SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 32
BRIEF PRACTICE
Sibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote Play-Based
Statements
of Children with Autism
Jessica S. Akers1,2 & Thomas S. Higbee1
& Joy S. Pollard1,3
& Kassidy S. Reinert1
Published online: 25 May 2018
# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2018
Abstract
We trained three typically developing children to implement a
script-fading procedure with their younger siblings with autism.
The number of contextually appropriate statements made by the
children with autism increased once treatment was initiated.
Participants continued to emit higher levels of contextually
appropriate statements after the scripts were completely faded
and at a
4- or 11-week follow-up. The typically developing siblings were
able to implement the script-fading procedure with high levels
of fidelity.
Keywords Script fading . Autism . Sibling . Play . Language
One of the diagnostic criteria of autism spectrum disorder
(ASD) is deficit in the area of social communication (DSM-
5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Script fad-
ing is a procedure that has been shown to increase the number
of appropriate vocalizations emitted by children with ASD
(Krantz & McClannahan, 1993). Script fading consists of
the following steps: (a) first, children are taught to emit the
scripted phrase using a textual (e.g., typed) or auditory (e.g.,
recorder) script and (b) the script is systematically faded such
that the child with ASD continues to emit the scripted phrase
in the absence of the script. Once script fading is initiated,
children often emit untaught phrases in addition to those that
were directly taught. While script fading has been shown to be
effective, researchers or instructors have served as
implementers in the majority of studies (Akers, Pyle,
Higbee, Pyle, & Gerencser, 2016). One notable exception is
the implementation of script fading by parents (Reagon &
Higbee, 2009). The parents in this study developed three
scripted statements and systematically faded the scripted state-
ments based on their child’s performance during play sessions.
The scripted statements (e.g., “Look, the car is going!”) were
related to one toy set, and two other toy sets were used to
assess for generalization. Results showed that play initiations
increased with both the target and generalization toy sets.
The results reported by Reagon and Higbee (2009) are
promising; however, it may be important to identify if
these results could be replicated with a more age appro-
priate play partner. For many children with ASD, a com-
mon play partner might be a typically developing sibling.
Given the effectiveness of script-fading procedures with
parents delivering and fading scripts, we sought to sys-
tematically replicate these results with siblings serving as
play partners. We measured the effects of the script fading
procedure on the number of contextually appropriate
statements made by children with ASD.
Method
Participants and Setting
Three children with ASD, who had previously attended or
were currently attending a university-based behavioral
Implications for practice
•Young children with ASD often do not emit appropriate play
statements
while playing with toys.
• Script fading is an effective intervention for teaching children
with ASD
to emit play-based statements.
• Typically developing siblings can implement script fading
with fidelity.
* Jessica S. Akers
[email protected]
1 Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA
2 Present address: Baylor University, Waco, TX, USA
3 Present address: Behavior Change Institute, Oakland, CA,
USA
Behavior Analysis in Practice (2018) 11:395–399
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-018-0257-5
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s40617-018-
0257-5&domain=pdf
mailto:[email protected]
preschool, participated in the study along with a typically
developing sibling. We recruited participants who (a) could
emit at least three-word phrases, (b) had a generalized imita-
tion repertoire, (c) engaged in low levels of destructive behav-
ior, and (d) played with toys appropriately but rarely
commented during play. Sadie (5) participated with her sister
Melissa (14), Cameron (7) participated with his brother
Landon (10), and Hank (4) participated with his sister
Mandy (6). A parent of each participant served as a research
assistant for sessions. We conducted sessions in participants’
homes in an open area that was cleared of distracting items.
Materials
Each sibling dyad was assigned three toy sets to interact with
during sessions (see Table 1). These toy sets were purchased
by the researcher and access was restricted outside of sessions.
We designated one toy set to be used for the script fading
intervention (hereafter called the “target toy”) and used the
other two toy sets to assess generalization. The parents devel-
oped three 3–4-word scripts which were recorded onMini-Me
™ voice recorders. Siblings wore MotivAider timers to signal
30-s intervals.
Measurement
Parents transcribed all statements made by participants during
sessions, and the transcription was then reviewed by the re-
searcher to total the number of contextually appropriate state-
ments. Statements were not scored if they were (a) not con-
textually appropriate (e.g., “I need gas,” when playing with
the playground), (b) one-word statements, (c) immediate rep-
etitions of statements, (d) excessive repetitions of statements,
defined as using the same statement more than four times, (f)
stereotypic phrases, individually identified for each participant
(e.g., “good job”), or (g) completely unintelligible.
A second coder collected data for 33% of sessions to assess
interobserver agreement (IOA). The second coder transcribed
and recorded the number of comments via recorded video.
IOA was calculated by dividing the number of agreements
by the number of agreements plus disagreements and
converting the result to a percentage. An agreement was de-
fined as both coders (i.e., the first and fourth authors) record-
ing the same statement as contextually appropriate. Mean
agreement was 95% (range from 75 to 100%) for Sadie,
96% (range from 75 to 100%) for Cameron, and 97% (range
from 50 to 100%) for Hank. During the session with 50%
agreement, Hank only made two comments; therefore, one
disagreement leads to this low percentage.
We assessed treatment fidelity for 50% of sessions. We
assessed for the following components, whether the sibling
(a) oriented to the participant, (b) presented a script every
30 s, (c) responded to the participant’s initiations, (d) used
the prompting procedure (described below), (e) did not ask
questions or provide directions, and (f) only made comments
about his/her own behavior. Treatment fidelity was calculated
by dividing the number of correctly implemented components
by the total number of components and converting the result to
a percentage. Mean fidelity of implementation was 93%
(range from 87 to 100%) for Sadie, 93% (range from 82 to
100%) for Cameron, and 88% (range from 62 to 100%) for
Hank.
Experimental Procedures
An adapted alternating treatment design embedded within a
multiple baseline design across participants was used to assess
the effects of the script-fading procedure on the number of
comments emitted by the participants. Prior to the study, a
brief multiple-stimulus without replacement preference as-
sessment (Carr, Nicolson, & Higbee, 2000) was conducted
to identify the top three toy sets.
Pretraining We trained the siblings to implement the
script-fading procedure using Behavioral Skills Training
(BST). Parents served as the role-play partner and provided
Table 1 Session materials
Toy sets Scripted statements
Sadie Fisher-Price Little People® Playground (target) 1. Go
down slide
Fisher-Price Little People® Happy Sounds Home (GS1) 2. I
want swing
Fisher-Price Little People® Wheelies Airport (GS2) 3. Cross
the bridge
Cameron Fisher Price World of Little People® Emergency Fire
Station (target) 1. Go get the hose
Fisher-Price Little People® Playground (GS1) 2. To the rescue
Fisher-Price Little People® Wheelies Airport (GS2) 3. Let us
save them
Hank Hot Wheels® City Tow and Tune Car Shop Play Set
(target) 1. Fix the car
Play-Doh® Brick Mill and Grinding Gravel Yard (GS1) 2.
Down the hill
Vtech® Go! Go! Smart Wheels Airport Playset (GS2) 3. Pick up
car
Target target toy set, GS generalization toy set
396 Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399
feedback to the sibling in conjunction with feedback pro-
vided by the researcher. We specifically trained the sib-
lings to (a) orient to the participant during play, (b) refrain
from asking questions or giving directions, (c) respond to
all of the participant’s verbalizations, and (d) to comment
on their own play actions. Siblings were instructed to pres-
ent an auditory script every 30 s and wait for participant to
emit the scripted phrase. If he or she did not repeat the
scripted phrase, the sibling first physically guided him or
her to press the voice recorder button. If this prompt was
ineffective, the sibling provided a verbal prompt (e.g.,
“say, here comes the car”). Training ended when the sib-
ling correctly implemented each component with their
parent with 95% or better accuracy. During pretraining,
the most common error was failing to respond to com-
ments made by the parent. The siblings met mastery after
one session that took approximately 30 min.
Baseline and Generalization Sessions We conducted three
3-min sessions per day (i.e., one for each toy set). Prior to each
session, the parent cleared the environment to remove possible
distractions and provided the sibling with the appropriate ma-
terials. The researcher video recorded sessions while the par-
ent simultaneously transcribed statements. The sibling began
sessions by saying, “let us play.” Throughout the sessions, the
siblings responded to all comments emitted by the child with
ASD, and there were no other programmed consequences for
commenting.
Script Fading The script-fading procedure was only imple-
mented with the target toy. These sessions followed the
same procedures as baseline and generalization sessions
with the exception of the presence of the scripts. The
sibling retained access of the recorders (i.e., scripts) and
every 30 s presented one of the three scripts by holding
the recorder in the participant’s line of vision and pro-
ceeding through the prompting steps when necessary.
We did not require play actions to match play statements
(e.g., the participant could say “cross the bridge” while
going down the slide); therefore, the sibling presented
scripts in a quasi-random order, unrelated to the child’s play
behavior. We initiated script fading once the child with ASD
independently followed the three scripts at 100% accuracy for
two consecutive sessions. We faded scripts one word at a time
from the end to the beginning with the final fading step being
complete removal of the script (i.e., including the recorder).
Follow-upWe assessed for maintenance 4 weeks after the
completion of training for Cameron and Hank. Sadie’s
follow-up sessions occurred 11 weeks after the comple-
tion of training due to an unforeseeable family incident.
These sessions followed baseline procedures and the
scripts were not present.
Results
Figure 1 displays the number of contextually appropriate
statements emitted by the three participants. The closed
data path denotes sessions for the target toy; this is the
only data path that includes scripted statements. The
large closed squares denote fading steps for the scripts.
Scripts were completely faded, including the removal of
the recorders, for all participants. Once we introduced
the script-fading procedures with the target toy, partici-
pants’ responding increased for the generalization toy
sets, as well as the target toy, indicating that commenting
behavior generalized across toy sets.
Sadie’s statements for the three toy sets greatly in-
creased from baseline (M = 6, target toy; M = 4.2, GS1;
and M = 4.4 GS2) to treatment (M = 17.77, target; M =
15.22, GS1; and M = 17.27, GS2). Scripts were
completely faded for Sadie in 17 sessions. We conducted
one booster session, denoted by the asterisk, before ses-
sion 46 because she was having a difficult time
responding appropriately to the scripts at the second fad-
ing level. During the booster session, Sadie was required
to accurately emit each scripted statement (without the
toy present) for five consecutive trials. After this booster
session, scripts were completely faded in seven sessions.
We conducted a follow-up session 11 weeks after the
final treatment session, and Sadie’s responding remained
at levels consistent with treatment even after this extend-
ed period of time.
Cameron’s statements for the three toy sets also in-
creased from baseline (M = 5.87, target toy; M = 7, GS1;
and M = 5.62, GS2) to treatment (M = 14.29, target; M =
15.16, GS1, and M = 13.87, GS2). We completely faded
the scripts for Cameron in 17 sessions. When playing
with the target toy set, Cameron emitted siren sound
effects (e.g., “weeeoooeee”) at a high rate, which ad-
versely effected his commenting. Therefore, at session
66 Landon began presenting scripts every 15 s instead
of every 30 s in order to interrupt these competing re-
sponses. After implementing this modification, Cameron
emitted more comments and continued to do so when the
scripts were completely faded. Cameron continued to
emit higher levels of contextually appropriate statements
during the 4-week follow-up session.
Hank’s statements also increased from baseline (M =
0.8, target toy; M = 0.9, GS1; and M = 0.4, GS2) to
treatment (M = 9.95, target; M = 6.17, GS1, and M =
8.26, GS2). We observed the most moderate treatment
effect for Hank; however, he also emitted the fewest
number of comments during baseline. We completely
