SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 46
Book Discussion Example
Ethical egoism fosters an environment of competition and
promotes lack of self-lessness. Because self-interest differs
from person to person, competition to fulfill personal self-
interest becomes acceptable. For example, many people who
have careers in the trade of business view personal gratification
and fulfillment as essential to success. Pursuing one’s self
interest demands that individual desires are elevated above
another’s. Ethical egoism does not allow consideration for
another person’s needs or permit aiding someone to reach their
personal goals (Debelijak & Krkac, 2008). This philosophy
fundamentally alters how society and individuals uphold or
value standards of morality. If one’s goal in life is to seek
personal satisfaction, then it is morally justified via ethical
egoism. However, there are serious ramifications to this self-
seeking ethical philosophy.
Ethical egoism advocates individual preservation above the
common welfare of society. If one’s desire is to steal believing
he is justified because his own self-interest is being fulfilled,
then he commits the crime with the belief that he is morally
justified in doing so. An action is moral so long as self-interest
is the only goal being achieved. Individual interests and social
welfare are often in conflict due to the reality that self-interest
does not permit self-sacrifice. Ethical egoism mandates that
social welfare is not a priority, not even a consideration when
contemplating how best to pursue one’s self-interest (Olson,
1961). This corrupt pattern of thinking creates a fixation on
individual preservation rather than universal prosperity.
Therefore, the only one benefiting from ethical egoism is a
single individual. Everyone’s needs are not met nor are they
considered important. It is a human being’s tendency to value or
pursue self-interest at the cost of common welfare and
prosperity.
Individualism states that the interests or desires of an
individual are regarded as supreme. Individualism is often
associated with the ethical egoism because an individual’s
desires are considered paramount above the collective society.
As a result, the individual relates to society or their community
based upon what said community can offer them as an
individual. There exists no internal motive to assist another
individual in pursing their desires or achieving their goals.
Ethical egoism is self-centered by nature which causes a
disconnect and lack of care for another’s prosperity (Gantt &
Burton, 2013).
I believe that the only benefit to pursuing self-interest is
temporary fulfillment of a particular desire. A society or
community cannot benefit from each individual seeking self-
satisfaction. This self-centered philosophy ultimately leads to
competition and misplaced animosity between people. It would
be impossible to encourage, uplift, or connect with others if we
are viewing each other as obstacles or hindrances to our selfish
desires. I find ethical egoism to personify the selfish, depraved
nature characteristic of human beings. If self-interest is the idol
to be worshipped, then other people will suffer the
concequences of such narrow-minded thinking. I believe that
many people benefit when we put aside our self-interest to
acknowledge and consider another person’s needs above our
own.
Works Cited:
Debelijak, J. & Krkac, K. (2008). “Me, myself & I”: Practical
egoism, selfishness, self-interest
and business ethics. Social Responsibility Journal, 4(1/2),
217-227.
Gantt, E. E., & Burton, J. (2013). Egoism, altruism, and the
ethical foundations of personhood.
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 53(4), 438-460.
Olson, R.G. (1961). Ethical egoism and social
welfare. Philosophy and Phenomenological
Research, 21(4), 528-536.
Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–392
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Computers in Human Behavior
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c
a t e / c o m p h u m b e h
Five days at outdoor education camp without screens improves
preteen
skills with nonverbal emotion cues
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036
0747-5632/� 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/).
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology,
University of
California, Los Angeles, 616 Via De La Paz, Pacific Palisades,
CA 90272, United
States. Tel.: +1 310 526 3316; fax: +1 310 230 7830.
E-mail address: [email protected] (Y.T. Uhls).
Yalda T. Uhls a,⇑ , Minas Michikyan b, Jordan Morris c, Debra
Garcia d,b, Gary W. Small e, Eleni Zgourou f,
Patricia M. Greenfield a
a Department of Psychology, University of California, Los
Angeles, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United States
b Department of Psychology, California State University, Los
Angeles, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United States
c Department of Social Welfare, University of California, Los
Angeles, United States
d Department of Education, University of California, Los
Angeles, United States
e Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences,
University of California, Los Angeles, United States
f Department of Education, University of North Carolina,
Chapel Hill, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United
States
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Article history:
Available online 15 August 2014
Keywords:
Social media
Nonverbal communication
Emotion
Adolescent
Social interaction
Development
A field experiment examined whether increasing opportunities
for face-to-face interaction while elimi-
nating the use of screen-based media and communication tools
improved nonverbal emotion–cue recog-
nition in preteens. Fifty-one preteens spent five days at an
overnight nature camp where television,
computers and mobile phones were not allowed; this group was
compared with school-based matched
controls (n = 54) that retained usual media practices. Both
groups took pre- and post-tests that required
participants to infer emotional states from photographs of facial
expressions and videotaped scenes with
verbal cues removed. Change scores for the two groups were
compared using gender, ethnicity, media
use, and age as covariates. After five days interacting face-to-
face without the use of any screen-based
media, preteens’ recognition of nonverbal emotion cues
improved significantly more than that of the con-
trol group for both facial expressions and videotaped scenes.
Implications are that the short-term effects
of increased opportunities for social interaction, combined with
time away from screen-based media and
digital communication tools, improves a preteen’s
understanding of nonverbal emotional cues.
� 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open
access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/).
1. Introduction study found that the amount of non-screen
playtime decreased
For several millennia, humans’ primary method for social learn-
ing and communication has been face to face. In the 21st
century,
as mobile technology and the Internet became available to most
of
the world’s population (Internet world stats, 2013), digital
media
have become an increasingly prevalent factor in the informal
learn-
ing environment (Greenfield, 2009). Children today, ages 8–18,
spend over 7½ h a day, seven days a week using media outside
of school (Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010). Moreover,
teenagers,
ages 12–17, report using phones to text message in their daily
lives
more than any other form of communication, including face-to-
face socializing (Lenhart, 2012). The extensive time that
children
and teenagers engage with media and communicate using
screens
may be taking time away from face-to-face communication and
some in-person activities (Giedd, 2012). Indeed, one
longitudinal
20% from 1997 to 2003, while screen activities (i.e., watching
tele-
vision, playing videogames and using the computer) increased
(Hofferth, 2010).
The advent of mobile technology enables today’s youth to
access and engage with screens 24/7 outside of school while in
cars, on vacations, in restaurants, and even in bed. A recent poll
found that children’s access to these kinds of devices has grown
fivefold in the last two years (Common Sense Media, 2013).
Extant
research indicates that, today, media exposure begins at early
ages,
consumes the majority of youth leisure time, and takes place in
many different environments and contexts. Such extensive use
of
new technology has raised concerns that children’s face-to-face
communication skills may be negatively affected (Bindley,
2011;
Giedd, 2012).
1.1. Face-to-face and mediated communication
When engaging in face-to-face communication, social informa-
tion is conveyed by vocal and visual cues within the context of
the
situation. Nonverbal communication, defined as communication
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.chb.2014.05
.036&domain=pdf
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/07475632
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh
388 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014)
387–392
without words, includes apparent behaviors such as facial
expres-
sion, eye contact, and tone of voice, as well as less obvious
mes-
sages such as posture and spatial distance between two or more
people (Knapp & Hall, 2010). The understanding of these kinds
of
nonverbal social cues is particularly important for social
interac-
tion because of the need to modify one’s own behavior in
response
to the reactions of others (Knapp & Hall, 2010). The capability
to
effectively process emotional cues is associated with many per-
sonal, social and academic outcomes (Knapp & Hall, 2010;
McClure & Nowicki, 2001). In addition, children who better
under-
stand emotional cues in a social environment may develop supe-
rior social skills and form more positive peer relationships
(Blakemore, 2003; Bosacki & Astington, 1999).
Long before digital media became ubiquitous, investigators
developed theories, such as the Cues-Filtered-Out theory, which
postulated that the lack of nonverbal cues in computer-mediated
interactions could lead to impersonal communication, (Culnan
&
Markus, 1987), while others pointed out deficits in computer-
med-
iated communication due to the lack of social-context cues
(Sproull
& Kiesler, 1988). More recently, an experiment exploring the
differ-
ence in emotional connectedness experienced by emerging
adults
using either in-person or digitally mediated communication
showed that bonding and affiliative cues were significantly
stron-
ger when friends communicated in person rather than by text
(Sherman, Michikyan, & Greenfeld, 2013). The extensive use of
dig-
ital media, often text-based and thus inherently lacking
nonverbal
emotional cues, may thus curtail the face-to-face experiences
nec-
essary to master important social skills, even though they are
used
for social communication (Giedd, 2012).
1.2. The video deficit
Research regarding what children do and do not learn about the
social world through screen-based media, in particular
television,
is robust (Guernsey, 2011; Richert, Robb, & Smith, 2011;
Wartella, 2012; Wartella, O’Keefe, & Scantlin, 2000). Much of
the
research concentrates on early learning from imitation, socially
contingent interaction (e.g. joint attention and gaze following),
and word learning (Flom & Johnson, 2010; Moore & Dunham,
1995). This body of research shows that young children learn
bet-
ter from live interaction than from screens. For example, Hayne,
Herbert, and Simcock (2003) performed a series of experiments
using matched live and videotaped models who performed a
series
of actions with a rattle and stuffed animals. Although children
imi-
tated televised models, the mean imitation scores were signifi-
cantly higher in the live condition. This discrepancy in
imitation
appears to last until 30 months of age and was coined the
‘‘video
deficit.’’
1.3. Reading nonverbal emotion cues: processes of development
and
learning
Features of face-to-face communication such as eye contact and
pointing are crucial when teaching young children about social
interaction and the world they live in. For example, gaze
following,
a well-studied mechanism in the literature on human develop-
ment, guides infants from around one year of age to learn about
objects and humans (Flom & Johnson, 2010). Humans also learn
from cues such as pointing when interacting socially (Moore &
Dunham, 1995). Once a child is able to attend to an object that
another person highlights, their ability to learn through social
interaction increases. These means of learning are available
only
when a child can see another’s face and physical being (Gross &
Ballif, 1991).
In-person interaction also develops the accurate understanding
of nonverbal emotion cues. For instance, cooperative interaction
among siblings in the third year of life has been shown to
predict
skill in affective labeling of facial expressions and
understanding
of emotions in dramatized puppet scenarios in the fourth year of
life
(Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991). The
chil-
dren’s positive behavior toward their siblings in the third year
of life
continued to predict more advanced understanding of emotions
at
six years of age (Brown & Dunn, 1996). These longitudinal
findings
point to in-person peer interaction as a key learning experience
in
the early acquisition of skill in reading nonverbal emotion cues.
As children grow older, their peer focus shifts from siblings to
unrelated peers, whom they usually meet in school. In
preadoles-
cence, the period under investigation in the present research,
social
interaction skill with peers, assessed in an in-person school
situa-
tion, was correlated with an understanding of feelings presented
in
narrative (Bosacki & Astington, 1999).
2. Research question and hypothesis: the present study
In the present study, we designed a field experiment to ask the
research question: Does children’s frequent screen use—and the
possibility that this extensive use replaces critical face-to-face
com-
munication—promote the development of emotion
understanding
to the same extent as in-person interactions? If not, a shift in
chil-
dren’s activities to solely in-person peer and adult
communication
could enhance skill in understanding the emotions of other
people.
Our experimental condition was a naturally occurring environ-
ment where children experienced extensive opportunities for
social
interaction, combined with no access to screens, for five days.
Our
participants were preteens in the sixth grade. We chose this age
group because: (1) by the time they reach early adolescence,
chil-
dren are able to integrate information from many nonverbal
cues,
including face, gesture and tone, to make inferences about
social sit-
uations (Knapp & Hall, 2010); (2) the understanding of social
emo-
tions and the ability to take into account another person’s
perspective are some of the most dramatic changes during
adoles-
cence (Dumontheil, Houlton, Christoff, & Blakemore, 2010);
and
(3) this is an age when many children begin to access personal
mobile technology and media use peaks (Rideout et al., 2010).
We investigated whether an absence of screens, and, accord-
ingly, increased opportunities for face-to-face communication,
gave children the context to be more sensitive in comprehending
nonverbal emotion cues. Our hypothesis was that, relative to a
matched control group that continued their usual daily activities
–including screen-based activities– both in and outside of
school,
children’s skill at recognizing emotion from nonverbal cues
would
improve after five days of increased opportunity for face-to-
face
interaction in an environment without screens.
3. Method
3.1. Design and Participants
The study design involved a pre- and post-test, and a no-inter-
vention matched control group. Both the experimental and
control
(i.e., no-intervention) groups were comprised of sixth graders
recruited from the same public school in Southern California.
The
experimental group included 51 children from the Spring 2012
class,
and the control included 54 children from the Fall 2012 class.
Given
that the two groups attended the same public school, the groups
were drawn from the same population and therefore matched on
many important demographic variables. In an average day of the
week during the school year, both groups reported spending
approx-
imately 4 1/2 h a day outside of school texting, watching
television,
and playing videogames (see Table 1 for key demographic
variables
for both groups).
Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–
392 389
The experimental group participated in the Pali Institute, an
outdoor education overnight camp facility, located 70 miles out-
side of Los Angeles, where neither electronic devices nor access
to any kind of screens was permitted. Opportunities for face-to-
face social interaction included living together in cabins, going
on hikes together, and working as a team to build emergency
shel-
ters. Our choice to use the camp as an intervention, rather than
ask
children to stop using media on their own or bring them into a
lab
environment, was a deliberate strategy that provided control, as
well as ecological and external validity.
The camp is educational; schoolchildren spend the day
immersed in activities meant to teach science through outdoor
instruction (see Table 2 for a description of the daily activities).
The school signed up their entire sixth-grade cohort to attend
the
camp (and planned for the control group to attend in the Spring
of 2013), and thus there was virtually no self-selection. In
choosing
the control group, we considered other groups, such as an over-
night camp that integrated screens into the daily activities, but
we determined that the selection effects outweighed the benefits
of matching on the overnight experience; in other words,
children
who are interested in these kinds of technology-oriented camps,
and whose parents could afford the cost (e.g., currently, UCLA
Tech
overnight camps are approximately $2000 for one week), would
not be a good match for children who were sent to an outdoor
nat-
ure camp underwritten by a public school district. In addition,
the
children’s social network was controlled for, because the same
children were together at camp with their peers from their sixth-
grade classes at school.
Participants in the control group attended school, with a typical
week of instruction at a California public elementary school
(i.e.,
history, English, math instruction, etc.), each day between the
pre- and post-test with no restrictions placed on their media use
by our research team. While we did not collect information
about
after school, beyond asking children about their typical day’s
media activities, a teacher at the school polled her class and
shared
with us that the majority of the children participate in sports
two
to four times a week. UCLA’s Institutional Review Board
approved
this study.
Table 1
Key demographics for the experimental and control groups.
Camp
Sample size and gender 51 (25 Boy
Age (yrs; mean ± SD) 11.86 ± .46
Range 11–
Ethnicity� White 26
Hispanic 9
African Am
Asian 9 (1
Other/mix
Parents’ education Mother:
Finished h
Some colle
Finished c
Beyond co
Father:
Finished h
Some colle
Finished c
Beyond co
Media use/ownership 22 (43%) O
51 (100%)
Media use (mean ± SD) hours per school day Texting: .9
Watching
Playing vid
Note: no variables were significantly different between
experimental and control groups
with change scores on the dependent variables. Percentages of
parents’ education do no
3.2. Measures
Participants in both conditions began by taking a one-time
online media-use survey to measure their daily media activities.
We created this Media Use Questionnaire using items from pre-
existing surveys (Pea et al., 2012; Uhls, 2013). In order to
create
a media-use variable to employ as a covariate, we added
together
the amount of time during an average school day participants
reported spending on computers, television, video games, and
cell
phones. For computers and cell phones, amount of time was
calcu-
lated by summing amount of time reported for specific activities
using these media (e.g., video chatting, texting, posting videos).
This measure was used as a control in our analysis.
After comprehensive piloting, for our dependent variables, we
