SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 22
Download to read offline
1
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
The role of divergent thinking and convergent thinking in the relation between
intelligence and literacy ability
Masterthesis
Universiteit Utrecht
Masteropleiding Pedagogische Wetenschappen
Masterprogramma Orthopedagogiek
Name: E. Bekker-Janssens
Student number: 4254694
Supervisor: dr. E.H. Kroesbergen
Second supervisor: dr. J.H. van de Beek
Date: 17-6-2015
Number of words: 3.662
2
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Voorwoord
Allereerst wil ik jou bedanken, Evelyn, voor alle tijd, ruimte en feedback die je me hebt
gegeven. Steeds opnieuw wist je me richting te geven zonder te sturen. Ik ben tevreden over
het eindresultaat en heb van het proces genoten.
Jan, bedankt voor het kritisch mee- kijken en denken. Lotte, Eveline, Sanne en Lisanne, dank
voor de fijne samenwerking bij het afnemen van alle tests en het scoren van de schrijftaken.
Oma Rita, bedankt voor al die keren dat Abel gezellig met Ties mocht spelen als ik uurtjes
tekort kwam. Bedankt lieve Anne, Evelien, Marijke, Marieke, Marion Mariette en Steffie. Ik
was een vreselijk nalatige zus /dochter /vriendin de afgelopen periode, maar jullie bleven
onverminderd begripvol.
En dan mijn mannen: Martijn, Stijn, Mees en Abel. Het was vast niet altijd leuk, mama achter
de laptop aan de keukentafel, bedolven onder artikelen, krabbels en boeken. Toch heb ik mij
van het begin af aan door jullie gesteund gevoeld in dit proces. Zonder woorden, maar
weldegelijk. Ik wil jullie daar ongelofelijk voor bedanken. Bedankt!
Peirce means "conceivable" very broadly. It allows any flight of imagination, provided this
imagination ultimately alights upon a possible practical effect; and thus many hypotheses
may seem at first glance to be excluded by the pragmatical maxim that are not really so
excluded.
3
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Abstract
The aim of this study was to get more in depth information as to why gifted children are
vulnerable to underachieving and low well-being. Based on preliminary research, it was
hypothesized that this might have to do with larger discrepancies between divergent and
convergent thinking above IQ 120, greater variability of academic performance and possible
relations between the two. It was expected that illuminating these relations might provide us
with essential information about the functioning of gifted children. For this purpose, 458
Dutch children from 12 regular primary schools participated in intelligence- and creativity
tests. Also scores of Dutch standardized literacy tests were taken into account. Based on
literature study and factor analysis, 6 variables were computed: intelligence, convergent
creativity (abstraction component), convergent creativity (innovation component), divergent
creativity, word reading and reading comprehension. Data were analysed using Kendall’s
Tau-B correlation and one-way between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA). It was found
that creative abstraction correlates positively with intelligence below the threshold, whereas
creative innovation only starts correlating above IQ 120. Discrepancies between convergent
and divergent creativity (convergent > divergent) were confirmed to be higher above the
threshold, as well as the variability between literacy scores. Discrepancies with divergent
thinking exceeding convergent thinking showed positive correlations with reading
comprehension. Possible explanations and suggestions for future research are being discussed.
Key words: intelligence, creativity, divergent thinking, convergent thinking, literacy
ability, underachieving, threshold theory, compensation theory, facilitation theory.
4
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
The role of divergent thinking and convergent thinking in the relation between intelligence
and literacy ability
Intelligence is the capacity to think logically and solve problems in novel situations,
independent of acquired knowledge (Cattell, 1963). Children with both high intelligence and
high creativity levels are referred to as being ‘gifted’ (Renzulli, 1986). Intelligence is known
to be a strong predictor of academic performance (Hansenne & Legrand, 2012). Yet, being
gifted is associated with underachieving (Kroesbergen, Van Hooijdonk, Middel-Lalleman,
Rijnders, & Van Viersen, 2015; Mann, 2006; Whitmore, 1980, cited in McCoach & Siegle,
2003) and low levels of well-being (Kroesbergen et al., 2015). This phenomenon can partly be
explained by the fact that our educational system is poorly adapted to the needs of gifted
children (Preckel, Gotz, & Frenzel, 2010). Educational fit and proper support are known to be
necessary conditions for gifted children to reach their full potential (Subotnik & Rickoff,
2010). This doesn’t explain though, why it is the gifted and not the highly intelligent child
that is underachieving and suffering from low well-being. Creativity seems to be of influence
on the relation between intelligence and academic performance. Yet, little is known about the
relation between these variables (Subotnik, Olszewski-Kubilius, & Worrell, 2011). The aim of
this study is to contribute to an increased insight in the relation between intelligence,
creativity and literacy ability in both gifted and non-gifted children.
Divergent and convergent thinking in the creative cognitive process
In studying the relation between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability it is
important to distinguish between divergent and convergent thinking (Barbot, Besencon and
Lubart, 2011; Cropley, 2006). Divergent thinking refers to the ability to view an idea from
different angles leading to various creative ideas. Divergent tasks are process-based and
focused on the quantity of a production. Divergent thinking is positively related to creative
potential (Runco & Acar, 2012) and can be measured by divergent thinking tests. The act of
combining various heterogeneous elements into a unique, original production refers to
convergent-integrative thinking (Barbot, 2011). Convergent-integrative tasks imply a product-
based approach, focusing on the quality of the production and leading to creative achievement
(Runco & Acar, 2012). In the current study, creativity is referred to as a cognitive process
consisting of both divergent and convergent thinking.
The relation between intelligence and creativity
Some research has shown that creativity test scores are independent from IQ scores,
whereas other research has confirmed a relationship between the two (Kim, 2005). According
to the threshold theory a positive linear correlation between intelligence and creativity is
5
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
present up to a certain point, usually IQ 120 (Jauk, Benedek, Dunst, & Neubauer, 2013). From
thereon up, the correlation between intelligence and creativity is said to be weak or absent
(Yamamoto, 1964a). The threshold theory has been extensively studied, with contradictory
and inconclusive results (Runco & Albert, 1986). This inconsistency may come from different
measures for creativity. Jung and colleagues (2009) provided neurobiological support for a
critical “threshold” regarding the relationship between intelligence and creativity, as measured
with divergent thinking tests. However, contemporary research shows that the breaking point
depends on the operationalization of the construct creativity. Quantitative criteria measure
divergent thinking, whereas qualitative criteria measure convergent thinking. It has been
found that quantitative criteria will lead to a lower breaking point than qualitative criteria
(Benedek, Franz, Heene, & Neubauer, 2012; Karwowski & Gralewski, 2013). Based on the
work of Benedek and collegues (2012) and Karwowski and collegues (2013), it could be
deduced that discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking might be larger in
gifted than in non-gifted children.
Figure 1. Threshold theory: significant correlations between creativity and intelligence below
the threshold (IQ = 120), with correlations above the threshold being weak or absent.
Literacy ability
In the Netherlands, primary schools mainly focus on two domains: mathematics and
literacy ability. Literacy ability consists of the sub-domains vocabulary, reading and spelling.
Two main skills in reading are word reading and reading comprehension (Oakhill & Chain,
2007). Word reading contains two fundamental aspects: decoding and automating. Once a
child is able to view each letter as a separate unit that corresponds to a particular sound, it can
6
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
learn to merge the sounds into words (Stanovich, 1986). Then, the child can be taught to
recognize combinations of letters. This is called automating (Perfetti, 1985). Word reading is
a necessary condition for reading comprehension (Clarke, Snowling, Truelove, & Hulme,
2010), which is the process of interacting with and giving meaning to a text (Bruner, 1985).
Literacy ability in gifted and non-gifted children
In the context of this research, literacy ability is particularly interesting because the
various subdomains seem to be differentially related to intelligence and creativity. With
regard to non-gifted children, strong positive correlations have been found between
intelligence and comprehensive reading (Stanovich, Cunningham & Freeman, 1984; Broos,
Fulker & De Vries, 1990), intelligence and vocabulary (Liegeois, Cross, Polkey, Harkness, &
Vargha-Khadem, 2008) and intelligence and word reading and spelling (Cornwall, 1992;
Strang, 1968). Positive correlations were also found between creativity and both reading
comprehension (Popov, 1992; Tien, Hsu, Tai, & Yang, 2014) and vocabulary (Al Issra, 1964).
Controversy exists about the relation between creativity and word reading and spelling.
Ritchie, Luciano, Hansell, Wright and Bates (2013) assessed reading, spelling and non word
repetition in a large sample, and studied associations with creativity. Based on the
compensatory theory, Ritchie and colleagues expected to find high creativity rates in children
with poor reading and spelling abilities. It was hypothesized that reading disability may
involve a compensatory cognitive benefit in the form of enhanced creativity (Chakravarty,
2009; Eide & Eide, 2011; Tafti, Hameedy, & Baghal, 2009). Yet, creativity was found to be
positively related to word reading and spelling, even when controlling for IQ (Ritchie et al.,
2013). These findings led to a novel theory: the theory of facilitation, which suggests that
fluency in written language may facilitate creative thought and curiosity (Ritchie et al., 2013).
In the current research, this theory of facilitation has been tested.
Little is known about the relation between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability in
gifted children. Based on the threshold theory, it could be hypothesized that above IQ 120
correlations between intelligence and creativity are weak or absent, and thus larger
discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking are conceivable. Finch, Neumeister,
Burney and Cook (2014) examined 61 gifted preschoolers and found that, despite intelligence
quotient scores in the very superior range (IQ > 130), great variability was observed on word
reading and spelling, with means ranging from average to superior levels. Large discrepancies
between divergent and convergent thinking are thought to be negatively related to (academic)
achievement (Chermahini & Hommel, 2010; Cropley, 2006; Lubow & Gewirtz, 1995; Wolf
& Mieg, 2010). In the current research, this hypothesis will be tested.
7
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Objectives and hypotheses
Based on the preliminary research, four hypotheses were formulated:
Hypothesis 1. Below the threshold (IQ = 120) intelligence and creativity correlate positively,
whereas above the threshold correlations are weak or absent.
Hypothesis 2. Discrepancies between divergent thinking and convergent thinking are larger
above than below the threshold (divergent > convergent).
Hypothesis 3. Variability in literacy ability scores is larger above than below the threshold.
Hypothesis 4. Discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking (divergent >
convergent) are negatively related to literacy ability scores.
Method
Participants
458 Dutch pupils from 12 regular primary schools participated in intelligence- and
creativity tests, after active consent was obtained from their parents. The participants were
recruited from 26 classes in grade 4 (n = 311), grade 5 (n = 73) and grade 6 (n = 74). Pupils in
grade 4 were offered a screening for gifted education simultaneously, resulting in a relatively
big sample size. The total sample consisted of boys (52%) and girls (48%) in the age of 8-13
years. The average age was 9.92 years (SD = 0.88).
Instruments
Intelligence: Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices has been used as a measure of
intelligence (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1992; Raven, 2000). Raven’s Standard Progressive
Matrices is a useful instrument to identify both high achievers and potential high achievers
who’s abilities have not yet fully developed (Kroesbergen et al.; 2015). In this study,
participants got 45 minutes to complete the task, the number of right answers was noted.
Abdel-Khalek (2005) found test-retest reliability ranging between .69 and .85 and Cronbach’s
alpha between .88 and .93.
Literacy ability: Literacy scores were retrieved from a national standardized
assessment, called CITO. This national literacy test consists of the items: spelling, reading
speed, reading comprehension and vocabulary. In the current research, the CITO literacy test
displayed Crohnbach’s alpha .523. To examine the underlying structure of the CITO literacy
scores more closely, data from all participants that had no missings on any of the literacy
measures (N=84) were subjected to principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. Two
factors were identified as underlying the four variables of the CITO literacy test. In total,
8
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
these factors accounted for 76% of the variance. Consequently, reading comprehension and
vocabulary were computed into one variable (referred to as: reading comprehension). The
other variable consisted of word reading and spelling (referred to as: word reading). Prior to
running the principal axis factoring, examination of the data indicated that most of the
variables were normally distributed and a linear relationship was identified among the
variables.
Creativity (divergent): Divergent thinking can be measured by the Torrance Test of
