SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 233
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 1
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS
AND ITS CONSEQUENCES FOR
RECIDIVISM*
TED CHIRICOS
KELLE BARRICK
WILLIAM BALES
College of Criminology and Criminal Justice
Florida State University
STEPHANIE BONTRAGER
Justice Research Associates
KEYWORDS: labeling, felony conviction, recidivism
Florida law allows judges to withhold adjudication of guilt for
individ-
uals who have been found guilty of a felony and are being
sentenced to
probation. Such individuals lose no civil rights and may
lawfully assert
they had not been convicted of a felony. Labeling theory would
predict
that the receipt of a felony label could increase the likelihood of
recidi-
vism. Reconviction data for 95,919 men and women who were
either
adjudicated or had adjudication withheld show that those
formally
labeled are significantly more likely to recidivate in 2 years
than those
who are not. Labeling effects are stronger for women, whites,
and those
who reach the age of 30 years without a prior conviction.
Second-level
indicators of county characteristics (e.g., crime rates or
concentrated
disadvantage) have no significant effect on the
adjudication/recidivism
relationship.
Traditional labeling theory explains the potential “escalating”
conse-
quences of a criminal or delinquent labeling experience in two
ways (Lof-
land, 1969; Sherman et al., 1992). The first consequence
involves a
* The authors would like to thank the anonymous reviewers as
well as Carter Hay,
Dan Mears, Brian Stults, and especially Xia Wang for their
helpful comments on
an earlier version of this article. Direct correspondence to Ted
Chiricos (e-mail:
[email protected]).
CRIMINOLOGY VOLUME 45 NUMBER 3 2007 547
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 2
22-AUG-07 10:10
548 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
transformation of identity,1 and the second emphasizes
structural impedi-
ments to conventional life that result from a labeling event.2
Although
labeling events have been variably operationalized to include
police con-
tact, arrest, conviction, and imprisonment, it is arguable that
felony convic-
tion is the most consequential in relation to the development of
structural
impediments. The label of “convicted felon” strips an individual
of the
right to vote, serve on juries, own firearms, or hold public
office. In many
states, convicted felons are prohibited from obtaining student
loans,
employment in state-licensed occupations, or employment with
state-
licensed companies. In addition, the label of convicted felon
may contrib-
ute to various informal exclusions that can make access to
noncriminal
activities more difficult and criminal alternatives more
attractive.3
The state of Florida has a law that allows individuals who have
been
found guilty of a felony, either by a judge, jury, or plea, to
literally avoid
the label of convicted felon. Judges have the option of
“withholding adju-
dication” of guilt for convicted felons who are being sentenced
to proba-
tion. The consequence of this unique labeling event is that
offenders who
are equivalent in terms of factual guilt can either be labeled a
convicted
felon or not. For those offenders who have adjudication
withheld—half of
the felony probationers in Florida in recent years—no civil
rights are lost
and such individuals may legitimately say on employment
applications and
elsewhere that a felony conviction did not occur. For those
offenders who
are formally adjudicated, all of the structural impediments of
being a con-
victed felon are possible.
Labeling theory would hypothesize that being formally
adjudicated
should increase the chances of recidivism. The research
question that we
address here is whether the labeling event of adjudication or the
withhold-
ing of adjudication has any effect on subsequent recidivism.4
Drawing on
1. Early research involving self-esteem or “self-typing” was
reported by Ageton and
Elliott (1974), Gibbs (1974), Harris (1976), Jensen (1972), and
Tittle (1972),
among others. More recently, “reflected appraisals of self”
(Heimer and Mat-
sueda, 1996; Matsueda, 1992) have been implicated in identity-
related labeling
research.
2. For an excellent discussion of structural impediments related
to labeling and sup-
portive research, see Sampson and Laub (1997: 147–52).
3. Discussions of the collateral consequences of felony
conviction have primarily
focused on the loss of voting rights (Hull, 2006; Manza and
Uggen, 2006; Uggen
and Manza, 2002). But a broader range of consequences from
imprisonment,
which presumes conviction, has also been described (Mauer and
Chesney-Lind,
2002; Western, 2002; Western and Pettit, 2005).
4. It is also possible, as specific deterrence theory would
hypothesize, that being
formally adjudicated could have the opposite effect of reducing
the chances of
recidivism. For discussions of specific deterrence, see, for
example, Paternoster
(1987), Nagin (1998), and Stafford and Warr (1993). At the
same time, a positive
relationship between formal sanctioning and subsequent
criminal activity could
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 3
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 549
recent theory and research, we also examine whether any
labeling effects
on recidivism are contingent on characteristics of the defendant,
such as
sex, race, or prior record, as well as on characteristics of the
county to
which the sentenced defendant returns, such as rates of crime or
levels of
social disadvantage. To assess these questions, we examine the
post-
sentencing recidivism experience of 95,919 men and women
who were
found guilty of a violent, property, or drug felony and sentenced
to proba-
tion in Florida between 2000 and 2002. This study is the first to
examine
the potential labeling effects of adult felony conviction.
Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) is used to assess the direct
effect of
having adjudication applied or withheld, as well as to assess the
effects of
other individual-level variables. In addition, we assess the
direct effect of
county-level characteristics on recidivism and the conditioning
relevance
that these second-level county characteristics may have on the
relationship
between adjudication status and recidivism.
LABELING EFFECTS: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES
A recurrent theme in the recent renewal of interest in labeling
effects
(Bernburg and Krohn, 2003; Bernburg, Krohn, and Rivera,
2006; Sampson
and Laub, 1997) is the recognition that consequences of formal
sanctions
may be contingent on the characteristics of offenders. As noted
by Pater-
noster and Iovanni (1989: 381), “we should not expect labeling
effects to
be invariant across societal subgroups.” With this in mind, four
individual-
level contingencies will inform parts of our analysis. They are
race, sex,
prior record, and “stakes in conformity.” Their conceptual
relevance for
potentially observed labeling effects is reviewed briefly here.
Race was one of the first issues to be raised in discussions of
contingent
labeling outcomes. Harris (1976: 433) argued that the effects of
official
labeling would be least consequential for those individuals most
excluded
from “ascriptive membership” in dominant groups—particularly
minori-
ties. Earlier, Jensen (1972) as well as Ageton and Elliott (1974)
hypothe-
sized that higher status persons—presumably including non-
minorities in
most contexts—may be more susceptible to labeling effects.
Similarly,
Paternoster and Iovanni suggested that persons who grant less
“legitimacy
to the legal order and its agents” may, as a result, be “more
impervious to
labeling effects” than others (1989: 382).
Alternatively, although not referencing race per se, Sampson
and Laub
(1997: 153) discussed the potential life-course consequences of
official
sanctions for the disadvantaged urban poor. They noted that
“among the
be the consequence of factors other than those attributed to
labeling. Among
these alternatives could be general strain (Agnew, 2006),
defiance (Sherman,
1993), and control–balance (Tittle, 1995).
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 4
22-AUG-07 10:10
550 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
disadvantaged, things seem to work differently. Deficits and
disadvantages
pile up faster and this has continuing negative consequences for
later
development. . . .” Building on that argument, Bernburg and
Krohn (2003)
hypothesized that labeling effects would be strongest among
“disadvan-
taged offenders” as distinguished by minority and poverty
status. Their
expectations were further supported by the observation that
official label-
ing of disadvantaged youth could be “enhanced by the negative
stereo-
types that are already associated with these youths in
mainstream culture”
(2003: 1290).
The relevance of sex for labeling outcomes has been
substantially over-
looked in criminological discourse. Even a book with the
promising title of
Labeling Women Deviant (Schur, 1983) makes no mention of
whether or
how labeling effects for women would be different from those
for men.
Regarding juveniles, Ageton and Elliott (1974) hypothesized
that “males
are more likely to be affected negatively by police contact than
females,”
but no rationale was associated with that expectation. Somewhat
later,
Ray and Downs (1986), also in the context of adolescents,
articulated the
opposite expectation. On the premise that “females are expected
to be
more attentive to interpersonal relationships than men,” they
hypothe-
sized that “labels may exert more of an influence on behavior
for females
than males” (1986: 171).
The possibility that women could be more adversely affected by
labeling
than men has also been noted in passing references that were
concerned
primarily with broader issues. For example, in his discussion of
“gender
blind criminology,” Messerschmidt (1993) conjectured how
females might
be factored into traditional criminological theories if one took
gender into
account. In this context, he speculated that because men
exercise greater
power in society than women, “men should therefore have
increased
opportunity to counteract official labeling . . .” (1993: 4).
Similarly, Gior-
dano, Cernkovich, and Lowery (2004) were developing
alternative expec-
tations concerning the relative prospects of women as compared
with men
for desistance from crime, without specific reference to formal
labeling. In
that context, they observed that “the greater social stigma
attached to
female in contrast to male involvement in antisocial behavior”
could in the
long run “be more limiting to life chances/opportunities for a
return to
conventional roles” (2004: 189).
Interestingly, the antithesis to labeling, deterrence, which
anticipates
that official sanctions will reduce rather than amplify criminal
involve-
ment, has generally argued that women are more likely to be
deterred
than men because they have more to lose if caught committing a
crime.
The argument has been made that women have been socialized
to feel
more responsible for social relationships than men (Blackwell
and
Eschholz, 2002) and to have a “greater investment in
conformity” than is
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 5
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 551
true for men (Richards and Tittle, 1981), both of which could
presumably
be lost as a consequence of a formal criminal sanction. If
sanctions are
expected to have stronger deterrent effects for women than for
men, then
logically, labeling effects should be weaker for women than
they are for
men. However, these arguments have been made in the context
of general
deterrence and not specific deterrence, which would be more
appropriate
here.
Expectations concerning the relevance of prior record for
potential
labeling effects have not been extensively developed, but a
limited consen-
sus favors the prospect that the consequences of formal
sanctions will be
greater for naı̈ ve, as opposed to repeat offenders. Noting that
some ana-
lysts have speculated that novice criminals would be more
susceptible to
deterrence, Horwitz and Wasserman (1979) offered “an
alternative
hypothesis.” They suggested that punishments of “increasing
severity will
lead to a greater increase in the rate of subsequent criminal
offenses for
primary than for secondary deviants” (1979: 57). Smith and
Gartin (1989)
articulated a similar hypothesis, although like Horwitz and
Wasserman,
they provided no supporting rationale. What that rationale could
be is sug-
gested by Paternoster and Iovanni (1989: 386) who observed
that once an
individual has been labeled, “it is doubtful that further
increments in label-
ing will continue to produce further deviance.” They attributed
this occur-
rence to a “leveling off” of labeling effects on the premise that
once an
individual has experienced social exclusions on the basis of a
label, “it is
not unreasonable to assume that further exclusion would have
little addi-
tional meaning . . .” (1989: 386).
Regarding “stakes in conformity,” Sherman et al. have shown
that the
issue has actually been used to hypothesize contradictory
labeling effects.
What they characterize as the “greater vulnerability” version of
labeling
theory expects that those individuals who “care more about the
opinions
of conventional society” will be more vulnerable to the
escalating effects
of labeling (1992: 682). From this point of view, nonminorities
and higher
status individuals would presumably be more vulnerable to
labeling effects
because they have more to lose. But it has been argued that
stakes in con-
formity could have the opposite consequence. Specifically,
persons with a
higher stake in conformity, such as those who are employed
and/or mar-
ried, could be more insulated from the negative consequences of
official
labels. Sherman et al. (1992) refer to this position as the “less
vulnerabil-
ity” approach to labeling effects. In this view, those individuals
who have
more to lose because of their informal social bonds may as a
consequence
have “other social resources that overcome the impact of
labeling” (1992:
682).
This “less vulnerability” position is implicit in the observation
by Samp-
son and Laub (1997: 152) that “sanctioning tends to aggravate
crime
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 6
22-AUG-07 10:10
552 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
among populations with low ‘stakes in conformity’.” They also
note, with
Braithwaite (1989) and Sherman (1993), that criminal sanctions
may actu-
ally promote future defiance of the law when applied to those
individuals
with weaker social bonds to conventional society (Sampson and
Laub,
1997: 153). Such a position is consistent with recent
ethnographic (Ander-
son, 1999) and autobiographic (Shakur, 1993) accounts of the
effects of
official sanctions on individuals from highly disadvantaged
inner-city envi-
ronments who presumably have fewer stakes in conformity than
others.
COMMUNITY-LEVEL CONTEXTS
However much individuals with particular characteristics may
vary in
their vulnerability to labeling effects, Paternoster and Iovanni
(1989: 373)
remind us that “the effect of status characteristics on labeling
outcomes is
not invariant, but varies substantially across different social
contexts.” In a
related manner, Sampson and Laub (1997: 153) noted that in
some social
environments, “deficits and disadvantages pile up faster” and as
a result
“one cannot ignore the effects of larger social contexts” in
studying the
life-course development of individuals who may have
experienced official
labeling and some of its negative consequences. Kubrin and
Stewart (2006:
172) have similarly argued that “where ex-offenders live greatly
affects
their ability to reintegrate into society” after being formally
sanctioned.
For these reasons, we have chosen to examine several social
contexts
aggregated at the county level that may interact with individual
traits in
producing greater or lesser labeling effects. To our knowledge,
this study is
the first study of labeling outcomes to do so. Specifically, we
include in our
analyses measures of concentrated disadvantage (CD),
concentrated
extremes (affluence and poverty), index crime rates, and police
presence.
The conceptual relevance of each measure for potentially
influencing
labeling outcomes is briefly reviewed here.
As noted by Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls (1997), among
others,
CD is a measure of “underclass concentration,” which involves
high levels
of minority presence, poverty, welfare, and female-headed
families. Such
places not only have frequently increased levels of crime
(Rountree, Land,
and Miethe, 1994; Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls, 1997;
Silver, 2000),
but they also have low levels of neighborhood resources and
social ser-
vices (Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley, 2002; Wilson,
1987). In
addition, as Sampson and Laub (1993: 295) have noted,
“counties charac-
terized by. . .a large concentration of the ‘underclass’. . .are
more likely
than other counties to be perceived as containing offensive and
threaten-
ing populations” and so are more likely to be more heavily
policed. Kubrin
and Stewart (2006) hypothesized and found higher levels of
recidivism
among felons released to community supervision in places with
higher
levels of CD. For these reasons, we might expect that those
individuals
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 7
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 553
who are labeled as convicted felons would see “deficits and
disadvan-
tages”—specifically recidivism—“pile up” more often in places
that have
higher levels of CD.
Whatever the value of taking account of the negative
consequences of
CD, Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley (2002: 446) have
suggested
that the common tactic of focusing on concentrated
disadvantage may
“. . .obscure the potential protective effects of affluent
neighborhoods.”
Morenoff, Sampson, and Raudenbush (2001: 528) have also
noted that
“the resources that affluent neighborhoods can mobilize are
theoretically
relevant to understanding the activation of social control . . . .”
They found
that a measure of concentrated affluence relative to poverty
(index of con-
centrated extremes, ICE) was related to lower levels of
homicide. Consis-
tent with that, Kubrin and Stewart (2006) hypothesized and
showed that
ICE was negatively related to the likelihood of recidivism for
ex-offenders
in Portland, Oregon. On that basis, we might expect that the
negative
effects of a felony convict label could be diminished in places
with higher
levels of ICE.
However, the ICE is really a measure of “relative inequality”
(Kubrin
and Stewart, 2006: 177) and not of “relative affluence” as
operationalized
by Sampson, Morenoff, and Earls (1999). It is reasonable to
imagine that
with extremes of poverty and affluence coexisting in the same
county, the
“protective effects of affluent neighborhoods” (Sampson,
Morenoff, and
Gannon-Rowley, 2002) could lead to “the activation of social
control”
(Morenoff, Sampson, and Raudenbush, 2001) in ways that result
in more
aggressive policing and prosecution, especially in poorer
sections of the
county. In that context, one might expect higher levels of ICE to
result in
higher levels of formal social control and recidivism generally
and in an
amplification of the effects of being labeled as a convicted
felon.
We also expect that convicted felons will be more likely to
recidivate in
places with higher index crime rates and with a stronger law
enforcement
presence. The rationale for the former expectation is that places
with
higher levels of crime, which will also likely have increased
levels of CD,
have more criminal opportunities, learning environments, and
role models
(Akers, 1998; Anderson, 1999; Warr, 2001). Individuals who
are labeled as
convicted felons and who may experience social exclusions not
exper-
ienced by their nonlabeled counterparts could find it easier to
resume
criminal activity in such a context, rather than in places with
lower levels
of crime. This occurrence may explain why, for example, Smith
and Pater-
noster (1990) found that the likelihood of recidivism for those
individuals
who had been referred to juvenile court in Florida was higher in
counties
with higher crime rates.
The expectation concerning higher recidivism for convicted
felons in
places with a stronger police presence is predicated on
Lofland’s (1969)
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 8
22-AUG-07 10:10
554 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
concept of “prevalent sensitivity.” His argument is that labels—
in this
case, recidivating felon—are more likely to be “imputed” in
places where
a higher prevalent sensitivity to misbehavior exists because
more “special-
ists” are deployed to detect and respond to it. In his terms: “as
the number
of imputational specialists increases . . . it is likely that the
number of peo-
ple imputed as deviant will also increase” (1969: 136). So if
individuals
labeled as convicted felons resume criminal activity, it seems
more likely
that they would be caught and officially recidivate where more
police
officers are present.
As a final conceptual issue, it can be noted that every adult
facing a
felony conviction, whether or not adjudication is withheld, has
been
arrested at least once and prosecuted. Assuming that those
experiences
constitute labeling events, the ideal situation described by
Bernburg and
Krohn (2003) of comparing samples from the general
(unlabeled) popula-
tion to assess “absolute labeling” effects is not available for the
label of
convicted felon. However, when it comes to generating the
structural
impediments that can accrue from labeling, the event of felony
conviction,
although “relative,” may nonetheless be relatively more
important than
any other for adults. Certainly, police contact, arrest, and
prosecution do
not cause individuals to lose their civil rights or to be legally
excluded
from the great variety of occupational opportunities denied to
those indi-
viduals who are convicted felons. By controlling for prior
convictions, both
adult and juvenile, an analysis of the consequences of having
adjudication
withheld or applied for convicted felons affords an estimate of
the inde-
pendent effect of “being labeled or not” in a relative labeling
context that
is most critical.
LABELING EFFECTS: PRIOR RESEARCH
Several systematic reviews of labeling effects research (Barrick,
2005;
Bernburg, 2002; Palamara, Cullen, and Gertsten, 1986;
Paternoster and
Iovanni, 1989) underscore several things. First, much evidence
that can be
inferred as relating to labeling is actually sanction effects
research, which
is often primarily concerned with deterrence. More important,
extraordi-
nary methodological variation exists in this research, in terms
of both the
specification of independent and dependent variables and the
elaboration
of contingent or subsample analyses. This variation makes a
brief sum-
mary of any research results or any patterns of support or
nonsupport for
labeling hypotheses all but impossible. Thus, we limit our
discussion of
prior research here to that which is most relevant to the analyses
that we
have undertaken. Specifically, we review studies that have
examined “con-
viction” as the key labeling event, and we review some
contingent results
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 9
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 555
of labeling research that anticipate the kinds of analyses that we
describe
below.
CONVICTION EFFECTS RESEARCH
We are aware of five studies that isolate conviction as a key
labeling
event. Two studies address misdemeanor charges of domestic
violence
(Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs, 1998; Ventura and
Davis, 2005),
another deals with drunk driving (Taxman and Piquero, 1998),
