SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 92
Parental Influence on Children
during Educational Television
Viewing in Immigrant Families
Yuting Zhaoa,b,* and Beth M. Phillipsa,b
aDepartment of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems,
Florida State
University, Tallahassee, FL USA
bFlorida Center for Reading Research, Tallahassee, FL USA
It has been suggested by researchers that educational television
programmes may support the language and literacy development
for children, especially those in immigrant families. In an
immigrant
family, many family characteristics appear to be related to
educa-
tional television programme viewing of children at home, for
exam-
ple, parental acculturation (the process of adapting to the new
culture) and parentalmediation (supervision and guidance) of
televi-
sion viewing. In the present work, the parental influence on
children
during educational television viewing was studied
quantitatively,
based on a sample (n=171) of immigrant families with children
aged
3–6years collected across the U.S. The results have revealed
that
significant differences existed between Asian and Hispanic
groups
in coviewingmediation and in their children’s educational
television
viewing. Furthermore, language in parental acculturation
signifi-
cantly predicted instructive and restrictive parental mediation,
and
parental occupation significantly predicted language in parental
acculturation. This study initiates the attention to the topic of
educa-
tional television viewing in immigrant families, which warrants
further investigations in the future as the Asian and Hispanic
immi-
grant population increases rapidly in the U.S. Copyright © 2013
John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Key words: parental influence; educational television;
immigrant
family; parental mediation; parental acculturation
The period frombirth to 6years old for a child is themost critical
and intensive one in
terms of developing language, as the brain continues growing.
Language develop-
ment, which occurs during this period, is fundamental for
children to be successful
in modern society (Hoff, 2005). As importantly, emergent
literacy development, as
*Correspondence to: Yuting Zhao, Department of Educational
Psychology and Learning
Systems, Florida State University, 3210 Stone Building, 1114
West Call Street, Tallahassee,
FL 32306, USA. E-mail: [email protected]
Infant and Child Development
Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013)
Published online 29 May 2013 in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/icd.1798
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
a later outcome of language development, has been related to
reading achievement
in school (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). This early childhood
period is the best and
most effective time for a child to acquire language and literacy
skills, especially when
immersed within a rich linguistic environment (Roberts,
Jurgens, & Burchinal, 2005).
However, some children from at-risk families (i.e. in poverty
and with low parental
education) are not as likely to be supported by their home
environment (Hart &
Risley, 1995; Neuman, 2006). A group that may be particularly
at-risk includes
children in immigrant families, who often face multiple risk
factors associated with
language learning. These factors are linked to the home literacy
environment and,
thus, ultimately to child outcomes (Storch & Whitehurst,
2001).These children, who
enter school with limited basic literacy skills, may experience
difficulties later in
school (Phillips & Lonigan, 2009). In an attempt to improve the
disadvantaged situ-
ation of children in at-risk families, some researchers have
found that educational
television is likely to be an aid to the aforementioned issue
(Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006).
In the present work, we studied the family characteristics that
could impact educa-
tional television viewing of children in a sample of immigrant
families potentially
having at-risk backgrounds. Specifically, several key constructs,
such as family
socio-economic status (SES), educational television programme
viewing, parental
mediation of children’s viewing and parental acculturation have
been investigated.
Each of these is briefly addressed later.
Family SES, Home Literacy Environment and Immigration
Status
A growing body of research indicates that SES is a complex and
multi-aspect
construct including family income, parents’ education and
parental occupation
(Phillips & Lonigan, 2005; Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). Family
SES explained 42%
of the variance in the rate of vocabulary growth of children at
the age of 3 years (Hart
& Risley, 1995). Low-SES status is a substantial disadvantage
for children’s emergent
literacy development (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). One of the
ways that family SES
might affect child outcomes is through the home literacy
environment (Storch &
Whitehurst, 2001). The home literacy environment refers to
literacy activities (e.g.
shared reading, rhyming games and watching educational
television) that parents
utilized to develop children’s language skills(Burgess, Hecht, &
Lonigan, 2002).
Families from low-SES backgrounds are often less likely to
provide a rich home
literacy environment (Phillips & Lonigan, 2009).
In general, immigrant families often facemore hardship than
native-born families,
including crowded living conditions, food insecurity, poor
health care and limited
English proficiency (Dinan, 2006; Fix, Zimmermann, & Passel,
2001). However, there
are some distinct family characteristics among different
immigrant groups.
According to the U.S. Census Current Population Reports (U.S.
Census Bureau,
2009a), Asian American families had the highest mean annual
income ($90 811),
compared with Hispanic American families ($52 229), African
American families
($46 046) and White families ($73 240). In terms of educational
level, approximately
48.6% of Asian Americans had a college degree or higher,
whereas only 11.8% of
Hispanic Americans possessed a college degree or higher (U.S.
Census Bureau,
2009b). In 2008, there were 1.4 million children aged 5–9years
speaking a language
other than English at home in the U.S. Recent surveys indicate
that among these chil-
dren, the largest group of children with spoken English
difficulty is Hispanic/Latino
children (22.9%), followed byAsian children (20.3%; National
Center for Educational
Statistics, 2010).
Given that family background characteristics conferring risk
frequently occur at
the same time, children in immigrant families are often less
likely to be supported
402 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
by their home literacy environment. Consistent with this
premise, Farver, Eppe,
and Ballon (2006) explored the home literacy environments of
Latino children
aged 4–5.5 years, and they found that several family
characteristics were signifi-
cantly related to the home literacy environment of Latino
children, including
family SES, parental literacy involvement, parents’ years in the
U.S. and parental
literacy habits.
Educational Television Programmes
Television has penetrated almost every aspect of family life
since its invention. It
was reported that 99% of the households (115.9 million) in the
U.S. owned at least
one television set in 2011 (Nielsen, 2011). A nationwide survey
by Rideout and
Hamel (2006) provided results that 99% of U.S. families with
children 6 years or
younger owned at least one television, and 43% of these
children watched televi-
sion for at least 2 hours in a typical day. Compared with other
activities, such as
reading books (40min), listening to music (48min) and playing
video games
(6min), watching television (59min) was the activity on which
children 4–6 years
spent the most time per day (Rideout & Hamel, 2006).
The current definition of educational television by the Federal
Communications
Commission (FCC) is ‘programming that furthers the
educational and informational
needs of children 16 years of age and under in any respect,
including the child’s intel-
lectual/cognitive or social/emotional needs’ (2006, p. 2).
Previous research suggests
that educational television programmes are different from
general television
programmes and that they may have distinct influences on
children’s development
(Anderson, Huston, Schmitt, Linebarger, & Wright, 2001;
Jennings, Hooker, &
Linebarger, 2009; Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009). For
example, most of the educa-
tional television programmes on the Public Broadcasting
Service (PBS) are research
based (Jennings et al., 2009). The content seems not only to
attract children to watch
but also to support the language and literacy development of
children (Jennings
et al., 2009; Uchikoshi, 2006; Wright et al., 2001). The
influence on literacy has been
extensively studied regarding emergent literacy (Anderson et
al., 2001; Linebarger
& Piotrowski, 2009; Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006), vocabulary
knowledge (Linebarger &
Piotrowski, 2010; Wright et al., 2001) and motivation to read
and write (Anderson
et al., 2001; Wright et al., 2001). Many of these studies utilized
experimental designs
(Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009; Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006), and
thus, one may appropri-
ately draw the conclusion from the empirical results that
educational television can
support the literacy development of children.
Given their educational characteristics, research-based
educational television
programmes may serve as an element of the home literacy
environment. Family
SES appears to relate to children’s educational television
viewing, although the pre-
vious literature on their relation was somewhat inconsistent.
Some research studies
suggested a positive relation (Huston, Wright, Marquis, &
Green, 1999; Huston &
Wright, 1997), whereas some failed to find the relation (Pinõn,
Huston, & Wright,
1989). Phillips and Lonigan’s results (2009) indicated a
negative relation between
family SES (i.e. parental education and family income) and
overall television viewing
amount, as well as educational television programme viewing.
These inconsistencies
may arise because first, some studies (Huston et al., 1999;
Huston & Wright, 1997)
only asked about one specific educational television
programme, Sesame Street.
Second, one study (Pinõn et al., 1989) was conducted two
decades ago, and the
results may not be generalizable tomore recent family
environments. There has been
a substantial increase in the percentage of young children,
particularly those from
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 403
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
middle and upper middle class households, attending preschools
rather than being
at home before attending kindergarten (U. S. Department of
Commerce, 2010), and
this may interact with the time allocated to television viewing
such that the overall
amount of television viewing by young children is simplymuch
lower than previous
in these families (Christakis & Garrison, 2009). In addition, the
explosion of cable
television and Internet-based media content available to
families with more
discretionary income might dilute the viewing time for free-
access PBS stations that
broadcast educational television. To further explore this topic,
the present study was
intended to offer additional insight into how family SES is
related to children’s
educational television viewing.
Parental Mediation
Most of the time, children watch television programmes at home
with their
parents; thus, inevitably, they are impacted by their parents’
decisions on the
content and amount of television viewing. Parental mediation
refers to just such
parental supervision and guidance of children’s television
viewing. A study by
Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, and Marseille (1999) included
telephone interviews
with 519 Dutch parents whose children were aged 5–12years.
They utilized
varimax-rotated exploratory factor analysis to extract three
factors (i.e. instructive,
restrictive and coviewing) of parental mediation of television
viewing. Instructive
mediation is that parents not only know very well the content
and the characters
but also discuss these aspects with their children and guide
them to think. Restrictive
mediation refers to the restriction from parents on the amount,
the times and the
content of children’s viewing. Coviewing mediation is that
parents are physically
present to watch television programmes together with their
children (not necessarily
creating educational occasions). The studies of Warren (2003,
2005) also supported
these three strategies of parental mediation through two surveys
in the U.S. from
129 middle-income families (87% Caucasian) and 306 low-
income families (69%
African American and 18% Caucasian), respectively. Warren
(2005) provided three
models examined by path analyses for these three types of
parental mediation and
found that parental education was a significant predictor for
parental mediation.
Atkin, Greenberg, and Baldwin (1991) earlier contextualized
these three types of
parental mediation of children’s television viewing (i.e.
instructive, restrictive and
coviewing mediation) within Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model
(i.e. microsystem,
mesosystem, exosystem and macrosystem). Bronfenbrenner
(1979) emphasized the
dynamics between a developing child and his or her
environment. The microsystem
is defined as bidirectional interactions and activities in a
person’s immediate
surroundings (Atkin et al., 1991). For instance, under the
circumstances of children’s
television viewing, parental involvement, children’s age and
interpersonal interac-
tions at home all belong to the bidirectional relationships in a
domestic microsystem.
Next, surrounding the microsystem is the mesosystem, which
provides connections
between microsystems to advance development. Exosystem
refers to multiple social
settings influencing the development of childrenwithout their
direct participation. In
this system, community life and family SES (i.e. parental
education and income)
seem to affect children’s television viewing. Macrosystem is the
outmost level that
consists of cultural consistencies. It is ‘various socioeconomic,
ethnic, religious, and
other sub-cultural groups, reflecting contrasting belief systems
and lifestyles’
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979, p. 26). In the present study, the ethnic
cultures of different
immigrant groups and parents’ different coping strategies in
adapting to the
new culture seem to serve as the macrosystem that may affect
children’s
404 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
television viewing (e.g. some Hispanic families’ values of
educacion and familism
and some Asian families’ value of collectivism, Bornstein &
Cote, 2003; Reese,
Balzano, Gallimore, & Goldenberg, 1995; Romero, Robinson,
Haydel, Mendoza,
& Killen, 2004).
Parental Acculturation
Parental acculturation is another variable that appears to affect
children’s
educational television programme viewing in immigrant
families. Two models have
described the phenomenon of acculturation. Gordon (1964)
claimed that accultura-
tion is the process by which a person is gradually absorbed into
the host culture
(linear or unidirectional models). However, the previous model
was extended to be
bidirectional and two-dimensional by Berry (1980); that is,
people are able to acquire a
new culture without necessarily losing their original one.
During the process of
parental acculturation (i.e. when parents in immigrant families
adapt to the host
culture), parents could employ acculturation strategies such as
integration (selectively
learning the new culture while maintaining the old one),
assimilation (being totally
absorbed into the new culture and not wanting to keep the old
one), separation
(separating themselves from the new culture) and
marginalization (trying to have
nothing to do with either the new culture or the old one; Sam,
2006). Portes and
Rumbaut (2006) claimed that there was no particular
acculturation pattern that fits
all individuals or different immigrant groups. A number of
applied studies, which
have been conductedwith the guidance of the two
acculturationmodels, led to some
relevant findings concerning the various aspects of
acculturation impacts. For
example, integration and assimilation relate positively to school
achievement (Eng
et al., 2008), parenting (White, Roosa, Weaver, & Nair, 2009)
and the home literacy
environment (Farver et al., 2006).
In summary, the existing literature suggests that family
characteristics are closely
linked to the home literacy environment and, thus, ultimately to
child outcomes.
Given that many immigrant children often struggle without
special help, their
problems have the potential to increase. Because educational
television programmes
may support the development of future literacy skills (Anderson
et al., 2001), and
PBS can be accessed by most of these children, parents could
help these children
develop literacy skills in part via educational television
viewing. Research also indi-
cates that children’s programme viewing is affected by parental
mediation, family
SES and the general home literacy environment (Farver et al.,
2006; Warren, 2003,
2005). As well, in immigrant families, parental acculturation
appears to serve as the
background construct that intertwines with family SES (Farver
et al., 2006). There-
fore, it is important to study parental acculturation and parental
mediation behav-
iours associatedwith children’s educational television viewing
in a high-risk sample.
The objective of this studywas to investigate relations among
family SES, parental
mediation, parental acculturation and children’s educational
television programme
viewing in immigrant families, in particular within both Asian
and Hispanic fami-
lies. Previous literature indicated that there was a significant
difference between
Asian and Hispanic families regarding family income and
parental education (U. S.
Census Bureau, 2009a, 2009b). In addition, Asian and Hispanic
immigrants are
currently the largest two immigrant groups in the U.S. (Dinan,
2006). The following
research questions were addressed: (RQ1) Is there a significant
difference between
