SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 23
Download to read offline
Child Language Teaching and Therapy
2014, Vol. 30(1) 41­–62
© The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0265659013514070
clt.sagepub.com
The role of grammar in the writing
curriculum: A review of the
literature
Debra Myhill
University of Exeter, UK; University of Wollongong, Australia
Annabel Watson
University of Exeter, UK
Abstract
For most Anglophone countries, the history of grammar teaching over the past 50 years is one of
contestation, debate and dissent: and 50 years on we are no closer to reaching a consensus about
the role of grammar in the English/Language Arts curriculum. The debate has been described
through the metaphor of battle and grammar wars (Kamler, 1995; Locke, 2005), frequently pitting
educational professionals against politicians, but also pitting one professional against another. At
the heart of the debate are differing perspectives on the value of grammar for the language learner
and opposing views of what educational benefits learning grammar may or may not accrue. At the
present time, several jurisdictions, including England and Australia, are creating new mandates for
grammar in the curriculum. This article reviews the literature on the teaching of grammar and its
role in the curriculum and indicates an emerging consensus on a fully-theorized conceptualization
of grammar in the curriculum.
Keywords
Curriculum, grammar, metalanguage, metalinguistic understanding, writing
I Introduction
For most Anglophone countries, the history of grammar teaching over the past 50 years is one of
contestation, debate and dissent: and 50 years on we are no closer to reaching a consensus about
the role of grammar in the English/Language Arts curriculum. The debate has been described
through the metaphor of battle and grammar wars (Kamler, 1995; Locke, 2005), frequently pitting
Corresponding author:
Debra Myhill, University of Exeter, Heavitree Road, Exeter EX1 2LU, UK.
Email: d.a.myhill@ex.ac.uk
514070CLT30110.1177/0265659013514070Child Language Teaching and TherapyMyhill and Watson
research-article2013
Article
42	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
educational professionals against politicians, but also pitting one professional against another. At
the heart of the debate are differing perspectives on the value of grammar for the language learner
and opposing views of what educational benefits learning grammar may or may not accrue. The
debate has been well rehearsed elsewhere (Braddock et al., 1963; Cameron and Bourne, 1988;
Christie, 2004; Hartwell, 1985; Hudson and Walmsley, 2005; Locke, 2009; Myhill and Jones,
2011; QCA, 1998), and only a brief overview will be offered here in order to frame the review that
follows.
The Dartmouth Conference in the USA in 1966 signalled a turning point in educational thinking
about grammar in the curriculum. Funded by the Carnegie Endowment, and organized by the
Modern Language Association (MLA) and the National Conference of Teachers of English
(NCTE), the Dartmouth Conference brought together teachers and educational researchers from
the UK and the USA to discuss the grammar issue. The conference was prompted by growing dis-
satisfaction with classroom practice in grammar teaching, which was largely characterized by
drills and exercises in labelling and identifying word classes and syntactical structures, and which
to many education professionals had no educational relevance and no impact on language develop-
ment. As Muller noted, the general consensus was that grammar teaching was ‘a waste of time’
(1967: 68). As a consequence of the Dartmouth Conference, many educational jurisdictions in the
USA, the UK, Australia, New Zealand and Canada moved to exclude formal grammar teaching
from the English curriculum, although, of course, there will always have been those teachers who
continued to teach grammar, despite changed policy mandates.
The widespread abandoning of grammar teaching following the Dartmouth Conference
activated two further strands in the grammar debate. First, whilst curriculum policy might
have eschewed the teaching of grammar, an underlying sense of barely disguised outrage at its
abandonment has surfaced repeatedly over the years, often from politicians or the general
public. This strand of discourse tends to have been preoccupied with language standards and
a pervading view that lack of attention to grammar was causing falling standards in language
users (for example, Macdonald, 1995; Paterson, 2010; Truss, 2003; Uttley, 2008). It positions
grammar as a form of ‘verbal hygiene’ (Cameron, 1995), which assumes a moral significance
and acts as guardian to the moral standards of the nation (Cameron, 1994; Pullman, 2005).
Second, the abandoning of grammar has fostered a strand of thinking about the role of knowl-
edge about language in the curriculum. Here linguists have argued for the place of grammar
knowledge, including its associated metalanguage, as a worthy and relevant body of knowl-
edge in its own right (Hudson, 2004). At the same time, a broader view of knowledge about
language places the emphasis on more inductive approaches, which investigate language in
social contexts (for example, Barton, 1999; Carter, 1990; Denham and Lobeck, 2005; Keith,
1990; NATE, 1997).
This article is framed by this debate and by recent international developments that appear to be
re-introducing grammar to the language curriculum. Through a systematic review of the literature,
the article will provide a critical outline of research into the role of grammar in the English or
Language Arts curriculum.
1  Grammar in the first language curriculum
As noted earlier, there appears to be an emerging trend to reintroduce grammar in the teaching of
English, particularly in England, the USA and Australia. In England, the first National Curriculum
for English (DES, 1990) adopted a knowledge about language approach, though with an emphasis
on Standard English, and subsequent iterations of the National Curriculum (DfE, 1995; DfEE,
1999, 2000) have subtly shifted these emphases (for more detail, see Myhill and Jones, 2011). The
Myhill and Watson	 43
1995 version, for example, refers more specifically to grammatical metalanguage, whereas the
2007 version has rather more emphasis on grammar in functional contexts. However, an irony of
the National Curriculum is that, although a statutory instrument, actual classroom practice was
much more significantly influenced by the assessment framework and, as grammar was not tested,
the degree to which it is was taught was largely dependent on individual teachers’ own predilec-
tions. Currently, however, a new draft National Curriculum presents a grammar annex (DfE,
2013a), which outlines, year by year, the grammatical terminology that students must learn and the
grammatical structures that they must master. Moreover, a new national test of grammar, spelling
and punctuation (DfE, 2013b) was introduced in 2013, which as high-stakes assessment will inevi-
tably have a significant impact on what is taught.
A more significant influence on classroom practice in England was the (non-statutory) National
Literacy Strategy, introduced in 1998 (DfE, 1998), which matched to the requirements of the
National Curriculum, but which was also accompanied by substantial training and resources. The
teaching of grammar was strongly encouraged in training materials, including two training DVDs
(Grammar for reading and Grammar for writing) that explicitly addressed grammar and in a man-
ner that was clearly attempting to be contextualized. However, the specified learning objectives
and assessment focuses underpinning the strategy left teachers the freedom to determine an appro-
priate context. This led to many lessons being conducted in the ‘context’ of a writing lesson, but
the lessons effectively de-contextualized grammar (Myhill, 2004, 2006; Wyse, 2006). At sentence
level, in particular, this led to lessons on ‘using complex sentences’ or ‘using connectives’, where
deployment of a particular grammatical feature became more important than any understanding of
a relationship between grammar and meaning.
Perhaps one reason for this discrepancy between policy-level principles and materials and class-
room practice is because in England there has been no clear articulation of a rationale for this
renewed emphasis on grammar. In contrast, in Australia where a National Curriculum is currently
being developed for the first time, the structure of the proposed English curriculum includes a
Language strand that provides for explicit grammatical knowledge: the articulated, purposeful, and
conscious understanding of how language works, which supports learners in ‘knowing how to
choose words and grammatical and textual structures that are more appropriate to the audience or
readership’ (ACARA, 2009: 3). The inclusion of a Language strand is built upon a rationale that
seeks to foster ‘a coherent, dynamic, and evolving body of knowledge about the English language
and how it works’ (ACARA, 2009: 10).
To an extent, the position in the USA is more akin to England than Australia, although there
is no National Curriculum mandating content. Instead, state intervention is principally stand-
ards-driven through the Common Core Standards (CCSSI, 2012), now adopted by all but five
US states. The Common Core Standards specify Language as a discrete standard, and two of the
three Language Anchor standards relate to accuracy and avoidance of error. Language Standard
1 requires command of ‘the conventions of standard English grammar and usage when writing
or speaking’ and, in similar vein, Language Standard 2 requires command of ‘the conventions
of standard English capitalization, punctuation, and spelling when writing’. However, Language
Strand 3 is considerably more meaning or pragmatics oriented, looking at knowledge of lan-
guage ‘to understand how language functions in different contexts, to make effective choices
for meaning or style, and to comprehend more fully when reading or listening’ (CCSSI, 2012).
The list of progressive skills in language that students have to master is principally directed
towards accuracy, yet teachers are reminded of the inseparability of language study from read-
ing, writing, speaking and listening contexts, and that students ‘must also be able to make
informed, skillful choices among the many ways to express themselves through language’
(CCSSI, 2012).
44	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
What is evident from this brief consideration of three Anglophone jurisdictions is that the peda-
gogical rationale for the re-emergence of grammar is not yet fully clear, particularly in England and
the USA. At policy level, the reasons for maintaining or re-introducing grammar appear to be
neither evidence based, nor clearly articulated. Indeed, many opposed to the re-introduction of
grammar would highlight the substantial body of research (reviewed later in this article) that points
very conclusively to the ineffectiveness of de-contextualized grammar in helping students improve
their writing. The new National Curriculum in England exemplifies this uncertainty, both in terms
of the absence of an evidence base and the ambiguity of the rationale for its inclusion. For example,
it mandates that children ‘learn the correct grammatical terms in English and that these terms are
integrated within teaching’ (DfE, 2013: 5) signalling simultaneously a suggestion that a rationale
for the inclusion of grammar is the learning of a discrete body of linguistic knowledge (which is,
of course, a defensible curriculum position to adopt) but also that this teaching should be ‘inte-
grated within teaching’. The meaning of ‘integrated’ is unclear as it would be impossible to teach
without integrating it into teaching, but it is perhaps implying a sense that the grammar should be
contextualized. If that is the case then the learning purpose of this is not communicated. Elsewhere,
the document argues that:
once pupils are familiar with a grammatical concept [for example ‘modal verb’], they should be encouraged
to apply and explore this concept in the grammar of their own speech and writing and to note where it is
used by others. (DfE, 2013c: 64)
There is no evidence base to support this assertion that learning progresses chronologically from
learning a grammatical concept to being able to apply it.
In non-English speaking countries, however, the position of grammar in the curriculum is
frequently neither contentious nor questioned. In both Europe and Asia, the teaching of gram-
mar as part of first language teaching is largely regarded as the norm; for example, van
Gelderen (2010) notes the routine presence of grammar in Dutch teaching. In the French-
speaking world, grammar is heavily drawn upon in order to develop students’ understanding
of the inflections in French spelling (Fayol et al., 1993, 1994, 2009) and this grammar is
based upon traditional explanations of rules that govern language patterns. However, in
Canada especially, grammaire nouvelle (‘new grammar’) is adopted as an alternative to tra-
ditional grammar teaching in French as a first language. Grammaire nouvelle is essentially
an inductive approach to securing grammatical understanding, with a reduced emphasis on
grammatical metalanguage, and a greater emphasis on fostering student observation, reflec-
tion, and active participation in exploring language (Poulin, 1980). Leger (no date) summa-
rizes the approach as:
a grammar of observation and reasoning based on effective manipulations, amongst other things, of words
and groups of words before analysing them and understanding their function. The teaching offers us
knowledge of French structures and allows us to build authentic procedures of reflection which drive
independence. [my translation] (Leger, no date: p. 3)
The manipulations that learners are encouraged to undertake involve subtraction, substitution, per-
mutation and transformation, designed to help develop independent grammatical understanding
(Boivin, 2009; Boivin and Pinsonneault, 2008; Nadeau and Fisher, 2009, 2011). Curiously,
although there are some resonances between grammaire nouvelle and a ‘grammar in context’
approach, there has been little cross-fertilization of ideas across the English–French language
borders.
Myhill and Watson	 45
2 Theoretical perspectives on the teaching of grammar
The historical tendency to focus consideration of grammar in the curriculum on whether it should be
included has led to a somewhat impoverished theoretical base for conceptualizing a role for grammar.
Often the ‘grammar debate’ has been framed by polemic (Macdonald, 1995) and claim and counter-
claim (Anderson, 1995; Cameron and Bourne, 1998; Clark, 2010; Crowley, 2003; Harris, 1962;
Hudson, 2004; Robinson, 1959; Sealey, 1999; Tomlinson, 1994; Wyse, 2001), but there is relatively
little coherent and developed articulation of the contribution that grammatical understanding might
make to students’learning about language.As noted earlier, there is a folk theorization of grammar as
central to supporting students’ accuracy as language users and in the eradication of errors from their
speech and writing. In essence, this is a theorization that gives primacy to Standard English and posi-
tions grammar as the tool by which Standard English is maintained. Drawing on historical discourses
that advocated school as the place where the ‘evil habits of speech contracted in the home and the
street’ (Board of Education [Newbolt Report] 1921: 59) could be overcome, and where school can
compensate for the linguistic disadvantage of the home (Bernstein, 1971), this perspective on gram-
mar tends to be adopted by those outside the profession. It is a largely prescriptive view of grammar,
focused on form, and predicated on assumptions that grammar prescribes how language should be
used and acts as the benchmark by which deviations should be judged. Theoretical arguments against
this position are more robust than those for it and include socio-cultural and socio-linguistic analyses
that highlight the cultural hegemony of this stance and its lack of understanding of language variation
and the descriptive grammars advocated by modern linguistics.
In contrast to this rather ill defined, though strongly held, theorization of grammar is the
approach of Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) and the seminal work of Halliday and the
Sydney School. SFL is essentially a meaning-oriented theorization of grammar, concerned to
explore the relationship between text and context, and looking not simply at lexical or syntactical
aspects of grammar but also the discourse elements of organization, development and cohesion
(Christie and Derewianka, 2006; Christie and Unsworth, 2006; Halliday, 1993, 1994, 2003; Hasan,
2002). It regards language as fundamentally a social semiotic system, ‘abstract semiosis’(Halliday,
2003: 5), and is concerned with how language works or functions. Carter (1990), for example,
describes this approach as ‘functionally oriented, related to the study of texts and responsive to
social purposes’ (p. 104). SFL differs from other functional grammars in its emphasis upon what
Halliday (1978) has called the metafunctions of language the ideational, the interpersonal and the
textual. The ideational metafunction refers to the communicative message, i.e. ‘what a text is
about’; the interpersonal metafunction refers to ‘how the self is expressed’ and ‘how the reader is
understood’; and the textual metafunction refers to the structural aspects of text.
From the perspective of the classroom, Halliday’s argument that any act of communication
requires the making of choices and that ‘the power of language resides in its organization as a huge
network of interrelated choices’ (2003: 8) is a direct contrast to the idea of grammar as the arbiter
of accuracy. Derewianka and Jones (2010: 9) sum this up succinctly:
Whereas traditional approaches conceive of grammar as a set of structures which can be assessed as
correct or incorrect, Halliday sees language as a resource, a meaning-making system through which we
interactively shape and interpret our world and ourselves.
Teaching grammar through an SFL theoretical frame supports learners’ ability to think grammati-
cally about language (Freebody et al., 2008; Macken-Horarik et al., 2011; Williams, 2005), and to
understand grammar’s potentiality as a meaning-making resource (Coffin, 2010; Schleppegrell,
2007, 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011, 2012).
46	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Clearly, knowledge about language and grammar as a meaning-making resource is at the heart
of SFL. The same emphasis on meaning, rather than form, is evident in the theorization of rhetori-
cal grammar, although it does not adopt the metalanguage of SFL and is a rather more eclectic and
fragmented body of theorization than the strongly coherent work of SFL. The notion of rhetorical
grammar is strong in the USA and stands in contrast to traditional pedagogical approaches that
position grammar as a ‘fix-it approach to weak writing’ (Micciche, 2004: 716). Rhetorical gram-
mar conceives of grammar as a tool for crafting and shaping language and written text, in ways that
connect grammar to ‘rhetorical and stylistic effects’(Paraskevas, 2006: 65) and that enables writers
to ‘understand and control’ (Kolln, 2006: xi). On one level, rhetorical grammar is about explicitly
showing young writers the repertoire that is available to them, what Myhill et al. have called ‘a
repertoire of infinite possibilities’(Myhill, 2011a; Myhill et al., 2011), and supporting the ability to
make conscious choices (Dawkins, 1995; Hancock, 2009; Petit, 2003). On another level, investi-
gating the ways in which meanings are created in texts is about teaching thinking about ‘the inter-
woven relationship between what we say and how we say it’ (Micciche, 2004: 718).
Systemic Functional Linguistics and rhetorical approaches to grammar share a common focus on
developing understanding about how language works, rather than simply regarding grammar as a
body of knowledge that describes, or prescribes, the system of language. Theoretically, the knowl-
edge that these approaches foster is metalinguistic knowledge. The most comprehensive investiga-
tion of metalinguistic knowledge and development is that of Gombert (1992). He suggests that
metalinguistic knowledge moves from an epilinguistic stage, where the knowledge is unconscious or
implicit, to a metalinguistic stage, where it is explicit. Alternative perspectives on the stages of meta-
linguistic development have been offered by Culioli (1990), Karmiloff-Smith (1996) and van Lier
(1998). Gombert categorizes metalinguistic knowledge as addressing one of six areas: metaphono-
logical, metalexical, metasemantic, metasyntactic, metatextual, and metapragmatic, although Myhill
(2011b) suggests that in older readers and writers, this categorization may be less appropriate as they
are principally concerned with the metapragmatic, i.e. how texts communicate in different contexts.
However, the most significant body of work on metalinguistic knowledge or development relates to
second language learners (e.g. Bialystok, 2007; ter Kuile et al., 2011), to young learners acquiring
language (Downing and Oliver, 1974; Karmiloff Smith et al., 1996; Tunmer et al., 1983), to spelling
development (e.g. Bourassa et al., 2006; Caravolas et al., 2005; Nunes et al., 2006; Thévenin et al.,
1999), and to reading comprehension (Heibert et al., 1984; Macgillivray, 1994; Zipke, 2007).
Nonetheless, metalinguistic knowledge and understanding is central to any consideration of the
role of grammar in the English curriculum, particularly the notions of explicit and implicit knowl-
edge, as they underpin the differing views of the value of grammar teaching. Gombert (1992) main-
tained that epilinguistic, implicit, grammatical knowledge precedes metalinguistic knowledge, and it
includes the unconscious knowledge that language users acquire from communication and interac-
tion, such as word order or how to form the past tense. Roehr (2008) contrasts implicit knowledge
that ‘cannot be brought into awareness or articulated’ with explicit metalinguistic knowledge that is
‘declarative knowledge that can be brought into awareness and that is potentially available for verbal
report’ (p. 179). The historical disputes over the value of grammar teaching hinge upon this distinc-
tion and arguments that first language speakers acquire grammar implicitly and therefore need lan-
guage experience rather than grammatical knowledge (Elbow, 1991) or, conversely, that explicit
grammatical knowledge gives writers both choice and control over their language use (Carter, 1990).
II  Empirical studies on the teaching of grammar
Although the issue of the value of grammar teaching has often been framed in terms of the rele-
vance of explicit and implicit grammatical knowledge, it is also true that one strong line of
Myhill and Watson	 47
argument has been located in discussions of the impact of teaching grammar on students’ language
use, particularly writing. In other words, does explicit knowledge of grammar support writing
development and attainment in writing, or as Elbow argued, ‘nothing helps your writing so much
as ignoring grammar’ (Elbow, 1981).
Over the past 50 years, there have been a number of research reviews or meta-analyses address-
ing the effect of grammar teaching on students’ learning (Andrews, 2005; Andrews et al., 2006;
Braddock et al., 1963; Hillocks, 1984; Wyse, 2001), all of which have concluded that ‘the teaching
of school grammar has little or no effect on students’(Hillocks and Smith, 1991) or even that it has
a ‘harmful effect on the improvement of writing’ (Braddock et al., 1963: 37). Indeed, Graham and
Perin’s (2007) meta-analysis investigating effective strategies for teaching writing found a nega-
tive effect for ‘the explicit and systematic teaching of the parts of speech and structure of sen-
tences’and the authors argue that this finding challenges ‘some educators’enthusiasm for traditional
grammar instruction as a focus of writing instruction for adolescents’ (2007: 21). A review by the
Evidence for Policy and Practice Information and Co-ordinating Centre (EPPI, 2004) concluded
that it could find no evidence for any benefit of grammar teaching on writing quality, although the
authors did also note that ‘the quality of seven out of ten of the primary studies included in the in-
depth review on the teaching of syntax is a limitation on the review, as is the lack of recent research’
(Andrews et al., 2006: 51).
In the end, the EPPI review draws its conclusions based on the analysis of just three studies:
Bateman and Zidonis (1966), Elley et al. (1975, 1979), and Fogel and Ehri (2000). It is worth look-
ing at these three studies more closely. The Bateman and Zidonis study, in the USA, focused on
sentence production in writing and investigated whether high school students could ‘learn to apply
the transformational rules of a generative grammar in their writing?’ (1966: 3). The sample size
was rather small: 50 students assigned to either the control or intervention, of whom only 41 were
still in the sample at the end of the study. The intervention group were taught explicitly about syn-
tactical structures for sentences, and written compositions from the two groups were analysed
using an analytical tool that captured the generative transformational grammar rules of sentence
construction and allowed them to compare development pre-treatment and post-treatment. An out-
come of the analysis was a structural complexity score for each piece of writing (1966: 8). The
statistical results were not conclusive, but the authors suggest that they did indicate a relationship
between ‘a knowledge of generative grammar and an ability to produce well-formed sentences of
greater structural complexity’(1966: 39). In contrast to this small-scale study, the Elley et al. study
is arguably the most robust research conducted in this field. They conducted a three-year longitu-
dinal study in New Zealand, involving a sample of 250 secondary-aged students of average ability
with eight classes, matched for ability. There were three treatment groups:
•	 Treatment 1: a transformational grammar course focusing on discovering facts about lan-
guage and its use. The course included teaching of sentence-combining, subordination, par-
ticiple modifiers, and deep and surface structures.
•	 Treatment 2: a reading–writing course with 40% spent free reading, 40% spent using a class
reader, and 20% spent writing, especially creative writing.
•	 Treatment 3: a course typical of New Zealand secondary English classes at the time, involv-
ing functional grammar, comprehension, and writing.
They found that there were no significant differences in the quality of writing produced post-test,
although the transformational grammar group did improve in their sentence-combining ability.
The Fogel and Ehri study (2000) was different from the previous two studies as it took as its
starting point an identified writing problem, the use of Black English Vernacular (BEV) in the
48	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
writing of low-attaining ethnic minority students in primary school. The study set out to ‘examine
how to structure dialect instruction so that it is effective in teaching SE [Standard English] forms
to students who use BEV in their writing’ (2000: 215). An experimental design was adopted with
three treatment groups:
•	 exposure plus text;
•	 exposure plus explicit instruction in strategies; and
•	 exposure, strategies, and guided practice in transforming BEV to SE and feedback (ESP)
According to the authors, treatments 1 and 2 mirror typical classroom practice. The results indi-
cated a statistically significant positive effect for group 3, leading the authors to conclude that the
guided practice had ‘clarified for students the link between features in their own non-standard
writing and features in SE’ (2000: 231). However, because of the small sample size, the EPPI
review does not rate this as strong evidence for a positive benefit.
The EPPI review noted the dearth of research in this area, but a much more recent study, con-
ducted in primary schools in the UK by Sheard et al. (2012), would appear to confirm its conclu-
sions. They investigated whether teaching grammar using electronic handsets that provided
students with instant feedback would improve their learning of grammatical terminology and
whether it would improve their writing. They conducted a 12 week study, drawing on a sample of