and two are
concerned with the “conviction” of juveniles for serious
offenses (Fagan,
Kupchik, and Liberman, 2003; Hagan and Palloni, 1990). To our
knowl-
edge, no research to date has examined the labeling effect of a
felony con-
viction for adults, which is the focus of the current study.
The two domestic violence studies that examine conviction
effects
involved midwestern cities and used rearrest within 1 year for
another
domestic violence charge as a measure of recidivism.
Multivariate logistic
analyses in both studies showed that conviction significantly
reduced the
likelihood of recidivism, thus supporting a deterrent effect more
than a
labeling effect (Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs, 1998:
394; Ventura
and Davis, 2005: 270).
Taxman and Piquero (1998) estimated the consequences of more
than
3,500 “drunk driving convictions” in Maryland between 1985
and 1993.
They measured recidivism by reconviction for drunk driving
within 3
years. Their primary concern was to distinguish between the
effects of
punitive as opposed to rehabilitative responses by the court. But
they also
made an important distinction—from a labeling perspective—
between
those who received a “guilty disposition” and those who
received “proba-
tion before judgement” (PBJ). The latter seems to be similar to
the “adju-
dication withheld” option that we examine for felony
convictions,
inasmuch as getting PBJ meant that one’s conviction was
“stayed” and
effectively expunged from the public record if probation was
successfully
completed (Taxman and Piquero, 1998: 133).
The authors report that for all offenders, conviction increased
the risk of
recidivism by 12 percent but that effect was not significant with
other con-
trols included. Separate analyses for first-time offenders
showed that a
guilty disposition increased the risk of recidivism by 27
percent, and this
was the only significant punishment or rehabilitation effect for
such
offenders (Taxman and Piquero, 1998: 135–7). This finding
supports an
interpretation that conviction for drunk driving had a labeling
effect that
was contingent on prior record, which is an issue that we
discuss more fully
in the next section.
Hagan and Palloni (1990) reanalyzed data from the Cambridge
Study in
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 10
22-AUG-07 10:10
556 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
Delinquency Development that were originally collected by
West and Far-
rington (1973). The primary dependent variables were self-
reports of
delinquency and crime (38 items) that were generated by
interviews with
randomly chosen London youth. The interviews were conducted
over a
period of years when the boys were between 8 and 24 years of
age. For
those who were interviewed, official data from the Criminal
Record Office
were used to create matched samples of youth who had been
“convicted”
by the age of 15 years and those who had not (Farrington, 1977:
114).5
In looking at self-reported delinquency subsequent to the
experience of
conviction, Hagan and Palloni (1990) controlled for individual
and family
attributes that could be taken as indicators of “characterological
and cul-
tural” traits that might predict delinquent involvement. They
also con-
trolled for prior reported delinquency. A key finding was that
being
convicted before the age of 15 years significantly increased the
likelihood
of delinquency at ages 16–17, 18–19, and 21–22 years
independent of the
effects of all other controls. This result clearly supports the
expectation
that the labeling experience of conviction contributes to
subsequent delin-
quent and criminal involvement (1990: 278–9).6
Fagan, Kupchick, and Liberman (2003) compared the
consequences for
juveniles of being convicted in an adult criminal court with
those of being
convicted in a juvenile court for felony assault, robbery, or
burglary. To do
this, they matched counties from northern New Jersey and from
New
York, which were all part of the same New York metro SMSA.
From the
New Jersey counties, they examined the consequences of
juvenile court
conviction, and from the New York counties, the same was done
for crimi-
nal court conviction. They tracked rearrest and reincarceration
outcomes
for at least 2 years after the completion of punishment for those
convicted.
An outcome called “any court action,” which the authors
describe as
“analogous to conviction,” was found to significantly increase
the chances
of rearrest for violent, property, and weapons crimes (not drugs)
for youth
convicted in either the juvenile or the criminal courts. However,
it was also
found that adolescent offenders convicted in criminal court
were rear-
rested and reincarcerated more often and more quickly for
violent, prop-
erty, and weapons offenses than those convicted in juvenile
court. The
reverse was true for drug crimes (Fagan, Kupchick, and
Liberman, 2003:
69). In sum, the available evidence on whether conviction is
related to
5. Farrington (1977: 114) describes the conviction data in this
manner: “searches of
the Criminal Record Office” uncovered a number of youth who
“had been found
guilty in court. . . .” The crimes for which these youth had been
convicted “were
mainly burglaries, thefts and unauthorized takings of motor
vehicles.”
6. The authors also report a significant interactive effect of
parent and son convic-
tion on subsequent delinquency, which was the primary
theoretical focus of their
study (Hagan and Palloni, 1990: 278–9).
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 11
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 557
recidivism is limited to juveniles, misdemeanor domestic
violence offend-
ers, and first-offending drunk drivers. How felony conviction
plays out in
the life course of adults remains to be seen.
CONTINGENT LABELING EFFECTS RESEARCH
It should be noted that some studies reporting what we call
contingent
labeling effects are not labeling studies per se. In fact, several
of them are
primarily concerned with specific (as distinct from general)
deterrence, but
if they report a relationship with recidivism subsequent to a
sanctioning
experience, we designate that as a potential labeling effect.
One consistent contingency seems to be prior record. As noted,
Taxman
and Piquero’s study (1998: 135–7) of drunk driving convictions
found sub-
stantially stronger labeling effects (increased recidivism) for
first-time
offenders than for their full sample. Similarly, Fagan,
Kupchick, and Liber-
man (2003: 41) report that the relatively stronger labeling
effects (recidi-
vism) for youth convicted in criminal as opposed to juvenile
court are
especially pronounced for those with no record of prior arrests.
Horwitz
and Wasserman (1979) found that more severe sanctions were
related to
higher rates of rearrest for juveniles in Newark, but that effect
held only
for those identified as first-time offenders on the basis of arrest
records.
DeJong (1997) analyzed the rearrest likelihood of those either
sentenced
to jail or not for misdemeanors7 in New York City. She reported
that the
experience of incarceration increased recidivism risk but only
for first-time
arrestees (1997: 571). The pattern of observed labeling effects
being con-
centrated among first offenders is consistent with the suggestion
by Pater-
noster and Iovanni (1989) that incremental labeling events may
be less
consequential than those that occur earlier in the life course of
an
individual.
Stakes in conformity have also been studied in relation to
sanction
effects. In general, what has been found is that apparent
labeling effects
are stronger for those who have a low stake in conformity or
weak ties to
conventional society. DeJong (1997) reported that incarceration
increased
the likelihood of rearrest especially for those with “low stakes”
as indi-
cated by having at least two of these traits: unmarried,
unemployed, and
not a high-school graduate. Sherman et al. (1992: 686) found
that formal
arrest for domestic violence significantly increased the
likelihood of rear-
rest in Milwaukee among those individuals who had a low stake
in con-
formity as measured by marital status and employment. Two
additional
7. Her sample was taken from “criminal court” as opposed to
“Supreme Court”
cases, which in New York involves primarily misdemeanors or
felony cases that
are negotiated down. Felonies that are not negotiated down are
tried in the
Supreme Court.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 12
22-AUG-07 10:10
558 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
domestic violence studies focused on those individuals with
“high” stakes
and concluded that sanctions for these people are associated
with less
recidivism, which the authors conclude is a stronger deterrent
effect (Pate
and Hamilton, 1992; Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs,
1998). The
negative relationship between stakes and recidivism for those
sanctioned
at least implies that those with lower stakes would experience
higher
recidivism (labeling effect) as reported by Dejong (1997) and
Sherman et
al. (1992).
A third contingency that has received some attention is race.
Several
early studies found that labeling was more consequential for
white
juveniles than for others. Ageton and Elliott (1974) reported the
race-spe-
cific effect for police contact and self-reported “delinquency
orientation”
among California youth. Harris (1975) found that New Jersey
youth who
had been incarcerated for longer periods expressed a higher
“relative
expected value of criminal choice” but that relationship held
only for
whites. Harris (1975: 97) hypothesized that the label might
mean more to
white youth because it came from a system “dominated by white
. . .
authorities,” whereas minority youth might perceive the label as
“created
and applied by ‘outsiders.’ ” In contrast, the most recent and
sophisticated
labeling work with juveniles has shown that juvenile justice
intervention
increased the chances of subsequent adult criminality only for
blacks
(Bernburg and Krohn, 2003: 1314).
Two domestic violence intervention studies report conflicting
results
concerning race in relation to the effects of arrest. Sherman et
al. (1992:
680) report that race did not condition the effect of arrest on
subsequent
domestic violence in Milwaukee. However, Berk et al. (1992:
705) found
that for pooled data from four cities, the labeling effect of arrest
on subse-
quent offending was 11 percent higher for blacks than for
whites and 47
percent higher for blacks who were unemployed than for
employed
whites.
Two reviews of the recidivism literature conclude that males are
consist-
ently more likely than females to resume criminal activity after
formal
sanctioning (Baumer, 1997; Gendreau, Little, and Goggin,
1996). Consis-
tent with this generalization, Giordano, Cernkovich, and Lowery
(2004)
reported longitudinal data from Ohio showing that previously
institution-
alized women were much less likely to recidivate than their
male counter-
parts. Similarly, Smith and Paternoster (1990) found that male
youth who
had been formally processed by the juvenile justice system in
Florida were
significantly more likely to recidivate. However, Taxman and
Piquero’s
(1998) study of drunk driving convictions, Sung’s (1993)
analysis of sen-
tenced drug offenders, and Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and
Gibbs’s
(1998) research on individuals arrested for domestic violence
each found
that sex was not a significant predictor of recidivism.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 13
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 559
On the basis of conceptual considerations that have been raised
and
prior research that has been reported, there are reasonably
consistent
expectations that we can bring to our analysis concerning the
relevance of
prior record and stakes in conformity for potential labeling
effects. But the
same cannot be said for sex or race. Specifically, both
conceptual and
empirical support exists for expecting stronger labeling effects
for less
experienced offenders and for those with lower stakes in
conformity. Con-
cerning the sex of offenders, the conceptual arguments favor
greater label-
ing consequences for women than for men, but the empirical
evidence
suggests otherwise. Finally, expectations concerning the
relative effect of
labeling by race are mixed on both conceptual and empirical
grounds.8
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
In this study, we assess the consequence for recidivism of
having adjudi-
cation applied or withheld for 71,548 male offenders and 24,371
female
offenders found guilty of a felony and sentenced to probation in
Florida
between 2000 and 2002. To the best of our knowledge, this is
the first
labeling research to examine the effect of adult felony
conviction, and the
first to examine whether community-level characteristics, such
as rates of
crime and CD, influence the relationship between the felony
conviction
and recidivism.
Our dependent variable, recidivism, is operationalized by
whether (yes
= 1) a felony probationer is convicted of another felony within 2
years of
being sentenced. This variable involves a new offense for which
an individ-
ual is sentenced to prison, probation, or jail, and it does not
include techni-
cal violations of the terms of probation. This measure of
recidivism only
captures those actually found guilty of another offense, and not
those who
may have been arrested and charged but had charges dropped or
were
found not guilty. Overall, 19 percent of our sample recidivated
within the
2-year follow-up period.9
8. It should be noted that several studies have examined
recidivism among felony
probationers, and with relative consistency, they report that
recidivism is more
likely for males, minorities, and persons with a more extensive
prior record (Ben-
edict and Huff-Corzine, 1997; Kruttschnitt, Uggen, and Shelton,
2000; Petersilia,
1985; Spohn and Holleran, 2002; Whitehead, 1991). However,
these were not
studies of labeling effects and the contingencies thereof,
because they involve
only individuals who have been labeled. Our concern is whether
these factors, as
well as stakes in conformity, have a conditional consequence
for observed label-
ing effects.
9. Arrest as an alternative measure of recidivism was
considered, but those data are
not available to us. The inclusion of probation revocation was
also considered but
rejected because the vast majority of those involved technical
violations and not
the commission of a new crime. Thus, the most complete and
homogeneous mea-
sure available to us in these data is reconviction.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 14
22-AUG-07 10:10
560 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
LEVEL 1 PREDICTORS
Our primary independent variable is whether (yes = 1) a person
being
sentenced to probation for a felony has adjudication formally
applied. This
outcome certifies the felony convict label. A total of 40 percent
of our
felony probationers were convicted in this manner.
Among the other factors used to model recidivism are
demographic
attributes of the defendant, his or her crime, and prior record.
The means
and standard deviations of the level 1 variables used are
described in table
1, which also shows the bivariate correlation matrix for all
variables of
interest. At the individual level, the Florida Sentencing
Guidelines
database was used to identify offenders as either non-Hispanic
blacks (yes
= 1) or Hispanic of whatever race (yes = 1). The surnames of all
offenders
who were not identified as Hispanic from the Guidelines data
were
checked against the U.S. Census list of Hispanic surnames
(Word and Per-
kins, 1996), and any individual whose name matched one of
those on the
list was coded as an Hispanic defendant. The use of the surname
list
afforded a more comprehensive identification of Hispanics,
which raises
their proportion in our sample from 11.5 percent to 14.4
percent. Unfortu-
nately, we cannot know how many females are identified as
Hispanic or
not by virtue of marriage. Also included as a predictor was the
defendant’s
age in years at sentencing.
The defendant’s crime was coded as either property (yes = 1) or
drug
related (yes = 1), with violent crime as the reference
category.10 This
choice was made because violent offenders were the least likely
to recidi-
vate11 and we expected the other types of crime to be more
consequential
in our recidivism models. The seriousness of the crime is
indicated by a
score generated by the Florida Sentencing Guidelines. Points
ranging from
4 to 116 are assigned to the primary offense, and additional
points are
assigned for additional offenses and a variety of other factors,
including
victim injury or the use of a firearm.12 A related variable,
which partially
reflects crime seriousness, is length of supervision in months
that is speci-
fied by the probation sentence.
Prior record is indicated by points that are assigned by the
Florida
10. “Other” crimes was such an eclectic category that we have
not included it in the
analysis. The most common of these crimes were Traffic (53
percent); Weapons,
Possession (16 percent); Escape (15 percent); and DUI, No
Injury (4 percent).
11. The relative frequency of recidivism was 25 percent for
property offenders, 23
percent for drug offenders, and 15 percent for violent offenders.
12. Additional points can be assigned on the basis of legal
status violations, commu-
nity sanction violations, prior serious felony points, violation of
the Law Enforce-
ment Protection Act, drug trafficking, grand theft of a motor
vehicle, commission
of the primary offense by a street gang member, and
involvement of domestic
violence.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 15
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 561
T
ab
le
1
.
C
o
rr
el
at
io
n
s,
M
ea
n
s,
a
n
d
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
D
ev
ia
ti
o
n
s
am
o
n
g
S
tu
d
y
V
ar
ia
b
le
s
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
1.
R
ec
o
n
vi
ct
io
n
1
2.
A
d
ju
d
ic
at
ed
.1
12
*
1
3.
M
al
e
.0
67
*
.0
82
*
1
4.
H
is
p
an
ic
–.
03
5*
–.
04
4*
.0
77
*
1
5.
B
la
ck
.1
02
*
.0
97
*
–.
03
1*
–.
30
9*
1
6.
W
h
it
e
–.
07
3*
–.
06
2*
–.
02
4*
–.
40
7*
–.
74
3*
1
7.
A
ge
–.
10
3*
.1
45
*
–.
04
3*
–.
02
5*
–.
08
9*
.1
03
*
1
8.
V
io
le
n
t
o
ff
en
se
–.
08
3*
–.
01
4*
.0
35
*
.0
62
*
–.
03
2*
–.
01
3*
.0
48
*
1
9.
P
ro
p
er
ty
o
ff
en
se
.0
31
*
–.
03
9*
–.
09
9*
.0
38
*
–.
04
1*
.0
12
*
–.
09
9*
–.
42
2*
1
10
.
D
ru
g
o
ff
en
se
.0
38
*
.0
51
*
.0
70
*
–.
09
1*
.0
68
*
–.
00
1
.0
59
*
–.
41
2*
–.
65
3*
1
11
.
P
ri
o
r
su
p
er
vi
si
o
n
v
io
la
ti
o
n
.1
54
*
.4
27
*
.0
39
*
–.
08
9*
.1
42
*
–.
07
3*
.1
37
*
–.
09
5*
–.
03
6*
.1
15
*
1
12
.
P
ri
o
r
re
co
rd
p
o
in
ts
(
L
n
)
.1
74
*
.4
87
*
.1
16
*
–.
12
8*
.1
35
*
–.
04
0*
.1
49
*
–.
09
3*
–.
06
4*
.1
42
*
.4
86
*
1
13
.
C
ri
m
e
se
ri
o
u
sn
es
s
(L
n
)
–.
00
2
.0
68
*
.1
11
*
.0
56
*
–.
01
1*
–.
02
9*
–.
04
7*
.4
61
*
–.
28
0*
–.
10
4*
–.
02
5*
–.
05
0*
1
14
.
S
u
p
er
vi
si
o
n
l
en
gt
h
(
L
n
)
–.
01
1*
.1
96
*
.0
21
*
–.
04
6*
–.
03
5*
.0
66
*
.0
06
*
.1
10
*
.0
39
*
–.
13
1*
.0
71
*
.0
77
*
.3
02
*
1
15
.
In
ve
rs
e
M
il
ls
r
at
io
–.
16
7*
–.
58
0*
–.
14
9*
.0
67
*
–.
15
4*
.1
01
*
–.
26
0*
–.
00
1
.0
74
*
–.
07
4*
–.
67
7*
–.
88
7*
–.
16
0*
–.
18
7*
1
16
.
IC
E
–.
00
4
–.
03
7*
.0
01
–.
11
6*
–.
05
3*
.1
33
*
.0
19
*
–.
05
6*
–.
03
7*
.0
83
*
.0
35
*
.0
35
*
–.
04
6*
.0
18
*
.0
90
*
1
17
.
D
is
ad
va
n
ta
ge
.0
15
*
.0
04
.0
16
*
.2
01
*
.1
66
*
–.
30
0*
–.
01
4*
.0
60
*
.0
32
*
–.
08
3*
–.
00
6
–.
06
6*
.0
76
*
–.
10
7*
–.
02
4*
–.
68
4*
1
18
.
C
ri
m
e
ra
te
.0
27
*
–.
00
5
.0
14
*
.2
35
*
.1
11
*
–.
27
2*
–.
01
4*
.0
41
*
.0
41
*
–.
07
6*
.0
10
*
–.
04
0*
.0
57
*
–.
13
7*
–.
01
0*
–.
32
3*
.6
41
*
1
19
.
P
o
li
ce
p
re
se
n
ce
.0
04
–.
00
3
.0
30
*
.2
02
*
.0
67
*
–.
20
6*
.0
22
*
.0
36
*
–.
00
7*
–.
02
3*
–.
00
6
–.
04
9*
.0
59
*
–.
08
3*
.0
03
–.
21
8*
.4
12
*
.5
95
*
1
M
ea
n
0.
19
0.
40
0.
75
0.
14
0.
36
0.
49
30
.9
6
0.
21
0.
40
0.
38
0.
22
–2
.4
6
2.
99
3.
21
1.
08
–0
.0
4
0.
01
56
61
23
1.
5
S
D
0.
39
0.
49
0.
44
0.
35
0.
48
0.
50
10
.5
5
0.
41
0.
49
0.
49
0.
41
3.
99
0.
68
0.
48
0.
56
0.
03
0.
88
15
62
31
.6
9
*p
<
.
05
.
N
O
T
E
:
N
=
9
5,
91
9
w
it
h
in
c
o
u
n
ty
;
N
=
6
7
b
et
w
ee
n
c
o
u
n
ti
es
.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 16
22-AUG-07 10:10
562 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
Guidelines to offense-specific prior convictions for a felony or
misde-
meanor as an adult or juvenile by a state, federal, military, or
foreign
court. As with other research using Guidelines data, this affords
an unusu-
ally comprehensive measure of an individual’s prior criminal
record.
Offenses that occurred more than 10 years before the current
offense are
not scored if the individual has been conviction free for that
time. Also
included among our predictors of recidivism is whether the
individual had
previously violated the terms of supervision while on probation
or commu-
nity control (yes = 1), which can be regarded as another
measure of prior
record. To correct for skewness, we used natural log values for
crime seri-
ousness, prior record, and supervision length.
It is possible that being adjudicated or not may be the
consequence of
uncontrolled factors that also predict an outcome of interest
such as recidi-
vism. In such a situation, the judge’s decision to adjudicate the
offender is
not independent of the residual terms in the equation for
recidivism, which
would result in biased estimates. To control for this “treatment
effect”
(Moffitt, 1999), which is somewhat analogous to “selection
bias,”13 we
computed inverse Mills ratios for inclusion in each model
estimating
recidivism.
The inverse Mills ratios were computed through a two-stage
process.
First, we estimated a probit model to predict adjudication,
including all
level 1 independent variables shown in table 1. We also
included exclusion
restrictions, which are “variables that affect the selection
process but not
the substantive equation of interest” (Bushway, Johnson, and
Slocum,
2007: 152). For this purpose, whether the offender went to trial
(1 = yes) is
used to predict adjudication but not recidivism, because this
variable is
arguably relevant for adjudication and not recidivism. In
addition, since
our data are drawn from 67 counties, we included 66 county
dummy vari-
ables and only kept those significant county dummy variables
for a
reduced probit model. The second step calculated the inverse
Mills ratio
using the predicted value for each offender in the probit
equation for adju-
dication from the first step, Zδ. Following Bushway, Johnson
and Slocum
13. Selection bias (Berk, 1983; Heckman, 1979) refers to a
situation wherein not all
members of a population are represented inasmuch as some have
been
“selected” in terms of variables that need to be accounted for
when predicting
subsequent outcomes. This process applies, for example, to
people who are sen-
tenced to prison (as opposed to probation) when one is
interested in the factors
that would predict differential sentence length. In other
instances, all members of
a population are represented, but some are accorded one
“treatment” as opposed
to another (Moffitt, 1999). This process applies to a situation
such as our own in
which all persons found guilty of a felony are either adjudicated
or have adjudi-
cation withheld. The object of controlling for “treatment effect”
is to account for
the influence of variables that may predict both the treatment
experienced and
its presumed consequence—in this case, recidivism.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 17
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 563
(2007: 161), the inverse Mills ratio was estimated for each case
by dividing
the normal density function evaluated at –Zδ by 1 minus the
normal
cumulative distribution function estimated at –Zδ.
One primary objective of this research is to determine whether