faded scripts for Hank in 16 sessions. Hank continued
to emit higher levels of appropriate comments at the
4-week follow-up session.
Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399 397
Discussion
The number of contextually appropriate statements increased
after the treatment was initiated for all three participants. It is
important to note that these increases in commenting were
observed in the absence of any artificial reinforcement.
Many social skill interventions include additional reinforcers,
which are necessary for behavior change. However, within
this intervention, we observed a change in behavior when
the only consequence that followed the emission of comments
was a verbal response from the sibling. We cannot state with
any certainty that the response functioned as a reinforcer, but it
is possible. Future researchers could directly assess whether
social responses function as reinforcers after implementing
script fading. We were able to completely fade the scripts for
participants without introducing any additional fading steps. It
is unlikely that this finding would be replicated across studies
as previous researchers have reported that complete fading
was not achieved (Akers et al., 2016). Future researchers
should consider investigating conditions which promote suc-
cessful fading.
This study extends the script-fading literature as it includes
naturalistic change agents (i.e., siblings) as the primary
implementer of the intervention and was conducted in the
natural environment (i.e., participants’ home). Siblings were
selected to implement this intervention because we deter-
mined that it was likely that they would serve as a play partner
for the child with ASD in the home environment. Despite the
young age of some of the siblings, all three implemented pro-
cedures with fidelity, for which we recorded data during 50%
of the sessions. Future researchers could assess whether typi-
cally developing peers could serve as implementers of the
script-fading procedure and to what extent this implementa-
tion would lead to a subsequent increase in commenting by
the child with ASD.
There are limitations of this study that are worth noting.
First, we did not specifically code for the complexity of the
statements. While the overall number of statements increased,
it is unknown whether there was an improvement in the qual-
ity of the comments. Future researchers could develop more
sensitive measures to identify the effects of script fading on
increasing comments with more advanced grammar and/or
content. In addition, coding statements as novel, delayed im-
itation, and variations of previously emitted statements may
provide useful information for future studies. Second, the ses-
sion duration was relatively short. We arbitrarily selected
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Cameron
15-s script
presentation
Scripts
completely
faded
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Sadie
Baseline Teaching Follow up
Target Toy
Generalization Toy 1
Generalization Toy 2
*
Scripts
completely faded
N
u
m
b
e
r
o
f
C
o
m
m
e
n
ts
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Sessions
Hank
Scripts
completely
faded
Fig. 1 The results for Sadie (top),
Cameron (middle), and Hank
(bottom). The large closed
squares represent script-fading
steps. *booster session
398 Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399
3-min sessions because we felt the short session length would
reduce the risk of participants’ losing motivation to play with
the toys. However, it is unknown whether participants’
responding would maintain during longer play sessions.
Future researchers may wish to investigate this further.
Third, the change in level between Cameron’s baseline and
initial treatment phase was less robust than for the other two
participants. Although the lack of an immediate effect is
concerning, the shift in level following the modification of
script presentation does provide a clear change from baseline
to treatment. Fourth, we did not require play statements to
match play actions (e.g., saying “down the ramp” while driv-
ing up). However, participants rarely, if ever, engaged in mis-
matched responses.While this did not become an issue for our
participants (likely because of our inclusion criteria), future
researchers could investigate procedures to increase corre-
spondence between play and language, as this may be a skill
deficit for many children with ASD.
A final limitation was our decision to train the siblings to
comment about their own behavior. This is a deviation from
the Reagon and Higbee study, and it is possible that the sib-
lings’ modeling appropriate comments had an effect on par-
ticipants’ commenting. Although siblings commented about
their own behavior across baseline and treatment, we did not
specifically hold the number of comments constant; therefore,
we cannot rule out the possibility that this alone led to an
increase in participants’ responding. We included this devia-
tion because we determined that due to the age of the siblings,
it was likely they would engage in some vocal verbal behavior
during play; therefore, we decided the best way to ensure they
refrained from asking questions or giving directions was to
teach them to comment about their own behavior. In addition,
we believed that training the siblings to refrain from speaking
unless they were responding to a participant comment would
have been a detriment to the social validity of the study as this
would not likely occur in the natural environment.
The results of this study further support the use of script
fading to increase play statements for children with ASD in
the home environment. In addition, this study demonstrates
the utility of incorporating siblings as play partners to promote
play-based commenting. These results again highlight the
generative effects of script-fading procedures in that all three
participants learned to initiate both scripted and unscripted
play statements as a result of being taught three scripted state-
ments with a single toy set.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no
conflict of
interest.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study were in
accor-
dance with the ethical standards of the institutional committee
and with
the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its latter amendments or
comparable
ethical standards.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from the
parents of
the children included in the study.
References
Akers, J. S., Pyle, N., Higbee, T. S., Pyle, D., & Gerencser, K.
R. (2016).
A synthesis of script fading effects with individuals with autism
spectrum disorder: A 20-year review. Review Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 3, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s40489-015-0062-9.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and
statistical
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American
Psychiatric Publishing.
Carr, J. E., Nicolson, A. C., & Higbee, T. S. (2000). Evaluation
of a brief
multiple-stimulus preference assessment in a naturalistic
context.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 353–357.
https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353.
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching children
with
autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a script-fading procedure.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 121–132.
https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121.
Reagon, K. A., & Higbee, T. S. (2009). Parent-implemented
script fading
to promote play-based verbal initiations in children with autism.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 659–664. https://doi.
org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659.
Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399 399
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0062-9
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0062-9
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659Sibling-Implemented
Script Fading to Promote Play-Based Statements of Children
with AutismAbstractMethodParticipants and
SettingMaterialsMeasurementExperimental
ProceduresResultsDiscussionReferences
ANALYSIS OF PRECURSORS TO MULTIPLY CONTROLLED
PROBLEM
BEHAVIOR: A REPLICATION
MEGAN A. BORLASE, JASON C. VLADESCU, APRIL N.
KISAMORE,
SHARON A. REEVE, AND JAMIE L. FETZER
CALDWELL UNIVERSITY
We replicated Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, and Bloom (2013) by
evaluating the efficacy of an
experimental methodology to identify precursors to aggression
displayed by an adolescent with
autism spectrum disorder. Using their trial-based precursor
analysis, we identified seven precur-
sors to aggression. Next, we compared the outcomes of separate
precursor and aggression func-
tional analyses and showed that both precursors and aggression
were multiply controlled by the
same variables.
Key words: aggression, functional analysis, precursor behavior
Researchers have evaluated procedures aimed
at identifying precursors to problem behavior in
an attempt to address some of the limits of
functional analysis (FA) methodology (e.g.,
Borrero & Borrero, 2008; Dracobly & Smith,
2012; Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, & Bloom,
2013; Herscovitch, Roscoe, Libby, Bourret, &
Ahearn, 2009; Smith & Churchill, 2002). Pre-
cursors are behaviors (e.g., vocalizing negatively,
feet stomping, hand flapping) that reliably occur
prior to, and are functionally related to, target
problem behavior. Thus, conducting FAs of pre-
cursors rather than target behavior may require
fewer resources, be less dangerous for the con-
sumer and therapist, and be more acceptable in
certain environments (e.g., schools).
Some researchers have largely relied on care-
giver report and informal observations to initially
determine potential precursors (e.g., Borrero &
Borrero, 2008; Dracobly & Smith, 2012). These
methods may be problematic as they may result
in potential false positives or false negatives
(Dracobly & Smith, 2012; Fritz et al., 2013).
A false positive occurs when a procedure incor-
rectly identifies a behavior as a precursor, whereas
a false negative occurs when a procedure fails to
identify a behavior as a precursor. False positives
and false negatives are problematic if practitioners
treat behaviors that are not precursors or fail to
treat those that are, which in turn may have min-
imal impact on the problem behavior targeted.
In an attempt to address this potential limitation,
Fritz et al. (2013) developed and evaluated an
experimental method for identifying precursors.
The experimenters conducted a trial-based analy-
sis with conditions similar to an FA. During each
trial, observers collected data on potential precur-
sors and target behavior and then calculated
conditional probabilities to determine likely pre-
cursors (i.e., behaviors that were followed by the
target behavior and did not occur in the absence
of the target behavior). Next, the experimenters
conducted independent FAs of the identified pre-
cursors and target behaviors for each participant
and found the same function for seven of eight
participants, whose target behavior was main-
tained by a single function. However, for one
participant, the outcome was only a partial match
(i.e., the precursor FA showed a tangible func-
tion, whereas the target behavior FA showed
both a tangible and an escape function).
The outcomes of Fritz et al. (2013) suggest
that their trial-based analysis was a valid experi-
mental method for determining precursors to
target behavior maintained by a single function;
Address correspondence to Jason C. Vladescu, Depart-
ment of Applied Behavior Analysis, Caldwell University,
120 Bloomfield Avenue, Caldwell, NJ 07006.
Email: [email protected]
doi: 10.1002/jaba.398
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2017, 50,
668–674 NUMBER 3 (SUMMER)
© 2017 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
668
however, it is less clear whether this method is
also effective for determining precursors to tar-
get behavior that is multiply controlled. There-
fore, the purpose of the current study was to
replicate the precursor analysis with an adoles-
cent with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who
engaged in aggression that was multiply
controlled.
METHOD
Participant and Setting
Brandon was a 13-year-old male diagnosed
with ASD whose target behavior was aggres-
sion. We conducted sessions in a room in a
center for individuals with ASD that contained
materials necessary for the session.
Precursor Analysis
We conducted a precursor analysis using
procedures similar to those described by Fritz
et al. (2013). We conducted three different test
trials (attention, demand, tangible) in which we
manipulated relevant antecedents and conse-
quences for aggression (i.e., hitting, pinching,
hair pulling, and biting others) in a similar
manner to an FA. After conducting the first
11 test trials, we observed aggression in
10 trials. Therefore, similar to Fritz et al., we
conducted an additional play trial to create an
approximately equal duration of trials with and
without aggression for which to do our ana-
lyses. Following the occurrence of aggression
during each attention, demand, and tangible
trial, the therapist delivered the trial-specific
consequence. Once aggression was not observed
for 30 s following the delivery of a consequence
or if aggression was not observed within 5 min
of the onset of a trial, the next trial started.
During attention trials, Brandon had access to
moderately preferred items while the therapist
engaged in an activity; however, if aggression
occurred, the therapist delivered attention.
During demand trials, the therapist used three-
step prompting to instruct Brandon to
complete a cleaning task; however, if aggression