42 primary schools, half of which were randomly assigned to use the electronic handsets and the
other as a control group. Grammatical knowledge was tested before and after the intervention using
discrete grammar questions and a paragraph revision writing task to ascertain whether the grammar
knowledge transferred to writing. The results indicated a strongly significant positive impact on
grammar knowledge on the experimental group, using the handsets, but no impact on writing.
However, one aspect of grammar teaching that has been repeatedly identified as successful is
sentence-combining, the practice of ‘teaching students to construct more complex and sophisti-
cated sentences through exercises in which two or more basic sentences are combined into a single
sentence’(Graham and Perin, 2007: 18). The EPPI review (2004; Andrews et al., 2006) maintained
that there was good evidence for the efficacy of sentence-combining, and it was one of the 11
instructional strategies identified by Graham and Perin (2007) as beneficial to writing. Sentence-
combining has a long history in the USA with a chain of studies (Daiker et al., 1978; Hake and
Williams, 1979; Hillocks and Mavrognes, 1986; Kinneavy, 1979; Marzano, 1976; O’Hare, 1971;
Phillips, 1996; Saddler and Graham, 2005; Savage, 1980) all reporting the success of sentence-
combining. Most were conducted in high schools or even at university level. The studies were
themselves followed by a flurry of similar studies (for an overview of this, see Connors, 2000),
most agreeing and confirming the efficacy of the technique. The only UK study to look at this, with
secondary school students (Keen, 2004), also found a positive impact. But despite this apparent
consistency in results, sentence-combining is problematic in terms of both the studies and the
teaching of writing. In general, the studies measure whether teaching students to combine sen-
tences, largely through exercises, improves their ability to create effectively combined sentences,
with very few studies looking at whether writing quality is improved. Several critics have pointed
out the shortcomings in these studies (Connors, 2000; Faigley, 1980; Witte, 1980), and Crowhurst
(1980) conducted a study that demonstrated that increased syntactic complexity, such as that
achieved through sentence-combining, was not correlated with writing quality. In addition, critics
have suggested that any success deriving from sentence-combining is attributable to discussions
about the ‘rhetorical principles characteristic of good writing’(1980: 298), not to the strategy itself.
Significantly, for example, Keen’s study included a considerable amount of contextualized discus-
sion of the effectiveness of different sentences. Summarizing the approach, Andrews et al. argued
Myhill and Watson	 49
that ‘sentence-combining suggests a pedagogy of applied knowledge – at its best, applied in situa-
tions of contextualized learning; at its worst, drilling’ (2006: 52), and it is probable that Andrews’
notion of applied knowledge rooted in contextualized learning is central to this issue.
Andrews’ observation that the success of sentence-combining might owe more to its contextu-
alization than to the approach per se is important. More recently, research in this area has begun to
look at the benefits of teaching grammar that is contextualized or embedded within the teaching of
writing. In the USA, Fearn and Farnan (2007) found strong positive effects on students’ writing in
high schools when teaching made connections between the grammar being taught and children’s
writing, and they claimed that ‘grammar instruction influences writing performance when gram-
mar and writing share one instructional context’(Fearn and Farnan, 2007: 16). In England, a mixed
method study, involving 13–14 year olds, and combining a large randomized controlled trial with
a qualitative design has found a strongly significant benefit for students of a contextualized
approach (Jones et al., 2013; Myhill et al., 2012). In this study, the instructional focus was the
teaching of writing, and the teaching materials were designed around a consideration of what
grammatical understanding might support learners in making authorial decisions in the composi-
tion of a particular text. It is the first robust study that has provided evidence of a positive impact
of grammar teaching on writing, but it is important to note that the learning focus was writing, not
grammar. Nonetheless, it offers a coherent and evidenced rationale for developing a purposeful
role for grammar in the curriculum.
III  Teacher perspectives on the teaching of grammar
Further insights into the role of grammar in the curriculum are provided by research studies or
professional publications that attend to teachers’ perspectives on the topic. Professional perspec-
tives on grammar are evident in numerous articles by teachers and teacher-educators, particularly
in the special ‘grammar’ editions of the English Journal, namely volume 85 (1996), volume 92
(2003) and volume 95 (2006). These articles tend to fall in to two camps: those which position
grammar predominantly as a matter of rule-learning and error-correction (e.g. Benjamin et al.,
2006; Brown, 1996; Rose, 1996), and those which position grammar as a tool for promoting meta-
linguistic awareness and rhetorical choice (e.g. Ehrenworth, 2003; Gold, 2006; Hagemann, 2003;
Jayman et al., 2006). The latter position is often informed, in articles from US practitioners, by
Weaver’s concept of contextualized grammar (1996, 2006) and Kolln’s concept of rhetorical gram-
mar (2006).
The professional literature reflects the complexity of defining what is meant by the terms ‘gram-
mar’ and ‘grammar teaching’, as Vavra (1996) notes. A handful of studies have explored how
teachers define or conceptualize grammar. The largest, a survey of 137 primary and secondary
school teachers in the UK, reported that teachers lacked confidence in defining grammar, particu-
larly in understanding ‘the relationship between implicit and explicit knowledge of language’, and
noted a strong association of explicit grammar teaching with prescriptivism and old-fashioned
teaching methods (QCA, 1998: 26). A similar problem of definition has been reported in the USA
from a smaller-scale study by Petruzella which noted confusion between grammatical rules, issues
of usage, and spelling and punctuation (1996: 69). This finding was echoed in Cajkler and Hislam’s
interviews with trainee primary teachers in the UK, with participants associating grammar with
phonics, spelling and punctuation (2002). UK studies by Pomphrey and Moger (1999) and Watson
(2012) report tension or inconsistency between prescriptive/descriptive and prescriptive/rhetorical
conceptualizations of grammar teaching among trainee and practising secondary English teachers
respectively, with the latter finding that teachers tend to immediately conceptualize grammar as
prescriptive, rule-bound and focused on correctness, while conversely offering a rhetorical view
50	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
focused on exploration, choice and effects when asked about how it might be useful in supporting
students’ writing development. All of these studies indicate that some degree of conceptual confu-
sion persists within the profession.
Research into teachers’ beliefs about grammar more generally provide evidence that UK teach-
ers tend to associate grammar with negative discourses of ‘old-fashioned’ teaching (QCA, 1998;
Watson, 2012), and that these ‘neo-conservative associations’(Myhill, 2006: 78) can occur even in
countries in which grammar is not such a contested subject, such as Flanders and the Netherlands
(van Gelderen, 2006). Investigations into teachers’ affective responses to grammar have typically
reported ‘anxiety’ (QCA, 1998: 26) and ‘apprehension’ (Watson, 2012), and a general lack of con-
fidence when it comes to dealing with grammar in the classroom, in both the UK (Kelly and
Safford, 2009) and USA (Hadjioannou and Hutchinson, 2010). Teachers and trainee teachers have
tended to be disparaging and hyper-critical in their self-evaluations (Pomphrey and Moger, 1999;
Watson, 2012), and Cajkler and Hislam have found that anxiety remains high even when trainee
teachers made improvements in their linguistic subject knowledge (2002). Teachers who feel anx-
ious or insecure about their own declarative grammatical knowledge are also more likely to hold
prescriptivist, rule-bound views of grammar (Harper and Rennie, 2009; Kamler, 1995; Macken-
Horarik, 2001). However, studies have also reported more positive attitudes. Both Turvey (2000),
working with secondary-level trainee teachers, and Cajkler and Hislam with primary-level (2002)
found that, regardless of their anxieties about subject knowledge, trainees valued the idea of gram-
mar teaching: Turvey found that her students felt that they had ‘missed out on something’ by not
being taught grammar themselves at school, and that this made it ‘all the more important that their
pupils should have it’ (2000: 143). Similarly, Watson found that some secondary English teachers
felt passionately that the negative discourse around grammar teaching is misleading, and that far
from being ‘the pit of doom’, grammar is actually ‘where freedom lies’, thanks to its focus on mak-
ing knowledge about language and the choices involved in writing explicit (2012: 32). Such reports
indicate that there may be willingness to learn about and practise grammar teaching even where
teachers or trainees are insecure in their own linguistic or pedagogical knowledge. The most recent
research into teachers’ beliefs has found that teachers tend to value the potential that a rhetorical
approach to grammar teaching focused on experimentation has to increase students’ metalinguistic
awareness and support them in consciously crafting their writing (Watson, 2012). There still per-
sists, however, a tendency to associate grammar with a restrictive discourse of rules and accuracy
(Watson, 2012) and to position grammar ‘as antipathetic to freedom and creativity’ (Wilson and
Myhill, 2012: 11).
Attitudes to the use of grammatical terminology have been explored in three studies. Two of
these indicate a belief that it is more suitable for use with higher rather than lower ability students
(Petruzella, 1996; QCA, 1998). The third study examined teachers’ personal epistemologies, their
‘beliefs about what knowledge is relevant and valuable in learning to write’ (Wilson and Myhill,
2012: 4), in relation to literary and linguistic metalanguage and poetry writing. This reported a
recurrent epistemological argument that ‘linguistic metalanguage was rule-bound and constrain-
ing, limiting young people’s writing in an unconstructive fashion’ (Wilson and Myhill, 2012: 10),
which counterpointed a view that literary metalanguage is ‘a valuable part of learning about litera-
ture’ (p. 13). This is consistent with Findlay’s findings that UK teachers regard grammar as ‘a
chore’ (2010: 4), and that they consistently value the literary aspects of the subject above the
linguistic.
The literature, therefore, indicates a continuing trend amongst practitioners to view grammar as
reactionary and restrictive, to value it less than literary aspects of the subject, and to be anxious
about teaching it. However, there is also evidence of a countervailing belief that grammar taught
Myhill and Watson	 51
with an exploratory and rhetorical approach can be valuable in increasing students’ metalinguistic
understanding and ability to consciously craft or design their writing.
IV  Teaching grammar and the demands on content and
pedagogical subject knowledge
While teachers’knowledge about grammar is not generally raised as a concern in non-Anglophone
countries, historical factors mean that it is a significant issue for many first-language English
teachers. Shulman (1987) has distinguished between subject content knowledge (knowledge of an
academic domain) and pedagogical content knowledge (knowledge of how to teach that academic
domain). The demands on teachers’ subject content knowledge about grammar have become
increasingly specific in Anglophone countries: in the USA, the Common Core State Standards for
Language Arts are heavily focused on grammatical constructions that students are expected to
master; in the UK grammar was made a mandatory part of the curriculum in 1988, with the non-
statutory but widely adopted National Strategies guidance (DfEE, 1998; DfES, 2001) including
detailed teaching objectives for students aged 5–14; Australia is currently developing a new
National Curriculum that includes a strand on Knowledge about Language aiming to foster ‘a
coherent, dynamic, and evolving body of knowledge about the English language and how it works’
(ACARA, 2009: 1); and in New Zealand, problems have occurred in trying to implement an inno-
vative syllabus with a strong grammar focus due to teachers’ ‘lack of knowledge about language’
(Gordon, 2005: 63).
While the extent to which students need to explicitly know about grammar is still hotly debated
(Locke, 2010), there is widespread agreement that teachers’ grammatical knowledge needs to be
richer and more substantive than the grammar they may need to teach to students. Teacher subject
knowledge requires ‘a higher degree of grammar consciousness than most direct learners are likely
to need or want’(Leech, 1994: 18), with an ability to be ‘conscious analysts of linguistic processes’
(Brumfit, 1997: 163) and possess ‘conscious awareness’ (Armstrong, 2004: 223) of how texts are
structured. It has been argued that teachers who understand grammatical forms may be better
placed to support developing writers (Andrews, 2005), to identify linguistic development in their
students (Gordon, 2005), and to ‘make the analysis explicit’ (Hudson, 2004: 113) when examining
texts with their students.
The problem that current English teachers have with attaining the level of grammatical subject
knowledge outlined above has arisen for two reasons: first, the fall from favour of grammar teach-
ing in Anglophone countries following the Dartmouth Conference in the USA in 1966, due to the
widespread view that the formal teaching of grammar had no beneficial impact on students’ lin-
guistic facility (Hudson and Walmsley, 2005); second, the tendency in the UK for teachers to fol-
low a literature degree route into teaching, along with is a shortage of applicants from a linguistics
route (Shortis and Blake, 2010). As a result, many current English teachers were not taught gram-
mar at school or university, a point also noted in the US context by Kolln and Hancock (2005), and
by Gordon in New Zealand (2005). Of course, teachers who have literature degrees and are keen
readers themselves do have a lot of knowledge about texts that they can draw on in language teach-
ing, and they also have a substantial amount of implicit grammatical knowledge about texts.
However, as Andrews (2005) points out, it is likely that ‘a teacher with a rich knowledge of gram-
matical constructions and a more general awareness of the forms and varieties of the language will
be in a better position to help young writers’ (2005: 75).
This subject knowledge problem extends into initial teacher training courses, with Kolln and
Hancock (2005) complaining that most pre-service programs for English teachers in the USA do
52	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
not address grammatical knowledge, and a number of UK studies reporting weaknesses in gram-
matical knowledge (Andrews, 1994, 1999; Bloor, 1986; Burgess et al., 2000; Chandler et al., 1988;
Hislam and Cajkler, 2006; Williamson and Hardman, 1995; Wray, 1993). In Australia, Louden et
al. (2005) conducted a survey which indicated that teachers do not feel confident about teaching
grammar when they complete their training, and Harper and Rennie’s pre-service teachers (2009)
‘showed limited understandings in their ability to analyse the parts and structure of sentences, and
their knowledge of metalinguistic terms did not seem to extend past the basic concepts of “noun”,
“verb” and “adjective” ’ (2009: 27). However, studies in the UK and Australia have indicated that
some teachers may be over-critical or over-anxious in their self-evaluations, with Cajkler and
Hislam (2002) finding that primary trainee teachers in the UK had reasonable knowledge of gram-
mar despite being anxious about their subject knowledge, and Hammond and Macken-Horarik
finding that primary teachers in Australia were confident in their knowledge of genres and text
types despite expressing a lack of confidence in their own knowledge of ‘rules of traditional gram-
mar’ (2001: 125). At the same time, in England, there has been criticism of the accuracy or appro-
priacy of curriculum materials prepared to support teaching grammar (Cajkler, 1999, 2002, 2004;
Cameron, 1997).
The combination of curricular expectation that students will have explicit knowledge of gram-
mar, and the tendency towards an absence of grammatical knowledge in the academic experiences
of English teachers, creates a clear challenge for pedagogical practice and student learning.
Research into teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge related to grammar teaching is limited, yet
there is evidence from second-language research that strong declarative grammatical knowledge
does not necessarily translate simply into effective teaching of language (Andrews, 2001; Borg,
1999, 2001, 2003). Pedagogical content knowledge is dependent upon teachers having a clear
understanding of the role that grammatical knowledge can play in the classroom, without which
teachers may generate mismatches between their own use of grammatical metalanguage and stu-
dents’ understanding (Berry, 1997) or unwittingly convey inappropriate messages to learners.
Lefstein (2009), for example, illustrates how teachers using policy materials underpinned by a
principally rhetorical notion of grammar can use them in meaningless or rule-bound prescriptive
ways, while the trainees in Cajkler and Hislam’s study (2002) understood grammatical knowledge
as essentially about the naming of grammatical constructions but did not understand that pedagogi-
cally ‘grammatical awareness is about making available a range of choices for writers to use for
particular purposes in particular contexts’ (2002: 176). In a more recent UK study, Myhill et al.
(2013) have identified a range of pedagogical problems faced by teachers who attempt to address
grammar. These include:
•	 the difficulty of defining and explaining grammatical terms, exacerbated by teachers’ ten-
dency to use semantic rather than functional definitions; also reported by Paraskevas
(2004);
•	 the difficulty of explaining sentences and syntactical features, exacerbated by the tendency to
reduce explanations of sentence types to issues of length and to use other non-grammatical
explanations;
•	 the difficulty of making meaningful connections between grammar and writing, seen par-
ticularly in the tendency to communicate highly generalized principles for writing that are
difficult for learners to operationalize meaningfully, such as ‘vary your sentences’, and the
tendency to talk vaguely and non-specifically about ‘effects’; and
•	 the difficulty of fostering metalinguistic discussion, a risky activity that requires teachers to
deal with misunderstandings, unexpected questions and unexpected answers without
advance preparation.
Myhill and Watson	 53
This study also, however, reported examples of teachers who were able to make meaningful con-
nections between the linguistic features being studied and specific context-relevant effects or pur-
poses, and who responded sensitively to students’ writing by explicitly drawing out effective
grammatical choices and challenging students to reflect metalinguistically on their own writing.
This focus on the relationship between teachers’ subject knowledge and pedagogical knowledge
relating to grammar is mirrored in Australia by Jones and Chen (2012), who have demonstrated
how teachers have been able to evolve new understandings of both grammatical knowledge and
pedagogical practice through participatory collaboration with a research team. Current research
therefore indicates the vital importance of addressing teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge
alongside subject knowledge of grammar.
V  Discussion
To an extent, what this review signals most clearly is that consideration of a fully theorized role for
grammar within the writing curriculum has largely been hijacked by political–professional debates
about grammar’s inclusion or not, and that the debate has not substantially developed since it
began in the early 1960s. The same arguments are voiced and re-voiced over time, but with little
re-theorization or advancement. Silently underpinning this long-standing debate, and perhaps
perpetuating it, is the fundamental distinction between prescriptive and descriptive views of
grammar. Whilst linguists have long theorized grammar as a description of language which will
change and evolve as language changes, public and political views of grammar have tended
strongly to prescriptive views of grammar. Hudson and Walmsley, in their analysis of the separate
development of academic linguistics and grammar teaching, draw attention to ‘an ever-widening
gap in England between the practice of professional grammarians, on the one hand, and the lay
public and practice in schools on the other’ (Hudson and Walmsley, 2005: 595). However, this
binary division between linguistics and educational perspectives on grammar is not wholly correct.
Following the eschewing of grammar teaching in Anglophone countries in the 1960s, an alterna-
tive view of grammar emerged that was characterized by a view of grammar as ‘knowledge about
language’, a view that was significantly influenced by sociolinguistics (e.g. the work of Peter
Trudgill; see, for example, Trudgill, 2000) and was descriptive in theorization. In England, the
emphasis in the Bullock Report (DES, 1975) on the importance of developing students’ under-
standing of language and its rejection of traditional grammar teaching was highly influential, and
more fully realized in the late 1980s in the government-commissioned national project, Language
in the National Curriculum (LINC), to investigate classroom approaches to developing knowledge
about language. The resultant materials, subsequently rejected by the government, drew heavily on
the work of Britton and Halliday, focusing on functionally oriented understandings of language in
context and on placing meaning at the centre of attention. These materials have had a significant
legacy: the dominant professional view of grammar within the writing curriculum is strongly ori-
ented towards a descriptive approach (Watson, 2012; Wyse, 2006). Therefore, it is possible to
argue that, despite the prolonged debate, one element of the theorization of grammar in the writing
curriculum from both a linguistic and theoretical perspective is that it is founded upon a theory of
descriptive, functionally oriented grammar. Given this foundation, the review also points to ways
in which this theorization may be developed further.
Within the theoretical perspectives offered by Systemic Functional Linguistics, and rhetorical
grammar – and the recent empirical work of Fearn and Farnan (2007), Macken-Horarik et al.
(2011), Myhill et al. (2012), and Schleppegrell (2007) – there are signs of an emerging consensus
that grammar may be important in developing learners’ understanding of how language works
and, specifically, how grammar choices are significant in shaping and constructing meaning.
54	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Cameron argued theoretically that ‘knowing grammar is knowing how more than knowing what’
(1997: 236), pointing out that grammatical terminology is simply the tool that facilitates lan-
guage investigation and analysis. To set this in a classroom context, this means that it is more
important to know how a passive construction alters the emphasis in information conveyed than
it is to know that it is a passive construction. The recent empirical work, described above, is
providing evidence of how this knowledge is realized in practice and how young writers develop
understanding that ‘it is through the rhetorical and syntactic forms they choose that the content
is constructed and evaluated’ (Schleppegrell, 2007: 122). This consensus is less concerned with
grammar as an arbiter of accuracy, and more concerned with ‘insights that go well beyond the
minimum needed to write conventionally or correctly’ (Hancock, 2009: 194). Carter and
McCarthy (2006: 7) draw attention to the notion of grammar as choice, as well as a grammar of
structure, and Myhill et al. (2011) have conceptualized the contextual teaching of grammar
within the writing curriculum as one that seeks to open up to young writers ‘a repertoire of infi-
nite possibilities’.
Taking this theorization one step further, fostering young writers’ awareness of the linguistic
choices available to them in writing and how those choices differently shape meaning is develop-
ing their metalinguistic knowledge of writing. Gombert’s (1992) taxonomy of metalinguistic
knowledge, as noted earlier, relates principally to oral language development, but the category of
metapragmatic knowledge is concerned with language in use, e.g. language in social contexts.
Arguably, adopting descriptive approaches to grammar and nurturing students’ abilities to make
choices and decisions in their writing is developing this metapragmatic knowledge, and rendering
it available to inform the process of writing. Such knowledge is explicit, what Roehr described as
‘declarative knowledge that can be brought into awareness’ (Roehr, 2008: 179) and goes beyond
the use of linguistic metalanguage to label and identify to include explicit knowledge of how lin-
guistic choices subtly shape or alter meanings. It may also be significant that the empirical studies
of Keen (2004) on sentence-combining, of Wyse (2006) on vocabulary choices, and Myhill et al.
(2011) all seem to be highlighting the importance of talk, or metalinguistic discussion, in enabling
this explicit metalinguistic knowledge of writing. Thus a more coherent theorization of a role for
grammar in the curriculum might be framed as the teaching of grammar which promotes students’
explicit metalinguistic understanding of how grammar choices shape meaning in texts and of the
writing choices available to them, founded upon a descriptive, functionally oriented understanding
of grammar.
VI  Conclusions
This review of the literature has illustrated clearly that the role of grammar in first language teach-
ing remains a source of dispute, a conclusion underlined by the political/professional debate in
England at present about the grammar test at age 11 and the emphasis on grammar in the revised
National Curriculum. The review also signals that robust empirical studies in this area are limited
and – apart from indicating that simply teaching grammar as the isolated naming and labelling of
word classes and syntactical structures is of little obvious benefit – the research is rather impover-
ished in offering any theoretically-grounded understanding of children’s learning of grammar and
how they transfer that learning to their own language use. It offers a way forward in theorizing a
role for grammar in the writing curriculum as a functionally oriented endeavour, developing stu-
dents’ metalinguistic thinking and decision-making in writing. It is also clear, however, that more
well designed studies in this area are much needed to provide richer and broader understanding of
how children develop metalinguistic understanding, how that learning transfers into their own
language use, and the pedagogies that support the development of that understanding.
Myhill and Watson	 55
Declaration of conflicting interest
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit
sectors.
References
Anderson H (1995) Monstrous plans? Standard English in the National Curriculum and Norwegian in the
‘Monsterplan’. English in Education 29: 14–19.
Andrews R (2005) Knowledge about the teaching of sentence grammar: The state of play. English Teaching:
Practice and Critique 4: 69–76.
Andrews R, Torgerson C, Beverton S, Freeman A, Locke T, Low G, Robinson A, and Zhu D (2006) The
effect of grammar teaching on writing development. British Educational Research Journal 32: 39–55.
Andrews S (1994) The grammatical knowledge and awareness of Hong Kong teachers of English. In: Bird