chose two well-validated tests, also extensively tested with chil-
dren, to assess the ability to decode emotional nonverbal
commu-
nication. Because the ability to accurately read emotion in the
facial expression of others is one of the most important
nonverbal
communication skills, we used the Faces subtests of the second
edition of the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Behavior
(DANVA2)
(Nowicki & Carton, 1993). This test has been validated with
partic-
ipants differing in age, gender, ethnicity, intellectual ability and
cultural background in over 385 studies (Nowicki, 2010). The
Faces
subtest includes 48 photos of faces (24 children and 24 adults)
with happy, sad, angry, and fearful emotions in both high and
low intensity. These photos of children and adult faces were
each
flashed onto a screen for 2 s after which participants recorded
on
a sheet the emotion the actor exhibited. Scoring assessed the
num-
ber of errors made in the identification of emotions.
Approximately
half the children in the control group and half the children in
the
experimental group were given adult faces first; the other half
in
each group were given the child faces first.
Our second test examined children’s ability to integrate and
accurately interpret different kinds of nonverbal cues within a
set-
ting that more clearly reflected real life. The Child and
Adolescent
Social Perception Measure (CASP), a measure that has been
vali-
dated for several different populations (Clikeman-Semrud,
Walkowiak, Wilkinson, & Minne, 2010; Guiltner, 2000; Magill-
Control
s; 26 girls) 54 (26 Boys; 28 girls)
11.81 ± .52
13 Range 11–13
(51%) White 11 (20%)
(18%) Hispanic 9 (17%)
erican 1 (2%) African American 1 (2%)
7%) Asian 19 (35%)
ed 6 (12%) Other/mixed 14 (26%)
Mother:
igh school 5 (10%) Finished high school 7 (12%)
ge 10 (20%) Some college 10 (18%)
ollege 15 (29%) Finished college 21 (37%)
llege 6 (12%) Beyond college 5 (9%)
Father:
igh school 6 (12%) Finished high school 9 (16%)
ge 9 (17%) Some college 7 (12%)
ollege 18 (35%) Finished college 21 (37%)
llege 5 (10%) Beyond college 3 (5%)
wn phone 26 (48%) Own phone
Computer at home 52 (96%) Computer at home
± 1.3 Texting: 1.1 ± 1.6
TV: 2.4 ± 1.4 Watching TV: 2.1 ± 1.6
eo games: 1.2 ± 1.3 Playing video games: 1.4 ± 1.4
except for *ethnicity: t(105) = �2.95, P < 0.01. However,
ethnicity was not correlated
t total 100% because some subjects did not know their parents’
educational history.
Table 2
Sample list of classes in a day at Pali Institute.
Class name Description
Forest
ecology
Students hike through the forest to explore and learn about the
ecosystems around them. They identify flora and fauna and
participate in hands-on
activities. Through these various activities, students understand
the history of the forest as the ecosystems come alive before
their eyes
Outdoor skills Mixing nature’s beauty with outdoor survival,
students learn the Ten Essentials for any outdoor trip. They
learn fire-building and cooking food in an
outdoor setting. While in the forest, they will band together as a
team to build emergency shelters. By the end of this class,
students understand basic
principles of exploring the great outdoors
Animal
survivor
Students are taught the importance and dynamics of food
chains/webs and how species depend on one another for
survival. In a fast-paced activity,
students are assigned an identity: carnivores, herbivores or
omnivores. They must search for food while avoiding predators
(their peers). Each student
begins the game with a certain number of lives and must have at
least one life remaining at the end to be a ‘‘survivor’’
Day hike Schools have the opportunity to select their focus for a
hike, such as birding, visiting a nature center and greenhouse,
or shortened versions of a double-
session forest ecology or outdoor skills class
Archery Students learn the history and mechanics of archery,
one of the oldest arts and means for survival. They are
introduced to the basic physics of a bow
and arrow, as well as the proper handling of this ancient device.
With this knowledge, they participate in target shooting.
Students gain an
understanding of the importance of archery and its influence on
society
Orienteering Students find their sense of direction while
engaging in one of several orienteering courses. During the
expedition, they learn how to navigate through
the forest by using compasses and coordinates. They gain an
understanding of the various skills involved in planning travel
from point A to point B
Note: (each lesson approximately 90 min); link to curriculum
for Pali Mountain outdoor education program:
<http://www.paliinstitute.com/oe.html>.
390 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014)
387–392
Evans, Koning, Cameron-Sadava, & Manyk, 1995), assesses the
social perception skills of children and adolescents using ten
video-
taped scenes in which actors (children and adults) perform a
rep-
resentative scenario in different situations typical of an
adolescent’s life (e.g., school, home) (Magill-Evans et al.,
1995). In
each scene, the verbal content is removed, requiring participants
to receive and interpret nonverbal social cues without speech
cues.
After watching the videotaped scenes, the test-taker is asked to
make a judgment about the emotional states of the actors.
To adapt the CASP for our design, we randomly separated the
videos into sets of five. The two sets were counterbalanced as
pre-
test and posttest in both the control and experimental groups.
Chil-
dren watched each of the five videos in sequence and were
given
several minutes to record a written description of the emotions
of each actor before moving on to the next one. These forms
were
then scored.
We used the existing coding system of the CASP to create a
total
emotion score based on the sum of number of accurate, partially
correct, or wrong answers for each character and scene. A
partici-
pant was given two points for each correctly identified emotion,
one point for a partially correct identification, and zero for an
incorrect identification or no identification. Different videos
had
different numbers of actors; in some videos an actor would
mani-
fest more than one emotion in different parts of the scene.
There-
fore, the maximum score varied from video to video. Because
the
maximum score on one form was 41, whereas the maximum
score
on the other form was 45, for our analysis, all scores were con-
verted into percentages, so that change scores would be
compara-
ble across both orders of administration.
The forward Digit Span (Wechsler, 2004) a subset of the
Wechs-
ler Intelligence Scale for children, was administered as a
distracter
task between the CASP and the DANVA2.
3.3. Procedure
3.3.1. Experimental group
Upon arrival to the camp on a Monday morning, and immedi-
ately after exiting the school bus, the entire sixth-grade class
took
the media use survey. Next, children were randomly assigned to
one of two administration groups to take the pre-test. A
moderator
in each group followed a scripted protocol to administer each
test.
In both groupings, children completed the DANVA2 and
watched
five videos from the CASP, with the distracter task in between
the measures. Group 1 first completed the DANVA2, followed
by the Digit Span, and next the CASP; whereas Group 2 first
completed the CASP, followed by the Digit Span, and
concluded
with the DANVA2.
The post-test was also administered at the camp, on the Friday
immediately before the children mounted school buses for their
ride home. Children stayed in the same groups that were
assigned
for the pre-test. We followed the same procedure of testing as in
the pre-test but did not re-administer the media use survey.
3.3.2. Control group
Children were kept with their classes (each class was one
group). Testing occurred on Monday and Friday at
approximately
the same time of school day as during the camp. We followed
the same procedure for administering the tests for the control as
we did for the experimental.
In both the experimental and control groups, we counterbal-
anced the testing order across two administration groups and pre
and post-tests (i.e., adult or children faces first for the
DANVA2
and set one or set two of the CASP videos).
3.4. Analysis
We began our analysis by conducting independent-sample t-
tests to compare each administration group within condition on
key socio-demographic characteristics, dependent variables at
pre-test, media use and social variables. Finding no significant
dif-
ferences, we combined data from the two administration groups
and next conducted the same analysis to confirm equivalent
soci-
odemographic characteristics and media use across conditions.
We
found only one significant difference between the experimental
and control condition; and that was a difference in ethnic
compo-
sition (t(105) = �2.95, P < 0.01) (see Table 1 for ethnic
breakdown in
each group). Following up this significant difference, we ran a
cor-
relational analysis to determine whether ethnicity was related to
any of the dependent variables; it was not, and thus was not
con-
sidered a significant factor in our analysis.
Statistical literature indicated that the reliability of gain scores
is higher than other variables in many practical situations with
designs similar to our experiment (i.e., nonrandomized control
group pretest and posttest designs); thus we chose change scores
as our dependent variable (Dimitrov & Rumrill, 2003). We
calcu-
lated change scores by measuring the difference between pretest
and posttest scores on each measure.
The DANVA change scores were calculated by subtracting post-
test errors from pretest errors and ranged from �10 to 31 with
positive numbers showing error reduction. The CASP change
scores
were calculated by subtracting the total emotion percentage cor-
http://www.paliinstitute.com/oe.html
Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–
392 391
rect on the pretest from the total emotion percentage correct on
the posttest and ranged from �14% to 31% with positive
numbers
showing improvement in the total emotion percentage correct.
Three coders achieved inter-rater reliability on 20% of the
CASP
responses (Cronbach’s alpha = .93).
We used these scores to investigate potential differences
between the experimental and control condition for both the
DAN-
VA2 and CASP. For both dependent variables, we ran univariate
analyses of covariance, the preferred method of analysis for this
design (Dimitrov & Rumrill, 2003), using gender, ethnicity, and
age, as well as a composite variable called media-use sum (i.e.,
sum of time spent watching television, playing video games,
using
cell phones, and using computers) as covariates, in order to
control
for demographics and prior media use.
4. Results
We found that children who were away from screens for five
days with many opportunities for in-person interaction
improved
significantly in reading facial emotion (DANVA 2), compared
to
those in the control group, who experienced their normal media
exposure during an equivalent five-day period (F5,88 = 4.06,
p < .05, d = .33). In the experimental condition, participants
went
from an average of 14.02 errors in the Faces pretest (including
both
child and adult faces) to an average of 9.41 errors in the
posttest (a
reduction of 4.61 errors), while the control group went from and
average of 12.24 to 9.81, which was a reduction of 2.43 errors
(we attribute this change to a practice effect). Thus, the group
that
attended camp without access to any screen-based media
improved significantly more than the control group, who experi-
enced their usual amount of screen time. Fig. 1 illustrates these
change scores.
We found a similar effect when using the videotaped scenarios
(CASP). Ability to correctly identify the emotion of actors was
sig-
nificantly greater for the children who had experienced five
days of
camp without personal media than for the control group
(F5,86 = 8.75, p < .01, d = .66). In the experimental condition,
scores
improved between pre- (i.e. M = 26% correct) and post-test (i.e.
M = 31% correct); in the control group, children’s scores stayed
flat
at 28% correct on pre- and posttest. Thus, children in the
experi-
mental group showed significant improvement in their ability to
recognize the nonverbal emotional cues in videotaped scenes,
while the emotion-reading cues of the control group showed no
change between pretest and posttest.
Fig. 1. Error reduction from pretest to posttest in assessing
emotion on DANVA2
faces in experimental and control group (F5,88 = 4.06, p <
0.05).
5. Discussion
In today’s world, digital media use begins at a very early age
(Common Sense Media, 2013) and takes up a large proportion
of
the informal learning environment (Greenfield, 2009), making it
essential to assess the effects of the substantial amount of time
children engage with media. This study provides evidence that,
in five days of being limited to in-person interaction without
access to any screen-based or media device for communication,
preteens improved on measures of nonverbal emotion
understand-
ing, significantly more than a control group.
We recognize that the design of this study makes it challenging
to tease out the separate effects of the group experience, the
nature
experience, and the withdrawal of screen-time; but it is likely
that
the augmentation of in-person communication necessitated by
the
absence of digital communication significantly contributed to
the
observed experimental effect. In other words, the time the
partic-
ipants spent engaging with other children and adults face-to-
face
seemed to make an important difference. The absence of screens
meant children could rely only on face-to-face interaction when
communicating during camp activities. Accordingly, the results
suggest that digital screen time, even when used for social
interac-
tion, could reduce time spent developing skills in reading
nonver-
bal cues of human emotion.
Another possibility for the observed effect is that nature activi-
ties could have caused the improvement in reading emotions
com-
municated through nonverbal cues. While other studies have
demonstrated the cognitive benefits from interacting with nature
(Atchley, Strayer, & Atchley, 2012; Berman, Jonides, & Kaplan,
2008) it is counterintuitive (and counter to the research on com-
municative learning) that being in nature, which is not an inher-
ently social activity and often is more isolating than urban
settings, could help someone learn to understand the emotions
of
other individuals.
Our findings are in line with developmental research pointing
to the importance of in-person peer interaction as a learning
pro-
cess that leads to skill in understanding the emotions of others
(Bosacki & Astington, 1999). These results are also in line with
find-
ings in neuroscience. For example, recent brain imaging with
adult
participants showed that the neural synchronization during face-
to-face dialog does not exist when communicating back to back
(Jiang et al., 2012).
5.1. Limitations and future research
As mentioned above, a limitation to our study is that we cannot
disentangle the effects of the three factors: the group
experience,
the nature experience, and the absence of screens, as these vari-
ables were all features of the experimental condition. We
hypoth-
esize that the effect of being in a setting that included
potentially
more opportunities for face-to-face group interaction than were
afforded in the control group was the critical factor. But even
with-
out being able to delineate all the conditions under which social
skill improvement would take place, this experiment does
suggest
that day-to-day, media-saturated environments interfere with a
preteen’s understanding of face-to-face communications, while
rich opportunities for in-person social interaction enhance
under-
standing of nonverbal emotion cues.
A next step is to attempt to generalize our findings by testing
the effects of eliminating screen time in the presence of a
different
set of activities, to determine whether withdrawing media expo-
sure or adding daily face-to-face interactions underlies the
improvement in recognizing nonverbal emotion cues. Because
skill
in reading emotional cues is essential to an individual’s ability
to
function in society (Gross & Ballif, 1991) further research is
called
for. In addition, it would be important to understand the extent
to
which these kinds of effects are lasting; one would expect that it
would be necessary to reduce screen time and increase face-to-
face
392 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014)
387–392
time on an ongoing basis in order to maintain or build on the
effects demonstrated in this short-term field experiment.
6. Conclusions
The results of this study should introduce a much-needed soci-
etal conversation about the costs and benefits of the enormous
amount of time children spend with screens, both inside and
out-
side the classroom. Given that a pre-requisite for effective
sociali-
zation is learning and practicing how to communicate with
others
in person (Eder & Nenga, 2003), face-to-face experiences must
be
emphasized in the socialization process. While digital media
pro-
vide many useful ways to communicate and learn, our study
sug-
gests that skills in reading human emotion may be diminished
when children’s face-to-face interaction is displaced by
technolog-
ically mediated communication. Today, even children under
2 years of age use mobile devices (Common Sense Media,
2013).
Moreover, computers and mobile tablets are rapidly entering
class-
rooms and being put in the hands of every child beginning as
early
as kindergarten (Partnership for 21st Century Skills, 2009;
Rotella,
2013) without sufficient attention to the potential costs (Cuban,
2001). Our hope is that this study will be a call to action for
research that thoroughly and systematically examines the effects
of digital media on children’s social development.
Author contributions are as follows. YTU, GS and PMG came
up with the idea. YTU and PMG developed and designed study.
YTU, MM, DG and PMG piloted the study and developed group
protocols for the measures. YTU, MM, JM, DG and EZ ran
participants. YTU, MM, JM, DG and EZ coded data. YTU, MM
and
PMG developed and/or ran data analyses. YTU and PMG wrote
the manuscript. All authors read and offered criticism of the
manuscript.
Acknowledgements
We thank Andy Wechsler, Jolie Nelson and Michael Urbach at
Pali Institute for welcoming our study at their outdoor camp.
We
also express our appreciation to the counselors who helped us
and to the campers who participated in the research, as well as
the school, the teachers, administrators, parents and students.
Thank you to research assistant Christine Clemmons for helping
with pilot testing and to Bridget Roy for helping run
participants
and code the CASP. All participants who contributed
substantially
to the study are listed in these acknowledgments.
References
Atchley, R. A., Strayer, D. L., & Atchley, P. (2012). Creativity
in the wild: Improving
creative reasoning through immersion in natural settings. PLoS
ONE, 7(12).
Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive
benefits of interacting
with nature. Psychological Science, 19, 1207–1212.
Bindley, K. (2011). When Children text all day, what happens to
their social skills?
Huffingtonpost.com.
<http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children-
texting-technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html> (Retrieved
13.03.13).
Blakemore, S. J. (2003). How does the brain deal with the social
world? Neuro
Report, 14, 1–10.
Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of mind in
preadolescence: Relations
between social understanding and social competence. Social
Development, 8,
237–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093.
Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion
understanding from three to
six years. Child Development, 67, 789–802. doi:
10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1996.tb01764.x.
Clikeman-Semrud, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Minne,
E. P. (2010). Direct and
indirect measures of social perception, behavior and emotional
functioning in
children with Asperger’s disorder, nonverbal learning disability,
or ADHD.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 509–519.
Common Sense Media. (2013). Zero to Eight: Children’s media
use in America. <http://