the Asian and Hispanic children in the amount of viewing of
educational television
programmes? (RQ2) Is there a significant difference between
the Asian and Hispanic
children in the types of parental mediation of television
viewing? (RQ3) Is there a
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 405
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
significant difference between the Asian and Hispanic children
in parental accultur-
ation patterns? (RQ4) Overall, what are the relations among
parental education and
occupation, parental acculturation, parental mediation of
television viewing, and
children’s amount of educational programme viewing? On the
basis of the previous
literature (e.g. Atkin et al., 1991; Warren, 2003, 2005;
Valkenburg et al., 1999), we
anticipated that the outcomes in parental mediation, parental
acculturation and
educational television viewing by their children would be
different between Asian
and Hispanic groups and that family SES indicators and parental
mediation behav-
iours would predict children’s educational television viewing
amount. For example,
we hypothesized that parental mediation operates as a partial
mediating variable for
the relation between parental acculturation and children’s
educational programme
viewing amount. In light of mixed prior findings on SES
indicators, we did not make
a specific directional prediction regarding their relation to
educational television
viewing but rather just posited that there would be significant
relations. Given the
absence of substantial prior investigation of the other possible
relations among
acculturation indicators, parental mediation behaviours and
children’s educational
television viewing, we considered these aspects of the model as
exploratory and
did not have explicit a priori hypotheses for each pathway.
METHOD
Participants
Eligible participants were parents from immigrant families in
the U.S. with children
between 3 and 6years old. According to the 2010 Census
Questionnaire Reference Book,
Hispanic Americans or Latino are immigrants who come from
Spain, Mexico, Puerto
Rico, Cuba or Spanish-speaking countries of Central and South
America. Asian
Americans are immigrants who come from Asian countries such
as China, Korea,
Vietnam, the Philippines, Japan, India, Indonesia, Malaysia,
Singapore, Thailand
and Cambodia (U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). Considering
acculturation processes
indicating that immigrants appear to be more assimilated into
the host culture after
three generations (Sam, 2006), it is likely that parents in
immigrant families were
most appropriate for the study if they were within three
generations of arrival in
the U.S. The first generation includes people who were born
outside the U.S., the
second generation includes people whowere born in the U.S. to
foreign-born parents
and the third generation includes people who were born in the
U.S. to U.S.-born
parents (Glick & White, 2004). A mechanism utilized for
increasing potential
participation was that the survey was available for completion
both in paper and
online. Prior research demonstrated that samples collected by
electronic survey were
diverse in terms of demographic features such as gender, SES,
age and geographic
region (Kim, Chelladurai, & Trail, 2007; Shannon& Bradshaw,
2002). Incentives were
also utilized in that participants were rewarded by lottery for
their completion of the
survey. Furthermore, schools, which helped to distribute some
of the surveys,
received a thank-you gift.
The sample (n=171) was composed of 45.0% Asians, 46.8%
Hispanics, 7.0%
European-Americans, and 1.2%AfricanAmericans. Participants
came from 17 differ-
ent states across the U.S. Fourteen participants were neither
Asian nor Hispanic, but
theywere eligible participants and thuswere retained in the
overall sample analyses.
Overall, 151 (21.0%) electronic survey responses were received
of 706 emailed cover
letters with survey links and 51 telephone call follow-ups, and
20 (6.0%) paper
surveys were received of 300 mailed surveys. It should be noted
that there was not
406 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
a specific way to know how many different people were given
the survey links or
copies by these schools or organizations, so the response rate
was an approximated
calculation (i.e. each cover letter was likely distributed to
multiple eligible individ-
uals).The demographic characteristics of children and families
overall and by groups
of Asian and Hispanic are presented in Table 1. Of the 171
returned surveys, 47
(27.5%) were completed by fathers and 124 (72.5%) were
completed by mothers.
Measures
A parent survey with four parts was created to measure
children’s educational
television viewing amounts, parental mediation and parental
acculturation. The first
part of the survey consisted of six items that investigated the
background of parents
including gender, ethnicity, generation, education, occupation
and household annual
income. The second part of the survey, including five questions,
asked parents about
their children’s background such as siblings, age and gender,
children’s language use
preference at home and school, and children’s television
viewing (viewing amount
and the content). In terms of viewing content, 29 educational
television programmes
Table 1. Child and family demographic characteristics overall
and by group
All Asian Hispanic
Sample size 171 77 80
Family characteristics
Parental education (years) Some college College High school
M= 14.54 (SD=4.27) M= 16.68 (SD= 4.01) M= 12.36 (SD=
3.09)
Family annual income
<$16 000 15.2% 22.1% 11.3%
$17 000–30 000 29.2% 23.4% 33.8%
$30 000–75 000 25.2% 26.0% 25.0%
$75 000–100 000 14.6% 14.3% 12.5%
$100 000–150 000 11.7% 7.8% 16.3%
>$150 000 4.1% 6.5% 1.3%
Number of children by
family (mean)
2.04 (SD=1.12) 1.82 (SD= 1.06) 2.28 (SD= 1.18)
Generation (parents) 88.8% First 94.7% First 83.8% First
11.2% Second 5.3% Second 16.3% Second
Child characteristics
Gender 49.4% boys 48.7% boys 47.5% boys
Age (years) M=4.74 (SD= 1.21) M=4.59 (SD= 1.61) M= 4.88
(SD=1.24)
Children numbers by age Age 3: 38 Age 3: 18 Age 3: 16
Age 4: 28 Age 4: 16 Age 4: 10
Age 5: 51 Age 5: 22 Age 5: 26
Age 6: 53 Age 6: 20 Age 6: 28
Born in the U.S. 73.8% 64.0% 87.3%
Language use at home 32.9% English 43.4% English 15.5%
English
36.5% Spanish 32.9% Chinese 77.5% Spanish
16.8% Chinese 23.7% Others 7.6% Others
14.9% Others
Language use at school 95.9% English 96.1% English 96.3%
English
Note. The age information for one child was missing for the
Asian group.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 407
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
from PBS were listed. Parents were asked to mark all
educational television
programmes that their child often watched (i.e. at least once a
week). The third part
of the survey contained 16 items – 15 of them (a= .90)were
adapted from the 5-point
Likert scale of parental mediation of television viewing
fromValkenburg et al. (1999),
who provided preliminary evidence of validity via factor
analysis. Several example
items are ‘how often do you try to help this child understand
what they see in an
educational television programme’ for instructive mediation,
‘how often do you
forbid your child to watch certain programmes’ for restrictive
mediation and ‘how
often do you watch together because you both like an
educational programme’ for
coviewingmediation. The fourth part of the survey (a= .70)was
adapted fromMarin
and Gamba (1996) and Stilling (1997) to measure parental
acculturation in three
aspects of language, food andmedia (22 items in total). The
validity of these accultur-
ation items was also examined by factor analysis (Marin &
Gamba, 1996), which
supported the categories of language (i.e. language use and
proficiency) and media.
The food category (two items) was added into the survey,
following suggestions by
Arends-Tóth and van de Vijer (2006) in assessment of
psychological acculturation.
Two item categories (i.e. American and ethnic) were
constructed in the acculturation
measure: a score of 33 and higher in the first category signified
high American
preferences and, in the second category, signified high ethnic
preferences. Through
use of these cut-off scores, acculturation patterns among
participants are presented
as four categories for descriptive purposes: integrated (high
American/high ethnic),
assimilated (high American/low ethnic), separated (low
American / high ethnic) and
marginalized (low American / low ethnic). However, for
statistical analyses, continu-
ous data for each aspect of acculturation were used. Example
items included ‘How
often do you speak English’, ‘How often do you eat your own
ethnic food’ and
‘How often do you watch television programmes in English’.
Procedure
Surveys were translated by native speakers in Spanish and
Chinese. The two
languages were selected because virtually all Hispanic
immigrants speak
Spanish and the largest population subgroup among Asian
Americans is Chinese
(23%; U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). The survey was distributed to
parents in both
English and the anticipated home language, so that parents
could select the language
in which they were most comfortable to respond. Before
distribution, back
translations were completed, and native speakers with an
educational psychology
and linguistics background checked all the wording of these
items. There were
several sources for recruiting eligible participants. First, the
authors contacted the
directors of centres and organizations for immigrants such as
the Hispanic American
Association and the AsianAmericanAssociation and asked for
their help to distribute
surveys. Second, the authors contacted international centres or
similar centres at
universities across the U.S., which served many international
students and their
families. Third, the authors contacted schools and child care
centres in metropolitan
areas known to have large eligible populations and asked for
their help to distribute
surveys. Fourth, the authors also collected some convenience
samples, such as
contacting schools and child care centres known to serve
immigrant populations that
have participated in prior research studies. Parents were
contacted by either the
researcher or their director (e.g. the director of a child care
centre or of a community
organization) via email or mail. Depending on their preferences,
each parent
participated by either visiting the website where they could
complete the survey or
returning the completed survey forms in the postage-paid
envelopes provided.
408 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
RESULTS
The results are presented in three parts: the first part includes
the sample descriptive
statistics; the second part includes one-way ANOVA tests of the
group differences
between Asian and Hispanic families in educational television
viewing amount,
and parental mediation and acculturation types; the third part is
the path analyses
of the overall relations among these variables.
Preliminary Analyses
Some transformations of coding were made to parental
demographic variables such
that categorical education levels were recalculated into average
years in which one
would achieve such educational levels (e.g. high school=
12years; Master’s degree=
18years). Standardized scores for categories of occupation were
utilized (Kim, Han,
Shin, Kim, & Lee, 2005). First, the nominal variables were
transformed into ordinal
variables according to aU.S. Census list of occupation ranks.
Then, each occupational
rank used by the Census corresponded to a standardized score;
these scores were
normalized and standardized from national income and
education distributions
associated with each occupation (e.g. manual workers = 46 and
managers = 59;
Green, 1970). Results indicated that more parents in Hispanic
families were born in
the U.S. than parents in Asian families, whereas Asian parents
had a higher educa-
tional level than did Hispanic parents (Table 1). Most families
reported income in
the range of $17 000–30 000; however, the nonsignificant
results of chi-square tests
for independence (w [5]2 = 9.50, p= .09) demonstrated that
there was no statistically
significant difference in annual income between Asian and
Hispanic families. The
correlation between education and occupation was statistically
significant and
positive (r= .68, p< .01).
The means and standard deviations for parental mediation and
parental
acculturation are presented in Table 2.The descriptive
categorization of acculturation
types indicated that the majority of the sample were classified
as integrated (70%
overall, 76% Asian families, 64% Hispanic families), whereas
approximately 24% of
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, one-way ANOVA and Welch
tests
All (n= 171) Asian (n=77) Hispanic (n= 80) ANOVA or
Welch F values
M SD M SD M SD
Educational TV viewing 12.33*
Parental mediation
Instructive 15.93 4.43 15.88 3.41 16.14 5.23 .14 (W)
Restrictive 16.86 4.86 17.61 3.58 16.20 5.82 3.28 (W)
Coviewing 17.76 3.92 16.19 3.30 19.62 3.42 39.72**
Parental acculturation
Language 30.81 5.88 31.28 4.79 30.08 6.83 1.62 (W)
Food 4.90 1.37 4.66 1.09 5.03 1.61 2.70 (W)
Media 24.84 3.23 24.45 3.39 25.13 3.20 1.63 (W)
Note. M=mean; SD= standard deviation. A (W) after an F value
means the assumption of homogeneity
of variances were violated and the Welch tests were conducted
and reported; the tests for the other
variables were one-way ANOVA.
*p< .01, **p< .001.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 409
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
the sample were classified as separated. None were
marginalized, and approxi-
mately 4% were assimilated; this represented no Asian families
but 11% of the
Hispanic families. There were significant, positive correlations
between instructive
mediation, and restrictive and coviewing mediation (r= .68, p<
.01; and r= .39,
p< .01), respectively (Table 3). Language, one aspect of
parental acculturation, was
positively correlated with instructive and restrictive – two types
of parental media-
tion (r= .31, p< .01; and r= .38, p< .01).
The hours that children viewed television, including educational
television, were
self-reported by parents both for weekdays and weekends.
Reported hours ranged
from 0 to 8hours per day. On average, children in immigrant
families watched PBS
shows 2.06hours per day, whereas they watched other general
television pro-
grammes 1.44hours per day. Restrictive mediation and parental
education were
negatively correlated with children’s educational programme
viewing amount
(r=�.20, p< .05, and r=�.27, p< .01, respectively; Table 3).
Asian and Hispanic Group Differences: One-way ANOVA Tests
One-way ANOVA tests were conducted to see whether there
were significant
differences between Asian and Hispanic families in children’s
educational televi-
sion viewing, parental mediation and acculturation (RQ1, RQ2
and RQ3, respec-
tively). To control for Type I error, the Bonferroni correction
(Shaffer, 1995) was
applied to each ANOVA test, and only results after correction
are reported as signif-
icant. A one-way ANOVA analysis indicated that the difference
in the amounts of
viewing educational programmes was statistically significant
between Asian and
Hispanic children, F(1, 152) = 12.33, p= .00, Z2 = .08; Table 2.
The effect size was
moderate (Ferguson, 2009). The assumption of homogeneity of
variances was
checked (Levene, 1960), and the assumption was met (p= .20).
One-way ANOVA were also conducted to test for group
differences in each of the
three types of parental mediation (i.e. instructive, restrictive
and coviewing). The re-
sults showed that the difference in frequency of coviewing
mediation was statistically
significant between Asian and Hispanic parents, F(1,
151)=39.72, p= .00, Z2 = .21;
Table 2, whereas there were no statistically significant
differences in instructive,
F(1, 151)= .13, p= .72, and restrictive mediation behaviours,
F(1, 151) =3.18,
Table 3. Correlations among observed variables
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1.Education 1
2.Occupation .68** 1
3.Instructive mediation .08 �.05 1
4.Restrictive mediation .18* .01 .68** 1
5.Coviewing mediation �.22* �.15 .39** .16* 1
6.Language .12 .30** .31** .38** .06 1
7.Food .07 .16* .26* .15 �.02 .31** 1
8.Media .07 .15 .15 .18* �.03 .41** .37** 1
9.ETVAmt �.27** �.05 �.08 �.20* .10 .00 .02 �.00 1
10.General TVAmt �.18* �.00 �.00 �.09 �.06 .14 .07 �.06
.31** 1
Note.ETV Amt= the amount of educational television
programmes that children watched every day;
General TVAmt= the amount of general television viewing
every day.
*p< .05, **p< .01.
410 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
p= .08, between Asian and Hispanic parents. The assumption of
homogeneity of
variances was met for coviewing mediation but violated for
instructive and re-
strictive mediation. The Welch test (Welch, 1951) is an
alternative F test to be
used when the homogeneity of variances is violated, and it was
recommended
by Lix, Keselman, and Keselman (1996) that the Welch test
could be used in
most one-way analyses regardless of the equality of group sizes
and sample
size. Therefore, the Welch test was conducted, and the results of
instructive me-
diation and restrictive mediation were F(1, 135)= .14, p= .71
and F(1, 131)=3.28,
p= .07, respectively. However, there were no significant
differences between the
Asian and Hispanic groups in instructive and restrictive
mediation frequencies.
With regard to the group differences in parental acculturation
aspects, the as-
sumption of homogeneity of variances was violated. Instead, the
Welch test (1951)
was conducted for the three aspects of parental acculturation
(i.e. language,
food and media) between the Asian and Hispanic groups. The
results were
F(1, 140)=1.62, p= .21 for language, F(1, 138)=2.70, p= .10 for
food and F(1, 149)=1.63,
p= .20 for media (Table 2), which show that there were no
differences between Asian
and Hispanic parents in the three aspects of parental
acculturation.
Overall Relations: Path Analysis
Given the few differences found between Asian and Hispanic
families, the
subgroups were combined for the overall model analysis. The
fourth research ques-
tion (RQ4), investigating the overall relations among parental
education and occupa-
tion, parental mediation, parental acculturation and children’s
educational television
viewing, was analysed by path analysis. Path analysis is the