Creative Thinking (Torrance, Ball, & Safter, 2008). Test-retest reliabilities were reported in
the .60-.80 range (Haensly & Torrance, 1990). Since we chose to focus on literacy ability, the
verbal version of this test was selected (instead of the figural version). Activity 5 of the
verbal test showed good to excellent inter-rater agreement (fluency: kappa = .815, flexibility:
kappa = .710 and originality: kappa = .631) and good reliability (α = .841). To investigate the
underlying structure of the test, data collected from 458 participants were subjected to
principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. One factor (with Eigenvalue 2.61) was
identified, accounting for around 87% of the variance. Thus, the three variables (fluency,
flexibility and originality) were computed into a new variable, called: “divergent creativity”.
Given the robust nature of factor analysis and the fact that a linear relationship was identified
among the variables, the fact that variables were not perfectly normally distributed was
considered not to be problematic.
Creativity (convergent): The Creative Writing Task was a composition on a specific
topic, written by primary school children. The test had to be completed in 30 minutes and was
conducted in order to assess domain specific creative achievement. The Creative Writing Task
was designed by Tsai-Ling Chu and Wei-Wen Lin (2013). Chu and Lin (2013) have found the
inter-rater reliability correlation coefficients between two raters to be positive for technical
goodness (r = .67, p < .001) and creativity (r = .73, p < .001). In the current study, children
were not given a specific topic (sentence) but an image as an initial stimulus. The
Experimental Scoring Manual for Minnesota Tests for Creative Thinking and Writing
(Yamamoto, 1964b) was used by three raters to evaluate the creative writing products. On
each scale one item had to be deleted, due to the nature of our stimulus (visual instead of
verbal). To assess inter-rater agreement, Kappa was calculated for the remaining 24 items (6
scales, 4 items on each scale). Kappa scores varied between excellent (richness: .786;
originality: 1), good (sensitivity: .715; imagination: .742) and acceptable (organisation: .610;
psychological insight: .679). For the Creative Writing Task Crohnbach’s alpha was .652. To
investigate the underlying structure of the Creative Writing Task, data collected from 418
9
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
participants was subjected to principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. Prior to running
the principal axis factoring, examination of data indicated that the variables were not perfectly
normally distributed. Given the robust nature of factor analysis, these deviations were not
considered problematic. Furthermore, a lineair relationship was identified among the
variables. Two factors (with Eigenvalues exceeding 1) were identified as underlying the six
variables of the Creative Writing Task. In total, these factors accounted for around 53% of the
variance in the Creative Writing Task data. The first factor consisted of the variables:
sensitivity, imagination, richness and organisation. This factor was computed into a new
variable, called: “convergent creativity: abstraction”. The second factor consisted of:
originality, psychological insight and organisation, and was computed into the new variable:
“convergent creativity: innovation”.
Procedure
The current study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Social
Sciences of Utrecht University. For pupils to participate in this study, parental informed
consent was provided. The examination was divided over two days and consisted of –amongst
others- the following tests: Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices [SPM], the Torrance Test
of Creative Thinking-Verbal [TTCT-V], and the Creative Writing Task [CWT]. These tests
were conducted in a class-wise manner by academic master students. SPM and TTCT-V were
introduced according to the guidelines. Then participants were given 45 minutes for the
completion of SPM and 30 minutes for the completion of TTCT-V. With regard to Creative
Writing Task, participants were offered a picture, showing either a Capricorn in the woods or
an old door with a key hole. Participants were asked to write a story about their picture in 30
minutes. In addition to these tests, the results of standardized literacy tests were retrieved
from teachers, offering information on the academic achievement of the participants with
regard to spelling, reading speed, vocabulary and reading comprehension. Also, teachers were
asked to select highly intelligent pupils as nominees for a gifted education programme. In
return, the school board was advised which pupils to select for gifted education.
Data-analysis
Raw scores were transformed into ratio-scores, so scores could be compared to each
other and separate groups could be formed based on percentile-scores. Children performing
within the top 10% on the intelligence test (percentile ≥ 90) were selected for the group
“gifted” (n = 33). The remaining group (percentile < 90) was labelled “non-gifted” (n = 271).
The assumptions of normality, linearity and homoscedasticity were assessed for each group
separately, and found not to be supported for the “gifted” group. A visual inspection of the
10
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
normal Q-Q and detrented Q-Q plots for each variable in the “non-gifted” group showed that
“comprehensive reading” was the only variable normally distributed, therefore Kendall’s Tau-
B correlation was selected to assess the size and direction of the linear relationship between
intelligence, creativity and literacy ability. One-Way-Between Groups Analysis of Variance
(ANOVA) was conducted to investigate: 1) if discrepancies between divergent and
convergent creativity differ between gifted and non-gifted children, 2) if the level of literacy
ability differs between gifted and non-gifted children. Discrepancies were calculated by
subtracting divergent creativity scores from convergent creativity scores. Inspection of
skewedness, kurtosis and Shapiro-Wilk statistics indicated that the assumption of normality
was not supported for each of the three conditions. Levene’s statistics was significant for
word reading F (1, 35) = 10.25, p = .003 and reading comprehension F (1, 33) = 4.63, p =
.039, but non-significant for intelligence, F (1, 41) = 2.67, p = .110, thus the assumption of
homogeneity of variance was violated.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 shows the number of children being nominated by their teacher for the gifted
education program. Statistics for boys and girls are being compared, as well as the number of
nominations within the gifted and non-gifted group of children. Table 2 offers an overview of
the means, standard deviations and ranges of the intelligence and creativity test scores of
gifted and non-gifted children.
Table 1
Sex, age and teacher nominations of gifted and non-gifted children
Variable Non-gifted Gifted
n 394 45
nboys 201 28
nnominatedboys 51 18
ngirls 193 17
nnominatedgirls 27 6
Mean age (SD) 9.9 (.87) 10.2 (.96)
Note. n = sample size; SD = standard deviation; nboys = number of teacher nominated boys;
ngirls = number of teacher nominated girls
11
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Table 2
Means, standard deviations and ranges of test scores of gifted and non-gifted children
Non-gifted Gifted
Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Raven 72.4 10.9 30-89 93.2 2.9 89-100
Reading comp. 38.5 8.9 0-95 56.5 15.4 19-92
Word reading 77.5 15.2 52-98 78.6 14.2 42-97
TTCT-V 42.8 20.9 0-100 42.5 19.1 6-86
CWT Abstraction 60.9 19.4 7-100 68.1 14.9 33-100
CWT Innovation 57.6 17.6 0-100 63.2 17.0 27-100
Note. Raven = fluid intelligence; Reading comp. = reading comprehension + vocabulary;
Word reading = word reading + spelling; TTCT-V = divergent creativity; CWT Abstraction =
abstraction component of convergent creativity; CWT Innovation = innovation component of
convergent creativity.
Hypothesis 1
To assess the size and direction of the linear relationship between intelligence,
creativity and literacy ability, Kendall’s Tau-B correlation coefficients (r ) were calculated.
Below the threshold, small positive correlations were found between intelligence and the
abstraction component of convergent creativity, r(377) = .178, p < .001. Above the threshold,
the abstraction component of convergent creativity showed weak negative correlations with
intelligence, r (40) = -.100, p = .415. Between intelligence and the innovation component of
convergent creativity, correlations were weak for non-gifted, and small and positive for gifted
children, r(40) = .156, p = .201. Correlations between intelligence and divergent thinking
[TTCT-V] were weak, both above and below the threshold (table 3).
Hypothesis 2
Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity were assessed using one-
way between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA). The ANOVA was statistically significant,
both for the abstraction and innovation component of convergent creativity, indicating that
discrepancies between convergent and divergent creativity (convergent > divergent) are larger in
gifted than in non-gifted children, as expected. Although not significant, non-gifted children showed
larger discrepancies with divergent creativity exceeding convergent creativity (table 4).
12
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Table 3
Kendall’s Tau-B correlation coefficients of gifted and non-gifted children
Raven CWT
abstraction
CWT
innovation
TTCT-V Word
reading
Reading
comprehension
Raven 1 -.100 .156 -.062 .160 -.142
CWT
Abstraction
.178** 1 .277* .236* -.070 .146
CWT
Innovation
.056 .345** 1 .069 .138 .007
TTCT-V .078* .114* -.012 1 .104 .162
Word
reading
.089* .101* .205** .076* 1 .182
Reading
comprehension
.270** .178** .104* .080* .209** 1
Raven = fluid intelligence; Reading comprehension = reading comprehension + vocabulary;
Word reading = word reading + spelling; TTCT-V = divergent creativity; CWT abstraction =
abstraction component of convergent creativity; CWT innovation = innovation component of
convergent creativity. Correlations for gifted children are presented above the diagonal, and
correlations for non-gifted children are presented below the diagonal. * p<.05 ** p<.01
Table 4
Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity in gifted and non-gifted children
Non-gifted Gifted ANOVA
n M SD n M SD F p η2
Abstract > Div
Innov > Div
Div > Abstract
362
362
89
17.4
13.9
17.4
25.7
26.9
14.5
35
35
3
26.7
23.0
12.2
20.4
23.9
3.4
5.2
3.7
.401
.023
.055
.528
.013
.009
4.43
Div > Innov 107 18.9 15.8 6 11.9 13.8 1.1 .293 .009
Note. Abstract = abstraction component of convergent creativity, Innov = innovation component of
convergent creativity, Div = divergent creativity
13
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Hypothesis 3
In order to analyse the variability of literacy scores, means, standard deviations and
ranges of the various test scores were retrieved (table 5). In addition, a one-way between
groups analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine if literacy ability scores
were different for gifted and non-gifted children. The ANOVA was statistically significant for
reading comprehension, F (1, 364) = 40.76, p < .000 , η2
= .101. The ANOVA was not
statistically significant for word reading and spelling.
Table 5
Variability in reading scores in gifted and non-gifted children
Non-gifted Gifted ANOVA
n M SD n M SD F p η2
Reading comp. 329 38.8 15.3 37 55.8 16.5 40.7 .000 .101
Word reading 341 75.9 11.9 33 78.2 14.5 1.05 .305 .002
Note. Reading comp. = reading comprehension + vocabulary; Word reading = word reading +
spelling
Hypothesis 4
Finally, the size and direction of the linear relationship between divergent-convergent
discrepancies and literacy ability was examined, using Kendall’s Tau-B correlation
coefficients (r ). Small positive correlations were found between discrepancies (divergent >
convergent) and reading comprehension, r(79) = .264, p = .001. No significant correlations
were found for convergent thinking exceeding divergent thinking.
Conclusion and discussion
In the current research it was confirmed that intelligence and convergent creativity
correlate positively below the threshold, yet correlations between intelligence and divergent
creativity were negligible. With regard to gifted children, correlations between intelligence
and creativity were absent for divergent creativity, as expected, yet small correlations were
found for the innovation component of convergent creativity. Below the threshold,
correlations for the innovation component were merely absent, indicating that the innovation
component of convergent creativity is only significant in gifted children (figure 4).
14
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Figure 4. Correlations between intelligence and creativity above and below the threshold
As shown in figure 5, discrepancies (convergent > divergent) were found to be larger in
gifted children. Also variability in literacy scores was confirmed to be larger above the
threshold, as shown in figure 6 and 7. It was found that the mean scores of word reading were
similar below and above the threshold, whereas gifted children outperformed non-gifted
children with regard to reading comprehension.
Figure 5. Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity, both above and below
the threshold
15
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Reading comprehension
Small correlations were found between reading comprehension and creative
discrepancies (divergent > convergent). In line with the theory of facilitation, the direction of
this correlation was positive, suggesting that a wide focus of attention may be useful in the
process of incorporating seemingly irrelevant and unrelated information to solve a problem or
gain more insight (Runco, 2004). Creative discrepancies (divergent > convergent) occurred
equally in gifted and non-gifted children, yet mean scores on reading comprehension were
higher for gifted children. Also the variability of the scores was significantly larger above the
threshold. This may be explained by the superior level of intelligence in gifted children,
which is associated with larger working memory capacity (Benedek, Jauk, Sommer,
Arendasy, Neubauer, 2014). Working memory is a primary resource for the control of
attention (Engle, 2002; Kane, Bleckley, Conway & Engle, 2001). It supports the active
maintenance of task-relevant information and the controlled search from memory (Unsworth
& Engle, 2007), which is highly relevant in reading comprehension. Yet, creativity is also