www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-childrens-
media-use-in-
america-2013>.
Cuban, L. (2001). Oversold and underused. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Culnan, M., & Markus, M. (1987). Information technologies. In
F. Jablin, L. Putnam, K.
Roberts, & L. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizatinal
communication
(pp. 420–443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Dimitrov, D. M., & Rumrill, P. D. (2003). Pretest–posttest
designs and measurement
of change. IOS Press, 20, 159–165.
Dumontheil, I., Houlton, R., Christoff, K., & Blakemore, S. J.
(2010). Development of
relational reasoning during adolescence. Developmental
Science, 13, F15–F24.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x.
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade,
L. (1991). Young
children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs:
Individual
differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 6, 1352–
1366. doi:
0.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01610.x.
Eder, D., & Nenga, S. K. (2003). Socialization in adolescence.
In J. Delamater (Ed.),
Handbook of social psychology (pp. 157–175). New York:
Kluwer Academic/
Plenum Publishers.
Flom, R., & Johnson, S. (2010). The effects of adults’ affective
expression and
direction of visual gaze on 12-month-olds’ visual preferences
following a 5
minute, 1-day or 1-month delay. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 29,
64–85.
Giedd, J. N. (2012). The digital revolution and adolescent brain
evolution. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 51, 101–105.
Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Technology and informal education:
What is taught, what
is learned. Science, 323, 69–71.
Gross, A. L., & Ballif, B. (1991). Children’s understanding of
emotion from facial
expression and situations: A review. Developmental Review, 11,
368–398.
Guernsey, L. (2011). Screentime: How electronic media – from
baby videos to
educational software – affects your child. Basic Books.
Guiltner, V. D. (2000). Validation of the child and adolescent
social perception measure.
Edmonton, Alberta: Department of Education Psychology.
Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from
television by 24- and 30-
month-olds. Developmental Science, 6, 254–261.
Hofferth, S. L. (2010). Home media and children’s achievement
and behavior. Child
Development, 81, 1598–1619.
Internet world stats. (2013). Internet World Stats. <http://
www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm>.
Jiang, J., Dai, B., Peng, D., Zhu, C., Liu, L., & Lu, C. (2012).
Neural synchronization
during face-to-face communication. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 32,
16064–16069. doi:10:1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012.
Knapp, M. L, & Hall, J. A (2010). Nonverbal communication in
human interaction
(Seventh.). Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Lenhart, A. (2012). Teens, smartphones and texting.
Washington DC: Pew Research
Center.
Magill-Evans, J., Koning, C., Cameron-Sadava, A., & Manyk,
K. (1995). The child and
adolescent social percetion measure. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 19, 151.
McClure, E., & Nowicki, S. Jr., (2001). Associations between
social anxiety and
nonverbal processing skill in preadolescent boys and girls.
Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 25, 3–19.
Moore, C., & Dunham, P. J. (Eds.). (1995). Joint attention: Its
origins and role in
development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nowicki, S. Jr., (2010). Manual for the receptive tests of the
DANVA2.
Nowicki, S., & Carton, J. (1993). The measurement of
emotional intensity from facial
expressions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 749–750.
Partnership for 21st Century Skills. (2009). 21st Century
student outcomes.
Washington DC.
Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford,
H., et al. (2012). Media
use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social
well-being
among 8–12 year old girls. Developmental Psychology, 48,
327–336. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030.
Richert, R. A., Robb, M., & Smith, E. I. (2011). Media as social
partners: The social
nature of young children’s learning from screen media. Child
Development, 82,
82–95.
Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010).
Generation M2: Media in the lives of
8–18 year-olds. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation.
Rotella, C. (2013). No child left untableted. New York Times.
NYC, NY. <http://
www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left-
untableted.html?_r=0>.
Sherman, L. E., Michikyan, M., & Greenfeld, P. M. (2013). The
effects of text, audio,
video, and in-person communication on bonding between
friends.
Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on
Cyberspace, 7. Article 3.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2013-2-3.
Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1988). Reducing social context cues:
Electronic mail in
organizational communication. Management Science, 32, 1492–
1512.
Uhls, Y. (2013). Values and new media: How social media
relates to preteen values.
Seattle, Wa: Presented at the Society for Research on Child
Development.
Wartella, E. (2012). The influence of media on young children’s
development. Zero
to Three, 18–22.
Wartella, E., O’Keefe, B., & Scantlin, R. (2000). Children and
interactive media: A
compendium of current research and directions for the future.
Markle Foundation.
Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
children. Pearson Assessment.
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0005
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0005
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0010
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0010
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children-texting-
technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children-texting-
technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0020
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035
http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-
childrens-media-use-in-america-2013
http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-
childrens-media-use-in-america-2013
http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-
childrens-media-use-in-america-2013
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0045
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0055
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0055
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0080
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0080
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0085
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0085
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0090
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0090
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0095
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0095
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0100
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0100
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0105
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0105
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0110
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0110
http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm
http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0125
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0125
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0130
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0130
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0135
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0135
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0145
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0145
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0150
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0155
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0155
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0175
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0175
http://www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left-
untableted.html?_r=0
http://www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left-
untableted.html?_r=0
http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2013-2-3
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0190
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0190
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0195
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0195
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0200
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0200
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0210Five
days at outdoor education camp without screens improves
preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues1 Introduction1.1
Face-to-face and mediated communication1.2 The video
deficit1.3 Reading nonverbal emotion cues: processes of
development and learning2 Research question and hypothesis:
the present study3 Method3.1 Design and Participants3.2
Measures3.3 Procedure3.3.1 Experimental group3.3.2 Control
group3.4 Analysis4 Results5 Discussion5.1 Limitations and
future research6 ConclusionsAcknowledgementsReferences
General guidelines for writing reaction papers
(Read this document fully! It’s 5 pages and contains important
information):
Reaction papers are thought papers where you critique an
article. As you read the assigned articles, point out 1) at least
one interesting fact that you learned from the introduction, 2)
study’s strengths, 3) the limitations of their research design (for
example, the way they defined or measured their variables, the
measures’ reliability/validity, their data collection technique
[e.g., self-report, lab visits, direct observation]), 4) implications
of their findings (so what do they findings mean in real world!.
In your implications section you must relate the study’s findings
to real life, and give it some context to make it relevant for lay
people), 5) future direction ideas (what would you want to test
next to build up on the findings of this research, and/or to
address its shortcomings).
These are some questions to have in mind as you read the
article:
· Did they account for confounding factors?
· What other factors could explain their findings?
· Were the findings substantial? Who will benefit from these?
· What were some of the considerations or little things that the
researchers took into account that strengthened their design?
· If you were to do subsequent investigations, what next steps
would you take?
· Also, if the article posed questions in your mind, mention the
questions and take a stab at giving answers too!
Show me that you’ve thought the article thorough. I evaluate
your reaction papers based on thedepth of your thoughts and
how sophisticated and well explained your arguments comments
are.
SUPER IMPORTANT NOTE regarding LIMITATIONS:
When pointing out the limitations, EXPLAIN how addressing
the limitation could mean getting different results. For example,
if the study’s participants are all socioeconomically advantaged
and you see this a limitation because it’s not nationally
representative, discuss how results of a mid/low SES sample
could be different. Simply saying that the results aren’t
“generalizable” IS NOT ENOUGH. You must justify your
argument for selecting a more diverse sample, otherwise there is
not enough evidence to suggest that the study’s findings are not
generalizable! Again, please realize that it is your explanations
and arguments that I evaluate, so don’t leave your comments
unexplained or unsupported.
SUPER IMPORTANT NOTE regarding STRENGHTS:
I have found that students are often confused as to what they
should consider a “strength” and what things are just “given
(must haves!)” in a work that is published in an academic
journal. Below are things that are NOT strengths, and rather
“given”, so please don’t include these as strengths of the
article! Violation of these can be considered a limitation:
· Random assignment
· Having conditions that differ on only one aspect
· Coders being blind to the study’s hypotheses
· Use of reliable and valid measures
· Citing relevant prior research
· High inter-rater reliability
· Having IRB approval
· Getting a baseline to compare post-intervention results with
To identify strengths, think about what steps the researchers
took into account for possible little things that could skew their
results. For instance, having “practice trials” before engaging
kids in a computer game, to account for differences in
familiarity with the game, device, etc. These are extra steps,
and thoughtful ones, that researchers take, and are considered
true strengths.
One more thing, in your reaction paper, refrain from statements
such as “the paper was great”, “I liked this finding”, “I thought
this finding was interesting”, without explaining why!! It’s okay
to like the study, but it’s important to reason why you found the
article interesting and important. Also, even if you really liked
the paper, you must still be able to able to play the role of a
sceptic and find few points to criticize the paper on. Your
comments must be deep and critical rather than superficial or
simply a reiteration of what was mentioned in the article.
Organization and Structure:
Start by summarizing the article in one very short paragraph
(NO more than 3-4 lines)! And then continue critiquing the
article. Reaction papers must be 2-3 pages. Note that most
students can’t write a thorough reaction paper in 2 pages, unless
they are true “concise writers”, so don’t stop at two pages,
unless you’ve touched on several key points about the article
(and don’t repeat the points you already mentioned, just to
cover a third page!). Be sure to cover the five main key parts
that were mentioned in the first paragraph, and know that your
focus should be on limitations.
Formatting:
· WORD DOC only! No PDFs please.
· Times New Roman font, 12 point, Double spaced, 1” margin
all sides.
· No cover page needed
· Citation of the assigned paper not needed. Any extra sources
must be cited
· APA style of writing
· Submit your paper on canvas by the due date
· I will accept reaction papers only if they’re submitted before
class time, AND if you are present in class to discuss your
paper. If you absolutely have to miss a class where we’re
discussing articles, you can make up the reaction paper by
choosing an article on the topic of the week and write a reaction
paper on the new article. Email me your reaction paper and the
article PDF. You may earn a maximum of 40 points (instead of
the typical 50 points) for this reaction paper. Only ONE
reaction paper can be made up this way. So if you miss more
than one article discussion day, I can’t give you the option to
make it up again.
· Note that on the week when you are the presenting group, you
will submit a single reaction paper collectively as a group. The
organizer should upload this reaction paper (under his/her
name) and the rest of group members don’t have to worry about
uploading anything.
REACTION PAPER GRADING RUBRIC
Performance category
Quality
Content (30 pts)
Organization(10 pts)
Grammar (10 pts); Correct choice of verb tenses, words,
avoidance of wordy phrases)
Exceeds expectations
Offers several high quality comments about the reading that
demonstrate a high level of understanding as well as
sophisticated analysis of the material. Embodies originality,
complexity, and depth, rather than just a presentation of the
obvious; shows evidence of effective inquiry and
argumentation. Clearly shows evidence of “Critical” thinking
Points: 25-30
Is stellar in construction, with compelling wording, smooth
transitions, and organizational clarity.
Points: 10
No errors whatsoever!
Points: 10
Meets expectations
Demonstrates a good level of understanding and raises many
points, but only one or none of the points demonstrate “deep”
analysis where KEY factors are discussed (i.e., sticks to
discussion of the obvious, like sample size and
generalizability). All or most of the points that are raised are
accurate
Points: 15-25
Language is generally appropriate to a professional audience
and organization is sound.
Points: 6-9
Between 1-4 grammatical or punctuation errors, or typos, but
they are minor and do not detract from the paper.
Points: 6-9
Needs improvement
Offers few comments, and they are superficial. Some comments
have accuracy issues (e.g., invalid criticisms, or discussion of a
limitation that was addressed in the paper by taking a specific
measure that resolved the issue completely).
Points: 5-15
Quality of writing may be inconsistent (i.e., quite good in some
sections of the paper and of lesser quality in other sections.
E.g., there are paragraphs containing critiquing points after
what seems to be a concluding paragraph); organization needs
improvement.
Points: 3-6
4-7 grammatical or punctuation errors, or typos.
Points: 3-6
Unsatisfactory
Sounds more like a summary than an analysis. Offers only one
or two comments which are on the superficial side.
Points: 0-5
Paper is incoherent, or unorganized with little agreement
between ideas.
Points: 0-3
More than 7 grammatical, punctuation errors or typos. Errors
impede understanding of content and require multiple readings
and guessing to figure out the message that’s being conveyed.
Points: 3-6
AG 401 Book/Video Discussion Rubric (Total Points 12)
Being scored on:
0 Unacceptable
1
Poor/Very Limited
2
OK/Good
3
Excellent
Criteria 1:
Word count and organization
100 words or less and/or single spaced.
Greater than 100 words without paragraphs and/or single
spaced.
Greater than 100 words with two or more paragraphs with a
minimum of one citations in each paragraph and/or single
spaced.
Greater than 100 words and at least three paragraphs with a
minimum of one citation in each paragraph AND double spaced.
Criteria 2:
Critical Thinking and use of ethical theory
No ethical theories applied
Minimal application of ethical theory
Good application of ethical theory
Detailed application of ethical theory
Criteria 3:
Number of ethical theory citations used in support. These
should add value and be meaningful.
No ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes). Pp,
video, topical and Open Web citations do not count.
2 or fewer ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes
from 2 or fewer sources). (NO Pp, video citations, topical or
Open Web). Sources are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical
theory sources (philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable:
CPP library or S-L textbook).
3 ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes from 3
different sources). (NO Pp, video citations, topical or Open
Web). Sources are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical theory
sources (philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable: CPP
library or S-L textbook).
3 or more ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes
from 3 different sources) used in support for all theories being
applied. (NO Pp, video citations, topical or Open Web). Sources
are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical theory sources
(philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable: CPP library or S-
L textbook).
Criteria 4:
APA citations and References section
Numerous improper APA citations and/or References errors.
Use APA book!
2 or more improper APA citations and/or References errors. Use
APA book!
1-2 improper APA citations and/or References errors. Use APA
book!
Proper APA citations and References section on sources used.
When you cite make sure and list the page number or paragraph
number. Use APA book!
Note:Extra points deducted for misspelling, poor grammar or
errors made repeatedly week after week after corrections and
feedback have already been given. Professor reserves the right
to deduct points, give warnings or zeros for submissions
slightly over 30% similarity (includes References section at
end). If you are having trouble with % similarity, paraphrase
MORE and direct quote less and include more original thought
to reduce. Zeros can and will be given for submissions that do
not follow the prompt or have % similarity exceeding the 30%
threshold.
Book Discussion ExampleEthical egoism fosters an environment of .docx

