SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 132
Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost Containment
Employee health is just one of the many reasons the
absenteeism rate continues to rise in
organizations and worldwide. When employees have poor
health, they are not able to perform at
their fullest potential. According to the Bureau of Labor
Statistics (2018), “About 4.2 million workers
in January 2018 missed work because they had an illness,
injury, or medical problem or appointment”
(para. 1). Employers are constantly reviewing the cost of
healthcare premiums while not taking a
closer examination of the actual �nancial and productivity loss
that occurs when employees are not
performing their assigned jobs. All absences should be tracked
(planned and unplanned). The human
resources (HR) professional will typically have data on
vacations, on-the-job injuries, the Family and
Medical Leave Act (FMLA) related absences and disabilities
since these absences are closely tied to
the employees’ pay and are easy to identify. Today, many health
insurance companies provide
employees or employers with an array of wellness program
options. Some employers may even offer
wellness education workshops and virtual care. To truly
understand the costs associated with health-
related absences, the employer can sift through reports on
obesity, high blood pressure, depression,
etc. The HR professional can also review articles published by
the Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention. “It is advisable to commit to health promotion with
a corresponding commitment to data
collection. Without data, evaluation is impossible” (Cascio &
Boudreau, 2011, p. 123).
To evaluate the effectiveness of a wellness program, you may
use one of the following methods:
Cost-effective analysis
Cost-bene�t and return-on-investment analysis
The employer must take the initiative, value the employees,
focus on a long-lasting employee or
employer relationships, and design wellness programs that �t
within the company culture. The
ultimate objective is to have healthier employees, reduce
productivity loss, and lower the
absenteeism rate and health insurance premiums.
Workplace Wellness Programs
On-site �tness center or health
club membership
Employee assistance program
Healthy snacks Yoga
Chair massage Meditation breaks
Reference:
Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018, March 29). TED: The
Economics Daily Image. Retrieved from http
s://www.bls.gov/opub/ted/2018/4-point-2-million-workers-have-
illness-related-work-absences-in-j
anuary-2018.htm
Wellness challenges Smoking cessation programs
Transit options
Additional Materials
From your course textbook, Process Metrics and Measurement
Complete Self-Assessment Guide,
review the following chapters:
Criterion #2: De�ne
Criterion #3: Measure
From the South University Online Library, review the following
article:
Meet the Wellness Programs That Save Companies Money
(https://www.thecampuscommon.com/library/ezproxy/ticketdem
ocs.asp?
sch=suo&turl=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&db=bth&AN=118686064&site=eds-live)
From the Internet, review the following:
Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018, March 29).TED: The
Economics Daily Image. Retrieved from
https://www.bls.gov/opub/ted/2018/4-point-2-million-workers-
have-illness-related-work-abs
ences-in-january-2018.htm
https://www.thecampuscommon.com/library/ezproxy/ticketdemo
cs.asp?sch=suo&turl=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?di
rect=true&db=bth&AN=118686064&site=eds-live
O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E
Gender Harassment: Broadening Our Understanding
of Sex-Based Harassment at Work
Emily A. Leskinen • Lilia M. Cortina •
Dana B. Kabat
Published online: 27 July 2010
� American Psychology-Law Society/Division 41 of the
American Psychological Association 2010
Abstract This study challenges the common legal and
organizational practice of privileging sexual advance forms
of sex-based harassment, while neglecting gender harass-
ment. Survey data came from women working in two male-
dominated contexts: the military and the legal profession.
Their responses to the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire
(SEQ) revealed five typical profiles of harassment: low
victimization, gender harassment, gender harassment with
unwanted sexual attention, moderate victimization, and
high victimization. The vast majority of harassment vic-
tims fell into one of the first two groups, which described
virtually no unwanted sexual advances. When compared to
non-victims, gender-harassed women showed significant
decrements in professional and psychological well-being.
These findings underscore the seriousness of gender
harassment, which merits greater attention by both law and
social science.
Keywords Gender harassment � Sexual harassment �
Working women � Well-being
In 1964, Title VII of the Civil Rights Act codified the ille-
gality of sex discrimination. However, it took another
13 years for a federal appellate court to recognize sexual
harassment as a form of sex discrimination (e.g., Barnes v.
Costle, 1977). The prevailing view of sexual harassment,
both then and now, sees unwanted sexual attention as ‘‘the
quintessential harassment’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1710). Among
legal scholars, a notable group has emerged who contend
that this conceptualization is too narrow, arguing that
‘‘gender harassment’’ should be included in legal under-
standings of sex-based harassment (Epstein, 1998; Franke
1995, 1997, 2004; Growe, 2007; Hébert, 2005; Shultz, 1998,
2003, 2006). Gender harassment refers to ‘‘a form of hostile
environment harassment that appears to be motivated by
hostility toward individuals who violate gender ideals rather
than by desire for those who meet them’’ (Berdahl, 2007a,
p. 425). In this article, we lend empirical support to the
assertion that gender harassment is a serious form of sex
discrimination that deserves more attention. Using survey
data from two samples of working women, we demonstrate
that most sexual harassment in traditionally male domains
entails gender harassment in the absence of sexual advan-
ces; we also show how these experiences are associated with
negative personal and professional outcomes.
Central Constructs
In its broadest sense, sex-based harassment
1
refers to
‘‘behavior that derogates, demeans, or humiliates an indi -
vidual based on that individual’s sex’’ (Berdahl, 2007b,
E. A. Leskinen (&) � D. B. Kabat
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan,
530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
L. M. Cortina
Departments of Psychology and Women’s Studies,
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
1
Psychology and the law have most commonly referred to this
phenomenon as ‘‘sexual harassment.’’ However, Berdahl
(2007b)
makes a compelling case that ‘‘sex-based harassment’’ is a
better
construct label, as it emphasizes sex (in the sense of femaleness
or
maleness) rather than sexuality or sexual desire. We use both
terms
interchangeably—‘‘sexual harassment’’ and ‘‘sex-based harass-
ment’’—to be consistent with the terminology of other literature
but
also encourage revision of that terminology.
123
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
DOI 10.1007/s10979-010-9241-5
p. 644). Subsumed under this umbrella term are (at least)
three related categories of behavior (e.g., Fitzgerald, Gelf-
and, & Drasgow, 1995; Fitzgerald, Swan, & Magley, 1997).
First, gender harassment refers to ‘‘a broad range of verbal
and nonverbal behaviors not aimed at sexual cooperation but
that convey insulting, hostile, and degrading attitudes about
women’’ (Fitzgerald et al., 1995, p. 430). Examples of
gender harassment include anti-female jokes, comments
that women do not belong in management, and crude terms
of address that denigrate women (e.g., referring to a cow -
orker as a ‘‘dumb slut’’). By contrast, unwanted sexual
attention involves expressions of romantic or sexual interest
that are unwelcome, unreciprocated, and offensive to the
recipient (e.g., unwanted touching, pressure for dates or
sexual behavior). The third category is sexual coercion:
bribes or threats that make the conditions of the victim’s
employment contingent on her sexual cooperation (e.g.,
offering a promotion in exchange for sexual favors, threat-
ening termination unless sexual demands are met).
Lim and Cortina (2005) elaborated on the relationships
among gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and
sexual coercion. They explained that ‘‘unwanted sexual
attention, as the name suggests, represents unwelcomed,
unreciprocated behaviors aimed at establishing some form
of sexual relationship. One could argue that sexual coerci on
is a specific, severe, rare form of unwanted sexual attention,
involving similar sexual advances coupled with bribery or
threats to force acquiescence’’ (p. 484). In stark contrast,
gender harassment communicates hostility that is devoid of
sexual interest. Gender harassment can include sexually
crude terminology or displays (for instance, calling a col -
league a ‘‘cunt’’ or telling a sexually graphic joke about her),
but these behaviors differ from unwanted sexual attention in
that they aim to insult and reject women, not pull them into a
sexual relationship. In colloquial terms, the difference
between unwanted sexual attention/coercion versus gender
harassment is analogous to the difference between a ‘‘come
on’’ versus a ‘‘put down’’ (Fitzgerald et al., 1995).
Gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and
sexual coercion refer to behaviors rather than legal con-
structs. That said, sexual coercion is roughly parallel to what
the law calls quid pro quo harassment, whereas unwanted
sexual attention and gender harassment together map onto
the legal category of hostile environment harassment (e.g.,
Fitzgerald et al., 1995; Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin, Gelfand,
& Magley, 1997). The next section elaborates on these legal
understandings of sexual harassment.
Legal Perspectives on Sexual Harassment
Title VII prohibits employers from discriminating against
any individual with regard to ‘‘compensation, terms,
conditions, or privileges of employment, because of such
individual’s…sex’’.2 When the federal courts first recog-
nized sexual harassment as a Title VII violation, they were
acting on cases in which women had lost jobs for failing to
comply with their employers’ sexual demands, termed
‘‘quid pro quo harassment’’ (beginning with Barnes v.
Costle, 1977). It took another decade before the U.S.
Supreme Court would rule that ‘‘hostile work environment
harassment’’ could constitute unlawful sexual harassment.
In Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson (1986), and later reaf-
firmed in Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc. (1993), the Court
described hostile environment sexual harassment as
occurring ‘‘[w]hen the workplace is permeated with ‘dis-
criminatory intimidation, ridicule, and insult’… that is
‘sufficiently severe or pervasive to alter the conditions of
the victim’s employment and create an abusive working
environment.’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc., 1993,
p. 21). Hostile environment sexual harassment is the pri -
mary focus of the current article.
According to both Meritor and Harris, to create a leg-
ally actionable hostile environment, the sexually harassing
conduct must be either severe or pervasive. In Harris, the
Court provided further guidance for determining whether a
hostile work environment is present: the harassing conduct
must pass both an objective test (a ‘‘reasonable’’ person
would find it hostile or abusive) and a subjective test (the
victim must have experienced it as abusive). Referring to
the objective test, it added that ‘‘whether an environment is
‘hostile’ or ‘abusive’ can be determined only by looking at
all the circumstances’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc.,
1993, p. 22).
Five years later, in Oncale v Sundowner Offshore Ser-
vices, Inc. (1998), the Supreme Court elaborated on what
should be considered when examining ‘‘all the circum-
stances,’’ explicitly mandating attention to the larger social
context. The Oncale decision also described types of
conduct that might be considered ‘‘severe.’’ Particularly
relevant to our article, it clearly stated that a motivation of
sexual desire is not a prerequisite for establishing objective
severity:
…harassing conduct need not be motivated by sexual
desire to support an inference of discrimination on
the basis of sex. A Trier of fact might reasonably find
such discrimination, for example, if a female victim
is harassed in such sex-specific and derogatory
terms…as to make it clear that the harasser is moti-
vated by general hostility to the presence of women
2
In addition, Title VII prohibits discrimination based on race,
color,
religion, or national origin. Due to space constraints, this article
only
details how Title VII has been interpreted to prohibit sexual
harassment.
26 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
in the workplace. (Oncale v Sundowner Offshore
Services, Inc., 1998, p. 80).
Despite this last development in judicial interpretations of
Title VII, the prevailing legal conception of sexual harass-
ment remains a highly sexualized one, in which sexually
advancing or threatening conduct is seen as ‘‘the essence of
harassment’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1716). The harassing
behaviors alleged in Oncale, although recognized as moti -
vated by hostility rather than desire, still involved sexually
predatory behavior (e.g., sodomy with a bar of soap, threa-
tened rape). The Supreme Court has never clearly stated
whether the harassing conduct itself (as opposed to the
motivation for the conduct) must involve some form of
sexual advance to violate Title VII. Some appellate decisions
have rejected this requirement of sexualized content; for
example, in Williams v. General Motors Corp. (1999), the
Sixth Circuit Court of Appeals stated that: ‘‘harassing
behavior that is not sexually explicit but is directed at women
and motivated by discriminatory animus against women
satisfies the ‘based on sex’ requirement.’’ The federal bench,
however, is far from unanimity on this issue. In the same
Williams v GMC (1999) case, one judge filed a dissenting
opinion, arguing vehemently that ‘‘…Title VII does not
proscribe ‘anti-female animus’ at all’’ and ‘‘the broad new
standard my colleagues have conjured here is not a correct
application of Title VII sex discrimination law presently on
the books.’’ Some courts routinely either dismiss hostile
environment cases that do not involve sexual conduct, or
they ‘‘disaggregate’’ sexual from nonsexual conduct and
then deem the latter to be irrelevant to a hostile environment
claim (see Franke, 2004, Growe, 2007, and Schultz, 2006 for
various post-Oncale case examples). Thus, the privileging of
the sexual advance in sexual harassment law continues.
To summarize, on many occasions the federal judiciary
has indicated, implicitly and explicitly, that offensive
behavior must reference sexuality to constitute unlawful
sex-based harassment. As a result, gender harassment
involving no sexual advances routinely gets neglected by
the law. This occurs even when the behavior fits all other
characteristics of a legally actionable hostile environment:
occurring ‘‘because of’’ the victim’s sex (interpreting
‘‘sex’’ to mean femaleness rather than sexuality); being
‘‘severe or pervasive’’ enough to adversely change the
conditions of her employment; and creating a work envi-
ronment that a ‘‘reasonable’’ person would find hostile or
abusive, and that the victim herself finds as such. Promi -
nent legal scholars have critiqued the exclusively sexual
view of sex-based harassment, arguing for instance that
‘‘most harassment is not designed to achieve sexual grati-
fication. Instead, it is used to preserve the sex segregation
of jobs by claiming the most highly rewarded forms of
work as masculine in composition and content’’ (Schultz,
2006, p. 22; see also Epstein, 1998; Franke, 1995, 1997,
2004; Growe, 2007; Shultz, 1998, 2003). At the same time
that these issues have been debated in law reviews, an
empirical literature on sexual harassment has developed in
psychology. How has psychological science made sense of
harassment based on sex and gender?
Psychological Research on Sexual Harassment
In psychology, researchers have examined lay perceptions
of sexual harassment more than any other aspect of sexual
harassment. These perceptions have differed over time,
between men and women, and across cultures (Cortina &
Berdahl, 2008; Rotundo, Nguyen, & Sackett, 2001). One
finding has not changed, however: when researchers have
compared perceptions of gender harassment versus
unwanted sexual attention/coercion, participants have
consistently rated the former as less severe, less offensive,
and less likely to represent what they see as ‘‘sexual
harassment’’ (e.g., Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo,
Reid, & Deaux, 1995; Tang, Yik, Cheung, & Choi, 1995).
Quite separate from studies of sexual harassment per-
ceptions within the lay public have been surveys of actual
harassment experiences among working adults. As in the
law, much of this work has concentrated on sexually
advancing behaviors. For instance, the U.S. Merit Systems
Protection Board (USMSPB) surveyed federal employees
in 1980, 1987, and 1994 about their encounters with
‘‘sexual harassment,’’ defined as ‘‘uninvited and unwel-
come sexual attention and/or behavior’’ (USMSPB, 1994,
p. vi). Employees were asked to indicate the extent that
they had experienced a list of specific acts, virtually all of
which contained some form of sexual advance or sexual
threat (from unwanted touching to pressure for dates to
sexual assault). These surveys were well-executed and
have had a major impact on the field. However, they
neglected gender harassment.
Unlike the USMSPB, some sexual harassment
researchers routinely include questions about gender
harassment in their surveys. This is true, for example, of
the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), which has
become the most widely used and validated measure of
sexual harassment experiences (Fitzgerald et al., 1988;
Fitzgerald, Magley, Drasgow, & Waldo, 1999; Stark,
Chernyshenko, Lancaster, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2002).
Some versions of the SEQ even assess two subtypes of
gender harassment—both ‘‘sexist’’ and ‘‘crude/offensive’’
behavior (Stark et al., 2002). When SEQ researchers have
divided harassment into its various subtypes, they have
found gender harassment to be the most common (e.g.,
Fitzgerald et al., 1988, 1999; Langhout et al., 2005;
Schneider, Swan, & Fitzgerald, 1997). Little SEQ research,
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 27
123
however, has focused on experiences of gender harassment
in isolation from other behaviors. Studies of sexual
harassment prevalence and outcomes—using the SEQ and
other instruments—typically collapse across the different
facets of behavior for an overall measure of ‘‘offensive sex-
related experiences in the workplace’’ (Fitzgerald, Swan
et al., 1997, p. 9; for examples from other research pro-
grams, see Barling, Rogers, & Kelloway, 2001; Culbertson
& Rosenfeld, 1994; Richman, Shinsako, Rospenda, Flah-
erty, & Freels, 2002). Although this practice permits
examination of sexual harassment as a holistic phenome-
non, and has yielded many important findings, it obscures
the unique experience and impact of gender harassment.
In addition to survey research, some psychologists have
approached the study of sexual harassment from an
experimental perspective. This work has also been char-
acterized by an emphasis on sexual attention. For instance,
lab studies have operationalized men’s sexually harassing
behavior only as the sexual touching of women (Perry,
Kulik, & Schmidtke, 1998; Pryor, 1987) or as sexually
suggestive questioning (Woodzicka & LaFrance, 2005). In
these examples and others, there was virtually no attention
to gender harassment.
3
While gender harassment has been neglected in research,
this behavior has also almost certainly gone unreported in
organizations. Studies have found that victims who perceive
the harassment as more severe are more likely to report their
experiences to a superior (Bergman, Langhout, Palmieri,
Cortina, & Fitzgerald, 2002). However, research into lay
perceptions of sexual harassment (described above; e.g.,
Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo et al., 1995; Tang
et al., 1995) suggests that gender harassment is widely
believed to be inconsequential, or somehow less important
than unwanted sexual attention in the workplace. Victims
should, therefore, be less likely to see gender harassment as
worthy of reporting, which means that organizational
authorities should be less likely to intervene (Langhout
et al., 2005). This makes it all the more imperative that
social science bring gender harassment to the fore, so that it
may be recognized as a legitimate and serious form of sex-
based discrimination in the workplace.
If It’s ‘‘Just’’ Gender Harassment, Why Should
We Care?
Because gender harassment has no explicit, sexually
predatory component to it (unlike unwanted sexual
attention or sexual coercion), it may seem less worthy of
scientific or legal scrutiny. However, past research on
everyday sexism has found that regular sexist interactions
decrease psychological well-being and predict symptoms
of psychological trauma (Berg, 2006; Swim, Hyers, Cohen,
& Ferguson, 2001). For example, Swim and colleagues
(2001) asked participants to keep track of instances of
ordinary sexist behavior (e.g., anti-female jokes, comments
reflecting gender stereotypes) observed or experienced in
any life setting. They found that these everyday sexist
encounters were associated with greater anger, anxiety, and
depression. To explain these negative outcomes, Swim and
colleagues (2001) argued that everyday sexism triggers
feelings of stereotype threat, defined as ‘‘being at risk of
confirming, as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype
about one’s group’’ (Steele & Aronson, 1995, p. 797).
Similar to everyday sexism, gender harassment may foster
stereotype threat in women, especially those working in
traditionally masculine domains. Gender harassment may
be used to cue women that they are inadequate, out of
place, and unable to perform at the level of men. The
associated experience of stereotype threat could set off a
cascade of negative outcomes in victims.
Research has demonstrated that sexual harassment is
linked with a wide range of victim outcomes (for recent
reviews, see Berdahl & Raver 2010; Cortina & Berdahl,
2008; Foote & Goodman-Delahunty, 2005). For instance,
studies have found that sexual harassment is associated
with decreased satisfaction with one’s job and professional
relationships, loss of productivity, and increased turnover
intentions and behaviors (e.g., Barling et al., 2001;
Langhout et al., 2005; Sims, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2005).
Moreover, the consequences of sexual harassment are not
constrained to the job site. Victims also report lower psy-
chological well-being, more physical health problems, and
even symptoms of traumatic stress (e.g., Culbertson &
Rosenfeld, 1994; Fitzgerald, Swan et al., 1997; Richman,
Shinsako, Rospenda, Flaherty, & Freels, 2002). By and
large, however, this research has analyzed ‘‘sexual
harassment’’ as a global phenomenon, failing to differen-
tiate among the subtypes of sex-harassing behavior. It is,
therefore, impossible to know from this work whether
gender harassment by itself would have the same adverse
implications for employee well-being.
Hypotheses
In sum, legal scholars have developed compelling theories
about the importance of gender harassment, which we
sought to test using large-scale survey research. In line with
prior studies, we hypothesized that gender harassme nt,
without unwanted sexual attention or coercion, would be
3
Notable exceptions exist, however, in the experimental
literature on
sexual harassment. For instance Schneider, Tomaka, and
Palacios
(2001) studied sexual harassment in the laboratory by exposing
women to sexist comments—a clear form of gender harassment.
28 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
the most common form of sex-based harassment that
women experience (Hypothesis 1). We also hypothesized
that women would report negative professional and per-
sonal outcomes, even when they ‘‘only’’ experience gender
harassment (Hypothesis 2). We tested these hypotheses
with survey data from women working in two male-dom-
inated domains: the U.S. Military (Study 1) and federal
legal practice (Study 2). Performing jobs that are highly
nontraditional for their gender, women in these domains
blur the boundaries between stereotypically ‘‘male’’ and
‘‘female’’ behavior. This makes them particularly vulner-
able to being scorned and rejected (e.g., gender harassed)
by colleagues who value rigid and clear distinctions
between the sexes (Berdahl, 2007a, b).
Study 1: The Military Survey
Participants and Procedure
Study 1 involved secondary analysis of survey data col -
lected by the U.S. Military. This survey began with a non-
proportional stratified, single stage random sample of
active-duty members from all branches of the U.S. Military
(Army, Navy, Air Force, Marines, and Coast Guard);
women and people of color were oversampled. The initial
sample contained 60,415 individuals, of whom 53,170
were deemed ‘‘eligible’’ (reasons for ineligibility were
various, such as inability to locate the sample member).
These individuals were invited to complete surveys either
on paper or online, and 19,960 usable surveys were
returned (38% response rate). The current study focused on
the 9,725 women who responded to the survey. Just over
one-half of these women identified as White (55%), one-
quarter as Black or African American (24%), and 11% as
Hispanic or Latina. Forty-eight percent of the respondents
reported some college, and 38% reported having at least a
4-year college degree. The number of years of active ser -
vice reported by members revealed a bi-modal distribution,
with 43% reporting less than 6 years and 33% reporting
10–20 years of active duty. For more information on this
sample and procedures, see Lipari and Lancaster (2003).
Measures
All participants completed the 2002 Department of Defense
Status of the Armed Forces Survey on Workplace and
Gender Relations.4 Descriptive statistics, coefficient
alphas, and intercorrelations for all variables analyzed in
Study 1 appear in Table 1. For multi-item scales, we
reverse-coded items as needed and then summed relevant
items to create scale-scores; higher scores reflect greater
levels of the underlying construct.
Sex-Based Harassment. To assess unwanted sex-
based experiences in the military, surveys contained an
updated version of the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire-
Department of Defense (SEQ-DoD) developed by Fitzgerald
and colleagues (1999; see also Stark et al., 2002).
Participants described how often over the prior 12 months
they had experienced various forms of unwanted, uninvited
‘‘sex/gender related talk and/or behavior’’ involving military
personnel, civilian employees, or contractors. They
responded on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 = never to
4 = very often. The measure consisted of 18 questions, in
which there are four subscales: (1) gender harassment: sexi st,
(2) gender harassment: crude, (3) unwanted sexual attention,
and (4) sexual coercion.
The gender harassment: sexist subscale measured
treatment that conveys explicit antipathy toward members
of one gender. The subscale consisted of four items, such
as ‘‘made offensive sexist remarks (for example, suggest-
ing that people of your gender are not suited for the kind of
work you do)’’ and ‘‘referred to people of your gender in
insulting or offensive terms.’’ Four items also assessed
experiences of gender harassment: crude behavior;
although sexual on the surface, this behavior expresses
animosity rather than attraction. Examples included:
‘‘made offensive remarks about your appearance, body, or
sexual activities’’ and ‘‘made gestures or used body lan-
guage of a sexual nature that embarrassed or offended
you.’’ The unwanted sexual attention subscale consisted of
six items, including ‘‘made unwanted attempts to establish
a romantic relationship with you despite your efforts to
discourage it’’ and ‘‘touched you in a way that made you
feel uncomfortable.’’ The sexual coercion subscale con-
tained four items, e.g., ‘‘implied faster promotions or better
treatment if you were sexually cooperative’’ For more
detail on this measure, including evidence of its high
reliability and validity (see Fitzgerald et al., 1999; Stark
et al., 2002).
Psychological Well-Being. Psychological well-being
was measured by the short, 5-item version of the Mental
Health Inventory (MHI-5) (Viet & Ware, 1983). On a scale
from 1 (little or none of the time) to 4 (all or most of the
time), survey respondents rated how often they had
experienced various psychological states over the prior
4 weeks. Examples of psychological states included in this
measure are: ‘‘felt calm and peaceful’’ and ‘‘felt so down in
the dumps that nothing could cheer you up.’’ Researchers
have found this scale to be reliable when used in the
general population (Berwick et al., 1991).
4
This is a recurring survey; prior surveys took place in 1995 and
1988.
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 29
123
Performance Decline Due to Poor Emotional
Health. In order to determine whether participants
experienced any difficulties during daily activities or
work as a result of mental health problems, surveys
included three questions adapted from the short-form
health survey (SF-36) used in the Medical Outcomes
Study (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Respondents rated how
often over the past 4 weeks had they experienced a
problem (such as ‘‘didn’t do work or other activities as
carefully as usual’’) with their work or other activities ‘‘as a
result of emotional problems (such as feeling depressed or
anxious).’’ Response options ranged from 1 (little or none
of the time) to 4 (all or most of the time).
Performance Decline Due to Poor Physical
Health. Surveys assessed health effects on work via
four items adapted from the SF-36 (Ware & Sherbourne,
1992). On a scale from 1 (little or none of the time) to 4 (all
or most of the time), respondents rated how often over the
past 4 weeks they had had problems with their work or
other daily activities ‘‘as a result of their physical health.’’
Examples of problems included: ‘‘were limited in the kind
of work or other activities you do’’ and ‘‘had diffi culty
performing the work or other activities you do (for
example, it took extra effort).’’
General Health. Surveys asked respondents about
their general health by having them respond to four
statements on a scale of 1 (definitely false) to 4 (definitely
true). Examples of statements included ‘‘I seem to get sick
a little easier than other people’’ (reverse coded), and ‘‘my
health is excellent.’’ These questions were adapted from the
SF-36 (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992).
Work Attitudes. Three measures tapped work
attitudes. On a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
(strongly agree), six items assessed coworker satisfaction;
examples included: ‘‘you are satisfied with the
relationships you have with your coworkers’’ and ‘‘there
is very little conflict among your coworkers.’’ This scale
was adapted from multiple sources, two items being taken
from the Job Satisfaction Survey (Spector, 1997), three
items adapted from the 1995 Armed Forces Sexual
Harassment Survey (Edwards, Elig, Edwards, & Riemer,
1997), and one item created for this survey.
Six items adapted from the 1995 Armed Forces Sexual
Harassment Survey (Edwards et al., 1997) measured work
satisfaction. These items included statements such as ‘‘you
like the kind of work you do’’ and ‘‘your work makes good
use of your skills.’’ Organizational commitment was
assessed using a modified version of Mowday, Steers, and
Porter’s (1979) Organizational Commitment Question-
naire. This scale contained four items, including ‘‘you areT
a
b
le
1
D
e
sc
ri
p
ti
v
e
st
a
ti
st
ic
s,
a
lp
h
a
c
o
e
ffi
c
ie
n
ts
,
a
n
d
c
o
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
s—
S
a
m
p
le
1
(M
il
it
a
ry
)
V
a
ri
a
b
le
s
M
S
D
a
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1
0
1
1
1
.
G
e
n
d
e
r
h
a
ra
ss
m
e
n
t:
S
e
x
is
t
2
.1
7
3
.2
4
.7
1
–
2
.
G
e
n
d
e
r
h
a
ra
ss
m
e
n
t:
C
ru
d
e
1
.6
1
2
.8
4
.6
7
.7
1
–
3
.
U
n
w
a
n
te
d
se
x
u
a
l
a
tt
e
n
ti
o
n
0
.8
4
2
.1
7
.7
5
.4
7
.6
6
–
4
.
S
e
x
u
a
l
c
o
e
rc
io
n
0
.2
3
1
.2
0
.7
9
.3
7
.4
7
.6
8
–
5
.
P
sy
c
h
o
lo
g
ic
a
l
w
e
ll
-b
e
in
g
1
5
.9
8
3
.2
2
.8
4
-
.2
8
-
.2
7
-
.2
1
-
.1
6
6
.
P
e
rf
o
rm
a
n
c
e
d
e
c
li
n
e
d
u
e
to
p
o
o
r
e
m
o
ti
o
n
a
l
h
e
a
lt
h
3
.9
3
1
.7
6
.9
1
.2
2
.2
2
.2
1
.1
6
-
.5
9
–
7
.
G
e
n
e
ra
l
h
e
a
lt
h
1
3
.3
7
2
.2
9
.7
7
-
.1
2
-
.1
3
-
.1
1
-
.0
9
.3
3
-
.2
5
–
8
.
P
e
rf
o
rm
a
n
c
e
d
e
c
li
n
e
d
u
e
to
p
o
o
r
p
h
y
si
c
a
l
h
e
a
lt
h
5
.1
9
2
.2
9
.8
9
.1
6
.1
6
.1
5
.1
4
-
.2
5
.3
1
-
.4
0
–
9
.
C
o
w
o
rk
e
r
sa
ti
sf
a
c
ti
o
n
2
1
.4
9
5
.0
5
.9
2
-
.3
1
-
.2
9
-
.1
9
-
.1
5
.3
2
-
.2
0
.1
1
-
.1
1
–
1
0
.
W
o
rk
S
a
ti
sf
a
c
ti
o
n
2
1
.3
7
5
.9
4
.9
1
-
.2
3
-
.2
0
-
.1
5
-
.1
1
.3
6
-
.2
5
.1
6
-
.1
5
.3
6
–
1
1
.
O
rg
a
n
iz
a
ti
o
n
a
l
c
o
m
m
it
m
e
n
t
1
6
.1
0
2
.9
2
.8
2
-
.1
3
-
.1
6
-
.1
3
-
.1
0
.2
9
-
.2
0
.1
8
-
.1
5
.2
7
.4
1
–
1
2
.
T
u
rn
o
v
e
r
in
te
n
ti
o
n
s
2
.9
1
1
.7
4
.8
0
.1
7
.1
5
.0
9
.0
8
-
.2
3
.1
7
-
.1
5
.1
5
-
.1
5
-
.2
6
-
.2
7
N
o
te
.
S
c
a
le
-s
c
o
re
s
w
e
re
d
e
ri
v
e
d
b
y
su
m
m
in
g
re
sp
o
n
se
s
a
c
ro
ss
a
ll
it
e
m
s
in
e
a
c
h
sc
a
le
fo
r
a
ll
w
o
m
e
n
in
th
e
sa
m
p
le
;
h
ig
h
e
r
sc
o
re
s
re
fl
e
c
te
d
g
re
a
te
r
le
v
e
ls
o
f
th
e
u
n
d
e
rl
y
in
g
c
o
n
st
ru
c
t.
A
ll
c
o
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
s
a
re
si
g
n
ifi
c
a
n
t,
p