predicted by inhibition and personality factors (Benedek et al., 2014). Creative achievement
depends on an adaptive engagement of inhibition in order to repulse stimuli that are too
obvious or irrelevant and at the same time keep creative thought flowing (Vartanian, 2009;
Zabelina & Robinson, 2010). Gifted children seem to have more difficulty controlling
attention due to an active imagination and a propensity to daydream (Baum & Olenchak,
2002; Cramond, 1994b). This might explain the wide range of scores in reading
comprehension.
Figure 6. Means and variability in reading comprehension test scores, both above and below
the threshold
16
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Word reading
The ability to narrow and sustain attention to information is needed for analytical tasks
or performing sequences of steps (like word reading). This can be more problematic for gifted
children, because of their varied interests (Baum & Olenchak, 2002; Cramond, 1994b). Strong
correlations were found between attention problems in gifted children and difficulty with
academic and preliteracy tasks like rapid automatized naming (Kiefer, & Martens, 2010).
Automaticity is traditionally thought to occur unconsciously and independent from top-down
control (Posner & Snyder, 1975; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977). Yet, according to the
attentional sensitization model of unconscious cognition (Kiefer & Martens, 2010),
automaticity also strongly relies on controlled top-down activity (Beaty, Silvia, Nusbaum,
Jauk, & Benedek, 2014).
Figure 7. Means and variability in word reading test scores, both above and below the
threshold
Summarizing, it can be concluded that large working memory capacity, combined
with elevated levels of divergent thinking is associated with superior reading comprehension.
The combination of these characteristics is frequently present in gifted children (figure 6).
However, in order to properly master the basic elements of the reading process, children also
need to be able to narrow and sustain attention. With regard to word reading, gifted children
with good attention control seem to outperform non-gifted children, whereas word reading
scores of gifted children with attention problems appear to be dramatically lower than those of
non-gifted peers (figure 7). This suggests that the ability and/or motivation for adaptive
17
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
inhibition and top-down control, is a key element in the word reading process of gifted
children.
Limitations and future research
It must be noted that participants were not randomly selected, data were not all
normally distributed and the sample size of the group of gifted children was small (n = 45).
However, the total sample size was considerable (n = 440) and so was the amount of data
taken from participants. The Creative Writing Task (utilized to assess convergent creativity) is
an unconventional instrument; reliability and validity have not been formally reviewed.
Nevertheless, it seems to compliment former research on the threshold theory, which was
based on divergent creativity only (Benedek, Franz, Heene, & Neubauer, 2012; Karwowski &
Gralewski, 2013). In order to gain more insight in (extremely) gifted children, it might be
relevant to further explore the innovation component of convergent creativity in future
research. With regard to the current research, implications for education are to view giftedness
from a developmental perspective, to be aware of the great variety within this group and to
aim for tailor made solutions.
18
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
References
Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2005). Reliability and factorial validity of the standard progressive
matrices among Kuwaiti children ages 8 to 15 years. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
101, 409-412. doi:10.2466/pms.101.2.409-412
Al-Issa, I (1964). Creativity and its relationship to age, vocabulary and personality of
schizophrenics. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 110(464), 74-79
Barbot, B., M., & Lubart, T. I. (2011). Assessing creativity in the classroom. The Open
Education Journal, 4, 58-66. doi: 10.2174/1874920801104010058
Baum, S. M., & Olenchak, F. R. (2002). The alphabet children: GT, AHDH, and more.
Exceptionality, 10, 77-91.
Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict
real-world creativity? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.
doi: 10.1037/a0035727.
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of
cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual
Differences, 53, 480-485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence,
creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of
executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73–83.
doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.007
Broos, A., Fulker, D. W., & De Fries, J. C. (1990). Reading performance and general
cognitive ability: A multivariate genetic analysis of twin data. Personality and
Individual Differences, 11, 141-146. doi: 10.1016/0191-886
Bruner, J. S. (1985). The role of interaction formats in language acquisition. In J. P. Forgas
(Ed.), Language and social situations, 31-46. New York: Springer Verlag.
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is
associated with increased creative achievement in high functioning individuals.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499-506.
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1-22. doi: 10.1037/h0046743
Chakravarty, A. (2009). Artistic talent in dyslexia: A hypothesis. Medical Hypotheses, 73,
569–571. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2009.05.034.
Chermahini, S. A., & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b)link between creativity and dopamine:
Spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking.
19
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Cognition, 115/3, 458-465. doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.007
Chu, T-L., & Lin, W-W. (2013). Uniqueness, integration or separation? Exploring the nature
of creativity through creative writing by elementary school students in Taiwan.
Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational
Psychology, 33:5, 582-595, doi: 10.1080/01443410.2013.821459
Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s
children’s reading comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial.
Psychological Science, 21, 1106-1116. doi. 10.1177/0956797610375449
Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming and verbal
memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25,
532-538.
Cramond, B. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and creativity—What is the
connection? Journal of Creative Behavior, 28, 193-210.
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3),
391-404. doi. 10.1207/s15326934crj1803_13
Eide, B. L., & Eide, F. F. (2011). The dyslexic advantage: Unlocking the hidden potential of
the dyslexic brain. London, UK: Hay House.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.
Finch, M. E. H., Neumeister, K. L. S., Burney, V. H., & Cook, A. L. (2014). The Relationship
of Cognitive and Executive Functioning With Achievement in Gifted Kindergarten
Children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 58, 167–182. doi: 10.1177/0016986214534889
Haensly, P. A., and Torrance, E. P. (1990). Assessment of creativity in children and
adolescents. In Reynoolds, C. R. and Kamphaus, R. W. (eds) Handbook of
Psychological and Educational Assessment of Children: Intelligence and
Achievement. New York: Gildford.
Hansenne, M., & Legrand, J. (2012). Creativity, emotional intelligence and school
performance. International Journal of Educational Research, 53, 264-268.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.03.015
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between
intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of
empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003.
Jung, R. E., Gaspararovic, C., Chaves, R. S., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Caprihan, A., &
Yeo, R. A. (2009). Biochemical support for the “threshold” theory of creativity: a
20
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5319-
5325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0588-09.2009
Karwowski, M., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Threshold hypothesis: fact or artefact? Thinking
Skills and Creativity, 8, 25-33. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003
Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task
sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 139, 464-489. doi.10.2478%2Fv10053-008-0102-4
Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? Journal of Secondary Gifted
Education, 16, 57-66. doi: 10.4219/jsge-2005-473
Kroesbergen, Van Hooijdonk, Middel-Lalleman, Rijnders, & van Viersen (2015). The
psychological well-being of early identified gifted children.
Liegeois, F., Cross, J. H., Polkey, C., Harkness, W., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2008) Language
after hemispherectomy in childhood: contributions from memory and intelligence.
Neuropsychologia, 46, 3101-3107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.001
Lubow, R. E., Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and
implications for schizophrenia. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (1): 87–103.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.87
Mann, E. L. (2006). Creativity: The essence of mathematics. Journal for the Education of
the Gifted, 30, 236-260. doi:10.4219/jeg-2006-264.
Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (2007). Issues of causality in children’s reading comprehension. In D.
S. McNamara (Eds.), Reading comprehension strategies. Theories, interventions and
technologies (47-71). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.
Preckel, F., Götz, T., & Frenzel, A. (2010). Ability grouping of gifted students: effects on
academic self-concept and boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80,
451-472. doi: 10.1348/000709909X480716
Popov, A. (1992). Creativity and reading comprehension. The Journal of Creative Behavior,
26(3), 206-212Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: change and
stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 375-386.
doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735.
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso
(Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55-85).
Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Renzulli, J. S. (1986). The three-ring conception of giftedness: a developmental model for
21
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
creative productivity. In R. Sternberg & J. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness
(pp. 332-357). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ritchie, S. J., Luciano, M., Hansell, N. K., Wright, M. J., Bates, T. C. (2013). The
relationship of reading ability to creativity: positive, not negative associations.
Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 171–176. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.009
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential.
Creativity Research Journal, 24, 66-75. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.652929
Runco, M. M., & Albert, R. S. (1986). The threshold theory regarding creativity and
intelligence: an empirical test with gifted and nongifted children. Creative Child and
Adult Quarterly J 1(4), 212-218. doi: 1988-07120-001
Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657-687. doi:
10.2466/pms.1984.59.3.711
Samuels, S. J., & Flor, R. F. (1997). The importance of automaticity for developing expertise
in reading. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Reading Difficulties, 13,
107–121.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information
processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual
differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
doi. 10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A., & Freeman, D. (1984). The relationship between early
reading acquisition and word decoding with and without context: A longitudinal study
of first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 668-677.
doi:10.1037/022-0663.76.4.668
Strang, R. (1968). Reading diagnosis and remediation (ERIC CRIER Reading Review Series).
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and
gifted education: a proposed direction forward based on psychological science.
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12, 3– 54.
doi: 10.1177/1529100611418056
Subotnik, R. F., & Rickoff, R. (2010). Should eminence based on outstanding innovation be
the goal of gifted education and talent development? Implications for policy and
research. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 358–364.
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.005
22
Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY
Tafti, M. A., Hameedy, M. A., & Baghal, N. M. (2009). Dyslexia, a deficit or a difference:
Comparing the creativity and memory skills of dyslexic and nondyslexic students in
Iran. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 37, 1009–1016.
doi: 10.2224/sbp.2009.37.8.1009
Tien, Y.-M., Hsu, L.-C., Tai, J. H.-Y, Yang, Y.-F. (2014). Deciphering syntactic processing of
gifted students with working memory and creativity. Chinese Journal of Psychology.
56(3), 257-276
Torrance, E. P., Ball, O. E., & Safter, T. H. (2008). Torrance® Test of Creative Thinking.
Streamlined Scoring Guide for Figural Forms A and B. Illinois: Scholastic Testing
Service, Inc.
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working
memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from
secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-
295X.114.1.104
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 57–59. doi: 10.1037/a0014781
Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness, conflict, and underachievement. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Wolf, K. M. & Mieg, H. A. (2010). Cognitive determinants of the success of inventors:
complex problem solving and deliberate use of divergent and convergent thinking,
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 443-462,
doi: 10.1080/09541440902916845
Yamatoto, K. (1964a). Threshold of intelligence in academic achievement of highly creative
students. The Journal of Experimental Education, 32, 401–405.
Yamamoto, K. (1964b). The Experimental Scoring Manual for Minnesota Tests for Creative
Thinking and Writing. Kent State University. Bureau of Educational Research.
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136–143. doi: 10.1037/a0017379