More Related Content

Similar to Book Discussion ExampleEthical egoism fosters an environment of .docx

Social Media Research
Social Media ResearchSocial Media Research
Social Media ResearchLuke Willson
 
How technology is actually affecting your brain what we actually know
How technology is actually affecting your brain   what we actually knowHow technology is actually affecting your brain   what we actually know
How technology is actually affecting your brain what we actually knowCharityComms
 
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...Foundation for Healthy Generations
 
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS .docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS               .docxRunning Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS               .docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS .docxtodd271
 
mental health.pptx
mental health.pptxmental health.pptx
mental health.pptxMFlora2
 
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3Ashley Lott
 
Right To Die Essay.pdf
Right To Die Essay.pdfRight To Die Essay.pdf
Right To Die Essay.pdfLaurel Connor
 
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarms
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarmsPSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarms
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarmsTamara Harms
 
Deviance Regulation Theory Analysis
Deviance Regulation Theory AnalysisDeviance Regulation Theory Analysis
Deviance Regulation Theory AnalysisMiles Priar
 

Similar to Book Discussion ExampleEthical egoism fosters an environment of .docx (9)

Social Media Research
Social Media ResearchSocial Media Research
Social Media Research
 
How technology is actually affecting your brain what we actually know
How technology is actually affecting your brain   what we actually knowHow technology is actually affecting your brain   what we actually know
How technology is actually affecting your brain what we actually know
 
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...
Moving Beyond the Individual: The Importance of Community and Organizational ...
 