occurred the therapist terminated instructions.
Prior to tangible trials, the therapist provided
Brandon access to high preference items for
30 s then removed the items. The removal of
the items initiated the tangible trial, and if
aggression occurred, the therapist returned the
items. During play trials, the therapist provided
Brandon noncontingent access to preferred
items and attention and did not present
demands.
We videotaped all precursor trials, which
were 5 min in duration (if no aggression
occurred) or shorter (if aggression occurred).
We used the videos to identify and define
potential precursors and to score the occurrence
or nonoccurrence of precursors across assess-
ment trials. We completed the following steps
to accomplish these tasks (similar to Fritz et al.,
2013): (a) two experimenters independently
viewed the videos and recorded potential pre-
cursors using a data sheet containing groups of
response topographies (e.g., vocalizations, loco-
motion); (b) the experimenters compared their
recordings and created definitions of each
potential precursor; and (c) the experimenters
independently viewed each video, recorded
whether or not each potential precursor
occurred during the trial, and compared their
recordings until 100% agreement was attained
for each potential precursor. If any discrepan-
cies occurred between experimenters, they
reviewed the video, modified the potential pre-
cursor definitions, and rescored the trial.
We calculated the following probabilities fol-
lowing the completion of the precursor analy-
sis: (a) the probability of the precursor given
the target [p(P|T)] by dividing the number of
trials with aggression and the precursor by the
number of trials with aggression, (b) the proba-
bility of the precursor given the absence of the
target [p(P| ~ T)] by dividing the number of
trials without aggression that contained the pre-
cursor by the number of trials without aggres-
sion, (c) the probability of aggression given the
669PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL
precursor [p(T|P)] by dividing the number of
trials with the precursor and aggression by the
number of trials with the precursor, and (d) the
probability of aggression given the absence of
the precursor [p(T| ~ P)] by dividing the num-
ber of trials without the precursor that con-
tained aggression by the number of trials
without the precursor. We also calculated the
unconditional probabilities of the precursor
behaviors [p(P)], by dividing the number trials
containing the precursor by the number of total
trials, and of the target behavior [p(T)], by
dividing the number of trials containing aggres-
sion by the number of total trials.
We selected precursors, if (a) the probability
of aggression given the precursor [p(T|P)]
was greater than the probability of aggression
given the absence of the precursor [ p(T| ~ P)]
and the unconditional probability of aggression
[p(T)], and (b) the probability of the precursor
given aggression [p(P|T)] was greater than the
probability of the precursor given the absence
of aggression [p(P| ~ T)] and the unconditional
probability of the precursor behaviors [p(P)]
(Fritz et al., 2013).
Functional Analysis
Following the precursor analysis, we con-
ducted independent FAs to confirm the func-
tional relation between the seven identified
precursors and aggression. Sessions lasted
10 min, and conditions were conducted in the
following order: attention, tangible, play, and
demand (Hammond, Iwata, Rooker, Fritz, &
Bloom, 2013) using procedures similar to
Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman
(1994). Brandon was seated at a table and the
therapist was positioned approximately
90–150 cm away from Brandon at the begin-
ning of all sessions. We conducted the precur-
sor FA first, during which condition-specific
consequences were provided contingent on the
occurrence of precursors and aggression was on
extinction. Next, we conducted the aggression
FA during which the condition-specific conse-
quences were provided contingent on the
occurrence of aggression and precursors were
on extinction. Materials, therapist interactions,
and condition-specific consequences were iden-
tical to those implemented during trials in the
precursor analysis.
During each FA, we collected data on the
frequency of aggression (as defined above in the
precursor analysis) and the frequency of the
seven precursor behaviors identified in the pre-
cursor analysis, which included standing up,
heavy breathing, approaching instructor, fold-
ing hands, knee hitting, rubbing face, and neg-
ative vocalizing. Operational definitions of all
precursors are available from the first author.
We calculated exact interval agreement per-
centages during 50% of FA sessions by compar-
ing the primary and secondary data collectors’
recorded frequencies of responses during 10-s
intervals during the FAs. Overall, mean IOA
scores were 97% (range, 90%–100%) and 95%
(range, 90%–100%) for the precursor and
aggression FAs, respectively.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The results of Brandon’s precursor analysis
are in Figure 1. Seven (denoted by the aster-
isks) of the 25 potential behaviors were identi-
fied as precursors based on the criteria
described above. Four of the precursors (knee
hitting, rubbing face, heavy breathing, and
folding hands) were perfectly correlated with
aggression [i.e., p(T|P) = 1.0] and never
occurred in trials in which aggression was not
observed [i.e., p(P| ~ T) = 0]. However, aggres-
sion still occurred in the absence of these four
precursors and the probability of these precur-
sors occurring given aggression was low. The
other three precursors (standing up, negative
vocalizing, and approaching instructor)
occurred less frequently [i.e., p(T|P) < 1.0] but
the relative conditional probabilities showed
MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.670
that these behaviors predicted the occurrence of
aggression.
Figure 2 depicts the results of precursor and
aggression FAs. We observed high rates of
precursors (left panel) and high rates of aggres-
sion (right panel) during the three test condi-
tions during precursor and aggression FAs,
respectively. This pattern of behavior suggests
that combined precursors and aggression were
multiply controlled by the same variables. In
addition, Brandon demonstrated reduced rates
of aggression during the precursor FA, which
replicates similar findings reported in previous
studies (e.g., Fritz et al., 2013).
Figure 1. The top panel depicts the conditional probability of
the target (T) given the presence [p(T|P)] and absence
[p(T| ~ P)] of potential precursors (P) and the unconditional
probability of the target [p(T)]. The bottom panel depicts
the conditional probability of potential precursors (P) given the
presence [p(P|T)] and absence [p(P| ~ T) of the target
and the unconditional probability of each precursor [p(P)]. The
asterisks denote behaviors identified as precursors.
671PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL
We calculated the proportion of each precur-
sor during the precursor FA (Figure 3) which
yielded several interesting outcomes. First, similar
to Fritz et al. (2013), not all identified precursors
occurred during the precursor FA. Knee hits and
heavy breathing did not occur during the precur-
sor FA despite being perfectly correlated with
aggression in the precursor analysis. It is unclear
why this occurred, but several explanations are
possible. It is possible that these precursors were
associated with increased response effort or did
not have as recent a history of reinforcement rela-
tive to the other topographies of precursors.
Alternatively, it may be the case that the precur-
sor analysis resulted in some false positives.
Second, the most common precursors
observed were negative vocalizing, approaching
instructor, and standing up. Although these
responses were not perfectly correlated with
aggression in the precursor analysis, they
occurred most frequently during the precursor
FA. The inclusion of standing up and approach-
ing instructor as precursor behaviors may be
considered a limitation, as these responses may
have been part of a response chain to aggression.
If these were early responses in the chain, then,
if reinforced, they would likely increase levels of
aggression (Dracobly & Smith, 2012). However,
data from the precursor FA do not support this
interpretation, as reduced levels of aggression
were observed when precursors were reinforced.
There is currently no established technology
to distinguish precursors from links in a chain
to a target behavior (Hagopian, Paclawskyj, &
Kuhn, 2005). Future research is needed to
develop such a technology. Third, a different
precursor occurred most frequently in each test
condition (e.g., vocalizing negatively in the
demand condition). This pattern of responding
may suggest different topographies of precursors
Figure 2. This graph depicts the precursor (left panel) and
aggression (right panel) FAs. The top and bottom panels
display the rate of combined precursors and aggression,
respectively.
MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.672
precede a multiply controlled target behavior
under different evocative situations. Future
research could further evaluate this possibility.
Several limitations are worth noting. The
current evaluation included only one partici-
pant, thus limiting external validity. Future
studies should replicate these findings with
additional participants. Another mitigating fac-
tor was the lack of an alone condition during
the FAs. The success of the treatment imple-
mented based on the results of the FAs (these
data are not included in the manuscript) sug-
gests that Brandon’s target behavior was
unlikely maintained by automatic reinforce-
ment; however, it is possible that some of the
precursor behaviors were. The inclusion of an
alone condition would have allowed an evalua-
tion of this possibility.
Although we attempted to ensure the oppor-
tunity to engage in all responses during the FAs
by seating Brandon at a table and positioning
the therapist a consistent distance away to
begin all sessions, some conditions required
therapist behavior that would necessarily
restrict Brandon’s ability to engage in some
responses. This limitation is exemplified by the
following situation: If the therapist used
physical guidance during the demand condi-
tion, Brandon would be unable to approach
the therapist. Despite these limitations, the cur-
rent evaluation provides support for an analysis
to identify precursors to a problem behavior.
REFERENCES
Borrero, C. S. W., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive
and experimental analyses of potential precursors to
problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 41, 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
2008.41-83
Dracobly, J. D., & Smith, R. G. (2012). Progressing from
identification and functional analysis of precursor
behavior to treatment of self-injurious behavior. Jour-
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 361–374.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361
Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Hammond, J. L., &
Bloom, S. E. (2013). Experimental analysis of precur-
sors to severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 46, 101–129. https://doi.org/10.
1002/jaba.27
Hagopian, L. P., Paclawskyj, T. R., & Kuhn, S. C.
(2005). The use of conditional probability analysis to
identify a response chain leading to the occurrence of
eye poking. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26,
393–397.
Hammond, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Rooker, G. W.,
Fritz, J. N., & Bloom, S. E. (2013). Effects of fixed
versus random condition sequencing during
Figure 3. The bottom graph depicts the proportion of precursors
that were scored in the precursor FA. Knee hits
and heavy breathing were not observed in any precursor FA
condition.
673PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-83
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-83
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.27
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.27
multielement functional analyses. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 46, 22–30. https://doi.org/10.
1002/jaba.7
Herscovitch, B., Roscoe, E. M., Libby, M. E.,
Bourret, J. C., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). A proce-
dure for identifying precursors to problem behavior.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 697–702.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-697
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. E., Slifer, K. J.,
Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a
functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209. https://doi.org/10.
1901/jaba.1994.27-197
Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification
of environmental determinants of behavior disorders
through functional analysis of precursor behaviors.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–136.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125
Received September 27, 2015
Final acceptance October 2, 2016
Action Editor, Claudia Dozier
MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.674
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.7
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.7
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-697
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-197
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-197
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125 ANALYSIS OF
PRECURSORS TO MULTIPLY CONTROLLED PROBLEM
BEHAVIOR: A REPLICATIONMETHODParticipant and
SettingPrecursor AnalysisFunctional AnalysisRESULTS AND
DISCUSSIONReferences
BRIEF PRACTICESibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote.docx