More Related Content

What's hot

What is intelligence ?
What is intelligence ?What is intelligence ?
What is intelligence ?Ravi Malik
 
Concept of intelligence
Concept of intelligenceConcept of intelligence
Concept of intelligenceSarfraz Ahmad
 
Aguiar ap intelligence and testing
Aguiar ap intelligence and testingAguiar ap intelligence and testing
Aguiar ap intelligence and testingMrAguiar
 
intelligence and Cognitive view of learning
 intelligence and Cognitive view of learning intelligence and Cognitive view of learning
intelligence and Cognitive view of learningALuvilu Lazarus
 
Intellectual development
Intellectual developmentIntellectual development
Intellectual developmentHennaAnsari
 
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligence
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligenceIntelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligence
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligenceAli Pk
 
Guilford's structure of intellect model
Guilford's structure of intellect modelGuilford's structure of intellect model
Guilford's structure of intellect modelBonnie Crerar
 
IQ Test- Intelligence theories
IQ Test- Intelligence theoriesIQ Test- Intelligence theories
IQ Test- Intelligence theoriesScholar hive
 
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TEST
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TESTINTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TEST
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TESTirshad narejo
 
Introduction To Intelligence
Introduction To IntelligenceIntroduction To Intelligence
Introduction To IntelligenceMansi_Garg
 
Intellgence
IntellgenceIntellgence
Intellgencer g
 
Intelligence meaning and types
Intelligence meaning and typesIntelligence meaning and types
Intelligence meaning and typesAtul Thakur
 
Algebra oflittlekids final
Algebra oflittlekids finalAlgebra oflittlekids final
Algebra oflittlekids finalMarta Montoro
 

What's hot (20)

What is intelligence ?
What is intelligence ?What is intelligence ?
What is intelligence ?
 
Concept of intelligence
Concept of intelligenceConcept of intelligence
Concept of intelligence
 
Intelligence testing
Intelligence testingIntelligence testing
Intelligence testing
 
Aguiar ap intelligence and testing
Aguiar ap intelligence and testingAguiar ap intelligence and testing
Aguiar ap intelligence and testing
 
intelligence and Cognitive view of learning
 intelligence and Cognitive view of learning intelligence and Cognitive view of learning
intelligence and Cognitive view of learning
 
Intellectual development
Intellectual developmentIntellectual development
Intellectual development
 
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligence
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligenceIntelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligence
Intelligence, IQ, Assessments, Pre-morbid intelligence
 
Guilford's structure of intellect model
Guilford's structure of intellect modelGuilford's structure of intellect model
Guilford's structure of intellect model
 
Ch11 ppt
Ch11 pptCh11 ppt
Ch11 ppt
 
IQ Test- Intelligence theories
IQ Test- Intelligence theoriesIQ Test- Intelligence theories
IQ Test- Intelligence theories
 
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TEST
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TESTINTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TEST
INTELLIGENCE AND EMOTIONAL TEST
 
Introduction To Intelligence
Introduction To IntelligenceIntroduction To Intelligence
Introduction To Intelligence
 
intelligece
intelligeceintelligece
intelligece
 
IQ TEST
IQ TESTIQ TEST
IQ TEST
 
Intellgence
IntellgenceIntellgence
Intellgence
 
Waispres
WaispresWaispres
Waispres
 
Intelligence meaning and types
Intelligence meaning and typesIntelligence meaning and types
Intelligence meaning and types
 
intelligence test
 intelligence test intelligence test
intelligence test
 
Algebra oflittlekids final
Algebra oflittlekids finalAlgebra oflittlekids final
Algebra oflittlekids final
 