estimation of expected
causal relations among observed variables (Kline, 2005). The
fully saturated model
with all possible pathways between variables was first tested
(Model 1; Table 4).
Because all the variables were multivariate normal, the
maximum likelihood estima-
tionmethodwas used to analyseModel 1. On the basis ofModel 1,
the significance of
paths and the modification index were checked. Modification
index values suggest
that adding any of the parameters listed in the output will
reduce the model chi-
square by at least 3.84. Nonsignificant paths (a> .05) were
removed or fixed to zero
one group by one group from large to small in order to achieve
a parsimonious
model (simple and good fit). Eight models were tested, and
some necessary modifi-
cations were made. The chi-square and model fit indices for
these models are
provided in Table 4.
A stepwise comparison procedure was used to select the final
model. The chi-
square difference test was used to evaluate the relative fit of
two nested models
(i.e. Models 7 and 8). The results indicated that the chi-square
difference test was
significant (Δw2 [1] = 4.38> w2critical value [1] = 3.84, a=
.05), which suggests that
Model 8 fits significantly better than Model 7. On the basis of
the stepwise model
comparison procedure, as well as the chi-square difference test,
Model 8 (see
Figure 1 for standardized parameters) was selected as the final
model. The results
suggested that parental education and restrictive mediation
significantly,
negatively, predicted the amount of children’s educational
television viewing and
that language in parental acculturation positively predicted
instructive and restric-
tive mediation. In addition, parental education was positively
related to parental
occupation. Given its negligible bivariate relation with
educational television view-
ing, the significant pathway between occupation and the
outcome is likely the
result of its suppressor role in the model and should not
necessarily be interpreted
(Maassen & Bakker, 2001).
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 411
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Ta
bl
e
4.
R
es
ul
ts
fr
om
a
st
ep
w
is
e
m
od
el
co
m
pa
ri
so
n
of
in
fl
ue
nc
es
on
ch
ild
re
n’
s
ed
uc
at
io
na
lt
el
ev
is
io
n
vi
ew
in
g
M
od
el
s
w2
df
C
FI
T
L
I
R
M
SE
A
SR
M
R
M
od
el
1:
Fu
lly
sa
tu
ra
te
d
m
od
el
.0
0
0
1.
00
1.
00
.0
0
.0
0
M
od
el
2:
R
em
ov
ed
fo
ur
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
s,
E
d
uc
at
io
n
➔
M
ed
ia
,
M
ed
ia
➔
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
,
Fo
od
➔
C
ov
ie
w
in
g,
an
d
Fo
od
➔
E
T
V
A
m
t
.1
1
4
1.
00
1.
13
.0
0
.0
0
M
od
el
3:
R
em
ov
ed
si
x
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
s,
E
d
uc
at
io
n
➔
Fo
od
,
Fo
od
➔
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
,
O
cc
up
at
io
n
➔
C
ov
ie
w
in
g,
In
st
ru
ct
iv
e
➔
E
T
V
A
m
t,
L
an
gu
ag
e
➔
E
T
V
A
m
t,
an
d
M
ed
ia
➔
E
T
V
A
m
t
1.
34
10
1.
00
1.
12
.0
0
.0
1
M
od
el
4:
R
em
ov
ed
fo
ur
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
s,
M
ed
ia
➔
In
st
ru
ct
iv
e,
E
d
uc
at
io
n
➔
In
st
ru
ct
iv
e,
O
cc
up
at
io
n
➔
In
st
ru
ct
iv
e,
an
d
M
ed
ia
➔
C
ov
ie
w
in
g
2.
66
14
1.
00
1.
12
.0
0
.0
2
M
od
el
5:
R
em
ov
ed
fo
ur
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
s,
O
cc
up
at
io
n
➔
M
ed
ia
,
O
cc
up
at
io
n
➔
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
,
L
an
gu
ag
e
➔
C
ov
ie
w
in
g,
an
d
C
ov
ie
w
in
g
➔
E
T
V
A
m
t
7.
68
18
1.
00
1.
08
.0
0
.0
3
M
od
el
6:
R
em
ov
ed
tw
o
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
s,
E
d
uc
at
io
n
➔
L
an
gu
ag
e
an
d
O
cc
up
at
io
n
➔
Fo
od
12
.4
5
20
1.
00
1.
05
.0
0
.0
4
M
od
el
7:
R
em
ov
ed
on
e
no
ns
ig
ni
fi
ca
nt
pa
th
,E
d
uc
at
io
n
➔
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
16
.1
9
21
1.
00
1.
03
.0
0
.0
5
M
od
el
8:
A
d
d
a
co
va
ri
an
ce
be
tw
ee
n
E
d
uc
at
io
n
an
d
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
11
.8
2
20
1.
00
1.
05
.0
0
.0
4
N
ot
e.
E
T
V
A
m
t=
th
e
am
ou
nt
of
ed
uc
at
io
na
l
te
le
vi
si
on
pr
og
ra
m
m
es
th
at
ch
ild
re
n
w
at
ch
ed
ev
er
y
d
ay
;
C
FI
=
co
m
pa
ra
ti
ve
fi
t
in
d
ex
;
T
L
I=
Tu
ck
er
–L
ew
is
in
d
ex
;
R
M
SE
A
=
ro
ot
m
ea
n
sq
ua
re
er
ro
r
of
ap
pr
ox
im
at
io
n;
SR
M
R
=
st
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
ro
ot
m
ea
n
sq
ua
re
re
si
d
ua
l.
412 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
The results of the overall model demonstrated that one aspect of
parental
mediation, restrictive mediation had significant correlations
with both language in
parental acculturation and educational television viewing by
children. There were
also indirect effects from parental occupation to educational
television viewing
amount (i.e. occupation➔ language➔ restrictive mediation➔
educational television
viewing amount; Table 5). In other words, parental occupation,
rather than parental
education, is more likely to affect parental acculturation
(language), and this effect
may continue to influence parental mediation patterns
(restrictive mediation).
Restrictive mediation may then affect children’s educational
television viewing
amount negatively.
DISCUSSION
This present study explored whether there were significant
differences among
immigrant families between Asian and Hispanic parents of
young children in pa-
rental mediation, parental acculturation and their children’s
educational
programme viewing, as well as the relations among these three
concepts (Figure 1).
The key findings of the present study included first, Hispanic
children watched
significantly more television overall and specifically more
educational television
than did Asian children, and second, Hispanic parents
significantly enacted more
coviewing mediation behaviours than did Asian parents. Third,
language in
parental acculturation significantly predicted instructive and
restrictive parental
mediation. Fourth, parental occupation significantly predicted
language in paren-
tal acculturation.
On average, children watched television 3.50 hours per day,
which is consistent
with a national survey finding that 43% of the children watched
television for at
Figure 1. Final pathmodel with standardized parameters (Model
8). Edu=Education; Occu=
Occupation; Lang=Language; Instr = Instructive mediation;
Restr =Restrictive mediation;
Coview=Coviewingmediation; ETVAmt= the amount of
educational television programmes
that children watched every day; PA=parental acculturation;
PM=parental mediation.
*p< .01, **p< .001.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 413
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Ta
bl
e
5.
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
d
ir
ec
t,
in
d
ir
ec
t
an
d
to
ta
le
ff
ec
ts
of
th
e
fi
na
lm
od
el
(M
od
el
8)
D
ep
en
d
en
t
va
ri
ab
le
s
In
d
ep
en
d
en
t
va
ri
ab
le
s
L
an
gu
ag
e
In
st
ru
ct
iv
e
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
E
T
V
A
m
t
D
ir
ec
t
In
d
ir
ec
t
To
ta
l
D
ir
ec
t
In
d
ir
ec
t
To
ta
l
D
ir
ec
t
In
d
ir
ec
t
To
ta
l
D
ir
ec
t
In
d
ir
ec
t
To
ta
l
E
d
uc
at
io
n
�.
37
2*
*
�.
37
2*
*
O
cc
up
at
io
n
.2
78
**
.2
78
**
.1
05
*
.1
05
*
.2
09
*
�.
01
8
.1
92
*
L
an
gu
ag
e
.2
20
*
.2
20
*
.3
77
**
.3
77
**
�.
06
3*
�.
06
3*
Fo
od
.1
72
*
.1
72
*
R
es
tr
ic
ti
ve
�.
16
8*
�.
16
8*
N
ot
e.
E
T
V
A
m
t,
th
e
am
ou
nt
of
ed
uc
at
io
na
lt
el
ev
is
io
n
pr
og
ra
m
m
es
th
at
ch
ild
re
n
w
at
ch
ed
ev
er
y
d
ay
.
*p
<
.0
5,
**
p
<
.0
01
414 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
least 2 hours in a typical day (Rideout & Hamel, 2006).
Children in Asian immi-
grant families spend significantly less time watching PBS shows
than children in
Hispanic immigrant families. There are two possible
explanations for this finding.
First, the demographic statistics in the sample showed that on
average, Asian
families reported a higher average parental educational level
than did the
Hispanic families, and parental education has previously (and in
the present study)
been found to be negatively related to overall television viewing
(Linebarger,
2010). Children of parents with low levels of education
typically watch more tele-
vision programmes in general, and the overall television
viewing amount seems to
drive the educational television programmes viewing amount
(Phillips & Lonigan,
2009). In accordance with this explanation, education was
negatively related to
educational television viewing within this sample. Second, more
Hispanic parents
reported engaging in coviewing mediation of television viewing
than did Asian
parents. The findings provide additional support for previous
research (Bryant &
Bryant, 2001) that children whose parents coviewed television
with them would
watch more television compared with children watching alone.
The results showed significant differences between Asian and
Hispanic groups
in coviewing mediation but not in instructive and restrictive
mediations. Hispanic
parents reported that their most frequent mediation type was
coviewing media-
tion. Parental education was significantly, negatively, related to
coviewing media-
tion (Warren, 2005), and Hispanic parents in this sample
reported a lower parental
education than Asian parents; thus, they might engage in more
coviewing media-
tion than the Asian parents. With regard to restrictive
mediation, the result
contrasted with that for coviewing mediation. Asian parents
reported more restric-
tive mediation compared with Hispanic parents. This outcome
may stem from
findings that education is often highly emphasized in Asian
immigrant families
(Eng et al., 2008). Further, they tend to set rules to regulate
their children, and
children are expected to have unquestioning obedience to their
parents. Under this
kind of parenting, children might experience more restrictive
mediation of television
viewing (Buerkel-Rothfuss & Buerkel, 2001). Consequently, it
was not surprising to
see that Asian parents were more restrictive of children’s
television viewing than
Hispanic parents in this sample. Parents who frequently engage
in restrictive media-
tion may restrict the overall television viewing of children,
which may accordingly
reduce viewing amounts for educational television programmes.
In the present study, restrictive mediation was significantly
positively correlated
to instructive mediation, such that parents who tended to set
rules for their children
on watching television were more likely to ask questions and
guide children to think
during viewing. When these two mediation behaviours are
combined, known as
active parental mediation (Warren, 2003, 2005), it may
facilitate good outcomes for
the language development of children. However,
restrictivemediation is not necessar-
ily good or bad, and it may depend on how parents engage in the
communications
with their children. That is, it may be better that parents
regulate children’s viewing
time and content without hurting their feelings and motivations
to learn.
Prior studies by Warren (2003, 2005) indicated that parents’
attitudes towards
television were related to the parental mediation types. Parents’
attitudes towards
television significantly and positively predicted instructive and
restrictive media-
tions but not coviewing mediation (Warren, 2005). This finding
suggests that the
influences of the macrosystem and exosystem from
Bronfenbrenner’s model
decreased and the influences of the mesosystem (parents’
attitudes) increased. This
model, taking acculturation as an indicator of attitude, might
explain our results that
there were no significant differences in instructive and
restrictive mediation types
between Asian and Hispanic families.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 415
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Conceptually, although some indicators within each (e.g.
restrictive and
coviewing mediation) were not highly correlated, parental
acculturation and paren-
tal mediation could be treated as two overall constructs for the
purposes of this
discussion. Parental acculturation significantly predicted
parental mediation in some
aspects (Figure 1). Parents with a higher acculturation level
appear to be more likely
to enact active parental mediation behaviours, inwhich parents
are actively involved
with their children by talking, explaining, asking questions and
setting rules. High
levels of acculturation include both integrated and assimilated
acculturation
patterns. Because the proportion of immigrants categorized as
assimilated was very
small, most of the participants were considered tomatch the
integrated acculturation
pattern. These participants may have high English proficiency,
high frequency of
English use, high frequency of American media use and high
motivation to use
media to learn American culture.
A study by Ying and Han (2008) indicated that parental
acculturation, especially
language acculturation (higher English language proficiency),
significantly predicted
active parental involvement at school and home (e.g. parents’
interactions with
teachers, parents helping children complete homework and
parents readingwith chil-
dren). A parallel could be drawn from this earlier study,
because parental mediation is
also a parental involvement behaviour related to children’s
television viewing. From
this perspective, the results that parental acculturation
significantly predicted parental
mediation are in line with the previous research.
Nonactive parentalmediation includes coviewingmediation,
inwhich parents are
just physically present to watch television together with their
children and do not
necessarily create educational occasions or have linguistic
interactions with their
children. Coviewing mediation was negatively correlated with
parental education.
It is likely that parents in this kind of mediation may just take it
for granted that
children could acquire literacy knowledge from educational
television automatically,
but ignore or not be aware of the parental role of guiding
children to think and
enhancing children’s learning outcomes through language
interactions while
watching. In addition, some parents from lower-SES families,
whomay have towork
two or three jobs to support families, may have no time or
energy to be actively in-
volved with children’s television viewing (i.e. instructive and
restrictive mediations).
Among the SES predictors, only parental occupation was
positively predictive of
parental acculturation. Immigrant professionals (i.e. thosewith a
higher occupational
coding) seem to be more likely than parents with blue collar
positions (e.g. manual
laborers) to be exposed to the whole American community
rather than only their
own ethnic community, and they may have more opportunities
to interact with the
American community. These opportunities may in turn provide
parents with
increased English proficiency and motivation to speak in this
language.
The assimilation rates, in contrast to integrated or separated
patterns, were very
low for both Asian and Hispanic parents, although both
subgroups reported a pro-
portion of second-generation parents. In past decades,
immigrants disproportionally
concentrated in several metropolitan locations such as Los
Angeles, San Francisco,
New York, Miami and Chicago, and these concentrations
provided a variety of large
immigrant communities in these metropolitan areas (i.e. the
Mexican community in
Los Angeles, Cuban community in Miami and Chinese
community in San Francisco
and New York; Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). Immigrants may have
less pressure and
motivation to give up their own cultures while still living in
their native cultural
community (e.g. China town, East LA and Little Italy) within
the new country (Fix
et al., 2001; Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). It is likely that these
individuals have just
selectively adapted to American society. Perhaps, this is the
reason that the majority
of Asian and Hispanic immigrants in this sample adopted the
strategy of integrated
416 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
acculturation, which is selectively learning the American
culture while maintaining
the native one. Future research should target larger proportions
of second-
generation and third-generation immigrant families to learn
whether these accultur-
ation patterns replicate.
Limitations
The primary limitation of this studywas that the sample sizewas
not large, as well as
convenience sampling; thus, the results of this studymay not
fully generalize to other
populations. As the sample was ascertained through
participation in community
centres and child care centres, it is not possible to determine its
full representative-
ness of the eligible immigrant population of families with young
children. We
provided potential participants in multiple states with the
opportunity to respond
either via paper or via electronic survey and provided the
survey in multiple lan-
guages to increase the generalizability. Moreover, the findings
of electronic surveys
have been found to be consistent with traditional data collection
and can be general-
ized to a larger population (Boyer, Olson, Calantone, &
Jackson, 2002; Gosling,
Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Shannon &
Bradshaw, 2002). In
further investigations, the goal is to increase the robustness of
the path models with
larger, ideally nationally representative samples.
A second limitation relates to the item investigating income,
which only asked
parents to mark a range of family annual income, did not allow
finer grain differen-
tiations between groups, although it likely increased
respondents’ comfort with
providing this type of sensitive material. It should also be noted
that despite the
experience and qualifications of some recent immigrants, they
are more likely to
‘enter at the bottom of their respective occupational ladders’
(Portes & Rumbaut,
2006, p. 58). For instance, immigrant professionals such as
engineers and doctors
often accept less desirable entry jobs within their professions or
even outside them;
immigrant manual workers, or illegal manual workers, have to
accept the most
arduous jobs with the lowest pay. Likewise, immigrant
entrepreneurs often start
small shops in the inner city, serving their own ethnic
community (Portes &
Rumbaut, 2006). Thus, although in general, parental education
was positively
related to parental occupation in the final model, their relation
may need to be
