SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 69
Dorling Kindersley RF/Thinkstock
Learning Objectives
By the end of the chapter you should be
able to:
• Describe the characteristics of communicators
that make them more persuasive
• Describe what characteristics of a message make
it more persuasive
• Describe how culture, age, and self-esteem
affect persuasion
• Differentiate the central route from the peripheral
route to persuasion within the elaboration likeli-
hood model
• Describe the persuasion techniques involving
initial small requests
Persuasion 7
Chapter Outline
7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader
• Credibility: Expertise and Trustworthiness
• Attractiveness and Likeability
7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
• Emotion
• Framing
• One-Sided and Two-Sided Messages
7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience
• Culture
• Age
• Self-Esteem
• Elaboration Likelihood Model
7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
• Foot-in-the-Door Technique
• Lowball Technique
• Legitimization-of-Paltry-Favors
• Reciprocity
• Door-in-the-Face Technique
• That’s-not-all Technique
• Scarcity
• Pique Technique
• Disrupt-Then-Reframe Technique
Chapter Summary
• Describe the use of reciprocity as a persuasion technique
• Explain techniques that begin with a large request
• Explain persuasion techniques that use scarcity
• Describe techniques that involve changing attention
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 141 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the
Persuader
Every day, other people try to persuade us. Advertisements on
television, the
Internet, or the radio attempt to persuade us to buy a product.
Family members,
friends, and employers ask us to do them a favor. Some of these
messages we
quickly dismiss, but others convince us to buy the extra
absorbent paper towels,
or bake cupcakes for that fundraiser.
Imagine watching an infomercial for an exercise machine. The
product is described
by an attractive and trim fitness expert to a mildly skeptical
person in front of an
enthusiastic studio audience. The machine is demonstrated, the
positive benefits
and ease of use of the machine are touted, and viewers are
offered the product
at a low, low price. By the end of the infomercial the skeptic is
convinced of the
machine’s miraculous powers and you find yourself picking up
the phone to order
one for yourself. What makes such communications persuasive?
Social psychology
can help us find the answers to these questions by applying the
scientific method to
different aspects of persuasion—the persuader, the message, and
the audience—as
well as investigating specific persuasion techniques. Knowing
more about persua-
sion may allow us to better resist being persuaded in the future.
Persuasive communication can be divided into four parts: the
communicator, the
message, the audience, and the technique (see Figure 7.1). First
we will deal with
what characteristics of persuaders make people more likely to
be persuaded. Next,
we will think about characteristics of the message that lead
people to change. Then,
we will explore what characteristics of the audience can lead
them to be persuaded.
Finally, we will cover a variety of persuasion techniques
advertisers, charities,
friends, or relatives might use to persuade.
7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader
Persuasive messages come from a source. The source may be
someone you respect or someone you dislike—someone who
knows a lot about a topic or someone who knows little. This
someone could be a friend or a stranger. Depending on the char-
acteristics of the persuader, people may be easily persuaded or
skeptical of the claims of a
message. Characteristics that have received a great deal of
attention from researchers are
credibility, attractiveness, and likeability.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 142 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the
Persuader
Figure 7.1: The who, what, to whom, and how of persuasion
The four variables that affect persuasion are the person doing
the persuading, the message being
transmitted, the audience receiving the message, and the
techniques used by the persuader.
Credibility: Expertise and Trustworthiness
As you watch an infomercial, a central communicator is likely
to offer arguments for the
product. Whether or not you listen to this person likely depends
on how credible you
view that person to be. Credibility has two aspects: expertise
and trustworthiness (Hov-
land, Janis, & Kelley, 1953). A communicator with expertise is
one who appears to have
knowledge and is able to communicate it. A trustworthy
communicator is one we believe
is giving us accurate information.
The Persuader
Important characteristics:
• Credibility
• Attractiveness and likeability
The Message
Important characteristics:
• Fear and guilt
• Framing
• One-sided and two-sided messages
• Narratives and rational appeals
The Techniques
Foot-in-the-door, low ball, legitimization-of-
paltry-favors, reciprocity, door-in-the-face,
that’s-not-all, scarcity, pique, disrupt-then-
reframe
The Audience
Important characteristics:
• Culture
• Age
• Self-esteem
• Elaboration Likelihood Model
Variables that
affect the
impact of
persuasion
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 143 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the
Persuader
Messages from expert sources are
persuasive when the message
includes coherent and high quality
arguments from within that expert’s
field of knowledge (DeBono & Har-
nish, 1988; Petty, Cacioppo, & Gold-
man, 1981). Messages that come
from an expert source but are of low
quality are less persuasive than mes-
sages coming from someone who is
less of an expert, but who has strong
arguments (Bohner, Ruder, & Erb,
2002; Tormala, Brinol, & Petty, 2006).
However, an excess of confidence
can sometimes limit the effectiveness
of the persuasion. For example, in
court an expert with a moderate
level of confidence appears to be
most persuasive, perhaps because a very high level of
confidence is viewed as arrogant or
unrealistic (Cramer, DeCoster, Harris, Fletcher, & Brodsky,
2011). Expert opinion is gener-
ally only persuasive within that expert’s domain of expertise.
For example, you might
believe what fitness experts say about exercise, but not what
they say about cake decorat-
ing. An exception to this would be involving children, due to
their place in society. For
example, using children to demonstrate the safety features in a
new vehicle may resonate
with an adult’s perceived role as protector and nurturer
(Pratkanis & Gliner, 2004).
At times, we may receive
a message and not have
the time or energy to think
carefully through the argu-
ments. In those instances
the trustworthiness of the
communicator can serve as
a cue regarding whether we
should trust the message. If
we perceive that the commu-
nicator is providing us with
accurate information, we
may not feel it necessary to
carefully examine the mes-
sage itself. When we perceive the communicator to be less
trustworthy, we may care-
fully examine the message to determine if we can trust it. A
message can, in this instance,
still be persuasive if it contains strong arguments. A nonexpert
who cites a study by the
American Heart Association showing that the exercise
equipment improves heart health
in 90% of users has a strong argument, despite lack of expertise.
An expert who points out
that the stainless steel frame of the equipment will match any
decor has a weak argument.
Strong messages from non-expert sources can be persuasive
because people carefully
examine the arguments (Priester & Petty, 2003). Such careful
examination creates stronger
and more long-lasting attitude change.
Expand Your Knowledge: Propaganda
Governments, political parties, organizations, and indi-
viduals have all used persuasive communications in
an effort to sway the public. The website Propaganda
Critic http://www.propagandacritic.com from the
Institute for Propaganda Analysis, offers a look at pro-
paganda, including a page on why investigating propa-
ganda matters and analysis of a number of propaganda
techniques.
PNC/Photodisc/Getty Images
Lawyers often retain expert witnesses to help strengthen
their client’s case in favor of the jury.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 144 7/16/13 9:49 AM
http://www.propagandacritic.com
CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the
Persuader
Attractiveness and Likeability
The attractiveness of the communicator is another factor in the
persuasiveness of the
message. In general, physically attractive communicators are
more persuasive than less
attractive communicators (Chaiken, 1979; Debevec, Madden, &
Kernan, 1986; DeBono &
Telesca, 1990). Physically attractive communicators may be
more persuasive because they
are viewed as more trustworthy than less attractive
communicators—research has shown
that we stereotype physically attractive people as having other
positive qualities such
as intelligence and honesty (Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, &
Longo, 1991; Langlois, et al.,
2000). We also tend to like physically attractive people more
and want to interact with
them (Reingen & Kernan, 1993). Even when a product is
unrelated to attractiveness, we
find attractive communicators more persuasive than less
attractive communicators (Prax-
marer, 2011).
Individuals we like are also more persuasive to us. You might
buy Girl Scout cookies from
the neighbor who you know and like, but you are likely to buy
fewer cookies from the Girl
Scout across town. For this reason, advertisers have found
tricky ways to make it appear
that an appeal is coming from someone we know and like. An
envelope that appears to be
addressed by hand and have a note inside is more likely to be
opened and read than one
that is clearly mass produced. In one study of this idea, a
number of car owners in Dallas
were sent an ad for car wax that appeared to have been ripped
from a magazine, with a
handwritten sticky note attached; if the recipient was named
Mary, for example, the note
would read “Mary—Try this. It works!—J,” with “J” being the
supposed sender. The ad
contained a mail-in card for a free sample of the car wax. The
ad was sent in an unmarked
white envelope, hand-addressed, with a first-class stamp.
Another set of car owners were
mailed the same ad, but in a typed envelope sent through
metered mail, and the ad inside
was simply printed on a sheet of paper with no note attached.
When the message in the ad
was strong and it appeared to come from someone they knew,
almost double the number
of car owners requested a free sample than if the message
appeared to be mass produced
(Howard & Kerin, 2004). A note from someone we might know
is more persuasive than a
message from a faceless company.
Should the communicators make their desires to persuade
explicit? To maintain credibility
and avoid reactance, advertisers generally avoid stating that
they are trying to persuade;
however, in some contexts such information can be helpful. You
would likely attribute
selfish motives to the fitness expert who is trying to sell
exercise equipment he designed.
But your doctor presumably has your best interests in mind, so
his or her obviously per-
suasive message about exercise may be received well (Campbell
& Kirmani, 2000; Eagly,
Wood, & Chaiken, 1978). Researchers have found that
physically attractive communica-
tors do well if they make their desire to persuade explicit.
Unattractive communicators
Test Yourself
• What are the two aspects of credibility?
Expertise and trustworthiness are the two aspects of credibility.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 145 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
When making persuasive appeals, communicators must carefully
consider the message they want to portray and how they want to
share that message. A per-suader might try to scare you so you
will adopt a healthy behavior. Should the
message be terrifying, or just a little frightening to motivate
you? Communicators must
also consider whether sharing the other side’s arguments is
helpful. If a phone company
knows that a rival phone has a feature that its own product does
not have, should it make
a mention of it in the advertising? As we discover in this
section, how a message is con-
structed makes all the difference in how persuasive it will be.
Emotion
Within a persuasive appeal, a communicator might attempt to
elicit an emotion. Emotions
contain both physiological and cognitive elements. For example,
when you are frightened
your blood pressure and your heart rate increase. The same
bodily state may be inter-
preted differently depending on the context. You might feel fear
if you are in a dark alley
and a stranger approaches. In the context of a thrilling video
game, that same racing heart
could be interpreted as excitement (Schachter & Singer, 1962).
Advertisers may capitalize
on this fact to help sell their products. A beer commercial with
a lot of beautiful, scantily
clad women may excite the men watching it—excitement that
could transfer to the brand
of beer the company is selling. When a camera follows a car
down a curvy road through
a majestic mountain landscape, we may feel a bit of awe at the
beauty of the scenery, and
we may transfer this sense of amazement to the car.
Test Yourself
• Who is more persuasive, an attractive communicator or an
unattractive communicator?
An attractive communicator is usually more persuasive.
• What types of communicators should make you aware of their
intent to persuade you?
Attractive persuaders and people we like are persuasive when
we know they are trying
to persuade us, as well as those who we know have our best
interests at heart (doctor).
are not very successful in this instance (Messner, Reinhard, &
Sporer, 2008). For the less
attractive, keeping persuasive intent hidden is a better strategy.
Similarly, disliked sources
should conceal persuasive intent, while liked sources are more
persuasive when they are
open about persuasive intent (Reinhard, Messner, & Sporer,
2006).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 146 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
One way persuaders may use emotion to con-
vince us to do something is to draw on the
“appropriate” emotions from our culture and
social context. We learn from those around us
what emotions are appropriate to feel (Averill,
1980). For example, convinced by your peers or
the media that your home should be clean and
smell like flowers, you buy a scented candle and
spend more time cleaning to avoid feeling embar-
rassed. Or, having learned from an advertiser that
a new car will make you happy, you purchase a
new vehicle.
Persuaders elicit specific emotions, such as fear, to
motivate people to act. If a health educator were
trying to convince smokers to quit smoking, would
scaring them about the potential consequences
of continuing to smoke be effective? Researchers
have long been interested in how persuasive mes-
sages that induce fear, what might be known as
scare tactics, influence persuasion (Hovland et al.,
1953, is an early example). The relationship was
hypothesized to follow the pattern of an inverted
U, with little persuasion at low levels of fear, the greatest
persuasion at moderate levels of
fear, and low persuasion at high levels of fear (see Figure 7.2).
At low fear levels persuasion
would be low because there is not enough motivation to change.
At a moderate rate of fear,
persuasion should be highest; here people would be motivated
to make a change but not
so scared that they become paralyzed. At high levels of fear,
according to this hypothesis,
persuasion once again becomes less likely, as people become
too frightened to process the
information and respond to it. At this level, individuals may
become defensive and ignore
the content of the message. At a low level of fear, an
antismoking message may not moti-
vate smokers to change; the consequences may not seem dire
enough to make the effort of
quitting worth it. At a high level of fear, smokers may dismiss a
message or become defen-
sive about their habit. When a message induces a moderate level
of fear smokers may be
able to process the message rather than becoming defensive and
be motivated to change.
Yet this hypothesis is problematic because the supporting
research is inconclusive (Eagly
& Chaiken, 1993; Janis & Leventhal, 1968). Some degree of
fear is motivating, so messages
that make it clear that the target is susceptible to the
consequences of a threat are most
effective (Maloney, Lapinski, & Witte, 2011). However, the
most important predictor of
behavior when confronted with scare tactics seems to not be the
level of fear aroused, but
the belief of individuals in their ability to engage in actions that
will allow them to avoid
the feared consequence (Ruiter, Abraham, & Kok, 2001).
Smokers might be exposed to a
message that evokes a great deal or just a little fear about lung
cancer, but if they do not
believe they can quit, thereby avoiding cancer, the level of fear
in the appeal does not seem
to matter much (Hoeken & Geurts, 2005; Timmers & van der
Wijst, 2007; Witte, 1998).
Getty Images News/Getty Images
A graphic warning label such as this might
scare people into rethinking their smoking
habits.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 147 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
Some persuasive appeals may
use guilt to attempt attitude
change. Charities, for example,
might describe people who are
hungry or sick or homeless in an
attempt to persuade someone to
give money or time. Guilt appeals
can be successful; the more guilty
people feel, the greater their
intention to donate. However, if
recipients are generally skeptical
of emotional appeals and believe
the communicator is manipula-
tive, they will not feel as much
guilt and therefore not be as likely
to donate (Hibbert, Smith, Davies,
& Ireland, 2007).
Rather than working to generate certain emotions, advertisers
can also appeal to con-
sumers by tapping into their current emotional experience.
Happy holiday shoppers are
likely to be persuaded by a happy message. The sweet and sappy
commercials for engage-
ment rings will do well with those who are in love and feeling
sweet and sappy toward
their significant other. A match between the emotional state of
the person and the emo-
tional overtones of the persuasive message is the most effective
(DeSteno, Petty, Rucker,
Wegener, & Braverman, 2004).
The complexity of the emotion presented in the message also
influences persuasion. When
people do not have a lot of time or energy to devote to
processing a message, their resources
are low. Sadness and happiness are relatively simple emotions,
and can be quickly pro-
cessed by the individual. When the resources of the person are
low and they are presented
with an ad with simple emotions like happiness or sadness, they
are more persuaded. If
resources were low and the effectiveness of the message relied
on the emotion of anxiety,
a relatively complex emotion, the message would be less
persuasive. For individuals with
more time and energy to process the message, anxiety within a
message could be more
persuasive than a simple emotion within that message (Lau-
Gesk & Meyers-Levy, 2009).
To generate an emotional response, persuaders might tell a
story. Researchers have found
that individuals are persuaded by stories if the stories are good,
even when arguments
themselves are weak. One element in persuasive stories is
something called transpor-
tation. In the context of story, transportation is the joining of
feelings, attention, and
thoughts (Green & Brock, 2000). The listener joins the
storyteller on a journey, and, for
the time being, focuses on the teller’s thoughts, feelings, and
experiences. As a result, the
listener comes away with a new or more informed perspective.
On the other hand, when
a message is shared not in a story but as a rational appeal, only
strong arguments lead to
attitude change (Escalas, 2007). People presented with personal
narratives of a health risk
viewed themselves as being more at risk and said they were
more likely to get tested than
those presented with statistical information on that health risk
(deWit, Das, & Vet, 2008).
Figure 7.2: The inverted-U model
In Hovland’s study, the relationship between fear and
persuasion was hypothesized to resemble an inverted U.
Highest Level of Persuasion
P
e
rs
u
a
s
io
n
Fear Levels
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 148 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
Framing
Messages can vary in whether they emphasize gains or losses. A
message that focuses on
benefits is described as being gain framed. If you were buying a
vehicle, for example, the
message that buying this car will keep you safe in a crash with
all of its safety features
emphasizes the things you would gain. A message that focuses
on losses is loss framed.
For example, a salesperson might suggest that if you do not buy
this car with all of its
safety features, your likelihood of experiencing major injury in
a crash is increased. By
emphasizing what you would lose, the message becomes loss
framed. Framing can influ-
ence responses to persuasive messages.
When dealing with prevention behaviors, such as exercising,
gain-framed messages tend
to be more effective. When encouraging detection behaviors,
such as going to the doc-
tor for a cancer screening, loss-framed messages tend to be
more effective (Ferrer, Klein,
Zajac, Land, & Ling, 2012; Rothman, Bartels, Wlaschin, &
Salovey, 2006). When people
are faced with a message and experiencing an emotion like fear,
loss-framed messages
are more persuasive. Fear is an emotion that leads to behavioral
inhibition, or movement
away from action. Anger and happiness are part of the
behavioral approach system, moti-
vating movement toward action. When people are angry or
happy gain-framed messages
are more persuasive (Gerend & Maner, 2011; Yan, Dillard, &
Shen, 2012).
Gain-framed messages create more positive emotions than loss-
framed emotions. How-
ever, researchers have also found that, while being in a positive
mood due to a gain-
framed message leads to persuasion, being in a negative mood
as a result of a loss-framed
message may motivate people to do something about the threat
(Van’T Riet, Ruiter, Werrij,
Candel, & De Vries, 2010). Being happy because you realize
from the infomercial all you
might gain from buying this exercise equipment might persuade
you that purchasing the
equipment is a good idea. Or, being sad because you realize
from the infomercial all the
terrible things that could happen if you do not buy the
equipment and begin exercising
may actually get you to pick up the phone and make an order.
When presenting a persuasive message, it is important for the
communicator to avoid
making the audience defensive. Affirming what others believe
and validating their con-
cerns can increase message scrutiny and lead to a change in
behavior (Correll, Spencer,
& Zanna, 2004). When students’ concerns about availability of
recycling containers were
Test Yourself
• What are the key elements to making a fear appeal persuasive?
The message needs to make it clear that the person is
susceptible to the threat, the
threat applies to the person, and that the person can avoid the
threat.
• Are messages that have happy messages persuasive?
Happy messages can be persuasive, particularly if the message
and the individual’s
mood or resources match.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 149 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
Test Yourself
• Is a message encouraging people to quit smoking that
emphasizes the benefit of years
of healthy living a gain-framed or a loss-framed message?
The message is gain framed because it emphasizes benefits or
gains rather than losses.
• Would a message that suggests people have yearly glaucoma
tests be more persuasive
if it was gain framed or loss framed?
Because the message involves detection behavior, rather than
prevention behavior, a
loss-framed message would be more persuasive.
validated (“we know it’s a long walk but . . .”), and they were
encouraged to use the con-
tainers that were available, even if it was inconvenient, their
recycling behavior increased
and lasted longer (Werner, Stoll, Birch, & White, 2002).
Reactance, the tendency to reas-
sert one’s freedom in the face of demands from others, can be
reduced by affirming what
that person believes. Acknowledging concerns works by
reducing criticism of a persua-
sive message, opening the individual up to consideration of the
message (Werner, White,
Byerly, & Stoll, 2009).
Another way to frame messages is by focusing on the long-term
or the short-term con-
sequences of an action, called temporal framing. A message
encouraging colonoscopies
might emphasize the peace of mind one would feel knowing that
any potentially cancer-
ous polyps have been removed. By focusing on current feelings
or consequences, such
messages use a short-term framing of a message. A colonoscopy
message that uses a long-
term frame might emphasize how removal of potentially
cancerous polyps could provide
years of life without colon cancer. When a message is about a
health risk and focuses
on short-term consequences, the risks of not engaging in the
behavior are perceived as
being more concrete (Chandran & Menon, 2004). Temporal
framing can influence the per-
suasiveness of gain-framed messages. College students
receiving gain-framed messages
that emphasized short-term consequences of drinking showed
less drinking than those
who received loss-framed messages emphasizing short-term
consequences or those that
described long-term consequences (Gerend & Cullen, 2008).
Some people are more sensi-
tive to short-term consequences of their actions and are,
therefore, more persuaded by
temporal frames that emphasize immediate consequences of
actions (Orbell & Hagger,
2006; Orbell & Kyriakaki, 2008).
One-Sided and Two-Sided Messages
Is presenting both sides of an argument advantageous or does it
weaken the message? For
example, if a car salesperson wanted to convince someone to
buy an electric car, would
just presenting the positive features of the car be best, or should
the salesperson point
out both the positive and negatives of owning an electric car? A
communicator must also
decide whether to simply describe the alternate position or also
refute it. Should the elec-
tric-car salesperson simply mention the drawbacks of owning
such a vehicle (the fully
charged vehicle goes only 100 miles), or also explain why those
drawbacks are not of great
concern (most trips people take are well under this distance)?
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 150 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
Generally, messages are most persuasive when they are first
encountered. Over time the
effect of a message gradually declines. For instance, you might
be quite motivated to exer-
cise after your doctor tells you about the importance of
exercise. A few months later, how-
ever, you find your gym clothes collecting dust in the corner.
However, in some instances
messages become more persuasive with time, a phenomenon
known as the sleeper effect
(Peterson & Thurstone, 1933/1970). For the sleeper effect to
occur, individuals need to
spend enough time thinking about the message that the message
remains in memory
(Priester, Wegner, Petty, & Fabrigar, 1999). People may
discount a message when they first
encounter it, but over time, forget their reasons for doing so,
leading to their persuasion
(Kumkale & Albarracin, 2004). If an unreliable source tells you
a story you may at first
discount the story because of the untrustworthiness of the
source. After some time, how-
ever, you may remember the story but forget who told it to you,
making the story more
persuasive a month after it was told to you. Some of the power
of narratives may be due
to the sleeper effect. The impression made by a narrative is
retained, and any reasons for
discounting it (it was just a fictional story) may be lost with
time (Appel & Richter, 2007).
Test Yourself
• Are one-sided or two-sided messages more persuasive?
Two-sided messages are more persuasive if they include
refutation of the opposing side.
If not, then one-sided messages are more persuasive.
For the most part, two-sided messages are more persuasive
when they provide a refuta-
tion of the option the communicator is arguing against. If both
sides are simply offered,
with no refutation, one-sided messages are more persuasive