Intelligence and Achievement
Intelligence and AchievementIntelligence and Achievement
Intelligence and Achievement
 

Viewers also liked

Creative out of the box thinking
Creative out of the box thinkingCreative out of the box thinking
Creative out of the box thinkingSameer Mathur
 
Development and Research
Development and ResearchDevelopment and Research
Development and Researchilaeadigital
 
Psychology Intelligences guilford
Psychology Intelligences guilfordPsychology Intelligences guilford
Psychology Intelligences guilfordNithya Patrick
 
How Do You Design For Creativity?
How Do You Design For Creativity?How Do You Design For Creativity?
How Do You Design For Creativity?Bud Caddell
 
Ideation Workshop
Ideation WorkshopIdeation Workshop
Ideation WorkshopRachel Liu
 

Viewers also liked (8)

Creative out of the box thinking
Creative out of the box thinkingCreative out of the box thinking
Creative out of the box thinking
 
Development and Research
Development and ResearchDevelopment and Research
Development and Research
 
Psychology Intelligences guilford
Psychology Intelligences guilfordPsychology Intelligences guilford
Psychology Intelligences guilford
 
Divergent, Emergent & Convergent Thinking
Divergent, Emergent & Convergent ThinkingDivergent, Emergent & Convergent Thinking
Divergent, Emergent & Convergent Thinking
 
Convergent & Divergent Thinking
Convergent & Divergent ThinkingConvergent & Divergent Thinking
Convergent & Divergent Thinking
 
How Do You Design For Creativity?
How Do You Design For Creativity?How Do You Design For Creativity?
How Do You Design For Creativity?
 
Ideation Workshop
Ideation WorkshopIdeation Workshop
Ideation Workshop
 
Creativity
CreativityCreativity
Creativity
 

Similar to Masterthesis E. Bekker-Janssens [4254694]

14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCEDr. Bhuvan Nagpal
 
Smart brain training in education
Smart brain training in educationSmart brain training in education
Smart brain training in educationRaise Your IQ
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review eckchela
 
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences Theory
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences TheoryThe Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences Theory
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences TheoryKristen Stacey
 
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docx
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docxMind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docx
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Asaricaoglu aarikan
Asaricaoglu aarikanAsaricaoglu aarikan
Asaricaoglu aarikanmikrop_2
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Ch13slides.ppt
Ch13slides.pptCh13slides.ppt
Ch13slides.pptEidTahir
 
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...Kassandra Chelzea Banalan
 
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very brief introduction
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very  brief introductionIntegral Education and the Brain; A very very  brief introduction
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very brief introductionSpencer Ames
 
EdunextIndia - Education Next Generation
EdunextIndia  - Education Next GenerationEdunextIndia  - Education Next Generation
EdunextIndia - Education Next GenerationEdunextIndia DMIT
 
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ab
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the abAccording to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ab
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ablatashiadegale
 
College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx
                College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx                College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx
College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docxhallettfaustina
 

Similar to Masterthesis E. Bekker-Janssens [4254694] (20)

14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
 
Intelligence revised
Intelligence revisedIntelligence revised
Intelligence revised
 
Smart brain training in education
Smart brain training in educationSmart brain training in education
Smart brain training in education
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review
EDR 8200-8: Develop a Final Literature Review
 
Intelligence
IntelligenceIntelligence
Intelligence
 
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences Theory
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences TheoryThe Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences Theory
The Theory Of Intelligence, And Multiple Intelligences Theory
 
PSY101 Week 5 intelligence
PSY101 Week 5 intelligencePSY101 Week 5 intelligence
PSY101 Week 5 intelligence
 
Neuroscience review evidence
Neuroscience review evidenceNeuroscience review evidence
Neuroscience review evidence
 
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docx
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docxMind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docx
Mind-wandering-in-children--Examining-task-unrelated-thou_2019_J.docx
 
Asaricaoglu aarikan
Asaricaoglu aarikanAsaricaoglu aarikan
Asaricaoglu aarikan
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Ch13slides.ppt
Ch13slides.pptCh13slides.ppt
Ch13slides.ppt
 
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...
Multiple Intelligences Profile of the First Two Sections of the Fourth Year ...
 
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very brief introduction
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very  brief introductionIntegral Education and the Brain; A very very  brief introduction
Integral Education and the Brain; A very very brief introduction
 
RUSSEL FINAL
RUSSEL FINALRUSSEL FINAL
RUSSEL FINAL
 
IDs Essay
IDs EssayIDs Essay
IDs Essay
 
EdunextIndia - Education Next Generation
EdunextIndia  - Education Next GenerationEdunextIndia  - Education Next Generation
EdunextIndia - Education Next Generation
 
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ab
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the abAccording to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ab
According to Nisbett (2012), Intelligence . . . involves the ab
 
College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx
                College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx                College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx
College of Doctoral StudiesLiterature Review.docx
 

Masterthesis E. Bekker-Janssens [4254694]