.0
0
1
30 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
willing to make sacrifices to help your Service’’ and ‘‘you
are glad that you are a part of your Service.’’
Turnover Intentions. In order to measure
respondents’ thoughts and intentions of leaving military
employment, five items were adapted from the 1999 Survey
of Active Duty Personnel Form A (1999 ADS). Using a
dichotomous yes/no scale, respondents indicated whether
over the prior 6 months they had, for example, ‘‘Thought
seriously about leaving the military’’ or ‘‘Discussed leaving
and/or civilian opportunities with family or friends.’’
Control Variables. We controlled for race, rank, and
service branch in all outcome analyses. Respondents self-
reported their race (coded 0 = minority and 1 = white).
They also provided their rank (i.e., paygrade) at the time of
the survey. Response options ranged from E-1 to E-9 for
enlisted personnel; from W-1 to W-5 for warrant officers;
and from O-1/O1E to O-6 or above for commissioned
officers. When releasing these data to the public, the
Defense Manpower Data Center (DMDC) collapsed these
choices into five ordered categories: 1 = E1–E4; 2 = E5–
E9; 3 = W1–W5; 4 = O1–O3; and 5 = O4–O6.
Participants self-reported their service branch as either
Army, Navy, Air Force, Marines, or Coast Guard.
Results
Profiles of Sex-Based Harassment
In order to test our first hypothesis, we used k-means cluster
analysis. k-means cluster analysis groups persons who are
similar on specified variables (see Hartigan, 1975, for more
information on this analytic approach). In the present study,
the k-means analysis classified women by the type and
amount of sex-based harassment they had experienced, as
indicated on the SEQ-DoD. Included in this analysis were
all women who reported experiencing at least one behavior
on the SEQ-DoD at least one time over the previous
12 months (n = 5,698). After standardizing these women’s
scores on the four subscales of the SEQ-DoD, we requested
two-, three-, four-, five-, and six-cluster solutions, and chose
the five-cluster solution for further analysis. We based this
decision on theoretical interest; we wanted to isolate women
who had experienced primarily gender harassment without
unwanted sexual attention or coercion. Profiles of means on
the z-scored SEQ-DoD scales appear in Fig. 1.
The largest group consisted of women who reported the
lowest levels of harassment (Group 1; n = 3,933). As seen
in Fig. 1, the experiences they described almost exclu-
sively consisted of sexist behavior. The second-largest
group (n = 1,161) contained women who had encountered
both subtypes of gender harassment—sexist and crude—
but very little unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion
(henceforth, this group will be referred to as Group 2, the
‘‘Gender Harassment’’ group). Group 3 (n = 429) dis-
closed episodes of unwanted sexual attention, in addition to
moderate levels of sexist and crude behavior. Group 4
(n = 138) had encountered moderate levels of all types of
harassment measured by the SEQ-DoD. Group 5 (n = 37),
the smallest profile group, reported the most frequent
harassment on all four subscales. In sum, 89.4% of
harassment victims fell into Group 1 or 2, which described
experiences of gender harassment but virtually no unwan-
ted sexual attention or coercion. This pattern of
victimization provides strong support for Hypothesis 1,
which had predicted gender harassment (in the absence of
sexual attention or coercion) to be the most common
manifestation of sex-based harassment.
-2
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
Gender
Harassment:
Sexist
Gender
Harassment:
Crude
Unwanted
Sexual
Attention
Sexual
Coercion
Z
-S
c
o
re
s
High Victimization (0.6%) -
Group 5
Moderate Victimization
(2.4%) - Group 4
Gender Harassment and
Unwanted Sexual
Attention (7.5%) - Group 3
Gender Harassment
(20.4%) - Group 2
Low Victimization (69%) -
Group 1
Fig. 1 Profiles of harassment
among military women
(n = 5,698)
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 31
123
In addition to the five groups revealed through cluster
analysis, we identified a sixth ‘‘Nonvictims’’ group, which
contained all women who had responded ‘‘never’’ to all
items of the SEQ-DoD. This group (n = 4,014) served as a
comparison group in subsequent analyses.
Table 2 reports demographic information for each of
the profile groups. Using chi-square and ANOVA analy-
ses, we found significant relationships between profile
membership and race, v2 (5, N = 9711) = 55.34, p
.001; rank, v2 (10, N = 9704) = 159.00, p  .001; years
served, F (5, 9664) = 81.90, p  .001; education level, F
(5, 9621) = 32.14, p  .001; and service branch, v2 (20,
N = 9712) = 246.04, p  .001. As seen in Table 2, the
racial make-up of each profile group generally mirrored
the overall sample. However, white respondents were
slightly more likely to be in the Nonvictims Group, Group
1 (Low Victimization), or Group 2 (Gender Harassment).
Ethnic minority respondents were more likely to appear in
Group 5 (High Victimization). Regarding military rank
and group membership, commissioned officers were dis-
proportionately likely to appear in Group 1 (Low
Victimization), whereas enlisted personnel were dispro-
portionately represented in Group 3 (Gender Harassment
with Unwanted Sexual Attention). In terms of tenure (i.e.,
years served) in the military, Nonvictims differed from all
other groups by having served the greatest average number
of years. Educational differences were most pronounced
for the Nonvictims and Group 1 (Low Victimization), who
had significantly higher educational levels than other
groups.
Outcomes of Sex-Based Harassment
For theoretical reasons, we were most interested in out-
comes for the group that had experienced primarily gender
harassment (Group 2: the Gender Harassment group
5
),
which we compared to outcomes for women in the Non-
victims group. A comparison of these two groups addresses
the question of whether experiences of ‘‘just’’ gender
harassment are associated with adverse consequences.
In order to equalize cell sizes for this analysis, we ran-
domly selected 1,000 women from the Gender Harassment
group and 1,000 women from the Nonvictims group. Using
multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), we then
compared these two groups on the mental health, physical
health, and organizational outcome variables. Covariates in
this analysis were racial minority status, military rank, and
service branch (to account for factors that could potentially
affect harassment risk). We found a significant multivariate
main effect of gender harassment on the collection of
outcomes, Wilks’ Lambda = .81, F (8, 1822) = 52.06,
p  .001.
As Table 3 shows, women in the Gender Harassment
group scored significantly lower than the Nonvictimized
women on all work attitudes (work satisfaction, coworker
satisfaction, and organizational commitment). They also
reported greater performance decline due to both physical
and emotional health, and they described less overall
psychological well-being and health satisfaction. Further-
more, women in the Gender Harassment group disclosed
greater thoughts and intentions of leaving their jobs.
Cohen’s effect sizes (d) ranged from .22 to .79, averaging
Table 2 Descriptive statistics for profile groups—Sample 1
(Military)
Profile group Non-
White
percent
Rank percent Years
served
Education Branch of service (%)
Enlisted Warrant
officer
Commissioned
officer
Mean SD Mean SD Army Navy Marine
corps
Air
Force
Coast
guard
Nonvictims (n = 4,014) 47.0 69.8 3.6 26.5 2.28 1.07 2.21 0.70
26.6 21.1 11.0 34.4 6.9
Group 1: Low Victimization
(n = 3,933)
42.1 68.2 3.3 28.4 2.07 1.06 2.21 0.70 28.9 21.4 13.8 26.8 9.1
Group 2: Gender Harassment
(n = 1,161)
39.8 75.3 3.6 20.9 1.82 1.02 2.11 0.69 27.6 24.4 17.5 19.8 10.7
Group 3: Gender Harassment
& Unwanted Sexual
Attention (n = 429)
51.3 91.6 0.9 7.5 1.47 0.81 1.87 0.63 34.5 24.7 14.7 15.4 10.7
Group 4: Moderate
Victimization (n = 138)
55.8 92.0 0.0 8.0 1.50 0.83 1.83 0.70 39.1 22.5 21.7 9.4 7.2
Group 5: High Victimization
(all types) (n = 37)
70.3 91.9 0.0 8.1 1.65 0.95 1.83 0.57 56.8 2.7 29.7 5.4 5.4
5
We did not include Group 1 (the ‘‘low victimization’’ group) in
outcome analyses, even though their experiences largely
consisted of
gender harassment, for two primary reasons. First, it is unlikely
that
the extremely low rates of harassing behavior described by this
group
would be seen as ‘‘sufficiently severe or pervasive’’ to be
actionable
under Title VII. In addition, we hope to avoid the criticism that
we are
‘‘making mountains out of molehills’’ by foregrounding
conduct that,
while offensive, is transient and rare.
32 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
.48; the largest differences emerged for coworker satis-
faction (.79), psychological well-being (.67), and work
satisfaction (.58). These findings provided strong support
for Hypothesis 2, which had predicted that experiences of
gender harassment (alone) would be associated with neg-
ative outcomes.
To provide an additional point of comparison, we also
combined Groups 3, 4, and 5 into a ‘‘Sexual Advance
Harassment’’ group (n = 604). All of these women, unlike
those in Groups 1 or 2, reported significant experiences of
unwanted sexual advances at work, in the form of sexual
attention and/or sexual coercion. We then conducted a
second MANCOVA, comparing the outcomes of this Sex-
ual Advance Harassment group to those of the Gender
Harassment group and the Nonvictims group. There was a
significant multivariate main effect of harassment-group-
membership on the collection of outcomes, Wilks’
Lambda = .77, F (16, 4748) = 40.82, p  .001. As
Table 3 shows, outcome means for the Gender Harassment
Victims fell in between those for the Nonvictims and Sexual
Advance Victims (although closer to the means of the latter
group). According to follow-up Tukey tests, all outcomes
for the gender-harassed women were significantly worse
than those of the nonvictimized women. Moreover, Sexual
Advance Victims showed significantly worse outcomes
than Gender Harassment Victims, with two exceptions:
there were no significant differences between the two
groups on work satisfaction and turnover intentions.
Study 2: The Attorney Survey
Participants and Procedure
We sought to cross-validate the results from Study 1 with
secondary analysis of data collected from women working
in a very different context: the legal profession. Participants
were drawn from a stratified random sample of attorneys
from a large federal judicial circuit. Surveys were sent to
9,223 individuals, yielding a 53% response rate. The current
study focused on the 1,425 women who responded to the
survey. Most of these women were white (93%). They
ranged in age from 24 to 79 years (M = 39.09, SD = 7.81).
Sixty-eight percent of the women were married or part-
nered, while 18% had never been married. These women
were highly educated, all holding at least a Juris Doctor, and
some holding additional graduate degrees. Similar to the
military sample, they worked in a traditionally masculine
occupation where women remain a minority. They com-
pleted a paper-and-pencil self-report survey. More
information about these participants and procedures appears
in Cortina et al. (2002) and Lim and Cortina (2005).
6
Measures
Table 4 displays the descriptive statistics, coefficient
alphas, and intercorrelations for all variables from Study 2.
Similar to Study 1, for multi-item scales, we reverse-coded
items as needed and then summed relevant items to create
scale-scores; higher scores reflect greater levels of the
underlying construct.
Sex-Based Harassment. Similar to Study 1, we used
items from the SEQ developed by Fitzgerald et al. (1995,
1988) to assess sex-based harassment. Participants
described how often they had experienced a list of
unwanted sexual or sexist behaviors over the past 5 years
Table 3 ANCOVAs for physical health, mental health, and work
outcomes—Sample 1 (Military)
Outcome measured Nonvictims Gender Harassment
Victims
Sexual Advance
Harassment Victims
df F p
Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI
1. Psychological well-being 16.76 (2.92) 16.58, 16.94 14.65
(3.43) 14.43, 14.86 13.62 (3.93) 13.31, 13.94 2, 2550 122.07
.001
2. Performance decline due to
poor emotional health
3.63 (1.49) 3.54, 3.73 4.47 (2.07) 4.34, 4.60 5.14 (2.51) 4.94,
5.34 2, 2562 75.91 .001
3. Health satisfaction 13.60 (2.10) 13.47, 13.73 13.04 (2.49)
12.89, 13.20 12.53 (2.62) 12.31, 12.74 2, 2550 24.06 .001
4. Performance decline due to
poor physical health
4.91 (2.09) 4.78, 5.04 5.62 (2.56) 5.46, 5.78 6.40 (3.16) 6.15,
6.66 2, 2555 37.90 .001
5. Coworker satisfaction 22.96 (4.50) 22.68, 23.24 18.89 (5.43)
18.55, 19.23 18.08 (5.73) 17.61, 18.54 2, 2532 169.93 .001
6. Work satisfaction 22.73 (5.44) 22.39, 23.07 18.96 ( 6.33)
18.56, 19.35 18.54 (6.62) 18.01, 19.07 2, 2554 89.16 .001
7. Organizational commitment 16.53 (2.66) 16.36, 16.70 15.49
(3.28) 15.28, 15.69 14.78 (3.35) 14.51, 15.05 2, 2555 29.15
.001
8. Turnover intentions 2.69 (1.80) 2.57, 2.80 3.44 (1.54) 3.35,
3.54 3.46 (1.58) 3.33, 3.58 2, 2558 53.49 .001
6
Although drawing on the same larger dataset, these two past
studies
have different foci from each other and from the current article.
Cortina et al. (2002) focused on gender differences in
experiences of
incivility and harassment in the legal profession. Lim and
Cortina
(2005) detailed how uncivil and harassing behaviors tend to co-
occur
and jointly affect personal and professional outcomes.
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 33
123
from judges, attorneys, trustees, marshals, court security
officers, and court personnel. They responded on a 5-point
scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = many times. The
measure consisted of nine questions, from which there are
three subscales: gender harassment, unwanted sexual
attention, and sexual coercion.
The gender harassment subscale consisted of two items:
‘‘made offensive remarks or jokes about women in your
presence?’’ and ‘‘publicly addressed you in unprofessional
terms (e.g., ‘honey,’ ‘dear’)?’’
7
The unwanted sexual
attention subscale contained four items, including:
‘‘attempted to establish a romantic or sexual relationship
despite your efforts to discourage it?’’ Two items com-
prised the sexual coercion subscale, such as, ‘‘implied
more favorable treatment of you or your client if you were
sexually cooperative?’’
Job-Related Outcomes. We used a three-item scale
(a = .74), developed for the purposes of this survey, to
measure attorneys’ intentions to change careers (e.g., ‘‘I
often think about leaving federal litigation’’). We measured
job stress with three items (a = .75), such as ‘‘my expe-
riences working in the federal court are more stressful than
I’d like.’’ Finally, in order to assess professional relationship
satisfaction, we used a three-item scale (a = .76) that
consisted of items such as ‘‘in general, I am satisfied with my
professional relationships with other attorneys in federal
court.’’ For all three scales, response options ranged from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Control Variables. We controlled for race and job
tenure in outcome analyses. Respondents self-reported
their race (coded 0 = minority and 1 = white). They also
reported the number of years they had actively practiced
law, including clerkships. Response options were 1
(0–5 years of practice), 2 (6–10 years of practice), 3 (11–
20 years of practice), 4 (21–30 years of practice), and 5
(31 ? years of practice); we collapsed response options 4
and 5 due to the small number of women who chose the
latter option.
Results
Profiles of Sex-Based Harassment
Similar to Study 1, we standardized the attorney women’s
scores on the subscales of the SEQ, including all women
who had reported at least one experience of an SEQ
behavior (n = 491). We again performed a k-means cluster
analysis of these subscales, and again chose a 5-cluster
solution. Profiles of attorney women’s means on the z-
scored SEQ appear in Fig. 2.
Mirroring the profiles from the military sample, the
largest group consisted of women who reported minimal
experiences of harassment (Group 1; n = 320). Those who
disclosed high levels of gender harassment, with almost no
unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion, made up the
second-largest group (Group 2; n = 134). Group 3
(n = 30) described episodes of unwanted sexual attention,
in addition to moderate levels gender harassment. Group 4
(n = 3) and Group 5 (n = 4) were quite small, and dis-
closed moderate and high levels of all types of harassment
measured by the SEQ, respectively. To summarize, over
90% of harassed women fell into one of the two groups
reporting little or no sexually advancing harassment
experiences (Group 1 or 2), further supporting Hypothesis
1. We also identified a group of Nonvictims (n = 338),
who had responded ‘‘never’’ to all items of the SEQ.
Demographic information for each of the profile groups
appears in Table 5. Using chi-square and F tests, we found
no significant relationships between profile membership
and race, v2 (5, N = 1317) = 4.24, p = .52; age, F (37,
1286) = 0.79, p = .81; or years practicing law F (5,
1333) = 0.98, p = .43.
Table 4 Descriptive statistics, alpha coefficients, and
correlations—Sample 2 (Attorneys)
Variables M SD a 1 2 3 4 5
1. Gender harassment 1.74 2.14 .69 –
2. Unwanted sexual attention 0.19 0.79 .74 .42** –
3. Sexual coercion 0.01 0.12 .84 .14** .41** –
4. Intention to change careers 6.00 2.93 .74 .02 .03 .00 –
5. Professional relationship satisfaction 12.04 2.24 .76 -.06* -
.04 -.02 -.36** –
6. Job stress 9.17 2.74 .75 .15** .04 .06* .32** -.34**
Note. Scale-scores were derived by summing responses across
all items in each scale for all women in the sample; higher
scores reflected greater
levels of the underlying construct
** p  .01, * p  .05
7
Unlike the military survey in Study 1, the brevity of this
subscale
precluded distinctions between ‘‘sexist’’ and ‘‘crude’’ gender
harass-
ment. Together, the two items assessed ‘‘gender harassment’’ as
a
global phenomenon.
34 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
Outcomes of Sex-Based Harassment
Again, for theoretical reasons, the group that experienced
high gender harassment (without sexual attention or coer-
cion; Group 2, n = 134) was the focus of our primary
outcome analysis. To serve as a comparison group of sim-
ilar size, we randomly selected 150 women from the
Nonvictims group. We then compared these 150 Nonvic-
tims to the 134 Gender Harassment victims on the three job-
related outcomes, using MANCOVA. Racial minority sta-
tus and years practicing law served as covariates. Again,
results suggested that there was a significant multivariate
main effect of gender harassment on the collection of out-
comes, Wilks’ Lambda = .96, F (3, 238) = 3.00, p  .05.
Follow-up univariate analyses (ANCOVAs), reported in
Table 6, revealed significant effects on job stress and sat-
isfaction with professional relationships. Compared to their
non-harassed counterparts, gender-harassed women repor-
ted significantly higher levels of job stress (d = .34) . They
also described less satisfaction with their relationships with
federal judges, other attorneys in the federal court, and
court personnel (d = .32). However, we did not find a
significant group difference in intention to change careers
(the means for both groups were similarly low: close to six,
on a scale that can range from 3 to 15). With the exception
of this last result, Hypothesis 2 was supported among
women attorneys.
To provide additional insight into group differences, we
again combined Groups 3, 4, and 5 into a Sexual Advance
Harassment group (n = 37); their outcome means appear in
Table 6. We compared the outcomes of this group with
those of the Nonvictims and Gender Harassment Victims
(using MANCOVA), finding a significant multivariate main
effect of harassment on outcomes, Wilks’ Lambda = .95, F
(6, 546) = 2.29, p  .05. Follow-up Tukey tests indicated
that the Sexual Advance group differed significantly from
the Nonvictims group in terms of professional relationship
satisfaction. The Gender Harassment Victims also reported
less professional relationship satisfaction, and more job
stress, than Nonvictims. Gender Harassment Victims did
not differ significantly from Sexual Advance Victims,
however, on any outcome. Put differently, we found that
gender-harassed women attorneys fared just as poorly as
those who had experienced sexual advance harassment.
General Discussion
This article draws attention to the incidence and correlates
of gender harassment in the workplace. Social science
-2
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
Gender
Harassment
Unwanted Sexual
Attention
Sexual Coercion
Z
-S
c
o
re
s
High Victimization (0.8%) -
Group 5
Moderate Victimization (0.6%)
- Group 4
Gender harassment and
Unwanted Sexual Attention
(6.1%) - Group 3
Gender Harassment (27.3%)
- Group 2
Low Victimization (65.2%) -
Group 1
Fig. 2 Profiles of harassment
among women attorneys
(n = 491)
Table 5 Descriptive statistics
for profile groups—Sample 2
(Attorneys)
Profile group Non-White Percent Age Years practicing law
M SD M SD
Nonvictims (n = 851) 5.2 39.37 8.09 2.23 0.91
Group 1: Low Victimization (n = 320) 6.7 38.32 7.31 2.21 0.90
Group 2: Gender Harassment (n = 134) 4.5 39.45 7.36 2.37 0.87
Group 3: Gender Harassment & Unwanted
Sexual Attention (n = 30)