(Allen, 1991; Buehl, Alexan-
der, Murphy, & Sperl, 2001). In the exercise equipment
infomercial, this means that the
communicator should discuss the drawbacks of owning one’s
own equipment and those
drawbacks should be refuted. Owning one’s own equipment, for
example, means taking
care of it and storing it, but, the fitness expert might argue, this
equipment takes almost
no care and can easily be stored in the corner of a room.
When in court, defense lawyers must decide whether to present
potentially incriminating
evidence the prosecution might reveal. Presenting information
that is potentially harm-
ful before the prosecution does can allow the defense to take
away some of the shock
value that information might otherwise have. The name of this
tactic is stealing thun-
der. Within the courtroom, stealing thunder increases the
credibility of the defense and
changes the way jurors think about the incriminating evidence
(Dolnik, Case & Williams,
2003; Williams, Bourgeois, & Croyle, 1993). By presenting
evidence that might otherwise
be condemning, the defense shows that it has done its research
and is aware of the cir-
cumstances. By presenting the evidence, the defense is then able
to take it apart for the
jurors, showing them why it is not condemning. This helps sway
the opinion of the jurors,
who might have otherwise formed a different opinion if the
prosecution had framed the
evidence. This tactic has been found to be more effective when
jurors are trying to make
quick decisions or are distracted or overwhelmed by the
evidence. When jurors carefully
consider the evidence, it has less of an effect (Howard, Brewer,
& Williams, 2006).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 151 7/16/13 9:49 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience
The audience of a persuasive message is vitally important.
Characteristics of the audi-ence may impact how a message is
received. People from different cultures think about and respond
to messages differently. Individuals of different ages and levels
of self-esteem may also differ in how easily they are persuaded.
In this section, we will
explore how the characteristics of the audience affect reception
of a message.
Culture
A large-scale difference in audiences for persuasive messages is
the cultural background
of the audience. In Chapter 3, different ways of viewing the self
were discussed. People
from Western cultures, like the United States, most often view
themselves as indepen-
dent and unique individuals; they are part of an independent
culture. People from many
Asian cultures, such as Korea, have a more interdependent view
of themselves; they are
part of an interdependent culture. In interdependent cultures,
people see themselves as
enmeshed within a social context, with the sense of self arising
out of social roles and
relationships.
Ryan McVay/Lifesize/Thinkstock
Western values usually emphasize independence and
uniqueness, while Asian values often focus more
on social roles and ingroups.
a b
Test Yourself
• If your arguments are weak should you use a narrative appeal
or a rational appeal?
With weak arguments you are better off with a narrative appeal.
Stories are persuasive
even when the arguments are weak. Rational appeals are not
persuasive when argu-
ments are weak.
• Are messages you initially discount ever persuasive?
Evidence of the sleeper effect suggests that over time arguments
that you thought
about but dismissed can become more persuasive.
iStockphoto/Thinkstock
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 152 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
Persuasive messages follow this pattern across cultures. For
example, persuasive mes-
sages in the United States focus more on uniqueness and
individual preferences. In Korea,
advertisements and other persuasive messages are more likely to
focus on harmony with
others, particularly family or other ingroups (Han & Shavitt,
1994). For example, an adver-
tisement in Korea might describe a product as promoting family
unity or the happiness
of others. Recent research has shown that this difference can
vary depending on genera-
tion within a culture. Affluent Generation X consumers in China
were more persuaded
than older Chinese consumers by an advertisement for a car that
emphasized uniqueness,
an independent culture value (Zhang, 2010). These
advertisements described the car as
allowing one to break away from the pack rather than being a
car a family could depend
on. With greater exposure to Western values, these young
people in China were more per-
suaded by advertisers who emphasized values of uniqueness
rather than family.
Age
Between ages of 3 and 5, children develop the ability to
understand that what is in their
mind is different from what is in the minds of others, a concept
called theory of mind.
They also come to understand that others can be persuaded. As
long as children believe
that others think exactly as they do and know what they know,
there is no need to per-
suade (Slaughter, Peterson, & Moore, 2013). When children
learn they can persuade oth-
ers they also realize that others may be trying to persuade them.
Because of this, children
under the age of 5 do not recognize that advertisers are
attempting to persuade them
(McAlister & Cornwell, 2009).
In looking at persuasion for younger, middle-aged, and older
adults, a paradox emerges.
Younger adults tend to have fluid attitudes that solidify and
remain the same through
middle and older adulthood. For example, although young adults
may dramatically
change their political attitudes, once established these attitudes
tend to remain the same
after young adulthood (Alwin, Cohen, & Newcomb, 1991;
Culter & Kaufman, 1975; Kros-
nick & Alwin, 1989). However, in a research setting,
participants of different ages show
approximately equal propensity for persuasion, sometimes even
more so for older adults
(Wang & Chen, 2006). Why might attitudes remain the same
over a lifetime, but show
evidence of malleability in a laboratory setting?
Over their lifespan, people seem to be equally likely to change
their attitudes. What many
young adults have that most middle-aged and older adults do not
have is a lot of new
life experiences. When middle-aged and older adults experience
new things, they are as
likely to change as younger adults (Tyler & Schuller, 1991).
What, then, seems to be driv-
ing the greater flexibility in young adulthood is not age but
experience. Older adults may
be slightly more vulnerable to persuasion in laboratory
persuasion tasks. This increased
susceptibility can be traced to a decline in processing capacity
with age. Older adults do
not have quite as much working memory capacity as younger
adults, and therefore, tend
to be more persuaded by simple cues, like the number of
arguments there are for a par-
ticular option (Wang & Chen, 2006).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 153 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
Test Yourself
• A message that focuses on family and relationships is more
likely to be persuasive in
what type of culture?
A message emphasizing interdependence is likely to be more
persuasive in interdepen-
dent cultures like those often found in Asia.
• Do older adults and middle-aged adults have the same capacity
to be persuaded as
younger adults?
Yes, though because of more stable life experiences the older
groups tend to have more
stable attitudes over time.
Self-Esteem
Individuals’ level of self-esteem can influence how easy it is to
persuade them. When it
comes to differences among people, William McGuire (1968),
an early researcher of per-
suasion, proposed that persuasion depended on both receptivity
and yielding. Receptivity
means that one has the ability and willingness to pay attention
and understand a message.
Yielding means that the individual changes his or her mind as a
result of the message. In
order for a message to be considered persuasive, the individual
must both be receptive to
the message, and yield to it.
When evaluating the effect of self-esteem on persuasion, both
receptivity and yielding
are important (Rhodes & Wood, 1992). Individuals with low
self-esteem who do not trust
their own opinion are likely to yield to what others say. But
those with low self-esteem
lack receptivity: they are less likely to pay attention and
remember a message. On the
other end of the spectrum, receptivity is high among those with
high self-esteem. Individ-
uals with high self-esteem are likely to pay attention to and
remember a message. Those
with high self-esteem, however, are confident in their own
judgments and therefore, less
persuaded by what others say. Those with moderate self-esteem
are likely to have the
combination of receptivity and yielding that makes persuasion
likely. While research find-
ings support this claim, there are many variables that interact
with self-esteem; given
the right circumstances, individuals with high or low self-
esteem are more persuadable
(Sanaktekin & Sunar, 2008).
Test Yourself
• How do receptivity and yielding relate to self-esteem?
Individuals with high self-esteem tend to be high in receptivity
but low in yielding while
those with low self-esteem tend to be low in receptivity but high
in yielding, leaving
those in the middle with moderate receptivity and yielding.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 154 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
Elaboration Likelihood Model
One model that brings together persuader, message, and
audience variables is the elabo-
ration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). According to
this model, people differ
in their motivation and ability to process a persuasive message
(see Figure 7.3). When
people are motivated and able to process a message, they will
take more time to think
about and evaluate the message. “Elaboration” refers to this
engagement with the mes-
sage. Those high on the spectrum of being willing and able to
process a message will
use what researchers have named the central route to
persuasion. When using the cen-
tral route, individuals spend time and energy thinking about a
message, evaluating the
strength of the persuasive arguments. If messages are strong,
people are likely to respond
with positive attitude change, leading to behavior that is
reflective of that change (Wu &
Shaffer, 1987). Stronger attitudes tend to last longer and are
less likely to be changed when
attacked (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Wu & Shaffer, 1987). But
when arguments in a message
are weak, those taking this route to persuasion may reject the
message.
Figure 7.3: The elaboration likelihood model
According to the elaboration likelihood model, level of
processing depends on motivation and ability to
process the message.
Based on Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The
elaboration likelihood model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19,
pp. 123–205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Low level of elaboration
(due to depleted self-control,
minimal time and energy available)
High level of elaboration
(due to relevance, time and energy
available, and need for cognition)
Central route Peripheral route
Thoughtful, attentive,
and deep processing
Quick and superficial
processing
The strength of the
persuasive argument
Various persuasive
cues
Leads to use of the
where messages are evaluated via
and changes in attitude depend on and changes in attitude
depend on
where messages are evaluated via
Leads to use of the
Message
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 155 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
Social Psychology in Depth: Word of Mouth and Persuasion
When making decisions about products to buy, restaurants to
visit, or movies to see, we
often ask our friends and relatives. A friend who raves about a
new movie is probably more
likely to get you to the theater than an advertisement on
television. This passing of infor-
mation about products or services through informal social
networks is known as “word of
mouth.” Traditionally, word of mouth has its strongest impact
when a relationship is close
and the message is rich or vivid (Brown & Reingen, 1987;
Kisielius & Sternthal, 1984; Swee-
ney, Soutar, & Mazzarol, 2008).
A new frontier for persuasion through word of mouth is the
Internet. Websites like Amazon
.com offer customer reviews of the products they sell. These
reviews, known as “electronic
word of mouth” or “e-word” of mouth, can significantly affect
popularity and sales (Ye, Law,
Gu, & Chen, 2011; Zhang, Ye, Law, & Li, 2010). E-word of
mouth is different from traditional
word of mouth because many of the comments or reviews one
might read about a product
or service are from strangers, not friends or relatives.
Online shoppers tend to use certain clues to evaluate e-word of
mouth. While considering
the e-word of mouth for a product, shoppers look at the
agreement among evaluations. If
all reviewers are in agreement, all positive or all negative, that
may sway a shopper (Chiou
& Cheng, 2003). If the reviews for a camera you were
considering purchasing were uni-
formly negative, would you buy the camera? Probably not. One
bad review among many,
however, will not necessarily scare off a shopper. A set of
perfect reviews may be seen as
suspicious, too good to be true (Doh & Hwang, 2009). Overall,
the greater the ratio of posi-
tive to negative reviews, the more positive the attitude of the
shopper and the greater the
intention to buy the product (Doh & Hwang, 2009).
(continued)
We tend to elaborate when a message is relevant to us and we
have the time and energy to
process it (Petty & Brock, 1981; Petty, Wells, & Brock, 1976).
For example, if a commercial
for exercise equipment were shown during a television show
you were watching, you
might listen closely if you were thinking about buying such
equipment and you had the
time to listen. If you had a gym membership and were happy
with your exercise routine,
you might largely ignore the message. Our elaboration of a
message can also depend on
how much we feel the need to evaluate aspects of our lives in
general. Need for cogni-
tion is a term researchers use to describe differences between
individuals concerning their
desire to engage in, and tendency to enjoy, thinking (Cacioppo
& Petty, 1982). In a scale
to assess need for cognition, research participants are asked
about their agreement with
statements such as “I really enjoy a task that involves coming
up with new solutions to
problems” or “I would prefer a task that is intellectual, difficult,
and important over one
that is somewhat important but does not require much thought.”
(Cacioppo & Petty, 1982,
p.120). Whether or not something is personally relevant, people
who are high in need for
cognition tend to take the central route to persuasion (Cacioppo,
Petty, & Morris, 1983).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 156 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the
Audience
Prior knowledge of a subject can influence people in a similar
way. If you knew a great
deal about exercise equipment, you would have an easier time
processing the infomercial.
In this instance, you would likely perceive a message with
strong arguments as more con-
vincing and quickly dismiss one with weak arguments or only
an attractive communicator
to recommend it (Wood & Kallgren, 1988). As with need for
cognition, even when a topic
is not personally relevant, someone with prior knowledge is still
more likely to use the
central route to persuasion (Chebat, Charlebois, & Gelinas-
Chebat, 2001). Even if you are
not presently in the market for exercise equipment, if you
stumble across an advertisement
for such equipment and have knowledge about equipment, you
are likely to pay attention.
When people are less motivated or do not have the time or
energy to process a message,
they use the peripheral route to persuasion. With this route,
individuals use other cues to
evaluate the persuasive arguments. Some cues include: the
number of arguments presented,
the supposed credibility of the source, and how many other
people seem to be persuaded
(Maheswaran & Chaiken, 1991; O’Keefe, 2002; Petty,
Caciopppo, & Schumann, 1983). For
example, an online shopper using the peripheral route might be
persuaded by the number
of reviews for a product rather than the quality of those reviews
(Sher & Lee, 2009). We
are more likely to take the peripheral route to persuasion when
our level of self-control is
depleted due to prior use (Fennis, Janssen, & Vohs, 2009). For
example, if you studied hard
for a test, using all of your self-control to keep working through
class material, an adver-
tisement with an attractive communicator would be more
persuasive to you, regardless of
the strength of that communicator’s arguments. Recall from
Chapter 2 that ego depletion
creates a problem for later volitional acts. The peripheral route
is used when we do not have
the time or energy to consider a persuasive message carefully—
when the ego is depleted.
The persuasive message itself can influence what route we
would take. Personalization
in websites is one way online retailers have sought to influence
the buying public (Tam &
Ho, 2005). Many companies that sell products online now keep
track of where consum-
ers look on their sites, what they click on, and what they
eventually buy. With all of that
information, the retailer is able to make suggestions for an
individual site visitor or for
Social Psychology in Depth: Word of Mouth and Persuasion
(continued)
Online reviews of products may be evaluated differently
depending on the shopper. Online
shoppers high in need for cognition were more persuaded by
reviews that were under-
standable, objective, and supported by relevant facts than by
those that were of lower
quality. The quality of the review has no impact on persuasion
for those with low need for
cognition (Lin, Lee, & Horng, 2011). A large number of reviews
is more convincing to those
low in need for cognition but does not affect persuasibility for
those high in need for cogni-
tion (Lin et al., 2011). Individuals who are largely unfamiliar
with a product are also more
swayed by the number of reviews for a product. Expertise of
reviewers, not number of
reviews, is more important to those who know more (SanJose-
Cabezudo, Gutierrez-Arranz,
& Gutierrez-Cillan, 2009).
The way individuals engage in e-word of mouth may be
different in different countries. In a
study of discussion boards in the United States and China,
Chinese participants were more
concerned about the country of origin, as opposed to the
quality, of the products they were
investigating. Researchers also found greater information
seeking but less information pro-
vision by the Chinese than the U.S. participants (Fong &
Burton, 2008).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 157 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Watching a series of infomercials or a shopping channel, you
may have noticed that some phrases keep coming up. Most
offers are “for a limited time only.” The shopping channel may
show you how many of the products are left and count
them down until they are all gone. Some infomercials ask you to
send no money now or
offer you a full refund if you are not completely satisfied.
Others offer the product and
then let you know that if you order now you could get a free
gift. All of these advertise-
ments are based on persuasion techniques researched by social
psychologists and others.
In this section, we will discuss several persuasive techniques.
The foot-in-the-door tech-
nique begins with a small request, and then moves to a larger
request. With the lowball
technique, a request is made and accepted before the full cost is
revealed. The legitimiza-
tion-of-paltry-favors technique validates small contributions.
Later in this section we will
look at persuasion techniques involving reciprocity, large initial
requests, scarcity, and
changing the focus of attention.
Foot-in-the-Door Technique
If you have ever bought a car, you may have found yourself
visiting a lot of dealerships or
used-car lots. At a dealership you might be asked to sit in a new
car or offered a test drive.
Test Yourself
• According to the elaboration likelihood model, if you had the
time and interest to listen
to a message, what type of message would be most persuasive?
When you have time and interest in a message you are more
likely to use the central
route to persuasion. Messages with strong arguments are most
persuasive when using
the central route.
• What is the relationship between need for cognition and the
two different routes to
persuasion?
People who are high in need for cognition tend to take the
central route to persuasion.
• Are attitudes stronger when someone has been persuaded using
the central route or
the peripheral route?
Messages that persuade using the central route result in strong
attitudes that are more
resistant to change and more likely to be related to behavior.
the general shopper who visits and shows interest in a product.
You may have visited a
website that, after you purchase or even just click on a few
products, offers suggestions
of other products you might be interested in; that site is using
personalization strategies.
Because personalization makes messages relevant to
individuals, according to the elabo-
ration likelihood model it should lead to greater central route
processing, and, when a
product is good, potentially more purchasing of that product.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 158 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
In doing all of this the salesperson might say that there is no
obligation, but is that really
true? With the foot-in-the-door technique, a small request is
made and agreed to, fol-
lowed by a larger request. The salesperson might first ask if you
would like to take a test
drive, and only after the test drive ask if you would like to
discuss financing options.
Because people have said yes to the first request (test drive)
they are more likely to say yes
to the second request (discussing options and potentially
moving to buy the car). The sec-
ond request is really the target for the salesperson (Freedman &
Fraser, 1966). Depending
on the situation, this technique has been known to bring 15% to
25% compliance with the
second request (Dillard, 1991).
Use of the foot-in-the-door
technique requires a delicate
balancing act. The first request
needs to be small enough to
be accepted, but the larger the
first request, the more likely
individuals will say yes to
the second request (Seligman,
Bush, & Kirsch, 1976). One
explanation for the foot-in-
the-door technique’s success
is related to self-perception
theory. Self-perception theory
involves attitude formation;
when we want to know what
our attitudes are, we look at our behavior. With foot-in-the-
door, the same process might be
happening as people look to their initial behavior (agreeing to a
small request) to determine
what they should do for another behavior (agreeing to the
larger, target request) (Burger,
1999; Burger & Caldwell, 2003). Foot-in-the-door technique is
also applied outside of com-
mercial purposes. When individuals visit a website for a
charitable organization, those who
agree to sign a petition are more likely to donate money than
those who are not asked to sign
(Gueguen & Jacob, 2001). In another study, researchers found
that young women were more
likely to agree to have coffee with a young man if they were
first asked to give him directions
or light his cigarette (Gueguen, Marchand, Pascual, & Lourel,
2008).
Lowball Technique
Seeing a commercial on a website for a laptop at a low price,
you click on the ad. You are
in the market for a new laptop computer so you know this price
is a good one. You quickly
decide on this computer and, excited about your potential
purchase, begin the ordering
process. Partway through providing your name, address, credit
card information, you
realize that the color you had wanted was going to be $30 extra.
Then you are offered a
lengthened power cord that will be able to reach more outlets,
as opposed to the shorter
one that comes with the laptop, for an additional $45.99. Then
you are offered antivirus
software for only $39. By the time you pay the added shipping
and processing fee, you
have spent almost $150 more on the laptop than you originally
intended.
This is an example of the lowball technique. An initial,
reasonable request is made, but after
the person agrees, additional items are added that make the
overall request less reasonable.
Because individuals have already made a commitment to the
item (e.g., the computer) they
Expand Your Knowledge: Influence by Cialdini
Persuasion researcher Robert Cialdini has spent
decades studying influence. His books are very acces-
sible, and may be particularly interesting if you are
planning to work in business or sales. His books are
titled Influence: Science and Practice, Influence: The
Psychology of Persuasion, and Yes!: 50 Scientifically
Proven Ways to Be Persuasive.
Cialdini’s website: http://www.influenceatwork.com/.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 159 7/16/13 9:50 AM
http://www.influenceatwork.com/
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
are reluctant to walk away. If they had known about all of the
costs initially, however, it is
unlikely they would have agreed (Cialdini, Cacioppo, Bassett, &
Miller, 1978; Gueguen, Pas-
cual, & Dagot, 2002). This tactic also frequently occurs outside
of sales. Someone might ask
you to hang up posters for an upcoming event as a favor. If you
agreed, and then later found
out you also needed to pick up the bulky, heavy posters from a
location 10 minutes away,
and transport them to where you are to hang them up, you might
still do it, even though
you might not have had you known all the details in the first
place (Burger & Petty, 1981).
Unlike foot-in-the-door, with lowball the initial request is the
target request. The initial
request is small, or at least reasonable, and it is only when the
full cost is revealed that
individuals realize the large commitment they have made. With
this technique, the per-
suader needs to be careful to offer the initial product at a price
people will be interested in
and then make the additional requests reasonable enough so that
the agreement continues
as the deal becomes less of a deal. Comparing the foot-in-the-
door and the lowball tech-
nique, researchers have found that the lowball technique seems
to work better at persuad-
ing people to comply than the foot-in-the-door technique (Joule,
1987).
Legitimization-of-Paltry-Favors
A neighbor child comes to your door, collecting money for a
well-known charity. Your
budget is tight and you already gave elsewhere, so you are
about to respond with a
polite no when the child says, “Even a penny will help.” You
have a penny, right in your
pocket, so you can’t really say no to the request. But you do not
feel right about con-
tributing just a penny. Reaching into your wallet, you pull out a
couple of dollars and
hand them over. Congratulations, you have just been persuaded
by the technique called
legitimization-of–paltry-favors.
As the name implies, the legitimization-of-paltry-favors
technique catches us by making
a very small contribution acceptable (Andrews, Carpenter,
Shaw, & Boster, 2008). It is dif-
ficult to refuse when even a very small amount is described as
legitimate. But few would
give a paltry amount even if it is acceptable, so we give more
than just that penny. Notice
that a penny was not suggested, but legitimized. With this
technique, it should be clear
that while a very small amount is okay, it is certainly not
desired. Such a technique tends
to increase the number of people who give while not affecting
the amount each individual
gives (Cialdini & Schroeder, 1976).
Test Yourself
• A teenager asks his mother if he can spend the weekend at a
friend’s house. Later he
asks if he could spend a week with his friend on a road trip.
Which persuasion technique
is this teenager using?
The foot-in-the-door technique. He first asks a relatively small
request and then follows
it with a larger request.
• When using the lowball technique why does the communicator
not just ask for the
entire request upfront?
If the communicator was to ask for what he or she wanted at the
beginning it is more
likely the person would refuse because the overall cost is too
high.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 160 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Test Yourself