  • 1. 1 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY The role of divergent thinking and convergent thinking in the relation between intelligence and literacy ability Masterthesis Universiteit Utrecht Masteropleiding Pedagogische Wetenschappen Masterprogramma Orthopedagogiek Name: E. Bekker-Janssens Student number: 4254694 Supervisor: dr. E.H. Kroesbergen Second supervisor: dr. J.H. van de Beek Date: 17-6-2015 Number of words: 3.662
  • 2. 2 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Voorwoord Allereerst wil ik jou bedanken, Evelyn, voor alle tijd, ruimte en feedback die je me hebt gegeven. Steeds opnieuw wist je me richting te geven zonder te sturen. Ik ben tevreden over het eindresultaat en heb van het proces genoten. Jan, bedankt voor het kritisch mee- kijken en denken. Lotte, Eveline, Sanne en Lisanne, dank voor de fijne samenwerking bij het afnemen van alle tests en het scoren van de schrijftaken. Oma Rita, bedankt voor al die keren dat Abel gezellig met Ties mocht spelen als ik uurtjes tekort kwam. Bedankt lieve Anne, Evelien, Marijke, Marieke, Marion Mariette en Steffie. Ik was een vreselijk nalatige zus /dochter /vriendin de afgelopen periode, maar jullie bleven onverminderd begripvol. En dan mijn mannen: Martijn, Stijn, Mees en Abel. Het was vast niet altijd leuk, mama achter de laptop aan de keukentafel, bedolven onder artikelen, krabbels en boeken. Toch heb ik mij van het begin af aan door jullie gesteund gevoeld in dit proces. Zonder woorden, maar weldegelijk. Ik wil jullie daar ongelofelijk voor bedanken. Bedankt! Peirce means "conceivable" very broadly. It allows any flight of imagination, provided this imagination ultimately alights upon a possible practical effect; and thus many hypotheses may seem at first glance to be excluded by the pragmatical maxim that are not really so excluded.
  • 3. 3 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Abstract The aim of this study was to get more in depth information as to why gifted children are vulnerable to underachieving and low well-being. Based on preliminary research, it was hypothesized that this might have to do with larger discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking above IQ 120, greater variability of academic performance and possible relations between the two. It was expected that illuminating these relations might provide us with essential information about the functioning of gifted children. For this purpose, 458 Dutch children from 12 regular primary schools participated in intelligence- and creativity tests. Also scores of Dutch standardized literacy tests were taken into account. Based on literature study and factor analysis, 6 variables were computed: intelligence, convergent creativity (abstraction component), convergent creativity (innovation component), divergent creativity, word reading and reading comprehension. Data were analysed using Kendall’s Tau-B correlation and one-way between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA). It was found that creative abstraction correlates positively with intelligence below the threshold, whereas creative innovation only starts correlating above IQ 120. Discrepancies between convergent and divergent creativity (convergent > divergent) were confirmed to be higher above the threshold, as well as the variability between literacy scores. Discrepancies with divergent thinking exceeding convergent thinking showed positive correlations with reading comprehension. Possible explanations and suggestions for future research are being discussed. Key words: intelligence, creativity, divergent thinking, convergent thinking, literacy ability, underachieving, threshold theory, compensation theory, facilitation theory.
  • 4. 4 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY The role of divergent thinking and convergent thinking in the relation between intelligence and literacy ability Intelligence is the capacity to think logically and solve problems in novel situations, independent of acquired knowledge (Cattell, 1963). Children with both high intelligence and high creativity levels are referred to as being ‘gifted’ (Renzulli, 1986). Intelligence is known to be a strong predictor of academic performance (Hansenne & Legrand, 2012). Yet, being gifted is associated with underachieving (Kroesbergen, Van Hooijdonk, Middel-Lalleman, Rijnders, & Van Viersen, 2015; Mann, 2006; Whitmore, 1980, cited in McCoach & Siegle, 2003) and low levels of well-being (Kroesbergen et al., 2015). This phenomenon can partly be explained by the fact that our educational system is poorly adapted to the needs of gifted children (Preckel, Gotz, & Frenzel, 2010). Educational fit and proper support are known to be necessary conditions for gifted children to reach their full potential (Subotnik & Rickoff, 2010). This doesn’t explain though, why it is the gifted and not the highly intelligent child that is underachieving and suffering from low well-being. Creativity seems to be of influence on the relation between intelligence and academic performance. Yet, little is known about the relation between these variables (Subotnik, Olszewski-Kubilius, & Worrell, 2011). The aim of this study is to contribute to an increased insight in the relation between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability in both gifted and non-gifted children. Divergent and convergent thinking in the creative cognitive process In studying the relation between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability it is important to distinguish between divergent and convergent thinking (Barbot, Besencon and Lubart, 2011; Cropley, 2006). Divergent thinking refers to the ability to view an idea from different angles leading to various creative ideas. Divergent tasks are process-based and focused on the quantity of a production. Divergent thinking is positively related to creative potential (Runco & Acar, 2012) and can be measured by divergent thinking tests. The act of combining various heterogeneous elements into a unique, original production refers to convergent-integrative thinking (Barbot, 2011). Convergent-integrative tasks imply a product- based approach, focusing on the quality of the production and leading to creative achievement (Runco & Acar, 2012). In the current study, creativity is referred to as a cognitive process consisting of both divergent and convergent thinking. The relation between intelligence and creativity Some research has shown that creativity test scores are independent from IQ scores, whereas other research has confirmed a relationship between the two (Kim, 2005). According to the threshold theory a positive linear correlation between intelligence and creativity is
  • 5. 5 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY present up to a certain point, usually IQ 120 (Jauk, Benedek, Dunst, & Neubauer, 2013). From thereon up, the correlation between intelligence and creativity is said to be weak or absent (Yamamoto, 1964a). The threshold theory has been extensively studied, with contradictory and inconclusive results (Runco & Albert, 1986). This inconsistency may come from different measures for creativity. Jung and colleagues (2009) provided neurobiological support for a critical “threshold” regarding the relationship between intelligence and creativity, as measured with divergent thinking tests. However, contemporary research shows that the breaking point depends on the operationalization of the construct creativity. Quantitative criteria measure divergent thinking, whereas qualitative criteria measure convergent thinking. It has been found that quantitative criteria will lead to a lower breaking point than qualitative criteria (Benedek, Franz, Heene, & Neubauer, 2012; Karwowski & Gralewski, 2013). Based on the work of Benedek and collegues (2012) and Karwowski and collegues (2013), it could be deduced that discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking might be larger in gifted than in non-gifted children. Figure 1. Threshold theory: significant correlations between creativity and intelligence below the threshold (IQ = 120), with correlations above the threshold being weak or absent. Literacy ability In the Netherlands, primary schools mainly focus on two domains: mathematics and literacy ability. Literacy ability consists of the sub-domains vocabulary, reading and spelling. Two main skills in reading are word reading and reading comprehension (Oakhill & Chain, 2007). Word reading contains two fundamental aspects: decoding and automating. Once a child is able to view each letter as a separate unit that corresponds to a particular sound, it can
  • 6. 6 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY learn to merge the sounds into words (Stanovich, 1986). Then, the child can be taught to recognize combinations of letters. This is called automating (Perfetti, 1985). Word reading is a necessary condition for reading comprehension (Clarke, Snowling, Truelove, & Hulme, 2010), which is the process of interacting with and giving meaning to a text (Bruner, 1985). Literacy ability in gifted and non-gifted children In the context of this research, literacy ability is particularly interesting because the various subdomains seem to be differentially related to intelligence and creativity. With regard to non-gifted children, strong positive correlations have been found between intelligence and comprehensive reading (Stanovich, Cunningham & Freeman, 1984; Broos, Fulker & De Vries, 1990), intelligence and vocabulary (Liegeois, Cross, Polkey, Harkness, & Vargha-Khadem, 2008) and intelligence and word reading and spelling (Cornwall, 1992; Strang, 1968). Positive correlations were also found between creativity and both reading comprehension (Popov, 1992; Tien, Hsu, Tai, & Yang, 2014) and vocabulary (Al Issra, 1964). Controversy exists about the relation between creativity and word reading and spelling. Ritchie, Luciano, Hansell, Wright and Bates (2013) assessed reading, spelling and non word repetition in a large sample, and studied associations with creativity. Based on the compensatory theory, Ritchie and colleagues expected to find high creativity rates in children with poor reading and spelling abilities. It was hypothesized that reading disability may involve a compensatory cognitive benefit in the form of enhanced creativity (Chakravarty, 2009; Eide & Eide, 2011; Tafti, Hameedy, & Baghal, 2009). Yet, creativity was found to be positively related to word reading and spelling, even when controlling for IQ (Ritchie et al., 2013). These findings led to a novel theory: the theory of facilitation, which suggests that fluency in written language may facilitate creative thought and curiosity (Ritchie et al., 2013). In the current research, this theory of facilitation has been tested. Little is known about the relation between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability in gifted children. Based on the threshold theory, it could be hypothesized that above IQ 120 correlations between intelligence and creativity are weak or absent, and thus larger discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking are conceivable. Finch, Neumeister, Burney and Cook (2014) examined 61 gifted preschoolers and found that, despite intelligence quotient scores in the very superior range (IQ > 130), great variability was observed on word reading and spelling, with means ranging from average to superior levels. Large discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking are thought to be negatively related to (academic) achievement (Chermahini & Hommel, 2010; Cropley, 2006; Lubow & Gewirtz, 1995; Wolf & Mieg, 2010). In the current research, this hypothesis will be tested.
  • 7. 7 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Objectives and hypotheses Based on the preliminary research, four hypotheses were formulated: Hypothesis 1. Below the threshold (IQ = 120) intelligence and creativity correlate positively, whereas above the threshold correlations are weak or absent. Hypothesis 2. Discrepancies between divergent thinking and convergent thinking are larger above than below the threshold (divergent > convergent). Hypothesis 3. Variability in literacy ability scores is larger above than below the threshold. Hypothesis 4. Discrepancies between divergent and convergent thinking (divergent > convergent) are negatively related to literacy ability scores. Method Participants 458 Dutch pupils from 12 regular primary schools participated in intelligence- and creativity tests, after active consent was obtained from their parents. The participants were recruited from 26 classes in grade 4 (n = 311), grade 5 (n = 73) and grade 6 (n = 74). Pupils in grade 4 were offered a screening for gifted education simultaneously, resulting in a relatively big sample size. The total sample consisted of boys (52%) and girls (48%) in the age of 8-13 years. The average age was 9.92 years (SD = 0.88). Instruments Intelligence: Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices has been used as a measure of intelligence (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1992; Raven, 2000). Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices is a useful instrument to identify both high achievers and potential high achievers who’s abilities have not yet fully developed (Kroesbergen et al.; 2015). In this study, participants got 45 minutes to complete the task, the number of right answers was noted. Abdel-Khalek (2005) found test-retest reliability ranging between .69 and .85 and Cronbach’s alpha between .88 and .93. Literacy ability: Literacy scores were retrieved from a national standardized assessment, called CITO. This national literacy test consists of the items: spelling, reading speed, reading comprehension and vocabulary. In the current research, the CITO literacy test displayed Crohnbach’s alpha .523. To examine the underlying structure of the CITO literacy scores more closely, data from all participants that had no missings on any of the literacy measures (N=84) were subjected to principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. Two factors were identified as underlying the four variables of the CITO literacy test. In total,