carefully scrutinized because of this possible underemployment
issue. Given that
immigrant parents who come as graduate students to the U.S.
may be low-
income families and other newly arrived immigrants may be
underemployed,
the validity of the measures of income and occupation as
indicators of other
behaviours may have been threatened. Third, because of the
difficulties of
conducting assessments with children in 17 states across the
U.S., language out-
comes of the children were not measured. On the basis of the
limitations, the
direction of the extended research is towards increasing the
sample size and
the accuracy of family SES measurement. Also, an experimental
design study
might illuminate the effects of different types of mediation
behaviours of educa-
tional television programme viewing on the language outcomes
of children in
immigrant families. Parenting variables (e.g. parenting style)
and cultural factors
(e.g. parental language use and values such as familism) might
also be considered
in future research. For example, immigrant families may watch
non-English
language channels such as those broadcasting in Spanish and
Chinese, a topic that
is worth exploring in future research. Also important for future
study would be
investigation of the popularity of native-language children’s
videos and websites
among immigrant families.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 417
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Conclusion
In conclusion, as addressed in the literature review, educational
television viewing
could help the language and literacy development for children
from at-risk back-
grounds. Accordingly, two conclusions of educational
implications could be drawn.
First, it would be helpful if parents from varying cultural
backgrounds could enact
active parental mediation behaviours (instructive and
restrictive)while their children
watch educational television. Active parental mediation
behaviours mean that
parents intentionally ask questions during viewing, guide
children to think, regulate
the viewing amount/content and always preview programmes.
Second, the results
suggests that parents are comfortable with exposing their
children to educational
television andmay also be comfortable with using educational
television as a supple-
mentary tool for teaching and learning in classrooms under the
circumstances of
careful previewing by researchers and teachers. Some studies
already showed that
educational television used in the classroom improved the
literacy development of
children from at-risk backgrounds (Phillips & Zhao, 2011;
Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006).
Few research studies existed to investigate the Asian and
Hispanic group differences
in educational television programmes viewing by children,
parental mediation and
parental acculturation, as well as the relations among these
three variables. To the
best of our knowledge, the present study serves as an initial
exploration into the topic
of educational television viewing of children in immigrant
families, and it awaits
replication from other studies on immigrants. Owing to the
rapid increase of the
Asian andHispanic immigrant population, the present
studywarrants further inves-
tigations in the future.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to Pam Burris, Galiya Tabulda, Kayla Sedgwick
and Smriti Jangra
for their comments on the article. The valuable suggestions
from Alysia Roehrig,
Barbara Foorman and Yanyun Yang are also appreciated.
REFERENCES
Anderson, D. R., Huston, A. C., Schmitt, K. L., Linebarger, D.
L., &Wright, J. C. (2001). Early
childhood television viewing and adolescent behavior: The
recontact study.Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(1), 1–143.
Arends-Tόth, J., & van de Vijer, F. J. R. (2006). Assessment of
psychological acculturation. In
D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of
acculturation psychology
(pp. 142–160). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Atkin, D. J., Greenberg, B. S., & Baldwin, T. F. (1991). The
home ecology of children’s television
viewing: Parental mediation and the new video environment.
Journal of Communication,
41(3), 40–52.
Berry, J. W.(1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In
A. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation:
Theory, models and some new findings (pp. 9–25). Boulder,
CO: Westview Press.
Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2003). Cultural and parenting
cognitions in acculturating
cultures: 2. Patterns of prediction and structural coherence.
Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 34(3), 350–373. DOI:
10.1177/0022022103034003007
Boyer, K. K., Olson, J. R., Calantone, R. J., & Jackson, E. C.
(2002). Print versus electronic sur-
veys: A comparison of two data collection methodologies.
Journal of Operations Management,
20(4), 357–373. DOI: 10.1016/S0272-6963(02)00004-9
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development:
Experiments by nature and
design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
418 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Bryant, J., & Bryant, J. A. (2001). Television and the American
family. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Buerkel-Rothfuss, N. L., & Buerkel, R. A. (2001). Family
mediation. In J. Bryant, & J. Alison
Bryant (Eds.), Television and the American family (2nd ed., pp.
355–376). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Burgess, S. R., Hecht, S. A., & Lonigan, C. J. (2002). Relations
of the home literacy environ-
ment (HLE) to the development of reading-related abilities: A
one-year longitudinal
study. Reading Research Quarterly, 37(4), 408–426.
Christakis, D. A., & Garrison, M. M. (2009). Preschool-aged
children’s television viewing in
child care settings. Pediatrics, 124, 1627–1632. DOI:
10.1542/peds.2009-0862
Dinan, K.A. (2006). Young children in immigrant families.
Report of YoungChildren in Immigrant
Families meeting held by Center for Children in Poverty
(NCCP) of Columbia University on
January 18–19, 2006. Retrieved from
http://www.nccp.org/publications/pub_661.html
Eng, S., Kanitkar, K., Cleveland, H. H., Herbert, R., Fischer, J.,
& Wiersma, J. D. (2008).
School achievement differences among Chinese and Filipino
American students:
Acculturation and the family. Educational Psychology: An
International Journal of Experimental
Educational Psychology, 28(5), 535–550. DOI:
10.1080/01443410701861308
Farver, J. M., Eppe, S., & Ballon, D. (2006). Acculturation and
family characteristics that
facilitate literacy development among Latino children. In M. H.
Bornstein, & L. R. Cote
(Eds.), Acculturation and parent–child relationships:
Measurement and development
(pp. 13–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Federal Communications Commission. (2006). Instruction for
FCC 398 children’s television
programming report. Retrieved from
http://transition.fcc.gov/Forms/Form398/398.pdf
Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for
clinicians and researchers. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532–538. DOI:
10.1037/a0015808
Fix, M. E., Zimmermann, W., & Passel, J. S. (2001). The
integration of immigrant families in
the United States. Retrieved from
http://www.urban.org/url.cfm?ID=410227
Glick, J. E., & White, M. J. (2004). Post-secondary school
participation of immigrant and
native youth: the role of familial resources and educational
expectations. Social Science
Research, 33(2), 272–299. DOI:
10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.06.001
Gordon,M. (1964). Assimilation inAmerican life: The role of
race, religion, and national origins
(pp. 9–25). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004).
Should we trust web-based
studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about
Internet questionnaires.
American Psychologist, 59(2), 93–104. DOI: 10.1037/0003-
066X.59.2.93
Green, L. W. (1970). Manual for scoring socioeconomic status
for research on health behav-
ior. Public Health Reports, 85(9), 815–827.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the
everyday experience of young
American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Hoff, E. (2005). Language development (3rd ed.). KY: Cengage
Learning.
Huston, A. C., &Wright, J. C. (1997). Mass media and
children’s development. In W. Damon
(Series Ed.), I. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Vol. Eds.), Handbook
of child psychology: Vol. 4.
Child psychology in practice (5th ed., pp. 999–1058). New
York: Wiley.
Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., Marquis, J., & Green, S. B. (1999).
How young children spend
their time: Television and other activities. Developmental
Psychology, 35(4), 912–925.
Jennings, N. A., Hooker, S. D., & Linebarger, D. L. (2009).
Educational television as mediated
literacy environments for preschoolers. Learning, Media and
Technology, 34(3), 229. DOI:
10.1080/17439880903141513
Kim, M., Chelladurai, P., & Trail, G. T. (2007). A model of
volunteer retention in youth sport.
Journal of Sport Management, 21(2), 151–171.
Kim, M. T., Han, H.-R., Shin, H. S., Kim, K. B., & Lee, H. B.
(2005). Factors associated with
depression experience of immigrant populations: A study of
Korean immigrants. Archives
of Psychiatric Nursing, 19(5), 217–225. DOI:
10.1016/j.apnu.2005.07.004
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural
equation modeling. NY: Guilford
Press.
Levene, H. (1960). Robust tests for equality of variances. In L.
Olkin (Ed.), Contributions to
probability and statistics (pp. 278–292). Palo Alto, CA:
Standard University Press.
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 419
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
http://www.nccp.org/publications/pub_661.html
http://transition.fcc.gov/Forms/Form398/398.pdf
http://www.urban.org/url.cfm?ID=410227
Linebarger, D. (2010). Do children living in low-income
families benefit from using high-
quality educational media? Presented at the Paper symposium
presented at the annual
meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
Denver, CO.
Linebarger, D. L., &Piotrowski, J. T. (2009). TV as storyteller:
How exposure to television
narratives impacts at-risk preschoolers’ story knowledge and
narrative skills. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 624–645. DOI:
10.1348/026151008X400445
Linebarger, D. L., & Piotrowski, J. T. (2010). Structure and
strategies in children-s educational
television: The roles of program type and learning strategies in
children’s learning. Child
Development, 81(5), 1582–1597. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2010.01493.x
Lix, L. M., Keselman, J. C., & Keselman, H. J. (1996).
Consequences of assumption violations
revisited: A quantitative review of alternatives to the one-way
analysis of variance “F”
test. Review of Educational Research, 66(4), 579–619. DOI:
10.2307/1170654
Maassen, G. H., & Bakker, A. B. (2001). Suppressor variables
in path models: Definitions and
interpretations. Sociological Methods & Research, 30, 241–270.
Marin, G., & Gamba, R. J. (1996). A new measurement of
acculturation for Hispanics: The
bidimensional acculturation scale for Hispanics (BAS).Hispanic
Journal of Behavioral Sciences,
18(3), 297–316. DOI: 10.1177/07399863960183002
National Center for Educational Statistics. (2010). The
condition of education 2010. Retrieved
April 7, 2011, from
http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010028
Neuman, S. B. (2006). The knowledge gap: Implications for
early education. InD. K. Dickinson,
& S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research:
Vol. 2 (pp. 29–40).
New York, NY: Guilford.
Nielsen. (2011). Television audience report 2010 & 2011.
Retrieved from http://www.nielsen.
com/us/en/insights/reports-downloads/2011/television-audience-
report-2010-2011.html
Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2005). Social correlates of
emergent literacy. In M. J.
Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A
handbook (pp. 173–187). Oxford,
UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the home
literacy environment of
preschool children: A cluster analytic approach. Scientific
Studies of Reading, 13, 146–174.
DOI: 10.1080/1088843090276953
Phillips, B. M., & Zhao, Y. (2011). Designing robust
vocabulary instruction for at-risk
preschoolers: Preliminary evidence for word acquisition.
Presented at Pacific Coast Research
Conference, San Diego, CA.
Pinõn, M. F., Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1989). Family
ecology and child characteristics
that predict young children’s educational television viewing.
Child Development, 60(4),
846–856. DOI: 10.2307/1131026
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A
portrait (3rd ed.). Berkeley and
Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.
Reese, L., Balzano, S., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (1995).
The concept of educacion: Latino
family values and American schooling. International Journal of
Educational Research, 23(1),
57–81. DOI: 10.1016/0883-0355(95)93535-4
Rideout, V., & Hamel, E. (2006). The media family: Electronic
media in the lives of infants,
toddlers, preschoolers, and their parents. Menlo Park, CA:
Kaiser Family Foundation.
http://www.kff.org/.
Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of
home literacy practices in
preschool children’s language and emergent literacy skills.
Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345–359.
Romero, A. J., Robinson, T. N., Haydel, K. F., Mendoza, F., &
Killen, J. D. (2004). Associa-
tions among familism, language preference, and education in
Mexican-American mothers
and their children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Pediatrics, 25(1), 34–40.
Sam, D. L. (2006). Acculturation: Conceptual background and
core components. In D. L. Sam,
& J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation
psychology (pp. 11–26).
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Shaffer, J. P. (1995). Multiple hypothesis testing. Annual
Review of Psychology, 46, 561–584.
Shannon, D. M., & Bradshaw, C. C. (2002). A comparison of
response rate, response
time, and costs of mail and electronic surveys. The Journal of
Experimental Education,
70(2), 179.
420 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010028
http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/reports-
downloads/2011/television-audience-report-2010-2011.html
http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/reports-
downloads/2011/television-audience-report-2010-2011.html
http://www.kff.org/
Stilling, E. A. (1997). The electronic melting pot hypothesis:
The cultivation of acculturation
among Hispanics through television viewing. Howard Journal of
Communications, 8, 77–100.
DOI: 10.1080/10646179709361744
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2001). The role of family
and home in the literacy devel-
opment of children from low-income backgrounds. New
Directions for Child and Adolescent
Development, 92, 53–71.
Uchikoshi, Y. (2005). Narrative development in bilingual
kindergarteners: Can Arthur help?
Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 464–478. DOI:
10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.464
Uchikoshi, Y. (2006). Early reading in bilingual kindergartners:
Can educational television
help? Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(1), 89. DOI:
10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_5
U.S. Census Bureau. (2009a). Current population reports.
Retrieved March 8, 2011, from
http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/cpstables/032010/hhinc/new0
1_000.htm
U.S. Census Bureau. (2009b). Educational attainment in the
United States: 2009. Retrieved
March 8, 2011, from
http://www.census.gov/hhes/socdemo/education/data/cps/2009/
tables.html
U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). 2010 Census questionnaire
reference book. Retrieved from
http://2010.census.gov/partners/pdf/langfiles/qrb_English.pdf
U.S. Department of Commerce (2010). Percentage of 3-, 4-, and
5-year old children enrolled in
preprimary programs, by attendance status, level of program,
and selected child and family
characteristics: October 2010. Retrieved from
http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/
indicator_epr.asp
Valkenburg, P. M., Krcmar, M., Peeters, A. L., & Marseille, N.
M. (1999). Developing a scale to
assess three styles of television mediation: “Instructive
mediation”, “restrictive mediation”,
and “social coviewing”. Journal of
Broadcasting&ElectronicMedia, 43(1), 52–66. DOI: 10.1080/
08838159909364474
Warren, R. (2003). Parental mediation of preschool children’s
television viewing. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(3), 394–417. DOI:
10.1207/s15506878jobem4703_5
Warren, R. (2005). Parental mediation of children’s television
viewing in low-income fami-
lies. Journal of Communication, 55(4), 847–863. DOI:
10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03026.x
Welch, B. L. (1951). On the Comparison of Several mean
values: An alternative approach.
Biometrika, 38(3/4), 330–336. DOI: 10.2307/2332579
White, R. M. B., Roosa, M. W., Weaver, S. R., & Nair, R. L.
(2009). Cultural and contextual in-
fluences on parenting in Mexican American families. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 71(1),
61–79.
Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C.,Murphy, K. C., St Peters, M. S.,
Pinõn,M. F., Scantlin, R., &Kotler, J.
(2001). The relations of early television viewing to school
readiness and vocabulary of
children from low-income families: The early window project.
Child Development, 72(5),
1347–1366.
Ying, Y.-W., & Han, M. (2008). Parental acculturation, parental
involvement, intergenerational
relationship and adolescent outcomes in immigrant Filipino
American families. Journal of
Immigrant & Refugee Studies, 6(1), 112. DOI:
10.1080/1536294080211935
Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 421
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22:
401–421 (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
http://2010.census.gov/partners/pdf/langfiles/qrb_English.pdf
http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_epr.asp
http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_epr.asp
Copyright of Infant & Child Development is the property of
John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
Parental influence on children's educational TV viewing