“larger
social contexts” (Sampson and Laub, 1997) play a role in
shaping the
potential labeling effects of felony conviction. The social
contexts that we
examine are characteristics of the county in which offenders
experienced
their probation supervision. We would prefer to have a lower
level of
aggregation, but data limitations relative to the situating of
offenders by
place preclude that possibility. Our second-level variables
include total
crime rate per 100,000 residents and a measure of police
presence, which is
the number of police per 100,000 residents. Both of these
measures are for
the year 2000 (Florida Department of Law Enforcement, 2005).
Our mea-
sure of CD uses 2000 Census data for percent black, percent of
families
receiving public assistance, percentage of persons living below
the poverty
level, and the percentage of families headed by a single mother.
Factor
loadings were used to weight the variables included in the CD
index. Sev-
eral other variables failed to load on this factor, including
unemployment
rate, percent Hispanic, and percent of the population under 18
years of
age.
To operationalize the ICE, we follow Kubrin and Stewart (2006)
and
Massey (2001). For a given county, the ICE index is computed
as [(number
of affluent families – number of poor families) / total number of
families].
In this context, “affluent” is defined as families that are 2
standard devia-
tions above the mean family income and “poor” is defined as
families
below the officially designated poverty level. We expect each of
the second
level variables to potentially interact with felony conviction in
the specifi-
cation of labeling effects. The means and standard deviations
for these
variables, as well as their bivariate correlations with first-level
and second-
level predictors are described in table 1.
ANALYTIC STRATEGY
HLM represents an improvement over traditional regression
techniques
when analyzing “nested” or multilevel data. According to
Raudenbush
and Bryk (2002), multilevel analysis using traditional
regression modeling
risks violation of two regression assumptions, the independence
of error
terms and homoscedasticity. Individuals nested within second-
level con-
texts—in our case, counties—will no longer represent
independent obser-
vations because they may share many of the same
characteristics. This
analysis results in correlated error terms and biased estimates of
standard
errors. HLM corrects for this problem “by incorporating into the
statistical
model a unique random effect for each organizational [county
level] equa-
tion” (Bryk and Raudenbush, 1992: 84). Second, tests of
significance for
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 18
22-AUG-07 10:10
564 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
second-level variables are accomplished in HLM by adjusting
the degrees
of freedom to represent the number of second-level units in the
analysis.
HLM also allows researchers to test the cross-level effects of
second-level
factors on individual-level relationships. The improved
estimation of inde-
pendent effects and cross-level interactions are of primary
interest in the
current research.
As discussed by Raudenbush and Bryk (2002), the use of
traditional
HLM with a dichotomous dependent variable such as recidivism
would
not be appropriate for two reasons. First, the assumption that
error terms
are normally distributed is violated with binary outcome data.
Also vio-
lated is the assumption of linearity in the relationship between
the inde-
pendent and the dependent variables and the assumption that the
variance
of the error terms be distributed equally across all values of the
dependent
variable. To overcome these problems, hierarchical generalized
linear
modeling (HGLM) is used with these data. All level 1 and level
2 variables
in the analysis are grand mean centered with the exception of
dichoto-
mous measures. Both Raudenbush and Bryk (2002) and Hox
(2002)
observe that grand mean centering as opposed to group mean
centering
facilitates the interpretation of coefficients, especially at level 1
and that,
without serious theoretical justification for doing otherwise, is
the pre-
ferred choice. Hox further notes that not using grand mean
centering in
models with cross-level interactions can lead to “serious
interpretation
problems” (2002: 56). In all analyses, unit-specific models with
robust stan-
dard errors are used. Nonsignificant variance components are
specified as
fixed.
RESEARCH FINDINGS
UNCONDITIONAL MODEL
The unconditional model is estimated primarily to measure the
magni-
tude of variation in recidivism across counties. Our variance
component
estimate, .037, is significant (p < .001), which is a common
indicator that
additional modeling using second-level predictors is warranted.
However,
the intraclass correlation of .006 suggests that although between
county
variation is significant, it is clearly not substantial.
Nonetheless, we will
proceed with second-level analyses.
LEVEL 1 RESULTS
The second stage of analysis estimates recidivism as a function
of indi-
vidual-level predictors. Results of the level 1 conditional
modeling are
described in table 2, which shows the effect of individual-level
predictors
on recidivism and the degree to which those effects vary across
counties
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 19
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 565
(variance component). All individual-level predictors are
significantly
related to recidivism except for crime seriousness.
In table 2, we see that odds of recidivism for property offenders
are 72
percent greater than violent offenders and that the odds that
black proba-
tioners will fail are 36 percent higher than for whites. Those
who had vio-
lated the terms of a previous probation have odds of recidivism
that are 22
percent greater than for those who had not, and males are 38
percent more
likely to fail than females. The odds of recidivism are lower for
Hispanics
than for non-Hispanic whites and are reduced for those with
longer terms
of probation supervision. Most importantly, table 2 shows that
indepen-
dent of the effects of all other predictors, having been convicted
of a fel-
ony increases the odds of recidivism by 17 percent when
compared with
those who had adjudication withheld.
Table 2. Level 1 Conditional HGLM Model of Recidivism
Odds Variance
Independent Variable Coefficient SE Ratio Component
Intercept –2.158 .061 .081*** .092***
Adjudicated .155 .032 1.167*** .016*
Male .320 .035 1.377*** .028***
Age at sentencing –.044 .002 .957*** .000*
Hispanic –.082 .041 .921* a
Black .308 .034 1.360*** .020**
Property .543 .037 1.721*** .018*
Drug .386 .046 1.470*** .042***
Prior supervision violation .199 .049 1.220*** .033*
Crime seriousness (Ln) .037 .027 1.037 a
Prior record points (Ln) .087 .022 1.091*** .007*
Supervision length (Ln) –.218 .031 .804*** .017**
Inverse Mills ratio –.472 .107 .624*** .156**
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
a Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed.
NOTE: N = 95,919 within county; N = 67 between counties.
As noted, both theory and prior research (sometimes
contradictory)
have raised the possibility that labeling effects may vary by a
person’s
race, sex, prior record, and stakes in conformity. To assess this
possibility,
we repeat the level 1 modeling described above with interaction
terms
involving adjudication status and the aforementioned individual
attributes.
Typically, stakes in conformity is operationalized in terms of
things that an
individual could potentially lose, such as a marital bond or
employment as
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 20
22-AUG-07 10:10
566 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
the result of criminal behavior. Unfortunately, those particular
data (as
well as educational attainment) are not available for Florida
probationers.
However, a theoretical analog to stakes in conformity is
Hirschi’s (1969)
“commitment to conventional lines of action.” It is presumed to
work the
same way as stakes in conformity, inasmuch as it provides an
individual
with “something to lose” in the event of delinquent or criminal
behavior.
It seems reasonable to suggest that individuals who have
achieved the age
of 30 years and have avoided a criminal or delinquent record
(up to the
current instance) have established more of a commitment to
conventional
behavior than those who are younger with a criminal record. As
such, they
have acquired a stake in conformity that threatens at least the
loss of repu-
tation and status as a consequence of criminal behavior. Thus,
we measure
“commitment” or stakes in conformity in terms of age and prior
record.
For our purpose, “high stakes” individuals are those who are
30+ years of
age without a prior conviction (felony or misdemeanor) as an
adult or
juvenile.
Table 3 shows the recidivism model with all level 1 estimators
and with
the addition of four interaction terms involving adjudication and
offender
characteristics. All level 1 predictors, except Hispanic, and
crime serious-
ness in three of four models, have a significant relationship
with recidi-
vism. The effect of adjudication on recidivism is significantly
higher for
females than for males. Table 3 also shows that the likelihood
of recidi-
vism, given adjudication is significantly higher for white
offenders than for
others. Prior record (yes/no) has no significant interaction with
adjudica-
tion status in predicting recidivism. With regard to our measure
of high
stakes in conformity, table 3 shows that probationers who had
achieved
the age of 30 years without a criminal or delinquent conviction
are signifi-
cantly more likely to recidivate given adjudication than those
with a prior
record before the age of 30 years.14
SECOND-LEVEL RESULTS
In this stage of our analysis, the slope for the relationship
between
recidivism and adjudication is modeled using county-level
contextual vari-
ables in conjunction with individual-level predictors. Strong
collinearity
problems are encountered when the ICE and CD are included in
the same
14. In separate models (not shown, results available on request),
the effect of adjudi-
cation is significant for men as well as for women; for
Hispanics, blacks, and
whites; for those with and without a prior record; and for those
with and without
high stakes. This finding suggests a robustness in the
consequences of adjudica-
tion for recidivism, notwithstanding the significance of
interaction terms in table
3 showing the effects of adjudication to be stronger for women,
whites, and those
with high stakes.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 21
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 567
T
ab
le
3
.
L
ev
el
1
C
o
n
d
it
io
n
al
H
G
L
M
M
o
d
el
w
it
h
A
d
ju
d
ic
at
io
n
a
n
d
O
ff
en
d
er
C
h
ar
ac
te
ri
st
ic
In
te
ra
ct
io
n
s
S
ex
P
ri
o
r
R
ec
o
rd
S
ta
ke
s
in
C
o
n
fo
rm
it
y
R
ac
e
In
d
ep
en
d
en
t
V
ar
ia
b
le
C
o
ef
fi
ci
en
t
O
d
d
s
R
at
io
C
o
ef
fi
ci
en
t
O
d
d
s
R
at
io
C
o
ef
fi
ci
en
t
O
d
d
s
R
at
io
a
C
o
ef
fi
ci
en
t
O
d
d
s
R
at
io
In
te
rc
ep
t
–2
.5
75
.0
76
**
*
–2
.5
46
.0
78
**
*
–2
.4
77
.0
84
**
*
–2
.5
43
.0
79
**
*
A
d
ju
d
ic
at
ed
.2
85
1.
33
0*
**
.1
52
1.
16
4*
**
.0
67
1.
06
9
.0
57
1.
05
9
M
al
e
.4
08
1.
50
3*
**
.3
16
1.
37
2*
**
.2
66
1.
30
5*
**
.3
17
1.
37
2*
**
A
ge
a
t
se
n
te
n
ci
n
g
–.
04
4
.9
57
**
*
–.
04
5
.9
56
**
*
–.
05
9
.9
43
**
*
–.
04
4
.9
57
**
*
H
is
p
an
ic
–.
07
9
.9
24
–.
08
1
.9
23
–.
02
9
.9
72
–.
01
8
.9
82
B
la
ck
.3
09
1.
36
2*
**
.3
16
1.
37
2*
**
.3
58
1.
43
0*
**
.3
80
1.
46
2*
**
P
ro
p
er
ty
.5
46
1.
72
6*
**
.5
64
1.
75
8*
**
.5
28
1.
69
5*
**
.5
45
1.
72
5*
**
D
ru
g
.3
87
1.
47
2*
**
.4
08
1.
50
4*
**
.3
90
1.
47
7*
**
.3
90
1.
47
7*
**
P
ri
o
r
su
p
er
vi
si
o
n
v
io
la
ti
o
n
.2
18
1.
24
4*
**
.1
80
1.
19
8*
*
.3
61
1.
43
5*
**
.2
17
1.
24
2*
**
C
ri
m
e
se
ri
o
u
sn
es
s
(L
n
)
.0
43
1.
04
4
.0
36
1.
03
7
.1
20
1.
12
7*
**
.0
40
1.
04
1
P
ri
o
r
re
co
rd
p
o
in
ts
(
L
n
)
.0
95
1.
10
0*
**
.0
60
1.
06
1*
*
.1
02
1.
10
7*
*
.0
93
1.
09
7*
**
S
u
p
er
vi
si
o
n
l
en
gt
h
(
L
n
)
–.
21
8
.8
04
**
*
–.
24
6
.7
82
**
*
–.
26
0
.7
71
**
*
–.
21
6
.8
06
**
*
In
ve
rs
e
M
il
ls
r
at
io
–.
45
0
.6
38
**
*
–.
59
1
.5
54
**
*
–.
15
2
.8
59
–.
45
6
.6
34
**
*
M
al
e
*
A
d
j
–.
20
4
.8
16
**
*
N
o
p
ri
o
r
p
ts
*
A
d
j
.0
20
1.
02
0
H
ig
h
s
ta
k
es
*
A
d
j
.2
03
1.
22
5*
*
W
h
it
e
*
A
d
j
.1
45
1.
15
6*
**
*p
<
.
05
;
**
p
<
.
01
;
**
*p
<
.
00
1.
a T
h
es
e
re
su
lt
s
ar
e
fr
o
m
a
r
ed
u
ce
d
s
am
p
le
i
n
cl
u
d
in
g
o
n
ly
t
h
o
se
w
it
h
h
ig
h
o
r
lo
w
s
ta
k
es
i
n
c
o
n
fo
rm
it
y
(N
=
4
1,
62
7)
.
N
O
T
E
S
:
N
o
n
si
gn
if
ic
an
t
va
ri
an
ce
c
o
m
p
o
n
en
ts
h
av
e
b
ee
n
f
ix
ed
;
N
=
9
5,
91
9
w
it
h
in
c
o
u
n
ty
;
N
=
6
7
b
et
w
ee
n
c
o
u
n
ti
es
.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 22
22-AUG-07 10:10
568 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
models. Like Kubrin and Stewart (2006), we resolve that issue
by develop-
ing estimates that keep these variables separate. However,
whereas
Kubrin and Stewart only examined those level 2 predictors, our
estimates
also include overall crime rates and police presence. These
models origi-
nally include all level 1 and level 2 predictors. The latter are
introduced
sequentially in two different estimates in order to keep ICE and
CD sepa-
rate. The sequence for introduction is as follows: ICE, crime
rate, and
police presence and, alternatively, CD, crime rate, and police
presence.
Following the criteria articulated by Raudenbush and Bryk
(2002: 268), we
first added the level 2 variables sequentially to the intercept
model and
retained only those that were significant. Then we sequentially
added the
level 2 variables to the adjudicated equation and again only
retained sig-
nificant variables.15
Table 4a. Combined Level 1 and Level 2 HGLM Model of
Recidivism (ICE)
Independent Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio
Intercept –2.546 .061 .078***
ICE 1.298 .517 3.662*
Adjudicated .154 .032 1.167***
Male .314 .035 1.369***
Age at sentencing –.045 .002 .956***
Hispanic –.083 .041 .921*
Black .312 .034 1.366***
Property .552 .036 1.738***
Drug .390 .046 1.477***
Prior supervision violation .184 .054 1.202**
Crime seriousness (Ln) .030 .028 1.031
Prior record points (Ln) .078 .024 1.082**
Supervision length (Ln) –.216 .032 .806***
Inverse Mills ratio –.509 .118 .601***
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
NOTES: Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed;
N = 95,919 within county;
N = 67 between counties.
15. Raudenbush and Bryk note that “the analyst will usually
want to develop a tenta-
tive model for the intercept . . . before proceeding to fit models
for the random
slopes” (2002: 267) and then, “the most direct evidence of
whether a level-2 pre-
dictor should be included is the magnitude of its estimated
effect and related
t-ratio. Predictors with t-ratios near or less than 1 are obvious
candidates for
exclusion from the model” (2002: 268).
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 23
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 569
Table 4b. Combined Level 1 and Level 2 HGLM Model of
Recidivism (CD)
Independent Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio
Intercept –2.530 .061 .080***
Concentrated Disadvantage –.060 .026 .942*
Adjudicated .157 .031 1.170***
Male .311 .036 1.364***
Age at sentencing –.044 .002 .957***
Hispanic –.082 .042 .921*
Black .314 .035 1.369***
Property .549 .036 1.731***
Drug .389 .046 1.475***
Prior supervision violation .192 .051 1.211**
Crime seriousness (Ln) .035 .026 1.036
Prior record points (Ln) .083 .022 1.087**
Supervision length (Ln) –.218 .031 .804***
Inverse Mills ratio –.486 .107 .615***
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
NOTES: Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed;
N = 95,919 within county;
N = 67 between counties.
Tables 4a and b show the results of estimating recidivism using
all level 1
predictors, including adjudication status and any significant
level 2
predictors. We are concerned with the direct effects of county-
level con-
texts on recidivism (intercept model) and more importantly,
with any pos-
sible interactions of level 2 variables and adjudication. Crime
rates and
police presence have no significant relationships either directly
with recidi-
vism or interactively with adjudication status. Neither the ICE
nor CD
interacts significantly with adjudication in the prediction of
recidivism.
However, concentrated extremes of affluence and poverty are
associated
with higher levels of recidivism across Florida counties,
whereas CD is
associated with slightly lower levels of recidivism.
The lack of interactive effects involving adjudication and level
2
predictors suggests that being convicted as a felon increases the
likelihood
of recidivism regardless of the characteristics of counties in
which proba-
tioners are supervised. As with the individual-level traits
discussed above
(table 3), this further illustrates the apparent robustness of the
effects of a
felony convict label inasmuch as those effects are not
conditioned either
by individual or “larger social contexts.” In addition, although
not a focus
of this research, our data for Florida counties show opposite
direct effects
of ICE and CD on recidivism than what Kubrin and Stewart
(2006) report
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 24
22-AUG-07 10:10
570 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
for Oregon census tracts.16
DISCUSSION
As noted, this study is the first one to examine the effects of
being
labeled a convicted felon on the recidivism of adults. The
option of with-
holding adjudication of guilt and the label of convicted felon
for those who
are legally guilty provides a unique opportunity to explore the
conse-
quences of labeling within the criminal justice system. The
decision to
withhold adjudication involves a situation wherein legally
equivalent
adults—in terms of guilt—are either labeled as a felon or not. In
terms of
social and legal significance, a more consequential label is not
available in
the criminal court system. Our principal findings can be briefly
summarized.
1. Being adjudicated guilty as a felon significantly and
substantially
increases the likelihood of recidivism in comparison with those
who have
adjudication withheld.
2. The effect of being adjudicated guilty on recidivism is
stronger for
whites than for blacks and Hispanics and for females as opposed
to males.
It is also stronger for those who reach the age of 30 years
without any
prior convictions compared with those with priors before
turning 30 years
old.
3. The community-level contexts that we examined have no
conse-
quence for the relationship between adjudication of guilt and
recidivism.
At a practical level, these results underscore the potential
benefits that
16. There are several methodological differences between the
Kubrin and Stewart
(2006) study and our own. Their study was performed in
Portland, Oregon and
ours in Florida. The proportion of non-Hispanic whites in
Multnomah County
(Portland) was substantially higher (79.4 percent) in the 2000
Census than in
Florida (66.5 percent). Their sample, which included
misdemeanants as well as
felons, was 68 percent white, 25 percent black, 4 percent
Hispanic, and 3 percent
other. Our sample is 49 percent white, 36 percent black, and 14
percent Hispanic
(others excluded). Their measure of recidivism is arrest,
whereas ours is reconvic-
tion for a felony. More important, perhaps, their data for ICE
and CD were
aggregated at the census tract level and ours at the county level.
At the lower
level of aggregation, it makes sense to expect the relative
presence of affluence to
provide for both social capital and collective efficacy in ways
that might reduce
recidivism overall. But at the county level, the affluent and poor
are likely not
living near one another and the potential benefits of having
affluence nearby may
not work in the same way. In fact, the social and physical
distance that may char-
acterize the affluent and poor in a county may work in ways that
afford the afflu-
ent the opportunity and means to more vigorously police poor
neighborhoods
and prosecute those who may be apprehended. This would not
only mitigate the
recidivism retarding effects of affluence noted by Kubrin and
Stewart, but it may
also aggravate the prospects of criminal activity resulting in a
formal charge and
conviction—our measure of recidivism.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 25
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 571
can accrue to individuals and to society—in this case, from less
criminal
activity—when a policy of minimizing harm (Clear, 1995;
Rubin, 1999) is
implemented in the administration of justice. The withholding
of adjudica-
tion for those who have been found guilty of a felony is a policy
that these
data suggest is directly related to lower levels of recidivism
than are found
among those who are formally labeled as a convicted felon. In
this case,
minimizing harm at the individual level has consequences for
reducing
harm in the broader community.
At a theoretical level, the pattern of findings suggests that the
conse-
quences of labeling are greater for those who would otherwise
be consid-
ered less likely to recidivate. As noted, research has generally
shown that
recidivism is more likely for males, minorities, and persons
with a more
extensive prior record (Benedict and Huff-Corzine, 1997;
Kruttschnitt,
Uggen, and Shelton, 2000; Spohn and Holleran, 2002;
Whitehead, 1991).
These data suggest that those who are generally more likely to
recidivate
are less disadvantaged by a formal label than are women,
whites, and
those without an early prior record. It could also be argued that
women,
whites, and those without an early prior record have more to
lose from
criminal stigma because crime is more often associated with
males, minori-
ties, and those with extensive criminal experience.
It also happens that the pattern of results reported here is
consistent
with some of the theoretical expectations raised earlier in this
article. Spe-
cifically, the diminished relevance of the felony convict label
for black and
Hispanic offenders was anticipated, even though prior research
is both
limited and inconsistent in this regard. For example, Harris
(1976: 433)
hypothesized that the effects of official labeling would be least
conse-
quential for those most excluded from “ascriptive membership”
in domi-
nant groups, whereas Paternoster and Iovanni (1989: 382)
hypothesized
that those who “grant less legitimacy to the legal order and its
agents”—
presumably including racial and ethnic minorities—could be
“more imper-
vious to labeling effects.” Assuming that, on average, whites
are less disad-
vantaged than blacks, our results run counter to the expectation
raised by
Sampson and Laub’s (1997: 153) that deficits would “pile up
faster” for
those who are already disadvantaged. These results also diverge
from
those of Bernburg and Krohn (2003) who found that labeling
effects were
stronger for African-American youth than for others. It is
possible that the
race-specific effects of formal labeling are different for adults
than for
juveniles, which is an issue that future research should carefully
explore.
The fact that a felony convict label more strongly predicts
recidivism for
females than for males is consistent with the scant theorizing
that has been
done about labeling effects for women, but it is inconsistent
with a limited
body of empirical evidence showing that labeling effects are
more conse-
quential for males. Messerschmidt (1993: 4) hypothesized that
because
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 26
22-AUG-07 10:10
572 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
men exercise greater power in society than women, “men should
. . . have
greater opportunity to counteract official labeling.” Giordano,