6.7 37.63 6.13 2.23 0.77
Group 4: Moderate Victimization (n = 3) 0.0 42.50 0.71 2.67
0.58
Group 5: High Victimization (all types) (n = 4) 25.0 35.00 2.31
1.75 0.50
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 35
123
research has often analyzed sex-based harassment as a
global phenomenon, failing to distinguish among the dif-
ferent facets of behavior; this practice may have obscured
the experiences of many harassed women, especially those
working in male-dominated fields. At the same time, many
federal judges have evaluated potentially harassing conduct
through a (hetero)sexualized lens, in which they privilege a
‘‘top-down, male-female sexual come-on image of
harassment’’ (Schultz, 2006, p. 26). This ‘‘sexual model of
sexual harassment’’ does not provide an explanation for
gender harassment that is devoid of sexual interest, which
we found to be the norm in women’s experiences of
harassment in traditionally masculine domains.
Key Findings
The first goal of this article was to investigate the preva-
lence of different dimensions of sexually harassing
conduct. Consistent with our hypothesis, gender harass-
ment in the absence of unwanted sexual attention or sexual
coercion was the most common manifestation of harass-
ment faced by women in the military and the law
(employment contexts which, importantly, were once the
exclusive province of men). In fact, in both settings, 9 out
of every 10 victims had experienced primarily gender
harassment, with virtually no unwanted sexual overtures.
Taken together, our empirical results support the legal
theory that ‘‘much of the time, harassment assumes a form
that has little or nothing to do with sexuality but everything
to do with gender’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1687). This conduct
is not about misguided attempts to draw women into sexual
relationships; quite the contrary, it rejects women and
attempts to drive them out of jobs where they are seen to
have no place. One could argue that, in these instances,
‘‘sexual harassment is used both to police and discipline the
gender outlaw: the woman who dares to do a man’s job is
made to pay’’ (Franke, 1997, p. 764). Had we collapsed
across the subtypes of harassing behavior, as many psy-
chologists do, this striking pattern of results would not have
surfaced.
The second goal of this article was to understand the
correlates of gender harassment for working women. When
comparing victims of gender harassment to women who
reported no harassment experiences, we found that ‘‘just’’
gender harassment was associated with multiple negative
outcomes. Specifically, in the military context, gender-
harassed women reported lower psychological well-being,
job performance, job commitment, and satisfaction with
their employment and health; they also described more
thoughts and intentions of leaving their jobs. These results
remained significant even after controlling for the women’s
race, rank, and service branch. Among attorneys, gender-
harassed women (compared to nonharassed women)
reported lower satisfaction with professional relationships
and higher job stress, above and beyond the effects of race
and job tenure. Thus, experiences of gender harassment
alone were associated with negative personal and profes-
sional outcomes in two very different contexts of work.
Implications for Sexual Harassment Jurisprudence
This research has important legal implications. Although
popular wisdom might suggest that the legal definition of
sexual harassment is fixed, in actuality the legal under-
standing changes as courts interpret and refine precedent.
Our research underscores the need to broaden legal and
scientific conceptualizations of sexual harassment, so that
gender harassment can be recognized as a harmful and
objectionable condition of employment, even when not
paired with unwanted sexual attention. Title VII of the
Civil Rights Act of 1964 says nothing about sexual
behavior. Definitions that limit sex-based harassment to
unwanted advances emerged over time as the courts
revised their interpretations of Title VII. Our results sug-
gest that further revision is in order, to prohibit not just
sexually predatory conduct, but also behavior that creates a
hostile work environment for members of one sex but
contains no sexual advance—that is, gender harassment. As
we have shown, gender harassment does not simply pro-
vide a backdrop for other kinds of harassment; it is the
modal form of sex-based harassment faced by women at
work (at least in male-dominated domains). Moreover, it
alters the terms of employment for targeted women, being
associated with a variety of negative professional
outcomes.
Table 6 ANCOVAs for job-related outcomes—Sample 2
(Attorneys)
Outcome measured Nonvictims Gender Harassment
Victims
Sexual Advance
Harassment Victims
df F p
Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI
1. Intention to change careers 5.81 (3.01) 5.32, 6.31 5.95 (2.93)
5.45, 6.46 6.51 (3.25) 5.43, 7.60 2, 307 0.98 .452
2. Professional relationship satisfaction 12.51 (2.14) 12.15,
12.86 11.81 (2.21) 11.43, 12.18 11.38 (2.97) 10.39, 12.37 2,
308 5.25 .006
3. Job stress 9.04 (2.84) 8.53, 9.55 10.01 (2.82) 9.51, 10.50
9.76 (3.67) 8.48, 11.04 2, 277 3.61 .032
36 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
Our outcome results suggest that harassment exclusively
consisting of gender-related hostility has adverse work-
related correlates. That is, the more that women experi -
enced gender harassment, the less satisfied they were with
their jobs and colleagues, the more they experienced stress
on the job, and the more they suffered health problems that
detracted from their job performance. Moreover, these
results were not trivial in magnitude, being associated with
large effect sizes in some cases (particularly for coworker
satisfaction and psychological well-being). Findings such
as these could be relevant to legal claims of hostile envi -
ronment sexual harassment.
As noted at the outset of this article, in Harris v Forklift
Systems, Inc. (1993) the Supreme Court stated that ‘‘all the
circumstances’’ must be considered when determining
whether an environment is ‘‘hostile’’ or ‘‘abusive,’’ in
violation of Title VII. The Court went on to say that these
circumstances may include a number of factors, including
whether the discriminatory conduct ‘‘unreasonably inter-
feres with an employee’s work performance’’ Harris v
Forklift Systems, Inc. (1993, p. 23). In the current study, we
documented that gender harassment on its own is linked
with a variety of adverse work outcomes, including but not
limited to performance decline. These outcomes do not
necessarily reflect traumatization or incapacitation of the
victim, but this is not a requirement of Title VII: ‘‘…Title
VII comes into play before the harassing conduct leads to a
nervous breakdown. A discriminatorily abusive work
environment, even one that does not seriously affect
employees’ psychological well-being, can and often will
detract from employees’ job performance, discourage
employees from remaining on the job, or keep them from
advancing in their careers’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc.,
1993, p. 21). Although we cannot draw definitive causal
conclusions from our correlational findings, our results are
consistent with these sorts of effects. They support the
possibility that ‘‘just’’ gender harassment can create a
hostile environment that disadvantages women.
Limitations
As with any research, our studies have their limitations. All
results were based on cross-sectional, correlational data,
precluding strong temporal or causal inferences. That said,
longitudinal studies of sexual harassment (e.g., Glomb,
Munson, Hulin, Berman, & Drasgow, 1999; Sims et al.,
2005) provide compelling evidence that our personal and
professional outcomes follow, rather than precede,
harassment experiences. These data were self-reported;
because of this, common method variance or response set
could potentially explain some of the significant findings.
Surveys were designed to minimize some of these prob-
lems: questions about mental health, physical health, and
job attitudes were asked prior to and independently of the
SEQ, so that responses about harassment did not bias
reports of health and attitudes. Also, we were only able to
use proxies for job and career turnover, with measures of
turnover intentions rather than actual turnover rates.
However, past research tells us that one of the best pre-
dictors of actual turnover is thoughts of turnover (e.g.,
Griffeth, Hom, & Gaertner, 2000). Finally, while we did
cross-validate results across two large samples that differed
by ethnicity and socio-economic status, both samples came
from male-dominated organizations. Gender parity has
increased in both industries, but as of 2008, only 34% of
lawyers were women (U.S. Department of Labor, 2009),
and as of September 2009, only 14% of active-duty mili-
tary personnel were women (Department of Defense,
2009).
We should also emphasize that our data combine the
experiences of hundreds of women, and just because gen-
der harassment correlates with negative outcomes in these
aggregate data does not mean that this behavior has neg-
ative outcomes for every individual victim. We found that
this is true on average. Whether it is true for any individual
woman is a determination that must be made on a case-by-
case basis. Our point is simply that courts and social sci -
entists should not automatically assume that ‘‘just gender
harassment’’ is, by definition, too trivial to create an abu-
sive work environment.
Future Directions
Social scientists continue to focus on sexual ‘‘come-on’’
forms of sexual harassment (e.g., de Haas, Timmerman, &
Höing, 2009; Woodzicka & LaFrance, 2005). When sur-
veys do include questions about gender harassment, during
the analysis of data these questions are often combined
with questions about unwanted sexual advances (e.g.,
Fitzgerald, Drasgow et al., 1997; Sims et al., 2005). This
happens even though there are major qualitative differ-
ences across the experiences; for example, being sexually
propositioned on one occasion is not the same as being
targeted with demeaning anti-female remarks on a daily
basis. Based on the current study, we recommend that more
research parse out experiences of gender harassment from
unwanted sexual attention/coercion, which will give rise to
new avenues of inquiry. For example, proponents of the-
ories as to why people harass others have generally looked
for one unifying explanation (e.g., Bargh, Raymond, Pryor,
& Strack, 1995; Berdahl, 2007b). It remains entirely pos-
sible that different goals motivate the different subtypes of
sex-based harassment.
Regarding construct labels, we second Berdahl’s
(2007b) recommendation that the term ‘‘sex-based harass-
ment’’ be used in lieu of ‘‘sexual harassment.’’ Research on
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 37
123
lay perceptions suggests that the latter term conjures up
narrow notions of unwanted sexual advances; it fails to
include gender harassment in the minds of many (e.g.,
Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo et al., 1995; Tang
et al., 1995). The term ‘‘sex-based harassment’’ is broader,
calling attention to both categories of behavior. It is also
closer to the original language of Title VII, which pro-
hibited harassment ‘‘based on sex.’’
The current study focused on two male-dominated pro-
fessions. Researchers have not yet determined how ‘‘just
gender harassment’’ operates in gender-balanced and
female-dominated industries. We would speculate that the
behavior might be less common in more female-integrated
contexts, where women are not perceived as ‘‘encroach-
ing’’ on ‘‘men’s territory.’’ In those settings, anti-female
hostility might also be less tolerated and more penalized,
and thus experienced by victims as less threatening. These
and other possibilities await future research.
In closing, we emphasize the need for scholars of sex-
based harassment, both in psychology and law, to continue
the interdisciplinary exchange of ideas. Social scientists
and legal theorists often struggle to converse, but consid-
erable efforts have been made to bridge the disciplinary
gap when it comes to this topic (for examples, see the
special issue on ‘‘sexual harassment’’ in Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law, 1999, and the special issue on
‘‘psychology, law, and the workplace’’ in Law and Human
Behavior, 2004). The arguments of prominent legal
scholars inspired the current psychological research. We
hope that, in turn, our results can inform further evolution
in legal thinking about harassment based on sex and
gender.
Acknowledgments We are grateful to Anna Kirkland, Abby
Stewart, and members of our research lab at the University of
Michigan for their valuable feedback on this research. Thanks
also to
the following individuals for their contributions to Study 2:
Louise F.
Fitzgerald, Leslie V. Freeman, Vicki J. Magley, Kimberly A.
Lon-
sway, Regina Day Langhout, Jill Hunter-Williams. This article
is
based on the master’s thesis of the first author, who presented
parts of
it in July 2008 at the Annual Meeting of the International
Society for
Political Psychology in Paris, France.
References
1999 Survey of Active Duty Personnel [Data file and
documentation
on CD-ROM]. (2001). Arlington, VA: DMDC.
Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995).
Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power ? sex
association and its consequences for sexual harassment and
aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
768–781. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.768.
Barling, J., Rogers, A., & Kelloway, E. (2001). Behind closed
doors:
In-home workers’ experience of sexual harassment and work-
place violence. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,
6(3), 255–269. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.6.3.255.
Barnes v. Costle, 561 F.2d 983 (D.C. Cir. 1977).
Berdahl, J. L. (2007a). The sexual harassment of uppity women.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 425–437. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.92.2.425.
Berdahl, J. L. (2007b). Harassment based on sex: Protecting
social
status in the context of gender hierarchy. Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 32(2), 641–658.
Berdahl, J. L., & Raver, J. L. (2010). Sexual harassment. In S.
Zedeck
(Ed.), Handbook of industrial/organizational psychology. Wash-
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Berg, S. H. (2006). Everyday sexism and post traumatic stress
disorder in women: A correlational study. Violence Against
Women, 12(10), 970–988. doi:10.1177/1077801206293082.
Bergman, M., Langhout, R. D., Palmieri, P., Cortina, L. M., &
Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). The (un)reasonableness of reporting:
Antecedents and consequences of reporting sexual harassment.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 230–242. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.87.2.230.
Berwick, D. M., Murphy, J. M., Goldman, P. A., Ware, J. E.,
Barsky,
A. J., & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance on a five-item
mental health screening test. Medical Care, 29, 169–176. doi:
10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008.
Cortina, L. M., & Berdahl, J. L. (2008). Sexual harassment in
organizations: A decade of research in review. In J. Barling &
C.
L. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of organizational
behavior (pp. 469–497). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. L., Magley, V. J., Freeman, L. V.,
Collinsworth, L. L., Hunter, M., et al. (2002). What’s gender
got
to do with it? Incivility in the federal courts. Law and Social
Inquiry, 27, 235–270.
Culbertson, A., & Rosenfeld, P. (1994). Assessment of sexual
harassment in the active-duty Navy. Military Psychology, 6(2),
69–93. doi:10.1207/s15327876mp0602_1.
de Haas, S., Timmerman, G., & Höing, M. (2009). Sexual
harassment
and health among male and female police officers. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 390–401. doi:10.1037/
a0017046.
Department of Defense. (2009). Active duty military personnel
by
service by rank/grade. Retrieved December 14, 2009, from
http://siadapp.dmdc.osd.mil/personnel/MILITARY/miltop.htm.
Edwards, J. E., Elig, T. W., Edwards, D. L., & Riemer, R. A.
(1997).
The 1995 Armed Forces Sexual Harassment Survey: Adminis-
tration, datasets, and codebook for Form B (report no. 95–015).
Arlington, VA: DMDC.
Epstein, L. B. (1998). What is a gender norm and why should
we
care? Implementing a new theory in sexual harassment law.
Stanford Law Review, 51, 161–182.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F., Hulin, C. L., Gelfand, M. J., &
Magley,
V. J. (1997). Antecedents and consequences of sexual harass-
ment in organizations: A test of an integrated model. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 82(4), 578–589. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.
82.4.578.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995).
Measuring
sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 425–445.
doi:10.1207/
s15324834basp1704_2.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Magley, V. J., Drasgow, F., & Waldo, C. R.
(1999).
Measuring sexual harassment in the military: The Sexual
Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ-DoD). Military Psychology,
11(3), 243–263. doi:10.1207/s15327876mp1103_3.
Fitzgerald, L. F., & Ormerod, A. J. (1991). Perceptions of
sexual
harassment: The influence of gender and academic context.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 281–294. doi:10.1111/j.
1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Schullman, S. L., Bailey, N., Richards, M.,
Swecker,
J., Gold, Y., et al. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of
38 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39
123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.768
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.6.3.255
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.425
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.425
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077801206293082
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.230
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.230
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp0602_1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017046
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017046
http://siadapp.dmdc.osd.mil/personnel/MILITARY/miltop.htm
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.4.578
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.4.578
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1704_2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1704_2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp1103_3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x
sexual harassment in academia and the workplace. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 32, 152–175. doi:10.1016/0001-8791
(88)90012-7.
Fitzgerald, L. F., Swan, S., & Magley, V. J. (1997). But was it
really
sexual harassment? Legal, behavioral, and psychological defini -
tions of the workplace victimization of women. In W.
O’Donohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment: Theory, research, and
treatment (pp. 5–28). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Foote, W. E., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2005). Evaluating
sexual
harassment: Psychological social, and legal considerations in
forensic evaluations. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Franke, K. M. (1995). The central mistake of sex discrimination
law:
The disaggregation of sex from gender. University of Pennsyl -
vania Law Review, 144, 1–99.
Franke, K. M. (1997). What’s wrong with sexual harassme nt?
Stanford Law Review, 49, 691–772.
Franke, K. M. (2004). What’s wrong with sexual harassment? In
C. A.
MacKinnon & R. B. Siegel (Eds.), Directions in sexual harass-
ment law (pp. 169–181). New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Glomb, T. M., Munson, L. J., Hulin, C. L., Berman, M., &
Drasgow,
F. (1999). Structural equation models of sexual harassment:
Longitudinal explorations and cross-sectional generalizations.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 14–28. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.84.1.14.
Griffeth, R. W., Hom, P. W., & Gaertner, S. (2000). A meta-
analysis
of antecedents and correlates of employee turnover: Update,
moderator tests, and research implications for the next millen-
nium. Journal of Management, 26, 463–488. doi:10.1177/014
920630002600305.
Growe, J. D. (2007). Reform the EEOC guidelines: Protect
employees
from gender discrimination as mandated by Title VII. Journal of
Law and Policy, 24, 275–300.
Harris v. Forklift Sys (1993) 114 S.Ct. 367.
Hartigan, J. A. (1975). Clustering algorithms. New York: Wiley.
Hébert, C. (2005). The disparate impact of sexual harassment:
Does
motive matter? Kansas Law Review, 53, 341–395.
Langhout, R. D., Bergman, M. E., Cortina, L. M., Fitzgerald, L.
F.,
Drasgow, F., & Williams, J. H. (2005). Sexual harassment
severity: Assessing situational and personal determinants and
outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 975–1007.
doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02156.x.
Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. (2005). Interpersonal mistreatment in