N, Falvey P, Tsui ABM, Allison D, and McNeill A (eds) Language and learning. Proceedings of ILEC
1993. Hong Kong: Institute of Language in Education, Education Department, 508–20.
Andrews S (1999) All these like little name things: A comparative study of language teachers’ explicit knowl-
edge of grammar and grammatical terminology. Language Awareness 8: 143–59.
Andrews S (2001) The language awareness of the L2 teacher: Its impact upon pedagogical practice. Language
Awareness 10: 75–90.
Armstrong K (2004) Sexing up the dossier: A semantic analysis of phrasal verbs for language teachers.
Language Awareness 14: 213–24.
Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (ACARA) (2009) Framing English. Sydney:
ACARA.
Barton G (1999) Grammar in context. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bateman DR and Zidonis FJ (1966) The effect of a study of transformational grammar on the writing of ninth
and tenth graders. Champagne, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Benjamin A and Jago C (2006) Teacher to Teacher: What is your most compelling reason for teaching gram-
mar? English Journal 95: 18–21.
Bernstein B (1971) Class, codes and control: Volume 1. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Berry R (1997) Teachers’ awareness of learners’ knowledge: The case of metalinguistic terminology.
Language Awareness 6: 136–46.
Bialystok E (2007) Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language
Learning 57: 45–77.
Bloor T (1986) What do language students know about grammar? British Journal of Language Teaching 24:
157–60.
Board of Education (1921) The teaching of English in England (Newbolt Report). London: HMSO.
Boivin M-C (2009) Manipulations syntaxiques et jugements de grammaticalité dans le travail en classe
d’élèves du secondaire [Syntactical manipulations and grammatical judgements in the work of second-
ary pupils]. In: Joaquim D and Simard C (ed.) Pratiques d’enseignement grammatical. Points de vue
de l’enseignant et de l’élève: Premier numéro de la collection Recherches en didactique du français.
Québec: Presses de l’Université Laval, 179–208.
Boivin M-C and Pinsonneault R (2008) Description grammaticale et transposition didactique: le cas des
infinitives et des participiales [Grammatical description and didactic transposition: The case of infini-
tives and participles]. Enjeux 71: 29–57.
Borg S (1999) The use of grammatical terminology in the second language classroom: A qualitative study of
teachers’ practices and cognitions. Applied Linguistics 20: 95–126.
Borg S (2001) Self-perception and practice in teaching grammar. ELT Journal 55: 21–29.
Borg S (2003) Teacher cognition in grammar teaching: A literature review. Language Awareness 12: 96–108.
56	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Bourassa D, Treiman R, and Kessler B (2006) Use of morphology in spelling by children with dyslexia and
typically developing children. Memory and Cognition 34: 703–14.
Braddock R, Lloyd-Jones R, and Schoer L (1963) Research in written composition. Urbana, IL: National
Council of Teachers of English.
Brumfit C (1997) The teacher as educational linguist. In: van Lier L and Corson D (eds) Encyclopedia of lan-
guage and education: Volume 6: Knowledge about language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers,
163–72.
Burgess T, Turvey A, and Quarshie R (2000) Teaching grammar: Working with student teachers. Changing
English 7: 7–21.
Cajkler W (1999) Misconceptions in the NLS: National literacy strategy or no linguistic sense? Use of English
50: 214–27.
Cajkler W (2002) Literacy across the curriculum at KS3: More muddle and confusion. Use of English 53:
151–64.
Cajkler W (2004) How a dead butler was killed: The way English national strategies maim grammatical parts.
Language and Education 18: 1–16.
Cajkler W and Hislam J (2002) Trainee teachers’ grammatical knowledge: The tension between public expec-
tations and individual competence. Language Awareness 11: 161–77.
Cameron D (1994) Dr Syntax and Mrs Grundy: Grammar, myths and morals. Changing English: studies in
reading and culture 2: 34–43.
Cameron D (1995) Verbal hygiene. London: Routledge.
Cameron D (1997) Sparing the rod: What teachers need to know about grammar. Changing English: studies
in reading and culture 4: 229–39.
Cameron D and Bourne J (1988) No common ground: Kingman, grammar and the nation. Language and
Education 2: 147–60.
Caravolas M, Kessler B, Hulme C, and Snowling M (2005) Effects of orthographic consistency, frequency,
and letter knowledge on children’s vowel spelling development. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology 92: 307–21.
Carter R (ed.) (1990) Knowledge about Language. London: Hodder and Stoughton.
Carter R and McCarthy M (2006) Cambridge grammar of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Chandler P, Robinson W, and Noyes P (1988) The level of linguistic knowledge and awareness amongst
student teachers training to be primary teachers. Language and Education 2: 161–74.
Christie F and Unsworth L (2006) Developing dimensions of an educational linguistics. In: Webster J,
Matthiessen C, and Hasan R (eds) Continuing discourse on language: A functional perspective. London:
Equinox, 217–50.
Clark U (2010) Grammar in the curriculum for English: What next? ChangingEnglish 17: 189–200.
Coffin C (2010) Language support in EAL contexts: Why systemic functional linguistics? Editorial. Special
Issue of NALDIC Quarterly: 2–5.
Common Core State Standards Initiative (CCSSI) (2012) Common Core State Standards for Language Arts.
Washington, DC: National Governors Association (NGA) Center and Council of Chief State School
Officers (CCSSO). Retrieved from: http://www.corestandards.org/ELA-Literacy (November 2013).
Connors RJ (2000) The erasure of the sentence. College Composition and Communication 52: 96–128.
Crowhurst M (1980) Syntactic complexity and teachers’ quality ratings of narrations and arguments. Research
in the Teaching of English 14: 223–31.
Crowley T (2003) Standard English and the politics of language. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Culioli A (1990) Pour une linguistique de l’enonciation: Tome 1 [Towards a linguistic enunciation: Volume
1]. Paris: Ophrys
Daiker DA, Kerek A, and Morenberg M (1978) Sentence-combining and syntactic maturity in freshman
English. College Composition and Communication 29: 36–41.
Christie F (2004) Revisiting some old themes: The role of grammar in the teaching of English. In: Foley JA
(ed.) Language, education and discourse: Functional approaches. London and New York: Continuum,
145–73.
Myhill and Watson	 57
Christie F and Derewianka B (2008) School discourse: Learning to write across the years of schooling.
London and New York: Continuum.
Dawkins J (1995) Teaching punctuation as a rhetorical tool. College Composition and Communication 46:
533–48.
Denham K and Lobeck A (2005) Language in the schools: Integrating linguistics into K12 teaching. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Derewianka B and Jones P (2010) From traditional to grammar to functional grammar: Bridging the divide.
Special Issue of NALDIC Quarterly: 6–15.
DES (Department of Education and Science) (1975) A language for life: The Bullock report. London: HMSO.
DES (Department of Education and Science) (1990) English in the National Curriculum. London: HMSO.
DfE (Department for Education) (1995) English in the National Curriculum. London: HMSO.
DfE (Department for Education) (2013a) The National Curriculum in England: Framework document:
Grammar annex. London: DfE. Retrieved from: https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/
uploads/attachment_data/file/210969/NC_framework_document_-_FINAL.pdf (November 2013).
DfE (Department for Education) (2013b) 2013 key stage 2 tests: English grammar, spelling, and punctuation
test. London: DfE. Retrieved from: http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/teachingandlearning/assess-
ment/keystage2/b00208296/ks2-2013/english-tests/grammar-punctuation-spelling-test (November
2013).
DfE (Department for Education) (2013c) English programmes of study: Key stages 1 and 2. DFE-00181-
2013. London: DfE.
DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (1998) The national literacy strategy: Framework for
teaching. London: DfEE.
DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (1999) The National Curriculum for England, English
Key Stages 1–4. London: DfEE.
DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (2000) The National Literacy Strategy: Grammar for
Writing. London: DfEE.
DfES (Department for Education and Skills) (2001) Framework for teaching English: Years 7, 8 and 9.
London: DfES.
Downing J and Oliver P (1974) The child’s concept of a word. Reading Research Quarterly 9: 568–82.
Ehrenworth M (2003) Grammar-comma-a new beginning. English Journal 92: 90–96.
Elbow P (1981) Writing with power: Techniques for mastering the writing process. New York: Oxford.
Elley WB, Barham IH, Lamb H, and Wylie M (1975) The role of grammar in a secondary school curriculum.
New Zealand Council for Educational Studies 10: 26–41.
Elley WB, Barham IH, Lamb H, and Wylie M (1979) The role of grammar in a secondary school curriculum:
Educational research series No 60. Wellington: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.
EPPI Review Group for English (2004) The effect of grammar teaching (syntax) in English on 5 to 16 year
olds’ accuracy and quality in written composition. London: Evidence for Policy and Practice Information
and Co-ordinating Centre (EPPI-Centre).
Faigley L (1980) Names in search of a concept: Maturity, fluency, complexity, and growth in written syntax.
College Composition and Communication 31: 291–300.
Fayol M, Largy P, and Lemaire P (1994) When cognitive overload enhances subject-verb agreement errors.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 47A: 473–64.
Fayol M, Zorman M, and Lété B (2009) Associations and dissociations in reading and spelling French:
Unexpectedly poor and good spellers. British Journal of Educational Psychology 2: 63–75.
Fayol M, Hickmann M, Bonnotte I, and Gombert JE (1993) The effects of narrative context on French verbal
inflections: A developmental perspective. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 22: 453–78.
Fearn L and Farnan N (2007) When is a verb using functional grammar to teach writing. Journal of Basic
Writing 26: 1–26.
Fogel H and Ehri LC (2000) Teaching elementary students who speak black English vernacular to write in
Standard English: Effects of dialect transformation practice. Contemporary Educational Psychology 25:
212–35.
58	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Freebody P, Maton K, and Martin J (2008) Talk, text, and knowledge in cumulative, integrated learning: A
response to ‘intellectual challenge’. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 31: 188–201.
Gold D (2006) But when do you teach grammar? Allaying community concerns about pedagogy. English
Journal 95: 42–47.
Gombert EJ (1992) Metalinguistic development. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Gordon E (2005) Grammar in New Zealand schools: Two case studies. English Teaching: Practice and
Critique 4: 48–68.
Graham S and Perin D (2007) A meta-analysis of writing instruction for adolescent students. Journal of
Educational Psychology 99: 445–76.
Hadjioannou X and Hutchinson M (2010) Putting the G back in English: Preparing pre-service teachers to
teach grammar. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 9: 90–105.
Hagemann JA (2003) Balancing content and form in the writing workshop. English Journal 92: 73–79.
Hake R and Williams JM (1979) Sentence expanding: Not can, or how, but when. In: Daiker D, Morenberg A,
and Kerek M (eds) Sentence-combining and the teaching of writing. Conway, AR: L&S Books, 134–46.
Halliday MAK (1978) Language as social semiotic: The social interpretation of language and meaning.
London: Edward Arnold.
Halliday MAK (1993) Towards a language-based theory of learning. 5: 93–116.
Halliday MAK (1994) An introduction to functional grammar. London: Hodder.
Halliday MAK (2003) Introduction: On the ‘architecture’ of human language. In: Webster J (ed.) On language
and linguistics: Volume 3 in the collected works of MAK Halliday. London and New York: Continuum,
1–29.
Hammond J and Macken-Horarik M (2001) Teachers’ voices, Teachers’ practices. Australian Journal of
Language and Literacy 24: 112–32.
Hancock C (2009) How Linguistics can Inform the Teaching of Writing. In: Beard R, Myhill D, Riley J, and
Nystrand M (eds) The Sage handbook of writing development. London: Sage, 194–208.
Harper H and Rennie J (2009) ‘I had to go out and get myself a book on grammar’: A study of pre-service
teachers’ knowledge about language. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 32: 22–37.
Harris RJ (1962) An experimental enquiry into the functions of and value of formal grammar in the teaching
of English. Unpublished PhD thesis, London University, UK.
Hartwell P (1985) Grammar, grammars, and the teaching of grammar. College English 47: 105–27.
Hasan R (2002) Ways of meaning, ways of learning: Code as an explanatory concept. British Journal of
Sociology of Education 23: 537–48.
Heibert E, Cioffi G, and Antonak R (1984) A developmental sequence in preschool children’s acquisition of
reading readiness skills and print awareness concepts. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 5:
115–26.
Hillocks G (1984) What works in teaching composition: A meta-analysis of experimental treatment studies.
American Journal of Education 93: 133–70.
Hillocks G and Mavrognes N (1986) Sentence combining. In: Hillocks G (ed.) Research on written composi-
tion: New directions for teaching. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, 142–46.
Hillocks G Jr and Smith M (1991) Grammar and usage. In: Flood J, Jensen JM, Lapp D, and Squire JS (eds)
Handbook of research on teaching the English language arts. New York: Macmillan.
Hislam J and Cajkler W (2006) Teacher trainees’ explicit knowledge of grammar and primary curricu-
lum requirements in England. In: Bartels N (ed.) Applied linguistics and language teacher education.
Netherlands: Springer, 295–312.
Hudson R (2004) Why education needs linguistics. Journal of Linguistics 40: 105–30.
Hudson R and Walmsley J (2005) The English patient: English grammar and teaching in the twentieth cen-
tury. Journal of Linguistics 41: 593–622.
Jayman J, Doolan L, Hoover M, Maas S, McHugh T, Mooney K, and Zepp A (2006) Sentence patterns:
Making meaning with a countywide grammar initiative. English Journal 95: 41–47.
Jones PT and Chen H (2012) Teachers’ knowledge about language: Issues of pedagogy and expertise.
Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 35: 147–72.
Myhill and Watson	 59
Jones SM, Myhill DA, and Bailey TC (2013) Grammar for writing? An investigation into the effect of contex-
tualised grammar teaching on student writing. Reading and Writing Online 26: 1241–63.
Kamler B (1995) The grammar wars or what do teachers need to know about grammar? English in Australia
114: 3–15.
Karmiloff-Smith A, Grant J, Sims K, Jones M-C, and Cuckle P (1996) Rethinking metalinguistic awareness:
Representing and accessing knowledge about what counts as a word. Cognition 58: 197–219.
Keen J (2004) Sentence-combining and redrafting processes in the writing of secondary school students in the
UK. Linguistics and Education: An International Research Journal 15: 17–97.
Keith G (1990) Language study at KS3. In: Carter R (ed.) Knowledge about language. London: Hodder and
Stoughton. 69–103.
Kelly A and Safford K (2009) Does teaching complex sentences have to be complicated? Lessons from chil-
dren’s online writing. Literacy 43: 118–22.
Kinneavy JL (1979) Sentence combining in a comprehensive language framework. In: Daiker D, Morenberg
A, and Kerek M (eds) Sentence-combining and the teaching of writing. Conway, AR: L&S Books,
60–76.
Kolln M (2006) Rhetorical grammar: Grammatical choices, rhetorical effects. New York: Longman.
Kolln M and Hancock C (2005) The story of English grammar in US Schools. English Teaching: Practice
and Critique 4: 11–31.
Leech GN (1994) Students’ grammar – teachers’ grammar – learners’ grammar. In: Bygate M, Tonkyn A, and
Williams E (eds) Grammar and the language teacher. New York: Prentice Hall, 17–30.
Lefstein A (2009) Rhetorical grammar and the grammar of schooling: Teaching ‘powerful verbs’ in the
English National Literacy Strategy. Linguistics and Education 20: 378–400.
Leger V (no date) Introduction à la nouvelle grammaire [An introduction to new grammar]. Retrieved from:
http://www.yumpu.com/fr/document/view/17000673/introduction-a-la-nouvelle-grammaire (November
2013).
Locke T (2005) Grammar wars: Beyond a truce. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 4: 1–10.
Locke T (2009) Grammar and writing: The international debate. In: Beard R, Myhill D, Nystrand M, and
Riley J (eds) International handbook of writing development. London: SAGE, 182–93.
Locke T (ed.) (2010) Beyond the grammar wars: A resource for teachers and students on developing lan-
guage knowledge in the English/literacy classroom. London: Routledge
Louden W, Rohl M, Gore J et al. (2005) Prepared to teach: An investigation into the preparation of teach-
ers to teach literacy and numeracy. Canberra, ACT: Australian Government Department of Education,
Science and Training.
MacDonald H (1995) Why Johnny can’t write: Teaching grammar and logic to college students. Public
Interest 120. Retrieved from: http://www.nationalaffairs.com/public_interest/detail/why-johnny-cant-
write (November 2013).
MacGillivray L (1994) Tacit shared understandings of a first-grade writing community. Journal of Reading
Behavior 26: 245–66.
Macken-Horarik M (2001) Something to shoot for: a systemic functional approach to teaching genre in sec-
ondary school science. In: Johns AM (ed.) Genre in the classroom: Multiple perspectives. London:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Macken-Horarik M, Love K, and Unsworth L (2011) A grammatics ‘good enough’ for school English in the
21st century: Four challenges in realising the potential. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy
34: 9–23.
Marzano RJ (1976) Sentence-combining myth. English Journal 65: 57–59.
Micciche L (2004) Making a case for rhetorical grammar. College Composition and Communication 55:
716–37.
Muller HJ (1967) The uses of English: Guidelines for the teaching of English from the Anglo-American con-
ference at Dartmouth College. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Myhill DA (2004) Making connections: Grammar and meaning. Secondary English Magazine 8: 23–26.
Myhill DA (2006) Designs on writing 2: Sentences. Secondary English Magazine 10: 25–28.
60	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Myhill DA (2011a) Grammar for designers: How grammar supports the development of writing. In: Ellis
S, McCartney E, and Bourne J (eds) Insight and impact: Applied linguistics and the primary school.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 81–92.
Myhill DA (2011b) ‘The ordeal of deliberate choice’: Metalinguistic development in secondary writers. In:
Berninger V (ed.) Past, present, and future contributions of cognitive writing research to cognitive psy-
chology. Psychology Press/Taylor Francis Group, 247–74.
Myhill DA and Jones SM (2011) Policing grammar: The place of grammar in literacy policy. In: Goodwyn A
and Fuller C (eds) The literacy game. London: Routledge, 45–62.
Myhill DA, Jones S, and Watson A (2013) Grammar matters: How teachers’ grammatical subject knowledge
impacts on the teaching of writing. Teaching and Teacher Education 36: 77–91. Retrieved from: http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2013.07.0052 (November 2103).
Myhill DA, Lines HE, and Watson A (2011) Making meaning with grammar: A repertoire of possibilities.
mETAphor 2: 1–10.
Myhill DA, Jones SM, Lines H, and Watson A (2012) Re-thinking grammar: The impact of embedded
grammar teaching on students’ writing and students’ metalinguistic understanding. Research Papers in
Education 27: 1–28.
Nadeau M and Fisher C (2009) Faut-il des connaissances explicites en grammaire pour réussir les accords en
français écrit? [Is explicit grammar knowledge needed to succeed with agreements in written French?].
In: Dolz J and Simard C (eds) Pratiques d’enseignement grammatical, points de vue de l’enseignant et
de l’élève; Recherches en didactique du français [Practices in grammar teaching, perspectives from
teachers and students; Research in teaching French]. Quebec: Presses de l’Université Laval, 209–31.
Nadeau M and Fisher C (2011) Les connaissances implicites et explicites en grammaire: Quelle impor-
tance pour l’enseignement? Quelles conséquences? [Implicit and explicit grammar knowledge: What
is the significance for teaching? What consequences?]. Bellaterra Journal of Teaching and Learning
Language and Literature 4: 1–31.
National Association for the Teaching of English (NATE) (1997) Position Paper No 1: Grammar. Sheffield:
NATE.
Nunes T, Bryant P, and Bindman M (2006) The effects of learning to spell on children’s awareness of mor-
phology. Reading and Writing 19: 767–87.
O’Hare F (1971) Sentence combining: Improving student writing without formal grammar instruction. NCTE
Research Report 15. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Paraskevas C (2004) Learning about grammar. International Journal of Learning 11: 93–99.
Paraskevas C (2006) Grammar apprenticeship. English Journal 95: 65–69.
Petit A (2003) The stylish semicolon: Teaching punctuation as rhetorical choice. English Journal 92: 66–72.
Petruzella BA (1996) Grammar instruction: What teachers say. English Journal 85: 68–72.
Phillips SE (1996) Sentence combining: A literature review. ERIC document number ED398589. Unpublished
literature review, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA.
Pomphrey C and Moger R (1999) Cross-subject dialogue about language: Attitudes and perceptions of PGCE
students of English and Modern Languages. Language Awareness 8: 223–36.
Poulin D (1980) Grammaire traditionnelle et grammaire nouvelle, ou De l’analyse à l’analyse structural
[Traditional grammar and new grammar, or From analysis to structural analysis]. Quebec Français 40:
29–32.
Pullman P (2005) Common sense has much to learn from moonshine. The Guardian 22 January 2005. Retrieved
from: http://www.guardian.co.uk/education/2005/jan/22/schools.wordsandlanguage (November 2013).
QCA (Qualifications | Curriculum Authority) (1998) The grammar papers: Perspectives on the teaching of
grammar in the National Curriculum. London: QCA.
Robinson N (1959) The relation between knowledge of English Grammar and ability in English composi-
tions. Unpublished MEd thesis, University of Manchester, UK.
Roehr K (2008) Metalinguistic knowledge and language ability in university-level L2 learners. Applied
Linguistics 29: 173–99.
Rose C (1996) The great debate: Teaching grammar and usage. English Journal 85: 59–63.
Myhill and Watson	 61
Saddler B and Graham S (2005) The effects of peer-assisted sentence-combining instruction on the writing
performance of more and less skilled young writers. Journal of Educational Psychology 97: 43–54.
Savage J (1980) Sentence combining: A positive practice. Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates.
Schleppegrell MJ (2007) The meaning in grammar. Research in the Teaching of English 42: 121–28.
Schleppegrell MJ (2008) Grammar, the sentence, and traditions of linguistic analysis. In: Bazerman C (ed.)
Handbook of writing research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 549–64.
Schleppegrell MJ (2009) Grammar for generation 1.5: A focus on meaning. In: Roberge M, Siegal M, and
Harklau L (eds) Generation 1.5 in college composition: Teaching academic writing to US-educated
learners of ESL. New York: Routledge, 221–34.
Schleppegrell MJ (2010) Supporting a ‘reading to write’ pedagogy with functional grammar. NALDIC
Quarterly 8: 26–31.
Schleppegrell MJ (2011) Supporting disciplinary learning through language analysis: Developing historical
literacy. In: Christie F and Maton K (eds) Disciplinarity: Functional linguistic and sociological perspec-
tives. London: Continuum, 197–216.
Schleppegrell MJ (2012) Systemic functional linguistics: Exploring meaning in language. In; Gee JP and
Handford M (eds) The Routledge handbook of discourse analysis. New York: Routledge, 21–34.
Sealey A (1999) Teaching primary school children about the English language: A critique of current policy
documents. Language Awareness 8: 84–97.
Sheard M, Chambers B, Slavin R, and Elliott L (2012) Effects of technology-enhanced formative assessment
on achievement in primary grammar. York: Institute for Effective Education.
Shortis T and Blake J (2010) Who’s prepared to teach school English? The degree level qualifications and
preparedness of initial teacher trainees in English. London: CLIE.
Shulman LS (1987) Knowledge and teaching: Foundations of the new reform. Harvard Educational Review
57: 1–22.
ter Kuile H, Veldhuis M, van Veen S, and Wicherts M (2011) Bilingual education, metalinguistic awareness,
and the understanding of an unknown language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 233–42.
Thévenin MG, Totereau C, Fayol M, and Jarousse JP (1999) L’apprentissage/enseignement de la morpholo-
gie écrite du nombre en français [The teaching/learning of the written morphology of number in French].
Revue Française de Pédagogie 126: 39–52.
Tomlinson D (1994) Errors in the research into the effectiveness of grammar teaching. English in Education
28: 20–26.
Trudgill P (2000) Sociolinguistics: An introduction to language and society. 4th edition. London: Penguin.
Truss L (2003) Eats, shoots and leaves: The zero tolerance approach to punctuation. London: Profile Books.
Tunmer WE, Bowey JA, and Grieve R (1983) The development of young children’s awareness of the word
as a unit of spoken language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 12: 567–94.
Turvey A (2000) Teaching grammar: Working with student teachers 2. Changing English 7: 139–52.
Uttley T (2008) Britain’s got talent? If you ask me, Britain’s got problems. Daily Mail 6 June 2008. Retrieved
from: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-1024582/Britains-Got-Talent-If-ask-Britains-got-
problems.html#ixzz0kKDkPGPy (November 2013).
van Gelderen A (2006) What we know without knowing it: Sense and nonsense in respect of linguistic reflec-
tion for students in elementary and secondary education. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 5:
44–54.
van Gelderen A (2010) Does explicit teaching of grammar help students to become better writers? Insights
from empirical research. In: Locke T (ed.) Beyond the grammar wars: A resource for teachers and
students on developing language knowledge in the English/literacy classroom. New York; Routledge,
109–28.
van Lier L (1998) The relationship between consciousness, interaction and language learning. Language
Awareness 7: 128–45.
Vavra E (1996) On not teaching grammar. English Journal 85: 32–37.
Watson A (2012) Navigating ‘The Pit Of Doom’: Affective responses to teaching ‘grammar’. English in
Education 46: 22–37.
Weaver C (1996) Teaching grammar in the context of writing. English Journal 85: 15–24.
62	 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)
Weaver C and Bush J (2006) Grammar intertwined throughout the writing process: An ‘inch wide and a mile
deep’. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 5: 77–101.
Williams G (2005) Grammatics in schools. In: Hasan R, Matthiessen C, and Webster J (eds) Continuing dis-
course on language: A functional perspective: Volume 1. London: Equinox, 281–310.
Williamson J and Hardman F (1995) Time for refilling the bath? A study of primary student-teachers’ gram-
matical knowledge. Language and Education 9: 23–45.
Wilson AC and Myhill DA (2012) Ways with words: Teachers’ personal epistemologies of the role of meta-
language in the teaching of poetry writing. Language and Education 26: 553–68.
Witte SP (1980) Sentence combining and the teaching of writing. College Composition and Communication
31: 433–37.
Wray D (1993) Student-teachers’ knowledge and beliefs about language. In: Bennett N and Carré C (eds)
Learning to teach. London: Routledge, 51–72.
Wyse D (2001) Grammar. For writing?: A critical review of empirical evidence. British Journal of Educational
Studies 49: 411–27.
Wyse D (2006) Pupils’ word choices and the teaching of grammar. Cambridge Journal of Education 36:
31–47.
Zipke M (2007) The role of metalinguistic awareness in the reading comprehension of sixth and seventh grad-
ers. Reading Psychology 28: 375–96.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.