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS .docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS               .docxRunning Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS               .docx
Running Head EFFECTS OF SOCIAL MEDIA ON THE YOUTHS .docx
 
mental health.pptx
mental health.pptxmental health.pptx
mental health.pptx
 
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3
Human Growth And Development Study Guide Ch 1,2,3
 
Right To Die Essay.pdf
Right To Die Essay.pdfRight To Die Essay.pdf
Right To Die Essay.pdf
 
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarms
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarmsPSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarms
PSYCH610researchproposalweek8TamaraHarms
 
Deviance Regulation Theory Analysis
Deviance Regulation Theory AnalysisDeviance Regulation Theory Analysis
Deviance Regulation Theory Analysis
 

More from jasoninnes20

1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx
1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx
1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-page APA 7 the edition No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx
1-page APA 7 the edition  No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx1-page APA 7 the edition  No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx
1-page APA 7 the edition No referenceDescription of Personal a.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docxjasoninnes20
 
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docxjasoninnes20
 
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docxjasoninnes20
 
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docxjasoninnes20
 

More from jasoninnes20 (20)

1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx
1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx
1-2paragraphsapa formatWelcome to Module 6. Divers.docx
 
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx
1-Post a two-paragraph summary of the lecture;  2- Review the li.docx
 
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx
1-What are the pros and cons of parole. Discuss!2-Discuss ways t.docx
 
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx
1-page (max) proposal including a Title, Executive Summary, Outline,.docx
 
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx
1-Identify the benefits of sharing your action research with oth.docx
 
1-page APA 7 the edition No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx
1-page APA 7 the edition  No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx1-page APA 7 the edition  No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx
1-page APA 7 the edition No referenceDescription of Personal a.docx
 
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx
1-Pretend that you are a new teacher.  You see that one of your st.docx
 
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx
1- What is the difference between a multi-valued attribute and a.docx
 
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx
1- What is a Relational Algebra What are the operators. Explain.docx
 
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx
1- Watch the movie Don Quixote, which is an adaptation of Cerv.docx
 
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx
1- reply to both below, no more than 75 words per each.  PSY 771.docx
 
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx
1- Pathogenesis 2- Organs affected in the body 3- Chain of i.docx
 
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx
1-  I can totally see where there would be tension between.docx
 
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx
1- One of the most difficult challenges leaders face is to integrate.docx
 
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx
1- Design one assignment of the Word Find (education word) and the o.docx
 
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx
1- This chapter suggests that emotional intelligence is an interpers.docx
 
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx
1-2 pages APA format1. overall purpose of site 2. resources .docx
 
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx
1-Define Energy.2- What is Potential energy3- What is K.docx
 
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx
1- Find one quote from chapter 7-9. Explain why this quote stood.docx
 
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx
1-Confucianism2-ShintoChoose one of the religious system.docx
 

Recently uploaded

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Jisc
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomnelietumpap1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
Field Attribute Index Feature in Odoo 17
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
 