the
workplace: The interface and impact of general incivility and
sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 483–
496.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.483.
Lipari, R. N., & Lancaster, A. R. (2003). Armed Forces 2002
Sexual
Harassment Survey. DMDC Report No. 2003–026. Arlington,
VA: Defense Manpower Data Center.
Loredo, C., Reid, A., & Deaux, K. (1995). Judgments and
definitions
of sexual harassment by high school students. Sex Roles, 32,
29–
45. doi:10.1007/BF01544756.
Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson, 477 U.S. 57 (1986).
Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. (1979). The
measurement of organizational commitment. Journal of Voca-
tional Behavior, 14, 224–247. doi:10.1016/0001-8791(79)
90072-1.
Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, 523 U.S. 75 (1998).
Perry, E., Kulik, C., & Schmidtke, J. (1998). Individual
differences in
the effectiveness of sexual harassment awareness training.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(8), 698–723. doi:
10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01727.x.
Pryor, J. B. (1987). Sexual harassment proclivities in men. Sex
Roles,
269–290. doi:10.1007/BF00288453.
Richman, J. A., Shinsako, S. A., Rospenda, K. M., Flaherty, J.
A., &
Freels, S. (2002). Workplace harassment/abuse and alcohol -
related outcomes: The mediating role of psychological distress.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(4), 412–419.
Rotundo, M., Nguyen, D., & Sackett, P. R. (2001). A meta-
analytic
review of gender differences in perceptions of sexual harass -
ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 914–922.
doi:10.1037/
0021-9010.86.5.914.
Schneider, K. T., Swan, S., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1997). Job-
related
psychological effects of sexual harassment in the workplace:
Empirical evidence from two organizations. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 82(3), 401–415. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.401.
Schneider, K. T., Tomaka, J., & Palacois, R. (2001). Women’s
cognitive, affective, and physiological reactions to a male
coworker’s sexist behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychol -
ogy, 31, 1995–2018. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00161.x.
Schultz, V. (1998). Reconceptualizing sexual harassment. Yale
Law
Journal, 107, 1683–1732.
Schultz, V. (2003). The sanitized workplace. Yale Law Journal,
112(8), 2061–2197.
Schultz, V. (2006). Understanding sexual harassment law in
action:
What has gone wrong and what we can do about it. Thomas
Jefferson Law Review, 29, 1–53.
Sims, C. S., Drasgow, F., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2005). The
effects of
sexual harassment on turnover in the military: Time-dependent
modeling. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6), 1141–1152.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1141.
Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction: Application, assessment,
cause, and consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stark, S., Chernyshenko, O. S., Lancaster, A. R., Drasgow, F.,
&
Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). Toward standardized measurement of
sexual harassment: Shortening the SEQ-DoD using item
response theory. Military Psychology, 14, 49–72. doi:10.1207/
S15327876MP1401_03.
Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the
intellectual
test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.69.5.797.
Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J.
(2001).
Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and
psychological impact from three daily diary studies. Journal of
Social Issues, 57(1), 31–53. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00200.
Tang, C. S.-K., Yik, M. S. M., Cheung, F. M. C., & Choi, P.-K.
(1995). How do Chinese college students define sexual harass -
ment? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 503–515. doi:
10.1177/088626095010004008.
Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, 42 U.S.C. § 2000e
(1994).
Retrieved May 15, 2009, from http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/
vii.html.
U.S. Department of Labor. (2009). Women in the labor force: A
databook. Retrieved December 14, 2009, from http://www.
bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf.
U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1994). Sexual
harassment in
the federal workplace: Trends, progress, continuing challenges.
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Viet, C. T., & Ware, J. E., Jr. (1983). The structure of
psychological
distress and well-being in general populations. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 730–742. doi:10.1037/
0022-006X.51.5.730.
Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item
short
form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item
selection. Medical Care, 30, 473–483. doi:10.1097/00005650-
199206000-00002.
Williams v. General Motors Corp., 187 F.3d 553 (6th Cir.
1999).
Woodzicka, J., & LaFrance, M. (2005). The effects of subtle
sexual
harassment on women’s performance in a job interview. Sex
Roles, 53(1), 67–77. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-4279-4.
Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 39
123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(88)90012-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(88)90012-7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.1.14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.1.14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014920630002600305
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014920630002600305
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02156.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.483
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01544756
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(79)90072-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(79)90072-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01727.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00288453
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.914
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.914
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.401
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00161.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1141
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327876MP1401_03
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327876MP1401_03
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00200
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/088626095010004008
http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/vii.html
http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/vii.html
http://www.bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf
http://www.bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.730
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.730
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-4279-4
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
c.10979_2010_Article_9241.pdfGender Harassment: Broadening
Our Understanding of Sex-Based Harassment at
WorkAbstractCentral ConstructsLegal Perspectives on Sexual
HarassmentPsychological Research on Sexual HarassmentIf It’s
‘‘Just’’ Gender Harassment, Why Should We
Care?HypothesesStudy 1: The Military SurveyParticipants and
ProcedureMeasuresSex-Based HarassmentPsychological Well-
BeingPerformance Decline Due to Poor Emotional
HealthPerformance Decline Due to Poor Physical HealthGeneral
HealthWork AttitudesTurnover IntentionsControl
VariablesResultsProfiles of Sex-Based HarassmentOutcomes of
Sex-Based HarassmentStudy 2: The Attorney SurveyParticipants
and ProcedureMeasuresSex-Based HarassmentJob-Related
OutcomesControl VariablesResultsProfiles of Sex-Based
HarassmentOutcomes of Sex-Based HarassmentGeneral
DiscussionKey FindingsImplications for Sexual Harassment
JurisprudenceLimitationsFuture
DirectionsAcknowledgmentsReferences
How Should We Respond to Sexual Harassment?
Taub, Amanda . New York Times (Online) , New York: New
York Times Company. Nov 29, 2017.
ProQuest document link
ABSTRACT (ENGLISH)
The legal system and social science offer principles to evaluate
each case and to explain why this moment is so
disorienting.
FULL TEXT
As accusations of sexual misconduct against famous men
accumulate , the sheer quantity of dispiriting news is
starting to create a confusing blur. The task of responding to
sexual harassment and assault feels simultaneously
more urgent and more daunting than ever.
Society is out of practice at this task; the same culture of
silence that protected harassers also suppressed the
public response to their crimes. Many people struggle even to
know which questions to ask, and worry that if they
ask the wrong ones, they might become part of the problem.
There is a temptation to simplify matters by viewing all
harassers and their offenses as equally awful, or,
alternatively, as equally misunderstood. But to be fair and
effective, any system needs to make distinctions: to sort
Harvey Weinstein from Roy Moore; and Louis C.K. and Matt
Lauer from Al Franken.
The legal system, while quite different from the court of public
opinion, offers principles and reasoning that we can
use to evaluate each case as it flares.
Slippery slopes and consequences
Until recently, all of those accused, no matter the severity of
their offenses, faced the same consequences:
generally none. Protected by their power and authority, they
kept their careers and reputations intact.
As that begins to change, some worry that we might bungle the
job. “Taking harassment seriously also requires
making serious distinctions,” Jonah Goldberg, a conservative
columnist, wrote recently for The Los Angeles Times.
“And yet Franken’s name is routinely listed alongside Moore’s
and Weinstein’s.”
Masha Gessen, writing in The New Yorker , worried we may be
on the verge of a “sex panic.”
Jane Curtin, a comedian who is a friend and former colleague of
Mr. Franken’s, compared the current atmosphere
to McCarthyism. “It’s just like the red menace,” she said in an
interview with The Times. “You don’t know who’s
going to be next.”
Many of those accused have lost their jobs, but for the most
part, they are not facing legal consequences. Yet
principles borrowed from criminal law can help us analyze
whether our response to their actions is just and fair.
Criminal punishment tends to rest on two broad principles: the
seriousness of the wrongdoing, and the
perpetrator’s intent in committing the crime.
Viewed through that lens, the accusations against Mr.
Weinstein, which include rape, and Mr. Moore, who is
accused of molesting teenage girls, are clearer-cut cases for
punishment than those against, say, Louis C.K., who
masturbated in front of adult women but did not touch them.
It’s also important that courts do not consider only the moment
of the crime itself in determining punishment. Our
system also punishes defendants who threaten witnesses or
obstruct justice, as well as others who help them do
so. Here again, the accusations against Mr. Weinstein are
especially extreme. According to a report by Ronan
Farrow in The New Yorker, he hired ex-Israeli intelligence
agents to intimidate victims and journalists into silence.
Dana Min Goodman and Julia Wolov, two of the women who
have accused Louis C.K. of misconduct, have said
they stayed silent for years in part because of pressure from
Dave Becky, Louis C.K.’s manager. Mr. Becky has
https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how-
should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-
2?accountid=28844
https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how-
should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-
2?accountid=28844
denied threatening them. But the women have said they feared
that speaking would bring retribution.
Accountability for harm
The question of punishment is merely one factor in considering
these cases. The wave of accusations has also led
to demands that society recognize and repair the harm of sexual
misconduct.
Caroline Framke, a culture critic for Vox, called for an
accounting of the “graveyard of potential cut short by
careless cruelty.”
The principles of civil law, which are intended to make victims
whole and ensure that no one profits from
wrongdoing, can offer useful guidance about what is fair, and
what is necessary.
A central principle is that the person at fault, not the victim,
should bear the cost of the harms of wrongdoing. In
law school, budding attorneys learn the “eggshell plaintiff”
rule, which says that defendants are responsible for all
of the harm they cause, even if the injuries were made more
extreme because, say, the victim’s skull was as thin as
an eggshell. Otherwise, the reasoning goes, the costs will fall
on the victims —a more unfair outcome than holding
perpetrators responsible for unexpectedly severe damages.
It is now becoming clear that there is not a one-to-one
correlation between the objective egregiousness of sexual
misconduct and the damage it can cause.
Louis C.K.’s actions may have been less extreme than Mr.
Weinstein’s. But Ms. Goodman and Ms. Wolov have said
they felt they could no longer work on projects involving him or
his manager —a category that grew to include a
large chunk of the comedy industry as Louis C.K.’s career took
off.
And the Emmy-award-winning writer Kater Gordon told The
Information that when Matthew Weiner, her boss on the
show “Mad Men,” told her that he “deserved to see her naked,”
he undermined her confidence and ambition. (Mr.
Weiner has said that he does not remember making that
comment, and would not speak that way to a colleague.)
Held up next to the allegations against Mr. Weinstein or Mr.
Moore, those words may seem like a misdemeanor.
But the harm was nevertheless severe, Ms. Gordon says,
because she left the television industry, abandoning a
promising career.
Women are often told to grow a “thicker skin” and become less
sensitive to harassment. But the eggshell plaintiff
rule suggests a different conclusion: that the harassers should
bear the costs of the harm they impose, even on
“thin-skinned” victims.
We must also consider harms that go beyond the immediate
victims. Less diverse workplaces offer women fewer
opportunities to find mentorship and achieve success; research
suggests such workplaces are also less profitable.
Holding particular harassers responsible for harms suffered by
an entire industry or gender is difficult; there are
too many contributing factors for it to be easy to apportion
blame. Collective harm may be more suited to
government- or society-level responses. But again, the harm is
there. The question is who ought to bear the cost.
Why it’s hard to think through these accusations
As more men are tarred as bad actors, and once-cherished public
figures become pariahs, imposing responsibility
can feel uncomfortable, even alarming.
People worry that we are sliding down a slippery slope to neo-
puritanism, or in the throes of a witch hunt for sexual
impropriety. Perhaps it will turn out that we are. But social
science research suggests that this discomfort is a
natural consequence of shifting social norms, not necessarily a
sign that the changes are going too far.
Humans are wired to conform to group judgments. Dan Kahan, a
professor at Yale Law School, argued in an
influential paper that we rely more on our peers’ opinions than
on actual laws to determine what behavior is right
or wrong.
In the famous “conformity study ”by the researcher Solomon
Asch, a majority of participants chose to select a
clearly incorrect answer to a question rather than defy the group
and cease being a peer in good standing.
Partisanship was a crucial element in the issue of sexual assault
during the 2016 presidential campaign, when
Donald J. Trump was heard on tape boasting about grabbing
women’s genitals. The ensuing public debate led
many women to discuss their experiences for the first time.
That was a shift away from the previous rules, in which victims
stayed silent. But the partisan aspect of the
episode meant that the new conversation about assault was still
a form of group morality and a way to conform to
group judgments. Opposing sexual assault became a way to call
Mr. Trump unfit for office, and so it fit within the
familiar context of partisan rivalry.
But the more recent accusations —affecting Democrats as well
as Republicans —have scrambled that partisan
logic and made such group moral decision-making more
difficult.
Meanwhile, the old norms of gender roles and hierarchies have
not disappeared, and may conflict with new
demands for accountability. There is no safe harbor of
conformity to be had.
It would be convenient if doing the right thing were easy. But
bringing long-hidden harms to the surface cannot
help disturbing the status quo. Accounting for years of
wrongdoing is costly, and dismantling hierarchies that
fostered harm can lead, in the short term, to chaos. Now society
must decide how many of those costs it is willing
to bear.
Source URL:
https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/29/upshot/sexual-
harassment-response-legal-system-
guidelines.html?partner=bloomberg
Credit: AMANDA TAUB
DETAILS
Subject: Principles; Law schools; Sexual harassment; Society;
Criminal law; Costs; Morality;
Sex crimes; Partisanship; Accountability
People: Goldberg, Jonah CK, Louis Curtin, Jane
Company / organization: Name: New Yorker Magazine Inc;
NAICS: 511120
Identifier / keyword: Sexual Harassment Suits and Litigation
(Civil) Women and Girls Weinstein, Harvey C
K, Louis Franken, Al Trump, Donald J
URL: https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/29/upshot/sexual-
harassment-response-legal-
system-guidelines.html?partner=bloomberg
Publication title: New York Times (Online); New York
Publication year: 2017
Publication date: Nov 29, 2017
Section: upshot
Publisher: New York Times Company
Place of publication: New York
Country of publication: United States, New York
Publication subject: General Interest Periodicals--United States
Source type: Blogs, Podcasts, &Websites
Language of publication: English
LINKS
Linking Service
Terms and Conditions Contact ProQuest
Document type: News
ProQuest document ID: 1969786545
Document URL: https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-
websites/how-should-we-respond-
sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-2?accountid=28844
Copyright: Copyright 2017 The New York Times Company
Last updated: 2017-11-30
Database: ProQuest One Academic
https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how-
should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-
2?accountid=28844
https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how-
should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-
2?accountid=28844
http://YV9QF3BQ4D.search.serialssolutions.com?ctx_ver=Z39.
88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-
8&rfr_id=info:sid/ProQ:nytimes&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:
mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.jtitle=New%20York%20Time
s%20(Online)&rft.atitle=How%20Should%20We%20Respond%
20to%20Sexual%20Harassment?&rft.au=Taub,%20Amanda&rft.
aulast=Taub&rft.aufirst=Amanda&rft.date=2017-11-
29&rft.volume=&rft.issue=&rft.spage=&rft.isbn=&rft.btitle=&r
ft.title=New%20York%20Times%20(Online)&rft.issn=&rft_id=i
nfo:doi/
https://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditions
http://about.proquest.com/go/pqissupportcontactHow Should We
Respond to Sexual Harassment?
4/10/2021 Sexual Harassment | U.S. Equal Employment
Opportunity Commission
https://www.eeoc.gov/sexual-harassment 1/2
U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission
Sexual Harassment
It is unlawful to harass a person (an applicant or employee)
because of that person's
sex. Harassment can include "sexual harassment" or unwelcome
sexual advances,
requests for sexual favors, and other verbal or physical
harassment of a sexual
nature.
Harassment does not have to be of a sexual nature, however,
and can include
o�ensive remarks about a person's sex. For example, it is
illegal to harass a woman
by making o�ensive comments about women in general.
Both victim and the harasser can be either a woman or a man,
and the victim and
harasser can be the same sex.
Although the law doesn't prohibit simple teasing, o�hand
comments, or isolated
incidents that are not very serious, harassment is illegal when it
is so frequent or
severe that it creates a hostile or o�ensive work environment or
when it results in an
adverse employment decision (such as the victim being fired or
demoted).
The harasser can be the victim's supervisor, a supervisor in
another area, a co-
worker, or someone who is not an employee of the employer,
such as a client or
customer.
Employer Coverage
15 or more employees
Time Limits
https://www.eeoc.gov/
4/10/2021 Sexual Harassment | U.S. Equal Employment
Opportunity Commission
https://www.eeoc.gov/sexual-harassment 2/2
180 days to file a charge
(https://www.eeoc.gov/employees/charge.cfm)
(may be extended by state laws)
Federal employees have 45 days to contact an EEO Counselor
(https://www.eeoc.gov/federal/fed_employees/complaint_overvi
Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost ContainmentEm
Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost ContainmentEm