More Related Content

What's hot

Presentation book review
Presentation book reviewPresentation book review
Presentation book reviewRajan Poudel
 
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...Dr. Seyed Hossein Fazeli
 
English acqusition programs final
English acqusition programs finalEnglish acqusition programs final
English acqusition programs finalesrbk
 
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published articleBudsaba Kanoksi
 
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...Dr. Seyed Hossein Fazeli
 
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english Víctor Elías Lugo Vásquez
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...Vissta Simanungkalit
 
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...inventionjournals
 
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015William Kritsonis
 
Wong vol6 (1)
Wong vol6 (1)Wong vol6 (1)
Wong vol6 (1)sawwah44
 
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-12013 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120Alexander Decker
 
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...Dr. Seyed Hossein Fazeli
 
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...Rachel Wicaksono
 

What's hot (20)

Presentation book review
Presentation book reviewPresentation book review
Presentation book review
 
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...
The exploring nature of definitions and classifications of language learning ...
 
English acqusition programs final
English acqusition programs finalEnglish acqusition programs final
English acqusition programs final
 
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article
2013.ICLLCS.Budsaba published article
 
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...
The exploring nature of language learning strategies (ll ss) and their relati...
 
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english
What role do expanding circle country users play in the spread of english
 
The babel tower of englishes presentation
The babel tower of englishes presentationThe babel tower of englishes presentation
The babel tower of englishes presentation
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Ae&be
Ae&beAe&be
Ae&be
 
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...
Gibson ferguson language planning and education (edinburgh textbooks in appli...
 
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...
English to Malay (Bahasa Melayu) Translation: Syntactical Issues Involving Ti...
 
Vocabulary Poster
Vocabulary PosterVocabulary Poster
Vocabulary Poster
 
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015
Ramos, norma teachers' native languages ije v3 n1 2015
 
Wong vol6 (1)
Wong vol6 (1)Wong vol6 (1)
Wong vol6 (1)
 
English in the world 1
English in the world 1English in the world 1
English in the world 1
 
Grammar 3
Grammar 3Grammar 3
Grammar 3
 
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-12013 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120
13 supakorn phoocharoensil 103-120
 
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...
The exploring nature of vocabulary acquisition and common main gaps in the cu...
 
A Toastmaster Approach
A Toastmaster ApproachA Toastmaster Approach
A Toastmaster Approach
 
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...
Raising awareness of English as a lingua franca in an 'internationalising' Br...
 