More Related Content

Similar to BRIEF PRACTICESibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote.docx

Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docx
Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docxFinal Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docx
Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docxAKHIL969626
 
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007guestf107fb1f
 
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)Meng Maggie Zhao
 
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principles
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principlesBrazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principles
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principlesKeith Brazendale
 
Problem Solving
Problem SolvingProblem Solving
Problem Solvingmcneeljc86
 
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docx
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docxPlayer Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docx
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docxinfantsuk
 
Neuropsychological Assessment of Attention
Neuropsychological Assessment of AttentionNeuropsychological Assessment of Attention
Neuropsychological Assessment of AttentionZahiruddin Othman
 
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL Skills
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL SkillsMMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL Skills
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL SkillsSandra Annette Rogers
 
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.com
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.comCh5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.com
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.comBegoña Pino
 
Understanding statistical word learning in a social context
Understanding statistical word learning in a social contextUnderstanding statistical word learning in a social context
Understanding statistical word learning in a social contextMichael Frank
 
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)1213anita
 
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docx
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docxChild Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docx
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Nuffield-Report-Final
Nuffield-Report-FinalNuffield-Report-Final
Nuffield-Report-FinalSafa Hashemi
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxcroysierkathey
 
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...David Pastor Calle
 
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples College
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples CollegeDescriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples College
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples CollegeErica Wambua
 
Dissertation Implicit Explicit Memory
Dissertation Implicit Explicit MemoryDissertation Implicit Explicit Memory
Dissertation Implicit Explicit MemoryMatthew Jones
 

Similar to BRIEF PRACTICESibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote.docx (20)

Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docx
Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docxFinal Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docx
Final Project Outline (Draft)PART I TIMELINEChildhood.docx
 
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007
Dietrich Kracke Esdp2007
 
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)
DRM adult and child poster.SRCD final (1)
 
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principles
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principlesBrazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principles
Brazendale 2015 - Maximizing childrens PA using the LET US Play principles
 
Problem Solving
Problem SolvingProblem Solving
Problem Solving
 
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docx
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docxPlayer Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docx
Player Profiling with Fallout 3Pieter Spronck, Iris Balema.docx
 
Neuropsychological Assessment of Attention
Neuropsychological Assessment of AttentionNeuropsychological Assessment of Attention
Neuropsychological Assessment of Attention
 
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL Skills
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL SkillsMMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL Skills
MMORPG with Strategic Language Learning Activities for ESL Skills
 
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.com
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.comCh5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.com
Ch5 Social interaction in individual vs. partner playing begonapino.com
 
Understanding statistical word learning in a social context
Understanding statistical word learning in a social contextUnderstanding statistical word learning in a social context
Understanding statistical word learning in a social context
 
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)
Speech Act (Andrew D. Cohen)
 
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docx
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docxChild Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docx
Child Gender Influences Paternal Behavior, Language, and Brain.docx
 
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docxRunning title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
Running title Communication disorders 3NameUniversit.docx
 
Nuffield-Report-Final
Nuffield-Report-FinalNuffield-Report-Final
Nuffield-Report-Final
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
 
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...
Brief Report: Increasing Communication Skills for an Elementary-Aged Student ...
 