  • 8. 8 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY these factors accounted for 76% of the variance. Consequently, reading comprehension and vocabulary were computed into one variable (referred to as: reading comprehension). The other variable consisted of word reading and spelling (referred to as: word reading). Prior to running the principal axis factoring, examination of the data indicated that most of the variables were normally distributed and a linear relationship was identified among the variables. Creativity (divergent): Divergent thinking can be measured by the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (Torrance, Ball, & Safter, 2008). Test-retest reliabilities were reported in the .60-.80 range (Haensly & Torrance, 1990). Since we chose to focus on literacy ability, the verbal version of this test was selected (instead of the figural version). Activity 5 of the verbal test showed good to excellent inter-rater agreement (fluency: kappa = .815, flexibility: kappa = .710 and originality: kappa = .631) and good reliability (α = .841). To investigate the underlying structure of the test, data collected from 458 participants were subjected to principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. One factor (with Eigenvalue 2.61) was identified, accounting for around 87% of the variance. Thus, the three variables (fluency, flexibility and originality) were computed into a new variable, called: “divergent creativity”. Given the robust nature of factor analysis and the fact that a linear relationship was identified among the variables, the fact that variables were not perfectly normally distributed was considered not to be problematic. Creativity (convergent): The Creative Writing Task was a composition on a specific topic, written by primary school children. The test had to be completed in 30 minutes and was conducted in order to assess domain specific creative achievement. The Creative Writing Task was designed by Tsai-Ling Chu and Wei-Wen Lin (2013). Chu and Lin (2013) have found the inter-rater reliability correlation coefficients between two raters to be positive for technical goodness (r = .67, p < .001) and creativity (r = .73, p < .001). In the current study, children were not given a specific topic (sentence) but an image as an initial stimulus. The Experimental Scoring Manual for Minnesota Tests for Creative Thinking and Writing (Yamamoto, 1964b) was used by three raters to evaluate the creative writing products. On each scale one item had to be deleted, due to the nature of our stimulus (visual instead of verbal). To assess inter-rater agreement, Kappa was calculated for the remaining 24 items (6 scales, 4 items on each scale). Kappa scores varied between excellent (richness: .786; originality: 1), good (sensitivity: .715; imagination: .742) and acceptable (organisation: .610; psychological insight: .679). For the Creative Writing Task Crohnbach’s alpha was .652. To investigate the underlying structure of the Creative Writing Task, data collected from 418
  • 9. 9 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY participants was subjected to principal axis factoring with varimax rotation. Prior to running the principal axis factoring, examination of data indicated that the variables were not perfectly normally distributed. Given the robust nature of factor analysis, these deviations were not considered problematic. Furthermore, a lineair relationship was identified among the variables. Two factors (with Eigenvalues exceeding 1) were identified as underlying the six variables of the Creative Writing Task. In total, these factors accounted for around 53% of the variance in the Creative Writing Task data. The first factor consisted of the variables: sensitivity, imagination, richness and organisation. This factor was computed into a new variable, called: “convergent creativity: abstraction”. The second factor consisted of: originality, psychological insight and organisation, and was computed into the new variable: “convergent creativity: innovation”. Procedure The current study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Social Sciences of Utrecht University. For pupils to participate in this study, parental informed consent was provided. The examination was divided over two days and consisted of –amongst others- the following tests: Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices [SPM], the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking-Verbal [TTCT-V], and the Creative Writing Task [CWT]. These tests were conducted in a class-wise manner by academic master students. SPM and TTCT-V were introduced according to the guidelines. Then participants were given 45 minutes for the completion of SPM and 30 minutes for the completion of TTCT-V. With regard to Creative Writing Task, participants were offered a picture, showing either a Capricorn in the woods or an old door with a key hole. Participants were asked to write a story about their picture in 30 minutes. In addition to these tests, the results of standardized literacy tests were retrieved from teachers, offering information on the academic achievement of the participants with regard to spelling, reading speed, vocabulary and reading comprehension. Also, teachers were asked to select highly intelligent pupils as nominees for a gifted education programme. In return, the school board was advised which pupils to select for gifted education. Data-analysis Raw scores were transformed into ratio-scores, so scores could be compared to each other and separate groups could be formed based on percentile-scores. Children performing within the top 10% on the intelligence test (percentile ≥ 90) were selected for the group “gifted” (n = 33). The remaining group (percentile < 90) was labelled “non-gifted” (n = 271). The assumptions of normality, linearity and homoscedasticity were assessed for each group separately, and found not to be supported for the “gifted” group. A visual inspection of the
  • 10. 10 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY normal Q-Q and detrented Q-Q plots for each variable in the “non-gifted” group showed that “comprehensive reading” was the only variable normally distributed, therefore Kendall’s Tau- B correlation was selected to assess the size and direction of the linear relationship between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability. One-Way-Between Groups Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted to investigate: 1) if discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity differ between gifted and non-gifted children, 2) if the level of literacy ability differs between gifted and non-gifted children. Discrepancies were calculated by subtracting divergent creativity scores from convergent creativity scores. Inspection of skewedness, kurtosis and Shapiro-Wilk statistics indicated that the assumption of normality was not supported for each of the three conditions. Levene’s statistics was significant for word reading F (1, 35) = 10.25, p = .003 and reading comprehension F (1, 33) = 4.63, p = .039, but non-significant for intelligence, F (1, 41) = 2.67, p = .110, thus the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated. Results Descriptive Statistics Table 1 shows the number of children being nominated by their teacher for the gifted education program. Statistics for boys and girls are being compared, as well as the number of nominations within the gifted and non-gifted group of children. Table 2 offers an overview of the means, standard deviations and ranges of the intelligence and creativity test scores of gifted and non-gifted children. Table 1 Sex, age and teacher nominations of gifted and non-gifted children Variable Non-gifted Gifted n 394 45 nboys 201 28 nnominatedboys 51 18 ngirls 193 17 nnominatedgirls 27 6 Mean age (SD) 9.9 (.87) 10.2 (.96) Note. n = sample size; SD = standard deviation; nboys = number of teacher nominated boys; ngirls = number of teacher nominated girls
  • 11. 11 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Table 2 Means, standard deviations and ranges of test scores of gifted and non-gifted children Non-gifted Gifted Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Raven 72.4 10.9 30-89 93.2 2.9 89-100 Reading comp. 38.5 8.9 0-95 56.5 15.4 19-92 Word reading 77.5 15.2 52-98 78.6 14.2 42-97 TTCT-V 42.8 20.9 0-100 42.5 19.1 6-86 CWT Abstraction 60.9 19.4 7-100 68.1 14.9 33-100 CWT Innovation 57.6 17.6 0-100 63.2 17.0 27-100 Note. Raven = fluid intelligence; Reading comp. = reading comprehension + vocabulary; Word reading = word reading + spelling; TTCT-V = divergent creativity; CWT Abstraction = abstraction component of convergent creativity; CWT Innovation = innovation component of convergent creativity. Hypothesis 1 To assess the size and direction of the linear relationship between intelligence, creativity and literacy ability, Kendall’s Tau-B correlation coefficients (r ) were calculated. Below the threshold, small positive correlations were found between intelligence and the abstraction component of convergent creativity, r(377) = .178, p < .001. Above the threshold, the abstraction component of convergent creativity showed weak negative correlations with intelligence, r (40) = -.100, p = .415. Between intelligence and the innovation component of convergent creativity, correlations were weak for non-gifted, and small and positive for gifted children, r(40) = .156, p = .201. Correlations between intelligence and divergent thinking [TTCT-V] were weak, both above and below the threshold (table 3). Hypothesis 2 Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity were assessed using one- way between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA). The ANOVA was statistically significant, both for the abstraction and innovation component of convergent creativity, indicating that discrepancies between convergent and divergent creativity (convergent > divergent) are larger in gifted than in non-gifted children, as expected. Although not significant, non-gifted children showed larger discrepancies with divergent creativity exceeding convergent creativity (table 4).
  • 12. 12 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Table 3 Kendall’s Tau-B correlation coefficients of gifted and non-gifted children Raven CWT abstraction CWT innovation TTCT-V Word reading Reading comprehension Raven 1 -.100 .156 -.062 .160 -.142 CWT Abstraction .178** 1 .277* .236* -.070 .146 CWT Innovation .056 .345** 1 .069 .138 .007 TTCT-V .078* .114* -.012 1 .104 .162 Word reading .089* .101* .205** .076* 1 .182 Reading comprehension .270** .178** .104* .080* .209** 1 Raven = fluid intelligence; Reading comprehension = reading comprehension + vocabulary; Word reading = word reading + spelling; TTCT-V = divergent creativity; CWT abstraction = abstraction component of convergent creativity; CWT innovation = innovation component of convergent creativity. Correlations for gifted children are presented above the diagonal, and correlations for non-gifted children are presented below the diagonal. * p<.05 ** p<.01 Table 4 Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity in gifted and non-gifted children Non-gifted Gifted ANOVA n M SD n M SD F p η2 Abstract > Div Innov > Div Div > Abstract 362 362 89 17.4 13.9 17.4 25.7 26.9 14.5 35 35 3 26.7 23.0 12.2 20.4 23.9 3.4 5.2 3.7 .401 .023 .055 .528 .013 .009 4.43 Div > Innov 107 18.9 15.8 6 11.9 13.8 1.1 .293 .009 Note. Abstract = abstraction component of convergent creativity, Innov = innovation component of convergent creativity, Div = divergent creativity
  • 13. 13 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Hypothesis 3 In order to analyse the variability of literacy scores, means, standard deviations and ranges of the various test scores were retrieved (table 5). In addition, a one-way between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine if literacy ability scores were different for gifted and non-gifted children. The ANOVA was statistically significant for reading comprehension, F (1, 364) = 40.76, p < .000 , η2 = .101. The ANOVA was not statistically significant for word reading and spelling. Table 5 Variability in reading scores in gifted and non-gifted children Non-gifted Gifted ANOVA n M SD n M SD F p η2 Reading comp. 329 38.8 15.3 37 55.8 16.5 40.7 .000 .101 Word reading 341 75.9 11.9 33 78.2 14.5 1.05 .305 .002 Note. Reading comp. = reading comprehension + vocabulary; Word reading = word reading + spelling Hypothesis 4 Finally, the size and direction of the linear relationship between divergent-convergent discrepancies and literacy ability was examined, using Kendall’s Tau-B correlation coefficients (r ). Small positive correlations were found between discrepancies (divergent > convergent) and reading comprehension, r(79) = .264, p = .001. No significant correlations were found for convergent thinking exceeding divergent thinking. Conclusion and discussion In the current research it was confirmed that intelligence and convergent creativity correlate positively below the threshold, yet correlations between intelligence and divergent creativity were negligible. With regard to gifted children, correlations between intelligence and creativity were absent for divergent creativity, as expected, yet small correlations were found for the innovation component of convergent creativity. Below the threshold, correlations for the innovation component were merely absent, indicating that the innovation component of convergent creativity is only significant in gifted children (figure 4).
  • 14. 14 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Figure 4. Correlations between intelligence and creativity above and below the threshold As shown in figure 5, discrepancies (convergent > divergent) were found to be larger in gifted children. Also variability in literacy scores was confirmed to be larger above the threshold, as shown in figure 6 and 7. It was found that the mean scores of word reading were similar below and above the threshold, whereas gifted children outperformed non-gifted children with regard to reading comprehension. Figure 5. Discrepancies between divergent and convergent creativity, both above and below the threshold