Viewers also liked

Development of Language Learning In Grammar
Development of Language Learning In GrammarDevelopment of Language Learning In Grammar
Development of Language Learning In GrammarNor Zyana
 
Grammatical categories and word classes
Grammatical categories and word classesGrammatical categories and word classes
Grammatical categories and word classesMaría Ortega
 
The nature of language slideshare.net
The nature of language slideshare.netThe nature of language slideshare.net
The nature of language slideshare.netElih Sutisna Yanto
 
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammar
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammarIntroduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammar
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammarConstance Chee
 
Teaching with-demonstration
Teaching with-demonstrationTeaching with-demonstration
Teaching with-demonstrationGineswilma
 
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-adminOlaf Lindström
 
O que um profissional de RH faz?
O que um profissional de RH faz?O que um profissional de RH faz?
O que um profissional de RH faz?Solides Tecnologia
 
Obatacles to Success
Obatacles to SuccessObatacles to Success
Obatacles to Successpatelmitul772
 
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paper
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paperScandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paper
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paperD.K. Das
 
Digital Consumer Response Report
Digital Consumer Response ReportDigital Consumer Response Report
Digital Consumer Response Reportwilliammarketing
 
NaturalLeadership
NaturalLeadershipNaturalLeadership
NaturalLeadershipal roessert
 
Visual Aids inPresentation
Visual Aids inPresentationVisual Aids inPresentation
Visual Aids inPresentationpatelmitul772
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Development of Language Learning In Grammar
Development of Language Learning In GrammarDevelopment of Language Learning In Grammar
Development of Language Learning In Grammar
 
Testing language areas and skills
Testing language areas and skillsTesting language areas and skills
Testing language areas and skills
 
Grammatical categories and word classes
Grammatical categories and word classesGrammatical categories and word classes
Grammatical categories and word classes
 
Learning Grammar
Learning GrammarLearning Grammar
Learning Grammar
 
The nature of language slideshare.net
The nature of language slideshare.netThe nature of language slideshare.net
The nature of language slideshare.net
 
Lesson Plan
Lesson Plan Lesson Plan
Lesson Plan
 
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammar
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammarIntroduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammar
Introduction to grammar & Approaches in teaching grammar
 
Teaching with-demonstration
Teaching with-demonstrationTeaching with-demonstration
Teaching with-demonstration
 
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin
#wpbar 26/3 - Förenkla WP-admin
 
O que um profissional de RH faz?
O que um profissional de RH faz?O que um profissional de RH faz?
O que um profissional de RH faz?
 
urban outfiters
urban outfitersurban outfiters
urban outfiters
 
Obatacles to Success
Obatacles to SuccessObatacles to Success
Obatacles to Success
 
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paper
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paperScandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paper
Scandinavia Oil-Gas Magazine May 2011 paper
 
Digital Consumer Response Report
Digital Consumer Response ReportDigital Consumer Response Report
Digital Consumer Response Report
 
Ikram CV-Final(02)
Ikram CV-Final(02)Ikram CV-Final(02)
Ikram CV-Final(02)
 
HND
HNDHND
HND
 
NaturalLeadership
NaturalLeadershipNaturalLeadership
NaturalLeadership
 
Visual Aids inPresentation
Visual Aids inPresentationVisual Aids inPresentation
Visual Aids inPresentation
 
Vetha manthra
Vetha manthraVetha manthra
Vetha manthra
 
Feasibility Study
Feasibility StudyFeasibility Study
Feasibility Study
 

Similar to Grammar myhill & annabel (2014)

A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language Teaching
A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language TeachingA Critical Overview Of Communicative Language Teaching
A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language TeachingJoe Andelija
 
Nassaji ( 2004)
Nassaji ( 2004)Nassaji ( 2004)
Nassaji ( 2004)illielas
 
hassans Contrastive Analysis
hassans Contrastive Analysishassans Contrastive Analysis
hassans Contrastive AnalysisHassans09
 
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)Hassans09
 
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.doc
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.docA_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.doc
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.docNEHAVORA16
 
Sherylll
SherylllSherylll
Sherylllbhobet
 
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciation
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching PronunciationA Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciation
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciationenglishonecfl
 
A historical survey 2
A historical survey 2A historical survey 2
A historical survey 2AnsulaNorsu
 
English language teaching, ch4
English language teaching, ch4English language teaching, ch4
English language teaching, ch4VivaAs
 
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisDr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisWilliam Kritsonis
 
Introduction
IntroductionIntroduction
Introductionmarcejos
 
Introduction
IntroductionIntroduction
Introductionmarcejos
 
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciation
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciationFuture English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciation
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciationabdullahcoskun14
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Hamizah Osman
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Hamizah Osman
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Hamizah Osman
 
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesTeaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesNhinie Indasari
 
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language Learners
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language LearnersResearch- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language Learners
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language LearnersNOR RUBA'YAH ABD RAHIM
 
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics Introduction
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics IntroductionAn Introduction To Applied Linguistics Introduction
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics IntroductionLori Moore
 
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language Learners
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language LearnersTeaching Grammar to Adult English Language Learners
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language LearnersAmber Gallup Rodriguez
 

Similar to Grammar myhill & annabel (2014) (20)

A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language Teaching
A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language TeachingA Critical Overview Of Communicative Language Teaching
A Critical Overview Of Communicative Language Teaching
 
Nassaji ( 2004)
Nassaji ( 2004)Nassaji ( 2004)
Nassaji ( 2004)
 
hassans Contrastive Analysis
hassans Contrastive Analysishassans Contrastive Analysis
hassans Contrastive Analysis
 
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)
Conrastive Analysis (Hassans Edu)
 
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.doc
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.docA_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.doc
A_STUDY_ON_THE_ISSUE_OF_CHANGING_THE_MED.doc
 
Sherylll
SherylllSherylll
Sherylll
 
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciation
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching PronunciationA Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciation
A Literature Review On Strategies For Teaching Pronunciation
 
A historical survey 2
A historical survey 2A historical survey 2
A historical survey 2
 
English language teaching, ch4
English language teaching, ch4English language teaching, ch4
English language teaching, ch4
 
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisDr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
 
Introduction
IntroductionIntroduction
Introduction
 
Introduction
IntroductionIntroduction
Introduction
 
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciation
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciationFuture English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciation
Future English teachers' attitudes towards EIL pronunciation
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
 
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
Language education in_asia_vol3_iss2_2012
 
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesTeaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
 
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language Learners
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language LearnersResearch- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language Learners
Research- based Vocabulary Instruction for English Language Learners
 
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics Introduction
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics IntroductionAn Introduction To Applied Linguistics Introduction
An Introduction To Applied Linguistics Introduction
 
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language Learners
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language LearnersTeaching Grammar to Adult English Language Learners
Teaching Grammar to Adult English Language Learners
 

Recently uploaded

BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 

Recently uploaded (20)

BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 

Grammar myhill & annabel (2014)