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples College
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples CollegeDescriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples College
Descriptive Essay Examples College.pdfDescriptive Essay Examples College
 
pedantry_poster
pedantry_posterpedantry_poster
pedantry_poster
 
Dissertation Implicit Explicit Memory
Dissertation Implicit Explicit MemoryDissertation Implicit Explicit Memory
Dissertation Implicit Explicit Memory
 
Alexithymia
AlexithymiaAlexithymia
Alexithymia
 

More from bartholomeocoombs

CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docx
CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docxCompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docx
CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docx
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docxCompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docx
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docxCompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docxCompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docx
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docxCompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docx
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docx
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docxCompetency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docx
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docx
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docxCompetency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docx
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docx
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docxCompetency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docx
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docx
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docxCompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docx
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docx
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docxCompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docx
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docx
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docxCompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docx
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docxbartholomeocoombs
 
COMPETENCIES734.3.4 Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docx
COMPETENCIES734.3.4  Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docxCOMPETENCIES734.3.4  Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docx
COMPETENCIES734.3.4 Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docx
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docxCompetences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docx
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docx
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docxCompensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docx
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docx
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docxCompensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docx
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docx
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docxCompete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docx
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docx
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docxCompensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docx
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docxbartholomeocoombs
 

More from bartholomeocoombs (20)

CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docx
CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docxCompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docx
CompetencyAnalyze how human resource standards and practices.docx
 
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docx
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docxCompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docx
CompetencyAnalyze financial statements to assess performance.docx
 
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docxCompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare.docx
 
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docxCompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docx
CompetencyAnalyze ethical and legal dilemmas that healthcare wor.docx
 
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docx
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docxCompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docx
CompetencyAnalyze collaboration tools to support organizatio.docx
 
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docx
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docxCompetency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docx
Competency Checklist and Professional Development Resources .docx
 
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docx
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docxCompetency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docx
Competency 6 Enagage with Communities and Organizations (3 hrs) (1 .docx
 
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docx
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docxCompetency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docx
Competency 2 Examine the organizational behavior within busines.docx
 
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docx
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docxCompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docx
CompetenciesEvaluate the challenges and benefits of employ.docx
 
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docx
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docxCompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docx
CompetenciesDescribe the supply chain management principle.docx
 
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docx
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docxCompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docx
CompetenciesABCDF1.1 Create oral, written, or visual .docx
 
COMPETENCIES734.3.4 Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docx
COMPETENCIES734.3.4  Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docxCOMPETENCIES734.3.4  Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docx
COMPETENCIES734.3.4 Healthcare Utilization and Finance.docx
 
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeWhat competencies were you able to dev.docx
 
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 parts.docx
 
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docxCompetencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docx
Competencies and KnowledgeThis assignment has 2 partsWhat.docx
 
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docx
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docxCompetences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docx
Competences, Learning Theories and MOOCsRecent Developments.docx
 
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docx
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docxCompensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docx
Compensation  & Benefits Class 700 words with referencesA stra.docx
 
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docx
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docxCompensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docx
Compensation, Benefits, Reward & Recognition Plan for V..docx
 
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docx
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docxCompete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docx
Compete the following tablesTheoryKey figuresKey concepts o.docx
 
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docx
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docxCompensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docx
Compensation Strategy for Knowledge WorkersTo prepare for this a.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/siemaillard
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文中 央社
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024Borja Sotomayor
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFVivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).Mohamed Rizk Khodair
 
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...EduSkills OECD
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....Ritu480198
 
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjStl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjMohammed Sikander
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategies
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategiesMajor project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategies
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategiesAmanpreetKaur157993
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopal
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopale-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopal
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi RajagopalEADTU
 
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17Celine George
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge App
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge AppAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge App
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge AppCeline George
 
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptx
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptxdemyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptx
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptxMohamed Rizk Khodair
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSean M. Fox
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
 
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).
Dementia (Alzheimer & vasular dementia).
 
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
 
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjStl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
 
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with Text Classification and Open Source"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with Text Classification and Open Source"Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with Text Classification and Open Source"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with Text Classification and Open Source"
 
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategies
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategiesMajor project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategies
Major project report on Tata Motors and its marketing strategies
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
 
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE .
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE                       .“O BEIJO” EM ARTE                       .
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE .
 
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopal
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopale-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopal
e-Sealing at EADTU by Kamakshi Rajagopal
 
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
 
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17
How to Analyse Profit of a Sales Order in Odoo 17
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge App
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge AppAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge App
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Knowledge App
 
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptx
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptxdemyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptx
demyelinated disorder: multiple sclerosis.pptx
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
 