More Related Content

Similar to Parental influence on children's educational TV viewing

Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docx
Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docxLinguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docx
Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docxSHIVA101531
 
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learningUniversitas Kanjuruhan Malang
 
Researching Practice Assignment 1
Researching Practice Assignment 1Researching Practice Assignment 1
Researching Practice Assignment 1Tess Jabbour
 
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceStrickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceNCFLLiteracyNow
 
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceStrickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceNCFLLiteracyNow
 
Family Literacy & The Achievement Gap
Family Literacy & The Achievement GapFamily Literacy & The Achievement Gap
Family Literacy & The Achievement GapNCFLLiteracyNow
 
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...acceptableshell02
 
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolDevelopment of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolmrwindy_3282
 
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolDevelopment of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolmrwindy_3282
 
Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610rhdiddl2009
 
Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610rhdiddl2009
 
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descr
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 DescrResponse 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descr
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descrmickietanger
 
If you don´t understand, how can you learn?
If you don´t understand, how can you learn? If you don´t understand, how can you learn?
If you don´t understand, how can you learn? João Soares
 
Educating Refugee Students--full version
Educating Refugee Students--full versionEducating Refugee Students--full version
Educating Refugee Students--full versionTravis Snow
 
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...EvaniaYafie
 
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishFull document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishCfreeland1
 
Epsl 0801-250-epru
Epsl 0801-250-epruEpsl 0801-250-epru
Epsl 0801-250-epruHDMcWhorter
 
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the l
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the lResponse 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the l
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the lmickietanger
 
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2elizabethsroberts85
 
Jurnal Pendidikan Khas
Jurnal Pendidikan KhasJurnal Pendidikan Khas
Jurnal Pendidikan KhasLaling Honey
 

Similar to Parental influence on children's educational TV viewing (20)

Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docx
Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docxLinguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docx
Linguistically and culturally diverse is an educational term.docx
 
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
 
Researching Practice Assignment 1
Researching Practice Assignment 1Researching Practice Assignment 1
Researching Practice Assignment 1
 
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceStrickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl Conference
 
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl ConferenceStrickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl Conference
 
Family Literacy & The Achievement Gap
Family Literacy & The Achievement GapFamily Literacy & The Achievement Gap
Family Literacy & The Achievement Gap
 
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...
Parent-child interaction therapy and language facilitation: the role of paren...
 
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolDevelopment of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
 
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschoolDevelopment of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool
 
Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610
 
Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610Final paper 취합0610
Final paper 취합0610
 
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descr
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 DescrResponse 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descr
Response 1Discussion 1 Week 9 Main PostQuestion 1 Descr
 
If you don´t understand, how can you learn?
If you don´t understand, how can you learn? If you don´t understand, how can you learn?
If you don´t understand, how can you learn?
 
Educating Refugee Students--full version
Educating Refugee Students--full versionEducating Refugee Students--full version
Educating Refugee Students--full version
 
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
The Role and Strategy to Stimulate Language Development in Early Childhood Du...
 
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishFull document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
 
Epsl 0801-250-epru
Epsl 0801-250-epruEpsl 0801-250-epru
Epsl 0801-250-epru
 
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the l
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the lResponse 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the l
Response 1· Explain how socioeconomic factors affect the l
 
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2
Power Point Bilingual Education Final 2
 
Jurnal Pendidikan Khas
Jurnal Pendidikan KhasJurnal Pendidikan Khas
Jurnal Pendidikan Khas
 

More from herbertwilson5999

Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docx
Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docxWrite a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docx
Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docxWrite at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docxWrite a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docxWrite a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docx
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docxWrite a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docx
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docx
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docxWrite a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docx
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docx
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docxWrite a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docx
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docx
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docxWrite at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docx
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docx
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docxWrite at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docx
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docxWrite at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docx
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docxWrite at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docx
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docx
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docxWrite at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docx
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docx
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docxWrite at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docx
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docx
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docxWrite As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docx
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docx
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docxWrite an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docx
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docx
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docxWrite an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docx
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docxherbertwilson5999
 
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docx
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docxWrite an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docx
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docxherbertwilson5999
 

More from herbertwilson5999 (20)

Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docx
Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docxWrite a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docx
Write a 5-7 page paper describing the historical development of info.docx
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree.docx
 
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docxWrite at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which youIdentify the.docx
 
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docxWrite a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. Plea.docx
 
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docxWrite a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docx
Write a 2 page paper analyzing the fact pattern scenario below. .docx
 
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docx
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docxWrite a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docx
Write a 100-word response in Spanish that addresses both of .docx
 
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docx
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docxWrite a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docx
Write a  Request for Proposal (approx. 3 - 4 pages in a word doc.docx
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to Physical Therapy Assistant th.docx
 
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docx
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docxWrite a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docx
Write a  5 page paper with  at-least three images that represent.docx
 
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docxWrite a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docx
Write a  5 paragraph essay related to the healthcare fieldthree maj.docx
 
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docx
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docxWrite at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docx
Write at least Ten sentences on your discussion. Compare and con.docx
 
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docx
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docxWrite at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docx
Write at least a three-page analysis using the case study on pages.docx
 
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docxWrite at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docx
Write at least a six-page paper, in which you Identify th.docx
 
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docx
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docxWrite at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docx
Write at least a paragraph for each.1) What is your understand.docx
 
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docx
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docxWrite at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docx
Write at least 500 words analyzing a subject you find in this .docx
 
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docx
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docxWrite at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docx
Write at least 750 words paper on Why is vulnerability assessme.docx
 
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docx
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docxWrite As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docx
Write As if You Are Writing in Your Journal (1st Person)Your T.docx
 
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docx
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docxWrite an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docx
Write an original, Scholarly Paper, addressing a topic relevant to t.docx
 
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docx
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docxWrite an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docx
Write an observation essay that explains the unique significance.docx
 
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docx
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docxWrite an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docx
Write an introduction in APA format in about 2 pages to describe.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersChitralekhaTherkar
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptxPoojaSen20
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 