Cernkovich, and Lowery (2004: 189) hypothesized that the
greater social
stigma attached to “antisocial” behavior by females could “be
more limit-
ing to life chances/opportunities for a return to conventional
roles” and
thus more conducive to recidivism. Although our results are
consistent
with those expectations, the process whereby labeling comes to
have more
consequence for women than for men cannot be known from
these data.
The finding that labeling effects are stronger for those who, in
Hirschi’s
(1969) terms, have established a more substantial “commitment
to conven-
tional action” by having reached at least 30 years of age without
a prior
record is consistent with the “greater vulnerability” labeling
hypothesis
(Sherman et al., 1992). In that view, it is expected that labeling
would be
more consequential for those with a higher stake in conformity
and who,
on that account, presumably “care more about the opinions of
conven-
tional society” (1992: 682). Another way to interpret this result
is that, as
Paternoster and Iovanni (1989) hypothesized, labeling
experiences have
diminishing consequences for those persons who have been
formally
labeled earlier in life.
Although not part of our original theoretical framework, it is
worth not-
ing that finding less recidivism for those having adjudication
withheld is
consistent with expectations from a “reintegrative shaming”
perspective.
As described by Braithwaite (1989: 55), reintegrative shaming
combines
“expressions of community disapproval” for a behavior with
“gestures of
reacceptance into the community of law-abiding citizens.” One
who has
been found guilty of a felony and sentenced to probation has
clearly
received an expression of community disapproval. At the same
time, not
being labeled a convicted felon (having adjudication withheld)
reduces the
prospect of formal and informal exclusions from the community
of law-
abiding citizens that traditionally accompany the label of
convicted felon.
In fact, Braithwaite (1989: 101) juxtaposes and contrasts
reintegrative
shaming with the central labeling concept of “stigmatization,”
which he
describes as “disintegrative shaming in which no effort is made
to recon-
cile the offender with the community.” What the decision to
impose or
withhold adjudication of guilt may amount to is a choice
between stigmati-
zation and reintegration—or at least diminished disintegration.
The avoid-
ance of the label in this instance not only reduces the
potentially
stigmatizing effects of labeling, but it may at the same time
increase the
reintegrative potential of the punitive response for the
individual involved.
The lack of an interaction between second-level variables and
adjudica-
tion status in these analyses could mean one of three things. On
the one
hand, it could indicate that the effect of adjudication on
recidivism is suffi-
ciently robust that it holds in a variety of community contexts.
Thus, it is
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 27
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 573
seemingly unaffected by levels of crime, police presence, CD,
and an ICE
of affluence and poverty. Alternatively, as the intraclass
correlation sug-
gests, there may be insufficient variation in recidivism across
counties to
matter for these purposes. Finally, it may be the case that
community con-
text is inadequately operationalized with data that are
aggregated at the
county level. Ideally, an assessment of the impact of community
character-
istics on the relationship between adjudication as a felon and
recidivism
would make use of neighborhood-level data, aggregated at the
census tract
or block level. Unfortunately, we cannot locate our probationers
more
precisely than by the county in which their probation
supervision took
place.
Another limitation of the current research is the potential for
omitted
variable biases at both levels of analysis. At the individual
level, it would
have been helpful if available data included socioeconomic
status as well
as three additional indicators of stakes in conformity—
employment, mari-
tal status, and educational attainment. At the second level of
aggregation,
measures of probation caseload could have relevance for the
likelihood of
recidivism, as could measures of social capital within different
social
contexts.
In sum, this research has shown that when adults are
adjudicated guilty
of a felony, a significant and substantial increase occurs in the
likelihood
of reconviction within 2 years. When combined with the recent
results
reported by Bernburg and his colleagues (Bernburg and Krohn,
2003;
Bernburg, Krohn, and Rivera, 2006) involving the labeling of
juveniles,
these data provide additional encouragement for the renewed
empirical
assessment of labeling theory. Moreover, research moving
forward in this
area should pay particular attention to whether and how labeling
effects
vary for different kinds of individuals and for broader social
contexts that
may be operationalized more finely than was possible in the
current
research.
REFERENCES
Ageton, Suzanne S., and Delbert Elliott. 1974. The effects of
legal process-
ing on delinquent orientations. Social Problems 22:87–100.
Agnew, Robert. 2006. Pressured Into Crime: An Overview of
General
Strain Theory. Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing.
Akers, Ronald L. 1998. Social Learning and Social Structure: A
General
Theory of Crime and Deviance. Boston, MA: Northeastern
University
Press.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 28
22-AUG-07 10:10
574 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
Anderson, Elijah. 1999. Code of the Street. New York: W. W.
Norton and
Company.
Barrick, Kelle. 2005. Labeling and recidivism: A quantitative
assessment
of the evidence. Presented at the annual meeting of the
American Soci-
ety of Criminology, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, November.
Baumer, Eric. 1997. Levels and predictors of recidivism: The
Malta experi-
ence. Criminology 35:601–28.
Benedict, W. Reed, and Lin Huff-Corzine. 1997. Return to the
scene of
the punishment: Recidivism of adult male property offenders on
felony
probation, 1986-1989. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency
34:237–53.
Berk, Richard A. 1983. An introduction to sample selection bias
in socio-
logical data. American Sociological Review 48:386–98.
Berk, Richard A., Alec Campbell, Ruth Klap, and Bruce
Western. 1992.
The deterrent effect of arrest in incidents of domestic violence:
A
Bayesian analysis of four field experiments. American
Sociological
Review 57:698–708.
Bernburg, Jon Gunnar. 2002. State reaction, life-course
outcomes and
structural disadvantage: A panel study of the impact of formal
criminal
labeling on the transition to adulthood. PhD Dissertation.
University
at Albany, State University of New York.
Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, and Marvin D. Krohn. 2003. Labeling,
life chances
and adult crime: The direct and indirect effects of official
intervention
in adolescence on crime in early adulthood. Criminology
41:1287–318.
Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, Marvin D. Krohn, and Craig J. Rivera.
2006. Offi-
cial labeling, criminal embeddedness, and subsequent
delinquency: A
longitudinal test of labeling theory. Journal of Research in
Crime and
Delinquency 43:67–88.
Blackwell, Brenda Sims, and Sarah Eschholz. 2002. Sex
differences and
rational choice: Traditional tests and new directions. In Rational
Choice and Criminal Behavior, eds. Alex R. Piquero and
Stephen G.
Tibbetts. New York: Routledge.
Braithwaite, John. 1989. Crime, Shame and Reintegration.
Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Bryk, Anthony S., and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 1992.
Hierarchical Linear
Models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 29
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 575
Bushway, Shawn, Brian D. Johnson, and Lee Ann Slocum. 2007.
Is the
magic still there? The use of the Heckman two-step correction
for
selection bias in criminology. Journal of Quantitative
Criminology
23:151-78.
Clear, Todd R. 1995. Harm in American Penology. Albany, NY:
SUNY
Press.
DeJong, Christine. 1997. Specific deterrence and survival
analysis: Inte-
grating theoretical and empirical models of recidivism.
Criminology
35:561–76.
Fagan, Jefferey A., Aaron Kupchick, and Akiva Liberman.
2003. Be care-
ful what you wish for. The comparative impacts of juvenile
versus crim-
inal court sanctions on recidivism among adolescent felony
offenders.
New York: Columbia Law School Public Law and Legal Theory
Work-
ing Group.
Farrington, David. 1977. The effects of public labeling. British
Journal of
Criminology 17:112–25.
Florida Department of Law Enforcement. 2005. Crime in
Florida, Florida
Uniform Crime Report, 2005. Tallahassee, FL: Florida
Statistical Anal-
ysis Center. http://www.fdle.state.fl.us/fsac/data_statistics.asp.
Gendreau, Paul, Tracy Little, and Claire Goggin. 1996. A meta-
analysis of
the predictors of adult offender recidivism: What works!
Criminology
34:575–607.
Gibbs, Jack. 1974. The effects of juvenile legal procedures on
juvenile
offenders’ self-attitudes. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency
11:51–5.
Giordano, Peggy C., Stephan A. Cernkovich, and Allen R.
Lowery. 2004.
A long-term follow-up of serious adolescent female offenders.
In
Aggression, Antisocial Behavior and Violence among Girls, eds.
Martha
Putallaz and Karen L. Bierman. New York: The Guilford Press.
Hagan, John, and Alberto Palloni. 1990. The social reproduction
of a crim-
inal class in working class London. American Journal of
Sociology
96:265–99.
Harris, Anthony. 1975. Imprisonment and the expected value of
criminal
choice: A specification and test of aspects of the labeling
perspective.
American Sociological Review 40:71–87.
Harris, Anthony. 1976. Race, commitment to deviance and
spoiled iden-
tity. American Sociological Review 41:432–42.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 30
22-AUG-07 10:10
576 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
Heckman, James J. 1979. Sample selection bias as a
specification error.
Econometrica 47:153–161.
Heimer, Karen, and Ross L. Matsueda. 1996. Role taking, role
commit-
ment and delinquency: A theory of differential social control.
Ameri-
can Sociological Review 59:365–90.
Hirschi, Travis. 1969. Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley:
University of Cali-
fornia Press.
Horwitz, Allan, and Michael Wasserman. 1979. The effect of
social control
on delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test. Sociological Focus
12:53–70.
Hox, Joop. 2002. Multilevel Analyses: Techniques and
Applications. Mah-
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hull, Elizabeth A. 2006. The Disenfranchisement of Ex-Felons.
Philadel-
phia, PA: Temple University Press.
Jensen, Gary. 1972. Delinquency and adolescent self-
conceptions: A study
of the personal relevance of infraction. Social Problems 20:84–
103.
Kruttschnitt, Candace, Christopher Uggen, and Kelly Shelton.
2000.
Predictors of desistence among sex offenders: The interaction of
for-
mal and informal social controls. Justice Quarterly 47:61–87.
Kubrin, Charis E., and Eric A. Stewart. 2006. Predicting who
reoffends:
The neglected role of neighborhood context in recidivism
studies.
Criminology. 44:165–98.
Lofland, John. 1969. Deviance and Identity. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Pren-
tice-Hall.
Manza, Jeff, and Christopher Uggen. 2006. Locked Out: Felon
Disen-
franchisement and American Democracy. New York: Oxford
Univer-
sity Press.
Massey, Douglas S. 2001. Ecology comes back to sociology. In
Does It
Take a Village? Community Effects on Children, Adolescents
and Fami-
lies, eds. Alan Booth and Ann Crouter. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Matsueda, Ross L. 1992. Reflected appraisals, parental labeling
and delin-
quency: Specifying a symbolic interactionist theory. American
Journal
of Sociology 97:1577–611.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 31
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 577
Mauer, Marc, and Meda Chesney-Lind. 2002. Invisible
Punishment: The
Collateral Consequences of Mass Imprisonment. New York: The
New
Press.
Messerschmidt, James W. 1993. Masculinities and Crime.
Lanham, MD:
Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, Inc.
Moffitt, Robert A. 1999. New developments in econometric
methods for
labor market analysis. In Handbook of Labor Economics, vol.
III, eds.
Orley Ashenfelter and David Card. North Holland, NY: Elsevier
Science.
Morenoff, Jeffrey D., Robert J. Sampson, and Stephen W.
Raudenbush.
2001. Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy and the
spatial
dynamics of urban violence. Criminology 39:517–59.
Nagin, Daniel S. 1998. Criminal deterrence research at the
outset of the
twenty-first century. In Crime and Justice: A Review of
Research, vol.
XXIII, ed. Michael Tonry. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press.
Palamara, Frances, Francis T. Cullen, and Joanne C. Gertsten.
1986. The
effect of police and mental health intervention on juvenile
deviance:
Specifying contingencies in the impact of formal reaction.
Journal of
Health and Social Behavior 27:90–105.
Pate, Antony M., and Edwin E. Hamilton. 1992. Formal and
informal
deterrents to domestic violence: The Dade County spouse
assault
experiment. American Sociological Review 57:691–97.
Paternoster, Raymond. 1987. The deterrent effect of the
perceived cer-
tainty and severity of punishment: A review of the evidence and
issues.
Justice Quarterly 4:173–217.
Paternoster, Raymond, and Leeann Iovanni. 1989. The labeling
perspec-
tive and delinquency: An elaboration of the theory and an
assessment
of the evidence. Justice Quarterly 6:359–93.
Petersilia, Joan. 1985. Probation and felony offenders. Federal
Probation
49:4–9.
Raudenbush, Stephen, and Anthony Bryk. 2002. Hierarchical
Linear Mod-
els: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks,
CA:
Sage.
Ray, Melvin C., and William R. Downs. 1986. An empirical test
of labeling
theory using longitudinal data. Journal of Research in Crime
and Delin-
quency 23:169–94.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 32
22-AUG-07 10:10
578 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
Richards, Pamela, and Charles R. Tittle. 1981. Gender and
perceived
chances of arrest. Social Forces 59:1182–99.
Rountree, Pamela Wilcox, Kenneth C. Land, and Terance D.
Miethe.
1994. Macro-micro integration in the study of victimization: A
hierar-
chical logistic model analysis across Seattle neighborhoods.
Criminol-
ogy 32:387–414.
Rubin, Edward L. 1999. Minimizing Harm: A New Crime Policy
for
America. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 1993. Structural
variation in juve-
nile court processing: Inequality, the underclass and social
control.
Law and Society Review 27:285–311.
Sampson, Robert, and John H. Laub. 1997. A life-course theory
of cumu-
lative disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In
Developmental
Theories of Crime and Delinquency, ed. Terence P. Thornberry.
New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Sampson, Robert J., Jeffrey D. Morenoff, and Felton Earls.
1999. Beyond
social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for
children.
American Sociological Review 64:644–60.
Sampson, Robert J., Jeffrey D. Morenoff, and Thomas Gannon-
Rowley.
2002. Assessing neighborhood effects: Social processes and new
direc-
tions in research. Annual Review of Sociology 28:443–78.
Sampson, Robert J., Stephen Raudenbush, and Felton Earls.
1997. Neigh-
borhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective
efficacy.
Science 277:918–24.
Schur, Edwin M. 1983. Labeling Women Deviant. Philadelphia,
PA: Tem-
ple University Press.
Shakur, Sanyika. 1993. Monster: The Autobiography of an L.A.
Gang
Member. New York: The Atlantic Monthly Press.
Sherman, Lawrence W. 1993. Defiance, deterrence and
irrelevance: A the-
ory of the criminal sanction. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delin-
quency 30:445–73.
Sherman, Lawrence W., Douglas A. Smith, Janell D. Schmidt,
and Dennis
P. Rogan. 1992. Crime, punishment and stake in conformity:
Legal and
informal control of domestic violence. American Sociological
Review
57:680–90.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 33
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 579
Silver, Eric. 2000. Extending social disorganization theory: A
multi-level
approach to the study of violence among persons with mental
illnesses.
Criminology 38:1043–73.
Smith, Douglas A., and Patrick Gartin. 1989. Specifying
specific deter-
rence: The influence of arrest on future criminal activity.
American
Sociological Review 54:94–106.
Smith, Douglas A., and Raymond Paternoster. 1990. Formal
processing
and future delinquency: Deviance amplification as selection
artifact.
Law and Society Review 24:1109–31.
Spohn, Cassia, and David Holleran. 2002. The effect of
imprisonment on
recidivism rates of felony offenders: A focus on drug offenders.
Crimi-
nology 40:329–57.
Stafford, Mark, and Mark Warr. 1993. A reconceptualization of
general
and specific deterrence. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency
30:123–35.
Sung, Hung-En. 2003. Differential impact of deterrence vs.
rehabilitation
as drug interventions on recidivism after 36 months. Journal of
Offender Rehabilitation 37:95–108.
Taxman, Faye S., and Alex Piquero. 1998. On preventing drunk
driving
recidivism: An examination of rehabilitation and punishment
approaches. Journal of Criminal Justice 26:129–43.
Thistlethwaite, Amy, John Wooldredge, and David Gibbs. 1998.
Severity
of dispositions and domestic violence recidivism. Crime and
Delin-
quency 44:388–98.
Tittle, Charles. 1972. Institutional living and self esteem. Social
Problems
20:65–77.
Tittle, Charles. 1995. Control Balance: Toward a General
Theory of Devi-
ance. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Ulmer, Jeffery T., and Brian Johnson. 2004. Sentencing in
context: A mul-
tilevel analysis. Criminology 42:137–77.
Ventura, Lois A., and Gabrielle Davis. 2005. Domestic
violence: Court
case conviction and recidivism. Violence Against Women
11:255–77.
Warr, Mark. 2001. The social origins of crime: Edwin
Sutherland and the
theory of differential association. In Explaining Crime and
Criminals,
eds. Raymond Paternoster and Ronet Bachman. Los Angeles,
CA:
Roxbury Publishing Company.
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 34
22-AUG-07 10:10
580 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
West, Donald, and David Farrington. 1973. Who Becomes
Delinquent?
London: Heinemann Educational.
Western, Bruce. 2002. The impact of incarceration on wage
mobility and
inequality. American Sociological Review 67:477–98.
Western, Bruce, and Becky Pettit. 2005. Black-white wage
inequality,
employment rates and incarceration. American Journal of
Sociology
111:553–78.
Whitehead, John T. 1991. The effectiveness of felony probation:
Results
from an eastern state. Justice Quarterly 8:525–43.
Wilson, William J. 1987. The Truly Disadvantaged. Chicago,
IL: The Uni-
versity of Chicago Press.
Word, David L., and Colby R. Perkins. 1996. Building a
Spanish surname
list for the 1990s—a new approach to an old problem. U.S.
Census
Bureau Technical Working Paper No. 13. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Commerce.
Ted Chiricos is the William Julius Wilson Professor of
Criminology in the
College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at Florida State
University.
His research interests include the effects of race and social
threat on jus-
tice outcomes, as well as the various factors that contribute to
punitiveness
in American culture.
Kelle Barrick is a doctoral student in the College of
Criminology and
Criminal Justice at Florida State University. Her current
interests include
criminological theory; the links among race, class, gender, and
crime; and
corporate crime. She is also the Richard Rachin Fellow for the
Journal of
Drug Issues.
William Bales is an associate professor in the College of
Criminology and
Criminal Justice at Florida State University. His interests
include sentenc-
ing research, assessing the effectiveness and consequences of
punishment
and correctional strategies, and community reentry issues
among incarcer-
ated adult and juvenile populations.
Stephanie Bontrager recently received her Ph.D. degree in
criminology
from Florida State University. She is a senior research analyst
with the
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 35
22-AUG-07 10:10
THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 581
Justice Research Center in Tallahassee, Florida and serves as
the project
director for the National Council on Crime and Delinquency
Tracking and
Follow-up Evaluation. Her research interests include the
relationship
between social threat and social control, the interaction between
macro
forms of social control, and the effects of economic, racial, and
gender
inequality on crime.
10.1177/0022427805280068ARTICLEJournal of Research in
Crime and DelinquencyBernburg et al. / Labeling Theory
Official Labeling,
Criminal Embeddedness,
and Subsequent
Delinquency
A Longitudinal Test of Labeling Theory
Jón Gunnar Bernburg
University of Iceland and Icelandic Research Council
Marvin D. Krohn
University at Albany–SUNY
Craig J. Rivera
Niagara University
This article examines the short-term impact of formal criminal
labeling on
involvement in deviant social networks and increased likelihood
of subsequent
delinquency. According to labeling theory, formal criminal
intervention
should affect the individual’s immediate social networks. In
many cases, the
stigma of the criminal status may increase the probability that
the individual
becomes involved in deviant social groups. The formal label
may thus ulti-
mately increase involvement in subsequent deviance. We use
panel data of a
sample of urban adolescents to examine whether involvement in
deviant social
groups mediates the relationship between juvenile justice
intervention and
subsequent delinquent behavior. Using measures from three
successive points
in time, the authors find that juvenile justice intervention
positively affects
subsequent involvement in serious delinquency through the
medium of
involvement in deviant social groups, namely, street gangs and
delinquent
peers.
Keywords: labeling theory; deviant peers; delinquency
In recent years, there has been a revived interest in the labeling
approach inthe field of criminology. After a period of criticism
and rejection of label-
ing theory (see Goode 1975; Hirschi 1980; Tittle 1980),
scholars have
repeatedly pointed out that by modifying the theory and
elaborating on the
social processes involved, the labeling approach can
complement some of
67
Journal of Research in Crime
and Delinquency
Volume 43 Number 1
February 2006 67-88
© 2006 Sage Publications
10.1177/0022427805280068
http://jrc.sagepub.com
hosted at
http://online.sagepub.com
at UTSA Libraries on November 12,
2015jrc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jrc.sagepub.com/
the more established theories of deviant behavior (Paternoster
and Iovanni
1989; Sampson and Laub 1997). A prominent theme that has
reemerged
in revisionist work on labeling theory has been an emphasis on
the social-
structural consequences of deviant labeling that trigger
processes leading to
movement into deviant groups (Bernburg and Krohn 2003;
Paternoster and
Iovanni 1989; Sampson and Laub 1997; Zhang and Messner
1994). In light
of the role that the individual’s social ties to unconventional
groups play in
criminological theory (Thornberry and Krohn 1997; Warr 2002),
such pro-
cesses should be of great interest to criminology. Yet existing
research bear-
ing upon this line of investigation has been both limited and
inconclusive
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx
server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx

More Related Content

Similar to server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx

ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxaudeleypearl
 
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxlorent8
 
Senior Thesis- New Thesis version
Senior Thesis- New Thesis versionSenior Thesis- New Thesis version
Senior Thesis- New Thesis versionLance Hagen
 
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docx
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docxAge, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docx
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docxnettletondevon
 
Gender differences in risk assessmen
Gender differences in risk assessmenGender differences in risk assessmen
Gender differences in risk assessmenJose Avila De Tomas
 
Jeff ifrah get familiarize yourself with variety of laws
Jeff ifrah  get familiarize yourself with variety of lawsJeff ifrah  get familiarize yourself with variety of laws
Jeff ifrah get familiarize yourself with variety of lawsJeff Ifrah
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaAlejandro Luna
 
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docxRacial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docxpoulterbarbara
 
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
 Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docxgertrudebellgrove
 
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docx
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docxPART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docx
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docxdanhaley45372
 
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduce
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduceWhy restorative justice_will_not_reduce
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduceBARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile Jus
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile JusBlended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile Jus
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile JusChantellPantoja184
 
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*UsamaShabir11
 
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALDissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALRyan Cockrell
 
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docx
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docxRunning head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docx
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docxcowinhelen
 

Similar to server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx (18)

ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
 
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docxReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
ReferencesBarrenger, S., Draine, J., Angell, B., & Herman, D. (2.docx
 
Senior Thesis- New Thesis version
Senior Thesis- New Thesis versionSenior Thesis- New Thesis version
Senior Thesis- New Thesis version
 
DMC Final
DMC FinalDMC Final
DMC Final
 
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docx
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docxAge, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docx
Age, RacialEthnic Minority Status, Genderand Misdemeanor Se.docx
 
Gender differences in risk assessmen
Gender differences in risk assessmenGender differences in risk assessmen
Gender differences in risk assessmen
 
Jeff ifrah get familiarize yourself with variety of laws
Jeff ifrah  get familiarize yourself with variety of lawsJeff ifrah  get familiarize yourself with variety of laws
Jeff ifrah get familiarize yourself with variety of laws
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
 
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docxRacial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
 
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
 Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
Racial Characteristics and the Imposition of the Death Pe.docx
 
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docx
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docxPART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docx
PART TWENTY-SEVENCONSENSUS AND CONFORMITYMARITAL STATUS .docx
 
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduce
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduceWhy restorative justice_will_not_reduce
Why restorative justice_will_not_reduce
 
The Right to Privacy
The Right to PrivacyThe Right to Privacy
The Right to Privacy
 
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile Jus
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile JusBlended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile Jus
Blended Sentencing Laws and the PunitiveTurn in Juvenile Jus
 
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
 
WritingSample1-499
WritingSample1-499WritingSample1-499
WritingSample1-499
 
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALDissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
 
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docx
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docxRunning head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docx
Running head INEFFECTIVENESS OF THE CAPITAL PUNISHMENT SYSTEM1.docx
 

More from klinda1

Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docx
Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docxSetting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docx
Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docxklinda1
 
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docx
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docxSetting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docx
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docxklinda1
 
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docx
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docxSetting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docx
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docxklinda1
 
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docx
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docxSet up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docx
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docxklinda1
 
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docx
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docxSet up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docx
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docxklinda1
 
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docx
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docxset FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docx
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docxklinda1
 
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docx
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docxSession 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docx
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docxklinda1
 
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docx
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docxSession 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docx
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docxklinda1
 
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docx
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docxSession 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docx
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docxklinda1
 
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docx
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docxSession 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docx
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docxklinda1
 
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docx
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docxService-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docx
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docxklinda1
 
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docx
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docxService Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docx
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docxklinda1
 
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docx
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docxServer FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docx
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docxklinda1
 
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docx
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docxService Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docx
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docxklinda1
 
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docx
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docxShare your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docx
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docxklinda1
 
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docx
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docxShareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docx
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docxklinda1
 
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docx
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docxShare your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docx
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docxklinda1
 
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docx
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docxShare your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docx
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docxklinda1
 
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docx
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docxShare your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docx
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docxklinda1
 
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docx
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docxShare your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docx
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docxklinda1
 

More from klinda1 (20)

Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docx
Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docxSetting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docx
Setting in Fiction       Think of your favorite story, boo.docx
 
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docx
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docxSetting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docx
Setting the Agenda © 2018 Laureate Education, Inc. 1 S.docx
 
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docx
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docxSetting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docx
Setting Goals and ObjectivesGilbert Burnham, MDJohns Hop.docx
 
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docx
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docxSet up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docx
Set up and diagram an Incident Command System for the following .docx
 
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docx
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docxSet up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docx
Set up a Ricardo-type comparative advantage numerical example with.docx
 
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docx
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docxset FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docx
set FOREIGN_KEY_CHECKS=false;-- --------------------------.docx
 
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docx
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docxSession 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docx
Session 3 Research Paper - Artist and InspirationThe author .docx
 
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docx
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docxSession 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docx
Session 2 Creativity Reflection PaperIn Echoes of Eden (.docx
 
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docx
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docxSession 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docx
Session 2 Business Strategies based on Value ChainAg.docx
 
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docx
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docxSession 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docx
Session 1 Module 2INTRODUCTION TO AUDITING .docx
 
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docx
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docxService-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docx
Service-Oriented Architecture Please respond to the followingSe.docx
 
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docx
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docxService Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docx
Service Project Consent LetterDear SirMadam,I wo.docx
 
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docx
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docxServer FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docx
Server FarmIP PhoneEnd-usersCorporate Computers.docx
 
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docx
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docxService Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docx
Service Area- The geographic area from which organization dr.docx
 
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docx
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docxShare your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docx
Share your written proposal with your manager, supervisor or other c.docx
 
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docx
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docxShareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docx
Shareholder or stakeholder That is the question.In recent years.docx
 
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docx
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docxShare your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docx
Share your thoughts and opinions on predictive versus adaptive S.docx
 
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docx
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docxShare your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docx
Share your thoughts on Chapters 4 and 5. How much experience do you .docx
 
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docx
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docxShare your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docx
Share your thoughtsYou are the most important generation. EVER..docx
 
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docx
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docxShare your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docx
Share your thoughts on some of the questions below. Research is not .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Mark Reed
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxLigayaBacuel1
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Celine George
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxGrade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxChelloAnnAsuncion2
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxGrade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptxRaw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 