More Related Content

Similar to Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost ContainmentEm

There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docx
There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docxThere are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docx
There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docxsusannr
 
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) wh
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) whIn terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) wh
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) whsimba35
 
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdfCH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdfbkbk37
 
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docx
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docxElements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docx
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docxSALU18
 
Assigment #1 - Brief the Mayor
Assigment #1 - Brief the MayorAssigment #1 - Brief the Mayor
Assigment #1 - Brief the MayorAmanda Weissman
 
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docx
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docxDiscussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docx
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docxtheresiarede
 
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health Management
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health ManagementHistorical factors that have influenced health care, Health Management
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health ManagementArdavan Shahroodi
 
Gender inequality in workplace
Gender inequality in workplaceGender inequality in workplace
Gender inequality in workplaceEnoch Reuben
 
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...Dania
 
Signature assignment organizational ethics & social responsibility
Signature assignment   organizational ethics & social responsibilitySignature assignment   organizational ethics & social responsibility
Signature assignment organizational ethics & social responsibilityAmartin2009
 
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLE
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLEDUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLE
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLEtalishaspadf
 
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And Workplace
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And WorkplaceA Review Of Discrimination In Employment And Workplace
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And WorkplaceLori Moore
 

Similar to Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost ContainmentEm (12)

There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docx
There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docxThere are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docx
There are 4 peer responses needed and the instructions and posts are.docx
 
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) wh
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) whIn terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) wh
In terms of the change model (unfreezing, change, and freezing) wh
 
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdfCH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
 
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docx
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docxElements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docx
Elements of a ContractSuppose that the Fabulous Hotel hires yo.docx
 
Assigment #1 - Brief the Mayor
Assigment #1 - Brief the MayorAssigment #1 - Brief the Mayor
Assigment #1 - Brief the Mayor
 
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docx
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docxDiscussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docx
Discussion 1 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders.docx
 
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health Management
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health ManagementHistorical factors that have influenced health care, Health Management
Historical factors that have influenced health care, Health Management
 
Gender inequality in workplace
Gender inequality in workplaceGender inequality in workplace
Gender inequality in workplace
 
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...
Conceptualising violence at work through a gender lens: Regulation and strate...
 