BRIEF PRACTICESibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote.docx

  • 1. BRIEF PRACTICE Sibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote Play-Based Statements of Children with Autism Jessica S. Akers1,2 & Thomas S. Higbee1 & Joy S. Pollard1,3 & Kassidy S. Reinert1 Published online: 25 May 2018 # Association for Behavior Analysis International 2018 Abstract We trained three typically developing children to implement a script-fading procedure with their younger siblings with autism. The number of contextually appropriate statements made by the children with autism increased once treatment was initiated. Participants continued to emit higher levels of contextually appropriate statements after the scripts were completely faded and at a 4- or 11-week follow-up. The typically developing siblings were able to implement the script-fading procedure with high levels of fidelity. Keywords Script fading . Autism . Sibling . Play . Language One of the diagnostic criteria of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is deficit in the area of social communication (DSM- 5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Script fad- ing is a procedure that has been shown to increase the number
  • 2. of appropriate vocalizations emitted by children with ASD (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993). Script fading consists of the following steps: (a) first, children are taught to emit the scripted phrase using a textual (e.g., typed) or auditory (e.g., recorder) script and (b) the script is systematically faded such that the child with ASD continues to emit the scripted phrase in the absence of the script. Once script fading is initiated, children often emit untaught phrases in addition to those that were directly taught. While script fading has been shown to be effective, researchers or instructors have served as implementers in the majority of studies (Akers, Pyle, Higbee, Pyle, & Gerencser, 2016). One notable exception is the implementation of script fading by parents (Reagon & Higbee, 2009). The parents in this study developed three scripted statements and systematically faded the scripted state- ments based on their child’s performance during play sessions. The scripted statements (e.g., “Look, the car is going!”) were related to one toy set, and two other toy sets were used to assess for generalization. Results showed that play initiations increased with both the target and generalization toy sets. The results reported by Reagon and Higbee (2009) are promising; however, it may be important to identify if these results could be replicated with a more age appro- priate play partner. For many children with ASD, a com- mon play partner might be a typically developing sibling. Given the effectiveness of script-fading procedures with parents delivering and fading scripts, we sought to sys- tematically replicate these results with siblings serving as play partners. We measured the effects of the script fading procedure on the number of contextually appropriate statements made by children with ASD. Method
  • 3. Participants and Setting Three children with ASD, who had previously attended or were currently attending a university-based behavioral Implications for practice •Young children with ASD often do not emit appropriate play statements while playing with toys. • Script fading is an effective intervention for teaching children with ASD to emit play-based statements. • Typically developing siblings can implement script fading with fidelity. * Jessica S. Akers [email protected] 1 Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA 2 Present address: Baylor University, Waco, TX, USA 3 Present address: Behavior Change Institute, Oakland, CA, USA Behavior Analysis in Practice (2018) 11:395–399 https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-018-0257-5 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s40617-018- 0257-5&domain=pdf mailto:[email protected] preschool, participated in the study along with a typically developing sibling. We recruited participants who (a) could emit at least three-word phrases, (b) had a generalized imita- tion repertoire, (c) engaged in low levels of destructive behav-
  • 4. ior, and (d) played with toys appropriately but rarely commented during play. Sadie (5) participated with her sister Melissa (14), Cameron (7) participated with his brother Landon (10), and Hank (4) participated with his sister Mandy (6). A parent of each participant served as a research assistant for sessions. We conducted sessions in participants’ homes in an open area that was cleared of distracting items. Materials Each sibling dyad was assigned three toy sets to interact with during sessions (see Table 1). These toy sets were purchased by the researcher and access was restricted outside of sessions. We designated one toy set to be used for the script fading intervention (hereafter called the “target toy”) and used the other two toy sets to assess generalization. The parents devel- oped three 3–4-word scripts which were recorded onMini-Me ™ voice recorders. Siblings wore MotivAider timers to signal 30-s intervals. Measurement Parents transcribed all statements made by participants during sessions, and the transcription was then reviewed by the re- searcher to total the number of contextually appropriate state- ments. Statements were not scored if they were (a) not con- textually appropriate (e.g., “I need gas,” when playing with the playground), (b) one-word statements, (c) immediate rep- etitions of statements, (d) excessive repetitions of statements, defined as using the same statement more than four times, (f) stereotypic phrases, individually identified for each participant (e.g., “good job”), or (g) completely unintelligible. A second coder collected data for 33% of sessions to assess interobserver agreement (IOA). The second coder transcribed
  • 5. and recorded the number of comments via recorded video. IOA was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and converting the result to a percentage. An agreement was de- fined as both coders (i.e., the first and fourth authors) record- ing the same statement as contextually appropriate. Mean agreement was 95% (range from 75 to 100%) for Sadie, 96% (range from 75 to 100%) for Cameron, and 97% (range from 50 to 100%) for Hank. During the session with 50% agreement, Hank only made two comments; therefore, one disagreement leads to this low percentage. We assessed treatment fidelity for 50% of sessions. We assessed for the following components, whether the sibling (a) oriented to the participant, (b) presented a script every 30 s, (c) responded to the participant’s initiations, (d) used the prompting procedure (described below), (e) did not ask questions or provide directions, and (f) only made comments about his/her own behavior. Treatment fidelity was calculated by dividing the number of correctly implemented components by the total number of components and converting the result to a percentage. Mean fidelity of implementation was 93% (range from 87 to 100%) for Sadie, 93% (range from 82 to 100%) for Cameron, and 88% (range from 62 to 100%) for Hank. Experimental Procedures An adapted alternating treatment design embedded within a multiple baseline design across participants was used to assess the effects of the script-fading procedure on the number of comments emitted by the participants. Prior to the study, a brief multiple-stimulus without replacement preference as- sessment (Carr, Nicolson, & Higbee, 2000) was conducted to identify the top three toy sets.
  • 6. Pretraining We trained the siblings to implement the script-fading procedure using Behavioral Skills Training (BST). Parents served as the role-play partner and provided Table 1 Session materials Toy sets Scripted statements Sadie Fisher-Price Little People® Playground (target) 1. Go down slide Fisher-Price Little People® Happy Sounds Home (GS1) 2. I want swing Fisher-Price Little People® Wheelies Airport (GS2) 3. Cross the bridge Cameron Fisher Price World of Little People® Emergency Fire Station (target) 1. Go get the hose Fisher-Price Little People® Playground (GS1) 2. To the rescue Fisher-Price Little People® Wheelies Airport (GS2) 3. Let us save them Hank Hot Wheels® City Tow and Tune Car Shop Play Set (target) 1. Fix the car Play-Doh® Brick Mill and Grinding Gravel Yard (GS1) 2. Down the hill Vtech® Go! Go! Smart Wheels Airport Playset (GS2) 3. Pick up car Target target toy set, GS generalization toy set 396 Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399
  • 7. feedback to the sibling in conjunction with feedback pro- vided by the researcher. We specifically trained the sib- lings to (a) orient to the participant during play, (b) refrain from asking questions or giving directions, (c) respond to all of the participant’s verbalizations, and (d) to comment on their own play actions. Siblings were instructed to pres- ent an auditory script every 30 s and wait for participant to emit the scripted phrase. If he or she did not repeat the scripted phrase, the sibling first physically guided him or her to press the voice recorder button. If this prompt was ineffective, the sibling provided a verbal prompt (e.g., “say, here comes the car”). Training ended when the sib- ling correctly implemented each component with their parent with 95% or better accuracy. During pretraining, the most common error was failing to respond to com- ments made by the parent. The siblings met mastery after one session that took approximately 30 min. Baseline and Generalization Sessions We conducted three 3-min sessions per day (i.e., one for each toy set). Prior to each session, the parent cleared the environment to remove possible distractions and provided the sibling with the appropriate ma- terials. The researcher video recorded sessions while the par- ent simultaneously transcribed statements. The sibling began sessions by saying, “let us play.” Throughout the sessions, the siblings responded to all comments emitted by the child with ASD, and there were no other programmed consequences for commenting. Script Fading The script-fading procedure was only imple- mented with the target toy. These sessions followed the same procedures as baseline and generalization sessions with the exception of the presence of the scripts. The
  • 8. sibling retained access of the recorders (i.e., scripts) and every 30 s presented one of the three scripts by holding the recorder in the participant’s line of vision and pro- ceeding through the prompting steps when necessary. We did not require play actions to match play statements (e.g., the participant could say “cross the bridge” while going down the slide); therefore, the sibling presented scripts in a quasi-random order, unrelated to the child’s play behavior. We initiated script fading once the child with ASD independently followed the three scripts at 100% accuracy for two consecutive sessions. We faded scripts one word at a time from the end to the beginning with the final fading step being complete removal of the script (i.e., including the recorder). Follow-upWe assessed for maintenance 4 weeks after the completion of training for Cameron and Hank. Sadie’s follow-up sessions occurred 11 weeks after the comple- tion of training due to an unforeseeable family incident. These sessions followed baseline procedures and the scripts were not present. Results Figure 1 displays the number of contextually appropriate statements emitted by the three participants. The closed data path denotes sessions for the target toy; this is the only data path that includes scripted statements. The large closed squares denote fading steps for the scripts. Scripts were completely faded, including the removal of the recorders, for all participants. Once we introduced the script-fading procedures with the target toy, partici- pants’ responding increased for the generalization toy sets, as well as the target toy, indicating that commenting behavior generalized across toy sets. Sadie’s statements for the three toy sets greatly in-
  • 9. creased from baseline (M = 6, target toy; M = 4.2, GS1; and M = 4.4 GS2) to treatment (M = 17.77, target; M = 15.22, GS1; and M = 17.27, GS2). Scripts were completely faded for Sadie in 17 sessions. We conducted one booster session, denoted by the asterisk, before ses- sion 46 because she was having a difficult time responding appropriately to the scripts at the second fad- ing level. During the booster session, Sadie was required to accurately emit each scripted statement (without the toy present) for five consecutive trials. After this booster session, scripts were completely faded in seven sessions. We conducted a follow-up session 11 weeks after the final treatment session, and Sadie’s responding remained at levels consistent with treatment even after this extend- ed period of time. Cameron’s statements for the three toy sets also in- creased from baseline (M = 5.87, target toy; M = 7, GS1; and M = 5.62, GS2) to treatment (M = 14.29, target; M = 15.16, GS1, and M = 13.87, GS2). We completely faded the scripts for Cameron in 17 sessions. When playing with the target toy set, Cameron emitted siren sound effects (e.g., “weeeoooeee”) at a high rate, which ad- versely effected his commenting. Therefore, at session 66 Landon began presenting scripts every 15 s instead of every 30 s in order to interrupt these competing re- sponses. After implementing this modification, Cameron emitted more comments and continued to do so when the scripts were completely faded. Cameron continued to emit higher levels of contextually appropriate statements during the 4-week follow-up session. Hank’s statements also increased from baseline (M = 0.8, target toy; M = 0.9, GS1; and M = 0.4, GS2) to treatment (M = 9.95, target; M = 6.17, GS1, and M = 8.26, GS2). We observed the most moderate treatment