server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq 1 22-AU.docx

  • 1. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 1 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS AND ITS CONSEQUENCES FOR RECIDIVISM* TED CHIRICOS KELLE BARRICK WILLIAM BALES College of Criminology and Criminal Justice Florida State University STEPHANIE BONTRAGER Justice Research Associates KEYWORDS: labeling, felony conviction, recidivism Florida law allows judges to withhold adjudication of guilt for individ- uals who have been found guilty of a felony and are being sentenced to probation. Such individuals lose no civil rights and may lawfully assert they had not been convicted of a felony. Labeling theory would predict that the receipt of a felony label could increase the likelihood of recidi- vism. Reconviction data for 95,919 men and women who were either adjudicated or had adjudication withheld show that those
  • 2. formally labeled are significantly more likely to recidivate in 2 years than those who are not. Labeling effects are stronger for women, whites, and those who reach the age of 30 years without a prior conviction. Second-level indicators of county characteristics (e.g., crime rates or concentrated disadvantage) have no significant effect on the adjudication/recidivism relationship. Traditional labeling theory explains the potential “escalating” conse- quences of a criminal or delinquent labeling experience in two ways (Lof- land, 1969; Sherman et al., 1992). The first consequence involves a * The authors would like to thank the anonymous reviewers as well as Carter Hay, Dan Mears, Brian Stults, and especially Xia Wang for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this article. Direct correspondence to Ted Chiricos (e-mail: [email protected]). CRIMINOLOGY VOLUME 45 NUMBER 3 2007 547 server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 2 22-AUG-07 10:10 548 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
  • 3. transformation of identity,1 and the second emphasizes structural impedi- ments to conventional life that result from a labeling event.2 Although labeling events have been variably operationalized to include police con- tact, arrest, conviction, and imprisonment, it is arguable that felony convic- tion is the most consequential in relation to the development of structural impediments. The label of “convicted felon” strips an individual of the right to vote, serve on juries, own firearms, or hold public office. In many states, convicted felons are prohibited from obtaining student loans, employment in state-licensed occupations, or employment with state- licensed companies. In addition, the label of convicted felon may contrib- ute to various informal exclusions that can make access to noncriminal activities more difficult and criminal alternatives more attractive.3 The state of Florida has a law that allows individuals who have been found guilty of a felony, either by a judge, jury, or plea, to literally avoid the label of convicted felon. Judges have the option of “withholding adju- dication” of guilt for convicted felons who are being sentenced to proba- tion. The consequence of this unique labeling event is that offenders who
  • 4. are equivalent in terms of factual guilt can either be labeled a convicted felon or not. For those offenders who have adjudication withheld—half of the felony probationers in Florida in recent years—no civil rights are lost and such individuals may legitimately say on employment applications and elsewhere that a felony conviction did not occur. For those offenders who are formally adjudicated, all of the structural impediments of being a con- victed felon are possible. Labeling theory would hypothesize that being formally adjudicated should increase the chances of recidivism. The research question that we address here is whether the labeling event of adjudication or the withhold- ing of adjudication has any effect on subsequent recidivism.4 Drawing on 1. Early research involving self-esteem or “self-typing” was reported by Ageton and Elliott (1974), Gibbs (1974), Harris (1976), Jensen (1972), and Tittle (1972), among others. More recently, “reflected appraisals of self” (Heimer and Mat- sueda, 1996; Matsueda, 1992) have been implicated in identity- related labeling research. 2. For an excellent discussion of structural impediments related to labeling and sup- portive research, see Sampson and Laub (1997: 147–52).
  • 5. 3. Discussions of the collateral consequences of felony conviction have primarily focused on the loss of voting rights (Hull, 2006; Manza and Uggen, 2006; Uggen and Manza, 2002). But a broader range of consequences from imprisonment, which presumes conviction, has also been described (Mauer and Chesney-Lind, 2002; Western, 2002; Western and Pettit, 2005). 4. It is also possible, as specific deterrence theory would hypothesize, that being formally adjudicated could have the opposite effect of reducing the chances of recidivism. For discussions of specific deterrence, see, for example, Paternoster (1987), Nagin (1998), and Stafford and Warr (1993). At the same time, a positive relationship between formal sanctioning and subsequent criminal activity could server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 3 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 549 recent theory and research, we also examine whether any labeling effects on recidivism are contingent on characteristics of the defendant, such as sex, race, or prior record, as well as on characteristics of the county to which the sentenced defendant returns, such as rates of crime or
  • 6. levels of social disadvantage. To assess these questions, we examine the post- sentencing recidivism experience of 95,919 men and women who were found guilty of a violent, property, or drug felony and sentenced to proba- tion in Florida between 2000 and 2002. This study is the first to examine the potential labeling effects of adult felony conviction. Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) is used to assess the direct effect of having adjudication applied or withheld, as well as to assess the effects of other individual-level variables. In addition, we assess the direct effect of county-level characteristics on recidivism and the conditioning relevance that these second-level county characteristics may have on the relationship between adjudication status and recidivism. LABELING EFFECTS: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES A recurrent theme in the recent renewal of interest in labeling effects (Bernburg and Krohn, 2003; Bernburg, Krohn, and Rivera, 2006; Sampson and Laub, 1997) is the recognition that consequences of formal sanctions may be contingent on the characteristics of offenders. As noted by Pater- noster and Iovanni (1989: 381), “we should not expect labeling effects to be invariant across societal subgroups.” With this in mind, four
  • 7. individual- level contingencies will inform parts of our analysis. They are race, sex, prior record, and “stakes in conformity.” Their conceptual relevance for potentially observed labeling effects is reviewed briefly here. Race was one of the first issues to be raised in discussions of contingent labeling outcomes. Harris (1976: 433) argued that the effects of official labeling would be least consequential for those individuals most excluded from “ascriptive membership” in dominant groups—particularly minori- ties. Earlier, Jensen (1972) as well as Ageton and Elliott (1974) hypothe- sized that higher status persons—presumably including non- minorities in most contexts—may be more susceptible to labeling effects. Similarly, Paternoster and Iovanni suggested that persons who grant less “legitimacy to the legal order and its agents” may, as a result, be “more impervious to labeling effects” than others (1989: 382). Alternatively, although not referencing race per se, Sampson and Laub (1997: 153) discussed the potential life-course consequences of official sanctions for the disadvantaged urban poor. They noted that “among the be the consequence of factors other than those attributed to labeling. Among
  • 8. these alternatives could be general strain (Agnew, 2006), defiance (Sherman, 1993), and control–balance (Tittle, 1995). server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 4 22-AUG-07 10:10 550 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER disadvantaged, things seem to work differently. Deficits and disadvantages pile up faster and this has continuing negative consequences for later development. . . .” Building on that argument, Bernburg and Krohn (2003) hypothesized that labeling effects would be strongest among “disadvan- taged offenders” as distinguished by minority and poverty status. Their expectations were further supported by the observation that official label- ing of disadvantaged youth could be “enhanced by the negative stereo- types that are already associated with these youths in mainstream culture” (2003: 1290). The relevance of sex for labeling outcomes has been substantially over- looked in criminological discourse. Even a book with the promising title of Labeling Women Deviant (Schur, 1983) makes no mention of whether or how labeling effects for women would be different from those
  • 9. for men. Regarding juveniles, Ageton and Elliott (1974) hypothesized that “males are more likely to be affected negatively by police contact than females,” but no rationale was associated with that expectation. Somewhat later, Ray and Downs (1986), also in the context of adolescents, articulated the opposite expectation. On the premise that “females are expected to be more attentive to interpersonal relationships than men,” they hypothe- sized that “labels may exert more of an influence on behavior for females than males” (1986: 171). The possibility that women could be more adversely affected by labeling than men has also been noted in passing references that were concerned primarily with broader issues. For example, in his discussion of “gender blind criminology,” Messerschmidt (1993) conjectured how females might be factored into traditional criminological theories if one took gender into account. In this context, he speculated that because men exercise greater power in society than women, “men should therefore have increased opportunity to counteract official labeling . . .” (1993: 4). Similarly, Gior- dano, Cernkovich, and Lowery (2004) were developing alternative expec- tations concerning the relative prospects of women as compared
  • 10. with men for desistance from crime, without specific reference to formal labeling. In that context, they observed that “the greater social stigma attached to female in contrast to male involvement in antisocial behavior” could in the long run “be more limiting to life chances/opportunities for a return to conventional roles” (2004: 189). Interestingly, the antithesis to labeling, deterrence, which anticipates that official sanctions will reduce rather than amplify criminal involve- ment, has generally argued that women are more likely to be deterred than men because they have more to lose if caught committing a crime. The argument has been made that women have been socialized to feel more responsible for social relationships than men (Blackwell and Eschholz, 2002) and to have a “greater investment in conformity” than is server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 5 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 551 true for men (Richards and Tittle, 1981), both of which could presumably be lost as a consequence of a formal criminal sanction. If
  • 11. sanctions are expected to have stronger deterrent effects for women than for men, then logically, labeling effects should be weaker for women than they are for men. However, these arguments have been made in the context of general deterrence and not specific deterrence, which would be more appropriate here. Expectations concerning the relevance of prior record for potential labeling effects have not been extensively developed, but a limited consen- sus favors the prospect that the consequences of formal sanctions will be greater for naı̈ ve, as opposed to repeat offenders. Noting that some ana- lysts have speculated that novice criminals would be more susceptible to deterrence, Horwitz and Wasserman (1979) offered “an alternative hypothesis.” They suggested that punishments of “increasing severity will lead to a greater increase in the rate of subsequent criminal offenses for primary than for secondary deviants” (1979: 57). Smith and Gartin (1989) articulated a similar hypothesis, although like Horwitz and Wasserman, they provided no supporting rationale. What that rationale could be is sug- gested by Paternoster and Iovanni (1989: 386) who observed that once an individual has been labeled, “it is doubtful that further
  • 12. increments in label- ing will continue to produce further deviance.” They attributed this occur- rence to a “leveling off” of labeling effects on the premise that once an individual has experienced social exclusions on the basis of a label, “it is not unreasonable to assume that further exclusion would have little addi- tional meaning . . .” (1989: 386). Regarding “stakes in conformity,” Sherman et al. have shown that the issue has actually been used to hypothesize contradictory labeling effects. What they characterize as the “greater vulnerability” version of labeling theory expects that those individuals who “care more about the opinions of conventional society” will be more vulnerable to the escalating effects of labeling (1992: 682). From this point of view, nonminorities and higher status individuals would presumably be more vulnerable to labeling effects because they have more to lose. But it has been argued that stakes in con- formity could have the opposite consequence. Specifically, persons with a higher stake in conformity, such as those who are employed and/or mar- ried, could be more insulated from the negative consequences of official labels. Sherman et al. (1992) refer to this position as the “less vulnerabil- ity” approach to labeling effects. In this view, those individuals
  • 13. who have more to lose because of their informal social bonds may as a consequence have “other social resources that overcome the impact of labeling” (1992: 682). This “less vulnerability” position is implicit in the observation by Samp- son and Laub (1997: 152) that “sanctioning tends to aggravate crime server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 6 22-AUG-07 10:10 552 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER among populations with low ‘stakes in conformity’.” They also note, with Braithwaite (1989) and Sherman (1993), that criminal sanctions may actu- ally promote future defiance of the law when applied to those individuals with weaker social bonds to conventional society (Sampson and Laub, 1997: 153). Such a position is consistent with recent ethnographic (Ander- son, 1999) and autobiographic (Shakur, 1993) accounts of the effects of official sanctions on individuals from highly disadvantaged inner-city envi- ronments who presumably have fewer stakes in conformity than others.
  • 14. COMMUNITY-LEVEL CONTEXTS However much individuals with particular characteristics may vary in their vulnerability to labeling effects, Paternoster and Iovanni (1989: 373) remind us that “the effect of status characteristics on labeling outcomes is not invariant, but varies substantially across different social contexts.” In a related manner, Sampson and Laub (1997: 153) noted that in some social environments, “deficits and disadvantages pile up faster” and as a result “one cannot ignore the effects of larger social contexts” in studying the life-course development of individuals who may have experienced official labeling and some of its negative consequences. Kubrin and Stewart (2006: 172) have similarly argued that “where ex-offenders live greatly affects their ability to reintegrate into society” after being formally sanctioned. For these reasons, we have chosen to examine several social contexts aggregated at the county level that may interact with individual traits in producing greater or lesser labeling effects. To our knowledge, this study is the first study of labeling outcomes to do so. Specifically, we include in our analyses measures of concentrated disadvantage (CD), concentrated extremes (affluence and poverty), index crime rates, and police
  • 15. presence. The conceptual relevance of each measure for potentially influencing labeling outcomes is briefly reviewed here. As noted by Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls (1997), among others, CD is a measure of “underclass concentration,” which involves high levels of minority presence, poverty, welfare, and female-headed families. Such places not only have frequently increased levels of crime (Rountree, Land, and Miethe, 1994; Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls, 1997; Silver, 2000), but they also have low levels of neighborhood resources and social ser- vices (Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley, 2002; Wilson, 1987). In addition, as Sampson and Laub (1993: 295) have noted, “counties charac- terized by. . .a large concentration of the ‘underclass’. . .are more likely than other counties to be perceived as containing offensive and threaten- ing populations” and so are more likely to be more heavily policed. Kubrin and Stewart (2006) hypothesized and found higher levels of recidivism among felons released to community supervision in places with higher levels of CD. For these reasons, we might expect that those individuals
  • 16. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 7 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 553 who are labeled as convicted felons would see “deficits and disadvan- tages”—specifically recidivism—“pile up” more often in places that have higher levels of CD. Whatever the value of taking account of the negative consequences of CD, Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley (2002: 446) have suggested that the common tactic of focusing on concentrated disadvantage may “. . .obscure the potential protective effects of affluent neighborhoods.” Morenoff, Sampson, and Raudenbush (2001: 528) have also noted that “the resources that affluent neighborhoods can mobilize are theoretically relevant to understanding the activation of social control . . . .” They found that a measure of concentrated affluence relative to poverty (index of con- centrated extremes, ICE) was related to lower levels of homicide. Consis- tent with that, Kubrin and Stewart (2006) hypothesized and showed that ICE was negatively related to the likelihood of recidivism for ex-offenders in Portland, Oregon. On that basis, we might expect that the negative effects of a felony convict label could be diminished in places
  • 17. with higher levels of ICE. However, the ICE is really a measure of “relative inequality” (Kubrin and Stewart, 2006: 177) and not of “relative affluence” as operationalized by Sampson, Morenoff, and Earls (1999). It is reasonable to imagine that with extremes of poverty and affluence coexisting in the same county, the “protective effects of affluent neighborhoods” (Sampson, Morenoff, and Gannon-Rowley, 2002) could lead to “the activation of social control” (Morenoff, Sampson, and Raudenbush, 2001) in ways that result in more aggressive policing and prosecution, especially in poorer sections of the county. In that context, one might expect higher levels of ICE to result in higher levels of formal social control and recidivism generally and in an amplification of the effects of being labeled as a convicted felon. We also expect that convicted felons will be more likely to recidivate in places with higher index crime rates and with a stronger law enforcement presence. The rationale for the former expectation is that places with higher levels of crime, which will also likely have increased levels of CD, have more criminal opportunities, learning environments, and role models
  • 18. (Akers, 1998; Anderson, 1999; Warr, 2001). Individuals who are labeled as convicted felons and who may experience social exclusions not exper- ienced by their nonlabeled counterparts could find it easier to resume criminal activity in such a context, rather than in places with lower levels of crime. This occurrence may explain why, for example, Smith and Pater- noster (1990) found that the likelihood of recidivism for those individuals who had been referred to juvenile court in Florida was higher in counties with higher crime rates. The expectation concerning higher recidivism for convicted felons in places with a stronger police presence is predicated on Lofland’s (1969) server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 8 22-AUG-07 10:10 554 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER concept of “prevalent sensitivity.” His argument is that labels— in this case, recidivating felon—are more likely to be “imputed” in places where a higher prevalent sensitivity to misbehavior exists because more “special- ists” are deployed to detect and respond to it. In his terms: “as the number
  • 19. of imputational specialists increases . . . it is likely that the number of peo- ple imputed as deviant will also increase” (1969: 136). So if individuals labeled as convicted felons resume criminal activity, it seems more likely that they would be caught and officially recidivate where more police officers are present. As a final conceptual issue, it can be noted that every adult facing a felony conviction, whether or not adjudication is withheld, has been arrested at least once and prosecuted. Assuming that those experiences constitute labeling events, the ideal situation described by Bernburg and Krohn (2003) of comparing samples from the general (unlabeled) popula- tion to assess “absolute labeling” effects is not available for the label of convicted felon. However, when it comes to generating the structural impediments that can accrue from labeling, the event of felony conviction, although “relative,” may nonetheless be relatively more important than any other for adults. Certainly, police contact, arrest, and prosecution do not cause individuals to lose their civil rights or to be legally excluded from the great variety of occupational opportunities denied to those indi- viduals who are convicted felons. By controlling for prior convictions, both
  • 20. adult and juvenile, an analysis of the consequences of having adjudication withheld or applied for convicted felons affords an estimate of the inde- pendent effect of “being labeled or not” in a relative labeling context that is most critical. LABELING EFFECTS: PRIOR RESEARCH Several systematic reviews of labeling effects research (Barrick, 2005; Bernburg, 2002; Palamara, Cullen, and Gertsten, 1986; Paternoster and Iovanni, 1989) underscore several things. First, much evidence that can be inferred as relating to labeling is actually sanction effects research, which is often primarily concerned with deterrence. More important, extraordi- nary methodological variation exists in this research, in terms of both the specification of independent and dependent variables and the elaboration of contingent or subsample analyses. This variation makes a brief sum- mary of any research results or any patterns of support or nonsupport for labeling hypotheses all but impossible. Thus, we limit our discussion of prior research here to that which is most relevant to the analyses that we have undertaken. Specifically, we review studies that have examined “con- viction” as the key labeling event, and we review some contingent results
  • 21. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 9 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 555 of labeling research that anticipate the kinds of analyses that we describe below. CONVICTION EFFECTS RESEARCH We are aware of five studies that isolate conviction as a key labeling event. Two studies address misdemeanor charges of domestic violence (Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs, 1998; Ventura and Davis, 2005), another deals with drunk driving (Taxman and Piquero, 1998), and two are concerned with the “conviction” of juveniles for serious offenses (Fagan, Kupchik, and Liberman, 2003; Hagan and Palloni, 1990). To our knowl- edge, no research to date has examined the labeling effect of a felony con- viction for adults, which is the focus of the current study. The two domestic violence studies that examine conviction effects involved midwestern cities and used rearrest within 1 year for another domestic violence charge as a measure of recidivism. Multivariate logistic
  • 22. analyses in both studies showed that conviction significantly reduced the likelihood of recidivism, thus supporting a deterrent effect more than a labeling effect (Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs, 1998: 394; Ventura and Davis, 2005: 270). Taxman and Piquero (1998) estimated the consequences of more than 3,500 “drunk driving convictions” in Maryland between 1985 and 1993. They measured recidivism by reconviction for drunk driving within 3 years. Their primary concern was to distinguish between the effects of punitive as opposed to rehabilitative responses by the court. But they also made an important distinction—from a labeling perspective— between those who received a “guilty disposition” and those who received “proba- tion before judgement” (PBJ). The latter seems to be similar to the “adju- dication withheld” option that we examine for felony convictions, inasmuch as getting PBJ meant that one’s conviction was “stayed” and effectively expunged from the public record if probation was successfully completed (Taxman and Piquero, 1998: 133). The authors report that for all offenders, conviction increased the risk of recidivism by 12 percent but that effect was not significant with other con-
  • 23. trols included. Separate analyses for first-time offenders showed that a guilty disposition increased the risk of recidivism by 27 percent, and this was the only significant punishment or rehabilitation effect for such offenders (Taxman and Piquero, 1998: 135–7). This finding supports an interpretation that conviction for drunk driving had a labeling effect that was contingent on prior record, which is an issue that we discuss more fully in the next section. Hagan and Palloni (1990) reanalyzed data from the Cambridge Study in server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 10 22-AUG-07 10:10 556 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER Delinquency Development that were originally collected by West and Far- rington (1973). The primary dependent variables were self- reports of delinquency and crime (38 items) that were generated by interviews with randomly chosen London youth. The interviews were conducted over a period of years when the boys were between 8 and 24 years of age. For those who were interviewed, official data from the Criminal Record Office
  • 24. were used to create matched samples of youth who had been “convicted” by the age of 15 years and those who had not (Farrington, 1977: 114).5 In looking at self-reported delinquency subsequent to the experience of conviction, Hagan and Palloni (1990) controlled for individual and family attributes that could be taken as indicators of “characterological and cul- tural” traits that might predict delinquent involvement. They also con- trolled for prior reported delinquency. A key finding was that being convicted before the age of 15 years significantly increased the likelihood of delinquency at ages 16–17, 18–19, and 21–22 years independent of the effects of all other controls. This result clearly supports the expectation that the labeling experience of conviction contributes to subsequent delin- quent and criminal involvement (1990: 278–9).6 Fagan, Kupchick, and Liberman (2003) compared the consequences for juveniles of being convicted in an adult criminal court with those of being convicted in a juvenile court for felony assault, robbery, or burglary. To do this, they matched counties from northern New Jersey and from New York, which were all part of the same New York metro SMSA. From the New Jersey counties, they examined the consequences of
  • 25. juvenile court conviction, and from the New York counties, the same was done for crimi- nal court conviction. They tracked rearrest and reincarceration outcomes for at least 2 years after the completion of punishment for those convicted. An outcome called “any court action,” which the authors describe as “analogous to conviction,” was found to significantly increase the chances of rearrest for violent, property, and weapons crimes (not drugs) for youth convicted in either the juvenile or the criminal courts. However, it was also found that adolescent offenders convicted in criminal court were rear- rested and reincarcerated more often and more quickly for violent, prop- erty, and weapons offenses than those convicted in juvenile court. The reverse was true for drug crimes (Fagan, Kupchick, and Liberman, 2003: 69). In sum, the available evidence on whether conviction is related to 5. Farrington (1977: 114) describes the conviction data in this manner: “searches of the Criminal Record Office” uncovered a number of youth who “had been found guilty in court. . . .” The crimes for which these youth had been convicted “were mainly burglaries, thefts and unauthorized takings of motor vehicles.”
  • 26. 6. The authors also report a significant interactive effect of parent and son convic- tion on subsequent delinquency, which was the primary theoretical focus of their study (Hagan and Palloni, 1990: 278–9). server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 11 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 557 recidivism is limited to juveniles, misdemeanor domestic violence offend- ers, and first-offending drunk drivers. How felony conviction plays out in the life course of adults remains to be seen. CONTINGENT LABELING EFFECTS RESEARCH It should be noted that some studies reporting what we call contingent labeling effects are not labeling studies per se. In fact, several of them are primarily concerned with specific (as distinct from general) deterrence, but if they report a relationship with recidivism subsequent to a sanctioning experience, we designate that as a potential labeling effect. One consistent contingency seems to be prior record. As noted, Taxman and Piquero’s study (1998: 135–7) of drunk driving convictions found sub- stantially stronger labeling effects (increased recidivism) for