Signature assignment organizational ethics & social responsibility
Signature assignment   organizational ethics & social responsibilitySignature assignment   organizational ethics & social responsibility
Signature assignment organizational ethics & social responsibility
 
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLE
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLEDUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLE
DUE IN 48 HOURSLABEL EACH DISCUSSION ACCORDING TO TITLE
 
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And Workplace
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And WorkplaceA Review Of Discrimination In Employment And Workplace
A Review Of Discrimination In Employment And Workplace
 

More from TanaMaeskm

Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docx
Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docxNine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docx
Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docx
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docxNewspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docx
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docxTanaMaeskm
 
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docx
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docxNew York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docx
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docxTanaMaeskm
 
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docx
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docxNice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docx
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docxTanaMaeskm
 
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docx
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docxNIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docx
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docx
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docxNice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docx
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docx
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docxNewsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docx
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docx
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docxNicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docx
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docxTanaMaeskm
 
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docx
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docxNew and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docx
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docxTanaMaeskm
 
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docx
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docxNew and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docx
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docxTanaMaeskm
 
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docx
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docxNew essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docx
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docx
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docxNeurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docx
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docx
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docxNeurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docx
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docx
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docxNeeds to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docx
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docxTanaMaeskm
 
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docx
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docxNeed to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docx
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docx
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docxNelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docx
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docx
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docxNegotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docx
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docxTanaMaeskm
 
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docx
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docxNeeds to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docx
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docxTanaMaeskm
 
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docx
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docxNeeds quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docx
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docxTanaMaeskm
 
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docx
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docxneed to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docx
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docxTanaMaeskm
 

More from TanaMaeskm (20)

Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docx
Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docxNine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docx
Nine-year-old Wandas teacher notices that for the past few weeks,.docx
 
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docx
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docxNewspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docx
Newspapers frequently feature stories on how various democratic prin.docx
 
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docx
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docxNew York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docx
New York UniversityType of InstitutionA four-year Private .docx
 
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docx
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docxNice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docx
Nice thought process and good example of foot into the door” ).docx
 
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docx
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docxNIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docx
NIST and Risk Governance and Risk Management Please respond to the.docx
 
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docx
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docxNice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docx
Nice thought process ;)!Some in social media agree with your v.docx
 
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docx
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docxNewsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docx
Newsletter pertaining to an oceanographic environmental issue 1500.docx
 
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docx
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docxNicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docx
Nicole Martins is the controller at UMC Corp., a publicly-traded man.docx
 
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docx
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docxNew and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docx
New and Orignal work. Please cite in MLA citation and use in text ci.docx
 
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docx
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docxNew and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docx
New and Origninal work. The topic is already provided below and I ne.docx
 
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docx
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docxNew essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docx
New essay -- minimum 300 words3 resources used NO cover sheet or.docx
 
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docx
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docxNeurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docx
Neurological DisordersNeurological disorders, such as headaches, s.docx
 
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docx
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docxNeurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docx
Neurodevelopmental and Neurocognitive Disorders Paper··I.docx
 
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docx
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docxNeeds to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docx
Needs to be done by 8pm central time!!!!!!An important aspect .docx
 
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docx
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docxNeed to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docx
Need to know about 504 plan and IEP.  I need to research the process.docx
 
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docx
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docxNelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docx
Nelson Carson is a 62-year-old man who presents to his private pract.docx
 
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docx
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docxNegotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docx
Negotiation strategiesUsing the text Negotiation Readings, Exerc.docx
 
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docx
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docxNeeds to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docx
Needs to be done in the next 3-4 hours .docx
 
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docx
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docxNeeds quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docx
Needs quotes and needs to be citied!!about 2 pages.NO PLAGARISM..docx
 
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docx
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docxneed to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docx
need to work on my present assignment using my last assignment as .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfPoh-Sun Goh
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...Poonam Aher Patil
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptRamjanShidvankar
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docxPoojaSen20
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxNikitaBankoti2
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxVishalSingh1417
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 

Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost ContainmentEm

  • 1. Employee Health, Wellness Programs, and Cost Containment Employee health is just one of the many reasons the absenteeism rate continues to rise in organizations and worldwide. When employees have poor health, they are not able to perform at their fullest potential. According to the Bureau of Labor Statistics (2018), “About 4.2 million workers in January 2018 missed work because they had an illness, injury, or medical problem or appointment” (para. 1). Employers are constantly reviewing the cost of healthcare premiums while not taking a closer examination of the actual �nancial and productivity loss that occurs when employees are not performing their assigned jobs. All absences should be tracked (planned and unplanned). The human resources (HR) professional will typically have data on vacations, on-the-job injuries, the Family and Medical Leave Act (FMLA) related absences and disabilities since these absences are closely tied to the employees’ pay and are easy to identify. Today, many health insurance companies provide employees or employers with an array of wellness program options. Some employers may even offer wellness education workshops and virtual care. To truly understand the costs associated with health- related absences, the employer can sift through reports on obesity, high blood pressure, depression, etc. The HR professional can also review articles published by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. “It is advisable to commit to health promotion with a corresponding commitment to data
  • 2. collection. Without data, evaluation is impossible” (Cascio & Boudreau, 2011, p. 123). To evaluate the effectiveness of a wellness program, you may use one of the following methods: Cost-effective analysis Cost-bene�t and return-on-investment analysis The employer must take the initiative, value the employees, focus on a long-lasting employee or employer relationships, and design wellness programs that �t within the company culture. The ultimate objective is to have healthier employees, reduce productivity loss, and lower the absenteeism rate and health insurance premiums. Workplace Wellness Programs On-site �tness center or health club membership Employee assistance program Healthy snacks Yoga Chair massage Meditation breaks
  • 3. Reference: Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018, March 29). TED: The Economics Daily Image. Retrieved from http s://www.bls.gov/opub/ted/2018/4-point-2-million-workers-have- illness-related-work-absences-in-j anuary-2018.htm Wellness challenges Smoking cessation programs Transit options Additional Materials From your course textbook, Process Metrics and Measurement Complete Self-Assessment Guide, review the following chapters: Criterion #2: De�ne Criterion #3: Measure From the South University Online Library, review the following article: Meet the Wellness Programs That Save Companies Money (https://www.thecampuscommon.com/library/ezproxy/ticketdem ocs.asp? sch=suo&turl=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx? direct=true&db=bth&AN=118686064&site=eds-live) From the Internet, review the following:
  • 4. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018, March 29).TED: The Economics Daily Image. Retrieved from https://www.bls.gov/opub/ted/2018/4-point-2-million-workers- have-illness-related-work-abs ences-in-january-2018.htm https://www.thecampuscommon.com/library/ezproxy/ticketdemo cs.asp?sch=suo&turl=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?di rect=true&db=bth&AN=118686064&site=eds-live O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E Gender Harassment: Broadening Our Understanding of Sex-Based Harassment at Work Emily A. Leskinen • Lilia M. Cortina • Dana B. Kabat Published online: 27 July 2010 � American Psychology-Law Society/Division 41 of the American Psychological Association 2010 Abstract This study challenges the common legal and organizational practice of privileging sexual advance forms of sex-based harassment, while neglecting gender harass- ment. Survey data came from women working in two male-
  • 5. dominated contexts: the military and the legal profession. Their responses to the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ) revealed five typical profiles of harassment: low victimization, gender harassment, gender harassment with unwanted sexual attention, moderate victimization, and high victimization. The vast majority of harassment vic- tims fell into one of the first two groups, which described virtually no unwanted sexual advances. When compared to non-victims, gender-harassed women showed significant decrements in professional and psychological well-being. These findings underscore the seriousness of gender harassment, which merits greater attention by both law and social science. Keywords Gender harassment � Sexual harassment � Working women � Well-being In 1964, Title VII of the Civil Rights Act codified the ille- gality of sex discrimination. However, it took another 13 years for a federal appellate court to recognize sexual harassment as a form of sex discrimination (e.g., Barnes v.
  • 6. Costle, 1977). The prevailing view of sexual harassment, both then and now, sees unwanted sexual attention as ‘‘the quintessential harassment’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1710). Among legal scholars, a notable group has emerged who contend that this conceptualization is too narrow, arguing that ‘‘gender harassment’’ should be included in legal under- standings of sex-based harassment (Epstein, 1998; Franke 1995, 1997, 2004; Growe, 2007; Hébert, 2005; Shultz, 1998, 2003, 2006). Gender harassment refers to ‘‘a form of hostile environment harassment that appears to be motivated by hostility toward individuals who violate gender ideals rather than by desire for those who meet them’’ (Berdahl, 2007a, p. 425). In this article, we lend empirical support to the assertion that gender harassment is a serious form of sex discrimination that deserves more attention. Using survey data from two samples of working women, we demonstrate that most sexual harassment in traditionally male domains entails gender harassment in the absence of sexual advan-
  • 7. ces; we also show how these experiences are associated with negative personal and professional outcomes. Central Constructs In its broadest sense, sex-based harassment 1 refers to ‘‘behavior that derogates, demeans, or humiliates an indi - vidual based on that individual’s sex’’ (Berdahl, 2007b, E. A. Leskinen (&) � D. B. Kabat Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA e-mail: [email protected] L. M. Cortina Departments of Psychology and Women’s Studies, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA 1 Psychology and the law have most commonly referred to this phenomenon as ‘‘sexual harassment.’’ However, Berdahl (2007b) makes a compelling case that ‘‘sex-based harassment’’ is a better
  • 8. construct label, as it emphasizes sex (in the sense of femaleness or maleness) rather than sexuality or sexual desire. We use both terms interchangeably—‘‘sexual harassment’’ and ‘‘sex-based harass- ment’’—to be consistent with the terminology of other literature but also encourage revision of that terminology. 123 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 DOI 10.1007/s10979-010-9241-5 p. 644). Subsumed under this umbrella term are (at least) three related categories of behavior (e.g., Fitzgerald, Gelf- and, & Drasgow, 1995; Fitzgerald, Swan, & Magley, 1997). First, gender harassment refers to ‘‘a broad range of verbal and nonverbal behaviors not aimed at sexual cooperation but that convey insulting, hostile, and degrading attitudes about women’’ (Fitzgerald et al., 1995, p. 430). Examples of
  • 9. gender harassment include anti-female jokes, comments that women do not belong in management, and crude terms of address that denigrate women (e.g., referring to a cow - orker as a ‘‘dumb slut’’). By contrast, unwanted sexual attention involves expressions of romantic or sexual interest that are unwelcome, unreciprocated, and offensive to the recipient (e.g., unwanted touching, pressure for dates or sexual behavior). The third category is sexual coercion: bribes or threats that make the conditions of the victim’s employment contingent on her sexual cooperation (e.g., offering a promotion in exchange for sexual favors, threat- ening termination unless sexual demands are met). Lim and Cortina (2005) elaborated on the relationships among gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and sexual coercion. They explained that ‘‘unwanted sexual attention, as the name suggests, represents unwelcomed, unreciprocated behaviors aimed at establishing some form of sexual relationship. One could argue that sexual coerci on
  • 10. is a specific, severe, rare form of unwanted sexual attention, involving similar sexual advances coupled with bribery or threats to force acquiescence’’ (p. 484). In stark contrast, gender harassment communicates hostility that is devoid of sexual interest. Gender harassment can include sexually crude terminology or displays (for instance, calling a col - league a ‘‘cunt’’ or telling a sexually graphic joke about her), but these behaviors differ from unwanted sexual attention in that they aim to insult and reject women, not pull them into a sexual relationship. In colloquial terms, the difference between unwanted sexual attention/coercion versus gender harassment is analogous to the difference between a ‘‘come on’’ versus a ‘‘put down’’ (Fitzgerald et al., 1995). Gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and sexual coercion refer to behaviors rather than legal con- structs. That said, sexual coercion is roughly parallel to what the law calls quid pro quo harassment, whereas unwanted sexual attention and gender harassment together map onto
  • 11. the legal category of hostile environment harassment (e.g., Fitzgerald et al., 1995; Fitzgerald, Drasgow, Hulin, Gelfand, & Magley, 1997). The next section elaborates on these legal understandings of sexual harassment. Legal Perspectives on Sexual Harassment Title VII prohibits employers from discriminating against any individual with regard to ‘‘compensation, terms, conditions, or privileges of employment, because of such individual’s…sex’’.2 When the federal courts first recog- nized sexual harassment as a Title VII violation, they were acting on cases in which women had lost jobs for failing to comply with their employers’ sexual demands, termed ‘‘quid pro quo harassment’’ (beginning with Barnes v. Costle, 1977). It took another decade before the U.S. Supreme Court would rule that ‘‘hostile work environment harassment’’ could constitute unlawful sexual harassment. In Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson (1986), and later reaf- firmed in Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc. (1993), the Court described hostile environment sexual harassment as
  • 12. occurring ‘‘[w]hen the workplace is permeated with ‘dis- criminatory intimidation, ridicule, and insult’… that is ‘sufficiently severe or pervasive to alter the conditions of the victim’s employment and create an abusive working environment.’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc., 1993, p. 21). Hostile environment sexual harassment is the pri - mary focus of the current article. According to both Meritor and Harris, to create a leg- ally actionable hostile environment, the sexually harassing conduct must be either severe or pervasive. In Harris, the Court provided further guidance for determining whether a hostile work environment is present: the harassing conduct must pass both an objective test (a ‘‘reasonable’’ person would find it hostile or abusive) and a subjective test (the victim must have experienced it as abusive). Referring to the objective test, it added that ‘‘whether an environment is ‘hostile’ or ‘abusive’ can be determined only by looking at all the circumstances’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc.,
  • 13. 1993, p. 22). Five years later, in Oncale v Sundowner Offshore Ser- vices, Inc. (1998), the Supreme Court elaborated on what should be considered when examining ‘‘all the circum- stances,’’ explicitly mandating attention to the larger social context. The Oncale decision also described types of conduct that might be considered ‘‘severe.’’ Particularly relevant to our article, it clearly stated that a motivation of sexual desire is not a prerequisite for establishing objective severity: …harassing conduct need not be motivated by sexual desire to support an inference of discrimination on the basis of sex. A Trier of fact might reasonably find such discrimination, for example, if a female victim is harassed in such sex-specific and derogatory terms…as to make it clear that the harasser is moti- vated by general hostility to the presence of women 2 In addition, Title VII prohibits discrimination based on race, color,
  • 14. religion, or national origin. Due to space constraints, this article only details how Title VII has been interpreted to prohibit sexual harassment. 26 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 in the workplace. (Oncale v Sundowner Offshore Services, Inc., 1998, p. 80). Despite this last development in judicial interpretations of Title VII, the prevailing legal conception of sexual harass- ment remains a highly sexualized one, in which sexually advancing or threatening conduct is seen as ‘‘the essence of harassment’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1716). The harassing behaviors alleged in Oncale, although recognized as moti - vated by hostility rather than desire, still involved sexually predatory behavior (e.g., sodomy with a bar of soap, threa- tened rape). The Supreme Court has never clearly stated whether the harassing conduct itself (as opposed to the
  • 15. motivation for the conduct) must involve some form of sexual advance to violate Title VII. Some appellate decisions have rejected this requirement of sexualized content; for example, in Williams v. General Motors Corp. (1999), the Sixth Circuit Court of Appeals stated that: ‘‘harassing behavior that is not sexually explicit but is directed at women and motivated by discriminatory animus against women satisfies the ‘based on sex’ requirement.’’ The federal bench, however, is far from unanimity on this issue. In the same Williams v GMC (1999) case, one judge filed a dissenting opinion, arguing vehemently that ‘‘…Title VII does not proscribe ‘anti-female animus’ at all’’ and ‘‘the broad new standard my colleagues have conjured here is not a correct application of Title VII sex discrimination law presently on the books.’’ Some courts routinely either dismiss hostile environment cases that do not involve sexual conduct, or they ‘‘disaggregate’’ sexual from nonsexual conduct and then deem the latter to be irrelevant to a hostile environment
  • 16. claim (see Franke, 2004, Growe, 2007, and Schultz, 2006 for various post-Oncale case examples). Thus, the privileging of the sexual advance in sexual harassment law continues. To summarize, on many occasions the federal judiciary has indicated, implicitly and explicitly, that offensive behavior must reference sexuality to constitute unlawful sex-based harassment. As a result, gender harassment involving no sexual advances routinely gets neglected by the law. This occurs even when the behavior fits all other characteristics of a legally actionable hostile environment: occurring ‘‘because of’’ the victim’s sex (interpreting ‘‘sex’’ to mean femaleness rather than sexuality); being ‘‘severe or pervasive’’ enough to adversely change the conditions of her employment; and creating a work envi- ronment that a ‘‘reasonable’’ person would find hostile or abusive, and that the victim herself finds as such. Promi - nent legal scholars have critiqued the exclusively sexual view of sex-based harassment, arguing for instance that
  • 17. ‘‘most harassment is not designed to achieve sexual grati- fication. Instead, it is used to preserve the sex segregation of jobs by claiming the most highly rewarded forms of work as masculine in composition and content’’ (Schultz, 2006, p. 22; see also Epstein, 1998; Franke, 1995, 1997, 2004; Growe, 2007; Shultz, 1998, 2003). At the same time that these issues have been debated in law reviews, an empirical literature on sexual harassment has developed in psychology. How has psychological science made sense of harassment based on sex and gender? Psychological Research on Sexual Harassment In psychology, researchers have examined lay perceptions of sexual harassment more than any other aspect of sexual harassment. These perceptions have differed over time, between men and women, and across cultures (Cortina & Berdahl, 2008; Rotundo, Nguyen, & Sackett, 2001). One finding has not changed, however: when researchers have compared perceptions of gender harassment versus
  • 18. unwanted sexual attention/coercion, participants have consistently rated the former as less severe, less offensive, and less likely to represent what they see as ‘‘sexual harassment’’ (e.g., Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo, Reid, & Deaux, 1995; Tang, Yik, Cheung, & Choi, 1995). Quite separate from studies of sexual harassment per- ceptions within the lay public have been surveys of actual harassment experiences among working adults. As in the law, much of this work has concentrated on sexually advancing behaviors. For instance, the U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board (USMSPB) surveyed federal employees in 1980, 1987, and 1994 about their encounters with ‘‘sexual harassment,’’ defined as ‘‘uninvited and unwel- come sexual attention and/or behavior’’ (USMSPB, 1994, p. vi). Employees were asked to indicate the extent that they had experienced a list of specific acts, virtually all of which contained some form of sexual advance or sexual threat (from unwanted touching to pressure for dates to
  • 19. sexual assault). These surveys were well-executed and have had a major impact on the field. However, they neglected gender harassment. Unlike the USMSPB, some sexual harassment researchers routinely include questions about gender harassment in their surveys. This is true, for example, of the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), which has become the most widely used and validated measure of sexual harassment experiences (Fitzgerald et al., 1988; Fitzgerald, Magley, Drasgow, & Waldo, 1999; Stark, Chernyshenko, Lancaster, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2002). Some versions of the SEQ even assess two subtypes of gender harassment—both ‘‘sexist’’ and ‘‘crude/offensive’’ behavior (Stark et al., 2002). When SEQ researchers have divided harassment into its various subtypes, they have found gender harassment to be the most common (e.g., Fitzgerald et al., 1988, 1999; Langhout et al., 2005; Schneider, Swan, & Fitzgerald, 1997). Little SEQ research,
  • 20. Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 27 123 however, has focused on experiences of gender harassment in isolation from other behaviors. Studies of sexual harassment prevalence and outcomes—using the SEQ and other instruments—typically collapse across the different facets of behavior for an overall measure of ‘‘offensive sex- related experiences in the workplace’’ (Fitzgerald, Swan et al., 1997, p. 9; for examples from other research pro- grams, see Barling, Rogers, & Kelloway, 2001; Culbertson & Rosenfeld, 1994; Richman, Shinsako, Rospenda, Flah- erty, & Freels, 2002). Although this practice permits examination of sexual harassment as a holistic phenome- non, and has yielded many important findings, it obscures the unique experience and impact of gender harassment. In addition to survey research, some psychologists have approached the study of sexual harassment from an
  • 21. experimental perspective. This work has also been char- acterized by an emphasis on sexual attention. For instance, lab studies have operationalized men’s sexually harassing behavior only as the sexual touching of women (Perry, Kulik, & Schmidtke, 1998; Pryor, 1987) or as sexually suggestive questioning (Woodzicka & LaFrance, 2005). In these examples and others, there was virtually no attention to gender harassment. 3 While gender harassment has been neglected in research, this behavior has also almost certainly gone unreported in organizations. Studies have found that victims who perceive the harassment as more severe are more likely to report their experiences to a superior (Bergman, Langhout, Palmieri, Cortina, & Fitzgerald, 2002). However, research into lay perceptions of sexual harassment (described above; e.g., Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo et al., 1995; Tang et al., 1995) suggests that gender harassment is widely believed to be inconsequential, or somehow less important
  • 22. than unwanted sexual attention in the workplace. Victims should, therefore, be less likely to see gender harassment as worthy of reporting, which means that organizational authorities should be less likely to intervene (Langhout et al., 2005). This makes it all the more imperative that social science bring gender harassment to the fore, so that it may be recognized as a legitimate and serious form of sex- based discrimination in the workplace. If It’s ‘‘Just’’ Gender Harassment, Why Should We Care? Because gender harassment has no explicit, sexually predatory component to it (unlike unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion), it may seem less worthy of scientific or legal scrutiny. However, past research on everyday sexism has found that regular sexist interactions decrease psychological well-being and predict symptoms of psychological trauma (Berg, 2006; Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001). For example, Swim and colleagues
  • 23. (2001) asked participants to keep track of instances of ordinary sexist behavior (e.g., anti-female jokes, comments reflecting gender stereotypes) observed or experienced in any life setting. They found that these everyday sexist encounters were associated with greater anger, anxiety, and depression. To explain these negative outcomes, Swim and colleagues (2001) argued that everyday sexism triggers feelings of stereotype threat, defined as ‘‘being at risk of confirming, as self-characteristic, a negative stereotype about one’s group’’ (Steele & Aronson, 1995, p. 797). Similar to everyday sexism, gender harassment may foster stereotype threat in women, especially those working in traditionally masculine domains. Gender harassment may be used to cue women that they are inadequate, out of place, and unable to perform at the level of men. The associated experience of stereotype threat could set off a cascade of negative outcomes in victims. Research has demonstrated that sexual harassment is
  • 24. linked with a wide range of victim outcomes (for recent reviews, see Berdahl & Raver 2010; Cortina & Berdahl, 2008; Foote & Goodman-Delahunty, 2005). For instance, studies have found that sexual harassment is associated with decreased satisfaction with one’s job and professional relationships, loss of productivity, and increased turnover intentions and behaviors (e.g., Barling et al., 2001; Langhout et al., 2005; Sims, Drasgow, & Fitzgerald, 2005). Moreover, the consequences of sexual harassment are not constrained to the job site. Victims also report lower psy- chological well-being, more physical health problems, and even symptoms of traumatic stress (e.g., Culbertson & Rosenfeld, 1994; Fitzgerald, Swan et al., 1997; Richman, Shinsako, Rospenda, Flaherty, & Freels, 2002). By and large, however, this research has analyzed ‘‘sexual harassment’’ as a global phenomenon, failing to differen- tiate among the subtypes of sex-harassing behavior. It is, therefore, impossible to know from this work whether
  • 25. gender harassment by itself would have the same adverse implications for employee well-being. Hypotheses In sum, legal scholars have developed compelling theories about the importance of gender harassment, which we sought to test using large-scale survey research. In line with prior studies, we hypothesized that gender harassme nt, without unwanted sexual attention or coercion, would be 3 Notable exceptions exist, however, in the experimental literature on sexual harassment. For instance Schneider, Tomaka, and Palacios (2001) studied sexual harassment in the laboratory by exposing women to sexist comments—a clear form of gender harassment. 28 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 the most common form of sex-based harassment that
  • 26. women experience (Hypothesis 1). We also hypothesized that women would report negative professional and per- sonal outcomes, even when they ‘‘only’’ experience gender harassment (Hypothesis 2). We tested these hypotheses with survey data from women working in two male-dom- inated domains: the U.S. Military (Study 1) and federal legal practice (Study 2). Performing jobs that are highly nontraditional for their gender, women in these domains blur the boundaries between stereotypically ‘‘male’’ and ‘‘female’’ behavior. This makes them particularly vulner- able to being scorned and rejected (e.g., gender harassed) by colleagues who value rigid and clear distinctions between the sexes (Berdahl, 2007a, b). Study 1: The Military Survey Participants and Procedure Study 1 involved secondary analysis of survey data col - lected by the U.S. Military. This survey began with a non- proportional stratified, single stage random sample of
  • 27. active-duty members from all branches of the U.S. Military (Army, Navy, Air Force, Marines, and Coast Guard); women and people of color were oversampled. The initial sample contained 60,415 individuals, of whom 53,170 were deemed ‘‘eligible’’ (reasons for ineligibility were various, such as inability to locate the sample member). These individuals were invited to complete surveys either on paper or online, and 19,960 usable surveys were returned (38% response rate). The current study focused on the 9,725 women who responded to the survey. Just over one-half of these women identified as White (55%), one- quarter as Black or African American (24%), and 11% as Hispanic or Latina. Forty-eight percent of the respondents reported some college, and 38% reported having at least a 4-year college degree. The number of years of active ser - vice reported by members revealed a bi-modal distribution, with 43% reporting less than 6 years and 33% reporting 10–20 years of active duty. For more information on this
  • 28. sample and procedures, see Lipari and Lancaster (2003). Measures All participants completed the 2002 Department of Defense Status of the Armed Forces Survey on Workplace and Gender Relations.4 Descriptive statistics, coefficient alphas, and intercorrelations for all variables analyzed in Study 1 appear in Table 1. For multi-item scales, we reverse-coded items as needed and then summed relevant items to create scale-scores; higher scores reflect greater levels of the underlying construct. Sex-Based Harassment. To assess unwanted sex- based experiences in the military, surveys contained an updated version of the Sexual Experiences Questionnaire- Department of Defense (SEQ-DoD) developed by Fitzgerald and colleagues (1999; see also Stark et al., 2002). Participants described how often over the prior 12 months they had experienced various forms of unwanted, uninvited ‘‘sex/gender related talk and/or behavior’’ involving military
  • 29. personnel, civilian employees, or contractors. They responded on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = very often. The measure consisted of 18 questions, in which there are four subscales: (1) gender harassment: sexi st, (2) gender harassment: crude, (3) unwanted sexual attention, and (4) sexual coercion. The gender harassment: sexist subscale measured treatment that conveys explicit antipathy toward members of one gender. The subscale consisted of four items, such as ‘‘made offensive sexist remarks (for example, suggest- ing that people of your gender are not suited for the kind of work you do)’’ and ‘‘referred to people of your gender in insulting or offensive terms.’’ Four items also assessed experiences of gender harassment: crude behavior; although sexual on the surface, this behavior expresses animosity rather than attraction. Examples included: ‘‘made offensive remarks about your appearance, body, or sexual activities’’ and ‘‘made gestures or used body lan-
  • 30. guage of a sexual nature that embarrassed or offended you.’’ The unwanted sexual attention subscale consisted of six items, including ‘‘made unwanted attempts to establish a romantic relationship with you despite your efforts to discourage it’’ and ‘‘touched you in a way that made you feel uncomfortable.’’ The sexual coercion subscale con- tained four items, e.g., ‘‘implied faster promotions or better treatment if you were sexually cooperative’’ For more detail on this measure, including evidence of its high reliability and validity (see Fitzgerald et al., 1999; Stark et al., 2002). Psychological Well-Being. Psychological well-being was measured by the short, 5-item version of the Mental Health Inventory (MHI-5) (Viet & Ware, 1983). On a scale from 1 (little or none of the time) to 4 (all or most of the time), survey respondents rated how often they had experienced various psychological states over the prior 4 weeks. Examples of psychological states included in this
  • 31. measure are: ‘‘felt calm and peaceful’’ and ‘‘felt so down in the dumps that nothing could cheer you up.’’ Researchers have found this scale to be reliable when used in the general population (Berwick et al., 1991). 4 This is a recurring survey; prior surveys took place in 1995 and 1988. Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 29 123 Performance Decline Due to Poor Emotional Health. In order to determine whether participants experienced any difficulties during daily activities or work as a result of mental health problems, surveys included three questions adapted from the short-form health survey (SF-36) used in the Medical Outcomes Study (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Respondents rated how often over the past 4 weeks had they experienced a problem (such as ‘‘didn’t do work or other activities as