  • 10. effect for Hank; however, he also emitted the fewest number of comments during baseline. We completely faded scripts for Hank in 16 sessions. Hank continued to emit higher levels of appropriate comments at the 4-week follow-up session. Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399 397 Discussion The number of contextually appropriate statements increased after the treatment was initiated for all three participants. It is important to note that these increases in commenting were observed in the absence of any artificial reinforcement. Many social skill interventions include additional reinforcers, which are necessary for behavior change. However, within this intervention, we observed a change in behavior when the only consequence that followed the emission of comments was a verbal response from the sibling. We cannot state with any certainty that the response functioned as a reinforcer, but it is possible. Future researchers could directly assess whether social responses function as reinforcers after implementing script fading. We were able to completely fade the scripts for participants without introducing any additional fading steps. It is unlikely that this finding would be replicated across studies as previous researchers have reported that complete fading was not achieved (Akers et al., 2016). Future researchers should consider investigating conditions which promote suc- cessful fading. This study extends the script-fading literature as it includes naturalistic change agents (i.e., siblings) as the primary implementer of the intervention and was conducted in the
  • 11. natural environment (i.e., participants’ home). Siblings were selected to implement this intervention because we deter- mined that it was likely that they would serve as a play partner for the child with ASD in the home environment. Despite the young age of some of the siblings, all three implemented pro- cedures with fidelity, for which we recorded data during 50% of the sessions. Future researchers could assess whether typi- cally developing peers could serve as implementers of the script-fading procedure and to what extent this implementa- tion would lead to a subsequent increase in commenting by the child with ASD. There are limitations of this study that are worth noting. First, we did not specifically code for the complexity of the statements. While the overall number of statements increased, it is unknown whether there was an improvement in the qual- ity of the comments. Future researchers could develop more sensitive measures to identify the effects of script fading on increasing comments with more advanced grammar and/or content. In addition, coding statements as novel, delayed im- itation, and variations of previously emitted statements may provide useful information for future studies. Second, the ses- sion duration was relatively short. We arbitrarily selected 0 5 10 15 20 25
  • 13. Generalization Toy 2 * Scripts completely faded N u m b e r o f C o m m e n ts 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 0 5 10
  • 14. 15 20 25 30 Sessions Hank Scripts completely faded Fig. 1 The results for Sadie (top), Cameron (middle), and Hank (bottom). The large closed squares represent script-fading steps. *booster session 398 Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399 3-min sessions because we felt the short session length would reduce the risk of participants’ losing motivation to play with the toys. However, it is unknown whether participants’ responding would maintain during longer play sessions. Future researchers may wish to investigate this further. Third, the change in level between Cameron’s baseline and initial treatment phase was less robust than for the other two participants. Although the lack of an immediate effect is
  • 15. concerning, the shift in level following the modification of script presentation does provide a clear change from baseline to treatment. Fourth, we did not require play statements to match play actions (e.g., saying “down the ramp” while driv- ing up). However, participants rarely, if ever, engaged in mis- matched responses.While this did not become an issue for our participants (likely because of our inclusion criteria), future researchers could investigate procedures to increase corre- spondence between play and language, as this may be a skill deficit for many children with ASD. A final limitation was our decision to train the siblings to comment about their own behavior. This is a deviation from the Reagon and Higbee study, and it is possible that the sib- lings’ modeling appropriate comments had an effect on par- ticipants’ commenting. Although siblings commented about their own behavior across baseline and treatment, we did not specifically hold the number of comments constant; therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that this alone led to an increase in participants’ responding. We included this devia- tion because we determined that due to the age of the siblings, it was likely they would engage in some vocal verbal behavior during play; therefore, we decided the best way to ensure they refrained from asking questions or giving directions was to teach them to comment about their own behavior. In addition, we believed that training the siblings to refrain from speaking unless they were responding to a participant comment would have been a detriment to the social validity of the study as this would not likely occur in the natural environment. The results of this study further support the use of script fading to increase play statements for children with ASD in the home environment. In addition, this study demonstrates the utility of incorporating siblings as play partners to promote play-based commenting. These results again highlight the
  • 16. generative effects of script-fading procedures in that all three participants learned to initiate both scripted and unscripted play statements as a result of being taught three scripted state- ments with a single toy set. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study were in accor- dance with the ethical standards of the institutional committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its latter amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from the parents of the children included in the study. References Akers, J. S., Pyle, N., Higbee, T. S., Pyle, D., & Gerencser, K. R. (2016). A synthesis of script fading effects with individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A 20-year review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s40489-015-0062-9. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
  • 17. Carr, J. E., Nicolson, A. C., & Higbee, T. S. (2000). Evaluation of a brief multiple-stimulus preference assessment in a naturalistic context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 353–357. https://doi. org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353. Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 121–132. https://doi. org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121. Reagon, K. A., & Higbee, T. S. (2009). Parent-implemented script fading to promote play-based verbal initiations in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 659–664. https://doi. org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659. Behav Analysis Practice (2018) 11:395–399 399 https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0062-9 https://doi.org/10.1007/s40489-015-0062-9 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.200.33-353 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659Sibling-Implemented Script Fading to Promote Play-Based Statements of Children with AutismAbstractMethodParticipants and SettingMaterialsMeasurementExperimental ProceduresResultsDiscussionReferences
  • 18. ANALYSIS OF PRECURSORS TO MULTIPLY CONTROLLED PROBLEM BEHAVIOR: A REPLICATION MEGAN A. BORLASE, JASON C. VLADESCU, APRIL N. KISAMORE, SHARON A. REEVE, AND JAMIE L. FETZER CALDWELL UNIVERSITY We replicated Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, and Bloom (2013) by evaluating the efficacy of an experimental methodology to identify precursors to aggression displayed by an adolescent with autism spectrum disorder. Using their trial-based precursor analysis, we identified seven precur- sors to aggression. Next, we compared the outcomes of separate precursor and aggression func- tional analyses and showed that both precursors and aggression were multiply controlled by the same variables. Key words: aggression, functional analysis, precursor behavior Researchers have evaluated procedures aimed at identifying precursors to problem behavior in an attempt to address some of the limits of functional analysis (FA) methodology (e.g., Borrero & Borrero, 2008; Dracobly & Smith, 2012; Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, & Bloom, 2013; Herscovitch, Roscoe, Libby, Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009; Smith & Churchill, 2002). Pre- cursors are behaviors (e.g., vocalizing negatively, feet stomping, hand flapping) that reliably occur
  • 19. prior to, and are functionally related to, target problem behavior. Thus, conducting FAs of pre- cursors rather than target behavior may require fewer resources, be less dangerous for the con- sumer and therapist, and be more acceptable in certain environments (e.g., schools). Some researchers have largely relied on care- giver report and informal observations to initially determine potential precursors (e.g., Borrero & Borrero, 2008; Dracobly & Smith, 2012). These methods may be problematic as they may result in potential false positives or false negatives (Dracobly & Smith, 2012; Fritz et al., 2013). A false positive occurs when a procedure incor- rectly identifies a behavior as a precursor, whereas a false negative occurs when a procedure fails to identify a behavior as a precursor. False positives and false negatives are problematic if practitioners treat behaviors that are not precursors or fail to treat those that are, which in turn may have min- imal impact on the problem behavior targeted. In an attempt to address this potential limitation, Fritz et al. (2013) developed and evaluated an experimental method for identifying precursors. The experimenters conducted a trial-based analy- sis with conditions similar to an FA. During each trial, observers collected data on potential precur- sors and target behavior and then calculated conditional probabilities to determine likely pre- cursors (i.e., behaviors that were followed by the target behavior and did not occur in the absence of the target behavior). Next, the experimenters conducted independent FAs of the identified pre- cursors and target behaviors for each participant
  • 20. and found the same function for seven of eight participants, whose target behavior was main- tained by a single function. However, for one participant, the outcome was only a partial match (i.e., the precursor FA showed a tangible func- tion, whereas the target behavior FA showed both a tangible and an escape function). The outcomes of Fritz et al. (2013) suggest that their trial-based analysis was a valid experi- mental method for determining precursors to target behavior maintained by a single function; Address correspondence to Jason C. Vladescu, Depart- ment of Applied Behavior Analysis, Caldwell University, 120 Bloomfield Avenue, Caldwell, NJ 07006. Email: [email protected] doi: 10.1002/jaba.398 JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2017, 50, 668–674 NUMBER 3 (SUMMER) © 2017 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 668 however, it is less clear whether this method is also effective for determining precursors to tar- get behavior that is multiply controlled. There- fore, the purpose of the current study was to replicate the precursor analysis with an adoles- cent with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who engaged in aggression that was multiply controlled.
  • 21. METHOD Participant and Setting Brandon was a 13-year-old male diagnosed with ASD whose target behavior was aggres- sion. We conducted sessions in a room in a center for individuals with ASD that contained materials necessary for the session. Precursor Analysis We conducted a precursor analysis using procedures similar to those described by Fritz et al. (2013). We conducted three different test trials (attention, demand, tangible) in which we manipulated relevant antecedents and conse- quences for aggression (i.e., hitting, pinching, hair pulling, and biting others) in a similar manner to an FA. After conducting the first 11 test trials, we observed aggression in 10 trials. Therefore, similar to Fritz et al., we conducted an additional play trial to create an approximately equal duration of trials with and without aggression for which to do our ana- lyses. Following the occurrence of aggression during each attention, demand, and tangible trial, the therapist delivered the trial-specific consequence. Once aggression was not observed for 30 s following the delivery of a consequence or if aggression was not observed within 5 min of the onset of a trial, the next trial started. During attention trials, Brandon had access to moderately preferred items while the therapist engaged in an activity; however, if aggression
