SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 218
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Developmental Review
journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate/dr
Natural conversations as a source of false memories
in children: Implications for the testimony
of young witnesses
Gabrielle F. Principe ⇑ , Erica Schindewolf
Department of Psychology, Ursinus College, United States
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Article history:
Available online 25 July 2012
Keywords:
Memory
Suggestibility
Children
Social interaction
Rumor
Eyewitness testimony
0273-2297/$ - see front matter � 2012 Elsevier In
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department o
United States. Fax: +1 610 409 3633.
E-mail address: [email protected] (G.F. Pri
Research on factors that can affect the accuracy of children’s
auto-
biographical remembering has important implications for under-
standing the abilities of young witnesses to provide legal
testimony. In this article, we review our own recent research on
one factor that has much potential to induce errors in children’s
event recall, namely natural memory sharing conversations with
peers and parents. Our studies provide compelling evidence that
not only can the content of conversations about the past intrude
into later memory but that such exchanges can prompt the
gener-
ation of entirely false narratives that are more detailed than true
accounts of experienced events. Further, our work show that
dee-
per and more creative participation in memory sharing dialogues
can boost the damaging effects of conversationally conveyed
mis-
information. Implications of this collection of findings for chil-
dren’s testimony are discussed.
� 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Introduction
Perhaps only one simple and straightforward claim can be made
about the accuracy of children’s
testimony: not all statements made by children are true.
Admittedly, exact accuracy is not the usual
goal of memory in everyday life. Most autobiographical
remembering is carried out for social pur-
poses, such as to build bonds and foster connectedness with
friends and family (see e.g., Nelson,
1993), and can serve these functions even when recollections do
not precisely represent the past. In
c. All rights reserved.
f Psychology, Ursinus College, PO Box 1000, Collegeville, PA
19426-1000,
ncipe).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02732297
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/dr
206 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
fact, many everyday situations encourage some degree of
unfaithfulness. Exaggerated, improvised, or
even fabricated stories can be more engaging or more amusing
to conversational partners than verid-
ical reports. These tendencies to embellish personal experiences
may be especially pronounced at
young ages given children’s proclivity for pretense and adults’
willingness to play along. To illustrate,
only young children can get away with fantastic stories of a
fairy who gives prizes for baby teeth or a
monster that lives under the bed (see e.g., Principe & Smith,
2007).
Against this backdrop of memory in everyday life, the
courtroom is a rather unusual setting for
children’s remembering. In the real world, accounts of personal
experiences are successful to the ex-
tent that they are relayed in a compelling or affecting manner.
In the legal system, precise accuracy
is the goal. Remembering is successful to the degree that
witnesses ‘‘tell the truth, the whole truth,
and nothing but the truth.’’ Forensic settings, therefore, put
unique demands on memory that are at
odds with the way that recollections of the past typically are
used. This contrast notwithstanding,
because many criminal offenses that bring children to court,
such as sexual abuse and other forms
of molestation, lack other witnesses or corroborating evidence,
children’s testimony often serves
as the sole piece of evidence against criminal defendants.
Likewise, children’s memories impact
many civil and family court cases. For example, children’s
accounts of parental transgressions, such
as domestic violence and substance abuse, as well as more
mundane events, such as daily home rou-
tines, commonly play a role in custody, support, and visitation
decisions. Considering the centrality
of children’s testimony in many legal situations, research on
factors that can compromise children’s
abilities to provide accurate accounts of the past has
considerable relevance to forensic professionals
and fact finders.
Given that children’s testimony is elicited in interviews, many
investigators have focused on the
mnemonic effects of various suggestive features of interviews.
This voluminous literature has revealed
that a range of factors, such as types of questions asked, the
sorts of ancillary aids used, and the char-
acteristics of interviewers, can seriously derail children’s
accuracy and even lead to entirely false ac-
counts (see Bruck, Ceci, & Hembrooke, 2002, for a review).
Despite the significance of this work for
developing effective interviewing protocols, researchers have
become increasingly concerned with
examining suggestive factors outside of the formal interview
context that also can contaminate mem-
ory. This move to exploring extra-interview factors has been
prompted by findings that even when
children are interviewed under optimally nonsuggestive
conditions, some nonetheless relay fabricated
stories in line with suggestions encountered from other sources,
such as parents (Poole & Lindsay,
2002) and television (Principe, Ornstein, Baker-ward, &
Gordon, 2000).
In everyday life, one common way to encounter suggestions is
during memory sharing conversa-
tions with others. A compelling reason for focusing on
conversational forms of suggestion concerns
the social nature of autobiographical memory. Sharing
memories through conversations with friends
and family members is a typical and frequent part of children’s
everyday social interactions. During
such exchanges, however, children constantly are encountering
others’ versions of the past. Different
versions can arise unwittingly when conversational partners
misremember what happened, but also
can occur when they purposefully exaggerate or even fabricate
details to tell, say, a more glamorous
story than give a precisely accurate account. Given that memory
is constructive (Bartlett, 1932), it is
within this realm that bits and pieces of the suggestions and
stories told by others may find their way
into children’s recollections of their experiences.
Emphasizing the social nature of remembering are theories of
collective memory (e.g., Hirst & Man-
ier, 2008; Reese & Fivush, 2008) that characterize memories of
shared experiences as dynamic repre-
sentations that are shaped by group conversational processes. In
this framework, as memories of the
past are reconstructed within a group, its members negotiate a
collective version of experience. Con-
sequently, individual representations are revised to become
progressively alike among group mem-
bers (see Harris, Paterson, & Kemp, 2008). However, when
misinformation is introduced into group
remembering, either deliberately by a confederate (Meade &
Roediger, 2002) or unknowingly by a
group member who experienced a slightly different version of
the event (Gabbert, Memon, & Allan,
2003), individuals are prone to later recall occurrences that
were nonexperienced but merely sug-
gested by their conversational partners.
The practical importance of studies of conversational sharing
for discussions of children’s testi-
mony comes from real world examples demonstrating that
witnesses often talk with one another.
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 207
Consider, for instance, Paterson and Kemp’s (2006a) finding
that over 80% of witnesses to a crime or
serious accident reported discussing the event with another
witness, or other work showing that it is
not uncommon for multiple witnesses to be questioned at the
same time (e.g., Garven, Wood, Malpass,
& Shaw, 1998). Issues of conversational contamination are
particularly relevant to situations involving
multiple abuse victims, as children caught up in these cases may
attend group therapy sessions or
community meetings where allegations are shared (Rabinowitz,
2004).
There are also reasons to suspect that young children may be
especially vulnerable to the contri-
butions of others in their constructions of experience. First,
young children’s difficulty keeping track
of the source of their memories (e.g., Poole & Lindsay, 2002)
may put them at increased risk for mis-
takenly attributing events relayed by others as their own actual
experiences. Second, young children
are somewhat dependant on others to help them figure out how
to represent and recount their expe-
riences. Such collaboration benefits children’s construals of
novel events and narrations of existing
memories (Nelson, 1993), but it might also lead to problematic
distortions in memory when others
incorrectly frame legally relevant events. Consider, for
instance, a father who frames sexual abuse
as a special game or a mother in a custody dispute who says,
‘‘Daddy hurts you when he gives you
a bath, doesn’t he?’’ Third, younger preschoolers do not yet
realize that others can have memories that
are false; rather they believe that the mind literally copies
experience and that everyone therefore has
only true memories (e.g., Perner, 1991). This tendency usually
is not problematic in the real world but
it can be in legal situations. To illustrate, when a child hears
from a friend that she saw Santa put pre-
sents under the Christmas tree or that their teacher Mr. Bob
does bad things, both claims are unques-
tionably believed. Finally, young children rarely receive
feedback on what a false memory feels like.
Adults do, for example, when they remember parking their car
on the second level of the garage
but find it on the first. Children, in contrast, get away with all
sorts of memory errors, such as claiming
to have spent the afternoon with an invisible friend.
As this brief analysis indicates, examination of the sorts of
conversational activities that might be
linked to later errors in remembering is central to an
understanding of children’s ability to provide tes-
timony in legal settings. However, because the extant literature
on memory errors focuses almost
exclusively on the effects of suggestive questioning or other
forms of scripted misinformation, we
know very little about the ways that memory may be
transformed in the normal course of discussing
the past with others. With these theoretical and applied issues in
mind, in the remainder of this article,
we offer an overview of a programmatic series of studies
carried out by our research group concerning
how knowledge gained from and within conversations with
agemates and adults can shape children’s
constructions of the present and reconstructions of the past.
Co-witness influence
Our exploration of mnemonic effects of memory sharing began
in the context of a study on the
influence of naturally occurring interactions with peer witnesses
on children’s memory for a personal
experience (Principe & Ceci, 2002). Given extant
demonstrations of the potency of collective remem-
bering in shaping individual memories in the direction of the
group, co-witness discussions might, at
least at times, cause children within a peer group to construct a
collaborative story that does not
veridically reflect the independent experiences of each group
member. This issue may be especially
important for legal cases involving multiple purported victims
because fact finders may rely on the
number and similarity of allegations to determine the credibility
of any single child’s testimony. It
is likely very compelling to hear child after child tell the same
story, especially if one believes that each
witness has arrived at the same storyline independently. In some
situations, however, the exact oppo-
site might be the case. The story may have been arrived at in a
collaborative manner among peers who
initially had very different representations of the event.
There are numerous real world examples that discussions among
co-witnesses can influence chil-
dren’s testimony. Consider, for instance, the following
exchange between a young witness and a foren-
sic investigator in the Wee Care Nursery School case in
Maplewood, New Jersey. In this case, Kelly
Michaels, a teacher at the school, was accused of sexually
molesting her students. Here a child reveals
that the source of her allegation was another child rather than
her own observation:
208 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
Interviewer: Do you know what [Kelly] did?
Child: She wasn’t supposed to touch somebody’s body. If you
want to touch somebody, touch your
own.
Interviewer: How do you know about her touching private
parts? Is that something you saw or
heard?
Child: Max told me.
(Ceci & Bruck, 1995, p. 150)
To explore co-witness contamination, three groups of 3- to 5-
year-olds participated in a staged
event at their preschools, namely an archaeology dig with a
confederate archeologist named Dr. Diggs.
Children used plastic hammers to dig pretend artifacts, such as
dinosaur bones, gold coins, and jewels,
out of specially constructed blocks of mortar mix and play sand.
Each dig included two ‘‘target’’ arti-
facts: a bottle with a map to a buried treasure and a rock with a
message written in a secret language.
One third of the children, those in the Witness condition, saw
Dr. Diggs ruin the target artifacts (here-
tofore referred to as target activities). He ‘‘accidentally’’
spilled coffee on the map, smearing the ink
and rendering the map illegible. He appeared upset and said, ‘‘I
messed up the map! Now I’ll never
find the buried treasure!’’ Dr. Diggs also dropped the rock,
shattering it into pieces, and said, ‘‘I’ve bro-
ken the rock! Now I’ll never know what the secret message
says!’’ A second third of the children, those
in the Classmate condition, did not witness the target activities
during the dig but were the classmates
of those in the Witness group. We expected that some of these
children would hear about the ruined
map and broken rock through natural conversations with their
classmates who saw these activities.
The remaining children in the Control condition were drawn
from different preschools than the Wit-
ness and Classmate children. These children did not have any
opportunities to interact with those who
saw the target activities nor did they see these activities
themselves. This group provided the likeli-
hood that target activities would be reported by a random
nonwitness without exposure to any peer
witnesses. Teachers were discouraged from initiating or
participating in conversations with children
about the dig.
Given evidence of the use of suggestive techniques in forensic
settings, we also examined whether
suggestive interviews might augment the influence of co-
witnesses. Following the dig, all children
were questioned on three occasions spread out over a 3-week
interval. Half of the children received
neutral interviews, whereas the remaining half were questioned
in a suggestive manner. Embedded
in the suggestive interviews were strongly worded leading
questions that implied that the target
activities had occurred. Thus these questions were in line with
what the Witnesses children had seen,
but inconsistent with the experience of the Classmate and
Control children.
Four weeks after the dig, a new interviewer questioned all of
the children in a neutral manner and
asked them to recall ‘‘only things that you remember happening
to you—things that you really did or
remember seeing with your own eyes.’’ The hierarchically
ordered interview began with an open-
ended prompt: ‘‘Tell me what happened when Dr. Diggs visited
your school.’’ After exhausting
open-ended recall, specific questions were asked if one or both
of the target activities had not yet been
reported (e.g., ‘‘Did anything happen to a treasure map?’’). For
each target activity relayed, children
were asked to elaborate (‘‘Tell me more about that.’’) Children
who made reports of target activities
also were asked for the source of their memories, that is,
whether they actually saw the target activity
Table 1
Mean percentages target activities reported as actually
occurring at the final interview as a function of
experimental group and degree of prompting.
Open-ended Specific Total
Witness/neutral interview 34 47 81
Classmate/neutral interview 16 15 31
Control/neutral interview 0 0 0
Witness/suggestive interview 68 23 91
Classmate/suggestive interview 50 36 86
Control/suggestive interview 23 33 57
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 209
occur with their own eyes or merely heard about it from
someone (e.g., ‘‘Did you see Dr. Diggs spill his
drink on the treasure map with your own eyes, or hear that he
did it?’’).
Table 1 shows the proportion of target activities reported and
the degree of prompting needed to
elicit the information at the 4-week interview. As shown,
children in the Witness conditions evi-
denced quite good recall of these actually experienced
activities, with both groups reporting over
80%. However, the Classmate children, who did not witness the
ruined map or the broken rock, also
reported many of these activities. Under both interviewing
conditions, the Classmate children wrongly
reported more target activities than the Control children,
demonstrating that natural contact with
peer witnesses can induce false accounts in non-witnesses. In
fact, many errant accounts were at
the open-ended level of questioning, indicating that the effects
of peer witnesses are not limited to
cued reports can but can result in abundant spontaneous errors.
Perhaps the most interesting finding
to emerge from this study is that when the Classmate children
were exposed to the suggestive inter-
views, they reported as many target activities as those in the
Witness conditions who actually expe-
rienced these activities. Thus the combined effects of exposure
to peer witnesses and suggestive
interviewing among the non-witnesses resulted in levels of
recall that were indistinguishable in terms
of magnitude from those of the Witness children.
Several other findings also are of interest. First, among those
non-witnesses who reported target
activities, the Classmate children were more likely than the
Control children to report actually seeing
these activities occur with their own eyes (as opposed to merely
having heard about them). Such
claims of seeing suggest that natural conversations with co-
witnesses not only can induce false re-
ports but they also can lead to source confusions. Second, given
that fact finders often consider detail
as an index of testimonial accuracy (Ceci, Kulkofsky, Klemfuss,
Sweeney, & Bruck, 2007), we explored
the narratives accompanying the reports of target activities. As
expected, the Witness children gener-
ated relatively detailed accounts of these experienced events.
Many non-witnesses, however, also re-
layed elaborate reports of these occurrences they never saw but
merely heard about, with many
embellishing with details that went beyond the Witness
children’s experiences but nonetheless were
consistent with them. Consider, for example, a Classmate child
who said that after spilling coffee on
the map ‘‘Dr. Diggs walked away and then we just got in big
trouble. . . all my friends and he had to
be punished for a whole weekend. . .The ladies in the cafeteria
cleaned it because he didn’t have a
mop. . .They took him away and put him in jail.’’ In fact, the
Classmate children’s false accounts of
the target activities were more voluminous than the true
narratives of the Witness children. This pat-
tern demonstrates that narrative detail is not diagnostic of
accuracy when children have been exposed
to peer witnesses, and is consistent with other work showing
that false accounts induced by other
forms of suggestion can be more elaborate than true reports
(e.g., Bruck et al., 2002; Poole & Lindsay,
2002). Interestingly, examination of the content of children’s
narratives indicated that this group dif-
ference occurred neither because the Classmate children more
completely relayed their peers’ expe-
riences or the interview suggestions nor because they more
readily invented fantastic or
idiosyncratic embellishments, but because they generated more
original constructions consistent with
the notions of a ruined treasure map and a broken rock.
Rumor mongering and remembering
In our next study, we sought to extend our (Principe & Ceci,
2002) demonstration of the impact of con-
versational interactions by determining whether peer
interactions can influence children’s reports of an
experience even when none of them actually witnessed the event
in question. To do this, we planted a
false rumor about an experienced event among some members of
preschool classrooms and examined
the degree to which the rumored information leaked into their
own and their classmates’ recollections
when later interviewed. We also explored the degree to which
the interfering effects of the rumor might
be exacerbated when paired with suggestive interviews that are
consistent with the rumor.
We chose to study rumor transmission because a large literature
in social psychology demonstrates
that rumors often are generated about events that are meaningful
and upsetting where the truth is
unclear (see Rosnow, 1991). These conditions sound a lot like
those created by the sorts of offenses
that usually bring children to court, such as sexual abuse and
other forms of maltreatment, because
210 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
they are unsettling and typically lack corroborating witnesses or
physical evidence. In such cases, ru-
mors may emerge to fill in the gaps of missing information or to
impose an explanation on an unset-
tling allegation. Considering that individuals generally assume
that information exchanged during
everyday conversations is true (Gilbert, 1995), shared rumors
likely have much potential to prompt
revisions in memory in line with overheard information.
There also are real world examples of rumor contamination. One
comes from a case in which chil-
dren who were absent from school on the day of a sniper attack
recalled seeing things that only their
peers, who were present, could have experienced. A rumor
allegedly began to circulate that a second
sniper had eluded police and was on the loose. When the
children were asked to describe the attack
several months later, many described in detail how the second
sniper had escaped and still was loose
in the neighborhood (Pynoos & Nader, 1989).
To examine whether rumor can leak into memory, four groups
of 3- to 5-year-olds saw a scripted
magic show in their preschools (Principe, Kanaya, Ceci, &
Singh, 2006) in which a magician named Ma-
gic Mumfry tried to pull a live rabbit out of his top hat. After
several failed and frenzied efforts, Mumfy
apologized and left the school. Immediately after the show,
children in the Overheard group overheard
a scripted conversation between two adults in which one alleged
that the trick failed because Mum-
fry’s rabbit had gotten loose in the school rather than residing
in his hat. We maximized children’s
attention to the rumor by having them stand quietly in a line
awaiting a sticker during the planned
conversation. Children in the Classmate group did not overhear
the adult conversation about the es-
caped rabbit but were the classmates of the Overheard children.
Of interest was whether these chil-
dren would learn about the alleged lost rabbit through natural
interactions with their classmates who
heard the rumor and whether details in line with the rumor
might leak into their later recollections.
Control children had no exposure to the rumor; they were not
the classmates of those who overheard
the rumor, nor did they overhear it themselves. The remaining
children in the Witness group had no
exposure to the other three groups but experienced the event
suggested by the rumor, namely seeing
Mumfry’s rabbit loose in their school after the failed trick. One
week later, all children were ques-
tioned in either a neutral or suggestive manner. Embedded in
the suggestive interviews were coercive
questions that implied that the interviewee had witnessed
Mumfry’s escaped rabbit, when in fact only
those in the Witness group did.
Two weeks after the show, all children were questioned by a
new, neutral interviewer in the same
hierarchical manner as in Principe and Ceci’s (2002) study. As
shown in Table 2, all of the Witness chil-
dren correctly recalled that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten loose in
their school. Table 2 also illustrates
the powerful effects of the rumor on children’s accounts. All
but one of the Overheard and Classmate
children wrongly reported a loose rabbit. Thus these children
were as likely as those who actually saw
a live rabbit to report that Mumfry’s rabbit was loose, thereby
eliminating differences in levels of recall
between true and false accounts. This pattern not only shows
that information overheard from adults
can lead to near ceiling levels of false reports of
nonexperienced events, it also indicates that rumors
transmitted by peers can be as detrimental as those spread by
adults. Moreover, the majority of the
Overheard and Classmate children’s reports of the escaped
rabbit were in response to open-ended
probes, demonstrating that errant rumors can lead to high levels
of spontaneous fabrications. Further,
many Overheard and Classmate children claimed to have seen,
as opposed to heard about, the loose
Table 2
Percentages of children who reported the target activity as
actually occurring at the final interview as
a function of experimental group and degree of prompting.
Group Open-ended Specific Total
Witness/neutral interview 90 10 100
Overheard/neutral interview 86 10 95
Classmate/neutral interview 86 14 100
Control/neutral interview 0 10 10
Witness/suggestive interview 87 13 100
Overheard/suggestive interview 87 13 100
Classmate/suggestive interview 91 9 100
Control/suggestive interview 9 50 59
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 211
rabbit. These reports of seeing a nonoccuring event represent a
considerably more extreme demon-
stration of peer-generated suggestibility than our prior study
(Principe & Ceci, 2002) considering that
none of these children witnessed the event in question. Further
demonstrating the potency of rumor,
the non-witness children described the rumored loose rabbit
with much elaborative detail that went
above and beyond the literal rumor. To illustrate, a Classmate
child said that, ‘‘The rabbit was in the
playground, and then it was over the gate and, the rabbit was
over, the rabbit jumped, hopped over the
gate. . .I tried catching him with a bucket but he bited me on the
finger. . .They found him in the potty.’’
In fact, the Overheard and Classmate children’s descriptions of
the rumored-but-nonoccurring loose
rabbit were twice as voluminous as the accounts of the Witness
children, demonstrating that false
narratives engendered by rumor can be much more elaborate
than true narratives generated on the
basis of experience.
Considering the ease with which suggestive questions can
induce false reports (see Ceci, Kulkofsky
et al., 2007), it is worth noting that the loose rabbit
misinformation engendered higher levels of error
when planted via a rumor than when suggested during an
interview. Those children who heard the
rumor from an adult or peers gave more errant reports of the
nonevent, were more likely to wrongly
recall seeing (as opposed to hearing about) it, and embellished
their accounts with more elaborative
detail compared to those for whom the very same false
information was suggested during an inter-
view. This finding is particularly noteworthy given that the
Overheard children were not instructed
to share the rumor with their peers but ended up naturally
propagating this information to them in
a manner that was more mnemonically damaging than an
aggressively suggestive interview.
Conflicting rumors
The major finding of our initial study on rumors was that
overheard false information that provides
a reasonable explanation for an earlier ambiguous event can
lead children to mistakenly recall details
consistent with the rumor (Principe et al., 2006). This finding
prompted us to consider whether the
effects of rumor might be less powerful in situations where the
rumored information conflicts with
the past rather merely fills a gap. This contrast was of interest
because when rumors only fill a gap,
overheard details can be imported into memory without
displacing or overwriting any experienced
details. But when rumors conflict with the past, there is a
contradiction between what was experi-
enced and what was overheard and children must resolve it.
One important factor that may moderate children’s resistance to
rumors that contradict with their
experiences is their ability to reason about conflicting mental
representations. This notion comes from
researchers in the theory of mind tradition who propose that
there is a major transition in children’s
understanding of representational processes between 3 and 6
years of age (e.g., Perner, 1991). Younger
preschoolers possess a copy theory of mind and believe that the
mind exactly represents the world
and that consequently everyone has the same true beliefs about
it. In contrast, older preschoolers de-
velop an interpretive theory of mind and recognize that
representation is a subjective process shaped
by experience as well as beliefs, expectations, goals, and so on.
Thus they realize that different people
can have different, even contradictory, representations of the
same experience.
With these issues in mind, 3- to 6-year-olds participated in our
usual magic show (Principe, Tingu-
ely, & Dobkowski, 2007). At the end of the show, one third of
the children, those in the Conflicting Rep-
resentation condition, experienced an extra activity that
provided a plausible explanation for the
failed trick but that conflicted with the rumor that they would
later overhear. Mumfry uncovered a
previously unseen cage that held a live rabbit. He explained that
he had found his missing rabbit
and that it was sick because when it is sick it will not leave its
cage, not even to do its favorite hat
trick. To encourage belief in this explanation, Mumfry got out
his vet kit, checked the rabbit with a
stethoscope, and gave it some ‘‘medicine.’’ Then he and the
rabbit left the school. Replicating our ori-
ginal procedure, these children then overheard an adult allege
that Mumfry’s trick failed because the
rabbit had gotten loose in the school. Importantly, the rumor
created a conflicting representation for
the failed trick for these children given their prior exposure to
the sick rabbit. A second third of the
children, those in the Nonconflicting Representation group,
overheard the same rumor conversation
but did not experience the extra sick rabbit activity. Thus for
these children the rumor provided a
212 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
plausible explanation for the failed trick. The remaining Control
children did not overhear the rumor
or interact with the children in the other two groups. One week
later, all children were questioned
using our standard interview protocol.
Replicating our earlier findings, nearly all of the children for
whom the rumor did not conflict with
their experiences reported that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten
loose in the school. Interestingly, there
were no effects of age in this group, suggesting that increased
resistance to gap-filling rumors may
not develop during the preschool years. However, the 5- and 6-
year-olds were better able than the
3- and 4-year-olds to resist the rumor when it conflicted with
their experiences than when it merely
filled a gap, whereas the 3- and 4-year-olds were equally likely
to be misled by both types of rumors.
All but one of the Conflicting Representation young children
reported a loose rabbit, whereas less than
half of the older children did. These findings qualify our earlier
work by revealing that younger pre-
schoolers may be more vulnerable than older preschoolers to
conflicting rumors.
Examination of children’s false narratives describing the
rumored loose rabbit suggests that their
developing ability to deal with conflicting representations
played some role in this age trend. The
younger Conflicting Representation children based the majority
of their false accounts on only one
representation of the failed trick—the nonoccurring rumor. The
older children in this condition, in con-
trast, imported many details consistent with the sick rabbit
activity they actually saw, suggesting that
they had more ready access to both representations and
consequently drew from both in constructing
their accounts. We also found that poorer performance on a
separate series of tasks that index conflict-