Parental influence on children's educational TV viewing

  • 1. Parental Influence on Children during Educational Television Viewing in Immigrant Families Yuting Zhaoa,b,* and Beth M. Phillipsa,b aDepartment of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL USA bFlorida Center for Reading Research, Tallahassee, FL USA It has been suggested by researchers that educational television programmes may support the language and literacy development for children, especially those in immigrant families. In an immigrant family, many family characteristics appear to be related to educa- tional television programme viewing of children at home, for exam- ple, parental acculturation (the process of adapting to the new culture) and parentalmediation (supervision and guidance) of televi- sion viewing. In the present work, the parental influence on children during educational television viewing was studied quantitatively, based on a sample (n=171) of immigrant families with children aged 3–6years collected across the U.S. The results have revealed that significant differences existed between Asian and Hispanic groups in coviewingmediation and in their children’s educational
  • 2. television viewing. Furthermore, language in parental acculturation signifi- cantly predicted instructive and restrictive parental mediation, and parental occupation significantly predicted language in parental acculturation. This study initiates the attention to the topic of educa- tional television viewing in immigrant families, which warrants further investigations in the future as the Asian and Hispanic immi- grant population increases rapidly in the U.S. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: parental influence; educational television; immigrant family; parental mediation; parental acculturation The period frombirth to 6years old for a child is themost critical and intensive one in terms of developing language, as the brain continues growing. Language develop- ment, which occurs during this period, is fundamental for children to be successful in modern society (Hoff, 2005). As importantly, emergent literacy development, as *Correspondence to: Yuting Zhao, Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems, Florida State University, 3210 Stone Building, 1114 West Call Street, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Infant and Child Development Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) Published online 29 May 2013 in Wiley Online Library
  • 3. (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/icd.1798 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. a later outcome of language development, has been related to reading achievement in school (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). This early childhood period is the best and most effective time for a child to acquire language and literacy skills, especially when immersed within a rich linguistic environment (Roberts, Jurgens, & Burchinal, 2005). However, some children from at-risk families (i.e. in poverty and with low parental education) are not as likely to be supported by their home environment (Hart & Risley, 1995; Neuman, 2006). A group that may be particularly at-risk includes children in immigrant families, who often face multiple risk factors associated with language learning. These factors are linked to the home literacy environment and, thus, ultimately to child outcomes (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001).These children, who enter school with limited basic literacy skills, may experience difficulties later in school (Phillips & Lonigan, 2009). In an attempt to improve the disadvantaged situ- ation of children in at-risk families, some researchers have found that educational television is likely to be an aid to the aforementioned issue (Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006). In the present work, we studied the family characteristics that could impact educa-
  • 4. tional television viewing of children in a sample of immigrant families potentially having at-risk backgrounds. Specifically, several key constructs, such as family socio-economic status (SES), educational television programme viewing, parental mediation of children’s viewing and parental acculturation have been investigated. Each of these is briefly addressed later. Family SES, Home Literacy Environment and Immigration Status A growing body of research indicates that SES is a complex and multi-aspect construct including family income, parents’ education and parental occupation (Phillips & Lonigan, 2005; Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). Family SES explained 42% of the variance in the rate of vocabulary growth of children at the age of 3 years (Hart & Risley, 1995). Low-SES status is a substantial disadvantage for children’s emergent literacy development (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). One of the ways that family SES might affect child outcomes is through the home literacy environment (Storch & Whitehurst, 2001). The home literacy environment refers to literacy activities (e.g. shared reading, rhyming games and watching educational television) that parents utilized to develop children’s language skills(Burgess, Hecht, & Lonigan, 2002). Families from low-SES backgrounds are often less likely to provide a rich home literacy environment (Phillips & Lonigan, 2009).
  • 5. In general, immigrant families often facemore hardship than native-born families, including crowded living conditions, food insecurity, poor health care and limited English proficiency (Dinan, 2006; Fix, Zimmermann, & Passel, 2001). However, there are some distinct family characteristics among different immigrant groups. According to the U.S. Census Current Population Reports (U.S. Census Bureau, 2009a), Asian American families had the highest mean annual income ($90 811), compared with Hispanic American families ($52 229), African American families ($46 046) and White families ($73 240). In terms of educational level, approximately 48.6% of Asian Americans had a college degree or higher, whereas only 11.8% of Hispanic Americans possessed a college degree or higher (U.S. Census Bureau, 2009b). In 2008, there were 1.4 million children aged 5–9years speaking a language other than English at home in the U.S. Recent surveys indicate that among these chil- dren, the largest group of children with spoken English difficulty is Hispanic/Latino children (22.9%), followed byAsian children (20.3%; National Center for Educational Statistics, 2010). Given that family background characteristics conferring risk frequently occur at the same time, children in immigrant families are often less likely to be supported
  • 6. 402 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd by their home literacy environment. Consistent with this premise, Farver, Eppe, and Ballon (2006) explored the home literacy environments of Latino children aged 4–5.5 years, and they found that several family characteristics were signifi- cantly related to the home literacy environment of Latino children, including family SES, parental literacy involvement, parents’ years in the U.S. and parental literacy habits. Educational Television Programmes Television has penetrated almost every aspect of family life since its invention. It was reported that 99% of the households (115.9 million) in the U.S. owned at least one television set in 2011 (Nielsen, 2011). A nationwide survey by Rideout and Hamel (2006) provided results that 99% of U.S. families with children 6 years or younger owned at least one television, and 43% of these children watched televi- sion for at least 2 hours in a typical day. Compared with other activities, such as reading books (40min), listening to music (48min) and playing video games
  • 7. (6min), watching television (59min) was the activity on which children 4–6 years spent the most time per day (Rideout & Hamel, 2006). The current definition of educational television by the Federal Communications Commission (FCC) is ‘programming that furthers the educational and informational needs of children 16 years of age and under in any respect, including the child’s intel- lectual/cognitive or social/emotional needs’ (2006, p. 2). Previous research suggests that educational television programmes are different from general television programmes and that they may have distinct influences on children’s development (Anderson, Huston, Schmitt, Linebarger, & Wright, 2001; Jennings, Hooker, & Linebarger, 2009; Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009). For example, most of the educa- tional television programmes on the Public Broadcasting Service (PBS) are research based (Jennings et al., 2009). The content seems not only to attract children to watch but also to support the language and literacy development of children (Jennings et al., 2009; Uchikoshi, 2006; Wright et al., 2001). The influence on literacy has been extensively studied regarding emergent literacy (Anderson et al., 2001; Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009; Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006), vocabulary knowledge (Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2010; Wright et al., 2001) and motivation to read and write (Anderson et al., 2001; Wright et al., 2001). Many of these studies utilized experimental designs
  • 8. (Linebarger & Piotrowski, 2009; Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006), and thus, one may appropri- ately draw the conclusion from the empirical results that educational television can support the literacy development of children. Given their educational characteristics, research-based educational television programmes may serve as an element of the home literacy environment. Family SES appears to relate to children’s educational television viewing, although the pre- vious literature on their relation was somewhat inconsistent. Some research studies suggested a positive relation (Huston, Wright, Marquis, & Green, 1999; Huston & Wright, 1997), whereas some failed to find the relation (Pinõn, Huston, & Wright, 1989). Phillips and Lonigan’s results (2009) indicated a negative relation between family SES (i.e. parental education and family income) and overall television viewing amount, as well as educational television programme viewing. These inconsistencies may arise because first, some studies (Huston et al., 1999; Huston & Wright, 1997) only asked about one specific educational television programme, Sesame Street. Second, one study (Pinõn et al., 1989) was conducted two decades ago, and the results may not be generalizable tomore recent family environments. There has been a substantial increase in the percentage of young children, particularly those from Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 403
  • 9. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd middle and upper middle class households, attending preschools rather than being at home before attending kindergarten (U. S. Department of Commerce, 2010), and this may interact with the time allocated to television viewing such that the overall amount of television viewing by young children is simplymuch lower than previous in these families (Christakis & Garrison, 2009). In addition, the explosion of cable television and Internet-based media content available to families with more discretionary income might dilute the viewing time for free- access PBS stations that broadcast educational television. To further explore this topic, the present study was intended to offer additional insight into how family SES is related to children’s educational television viewing. Parental Mediation Most of the time, children watch television programmes at home with their parents; thus, inevitably, they are impacted by their parents’ decisions on the content and amount of television viewing. Parental mediation refers to just such parental supervision and guidance of children’s television
  • 10. viewing. A study by Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, and Marseille (1999) included telephone interviews with 519 Dutch parents whose children were aged 5–12years. They utilized varimax-rotated exploratory factor analysis to extract three factors (i.e. instructive, restrictive and coviewing) of parental mediation of television viewing. Instructive mediation is that parents not only know very well the content and the characters but also discuss these aspects with their children and guide them to think. Restrictive mediation refers to the restriction from parents on the amount, the times and the content of children’s viewing. Coviewing mediation is that parents are physically present to watch television programmes together with their children (not necessarily creating educational occasions). The studies of Warren (2003, 2005) also supported these three strategies of parental mediation through two surveys in the U.S. from 129 middle-income families (87% Caucasian) and 306 low- income families (69% African American and 18% Caucasian), respectively. Warren (2005) provided three models examined by path analyses for these three types of parental mediation and found that parental education was a significant predictor for parental mediation. Atkin, Greenberg, and Baldwin (1991) earlier contextualized these three types of parental mediation of children’s television viewing (i.e. instructive, restrictive and
  • 11. coviewing mediation) within Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model (i.e. microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem and macrosystem). Bronfenbrenner (1979) emphasized the dynamics between a developing child and his or her environment. The microsystem is defined as bidirectional interactions and activities in a person’s immediate surroundings (Atkin et al., 1991). For instance, under the circumstances of children’s television viewing, parental involvement, children’s age and interpersonal interac- tions at home all belong to the bidirectional relationships in a domestic microsystem. Next, surrounding the microsystem is the mesosystem, which provides connections between microsystems to advance development. Exosystem refers to multiple social settings influencing the development of childrenwithout their direct participation. In this system, community life and family SES (i.e. parental education and income) seem to affect children’s television viewing. Macrosystem is the outmost level that consists of cultural consistencies. It is ‘various socioeconomic, ethnic, religious, and other sub-cultural groups, reflecting contrasting belief systems and lifestyles’ (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, p. 26). In the present study, the ethnic cultures of different immigrant groups and parents’ different coping strategies in adapting to the new culture seem to serve as the macrosystem that may affect children’s 404 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips
  • 12. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd television viewing (e.g. some Hispanic families’ values of educacion and familism and some Asian families’ value of collectivism, Bornstein & Cote, 2003; Reese, Balzano, Gallimore, & Goldenberg, 1995; Romero, Robinson, Haydel, Mendoza, & Killen, 2004). Parental Acculturation Parental acculturation is another variable that appears to affect children’s educational television programme viewing in immigrant families. Two models have described the phenomenon of acculturation. Gordon (1964) claimed that accultura- tion is the process by which a person is gradually absorbed into the host culture (linear or unidirectional models). However, the previous model was extended to be bidirectional and two-dimensional by Berry (1980); that is, people are able to acquire a new culture without necessarily losing their original one. During the process of parental acculturation (i.e. when parents in immigrant families adapt to the host culture), parents could employ acculturation strategies such as integration (selectively learning the new culture while maintaining the old one),
  • 13. assimilation (being totally absorbed into the new culture and not wanting to keep the old one), separation (separating themselves from the new culture) and marginalization (trying to have nothing to do with either the new culture or the old one; Sam, 2006). Portes and Rumbaut (2006) claimed that there was no particular acculturation pattern that fits all individuals or different immigrant groups. A number of applied studies, which have been conductedwith the guidance of the two acculturationmodels, led to some relevant findings concerning the various aspects of acculturation impacts. For example, integration and assimilation relate positively to school achievement (Eng et al., 2008), parenting (White, Roosa, Weaver, & Nair, 2009) and the home literacy environment (Farver et al., 2006). In summary, the existing literature suggests that family characteristics are closely linked to the home literacy environment and, thus, ultimately to child outcomes. Given that many immigrant children often struggle without special help, their problems have the potential to increase. Because educational television programmes may support the development of future literacy skills (Anderson et al., 2001), and PBS can be accessed by most of these children, parents could help these children develop literacy skills in part via educational television viewing. Research also indi- cates that children’s programme viewing is affected by parental
  • 14. mediation, family SES and the general home literacy environment (Farver et al., 2006; Warren, 2003, 2005). As well, in immigrant families, parental acculturation appears to serve as the background construct that intertwines with family SES (Farver et al., 2006). There- fore, it is important to study parental acculturation and parental mediation behav- iours associatedwith children’s educational television viewing in a high-risk sample. The objective of this studywas to investigate relations among family SES, parental mediation, parental acculturation and children’s educational television programme viewing in immigrant families, in particular within both Asian and Hispanic fami- lies. Previous literature indicated that there was a significant difference between Asian and Hispanic families regarding family income and parental education (U. S. Census Bureau, 2009a, 2009b). In addition, Asian and Hispanic immigrants are currently the largest two immigrant groups in the U.S. (Dinan, 2006). The following research questions were addressed: (RQ1) Is there a significant difference between the Asian and Hispanic children in the amount of viewing of educational television programmes? (RQ2) Is there a significant difference between the Asian and Hispanic children in the types of parental mediation of television viewing? (RQ3) Is there a Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 405
  • 15. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd significant difference between the Asian and Hispanic children in parental accultur- ation patterns? (RQ4) Overall, what are the relations among parental education and occupation, parental acculturation, parental mediation of television viewing, and children’s amount of educational programme viewing? On the basis of the previous literature (e.g. Atkin et al., 1991; Warren, 2003, 2005; Valkenburg et al., 1999), we anticipated that the outcomes in parental mediation, parental acculturation and educational television viewing by their children would be different between Asian and Hispanic groups and that family SES indicators and parental mediation behav- iours would predict children’s educational television viewing amount. For example, we hypothesized that parental mediation operates as a partial mediating variable for the relation between parental acculturation and children’s educational programme viewing amount. In light of mixed prior findings on SES indicators, we did not make a specific directional prediction regarding their relation to educational television viewing but rather just posited that there would be significant relations. Given the absence of substantial prior investigation of the other possible
  • 16. relations among acculturation indicators, parental mediation behaviours and children’s educational television viewing, we considered these aspects of the model as exploratory and did not have explicit a priori hypotheses for each pathway. METHOD Participants Eligible participants were parents from immigrant families in the U.S. with children between 3 and 6years old. According to the 2010 Census Questionnaire Reference Book, Hispanic Americans or Latino are immigrants who come from Spain, Mexico, Puerto Rico, Cuba or Spanish-speaking countries of Central and South America. Asian Americans are immigrants who come from Asian countries such as China, Korea, Vietnam, the Philippines, Japan, India, Indonesia, Malaysia, Singapore, Thailand and Cambodia (U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). Considering acculturation processes indicating that immigrants appear to be more assimilated into the host culture after three generations (Sam, 2006), it is likely that parents in immigrant families were most appropriate for the study if they were within three generations of arrival in the U.S. The first generation includes people who were born outside the U.S., the second generation includes people whowere born in the U.S. to foreign-born parents and the third generation includes people who were born in the
  • 17. U.S. to U.S.-born parents (Glick & White, 2004). A mechanism utilized for increasing potential participation was that the survey was available for completion both in paper and online. Prior research demonstrated that samples collected by electronic survey were diverse in terms of demographic features such as gender, SES, age and geographic region (Kim, Chelladurai, & Trail, 2007; Shannon& Bradshaw, 2002). Incentives were also utilized in that participants were rewarded by lottery for their completion of the survey. Furthermore, schools, which helped to distribute some of the surveys, received a thank-you gift. The sample (n=171) was composed of 45.0% Asians, 46.8% Hispanics, 7.0% European-Americans, and 1.2%AfricanAmericans. Participants came from 17 differ- ent states across the U.S. Fourteen participants were neither Asian nor Hispanic, but theywere eligible participants and thuswere retained in the overall sample analyses. Overall, 151 (21.0%) electronic survey responses were received of 706 emailed cover letters with survey links and 51 telephone call follow-ups, and 20 (6.0%) paper surveys were received of 300 mailed surveys. It should be noted that there was not 406 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013)
  • 18. DOI: 10.1002/icd a specific way to know how many different people were given the survey links or copies by these schools or organizations, so the response rate was an approximated calculation (i.e. each cover letter was likely distributed to multiple eligible individ- uals).The demographic characteristics of children and families overall and by groups of Asian and Hispanic are presented in Table 1. Of the 171 returned surveys, 47 (27.5%) were completed by fathers and 124 (72.5%) were completed by mothers. Measures A parent survey with four parts was created to measure children’s educational television viewing amounts, parental mediation and parental acculturation. The first part of the survey consisted of six items that investigated the background of parents including gender, ethnicity, generation, education, occupation and household annual income. The second part of the survey, including five questions, asked parents about their children’s background such as siblings, age and gender, children’s language use preference at home and school, and children’s television viewing (viewing amount and the content). In terms of viewing content, 29 educational television programmes