Book Discussion ExampleEthical egoism fosters an environment of .docx

  • 1. Book Discussion Example Ethical egoism fosters an environment of competition and promotes lack of self-lessness. Because self-interest differs from person to person, competition to fulfill personal self- interest becomes acceptable. For example, many people who have careers in the trade of business view personal gratification and fulfillment as essential to success. Pursuing one’s self interest demands that individual desires are elevated above another’s. Ethical egoism does not allow consideration for another person’s needs or permit aiding someone to reach their personal goals (Debelijak & Krkac, 2008). This philosophy fundamentally alters how society and individuals uphold or value standards of morality. If one’s goal in life is to seek personal satisfaction, then it is morally justified via ethical egoism. However, there are serious ramifications to this self- seeking ethical philosophy. Ethical egoism advocates individual preservation above the common welfare of society. If one’s desire is to steal believing he is justified because his own self-interest is being fulfilled, then he commits the crime with the belief that he is morally justified in doing so. An action is moral so long as self-interest is the only goal being achieved. Individual interests and social welfare are often in conflict due to the reality that self-interest does not permit self-sacrifice. Ethical egoism mandates that social welfare is not a priority, not even a consideration when contemplating how best to pursue one’s self-interest (Olson, 1961). This corrupt pattern of thinking creates a fixation on individual preservation rather than universal prosperity. Therefore, the only one benefiting from ethical egoism is a single individual. Everyone’s needs are not met nor are they considered important. It is a human being’s tendency to value or pursue self-interest at the cost of common welfare and prosperity. Individualism states that the interests or desires of an
  • 2. individual are regarded as supreme. Individualism is often associated with the ethical egoism because an individual’s desires are considered paramount above the collective society. As a result, the individual relates to society or their community based upon what said community can offer them as an individual. There exists no internal motive to assist another individual in pursing their desires or achieving their goals. Ethical egoism is self-centered by nature which causes a disconnect and lack of care for another’s prosperity (Gantt & Burton, 2013). I believe that the only benefit to pursuing self-interest is temporary fulfillment of a particular desire. A society or community cannot benefit from each individual seeking self- satisfaction. This self-centered philosophy ultimately leads to competition and misplaced animosity between people. It would be impossible to encourage, uplift, or connect with others if we are viewing each other as obstacles or hindrances to our selfish desires. I find ethical egoism to personify the selfish, depraved nature characteristic of human beings. If self-interest is the idol to be worshipped, then other people will suffer the concequences of such narrow-minded thinking. I believe that many people benefit when we put aside our self-interest to acknowledge and consider another person’s needs above our own. Works Cited: Debelijak, J. & Krkac, K. (2008). “Me, myself & I”: Practical egoism, selfishness, self-interest and business ethics. Social Responsibility Journal, 4(1/2), 217-227. Gantt, E. E., & Burton, J. (2013). Egoism, altruism, and the ethical foundations of personhood. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 53(4), 438-460.
  • 3. Olson, R.G. (1961). Ethical egoism and social welfare. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 21(4), 528-536. Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–392 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / c o m p h u m b e h Five days at outdoor education camp without screens improves preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036 0747-5632/� 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/). ⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, 616 Via De La Paz, Pacific Palisades, CA 90272, United States. Tel.: +1 310 526 3316; fax: +1 310 230 7830. E-mail address: [email protected] (Y.T. Uhls). Yalda T. Uhls a,⇑ , Minas Michikyan b, Jordan Morris c, Debra Garcia d,b, Gary W. Small e, Eleni Zgourou f, Patricia M. Greenfield a a Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United States b Department of Psychology, California State University, Los
  • 4. Angeles, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United States c Department of Social Welfare, University of California, Los Angeles, United States d Department of Education, University of California, Los Angeles, United States e Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, United States f Department of Education, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, Children’s Digital Media Center @ LA, United States a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Available online 15 August 2014 Keywords: Social media Nonverbal communication Emotion Adolescent Social interaction Development A field experiment examined whether increasing opportunities for face-to-face interaction while elimi- nating the use of screen-based media and communication tools improved nonverbal emotion–cue recog- nition in preteens. Fifty-one preteens spent five days at an overnight nature camp where television, computers and mobile phones were not allowed; this group was compared with school-based matched controls (n = 54) that retained usual media practices. Both groups took pre- and post-tests that required participants to infer emotional states from photographs of facial expressions and videotaped scenes with
  • 5. verbal cues removed. Change scores for the two groups were compared using gender, ethnicity, media use, and age as covariates. After five days interacting face-to- face without the use of any screen-based media, preteens’ recognition of nonverbal emotion cues improved significantly more than that of the con- trol group for both facial expressions and videotaped scenes. Implications are that the short-term effects of increased opportunities for social interaction, combined with time away from screen-based media and digital communication tools, improves a preteen’s understanding of nonverbal emotional cues. � 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/). 1. Introduction study found that the amount of non-screen playtime decreased For several millennia, humans’ primary method for social learn- ing and communication has been face to face. In the 21st century, as mobile technology and the Internet became available to most of the world’s population (Internet world stats, 2013), digital media have become an increasingly prevalent factor in the informal learn- ing environment (Greenfield, 2009). Children today, ages 8–18, spend over 7½ h a day, seven days a week using media outside of school (Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010). Moreover, teenagers, ages 12–17, report using phones to text message in their daily lives more than any other form of communication, including face-to- face socializing (Lenhart, 2012). The extensive time that children
  • 6. and teenagers engage with media and communicate using screens may be taking time away from face-to-face communication and some in-person activities (Giedd, 2012). Indeed, one longitudinal 20% from 1997 to 2003, while screen activities (i.e., watching tele- vision, playing videogames and using the computer) increased (Hofferth, 2010). The advent of mobile technology enables today’s youth to access and engage with screens 24/7 outside of school while in cars, on vacations, in restaurants, and even in bed. A recent poll found that children’s access to these kinds of devices has grown fivefold in the last two years (Common Sense Media, 2013). Extant research indicates that, today, media exposure begins at early ages, consumes the majority of youth leisure time, and takes place in many different environments and contexts. Such extensive use of new technology has raised concerns that children’s face-to-face communication skills may be negatively affected (Bindley, 2011; Giedd, 2012). 1.1. Face-to-face and mediated communication When engaging in face-to-face communication, social informa- tion is conveyed by vocal and visual cues within the context of the situation. Nonverbal communication, defined as communication http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.chb.2014.05 .036&domain=pdf http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
  • 7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/07475632 http://www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh 388 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–392 without words, includes apparent behaviors such as facial expres- sion, eye contact, and tone of voice, as well as less obvious mes- sages such as posture and spatial distance between two or more people (Knapp & Hall, 2010). The understanding of these kinds of nonverbal social cues is particularly important for social interac- tion because of the need to modify one’s own behavior in response to the reactions of others (Knapp & Hall, 2010). The capability to effectively process emotional cues is associated with many per- sonal, social and academic outcomes (Knapp & Hall, 2010; McClure & Nowicki, 2001). In addition, children who better under- stand emotional cues in a social environment may develop supe- rior social skills and form more positive peer relationships (Blakemore, 2003; Bosacki & Astington, 1999). Long before digital media became ubiquitous, investigators developed theories, such as the Cues-Filtered-Out theory, which postulated that the lack of nonverbal cues in computer-mediated interactions could lead to impersonal communication, (Culnan &
  • 8. Markus, 1987), while others pointed out deficits in computer- med- iated communication due to the lack of social-context cues (Sproull & Kiesler, 1988). More recently, an experiment exploring the differ- ence in emotional connectedness experienced by emerging adults using either in-person or digitally mediated communication showed that bonding and affiliative cues were significantly stron- ger when friends communicated in person rather than by text (Sherman, Michikyan, & Greenfeld, 2013). The extensive use of dig- ital media, often text-based and thus inherently lacking nonverbal emotional cues, may thus curtail the face-to-face experiences nec- essary to master important social skills, even though they are used for social communication (Giedd, 2012). 1.2. The video deficit Research regarding what children do and do not learn about the social world through screen-based media, in particular television, is robust (Guernsey, 2011; Richert, Robb, & Smith, 2011; Wartella, 2012; Wartella, O’Keefe, & Scantlin, 2000). Much of the research concentrates on early learning from imitation, socially contingent interaction (e.g. joint attention and gaze following), and word learning (Flom & Johnson, 2010; Moore & Dunham, 1995). This body of research shows that young children learn bet- ter from live interaction than from screens. For example, Hayne,
  • 9. Herbert, and Simcock (2003) performed a series of experiments using matched live and videotaped models who performed a series of actions with a rattle and stuffed animals. Although children imi- tated televised models, the mean imitation scores were signifi- cantly higher in the live condition. This discrepancy in imitation appears to last until 30 months of age and was coined the ‘‘video deficit.’’ 1.3. Reading nonverbal emotion cues: processes of development and learning Features of face-to-face communication such as eye contact and pointing are crucial when teaching young children about social interaction and the world they live in. For example, gaze following, a well-studied mechanism in the literature on human develop- ment, guides infants from around one year of age to learn about objects and humans (Flom & Johnson, 2010). Humans also learn from cues such as pointing when interacting socially (Moore & Dunham, 1995). Once a child is able to attend to an object that another person highlights, their ability to learn through social interaction increases. These means of learning are available only when a child can see another’s face and physical being (Gross & Ballif, 1991). In-person interaction also develops the accurate understanding of nonverbal emotion cues. For instance, cooperative interaction among siblings in the third year of life has been shown to predict skill in affective labeling of facial expressions and
  • 10. understanding of emotions in dramatized puppet scenarios in the fourth year of life (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991). The chil- dren’s positive behavior toward their siblings in the third year of life continued to predict more advanced understanding of emotions at six years of age (Brown & Dunn, 1996). These longitudinal findings point to in-person peer interaction as a key learning experience in the early acquisition of skill in reading nonverbal emotion cues. As children grow older, their peer focus shifts from siblings to unrelated peers, whom they usually meet in school. In preadoles- cence, the period under investigation in the present research, social interaction skill with peers, assessed in an in-person school situa- tion, was correlated with an understanding of feelings presented in narrative (Bosacki & Astington, 1999). 2. Research question and hypothesis: the present study In the present study, we designed a field experiment to ask the research question: Does children’s frequent screen use—and the possibility that this extensive use replaces critical face-to-face com- munication—promote the development of emotion understanding to the same extent as in-person interactions? If not, a shift in chil- dren’s activities to solely in-person peer and adult
  • 11. communication could enhance skill in understanding the emotions of other people. Our experimental condition was a naturally occurring environ- ment where children experienced extensive opportunities for social interaction, combined with no access to screens, for five days. Our participants were preteens in the sixth grade. We chose this age group because: (1) by the time they reach early adolescence, chil- dren are able to integrate information from many nonverbal cues, including face, gesture and tone, to make inferences about social sit- uations (Knapp & Hall, 2010); (2) the understanding of social emo- tions and the ability to take into account another person’s perspective are some of the most dramatic changes during adoles- cence (Dumontheil, Houlton, Christoff, & Blakemore, 2010); and (3) this is an age when many children begin to access personal mobile technology and media use peaks (Rideout et al., 2010). We investigated whether an absence of screens, and, accord- ingly, increased opportunities for face-to-face communication, gave children the context to be more sensitive in comprehending nonverbal emotion cues. Our hypothesis was that, relative to a matched control group that continued their usual daily activities –including screen-based activities– both in and outside of school, children’s skill at recognizing emotion from nonverbal cues would improve after five days of increased opportunity for face-to-
  • 12. face interaction in an environment without screens. 3. Method 3.1. Design and Participants The study design involved a pre- and post-test, and a no-inter- vention matched control group. Both the experimental and control (i.e., no-intervention) groups were comprised of sixth graders recruited from the same public school in Southern California. The experimental group included 51 children from the Spring 2012 class, and the control included 54 children from the Fall 2012 class. Given that the two groups attended the same public school, the groups were drawn from the same population and therefore matched on many important demographic variables. In an average day of the week during the school year, both groups reported spending approx- imately 4 1/2 h a day outside of school texting, watching television, and playing videogames (see Table 1 for key demographic variables for both groups). Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387– 392 389 The experimental group participated in the Pali Institute, an outdoor education overnight camp facility, located 70 miles out- side of Los Angeles, where neither electronic devices nor access to any kind of screens was permitted. Opportunities for face-to- face social interaction included living together in cabins, going
  • 13. on hikes together, and working as a team to build emergency shel- ters. Our choice to use the camp as an intervention, rather than ask children to stop using media on their own or bring them into a lab environment, was a deliberate strategy that provided control, as well as ecological and external validity. The camp is educational; schoolchildren spend the day immersed in activities meant to teach science through outdoor instruction (see Table 2 for a description of the daily activities). The school signed up their entire sixth-grade cohort to attend the camp (and planned for the control group to attend in the Spring of 2013), and thus there was virtually no self-selection. In choosing the control group, we considered other groups, such as an over- night camp that integrated screens into the daily activities, but we determined that the selection effects outweighed the benefits of matching on the overnight experience; in other words, children who are interested in these kinds of technology-oriented camps, and whose parents could afford the cost (e.g., currently, UCLA Tech overnight camps are approximately $2000 for one week), would not be a good match for children who were sent to an outdoor nat- ure camp underwritten by a public school district. In addition, the children’s social network was controlled for, because the same children were together at camp with their peers from their sixth- grade classes at school. Participants in the control group attended school, with a typical week of instruction at a California public elementary school
  • 14. (i.e., history, English, math instruction, etc.), each day between the pre- and post-test with no restrictions placed on their media use by our research team. While we did not collect information about after school, beyond asking children about their typical day’s media activities, a teacher at the school polled her class and shared with us that the majority of the children participate in sports two to four times a week. UCLA’s Institutional Review Board approved this study. Table 1 Key demographics for the experimental and control groups. Camp Sample size and gender 51 (25 Boy Age (yrs; mean ± SD) 11.86 ± .46 Range 11– Ethnicity� White 26 Hispanic 9 African Am Asian 9 (1 Other/mix Parents’ education Mother: Finished h Some colle Finished c Beyond co Father: Finished h
  • 15. Some colle Finished c Beyond co Media use/ownership 22 (43%) O 51 (100%) Media use (mean ± SD) hours per school day Texting: .9 Watching Playing vid Note: no variables were significantly different between experimental and control groups with change scores on the dependent variables. Percentages of parents’ education do no 3.2. Measures Participants in both conditions began by taking a one-time online media-use survey to measure their daily media activities. We created this Media Use Questionnaire using items from pre- existing surveys (Pea et al., 2012; Uhls, 2013). In order to create a media-use variable to employ as a covariate, we added together the amount of time during an average school day participants reported spending on computers, television, video games, and cell phones. For computers and cell phones, amount of time was calcu- lated by summing amount of time reported for specific activities using these media (e.g., video chatting, texting, posting videos). This measure was used as a control in our analysis. After comprehensive piloting, for our dependent variables, we chose two well-validated tests, also extensively tested with chil- dren, to assess the ability to decode emotional nonverbal