• If someone gives you a free sample, are you more likely to
buy the product?
Yes. Because of reciprocity, we feel some pressure to give back
to those who have given
us something.
Reciprocity
Wandering through the supermar-
ket, you come upon a small table
that is offering free samples of gra-
nola bars. You are hungry and the
samples look good, so you take one.
Smiling you thank the person offer-
ing the samples, and you reach for a
box of the bars. Considering that gra-
nola bars were not on your shopping
list and you may not have known
this brand even existed before your
encounter with the sample, why
would you buy this product?
The technique being used here is rec-
iprocity. Reciprocity is considered a
rule of social behavior that appears
to be present in most cultures, if not
all. When others do something for us, we feel obligated to do
something for them (Gould-
ner, 1960; Wright, 1994). Communicators use our natural
tendency toward reciprocity to
get us to do what we otherwise might not. When provided with
something we did not ask
for, we tend to reciprocate, at times giving more in return than
was invested in the original
gift. In one study published in the 1970s, some research
participants were given a 10-cent
soda. Later in the study, when the participants were asked to
buy 25-cent raffle tickets, the
ones who had been given the soda were more likely to buy the
raffle tickets than those
who had not been given a soda (Regan, 1971). In another
example of reciprocity, research-
ers have found that food servers who provide their customers
with mints or candy get
increased tips (Lynn & McCall, 2000). Although reciprocity
will be slightly higher in a
public context, we still tend to reciprocate, even when our
behavior is private (Whatley,
Webster, Smith, & Rhodes, 1999).
Even an offer of reciprocity that is refused is helpful. In a field
study in France, researchers
asked smokers on the street for a cigarette. When the
researchers offered a small amount
of money in exchange the money was usually refused, but the
smokers were more likely
to give up a cigarette (Guéguen & Pascual, 2003). Reciprocity
is so natural for humans
that those who are persuaded do not always recognize
reciprocity as the reason for their
change in attitude or behavior (Cialdini, Green, & Rusch, 1992).
Corbis/SuperStock
After sampling free food at a supermarket, you may feel
inclined to purchase that food in reciprocation of the free
sample. The supermarket has persuaded you to buy via
reciprocity.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 161 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Social Psychology in Depth: Resistance to Persuasion
Persuasive messages are designed to change our minds but we
are not without defense.
In the battle for our wallets and our minds, resistance to
persuasion plays a part. One way
in which we resist persuasive messages is through arguing our
own point of view, called
counterarguing. Hearing a message you do not agree with, you
come up with a number of
arguments why your position is right and the alternate position
is wrong. Such an activity will
have an effect on your initial attitude. When people
counterargue successfully, they become
more certain of their initial attitude, particularly when the other
message comes from an
expert source (Tormala & Petty, 2004a; Tormala & Petty,
2004b). However, when people
believe they have done a poor job at counterarguing, they may
actually become less certain
of their attitudes. The attitudes themselves may not change, but
they are now more vulnera-
ble to future attacks and are less likely to predict behavior
(Tormala, Clarkson, & Petty, 2006).
At times we resist persuasion not because of well-thought-out
arguments but simply
because we are reacting to manipulation. Reactance is our
response to threatened free-
dom; we do the opposite of what someone wants us to do
because we want to reassert our
right to make our own decisions. If a high-pressure salesperson
is advocating a particular
product, we may choose another because we want to assert our
freedom to make our
own choices. We generally think of reactance as a simple knee-
jerk reaction to a threat to
freedom, but reactance can also lead us to the kind of
counterarguing described in the first
paragraph (Silvia, 2006).
While we can and do resist persuasive messages, our view of
our vulnerability to persuasion
is distorted. We believe we are less vulnerable to persuasion
than others (Douglas & Sutton,
2004). While we tend to judge the vulnerability of others
accurately, we judge ourselves as
being less vulnerable to persuasion than we really are. When
our attitudes do change, we
underestimate the degree of change (Bem & McConnell, 1970;
Markus, 1986). For example,
if a salesperson convinced us to buy a product, we might say we
were leaning toward that
choice already even if we were not.
When it comes to resistance to persuasion, our vulnerability is
higher when we have exerted
self-control. Previous experiences that have resulted in ego
depletion, such as controlling
our emotions, making decisions, or engaging in an undesired
task, make us more vulnerable
to persuasive messages. With depleted resources we are less
able to counterargue (Burkley,
2008; Wheeler, Brinol, & Hermann, 2007). The reverse also
appears to be true: Engaging
in resistance to persuasion makes us less able to engage in tasks
requiring our self-control
(Burkley, 2008).
Door-in-the-Face Technique
As you watch an infomercial for exercise equipment, the
advertiser shows four different
machines and the price for each. As you contemplate the
expense of such equipment, you
quickly reject the idea of ever owning exercise equipment. A
request to buy four different
machines is too much. But wait, the advertiser says, you can
afford exercise equipment.
The advertiser then displays a machine that will do everything
the first four machines can
and is small and relatively inexpensive. Relieved that there is a
product you can afford,
you pick up the phone to order one of your own.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 162 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Unlike foot-in-the-door, where a small request is followed by a
larger request, the target
request, the door-in-the-face technique begins with a large
request. When the message
recipient says no to the first request, the persuader follows with
a second, more reasonable
request, the actual target of the communication. In the original
study on the technique,
college students were asked if they would be willing to counsel
juvenile delinquents for
2 hours a week for 2 years. The students declined. Then, those
students and others were
asked if they would be willing to take the juveniles to the zoo
for a day. Of the students
who had not been previously asked to make a 2-year
commitment, about 17% agreed to
chaperone the kids. Of the students who had been asked and
refused the 2-year commit-
ment, 50% agreed to the chaperoning (Cialdini et al., 1975).
One explanation for why this technique works has to do with the
discomfort we feel when
we refuse someone’s request. In the second request the
persuader makes a concession, so
we feel as though we should make a reciprocal concession
(Turner, Tamborini, Limon, &
Zuckerman-Hyman, 2007). Wanting the students’ help with
juveniles for 2 years, the per-
suader appears to be making a concession with just a day at the
zoo. Wanting to do noth-
ing, the students make a concession from nothing to a day at the
zoo. Further research has
found that emphasizing the concession made by the person
making the request increases
persuasion or feelings of obligation (Abrahams & Bell, 1994;
Ebster & Neumayr, 2008).
Perhaps because of this, compliance is highest when requests
are close to one another in
time (ideally within the same interaction), and are made by the
same individual (Cann,
Sherman, & Elkes, 1975; Guéguen, Jacob, & Meineri, 2011;
O’Keefe & Hale, 2001). It seems
the person needs to see a clear connection between the first
request and the second request
in order to feel a need for reciprocal concession.
As with the foot-in-the-door technique, there needs to be a
balance between the levels of
demand of the requests. In the initial study on the technique, the
first request was a very
demanding request that everyone was expected to refuse
(counseling juveniles 2 hours per
week for 2 years). When moderate initial requests are used,
compliance actually declines
in some studies (Even-Chen, Yinon, & Bizman, 1978). In a
meta-analysis of studies on the
technique, researchers found that the technique works better in
gaining verbal agreement
rather than persuading people to behave differently (Feeley,
Anker, & Aloe, 2012).
Studies comparing the amount of persuasion found for the door-
in-the-face technique in
contrast with the foot-in-the-door technique find that the door-
in-the-face technique tends
to be more effective (Pascual & Guéguen, 2005; Rodafinos,
Vucevic, & Sideridis, 2005). For
example, in one study researchers were able to get children to
do more academic work by
using the door-in-the-face technique than they got when they
used the foot-in-the-door
technique, or simply asking the children to do the work (Chan &
Au, 2011). In another
study, when comparing the foot-in-the-door, door-in-the-face,
and the lowball technique,
the door-in-the-face technique once again came out on top. The
door-in-the-face technique
resulted in the most donated money in comparison with the
other two techniques (Wang,
Brownstein, & Katzey, 1989).
That’s-not-all Technique
On late-night television you might find an infomercial that
explains an exercise machine
and the wonderful things it does. After offering the product and
telling you the price,
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 163 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
the announcer says, “and that’s not all” and offers some deluxe
exercise towels that you
will get with your purchase. When sellers offer a product and,
before the customer can
respond, offer something else with the sale or offer to lower the
price, they are using the
that’s-not-all technique.
This technique might be effective due to reciprocity. The seller
is seen as willing to negoti-
ate the sale of his or her product, so the buyer should
reciprocate that flexibility by buying.
Another factor may be a concept known as anchoring. The
initial price for the individual
piece is seen as the anchor. When the price is lowered or
additional things are added, the
consumer sees this as a good deal. If originally offered the
entire package at the lower
price, consumers take that as the anchor and do not view it as a
good deal (Burger, 1986).
The that’s-not-all technique seems to work best when the initial
request is reasonable
(Burger, Reed, DeCesare, Rauner, & Rozolis, 1999). This means
that when using the tech-
nique, sellers need to be careful in setting the initial price. A
high initial price can shock
people into considering whether they really need the product or
really need to spend that
much money. High initial prices tend to lead to greater refusal
when the that’s-not-all
technique is used. Adding additional items to the initial offering
may not alleviate that
initial shock (Pollock, Smith, Knowles, & Bruce, 1998).
Scarcity
Often advertisements for products will emphasize the fact that
the item is only available for
a limited time. Deals available on the day after Thanksgiving
are sometimes only available
for a few hours after the store has opened. Why do advertisers
limit the time frame for such
bargains? Overall, people do not like it when their freedom to
act is restricted. When prod-
ucts may not be available for long, we tend to buy those
products so we can access them if
we want. This type of behavior is called reactance. Reactance is
acting in a way that pro-
tects one’s freedom. Although we may have never intended to
buy a certain product in the
first place, if we think it will no longer be available we may
purchase it. Researchers have
found evidence of reactance in a wide variety of circumstances.
When we feel like others are
restricting our freedom in a parking lot by waiting for our
parking space, we may actually
stay in the space longer than if no one was waiting (Ruback &
Juieng, 1997).
Test Yourself
• Why does an initial refusal with the door-in-the-face
technique lead to later persuasion?
Because the initial request is refused and the communicator
makes a concession to a
smaller request, the target of the technique makes a reciprocal
concession, agreeing to
that smaller request.
• How does the that’s-not-all technique work?
With the that’s-not-all technique before potential buyers can
refuse an initial request
additional positive incentives are added.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 164 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Limited-time offers are not the only type of offers for which we
have this reaction. We may
also buy when we believe there are only a few of the product in
existence, in other words,
when the number of items is restricted. Any advertisement that
refers to the product as
being a limited edition or in short supply leads us to a similar
reaction. This reaction may
rely in part on our need for uniqueness (Lynn, 1991). We do not
want to look like or have
the same things as everyone else, so we buy rare products to
express our individuality.
When commercials offer a product for a limited time or describe
the limited number avail-
able, the advertisers are using scarcity.
When products are seen as available for only a limited time, it
is possible that respondents
actually increase their scrutiny of the persuasive message.
Knowing the product may only
be available briefly provides motivation to think about the
merits of the message (Bran-
non & Brock, 2001). Scarcity works particularly well in
romantic contexts (such as when
watching a romantic movie) but can backfire when in the
context of fearful events (such
as when watching a scary movie). Romantic situations remind
us how scarce opportuni-
ties can be; after all, how often does Mr. or Ms. Right come
along? In these situations you
want to stand out from the crowd so you can be noticed by that
potential significant other
or impress the partner you have. When we are thinking of
romance, therefore, a commer-
cial that tells us to get one of the few products available is more
likely to be persuasive
(Griskevicius et al., 2009).
Test Yourself
• The countdown clock on an infomercial for the time a product
is available is relying on
what tendency?
This technique relies on the human tendency to react when
one’s freedom is being
threatened, called reactance. When we think a product may not
be around very much
longer we move to buy it so it can remain available to us.
Pique Technique
Within some contexts, we automatically refuse anyone’s
attempts to persuade us. When
encountering a panhandler, for example, the majority of people
walk on by and do not give
any money. Two techniques involve disrupting that automatic
tendency toward refusal,
making compliance more likely—the pique technique and the
disrupt-then-reframe tech-
nique. The pique technique works by disrupting attention with
an odd request. Rather
than asking passersby if they could spare some change, a
panhandler might ask if the
person could spare 37 cents. Imagine your own response to such
a request. Though you
might have simply kept going the odd request makes you stop
and perhaps ask why the
person wants exactly 37 cents. In research on the technique,
unique requests resulted in an
almost 60% increase in compliance (Santos, Leve, & Pratkanis,
1994).
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 165 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques
Does the reason for wanting 37 cents matter? When a person
asking for 37 cents gave
either a specific reason (“that’s all I need to have enough for
the bus fare”), or a vague
reason (“I need to buy something”), the amount of compliance
with the request did not
change. Although people did give more money when they asked
a question rather than if
the requester just used the pique technique, whether there was a
specific reason given or a
vague reason the average amount of money given was
essentially the same (Burger, Hor-
nisher, Martin, Newman, & Pringle, 2007). It seems that the
actual reason does not matter,
only that the passerby was intrigued enough to ask about the
request.
Disrupt-Then-Reframe Technique
Another technique relying on disrupting the general tendency to
refuse requests is the
disrupt-then-reframe technique. With this technique, a request is
made in an unusual way
and then the requester reframes that request. For example, in
door-to-door sales of Christ-
mas cards, women described that they were selling the cards to
raise money for a school for
the developmentally disabled. If people said they wanted to
know the price of the cards the
woman said “They’re 300 pennies. . . that’s $3. It’s a bargain.”
The odd way of stating the
price, 300 pennies, changed the way people thought about the
request, and then, by stating
such a price is a bargain, the 300-penny price was reframed
(Davis & Knowles, 1999).
One hypothesis for how this works is that the unusual request
interrupts usual thinking
processing so people do not have a ready response. The
reframing then provides them
with an easy response. “It’s a bargain” gives people something
to latch on to while they are
trying to process what 300 pennies really means and whether
300 pennies for a few Christ-
mas cards is a good or a bad deal. Although the technique works
in both requests related
to charity and in sales settings, it is more effective in charity
related contexts (Carpenter &
Boster, 2009; Fennis, Das, & Pruyn, 2006).
Test Yourself
• How does a communicator pique attention using the pique
technique?
With the pique technique the communicator makes an unusual
request, such as asking
for 37 cents, thereby piquing attention.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 166 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7
Table 7.1 Persuasion techniques: Definitions and examples
Technique Definition Example
Foot-in-
the-door
An initial small request is made and
accepted. A large request, the target
request, is then made.
You are asked to sign a petition to
support blood donation. After you sign,
you are asked to donate blood.
Lowball An initial request, the target request, is
made, but only later are the full costs
are revealed.
You are asked to volunteer 20 minutes
of your time. Only later is it revealed
that the time will involve blood
donation, with accompanying needles
and slight pain.
Legitimization-
of-paltry-
favors
Small favors are described as
acceptable, although not desired.
A small donation to support blood
drives, just 25 cents, is acceptable,
although a larger donation would be
appreciated.
Reciprocity A request is made after a gift has been
given.
After receiving a cookie you are asked if
you would be willing to donate blood.
Door-in-
the-face
A large request is made and refused.
Then the target request is made.
You are asked if you could volunteer 2
hours a week for the next year. When
you refuse, you are asked if you could
spend just a half hour now donating
blood.
That’s-not-all A large request is made, but before
the individual can refuse additional
incentives are added.
You are asked to donate blood, but
before you say no you are told you will
get a cookie and a sticker and your
name will be published in the paper.
Scarcity Products or opportunities are
presented as being limited in number
or as only available for a limited time.
The blood bank tells you the special
post-donation cookies are only
available today for the first 20 people
who give blood.
Pique Attention is disrupted by an oddly
framed request.
You are asked if you want to save the
lives of three children today.
Disrupt-then-
reframe
An unusual request is made and then
framed in a positive way.
When asked to give blood you are told
it will only take 3,600 seconds, a very
short time to give.
Conclusion
In evaluating persuasion we need to take into account where the
message comes from,
what the message contains, and the intended audience. All of
these factors interact with
one another, so a particular communicator may be quite
persuasive using a certain type of
message with a certain audience but less persuasive when
conditions change. Persuaders
have a large and varied bag of tricks. Each persuasion technique
can affect behavior or
attitudes when used carefully and correctly.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 167 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Chapter Summary
Chapter Summary
Who—Characteristics of the Persuader
Those who have credibility are most persuasive. Credible
sources convince their audience
they are expert and trustworthy. Attractive sources tend to be
more persuasive. Persua-
sion is also higher with sources we like.
What—Characteristics of the Message
Some amount of fear and guilt can make a message more
persuasive. Whether a per-
suader wants to use a gain-framed message or a loss-framed
message depends on the type
of behavior being encouraged. Two-sided messages are more
persuasive than one-sided
messages, but only if refutations of the opposing arguments are
offered. When a message
is not a strong one it can become more persuasive with time
through the sleeper effect.
To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience
Culture, self-esteem, and age all affect persuasion. The
elaboration likelihood model helps
us sort out when persuasive messages might be persuasive.
When people are attentive
and have the time and energy to think about a message, the
central route can be more
persuasive. When people are distracted or uninterested,
messages that take the peripheral
route are more persuasive.
How—Persuasion Techniques
With the foot-in-the-door technique persuaders make small
requests and then a larger,
target request. Compliance with the larger request is higher
when an initial request is
accepted than if no initial request is made. For the lowball
technique the target request is
made and accepted because it seems reasonable. Only later are
additional costs revealed.
When small favors are legitimized, larger favors may follow
with the legitimization-of-
paltry-favors technique. Small gifts may provide large returns
with reciprocity. When
given a gift we feel compelled to give something in return.
Persuasion following large
requests are hallmarks of the door-in-the-face technique and the
that’s-not-all technique.
When large requests are refused and then a smaller target
request is offered, compliance
with the second request is higher; this is the door-in-the-face
technique. With the that’s-
not-all technique, a large request that might be refused may be
accepted if additional
things are added to it before the target of the request can refuse.
Techniques involving
scarcity stress that products or options may be available in
limited quantities or for a
limited time so that the products will receive special
consideration. Feeling our freedom
threated, we respond by buying the product or taking the option
provided by the per-
suader. Gaining our attention or disrupting our thought
processes and then reframing the
request are used to increase persuasion with the pique technique
and the disrupt-then-
reframe technique.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 168 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Key Terms
central route to persuasion According to
the elaboration likelihood model, the route
taken when individuals have the time
and energy to process a message deeply,
evaluating the strength of the persuasive
arguments.
credibility Believability; a factor in
whether a message is persuasive. Its two
aspects are expertise and trustworthiness.
disrupt-then-reframe technique A per-
suasion technique in which a request is
made in an unusual way and then the
requester positively reframes that request.
door-in-the-face technique Persuasion
technique that involves beginning with a
large request, which is refused, and fol-
lowing with a smaller request, the target
request.
elaboration likelihood model A model
that proposes two routes to persuasion:
the central route, used when individuals
have time and energy to engage with the
message, and the peripheral route, used
when individuals are lacking in time and
energy and rely on other cues to help them
make decisions.
expertise One aspect of credibility, along
with trustworthiness. Expertise is found
in a communicator who appears to have
knowledge and is able to communicate it.
foot-in-the-door technique Persuasion
technique that involves making a small
request followed by a larger request, the
target request.
gain framed A message that focuses on
the benefits of a behavior.
Critical Thinking Questions
1. What other communicator characteristics do you think might
make a communi-
cator more persuasive?
2. Consider the last infomercial you have seen. How did
characteristics of the com-
municator, the message, and your own interest in the product
affect the degree
to which you were persuaded?
3. If you are currently trying to persuade another person or
group of people, how
might you use the content from this chapter to help you?
4. In what circumstances might taking the peripheral route to
persuasion be more
appropriate? When might the central route be more appropriate?
5. Look through a magazine or your junk mail. What persuasion
techniques seem to
be used in those media?
6. Have you noticed use of the persuasion techniques covered in
the chapter while
watching commercials?
7. How might you arm yourself against falling victim to these
techniques?
Key Terms
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 169 7/16/13 9:50 AM
CHAPTER 7Key Terms
legitimization-of-paltry-favors technique
A persuasion technique that involves mak-
ing a very small contribution acceptable.
It is difficult to not give when even a very
small amount is described as legitimate.
loss framed A message that focuses on
what might be lost if a behavior is not done.
lowball technique A persuasion tech-
nique involving an initial, reasonable
request to which additional things are
added that make the overall request less
reasonable. People tend to maintain com-
mitment to the request, despite the fact
they would likely have refused if they had
known all the costs initially.
need for cognition An individual differ-
ence in how much people tend to engage
in and enjoy thinking.
peripheral route to persuasion According
to the elaboration likelihood model, this is
the route taken when people do not have
the time or energy to process a message.
With this route, individuals use other cues
to evaluate the persuasive arguments.
pique technique A persuasion technique
that works by disrupting attention with an
odd request.
reactance The behavior one undertakes
when one’s freedom is threatened, such as
when an item is available for a limited time.
reciprocity The tendency for people to feel
that when others do something for them,
they must reciprocate.
scarcity A persuasion technique where
a product or option appears to only be
available for a limited time or in a limited
amount.
sleeper effect Occurs when a message
becomes more persuasive with time.
stealing thunder Presenting information
that is potentially harmful before the oppo-
site side can in order to take away some
of the shock value that information might
otherwise have.
that’s-not-all technique A persuasion
technique involving making an offer
and, before the customer can respond,
lowering the price or offering additional
products.
transportation The joining of feelings,
attention, and thoughts in the context of a
story. Transportation involves getting lost
in a story.
trustworthy One aspect of credibility,
along with expertise. Describes a commu-
nicator who appears to be sharing accurate
information.
fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 170 7/16/13 9:50 AM
Francois Lyotard, selections from The Postmodern Condition
Link of the text:
https://www.abdn.ac.uk/idav/documents/Lyotard_-
_Postmodern_Condition.pdf
Research topic has to be found from this part of the text:
Education and Its Legitimation through Performativity. (Starts
end of page 47).
Determine what aspect of the text you have selected will be the
focus of this research process and of the potential paper. Once
you have done these things—selected your text and decided
what the basic topic of your research will be—you may move on
to the follow parts for this assignment.