  • 1. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 2014, Vol. 30(1) 41­–62 © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0265659013514070 clt.sagepub.com The role of grammar in the writing curriculum: A review of the literature Debra Myhill University of Exeter, UK; University of Wollongong, Australia Annabel Watson University of Exeter, UK Abstract For most Anglophone countries, the history of grammar teaching over the past 50 years is one of contestation, debate and dissent: and 50 years on we are no closer to reaching a consensus about the role of grammar in the English/Language Arts curriculum. The debate has been described through the metaphor of battle and grammar wars (Kamler, 1995; Locke, 2005), frequently pitting educational professionals against politicians, but also pitting one professional against another. At the heart of the debate are differing perspectives on the value of grammar for the language learner and opposing views of what educational benefits learning grammar may or may not accrue. At the present time, several jurisdictions, including England and Australia, are creating new mandates for grammar in the curriculum. This article reviews the literature on the teaching of grammar and its role in the curriculum and indicates an emerging consensus on a fully-theorized conceptualization of grammar in the curriculum. Keywords Curriculum, grammar, metalanguage, metalinguistic understanding, writing I Introduction For most Anglophone countries, the history of grammar teaching over the past 50 years is one of contestation, debate and dissent: and 50 years on we are no closer to reaching a consensus about the role of grammar in the English/Language Arts curriculum. The debate has been described through the metaphor of battle and grammar wars (Kamler, 1995; Locke, 2005), frequently pitting Corresponding author: Debra Myhill, University of Exeter, Heavitree Road, Exeter EX1 2LU, UK. Email: d.a.myhill@ex.ac.uk 514070CLT30110.1177/0265659013514070Child Language Teaching and TherapyMyhill and Watson research-article2013 Article
  • 2. 42 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) educational professionals against politicians, but also pitting one professional against another. At the heart of the debate are differing perspectives on the value of grammar for the language learner and opposing views of what educational benefits learning grammar may or may not accrue. The debate has been well rehearsed elsewhere (Braddock et al., 1963; Cameron and Bourne, 1988; Christie, 2004; Hartwell, 1985; Hudson and Walmsley, 2005; Locke, 2009; Myhill and Jones, 2011; QCA, 1998), and only a brief overview will be offered here in order to frame the review that follows. The Dartmouth Conference in the USA in 1966 signalled a turning point in educational thinking about grammar in the curriculum. Funded by the Carnegie Endowment, and organized by the Modern Language Association (MLA) and the National Conference of Teachers of English (NCTE), the Dartmouth Conference brought together teachers and educational researchers from the UK and the USA to discuss the grammar issue. The conference was prompted by growing dis- satisfaction with classroom practice in grammar teaching, which was largely characterized by drills and exercises in labelling and identifying word classes and syntactical structures, and which to many education professionals had no educational relevance and no impact on language develop- ment. As Muller noted, the general consensus was that grammar teaching was ‘a waste of time’ (1967: 68). As a consequence of the Dartmouth Conference, many educational jurisdictions in the USA, the UK, Australia, New Zealand and Canada moved to exclude formal grammar teaching from the English curriculum, although, of course, there will always have been those teachers who continued to teach grammar, despite changed policy mandates. The widespread abandoning of grammar teaching following the Dartmouth Conference activated two further strands in the grammar debate. First, whilst curriculum policy might have eschewed the teaching of grammar, an underlying sense of barely disguised outrage at its abandonment has surfaced repeatedly over the years, often from politicians or the general public. This strand of discourse tends to have been preoccupied with language standards and a pervading view that lack of attention to grammar was causing falling standards in language users (for example, Macdonald, 1995; Paterson, 2010; Truss, 2003; Uttley, 2008). It positions grammar as a form of ‘verbal hygiene’ (Cameron, 1995), which assumes a moral significance and acts as guardian to the moral standards of the nation (Cameron, 1994; Pullman, 2005). Second, the abandoning of grammar has fostered a strand of thinking about the role of knowl- edge about language in the curriculum. Here linguists have argued for the place of grammar knowledge, including its associated metalanguage, as a worthy and relevant body of knowl- edge in its own right (Hudson, 2004). At the same time, a broader view of knowledge about language places the emphasis on more inductive approaches, which investigate language in social contexts (for example, Barton, 1999; Carter, 1990; Denham and Lobeck, 2005; Keith, 1990; NATE, 1997). This article is framed by this debate and by recent international developments that appear to be re-introducing grammar to the language curriculum. Through a systematic review of the literature, the article will provide a critical outline of research into the role of grammar in the English or Language Arts curriculum. 1  Grammar in the first language curriculum As noted earlier, there appears to be an emerging trend to reintroduce grammar in the teaching of English, particularly in England, the USA and Australia. In England, the first National Curriculum for English (DES, 1990) adopted a knowledge about language approach, though with an emphasis on Standard English, and subsequent iterations of the National Curriculum (DfE, 1995; DfEE, 1999, 2000) have subtly shifted these emphases (for more detail, see Myhill and Jones, 2011). The
  • 3. Myhill and Watson 43 1995 version, for example, refers more specifically to grammatical metalanguage, whereas the 2007 version has rather more emphasis on grammar in functional contexts. However, an irony of the National Curriculum is that, although a statutory instrument, actual classroom practice was much more significantly influenced by the assessment framework and, as grammar was not tested, the degree to which it is was taught was largely dependent on individual teachers’ own predilec- tions. Currently, however, a new draft National Curriculum presents a grammar annex (DfE, 2013a), which outlines, year by year, the grammatical terminology that students must learn and the grammatical structures that they must master. Moreover, a new national test of grammar, spelling and punctuation (DfE, 2013b) was introduced in 2013, which as high-stakes assessment will inevi- tably have a significant impact on what is taught. A more significant influence on classroom practice in England was the (non-statutory) National Literacy Strategy, introduced in 1998 (DfE, 1998), which matched to the requirements of the National Curriculum, but which was also accompanied by substantial training and resources. The teaching of grammar was strongly encouraged in training materials, including two training DVDs (Grammar for reading and Grammar for writing) that explicitly addressed grammar and in a man- ner that was clearly attempting to be contextualized. However, the specified learning objectives and assessment focuses underpinning the strategy left teachers the freedom to determine an appro- priate context. This led to many lessons being conducted in the ‘context’ of a writing lesson, but the lessons effectively de-contextualized grammar (Myhill, 2004, 2006; Wyse, 2006). At sentence level, in particular, this led to lessons on ‘using complex sentences’ or ‘using connectives’, where deployment of a particular grammatical feature became more important than any understanding of a relationship between grammar and meaning. Perhaps one reason for this discrepancy between policy-level principles and materials and class- room practice is because in England there has been no clear articulation of a rationale for this renewed emphasis on grammar. In contrast, in Australia where a National Curriculum is currently being developed for the first time, the structure of the proposed English curriculum includes a Language strand that provides for explicit grammatical knowledge: the articulated, purposeful, and conscious understanding of how language works, which supports learners in ‘knowing how to choose words and grammatical and textual structures that are more appropriate to the audience or readership’ (ACARA, 2009: 3). The inclusion of a Language strand is built upon a rationale that seeks to foster ‘a coherent, dynamic, and evolving body of knowledge about the English language and how it works’ (ACARA, 2009: 10). To an extent, the position in the USA is more akin to England than Australia, although there is no National Curriculum mandating content. Instead, state intervention is principally stand- ards-driven through the Common Core Standards (CCSSI, 2012), now adopted by all but five US states. The Common Core Standards specify Language as a discrete standard, and two of the three Language Anchor standards relate to accuracy and avoidance of error. Language Standard 1 requires command of ‘the conventions of standard English grammar and usage when writing or speaking’ and, in similar vein, Language Standard 2 requires command of ‘the conventions of standard English capitalization, punctuation, and spelling when writing’. However, Language Strand 3 is considerably more meaning or pragmatics oriented, looking at knowledge of lan- guage ‘to understand how language functions in different contexts, to make effective choices for meaning or style, and to comprehend more fully when reading or listening’ (CCSSI, 2012). The list of progressive skills in language that students have to master is principally directed towards accuracy, yet teachers are reminded of the inseparability of language study from read- ing, writing, speaking and listening contexts, and that students ‘must also be able to make informed, skillful choices among the many ways to express themselves through language’ (CCSSI, 2012).
  • 4. 44 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) What is evident from this brief consideration of three Anglophone jurisdictions is that the peda- gogical rationale for the re-emergence of grammar is not yet fully clear, particularly in England and the USA. At policy level, the reasons for maintaining or re-introducing grammar appear to be neither evidence based, nor clearly articulated. Indeed, many opposed to the re-introduction of grammar would highlight the substantial body of research (reviewed later in this article) that points very conclusively to the ineffectiveness of de-contextualized grammar in helping students improve their writing. The new National Curriculum in England exemplifies this uncertainty, both in terms of the absence of an evidence base and the ambiguity of the rationale for its inclusion. For example, it mandates that children ‘learn the correct grammatical terms in English and that these terms are integrated within teaching’ (DfE, 2013: 5) signalling simultaneously a suggestion that a rationale for the inclusion of grammar is the learning of a discrete body of linguistic knowledge (which is, of course, a defensible curriculum position to adopt) but also that this teaching should be ‘inte- grated within teaching’. The meaning of ‘integrated’ is unclear as it would be impossible to teach without integrating it into teaching, but it is perhaps implying a sense that the grammar should be contextualized. If that is the case then the learning purpose of this is not communicated. Elsewhere, the document argues that: once pupils are familiar with a grammatical concept [for example ‘modal verb’], they should be encouraged to apply and explore this concept in the grammar of their own speech and writing and to note where it is used by others. (DfE, 2013c: 64) There is no evidence base to support this assertion that learning progresses chronologically from learning a grammatical concept to being able to apply it. In non-English speaking countries, however, the position of grammar in the curriculum is frequently neither contentious nor questioned. In both Europe and Asia, the teaching of gram- mar as part of first language teaching is largely regarded as the norm; for example, van Gelderen (2010) notes the routine presence of grammar in Dutch teaching. In the French- speaking world, grammar is heavily drawn upon in order to develop students’ understanding of the inflections in French spelling (Fayol et al., 1993, 1994, 2009) and this grammar is based upon traditional explanations of rules that govern language patterns. However, in Canada especially, grammaire nouvelle (‘new grammar’) is adopted as an alternative to tra- ditional grammar teaching in French as a first language. Grammaire nouvelle is essentially an inductive approach to securing grammatical understanding, with a reduced emphasis on grammatical metalanguage, and a greater emphasis on fostering student observation, reflec- tion, and active participation in exploring language (Poulin, 1980). Leger (no date) summa- rizes the approach as: a grammar of observation and reasoning based on effective manipulations, amongst other things, of words and groups of words before analysing them and understanding their function. The teaching offers us knowledge of French structures and allows us to build authentic procedures of reflection which drive independence. [my translation] (Leger, no date: p. 3) The manipulations that learners are encouraged to undertake involve subtraction, substitution, per- mutation and transformation, designed to help develop independent grammatical understanding (Boivin, 2009; Boivin and Pinsonneault, 2008; Nadeau and Fisher, 2009, 2011). Curiously, although there are some resonances between grammaire nouvelle and a ‘grammar in context’ approach, there has been little cross-fertilization of ideas across the English–French language borders.
  • 5. Myhill and Watson 45 2 Theoretical perspectives on the teaching of grammar The historical tendency to focus consideration of grammar in the curriculum on whether it should be included has led to a somewhat impoverished theoretical base for conceptualizing a role for grammar. Often the ‘grammar debate’ has been framed by polemic (Macdonald, 1995) and claim and counter- claim (Anderson, 1995; Cameron and Bourne, 1998; Clark, 2010; Crowley, 2003; Harris, 1962; Hudson, 2004; Robinson, 1959; Sealey, 1999; Tomlinson, 1994; Wyse, 2001), but there is relatively little coherent and developed articulation of the contribution that grammatical understanding might make to students’learning about language.As noted earlier, there is a folk theorization of grammar as central to supporting students’ accuracy as language users and in the eradication of errors from their speech and writing. In essence, this is a theorization that gives primacy to Standard English and posi- tions grammar as the tool by which Standard English is maintained. Drawing on historical discourses that advocated school as the place where the ‘evil habits of speech contracted in the home and the street’ (Board of Education [Newbolt Report] 1921: 59) could be overcome, and where school can compensate for the linguistic disadvantage of the home (Bernstein, 1971), this perspective on gram- mar tends to be adopted by those outside the profession. It is a largely prescriptive view of grammar, focused on form, and predicated on assumptions that grammar prescribes how language should be used and acts as the benchmark by which deviations should be judged. Theoretical arguments against this position are more robust than those for it and include socio-cultural and socio-linguistic analyses that highlight the cultural hegemony of this stance and its lack of understanding of language variation and the descriptive grammars advocated by modern linguistics. In contrast to this rather ill defined, though strongly held, theorization of grammar is the approach of Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) and the seminal work of Halliday and the Sydney School. SFL is essentially a meaning-oriented theorization of grammar, concerned to explore the relationship between text and context, and looking not simply at lexical or syntactical aspects of grammar but also the discourse elements of organization, development and cohesion (Christie and Derewianka, 2006; Christie and Unsworth, 2006; Halliday, 1993, 1994, 2003; Hasan, 2002). It regards language as fundamentally a social semiotic system, ‘abstract semiosis’(Halliday, 2003: 5), and is concerned with how language works or functions. Carter (1990), for example, describes this approach as ‘functionally oriented, related to the study of texts and responsive to social purposes’ (p. 104). SFL differs from other functional grammars in its emphasis upon what Halliday (1978) has called the metafunctions of language the ideational, the interpersonal and the textual. The ideational metafunction refers to the communicative message, i.e. ‘what a text is about’; the interpersonal metafunction refers to ‘how the self is expressed’ and ‘how the reader is understood’; and the textual metafunction refers to the structural aspects of text. From the perspective of the classroom, Halliday’s argument that any act of communication requires the making of choices and that ‘the power of language resides in its organization as a huge network of interrelated choices’ (2003: 8) is a direct contrast to the idea of grammar as the arbiter of accuracy. Derewianka and Jones (2010: 9) sum this up succinctly: Whereas traditional approaches conceive of grammar as a set of structures which can be assessed as correct or incorrect, Halliday sees language as a resource, a meaning-making system through which we interactively shape and interpret our world and ourselves. Teaching grammar through an SFL theoretical frame supports learners’ ability to think grammati- cally about language (Freebody et al., 2008; Macken-Horarik et al., 2011; Williams, 2005), and to understand grammar’s potentiality as a meaning-making resource (Coffin, 2010; Schleppegrell, 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011, 2012).
  • 6. 46 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Clearly, knowledge about language and grammar as a meaning-making resource is at the heart of SFL. The same emphasis on meaning, rather than form, is evident in the theorization of rhetori- cal grammar, although it does not adopt the metalanguage of SFL and is a rather more eclectic and fragmented body of theorization than the strongly coherent work of SFL. The notion of rhetorical grammar is strong in the USA and stands in contrast to traditional pedagogical approaches that position grammar as a ‘fix-it approach to weak writing’ (Micciche, 2004: 716). Rhetorical gram- mar conceives of grammar as a tool for crafting and shaping language and written text, in ways that connect grammar to ‘rhetorical and stylistic effects’(Paraskevas, 2006: 65) and that enables writers to ‘understand and control’ (Kolln, 2006: xi). On one level, rhetorical grammar is about explicitly showing young writers the repertoire that is available to them, what Myhill et al. have called ‘a repertoire of infinite possibilities’(Myhill, 2011a; Myhill et al., 2011), and supporting the ability to make conscious choices (Dawkins, 1995; Hancock, 2009; Petit, 2003). On another level, investi- gating the ways in which meanings are created in texts is about teaching thinking about ‘the inter- woven relationship between what we say and how we say it’ (Micciche, 2004: 718). Systemic Functional Linguistics and rhetorical approaches to grammar share a common focus on developing understanding about how language works, rather than simply regarding grammar as a body of knowledge that describes, or prescribes, the system of language. Theoretically, the knowl- edge that these approaches foster is metalinguistic knowledge. The most comprehensive investiga- tion of metalinguistic knowledge and development is that of Gombert (1992). He suggests that metalinguistic knowledge moves from an epilinguistic stage, where the knowledge is unconscious or implicit, to a metalinguistic stage, where it is explicit. Alternative perspectives on the stages of meta- linguistic development have been offered by Culioli (1990), Karmiloff-Smith (1996) and van Lier (1998). Gombert categorizes metalinguistic knowledge as addressing one of six areas: metaphono- logical, metalexical, metasemantic, metasyntactic, metatextual, and metapragmatic, although Myhill (2011b) suggests that in older readers and writers, this categorization may be less appropriate as they are principally concerned with the metapragmatic, i.e. how texts communicate in different contexts. However, the most significant body of work on metalinguistic knowledge or development relates to second language learners (e.g. Bialystok, 2007; ter Kuile et al., 2011), to young learners acquiring language (Downing and Oliver, 1974; Karmiloff Smith et al., 1996; Tunmer et al., 1983), to spelling development (e.g. Bourassa et al., 2006; Caravolas et al., 2005; Nunes et al., 2006; Thévenin et al., 1999), and to reading comprehension (Heibert et al., 1984; Macgillivray, 1994; Zipke, 2007). Nonetheless, metalinguistic knowledge and understanding is central to any consideration of the role of grammar in the English curriculum, particularly the notions of explicit and implicit knowl- edge, as they underpin the differing views of the value of grammar teaching. Gombert (1992) main- tained that epilinguistic, implicit, grammatical knowledge precedes metalinguistic knowledge, and it includes the unconscious knowledge that language users acquire from communication and interac- tion, such as word order or how to form the past tense. Roehr (2008) contrasts implicit knowledge that ‘cannot be brought into awareness or articulated’ with explicit metalinguistic knowledge that is ‘declarative knowledge that can be brought into awareness and that is potentially available for verbal report’ (p. 179). The historical disputes over the value of grammar teaching hinge upon this distinc- tion and arguments that first language speakers acquire grammar implicitly and therefore need lan- guage experience rather than grammatical knowledge (Elbow, 1991) or, conversely, that explicit grammatical knowledge gives writers both choice and control over their language use (Carter, 1990). II  Empirical studies on the teaching of grammar Although the issue of the value of grammar teaching has often been framed in terms of the rele- vance of explicit and implicit grammatical knowledge, it is also true that one strong line of
  • 7. Myhill and Watson 47 argument has been located in discussions of the impact of teaching grammar on students’ language use, particularly writing. In other words, does explicit knowledge of grammar support writing development and attainment in writing, or as Elbow argued, ‘nothing helps your writing so much as ignoring grammar’ (Elbow, 1981). Over the past 50 years, there have been a number of research reviews or meta-analyses address- ing the effect of grammar teaching on students’ learning (Andrews, 2005; Andrews et al., 2006; Braddock et al., 1963; Hillocks, 1984; Wyse, 2001), all of which have concluded that ‘the teaching of school grammar has little or no effect on students’(Hillocks and Smith, 1991) or even that it has a ‘harmful effect on the improvement of writing’ (Braddock et al., 1963: 37). Indeed, Graham and Perin’s (2007) meta-analysis investigating effective strategies for teaching writing found a nega- tive effect for ‘the explicit and systematic teaching of the parts of speech and structure of sen- tences’and the authors argue that this finding challenges ‘some educators’enthusiasm for traditional grammar instruction as a focus of writing instruction for adolescents’ (2007: 21). A review by the Evidence for Policy and Practice Information and Co-ordinating Centre (EPPI, 2004) concluded that it could find no evidence for any benefit of grammar teaching on writing quality, although the authors did also note that ‘the quality of seven out of ten of the primary studies included in the in- depth review on the teaching of syntax is a limitation on the review, as is the lack of recent research’ (Andrews et al., 2006: 51). In the end, the EPPI review draws its conclusions based on the analysis of just three studies: Bateman and Zidonis (1966), Elley et al. (1975, 1979), and Fogel and Ehri (2000). It is worth look- ing at these three studies more closely. The Bateman and Zidonis study, in the USA, focused on sentence production in writing and investigated whether high school students could ‘learn to apply the transformational rules of a generative grammar in their writing?’ (1966: 3). The sample size was rather small: 50 students assigned to either the control or intervention, of whom only 41 were still in the sample at the end of the study. The intervention group were taught explicitly about syn- tactical structures for sentences, and written compositions from the two groups were analysed using an analytical tool that captured the generative transformational grammar rules of sentence construction and allowed them to compare development pre-treatment and post-treatment. An out- come of the analysis was a structural complexity score for each piece of writing (1966: 8). The statistical results were not conclusive, but the authors suggest that they did indicate a relationship between ‘a knowledge of generative grammar and an ability to produce well-formed sentences of greater structural complexity’(1966: 39). In contrast to this small-scale study, the Elley et al. study is arguably the most robust research conducted in this field. They conducted a three-year longitu- dinal study in New Zealand, involving a sample of 250 secondary-aged students of average ability with eight classes, matched for ability. There were three treatment groups: • Treatment 1: a transformational grammar course focusing on discovering facts about lan- guage and its use. The course included teaching of sentence-combining, subordination, par- ticiple modifiers, and deep and surface structures. • Treatment 2: a reading–writing course with 40% spent free reading, 40% spent using a class reader, and 20% spent writing, especially creative writing. • Treatment 3: a course typical of New Zealand secondary English classes at the time, involv- ing functional grammar, comprehension, and writing. They found that there were no significant differences in the quality of writing produced post-test, although the transformational grammar group did improve in their sentence-combining ability. The Fogel and Ehri study (2000) was different from the previous two studies as it took as its starting point an identified writing problem, the use of Black English Vernacular (BEV) in the