  • 16. commu- nication. Because the ability to accurately read emotion in the facial expression of others is one of the most important nonverbal communication skills, we used the Faces subtests of the second edition of the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Behavior (DANVA2) (Nowicki & Carton, 1993). This test has been validated with partic- ipants differing in age, gender, ethnicity, intellectual ability and cultural background in over 385 studies (Nowicki, 2010). The Faces subtest includes 48 photos of faces (24 children and 24 adults) with happy, sad, angry, and fearful emotions in both high and low intensity. These photos of children and adult faces were each flashed onto a screen for 2 s after which participants recorded on a sheet the emotion the actor exhibited. Scoring assessed the num- ber of errors made in the identification of emotions. Approximately half the children in the control group and half the children in the experimental group were given adult faces first; the other half in each group were given the child faces first. Our second test examined children’s ability to integrate and accurately interpret different kinds of nonverbal cues within a set- ting that more clearly reflected real life. The Child and Adolescent Social Perception Measure (CASP), a measure that has been vali- dated for several different populations (Clikeman-Semrud,
  • 17. Walkowiak, Wilkinson, & Minne, 2010; Guiltner, 2000; Magill- Control s; 26 girls) 54 (26 Boys; 28 girls) 11.81 ± .52 13 Range 11–13 (51%) White 11 (20%) (18%) Hispanic 9 (17%) erican 1 (2%) African American 1 (2%) 7%) Asian 19 (35%) ed 6 (12%) Other/mixed 14 (26%) Mother: igh school 5 (10%) Finished high school 7 (12%) ge 10 (20%) Some college 10 (18%) ollege 15 (29%) Finished college 21 (37%) llege 6 (12%) Beyond college 5 (9%) Father: igh school 6 (12%) Finished high school 9 (16%) ge 9 (17%) Some college 7 (12%) ollege 18 (35%) Finished college 21 (37%) llege 5 (10%) Beyond college 3 (5%) wn phone 26 (48%) Own phone Computer at home 52 (96%) Computer at home ± 1.3 Texting: 1.1 ± 1.6 TV: 2.4 ± 1.4 Watching TV: 2.1 ± 1.6 eo games: 1.2 ± 1.3 Playing video games: 1.4 ± 1.4 except for *ethnicity: t(105) = �2.95, P < 0.01. However,
  • 18. ethnicity was not correlated t total 100% because some subjects did not know their parents’ educational history. Table 2 Sample list of classes in a day at Pali Institute. Class name Description Forest ecology Students hike through the forest to explore and learn about the ecosystems around them. They identify flora and fauna and participate in hands-on activities. Through these various activities, students understand the history of the forest as the ecosystems come alive before their eyes Outdoor skills Mixing nature’s beauty with outdoor survival, students learn the Ten Essentials for any outdoor trip. They learn fire-building and cooking food in an outdoor setting. While in the forest, they will band together as a team to build emergency shelters. By the end of this class, students understand basic principles of exploring the great outdoors Animal survivor Students are taught the importance and dynamics of food chains/webs and how species depend on one another for survival. In a fast-paced activity, students are assigned an identity: carnivores, herbivores or
  • 19. omnivores. They must search for food while avoiding predators (their peers). Each student begins the game with a certain number of lives and must have at least one life remaining at the end to be a ‘‘survivor’’ Day hike Schools have the opportunity to select their focus for a hike, such as birding, visiting a nature center and greenhouse, or shortened versions of a double- session forest ecology or outdoor skills class Archery Students learn the history and mechanics of archery, one of the oldest arts and means for survival. They are introduced to the basic physics of a bow and arrow, as well as the proper handling of this ancient device. With this knowledge, they participate in target shooting. Students gain an understanding of the importance of archery and its influence on society Orienteering Students find their sense of direction while engaging in one of several orienteering courses. During the expedition, they learn how to navigate through the forest by using compasses and coordinates. They gain an understanding of the various skills involved in planning travel from point A to point B Note: (each lesson approximately 90 min); link to curriculum for Pali Mountain outdoor education program: <http://www.paliinstitute.com/oe.html>. 390 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–392 Evans, Koning, Cameron-Sadava, & Manyk, 1995), assesses the social perception skills of children and adolescents using ten video- taped scenes in which actors (children and adults) perform a
  • 20. rep- resentative scenario in different situations typical of an adolescent’s life (e.g., school, home) (Magill-Evans et al., 1995). In each scene, the verbal content is removed, requiring participants to receive and interpret nonverbal social cues without speech cues. After watching the videotaped scenes, the test-taker is asked to make a judgment about the emotional states of the actors. To adapt the CASP for our design, we randomly separated the videos into sets of five. The two sets were counterbalanced as pre- test and posttest in both the control and experimental groups. Chil- dren watched each of the five videos in sequence and were given several minutes to record a written description of the emotions of each actor before moving on to the next one. These forms were then scored. We used the existing coding system of the CASP to create a total emotion score based on the sum of number of accurate, partially correct, or wrong answers for each character and scene. A partici- pant was given two points for each correctly identified emotion, one point for a partially correct identification, and zero for an incorrect identification or no identification. Different videos had different numbers of actors; in some videos an actor would mani- fest more than one emotion in different parts of the scene. There- fore, the maximum score varied from video to video. Because
  • 21. the maximum score on one form was 41, whereas the maximum score on the other form was 45, for our analysis, all scores were con- verted into percentages, so that change scores would be compara- ble across both orders of administration. The forward Digit Span (Wechsler, 2004) a subset of the Wechs- ler Intelligence Scale for children, was administered as a distracter task between the CASP and the DANVA2. 3.3. Procedure 3.3.1. Experimental group Upon arrival to the camp on a Monday morning, and immedi- ately after exiting the school bus, the entire sixth-grade class took the media use survey. Next, children were randomly assigned to one of two administration groups to take the pre-test. A moderator in each group followed a scripted protocol to administer each test. In both groupings, children completed the DANVA2 and watched five videos from the CASP, with the distracter task in between the measures. Group 1 first completed the DANVA2, followed by the Digit Span, and next the CASP; whereas Group 2 first completed the CASP, followed by the Digit Span, and concluded with the DANVA2. The post-test was also administered at the camp, on the Friday immediately before the children mounted school buses for their
  • 22. ride home. Children stayed in the same groups that were assigned for the pre-test. We followed the same procedure of testing as in the pre-test but did not re-administer the media use survey. 3.3.2. Control group Children were kept with their classes (each class was one group). Testing occurred on Monday and Friday at approximately the same time of school day as during the camp. We followed the same procedure for administering the tests for the control as we did for the experimental. In both the experimental and control groups, we counterbal- anced the testing order across two administration groups and pre and post-tests (i.e., adult or children faces first for the DANVA2 and set one or set two of the CASP videos). 3.4. Analysis We began our analysis by conducting independent-sample t- tests to compare each administration group within condition on key socio-demographic characteristics, dependent variables at pre-test, media use and social variables. Finding no significant dif- ferences, we combined data from the two administration groups and next conducted the same analysis to confirm equivalent soci- odemographic characteristics and media use across conditions. We found only one significant difference between the experimental and control condition; and that was a difference in ethnic compo- sition (t(105) = �2.95, P < 0.01) (see Table 1 for ethnic
  • 23. breakdown in each group). Following up this significant difference, we ran a cor- relational analysis to determine whether ethnicity was related to any of the dependent variables; it was not, and thus was not con- sidered a significant factor in our analysis. Statistical literature indicated that the reliability of gain scores is higher than other variables in many practical situations with designs similar to our experiment (i.e., nonrandomized control group pretest and posttest designs); thus we chose change scores as our dependent variable (Dimitrov & Rumrill, 2003). We calcu- lated change scores by measuring the difference between pretest and posttest scores on each measure. The DANVA change scores were calculated by subtracting post- test errors from pretest errors and ranged from �10 to 31 with positive numbers showing error reduction. The CASP change scores were calculated by subtracting the total emotion percentage cor- http://www.paliinstitute.com/oe.html Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387– 392 391 rect on the pretest from the total emotion percentage correct on the posttest and ranged from �14% to 31% with positive numbers showing improvement in the total emotion percentage correct. Three coders achieved inter-rater reliability on 20% of the CASP responses (Cronbach’s alpha = .93).
  • 24. We used these scores to investigate potential differences between the experimental and control condition for both the DAN- VA2 and CASP. For both dependent variables, we ran univariate analyses of covariance, the preferred method of analysis for this design (Dimitrov & Rumrill, 2003), using gender, ethnicity, and age, as well as a composite variable called media-use sum (i.e., sum of time spent watching television, playing video games, using cell phones, and using computers) as covariates, in order to control for demographics and prior media use. 4. Results We found that children who were away from screens for five days with many opportunities for in-person interaction improved significantly in reading facial emotion (DANVA 2), compared to those in the control group, who experienced their normal media exposure during an equivalent five-day period (F5,88 = 4.06, p < .05, d = .33). In the experimental condition, participants went from an average of 14.02 errors in the Faces pretest (including both child and adult faces) to an average of 9.41 errors in the posttest (a reduction of 4.61 errors), while the control group went from and average of 12.24 to 9.81, which was a reduction of 2.43 errors (we attribute this change to a practice effect). Thus, the group that attended camp without access to any screen-based media improved significantly more than the control group, who experi- enced their usual amount of screen time. Fig. 1 illustrates these change scores.
  • 25. We found a similar effect when using the videotaped scenarios (CASP). Ability to correctly identify the emotion of actors was sig- nificantly greater for the children who had experienced five days of camp without personal media than for the control group (F5,86 = 8.75, p < .01, d = .66). In the experimental condition, scores improved between pre- (i.e. M = 26% correct) and post-test (i.e. M = 31% correct); in the control group, children’s scores stayed flat at 28% correct on pre- and posttest. Thus, children in the experi- mental group showed significant improvement in their ability to recognize the nonverbal emotional cues in videotaped scenes, while the emotion-reading cues of the control group showed no change between pretest and posttest. Fig. 1. Error reduction from pretest to posttest in assessing emotion on DANVA2 faces in experimental and control group (F5,88 = 4.06, p < 0.05). 5. Discussion In today’s world, digital media use begins at a very early age (Common Sense Media, 2013) and takes up a large proportion of the informal learning environment (Greenfield, 2009), making it essential to assess the effects of the substantial amount of time children engage with media. This study provides evidence that, in five days of being limited to in-person interaction without access to any screen-based or media device for communication, preteens improved on measures of nonverbal emotion understand- ing, significantly more than a control group. We recognize that the design of this study makes it challenging
  • 26. to tease out the separate effects of the group experience, the nature experience, and the withdrawal of screen-time; but it is likely that the augmentation of in-person communication necessitated by the absence of digital communication significantly contributed to the observed experimental effect. In other words, the time the partic- ipants spent engaging with other children and adults face-to- face seemed to make an important difference. The absence of screens meant children could rely only on face-to-face interaction when communicating during camp activities. Accordingly, the results suggest that digital screen time, even when used for social interac- tion, could reduce time spent developing skills in reading nonver- bal cues of human emotion. Another possibility for the observed effect is that nature activi- ties could have caused the improvement in reading emotions com- municated through nonverbal cues. While other studies have demonstrated the cognitive benefits from interacting with nature (Atchley, Strayer, & Atchley, 2012; Berman, Jonides, & Kaplan, 2008) it is counterintuitive (and counter to the research on com- municative learning) that being in nature, which is not an inher- ently social activity and often is more isolating than urban settings, could help someone learn to understand the emotions of other individuals. Our findings are in line with developmental research pointing to the importance of in-person peer interaction as a learning
  • 27. pro- cess that leads to skill in understanding the emotions of others (Bosacki & Astington, 1999). These results are also in line with find- ings in neuroscience. For example, recent brain imaging with adult participants showed that the neural synchronization during face- to-face dialog does not exist when communicating back to back (Jiang et al., 2012). 5.1. Limitations and future research As mentioned above, a limitation to our study is that we cannot disentangle the effects of the three factors: the group experience, the nature experience, and the absence of screens, as these vari- ables were all features of the experimental condition. We hypoth- esize that the effect of being in a setting that included potentially more opportunities for face-to-face group interaction than were afforded in the control group was the critical factor. But even with- out being able to delineate all the conditions under which social skill improvement would take place, this experiment does suggest that day-to-day, media-saturated environments interfere with a preteen’s understanding of face-to-face communications, while rich opportunities for in-person social interaction enhance under- standing of nonverbal emotion cues. A next step is to attempt to generalize our findings by testing the effects of eliminating screen time in the presence of a different set of activities, to determine whether withdrawing media expo- sure or adding daily face-to-face interactions underlies the
  • 28. improvement in recognizing nonverbal emotion cues. Because skill in reading emotional cues is essential to an individual’s ability to function in society (Gross & Ballif, 1991) further research is called for. In addition, it would be important to understand the extent to which these kinds of effects are lasting; one would expect that it would be necessary to reduce screen time and increase face-to- face 392 Y.T. Uhls et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 39 (2014) 387–392 time on an ongoing basis in order to maintain or build on the effects demonstrated in this short-term field experiment. 6. Conclusions The results of this study should introduce a much-needed soci- etal conversation about the costs and benefits of the enormous amount of time children spend with screens, both inside and out- side the classroom. Given that a pre-requisite for effective sociali- zation is learning and practicing how to communicate with others in person (Eder & Nenga, 2003), face-to-face experiences must be emphasized in the socialization process. While digital media pro- vide many useful ways to communicate and learn, our study sug- gests that skills in reading human emotion may be diminished when children’s face-to-face interaction is displaced by
  • 29. technolog- ically mediated communication. Today, even children under 2 years of age use mobile devices (Common Sense Media, 2013). Moreover, computers and mobile tablets are rapidly entering class- rooms and being put in the hands of every child beginning as early as kindergarten (Partnership for 21st Century Skills, 2009; Rotella, 2013) without sufficient attention to the potential costs (Cuban, 2001). Our hope is that this study will be a call to action for research that thoroughly and systematically examines the effects of digital media on children’s social development. Author contributions are as follows. YTU, GS and PMG came up with the idea. YTU and PMG developed and designed study. YTU, MM, DG and PMG piloted the study and developed group protocols for the measures. YTU, MM, JM, DG and EZ ran participants. YTU, MM, JM, DG and EZ coded data. YTU, MM and PMG developed and/or ran data analyses. YTU and PMG wrote the manuscript. All authors read and offered criticism of the manuscript. Acknowledgements We thank Andy Wechsler, Jolie Nelson and Michael Urbach at Pali Institute for welcoming our study at their outdoor camp. We also express our appreciation to the counselors who helped us and to the campers who participated in the research, as well as the school, the teachers, administrators, parents and students. Thank you to research assistant Christine Clemmons for helping with pilot testing and to Bridget Roy for helping run participants
  • 30. and code the CASP. All participants who contributed substantially to the study are listed in these acknowledgments. References Atchley, R. A., Strayer, D. L., & Atchley, P. (2012). Creativity in the wild: Improving creative reasoning through immersion in natural settings. PLoS ONE, 7(12). Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychological Science, 19, 1207–1212. Bindley, K. (2011). When Children text all day, what happens to their social skills? Huffingtonpost.com. <http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children- texting-technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html> (Retrieved 13.03.13). Blakemore, S. J. (2003). How does the brain deal with the social world? Neuro Report, 14, 1–10. Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations between social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8, 237–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093. Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding from three to six years. Child Development, 67, 789–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