More Related Content

Similar to Dorling Kindersley RFThinkstockLearning Objectives By.docx

Research on communication
Research on communicationResearch on communication
Research on communicationriyankadaga
 
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)riyankadaga
 
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docx
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docxTrust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docx
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docxturveycharlyn
 
Credibility Advantage 2.0
Credibility Advantage 2.0Credibility Advantage 2.0
Credibility Advantage 2.0Pam Holloway
 
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docx
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docxMcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docx
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docxARIV4
 
Module 3& 4 Business Communication
Module 3& 4 Business CommunicationModule 3& 4 Business Communication
Module 3& 4 Business CommunicationDeepak Mehra
 
Communication Process, Principles, and Ethics
Communication Process, Principles, and EthicsCommunication Process, Principles, and Ethics
Communication Process, Principles, and EthicsDaryl Casiano
 
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...Bruce Jeffers
 
The speed of trust
The speed of trustThe speed of trust
The speed of trustkerrigans
 
Presentation on Persuasion
Presentation on PersuasionPresentation on Persuasion
Presentation on PersuasionNii Ako Odoi
 
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. Covey
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. CoveyImportance of Speed of Trust by S. Covey
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. CoveyJigarUndavia2
 
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptx
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptxObtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptx
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptxRussel Carilla
 
Public Relations Theory
Public Relations TheoryPublic Relations Theory
Public Relations TheoryBrett Atwood
 

Similar to Dorling Kindersley RFThinkstockLearning Objectives By.docx (20)

Research on communication
Research on communicationResearch on communication
Research on communication
 
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)
Research on communication(Cross cultural and Corporate)
 
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docx
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docxTrust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docx
Trust, Justice, & EthicsMost Trusted Companies100 Best.docx
 
Communication ppt
Communication pptCommunication ppt
Communication ppt
 
Credibility Advantage 2.0
Credibility Advantage 2.0Credibility Advantage 2.0
Credibility Advantage 2.0
 
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docx
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docxMcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docx
Mcm 380 Persuasive CommunicationProcessing Persuasive Communi.docx
 
Module 3& 4 Business Communication
Module 3& 4 Business CommunicationModule 3& 4 Business Communication
Module 3& 4 Business Communication
 
INFORMATION-1.pptx
INFORMATION-1.pptxINFORMATION-1.pptx
INFORMATION-1.pptx
 
Communication Process, Principles, and Ethics
Communication Process, Principles, and EthicsCommunication Process, Principles, and Ethics
Communication Process, Principles, and Ethics
 
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...
Bruce Jeffers ::: Theoretical Background for Using Celebrity Appeal in Produc...
 