ing representation understanding was associated with increased
proneness to report the rumored tar-
get activity and heightened levels of descriptive detail
consistent with the rumor. This relation
remained significant after controlling for age, supporting the
notion that the development of a concep-
tual understanding of the mind, particularly the ability to reason
about conflicting mental represen-
tations, may be important for resisting suggestions that
contradict with children’s experiences. It is
worth noting that the distinction between conflicting and
nonconflicting misinformation generally
is not made in the suggestibility literature, but these findings
suggest that this may be an important
contrast useful for explaining age trends.
Child-generated suggestibility
These findings demonstrating the potency of rumors planted by
adults prompted us to consider the
mnemonic effects of rumors generated by children themselves.
This issue was of interest because of a
growing attention in the memory literature to autosuggestibility
(Brainerd & Reyna, 1995), or errors in
memory that emanate from internal, constructive processes in
which an individual’s own beliefs,
expectations, goals, and so on, distort their recollections. For
instance, autosuggestion errors can occur
when people make causal inferences during an event and later
mistake their inferences for memories
of the actual experience. To illustrate, when shown a slide
sequence of a familiar event (e.g., grocery
shopping) that shows an effect (e.g., oranges on the supermarket
floor) but not its cause, many people
wrongly report seeing the most probable cause of the observed
effect (e.g., a woman pulling an orange
from the bottom of the stack). Hannigan and Reinitz (2001)
attribute this tendency to a reality mon-
itoring error that occurs when people misattribute their memory
of an internally generated inference
as arising from an externally experienced event. Given that
children as young 3 make inferences about
the causes of observed events (e.g., Sophian & Huber, 1984),
we wondered whether they may be vul-
nerable to causal inference errors about experienced events.
In this study, 3- to 6-year-olds watched a modified version of
our usual magic show in which Mum-
fry failed at two tricks: pulling a rabbit out of his hat and
producing a baked cake from a cake pan
(Principe, Guiliano, & Root, 2008). For the cake trick, Mumfry
put several real ingredients into cake
pan, covered it, and promised a fully baked cake. However,
when he opened the lid, the pan was
empty. To prompt the generation of rumors, one third of the
children, those in the Clue condition,
were exposed to two sets of clues: carrot ends with ‘‘teeth
marks’’ and a plate with cake crumbs
and a dirty fork. The carrot and cake ‘‘clues’’ were expected to
induce two inferences (heretofore re-
ferred to as target activities): one about the cause of the failed
hat trick (i.e., the rabbit got loose in
the school) and one about the cause of the vanished cake (i.e.,
someone ate the cake). To investigate
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 213
whether these children would naturally propagate their
inferences about the escaped rabbit and the
eaten cake to peers, the children in the Classmate condition
were the classmates of those in the Clue
group, but were not exposed to the clues. The remaining
children in the Control condition were not the
classmates of those who saw the clues, nor did they see the
clues themselves.
The results indicated that the clue manipulations prompted the
generation of rumors (or explana-
tions) about what happened to the missing rabbit and cake that
made their way into children’s later
memory reports. When interviewed one week after the show,
nearly 80% of the Clue children reported
that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten loose or that someone had eaten
the cake. Forty percent of these errors
were at the open-ended level of questioning. Interestingly, the
clues were more mnemonically damag-
ing to the 5- and 6-year-olds than the 3- and 4-year-olds. Not
only were the older children more likely
than the younger children to mistakenly report their inferences
during free recall, their reports of non-
experienced-but-inferred occurrences were embellished with
double the amount of detail. This pattern
might seem counterintuitive because younger children typically
are more suggestible than older chil-
dren. But given that causal inferencing ability develops rapidly
during the preschool years (e.g., Sophian
& Huber, 1984), a reverse developmental trend in interference
errors makes sense on empirical
grounds. Assuming the older children more readily generated
relevant inferences following the clues,
they likely had created for themselves more opportunities than
the younger children to make causal
inference errors. Supporting this explanation is that compared to
the younger children, the older chil-
dren’s false accounts contained more logical inferences based
on the clues and they more often elabo-
rated on their inferences in a rational manner. The younger
children, in contrast, were more likely to
recount illogical or even impossible details about the target
activities. Considering that fact finders tend
to view detail as diagnostic of accuracy, these findings have
some applied relevance as they suggest that
when condition are ripe for causal inferences, older children
may be more prone to construct false re-
ports that are more compelling or believable than those
produced by younger children.
Examination of the Classmate children’s performance indicated
that the Clue children readily prop-
agated their inferences to peers. Nearly 40% of the Classmate
children reported one or both of the tar-
get activities, and the majority of these claims occurred during
free recall. Interesting, the spread of
inferred information was particularly potent among the 3- and
4-year-olds, as they were more likely
than the older children to report actually seeing events that their
peers merely inferred. These data
provide original evidence that rumors generated by children
themselves, rather than those planted
by adults, can intrude into the recollections of peers. This is an
important extension of our earlier work
because although much is known about how adults can induce
errors in children’s memory, little is
understood about how children themselves can affect other
children’s recall. It is noteworthy, how-
ever, that the Clue children made more false reports than the
Classmate children, demonstrating that
the children generally were more influenced by their own
conclusions for the failed tricks than their
peers’. This rationale is in line with findings in the source
monitoring literature that internally gener-
ated events are more easily confused with one’s own
experiences than are externally suggested events
(e.g., Lindsay, Johnson, & Kwon, 1991).
The more frequent errors among the Clue relative to the
Classmate group notwithstanding, higher
levels of details accompanied the Classmate children’s reports
of the target activities. This finding sug-
gests that children may be especially likely to embellish
information picked up from peers—a trend
that we found in our original Mumfry study (Principe et al.,
2006) and in follow-up work reported be-
low. Examination of the content of children’s narratives made it
clear that the rise in elaboration
among the Classmate children was driven by an increase in
statements consistent with the notions
of a loose rabbit and eaten cake but beyond inferences that
could be derived directly from the clues.
Perhaps because the Classmate children did not see the clues
themselves but based their ideas on their
peers’ stories about the causes of the failed tricks, they were
less limited than the Clue children to
describing the simple inferences implied by the clues.
Social processes
In our three rumor studies described so far, levels of errant
reports are higher and the accompany-
ing false narratives more voluminous than are typical of
suggestibility research in which children are
214 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
exposed to misinformation during interviews (e.g., Bruck et al.,
2002) or in other private contexts (e.g.,
Leichtman & Ceci, 1995; Poole & Lindsay, 2002). Of interest
therefore is what gives rumor its potency?
One clue comes from studies in the adult social influence
literature that demonstrate the very same
misinformation more easily intrudes into memory if it is
encountered in a social context via a confed-
erate than if it is picked up in a nonsocial manner, such as
through written suggestions or leading
questions (Paterson & Kemp, 2006b), with the magnitude of
social conformity building as exposure
to others’ erroneous responses increases (Virj, Pannell, & Ost,
2005). Given that children in our rumor
studies interact freely with their entire class after the rumor is
planted, it is possible that this social
experience, rather than the rumor itself, drives the exceptionally
high levels of false reports and ficti-
tious elaboration. The purpose of our next study, therefore, was
to examine whether opportunities for
natural discussions with others following exposure to a false
rumor can exacerbate its effects on later
recall.
To investigate this question, 3- to 5-year-olds watched our
Mumfry show and then overheard the
loose rabbit rumor (Principe, Daley, & Kauth, 2010).
Immediately after overhearing the rumor, some
of the children were given 30 min of free play time to interact
naturally with one another. Other chil-
dren were engaged in a structured ‘‘circle time’’ activity for 30
min that prohibited them from talking
about the rumor. After these 30 min, all children went home for
the day and we took several mea-
sures to ensure that none of the children had any further
interactions with their schoolmates that
day.
When interviewed 1 week later, those children who heard the
rumor and then interacted freely
with peers made more false reports of the rumored occurrence,
were more likely to admit to seeing
this nonevent, and described this nonoccurrence in more detail
than those who did not have the
opportunity to naturally converse with their peers immediately
after the rumor. These findings dem-
onstrate that the infusion of misinformation into a group has
more powerful effects on memory than if
the very same information is encountered individually without
the opportunity for immediate collab-
orative reflection. Further, analysis of the content of children’s
narratives revealed that more than one-
third of the details reported by those who interacted naturally
following the rumor overlapped with
something that someone else in their classroom had uttered,
whereas those who were denied an
opportunity to interact evidenced only a 9% overlap in their
reports (most of which were mere verba-
tim repetitions of the rumor). This group difference in shared
utterances clearly indicates that the nat-
ural conversations that occurred in the classrooms immediately
following the provision the rumor had
a powerful effect in shaping children’s reports one week later.
The ease of contamination brought about by socially provided
misinformation notwithstanding, a
second social factor that might impact the transmission and
mnemonic effects of rumored information
within a group is the group’s prior history. Supporting this
notion are findings in the classical social
influence literature demonstrating that the magnitude of
conformity escalates with group cohesive-
ness (Wren, 1999). Further, experimental work in the rumor
literature demonstrates that the trans-
mission of unfounded information within a group increases with
the level of familiarity between
participants, or a desire to establish such a level (Rosnow,
1991).
To examine whether familiarity might exacerbate the interfering
effects of errant rumor on mem-
ory, 3- to 5-year-olds were assigned to one of two conditions
that differed in terms of their social
history (Principe et al., 2010). Half of the children had been
classmates for at least 6 months, whereas
the remaining half were complete strangers. We carried out the
magic shows in an unfamiliar loca-
tion to ensure that the group’s social history rather than the
familiarity of the setting drove any
group differences in memory. Once the rumor was planted, all
of the children engaged in free play
for 30 min.
As expected, the rumor induced greater memory contamination
if it was planted among familiar
peers than if it was encountered among strangers. Those
children who interacted freely with their pre-
school classmates made more false reports of the rumored
occurrence, were more likely to claim to
have seen this nonevent, and provided more elaborate narratives
compared to those children who
interacted with unfamiliar peers following the rumor. The
familiar children also displayed greater
overlap in the content of their errant accounts compared to
unfamiliar children, demonstrating that
a group’s social history can augment the degree to which
narrative details invented during natural
interactions later become infused into children’s individual
reports.
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 215
Representational changes
Given our findings that overhearing an errant rumor—either
from an adult or classmates—can lead
children to make detailed false reports, we next sought to
explore the extent to which such accounts
are driven by changes in children’s memory representations that
lead to a genuine belief that the ru-
mored information was actually seen. The alternative is that
children’s errant reports following rumor
are driven merely by social demands to relay information they
knew they had not experienced but
was only suggested. This theoretically significant distinction is
also important for discussions of tes-
timony. If rumor can bring about errant beliefs about witnessing
events that only were overheard,
then there may be little that forensic interviewers can do to
mitigate rumors’ effects. But if reports
of rumored occurrences are merely driven by compliance to
social pressures, such effects may be re-
duced by protocols that boost the retrieval and reporting of
information in memory, such as the Re-
vised Cognitive Interview or the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development
Investigative Interview (see e.g., Lamb, Hershkowitz, Orbach,
& Esplin, 2008).
To examine this issue, we used a warning manipulation that
minimized social pressures to report
an overheard rumor about an experienced event and we
examined the qualitative characteristics of
children’s false narratives prompted by the rumor (Principe,
Haines, Adkins, & Guiliano, 2010). Three-
to 6-year-olds watched a magic show in their schools and we
replicated the Overheard, Classmate, and
Control procedures from our rumor original study. When
interviewed one week later, half of the Over-
heard and Classmate children received a series of emphatic
warnings that eliminated social demands
to report the rumor by telling them that any information
overheard after the show was wrong and
therefore should not be reported. Thus if children were able to
discern the correct source of the rumor,
there were no social pressures to relay it.
Demonstrating that compliance to social pressures can lead to
false claims, the warning reduced
false reports in line with the rumor when it was planted by an
adult. However, when the rumor
was picked up from peers, the warning decreased false reports
among 5- and 6-year-olds, but not
3- and 4-year-olds. The warning, however, had no effect on
children’s claims of actually seeing the
loose rabbit. That is, when children who reported the loose
rabbit were probed for the source of their
memory, warned and nonwarned children were equally liked to
claim to have seen the loose rabbit
with their own eyes (as opposed to merely having heard about
it). This pattern suggests that social
demands produced at least some false reports of the rumored
occurrence, but that demand character-
istics had little or nothing to do with children’s claims of seeing
the rumor.
Considering this evidence that claims of seeing generally were
not due to social demands, to what
extent did they reflect a genuine belief in having seen the
rumored event? Analyses of children’s false
narratives provide some insight. Replicating findings of all of
our prior rumor studies, those children
who recalled seeing a loose rabbit provided more voluminous
false narratives consistent with the
theme of the rumor than those who did not admit to seeing it.
This pattern suggests that when mem-
ories for rumored events contain much elaborative detail,
children may be prone for mistaking them
for real experiences. However, analysis of the content of
children’s false narratives indicated that there
was more to the story. Those children who did and did not recall
seeing the rumored occurrence gen-
erated different narrative profiles. Specifically, those children
who reported seeing a loose rabbit de-
scribed this rumored event with relatively more perceptual (e.g.,
color and sound) and contextual (e.g.,
spatial location and temporal order) detail than either those who
were unable to determine a source of
their false reports or who claimed another source.
This distinction is important because according to Johnson and
colleagues’ source-monitoring
framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993), individuals
distinguish the source of information
in memory by evaluating certain characteristics of the
representation at retrieval. In this view, expe-
rienced events are represented with more perceptual, contextual,
semantic, and affective information
than imagined, suggested, or otherwise nonexperienced events,
and the differing profiles of these two
classes of memories serve as cues to discriminate their origins.
Thus given that perceptual and contex-
tual details are characteristic of experienced sources, it may be
that the generation of these qualities in
representations of the rumor interfered with the usual source-
judgment process and consequently led
some children to misattribute it as a witnessed event. This
interpretation presumes that rumored
events can come to be represented similarly to experienced
events and that such representational
216 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
changes put children at risk for wrongly judging heard about
events as seen. The point is that children
who made false claims of seeing might not have been engaging
in faulty reasoning about source, but
rather were dealing with a memory that was uncharacteristic of
its class. This interpretation is in line
with other work showing the usually successful source judgment
process can go awry when represen-
tations of nonexperienced events develop qualities typical of
real experiences (e.g., Blandon-Gitlin,
Pezdek, Lindsay, & Hagen, 2009).
The current results also suggest developmental improvements in
children’s vulnerability to make
false claims of seeing rumored events. As expected, 3- and 4-
year-olds were more likely than 5- and 6-
year-olds to recall seeing the rumor. Common explanations of
these sorts of heightened source errors
among younger children are their immaturities in theory-of-
mind understanding, representational
ability, and the strategy of using memory characteristics to
identify source (see e.g., Sluzenski, New-
combe, & Ottinger, 2004). We found, however, more abundant
perceptual detail in the false accounts
of the younger versus older children. This raises another
possibility, namely that young children may
be particularly prone to generate perceptual images in response
to postevent suggestions and subse-
quently infuse them into their representations of experience.
Indeed, such a tendency would constrain
the efficiency with which source-monitoring processes can
operate.
We also found that the Classmate children were more
suggestible than the Overheard children. In
particular, the Classmate children made more frequent reports
of seeing and gave more lengthy
descriptions of the rumored occurrence relative to the
Overheard children. Examination of false nar-
ratives indicated that the Classmate children reported
proportionately more perceptual and contex-
tual detail than the Overheard children. This pattern suggests
that what makes rumors picked up
from peers particularly potent is that this mode of transmission
can lead to an abundance of percep-
tual and contextual detail in memory that children are prone to
judge as indicative of an authentic
witnessed experience.
Consistent with current conceptualizations of memory (Ceci,
Papierno, & Kulkofsky, 2007), the re-
sults of this study highlight the notion that individual and
developmental differences in underlying
representations play an important role in children’s
suggestibility. These data also have implications
for children as witnesses. First, the effectiveness of the warning
in reducing false claims suggests that
legal professionals would be prudent to consider instructions to
young witnesses to ignore any heard
about information if it is known that a false rumor has been
circulating. Second, we found that even
though the warning reduced reports of rumored information, it
did not affect children’s ability to cor-
rectly report the actually experienced portions of the magic
show. This finding indicates that legal pro-
fessionals may not need to be concerned that warnings will
reduce false detail at the expense of true
information. We found, however, that the warning was
associated with an increase in the provision of
constructive details and a corresponding decrease in reports of
fantastic details about the rumored
occurrence, suggesting that false accounts of rumored-but-
nonexperienced events that persist follow-
ing a warning might appear particularly believable and
compelling.
Linking rumor mongering to later false reports
In our next study, we sought to investigate directly the content
of children’s post-rumor conversa-
tions to begin to make claims about which specific qualities of
these discussions are linked to later
errors in remembering (Principe, Cherson, DiPuppo, &
Schindewolf, in press). To do so, we replicated
our original magic show procedures and formed three groups:
Overheard, Classmate, and Control. To
document the transmission of rumored information among peers,
we recorded children’s natural con-
versations for 20 min following their exposure to the rumor.
Children wore a small belt pack with a
digital audio recorder and a microphone that attached to their
shirts. Children were interviewed at
both 1 and 4 weeks following the show. We added this 4-week
delay to explore the decay rate of over-
heard rumors.
Children’s performance at the 1-week interview replicated our
earlier results. Nearly all of the
Overheard and Classmate children, but none of the Control
children, reported that Mumfry’s rabbit
was loose. Likewise, many Overheard and Classmate children
recalled seeing the alleged loose rabbit
and many provided high levels of constructive embellishments
in line with the rumor, with the Class-
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 217
mate children being more likely to make claims of seeing and
generating more voluminous false ac-
counts. Reports of the loose rabbit and recollections of seeing
remained high across the 4-week delay.
Only the older Overheard children evidenced any decline in the
volume of their false narratives over
the delay. These patterns indicate that the memory alterations
engendered by false rumor do not dis-
sipate rapidly but rather reflect more lasting changes in
remembering.
Examination of children’ natural exchanges with peers in their
classrooms following their exposure
to the rumor revealed a remarkable amount of dialogue going on
among those who heard the rumor
directly from the adult as well as those who had picked it up
secondhand from their peers. In fact,
every child in both groups uttered at least one statement about
the alleged loose rabbit, demonstrat-
ing that every Overheard and Classmate child encoded the
rumor and was actively engaged, albeit in
varying degrees, in circulating the rumored information.
Analysis of the content of children’s natural talk in their
classrooms showed that they did not
merely stick to propagating the rumor verbatim to peers. Nor
was there much fantastic talk in the
post-rumor dialogues, suggesting that the children generally did
not interpret the rumor as an invita-
tion to engage in pretense. Most of the information transmitted
was made up of constructive utter-
ances in line with the theme of rumor but above and beyond its
literal content. Thus the children
were inventing and sharing new details that generally were
believable. Considering findings in the
adult literature that believability is necessary for rumors to be
spread readily and widely (see Rosnow,
1991), the constructive nature of the information generated and
circulated in the classrooms likely
boosted the influence of the rumor on children’s subsequent
remembering.
Next we explored the degree to which things said in the
interview originated in the classroom on
the day that the rumor was spread. At the 1-week interview,
20% of the details relayed about the ru-
mored loose rabbit overlapped with what they themselves had
uttered in the classroom. Thirty-two
percent of their narrative reports during the interview originated
in their classmates’ transmissions.
At the 4-week interview, 13% of children’s false reports
overlapped with their own transmissions in
the classroom, and 23% overlapped with their classmates
contributions. These findings provide direct
evidence that the very narrative details invented and circulated
among children following their expo-
sure to the rumor intruded into their own and their peers’
subsequent individual accounts of the event
out to a 4-week delay. Thus the children were not merely
fabricating constructions about the rumored
occurrence on the fly during the interview but also were
remembering a good deal of the transmis-
sions that had originated one or four weeks earlier in the natural
dialogues on the day the rumor
was planted. Further, these findings of the ready infusion of the
content of peer dialogues into later
memory provides some insight into why the effects of postevent
misinformation are exacerbated
when it is encountered in a group rather than individually
without the opportunity for co-witness
exchange.
Consistent with our earlier findings, the rumor was more
damaging to memory when it was
gleaned from agemates than when it was overheard from an
adult. At both interviews, the Classmate
children made more frequent reports of seeing the rumored
occurrence and they offered more lengthy
false narratives than the Overheard children. Further, among
those children who erroneously reported
the rumored event at both interviews, the Classmate children
evidenced no decline in narrative detail
across the 4-week delay. The older Overheard children,
however, displayed a drop in elaborative detail
from the 1-week to the 4-week interview.
In line with findings that the Overheard and Classmate children
remembered differently, compar-
ison of their post-rumor talk revealed that the Classmate
children also talked differently than the
Overheard children about the alleged occurrence on the day the
rumor was planted. Compared to
the Overheard children, the Classmate children uttered more
original transmissions. Both groups of
children improvised quite freely—overall 58% of children’s
utterances in the classroom were novel
(i.e., no other child in the classroom had yet uttered), but the
Classmate children relayed more than
twice as many original transmissions as their Overheard peers,
demonstrating that they engaged in
much more inventive rumor mongering than their agemates.
Further, the Classmate children also
were more affected than the Overheard children by what went
on in the classrooms on the day the
rumor was planted. At both the 1- and 4-week interviews, the
Classmate children evidenced a greater
overlap than the Overheard children between things they
themselves had said as well as things their
classmates had uttered on the day the rumor was planted and
their subsequent interview reports.
218 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
Likewise, at both interviews, the Overhead children relayed
higher levels of nonoverlapping informa-
tion, suggesting that they were more likely than the Classmate
children to fabricate their interview
reports on the fly.
These findings of heightened peer-suggestibility are at odds
with the typical finding in the suggest-
ibility literature that children are more easily misled by adults
than peers (e.g., Ceci, Toglia, & Ross,
1990). An important contrast between the usual suggestibility
study and the current work concerns
the differing manners by which suggestions were delivered by
peers. In typical suggestibility research,
children make scripted suggestions during formal interviews,
whereas in our paradigm, familiar chil-
dren transmitted freely varying suggestions—without being
instructed to do so—during the course of
naturally occurring interactions. This contrast suggests that the
naturalness of the reception context
might boost the impact of misinformation. Consistent with this
interpretation are findings in the ru-
mor literature that rumors are more readily spread when picked
up from a peer than an authority fig-
ure (Jaeger, Anthony, & Rosnow, 1980), with higher degrees of
propagation within an individual’s own
social group (Almirol, 1981). Also supporting this explanation
is the finding that the Overheard chil-
dren generally did not constrain themselves to stay true to the
information transmitted by a presumed
authority figure; rather their propagations in the classrooms
were highly inventive. Thus, it may be
the improvised nature of the Overheard children’s original
transmissions and the Classmate children’s
subsequent tendencies to do more constructive spinning of the
rumor that made peer suggestions par-
ticularly powerful.
In line with prior findings, the older children provided more
errant narrative detail at both inter-
views than the younger children. We also found that the 5- and
6-year-olds were the master rumor
propagators in the classroom. Compared to the 3- and 4-year-
olds, they generated more constructive
utterances and improvised more original transmissions about the
rumored occurrence. This deeper
and more inventive participation in rumor exchanges with their
peers suggests that the older children
simply had created for themselves more opportunities than the
younger children to be influenced by
what went on in the classrooms. Supporting this notion is that
the older children provided more inter-
view details that overlapped with their own utterances (1-week
interview) and their classmates’
utterances (1- and 4-week interviews) in the classroom than the
younger children. Given that fact
finders tend to judge detail as an index of accuracy, these
findings suggest that when conditions
are ripe for rumor mongering, older children may be more prone
to construct false narratives that
are more compelling or believable than those produced by
younger children.
Maternal suggestibility and memory sharing style
These findings demonstrating the mnemonic effects of peer
discussions following misinformation
notwithstanding, little is known about how conversations with
parents who have been exposed to
misleading information might affect memory. It is well known
that young children rely heavily on
their parents to help them frame and guide their formulations of
experience. But what happens when
parents have preexisting beliefs about what has happened to
their children? Both case study (see Ceci
& Bruck, 1995) and empirical investigations (White, Leichtman,
& Ceci, 1997) show that when inter-
viewers believe unfounded allegations to be true, they are prone
to shape their interviews with chil-
dren to elicit untrue statements consistent with their extant
beliefs and consequently put children at
increase risk of making false claims in line the unfounded
beliefs. Little is known, however, about
whether parents’ false beliefs might similarly drive children’s
accounts.
Understanding the influence of parents’ beliefs also has
implications for discussions of the testi-
mony of young witnesses. Consider, for instance, the case of
Lillie and Reed v Newcastle City Council
& Ors (see Bruck, Ceci, & Principe, 2006) in which, on the
basis of a 2-year-olds’ uncorroborated alle-
gations, parents of children in a daycare center were told that a
child in the center may have been sex-
ually abused and then were asked to talk with their children
about the possible abuse. Similarly, in the
McMartin Preschool case in Manhattan Beach, California,
prompted by a mother’s allegation that her
son had been sexually molested by a school aid, Police Chief
Harry Kuhlmeyer sent a letter to nearly
200 mothers whose children attended the school (see Ceci &
Bruck, 1995). The letter urged mothers to