  • 31. 8624.1996.tb01764.x. Clikeman-Semrud, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Minne, E. P. (2010). Direct and indirect measures of social perception, behavior and emotional functioning in children with Asperger’s disorder, nonverbal learning disability, or ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 509–519. Common Sense Media. (2013). Zero to Eight: Children’s media use in America. <http:// www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-childrens- media-use-in- america-2013>. Cuban, L. (2001). Oversold and underused. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Culnan, M., & Markus, M. (1987). Information technologies. In F. Jablin, L. Putnam, K. Roberts, & L. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizatinal communication (pp. 420–443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Dimitrov, D. M., & Rumrill, P. D. (2003). Pretest–posttest designs and measurement of change. IOS Press, 20, 159–165. Dumontheil, I., Houlton, R., Christoff, K., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Development of relational reasoning during adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, F15–F24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x. Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young
  • 32. children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 6, 1352– 1366. doi: 0.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01610.x. Eder, D., & Nenga, S. K. (2003). Socialization in adolescence. In J. Delamater (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 157–175). New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Publishers. Flom, R., & Johnson, S. (2010). The effects of adults’ affective expression and direction of visual gaze on 12-month-olds’ visual preferences following a 5 minute, 1-day or 1-month delay. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 64–85. Giedd, J. N. (2012). The digital revolution and adolescent brain evolution. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 101–105. Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Technology and informal education: What is taught, what is learned. Science, 323, 69–71. Gross, A. L., & Ballif, B. (1991). Children’s understanding of emotion from facial expression and situations: A review. Developmental Review, 11, 368–398. Guernsey, L. (2011). Screentime: How electronic media – from baby videos to educational software – affects your child. Basic Books.
  • 33. Guiltner, V. D. (2000). Validation of the child and adolescent social perception measure. Edmonton, Alberta: Department of Education Psychology. Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from television by 24- and 30- month-olds. Developmental Science, 6, 254–261. Hofferth, S. L. (2010). Home media and children’s achievement and behavior. Child Development, 81, 1598–1619. Internet world stats. (2013). Internet World Stats. <http:// www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm>. Jiang, J., Dai, B., Peng, D., Zhu, C., Liu, L., & Lu, C. (2012). Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 16064–16069. doi:10:1523/JNEUROSCI.2926-12.2012. Knapp, M. L, & Hall, J. A (2010). Nonverbal communication in human interaction (Seventh.). Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Lenhart, A. (2012). Teens, smartphones and texting. Washington DC: Pew Research Center. Magill-Evans, J., Koning, C., Cameron-Sadava, A., & Manyk, K. (1995). The child and adolescent social percetion measure. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 151. McClure, E., & Nowicki, S. Jr., (2001). Associations between
  • 34. social anxiety and nonverbal processing skill in preadolescent boys and girls. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 3–19. Moore, C., & Dunham, P. J. (Eds.). (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Nowicki, S. Jr., (2010). Manual for the receptive tests of the DANVA2. Nowicki, S., & Carton, J. (1993). The measurement of emotional intensity from facial expressions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 749–750. Partnership for 21st Century Skills. (2009). 21st Century student outcomes. Washington DC. Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford, H., et al. (2012). Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8–12 year old girls. Developmental Psychology, 48, 327–336. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030. Richert, R. A., Robb, M., & Smith, E. I. (2011). Media as social partners: The social nature of young children’s learning from screen media. Child Development, 82, 82–95. Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation M2: Media in the lives of
  • 35. 8–18 year-olds. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation. Rotella, C. (2013). No child left untableted. New York Times. NYC, NY. <http:// www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left- untableted.html?_r=0>. Sherman, L. E., Michikyan, M., & Greenfeld, P. M. (2013). The effects of text, audio, video, and in-person communication on bonding between friends. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 7. Article 3. http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2013-2-3. Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1988). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science, 32, 1492– 1512. Uhls, Y. (2013). Values and new media: How social media relates to preteen values. Seattle, Wa: Presented at the Society for Research on Child Development. Wartella, E. (2012). The influence of media on young children’s development. Zero to Three, 18–22. Wartella, E., O’Keefe, B., & Scantlin, R. (2000). Children and interactive media: A compendium of current research and directions for the future. Markle Foundation. Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children. Pearson Assessment.
  • 36. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0005 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0005 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0010 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0010 http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children-texting- technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2011/12/09/children-texting- technology-social-skills_n_1137570.html http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0020 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0035 http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight- childrens-media-use-in-america-2013 http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight- childrens-media-use-in-america-2013 http://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight- childrens-media-use-in-america-2013 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0045 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0050 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0055 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0055 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0220
  • 37. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0070 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0080 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0080 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0085 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0085 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0090 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0090 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0095 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0095 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0100 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0100 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0105 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0105 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0110 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0110 http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm http://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0120 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0125 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0125 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0130 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0130 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0135 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0135 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0145
  • 38. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0145 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0150 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0155 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0155 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027030 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0170 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0175 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0175 http://www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left- untableted.html?_r=0 http://www.nytimes.com/2013/09/15/magazine/no-child-left- untableted.html?_r=0 http://dx.doi.org/10.5817/CP2013-2-3 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0190 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0190 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0195 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0195 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0200 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0200 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0747-5632(14)00322-7/h0210Five days at outdoor education camp without screens improves preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues1 Introduction1.1 Face-to-face and mediated communication1.2 The video deficit1.3 Reading nonverbal emotion cues: processes of development and learning2 Research question and hypothesis: the present study3 Method3.1 Design and Participants3.2 Measures3.3 Procedure3.3.1 Experimental group3.3.2 Control group3.4 Analysis4 Results5 Discussion5.1 Limitations and future research6 ConclusionsAcknowledgementsReferences General guidelines for writing reaction papers
  • 39. (Read this document fully! It’s 5 pages and contains important information): Reaction papers are thought papers where you critique an article. As you read the assigned articles, point out 1) at least one interesting fact that you learned from the introduction, 2) study’s strengths, 3) the limitations of their research design (for example, the way they defined or measured their variables, the measures’ reliability/validity, their data collection technique [e.g., self-report, lab visits, direct observation]), 4) implications of their findings (so what do they findings mean in real world!. In your implications section you must relate the study’s findings to real life, and give it some context to make it relevant for lay people), 5) future direction ideas (what would you want to test next to build up on the findings of this research, and/or to address its shortcomings). These are some questions to have in mind as you read the article: · Did they account for confounding factors? · What other factors could explain their findings? · Were the findings substantial? Who will benefit from these? · What were some of the considerations or little things that the researchers took into account that strengthened their design? · If you were to do subsequent investigations, what next steps would you take? · Also, if the article posed questions in your mind, mention the questions and take a stab at giving answers too! Show me that you’ve thought the article thorough. I evaluate your reaction papers based on thedepth of your thoughts and how sophisticated and well explained your arguments comments are. SUPER IMPORTANT NOTE regarding LIMITATIONS: When pointing out the limitations, EXPLAIN how addressing the limitation could mean getting different results. For example, if the study’s participants are all socioeconomically advantaged
  • 40. and you see this a limitation because it’s not nationally representative, discuss how results of a mid/low SES sample could be different. Simply saying that the results aren’t “generalizable” IS NOT ENOUGH. You must justify your argument for selecting a more diverse sample, otherwise there is not enough evidence to suggest that the study’s findings are not generalizable! Again, please realize that it is your explanations and arguments that I evaluate, so don’t leave your comments unexplained or unsupported. SUPER IMPORTANT NOTE regarding STRENGHTS: I have found that students are often confused as to what they should consider a “strength” and what things are just “given (must haves!)” in a work that is published in an academic journal. Below are things that are NOT strengths, and rather “given”, so please don’t include these as strengths of the article! Violation of these can be considered a limitation: · Random assignment · Having conditions that differ on only one aspect · Coders being blind to the study’s hypotheses · Use of reliable and valid measures · Citing relevant prior research · High inter-rater reliability · Having IRB approval · Getting a baseline to compare post-intervention results with To identify strengths, think about what steps the researchers took into account for possible little things that could skew their results. For instance, having “practice trials” before engaging kids in a computer game, to account for differences in familiarity with the game, device, etc. These are extra steps, and thoughtful ones, that researchers take, and are considered true strengths. One more thing, in your reaction paper, refrain from statements such as “the paper was great”, “I liked this finding”, “I thought this finding was interesting”, without explaining why!! It’s okay to like the study, but it’s important to reason why you found the article interesting and important. Also, even if you really liked
  • 41. the paper, you must still be able to able to play the role of a sceptic and find few points to criticize the paper on. Your comments must be deep and critical rather than superficial or simply a reiteration of what was mentioned in the article. Organization and Structure: Start by summarizing the article in one very short paragraph (NO more than 3-4 lines)! And then continue critiquing the article. Reaction papers must be 2-3 pages. Note that most students can’t write a thorough reaction paper in 2 pages, unless they are true “concise writers”, so don’t stop at two pages, unless you’ve touched on several key points about the article (and don’t repeat the points you already mentioned, just to cover a third page!). Be sure to cover the five main key parts that were mentioned in the first paragraph, and know that your focus should be on limitations. Formatting: · WORD DOC only! No PDFs please. · Times New Roman font, 12 point, Double spaced, 1” margin all sides. · No cover page needed · Citation of the assigned paper not needed. Any extra sources must be cited · APA style of writing · Submit your paper on canvas by the due date · I will accept reaction papers only if they’re submitted before class time, AND if you are present in class to discuss your paper. If you absolutely have to miss a class where we’re discussing articles, you can make up the reaction paper by choosing an article on the topic of the week and write a reaction paper on the new article. Email me your reaction paper and the article PDF. You may earn a maximum of 40 points (instead of the typical 50 points) for this reaction paper. Only ONE reaction paper can be made up this way. So if you miss more than one article discussion day, I can’t give you the option to make it up again. · Note that on the week when you are the presenting group, you
  • 42. will submit a single reaction paper collectively as a group. The organizer should upload this reaction paper (under his/her name) and the rest of group members don’t have to worry about uploading anything. REACTION PAPER GRADING RUBRIC Performance category Quality Content (30 pts) Organization(10 pts) Grammar (10 pts); Correct choice of verb tenses, words, avoidance of wordy phrases) Exceeds expectations Offers several high quality comments about the reading that demonstrate a high level of understanding as well as sophisticated analysis of the material. Embodies originality, complexity, and depth, rather than just a presentation of the obvious; shows evidence of effective inquiry and argumentation. Clearly shows evidence of “Critical” thinking Points: 25-30 Is stellar in construction, with compelling wording, smooth transitions, and organizational clarity. Points: 10 No errors whatsoever! Points: 10 Meets expectations Demonstrates a good level of understanding and raises many points, but only one or none of the points demonstrate “deep” analysis where KEY factors are discussed (i.e., sticks to discussion of the obvious, like sample size and generalizability). All or most of the points that are raised are accurate Points: 15-25 Language is generally appropriate to a professional audience
  • 43. and organization is sound. Points: 6-9 Between 1-4 grammatical or punctuation errors, or typos, but they are minor and do not detract from the paper. Points: 6-9 Needs improvement Offers few comments, and they are superficial. Some comments have accuracy issues (e.g., invalid criticisms, or discussion of a limitation that was addressed in the paper by taking a specific measure that resolved the issue completely). Points: 5-15 Quality of writing may be inconsistent (i.e., quite good in some sections of the paper and of lesser quality in other sections. E.g., there are paragraphs containing critiquing points after what seems to be a concluding paragraph); organization needs improvement. Points: 3-6 4-7 grammatical or punctuation errors, or typos. Points: 3-6 Unsatisfactory Sounds more like a summary than an analysis. Offers only one or two comments which are on the superficial side. Points: 0-5 Paper is incoherent, or unorganized with little agreement between ideas. Points: 0-3 More than 7 grammatical, punctuation errors or typos. Errors impede understanding of content and require multiple readings and guessing to figure out the message that’s being conveyed. Points: 3-6
  • 44. AG 401 Book/Video Discussion Rubric (Total Points 12) Being scored on: 0 Unacceptable 1 Poor/Very Limited 2 OK/Good 3 Excellent Criteria 1: Word count and organization 100 words or less and/or single spaced. Greater than 100 words without paragraphs and/or single spaced. Greater than 100 words with two or more paragraphs with a minimum of one citations in each paragraph and/or single spaced. Greater than 100 words and at least three paragraphs with a minimum of one citation in each paragraph AND double spaced. Criteria 2: Critical Thinking and use of ethical theory No ethical theories applied Minimal application of ethical theory Good application of ethical theory Detailed application of ethical theory Criteria 3: Number of ethical theory citations used in support. These should add value and be meaningful. No ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes). Pp, video, topical and Open Web citations do not count. 2 or fewer ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes from 2 or fewer sources). (NO Pp, video citations, topical or Open Web). Sources are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical theory sources (philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable: CPP library or S-L textbook).
  • 45. 3 ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes from 3 different sources). (NO Pp, video citations, topical or Open Web). Sources are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical theory sources (philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable: CPP library or S-L textbook). 3 or more ethical theory citations (paraphrase or direct quotes from 3 different sources) used in support for all theories being applied. (NO Pp, video citations, topical or Open Web). Sources are well-rounded, peer-reviewed ethical theory sources (philosophical in nature). (Must be reputable: CPP library or S- L textbook). Criteria 4: APA citations and References section Numerous improper APA citations and/or References errors. Use APA book! 2 or more improper APA citations and/or References errors. Use APA book! 1-2 improper APA citations and/or References errors. Use APA book! Proper APA citations and References section on sources used. When you cite make sure and list the page number or paragraph number. Use APA book! Note:Extra points deducted for misspelling, poor grammar or errors made repeatedly week after week after corrections and feedback have already been given. Professor reserves the right to deduct points, give warnings or zeros for submissions slightly over 30% similarity (includes References section at end). If you are having trouble with % similarity, paraphrase MORE and direct quote less and include more original thought to reduce. Zeros can and will be given for submissions that do not follow the prompt or have % similarity exceeding the 30% threshold.