  • 27. first-time offenders than for their full sample. Similarly, Fagan, Kupchick, and Liber- man (2003: 41) report that the relatively stronger labeling effects (recidi- vism) for youth convicted in criminal as opposed to juvenile court are especially pronounced for those with no record of prior arrests. Horwitz and Wasserman (1979) found that more severe sanctions were related to higher rates of rearrest for juveniles in Newark, but that effect held only for those identified as first-time offenders on the basis of arrest records. DeJong (1997) analyzed the rearrest likelihood of those either sentenced to jail or not for misdemeanors7 in New York City. She reported that the experience of incarceration increased recidivism risk but only for first-time arrestees (1997: 571). The pattern of observed labeling effects being con- centrated among first offenders is consistent with the suggestion by Pater- noster and Iovanni (1989) that incremental labeling events may be less consequential than those that occur earlier in the life course of an individual. Stakes in conformity have also been studied in relation to sanction effects. In general, what has been found is that apparent labeling effects are stronger for those who have a low stake in conformity or
  • 28. weak ties to conventional society. DeJong (1997) reported that incarceration increased the likelihood of rearrest especially for those with “low stakes” as indi- cated by having at least two of these traits: unmarried, unemployed, and not a high-school graduate. Sherman et al. (1992: 686) found that formal arrest for domestic violence significantly increased the likelihood of rear- rest in Milwaukee among those individuals who had a low stake in con- formity as measured by marital status and employment. Two additional 7. Her sample was taken from “criminal court” as opposed to “Supreme Court” cases, which in New York involves primarily misdemeanors or felony cases that are negotiated down. Felonies that are not negotiated down are tried in the Supreme Court. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 12 22-AUG-07 10:10 558 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER domestic violence studies focused on those individuals with “high” stakes and concluded that sanctions for these people are associated with less recidivism, which the authors conclude is a stronger deterrent
  • 29. effect (Pate and Hamilton, 1992; Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs, 1998). The negative relationship between stakes and recidivism for those sanctioned at least implies that those with lower stakes would experience higher recidivism (labeling effect) as reported by Dejong (1997) and Sherman et al. (1992). A third contingency that has received some attention is race. Several early studies found that labeling was more consequential for white juveniles than for others. Ageton and Elliott (1974) reported the race-spe- cific effect for police contact and self-reported “delinquency orientation” among California youth. Harris (1975) found that New Jersey youth who had been incarcerated for longer periods expressed a higher “relative expected value of criminal choice” but that relationship held only for whites. Harris (1975: 97) hypothesized that the label might mean more to white youth because it came from a system “dominated by white . . . authorities,” whereas minority youth might perceive the label as “created and applied by ‘outsiders.’ ” In contrast, the most recent and sophisticated labeling work with juveniles has shown that juvenile justice intervention increased the chances of subsequent adult criminality only for
  • 30. blacks (Bernburg and Krohn, 2003: 1314). Two domestic violence intervention studies report conflicting results concerning race in relation to the effects of arrest. Sherman et al. (1992: 680) report that race did not condition the effect of arrest on subsequent domestic violence in Milwaukee. However, Berk et al. (1992: 705) found that for pooled data from four cities, the labeling effect of arrest on subse- quent offending was 11 percent higher for blacks than for whites and 47 percent higher for blacks who were unemployed than for employed whites. Two reviews of the recidivism literature conclude that males are consist- ently more likely than females to resume criminal activity after formal sanctioning (Baumer, 1997; Gendreau, Little, and Goggin, 1996). Consis- tent with this generalization, Giordano, Cernkovich, and Lowery (2004) reported longitudinal data from Ohio showing that previously institution- alized women were much less likely to recidivate than their male counter- parts. Similarly, Smith and Paternoster (1990) found that male youth who had been formally processed by the juvenile justice system in Florida were significantly more likely to recidivate. However, Taxman and
  • 31. Piquero’s (1998) study of drunk driving convictions, Sung’s (1993) analysis of sen- tenced drug offenders, and Thistlethwaite, Wooldredge, and Gibbs’s (1998) research on individuals arrested for domestic violence each found that sex was not a significant predictor of recidivism. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 13 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 559 On the basis of conceptual considerations that have been raised and prior research that has been reported, there are reasonably consistent expectations that we can bring to our analysis concerning the relevance of prior record and stakes in conformity for potential labeling effects. But the same cannot be said for sex or race. Specifically, both conceptual and empirical support exists for expecting stronger labeling effects for less experienced offenders and for those with lower stakes in conformity. Con- cerning the sex of offenders, the conceptual arguments favor greater label- ing consequences for women than for men, but the empirical evidence suggests otherwise. Finally, expectations concerning the relative effect of
  • 32. labeling by race are mixed on both conceptual and empirical grounds.8 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY In this study, we assess the consequence for recidivism of having adjudi- cation applied or withheld for 71,548 male offenders and 24,371 female offenders found guilty of a felony and sentenced to probation in Florida between 2000 and 2002. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first labeling research to examine the effect of adult felony conviction, and the first to examine whether community-level characteristics, such as rates of crime and CD, influence the relationship between the felony conviction and recidivism. Our dependent variable, recidivism, is operationalized by whether (yes = 1) a felony probationer is convicted of another felony within 2 years of being sentenced. This variable involves a new offense for which an individ- ual is sentenced to prison, probation, or jail, and it does not include techni- cal violations of the terms of probation. This measure of recidivism only captures those actually found guilty of another offense, and not those who may have been arrested and charged but had charges dropped or were found not guilty. Overall, 19 percent of our sample recidivated
  • 33. within the 2-year follow-up period.9 8. It should be noted that several studies have examined recidivism among felony probationers, and with relative consistency, they report that recidivism is more likely for males, minorities, and persons with a more extensive prior record (Ben- edict and Huff-Corzine, 1997; Kruttschnitt, Uggen, and Shelton, 2000; Petersilia, 1985; Spohn and Holleran, 2002; Whitehead, 1991). However, these were not studies of labeling effects and the contingencies thereof, because they involve only individuals who have been labeled. Our concern is whether these factors, as well as stakes in conformity, have a conditional consequence for observed label- ing effects. 9. Arrest as an alternative measure of recidivism was considered, but those data are not available to us. The inclusion of probation revocation was also considered but rejected because the vast majority of those involved technical violations and not the commission of a new crime. Thus, the most complete and homogeneous mea- sure available to us in these data is reconviction. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 14 22-AUG-07 10:10
  • 34. 560 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER LEVEL 1 PREDICTORS Our primary independent variable is whether (yes = 1) a person being sentenced to probation for a felony has adjudication formally applied. This outcome certifies the felony convict label. A total of 40 percent of our felony probationers were convicted in this manner. Among the other factors used to model recidivism are demographic attributes of the defendant, his or her crime, and prior record. The means and standard deviations of the level 1 variables used are described in table 1, which also shows the bivariate correlation matrix for all variables of interest. At the individual level, the Florida Sentencing Guidelines database was used to identify offenders as either non-Hispanic blacks (yes = 1) or Hispanic of whatever race (yes = 1). The surnames of all offenders who were not identified as Hispanic from the Guidelines data were checked against the U.S. Census list of Hispanic surnames (Word and Per- kins, 1996), and any individual whose name matched one of those on the list was coded as an Hispanic defendant. The use of the surname list afforded a more comprehensive identification of Hispanics, which raises
  • 35. their proportion in our sample from 11.5 percent to 14.4 percent. Unfortu- nately, we cannot know how many females are identified as Hispanic or not by virtue of marriage. Also included as a predictor was the defendant’s age in years at sentencing. The defendant’s crime was coded as either property (yes = 1) or drug related (yes = 1), with violent crime as the reference category.10 This choice was made because violent offenders were the least likely to recidi- vate11 and we expected the other types of crime to be more consequential in our recidivism models. The seriousness of the crime is indicated by a score generated by the Florida Sentencing Guidelines. Points ranging from 4 to 116 are assigned to the primary offense, and additional points are assigned for additional offenses and a variety of other factors, including victim injury or the use of a firearm.12 A related variable, which partially reflects crime seriousness, is length of supervision in months that is speci- fied by the probation sentence. Prior record is indicated by points that are assigned by the Florida 10. “Other” crimes was such an eclectic category that we have not included it in the analysis. The most common of these crimes were Traffic (53
  • 36. percent); Weapons, Possession (16 percent); Escape (15 percent); and DUI, No Injury (4 percent). 11. The relative frequency of recidivism was 25 percent for property offenders, 23 percent for drug offenders, and 15 percent for violent offenders. 12. Additional points can be assigned on the basis of legal status violations, commu- nity sanction violations, prior serious felony points, violation of the Law Enforce- ment Protection Act, drug trafficking, grand theft of a motor vehicle, commission of the primary offense by a street gang member, and involvement of domestic violence. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 15 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 561 T ab le 1 . C o rr
  • 74. 6 7 b et w ee n c o u n ti es . server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 16 22-AUG-07 10:10 562 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER Guidelines to offense-specific prior convictions for a felony or misde- meanor as an adult or juvenile by a state, federal, military, or foreign court. As with other research using Guidelines data, this affords an unusu- ally comprehensive measure of an individual’s prior criminal
  • 75. record. Offenses that occurred more than 10 years before the current offense are not scored if the individual has been conviction free for that time. Also included among our predictors of recidivism is whether the individual had previously violated the terms of supervision while on probation or commu- nity control (yes = 1), which can be regarded as another measure of prior record. To correct for skewness, we used natural log values for crime seri- ousness, prior record, and supervision length. It is possible that being adjudicated or not may be the consequence of uncontrolled factors that also predict an outcome of interest such as recidi- vism. In such a situation, the judge’s decision to adjudicate the offender is not independent of the residual terms in the equation for recidivism, which would result in biased estimates. To control for this “treatment effect” (Moffitt, 1999), which is somewhat analogous to “selection bias,”13 we computed inverse Mills ratios for inclusion in each model estimating recidivism. The inverse Mills ratios were computed through a two-stage process. First, we estimated a probit model to predict adjudication, including all level 1 independent variables shown in table 1. We also
  • 76. included exclusion restrictions, which are “variables that affect the selection process but not the substantive equation of interest” (Bushway, Johnson, and Slocum, 2007: 152). For this purpose, whether the offender went to trial (1 = yes) is used to predict adjudication but not recidivism, because this variable is arguably relevant for adjudication and not recidivism. In addition, since our data are drawn from 67 counties, we included 66 county dummy vari- ables and only kept those significant county dummy variables for a reduced probit model. The second step calculated the inverse Mills ratio using the predicted value for each offender in the probit equation for adju- dication from the first step, Zδ. Following Bushway, Johnson and Slocum 13. Selection bias (Berk, 1983; Heckman, 1979) refers to a situation wherein not all members of a population are represented inasmuch as some have been “selected” in terms of variables that need to be accounted for when predicting subsequent outcomes. This process applies, for example, to people who are sen- tenced to prison (as opposed to probation) when one is interested in the factors that would predict differential sentence length. In other instances, all members of a population are represented, but some are accorded one “treatment” as opposed
  • 77. to another (Moffitt, 1999). This process applies to a situation such as our own in which all persons found guilty of a felony are either adjudicated or have adjudi- cation withheld. The object of controlling for “treatment effect” is to account for the influence of variables that may predict both the treatment experienced and its presumed consequence—in this case, recidivism. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 17 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 563 (2007: 161), the inverse Mills ratio was estimated for each case by dividing the normal density function evaluated at –Zδ by 1 minus the normal cumulative distribution function estimated at –Zδ. One primary objective of this research is to determine whether “larger social contexts” (Sampson and Laub, 1997) play a role in shaping the potential labeling effects of felony conviction. The social contexts that we examine are characteristics of the county in which offenders experienced their probation supervision. We would prefer to have a lower level of aggregation, but data limitations relative to the situating of offenders by place preclude that possibility. Our second-level variables
  • 78. include total crime rate per 100,000 residents and a measure of police presence, which is the number of police per 100,000 residents. Both of these measures are for the year 2000 (Florida Department of Law Enforcement, 2005). Our mea- sure of CD uses 2000 Census data for percent black, percent of families receiving public assistance, percentage of persons living below the poverty level, and the percentage of families headed by a single mother. Factor loadings were used to weight the variables included in the CD index. Sev- eral other variables failed to load on this factor, including unemployment rate, percent Hispanic, and percent of the population under 18 years of age. To operationalize the ICE, we follow Kubrin and Stewart (2006) and Massey (2001). For a given county, the ICE index is computed as [(number of affluent families – number of poor families) / total number of families]. In this context, “affluent” is defined as families that are 2 standard devia- tions above the mean family income and “poor” is defined as families below the officially designated poverty level. We expect each of the second level variables to potentially interact with felony conviction in the specifi- cation of labeling effects. The means and standard deviations
  • 79. for these variables, as well as their bivariate correlations with first-level and second- level predictors are described in table 1. ANALYTIC STRATEGY HLM represents an improvement over traditional regression techniques when analyzing “nested” or multilevel data. According to Raudenbush and Bryk (2002), multilevel analysis using traditional regression modeling risks violation of two regression assumptions, the independence of error terms and homoscedasticity. Individuals nested within second- level con- texts—in our case, counties—will no longer represent independent obser- vations because they may share many of the same characteristics. This analysis results in correlated error terms and biased estimates of standard errors. HLM corrects for this problem “by incorporating into the statistical model a unique random effect for each organizational [county level] equa- tion” (Bryk and Raudenbush, 1992: 84). Second, tests of significance for server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 18 22-AUG-07 10:10 564 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER
  • 80. second-level variables are accomplished in HLM by adjusting the degrees of freedom to represent the number of second-level units in the analysis. HLM also allows researchers to test the cross-level effects of second-level factors on individual-level relationships. The improved estimation of inde- pendent effects and cross-level interactions are of primary interest in the current research. As discussed by Raudenbush and Bryk (2002), the use of traditional HLM with a dichotomous dependent variable such as recidivism would not be appropriate for two reasons. First, the assumption that error terms are normally distributed is violated with binary outcome data. Also vio- lated is the assumption of linearity in the relationship between the inde- pendent and the dependent variables and the assumption that the variance of the error terms be distributed equally across all values of the dependent variable. To overcome these problems, hierarchical generalized linear modeling (HGLM) is used with these data. All level 1 and level 2 variables in the analysis are grand mean centered with the exception of dichoto- mous measures. Both Raudenbush and Bryk (2002) and Hox (2002) observe that grand mean centering as opposed to group mean
  • 81. centering facilitates the interpretation of coefficients, especially at level 1 and that, without serious theoretical justification for doing otherwise, is the pre- ferred choice. Hox further notes that not using grand mean centering in models with cross-level interactions can lead to “serious interpretation problems” (2002: 56). In all analyses, unit-specific models with robust stan- dard errors are used. Nonsignificant variance components are specified as fixed. RESEARCH FINDINGS UNCONDITIONAL MODEL The unconditional model is estimated primarily to measure the magni- tude of variation in recidivism across counties. Our variance component estimate, .037, is significant (p < .001), which is a common indicator that additional modeling using second-level predictors is warranted. However, the intraclass correlation of .006 suggests that although between county variation is significant, it is clearly not substantial. Nonetheless, we will proceed with second-level analyses. LEVEL 1 RESULTS The second stage of analysis estimates recidivism as a function
  • 82. of indi- vidual-level predictors. Results of the level 1 conditional modeling are described in table 2, which shows the effect of individual-level predictors on recidivism and the degree to which those effects vary across counties server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 19 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 565 (variance component). All individual-level predictors are significantly related to recidivism except for crime seriousness. In table 2, we see that odds of recidivism for property offenders are 72 percent greater than violent offenders and that the odds that black proba- tioners will fail are 36 percent higher than for whites. Those who had vio- lated the terms of a previous probation have odds of recidivism that are 22 percent greater than for those who had not, and males are 38 percent more likely to fail than females. The odds of recidivism are lower for Hispanics than for non-Hispanic whites and are reduced for those with longer terms of probation supervision. Most importantly, table 2 shows that indepen- dent of the effects of all other predictors, having been convicted
  • 83. of a fel- ony increases the odds of recidivism by 17 percent when compared with those who had adjudication withheld. Table 2. Level 1 Conditional HGLM Model of Recidivism Odds Variance Independent Variable Coefficient SE Ratio Component Intercept –2.158 .061 .081*** .092*** Adjudicated .155 .032 1.167*** .016* Male .320 .035 1.377*** .028*** Age at sentencing –.044 .002 .957*** .000* Hispanic –.082 .041 .921* a Black .308 .034 1.360*** .020** Property .543 .037 1.721*** .018* Drug .386 .046 1.470*** .042*** Prior supervision violation .199 .049 1.220*** .033* Crime seriousness (Ln) .037 .027 1.037 a Prior record points (Ln) .087 .022 1.091*** .007* Supervision length (Ln) –.218 .031 .804*** .017** Inverse Mills ratio –.472 .107 .624*** .156** *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. a Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed. NOTE: N = 95,919 within county; N = 67 between counties. As noted, both theory and prior research (sometimes contradictory) have raised the possibility that labeling effects may vary by a person’s race, sex, prior record, and stakes in conformity. To assess this possibility,
  • 84. we repeat the level 1 modeling described above with interaction terms involving adjudication status and the aforementioned individual attributes. Typically, stakes in conformity is operationalized in terms of things that an individual could potentially lose, such as a marital bond or employment as server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 20 22-AUG-07 10:10 566 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER the result of criminal behavior. Unfortunately, those particular data (as well as educational attainment) are not available for Florida probationers. However, a theoretical analog to stakes in conformity is Hirschi’s (1969) “commitment to conventional lines of action.” It is presumed to work the same way as stakes in conformity, inasmuch as it provides an individual with “something to lose” in the event of delinquent or criminal behavior. It seems reasonable to suggest that individuals who have achieved the age of 30 years and have avoided a criminal or delinquent record (up to the current instance) have established more of a commitment to conventional behavior than those who are younger with a criminal record. As
  • 85. such, they have acquired a stake in conformity that threatens at least the loss of repu- tation and status as a consequence of criminal behavior. Thus, we measure “commitment” or stakes in conformity in terms of age and prior record. For our purpose, “high stakes” individuals are those who are 30+ years of age without a prior conviction (felony or misdemeanor) as an adult or juvenile. Table 3 shows the recidivism model with all level 1 estimators and with the addition of four interaction terms involving adjudication and offender characteristics. All level 1 predictors, except Hispanic, and crime serious- ness in three of four models, have a significant relationship with recidi- vism. The effect of adjudication on recidivism is significantly higher for females than for males. Table 3 also shows that the likelihood of recidi- vism, given adjudication is significantly higher for white offenders than for others. Prior record (yes/no) has no significant interaction with adjudica- tion status in predicting recidivism. With regard to our measure of high stakes in conformity, table 3 shows that probationers who had achieved the age of 30 years without a criminal or delinquent conviction are signifi- cantly more likely to recidivate given adjudication than those
  • 86. with a prior record before the age of 30 years.14 SECOND-LEVEL RESULTS In this stage of our analysis, the slope for the relationship between recidivism and adjudication is modeled using county-level contextual vari- ables in conjunction with individual-level predictors. Strong collinearity problems are encountered when the ICE and CD are included in the same 14. In separate models (not shown, results available on request), the effect of adjudi- cation is significant for men as well as for women; for Hispanics, blacks, and whites; for those with and without a prior record; and for those with and without high stakes. This finding suggests a robustness in the consequences of adjudica- tion for recidivism, notwithstanding the significance of interaction terms in table 3 showing the effects of adjudication to be stronger for women, whites, and those with high stakes. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 21 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 567 T
  • 120. c o u n ti es . server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 22 22-AUG-07 10:10 568 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER models. Like Kubrin and Stewart (2006), we resolve that issue by develop- ing estimates that keep these variables separate. However, whereas Kubrin and Stewart only examined those level 2 predictors, our estimates also include overall crime rates and police presence. These models origi- nally include all level 1 and level 2 predictors. The latter are introduced sequentially in two different estimates in order to keep ICE and CD sepa- rate. The sequence for introduction is as follows: ICE, crime rate, and police presence and, alternatively, CD, crime rate, and police presence. Following the criteria articulated by Raudenbush and Bryk
  • 121. (2002: 268), we first added the level 2 variables sequentially to the intercept model and retained only those that were significant. Then we sequentially added the level 2 variables to the adjudicated equation and again only retained sig- nificant variables.15 Table 4a. Combined Level 1 and Level 2 HGLM Model of Recidivism (ICE) Independent Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio Intercept –2.546 .061 .078*** ICE 1.298 .517 3.662* Adjudicated .154 .032 1.167*** Male .314 .035 1.369*** Age at sentencing –.045 .002 .956*** Hispanic –.083 .041 .921* Black .312 .034 1.366*** Property .552 .036 1.738*** Drug .390 .046 1.477*** Prior supervision violation .184 .054 1.202** Crime seriousness (Ln) .030 .028 1.031 Prior record points (Ln) .078 .024 1.082** Supervision length (Ln) –.216 .032 .806*** Inverse Mills ratio –.509 .118 .601*** *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. NOTES: Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed; N = 95,919 within county; N = 67 between counties. 15. Raudenbush and Bryk note that “the analyst will usually
  • 122. want to develop a tenta- tive model for the intercept . . . before proceeding to fit models for the random slopes” (2002: 267) and then, “the most direct evidence of whether a level-2 pre- dictor should be included is the magnitude of its estimated effect and related t-ratio. Predictors with t-ratios near or less than 1 are obvious candidates for exclusion from the model” (2002: 268). server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 23 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 569 Table 4b. Combined Level 1 and Level 2 HGLM Model of Recidivism (CD) Independent Variable Coefficient SE Odds Ratio Intercept –2.530 .061 .080*** Concentrated Disadvantage –.060 .026 .942* Adjudicated .157 .031 1.170*** Male .311 .036 1.364*** Age at sentencing –.044 .002 .957*** Hispanic –.082 .042 .921* Black .314 .035 1.369*** Property .549 .036 1.731*** Drug .389 .046 1.475*** Prior supervision violation .192 .051 1.211** Crime seriousness (Ln) .035 .026 1.036 Prior record points (Ln) .083 .022 1.087**
  • 123. Supervision length (Ln) –.218 .031 .804*** Inverse Mills ratio –.486 .107 .615*** *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. NOTES: Nonsignificant variance components have been fixed; N = 95,919 within county; N = 67 between counties. Tables 4a and b show the results of estimating recidivism using all level 1 predictors, including adjudication status and any significant level 2 predictors. We are concerned with the direct effects of county- level con- texts on recidivism (intercept model) and more importantly, with any pos- sible interactions of level 2 variables and adjudication. Crime rates and police presence have no significant relationships either directly with recidi- vism or interactively with adjudication status. Neither the ICE nor CD interacts significantly with adjudication in the prediction of recidivism. However, concentrated extremes of affluence and poverty are associated with higher levels of recidivism across Florida counties, whereas CD is associated with slightly lower levels of recidivism. The lack of interactive effects involving adjudication and level 2 predictors suggests that being convicted as a felon increases the likelihood of recidivism regardless of the characteristics of counties in which proba-