question their children and suggested events that might have
taken place: ‘‘Please question your child
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 219
to see if he or she has been a witness to any crime or if he or
she has been a victim. Our investigation
indicates that possible criminal acts include: oral sex, fondling
of genitals, buttock or chest area, and
sodomy.’’ Given that parents in both of these cases were told
that abuse was possible, some may have
believed in the defendants’ guilt and inadvertently molded their
memory discussions with their chil-
dren to elicit accounts in line with their beliefs.
To explore whether discussions with misinformed parents might
affect children’s later memory, we
had children watch our usual magic show and then gave their
mothers a letter that asked them to nat-
urally discuss the show with their children (Principe, DiPuppo,
& Gammel, 2011). In the letters of half
of these mothers, those in the Suggestive Letter condition, was
a suggestion that Mumfry’s rabbit may
or may not have gotten loose in the school on the day of the
magic show. The letters of mothers in the
Neutral Letter condition was identical except for the suggestion.
Both sets of mothers were asked to
hold their discussion on the morning of our interview which
took place 1-week after the show. We
gave mothers a digital recorder to record their conversations. A
third group, the Control condition, nei-
ther received the suggestion nor were they asked to talk about
the event with their children.
Results indicated that the letter was a potent form of
suggestion. During the interview, the Sugges-
tive Letter children were more likely to wrongly report a loose
rabbit and to claim to have seen this
rumored-but-nonconcurring event than the Neutral or Control
children, providing original evidence
that parents can be a natural source of report contamination
when they have been misinformed. Ear-
lier work has shown that parents can be a source of memory
error when they suggest experimenter-
provided scripted nonoccurrences (e.g., Poole & Lindsay, 2002),
but this is the first study to demon-
strate that misinformation encountered by parents can leak into
children’s later accounts when they
are not asked to suggest the nonevent to their children but
merely to talk with their children in a nat-
ural manner.
Admittedly, the current suggestibility manipulation was rather
weak compared to the more direct
and coercive forms used in most of the literature (e.g., Bruck et
al., 2002). Nonetheless, children’s error
rates during the interview in the current study are comparable to
those in research on suggestibility
that use more direct forms (60% of the Suggestive Letter
children claimed that the rabbit was loose,
compared to less than 10% in the other two groups). Not only
was a substantial proportion of chil-
dren’s false reports at the open-ended level of questioning
(20%), their errors also were accompanied
by a fair degree of elaborative detail in line with the theme of
the suggestion to their mothers.
Given the well-known distinction in style that mothers take on
when remembering with their chil-
dren, namely high versus low elaborative (see Nelson and
Fivush (2004) for a review), we next sought
to explore whether the influence of our misinformation
manipulation varied as a function of maternal
style. This issue was of interest because maternal style
influences how children represent and remem-
ber the past. High-elaborative mothers scaffold children’s
accounts by asking many –wh questions and
encourage extended narratives by providing more and more
memory information with each succeed-
ing question. Low-elaborative mothers in contrast, provide less
structure and ask their children few
and redundant questions about the past. Despite much evidence
that children of high-elaborative
mothers tend to relay the past in a more elaborated and coherent
manner than children whose moth-
ers use a low elaborative style, unknown is whether maternal
style might affect children’s remember-
ing when mothers have been exposed to misinformation. Given
the tendencies of high elaborative
mothers to provide new memory information to aid their
children’s recollections, when these mothers
hold false beliefs about their children’s experiences they may be
more likely than low elaborative
mothers to weave their own beliefs into the ongoing co-
constructed narrative and consequently more
greatly affect their children’s later accounts.
In line with the prediction that children of high-elaborative
mothers tend to display more sophis-
ticated autobiographical memory skills, we found that these
children, compared to those of low-elab-
orative mothers, provided more detailed narratives of the
actually experienced portions of the magic
show and were less likely to make errors when describing these
actual experiences. Despite these po-
sitive associations between maternal elaborativeness and
children’s recall of experienced events, we
found that children with high elaborative mothers were twice as
likely as those with low elaborative
mothers to wrongly report a loose rabbit. Likewise, of those
who made a false report, children whose
mothers displayed a high elaborative style described the
suggested nonevent in more narrative detail
than those whose mothers talked in a low elaborative fashion.
Thus, these results demonstrate that
220 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
the high elaborative style usually associated with more skilled
autobiographical remembering is also
linked with increases in children’s memory error when mothers
are exposed to misinformation about
their children’s experiences.
Interestingly, there were no differences in the qualities of
mothers’ conversations as a function of
experimental group, suggesting that misinformation exposure
did not affect maternal style. However,
examination of mothers’ contributions during the mother–child
conversation suggests that maternal
style affected how mothers used the misinformation in their
discussions with their children. In partic-
ular, high elaborative mothers provided more than three times
the elaborations consistent with the
theme of the suggestion than those who were low elaborative,
indicating that after exposure to the
misinformation the high elaborative mothers created a more
coercive memory sharing environment
than the low elaborative mothers. Importantly, these mothers
were not instructed to provide elabo-
rations consistent with the suggestion or offer new information
if their children were not remember-
ing a loose rabbit. Rather, they were merely instructed to ask
their children if the magician’s rabbit had
gotten loose. These findings suggest that mothers with a high
elaborative predisposition often went
above and beyond a simple ‘‘Was the rabbit loose?’’ question
and offered new and necessarily false
details describing the suggestion. Interestingly, there were no
differences in the extent to which
low- versus high-elaborative mothers believed the loose rabbit
suggestion to be true, suggesting that
maternal style may override any influence of belief on how
mothers question their children about the
past.
Conclusion and implications
This collection of studies demonstrates the importance of
considering the mnemonic consequences
of natural conversational interactions for discussions of
children’s memory and suggestibility. Our
findings show that not only can conversations with co-witnesses
be a potent source of errors in auto-
biographical remembering, but that interactions with others can
lead to false reports even when they
have not witnessed the event in question but merely have been
exposed to misinformation about it.
These investigations also demonstrate several conditions under
which the potential of natural conver-
sations to taint memory are exacerbated. Memory sharing
conversations with familiar peers as op-
posed to strangers more readily shape subsequent memory, and
discussions about the past with
high-elaborative mothers exhibit greater influence than those
with low-elaborative mothers. Also,
the nature of the shared false information can impact its
mnemonic effects. With age, children develop
some resistance to being misled by conversationally conveyed
misinformation when it conflicts with
their actual experiences, but they also become more likely to
generate their own causal inferences
about events that can put themselves as well as their peers at
increased risk of wrongly recollecting
occurrences that were merely inferred.
Researchers interested in children’s memory generally have
overlooked peers as a source of error.
Our findings of high levels of peer-generated suggestibility,
however, underscore the importance of a
continuing examination of peers. Importantly, none of the
children in our studies were asked to invent
or share information with their classmates. Rather they naturally
generated and spread misinforma-
tion in such a manner that it that intruded into their own and
their peers’ subsequent autobiograph-
ical accounts. In fact, on many measures of memory,
misinformation picked up from peers was more
mnemonically damaging than suggestions planted by adults.
Indeed, inspection of the content of chil-
dren’s false accounts suggests that what might make peer
transmitted misinformation particularly po-
tent is that it leads to the generation of perceptual and
contextual images in memory that children
wrongly judge as indicative of an authentic experience. Future
work is needed to more fully explore
this interpretation and the sorts of conversational exchanges
that can prompt the generation of high
levels of nonexperienced perceptual and contextual detail and
consequent reports of seeing. Nonethe-
less, our examination of children’s classroom conversations
suggests that these representational dif-
ferences as a function of informant might be linked to
differences in the ways that children talk
with others following the reception of misinformation from
peers versus an adult. Specifically, our
findings suggest that children who glean false information from
peers engage in deeper and more
inventive rumor mongering than those who overhear the very
same information from an adult. Final-
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 221
ly, our findings of elevated errors with familiar peers as
opposed to strangers show that social history
is also an important variable in understanding conversational
suggestibility. Consequently, an impor-
tant question for future work is how fast and over what course
does familiarity augment the potential
for memory contamination.
Admittedly, an exploration of conversational processes in
remembering has not been completely
off the radar screen for investigators exploring children’s
autobiographical memory. Developmental
researchers have long explored the effects of mothers’ memory
sharing style on children’s event nar-
ratives. Accuracy for a specific event, however, has rarely been
a dependent variable in this line of
work. Our findings not only show that mothers can be a potent
source of errors in remembering,
but that high maternal elaborativeness—a quality associated
with increases in children’s memory
and narrative skills—is linked with decreases in accuracy when
mothers have been exposed to misin-
formation about their children’s experiences. Given that this
link is correlational, an important next
step is to carry out experimental studies of maternal style to
bolster the causal argument. Also of inter-
est in future work is exploring in a more fine-grained manner
how the content of mother’s elaborative
statements and questions influence children’s independent
remembering. This sort of analysis would
deepen our understanding of how a high elaborative style can
both help and hinder children’s memory
when mothers and their children disagree on the facts of a past
event. Indeed, mother–child negoti-
ation is a critical context for children’s developing abilities to
represent and recollect their experi-
ences, but a closer look at such conversations may provide some
insight into how memory
exchanges teach young children that others can represent the
past differently and that memory is a
subjective, dynamic process rather than a static product.
Our work also holds some applied relevance to legal settings
involving young witnesses as it re-
veals powerful sources of memory error not readily eliminated
by common techniques used to min-
imize reporting errors in forensic interviews (e.g., exclusive
nonsuggestive questioning, videotaping
interviews). Caution, of course, is required in generalizing our
findings to situations involving child
witnesses (e.g., our to-be-remembered events are enjoyable,
children are not pressured to make false
reports, interviewers are not rewarded for eliciting certain
claims). Nonetheless, our data provides
clear evidence that even when children are interviewed in a
supportive and neutral manner, misinfor-
mation from peers and parents can contaminate their reports. In
fact, because the level of peer influ-
ence in our experimental paradigms is likely pale compared to
that which occurs in group therapy
sessions or joint police interviews, our findings underscore the
importance of considering the poten-
tial contaminating influence of peers in legal cases involving
multiple child witnesses. Likewise, our
findings suggest that exposing mothers to unfounded claims,
such as Police Chief Harry Kuhlmeyer
did in the McMartin case described above, might engender false
allegations, especially if mothers talk
with their children in a high elaborative manner. As such,
experimental studies that suggest nonoccur-
ring experiences to mothers may be relevant for forensic
situations where unfounded allegations
emerge and families are asked to play a role in diagnosing the
truthfulness of such allegations in their
own children.
Further, the current body of work demonstrates that the
tendencies of judges and juries to use cer-
tain qualities of children’s testimony, such as corroboration,
spontaneity, elaborativeness, and consis-
tency, as markers of accuracy are unwarranted. For instance, the
near ceiling levels of reports of
rumored-but-nonoccurring events in our studies show that
corroboration can occur even when none
of the witnesses are accurate. Second, children’s spontaneous
and elaboratively detailed accounts of
nonevents following rumor provide compelling evidence that
these qualities of children’s accounts
are not reliable indexes for gauging accuracy. Third, the high
levels of consistency in reports of the ru-
mored event across a 4-week delay make it clear that
consistency across interviews is not diagnostic
of truthfulness.
In summary, this collection of findings provides unique insight
into why memory sharing conver-
sations can be such a potent source of children’s report
contamination. Not only do children and par-
ents naturally share misinformation in conversations about the
past, but they also improvise
embellishments that go above and beyond the literal
suggestions—a tendency that seems to put them-
selves and their conversational partners at risk for
representational changes and consequent memory
errors. Further, certain qualities of the informer, such as age,
familiarity, and conversational style, can
affect the nature of misinformation delivery in such a manner
that it incites the receiver in to engage
222 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223
in a more or less co-constructive spinning of the past that has
implications for later accuracy. Taken
together, these patterns show that it is not misinformation per se
that affects children’s autobiograph-
ical remembering, but rather how it is encountered and shared
with others.
Acknowledgment
This research was supported by Grants MH12619 and
MH076811 from the National Institutes of
Health.
References
Almirol, E. B. (1981). Chasing the elusive butterfly: Gossip and
the pursuit of reputation. Ethnicity, 8, 293–304.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental
and social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press.
Blandon-Gitlin, I., Pezdek, K., Lindsay, D. S., & Hagen, L.
(2009). Criteria-based content analysis of true and suggested
accounts of
events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 901–917.
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1995). Autosuggestibility in
memory development. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 65–101.
Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (2002). The nature of
children’s true and false narratives. Developmental Review, 22,
520–554.
Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Principe, G. F. (2006). The child and
the law. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.), & K. A.
Renniger & I.
E. Sigel (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4).
Child psychology in practice (6th ed., pp. 776–816). New York:
Wiley.
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A
scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC:
American
Psychological Association.
Ceci, S. J., Kulkofsky, S., Klemfuss, J. Z., Sweeney, C. D., &
Bruck, M. (2007). Unwarranted assumptions about children’s
testimonial accuracy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3,
311–328.
Ceci, S. J., Papierno, P. B., & Kulkofsky, S. (2007).
Representational constraints on children’s suggestibility.
Psychological Science,
18, 503–509.
Ceci, S. J., Toglia, M., & Ross, D. (1990). The suggestibility of
preschoolers’ recollections: Historical perspectives on current
problems. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and
remembering in young children (pp. 285–300). New York:
Cambridge.
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory
conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memory
for an event?
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533–543.
Garven, S., Wood, J. M., Malpass, R., & Shaw, J. S. (1998).
More than suggestion: consequences of the interviewing
techniques
from the McMartin preschool case. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 83, 347–359.
Gilbert, D. T. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 121–138.
Hannigan, S. L., & Reinitz, M. T. (2001). A demonstration and
comparison of two types of inference-based memory errors.
Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
27, 931–940.
Harris, C. B., Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2008).
Collaborative recall and collective memory: What happens when
we
remember together? Memory, 16, 213–230.
Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (2008). Towards a psychology of
collective memory. Memory, 16, 183–200.
Jaeger, M. E., Anthony, S., & Rosnow, R. L. (1980). Who hears
what from whom and with what effect: A study of rumor.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 473–478.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source
monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.
Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, I., Orbach, Y., & Esplin, P. W.
(2008). Tell me what happened: Structured investigative
interviews of child
victims and witnesses. West Sussex, England: Wiley.
Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of
stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports.
Developmental
Psychology, 31, 568–578.
Lindsay, D. S., Johnson, M. K., & Kwon, P. (1991).
Developmental changes in source monitoring. Journal of
Experimental Child
Psychology, 52, 297–318.
Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. III, (2002). Explorations in the
social contagion of memory. Memory & Cognition, 30, 995–
1009.
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of
autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 7–14.
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of
autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental
theory.
Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511.
Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006a). Co-witnesses talk: A
survey of eyewitness discussion. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12,
181–191.
Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006b). Comparing methods of
encountering post-event information: The power of co-witness
suggestion. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1083–1099.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Poole, D., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). Reducing child witnesses’
false reports of misinformation from parents. Journal of
Experimental
Child Psychology, 81, 117–140.
Principe, G. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). ‘‘I saw it with my own
ears’’: The influence of peer conversations and suggestive
questions on
preschoolers’ event memories. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 83, 1–25.
Principe, G. F., Daley, L., & Kauth, K. (2010). Social processes
affecting the mnemonic consequences of rumors on children’s
memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 479–
493.
Principe, G. F., Cherson, M., DiPuppo, J., & Schindewolf, E.
(in press). Children’s natural conversations following exposure
to a
rumor: Linkages to later false reports. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology.
G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32
(2012) 205–223 223
Principe, G. F., DiPuppo, J., & Gammel, J. (2011). Leaking
misinformation to mothers: Effects of maternal conversational
style on
young children’s suggestibility for a past event. Poster
presented at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
Principe, G. F., Guiliano, S., & Root, C. (2008).
Rumormongering and remembering: How rumors originating in
children’s
inferences can affect memory. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 99, 135–155.
Principe, G. F., Haines, B., Adkins, A., & Guiliano, S. (2010).
False rumors and true belief: Memory processes underlying
children’s
errant reports of rumored events. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 107, 407–422.
Principe, G. F., Kanaya, T., Ceci, S. J., & Singh, M. (2006).
Believing is seeing: How rumors can engender false memories
in
preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 243–248.
Principe, G. F., Ornstein, P. A., Baker-ward, L., & Gordon, B.
N. (2000). The effects of intervening experiences on children’s
memory for a physical examination. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 14, 59–80.
Principe, G. F., & Smith, E. (2007). The tooth, the whole tooth,
and nothing but the tooth: How belief in the Tooth Fairy can
engender false memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1–
18.
Principe, G. F., Tinguely, A., & Dobkowski, N. (2007). Mixing
memories: The effects of rumors that conflict with children’s
experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 1–
19.
Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1989). Children’s memory and
proximity to violence. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 236–241.
Rabinowitz, D. (2004). No crueler tyrannies: Accusation, false
witness, and other terrors of our times. New York: Free Press.
Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (2008). The development of collective
remembering. Memory, 16, 201–212.
Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Inside rumor: A personal journey.
American Psychologist, 46, 484–496.
Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N. S., & Ottinger, W. (2004).
Changes in reality monitoring and episodic memory in early
childhood.
Developmental Science, 7, 225–245.
Sophian, C., & Huber, A. (1984). Early developments in
children’s causal judgments. Child Development, 55, 512–526.
Virj, A., Pannell, H., & Ost, J. (2005). The influence of social
pressure and black clothing on crime judgments. Psychology,
Crime, &
Law, 11, 265–274.
White, T. L., Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1997). The good,
the bad, and the ugly: Accuracy, inaccuracy, and elaboration in
preschoolers’ reports about a past event. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 11, S37–S54.
Wren, K. (1999). Social influences. New York: Routledge.
Natural conversations as a source of false memories in children:
Implications for the testimony of young
witnessesIntroductionCo-witness influenceRumor mongering
and rememberingConflicting rumorsChild-generated
suggestibilitySocial processesRepresentational changesLinking
rumor mongering to later false reportsMaternal suggestibility
and memory sharing styleConclusion and
implicationsAcknowledgmentReferences
Chapter 9: Patient Safety, Quality and Value
Harry Burke MD PhD
Learning Objectives
After reviewing the presentation, viewers should be able to:
Define safety, quality, near miss, and unsafe action
List the safety and quality factors that justified the clinical
implementation of electronic health record systems
Discuss three reasons why the electronic health record is central
to safety, quality, and value
List three issues that clinicians have with the current electronic
health record systems and discuss how these problems affect
safety and quality
Describe a specific electronic patient safety measurement
system and a specific electronic safety reporting system
Describe two integrated clinical decision support systems and
discuss how they may improve safety and quality
Patient Safety-Related Definitions
Safety: minimization of the risk and occurrence of patient harm
events
Harm: inappropriate or avoidable psychological or physical
injury to patient and/or family
Adverse Events: “an injury resulting from a medical
intervention”
Preventable Adverse Events: “errors that result in an adverse
event that are preventable”
Overuse: “the delivery of care of little or no value” e.g.
widespread use of antibiotics for viral infections
Underuse: “the failure to deliver appropriate care” e.g.
vaccines or cancer screening
Misuse: “the use of certain services in situations where they are
not clinically indicated” e.g. MRI for routine low back pain
Introduction
Medical errors are unfortunately common in healthcare, in spite
of sophisticated hospitals and well trained clinicians
Often it is breakdowns in protocol and communication, and not
individual errors
Technology has potential to reduce medical errors (particularly
medication errors) by:
Improving communication between physicians and patients
Improving clinical decision support
Decreasing diagnostic errors
Unfortunately, technology also has the potential to create
unique new errors that cause harm
Medical Errors
Errors can be related to diagnosis, treatment and preventive
care. Furthermore, medical errors can be errors of commission
or omission and fortunately not all errors result in an injury and
not all medical errors are preventable
Most common outpatient errors:
Prescribing medications
Getting the correct laboratory test for the correct patient at the
correct time
Filing system errors
Dispensing medications and responding to abnormal test results
5
While many would argue that treatment errors are the most
common category of medical errors, diagnostic errors accounted
for the largest percentage of malpractice claims, surpassing
treatment errors in one study
Diagnostic errors can result from missed, wrong or delayed
diagnoses and are more likely in the outpatient setting. This is
somewhat surprising given the fact that US physicians tend to
practice “defensive medicine”
Over-diagnosis may also cause medical errors but this has been
less well studied
Medical Errors
Unsafe healthcare lowers quality but safe medicine is not
always high quality
From the National Academy of Medicine’s perspective, quality
is a set of six aspirational goals: medical care should be safe,
effective, timely, efficient, patient-centered, and equitable
Value relates to how important something is to use
Cost-effective?
Necessary?
Affect morbidity, mortality or quality of life?
Quality, Safety and Value
Most adverse events result from unsafe actions or inactions by
anyone on the healthcare team, including the patient
Missed care is “any aspect of required care that is omitted either
in part or in whole or delayed”
Many of the above go unreported
Unsafe Actions
Most near-miss events are not reported. Many are not witnessed
The tendency is the blame the individual, but healthcare is
complex and there are often “system errors”
Most safety systems are retrospective; we need to move to be
proactive
We need good data, such as the ratio of detected unsafe actions
divided by the opportunity of an unsafe action, over a specified
time interval
Reporting Unsafe Actions
9
Patient Safety Reporting System: event is recorded and if it is a
sentinel event, it is investigated.
Most systems are not integrated with the EHR
Root Cause Analysis: common approach to determine the cause
of an adverse event. This has limitations
HEDIS measures can help track quality issues
Patient Safety Systems
Current reimbursement models mandate quality measures, e.g.
Medicare Patient Safety Monitoring System, now operated by
AHRQ. The new system is known as the Quality and Safety
Review System. Still labor intensive and manual
Global Trigger Tool: evaluates hospital safety. Said to detect
90% of adverse events. Select 10 discharge records and two
reviewers review the chart for any of the 53 “triggers”
Patient Safety Systems
Paper records have multiple disadvantages, as pointed out in the
EHR chapter
Expectations have been very high regarding the EHR’s impact
on safety, quality and value
Unfortunately, results have been mixed and there has not been a
prospective study conducted to prove the EHR’s benefit towards
safety and quality
Using the EHR to Improve Safety, Quality and Value
High expectations that CDS that is part of EHRs will improve
safety
As per multiple chapters in the textbook, CDS has mixed
reviews, in terms of safety and quality
Adverse events regarding CDS, includes ”alert fatigue”
The FDA will regulate software that is related to treatment and
decision making
Clinical Decision Support
Results in altered workflow and decreased efficiency.
Physicians are staying late to complete notes in the EHR
In an effort to save time physicians may “cut and paste” old
histories into the EHR, creating new problems
EHRs may create new safety issues “e-iatrogenesis”
Because of the multiple issues, it is very common to see offices
and hospitals change EHRs, not always solving the problem
Clinician’s Issues with EHRs
Roughly 2/3 of EHR data is unstructured (free text) so it is not
computable.
While natural language processing (NLP) may help solve this,
we are a long ways away from resolution
Multiple open source and commercial NLP programs exist but
they require a great deal of time and expertise to match the
results a manual chart review would produce
Clinician’s Issues with EHRs
Governmental Organizations Involved with Patient Safety
US Federal Agencies:
Department of Health and Human Services (HHS)
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ)
Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS)
Non-reimbursable complications: (3 examples)
Objects left in a patient during surgery and blood
incompatibility
Catheter-associated urinary tract infections
Pressure ulcers (bed sores)
Hospitals must assemble, analyze and trend clinical and
administrative data to capture baseline data and measure
improvement over time
Health IT-based interventions are expected to assist
Governmental Organizations
Office of the National Coordinator for HIT
Learn: “Increase the quantity and quality of data and knowledge
about health IT safety.”
Improve: “Target resources and corrective actions to improve
health IT safety and patient safety”
Safety goals will be aligned with meaningful use objectives.
Lead: “Promote a culture of safety related to health IT”
Governmental Organizations
The Food and Drug Administration
MedWatch: posts drug alerts and offers online reporting area
Center for Devices and Radiological Health (CDRH)
Plan to regulate mobile medical applications designed for use
on smartphones
State Patient Safety Programs: By 2010, 27 states and the
District of Columbia passed legislation or regulation related to
hospital reporting of adverse events to a state agency
Meaningful Use Objectives and Potential Impact on Patient
Safety
Objective: Use computerized provider order entry (CPOE) for
medication, laboratory, and radiology orders directly entered by
any licensed healthcare professional who can enter orders into
the medical record per state, local, and professional guidelines
Objective: Use clinical decision support to improve
performance on high-priority health conditions
Meaningful Use Objectives and Potential Impact on Patient
Safety
Objective: Automatically track medications from order to
administration using assistive technologies in conjunction with
an electronic medication administration record (eMAR)
Objective: Generate and transmit discharge prescriptions
electronically (eRx)
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx
Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx

Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different CognitiveCritically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitivesazoliver
 
McDonald 6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docx
McDonald     6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docxMcDonald     6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docx
McDonald 6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docxalfredacavx97
 
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in Adulthood
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in AdulthoodNumber of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in Adulthood
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in AdulthoodMary Lopez
 
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...inventionjournals
 
National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx
  National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx  National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx
National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docxjoyjonna282
 
Dissertation report
Dissertation reportDissertation report
Dissertation reportMathew Aspey
 
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docx
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docxChild and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docx
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docxbissacr
 
Master thesis Lies Polet
Master thesis Lies PoletMaster thesis Lies Polet
Master thesis Lies PoletLies Polet
 
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David Metaloro
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David MetaloroLiterature Review of Family Breakdown-David Metaloro
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David MetaloroDavid Metaloro
 
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureConfiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureBASPCAN
 
False memory research and its implications on children
False memory research and its implications on childrenFalse memory research and its implications on children
False memory research and its implications on childrenSarmad Agha
 
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdfADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdfCatalinaAguiar2
 
Beyond the Birds and the Bees: Adolescent Sex-Based Communication
Beyond the Birds and the Bees:  Adolescent Sex-Based Communication Beyond the Birds and the Bees:  Adolescent Sex-Based Communication
Beyond the Birds and the Bees: Adolescent Sex-Based Communication Daniella Singleton
 
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory 081909
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory  081909Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory  081909
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory 081909sgendron_stu_argosy_edu
 
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?Stella Pedrosa
 
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationship
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationshipWelcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationship
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationshipHazel Lee Weiyi
 
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docx
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docxThere are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docx
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docxrorye
 
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay Example
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay ExampleEssay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay Example
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay ExampleJessica Turner
 

Similar to Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx (20)

Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different CognitiveCritically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
Critically Evaluate The Role Of Three Different Cognitive
 
McDonald 6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docx
McDonald     6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docxMcDonald     6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docx
McDonald 6Name Sonja McDonaldCourse Name ENG 111-2902.docx
 
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in Adulthood
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in AdulthoodNumber of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in Adulthood
Number of Siblings in Childhood and the Likelihood of Divorce in Adulthood
 
M4_A1_Weber_Jennifer
M4_A1_Weber_JenniferM4_A1_Weber_Jennifer
M4_A1_Weber_Jennifer
 
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...
Sibling Birth Spacing Influence on Extroversion, Introversion and Aggressiven...
 