Effective communication
Effective communicationEffective communication
Effective communication
 
The speed of trust
The speed of trustThe speed of trust
The speed of trust
 
Presentation on Persuasion
Presentation on PersuasionPresentation on Persuasion
Presentation on Persuasion
 
Speed of Trust
Speed of TrustSpeed of Trust
Speed of Trust
 
Trust Building
Trust BuildingTrust Building
Trust Building
 
GROUP 1.pptx
GROUP 1.pptxGROUP 1.pptx
GROUP 1.pptx
 
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. Covey
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. CoveyImportance of Speed of Trust by S. Covey
Importance of Speed of Trust by S. Covey
 
Ch 6 summary
Ch 6 summaryCh 6 summary
Ch 6 summary
 
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptx
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptxObtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptx
Obtaining, Providing and Disseminating Information.pptx
 
Public Relations Theory
Public Relations TheoryPublic Relations Theory
Public Relations Theory
 

More from jacksnathalie

OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docx
OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docxOverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docx
OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docx
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docxOverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docx
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docx
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docxOverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docx
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docx
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docxOverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docx
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docx
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docxOverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docx
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docx
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docxOverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docx
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docx
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docxOverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docx
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docx
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docxOverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docx
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docx
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docxOverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docx
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docx
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docxOverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docx
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docx
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docxOverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docx
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docxjacksnathalie
 
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docxOverview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docxjacksnathalie
 
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docx
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docxOverall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docx
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docxjacksnathalie
 
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docx
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docxOverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docx
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docxjacksnathalie
 
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docxOverall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docxjacksnathalie
 
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docxOverall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docxjacksnathalie
 
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all of the assignment req.docx
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all  of the assignment req.docxOverall feedbackYou addressed most all  of the assignment req.docx
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all of the assignment req.docxjacksnathalie
 
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docx
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docxOverall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docx
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docxjacksnathalie
 
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docxOverview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docxjacksnathalie
 
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docx
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docxOver the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docx
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docxjacksnathalie
 

More from jacksnathalie (20)

OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docx
OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docxOverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docx
OverviewThe US is currently undergoing an energy boom largel.docx
 
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docx
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docxOverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docx
OverviewThe United Nations (UN) has hired you as a consultan.docx
 
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docx
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docxOverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docx
OverviewThis project will allow you to write a program to get mo.docx
 
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docx
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docxOverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docx
OverviewThis week, we begin our examination of contemporary resp.docx
 
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docx
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docxOverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docx
OverviewProgress monitoring is a type of formative assessment in.docx
 
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docx
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docxOverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docx
OverviewThe work you do throughout the modules culminates into a.docx
 
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docx
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docxOverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docx
OverviewThis discussion is about organizational design and.docx
 
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docx
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docxOverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docx
OverviewScholarly dissemination is essential for any doctora.docx
 
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docx
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docxOverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docx
OverviewRegardless of whether you own a business or are a s.docx
 
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docx
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docxOverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docx
OverviewImagine you have been hired as a consultant for th.docx
 
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docx
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docxOverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docx
OverviewDevelop a 4–6-page position about a specific health care.docx
 
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docxOverview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 6 discussion board is to exam.docx
 
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docx
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docxOverall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docx
Overall Scenario Always Fresh Foods Inc. is a food distributor w.docx
 
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docx
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docxOverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docx
OverviewCreate a 15-minute oral presentation (3–4 pages) that .docx
 
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docxOverall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with y.docx
 
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docxOverall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docx
Overall CommentsHi Khanh,Overall you made a nice start with.docx
 
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all of the assignment req.docx
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all  of the assignment req.docxOverall feedbackYou addressed most all  of the assignment req.docx
Overall feedbackYou addressed most all of the assignment req.docx
 
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docx
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docxOverall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docx
Overall Comments Overall you made a nice start with your U02a1 .docx
 
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docxOverview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docx
Overview This purpose of the week 12 discussion board is to e.docx
 
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docx
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docxOver the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docx
Over the years, the style and practice of leadership within law .docx
 

Recently uploaded

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxabhijeetpadhi001
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 

Recently uploaded (20)