  • 124. tioners are supervised. As with the individual-level traits discussed above (table 3), this further illustrates the apparent robustness of the effects of a felony convict label inasmuch as those effects are not conditioned either by individual or “larger social contexts.” In addition, although not a focus of this research, our data for Florida counties show opposite direct effects of ICE and CD on recidivism than what Kubrin and Stewart (2006) report server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 24 22-AUG-07 10:10 570 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER for Oregon census tracts.16 DISCUSSION As noted, this study is the first one to examine the effects of being labeled a convicted felon on the recidivism of adults. The option of with- holding adjudication of guilt and the label of convicted felon for those who are legally guilty provides a unique opportunity to explore the conse- quences of labeling within the criminal justice system. The decision to withhold adjudication involves a situation wherein legally equivalent
  • 125. adults—in terms of guilt—are either labeled as a felon or not. In terms of social and legal significance, a more consequential label is not available in the criminal court system. Our principal findings can be briefly summarized. 1. Being adjudicated guilty as a felon significantly and substantially increases the likelihood of recidivism in comparison with those who have adjudication withheld. 2. The effect of being adjudicated guilty on recidivism is stronger for whites than for blacks and Hispanics and for females as opposed to males. It is also stronger for those who reach the age of 30 years without any prior convictions compared with those with priors before turning 30 years old. 3. The community-level contexts that we examined have no conse- quence for the relationship between adjudication of guilt and recidivism. At a practical level, these results underscore the potential benefits that 16. There are several methodological differences between the Kubrin and Stewart (2006) study and our own. Their study was performed in Portland, Oregon and ours in Florida. The proportion of non-Hispanic whites in
  • 126. Multnomah County (Portland) was substantially higher (79.4 percent) in the 2000 Census than in Florida (66.5 percent). Their sample, which included misdemeanants as well as felons, was 68 percent white, 25 percent black, 4 percent Hispanic, and 3 percent other. Our sample is 49 percent white, 36 percent black, and 14 percent Hispanic (others excluded). Their measure of recidivism is arrest, whereas ours is reconvic- tion for a felony. More important, perhaps, their data for ICE and CD were aggregated at the census tract level and ours at the county level. At the lower level of aggregation, it makes sense to expect the relative presence of affluence to provide for both social capital and collective efficacy in ways that might reduce recidivism overall. But at the county level, the affluent and poor are likely not living near one another and the potential benefits of having affluence nearby may not work in the same way. In fact, the social and physical distance that may char- acterize the affluent and poor in a county may work in ways that afford the afflu- ent the opportunity and means to more vigorously police poor neighborhoods and prosecute those who may be apprehended. This would not only mitigate the recidivism retarding effects of affluence noted by Kubrin and Stewart, but it may also aggravate the prospects of criminal activity resulting in a formal charge and conviction—our measure of recidivism.
  • 127. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 25 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 571 can accrue to individuals and to society—in this case, from less criminal activity—when a policy of minimizing harm (Clear, 1995; Rubin, 1999) is implemented in the administration of justice. The withholding of adjudica- tion for those who have been found guilty of a felony is a policy that these data suggest is directly related to lower levels of recidivism than are found among those who are formally labeled as a convicted felon. In this case, minimizing harm at the individual level has consequences for reducing harm in the broader community. At a theoretical level, the pattern of findings suggests that the conse- quences of labeling are greater for those who would otherwise be consid- ered less likely to recidivate. As noted, research has generally shown that recidivism is more likely for males, minorities, and persons with a more extensive prior record (Benedict and Huff-Corzine, 1997; Kruttschnitt, Uggen, and Shelton, 2000; Spohn and Holleran, 2002; Whitehead, 1991).
  • 128. These data suggest that those who are generally more likely to recidivate are less disadvantaged by a formal label than are women, whites, and those without an early prior record. It could also be argued that women, whites, and those without an early prior record have more to lose from criminal stigma because crime is more often associated with males, minori- ties, and those with extensive criminal experience. It also happens that the pattern of results reported here is consistent with some of the theoretical expectations raised earlier in this article. Spe- cifically, the diminished relevance of the felony convict label for black and Hispanic offenders was anticipated, even though prior research is both limited and inconsistent in this regard. For example, Harris (1976: 433) hypothesized that the effects of official labeling would be least conse- quential for those most excluded from “ascriptive membership” in domi- nant groups, whereas Paternoster and Iovanni (1989: 382) hypothesized that those who “grant less legitimacy to the legal order and its agents”— presumably including racial and ethnic minorities—could be “more imper- vious to labeling effects.” Assuming that, on average, whites are less disad- vantaged than blacks, our results run counter to the expectation raised by
  • 129. Sampson and Laub’s (1997: 153) that deficits would “pile up faster” for those who are already disadvantaged. These results also diverge from those of Bernburg and Krohn (2003) who found that labeling effects were stronger for African-American youth than for others. It is possible that the race-specific effects of formal labeling are different for adults than for juveniles, which is an issue that future research should carefully explore. The fact that a felony convict label more strongly predicts recidivism for females than for males is consistent with the scant theorizing that has been done about labeling effects for women, but it is inconsistent with a limited body of empirical evidence showing that labeling effects are more conse- quential for males. Messerschmidt (1993: 4) hypothesized that because server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 26 22-AUG-07 10:10 572 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER men exercise greater power in society than women, “men should . . . have greater opportunity to counteract official labeling.” Giordano, Cernkovich, and Lowery (2004: 189) hypothesized that the greater social
  • 130. stigma attached to “antisocial” behavior by females could “be more limit- ing to life chances/opportunities for a return to conventional roles” and thus more conducive to recidivism. Although our results are consistent with those expectations, the process whereby labeling comes to have more consequence for women than for men cannot be known from these data. The finding that labeling effects are stronger for those who, in Hirschi’s (1969) terms, have established a more substantial “commitment to conven- tional action” by having reached at least 30 years of age without a prior record is consistent with the “greater vulnerability” labeling hypothesis (Sherman et al., 1992). In that view, it is expected that labeling would be more consequential for those with a higher stake in conformity and who, on that account, presumably “care more about the opinions of conven- tional society” (1992: 682). Another way to interpret this result is that, as Paternoster and Iovanni (1989) hypothesized, labeling experiences have diminishing consequences for those persons who have been formally labeled earlier in life. Although not part of our original theoretical framework, it is worth not- ing that finding less recidivism for those having adjudication
  • 131. withheld is consistent with expectations from a “reintegrative shaming” perspective. As described by Braithwaite (1989: 55), reintegrative shaming combines “expressions of community disapproval” for a behavior with “gestures of reacceptance into the community of law-abiding citizens.” One who has been found guilty of a felony and sentenced to probation has clearly received an expression of community disapproval. At the same time, not being labeled a convicted felon (having adjudication withheld) reduces the prospect of formal and informal exclusions from the community of law- abiding citizens that traditionally accompany the label of convicted felon. In fact, Braithwaite (1989: 101) juxtaposes and contrasts reintegrative shaming with the central labeling concept of “stigmatization,” which he describes as “disintegrative shaming in which no effort is made to recon- cile the offender with the community.” What the decision to impose or withhold adjudication of guilt may amount to is a choice between stigmati- zation and reintegration—or at least diminished disintegration. The avoid- ance of the label in this instance not only reduces the potentially stigmatizing effects of labeling, but it may at the same time increase the
  • 132. reintegrative potential of the punitive response for the individual involved. The lack of an interaction between second-level variables and adjudica- tion status in these analyses could mean one of three things. On the one hand, it could indicate that the effect of adjudication on recidivism is suffi- ciently robust that it holds in a variety of community contexts. Thus, it is server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 27 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 573 seemingly unaffected by levels of crime, police presence, CD, and an ICE of affluence and poverty. Alternatively, as the intraclass correlation sug- gests, there may be insufficient variation in recidivism across counties to matter for these purposes. Finally, it may be the case that community con- text is inadequately operationalized with data that are aggregated at the county level. Ideally, an assessment of the impact of community character- istics on the relationship between adjudication as a felon and recidivism would make use of neighborhood-level data, aggregated at the census tract or block level. Unfortunately, we cannot locate our probationers
  • 133. more precisely than by the county in which their probation supervision took place. Another limitation of the current research is the potential for omitted variable biases at both levels of analysis. At the individual level, it would have been helpful if available data included socioeconomic status as well as three additional indicators of stakes in conformity— employment, mari- tal status, and educational attainment. At the second level of aggregation, measures of probation caseload could have relevance for the likelihood of recidivism, as could measures of social capital within different social contexts. In sum, this research has shown that when adults are adjudicated guilty of a felony, a significant and substantial increase occurs in the likelihood of reconviction within 2 years. When combined with the recent results reported by Bernburg and his colleagues (Bernburg and Krohn, 2003; Bernburg, Krohn, and Rivera, 2006) involving the labeling of juveniles, these data provide additional encouragement for the renewed empirical assessment of labeling theory. Moreover, research moving forward in this area should pay particular attention to whether and how labeling
  • 134. effects vary for different kinds of individuals and for broader social contexts that may be operationalized more finely than was possible in the current research. REFERENCES Ageton, Suzanne S., and Delbert Elliott. 1974. The effects of legal process- ing on delinquent orientations. Social Problems 22:87–100. Agnew, Robert. 2006. Pressured Into Crime: An Overview of General Strain Theory. Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing. Akers, Ronald L. 1998. Social Learning and Social Structure: A General Theory of Crime and Deviance. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 28 22-AUG-07 10:10 574 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER Anderson, Elijah. 1999. Code of the Street. New York: W. W. Norton and Company. Barrick, Kelle. 2005. Labeling and recidivism: A quantitative assessment
  • 135. of the evidence. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Soci- ety of Criminology, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, November. Baumer, Eric. 1997. Levels and predictors of recidivism: The Malta experi- ence. Criminology 35:601–28. Benedict, W. Reed, and Lin Huff-Corzine. 1997. Return to the scene of the punishment: Recidivism of adult male property offenders on felony probation, 1986-1989. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 34:237–53. Berk, Richard A. 1983. An introduction to sample selection bias in socio- logical data. American Sociological Review 48:386–98. Berk, Richard A., Alec Campbell, Ruth Klap, and Bruce Western. 1992. The deterrent effect of arrest in incidents of domestic violence: A Bayesian analysis of four field experiments. American Sociological Review 57:698–708. Bernburg, Jon Gunnar. 2002. State reaction, life-course outcomes and structural disadvantage: A panel study of the impact of formal criminal labeling on the transition to adulthood. PhD Dissertation. University at Albany, State University of New York.
  • 136. Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, and Marvin D. Krohn. 2003. Labeling, life chances and adult crime: The direct and indirect effects of official intervention in adolescence on crime in early adulthood. Criminology 41:1287–318. Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, Marvin D. Krohn, and Craig J. Rivera. 2006. Offi- cial labeling, criminal embeddedness, and subsequent delinquency: A longitudinal test of labeling theory. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 43:67–88. Blackwell, Brenda Sims, and Sarah Eschholz. 2002. Sex differences and rational choice: Traditional tests and new directions. In Rational Choice and Criminal Behavior, eds. Alex R. Piquero and Stephen G. Tibbetts. New York: Routledge. Braithwaite, John. 1989. Crime, Shame and Reintegration. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Bryk, Anthony S., and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 1992. Hierarchical Linear Models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 29 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 575
  • 137. Bushway, Shawn, Brian D. Johnson, and Lee Ann Slocum. 2007. Is the magic still there? The use of the Heckman two-step correction for selection bias in criminology. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 23:151-78. Clear, Todd R. 1995. Harm in American Penology. Albany, NY: SUNY Press. DeJong, Christine. 1997. Specific deterrence and survival analysis: Inte- grating theoretical and empirical models of recidivism. Criminology 35:561–76. Fagan, Jefferey A., Aaron Kupchick, and Akiva Liberman. 2003. Be care- ful what you wish for. The comparative impacts of juvenile versus crim- inal court sanctions on recidivism among adolescent felony offenders. New York: Columbia Law School Public Law and Legal Theory Work- ing Group. Farrington, David. 1977. The effects of public labeling. British Journal of Criminology 17:112–25. Florida Department of Law Enforcement. 2005. Crime in Florida, Florida Uniform Crime Report, 2005. Tallahassee, FL: Florida
  • 138. Statistical Anal- ysis Center. http://www.fdle.state.fl.us/fsac/data_statistics.asp. Gendreau, Paul, Tracy Little, and Claire Goggin. 1996. A meta- analysis of the predictors of adult offender recidivism: What works! Criminology 34:575–607. Gibbs, Jack. 1974. The effects of juvenile legal procedures on juvenile offenders’ self-attitudes. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 11:51–5. Giordano, Peggy C., Stephan A. Cernkovich, and Allen R. Lowery. 2004. A long-term follow-up of serious adolescent female offenders. In Aggression, Antisocial Behavior and Violence among Girls, eds. Martha Putallaz and Karen L. Bierman. New York: The Guilford Press. Hagan, John, and Alberto Palloni. 1990. The social reproduction of a crim- inal class in working class London. American Journal of Sociology 96:265–99. Harris, Anthony. 1975. Imprisonment and the expected value of criminal choice: A specification and test of aspects of the labeling perspective. American Sociological Review 40:71–87. Harris, Anthony. 1976. Race, commitment to deviance and
  • 139. spoiled iden- tity. American Sociological Review 41:432–42. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 30 22-AUG-07 10:10 576 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER Heckman, James J. 1979. Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica 47:153–161. Heimer, Karen, and Ross L. Matsueda. 1996. Role taking, role commit- ment and delinquency: A theory of differential social control. Ameri- can Sociological Review 59:365–90. Hirschi, Travis. 1969. Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley: University of Cali- fornia Press. Horwitz, Allan, and Michael Wasserman. 1979. The effect of social control on delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test. Sociological Focus 12:53–70. Hox, Joop. 2002. Multilevel Analyses: Techniques and Applications. Mah- wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hull, Elizabeth A. 2006. The Disenfranchisement of Ex-Felons. Philadel- phia, PA: Temple University Press.
  • 140. Jensen, Gary. 1972. Delinquency and adolescent self- conceptions: A study of the personal relevance of infraction. Social Problems 20:84– 103. Kruttschnitt, Candace, Christopher Uggen, and Kelly Shelton. 2000. Predictors of desistence among sex offenders: The interaction of for- mal and informal social controls. Justice Quarterly 47:61–87. Kubrin, Charis E., and Eric A. Stewart. 2006. Predicting who reoffends: The neglected role of neighborhood context in recidivism studies. Criminology. 44:165–98. Lofland, John. 1969. Deviance and Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- tice-Hall. Manza, Jeff, and Christopher Uggen. 2006. Locked Out: Felon Disen- franchisement and American Democracy. New York: Oxford Univer- sity Press. Massey, Douglas S. 2001. Ecology comes back to sociology. In Does It Take a Village? Community Effects on Children, Adolescents and Fami- lies, eds. Alan Booth and Ann Crouter. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Matsueda, Ross L. 1992. Reflected appraisals, parental labeling
  • 141. and delin- quency: Specifying a symbolic interactionist theory. American Journal of Sociology 97:1577–611. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 31 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 577 Mauer, Marc, and Meda Chesney-Lind. 2002. Invisible Punishment: The Collateral Consequences of Mass Imprisonment. New York: The New Press. Messerschmidt, James W. 1993. Masculinities and Crime. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, Inc. Moffitt, Robert A. 1999. New developments in econometric methods for labor market analysis. In Handbook of Labor Economics, vol. III, eds. Orley Ashenfelter and David Card. North Holland, NY: Elsevier Science. Morenoff, Jeffrey D., Robert J. Sampson, and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 2001. Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology 39:517–59. Nagin, Daniel S. 1998. Criminal deterrence research at the
  • 142. outset of the twenty-first century. In Crime and Justice: A Review of Research, vol. XXIII, ed. Michael Tonry. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Palamara, Frances, Francis T. Cullen, and Joanne C. Gertsten. 1986. The effect of police and mental health intervention on juvenile deviance: Specifying contingencies in the impact of formal reaction. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 27:90–105. Pate, Antony M., and Edwin E. Hamilton. 1992. Formal and informal deterrents to domestic violence: The Dade County spouse assault experiment. American Sociological Review 57:691–97. Paternoster, Raymond. 1987. The deterrent effect of the perceived cer- tainty and severity of punishment: A review of the evidence and issues. Justice Quarterly 4:173–217. Paternoster, Raymond, and Leeann Iovanni. 1989. The labeling perspec- tive and delinquency: An elaboration of the theory and an assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly 6:359–93. Petersilia, Joan. 1985. Probation and felony offenders. Federal Probation 49:4–9.
  • 143. Raudenbush, Stephen, and Anthony Bryk. 2002. Hierarchical Linear Mod- els: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ray, Melvin C., and William R. Downs. 1986. An empirical test of labeling theory using longitudinal data. Journal of Research in Crime and Delin- quency 23:169–94. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 32 22-AUG-07 10:10 578 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER Richards, Pamela, and Charles R. Tittle. 1981. Gender and perceived chances of arrest. Social Forces 59:1182–99. Rountree, Pamela Wilcox, Kenneth C. Land, and Terance D. Miethe. 1994. Macro-micro integration in the study of victimization: A hierar- chical logistic model analysis across Seattle neighborhoods. Criminol- ogy 32:387–414. Rubin, Edward L. 1999. Minimizing Harm: A New Crime Policy for America. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 1993. Structural
  • 144. variation in juve- nile court processing: Inequality, the underclass and social control. Law and Society Review 27:285–311. Sampson, Robert, and John H. Laub. 1997. A life-course theory of cumu- lative disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In Developmental Theories of Crime and Delinquency, ed. Terence P. Thornberry. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Sampson, Robert J., Jeffrey D. Morenoff, and Felton Earls. 1999. Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review 64:644–60. Sampson, Robert J., Jeffrey D. Morenoff, and Thomas Gannon- Rowley. 2002. Assessing neighborhood effects: Social processes and new direc- tions in research. Annual Review of Sociology 28:443–78. Sampson, Robert J., Stephen Raudenbush, and Felton Earls. 1997. Neigh- borhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277:918–24. Schur, Edwin M. 1983. Labeling Women Deviant. Philadelphia, PA: Tem- ple University Press. Shakur, Sanyika. 1993. Monster: The Autobiography of an L.A.
  • 145. Gang Member. New York: The Atlantic Monthly Press. Sherman, Lawrence W. 1993. Defiance, deterrence and irrelevance: A the- ory of the criminal sanction. Journal of Research in Crime and Delin- quency 30:445–73. Sherman, Lawrence W., Douglas A. Smith, Janell D. Schmidt, and Dennis P. Rogan. 1992. Crime, punishment and stake in conformity: Legal and informal control of domestic violence. American Sociological Review 57:680–90. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 33 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 579 Silver, Eric. 2000. Extending social disorganization theory: A multi-level approach to the study of violence among persons with mental illnesses. Criminology 38:1043–73. Smith, Douglas A., and Patrick Gartin. 1989. Specifying specific deter- rence: The influence of arrest on future criminal activity. American Sociological Review 54:94–106.
  • 146. Smith, Douglas A., and Raymond Paternoster. 1990. Formal processing and future delinquency: Deviance amplification as selection artifact. Law and Society Review 24:1109–31. Spohn, Cassia, and David Holleran. 2002. The effect of imprisonment on recidivism rates of felony offenders: A focus on drug offenders. Crimi- nology 40:329–57. Stafford, Mark, and Mark Warr. 1993. A reconceptualization of general and specific deterrence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 30:123–35. Sung, Hung-En. 2003. Differential impact of deterrence vs. rehabilitation as drug interventions on recidivism after 36 months. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation 37:95–108. Taxman, Faye S., and Alex Piquero. 1998. On preventing drunk driving recidivism: An examination of rehabilitation and punishment approaches. Journal of Criminal Justice 26:129–43. Thistlethwaite, Amy, John Wooldredge, and David Gibbs. 1998. Severity of dispositions and domestic violence recidivism. Crime and Delin- quency 44:388–98. Tittle, Charles. 1972. Institutional living and self esteem. Social Problems
  • 147. 20:65–77. Tittle, Charles. 1995. Control Balance: Toward a General Theory of Devi- ance. Boulder, CO: Westview. Ulmer, Jeffery T., and Brian Johnson. 2004. Sentencing in context: A mul- tilevel analysis. Criminology 42:137–77. Ventura, Lois A., and Gabrielle Davis. 2005. Domestic violence: Court case conviction and recidivism. Violence Against Women 11:255–77. Warr, Mark. 2001. The social origins of crime: Edwin Sutherland and the theory of differential association. In Explaining Crime and Criminals, eds. Raymond Paternoster and Ronet Bachman. Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing Company. server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 34 22-AUG-07 10:10 580 CHIRICOS, BARRICK, BALES & BONTRAGER West, Donald, and David Farrington. 1973. Who Becomes Delinquent? London: Heinemann Educational. Western, Bruce. 2002. The impact of incarceration on wage mobility and
  • 148. inequality. American Sociological Review 67:477–98. Western, Bruce, and Becky Pettit. 2005. Black-white wage inequality, employment rates and incarceration. American Journal of Sociology 111:553–78. Whitehead, John T. 1991. The effectiveness of felony probation: Results from an eastern state. Justice Quarterly 8:525–43. Wilson, William J. 1987. The Truly Disadvantaged. Chicago, IL: The Uni- versity of Chicago Press. Word, David L., and Colby R. Perkins. 1996. Building a Spanish surname list for the 1990s—a new approach to an old problem. U.S. Census Bureau Technical Working Paper No. 13. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce. Ted Chiricos is the William Julius Wilson Professor of Criminology in the College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at Florida State University. His research interests include the effects of race and social threat on jus- tice outcomes, as well as the various factors that contribute to punitiveness in American culture. Kelle Barrick is a doctoral student in the College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at Florida State University. Her current
  • 149. interests include criminological theory; the links among race, class, gender, and crime; and corporate crime. She is also the Richard Rachin Fellow for the Journal of Drug Issues. William Bales is an associate professor in the College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at Florida State University. His interests include sentenc- ing research, assessing the effectiveness and consequences of punishment and correctional strategies, and community reentry issues among incarcer- ated adult and juvenile populations. Stephanie Bontrager recently received her Ph.D. degree in criminology from Florida State University. She is a senior research analyst with the server05productnCCRY45-3CRY305.txt unknown Seq: 35 22-AUG-07 10:10 THE LABELING OF CONVICTED FELONS 581 Justice Research Center in Tallahassee, Florida and serves as the project director for the National Council on Crime and Delinquency Tracking and Follow-up Evaluation. Her research interests include the relationship between social threat and social control, the interaction between
  • 150. macro forms of social control, and the effects of economic, racial, and gender inequality on crime. 10.1177/0022427805280068ARTICLEJournal of Research in Crime and DelinquencyBernburg et al. / Labeling Theory Official Labeling, Criminal Embeddedness, and Subsequent Delinquency A Longitudinal Test of Labeling Theory Jón Gunnar Bernburg University of Iceland and Icelandic Research Council Marvin D. Krohn University at Albany–SUNY Craig J. Rivera Niagara University This article examines the short-term impact of formal criminal labeling on involvement in deviant social networks and increased likelihood of subsequent delinquency. According to labeling theory, formal criminal intervention should affect the individual’s immediate social networks. In many cases, the stigma of the criminal status may increase the probability that the individual
  • 151. becomes involved in deviant social groups. The formal label may thus ulti- mately increase involvement in subsequent deviance. We use panel data of a sample of urban adolescents to examine whether involvement in deviant social groups mediates the relationship between juvenile justice intervention and subsequent delinquent behavior. Using measures from three successive points in time, the authors find that juvenile justice intervention positively affects subsequent involvement in serious delinquency through the medium of involvement in deviant social groups, namely, street gangs and delinquent peers. Keywords: labeling theory; deviant peers; delinquency In recent years, there has been a revived interest in the labeling approach inthe field of criminology. After a period of criticism and rejection of label- ing theory (see Goode 1975; Hirschi 1980; Tittle 1980), scholars have repeatedly pointed out that by modifying the theory and elaborating on the social processes involved, the labeling approach can complement some of 67 Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency Volume 43 Number 1
  • 152. February 2006 67-88 © 2006 Sage Publications 10.1177/0022427805280068 http://jrc.sagepub.com hosted at http://online.sagepub.com at UTSA Libraries on November 12, 2015jrc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jrc.sagepub.com/ the more established theories of deviant behavior (Paternoster and Iovanni 1989; Sampson and Laub 1997). A prominent theme that has reemerged in revisionist work on labeling theory has been an emphasis on the social- structural consequences of deviant labeling that trigger processes leading to movement into deviant groups (Bernburg and Krohn 2003; Paternoster and Iovanni 1989; Sampson and Laub 1997; Zhang and Messner 1994). In light of the role that the individual’s social ties to unconventional groups play in criminological theory (Thornberry and Krohn 1997; Warr 2002), such pro- cesses should be of great interest to criminology. Yet existing research bear- ing upon this line of investigation has been both limited and inconclusive