Buddhism Essays
Buddhism EssaysBuddhism Essays
Buddhism Essays
 
National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx
  National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx  National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx
National Council on Family Relations and Wiley are collabora.docx
 
Dissertation report
Dissertation reportDissertation report
Dissertation report
 
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docx
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docxChild and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docx
Child and Family Social Work 2002, 7, pp 35–45 © 2002 Blackwel.docx
 
Master thesis Lies Polet
Master thesis Lies PoletMaster thesis Lies Polet
Master thesis Lies Polet
 
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David Metaloro
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David MetaloroLiterature Review of Family Breakdown-David Metaloro
Literature Review of Family Breakdown-David Metaloro
 
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literatureConfiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
Confiding in Friends: A systematic review of the literature
 
False memory research and its implications on children
False memory research and its implications on childrenFalse memory research and its implications on children
False memory research and its implications on children
 
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdfADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
 
Beyond the Birds and the Bees: Adolescent Sex-Based Communication
Beyond the Birds and the Bees:  Adolescent Sex-Based Communication Beyond the Birds and the Bees:  Adolescent Sex-Based Communication
Beyond the Birds and the Bees: Adolescent Sex-Based Communication
 
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory 081909
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory  081909Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory  081909
Cognitive Abilities Psych Application: Environ Ecological Theory 081909
 
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?
THE USE OF SOCIAL NETWORKS ONLINE: A REASON OF INTERGENERATIONAL CONFLICTS?
 
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationship
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationshipWelcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationship
Welcome-to-Facebook-How-Facebook-influences-Parent-child-relationship
 
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docx
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docxThere are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docx
There are a wide variety of factors that can influence andor cr.docx
 
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay Example
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay ExampleEssay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay Example
Essay On Food Inc. Reflection Paper: Organic Foods Free Essay Example
 

More from cuddietheresa

1. A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx
1.  A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx1.  A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx
1. A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docxcuddietheresa
 
1. Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx
1.   Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx1.   Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx
1. Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docxcuddietheresa
 
1. As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx
1.  As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx1.  As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx
1. As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docxcuddietheresa
 
1. An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx
1.  An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx1.  An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx
1. An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docxcuddietheresa
 
1. According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx
1.  According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx1.  According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx
1. According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docxcuddietheresa
 
1. Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx
1.     Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx1.     Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx
1. Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docxcuddietheresa
 
1. The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx
1.   The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx1.   The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx
1. The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docxcuddietheresa
 
1. Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx
1.   Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx1.   Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx
1. Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docxcuddietheresa
 
1. All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx
1.  All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx1.  All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx
1. All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docxcuddietheresa
 
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docxcuddietheresa
 
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docxcuddietheresa
 
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docxcuddietheresa
 
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docxcuddietheresa
 

More from cuddietheresa (20)

1. A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx
1.  A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx1.  A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx
1. A corporations distribution of additional shares of its own s.docx
 
1. Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx
1.   Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx1.   Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx
1. Like the modernists, postmodern writers focused on subjective e.docx
 
1. As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx
1.  As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx1.  As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx
1. As the degree of freedom increase indefinitely, the t distribu.docx
 
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx
1-Explain how the topography of the United states can affect the wea.docx
 
1. An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx
1.  An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx1.  An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx
1. An exporter faced with exposure to a depreciating currency can.docx
 
1. According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx
1.  According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx1.  According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx
1. According to the central limit theorem, a population which is .docx
 
1. Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx
1.     Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx1.     Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx
1. Which of the following is not a class of essential nutrient.docx
 
1. The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx
1.   The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx1.   The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx
1. The process by which one group takes on the cultural and other .docx
 
1. Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx
1.   Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx1.   Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx
1. Milestone InvestingCompare and contrast the interests of .docx
 
1. All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx
1.  All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx1.  All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx
1. All dogs are warm-blooded. All warm-blooded creatures are mamm.docx
 
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx
1-3 Final Project Milestone #1 ProposalThroughout this course.docx
 
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx
1-Please explain Ethical Universalism. Should organizations be socia.docx
 
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx
1-an explanation of why the Marbury v. Madison case is a landmar.docx
 
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx
1-Discuss research that supports the hypothesis that a person’s ac.docx
 
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx
1-Imagine you are a historian, and the only existing sources of evid.docx
 
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx
1-How does relative humidity affect the comfort of people Can you e.docx
 
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx
1-1) In general, what is the effect of one party being mistaken abou.docx
 
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx
1- How did the United States become involved in the politics of Sout.docx
 
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx
1- I need someone who read the book (V for Vendetta) and saw the mov.docx
 
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx
1- Define arbitration.2- Who is responsible for paying an arbitr.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 

Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223Contents lists availab.docx