  • 22. occurred, the therapist delivered attention. During demand trials, the therapist used three- step prompting to instruct Brandon to complete a cleaning task; however, if aggression occurred the therapist terminated instructions. Prior to tangible trials, the therapist provided Brandon access to high preference items for 30 s then removed the items. The removal of the items initiated the tangible trial, and if aggression occurred, the therapist returned the items. During play trials, the therapist provided Brandon noncontingent access to preferred items and attention and did not present demands. We videotaped all precursor trials, which were 5 min in duration (if no aggression occurred) or shorter (if aggression occurred). We used the videos to identify and define potential precursors and to score the occurrence or nonoccurrence of precursors across assess- ment trials. We completed the following steps to accomplish these tasks (similar to Fritz et al., 2013): (a) two experimenters independently viewed the videos and recorded potential pre- cursors using a data sheet containing groups of response topographies (e.g., vocalizations, loco- motion); (b) the experimenters compared their recordings and created definitions of each potential precursor; and (c) the experimenters independently viewed each video, recorded whether or not each potential precursor occurred during the trial, and compared their recordings until 100% agreement was attained for each potential precursor. If any discrepan-
  • 23. cies occurred between experimenters, they reviewed the video, modified the potential pre- cursor definitions, and rescored the trial. We calculated the following probabilities fol- lowing the completion of the precursor analy- sis: (a) the probability of the precursor given the target [p(P|T)] by dividing the number of trials with aggression and the precursor by the number of trials with aggression, (b) the proba- bility of the precursor given the absence of the target [p(P| ~ T)] by dividing the number of trials without aggression that contained the pre- cursor by the number of trials without aggres- sion, (c) the probability of aggression given the 669PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL precursor [p(T|P)] by dividing the number of trials with the precursor and aggression by the number of trials with the precursor, and (d) the probability of aggression given the absence of the precursor [p(T| ~ P)] by dividing the num- ber of trials without the precursor that con- tained aggression by the number of trials without the precursor. We also calculated the unconditional probabilities of the precursor behaviors [p(P)], by dividing the number trials containing the precursor by the number of total trials, and of the target behavior [p(T)], by dividing the number of trials containing aggres- sion by the number of total trials. We selected precursors, if (a) the probability
  • 24. of aggression given the precursor [p(T|P)] was greater than the probability of aggression given the absence of the precursor [ p(T| ~ P)] and the unconditional probability of aggression [p(T)], and (b) the probability of the precursor given aggression [p(P|T)] was greater than the probability of the precursor given the absence of aggression [p(P| ~ T)] and the unconditional probability of the precursor behaviors [p(P)] (Fritz et al., 2013). Functional Analysis Following the precursor analysis, we con- ducted independent FAs to confirm the func- tional relation between the seven identified precursors and aggression. Sessions lasted 10 min, and conditions were conducted in the following order: attention, tangible, play, and demand (Hammond, Iwata, Rooker, Fritz, & Bloom, 2013) using procedures similar to Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1994). Brandon was seated at a table and the therapist was positioned approximately 90–150 cm away from Brandon at the begin- ning of all sessions. We conducted the precur- sor FA first, during which condition-specific consequences were provided contingent on the occurrence of precursors and aggression was on extinction. Next, we conducted the aggression FA during which the condition-specific conse- quences were provided contingent on the occurrence of aggression and precursors were on extinction. Materials, therapist interactions, and condition-specific consequences were iden-
  • 25. tical to those implemented during trials in the precursor analysis. During each FA, we collected data on the frequency of aggression (as defined above in the precursor analysis) and the frequency of the seven precursor behaviors identified in the pre- cursor analysis, which included standing up, heavy breathing, approaching instructor, fold- ing hands, knee hitting, rubbing face, and neg- ative vocalizing. Operational definitions of all precursors are available from the first author. We calculated exact interval agreement per- centages during 50% of FA sessions by compar- ing the primary and secondary data collectors’ recorded frequencies of responses during 10-s intervals during the FAs. Overall, mean IOA scores were 97% (range, 90%–100%) and 95% (range, 90%–100%) for the precursor and aggression FAs, respectively. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results of Brandon’s precursor analysis are in Figure 1. Seven (denoted by the aster- isks) of the 25 potential behaviors were identi- fied as precursors based on the criteria described above. Four of the precursors (knee hitting, rubbing face, heavy breathing, and folding hands) were perfectly correlated with aggression [i.e., p(T|P) = 1.0] and never occurred in trials in which aggression was not observed [i.e., p(P| ~ T) = 0]. However, aggres- sion still occurred in the absence of these four precursors and the probability of these precur-
  • 26. sors occurring given aggression was low. The other three precursors (standing up, negative vocalizing, and approaching instructor) occurred less frequently [i.e., p(T|P) < 1.0] but the relative conditional probabilities showed MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.670 that these behaviors predicted the occurrence of aggression. Figure 2 depicts the results of precursor and aggression FAs. We observed high rates of precursors (left panel) and high rates of aggres- sion (right panel) during the three test condi- tions during precursor and aggression FAs, respectively. This pattern of behavior suggests that combined precursors and aggression were multiply controlled by the same variables. In addition, Brandon demonstrated reduced rates of aggression during the precursor FA, which replicates similar findings reported in previous studies (e.g., Fritz et al., 2013). Figure 1. The top panel depicts the conditional probability of the target (T) given the presence [p(T|P)] and absence [p(T| ~ P)] of potential precursors (P) and the unconditional probability of the target [p(T)]. The bottom panel depicts the conditional probability of potential precursors (P) given the presence [p(P|T)] and absence [p(P| ~ T) of the target and the unconditional probability of each precursor [p(P)]. The asterisks denote behaviors identified as precursors.
  • 27. 671PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL We calculated the proportion of each precur- sor during the precursor FA (Figure 3) which yielded several interesting outcomes. First, similar to Fritz et al. (2013), not all identified precursors occurred during the precursor FA. Knee hits and heavy breathing did not occur during the precur- sor FA despite being perfectly correlated with aggression in the precursor analysis. It is unclear why this occurred, but several explanations are possible. It is possible that these precursors were associated with increased response effort or did not have as recent a history of reinforcement rela- tive to the other topographies of precursors. Alternatively, it may be the case that the precur- sor analysis resulted in some false positives. Second, the most common precursors observed were negative vocalizing, approaching instructor, and standing up. Although these responses were not perfectly correlated with aggression in the precursor analysis, they occurred most frequently during the precursor FA. The inclusion of standing up and approach- ing instructor as precursor behaviors may be considered a limitation, as these responses may have been part of a response chain to aggression. If these were early responses in the chain, then, if reinforced, they would likely increase levels of aggression (Dracobly & Smith, 2012). However, data from the precursor FA do not support this interpretation, as reduced levels of aggression
  • 28. were observed when precursors were reinforced. There is currently no established technology to distinguish precursors from links in a chain to a target behavior (Hagopian, Paclawskyj, & Kuhn, 2005). Future research is needed to develop such a technology. Third, a different precursor occurred most frequently in each test condition (e.g., vocalizing negatively in the demand condition). This pattern of responding may suggest different topographies of precursors Figure 2. This graph depicts the precursor (left panel) and aggression (right panel) FAs. The top and bottom panels display the rate of combined precursors and aggression, respectively. MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.672 precede a multiply controlled target behavior under different evocative situations. Future research could further evaluate this possibility. Several limitations are worth noting. The current evaluation included only one partici- pant, thus limiting external validity. Future studies should replicate these findings with additional participants. Another mitigating fac- tor was the lack of an alone condition during the FAs. The success of the treatment imple- mented based on the results of the FAs (these data are not included in the manuscript) sug- gests that Brandon’s target behavior was unlikely maintained by automatic reinforce-
  • 29. ment; however, it is possible that some of the precursor behaviors were. The inclusion of an alone condition would have allowed an evalua- tion of this possibility. Although we attempted to ensure the oppor- tunity to engage in all responses during the FAs by seating Brandon at a table and positioning the therapist a consistent distance away to begin all sessions, some conditions required therapist behavior that would necessarily restrict Brandon’s ability to engage in some responses. This limitation is exemplified by the following situation: If the therapist used physical guidance during the demand condi- tion, Brandon would be unable to approach the therapist. Despite these limitations, the cur- rent evaluation provides support for an analysis to identify precursors to a problem behavior. REFERENCES Borrero, C. S. W., & Borrero, J. C. (2008). Descriptive and experimental analyses of potential precursors to problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. 2008.41-83 Dracobly, J. D., & Smith, R. G. (2012). Progressing from identification and functional analysis of precursor behavior to treatment of self-injurious behavior. Jour- nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 361–374. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361 Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Hammond, J. L., &
  • 30. Bloom, S. E. (2013). Experimental analysis of precur- sors to severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 101–129. https://doi.org/10. 1002/jaba.27 Hagopian, L. P., Paclawskyj, T. R., & Kuhn, S. C. (2005). The use of conditional probability analysis to identify a response chain leading to the occurrence of eye poking. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 393–397. Hammond, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Rooker, G. W., Fritz, J. N., & Bloom, S. E. (2013). Effects of fixed versus random condition sequencing during Figure 3. The bottom graph depicts the proportion of precursors that were scored in the precursor FA. Knee hits and heavy breathing were not observed in any precursor FA condition. 673PRECURSORS AND MULTIPLE CONTROL https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-83 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-83 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361 https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.27 https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.27 multielement functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 22–30. https://doi.org/10. 1002/jaba.7 Herscovitch, B., Roscoe, E. M., Libby, M. E., Bourret, J. C., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). A proce- dure for identifying precursors to problem behavior.
  • 31. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 697–702. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-697 Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. E., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209. https://doi.org/10. 1901/jaba.1994.27-197 Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification of environmental determinants of behavior disorders through functional analysis of precursor behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–136. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125 Received September 27, 2015 Final acceptance October 2, 2016 Action Editor, Claudia Dozier MEGAN A. BORLASE et al.674 https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.7 https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.7 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-697 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-197 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-197 https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125 ANALYSIS OF PRECURSORS TO MULTIPLY CONTROLLED PROBLEM BEHAVIOR: A REPLICATIONMETHODParticipant and SettingPrecursor AnalysisFunctional AnalysisRESULTS AND DISCUSSIONReferences