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 

Dorling Kindersley RFThinkstockLearning Objectives By.docx

  • 1. Dorling Kindersley RF/Thinkstock Learning Objectives By the end of the chapter you should be able to: • Describe the characteristics of communicators that make them more persuasive • Describe what characteristics of a message make it more persuasive • Describe how culture, age, and self-esteem affect persuasion • Differentiate the central route from the peripheral route to persuasion within the elaboration likeli- hood model • Describe the persuasion techniques involving initial small requests Persuasion 7 Chapter Outline 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader • Credibility: Expertise and Trustworthiness • Attractiveness and Likeability 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message
  • 2. • Emotion • Framing • One-Sided and Two-Sided Messages 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience • Culture • Age • Self-Esteem • Elaboration Likelihood Model 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques • Foot-in-the-Door Technique • Lowball Technique • Legitimization-of-Paltry-Favors • Reciprocity • Door-in-the-Face Technique • That’s-not-all Technique • Scarcity • Pique Technique • Disrupt-Then-Reframe Technique Chapter Summary • Describe the use of reciprocity as a persuasion technique • Explain techniques that begin with a large request • Explain persuasion techniques that use scarcity • Describe techniques that involve changing attention fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 141 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the
  • 3. Persuader Every day, other people try to persuade us. Advertisements on television, the Internet, or the radio attempt to persuade us to buy a product. Family members, friends, and employers ask us to do them a favor. Some of these messages we quickly dismiss, but others convince us to buy the extra absorbent paper towels, or bake cupcakes for that fundraiser. Imagine watching an infomercial for an exercise machine. The product is described by an attractive and trim fitness expert to a mildly skeptical person in front of an enthusiastic studio audience. The machine is demonstrated, the positive benefits and ease of use of the machine are touted, and viewers are offered the product at a low, low price. By the end of the infomercial the skeptic is convinced of the machine’s miraculous powers and you find yourself picking up the phone to order one for yourself. What makes such communications persuasive? Social psychology can help us find the answers to these questions by applying the scientific method to different aspects of persuasion—the persuader, the message, and the audience—as well as investigating specific persuasion techniques. Knowing more about persua- sion may allow us to better resist being persuaded in the future. Persuasive communication can be divided into four parts: the communicator, the
  • 4. message, the audience, and the technique (see Figure 7.1). First we will deal with what characteristics of persuaders make people more likely to be persuaded. Next, we will think about characteristics of the message that lead people to change. Then, we will explore what characteristics of the audience can lead them to be persuaded. Finally, we will cover a variety of persuasion techniques advertisers, charities, friends, or relatives might use to persuade. 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader Persuasive messages come from a source. The source may be someone you respect or someone you dislike—someone who knows a lot about a topic or someone who knows little. This someone could be a friend or a stranger. Depending on the char- acteristics of the persuader, people may be easily persuaded or skeptical of the claims of a message. Characteristics that have received a great deal of attention from researchers are credibility, attractiveness, and likeability. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 142 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader Figure 7.1: The who, what, to whom, and how of persuasion The four variables that affect persuasion are the person doing the persuading, the message being transmitted, the audience receiving the message, and the
  • 5. techniques used by the persuader. Credibility: Expertise and Trustworthiness As you watch an infomercial, a central communicator is likely to offer arguments for the product. Whether or not you listen to this person likely depends on how credible you view that person to be. Credibility has two aspects: expertise and trustworthiness (Hov- land, Janis, & Kelley, 1953). A communicator with expertise is one who appears to have knowledge and is able to communicate it. A trustworthy communicator is one we believe is giving us accurate information. The Persuader Important characteristics: • Credibility • Attractiveness and likeability The Message Important characteristics: • Fear and guilt • Framing • One-sided and two-sided messages • Narratives and rational appeals The Techniques Foot-in-the-door, low ball, legitimization-of- paltry-favors, reciprocity, door-in-the-face, that’s-not-all, scarcity, pique, disrupt-then- reframe The Audience Important characteristics: • Culture
  • 6. • Age • Self-esteem • Elaboration Likelihood Model Variables that affect the impact of persuasion fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 143 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader Messages from expert sources are persuasive when the message includes coherent and high quality arguments from within that expert’s field of knowledge (DeBono & Har- nish, 1988; Petty, Cacioppo, & Gold- man, 1981). Messages that come from an expert source but are of low quality are less persuasive than mes- sages coming from someone who is less of an expert, but who has strong arguments (Bohner, Ruder, & Erb, 2002; Tormala, Brinol, & Petty, 2006). However, an excess of confidence can sometimes limit the effectiveness of the persuasion. For example, in court an expert with a moderate level of confidence appears to be most persuasive, perhaps because a very high level of
  • 7. confidence is viewed as arrogant or unrealistic (Cramer, DeCoster, Harris, Fletcher, & Brodsky, 2011). Expert opinion is gener- ally only persuasive within that expert’s domain of expertise. For example, you might believe what fitness experts say about exercise, but not what they say about cake decorat- ing. An exception to this would be involving children, due to their place in society. For example, using children to demonstrate the safety features in a new vehicle may resonate with an adult’s perceived role as protector and nurturer (Pratkanis & Gliner, 2004). At times, we may receive a message and not have the time or energy to think carefully through the argu- ments. In those instances the trustworthiness of the communicator can serve as a cue regarding whether we should trust the message. If we perceive that the commu- nicator is providing us with accurate information, we may not feel it necessary to carefully examine the mes- sage itself. When we perceive the communicator to be less trustworthy, we may care- fully examine the message to determine if we can trust it. A message can, in this instance, still be persuasive if it contains strong arguments. A nonexpert who cites a study by the American Heart Association showing that the exercise
  • 8. equipment improves heart health in 90% of users has a strong argument, despite lack of expertise. An expert who points out that the stainless steel frame of the equipment will match any decor has a weak argument. Strong messages from non-expert sources can be persuasive because people carefully examine the arguments (Priester & Petty, 2003). Such careful examination creates stronger and more long-lasting attitude change. Expand Your Knowledge: Propaganda Governments, political parties, organizations, and indi- viduals have all used persuasive communications in an effort to sway the public. The website Propaganda Critic http://www.propagandacritic.com from the Institute for Propaganda Analysis, offers a look at pro- paganda, including a page on why investigating propa- ganda matters and analysis of a number of propaganda techniques. PNC/Photodisc/Getty Images Lawyers often retain expert witnesses to help strengthen their client’s case in favor of the jury. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 144 7/16/13 9:49 AM http://www.propagandacritic.com CHAPTER 7Section 7.1 Who—Characteristics of the Persuader Attractiveness and Likeability
  • 9. The attractiveness of the communicator is another factor in the persuasiveness of the message. In general, physically attractive communicators are more persuasive than less attractive communicators (Chaiken, 1979; Debevec, Madden, & Kernan, 1986; DeBono & Telesca, 1990). Physically attractive communicators may be more persuasive because they are viewed as more trustworthy than less attractive communicators—research has shown that we stereotype physically attractive people as having other positive qualities such as intelligence and honesty (Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, & Longo, 1991; Langlois, et al., 2000). We also tend to like physically attractive people more and want to interact with them (Reingen & Kernan, 1993). Even when a product is unrelated to attractiveness, we find attractive communicators more persuasive than less attractive communicators (Prax- marer, 2011). Individuals we like are also more persuasive to us. You might buy Girl Scout cookies from the neighbor who you know and like, but you are likely to buy fewer cookies from the Girl Scout across town. For this reason, advertisers have found tricky ways to make it appear that an appeal is coming from someone we know and like. An envelope that appears to be addressed by hand and have a note inside is more likely to be opened and read than one that is clearly mass produced. In one study of this idea, a number of car owners in Dallas were sent an ad for car wax that appeared to have been ripped from a magazine, with a
  • 10. handwritten sticky note attached; if the recipient was named Mary, for example, the note would read “Mary—Try this. It works!—J,” with “J” being the supposed sender. The ad contained a mail-in card for a free sample of the car wax. The ad was sent in an unmarked white envelope, hand-addressed, with a first-class stamp. Another set of car owners were mailed the same ad, but in a typed envelope sent through metered mail, and the ad inside was simply printed on a sheet of paper with no note attached. When the message in the ad was strong and it appeared to come from someone they knew, almost double the number of car owners requested a free sample than if the message appeared to be mass produced (Howard & Kerin, 2004). A note from someone we might know is more persuasive than a message from a faceless company. Should the communicators make their desires to persuade explicit? To maintain credibility and avoid reactance, advertisers generally avoid stating that they are trying to persuade; however, in some contexts such information can be helpful. You would likely attribute selfish motives to the fitness expert who is trying to sell exercise equipment he designed. But your doctor presumably has your best interests in mind, so his or her obviously per- suasive message about exercise may be received well (Campbell & Kirmani, 2000; Eagly, Wood, & Chaiken, 1978). Researchers have found that physically attractive communica- tors do well if they make their desire to persuade explicit. Unattractive communicators
  • 11. Test Yourself • What are the two aspects of credibility? Expertise and trustworthiness are the two aspects of credibility. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 145 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message When making persuasive appeals, communicators must carefully consider the message they want to portray and how they want to share that message. A per-suader might try to scare you so you will adopt a healthy behavior. Should the message be terrifying, or just a little frightening to motivate you? Communicators must also consider whether sharing the other side’s arguments is helpful. If a phone company knows that a rival phone has a feature that its own product does not have, should it make a mention of it in the advertising? As we discover in this section, how a message is con- structed makes all the difference in how persuasive it will be. Emotion Within a persuasive appeal, a communicator might attempt to elicit an emotion. Emotions contain both physiological and cognitive elements. For example, when you are frightened your blood pressure and your heart rate increase. The same bodily state may be inter-
  • 12. preted differently depending on the context. You might feel fear if you are in a dark alley and a stranger approaches. In the context of a thrilling video game, that same racing heart could be interpreted as excitement (Schachter & Singer, 1962). Advertisers may capitalize on this fact to help sell their products. A beer commercial with a lot of beautiful, scantily clad women may excite the men watching it—excitement that could transfer to the brand of beer the company is selling. When a camera follows a car down a curvy road through a majestic mountain landscape, we may feel a bit of awe at the beauty of the scenery, and we may transfer this sense of amazement to the car. Test Yourself • Who is more persuasive, an attractive communicator or an unattractive communicator? An attractive communicator is usually more persuasive. • What types of communicators should make you aware of their intent to persuade you? Attractive persuaders and people we like are persuasive when we know they are trying to persuade us, as well as those who we know have our best interests at heart (doctor). are not very successful in this instance (Messner, Reinhard, & Sporer, 2008). For the less attractive, keeping persuasive intent hidden is a better strategy. Similarly, disliked sources should conceal persuasive intent, while liked sources are more
  • 13. persuasive when they are open about persuasive intent (Reinhard, Messner, & Sporer, 2006). fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 146 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message One way persuaders may use emotion to con- vince us to do something is to draw on the “appropriate” emotions from our culture and social context. We learn from those around us what emotions are appropriate to feel (Averill, 1980). For example, convinced by your peers or the media that your home should be clean and smell like flowers, you buy a scented candle and spend more time cleaning to avoid feeling embar- rassed. Or, having learned from an advertiser that a new car will make you happy, you purchase a new vehicle. Persuaders elicit specific emotions, such as fear, to motivate people to act. If a health educator were trying to convince smokers to quit smoking, would scaring them about the potential consequences of continuing to smoke be effective? Researchers have long been interested in how persuasive mes- sages that induce fear, what might be known as scare tactics, influence persuasion (Hovland et al., 1953, is an early example). The relationship was hypothesized to follow the pattern of an inverted U, with little persuasion at low levels of fear, the greatest persuasion at moderate levels of
  • 14. fear, and low persuasion at high levels of fear (see Figure 7.2). At low fear levels persuasion would be low because there is not enough motivation to change. At a moderate rate of fear, persuasion should be highest; here people would be motivated to make a change but not so scared that they become paralyzed. At high levels of fear, according to this hypothesis, persuasion once again becomes less likely, as people become too frightened to process the information and respond to it. At this level, individuals may become defensive and ignore the content of the message. At a low level of fear, an antismoking message may not moti- vate smokers to change; the consequences may not seem dire enough to make the effort of quitting worth it. At a high level of fear, smokers may dismiss a message or become defen- sive about their habit. When a message induces a moderate level of fear smokers may be able to process the message rather than becoming defensive and be motivated to change. Yet this hypothesis is problematic because the supporting research is inconclusive (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; Janis & Leventhal, 1968). Some degree of fear is motivating, so messages that make it clear that the target is susceptible to the consequences of a threat are most effective (Maloney, Lapinski, & Witte, 2011). However, the most important predictor of behavior when confronted with scare tactics seems to not be the level of fear aroused, but the belief of individuals in their ability to engage in actions that will allow them to avoid the feared consequence (Ruiter, Abraham, & Kok, 2001).
  • 15. Smokers might be exposed to a message that evokes a great deal or just a little fear about lung cancer, but if they do not believe they can quit, thereby avoiding cancer, the level of fear in the appeal does not seem to matter much (Hoeken & Geurts, 2005; Timmers & van der Wijst, 2007; Witte, 1998). Getty Images News/Getty Images A graphic warning label such as this might scare people into rethinking their smoking habits. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 147 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message Some persuasive appeals may use guilt to attempt attitude change. Charities, for example, might describe people who are hungry or sick or homeless in an attempt to persuade someone to give money or time. Guilt appeals can be successful; the more guilty people feel, the greater their intention to donate. However, if recipients are generally skeptical of emotional appeals and believe the communicator is manipula- tive, they will not feel as much guilt and therefore not be as likely to donate (Hibbert, Smith, Davies,
  • 16. & Ireland, 2007). Rather than working to generate certain emotions, advertisers can also appeal to con- sumers by tapping into their current emotional experience. Happy holiday shoppers are likely to be persuaded by a happy message. The sweet and sappy commercials for engage- ment rings will do well with those who are in love and feeling sweet and sappy toward their significant other. A match between the emotional state of the person and the emo- tional overtones of the persuasive message is the most effective (DeSteno, Petty, Rucker, Wegener, & Braverman, 2004). The complexity of the emotion presented in the message also influences persuasion. When people do not have a lot of time or energy to devote to processing a message, their resources are low. Sadness and happiness are relatively simple emotions, and can be quickly pro- cessed by the individual. When the resources of the person are low and they are presented with an ad with simple emotions like happiness or sadness, they are more persuaded. If resources were low and the effectiveness of the message relied on the emotion of anxiety, a relatively complex emotion, the message would be less persuasive. For individuals with more time and energy to process the message, anxiety within a message could be more persuasive than a simple emotion within that message (Lau- Gesk & Meyers-Levy, 2009). To generate an emotional response, persuaders might tell a
  • 17. story. Researchers have found that individuals are persuaded by stories if the stories are good, even when arguments themselves are weak. One element in persuasive stories is something called transpor- tation. In the context of story, transportation is the joining of feelings, attention, and thoughts (Green & Brock, 2000). The listener joins the storyteller on a journey, and, for the time being, focuses on the teller’s thoughts, feelings, and experiences. As a result, the listener comes away with a new or more informed perspective. On the other hand, when a message is shared not in a story but as a rational appeal, only strong arguments lead to attitude change (Escalas, 2007). People presented with personal narratives of a health risk viewed themselves as being more at risk and said they were more likely to get tested than those presented with statistical information on that health risk (deWit, Das, & Vet, 2008). Figure 7.2: The inverted-U model In Hovland’s study, the relationship between fear and persuasion was hypothesized to resemble an inverted U. Highest Level of Persuasion P e rs u a s
  • 18. io n Fear Levels fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 148 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message Framing Messages can vary in whether they emphasize gains or losses. A message that focuses on benefits is described as being gain framed. If you were buying a vehicle, for example, the message that buying this car will keep you safe in a crash with all of its safety features emphasizes the things you would gain. A message that focuses on losses is loss framed. For example, a salesperson might suggest that if you do not buy this car with all of its safety features, your likelihood of experiencing major injury in a crash is increased. By emphasizing what you would lose, the message becomes loss framed. Framing can influ- ence responses to persuasive messages. When dealing with prevention behaviors, such as exercising, gain-framed messages tend to be more effective. When encouraging detection behaviors, such as going to the doc- tor for a cancer screening, loss-framed messages tend to be more effective (Ferrer, Klein, Zajac, Land, & Ling, 2012; Rothman, Bartels, Wlaschin, &
  • 19. Salovey, 2006). When people are faced with a message and experiencing an emotion like fear, loss-framed messages are more persuasive. Fear is an emotion that leads to behavioral inhibition, or movement away from action. Anger and happiness are part of the behavioral approach system, moti- vating movement toward action. When people are angry or happy gain-framed messages are more persuasive (Gerend & Maner, 2011; Yan, Dillard, & Shen, 2012). Gain-framed messages create more positive emotions than loss- framed emotions. How- ever, researchers have also found that, while being in a positive mood due to a gain- framed message leads to persuasion, being in a negative mood as a result of a loss-framed message may motivate people to do something about the threat (Van’T Riet, Ruiter, Werrij, Candel, & De Vries, 2010). Being happy because you realize from the infomercial all you might gain from buying this exercise equipment might persuade you that purchasing the equipment is a good idea. Or, being sad because you realize from the infomercial all the terrible things that could happen if you do not buy the equipment and begin exercising may actually get you to pick up the phone and make an order. When presenting a persuasive message, it is important for the communicator to avoid making the audience defensive. Affirming what others believe and validating their con- cerns can increase message scrutiny and lead to a change in behavior (Correll, Spencer,
  • 20. & Zanna, 2004). When students’ concerns about availability of recycling containers were Test Yourself • What are the key elements to making a fear appeal persuasive? The message needs to make it clear that the person is susceptible to the threat, the threat applies to the person, and that the person can avoid the threat. • Are messages that have happy messages persuasive? Happy messages can be persuasive, particularly if the message and the individual’s mood or resources match. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 149 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message Test Yourself • Is a message encouraging people to quit smoking that emphasizes the benefit of years of healthy living a gain-framed or a loss-framed message? The message is gain framed because it emphasizes benefits or gains rather than losses. • Would a message that suggests people have yearly glaucoma tests be more persuasive if it was gain framed or loss framed?
  • 21. Because the message involves detection behavior, rather than prevention behavior, a loss-framed message would be more persuasive. validated (“we know it’s a long walk but . . .”), and they were encouraged to use the con- tainers that were available, even if it was inconvenient, their recycling behavior increased and lasted longer (Werner, Stoll, Birch, & White, 2002). Reactance, the tendency to reas- sert one’s freedom in the face of demands from others, can be reduced by affirming what that person believes. Acknowledging concerns works by reducing criticism of a persua- sive message, opening the individual up to consideration of the message (Werner, White, Byerly, & Stoll, 2009). Another way to frame messages is by focusing on the long-term or the short-term con- sequences of an action, called temporal framing. A message encouraging colonoscopies might emphasize the peace of mind one would feel knowing that any potentially cancer- ous polyps have been removed. By focusing on current feelings or consequences, such messages use a short-term framing of a message. A colonoscopy message that uses a long- term frame might emphasize how removal of potentially cancerous polyps could provide years of life without colon cancer. When a message is about a health risk and focuses on short-term consequences, the risks of not engaging in the behavior are perceived as being more concrete (Chandran & Menon, 2004). Temporal
  • 22. framing can influence the per- suasiveness of gain-framed messages. College students receiving gain-framed messages that emphasized short-term consequences of drinking showed less drinking than those who received loss-framed messages emphasizing short-term consequences or those that described long-term consequences (Gerend & Cullen, 2008). Some people are more sensi- tive to short-term consequences of their actions and are, therefore, more persuaded by temporal frames that emphasize immediate consequences of actions (Orbell & Hagger, 2006; Orbell & Kyriakaki, 2008). One-Sided and Two-Sided Messages Is presenting both sides of an argument advantageous or does it weaken the message? For example, if a car salesperson wanted to convince someone to buy an electric car, would just presenting the positive features of the car be best, or should the salesperson point out both the positive and negatives of owning an electric car? A communicator must also decide whether to simply describe the alternate position or also refute it. Should the elec- tric-car salesperson simply mention the drawbacks of owning such a vehicle (the fully charged vehicle goes only 100 miles), or also explain why those drawbacks are not of great concern (most trips people take are well under this distance)? fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 150 7/16/13 9:49 AM
  • 23. CHAPTER 7Section 7.2 What—Characteristics of the Message Generally, messages are most persuasive when they are first encountered. Over time the effect of a message gradually declines. For instance, you might be quite motivated to exer- cise after your doctor tells you about the importance of exercise. A few months later, how- ever, you find your gym clothes collecting dust in the corner. However, in some instances messages become more persuasive with time, a phenomenon known as the sleeper effect (Peterson & Thurstone, 1933/1970). For the sleeper effect to occur, individuals need to spend enough time thinking about the message that the message remains in memory (Priester, Wegner, Petty, & Fabrigar, 1999). People may discount a message when they first encounter it, but over time, forget their reasons for doing so, leading to their persuasion (Kumkale & Albarracin, 2004). If an unreliable source tells you a story you may at first discount the story because of the untrustworthiness of the source. After some time, how- ever, you may remember the story but forget who told it to you, making the story more persuasive a month after it was told to you. Some of the power of narratives may be due to the sleeper effect. The impression made by a narrative is retained, and any reasons for discounting it (it was just a fictional story) may be lost with time (Appel & Richter, 2007). Test Yourself • Are one-sided or two-sided messages more persuasive?