  • 1. Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Developmental Review journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate/dr Natural conversations as a source of false memories in children: Implications for the testimony of young witnesses Gabrielle F. Principe ⇑ , Erica Schindewolf Department of Psychology, Ursinus College, United States a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Available online 25 July 2012 Keywords: Memory Suggestibility Children Social interaction Rumor Eyewitness testimony 0273-2297/$ - see front matter � 2012 Elsevier In http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003 ⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department o United States. Fax: +1 610 409 3633. E-mail address: [email protected] (G.F. Pri Research on factors that can affect the accuracy of children’s
  • 2. auto- biographical remembering has important implications for under- standing the abilities of young witnesses to provide legal testimony. In this article, we review our own recent research on one factor that has much potential to induce errors in children’s event recall, namely natural memory sharing conversations with peers and parents. Our studies provide compelling evidence that not only can the content of conversations about the past intrude into later memory but that such exchanges can prompt the gener- ation of entirely false narratives that are more detailed than true accounts of experienced events. Further, our work show that dee- per and more creative participation in memory sharing dialogues can boost the damaging effects of conversationally conveyed mis- information. Implications of this collection of findings for chil- dren’s testimony are discussed. � 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Perhaps only one simple and straightforward claim can be made about the accuracy of children’s testimony: not all statements made by children are true. Admittedly, exact accuracy is not the usual goal of memory in everyday life. Most autobiographical remembering is carried out for social pur- poses, such as to build bonds and foster connectedness with friends and family (see e.g., Nelson, 1993), and can serve these functions even when recollections do not precisely represent the past. In c. All rights reserved. f Psychology, Ursinus College, PO Box 1000, Collegeville, PA 19426-1000,
  • 3. ncipe). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003 mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.003 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02732297 http://www.elsevier.com/locate/dr 206 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 fact, many everyday situations encourage some degree of unfaithfulness. Exaggerated, improvised, or even fabricated stories can be more engaging or more amusing to conversational partners than verid- ical reports. These tendencies to embellish personal experiences may be especially pronounced at young ages given children’s proclivity for pretense and adults’ willingness to play along. To illustrate, only young children can get away with fantastic stories of a fairy who gives prizes for baby teeth or a monster that lives under the bed (see e.g., Principe & Smith, 2007). Against this backdrop of memory in everyday life, the courtroom is a rather unusual setting for children’s remembering. In the real world, accounts of personal experiences are successful to the ex- tent that they are relayed in a compelling or affecting manner. In the legal system, precise accuracy is the goal. Remembering is successful to the degree that witnesses ‘‘tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth.’’ Forensic settings, therefore, put unique demands on memory that are at odds with the way that recollections of the past typically are
  • 4. used. This contrast notwithstanding, because many criminal offenses that bring children to court, such as sexual abuse and other forms of molestation, lack other witnesses or corroborating evidence, children’s testimony often serves as the sole piece of evidence against criminal defendants. Likewise, children’s memories impact many civil and family court cases. For example, children’s accounts of parental transgressions, such as domestic violence and substance abuse, as well as more mundane events, such as daily home rou- tines, commonly play a role in custody, support, and visitation decisions. Considering the centrality of children’s testimony in many legal situations, research on factors that can compromise children’s abilities to provide accurate accounts of the past has considerable relevance to forensic professionals and fact finders. Given that children’s testimony is elicited in interviews, many investigators have focused on the mnemonic effects of various suggestive features of interviews. This voluminous literature has revealed that a range of factors, such as types of questions asked, the sorts of ancillary aids used, and the char- acteristics of interviewers, can seriously derail children’s accuracy and even lead to entirely false ac- counts (see Bruck, Ceci, & Hembrooke, 2002, for a review). Despite the significance of this work for developing effective interviewing protocols, researchers have become increasingly concerned with examining suggestive factors outside of the formal interview context that also can contaminate mem- ory. This move to exploring extra-interview factors has been prompted by findings that even when children are interviewed under optimally nonsuggestive
  • 5. conditions, some nonetheless relay fabricated stories in line with suggestions encountered from other sources, such as parents (Poole & Lindsay, 2002) and television (Principe, Ornstein, Baker-ward, & Gordon, 2000). In everyday life, one common way to encounter suggestions is during memory sharing conversa- tions with others. A compelling reason for focusing on conversational forms of suggestion concerns the social nature of autobiographical memory. Sharing memories through conversations with friends and family members is a typical and frequent part of children’s everyday social interactions. During such exchanges, however, children constantly are encountering others’ versions of the past. Different versions can arise unwittingly when conversational partners misremember what happened, but also can occur when they purposefully exaggerate or even fabricate details to tell, say, a more glamorous story than give a precisely accurate account. Given that memory is constructive (Bartlett, 1932), it is within this realm that bits and pieces of the suggestions and stories told by others may find their way into children’s recollections of their experiences. Emphasizing the social nature of remembering are theories of collective memory (e.g., Hirst & Man- ier, 2008; Reese & Fivush, 2008) that characterize memories of shared experiences as dynamic repre- sentations that are shaped by group conversational processes. In this framework, as memories of the past are reconstructed within a group, its members negotiate a collective version of experience. Con- sequently, individual representations are revised to become progressively alike among group mem-
  • 6. bers (see Harris, Paterson, & Kemp, 2008). However, when misinformation is introduced into group remembering, either deliberately by a confederate (Meade & Roediger, 2002) or unknowingly by a group member who experienced a slightly different version of the event (Gabbert, Memon, & Allan, 2003), individuals are prone to later recall occurrences that were nonexperienced but merely sug- gested by their conversational partners. The practical importance of studies of conversational sharing for discussions of children’s testi- mony comes from real world examples demonstrating that witnesses often talk with one another. G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 207 Consider, for instance, Paterson and Kemp’s (2006a) finding that over 80% of witnesses to a crime or serious accident reported discussing the event with another witness, or other work showing that it is not uncommon for multiple witnesses to be questioned at the same time (e.g., Garven, Wood, Malpass, & Shaw, 1998). Issues of conversational contamination are particularly relevant to situations involving multiple abuse victims, as children caught up in these cases may attend group therapy sessions or community meetings where allegations are shared (Rabinowitz, 2004). There are also reasons to suspect that young children may be especially vulnerable to the contri- butions of others in their constructions of experience. First, young children’s difficulty keeping track
  • 7. of the source of their memories (e.g., Poole & Lindsay, 2002) may put them at increased risk for mis- takenly attributing events relayed by others as their own actual experiences. Second, young children are somewhat dependant on others to help them figure out how to represent and recount their expe- riences. Such collaboration benefits children’s construals of novel events and narrations of existing memories (Nelson, 1993), but it might also lead to problematic distortions in memory when others incorrectly frame legally relevant events. Consider, for instance, a father who frames sexual abuse as a special game or a mother in a custody dispute who says, ‘‘Daddy hurts you when he gives you a bath, doesn’t he?’’ Third, younger preschoolers do not yet realize that others can have memories that are false; rather they believe that the mind literally copies experience and that everyone therefore has only true memories (e.g., Perner, 1991). This tendency usually is not problematic in the real world but it can be in legal situations. To illustrate, when a child hears from a friend that she saw Santa put pre- sents under the Christmas tree or that their teacher Mr. Bob does bad things, both claims are unques- tionably believed. Finally, young children rarely receive feedback on what a false memory feels like. Adults do, for example, when they remember parking their car on the second level of the garage but find it on the first. Children, in contrast, get away with all sorts of memory errors, such as claiming to have spent the afternoon with an invisible friend. As this brief analysis indicates, examination of the sorts of conversational activities that might be linked to later errors in remembering is central to an understanding of children’s ability to provide tes-
  • 8. timony in legal settings. However, because the extant literature on memory errors focuses almost exclusively on the effects of suggestive questioning or other forms of scripted misinformation, we know very little about the ways that memory may be transformed in the normal course of discussing the past with others. With these theoretical and applied issues in mind, in the remainder of this article, we offer an overview of a programmatic series of studies carried out by our research group concerning how knowledge gained from and within conversations with agemates and adults can shape children’s constructions of the present and reconstructions of the past. Co-witness influence Our exploration of mnemonic effects of memory sharing began in the context of a study on the influence of naturally occurring interactions with peer witnesses on children’s memory for a personal experience (Principe & Ceci, 2002). Given extant demonstrations of the potency of collective remem- bering in shaping individual memories in the direction of the group, co-witness discussions might, at least at times, cause children within a peer group to construct a collaborative story that does not veridically reflect the independent experiences of each group member. This issue may be especially important for legal cases involving multiple purported victims because fact finders may rely on the number and similarity of allegations to determine the credibility of any single child’s testimony. It is likely very compelling to hear child after child tell the same story, especially if one believes that each witness has arrived at the same storyline independently. In some situations, however, the exact oppo- site might be the case. The story may have been arrived at in a
  • 9. collaborative manner among peers who initially had very different representations of the event. There are numerous real world examples that discussions among co-witnesses can influence chil- dren’s testimony. Consider, for instance, the following exchange between a young witness and a foren- sic investigator in the Wee Care Nursery School case in Maplewood, New Jersey. In this case, Kelly Michaels, a teacher at the school, was accused of sexually molesting her students. Here a child reveals that the source of her allegation was another child rather than her own observation: 208 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 Interviewer: Do you know what [Kelly] did? Child: She wasn’t supposed to touch somebody’s body. If you want to touch somebody, touch your own. Interviewer: How do you know about her touching private parts? Is that something you saw or heard? Child: Max told me. (Ceci & Bruck, 1995, p. 150) To explore co-witness contamination, three groups of 3- to 5- year-olds participated in a staged event at their preschools, namely an archaeology dig with a confederate archeologist named Dr. Diggs.
  • 10. Children used plastic hammers to dig pretend artifacts, such as dinosaur bones, gold coins, and jewels, out of specially constructed blocks of mortar mix and play sand. Each dig included two ‘‘target’’ arti- facts: a bottle with a map to a buried treasure and a rock with a message written in a secret language. One third of the children, those in the Witness condition, saw Dr. Diggs ruin the target artifacts (here- tofore referred to as target activities). He ‘‘accidentally’’ spilled coffee on the map, smearing the ink and rendering the map illegible. He appeared upset and said, ‘‘I messed up the map! Now I’ll never find the buried treasure!’’ Dr. Diggs also dropped the rock, shattering it into pieces, and said, ‘‘I’ve bro- ken the rock! Now I’ll never know what the secret message says!’’ A second third of the children, those in the Classmate condition, did not witness the target activities during the dig but were the classmates of those in the Witness group. We expected that some of these children would hear about the ruined map and broken rock through natural conversations with their classmates who saw these activities. The remaining children in the Control condition were drawn from different preschools than the Wit- ness and Classmate children. These children did not have any opportunities to interact with those who saw the target activities nor did they see these activities themselves. This group provided the likeli- hood that target activities would be reported by a random nonwitness without exposure to any peer witnesses. Teachers were discouraged from initiating or participating in conversations with children about the dig. Given evidence of the use of suggestive techniques in forensic settings, we also examined whether
  • 11. suggestive interviews might augment the influence of co- witnesses. Following the dig, all children were questioned on three occasions spread out over a 3-week interval. Half of the children received neutral interviews, whereas the remaining half were questioned in a suggestive manner. Embedded in the suggestive interviews were strongly worded leading questions that implied that the target activities had occurred. Thus these questions were in line with what the Witnesses children had seen, but inconsistent with the experience of the Classmate and Control children. Four weeks after the dig, a new interviewer questioned all of the children in a neutral manner and asked them to recall ‘‘only things that you remember happening to you—things that you really did or remember seeing with your own eyes.’’ The hierarchically ordered interview began with an open- ended prompt: ‘‘Tell me what happened when Dr. Diggs visited your school.’’ After exhausting open-ended recall, specific questions were asked if one or both of the target activities had not yet been reported (e.g., ‘‘Did anything happen to a treasure map?’’). For each target activity relayed, children were asked to elaborate (‘‘Tell me more about that.’’) Children who made reports of target activities also were asked for the source of their memories, that is, whether they actually saw the target activity Table 1 Mean percentages target activities reported as actually occurring at the final interview as a function of experimental group and degree of prompting. Open-ended Specific Total
  • 12. Witness/neutral interview 34 47 81 Classmate/neutral interview 16 15 31 Control/neutral interview 0 0 0 Witness/suggestive interview 68 23 91 Classmate/suggestive interview 50 36 86 Control/suggestive interview 23 33 57 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 209 occur with their own eyes or merely heard about it from someone (e.g., ‘‘Did you see Dr. Diggs spill his drink on the treasure map with your own eyes, or hear that he did it?’’). Table 1 shows the proportion of target activities reported and the degree of prompting needed to elicit the information at the 4-week interview. As shown, children in the Witness conditions evi- denced quite good recall of these actually experienced activities, with both groups reporting over 80%. However, the Classmate children, who did not witness the ruined map or the broken rock, also reported many of these activities. Under both interviewing conditions, the Classmate children wrongly reported more target activities than the Control children, demonstrating that natural contact with peer witnesses can induce false accounts in non-witnesses. In fact, many errant accounts were at the open-ended level of questioning, indicating that the effects of peer witnesses are not limited to cued reports can but can result in abundant spontaneous errors. Perhaps the most interesting finding to emerge from this study is that when the Classmate children were exposed to the suggestive inter-
  • 13. views, they reported as many target activities as those in the Witness conditions who actually expe- rienced these activities. Thus the combined effects of exposure to peer witnesses and suggestive interviewing among the non-witnesses resulted in levels of recall that were indistinguishable in terms of magnitude from those of the Witness children. Several other findings also are of interest. First, among those non-witnesses who reported target activities, the Classmate children were more likely than the Control children to report actually seeing these activities occur with their own eyes (as opposed to merely having heard about them). Such claims of seeing suggest that natural conversations with co- witnesses not only can induce false re- ports but they also can lead to source confusions. Second, given that fact finders often consider detail as an index of testimonial accuracy (Ceci, Kulkofsky, Klemfuss, Sweeney, & Bruck, 2007), we explored the narratives accompanying the reports of target activities. As expected, the Witness children gener- ated relatively detailed accounts of these experienced events. Many non-witnesses, however, also re- layed elaborate reports of these occurrences they never saw but merely heard about, with many embellishing with details that went beyond the Witness children’s experiences but nonetheless were consistent with them. Consider, for example, a Classmate child who said that after spilling coffee on the map ‘‘Dr. Diggs walked away and then we just got in big trouble. . . all my friends and he had to be punished for a whole weekend. . .The ladies in the cafeteria cleaned it because he didn’t have a mop. . .They took him away and put him in jail.’’ In fact, the Classmate children’s false accounts of
  • 14. the target activities were more voluminous than the true narratives of the Witness children. This pat- tern demonstrates that narrative detail is not diagnostic of accuracy when children have been exposed to peer witnesses, and is consistent with other work showing that false accounts induced by other forms of suggestion can be more elaborate than true reports (e.g., Bruck et al., 2002; Poole & Lindsay, 2002). Interestingly, examination of the content of children’s narratives indicated that this group dif- ference occurred neither because the Classmate children more completely relayed their peers’ expe- riences or the interview suggestions nor because they more readily invented fantastic or idiosyncratic embellishments, but because they generated more original constructions consistent with the notions of a ruined treasure map and a broken rock. Rumor mongering and remembering In our next study, we sought to extend our (Principe & Ceci, 2002) demonstration of the impact of con- versational interactions by determining whether peer interactions can influence children’s reports of an experience even when none of them actually witnessed the event in question. To do this, we planted a false rumor about an experienced event among some members of preschool classrooms and examined the degree to which the rumored information leaked into their own and their classmates’ recollections when later interviewed. We also explored the degree to which the interfering effects of the rumor might be exacerbated when paired with suggestive interviews that are consistent with the rumor. We chose to study rumor transmission because a large literature in social psychology demonstrates
  • 15. that rumors often are generated about events that are meaningful and upsetting where the truth is unclear (see Rosnow, 1991). These conditions sound a lot like those created by the sorts of offenses that usually bring children to court, such as sexual abuse and other forms of maltreatment, because 210 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 they are unsettling and typically lack corroborating witnesses or physical evidence. In such cases, ru- mors may emerge to fill in the gaps of missing information or to impose an explanation on an unset- tling allegation. Considering that individuals generally assume that information exchanged during everyday conversations is true (Gilbert, 1995), shared rumors likely have much potential to prompt revisions in memory in line with overheard information. There also are real world examples of rumor contamination. One comes from a case in which chil- dren who were absent from school on the day of a sniper attack recalled seeing things that only their peers, who were present, could have experienced. A rumor allegedly began to circulate that a second sniper had eluded police and was on the loose. When the children were asked to describe the attack several months later, many described in detail how the second sniper had escaped and still was loose in the neighborhood (Pynoos & Nader, 1989). To examine whether rumor can leak into memory, four groups of 3- to 5-year-olds saw a scripted magic show in their preschools (Principe, Kanaya, Ceci, &
  • 16. Singh, 2006) in which a magician named Ma- gic Mumfry tried to pull a live rabbit out of his top hat. After several failed and frenzied efforts, Mumfy apologized and left the school. Immediately after the show, children in the Overheard group overheard a scripted conversation between two adults in which one alleged that the trick failed because Mum- fry’s rabbit had gotten loose in the school rather than residing in his hat. We maximized children’s attention to the rumor by having them stand quietly in a line awaiting a sticker during the planned conversation. Children in the Classmate group did not overhear the adult conversation about the es- caped rabbit but were the classmates of the Overheard children. Of interest was whether these chil- dren would learn about the alleged lost rabbit through natural interactions with their classmates who heard the rumor and whether details in line with the rumor might leak into their later recollections. Control children had no exposure to the rumor; they were not the classmates of those who overheard the rumor, nor did they overhear it themselves. The remaining children in the Witness group had no exposure to the other three groups but experienced the event suggested by the rumor, namely seeing Mumfry’s rabbit loose in their school after the failed trick. One week later, all children were ques- tioned in either a neutral or suggestive manner. Embedded in the suggestive interviews were coercive questions that implied that the interviewee had witnessed Mumfry’s escaped rabbit, when in fact only those in the Witness group did. Two weeks after the show, all children were questioned by a new, neutral interviewer in the same hierarchical manner as in Principe and Ceci’s (2002) study. As
  • 17. shown in Table 2, all of the Witness chil- dren correctly recalled that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten loose in their school. Table 2 also illustrates the powerful effects of the rumor on children’s accounts. All but one of the Overheard and Classmate children wrongly reported a loose rabbit. Thus these children were as likely as those who actually saw a live rabbit to report that Mumfry’s rabbit was loose, thereby eliminating differences in levels of recall between true and false accounts. This pattern not only shows that information overheard from adults can lead to near ceiling levels of false reports of nonexperienced events, it also indicates that rumors transmitted by peers can be as detrimental as those spread by adults. Moreover, the majority of the Overheard and Classmate children’s reports of the escaped rabbit were in response to open-ended probes, demonstrating that errant rumors can lead to high levels of spontaneous fabrications. Further, many Overheard and Classmate children claimed to have seen, as opposed to heard about, the loose Table 2 Percentages of children who reported the target activity as actually occurring at the final interview as a function of experimental group and degree of prompting. Group Open-ended Specific Total Witness/neutral interview 90 10 100 Overheard/neutral interview 86 10 95 Classmate/neutral interview 86 14 100 Control/neutral interview 0 10 10 Witness/suggestive interview 87 13 100 Overheard/suggestive interview 87 13 100 Classmate/suggestive interview 91 9 100 Control/suggestive interview 9 50 59
  • 18. G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 211 rabbit. These reports of seeing a nonoccuring event represent a considerably more extreme demon- stration of peer-generated suggestibility than our prior study (Principe & Ceci, 2002) considering that none of these children witnessed the event in question. Further demonstrating the potency of rumor, the non-witness children described the rumored loose rabbit with much elaborative detail that went above and beyond the literal rumor. To illustrate, a Classmate child said that, ‘‘The rabbit was in the playground, and then it was over the gate and, the rabbit was over, the rabbit jumped, hopped over the gate. . .I tried catching him with a bucket but he bited me on the finger. . .They found him in the potty.’’ In fact, the Overheard and Classmate children’s descriptions of the rumored-but-nonoccurring loose rabbit were twice as voluminous as the accounts of the Witness children, demonstrating that false narratives engendered by rumor can be much more elaborate than true narratives generated on the basis of experience. Considering the ease with which suggestive questions can induce false reports (see Ceci, Kulkofsky et al., 2007), it is worth noting that the loose rabbit misinformation engendered higher levels of error when planted via a rumor than when suggested during an interview. Those children who heard the rumor from an adult or peers gave more errant reports of the nonevent, were more likely to wrongly recall seeing (as opposed to hearing about) it, and embellished
  • 19. their accounts with more elaborative detail compared to those for whom the very same false information was suggested during an inter- view. This finding is particularly noteworthy given that the Overheard children were not instructed to share the rumor with their peers but ended up naturally propagating this information to them in a manner that was more mnemonically damaging than an aggressively suggestive interview. Conflicting rumors The major finding of our initial study on rumors was that overheard false information that provides a reasonable explanation for an earlier ambiguous event can lead children to mistakenly recall details consistent with the rumor (Principe et al., 2006). This finding prompted us to consider whether the effects of rumor might be less powerful in situations where the rumored information conflicts with the past rather merely fills a gap. This contrast was of interest because when rumors only fill a gap, overheard details can be imported into memory without displacing or overwriting any experienced details. But when rumors conflict with the past, there is a contradiction between what was experi- enced and what was overheard and children must resolve it. One important factor that may moderate children’s resistance to rumors that contradict with their experiences is their ability to reason about conflicting mental representations. This notion comes from researchers in the theory of mind tradition who propose that there is a major transition in children’s understanding of representational processes between 3 and 6 years of age (e.g., Perner, 1991). Younger preschoolers possess a copy theory of mind and believe that the
  • 20. mind exactly represents the world and that consequently everyone has the same true beliefs about it. In contrast, older preschoolers de- velop an interpretive theory of mind and recognize that representation is a subjective process shaped by experience as well as beliefs, expectations, goals, and so on. Thus they realize that different people can have different, even contradictory, representations of the same experience. With these issues in mind, 3- to 6-year-olds participated in our usual magic show (Principe, Tingu- ely, & Dobkowski, 2007). At the end of the show, one third of the children, those in the Conflicting Rep- resentation condition, experienced an extra activity that provided a plausible explanation for the failed trick but that conflicted with the rumor that they would later overhear. Mumfry uncovered a previously unseen cage that held a live rabbit. He explained that he had found his missing rabbit and that it was sick because when it is sick it will not leave its cage, not even to do its favorite hat trick. To encourage belief in this explanation, Mumfry got out his vet kit, checked the rabbit with a stethoscope, and gave it some ‘‘medicine.’’ Then he and the rabbit left the school. Replicating our ori- ginal procedure, these children then overheard an adult allege that Mumfry’s trick failed because the rabbit had gotten loose in the school. Importantly, the rumor created a conflicting representation for the failed trick for these children given their prior exposure to the sick rabbit. A second third of the children, those in the Nonconflicting Representation group, overheard the same rumor conversation but did not experience the extra sick rabbit activity. Thus for these children the rumor provided a
  • 21. 212 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 plausible explanation for the failed trick. The remaining Control children did not overhear the rumor or interact with the children in the other two groups. One week later, all children were questioned using our standard interview protocol. Replicating our earlier findings, nearly all of the children for whom the rumor did not conflict with their experiences reported that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten loose in the school. Interestingly, there were no effects of age in this group, suggesting that increased resistance to gap-filling rumors may not develop during the preschool years. However, the 5- and 6- year-olds were better able than the 3- and 4-year-olds to resist the rumor when it conflicted with their experiences than when it merely filled a gap, whereas the 3- and 4-year-olds were equally likely to be misled by both types of rumors. All but one of the Conflicting Representation young children reported a loose rabbit, whereas less than half of the older children did. These findings qualify our earlier work by revealing that younger pre- schoolers may be more vulnerable than older preschoolers to conflicting rumors. Examination of children’s false narratives describing the rumored loose rabbit suggests that their developing ability to deal with conflicting representations played some role in this age trend. The younger Conflicting Representation children based the majority of their false accounts on only one