  • 15. 15 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Reading comprehension Small correlations were found between reading comprehension and creative discrepancies (divergent > convergent). In line with the theory of facilitation, the direction of this correlation was positive, suggesting that a wide focus of attention may be useful in the process of incorporating seemingly irrelevant and unrelated information to solve a problem or gain more insight (Runco, 2004). Creative discrepancies (divergent > convergent) occurred equally in gifted and non-gifted children, yet mean scores on reading comprehension were higher for gifted children. Also the variability of the scores was significantly larger above the threshold. This may be explained by the superior level of intelligence in gifted children, which is associated with larger working memory capacity (Benedek, Jauk, Sommer, Arendasy, Neubauer, 2014). Working memory is a primary resource for the control of attention (Engle, 2002; Kane, Bleckley, Conway & Engle, 2001). It supports the active maintenance of task-relevant information and the controlled search from memory (Unsworth & Engle, 2007), which is highly relevant in reading comprehension. Yet, creativity is also predicted by inhibition and personality factors (Benedek et al., 2014). Creative achievement depends on an adaptive engagement of inhibition in order to repulse stimuli that are too obvious or irrelevant and at the same time keep creative thought flowing (Vartanian, 2009; Zabelina & Robinson, 2010). Gifted children seem to have more difficulty controlling attention due to an active imagination and a propensity to daydream (Baum & Olenchak, 2002; Cramond, 1994b). This might explain the wide range of scores in reading comprehension. Figure 6. Means and variability in reading comprehension test scores, both above and below the threshold
  • 16. 16 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Word reading The ability to narrow and sustain attention to information is needed for analytical tasks or performing sequences of steps (like word reading). This can be more problematic for gifted children, because of their varied interests (Baum & Olenchak, 2002; Cramond, 1994b). Strong correlations were found between attention problems in gifted children and difficulty with academic and preliteracy tasks like rapid automatized naming (Kiefer, & Martens, 2010). Automaticity is traditionally thought to occur unconsciously and independent from top-down control (Posner & Snyder, 1975; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977). Yet, according to the attentional sensitization model of unconscious cognition (Kiefer & Martens, 2010), automaticity also strongly relies on controlled top-down activity (Beaty, Silvia, Nusbaum, Jauk, & Benedek, 2014). Figure 7. Means and variability in word reading test scores, both above and below the threshold Summarizing, it can be concluded that large working memory capacity, combined with elevated levels of divergent thinking is associated with superior reading comprehension. The combination of these characteristics is frequently present in gifted children (figure 6). However, in order to properly master the basic elements of the reading process, children also need to be able to narrow and sustain attention. With regard to word reading, gifted children with good attention control seem to outperform non-gifted children, whereas word reading scores of gifted children with attention problems appear to be dramatically lower than those of non-gifted peers (figure 7). This suggests that the ability and/or motivation for adaptive
  • 17. 17 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY inhibition and top-down control, is a key element in the word reading process of gifted children. Limitations and future research It must be noted that participants were not randomly selected, data were not all normally distributed and the sample size of the group of gifted children was small (n = 45). However, the total sample size was considerable (n = 440) and so was the amount of data taken from participants. The Creative Writing Task (utilized to assess convergent creativity) is an unconventional instrument; reliability and validity have not been formally reviewed. Nevertheless, it seems to compliment former research on the threshold theory, which was based on divergent creativity only (Benedek, Franz, Heene, & Neubauer, 2012; Karwowski & Gralewski, 2013). In order to gain more insight in (extremely) gifted children, it might be relevant to further explore the innovation component of convergent creativity in future research. With regard to the current research, implications for education are to view giftedness from a developmental perspective, to be aware of the great variety within this group and to aim for tailor made solutions.
  • 18. 18 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY References Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2005). Reliability and factorial validity of the standard progressive matrices among Kuwaiti children ages 8 to 15 years. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101, 409-412. doi:10.2466/pms.101.2.409-412 Al-Issa, I (1964). Creativity and its relationship to age, vocabulary and personality of schizophrenics. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 110(464), 74-79 Barbot, B., M., & Lubart, T. I. (2011). Assessing creativity in the classroom. The Open Education Journal, 4, 58-66. doi: 10.2174/1874920801104010058 Baum, S. M., & Olenchak, F. R. (2002). The alphabet children: GT, AHDH, and more. Exceptionality, 10, 77-91. Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. doi: 10.1037/a0035727. Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480-485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014 Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.007 Broos, A., Fulker, D. W., & De Fries, J. C. (1990). Reading performance and general cognitive ability: A multivariate genetic analysis of twin data. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 141-146. doi: 10.1016/0191-886 Bruner, J. S. (1985). The role of interaction formats in language acquisition. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Language and social situations, 31-46. New York: Springer Verlag. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499-506. Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1-22. doi: 10.1037/h0046743 Chakravarty, A. (2009). Artistic talent in dyslexia: A hypothesis. Medical Hypotheses, 73, 569–571. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2009.05.034. Chermahini, S. A., & Hommel, B. (2010). The (b)link between creativity and dopamine: Spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking.
  • 19. 19 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Cognition, 115/3, 458-465. doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.007 Chu, T-L., & Lin, W-W. (2013). Uniqueness, integration or separation? Exploring the nature of creativity through creative writing by elementary school students in Taiwan. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 33:5, 582-595, doi: 10.1080/01443410.2013.821459 Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s children’s reading comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21, 1106-1116. doi. 10.1177/0956797610375449 Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532-538. Cramond, B. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and creativity—What is the connection? Journal of Creative Behavior, 28, 193-210. Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391-404. doi. 10.1207/s15326934crj1803_13 Eide, B. L., & Eide, F. F. (2011). The dyslexic advantage: Unlocking the hidden potential of the dyslexic brain. London, UK: Hay House. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. Finch, M. E. H., Neumeister, K. L. S., Burney, V. H., & Cook, A. L. (2014). The Relationship of Cognitive and Executive Functioning With Achievement in Gifted Kindergarten Children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 58, 167–182. doi: 10.1177/0016986214534889 Haensly, P. A., and Torrance, E. P. (1990). Assessment of creativity in children and adolescents. In Reynoolds, C. R. and Kamphaus, R. W. (eds) Handbook of Psychological and Educational Assessment of Children: Intelligence and Achievement. New York: Gildford. Hansenne, M., & Legrand, J. (2012). Creativity, emotional intelligence and school performance. International Journal of Educational Research, 53, 264-268. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.03.015 Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003. Jung, R. E., Gaspararovic, C., Chaves, R. S., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Caprihan, A., & Yeo, R. A. (2009). Biochemical support for the “threshold” theory of creativity: a
  • 20. 20 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5319- 5325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0588-09.2009 Karwowski, M., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Threshold hypothesis: fact or artefact? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 25-33. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003 Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 464-489. doi.10.2478%2Fv10053-008-0102-4 Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 16, 57-66. doi: 10.4219/jsge-2005-473 Kroesbergen, Van Hooijdonk, Middel-Lalleman, Rijnders, & van Viersen (2015). The psychological well-being of early identified gifted children. Liegeois, F., Cross, J. H., Polkey, C., Harkness, W., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2008) Language after hemispherectomy in childhood: contributions from memory and intelligence. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3101-3107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.001 Lubow, R. E., Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (1): 87–103. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.87 Mann, E. L. (2006). Creativity: The essence of mathematics. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30, 236-260. doi:10.4219/jeg-2006-264. Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (2007). Issues of causality in children’s reading comprehension. In D. S. McNamara (Eds.), Reading comprehension strategies. Theories, interventions and technologies (47-71). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press. Preckel, F., Götz, T., & Frenzel, A. (2010). Ability grouping of gifted students: effects on academic self-concept and boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 451-472. doi: 10.1348/000709909X480716 Popov, A. (1992). Creativity and reading comprehension. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 26(3), 206-212Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 375-386. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55-85). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Renzulli, J. S. (1986). The three-ring conception of giftedness: a developmental model for
  • 21. 21 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY creative productivity. In R. Sternberg & J. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 332-357). New York: Cambridge University Press. Ritchie, S. J., Luciano, M., Hansell, N. K., Wright, M. J., Bates, T. C. (2013). The relationship of reading ability to creativity: positive, not negative associations. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 171–176. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.009 Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 66-75. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.652929 Runco, M. M., & Albert, R. S. (1986). The threshold theory regarding creativity and intelligence: an empirical test with gifted and nongifted children. Creative Child and Adult Quarterly J 1(4), 212-218. doi: 1988-07120-001 Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657-687. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.59.3.711 Samuels, S. J., & Flor, R. F. (1997). The importance of automaticity for developing expertise in reading. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Reading Difficulties, 13, 107–121. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407. doi. 10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1 Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A., & Freeman, D. (1984). The relationship between early reading acquisition and word decoding with and without context: A longitudinal study of first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 668-677. doi:10.1037/022-0663.76.4.668 Strang, R. (1968). Reading diagnosis and remediation (ERIC CRIER Reading Review Series). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Subotnik, R. F., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., & Worrell F. C. (2011). Rethinking giftedness and gifted education: a proposed direction forward based on psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12, 3– 54. doi: 10.1177/1529100611418056 Subotnik, R. F., & Rickoff, R. (2010). Should eminence based on outstanding innovation be the goal of gifted education and talent development? Implications for policy and research. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 358–364. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.005
  • 22. 22 Running head: INTELLIGENCE, CREATIVITY AND LITERACY ABILITY Tafti, M. A., Hameedy, M. A., & Baghal, N. M. (2009). Dyslexia, a deficit or a difference: Comparing the creativity and memory skills of dyslexic and nondyslexic students in Iran. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 37, 1009–1016. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2009.37.8.1009 Tien, Y.-M., Hsu, L.-C., Tai, J. H.-Y, Yang, Y.-F. (2014). Deciphering syntactic processing of gifted students with working memory and creativity. Chinese Journal of Psychology. 56(3), 257-276 Torrance, E. P., Ball, O. E., & Safter, T. H. (2008). Torrance® Test of Creative Thinking. Streamlined Scoring Guide for Figural Forms A and B. Illinois: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc. Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104–132. doi: 10.1037/0033- 295X.114.1.104 Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 57–59. doi: 10.1037/a0014781 Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness, conflict, and underachievement. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Wolf, K. M. & Mieg, H. A. (2010). Cognitive determinants of the success of inventors: complex problem solving and deliberate use of divergent and convergent thinking, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 443-462, doi: 10.1080/09541440902916845 Yamatoto, K. (1964a). Threshold of intelligence in academic achievement of highly creative students. The Journal of Experimental Education, 32, 401–405. Yamamoto, K. (1964b). The Experimental Scoring Manual for Minnesota Tests for Creative Thinking and Writing. Kent State University. Bureau of Educational Research. Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136–143. doi: 10.1037/a0017379