  • 24. Two-sided messages are more persuasive if they include refutation of the opposing side. If not, then one-sided messages are more persuasive. For the most part, two-sided messages are more persuasive when they provide a refuta- tion of the option the communicator is arguing against. If both sides are simply offered, with no refutation, one-sided messages are more persuasive (Allen, 1991; Buehl, Alexan- der, Murphy, & Sperl, 2001). In the exercise equipment infomercial, this means that the communicator should discuss the drawbacks of owning one’s own equipment and those drawbacks should be refuted. Owning one’s own equipment, for example, means taking care of it and storing it, but, the fitness expert might argue, this equipment takes almost no care and can easily be stored in the corner of a room. When in court, defense lawyers must decide whether to present potentially incriminating evidence the prosecution might reveal. Presenting information that is potentially harm- ful before the prosecution does can allow the defense to take away some of the shock value that information might otherwise have. The name of this tactic is stealing thun- der. Within the courtroom, stealing thunder increases the credibility of the defense and changes the way jurors think about the incriminating evidence (Dolnik, Case & Williams, 2003; Williams, Bourgeois, & Croyle, 1993). By presenting evidence that might otherwise be condemning, the defense shows that it has done its research
  • 25. and is aware of the cir- cumstances. By presenting the evidence, the defense is then able to take it apart for the jurors, showing them why it is not condemning. This helps sway the opinion of the jurors, who might have otherwise formed a different opinion if the prosecution had framed the evidence. This tactic has been found to be more effective when jurors are trying to make quick decisions or are distracted or overwhelmed by the evidence. When jurors carefully consider the evidence, it has less of an effect (Howard, Brewer, & Williams, 2006). fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 151 7/16/13 9:49 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience The audience of a persuasive message is vitally important. Characteristics of the audi-ence may impact how a message is received. People from different cultures think about and respond to messages differently. Individuals of different ages and levels of self-esteem may also differ in how easily they are persuaded. In this section, we will explore how the characteristics of the audience affect reception of a message. Culture A large-scale difference in audiences for persuasive messages is the cultural background of the audience. In Chapter 3, different ways of viewing the self
  • 26. were discussed. People from Western cultures, like the United States, most often view themselves as indepen- dent and unique individuals; they are part of an independent culture. People from many Asian cultures, such as Korea, have a more interdependent view of themselves; they are part of an interdependent culture. In interdependent cultures, people see themselves as enmeshed within a social context, with the sense of self arising out of social roles and relationships. Ryan McVay/Lifesize/Thinkstock Western values usually emphasize independence and uniqueness, while Asian values often focus more on social roles and ingroups. a b Test Yourself • If your arguments are weak should you use a narrative appeal or a rational appeal? With weak arguments you are better off with a narrative appeal. Stories are persuasive even when the arguments are weak. Rational appeals are not persuasive when argu- ments are weak. • Are messages you initially discount ever persuasive? Evidence of the sleeper effect suggests that over time arguments that you thought
  • 27. about but dismissed can become more persuasive. iStockphoto/Thinkstock fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 152 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience Persuasive messages follow this pattern across cultures. For example, persuasive mes- sages in the United States focus more on uniqueness and individual preferences. In Korea, advertisements and other persuasive messages are more likely to focus on harmony with others, particularly family or other ingroups (Han & Shavitt, 1994). For example, an adver- tisement in Korea might describe a product as promoting family unity or the happiness of others. Recent research has shown that this difference can vary depending on genera- tion within a culture. Affluent Generation X consumers in China were more persuaded than older Chinese consumers by an advertisement for a car that emphasized uniqueness, an independent culture value (Zhang, 2010). These advertisements described the car as allowing one to break away from the pack rather than being a car a family could depend on. With greater exposure to Western values, these young people in China were more per- suaded by advertisers who emphasized values of uniqueness rather than family.
  • 28. Age Between ages of 3 and 5, children develop the ability to understand that what is in their mind is different from what is in the minds of others, a concept called theory of mind. They also come to understand that others can be persuaded. As long as children believe that others think exactly as they do and know what they know, there is no need to per- suade (Slaughter, Peterson, & Moore, 2013). When children learn they can persuade oth- ers they also realize that others may be trying to persuade them. Because of this, children under the age of 5 do not recognize that advertisers are attempting to persuade them (McAlister & Cornwell, 2009). In looking at persuasion for younger, middle-aged, and older adults, a paradox emerges. Younger adults tend to have fluid attitudes that solidify and remain the same through middle and older adulthood. For example, although young adults may dramatically change their political attitudes, once established these attitudes tend to remain the same after young adulthood (Alwin, Cohen, & Newcomb, 1991; Culter & Kaufman, 1975; Kros- nick & Alwin, 1989). However, in a research setting, participants of different ages show approximately equal propensity for persuasion, sometimes even more so for older adults (Wang & Chen, 2006). Why might attitudes remain the same over a lifetime, but show evidence of malleability in a laboratory setting? Over their lifespan, people seem to be equally likely to change
  • 29. their attitudes. What many young adults have that most middle-aged and older adults do not have is a lot of new life experiences. When middle-aged and older adults experience new things, they are as likely to change as younger adults (Tyler & Schuller, 1991). What, then, seems to be driv- ing the greater flexibility in young adulthood is not age but experience. Older adults may be slightly more vulnerable to persuasion in laboratory persuasion tasks. This increased susceptibility can be traced to a decline in processing capacity with age. Older adults do not have quite as much working memory capacity as younger adults, and therefore, tend to be more persuaded by simple cues, like the number of arguments there are for a par- ticular option (Wang & Chen, 2006). fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 153 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience Test Yourself • A message that focuses on family and relationships is more likely to be persuasive in what type of culture? A message emphasizing interdependence is likely to be more persuasive in interdepen- dent cultures like those often found in Asia.
  • 30. • Do older adults and middle-aged adults have the same capacity to be persuaded as younger adults? Yes, though because of more stable life experiences the older groups tend to have more stable attitudes over time. Self-Esteem Individuals’ level of self-esteem can influence how easy it is to persuade them. When it comes to differences among people, William McGuire (1968), an early researcher of per- suasion, proposed that persuasion depended on both receptivity and yielding. Receptivity means that one has the ability and willingness to pay attention and understand a message. Yielding means that the individual changes his or her mind as a result of the message. In order for a message to be considered persuasive, the individual must both be receptive to the message, and yield to it. When evaluating the effect of self-esteem on persuasion, both receptivity and yielding are important (Rhodes & Wood, 1992). Individuals with low self-esteem who do not trust their own opinion are likely to yield to what others say. But those with low self-esteem lack receptivity: they are less likely to pay attention and remember a message. On the other end of the spectrum, receptivity is high among those with high self-esteem. Individ- uals with high self-esteem are likely to pay attention to and remember a message. Those with high self-esteem, however, are confident in their own
  • 31. judgments and therefore, less persuaded by what others say. Those with moderate self-esteem are likely to have the combination of receptivity and yielding that makes persuasion likely. While research find- ings support this claim, there are many variables that interact with self-esteem; given the right circumstances, individuals with high or low self- esteem are more persuadable (Sanaktekin & Sunar, 2008). Test Yourself • How do receptivity and yielding relate to self-esteem? Individuals with high self-esteem tend to be high in receptivity but low in yielding while those with low self-esteem tend to be low in receptivity but high in yielding, leaving those in the middle with moderate receptivity and yielding. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 154 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience Elaboration Likelihood Model One model that brings together persuader, message, and audience variables is the elabo- ration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). According to this model, people differ in their motivation and ability to process a persuasive message (see Figure 7.3). When people are motivated and able to process a message, they will
  • 32. take more time to think about and evaluate the message. “Elaboration” refers to this engagement with the mes- sage. Those high on the spectrum of being willing and able to process a message will use what researchers have named the central route to persuasion. When using the cen- tral route, individuals spend time and energy thinking about a message, evaluating the strength of the persuasive arguments. If messages are strong, people are likely to respond with positive attitude change, leading to behavior that is reflective of that change (Wu & Shaffer, 1987). Stronger attitudes tend to last longer and are less likely to be changed when attacked (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Wu & Shaffer, 1987). But when arguments in a message are weak, those taking this route to persuasion may reject the message. Figure 7.3: The elaboration likelihood model According to the elaboration likelihood model, level of processing depends on motivation and ability to process the message. Based on Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Low level of elaboration (due to depleted self-control, minimal time and energy available) High level of elaboration
  • 33. (due to relevance, time and energy available, and need for cognition) Central route Peripheral route Thoughtful, attentive, and deep processing Quick and superficial processing The strength of the persuasive argument Various persuasive cues Leads to use of the where messages are evaluated via and changes in attitude depend on and changes in attitude depend on where messages are evaluated via Leads to use of the Message fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 155 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience
  • 34. Social Psychology in Depth: Word of Mouth and Persuasion When making decisions about products to buy, restaurants to visit, or movies to see, we often ask our friends and relatives. A friend who raves about a new movie is probably more likely to get you to the theater than an advertisement on television. This passing of infor- mation about products or services through informal social networks is known as “word of mouth.” Traditionally, word of mouth has its strongest impact when a relationship is close and the message is rich or vivid (Brown & Reingen, 1987; Kisielius & Sternthal, 1984; Swee- ney, Soutar, & Mazzarol, 2008). A new frontier for persuasion through word of mouth is the Internet. Websites like Amazon .com offer customer reviews of the products they sell. These reviews, known as “electronic word of mouth” or “e-word” of mouth, can significantly affect popularity and sales (Ye, Law, Gu, & Chen, 2011; Zhang, Ye, Law, & Li, 2010). E-word of mouth is different from traditional word of mouth because many of the comments or reviews one might read about a product or service are from strangers, not friends or relatives. Online shoppers tend to use certain clues to evaluate e-word of mouth. While considering the e-word of mouth for a product, shoppers look at the agreement among evaluations. If all reviewers are in agreement, all positive or all negative, that may sway a shopper (Chiou & Cheng, 2003). If the reviews for a camera you were
  • 35. considering purchasing were uni- formly negative, would you buy the camera? Probably not. One bad review among many, however, will not necessarily scare off a shopper. A set of perfect reviews may be seen as suspicious, too good to be true (Doh & Hwang, 2009). Overall, the greater the ratio of posi- tive to negative reviews, the more positive the attitude of the shopper and the greater the intention to buy the product (Doh & Hwang, 2009). (continued) We tend to elaborate when a message is relevant to us and we have the time and energy to process it (Petty & Brock, 1981; Petty, Wells, & Brock, 1976). For example, if a commercial for exercise equipment were shown during a television show you were watching, you might listen closely if you were thinking about buying such equipment and you had the time to listen. If you had a gym membership and were happy with your exercise routine, you might largely ignore the message. Our elaboration of a message can also depend on how much we feel the need to evaluate aspects of our lives in general. Need for cogni- tion is a term researchers use to describe differences between individuals concerning their desire to engage in, and tendency to enjoy, thinking (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). In a scale to assess need for cognition, research participants are asked about their agreement with statements such as “I really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to problems” or “I would prefer a task that is intellectual, difficult,
  • 36. and important over one that is somewhat important but does not require much thought.” (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982, p.120). Whether or not something is personally relevant, people who are high in need for cognition tend to take the central route to persuasion (Cacioppo, Petty, & Morris, 1983). fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 156 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.3 To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience Prior knowledge of a subject can influence people in a similar way. If you knew a great deal about exercise equipment, you would have an easier time processing the infomercial. In this instance, you would likely perceive a message with strong arguments as more con- vincing and quickly dismiss one with weak arguments or only an attractive communicator to recommend it (Wood & Kallgren, 1988). As with need for cognition, even when a topic is not personally relevant, someone with prior knowledge is still more likely to use the central route to persuasion (Chebat, Charlebois, & Gelinas- Chebat, 2001). Even if you are not presently in the market for exercise equipment, if you stumble across an advertisement for such equipment and have knowledge about equipment, you are likely to pay attention. When people are less motivated or do not have the time or energy to process a message,
  • 37. they use the peripheral route to persuasion. With this route, individuals use other cues to evaluate the persuasive arguments. Some cues include: the number of arguments presented, the supposed credibility of the source, and how many other people seem to be persuaded (Maheswaran & Chaiken, 1991; O’Keefe, 2002; Petty, Caciopppo, & Schumann, 1983). For example, an online shopper using the peripheral route might be persuaded by the number of reviews for a product rather than the quality of those reviews (Sher & Lee, 2009). We are more likely to take the peripheral route to persuasion when our level of self-control is depleted due to prior use (Fennis, Janssen, & Vohs, 2009). For example, if you studied hard for a test, using all of your self-control to keep working through class material, an adver- tisement with an attractive communicator would be more persuasive to you, regardless of the strength of that communicator’s arguments. Recall from Chapter 2 that ego depletion creates a problem for later volitional acts. The peripheral route is used when we do not have the time or energy to consider a persuasive message carefully— when the ego is depleted. The persuasive message itself can influence what route we would take. Personalization in websites is one way online retailers have sought to influence the buying public (Tam & Ho, 2005). Many companies that sell products online now keep track of where consum- ers look on their sites, what they click on, and what they eventually buy. With all of that information, the retailer is able to make suggestions for an
  • 38. individual site visitor or for Social Psychology in Depth: Word of Mouth and Persuasion (continued) Online reviews of products may be evaluated differently depending on the shopper. Online shoppers high in need for cognition were more persuaded by reviews that were under- standable, objective, and supported by relevant facts than by those that were of lower quality. The quality of the review has no impact on persuasion for those with low need for cognition (Lin, Lee, & Horng, 2011). A large number of reviews is more convincing to those low in need for cognition but does not affect persuasibility for those high in need for cogni- tion (Lin et al., 2011). Individuals who are largely unfamiliar with a product are also more swayed by the number of reviews for a product. Expertise of reviewers, not number of reviews, is more important to those who know more (SanJose- Cabezudo, Gutierrez-Arranz, & Gutierrez-Cillan, 2009). The way individuals engage in e-word of mouth may be different in different countries. In a study of discussion boards in the United States and China, Chinese participants were more concerned about the country of origin, as opposed to the quality, of the products they were investigating. Researchers also found greater information seeking but less information pro- vision by the Chinese than the U.S. participants (Fong & Burton, 2008).
  • 39. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 157 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Watching a series of infomercials or a shopping channel, you may have noticed that some phrases keep coming up. Most offers are “for a limited time only.” The shopping channel may show you how many of the products are left and count them down until they are all gone. Some infomercials ask you to send no money now or offer you a full refund if you are not completely satisfied. Others offer the product and then let you know that if you order now you could get a free gift. All of these advertise- ments are based on persuasion techniques researched by social psychologists and others. In this section, we will discuss several persuasive techniques. The foot-in-the-door tech- nique begins with a small request, and then moves to a larger request. With the lowball technique, a request is made and accepted before the full cost is revealed. The legitimiza- tion-of-paltry-favors technique validates small contributions. Later in this section we will look at persuasion techniques involving reciprocity, large initial requests, scarcity, and changing the focus of attention. Foot-in-the-Door Technique If you have ever bought a car, you may have found yourself visiting a lot of dealerships or
  • 40. used-car lots. At a dealership you might be asked to sit in a new car or offered a test drive. Test Yourself • According to the elaboration likelihood model, if you had the time and interest to listen to a message, what type of message would be most persuasive? When you have time and interest in a message you are more likely to use the central route to persuasion. Messages with strong arguments are most persuasive when using the central route. • What is the relationship between need for cognition and the two different routes to persuasion? People who are high in need for cognition tend to take the central route to persuasion. • Are attitudes stronger when someone has been persuaded using the central route or the peripheral route? Messages that persuade using the central route result in strong attitudes that are more resistant to change and more likely to be related to behavior. the general shopper who visits and shows interest in a product. You may have visited a website that, after you purchase or even just click on a few products, offers suggestions of other products you might be interested in; that site is using personalization strategies.
  • 41. Because personalization makes messages relevant to individuals, according to the elabo- ration likelihood model it should lead to greater central route processing, and, when a product is good, potentially more purchasing of that product. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 158 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques In doing all of this the salesperson might say that there is no obligation, but is that really true? With the foot-in-the-door technique, a small request is made and agreed to, fol- lowed by a larger request. The salesperson might first ask if you would like to take a test drive, and only after the test drive ask if you would like to discuss financing options. Because people have said yes to the first request (test drive) they are more likely to say yes to the second request (discussing options and potentially moving to buy the car). The sec- ond request is really the target for the salesperson (Freedman & Fraser, 1966). Depending on the situation, this technique has been known to bring 15% to 25% compliance with the second request (Dillard, 1991). Use of the foot-in-the-door technique requires a delicate balancing act. The first request needs to be small enough to be accepted, but the larger the first request, the more likely
  • 42. individuals will say yes to the second request (Seligman, Bush, & Kirsch, 1976). One explanation for the foot-in- the-door technique’s success is related to self-perception theory. Self-perception theory involves attitude formation; when we want to know what our attitudes are, we look at our behavior. With foot-in-the- door, the same process might be happening as people look to their initial behavior (agreeing to a small request) to determine what they should do for another behavior (agreeing to the larger, target request) (Burger, 1999; Burger & Caldwell, 2003). Foot-in-the-door technique is also applied outside of com- mercial purposes. When individuals visit a website for a charitable organization, those who agree to sign a petition are more likely to donate money than those who are not asked to sign (Gueguen & Jacob, 2001). In another study, researchers found that young women were more likely to agree to have coffee with a young man if they were first asked to give him directions or light his cigarette (Gueguen, Marchand, Pascual, & Lourel, 2008). Lowball Technique Seeing a commercial on a website for a laptop at a low price, you click on the ad. You are in the market for a new laptop computer so you know this price is a good one. You quickly decide on this computer and, excited about your potential purchase, begin the ordering process. Partway through providing your name, address, credit
  • 43. card information, you realize that the color you had wanted was going to be $30 extra. Then you are offered a lengthened power cord that will be able to reach more outlets, as opposed to the shorter one that comes with the laptop, for an additional $45.99. Then you are offered antivirus software for only $39. By the time you pay the added shipping and processing fee, you have spent almost $150 more on the laptop than you originally intended. This is an example of the lowball technique. An initial, reasonable request is made, but after the person agrees, additional items are added that make the overall request less reasonable. Because individuals have already made a commitment to the item (e.g., the computer) they Expand Your Knowledge: Influence by Cialdini Persuasion researcher Robert Cialdini has spent decades studying influence. His books are very acces- sible, and may be particularly interesting if you are planning to work in business or sales. His books are titled Influence: Science and Practice, Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion, and Yes!: 50 Scientifically Proven Ways to Be Persuasive. Cialdini’s website: http://www.influenceatwork.com/. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 159 7/16/13 9:50 AM http://www.influenceatwork.com/
  • 44. CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques are reluctant to walk away. If they had known about all of the costs initially, however, it is unlikely they would have agreed (Cialdini, Cacioppo, Bassett, & Miller, 1978; Gueguen, Pas- cual, & Dagot, 2002). This tactic also frequently occurs outside of sales. Someone might ask you to hang up posters for an upcoming event as a favor. If you agreed, and then later found out you also needed to pick up the bulky, heavy posters from a location 10 minutes away, and transport them to where you are to hang them up, you might still do it, even though you might not have had you known all the details in the first place (Burger & Petty, 1981). Unlike foot-in-the-door, with lowball the initial request is the target request. The initial request is small, or at least reasonable, and it is only when the full cost is revealed that individuals realize the large commitment they have made. With this technique, the per- suader needs to be careful to offer the initial product at a price people will be interested in and then make the additional requests reasonable enough so that the agreement continues as the deal becomes less of a deal. Comparing the foot-in-the- door and the lowball tech- nique, researchers have found that the lowball technique seems to work better at persuad- ing people to comply than the foot-in-the-door technique (Joule, 1987). Legitimization-of-Paltry-Favors A neighbor child comes to your door, collecting money for a
  • 45. well-known charity. Your budget is tight and you already gave elsewhere, so you are about to respond with a polite no when the child says, “Even a penny will help.” You have a penny, right in your pocket, so you can’t really say no to the request. But you do not feel right about con- tributing just a penny. Reaching into your wallet, you pull out a couple of dollars and hand them over. Congratulations, you have just been persuaded by the technique called legitimization-of–paltry-favors. As the name implies, the legitimization-of-paltry-favors technique catches us by making a very small contribution acceptable (Andrews, Carpenter, Shaw, & Boster, 2008). It is dif- ficult to refuse when even a very small amount is described as legitimate. But few would give a paltry amount even if it is acceptable, so we give more than just that penny. Notice that a penny was not suggested, but legitimized. With this technique, it should be clear that while a very small amount is okay, it is certainly not desired. Such a technique tends to increase the number of people who give while not affecting the amount each individual gives (Cialdini & Schroeder, 1976). Test Yourself • A teenager asks his mother if he can spend the weekend at a friend’s house. Later he asks if he could spend a week with his friend on a road trip. Which persuasion technique is this teenager using?
  • 46. The foot-in-the-door technique. He first asks a relatively small request and then follows it with a larger request. • When using the lowball technique why does the communicator not just ask for the entire request upfront? If the communicator was to ask for what he or she wanted at the beginning it is more likely the person would refuse because the overall cost is too high. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 160 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Test Yourself • If someone gives you a free sample, are you more likely to buy the product? Yes. Because of reciprocity, we feel some pressure to give back to those who have given us something. Reciprocity Wandering through the supermar- ket, you come upon a small table that is offering free samples of gra- nola bars. You are hungry and the samples look good, so you take one. Smiling you thank the person offer-