  • 19. Table 1. Child and family demographic characteristics overall and by group All Asian Hispanic Sample size 171 77 80 Family characteristics Parental education (years) Some college College High school M= 14.54 (SD=4.27) M= 16.68 (SD= 4.01) M= 12.36 (SD= 3.09) Family annual income <$16 000 15.2% 22.1% 11.3% $17 000–30 000 29.2% 23.4% 33.8% $30 000–75 000 25.2% 26.0% 25.0% $75 000–100 000 14.6% 14.3% 12.5% $100 000–150 000 11.7% 7.8% 16.3% >$150 000 4.1% 6.5% 1.3% Number of children by family (mean) 2.04 (SD=1.12) 1.82 (SD= 1.06) 2.28 (SD= 1.18) Generation (parents) 88.8% First 94.7% First 83.8% First 11.2% Second 5.3% Second 16.3% Second Child characteristics Gender 49.4% boys 48.7% boys 47.5% boys Age (years) M=4.74 (SD= 1.21) M=4.59 (SD= 1.61) M= 4.88 (SD=1.24) Children numbers by age Age 3: 38 Age 3: 18 Age 3: 16 Age 4: 28 Age 4: 16 Age 4: 10 Age 5: 51 Age 5: 22 Age 5: 26 Age 6: 53 Age 6: 20 Age 6: 28
  • 20. Born in the U.S. 73.8% 64.0% 87.3% Language use at home 32.9% English 43.4% English 15.5% English 36.5% Spanish 32.9% Chinese 77.5% Spanish 16.8% Chinese 23.7% Others 7.6% Others 14.9% Others Language use at school 95.9% English 96.1% English 96.3% English Note. The age information for one child was missing for the Asian group. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 407 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd from PBS were listed. Parents were asked to mark all educational television programmes that their child often watched (i.e. at least once a week). The third part of the survey contained 16 items – 15 of them (a= .90)were adapted from the 5-point Likert scale of parental mediation of television viewing fromValkenburg et al. (1999), who provided preliminary evidence of validity via factor analysis. Several example items are ‘how often do you try to help this child understand what they see in an educational television programme’ for instructive mediation, ‘how often do you
  • 21. forbid your child to watch certain programmes’ for restrictive mediation and ‘how often do you watch together because you both like an educational programme’ for coviewingmediation. The fourth part of the survey (a= .70)was adapted fromMarin and Gamba (1996) and Stilling (1997) to measure parental acculturation in three aspects of language, food andmedia (22 items in total). The validity of these accultur- ation items was also examined by factor analysis (Marin & Gamba, 1996), which supported the categories of language (i.e. language use and proficiency) and media. The food category (two items) was added into the survey, following suggestions by Arends-Tóth and van de Vijer (2006) in assessment of psychological acculturation. Two item categories (i.e. American and ethnic) were constructed in the acculturation measure: a score of 33 and higher in the first category signified high American preferences and, in the second category, signified high ethnic preferences. Through use of these cut-off scores, acculturation patterns among participants are presented as four categories for descriptive purposes: integrated (high American/high ethnic), assimilated (high American/low ethnic), separated (low American / high ethnic) and marginalized (low American / low ethnic). However, for statistical analyses, continu- ous data for each aspect of acculturation were used. Example items included ‘How often do you speak English’, ‘How often do you eat your own ethnic food’ and
  • 22. ‘How often do you watch television programmes in English’. Procedure Surveys were translated by native speakers in Spanish and Chinese. The two languages were selected because virtually all Hispanic immigrants speak Spanish and the largest population subgroup among Asian Americans is Chinese (23%; U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). The survey was distributed to parents in both English and the anticipated home language, so that parents could select the language in which they were most comfortable to respond. Before distribution, back translations were completed, and native speakers with an educational psychology and linguistics background checked all the wording of these items. There were several sources for recruiting eligible participants. First, the authors contacted the directors of centres and organizations for immigrants such as the Hispanic American Association and the AsianAmericanAssociation and asked for their help to distribute surveys. Second, the authors contacted international centres or similar centres at universities across the U.S., which served many international students and their families. Third, the authors contacted schools and child care centres in metropolitan areas known to have large eligible populations and asked for their help to distribute surveys. Fourth, the authors also collected some convenience samples, such as
  • 23. contacting schools and child care centres known to serve immigrant populations that have participated in prior research studies. Parents were contacted by either the researcher or their director (e.g. the director of a child care centre or of a community organization) via email or mail. Depending on their preferences, each parent participated by either visiting the website where they could complete the survey or returning the completed survey forms in the postage-paid envelopes provided. 408 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd RESULTS The results are presented in three parts: the first part includes the sample descriptive statistics; the second part includes one-way ANOVA tests of the group differences between Asian and Hispanic families in educational television viewing amount, and parental mediation and acculturation types; the third part is the path analyses of the overall relations among these variables. Preliminary Analyses Some transformations of coding were made to parental
  • 24. demographic variables such that categorical education levels were recalculated into average years in which one would achieve such educational levels (e.g. high school= 12years; Master’s degree= 18years). Standardized scores for categories of occupation were utilized (Kim, Han, Shin, Kim, & Lee, 2005). First, the nominal variables were transformed into ordinal variables according to aU.S. Census list of occupation ranks. Then, each occupational rank used by the Census corresponded to a standardized score; these scores were normalized and standardized from national income and education distributions associated with each occupation (e.g. manual workers = 46 and managers = 59; Green, 1970). Results indicated that more parents in Hispanic families were born in the U.S. than parents in Asian families, whereas Asian parents had a higher educa- tional level than did Hispanic parents (Table 1). Most families reported income in the range of $17 000–30 000; however, the nonsignificant results of chi-square tests for independence (w [5]2 = 9.50, p= .09) demonstrated that there was no statistically significant difference in annual income between Asian and Hispanic families. The correlation between education and occupation was statistically significant and positive (r= .68, p< .01). The means and standard deviations for parental mediation and parental acculturation are presented in Table 2.The descriptive
  • 25. categorization of acculturation types indicated that the majority of the sample were classified as integrated (70% overall, 76% Asian families, 64% Hispanic families), whereas approximately 24% of Table 2. Descriptive statistics, one-way ANOVA and Welch tests All (n= 171) Asian (n=77) Hispanic (n= 80) ANOVA or Welch F values M SD M SD M SD Educational TV viewing 12.33* Parental mediation Instructive 15.93 4.43 15.88 3.41 16.14 5.23 .14 (W) Restrictive 16.86 4.86 17.61 3.58 16.20 5.82 3.28 (W) Coviewing 17.76 3.92 16.19 3.30 19.62 3.42 39.72** Parental acculturation Language 30.81 5.88 31.28 4.79 30.08 6.83 1.62 (W) Food 4.90 1.37 4.66 1.09 5.03 1.61 2.70 (W) Media 24.84 3.23 24.45 3.39 25.13 3.20 1.63 (W) Note. M=mean; SD= standard deviation. A (W) after an F value means the assumption of homogeneity of variances were violated and the Welch tests were conducted and reported; the tests for the other variables were one-way ANOVA. *p< .01, **p< .001. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 409 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013)
  • 26. DOI: 10.1002/icd the sample were classified as separated. None were marginalized, and approxi- mately 4% were assimilated; this represented no Asian families but 11% of the Hispanic families. There were significant, positive correlations between instructive mediation, and restrictive and coviewing mediation (r= .68, p< .01; and r= .39, p< .01), respectively (Table 3). Language, one aspect of parental acculturation, was positively correlated with instructive and restrictive – two types of parental media- tion (r= .31, p< .01; and r= .38, p< .01). The hours that children viewed television, including educational television, were self-reported by parents both for weekdays and weekends. Reported hours ranged from 0 to 8hours per day. On average, children in immigrant families watched PBS shows 2.06hours per day, whereas they watched other general television pro- grammes 1.44hours per day. Restrictive mediation and parental education were negatively correlated with children’s educational programme viewing amount (r=�.20, p< .05, and r=�.27, p< .01, respectively; Table 3). Asian and Hispanic Group Differences: One-way ANOVA Tests One-way ANOVA tests were conducted to see whether there were significant
  • 27. differences between Asian and Hispanic families in children’s educational televi- sion viewing, parental mediation and acculturation (RQ1, RQ2 and RQ3, respec- tively). To control for Type I error, the Bonferroni correction (Shaffer, 1995) was applied to each ANOVA test, and only results after correction are reported as signif- icant. A one-way ANOVA analysis indicated that the difference in the amounts of viewing educational programmes was statistically significant between Asian and Hispanic children, F(1, 152) = 12.33, p= .00, Z2 = .08; Table 2. The effect size was moderate (Ferguson, 2009). The assumption of homogeneity of variances was checked (Levene, 1960), and the assumption was met (p= .20). One-way ANOVA were also conducted to test for group differences in each of the three types of parental mediation (i.e. instructive, restrictive and coviewing). The re- sults showed that the difference in frequency of coviewing mediation was statistically significant between Asian and Hispanic parents, F(1, 151)=39.72, p= .00, Z2 = .21; Table 2, whereas there were no statistically significant differences in instructive, F(1, 151)= .13, p= .72, and restrictive mediation behaviours, F(1, 151) =3.18, Table 3. Correlations among observed variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1.Education 1
  • 28. 2.Occupation .68** 1 3.Instructive mediation .08 �.05 1 4.Restrictive mediation .18* .01 .68** 1 5.Coviewing mediation �.22* �.15 .39** .16* 1 6.Language .12 .30** .31** .38** .06 1 7.Food .07 .16* .26* .15 �.02 .31** 1 8.Media .07 .15 .15 .18* �.03 .41** .37** 1 9.ETVAmt �.27** �.05 �.08 �.20* .10 .00 .02 �.00 1 10.General TVAmt �.18* �.00 �.00 �.09 �.06 .14 .07 �.06 .31** 1 Note.ETV Amt= the amount of educational television programmes that children watched every day; General TVAmt= the amount of general television viewing every day. *p< .05, **p< .01. 410 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd p= .08, between Asian and Hispanic parents. The assumption of homogeneity of variances was met for coviewing mediation but violated for instructive and re- strictive mediation. The Welch test (Welch, 1951) is an alternative F test to be used when the homogeneity of variances is violated, and it was recommended by Lix, Keselman, and Keselman (1996) that the Welch test could be used in most one-way analyses regardless of the equality of group sizes
  • 29. and sample size. Therefore, the Welch test was conducted, and the results of instructive me- diation and restrictive mediation were F(1, 135)= .14, p= .71 and F(1, 131)=3.28, p= .07, respectively. However, there were no significant differences between the Asian and Hispanic groups in instructive and restrictive mediation frequencies. With regard to the group differences in parental acculturation aspects, the as- sumption of homogeneity of variances was violated. Instead, the Welch test (1951) was conducted for the three aspects of parental acculturation (i.e. language, food and media) between the Asian and Hispanic groups. The results were F(1, 140)=1.62, p= .21 for language, F(1, 138)=2.70, p= .10 for food and F(1, 149)=1.63, p= .20 for media (Table 2), which show that there were no differences between Asian and Hispanic parents in the three aspects of parental acculturation. Overall Relations: Path Analysis Given the few differences found between Asian and Hispanic families, the subgroups were combined for the overall model analysis. The fourth research ques- tion (RQ4), investigating the overall relations among parental education and occupa- tion, parental mediation, parental acculturation and children’s educational television viewing, was analysed by path analysis. Path analysis is the
  • 30. estimation of expected causal relations among observed variables (Kline, 2005). The fully saturated model with all possible pathways between variables was first tested (Model 1; Table 4). Because all the variables were multivariate normal, the maximum likelihood estima- tionmethodwas used to analyseModel 1. On the basis ofModel 1, the significance of paths and the modification index were checked. Modification index values suggest that adding any of the parameters listed in the output will reduce the model chi- square by at least 3.84. Nonsignificant paths (a> .05) were removed or fixed to zero one group by one group from large to small in order to achieve a parsimonious model (simple and good fit). Eight models were tested, and some necessary modifi- cations were made. The chi-square and model fit indices for these models are provided in Table 4. A stepwise comparison procedure was used to select the final model. The chi- square difference test was used to evaluate the relative fit of two nested models (i.e. Models 7 and 8). The results indicated that the chi-square difference test was significant (Δw2 [1] = 4.38> w2critical value [1] = 3.84, a= .05), which suggests that Model 8 fits significantly better than Model 7. On the basis of the stepwise model comparison procedure, as well as the chi-square difference test, Model 8 (see Figure 1 for standardized parameters) was selected as the final
  • 31. model. The results suggested that parental education and restrictive mediation significantly, negatively, predicted the amount of children’s educational television viewing and that language in parental acculturation positively predicted instructive and restric- tive mediation. In addition, parental education was positively related to parental occupation. Given its negligible bivariate relation with educational television view- ing, the significant pathway between occupation and the outcome is likely the result of its suppressor role in the model and should not necessarily be interpreted (Maassen & Bakker, 2001). Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 411 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd Ta bl e 4. R es ul ts fr
  • 57. d ua l. 412 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd The results of the overall model demonstrated that one aspect of parental mediation, restrictive mediation had significant correlations with both language in parental acculturation and educational television viewing by children. There were also indirect effects from parental occupation to educational television viewing amount (i.e. occupation➔ language➔ restrictive mediation➔ educational television viewing amount; Table 5). In other words, parental occupation, rather than parental education, is more likely to affect parental acculturation (language), and this effect may continue to influence parental mediation patterns (restrictive mediation). Restrictive mediation may then affect children’s educational television viewing amount negatively. DISCUSSION This present study explored whether there were significant
  • 58. differences among immigrant families between Asian and Hispanic parents of young children in pa- rental mediation, parental acculturation and their children’s educational programme viewing, as well as the relations among these three concepts (Figure 1). The key findings of the present study included first, Hispanic children watched significantly more television overall and specifically more educational television than did Asian children, and second, Hispanic parents significantly enacted more coviewing mediation behaviours than did Asian parents. Third, language in parental acculturation significantly predicted instructive and restrictive parental mediation. Fourth, parental occupation significantly predicted language in paren- tal acculturation. On average, children watched television 3.50 hours per day, which is consistent with a national survey finding that 43% of the children watched television for at Figure 1. Final pathmodel with standardized parameters (Model 8). Edu=Education; Occu= Occupation; Lang=Language; Instr = Instructive mediation; Restr =Restrictive mediation; Coview=Coviewingmediation; ETVAmt= the amount of educational television programmes that children watched every day; PA=parental acculturation; PM=parental mediation. *p< .01, **p< .001.
  • 59. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 413 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd Ta bl e 5. St an d ar d iz ed d ir ec t, in d ir ec t an d to ta
  • 69. 01 414 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd least 2 hours in a typical day (Rideout & Hamel, 2006). Children in Asian immi- grant families spend significantly less time watching PBS shows than children in Hispanic immigrant families. There are two possible explanations for this finding. First, the demographic statistics in the sample showed that on average, Asian families reported a higher average parental educational level than did the Hispanic families, and parental education has previously (and in the present study) been found to be negatively related to overall television viewing (Linebarger, 2010). Children of parents with low levels of education typically watch more tele- vision programmes in general, and the overall television viewing amount seems to drive the educational television programmes viewing amount (Phillips & Lonigan, 2009). In accordance with this explanation, education was negatively related to educational television viewing within this sample. Second, more Hispanic parents reported engaging in coviewing mediation of television viewing than did Asian
  • 70. parents. The findings provide additional support for previous research (Bryant & Bryant, 2001) that children whose parents coviewed television with them would watch more television compared with children watching alone. The results showed significant differences between Asian and Hispanic groups in coviewing mediation but not in instructive and restrictive mediations. Hispanic parents reported that their most frequent mediation type was coviewing media- tion. Parental education was significantly, negatively, related to coviewing media- tion (Warren, 2005), and Hispanic parents in this sample reported a lower parental education than Asian parents; thus, they might engage in more coviewing media- tion than the Asian parents. With regard to restrictive mediation, the result contrasted with that for coviewing mediation. Asian parents reported more restric- tive mediation compared with Hispanic parents. This outcome may stem from findings that education is often highly emphasized in Asian immigrant families (Eng et al., 2008). Further, they tend to set rules to regulate their children, and children are expected to have unquestioning obedience to their parents. Under this kind of parenting, children might experience more restrictive mediation of television viewing (Buerkel-Rothfuss & Buerkel, 2001). Consequently, it was not surprising to see that Asian parents were more restrictive of children’s television viewing than
  • 71. Hispanic parents in this sample. Parents who frequently engage in restrictive media- tion may restrict the overall television viewing of children, which may accordingly reduce viewing amounts for educational television programmes. In the present study, restrictive mediation was significantly positively correlated to instructive mediation, such that parents who tended to set rules for their children on watching television were more likely to ask questions and guide children to think during viewing. When these two mediation behaviours are combined, known as active parental mediation (Warren, 2003, 2005), it may facilitate good outcomes for the language development of children. However, restrictivemediation is not necessar- ily good or bad, and it may depend on how parents engage in the communications with their children. That is, it may be better that parents regulate children’s viewing time and content without hurting their feelings and motivations to learn. Prior studies by Warren (2003, 2005) indicated that parents’ attitudes towards television were related to the parental mediation types. Parents’ attitudes towards television significantly and positively predicted instructive and restrictive media- tions but not coviewing mediation (Warren, 2005). This finding suggests that the influences of the macrosystem and exosystem from Bronfenbrenner’s model decreased and the influences of the mesosystem (parents’
  • 72. attitudes) increased. This model, taking acculturation as an indicator of attitude, might explain our results that there were no significant differences in instructive and restrictive mediation types between Asian and Hispanic families. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 415 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd Conceptually, although some indicators within each (e.g. restrictive and coviewing mediation) were not highly correlated, parental acculturation and paren- tal mediation could be treated as two overall constructs for the purposes of this discussion. Parental acculturation significantly predicted parental mediation in some aspects (Figure 1). Parents with a higher acculturation level appear to be more likely to enact active parental mediation behaviours, inwhich parents are actively involved with their children by talking, explaining, asking questions and setting rules. High levels of acculturation include both integrated and assimilated acculturation patterns. Because the proportion of immigrants categorized as assimilated was very small, most of the participants were considered tomatch the integrated acculturation pattern. These participants may have high English proficiency,