  • 22. representation of the failed trick—the nonoccurring rumor. The older children in this condition, in con- trast, imported many details consistent with the sick rabbit activity they actually saw, suggesting that they had more ready access to both representations and consequently drew from both in constructing their accounts. We also found that poorer performance on a separate series of tasks that index conflict- ing representation understanding was associated with increased proneness to report the rumored tar- get activity and heightened levels of descriptive detail consistent with the rumor. This relation remained significant after controlling for age, supporting the notion that the development of a concep- tual understanding of the mind, particularly the ability to reason about conflicting mental represen- tations, may be important for resisting suggestions that contradict with children’s experiences. It is worth noting that the distinction between conflicting and nonconflicting misinformation generally is not made in the suggestibility literature, but these findings suggest that this may be an important contrast useful for explaining age trends. Child-generated suggestibility These findings demonstrating the potency of rumors planted by adults prompted us to consider the mnemonic effects of rumors generated by children themselves. This issue was of interest because of a growing attention in the memory literature to autosuggestibility (Brainerd & Reyna, 1995), or errors in memory that emanate from internal, constructive processes in which an individual’s own beliefs, expectations, goals, and so on, distort their recollections. For instance, autosuggestion errors can occur when people make causal inferences during an event and later
  • 23. mistake their inferences for memories of the actual experience. To illustrate, when shown a slide sequence of a familiar event (e.g., grocery shopping) that shows an effect (e.g., oranges on the supermarket floor) but not its cause, many people wrongly report seeing the most probable cause of the observed effect (e.g., a woman pulling an orange from the bottom of the stack). Hannigan and Reinitz (2001) attribute this tendency to a reality mon- itoring error that occurs when people misattribute their memory of an internally generated inference as arising from an externally experienced event. Given that children as young 3 make inferences about the causes of observed events (e.g., Sophian & Huber, 1984), we wondered whether they may be vul- nerable to causal inference errors about experienced events. In this study, 3- to 6-year-olds watched a modified version of our usual magic show in which Mum- fry failed at two tricks: pulling a rabbit out of his hat and producing a baked cake from a cake pan (Principe, Guiliano, & Root, 2008). For the cake trick, Mumfry put several real ingredients into cake pan, covered it, and promised a fully baked cake. However, when he opened the lid, the pan was empty. To prompt the generation of rumors, one third of the children, those in the Clue condition, were exposed to two sets of clues: carrot ends with ‘‘teeth marks’’ and a plate with cake crumbs and a dirty fork. The carrot and cake ‘‘clues’’ were expected to induce two inferences (heretofore re- ferred to as target activities): one about the cause of the failed hat trick (i.e., the rabbit got loose in the school) and one about the cause of the vanished cake (i.e., someone ate the cake). To investigate
  • 24. G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 213 whether these children would naturally propagate their inferences about the escaped rabbit and the eaten cake to peers, the children in the Classmate condition were the classmates of those in the Clue group, but were not exposed to the clues. The remaining children in the Control condition were not the classmates of those who saw the clues, nor did they see the clues themselves. The results indicated that the clue manipulations prompted the generation of rumors (or explana- tions) about what happened to the missing rabbit and cake that made their way into children’s later memory reports. When interviewed one week after the show, nearly 80% of the Clue children reported that Mumfry’s rabbit had gotten loose or that someone had eaten the cake. Forty percent of these errors were at the open-ended level of questioning. Interestingly, the clues were more mnemonically damag- ing to the 5- and 6-year-olds than the 3- and 4-year-olds. Not only were the older children more likely than the younger children to mistakenly report their inferences during free recall, their reports of non- experienced-but-inferred occurrences were embellished with double the amount of detail. This pattern might seem counterintuitive because younger children typically are more suggestible than older chil- dren. But given that causal inferencing ability develops rapidly during the preschool years (e.g., Sophian & Huber, 1984), a reverse developmental trend in interference errors makes sense on empirical grounds. Assuming the older children more readily generated
  • 25. relevant inferences following the clues, they likely had created for themselves more opportunities than the younger children to make causal inference errors. Supporting this explanation is that compared to the younger children, the older chil- dren’s false accounts contained more logical inferences based on the clues and they more often elabo- rated on their inferences in a rational manner. The younger children, in contrast, were more likely to recount illogical or even impossible details about the target activities. Considering that fact finders tend to view detail as diagnostic of accuracy, these findings have some applied relevance as they suggest that when condition are ripe for causal inferences, older children may be more prone to construct false re- ports that are more compelling or believable than those produced by younger children. Examination of the Classmate children’s performance indicated that the Clue children readily prop- agated their inferences to peers. Nearly 40% of the Classmate children reported one or both of the tar- get activities, and the majority of these claims occurred during free recall. Interesting, the spread of inferred information was particularly potent among the 3- and 4-year-olds, as they were more likely than the older children to report actually seeing events that their peers merely inferred. These data provide original evidence that rumors generated by children themselves, rather than those planted by adults, can intrude into the recollections of peers. This is an important extension of our earlier work because although much is known about how adults can induce errors in children’s memory, little is understood about how children themselves can affect other children’s recall. It is noteworthy, how-
  • 26. ever, that the Clue children made more false reports than the Classmate children, demonstrating that the children generally were more influenced by their own conclusions for the failed tricks than their peers’. This rationale is in line with findings in the source monitoring literature that internally gener- ated events are more easily confused with one’s own experiences than are externally suggested events (e.g., Lindsay, Johnson, & Kwon, 1991). The more frequent errors among the Clue relative to the Classmate group notwithstanding, higher levels of details accompanied the Classmate children’s reports of the target activities. This finding sug- gests that children may be especially likely to embellish information picked up from peers—a trend that we found in our original Mumfry study (Principe et al., 2006) and in follow-up work reported be- low. Examination of the content of children’s narratives made it clear that the rise in elaboration among the Classmate children was driven by an increase in statements consistent with the notions of a loose rabbit and eaten cake but beyond inferences that could be derived directly from the clues. Perhaps because the Classmate children did not see the clues themselves but based their ideas on their peers’ stories about the causes of the failed tricks, they were less limited than the Clue children to describing the simple inferences implied by the clues. Social processes In our three rumor studies described so far, levels of errant reports are higher and the accompany- ing false narratives more voluminous than are typical of suggestibility research in which children are
  • 27. 214 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 exposed to misinformation during interviews (e.g., Bruck et al., 2002) or in other private contexts (e.g., Leichtman & Ceci, 1995; Poole & Lindsay, 2002). Of interest therefore is what gives rumor its potency? One clue comes from studies in the adult social influence literature that demonstrate the very same misinformation more easily intrudes into memory if it is encountered in a social context via a confed- erate than if it is picked up in a nonsocial manner, such as through written suggestions or leading questions (Paterson & Kemp, 2006b), with the magnitude of social conformity building as exposure to others’ erroneous responses increases (Virj, Pannell, & Ost, 2005). Given that children in our rumor studies interact freely with their entire class after the rumor is planted, it is possible that this social experience, rather than the rumor itself, drives the exceptionally high levels of false reports and ficti- tious elaboration. The purpose of our next study, therefore, was to examine whether opportunities for natural discussions with others following exposure to a false rumor can exacerbate its effects on later recall. To investigate this question, 3- to 5-year-olds watched our Mumfry show and then overheard the loose rabbit rumor (Principe, Daley, & Kauth, 2010). Immediately after overhearing the rumor, some of the children were given 30 min of free play time to interact naturally with one another. Other chil- dren were engaged in a structured ‘‘circle time’’ activity for 30 min that prohibited them from talking
  • 28. about the rumor. After these 30 min, all children went home for the day and we took several mea- sures to ensure that none of the children had any further interactions with their schoolmates that day. When interviewed 1 week later, those children who heard the rumor and then interacted freely with peers made more false reports of the rumored occurrence, were more likely to admit to seeing this nonevent, and described this nonoccurrence in more detail than those who did not have the opportunity to naturally converse with their peers immediately after the rumor. These findings dem- onstrate that the infusion of misinformation into a group has more powerful effects on memory than if the very same information is encountered individually without the opportunity for immediate collab- orative reflection. Further, analysis of the content of children’s narratives revealed that more than one- third of the details reported by those who interacted naturally following the rumor overlapped with something that someone else in their classroom had uttered, whereas those who were denied an opportunity to interact evidenced only a 9% overlap in their reports (most of which were mere verba- tim repetitions of the rumor). This group difference in shared utterances clearly indicates that the nat- ural conversations that occurred in the classrooms immediately following the provision the rumor had a powerful effect in shaping children’s reports one week later. The ease of contamination brought about by socially provided misinformation notwithstanding, a second social factor that might impact the transmission and mnemonic effects of rumored information
  • 29. within a group is the group’s prior history. Supporting this notion are findings in the classical social influence literature demonstrating that the magnitude of conformity escalates with group cohesive- ness (Wren, 1999). Further, experimental work in the rumor literature demonstrates that the trans- mission of unfounded information within a group increases with the level of familiarity between participants, or a desire to establish such a level (Rosnow, 1991). To examine whether familiarity might exacerbate the interfering effects of errant rumor on mem- ory, 3- to 5-year-olds were assigned to one of two conditions that differed in terms of their social history (Principe et al., 2010). Half of the children had been classmates for at least 6 months, whereas the remaining half were complete strangers. We carried out the magic shows in an unfamiliar loca- tion to ensure that the group’s social history rather than the familiarity of the setting drove any group differences in memory. Once the rumor was planted, all of the children engaged in free play for 30 min. As expected, the rumor induced greater memory contamination if it was planted among familiar peers than if it was encountered among strangers. Those children who interacted freely with their pre- school classmates made more false reports of the rumored occurrence, were more likely to claim to have seen this nonevent, and provided more elaborate narratives compared to those children who interacted with unfamiliar peers following the rumor. The familiar children also displayed greater overlap in the content of their errant accounts compared to
  • 30. unfamiliar children, demonstrating that a group’s social history can augment the degree to which narrative details invented during natural interactions later become infused into children’s individual reports. G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 215 Representational changes Given our findings that overhearing an errant rumor—either from an adult or classmates—can lead children to make detailed false reports, we next sought to explore the extent to which such accounts are driven by changes in children’s memory representations that lead to a genuine belief that the ru- mored information was actually seen. The alternative is that children’s errant reports following rumor are driven merely by social demands to relay information they knew they had not experienced but was only suggested. This theoretically significant distinction is also important for discussions of tes- timony. If rumor can bring about errant beliefs about witnessing events that only were overheard, then there may be little that forensic interviewers can do to mitigate rumors’ effects. But if reports of rumored occurrences are merely driven by compliance to social pressures, such effects may be re- duced by protocols that boost the retrieval and reporting of information in memory, such as the Re- vised Cognitive Interview or the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Investigative Interview (see e.g., Lamb, Hershkowitz, Orbach, & Esplin, 2008).
  • 31. To examine this issue, we used a warning manipulation that minimized social pressures to report an overheard rumor about an experienced event and we examined the qualitative characteristics of children’s false narratives prompted by the rumor (Principe, Haines, Adkins, & Guiliano, 2010). Three- to 6-year-olds watched a magic show in their schools and we replicated the Overheard, Classmate, and Control procedures from our rumor original study. When interviewed one week later, half of the Over- heard and Classmate children received a series of emphatic warnings that eliminated social demands to report the rumor by telling them that any information overheard after the show was wrong and therefore should not be reported. Thus if children were able to discern the correct source of the rumor, there were no social pressures to relay it. Demonstrating that compliance to social pressures can lead to false claims, the warning reduced false reports in line with the rumor when it was planted by an adult. However, when the rumor was picked up from peers, the warning decreased false reports among 5- and 6-year-olds, but not 3- and 4-year-olds. The warning, however, had no effect on children’s claims of actually seeing the loose rabbit. That is, when children who reported the loose rabbit were probed for the source of their memory, warned and nonwarned children were equally liked to claim to have seen the loose rabbit with their own eyes (as opposed to merely having heard about it). This pattern suggests that social demands produced at least some false reports of the rumored occurrence, but that demand character- istics had little or nothing to do with children’s claims of seeing
  • 32. the rumor. Considering this evidence that claims of seeing generally were not due to social demands, to what extent did they reflect a genuine belief in having seen the rumored event? Analyses of children’s false narratives provide some insight. Replicating findings of all of our prior rumor studies, those children who recalled seeing a loose rabbit provided more voluminous false narratives consistent with the theme of the rumor than those who did not admit to seeing it. This pattern suggests that when mem- ories for rumored events contain much elaborative detail, children may be prone for mistaking them for real experiences. However, analysis of the content of children’s false narratives indicated that there was more to the story. Those children who did and did not recall seeing the rumored occurrence gen- erated different narrative profiles. Specifically, those children who reported seeing a loose rabbit de- scribed this rumored event with relatively more perceptual (e.g., color and sound) and contextual (e.g., spatial location and temporal order) detail than either those who were unable to determine a source of their false reports or who claimed another source. This distinction is important because according to Johnson and colleagues’ source-monitoring framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993), individuals distinguish the source of information in memory by evaluating certain characteristics of the representation at retrieval. In this view, expe- rienced events are represented with more perceptual, contextual, semantic, and affective information than imagined, suggested, or otherwise nonexperienced events, and the differing profiles of these two
  • 33. classes of memories serve as cues to discriminate their origins. Thus given that perceptual and contex- tual details are characteristic of experienced sources, it may be that the generation of these qualities in representations of the rumor interfered with the usual source- judgment process and consequently led some children to misattribute it as a witnessed event. This interpretation presumes that rumored events can come to be represented similarly to experienced events and that such representational 216 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 changes put children at risk for wrongly judging heard about events as seen. The point is that children who made false claims of seeing might not have been engaging in faulty reasoning about source, but rather were dealing with a memory that was uncharacteristic of its class. This interpretation is in line with other work showing the usually successful source judgment process can go awry when represen- tations of nonexperienced events develop qualities typical of real experiences (e.g., Blandon-Gitlin, Pezdek, Lindsay, & Hagen, 2009). The current results also suggest developmental improvements in children’s vulnerability to make false claims of seeing rumored events. As expected, 3- and 4- year-olds were more likely than 5- and 6- year-olds to recall seeing the rumor. Common explanations of these sorts of heightened source errors among younger children are their immaturities in theory-of- mind understanding, representational ability, and the strategy of using memory characteristics to
  • 34. identify source (see e.g., Sluzenski, New- combe, & Ottinger, 2004). We found, however, more abundant perceptual detail in the false accounts of the younger versus older children. This raises another possibility, namely that young children may be particularly prone to generate perceptual images in response to postevent suggestions and subse- quently infuse them into their representations of experience. Indeed, such a tendency would constrain the efficiency with which source-monitoring processes can operate. We also found that the Classmate children were more suggestible than the Overheard children. In particular, the Classmate children made more frequent reports of seeing and gave more lengthy descriptions of the rumored occurrence relative to the Overheard children. Examination of false nar- ratives indicated that the Classmate children reported proportionately more perceptual and contex- tual detail than the Overheard children. This pattern suggests that what makes rumors picked up from peers particularly potent is that this mode of transmission can lead to an abundance of percep- tual and contextual detail in memory that children are prone to judge as indicative of an authentic witnessed experience. Consistent with current conceptualizations of memory (Ceci, Papierno, & Kulkofsky, 2007), the re- sults of this study highlight the notion that individual and developmental differences in underlying representations play an important role in children’s suggestibility. These data also have implications for children as witnesses. First, the effectiveness of the warning in reducing false claims suggests that
  • 35. legal professionals would be prudent to consider instructions to young witnesses to ignore any heard about information if it is known that a false rumor has been circulating. Second, we found that even though the warning reduced reports of rumored information, it did not affect children’s ability to cor- rectly report the actually experienced portions of the magic show. This finding indicates that legal pro- fessionals may not need to be concerned that warnings will reduce false detail at the expense of true information. We found, however, that the warning was associated with an increase in the provision of constructive details and a corresponding decrease in reports of fantastic details about the rumored occurrence, suggesting that false accounts of rumored-but- nonexperienced events that persist follow- ing a warning might appear particularly believable and compelling. Linking rumor mongering to later false reports In our next study, we sought to investigate directly the content of children’s post-rumor conversa- tions to begin to make claims about which specific qualities of these discussions are linked to later errors in remembering (Principe, Cherson, DiPuppo, & Schindewolf, in press). To do so, we replicated our original magic show procedures and formed three groups: Overheard, Classmate, and Control. To document the transmission of rumored information among peers, we recorded children’s natural con- versations for 20 min following their exposure to the rumor. Children wore a small belt pack with a digital audio recorder and a microphone that attached to their shirts. Children were interviewed at both 1 and 4 weeks following the show. We added this 4-week delay to explore the decay rate of over-
  • 36. heard rumors. Children’s performance at the 1-week interview replicated our earlier results. Nearly all of the Overheard and Classmate children, but none of the Control children, reported that Mumfry’s rabbit was loose. Likewise, many Overheard and Classmate children recalled seeing the alleged loose rabbit and many provided high levels of constructive embellishments in line with the rumor, with the Class- G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 217 mate children being more likely to make claims of seeing and generating more voluminous false ac- counts. Reports of the loose rabbit and recollections of seeing remained high across the 4-week delay. Only the older Overheard children evidenced any decline in the volume of their false narratives over the delay. These patterns indicate that the memory alterations engendered by false rumor do not dis- sipate rapidly but rather reflect more lasting changes in remembering. Examination of children’ natural exchanges with peers in their classrooms following their exposure to the rumor revealed a remarkable amount of dialogue going on among those who heard the rumor directly from the adult as well as those who had picked it up secondhand from their peers. In fact, every child in both groups uttered at least one statement about the alleged loose rabbit, demonstrat- ing that every Overheard and Classmate child encoded the rumor and was actively engaged, albeit in
  • 37. varying degrees, in circulating the rumored information. Analysis of the content of children’s natural talk in their classrooms showed that they did not merely stick to propagating the rumor verbatim to peers. Nor was there much fantastic talk in the post-rumor dialogues, suggesting that the children generally did not interpret the rumor as an invita- tion to engage in pretense. Most of the information transmitted was made up of constructive utter- ances in line with the theme of rumor but above and beyond its literal content. Thus the children were inventing and sharing new details that generally were believable. Considering findings in the adult literature that believability is necessary for rumors to be spread readily and widely (see Rosnow, 1991), the constructive nature of the information generated and circulated in the classrooms likely boosted the influence of the rumor on children’s subsequent remembering. Next we explored the degree to which things said in the interview originated in the classroom on the day that the rumor was spread. At the 1-week interview, 20% of the details relayed about the ru- mored loose rabbit overlapped with what they themselves had uttered in the classroom. Thirty-two percent of their narrative reports during the interview originated in their classmates’ transmissions. At the 4-week interview, 13% of children’s false reports overlapped with their own transmissions in the classroom, and 23% overlapped with their classmates contributions. These findings provide direct evidence that the very narrative details invented and circulated among children following their expo- sure to the rumor intruded into their own and their peers’
  • 38. subsequent individual accounts of the event out to a 4-week delay. Thus the children were not merely fabricating constructions about the rumored occurrence on the fly during the interview but also were remembering a good deal of the transmis- sions that had originated one or four weeks earlier in the natural dialogues on the day the rumor was planted. Further, these findings of the ready infusion of the content of peer dialogues into later memory provides some insight into why the effects of postevent misinformation are exacerbated when it is encountered in a group rather than individually without the opportunity for co-witness exchange. Consistent with our earlier findings, the rumor was more damaging to memory when it was gleaned from agemates than when it was overheard from an adult. At both interviews, the Classmate children made more frequent reports of seeing the rumored occurrence and they offered more lengthy false narratives than the Overheard children. Further, among those children who erroneously reported the rumored event at both interviews, the Classmate children evidenced no decline in narrative detail across the 4-week delay. The older Overheard children, however, displayed a drop in elaborative detail from the 1-week to the 4-week interview. In line with findings that the Overheard and Classmate children remembered differently, compar- ison of their post-rumor talk revealed that the Classmate children also talked differently than the Overheard children about the alleged occurrence on the day the rumor was planted. Compared to the Overheard children, the Classmate children uttered more
  • 39. original transmissions. Both groups of children improvised quite freely—overall 58% of children’s utterances in the classroom were novel (i.e., no other child in the classroom had yet uttered), but the Classmate children relayed more than twice as many original transmissions as their Overheard peers, demonstrating that they engaged in much more inventive rumor mongering than their agemates. Further, the Classmate children also were more affected than the Overheard children by what went on in the classrooms on the day the rumor was planted. At both the 1- and 4-week interviews, the Classmate children evidenced a greater overlap than the Overheard children between things they themselves had said as well as things their classmates had uttered on the day the rumor was planted and their subsequent interview reports. 218 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 Likewise, at both interviews, the Overhead children relayed higher levels of nonoverlapping informa- tion, suggesting that they were more likely than the Classmate children to fabricate their interview reports on the fly. These findings of heightened peer-suggestibility are at odds with the typical finding in the suggest- ibility literature that children are more easily misled by adults than peers (e.g., Ceci, Toglia, & Ross, 1990). An important contrast between the usual suggestibility study and the current work concerns the differing manners by which suggestions were delivered by peers. In typical suggestibility research,
  • 40. children make scripted suggestions during formal interviews, whereas in our paradigm, familiar chil- dren transmitted freely varying suggestions—without being instructed to do so—during the course of naturally occurring interactions. This contrast suggests that the naturalness of the reception context might boost the impact of misinformation. Consistent with this interpretation are findings in the ru- mor literature that rumors are more readily spread when picked up from a peer than an authority fig- ure (Jaeger, Anthony, & Rosnow, 1980), with higher degrees of propagation within an individual’s own social group (Almirol, 1981). Also supporting this explanation is the finding that the Overheard chil- dren generally did not constrain themselves to stay true to the information transmitted by a presumed authority figure; rather their propagations in the classrooms were highly inventive. Thus, it may be the improvised nature of the Overheard children’s original transmissions and the Classmate children’s subsequent tendencies to do more constructive spinning of the rumor that made peer suggestions par- ticularly powerful. In line with prior findings, the older children provided more errant narrative detail at both inter- views than the younger children. We also found that the 5- and 6-year-olds were the master rumor propagators in the classroom. Compared to the 3- and 4-year- olds, they generated more constructive utterances and improvised more original transmissions about the rumored occurrence. This deeper and more inventive participation in rumor exchanges with their peers suggests that the older children simply had created for themselves more opportunities than the younger children to be influenced by
  • 41. what went on in the classrooms. Supporting this notion is that the older children provided more inter- view details that overlapped with their own utterances (1-week interview) and their classmates’ utterances (1- and 4-week interviews) in the classroom than the younger children. Given that fact finders tend to judge detail as an index of accuracy, these findings suggest that when conditions are ripe for rumor mongering, older children may be more prone to construct false narratives that are more compelling or believable than those produced by younger children. Maternal suggestibility and memory sharing style These findings demonstrating the mnemonic effects of peer discussions following misinformation notwithstanding, little is known about how conversations with parents who have been exposed to misleading information might affect memory. It is well known that young children rely heavily on their parents to help them frame and guide their formulations of experience. But what happens when parents have preexisting beliefs about what has happened to their children? Both case study (see Ceci & Bruck, 1995) and empirical investigations (White, Leichtman, & Ceci, 1997) show that when inter- viewers believe unfounded allegations to be true, they are prone to shape their interviews with chil- dren to elicit untrue statements consistent with their extant beliefs and consequently put children at increase risk of making false claims in line the unfounded beliefs. Little is known, however, about whether parents’ false beliefs might similarly drive children’s accounts. Understanding the influence of parents’ beliefs also has