  • 47. ing the samples, and you reach for a box of the bars. Considering that gra- nola bars were not on your shopping list and you may not have known this brand even existed before your encounter with the sample, why would you buy this product? The technique being used here is rec- iprocity. Reciprocity is considered a rule of social behavior that appears to be present in most cultures, if not all. When others do something for us, we feel obligated to do something for them (Gould- ner, 1960; Wright, 1994). Communicators use our natural tendency toward reciprocity to get us to do what we otherwise might not. When provided with something we did not ask for, we tend to reciprocate, at times giving more in return than was invested in the original gift. In one study published in the 1970s, some research participants were given a 10-cent soda. Later in the study, when the participants were asked to buy 25-cent raffle tickets, the ones who had been given the soda were more likely to buy the raffle tickets than those who had not been given a soda (Regan, 1971). In another example of reciprocity, research- ers have found that food servers who provide their customers with mints or candy get increased tips (Lynn & McCall, 2000). Although reciprocity will be slightly higher in a public context, we still tend to reciprocate, even when our behavior is private (Whatley, Webster, Smith, & Rhodes, 1999).
  • 48. Even an offer of reciprocity that is refused is helpful. In a field study in France, researchers asked smokers on the street for a cigarette. When the researchers offered a small amount of money in exchange the money was usually refused, but the smokers were more likely to give up a cigarette (Guéguen & Pascual, 2003). Reciprocity is so natural for humans that those who are persuaded do not always recognize reciprocity as the reason for their change in attitude or behavior (Cialdini, Green, & Rusch, 1992). Corbis/SuperStock After sampling free food at a supermarket, you may feel inclined to purchase that food in reciprocation of the free sample. The supermarket has persuaded you to buy via reciprocity. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 161 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Social Psychology in Depth: Resistance to Persuasion Persuasive messages are designed to change our minds but we are not without defense. In the battle for our wallets and our minds, resistance to persuasion plays a part. One way in which we resist persuasive messages is through arguing our own point of view, called counterarguing. Hearing a message you do not agree with, you come up with a number of arguments why your position is right and the alternate position
  • 49. is wrong. Such an activity will have an effect on your initial attitude. When people counterargue successfully, they become more certain of their initial attitude, particularly when the other message comes from an expert source (Tormala & Petty, 2004a; Tormala & Petty, 2004b). However, when people believe they have done a poor job at counterarguing, they may actually become less certain of their attitudes. The attitudes themselves may not change, but they are now more vulnera- ble to future attacks and are less likely to predict behavior (Tormala, Clarkson, & Petty, 2006). At times we resist persuasion not because of well-thought-out arguments but simply because we are reacting to manipulation. Reactance is our response to threatened free- dom; we do the opposite of what someone wants us to do because we want to reassert our right to make our own decisions. If a high-pressure salesperson is advocating a particular product, we may choose another because we want to assert our freedom to make our own choices. We generally think of reactance as a simple knee- jerk reaction to a threat to freedom, but reactance can also lead us to the kind of counterarguing described in the first paragraph (Silvia, 2006). While we can and do resist persuasive messages, our view of our vulnerability to persuasion is distorted. We believe we are less vulnerable to persuasion than others (Douglas & Sutton, 2004). While we tend to judge the vulnerability of others accurately, we judge ourselves as
  • 50. being less vulnerable to persuasion than we really are. When our attitudes do change, we underestimate the degree of change (Bem & McConnell, 1970; Markus, 1986). For example, if a salesperson convinced us to buy a product, we might say we were leaning toward that choice already even if we were not. When it comes to resistance to persuasion, our vulnerability is higher when we have exerted self-control. Previous experiences that have resulted in ego depletion, such as controlling our emotions, making decisions, or engaging in an undesired task, make us more vulnerable to persuasive messages. With depleted resources we are less able to counterargue (Burkley, 2008; Wheeler, Brinol, & Hermann, 2007). The reverse also appears to be true: Engaging in resistance to persuasion makes us less able to engage in tasks requiring our self-control (Burkley, 2008). Door-in-the-Face Technique As you watch an infomercial for exercise equipment, the advertiser shows four different machines and the price for each. As you contemplate the expense of such equipment, you quickly reject the idea of ever owning exercise equipment. A request to buy four different machines is too much. But wait, the advertiser says, you can afford exercise equipment. The advertiser then displays a machine that will do everything the first four machines can and is small and relatively inexpensive. Relieved that there is a product you can afford, you pick up the phone to order one of your own.
  • 51. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 162 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Unlike foot-in-the-door, where a small request is followed by a larger request, the target request, the door-in-the-face technique begins with a large request. When the message recipient says no to the first request, the persuader follows with a second, more reasonable request, the actual target of the communication. In the original study on the technique, college students were asked if they would be willing to counsel juvenile delinquents for 2 hours a week for 2 years. The students declined. Then, those students and others were asked if they would be willing to take the juveniles to the zoo for a day. Of the students who had not been previously asked to make a 2-year commitment, about 17% agreed to chaperone the kids. Of the students who had been asked and refused the 2-year commit- ment, 50% agreed to the chaperoning (Cialdini et al., 1975). One explanation for why this technique works has to do with the discomfort we feel when we refuse someone’s request. In the second request the persuader makes a concession, so we feel as though we should make a reciprocal concession (Turner, Tamborini, Limon, & Zuckerman-Hyman, 2007). Wanting the students’ help with juveniles for 2 years, the per- suader appears to be making a concession with just a day at the
  • 52. zoo. Wanting to do noth- ing, the students make a concession from nothing to a day at the zoo. Further research has found that emphasizing the concession made by the person making the request increases persuasion or feelings of obligation (Abrahams & Bell, 1994; Ebster & Neumayr, 2008). Perhaps because of this, compliance is highest when requests are close to one another in time (ideally within the same interaction), and are made by the same individual (Cann, Sherman, & Elkes, 1975; Guéguen, Jacob, & Meineri, 2011; O’Keefe & Hale, 2001). It seems the person needs to see a clear connection between the first request and the second request in order to feel a need for reciprocal concession. As with the foot-in-the-door technique, there needs to be a balance between the levels of demand of the requests. In the initial study on the technique, the first request was a very demanding request that everyone was expected to refuse (counseling juveniles 2 hours per week for 2 years). When moderate initial requests are used, compliance actually declines in some studies (Even-Chen, Yinon, & Bizman, 1978). In a meta-analysis of studies on the technique, researchers found that the technique works better in gaining verbal agreement rather than persuading people to behave differently (Feeley, Anker, & Aloe, 2012). Studies comparing the amount of persuasion found for the door- in-the-face technique in contrast with the foot-in-the-door technique find that the door- in-the-face technique tends
  • 53. to be more effective (Pascual & Guéguen, 2005; Rodafinos, Vucevic, & Sideridis, 2005). For example, in one study researchers were able to get children to do more academic work by using the door-in-the-face technique than they got when they used the foot-in-the-door technique, or simply asking the children to do the work (Chan & Au, 2011). In another study, when comparing the foot-in-the-door, door-in-the-face, and the lowball technique, the door-in-the-face technique once again came out on top. The door-in-the-face technique resulted in the most donated money in comparison with the other two techniques (Wang, Brownstein, & Katzey, 1989). That’s-not-all Technique On late-night television you might find an infomercial that explains an exercise machine and the wonderful things it does. After offering the product and telling you the price, fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 163 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques the announcer says, “and that’s not all” and offers some deluxe exercise towels that you will get with your purchase. When sellers offer a product and, before the customer can respond, offer something else with the sale or offer to lower the price, they are using the that’s-not-all technique.
  • 54. This technique might be effective due to reciprocity. The seller is seen as willing to negoti- ate the sale of his or her product, so the buyer should reciprocate that flexibility by buying. Another factor may be a concept known as anchoring. The initial price for the individual piece is seen as the anchor. When the price is lowered or additional things are added, the consumer sees this as a good deal. If originally offered the entire package at the lower price, consumers take that as the anchor and do not view it as a good deal (Burger, 1986). The that’s-not-all technique seems to work best when the initial request is reasonable (Burger, Reed, DeCesare, Rauner, & Rozolis, 1999). This means that when using the tech- nique, sellers need to be careful in setting the initial price. A high initial price can shock people into considering whether they really need the product or really need to spend that much money. High initial prices tend to lead to greater refusal when the that’s-not-all technique is used. Adding additional items to the initial offering may not alleviate that initial shock (Pollock, Smith, Knowles, & Bruce, 1998). Scarcity Often advertisements for products will emphasize the fact that the item is only available for a limited time. Deals available on the day after Thanksgiving are sometimes only available for a few hours after the store has opened. Why do advertisers limit the time frame for such bargains? Overall, people do not like it when their freedom to act is restricted. When prod-
  • 55. ucts may not be available for long, we tend to buy those products so we can access them if we want. This type of behavior is called reactance. Reactance is acting in a way that pro- tects one’s freedom. Although we may have never intended to buy a certain product in the first place, if we think it will no longer be available we may purchase it. Researchers have found evidence of reactance in a wide variety of circumstances. When we feel like others are restricting our freedom in a parking lot by waiting for our parking space, we may actually stay in the space longer than if no one was waiting (Ruback & Juieng, 1997). Test Yourself • Why does an initial refusal with the door-in-the-face technique lead to later persuasion? Because the initial request is refused and the communicator makes a concession to a smaller request, the target of the technique makes a reciprocal concession, agreeing to that smaller request. • How does the that’s-not-all technique work? With the that’s-not-all technique before potential buyers can refuse an initial request additional positive incentives are added. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 164 7/16/13 9:50 AM
  • 56. CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Limited-time offers are not the only type of offers for which we have this reaction. We may also buy when we believe there are only a few of the product in existence, in other words, when the number of items is restricted. Any advertisement that refers to the product as being a limited edition or in short supply leads us to a similar reaction. This reaction may rely in part on our need for uniqueness (Lynn, 1991). We do not want to look like or have the same things as everyone else, so we buy rare products to express our individuality. When commercials offer a product for a limited time or describe the limited number avail- able, the advertisers are using scarcity. When products are seen as available for only a limited time, it is possible that respondents actually increase their scrutiny of the persuasive message. Knowing the product may only be available briefly provides motivation to think about the merits of the message (Bran- non & Brock, 2001). Scarcity works particularly well in romantic contexts (such as when watching a romantic movie) but can backfire when in the context of fearful events (such as when watching a scary movie). Romantic situations remind us how scarce opportuni- ties can be; after all, how often does Mr. or Ms. Right come along? In these situations you want to stand out from the crowd so you can be noticed by that potential significant other or impress the partner you have. When we are thinking of romance, therefore, a commer-
  • 57. cial that tells us to get one of the few products available is more likely to be persuasive (Griskevicius et al., 2009). Test Yourself • The countdown clock on an infomercial for the time a product is available is relying on what tendency? This technique relies on the human tendency to react when one’s freedom is being threatened, called reactance. When we think a product may not be around very much longer we move to buy it so it can remain available to us. Pique Technique Within some contexts, we automatically refuse anyone’s attempts to persuade us. When encountering a panhandler, for example, the majority of people walk on by and do not give any money. Two techniques involve disrupting that automatic tendency toward refusal, making compliance more likely—the pique technique and the disrupt-then-reframe tech- nique. The pique technique works by disrupting attention with an odd request. Rather than asking passersby if they could spare some change, a panhandler might ask if the person could spare 37 cents. Imagine your own response to such a request. Though you might have simply kept going the odd request makes you stop and perhaps ask why the person wants exactly 37 cents. In research on the technique, unique requests resulted in an almost 60% increase in compliance (Santos, Leve, & Pratkanis,
  • 58. 1994). fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 165 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Section 7.4 How—Persuasion Techniques Does the reason for wanting 37 cents matter? When a person asking for 37 cents gave either a specific reason (“that’s all I need to have enough for the bus fare”), or a vague reason (“I need to buy something”), the amount of compliance with the request did not change. Although people did give more money when they asked a question rather than if the requester just used the pique technique, whether there was a specific reason given or a vague reason the average amount of money given was essentially the same (Burger, Hor- nisher, Martin, Newman, & Pringle, 2007). It seems that the actual reason does not matter, only that the passerby was intrigued enough to ask about the request. Disrupt-Then-Reframe Technique Another technique relying on disrupting the general tendency to refuse requests is the disrupt-then-reframe technique. With this technique, a request is made in an unusual way and then the requester reframes that request. For example, in door-to-door sales of Christ- mas cards, women described that they were selling the cards to raise money for a school for the developmentally disabled. If people said they wanted to know the price of the cards the
  • 59. woman said “They’re 300 pennies. . . that’s $3. It’s a bargain.” The odd way of stating the price, 300 pennies, changed the way people thought about the request, and then, by stating such a price is a bargain, the 300-penny price was reframed (Davis & Knowles, 1999). One hypothesis for how this works is that the unusual request interrupts usual thinking processing so people do not have a ready response. The reframing then provides them with an easy response. “It’s a bargain” gives people something to latch on to while they are trying to process what 300 pennies really means and whether 300 pennies for a few Christ- mas cards is a good or a bad deal. Although the technique works in both requests related to charity and in sales settings, it is more effective in charity related contexts (Carpenter & Boster, 2009; Fennis, Das, & Pruyn, 2006). Test Yourself • How does a communicator pique attention using the pique technique? With the pique technique the communicator makes an unusual request, such as asking for 37 cents, thereby piquing attention. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 166 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7
  • 60. Table 7.1 Persuasion techniques: Definitions and examples Technique Definition Example Foot-in- the-door An initial small request is made and accepted. A large request, the target request, is then made. You are asked to sign a petition to support blood donation. After you sign, you are asked to donate blood. Lowball An initial request, the target request, is made, but only later are the full costs are revealed. You are asked to volunteer 20 minutes of your time. Only later is it revealed that the time will involve blood donation, with accompanying needles and slight pain. Legitimization- of-paltry- favors Small favors are described as acceptable, although not desired. A small donation to support blood drives, just 25 cents, is acceptable, although a larger donation would be appreciated.
  • 61. Reciprocity A request is made after a gift has been given. After receiving a cookie you are asked if you would be willing to donate blood. Door-in- the-face A large request is made and refused. Then the target request is made. You are asked if you could volunteer 2 hours a week for the next year. When you refuse, you are asked if you could spend just a half hour now donating blood. That’s-not-all A large request is made, but before the individual can refuse additional incentives are added. You are asked to donate blood, but before you say no you are told you will get a cookie and a sticker and your name will be published in the paper. Scarcity Products or opportunities are presented as being limited in number or as only available for a limited time. The blood bank tells you the special post-donation cookies are only available today for the first 20 people who give blood.
  • 62. Pique Attention is disrupted by an oddly framed request. You are asked if you want to save the lives of three children today. Disrupt-then- reframe An unusual request is made and then framed in a positive way. When asked to give blood you are told it will only take 3,600 seconds, a very short time to give. Conclusion In evaluating persuasion we need to take into account where the message comes from, what the message contains, and the intended audience. All of these factors interact with one another, so a particular communicator may be quite persuasive using a certain type of message with a certain audience but less persuasive when conditions change. Persuaders have a large and varied bag of tricks. Each persuasion technique can affect behavior or attitudes when used carefully and correctly. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 167 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Chapter Summary
  • 63. Chapter Summary Who—Characteristics of the Persuader Those who have credibility are most persuasive. Credible sources convince their audience they are expert and trustworthy. Attractive sources tend to be more persuasive. Persua- sion is also higher with sources we like. What—Characteristics of the Message Some amount of fear and guilt can make a message more persuasive. Whether a per- suader wants to use a gain-framed message or a loss-framed message depends on the type of behavior being encouraged. Two-sided messages are more persuasive than one-sided messages, but only if refutations of the opposing arguments are offered. When a message is not a strong one it can become more persuasive with time through the sleeper effect. To Whom—Characteristics of the Audience Culture, self-esteem, and age all affect persuasion. The elaboration likelihood model helps us sort out when persuasive messages might be persuasive. When people are attentive and have the time and energy to think about a message, the central route can be more persuasive. When people are distracted or uninterested, messages that take the peripheral route are more persuasive. How—Persuasion Techniques With the foot-in-the-door technique persuaders make small requests and then a larger, target request. Compliance with the larger request is higher
  • 64. when an initial request is accepted than if no initial request is made. For the lowball technique the target request is made and accepted because it seems reasonable. Only later are additional costs revealed. When small favors are legitimized, larger favors may follow with the legitimization-of- paltry-favors technique. Small gifts may provide large returns with reciprocity. When given a gift we feel compelled to give something in return. Persuasion following large requests are hallmarks of the door-in-the-face technique and the that’s-not-all technique. When large requests are refused and then a smaller target request is offered, compliance with the second request is higher; this is the door-in-the-face technique. With the that’s- not-all technique, a large request that might be refused may be accepted if additional things are added to it before the target of the request can refuse. Techniques involving scarcity stress that products or options may be available in limited quantities or for a limited time so that the products will receive special consideration. Feeling our freedom threated, we respond by buying the product or taking the option provided by the per- suader. Gaining our attention or disrupting our thought processes and then reframing the request are used to increase persuasion with the pique technique and the disrupt-then- reframe technique. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 168 7/16/13 9:50 AM
  • 65. CHAPTER 7Key Terms central route to persuasion According to the elaboration likelihood model, the route taken when individuals have the time and energy to process a message deeply, evaluating the strength of the persuasive arguments. credibility Believability; a factor in whether a message is persuasive. Its two aspects are expertise and trustworthiness. disrupt-then-reframe technique A per- suasion technique in which a request is made in an unusual way and then the requester positively reframes that request. door-in-the-face technique Persuasion technique that involves beginning with a large request, which is refused, and fol- lowing with a smaller request, the target request. elaboration likelihood model A model that proposes two routes to persuasion: the central route, used when individuals have time and energy to engage with the message, and the peripheral route, used when individuals are lacking in time and energy and rely on other cues to help them make decisions. expertise One aspect of credibility, along with trustworthiness. Expertise is found
  • 66. in a communicator who appears to have knowledge and is able to communicate it. foot-in-the-door technique Persuasion technique that involves making a small request followed by a larger request, the target request. gain framed A message that focuses on the benefits of a behavior. Critical Thinking Questions 1. What other communicator characteristics do you think might make a communi- cator more persuasive? 2. Consider the last infomercial you have seen. How did characteristics of the com- municator, the message, and your own interest in the product affect the degree to which you were persuaded? 3. If you are currently trying to persuade another person or group of people, how might you use the content from this chapter to help you? 4. In what circumstances might taking the peripheral route to persuasion be more appropriate? When might the central route be more appropriate? 5. Look through a magazine or your junk mail. What persuasion techniques seem to be used in those media? 6. Have you noticed use of the persuasion techniques covered in
  • 67. the chapter while watching commercials? 7. How might you arm yourself against falling victim to these techniques? Key Terms fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 169 7/16/13 9:50 AM CHAPTER 7Key Terms legitimization-of-paltry-favors technique A persuasion technique that involves mak- ing a very small contribution acceptable. It is difficult to not give when even a very small amount is described as legitimate. loss framed A message that focuses on what might be lost if a behavior is not done. lowball technique A persuasion tech- nique involving an initial, reasonable request to which additional things are added that make the overall request less reasonable. People tend to maintain com- mitment to the request, despite the fact they would likely have refused if they had known all the costs initially. need for cognition An individual differ- ence in how much people tend to engage in and enjoy thinking.
  • 68. peripheral route to persuasion According to the elaboration likelihood model, this is the route taken when people do not have the time or energy to process a message. With this route, individuals use other cues to evaluate the persuasive arguments. pique technique A persuasion technique that works by disrupting attention with an odd request. reactance The behavior one undertakes when one’s freedom is threatened, such as when an item is available for a limited time. reciprocity The tendency for people to feel that when others do something for them, they must reciprocate. scarcity A persuasion technique where a product or option appears to only be available for a limited time or in a limited amount. sleeper effect Occurs when a message becomes more persuasive with time. stealing thunder Presenting information that is potentially harmful before the oppo- site side can in order to take away some of the shock value that information might otherwise have. that’s-not-all technique A persuasion technique involving making an offer and, before the customer can respond,
  • 69. lowering the price or offering additional products. transportation The joining of feelings, attention, and thoughts in the context of a story. Transportation involves getting lost in a story. trustworthy One aspect of credibility, along with expertise. Describes a commu- nicator who appears to be sharing accurate information. fee85798_07_c07_141-170.indd 170 7/16/13 9:50 AM Francois Lyotard, selections from The Postmodern Condition Link of the text: https://www.abdn.ac.uk/idav/documents/Lyotard_- _Postmodern_Condition.pdf Research topic has to be found from this part of the text: Education and Its Legitimation through Performativity. (Starts end of page 47). Determine what aspect of the text you have selected will be the focus of this research process and of the potential paper. Once you have done these things—selected your text and decided what the basic topic of your research will be—you may move on to the follow parts for this assignment.