  • 8. 48 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) writing of low-attaining ethnic minority students in primary school. The study set out to ‘examine how to structure dialect instruction so that it is effective in teaching SE [Standard English] forms to students who use BEV in their writing’ (2000: 215). An experimental design was adopted with three treatment groups: • exposure plus text; • exposure plus explicit instruction in strategies; and • exposure, strategies, and guided practice in transforming BEV to SE and feedback (ESP) According to the authors, treatments 1 and 2 mirror typical classroom practice. The results indi- cated a statistically significant positive effect for group 3, leading the authors to conclude that the guided practice had ‘clarified for students the link between features in their own non-standard writing and features in SE’ (2000: 231). However, because of the small sample size, the EPPI review does not rate this as strong evidence for a positive benefit. The EPPI review noted the dearth of research in this area, but a much more recent study, con- ducted in primary schools in the UK by Sheard et al. (2012), would appear to confirm its conclu- sions. They investigated whether teaching grammar using electronic handsets that provided students with instant feedback would improve their learning of grammatical terminology and whether it would improve their writing. They conducted a 12 week study, drawing on a sample of 42 primary schools, half of which were randomly assigned to use the electronic handsets and the other as a control group. Grammatical knowledge was tested before and after the intervention using discrete grammar questions and a paragraph revision writing task to ascertain whether the grammar knowledge transferred to writing. The results indicated a strongly significant positive impact on grammar knowledge on the experimental group, using the handsets, but no impact on writing. However, one aspect of grammar teaching that has been repeatedly identified as successful is sentence-combining, the practice of ‘teaching students to construct more complex and sophisti- cated sentences through exercises in which two or more basic sentences are combined into a single sentence’(Graham and Perin, 2007: 18). The EPPI review (2004; Andrews et al., 2006) maintained that there was good evidence for the efficacy of sentence-combining, and it was one of the 11 instructional strategies identified by Graham and Perin (2007) as beneficial to writing. Sentence- combining has a long history in the USA with a chain of studies (Daiker et al., 1978; Hake and Williams, 1979; Hillocks and Mavrognes, 1986; Kinneavy, 1979; Marzano, 1976; O’Hare, 1971; Phillips, 1996; Saddler and Graham, 2005; Savage, 1980) all reporting the success of sentence- combining. Most were conducted in high schools or even at university level. The studies were themselves followed by a flurry of similar studies (for an overview of this, see Connors, 2000), most agreeing and confirming the efficacy of the technique. The only UK study to look at this, with secondary school students (Keen, 2004), also found a positive impact. But despite this apparent consistency in results, sentence-combining is problematic in terms of both the studies and the teaching of writing. In general, the studies measure whether teaching students to combine sen- tences, largely through exercises, improves their ability to create effectively combined sentences, with very few studies looking at whether writing quality is improved. Several critics have pointed out the shortcomings in these studies (Connors, 2000; Faigley, 1980; Witte, 1980), and Crowhurst (1980) conducted a study that demonstrated that increased syntactic complexity, such as that achieved through sentence-combining, was not correlated with writing quality. In addition, critics have suggested that any success deriving from sentence-combining is attributable to discussions about the ‘rhetorical principles characteristic of good writing’(1980: 298), not to the strategy itself. Significantly, for example, Keen’s study included a considerable amount of contextualized discus- sion of the effectiveness of different sentences. Summarizing the approach, Andrews et al. argued
  • 9. Myhill and Watson 49 that ‘sentence-combining suggests a pedagogy of applied knowledge – at its best, applied in situa- tions of contextualized learning; at its worst, drilling’ (2006: 52), and it is probable that Andrews’ notion of applied knowledge rooted in contextualized learning is central to this issue. Andrews’ observation that the success of sentence-combining might owe more to its contextu- alization than to the approach per se is important. More recently, research in this area has begun to look at the benefits of teaching grammar that is contextualized or embedded within the teaching of writing. In the USA, Fearn and Farnan (2007) found strong positive effects on students’ writing in high schools when teaching made connections between the grammar being taught and children’s writing, and they claimed that ‘grammar instruction influences writing performance when gram- mar and writing share one instructional context’(Fearn and Farnan, 2007: 16). In England, a mixed method study, involving 13–14 year olds, and combining a large randomized controlled trial with a qualitative design has found a strongly significant benefit for students of a contextualized approach (Jones et al., 2013; Myhill et al., 2012). In this study, the instructional focus was the teaching of writing, and the teaching materials were designed around a consideration of what grammatical understanding might support learners in making authorial decisions in the composi- tion of a particular text. It is the first robust study that has provided evidence of a positive impact of grammar teaching on writing, but it is important to note that the learning focus was writing, not grammar. Nonetheless, it offers a coherent and evidenced rationale for developing a purposeful role for grammar in the curriculum. III  Teacher perspectives on the teaching of grammar Further insights into the role of grammar in the curriculum are provided by research studies or professional publications that attend to teachers’ perspectives on the topic. Professional perspec- tives on grammar are evident in numerous articles by teachers and teacher-educators, particularly in the special ‘grammar’ editions of the English Journal, namely volume 85 (1996), volume 92 (2003) and volume 95 (2006). These articles tend to fall in to two camps: those which position grammar predominantly as a matter of rule-learning and error-correction (e.g. Benjamin et al., 2006; Brown, 1996; Rose, 1996), and those which position grammar as a tool for promoting meta- linguistic awareness and rhetorical choice (e.g. Ehrenworth, 2003; Gold, 2006; Hagemann, 2003; Jayman et al., 2006). The latter position is often informed, in articles from US practitioners, by Weaver’s concept of contextualized grammar (1996, 2006) and Kolln’s concept of rhetorical gram- mar (2006). The professional literature reflects the complexity of defining what is meant by the terms ‘gram- mar’ and ‘grammar teaching’, as Vavra (1996) notes. A handful of studies have explored how teachers define or conceptualize grammar. The largest, a survey of 137 primary and secondary school teachers in the UK, reported that teachers lacked confidence in defining grammar, particu- larly in understanding ‘the relationship between implicit and explicit knowledge of language’, and noted a strong association of explicit grammar teaching with prescriptivism and old-fashioned teaching methods (QCA, 1998: 26). A similar problem of definition has been reported in the USA from a smaller-scale study by Petruzella which noted confusion between grammatical rules, issues of usage, and spelling and punctuation (1996: 69). This finding was echoed in Cajkler and Hislam’s interviews with trainee primary teachers in the UK, with participants associating grammar with phonics, spelling and punctuation (2002). UK studies by Pomphrey and Moger (1999) and Watson (2012) report tension or inconsistency between prescriptive/descriptive and prescriptive/rhetorical conceptualizations of grammar teaching among trainee and practising secondary English teachers respectively, with the latter finding that teachers tend to immediately conceptualize grammar as prescriptive, rule-bound and focused on correctness, while conversely offering a rhetorical view
  • 10. 50 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) focused on exploration, choice and effects when asked about how it might be useful in supporting students’ writing development. All of these studies indicate that some degree of conceptual confu- sion persists within the profession. Research into teachers’ beliefs about grammar more generally provide evidence that UK teach- ers tend to associate grammar with negative discourses of ‘old-fashioned’ teaching (QCA, 1998; Watson, 2012), and that these ‘neo-conservative associations’(Myhill, 2006: 78) can occur even in countries in which grammar is not such a contested subject, such as Flanders and the Netherlands (van Gelderen, 2006). Investigations into teachers’ affective responses to grammar have typically reported ‘anxiety’ (QCA, 1998: 26) and ‘apprehension’ (Watson, 2012), and a general lack of con- fidence when it comes to dealing with grammar in the classroom, in both the UK (Kelly and Safford, 2009) and USA (Hadjioannou and Hutchinson, 2010). Teachers and trainee teachers have tended to be disparaging and hyper-critical in their self-evaluations (Pomphrey and Moger, 1999; Watson, 2012), and Cajkler and Hislam have found that anxiety remains high even when trainee teachers made improvements in their linguistic subject knowledge (2002). Teachers who feel anx- ious or insecure about their own declarative grammatical knowledge are also more likely to hold prescriptivist, rule-bound views of grammar (Harper and Rennie, 2009; Kamler, 1995; Macken- Horarik, 2001). However, studies have also reported more positive attitudes. Both Turvey (2000), working with secondary-level trainee teachers, and Cajkler and Hislam with primary-level (2002) found that, regardless of their anxieties about subject knowledge, trainees valued the idea of gram- mar teaching: Turvey found that her students felt that they had ‘missed out on something’ by not being taught grammar themselves at school, and that this made it ‘all the more important that their pupils should have it’ (2000: 143). Similarly, Watson found that some secondary English teachers felt passionately that the negative discourse around grammar teaching is misleading, and that far from being ‘the pit of doom’, grammar is actually ‘where freedom lies’, thanks to its focus on mak- ing knowledge about language and the choices involved in writing explicit (2012: 32). Such reports indicate that there may be willingness to learn about and practise grammar teaching even where teachers or trainees are insecure in their own linguistic or pedagogical knowledge. The most recent research into teachers’ beliefs has found that teachers tend to value the potential that a rhetorical approach to grammar teaching focused on experimentation has to increase students’ metalinguistic awareness and support them in consciously crafting their writing (Watson, 2012). There still per- sists, however, a tendency to associate grammar with a restrictive discourse of rules and accuracy (Watson, 2012) and to position grammar ‘as antipathetic to freedom and creativity’ (Wilson and Myhill, 2012: 11). Attitudes to the use of grammatical terminology have been explored in three studies. Two of these indicate a belief that it is more suitable for use with higher rather than lower ability students (Petruzella, 1996; QCA, 1998). The third study examined teachers’ personal epistemologies, their ‘beliefs about what knowledge is relevant and valuable in learning to write’ (Wilson and Myhill, 2012: 4), in relation to literary and linguistic metalanguage and poetry writing. This reported a recurrent epistemological argument that ‘linguistic metalanguage was rule-bound and constrain- ing, limiting young people’s writing in an unconstructive fashion’ (Wilson and Myhill, 2012: 10), which counterpointed a view that literary metalanguage is ‘a valuable part of learning about litera- ture’ (p. 13). This is consistent with Findlay’s findings that UK teachers regard grammar as ‘a chore’ (2010: 4), and that they consistently value the literary aspects of the subject above the linguistic. The literature, therefore, indicates a continuing trend amongst practitioners to view grammar as reactionary and restrictive, to value it less than literary aspects of the subject, and to be anxious about teaching it. However, there is also evidence of a countervailing belief that grammar taught
  • 11. Myhill and Watson 51 with an exploratory and rhetorical approach can be valuable in increasing students’ metalinguistic understanding and ability to consciously craft or design their writing. IV  Teaching grammar and the demands on content and pedagogical subject knowledge While teachers’knowledge about grammar is not generally raised as a concern in non-Anglophone countries, historical factors mean that it is a significant issue for many first-language English teachers. Shulman (1987) has distinguished between subject content knowledge (knowledge of an academic domain) and pedagogical content knowledge (knowledge of how to teach that academic domain). The demands on teachers’ subject content knowledge about grammar have become increasingly specific in Anglophone countries: in the USA, the Common Core State Standards for Language Arts are heavily focused on grammatical constructions that students are expected to master; in the UK grammar was made a mandatory part of the curriculum in 1988, with the non- statutory but widely adopted National Strategies guidance (DfEE, 1998; DfES, 2001) including detailed teaching objectives for students aged 5–14; Australia is currently developing a new National Curriculum that includes a strand on Knowledge about Language aiming to foster ‘a coherent, dynamic, and evolving body of knowledge about the English language and how it works’ (ACARA, 2009: 1); and in New Zealand, problems have occurred in trying to implement an inno- vative syllabus with a strong grammar focus due to teachers’ ‘lack of knowledge about language’ (Gordon, 2005: 63). While the extent to which students need to explicitly know about grammar is still hotly debated (Locke, 2010), there is widespread agreement that teachers’ grammatical knowledge needs to be richer and more substantive than the grammar they may need to teach to students. Teacher subject knowledge requires ‘a higher degree of grammar consciousness than most direct learners are likely to need or want’(Leech, 1994: 18), with an ability to be ‘conscious analysts of linguistic processes’ (Brumfit, 1997: 163) and possess ‘conscious awareness’ (Armstrong, 2004: 223) of how texts are structured. It has been argued that teachers who understand grammatical forms may be better placed to support developing writers (Andrews, 2005), to identify linguistic development in their students (Gordon, 2005), and to ‘make the analysis explicit’ (Hudson, 2004: 113) when examining texts with their students. The problem that current English teachers have with attaining the level of grammatical subject knowledge outlined above has arisen for two reasons: first, the fall from favour of grammar teach- ing in Anglophone countries following the Dartmouth Conference in the USA in 1966, due to the widespread view that the formal teaching of grammar had no beneficial impact on students’ lin- guistic facility (Hudson and Walmsley, 2005); second, the tendency in the UK for teachers to fol- low a literature degree route into teaching, along with is a shortage of applicants from a linguistics route (Shortis and Blake, 2010). As a result, many current English teachers were not taught gram- mar at school or university, a point also noted in the US context by Kolln and Hancock (2005), and by Gordon in New Zealand (2005). Of course, teachers who have literature degrees and are keen readers themselves do have a lot of knowledge about texts that they can draw on in language teach- ing, and they also have a substantial amount of implicit grammatical knowledge about texts. However, as Andrews (2005) points out, it is likely that ‘a teacher with a rich knowledge of gram- matical constructions and a more general awareness of the forms and varieties of the language will be in a better position to help young writers’ (2005: 75). This subject knowledge problem extends into initial teacher training courses, with Kolln and Hancock (2005) complaining that most pre-service programs for English teachers in the USA do
  • 12. 52 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) not address grammatical knowledge, and a number of UK studies reporting weaknesses in gram- matical knowledge (Andrews, 1994, 1999; Bloor, 1986; Burgess et al., 2000; Chandler et al., 1988; Hislam and Cajkler, 2006; Williamson and Hardman, 1995; Wray, 1993). In Australia, Louden et al. (2005) conducted a survey which indicated that teachers do not feel confident about teaching grammar when they complete their training, and Harper and Rennie’s pre-service teachers (2009) ‘showed limited understandings in their ability to analyse the parts and structure of sentences, and their knowledge of metalinguistic terms did not seem to extend past the basic concepts of “noun”, “verb” and “adjective” ’ (2009: 27). However, studies in the UK and Australia have indicated that some teachers may be over-critical or over-anxious in their self-evaluations, with Cajkler and Hislam (2002) finding that primary trainee teachers in the UK had reasonable knowledge of gram- mar despite being anxious about their subject knowledge, and Hammond and Macken-Horarik finding that primary teachers in Australia were confident in their knowledge of genres and text types despite expressing a lack of confidence in their own knowledge of ‘rules of traditional gram- mar’ (2001: 125). At the same time, in England, there has been criticism of the accuracy or appro- priacy of curriculum materials prepared to support teaching grammar (Cajkler, 1999, 2002, 2004; Cameron, 1997). The combination of curricular expectation that students will have explicit knowledge of gram- mar, and the tendency towards an absence of grammatical knowledge in the academic experiences of English teachers, creates a clear challenge for pedagogical practice and student learning. Research into teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge related to grammar teaching is limited, yet there is evidence from second-language research that strong declarative grammatical knowledge does not necessarily translate simply into effective teaching of language (Andrews, 2001; Borg, 1999, 2001, 2003). Pedagogical content knowledge is dependent upon teachers having a clear understanding of the role that grammatical knowledge can play in the classroom, without which teachers may generate mismatches between their own use of grammatical metalanguage and stu- dents’ understanding (Berry, 1997) or unwittingly convey inappropriate messages to learners. Lefstein (2009), for example, illustrates how teachers using policy materials underpinned by a principally rhetorical notion of grammar can use them in meaningless or rule-bound prescriptive ways, while the trainees in Cajkler and Hislam’s study (2002) understood grammatical knowledge as essentially about the naming of grammatical constructions but did not understand that pedagogi- cally ‘grammatical awareness is about making available a range of choices for writers to use for particular purposes in particular contexts’ (2002: 176). In a more recent UK study, Myhill et al. (2013) have identified a range of pedagogical problems faced by teachers who attempt to address grammar. These include: • the difficulty of defining and explaining grammatical terms, exacerbated by teachers’ ten- dency to use semantic rather than functional definitions; also reported by Paraskevas (2004); • the difficulty of explaining sentences and syntactical features, exacerbated by the tendency to reduce explanations of sentence types to issues of length and to use other non-grammatical explanations; • the difficulty of making meaningful connections between grammar and writing, seen par- ticularly in the tendency to communicate highly generalized principles for writing that are difficult for learners to operationalize meaningfully, such as ‘vary your sentences’, and the tendency to talk vaguely and non-specifically about ‘effects’; and • the difficulty of fostering metalinguistic discussion, a risky activity that requires teachers to deal with misunderstandings, unexpected questions and unexpected answers without advance preparation.
  • 13. Myhill and Watson 53 This study also, however, reported examples of teachers who were able to make meaningful con- nections between the linguistic features being studied and specific context-relevant effects or pur- poses, and who responded sensitively to students’ writing by explicitly drawing out effective grammatical choices and challenging students to reflect metalinguistically on their own writing. This focus on the relationship between teachers’ subject knowledge and pedagogical knowledge relating to grammar is mirrored in Australia by Jones and Chen (2012), who have demonstrated how teachers have been able to evolve new understandings of both grammatical knowledge and pedagogical practice through participatory collaboration with a research team. Current research therefore indicates the vital importance of addressing teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge alongside subject knowledge of grammar. V  Discussion To an extent, what this review signals most clearly is that consideration of a fully theorized role for grammar within the writing curriculum has largely been hijacked by political–professional debates about grammar’s inclusion or not, and that the debate has not substantially developed since it began in the early 1960s. The same arguments are voiced and re-voiced over time, but with little re-theorization or advancement. Silently underpinning this long-standing debate, and perhaps perpetuating it, is the fundamental distinction between prescriptive and descriptive views of grammar. Whilst linguists have long theorized grammar as a description of language which will change and evolve as language changes, public and political views of grammar have tended strongly to prescriptive views of grammar. Hudson and Walmsley, in their analysis of the separate development of academic linguistics and grammar teaching, draw attention to ‘an ever-widening gap in England between the practice of professional grammarians, on the one hand, and the lay public and practice in schools on the other’ (Hudson and Walmsley, 2005: 595). However, this binary division between linguistics and educational perspectives on grammar is not wholly correct. Following the eschewing of grammar teaching in Anglophone countries in the 1960s, an alterna- tive view of grammar emerged that was characterized by a view of grammar as ‘knowledge about language’, a view that was significantly influenced by sociolinguistics (e.g. the work of Peter Trudgill; see, for example, Trudgill, 2000) and was descriptive in theorization. In England, the emphasis in the Bullock Report (DES, 1975) on the importance of developing students’ under- standing of language and its rejection of traditional grammar teaching was highly influential, and more fully realized in the late 1980s in the government-commissioned national project, Language in the National Curriculum (LINC), to investigate classroom approaches to developing knowledge about language. The resultant materials, subsequently rejected by the government, drew heavily on the work of Britton and Halliday, focusing on functionally oriented understandings of language in context and on placing meaning at the centre of attention. These materials have had a significant legacy: the dominant professional view of grammar within the writing curriculum is strongly ori- ented towards a descriptive approach (Watson, 2012; Wyse, 2006). Therefore, it is possible to argue that, despite the prolonged debate, one element of the theorization of grammar in the writing curriculum from both a linguistic and theoretical perspective is that it is founded upon a theory of descriptive, functionally oriented grammar. Given this foundation, the review also points to ways in which this theorization may be developed further. Within the theoretical perspectives offered by Systemic Functional Linguistics, and rhetorical grammar – and the recent empirical work of Fearn and Farnan (2007), Macken-Horarik et al. (2011), Myhill et al. (2012), and Schleppegrell (2007) – there are signs of an emerging consensus that grammar may be important in developing learners’ understanding of how language works and, specifically, how grammar choices are significant in shaping and constructing meaning.
  • 14. 54 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Cameron argued theoretically that ‘knowing grammar is knowing how more than knowing what’ (1997: 236), pointing out that grammatical terminology is simply the tool that facilitates lan- guage investigation and analysis. To set this in a classroom context, this means that it is more important to know how a passive construction alters the emphasis in information conveyed than it is to know that it is a passive construction. The recent empirical work, described above, is providing evidence of how this knowledge is realized in practice and how young writers develop understanding that ‘it is through the rhetorical and syntactic forms they choose that the content is constructed and evaluated’ (Schleppegrell, 2007: 122). This consensus is less concerned with grammar as an arbiter of accuracy, and more concerned with ‘insights that go well beyond the minimum needed to write conventionally or correctly’ (Hancock, 2009: 194). Carter and McCarthy (2006: 7) draw attention to the notion of grammar as choice, as well as a grammar of structure, and Myhill et al. (2011) have conceptualized the contextual teaching of grammar within the writing curriculum as one that seeks to open up to young writers ‘a repertoire of infi- nite possibilities’. Taking this theorization one step further, fostering young writers’ awareness of the linguistic choices available to them in writing and how those choices differently shape meaning is develop- ing their metalinguistic knowledge of writing. Gombert’s (1992) taxonomy of metalinguistic knowledge, as noted earlier, relates principally to oral language development, but the category of metapragmatic knowledge is concerned with language in use, e.g. language in social contexts. Arguably, adopting descriptive approaches to grammar and nurturing students’ abilities to make choices and decisions in their writing is developing this metapragmatic knowledge, and rendering it available to inform the process of writing. Such knowledge is explicit, what Roehr described as ‘declarative knowledge that can be brought into awareness’ (Roehr, 2008: 179) and goes beyond the use of linguistic metalanguage to label and identify to include explicit knowledge of how lin- guistic choices subtly shape or alter meanings. It may also be significant that the empirical studies of Keen (2004) on sentence-combining, of Wyse (2006) on vocabulary choices, and Myhill et al. (2011) all seem to be highlighting the importance of talk, or metalinguistic discussion, in enabling this explicit metalinguistic knowledge of writing. Thus a more coherent theorization of a role for grammar in the curriculum might be framed as the teaching of grammar which promotes students’ explicit metalinguistic understanding of how grammar choices shape meaning in texts and of the writing choices available to them, founded upon a descriptive, functionally oriented understanding of grammar. VI  Conclusions This review of the literature has illustrated clearly that the role of grammar in first language teach- ing remains a source of dispute, a conclusion underlined by the political/professional debate in England at present about the grammar test at age 11 and the emphasis on grammar in the revised National Curriculum. The review also signals that robust empirical studies in this area are limited and – apart from indicating that simply teaching grammar as the isolated naming and labelling of word classes and syntactical structures is of little obvious benefit – the research is rather impover- ished in offering any theoretically-grounded understanding of children’s learning of grammar and how they transfer that learning to their own language use. It offers a way forward in theorizing a role for grammar in the writing curriculum as a functionally oriented endeavour, developing stu- dents’ metalinguistic thinking and decision-making in writing. It is also clear, however, that more well designed studies in this area are much needed to provide richer and broader understanding of how children develop metalinguistic understanding, how that learning transfers into their own language use, and the pedagogies that support the development of that understanding.