  • 73. high frequency of English use, high frequency of American media use and high motivation to use media to learn American culture. A study by Ying and Han (2008) indicated that parental acculturation, especially language acculturation (higher English language proficiency), significantly predicted active parental involvement at school and home (e.g. parents’ interactions with teachers, parents helping children complete homework and parents readingwith chil- dren). A parallel could be drawn from this earlier study, because parental mediation is also a parental involvement behaviour related to children’s television viewing. From this perspective, the results that parental acculturation significantly predicted parental mediation are in line with the previous research. Nonactive parentalmediation includes coviewingmediation, inwhich parents are just physically present to watch television together with their children and do not necessarily create educational occasions or have linguistic interactions with their children. Coviewing mediation was negatively correlated with parental education. It is likely that parents in this kind of mediation may just take it for granted that children could acquire literacy knowledge from educational television automatically, but ignore or not be aware of the parental role of guiding children to think and enhancing children’s learning outcomes through language
  • 74. interactions while watching. In addition, some parents from lower-SES families, whomay have towork two or three jobs to support families, may have no time or energy to be actively in- volved with children’s television viewing (i.e. instructive and restrictive mediations). Among the SES predictors, only parental occupation was positively predictive of parental acculturation. Immigrant professionals (i.e. thosewith a higher occupational coding) seem to be more likely than parents with blue collar positions (e.g. manual laborers) to be exposed to the whole American community rather than only their own ethnic community, and they may have more opportunities to interact with the American community. These opportunities may in turn provide parents with increased English proficiency and motivation to speak in this language. The assimilation rates, in contrast to integrated or separated patterns, were very low for both Asian and Hispanic parents, although both subgroups reported a pro- portion of second-generation parents. In past decades, immigrants disproportionally concentrated in several metropolitan locations such as Los Angeles, San Francisco, New York, Miami and Chicago, and these concentrations provided a variety of large immigrant communities in these metropolitan areas (i.e. the Mexican community in Los Angeles, Cuban community in Miami and Chinese
  • 75. community in San Francisco and New York; Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). Immigrants may have less pressure and motivation to give up their own cultures while still living in their native cultural community (e.g. China town, East LA and Little Italy) within the new country (Fix et al., 2001; Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). It is likely that these individuals have just selectively adapted to American society. Perhaps, this is the reason that the majority of Asian and Hispanic immigrants in this sample adopted the strategy of integrated 416 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd acculturation, which is selectively learning the American culture while maintaining the native one. Future research should target larger proportions of second- generation and third-generation immigrant families to learn whether these accultur- ation patterns replicate. Limitations The primary limitation of this studywas that the sample sizewas not large, as well as convenience sampling; thus, the results of this studymay not fully generalize to other
  • 76. populations. As the sample was ascertained through participation in community centres and child care centres, it is not possible to determine its full representative- ness of the eligible immigrant population of families with young children. We provided potential participants in multiple states with the opportunity to respond either via paper or via electronic survey and provided the survey in multiple lan- guages to increase the generalizability. Moreover, the findings of electronic surveys have been found to be consistent with traditional data collection and can be general- ized to a larger population (Boyer, Olson, Calantone, & Jackson, 2002; Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & John, 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Shannon & Bradshaw, 2002). In further investigations, the goal is to increase the robustness of the path models with larger, ideally nationally representative samples. A second limitation relates to the item investigating income, which only asked parents to mark a range of family annual income, did not allow finer grain differen- tiations between groups, although it likely increased respondents’ comfort with providing this type of sensitive material. It should also be noted that despite the experience and qualifications of some recent immigrants, they are more likely to ‘enter at the bottom of their respective occupational ladders’ (Portes & Rumbaut, 2006, p. 58). For instance, immigrant professionals such as engineers and doctors
  • 77. often accept less desirable entry jobs within their professions or even outside them; immigrant manual workers, or illegal manual workers, have to accept the most arduous jobs with the lowest pay. Likewise, immigrant entrepreneurs often start small shops in the inner city, serving their own ethnic community (Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). Thus, although in general, parental education was positively related to parental occupation in the final model, their relation may need to be carefully scrutinized because of this possible underemployment issue. Given that immigrant parents who come as graduate students to the U.S. may be low- income families and other newly arrived immigrants may be underemployed, the validity of the measures of income and occupation as indicators of other behaviours may have been threatened. Third, because of the difficulties of conducting assessments with children in 17 states across the U.S., language out- comes of the children were not measured. On the basis of the limitations, the direction of the extended research is towards increasing the sample size and the accuracy of family SES measurement. Also, an experimental design study might illuminate the effects of different types of mediation behaviours of educa- tional television programme viewing on the language outcomes of children in immigrant families. Parenting variables (e.g. parenting style) and cultural factors
  • 78. (e.g. parental language use and values such as familism) might also be considered in future research. For example, immigrant families may watch non-English language channels such as those broadcasting in Spanish and Chinese, a topic that is worth exploring in future research. Also important for future study would be investigation of the popularity of native-language children’s videos and websites among immigrant families. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 417 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd Conclusion In conclusion, as addressed in the literature review, educational television viewing could help the language and literacy development for children from at-risk back- grounds. Accordingly, two conclusions of educational implications could be drawn. First, it would be helpful if parents from varying cultural backgrounds could enact active parental mediation behaviours (instructive and restrictive)while their children watch educational television. Active parental mediation behaviours mean that parents intentionally ask questions during viewing, guide children to think, regulate
  • 79. the viewing amount/content and always preview programmes. Second, the results suggests that parents are comfortable with exposing their children to educational television andmay also be comfortable with using educational television as a supple- mentary tool for teaching and learning in classrooms under the circumstances of careful previewing by researchers and teachers. Some studies already showed that educational television used in the classroom improved the literacy development of children from at-risk backgrounds (Phillips & Zhao, 2011; Uchikoshi, 2005, 2006). Few research studies existed to investigate the Asian and Hispanic group differences in educational television programmes viewing by children, parental mediation and parental acculturation, as well as the relations among these three variables. To the best of our knowledge, the present study serves as an initial exploration into the topic of educational television viewing of children in immigrant families, and it awaits replication from other studies on immigrants. Owing to the rapid increase of the Asian andHispanic immigrant population, the present studywarrants further inves- tigations in the future. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are grateful to Pam Burris, Galiya Tabulda, Kayla Sedgwick and Smriti Jangra for their comments on the article. The valuable suggestions from Alysia Roehrig,
  • 80. Barbara Foorman and Yanyun Yang are also appreciated. REFERENCES Anderson, D. R., Huston, A. C., Schmitt, K. L., Linebarger, D. L., &Wright, J. C. (2001). Early childhood television viewing and adolescent behavior: The recontact study.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(1), 1–143. Arends-Tόth, J., & van de Vijer, F. J. R. (2006). Assessment of psychological acculturation. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 142–160). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Atkin, D. J., Greenberg, B. S., & Baldwin, T. F. (1991). The home ecology of children’s television viewing: Parental mediation and the new video environment. Journal of Communication, 41(3), 40–52. Berry, J. W.(1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In A. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, models and some new findings (pp. 9–25). Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2003). Cultural and parenting cognitions in acculturating cultures: 2. Patterns of prediction and structural coherence. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(3), 350–373. DOI: 10.1177/0022022103034003007 Boyer, K. K., Olson, J. R., Calantone, R. J., & Jackson, E. C. (2002). Print versus electronic sur-
  • 81. veys: A comparison of two data collection methodologies. Journal of Operations Management, 20(4), 357–373. DOI: 10.1016/S0272-6963(02)00004-9 Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 418 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd Bryant, J., & Bryant, J. A. (2001). Television and the American family. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Buerkel-Rothfuss, N. L., & Buerkel, R. A. (2001). Family mediation. In J. Bryant, & J. Alison Bryant (Eds.), Television and the American family (2nd ed., pp. 355–376). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Burgess, S. R., Hecht, S. A., & Lonigan, C. J. (2002). Relations of the home literacy environ- ment (HLE) to the development of reading-related abilities: A one-year longitudinal study. Reading Research Quarterly, 37(4), 408–426. Christakis, D. A., & Garrison, M. M. (2009). Preschool-aged children’s television viewing in child care settings. Pediatrics, 124, 1627–1632. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2009-0862
  • 82. Dinan, K.A. (2006). Young children in immigrant families. Report of YoungChildren in Immigrant Families meeting held by Center for Children in Poverty (NCCP) of Columbia University on January 18–19, 2006. Retrieved from http://www.nccp.org/publications/pub_661.html Eng, S., Kanitkar, K., Cleveland, H. H., Herbert, R., Fischer, J., & Wiersma, J. D. (2008). School achievement differences among Chinese and Filipino American students: Acculturation and the family. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 28(5), 535–550. DOI: 10.1080/01443410701861308 Farver, J. M., Eppe, S., & Ballon, D. (2006). Acculturation and family characteristics that facilitate literacy development among Latino children. In M. H. Bornstein, & L. R. Cote (Eds.), Acculturation and parent–child relationships: Measurement and development (pp. 13–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Federal Communications Commission. (2006). Instruction for FCC 398 children’s television programming report. Retrieved from http://transition.fcc.gov/Forms/Form398/398.pdf Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532–538. DOI: 10.1037/a0015808 Fix, M. E., Zimmermann, W., & Passel, J. S. (2001). The
  • 83. integration of immigrant families in the United States. Retrieved from http://www.urban.org/url.cfm?ID=410227 Glick, J. E., & White, M. J. (2004). Post-secondary school participation of immigrant and native youth: the role of familial resources and educational expectations. Social Science Research, 33(2), 272–299. DOI: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.06.001 Gordon,M. (1964). Assimilation inAmerican life: The role of race, religion, and national origins (pp. 9–25). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59(2), 93–104. DOI: 10.1037/0003- 066X.59.2.93 Green, L. W. (1970). Manual for scoring socioeconomic status for research on health behav- ior. Public Health Reports, 85(9), 815–827. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Hoff, E. (2005). Language development (3rd ed.). KY: Cengage Learning. Huston, A. C., &Wright, J. C. (1997). Mass media and children’s development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), I. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Vol. Eds.), Handbook
  • 84. of child psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (5th ed., pp. 999–1058). New York: Wiley. Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., Marquis, J., & Green, S. B. (1999). How young children spend their time: Television and other activities. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 912–925. Jennings, N. A., Hooker, S. D., & Linebarger, D. L. (2009). Educational television as mediated literacy environments for preschoolers. Learning, Media and Technology, 34(3), 229. DOI: 10.1080/17439880903141513 Kim, M., Chelladurai, P., & Trail, G. T. (2007). A model of volunteer retention in youth sport. Journal of Sport Management, 21(2), 151–171. Kim, M. T., Han, H.-R., Shin, H. S., Kim, K. B., & Lee, H. B. (2005). Factors associated with depression experience of immigrant populations: A study of Korean immigrants. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 19(5), 217–225. DOI: 10.1016/j.apnu.2005.07.004 Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford Press. Levene, H. (1960). Robust tests for equality of variances. In L. Olkin (Ed.), Contributions to probability and statistics (pp. 278–292). Palo Alto, CA: Standard University Press. Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 419
  • 85. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd http://www.nccp.org/publications/pub_661.html http://transition.fcc.gov/Forms/Form398/398.pdf http://www.urban.org/url.cfm?ID=410227 Linebarger, D. (2010). Do children living in low-income families benefit from using high- quality educational media? Presented at the Paper symposium presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Denver, CO. Linebarger, D. L., &Piotrowski, J. T. (2009). TV as storyteller: How exposure to television narratives impacts at-risk preschoolers’ story knowledge and narrative skills. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 624–645. DOI: 10.1348/026151008X400445 Linebarger, D. L., & Piotrowski, J. T. (2010). Structure and strategies in children-s educational television: The roles of program type and learning strategies in children’s learning. Child Development, 81(5), 1582–1597. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2010.01493.x Lix, L. M., Keselman, J. C., & Keselman, H. J. (1996). Consequences of assumption violations revisited: A quantitative review of alternatives to the one-way analysis of variance “F” test. Review of Educational Research, 66(4), 579–619. DOI:
  • 86. 10.2307/1170654 Maassen, G. H., & Bakker, A. B. (2001). Suppressor variables in path models: Definitions and interpretations. Sociological Methods & Research, 30, 241–270. Marin, G., & Gamba, R. J. (1996). A new measurement of acculturation for Hispanics: The bidimensional acculturation scale for Hispanics (BAS).Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18(3), 297–316. DOI: 10.1177/07399863960183002 National Center for Educational Statistics. (2010). The condition of education 2010. Retrieved April 7, 2011, from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010028 Neuman, S. B. (2006). The knowledge gap: Implications for early education. InD. K. Dickinson, & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research: Vol. 2 (pp. 29–40). New York, NY: Guilford. Nielsen. (2011). Television audience report 2010 & 2011. Retrieved from http://www.nielsen. com/us/en/insights/reports-downloads/2011/television-audience- report-2010-2011.html Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2005). Social correlates of emergent literacy. In M. J. Snowling, & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 173–187). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. Phillips, B. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the home literacy environment of
  • 87. preschool children: A cluster analytic approach. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 146–174. DOI: 10.1080/1088843090276953 Phillips, B. M., & Zhao, Y. (2011). Designing robust vocabulary instruction for at-risk preschoolers: Preliminary evidence for word acquisition. Presented at Pacific Coast Research Conference, San Diego, CA. Pinõn, M. F., Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1989). Family ecology and child characteristics that predict young children’s educational television viewing. Child Development, 60(4), 846–856. DOI: 10.2307/1131026 Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A portrait (3rd ed.). Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press. Reese, L., Balzano, S., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (1995). The concept of educacion: Latino family values and American schooling. International Journal of Educational Research, 23(1), 57–81. DOI: 10.1016/0883-0355(95)93535-4 Rideout, V., & Hamel, E. (2006). The media family: Electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers, preschoolers, and their parents. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation. http://www.kff.org/. Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children’s language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language,
  • 88. and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345–359. Romero, A. J., Robinson, T. N., Haydel, K. F., Mendoza, F., & Killen, J. D. (2004). Associa- tions among familism, language preference, and education in Mexican-American mothers and their children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(1), 34–40. Sam, D. L. (2006). Acculturation: Conceptual background and core components. In D. L. Sam, & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology (pp. 11–26). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Shaffer, J. P. (1995). Multiple hypothesis testing. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 561–584. Shannon, D. M., & Bradshaw, C. C. (2002). A comparison of response rate, response time, and costs of mail and electronic surveys. The Journal of Experimental Education, 70(2), 179. 420 Y. Zhao and B.M. Phillips Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2010028 http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/reports- downloads/2011/television-audience-report-2010-2011.html http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/reports- downloads/2011/television-audience-report-2010-2011.html http://www.kff.org/
  • 89. Stilling, E. A. (1997). The electronic melting pot hypothesis: The cultivation of acculturation among Hispanics through television viewing. Howard Journal of Communications, 8, 77–100. DOI: 10.1080/10646179709361744 Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2001). The role of family and home in the literacy devel- opment of children from low-income backgrounds. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 53–71. Uchikoshi, Y. (2005). Narrative development in bilingual kindergarteners: Can Arthur help? Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 464–478. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.464 Uchikoshi, Y. (2006). Early reading in bilingual kindergartners: Can educational television help? Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(1), 89. DOI: 10.1207/s1532799xssr1001_5 U.S. Census Bureau. (2009a). Current population reports. Retrieved March 8, 2011, from http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/cpstables/032010/hhinc/new0 1_000.htm U.S. Census Bureau. (2009b). Educational attainment in the United States: 2009. Retrieved March 8, 2011, from http://www.census.gov/hhes/socdemo/education/data/cps/2009/ tables.html U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). 2010 Census questionnaire
  • 90. reference book. Retrieved from http://2010.census.gov/partners/pdf/langfiles/qrb_English.pdf U.S. Department of Commerce (2010). Percentage of 3-, 4-, and 5-year old children enrolled in preprimary programs, by attendance status, level of program, and selected child and family characteristics: October 2010. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/ indicator_epr.asp Valkenburg, P. M., Krcmar, M., Peeters, A. L., & Marseille, N. M. (1999). Developing a scale to assess three styles of television mediation: “Instructive mediation”, “restrictive mediation”, and “social coviewing”. Journal of Broadcasting&ElectronicMedia, 43(1), 52–66. DOI: 10.1080/ 08838159909364474 Warren, R. (2003). Parental mediation of preschool children’s television viewing. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47(3), 394–417. DOI: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4703_5 Warren, R. (2005). Parental mediation of children’s television viewing in low-income fami- lies. Journal of Communication, 55(4), 847–863. DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.tb03026.x Welch, B. L. (1951). On the Comparison of Several mean values: An alternative approach. Biometrika, 38(3/4), 330–336. DOI: 10.2307/2332579 White, R. M. B., Roosa, M. W., Weaver, S. R., & Nair, R. L. (2009). Cultural and contextual in- fluences on parenting in Mexican American families. Journal of
  • 91. Marriage and Family, 71(1), 61–79. Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C.,Murphy, K. C., St Peters, M. S., Pinõn,M. F., Scantlin, R., &Kotler, J. (2001). The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: The early window project. Child Development, 72(5), 1347–1366. Ying, Y.-W., & Han, M. (2008). Parental acculturation, parental involvement, intergenerational relationship and adolescent outcomes in immigrant Filipino American families. Journal of Immigrant & Refugee Studies, 6(1), 112. DOI: 10.1080/1536294080211935 Educational TV Viewing in Immigrant Families 421 Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. 22: 401–421 (2013) DOI: 10.1002/icd http://2010.census.gov/partners/pdf/langfiles/qrb_English.pdf http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_epr.asp http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_epr.asp Copyright of Infant & Child Development is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.