  • 42. implications for discussions of the testi- mony of young witnesses. Consider, for instance, the case of Lillie and Reed v Newcastle City Council & Ors (see Bruck, Ceci, & Principe, 2006) in which, on the basis of a 2-year-olds’ uncorroborated alle- gations, parents of children in a daycare center were told that a child in the center may have been sex- ually abused and then were asked to talk with their children about the possible abuse. Similarly, in the McMartin Preschool case in Manhattan Beach, California, prompted by a mother’s allegation that her son had been sexually molested by a school aid, Police Chief Harry Kuhlmeyer sent a letter to nearly 200 mothers whose children attended the school (see Ceci & Bruck, 1995). The letter urged mothers to question their children and suggested events that might have taken place: ‘‘Please question your child G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 219 to see if he or she has been a witness to any crime or if he or she has been a victim. Our investigation indicates that possible criminal acts include: oral sex, fondling of genitals, buttock or chest area, and sodomy.’’ Given that parents in both of these cases were told that abuse was possible, some may have believed in the defendants’ guilt and inadvertently molded their memory discussions with their chil- dren to elicit accounts in line with their beliefs. To explore whether discussions with misinformed parents might affect children’s later memory, we had children watch our usual magic show and then gave their mothers a letter that asked them to nat-
  • 43. urally discuss the show with their children (Principe, DiPuppo, & Gammel, 2011). In the letters of half of these mothers, those in the Suggestive Letter condition, was a suggestion that Mumfry’s rabbit may or may not have gotten loose in the school on the day of the magic show. The letters of mothers in the Neutral Letter condition was identical except for the suggestion. Both sets of mothers were asked to hold their discussion on the morning of our interview which took place 1-week after the show. We gave mothers a digital recorder to record their conversations. A third group, the Control condition, nei- ther received the suggestion nor were they asked to talk about the event with their children. Results indicated that the letter was a potent form of suggestion. During the interview, the Sugges- tive Letter children were more likely to wrongly report a loose rabbit and to claim to have seen this rumored-but-nonconcurring event than the Neutral or Control children, providing original evidence that parents can be a natural source of report contamination when they have been misinformed. Ear- lier work has shown that parents can be a source of memory error when they suggest experimenter- provided scripted nonoccurrences (e.g., Poole & Lindsay, 2002), but this is the first study to demon- strate that misinformation encountered by parents can leak into children’s later accounts when they are not asked to suggest the nonevent to their children but merely to talk with their children in a nat- ural manner. Admittedly, the current suggestibility manipulation was rather weak compared to the more direct and coercive forms used in most of the literature (e.g., Bruck et
  • 44. al., 2002). Nonetheless, children’s error rates during the interview in the current study are comparable to those in research on suggestibility that use more direct forms (60% of the Suggestive Letter children claimed that the rabbit was loose, compared to less than 10% in the other two groups). Not only was a substantial proportion of chil- dren’s false reports at the open-ended level of questioning (20%), their errors also were accompanied by a fair degree of elaborative detail in line with the theme of the suggestion to their mothers. Given the well-known distinction in style that mothers take on when remembering with their chil- dren, namely high versus low elaborative (see Nelson and Fivush (2004) for a review), we next sought to explore whether the influence of our misinformation manipulation varied as a function of maternal style. This issue was of interest because maternal style influences how children represent and remem- ber the past. High-elaborative mothers scaffold children’s accounts by asking many –wh questions and encourage extended narratives by providing more and more memory information with each succeed- ing question. Low-elaborative mothers in contrast, provide less structure and ask their children few and redundant questions about the past. Despite much evidence that children of high-elaborative mothers tend to relay the past in a more elaborated and coherent manner than children whose moth- ers use a low elaborative style, unknown is whether maternal style might affect children’s remember- ing when mothers have been exposed to misinformation. Given the tendencies of high elaborative mothers to provide new memory information to aid their children’s recollections, when these mothers
  • 45. hold false beliefs about their children’s experiences they may be more likely than low elaborative mothers to weave their own beliefs into the ongoing co- constructed narrative and consequently more greatly affect their children’s later accounts. In line with the prediction that children of high-elaborative mothers tend to display more sophis- ticated autobiographical memory skills, we found that these children, compared to those of low-elab- orative mothers, provided more detailed narratives of the actually experienced portions of the magic show and were less likely to make errors when describing these actual experiences. Despite these po- sitive associations between maternal elaborativeness and children’s recall of experienced events, we found that children with high elaborative mothers were twice as likely as those with low elaborative mothers to wrongly report a loose rabbit. Likewise, of those who made a false report, children whose mothers displayed a high elaborative style described the suggested nonevent in more narrative detail than those whose mothers talked in a low elaborative fashion. Thus, these results demonstrate that 220 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 the high elaborative style usually associated with more skilled autobiographical remembering is also linked with increases in children’s memory error when mothers are exposed to misinformation about their children’s experiences. Interestingly, there were no differences in the qualities of
  • 46. mothers’ conversations as a function of experimental group, suggesting that misinformation exposure did not affect maternal style. However, examination of mothers’ contributions during the mother–child conversation suggests that maternal style affected how mothers used the misinformation in their discussions with their children. In partic- ular, high elaborative mothers provided more than three times the elaborations consistent with the theme of the suggestion than those who were low elaborative, indicating that after exposure to the misinformation the high elaborative mothers created a more coercive memory sharing environment than the low elaborative mothers. Importantly, these mothers were not instructed to provide elabo- rations consistent with the suggestion or offer new information if their children were not remember- ing a loose rabbit. Rather, they were merely instructed to ask their children if the magician’s rabbit had gotten loose. These findings suggest that mothers with a high elaborative predisposition often went above and beyond a simple ‘‘Was the rabbit loose?’’ question and offered new and necessarily false details describing the suggestion. Interestingly, there were no differences in the extent to which low- versus high-elaborative mothers believed the loose rabbit suggestion to be true, suggesting that maternal style may override any influence of belief on how mothers question their children about the past. Conclusion and implications This collection of studies demonstrates the importance of considering the mnemonic consequences of natural conversational interactions for discussions of children’s memory and suggestibility. Our
  • 47. findings show that not only can conversations with co-witnesses be a potent source of errors in auto- biographical remembering, but that interactions with others can lead to false reports even when they have not witnessed the event in question but merely have been exposed to misinformation about it. These investigations also demonstrate several conditions under which the potential of natural conver- sations to taint memory are exacerbated. Memory sharing conversations with familiar peers as op- posed to strangers more readily shape subsequent memory, and discussions about the past with high-elaborative mothers exhibit greater influence than those with low-elaborative mothers. Also, the nature of the shared false information can impact its mnemonic effects. With age, children develop some resistance to being misled by conversationally conveyed misinformation when it conflicts with their actual experiences, but they also become more likely to generate their own causal inferences about events that can put themselves as well as their peers at increased risk of wrongly recollecting occurrences that were merely inferred. Researchers interested in children’s memory generally have overlooked peers as a source of error. Our findings of high levels of peer-generated suggestibility, however, underscore the importance of a continuing examination of peers. Importantly, none of the children in our studies were asked to invent or share information with their classmates. Rather they naturally generated and spread misinforma- tion in such a manner that it that intruded into their own and their peers’ subsequent autobiograph- ical accounts. In fact, on many measures of memory, misinformation picked up from peers was more
  • 48. mnemonically damaging than suggestions planted by adults. Indeed, inspection of the content of chil- dren’s false accounts suggests that what might make peer transmitted misinformation particularly po- tent is that it leads to the generation of perceptual and contextual images in memory that children wrongly judge as indicative of an authentic experience. Future work is needed to more fully explore this interpretation and the sorts of conversational exchanges that can prompt the generation of high levels of nonexperienced perceptual and contextual detail and consequent reports of seeing. Nonethe- less, our examination of children’s classroom conversations suggests that these representational dif- ferences as a function of informant might be linked to differences in the ways that children talk with others following the reception of misinformation from peers versus an adult. Specifically, our findings suggest that children who glean false information from peers engage in deeper and more inventive rumor mongering than those who overhear the very same information from an adult. Final- G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 221 ly, our findings of elevated errors with familiar peers as opposed to strangers show that social history is also an important variable in understanding conversational suggestibility. Consequently, an impor- tant question for future work is how fast and over what course does familiarity augment the potential for memory contamination. Admittedly, an exploration of conversational processes in
  • 49. remembering has not been completely off the radar screen for investigators exploring children’s autobiographical memory. Developmental researchers have long explored the effects of mothers’ memory sharing style on children’s event nar- ratives. Accuracy for a specific event, however, has rarely been a dependent variable in this line of work. Our findings not only show that mothers can be a potent source of errors in remembering, but that high maternal elaborativeness—a quality associated with increases in children’s memory and narrative skills—is linked with decreases in accuracy when mothers have been exposed to misin- formation about their children’s experiences. Given that this link is correlational, an important next step is to carry out experimental studies of maternal style to bolster the causal argument. Also of inter- est in future work is exploring in a more fine-grained manner how the content of mother’s elaborative statements and questions influence children’s independent remembering. This sort of analysis would deepen our understanding of how a high elaborative style can both help and hinder children’s memory when mothers and their children disagree on the facts of a past event. Indeed, mother–child negoti- ation is a critical context for children’s developing abilities to represent and recollect their experi- ences, but a closer look at such conversations may provide some insight into how memory exchanges teach young children that others can represent the past differently and that memory is a subjective, dynamic process rather than a static product. Our work also holds some applied relevance to legal settings involving young witnesses as it re- veals powerful sources of memory error not readily eliminated
  • 50. by common techniques used to min- imize reporting errors in forensic interviews (e.g., exclusive nonsuggestive questioning, videotaping interviews). Caution, of course, is required in generalizing our findings to situations involving child witnesses (e.g., our to-be-remembered events are enjoyable, children are not pressured to make false reports, interviewers are not rewarded for eliciting certain claims). Nonetheless, our data provides clear evidence that even when children are interviewed in a supportive and neutral manner, misinfor- mation from peers and parents can contaminate their reports. In fact, because the level of peer influ- ence in our experimental paradigms is likely pale compared to that which occurs in group therapy sessions or joint police interviews, our findings underscore the importance of considering the poten- tial contaminating influence of peers in legal cases involving multiple child witnesses. Likewise, our findings suggest that exposing mothers to unfounded claims, such as Police Chief Harry Kuhlmeyer did in the McMartin case described above, might engender false allegations, especially if mothers talk with their children in a high elaborative manner. As such, experimental studies that suggest nonoccur- ring experiences to mothers may be relevant for forensic situations where unfounded allegations emerge and families are asked to play a role in diagnosing the truthfulness of such allegations in their own children. Further, the current body of work demonstrates that the tendencies of judges and juries to use cer- tain qualities of children’s testimony, such as corroboration, spontaneity, elaborativeness, and consis- tency, as markers of accuracy are unwarranted. For instance, the
  • 51. near ceiling levels of reports of rumored-but-nonoccurring events in our studies show that corroboration can occur even when none of the witnesses are accurate. Second, children’s spontaneous and elaboratively detailed accounts of nonevents following rumor provide compelling evidence that these qualities of children’s accounts are not reliable indexes for gauging accuracy. Third, the high levels of consistency in reports of the ru- mored event across a 4-week delay make it clear that consistency across interviews is not diagnostic of truthfulness. In summary, this collection of findings provides unique insight into why memory sharing conver- sations can be such a potent source of children’s report contamination. Not only do children and par- ents naturally share misinformation in conversations about the past, but they also improvise embellishments that go above and beyond the literal suggestions—a tendency that seems to put them- selves and their conversational partners at risk for representational changes and consequent memory errors. Further, certain qualities of the informer, such as age, familiarity, and conversational style, can affect the nature of misinformation delivery in such a manner that it incites the receiver in to engage 222 G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 in a more or less co-constructive spinning of the past that has implications for later accuracy. Taken together, these patterns show that it is not misinformation per se that affects children’s autobiograph-
  • 52. ical remembering, but rather how it is encountered and shared with others. Acknowledgment This research was supported by Grants MH12619 and MH076811 from the National Institutes of Health. References Almirol, E. B. (1981). Chasing the elusive butterfly: Gossip and the pursuit of reputation. Ethnicity, 8, 293–304. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press. Blandon-Gitlin, I., Pezdek, K., Lindsay, D. S., & Hagen, L. (2009). Criteria-based content analysis of true and suggested accounts of events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 901–917. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1995). Autosuggestibility in memory development. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 65–101. Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (2002). The nature of children’s true and false narratives. Developmental Review, 22, 520–554. Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Principe, G. F. (2006). The child and the law. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.), & K. A. Renniger & I. E. Sigel (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4). Child psychology in practice (6th ed., pp. 776–816). New York: Wiley. Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • 53. Ceci, S. J., Kulkofsky, S., Klemfuss, J. Z., Sweeney, C. D., & Bruck, M. (2007). Unwarranted assumptions about children’s testimonial accuracy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 311–328. Ceci, S. J., Papierno, P. B., & Kulkofsky, S. (2007). Representational constraints on children’s suggestibility. Psychological Science, 18, 503–509. Ceci, S. J., Toglia, M., & Ross, D. (1990). The suggestibility of preschoolers’ recollections: Historical perspectives on current problems. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 285–300). New York: Cambridge. Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memory for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533–543. Garven, S., Wood, J. M., Malpass, R., & Shaw, J. S. (1998). More than suggestion: consequences of the interviewing techniques from the McMartin preschool case. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 347–359. Gilbert, D. T. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 121–138. Hannigan, S. L., & Reinitz, M. T. (2001). A demonstration and comparison of two types of inference-based memory errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 931–940.
  • 54. Harris, C. B., Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2008). Collaborative recall and collective memory: What happens when we remember together? Memory, 16, 213–230. Hirst, W., & Manier, D. (2008). Towards a psychology of collective memory. Memory, 16, 183–200. Jaeger, M. E., Anthony, S., & Rosnow, R. L. (1980). Who hears what from whom and with what effect: A study of rumor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 473–478. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28. Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, I., Orbach, Y., & Esplin, P. W. (2008). Tell me what happened: Structured investigative interviews of child victims and witnesses. West Sussex, England: Wiley. Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1995). The effects of stereotypes and suggestions on preschoolers’ reports. Developmental Psychology, 31, 568–578. Lindsay, D. S., Johnson, M. K., & Kwon, P. (1991). Developmental changes in source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52, 297–318. Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. III, (2002). Explorations in the social contagion of memory. Memory & Cognition, 30, 995– 1009. Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 7–14. Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory.
  • 55. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511. Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006a). Co-witnesses talk: A survey of eyewitness discussion. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12, 181–191. Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006b). Comparing methods of encountering post-event information: The power of co-witness suggestion. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1083–1099. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Poole, D., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). Reducing child witnesses’ false reports of misinformation from parents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 117–140. Principe, G. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). ‘‘I saw it with my own ears’’: The influence of peer conversations and suggestive questions on preschoolers’ event memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 1–25. Principe, G. F., Daley, L., & Kauth, K. (2010). Social processes affecting the mnemonic consequences of rumors on children’s memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 479– 493. Principe, G. F., Cherson, M., DiPuppo, J., & Schindewolf, E. (in press). Children’s natural conversations following exposure to a rumor: Linkages to later false reports. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
  • 56. G.F. Principe, E. Schindewolf / Developmental Review 32 (2012) 205–223 223 Principe, G. F., DiPuppo, J., & Gammel, J. (2011). Leaking misinformation to mothers: Effects of maternal conversational style on young children’s suggestibility for a past event. Poster presented at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Principe, G. F., Guiliano, S., & Root, C. (2008). Rumormongering and remembering: How rumors originating in children’s inferences can affect memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 135–155. Principe, G. F., Haines, B., Adkins, A., & Guiliano, S. (2010). False rumors and true belief: Memory processes underlying children’s errant reports of rumored events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 407–422. Principe, G. F., Kanaya, T., Ceci, S. J., & Singh, M. (2006). Believing is seeing: How rumors can engender false memories in preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 243–248. Principe, G. F., Ornstein, P. A., Baker-ward, L., & Gordon, B. N. (2000). The effects of intervening experiences on children’s memory for a physical examination. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 59–80. Principe, G. F., & Smith, E. (2007). The tooth, the whole tooth, and nothing but the tooth: How belief in the Tooth Fairy can engender false memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1– 18.
  • 57. Principe, G. F., Tinguely, A., & Dobkowski, N. (2007). Mixing memories: The effects of rumors that conflict with children’s experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 1– 19. Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1989). Children’s memory and proximity to violence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 236–241. Rabinowitz, D. (2004). No crueler tyrannies: Accusation, false witness, and other terrors of our times. New York: Free Press. Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (2008). The development of collective remembering. Memory, 16, 201–212. Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Inside rumor: A personal journey. American Psychologist, 46, 484–496. Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N. S., & Ottinger, W. (2004). Changes in reality monitoring and episodic memory in early childhood. Developmental Science, 7, 225–245. Sophian, C., & Huber, A. (1984). Early developments in children’s causal judgments. Child Development, 55, 512–526. Virj, A., Pannell, H., & Ost, J. (2005). The influence of social pressure and black clothing on crime judgments. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 11, 265–274. White, T. L., Leichtman, M. D., & Ceci, S. J. (1997). The good, the bad, and the ugly: Accuracy, inaccuracy, and elaboration in preschoolers’ reports about a past event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, S37–S54. Wren, K. (1999). Social influences. New York: Routledge. Natural conversations as a source of false memories in children:
  • 58. Implications for the testimony of young witnessesIntroductionCo-witness influenceRumor mongering and rememberingConflicting rumorsChild-generated suggestibilitySocial processesRepresentational changesLinking rumor mongering to later false reportsMaternal suggestibility and memory sharing styleConclusion and implicationsAcknowledgmentReferences Chapter 9: Patient Safety, Quality and Value Harry Burke MD PhD Learning Objectives After reviewing the presentation, viewers should be able to: Define safety, quality, near miss, and unsafe action List the safety and quality factors that justified the clinical implementation of electronic health record systems Discuss three reasons why the electronic health record is central to safety, quality, and value List three issues that clinicians have with the current electronic
  • 59. health record systems and discuss how these problems affect safety and quality Describe a specific electronic patient safety measurement system and a specific electronic safety reporting system Describe two integrated clinical decision support systems and discuss how they may improve safety and quality Patient Safety-Related Definitions Safety: minimization of the risk and occurrence of patient harm events Harm: inappropriate or avoidable psychological or physical injury to patient and/or family Adverse Events: “an injury resulting from a medical intervention” Preventable Adverse Events: “errors that result in an adverse event that are preventable” Overuse: “the delivery of care of little or no value” e.g. widespread use of antibiotics for viral infections Underuse: “the failure to deliver appropriate care” e.g. vaccines or cancer screening Misuse: “the use of certain services in situations where they are not clinically indicated” e.g. MRI for routine low back pain
  • 60. Introduction Medical errors are unfortunately common in healthcare, in spite of sophisticated hospitals and well trained clinicians Often it is breakdowns in protocol and communication, and not individual errors Technology has potential to reduce medical errors (particularly medication errors) by: Improving communication between physicians and patients Improving clinical decision support Decreasing diagnostic errors Unfortunately, technology also has the potential to create unique new errors that cause harm Medical Errors Errors can be related to diagnosis, treatment and preventive care. Furthermore, medical errors can be errors of commission or omission and fortunately not all errors result in an injury and not all medical errors are preventable Most common outpatient errors: Prescribing medications Getting the correct laboratory test for the correct patient at the
  • 61. correct time Filing system errors Dispensing medications and responding to abnormal test results 5 While many would argue that treatment errors are the most common category of medical errors, diagnostic errors accounted for the largest percentage of malpractice claims, surpassing treatment errors in one study Diagnostic errors can result from missed, wrong or delayed diagnoses and are more likely in the outpatient setting. This is somewhat surprising given the fact that US physicians tend to practice “defensive medicine” Over-diagnosis may also cause medical errors but this has been less well studied Medical Errors Unsafe healthcare lowers quality but safe medicine is not
  • 62. always high quality From the National Academy of Medicine’s perspective, quality is a set of six aspirational goals: medical care should be safe, effective, timely, efficient, patient-centered, and equitable Value relates to how important something is to use Cost-effective? Necessary? Affect morbidity, mortality or quality of life? Quality, Safety and Value Most adverse events result from unsafe actions or inactions by anyone on the healthcare team, including the patient Missed care is “any aspect of required care that is omitted either in part or in whole or delayed” Many of the above go unreported Unsafe Actions Most near-miss events are not reported. Many are not witnessed
  • 63. The tendency is the blame the individual, but healthcare is complex and there are often “system errors” Most safety systems are retrospective; we need to move to be proactive We need good data, such as the ratio of detected unsafe actions divided by the opportunity of an unsafe action, over a specified time interval Reporting Unsafe Actions 9 Patient Safety Reporting System: event is recorded and if it is a sentinel event, it is investigated. Most systems are not integrated with the EHR Root Cause Analysis: common approach to determine the cause of an adverse event. This has limitations HEDIS measures can help track quality issues Patient Safety Systems
  • 64. Current reimbursement models mandate quality measures, e.g. Medicare Patient Safety Monitoring System, now operated by AHRQ. The new system is known as the Quality and Safety Review System. Still labor intensive and manual Global Trigger Tool: evaluates hospital safety. Said to detect 90% of adverse events. Select 10 discharge records and two reviewers review the chart for any of the 53 “triggers” Patient Safety Systems Paper records have multiple disadvantages, as pointed out in the EHR chapter Expectations have been very high regarding the EHR’s impact on safety, quality and value Unfortunately, results have been mixed and there has not been a prospective study conducted to prove the EHR’s benefit towards safety and quality Using the EHR to Improve Safety, Quality and Value
  • 65. High expectations that CDS that is part of EHRs will improve safety As per multiple chapters in the textbook, CDS has mixed reviews, in terms of safety and quality Adverse events regarding CDS, includes ”alert fatigue” The FDA will regulate software that is related to treatment and decision making Clinical Decision Support Results in altered workflow and decreased efficiency. Physicians are staying late to complete notes in the EHR In an effort to save time physicians may “cut and paste” old histories into the EHR, creating new problems EHRs may create new safety issues “e-iatrogenesis” Because of the multiple issues, it is very common to see offices and hospitals change EHRs, not always solving the problem Clinician’s Issues with EHRs
  • 66. Roughly 2/3 of EHR data is unstructured (free text) so it is not computable. While natural language processing (NLP) may help solve this, we are a long ways away from resolution Multiple open source and commercial NLP programs exist but they require a great deal of time and expertise to match the results a manual chart review would produce Clinician’s Issues with EHRs Governmental Organizations Involved with Patient Safety US Federal Agencies: Department of Health and Human Services (HHS) Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) Non-reimbursable complications: (3 examples) Objects left in a patient during surgery and blood incompatibility Catheter-associated urinary tract infections Pressure ulcers (bed sores) Hospitals must assemble, analyze and trend clinical and administrative data to capture baseline data and measure improvement over time Health IT-based interventions are expected to assist
  • 67. Governmental Organizations Office of the National Coordinator for HIT Learn: “Increase the quantity and quality of data and knowledge about health IT safety.” Improve: “Target resources and corrective actions to improve health IT safety and patient safety” Safety goals will be aligned with meaningful use objectives. Lead: “Promote a culture of safety related to health IT” Governmental Organizations The Food and Drug Administration MedWatch: posts drug alerts and offers online reporting area Center for Devices and Radiological Health (CDRH) Plan to regulate mobile medical applications designed for use on smartphones State Patient Safety Programs: By 2010, 27 states and the District of Columbia passed legislation or regulation related to hospital reporting of adverse events to a state agency
  • 68. Meaningful Use Objectives and Potential Impact on Patient Safety Objective: Use computerized provider order entry (CPOE) for medication, laboratory, and radiology orders directly entered by any licensed healthcare professional who can enter orders into the medical record per state, local, and professional guidelines Objective: Use clinical decision support to improve performance on high-priority health conditions Meaningful Use Objectives and Potential Impact on Patient Safety Objective: Automatically track medications from order to administration using assistive technologies in conjunction with an electronic medication administration record (eMAR) Objective: Generate and transmit discharge prescriptions electronically (eRx)