  • 15. Myhill and Watson 55 Declaration of conflicting interest The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest. Funding This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. References Anderson H (1995) Monstrous plans? Standard English in the National Curriculum and Norwegian in the ‘Monsterplan’. English in Education 29: 14–19. Andrews R (2005) Knowledge about the teaching of sentence grammar: The state of play. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 4: 69–76. Andrews R, Torgerson C, Beverton S, Freeman A, Locke T, Low G, Robinson A, and Zhu D (2006) The effect of grammar teaching on writing development. British Educational Research Journal 32: 39–55. Andrews S (1994) The grammatical knowledge and awareness of Hong Kong teachers of English. In: Bird N, Falvey P, Tsui ABM, Allison D, and McNeill A (eds) Language and learning. Proceedings of ILEC 1993. Hong Kong: Institute of Language in Education, Education Department, 508–20. Andrews S (1999) All these like little name things: A comparative study of language teachers’ explicit knowl- edge of grammar and grammatical terminology. Language Awareness 8: 143–59. Andrews S (2001) The language awareness of the L2 teacher: Its impact upon pedagogical practice. Language Awareness 10: 75–90. Armstrong K (2004) Sexing up the dossier: A semantic analysis of phrasal verbs for language teachers. Language Awareness 14: 213–24. Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (ACARA) (2009) Framing English. Sydney: ACARA. Barton G (1999) Grammar in context. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bateman DR and Zidonis FJ (1966) The effect of a study of transformational grammar on the writing of ninth and tenth graders. Champagne, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Benjamin A and Jago C (2006) Teacher to Teacher: What is your most compelling reason for teaching gram- mar? English Journal 95: 18–21. Bernstein B (1971) Class, codes and control: Volume 1. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Berry R (1997) Teachers’ awareness of learners’ knowledge: The case of metalinguistic terminology. Language Awareness 6: 136–46. Bialystok E (2007) Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning 57: 45–77. Bloor T (1986) What do language students know about grammar? British Journal of Language Teaching 24: 157–60. Board of Education (1921) The teaching of English in England (Newbolt Report). London: HMSO. Boivin M-C (2009) Manipulations syntaxiques et jugements de grammaticalité dans le travail en classe d’élèves du secondaire [Syntactical manipulations and grammatical judgements in the work of second- ary pupils]. In: Joaquim D and Simard C (ed.) Pratiques d’enseignement grammatical. Points de vue de l’enseignant et de l’élève: Premier numéro de la collection Recherches en didactique du français. Québec: Presses de l’Université Laval, 179–208. Boivin M-C and Pinsonneault R (2008) Description grammaticale et transposition didactique: le cas des infinitives et des participiales [Grammatical description and didactic transposition: The case of infini- tives and participles]. Enjeux 71: 29–57. Borg S (1999) The use of grammatical terminology in the second language classroom: A qualitative study of teachers’ practices and cognitions. Applied Linguistics 20: 95–126. Borg S (2001) Self-perception and practice in teaching grammar. ELT Journal 55: 21–29. Borg S (2003) Teacher cognition in grammar teaching: A literature review. Language Awareness 12: 96–108.
  • 16. 56 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Bourassa D, Treiman R, and Kessler B (2006) Use of morphology in spelling by children with dyslexia and typically developing children. Memory and Cognition 34: 703–14. Braddock R, Lloyd-Jones R, and Schoer L (1963) Research in written composition. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Brumfit C (1997) The teacher as educational linguist. In: van Lier L and Corson D (eds) Encyclopedia of lan- guage and education: Volume 6: Knowledge about language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 163–72. Burgess T, Turvey A, and Quarshie R (2000) Teaching grammar: Working with student teachers. Changing English 7: 7–21. Cajkler W (1999) Misconceptions in the NLS: National literacy strategy or no linguistic sense? Use of English 50: 214–27. Cajkler W (2002) Literacy across the curriculum at KS3: More muddle and confusion. Use of English 53: 151–64. Cajkler W (2004) How a dead butler was killed: The way English national strategies maim grammatical parts. Language and Education 18: 1–16. Cajkler W and Hislam J (2002) Trainee teachers’ grammatical knowledge: The tension between public expec- tations and individual competence. Language Awareness 11: 161–77. Cameron D (1994) Dr Syntax and Mrs Grundy: Grammar, myths and morals. Changing English: studies in reading and culture 2: 34–43. Cameron D (1995) Verbal hygiene. London: Routledge. Cameron D (1997) Sparing the rod: What teachers need to know about grammar. Changing English: studies in reading and culture 4: 229–39. Cameron D and Bourne J (1988) No common ground: Kingman, grammar and the nation. Language and Education 2: 147–60. Caravolas M, Kessler B, Hulme C, and Snowling M (2005) Effects of orthographic consistency, frequency, and letter knowledge on children’s vowel spelling development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 92: 307–21. Carter R (ed.) (1990) Knowledge about Language. London: Hodder and Stoughton. Carter R and McCarthy M (2006) Cambridge grammar of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Chandler P, Robinson W, and Noyes P (1988) The level of linguistic knowledge and awareness amongst student teachers training to be primary teachers. Language and Education 2: 161–74. Christie F and Unsworth L (2006) Developing dimensions of an educational linguistics. In: Webster J, Matthiessen C, and Hasan R (eds) Continuing discourse on language: A functional perspective. London: Equinox, 217–50. Clark U (2010) Grammar in the curriculum for English: What next? ChangingEnglish 17: 189–200. Coffin C (2010) Language support in EAL contexts: Why systemic functional linguistics? Editorial. Special Issue of NALDIC Quarterly: 2–5. Common Core State Standards Initiative (CCSSI) (2012) Common Core State Standards for Language Arts. Washington, DC: National Governors Association (NGA) Center and Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO). Retrieved from: http://www.corestandards.org/ELA-Literacy (November 2013). Connors RJ (2000) The erasure of the sentence. College Composition and Communication 52: 96–128. Crowhurst M (1980) Syntactic complexity and teachers’ quality ratings of narrations and arguments. Research in the Teaching of English 14: 223–31. Crowley T (2003) Standard English and the politics of language. London: Palgrave Macmillan. Culioli A (1990) Pour une linguistique de l’enonciation: Tome 1 [Towards a linguistic enunciation: Volume 1]. Paris: Ophrys Daiker DA, Kerek A, and Morenberg M (1978) Sentence-combining and syntactic maturity in freshman English. College Composition and Communication 29: 36–41. Christie F (2004) Revisiting some old themes: The role of grammar in the teaching of English. In: Foley JA (ed.) Language, education and discourse: Functional approaches. London and New York: Continuum, 145–73.
  • 17. Myhill and Watson 57 Christie F and Derewianka B (2008) School discourse: Learning to write across the years of schooling. London and New York: Continuum. Dawkins J (1995) Teaching punctuation as a rhetorical tool. College Composition and Communication 46: 533–48. Denham K and Lobeck A (2005) Language in the schools: Integrating linguistics into K12 teaching. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Derewianka B and Jones P (2010) From traditional to grammar to functional grammar: Bridging the divide. Special Issue of NALDIC Quarterly: 6–15. DES (Department of Education and Science) (1975) A language for life: The Bullock report. London: HMSO. DES (Department of Education and Science) (1990) English in the National Curriculum. London: HMSO. DfE (Department for Education) (1995) English in the National Curriculum. London: HMSO. DfE (Department for Education) (2013a) The National Curriculum in England: Framework document: Grammar annex. London: DfE. Retrieved from: https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/ uploads/attachment_data/file/210969/NC_framework_document_-_FINAL.pdf (November 2013). DfE (Department for Education) (2013b) 2013 key stage 2 tests: English grammar, spelling, and punctuation test. London: DfE. Retrieved from: http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/teachingandlearning/assess- ment/keystage2/b00208296/ks2-2013/english-tests/grammar-punctuation-spelling-test (November 2013). DfE (Department for Education) (2013c) English programmes of study: Key stages 1 and 2. DFE-00181- 2013. London: DfE. DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (1998) The national literacy strategy: Framework for teaching. London: DfEE. DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (1999) The National Curriculum for England, English Key Stages 1–4. London: DfEE. DfEE (Department for Education and Employment) (2000) The National Literacy Strategy: Grammar for Writing. London: DfEE. DfES (Department for Education and Skills) (2001) Framework for teaching English: Years 7, 8 and 9. London: DfES. Downing J and Oliver P (1974) The child’s concept of a word. Reading Research Quarterly 9: 568–82. Ehrenworth M (2003) Grammar-comma-a new beginning. English Journal 92: 90–96. Elbow P (1981) Writing with power: Techniques for mastering the writing process. New York: Oxford. Elley WB, Barham IH, Lamb H, and Wylie M (1975) The role of grammar in a secondary school curriculum. New Zealand Council for Educational Studies 10: 26–41. Elley WB, Barham IH, Lamb H, and Wylie M (1979) The role of grammar in a secondary school curriculum: Educational research series No 60. Wellington: New Zealand Council for Educational Research. EPPI Review Group for English (2004) The effect of grammar teaching (syntax) in English on 5 to 16 year olds’ accuracy and quality in written composition. London: Evidence for Policy and Practice Information and Co-ordinating Centre (EPPI-Centre). Faigley L (1980) Names in search of a concept: Maturity, fluency, complexity, and growth in written syntax. College Composition and Communication 31: 291–300. Fayol M, Largy P, and Lemaire P (1994) When cognitive overload enhances subject-verb agreement errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 47A: 473–64. Fayol M, Zorman M, and Lété B (2009) Associations and dissociations in reading and spelling French: Unexpectedly poor and good spellers. British Journal of Educational Psychology 2: 63–75. Fayol M, Hickmann M, Bonnotte I, and Gombert JE (1993) The effects of narrative context on French verbal inflections: A developmental perspective. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 22: 453–78. Fearn L and Farnan N (2007) When is a verb using functional grammar to teach writing. Journal of Basic Writing 26: 1–26. Fogel H and Ehri LC (2000) Teaching elementary students who speak black English vernacular to write in Standard English: Effects of dialect transformation practice. Contemporary Educational Psychology 25: 212–35.
  • 18. 58 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Freebody P, Maton K, and Martin J (2008) Talk, text, and knowledge in cumulative, integrated learning: A response to ‘intellectual challenge’. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 31: 188–201. Gold D (2006) But when do you teach grammar? Allaying community concerns about pedagogy. English Journal 95: 42–47. Gombert EJ (1992) Metalinguistic development. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Gordon E (2005) Grammar in New Zealand schools: Two case studies. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 4: 48–68. Graham S and Perin D (2007) A meta-analysis of writing instruction for adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology 99: 445–76. Hadjioannou X and Hutchinson M (2010) Putting the G back in English: Preparing pre-service teachers to teach grammar. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 9: 90–105. Hagemann JA (2003) Balancing content and form in the writing workshop. English Journal 92: 73–79. Hake R and Williams JM (1979) Sentence expanding: Not can, or how, but when. In: Daiker D, Morenberg A, and Kerek M (eds) Sentence-combining and the teaching of writing. Conway, AR: L&S Books, 134–46. Halliday MAK (1978) Language as social semiotic: The social interpretation of language and meaning. London: Edward Arnold. Halliday MAK (1993) Towards a language-based theory of learning. 5: 93–116. Halliday MAK (1994) An introduction to functional grammar. London: Hodder. Halliday MAK (2003) Introduction: On the ‘architecture’ of human language. In: Webster J (ed.) On language and linguistics: Volume 3 in the collected works of MAK Halliday. London and New York: Continuum, 1–29. Hammond J and Macken-Horarik M (2001) Teachers’ voices, Teachers’ practices. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 24: 112–32. Hancock C (2009) How Linguistics can Inform the Teaching of Writing. In: Beard R, Myhill D, Riley J, and Nystrand M (eds) The Sage handbook of writing development. London: Sage, 194–208. Harper H and Rennie J (2009) ‘I had to go out and get myself a book on grammar’: A study of pre-service teachers’ knowledge about language. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 32: 22–37. Harris RJ (1962) An experimental enquiry into the functions of and value of formal grammar in the teaching of English. Unpublished PhD thesis, London University, UK. Hartwell P (1985) Grammar, grammars, and the teaching of grammar. College English 47: 105–27. Hasan R (2002) Ways of meaning, ways of learning: Code as an explanatory concept. British Journal of Sociology of Education 23: 537–48. Heibert E, Cioffi G, and Antonak R (1984) A developmental sequence in preschool children’s acquisition of reading readiness skills and print awareness concepts. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 5: 115–26. Hillocks G (1984) What works in teaching composition: A meta-analysis of experimental treatment studies. American Journal of Education 93: 133–70. Hillocks G and Mavrognes N (1986) Sentence combining. In: Hillocks G (ed.) Research on written composi- tion: New directions for teaching. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, 142–46. Hillocks G Jr and Smith M (1991) Grammar and usage. In: Flood J, Jensen JM, Lapp D, and Squire JS (eds) Handbook of research on teaching the English language arts. New York: Macmillan. Hislam J and Cajkler W (2006) Teacher trainees’ explicit knowledge of grammar and primary curricu- lum requirements in England. In: Bartels N (ed.) Applied linguistics and language teacher education. Netherlands: Springer, 295–312. Hudson R (2004) Why education needs linguistics. Journal of Linguistics 40: 105–30. Hudson R and Walmsley J (2005) The English patient: English grammar and teaching in the twentieth cen- tury. Journal of Linguistics 41: 593–622. Jayman J, Doolan L, Hoover M, Maas S, McHugh T, Mooney K, and Zepp A (2006) Sentence patterns: Making meaning with a countywide grammar initiative. English Journal 95: 41–47. Jones PT and Chen H (2012) Teachers’ knowledge about language: Issues of pedagogy and expertise. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 35: 147–72.
  • 19. Myhill and Watson 59 Jones SM, Myhill DA, and Bailey TC (2013) Grammar for writing? An investigation into the effect of contex- tualised grammar teaching on student writing. Reading and Writing Online 26: 1241–63. Kamler B (1995) The grammar wars or what do teachers need to know about grammar? English in Australia 114: 3–15. Karmiloff-Smith A, Grant J, Sims K, Jones M-C, and Cuckle P (1996) Rethinking metalinguistic awareness: Representing and accessing knowledge about what counts as a word. Cognition 58: 197–219. Keen J (2004) Sentence-combining and redrafting processes in the writing of secondary school students in the UK. Linguistics and Education: An International Research Journal 15: 17–97. Keith G (1990) Language study at KS3. In: Carter R (ed.) Knowledge about language. London: Hodder and Stoughton. 69–103. Kelly A and Safford K (2009) Does teaching complex sentences have to be complicated? Lessons from chil- dren’s online writing. Literacy 43: 118–22. Kinneavy JL (1979) Sentence combining in a comprehensive language framework. In: Daiker D, Morenberg A, and Kerek M (eds) Sentence-combining and the teaching of writing. Conway, AR: L&S Books, 60–76. Kolln M (2006) Rhetorical grammar: Grammatical choices, rhetorical effects. New York: Longman. Kolln M and Hancock C (2005) The story of English grammar in US Schools. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 4: 11–31. Leech GN (1994) Students’ grammar – teachers’ grammar – learners’ grammar. In: Bygate M, Tonkyn A, and Williams E (eds) Grammar and the language teacher. New York: Prentice Hall, 17–30. Lefstein A (2009) Rhetorical grammar and the grammar of schooling: Teaching ‘powerful verbs’ in the English National Literacy Strategy. Linguistics and Education 20: 378–400. Leger V (no date) Introduction à la nouvelle grammaire [An introduction to new grammar]. Retrieved from: http://www.yumpu.com/fr/document/view/17000673/introduction-a-la-nouvelle-grammaire (November 2013). Locke T (2005) Grammar wars: Beyond a truce. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 4: 1–10. Locke T (2009) Grammar and writing: The international debate. In: Beard R, Myhill D, Nystrand M, and Riley J (eds) International handbook of writing development. London: SAGE, 182–93. Locke T (ed.) (2010) Beyond the grammar wars: A resource for teachers and students on developing lan- guage knowledge in the English/literacy classroom. London: Routledge Louden W, Rohl M, Gore J et al. (2005) Prepared to teach: An investigation into the preparation of teach- ers to teach literacy and numeracy. Canberra, ACT: Australian Government Department of Education, Science and Training. MacDonald H (1995) Why Johnny can’t write: Teaching grammar and logic to college students. Public Interest 120. Retrieved from: http://www.nationalaffairs.com/public_interest/detail/why-johnny-cant- write (November 2013). MacGillivray L (1994) Tacit shared understandings of a first-grade writing community. Journal of Reading Behavior 26: 245–66. Macken-Horarik M (2001) Something to shoot for: a systemic functional approach to teaching genre in sec- ondary school science. In: Johns AM (ed.) Genre in the classroom: Multiple perspectives. London: Lawrence Erlbaum. Macken-Horarik M, Love K, and Unsworth L (2011) A grammatics ‘good enough’ for school English in the 21st century: Four challenges in realising the potential. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 34: 9–23. Marzano RJ (1976) Sentence-combining myth. English Journal 65: 57–59. Micciche L (2004) Making a case for rhetorical grammar. College Composition and Communication 55: 716–37. Muller HJ (1967) The uses of English: Guidelines for the teaching of English from the Anglo-American con- ference at Dartmouth College. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Myhill DA (2004) Making connections: Grammar and meaning. Secondary English Magazine 8: 23–26. Myhill DA (2006) Designs on writing 2: Sentences. Secondary English Magazine 10: 25–28.
  • 20. 60 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Myhill DA (2011a) Grammar for designers: How grammar supports the development of writing. In: Ellis S, McCartney E, and Bourne J (eds) Insight and impact: Applied linguistics and the primary school. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 81–92. Myhill DA (2011b) ‘The ordeal of deliberate choice’: Metalinguistic development in secondary writers. In: Berninger V (ed.) Past, present, and future contributions of cognitive writing research to cognitive psy- chology. Psychology Press/Taylor Francis Group, 247–74. Myhill DA and Jones SM (2011) Policing grammar: The place of grammar in literacy policy. In: Goodwyn A and Fuller C (eds) The literacy game. London: Routledge, 45–62. Myhill DA, Jones S, and Watson A (2013) Grammar matters: How teachers’ grammatical subject knowledge impacts on the teaching of writing. Teaching and Teacher Education 36: 77–91. Retrieved from: http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2013.07.0052 (November 2103). Myhill DA, Lines HE, and Watson A (2011) Making meaning with grammar: A repertoire of possibilities. mETAphor 2: 1–10. Myhill DA, Jones SM, Lines H, and Watson A (2012) Re-thinking grammar: The impact of embedded grammar teaching on students’ writing and students’ metalinguistic understanding. Research Papers in Education 27: 1–28. Nadeau M and Fisher C (2009) Faut-il des connaissances explicites en grammaire pour réussir les accords en français écrit? [Is explicit grammar knowledge needed to succeed with agreements in written French?]. In: Dolz J and Simard C (eds) Pratiques d’enseignement grammatical, points de vue de l’enseignant et de l’élève; Recherches en didactique du français [Practices in grammar teaching, perspectives from teachers and students; Research in teaching French]. Quebec: Presses de l’Université Laval, 209–31. Nadeau M and Fisher C (2011) Les connaissances implicites et explicites en grammaire: Quelle impor- tance pour l’enseignement? Quelles conséquences? [Implicit and explicit grammar knowledge: What is the significance for teaching? What consequences?]. Bellaterra Journal of Teaching and Learning Language and Literature 4: 1–31. National Association for the Teaching of English (NATE) (1997) Position Paper No 1: Grammar. Sheffield: NATE. Nunes T, Bryant P, and Bindman M (2006) The effects of learning to spell on children’s awareness of mor- phology. Reading and Writing 19: 767–87. O’Hare F (1971) Sentence combining: Improving student writing without formal grammar instruction. NCTE Research Report 15. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Paraskevas C (2004) Learning about grammar. International Journal of Learning 11: 93–99. Paraskevas C (2006) Grammar apprenticeship. English Journal 95: 65–69. Petit A (2003) The stylish semicolon: Teaching punctuation as rhetorical choice. English Journal 92: 66–72. Petruzella BA (1996) Grammar instruction: What teachers say. English Journal 85: 68–72. Phillips SE (1996) Sentence combining: A literature review. ERIC document number ED398589. Unpublished literature review, University of Texas, Dallas, TX, USA. Pomphrey C and Moger R (1999) Cross-subject dialogue about language: Attitudes and perceptions of PGCE students of English and Modern Languages. Language Awareness 8: 223–36. Poulin D (1980) Grammaire traditionnelle et grammaire nouvelle, ou De l’analyse à l’analyse structural [Traditional grammar and new grammar, or From analysis to structural analysis]. Quebec Français 40: 29–32. Pullman P (2005) Common sense has much to learn from moonshine. The Guardian 22 January 2005. Retrieved from: http://www.guardian.co.uk/education/2005/jan/22/schools.wordsandlanguage (November 2013). QCA (Qualifications | Curriculum Authority) (1998) The grammar papers: Perspectives on the teaching of grammar in the National Curriculum. London: QCA. Robinson N (1959) The relation between knowledge of English Grammar and ability in English composi- tions. Unpublished MEd thesis, University of Manchester, UK. Roehr K (2008) Metalinguistic knowledge and language ability in university-level L2 learners. Applied Linguistics 29: 173–99. Rose C (1996) The great debate: Teaching grammar and usage. English Journal 85: 59–63.
  • 21. Myhill and Watson 61 Saddler B and Graham S (2005) The effects of peer-assisted sentence-combining instruction on the writing performance of more and less skilled young writers. Journal of Educational Psychology 97: 43–54. Savage J (1980) Sentence combining: A positive practice. Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates. Schleppegrell MJ (2007) The meaning in grammar. Research in the Teaching of English 42: 121–28. Schleppegrell MJ (2008) Grammar, the sentence, and traditions of linguistic analysis. In: Bazerman C (ed.) Handbook of writing research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 549–64. Schleppegrell MJ (2009) Grammar for generation 1.5: A focus on meaning. In: Roberge M, Siegal M, and Harklau L (eds) Generation 1.5 in college composition: Teaching academic writing to US-educated learners of ESL. New York: Routledge, 221–34. Schleppegrell MJ (2010) Supporting a ‘reading to write’ pedagogy with functional grammar. NALDIC Quarterly 8: 26–31. Schleppegrell MJ (2011) Supporting disciplinary learning through language analysis: Developing historical literacy. In: Christie F and Maton K (eds) Disciplinarity: Functional linguistic and sociological perspec- tives. London: Continuum, 197–216. Schleppegrell MJ (2012) Systemic functional linguistics: Exploring meaning in language. In; Gee JP and Handford M (eds) The Routledge handbook of discourse analysis. New York: Routledge, 21–34. Sealey A (1999) Teaching primary school children about the English language: A critique of current policy documents. Language Awareness 8: 84–97. Sheard M, Chambers B, Slavin R, and Elliott L (2012) Effects of technology-enhanced formative assessment on achievement in primary grammar. York: Institute for Effective Education. Shortis T and Blake J (2010) Who’s prepared to teach school English? The degree level qualifications and preparedness of initial teacher trainees in English. London: CLIE. Shulman LS (1987) Knowledge and teaching: Foundations of the new reform. Harvard Educational Review 57: 1–22. ter Kuile H, Veldhuis M, van Veen S, and Wicherts M (2011) Bilingual education, metalinguistic awareness, and the understanding of an unknown language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 233–42. Thévenin MG, Totereau C, Fayol M, and Jarousse JP (1999) L’apprentissage/enseignement de la morpholo- gie écrite du nombre en français [The teaching/learning of the written morphology of number in French]. Revue Française de Pédagogie 126: 39–52. Tomlinson D (1994) Errors in the research into the effectiveness of grammar teaching. English in Education 28: 20–26. Trudgill P (2000) Sociolinguistics: An introduction to language and society. 4th edition. London: Penguin. Truss L (2003) Eats, shoots and leaves: The zero tolerance approach to punctuation. London: Profile Books. Tunmer WE, Bowey JA, and Grieve R (1983) The development of young children’s awareness of the word as a unit of spoken language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 12: 567–94. Turvey A (2000) Teaching grammar: Working with student teachers 2. Changing English 7: 139–52. Uttley T (2008) Britain’s got talent? If you ask me, Britain’s got problems. Daily Mail 6 June 2008. Retrieved from: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-1024582/Britains-Got-Talent-If-ask-Britains-got- problems.html#ixzz0kKDkPGPy (November 2013). van Gelderen A (2006) What we know without knowing it: Sense and nonsense in respect of linguistic reflec- tion for students in elementary and secondary education. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 5: 44–54. van Gelderen A (2010) Does explicit teaching of grammar help students to become better writers? Insights from empirical research. In: Locke T (ed.) Beyond the grammar wars: A resource for teachers and students on developing language knowledge in the English/literacy classroom. New York; Routledge, 109–28. van Lier L (1998) The relationship between consciousness, interaction and language learning. Language Awareness 7: 128–45. Vavra E (1996) On not teaching grammar. English Journal 85: 32–37. Watson A (2012) Navigating ‘The Pit Of Doom’: Affective responses to teaching ‘grammar’. English in Education 46: 22–37. Weaver C (1996) Teaching grammar in the context of writing. English Journal 85: 15–24.
  • 22. 62 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1) Weaver C and Bush J (2006) Grammar intertwined throughout the writing process: An ‘inch wide and a mile deep’. English Teaching: Practice and Critique 5: 77–101. Williams G (2005) Grammatics in schools. In: Hasan R, Matthiessen C, and Webster J (eds) Continuing dis- course on language: A functional perspective: Volume 1. London: Equinox, 281–310. Williamson J and Hardman F (1995) Time for refilling the bath? A study of primary student-teachers’ gram- matical knowledge. Language and Education 9: 23–45. Wilson AC and Myhill DA (2012) Ways with words: Teachers’ personal epistemologies of the role of meta- language in the teaching of poetry writing. Language and Education 26: 553–68. Witte SP (1980) Sentence combining and the teaching of writing. College Composition and Communication 31: 433–37. Wray D (1993) Student-teachers’ knowledge and beliefs about language. In: Bennett N and Carré C (eds) Learning to teach. London: Routledge, 51–72. Wyse D (2001) Grammar. For writing?: A critical review of empirical evidence. British Journal of Educational Studies 49: 411–27. Wyse D (2006) Pupils’ word choices and the teaching of grammar. Cambridge Journal of Education 36: 31–47. Zipke M (2007) The role of metalinguistic awareness in the reading comprehension of sixth and seventh grad- ers. Reading Psychology 28: 375–96.
  • 23. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.