  • 32. carefully as usual’’) with their work or other activities ‘‘as a result of emotional problems (such as feeling depressed or anxious).’’ Response options ranged from 1 (little or none of the time) to 4 (all or most of the time). Performance Decline Due to Poor Physical Health. Surveys assessed health effects on work via four items adapted from the SF-36 (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). On a scale from 1 (little or none of the time) to 4 (all or most of the time), respondents rated how often over the past 4 weeks they had had problems with their work or other daily activities ‘‘as a result of their physical health.’’ Examples of problems included: ‘‘were limited in the kind of work or other activities you do’’ and ‘‘had diffi culty performing the work or other activities you do (for example, it took extra effort).’’ General Health. Surveys asked respondents about their general health by having them respond to four statements on a scale of 1 (definitely false) to 4 (definitely
  • 33. true). Examples of statements included ‘‘I seem to get sick a little easier than other people’’ (reverse coded), and ‘‘my health is excellent.’’ These questions were adapted from the SF-36 (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). Work Attitudes. Three measures tapped work attitudes. On a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), six items assessed coworker satisfaction; examples included: ‘‘you are satisfied with the relationships you have with your coworkers’’ and ‘‘there is very little conflict among your coworkers.’’ This scale was adapted from multiple sources, two items being taken from the Job Satisfaction Survey (Spector, 1997), three items adapted from the 1995 Armed Forces Sexual Harassment Survey (Edwards, Elig, Edwards, & Riemer, 1997), and one item created for this survey. Six items adapted from the 1995 Armed Forces Sexual Harassment Survey (Edwards et al., 1997) measured work satisfaction. These items included statements such as ‘‘you
  • 34. like the kind of work you do’’ and ‘‘your work makes good use of your skills.’’ Organizational commitment was assessed using a modified version of Mowday, Steers, and Porter’s (1979) Organizational Commitment Question- naire. This scale contained four items, including ‘‘you areT a b le 1 D e sc ri p ti v e st a ti st ic
  • 64. ti o n s a re si g n ifi c a n t, p .0 0 1 30 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 willing to make sacrifices to help your Service’’ and ‘‘you
  • 65. are glad that you are a part of your Service.’’ Turnover Intentions. In order to measure respondents’ thoughts and intentions of leaving military employment, five items were adapted from the 1999 Survey of Active Duty Personnel Form A (1999 ADS). Using a dichotomous yes/no scale, respondents indicated whether over the prior 6 months they had, for example, ‘‘Thought seriously about leaving the military’’ or ‘‘Discussed leaving and/or civilian opportunities with family or friends.’’ Control Variables. We controlled for race, rank, and service branch in all outcome analyses. Respondents self- reported their race (coded 0 = minority and 1 = white). They also provided their rank (i.e., paygrade) at the time of the survey. Response options ranged from E-1 to E-9 for enlisted personnel; from W-1 to W-5 for warrant officers; and from O-1/O1E to O-6 or above for commissioned officers. When releasing these data to the public, the Defense Manpower Data Center (DMDC) collapsed these
  • 66. choices into five ordered categories: 1 = E1–E4; 2 = E5– E9; 3 = W1–W5; 4 = O1–O3; and 5 = O4–O6. Participants self-reported their service branch as either Army, Navy, Air Force, Marines, or Coast Guard. Results Profiles of Sex-Based Harassment In order to test our first hypothesis, we used k-means cluster analysis. k-means cluster analysis groups persons who are similar on specified variables (see Hartigan, 1975, for more information on this analytic approach). In the present study, the k-means analysis classified women by the type and amount of sex-based harassment they had experienced, as indicated on the SEQ-DoD. Included in this analysis were all women who reported experiencing at least one behavior on the SEQ-DoD at least one time over the previous 12 months (n = 5,698). After standardizing these women’s scores on the four subscales of the SEQ-DoD, we requested two-, three-, four-, five-, and six-cluster solutions, and chose
  • 67. the five-cluster solution for further analysis. We based this decision on theoretical interest; we wanted to isolate women who had experienced primarily gender harassment without unwanted sexual attention or coercion. Profiles of means on the z-scored SEQ-DoD scales appear in Fig. 1. The largest group consisted of women who reported the lowest levels of harassment (Group 1; n = 3,933). As seen in Fig. 1, the experiences they described almost exclu- sively consisted of sexist behavior. The second-largest group (n = 1,161) contained women who had encountered both subtypes of gender harassment—sexist and crude— but very little unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion (henceforth, this group will be referred to as Group 2, the ‘‘Gender Harassment’’ group). Group 3 (n = 429) dis- closed episodes of unwanted sexual attention, in addition to moderate levels of sexist and crude behavior. Group 4 (n = 138) had encountered moderate levels of all types of harassment measured by the SEQ-DoD. Group 5 (n = 37),
  • 68. the smallest profile group, reported the most frequent harassment on all four subscales. In sum, 89.4% of harassment victims fell into Group 1 or 2, which described experiences of gender harassment but virtually no unwan- ted sexual attention or coercion. This pattern of victimization provides strong support for Hypothesis 1, which had predicted gender harassment (in the absence of sexual attention or coercion) to be the most common manifestation of sex-based harassment. -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14
  • 69. Gender Harassment: Sexist Gender Harassment: Crude Unwanted Sexual Attention Sexual Coercion Z -S c o re s High Victimization (0.6%) - Group 5 Moderate Victimization (2.4%) - Group 4 Gender Harassment and Unwanted Sexual
  • 70. Attention (7.5%) - Group 3 Gender Harassment (20.4%) - Group 2 Low Victimization (69%) - Group 1 Fig. 1 Profiles of harassment among military women (n = 5,698) Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 31 123 In addition to the five groups revealed through cluster analysis, we identified a sixth ‘‘Nonvictims’’ group, which contained all women who had responded ‘‘never’’ to all items of the SEQ-DoD. This group (n = 4,014) served as a comparison group in subsequent analyses. Table 2 reports demographic information for each of the profile groups. Using chi-square and ANOVA analy- ses, we found significant relationships between profile membership and race, v2 (5, N = 9711) = 55.34, p
  • 71. .001; rank, v2 (10, N = 9704) = 159.00, p .001; years served, F (5, 9664) = 81.90, p .001; education level, F (5, 9621) = 32.14, p .001; and service branch, v2 (20, N = 9712) = 246.04, p .001. As seen in Table 2, the racial make-up of each profile group generally mirrored the overall sample. However, white respondents were slightly more likely to be in the Nonvictims Group, Group 1 (Low Victimization), or Group 2 (Gender Harassment). Ethnic minority respondents were more likely to appear in Group 5 (High Victimization). Regarding military rank and group membership, commissioned officers were dis- proportionately likely to appear in Group 1 (Low Victimization), whereas enlisted personnel were dispro- portionately represented in Group 3 (Gender Harassment with Unwanted Sexual Attention). In terms of tenure (i.e., years served) in the military, Nonvictims differed from all other groups by having served the greatest average number of years. Educational differences were most pronounced for the Nonvictims and Group 1 (Low Victimization), who had significantly higher educational levels than other
  • 72. groups. Outcomes of Sex-Based Harassment For theoretical reasons, we were most interested in out- comes for the group that had experienced primarily gender harassment (Group 2: the Gender Harassment group 5 ), which we compared to outcomes for women in the Non- victims group. A comparison of these two groups addresses the question of whether experiences of ‘‘just’’ gender harassment are associated with adverse consequences. In order to equalize cell sizes for this analysis, we ran- domly selected 1,000 women from the Gender Harassment group and 1,000 women from the Nonvictims group. Using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), we then compared these two groups on the mental health, physical health, and organizational outcome variables. Covariates in this analysis were racial minority status, military rank, and service branch (to account for factors that could potentially
  • 73. affect harassment risk). We found a significant multivariate main effect of gender harassment on the collection of outcomes, Wilks’ Lambda = .81, F (8, 1822) = 52.06, p .001. As Table 3 shows, women in the Gender Harassment group scored significantly lower than the Nonvictimized women on all work attitudes (work satisfaction, coworker satisfaction, and organizational commitment). They also reported greater performance decline due to both physical and emotional health, and they described less overall psychological well-being and health satisfaction. Further- more, women in the Gender Harassment group disclosed greater thoughts and intentions of leaving their jobs. Cohen’s effect sizes (d) ranged from .22 to .79, averaging Table 2 Descriptive statistics for profile groups—Sample 1 (Military) Profile group Non- White percent
  • 74. Rank percent Years served Education Branch of service (%) Enlisted Warrant officer Commissioned officer Mean SD Mean SD Army Navy Marine corps Air Force Coast guard Nonvictims (n = 4,014) 47.0 69.8 3.6 26.5 2.28 1.07 2.21 0.70 26.6 21.1 11.0 34.4 6.9 Group 1: Low Victimization (n = 3,933) 42.1 68.2 3.3 28.4 2.07 1.06 2.21 0.70 28.9 21.4 13.8 26.8 9.1 Group 2: Gender Harassment (n = 1,161)
  • 75. 39.8 75.3 3.6 20.9 1.82 1.02 2.11 0.69 27.6 24.4 17.5 19.8 10.7 Group 3: Gender Harassment & Unwanted Sexual Attention (n = 429) 51.3 91.6 0.9 7.5 1.47 0.81 1.87 0.63 34.5 24.7 14.7 15.4 10.7 Group 4: Moderate Victimization (n = 138) 55.8 92.0 0.0 8.0 1.50 0.83 1.83 0.70 39.1 22.5 21.7 9.4 7.2 Group 5: High Victimization (all types) (n = 37) 70.3 91.9 0.0 8.1 1.65 0.95 1.83 0.57 56.8 2.7 29.7 5.4 5.4 5 We did not include Group 1 (the ‘‘low victimization’’ group) in outcome analyses, even though their experiences largely consisted of gender harassment, for two primary reasons. First, it is unlikely that the extremely low rates of harassing behavior described by this group would be seen as ‘‘sufficiently severe or pervasive’’ to be actionable under Title VII. In addition, we hope to avoid the criticism that
  • 76. we are ‘‘making mountains out of molehills’’ by foregrounding conduct that, while offensive, is transient and rare. 32 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 .48; the largest differences emerged for coworker satis- faction (.79), psychological well-being (.67), and work satisfaction (.58). These findings provided strong support for Hypothesis 2, which had predicted that experiences of gender harassment (alone) would be associated with neg- ative outcomes. To provide an additional point of comparison, we also combined Groups 3, 4, and 5 into a ‘‘Sexual Advance Harassment’’ group (n = 604). All of these women, unlike those in Groups 1 or 2, reported significant experiences of unwanted sexual advances at work, in the form of sexual attention and/or sexual coercion. We then conducted a
  • 77. second MANCOVA, comparing the outcomes of this Sex- ual Advance Harassment group to those of the Gender Harassment group and the Nonvictims group. There was a significant multivariate main effect of harassment-group- membership on the collection of outcomes, Wilks’ Lambda = .77, F (16, 4748) = 40.82, p .001. As Table 3 shows, outcome means for the Gender Harassment Victims fell in between those for the Nonvictims and Sexual Advance Victims (although closer to the means of the latter group). According to follow-up Tukey tests, all outcomes for the gender-harassed women were significantly worse than those of the nonvictimized women. Moreover, Sexual Advance Victims showed significantly worse outcomes than Gender Harassment Victims, with two exceptions: there were no significant differences between the two groups on work satisfaction and turnover intentions. Study 2: The Attorney Survey Participants and Procedure
  • 78. We sought to cross-validate the results from Study 1 with secondary analysis of data collected from women working in a very different context: the legal profession. Participants were drawn from a stratified random sample of attorneys from a large federal judicial circuit. Surveys were sent to 9,223 individuals, yielding a 53% response rate. The current study focused on the 1,425 women who responded to the survey. Most of these women were white (93%). They ranged in age from 24 to 79 years (M = 39.09, SD = 7.81). Sixty-eight percent of the women were married or part- nered, while 18% had never been married. These women were highly educated, all holding at least a Juris Doctor, and some holding additional graduate degrees. Similar to the military sample, they worked in a traditionally masculine occupation where women remain a minority. They com- pleted a paper-and-pencil self-report survey. More information about these participants and procedures appears in Cortina et al. (2002) and Lim and Cortina (2005). 6
  • 79. Measures Table 4 displays the descriptive statistics, coefficient alphas, and intercorrelations for all variables from Study 2. Similar to Study 1, for multi-item scales, we reverse-coded items as needed and then summed relevant items to create scale-scores; higher scores reflect greater levels of the underlying construct. Sex-Based Harassment. Similar to Study 1, we used items from the SEQ developed by Fitzgerald et al. (1995, 1988) to assess sex-based harassment. Participants described how often they had experienced a list of unwanted sexual or sexist behaviors over the past 5 years Table 3 ANCOVAs for physical health, mental health, and work outcomes—Sample 1 (Military) Outcome measured Nonvictims Gender Harassment Victims Sexual Advance Harassment Victims
  • 80. df F p Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI 1. Psychological well-being 16.76 (2.92) 16.58, 16.94 14.65 (3.43) 14.43, 14.86 13.62 (3.93) 13.31, 13.94 2, 2550 122.07 .001 2. Performance decline due to poor emotional health 3.63 (1.49) 3.54, 3.73 4.47 (2.07) 4.34, 4.60 5.14 (2.51) 4.94, 5.34 2, 2562 75.91 .001 3. Health satisfaction 13.60 (2.10) 13.47, 13.73 13.04 (2.49) 12.89, 13.20 12.53 (2.62) 12.31, 12.74 2, 2550 24.06 .001 4. Performance decline due to poor physical health 4.91 (2.09) 4.78, 5.04 5.62 (2.56) 5.46, 5.78 6.40 (3.16) 6.15, 6.66 2, 2555 37.90 .001 5. Coworker satisfaction 22.96 (4.50) 22.68, 23.24 18.89 (5.43) 18.55, 19.23 18.08 (5.73) 17.61, 18.54 2, 2532 169.93 .001 6. Work satisfaction 22.73 (5.44) 22.39, 23.07 18.96 ( 6.33) 18.56, 19.35 18.54 (6.62) 18.01, 19.07 2, 2554 89.16 .001 7. Organizational commitment 16.53 (2.66) 16.36, 16.70 15.49 (3.28) 15.28, 15.69 14.78 (3.35) 14.51, 15.05 2, 2555 29.15 .001 8. Turnover intentions 2.69 (1.80) 2.57, 2.80 3.44 (1.54) 3.35, 3.54 3.46 (1.58) 3.33, 3.58 2, 2558 53.49 .001 6 Although drawing on the same larger dataset, these two past studies
  • 81. have different foci from each other and from the current article. Cortina et al. (2002) focused on gender differences in experiences of incivility and harassment in the legal profession. Lim and Cortina (2005) detailed how uncivil and harassing behaviors tend to co- occur and jointly affect personal and professional outcomes. Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 33 123 from judges, attorneys, trustees, marshals, court security officers, and court personnel. They responded on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = many times. The measure consisted of nine questions, from which there are three subscales: gender harassment, unwanted sexual attention, and sexual coercion. The gender harassment subscale consisted of two items: ‘‘made offensive remarks or jokes about women in your
  • 82. presence?’’ and ‘‘publicly addressed you in unprofessional terms (e.g., ‘honey,’ ‘dear’)?’’ 7 The unwanted sexual attention subscale contained four items, including: ‘‘attempted to establish a romantic or sexual relationship despite your efforts to discourage it?’’ Two items com- prised the sexual coercion subscale, such as, ‘‘implied more favorable treatment of you or your client if you were sexually cooperative?’’ Job-Related Outcomes. We used a three-item scale (a = .74), developed for the purposes of this survey, to measure attorneys’ intentions to change careers (e.g., ‘‘I often think about leaving federal litigation’’). We measured job stress with three items (a = .75), such as ‘‘my expe- riences working in the federal court are more stressful than I’d like.’’ Finally, in order to assess professional relationship satisfaction, we used a three-item scale (a = .76) that consisted of items such as ‘‘in general, I am satisfied with my professional relationships with other attorneys in federal
  • 83. court.’’ For all three scales, response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Control Variables. We controlled for race and job tenure in outcome analyses. Respondents self-reported their race (coded 0 = minority and 1 = white). They also reported the number of years they had actively practiced law, including clerkships. Response options were 1 (0–5 years of practice), 2 (6–10 years of practice), 3 (11– 20 years of practice), 4 (21–30 years of practice), and 5 (31 ? years of practice); we collapsed response options 4 and 5 due to the small number of women who chose the latter option. Results Profiles of Sex-Based Harassment Similar to Study 1, we standardized the attorney women’s scores on the subscales of the SEQ, including all women who had reported at least one experience of an SEQ behavior (n = 491). We again performed a k-means cluster
  • 84. analysis of these subscales, and again chose a 5-cluster solution. Profiles of attorney women’s means on the z- scored SEQ appear in Fig. 2. Mirroring the profiles from the military sample, the largest group consisted of women who reported minimal experiences of harassment (Group 1; n = 320). Those who disclosed high levels of gender harassment, with almost no unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion, made up the second-largest group (Group 2; n = 134). Group 3 (n = 30) described episodes of unwanted sexual attention, in addition to moderate levels gender harassment. Group 4 (n = 3) and Group 5 (n = 4) were quite small, and dis- closed moderate and high levels of all types of harassment measured by the SEQ, respectively. To summarize, over 90% of harassed women fell into one of the two groups reporting little or no sexually advancing harassment experiences (Group 1 or 2), further supporting Hypothesis 1. We also identified a group of Nonvictims (n = 338),
  • 85. who had responded ‘‘never’’ to all items of the SEQ. Demographic information for each of the profile groups appears in Table 5. Using chi-square and F tests, we found no significant relationships between profile membership and race, v2 (5, N = 1317) = 4.24, p = .52; age, F (37, 1286) = 0.79, p = .81; or years practicing law F (5, 1333) = 0.98, p = .43. Table 4 Descriptive statistics, alpha coefficients, and correlations—Sample 2 (Attorneys) Variables M SD a 1 2 3 4 5 1. Gender harassment 1.74 2.14 .69 – 2. Unwanted sexual attention 0.19 0.79 .74 .42** – 3. Sexual coercion 0.01 0.12 .84 .14** .41** – 4. Intention to change careers 6.00 2.93 .74 .02 .03 .00 – 5. Professional relationship satisfaction 12.04 2.24 .76 -.06* - .04 -.02 -.36** – 6. Job stress 9.17 2.74 .75 .15** .04 .06* .32** -.34** Note. Scale-scores were derived by summing responses across all items in each scale for all women in the sample; higher scores reflected greater levels of the underlying construct
  • 86. ** p .01, * p .05 7 Unlike the military survey in Study 1, the brevity of this subscale precluded distinctions between ‘‘sexist’’ and ‘‘crude’’ gender harass- ment. Together, the two items assessed ‘‘gender harassment’’ as a global phenomenon. 34 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 Outcomes of Sex-Based Harassment Again, for theoretical reasons, the group that experienced high gender harassment (without sexual attention or coer- cion; Group 2, n = 134) was the focus of our primary outcome analysis. To serve as a comparison group of sim- ilar size, we randomly selected 150 women from the Nonvictims group. We then compared these 150 Nonvic- tims to the 134 Gender Harassment victims on the three job-
  • 87. related outcomes, using MANCOVA. Racial minority sta- tus and years practicing law served as covariates. Again, results suggested that there was a significant multivariate main effect of gender harassment on the collection of out- comes, Wilks’ Lambda = .96, F (3, 238) = 3.00, p .05. Follow-up univariate analyses (ANCOVAs), reported in Table 6, revealed significant effects on job stress and sat- isfaction with professional relationships. Compared to their non-harassed counterparts, gender-harassed women repor- ted significantly higher levels of job stress (d = .34) . They also described less satisfaction with their relationships with federal judges, other attorneys in the federal court, and court personnel (d = .32). However, we did not find a significant group difference in intention to change careers (the means for both groups were similarly low: close to six, on a scale that can range from 3 to 15). With the exception of this last result, Hypothesis 2 was supported among women attorneys. To provide additional insight into group differences, we
  • 88. again combined Groups 3, 4, and 5 into a Sexual Advance Harassment group (n = 37); their outcome means appear in Table 6. We compared the outcomes of this group with those of the Nonvictims and Gender Harassment Victims (using MANCOVA), finding a significant multivariate main effect of harassment on outcomes, Wilks’ Lambda = .95, F (6, 546) = 2.29, p .05. Follow-up Tukey tests indicated that the Sexual Advance group differed significantly from the Nonvictims group in terms of professional relationship satisfaction. The Gender Harassment Victims also reported less professional relationship satisfaction, and more job stress, than Nonvictims. Gender Harassment Victims did not differ significantly from Sexual Advance Victims, however, on any outcome. Put differently, we found that gender-harassed women attorneys fared just as poorly as those who had experienced sexual advance harassment. General Discussion This article draws attention to the incidence and correlates
  • 89. of gender harassment in the workplace. Social science -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 Gender Harassment Unwanted Sexual Attention Sexual Coercion Z -S c o re s
  • 90. High Victimization (0.8%) - Group 5 Moderate Victimization (0.6%) - Group 4 Gender harassment and Unwanted Sexual Attention (6.1%) - Group 3 Gender Harassment (27.3%) - Group 2 Low Victimization (65.2%) - Group 1 Fig. 2 Profiles of harassment among women attorneys (n = 491) Table 5 Descriptive statistics for profile groups—Sample 2 (Attorneys) Profile group Non-White Percent Age Years practicing law M SD M SD Nonvictims (n = 851) 5.2 39.37 8.09 2.23 0.91 Group 1: Low Victimization (n = 320) 6.7 38.32 7.31 2.21 0.90 Group 2: Gender Harassment (n = 134) 4.5 39.45 7.36 2.37 0.87
  • 91. Group 3: Gender Harassment & Unwanted Sexual Attention (n = 30) 6.7 37.63 6.13 2.23 0.77 Group 4: Moderate Victimization (n = 3) 0.0 42.50 0.71 2.67 0.58 Group 5: High Victimization (all types) (n = 4) 25.0 35.00 2.31 1.75 0.50 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 35 123 research has often analyzed sex-based harassment as a global phenomenon, failing to distinguish among the dif- ferent facets of behavior; this practice may have obscured the experiences of many harassed women, especially those working in male-dominated fields. At the same time, many federal judges have evaluated potentially harassing conduct through a (hetero)sexualized lens, in which they privilege a ‘‘top-down, male-female sexual come-on image of harassment’’ (Schultz, 2006, p. 26). This ‘‘sexual model of
  • 92. sexual harassment’’ does not provide an explanation for gender harassment that is devoid of sexual interest, which we found to be the norm in women’s experiences of harassment in traditionally masculine domains. Key Findings The first goal of this article was to investigate the preva- lence of different dimensions of sexually harassing conduct. Consistent with our hypothesis, gender harass- ment in the absence of unwanted sexual attention or sexual coercion was the most common manifestation of harass- ment faced by women in the military and the law (employment contexts which, importantly, were once the exclusive province of men). In fact, in both settings, 9 out of every 10 victims had experienced primarily gender harassment, with virtually no unwanted sexual overtures. Taken together, our empirical results support the legal theory that ‘‘much of the time, harassment assumes a form that has little or nothing to do with sexuality but everything
  • 93. to do with gender’’ (Schultz, 1998, p. 1687). This conduct is not about misguided attempts to draw women into sexual relationships; quite the contrary, it rejects women and attempts to drive them out of jobs where they are seen to have no place. One could argue that, in these instances, ‘‘sexual harassment is used both to police and discipline the gender outlaw: the woman who dares to do a man’s job is made to pay’’ (Franke, 1997, p. 764). Had we collapsed across the subtypes of harassing behavior, as many psy- chologists do, this striking pattern of results would not have surfaced. The second goal of this article was to understand the correlates of gender harassment for working women. When comparing victims of gender harassment to women who reported no harassment experiences, we found that ‘‘just’’ gender harassment was associated with multiple negative outcomes. Specifically, in the military context, gender- harassed women reported lower psychological well-being,
  • 94. job performance, job commitment, and satisfaction with their employment and health; they also described more thoughts and intentions of leaving their jobs. These results remained significant even after controlling for the women’s race, rank, and service branch. Among attorneys, gender- harassed women (compared to nonharassed women) reported lower satisfaction with professional relationships and higher job stress, above and beyond the effects of race and job tenure. Thus, experiences of gender harassment alone were associated with negative personal and profes- sional outcomes in two very different contexts of work. Implications for Sexual Harassment Jurisprudence This research has important legal implications. Although popular wisdom might suggest that the legal definition of sexual harassment is fixed, in actuality the legal under- standing changes as courts interpret and refine precedent. Our research underscores the need to broaden legal and scientific conceptualizations of sexual harassment, so that
  • 95. gender harassment can be recognized as a harmful and objectionable condition of employment, even when not paired with unwanted sexual attention. Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964 says nothing about sexual behavior. Definitions that limit sex-based harassment to unwanted advances emerged over time as the courts revised their interpretations of Title VII. Our results sug- gest that further revision is in order, to prohibit not just sexually predatory conduct, but also behavior that creates a hostile work environment for members of one sex but contains no sexual advance—that is, gender harassment. As we have shown, gender harassment does not simply pro- vide a backdrop for other kinds of harassment; it is the modal form of sex-based harassment faced by women at work (at least in male-dominated domains). Moreover, it alters the terms of employment for targeted women, being associated with a variety of negative professional outcomes.
  • 96. Table 6 ANCOVAs for job-related outcomes—Sample 2 (Attorneys) Outcome measured Nonvictims Gender Harassment Victims Sexual Advance Harassment Victims df F p Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI Mean (SD) 95% CI 1. Intention to change careers 5.81 (3.01) 5.32, 6.31 5.95 (2.93) 5.45, 6.46 6.51 (3.25) 5.43, 7.60 2, 307 0.98 .452 2. Professional relationship satisfaction 12.51 (2.14) 12.15, 12.86 11.81 (2.21) 11.43, 12.18 11.38 (2.97) 10.39, 12.37 2, 308 5.25 .006 3. Job stress 9.04 (2.84) 8.53, 9.55 10.01 (2.82) 9.51, 10.50 9.76 (3.67) 8.48, 11.04 2, 277 3.61 .032 36 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 Our outcome results suggest that harassment exclusively consisting of gender-related hostility has adverse work- related correlates. That is, the more that women experi -
  • 97. enced gender harassment, the less satisfied they were with their jobs and colleagues, the more they experienced stress on the job, and the more they suffered health problems that detracted from their job performance. Moreover, these results were not trivial in magnitude, being associated with large effect sizes in some cases (particularly for coworker satisfaction and psychological well-being). Findings such as these could be relevant to legal claims of hostile envi - ronment sexual harassment. As noted at the outset of this article, in Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc. (1993) the Supreme Court stated that ‘‘all the circumstances’’ must be considered when determining whether an environment is ‘‘hostile’’ or ‘‘abusive,’’ in violation of Title VII. The Court went on to say that these circumstances may include a number of factors, including whether the discriminatory conduct ‘‘unreasonably inter- feres with an employee’s work performance’’ Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc. (1993, p. 23). In the current study, we
  • 98. documented that gender harassment on its own is linked with a variety of adverse work outcomes, including but not limited to performance decline. These outcomes do not necessarily reflect traumatization or incapacitation of the victim, but this is not a requirement of Title VII: ‘‘…Title VII comes into play before the harassing conduct leads to a nervous breakdown. A discriminatorily abusive work environment, even one that does not seriously affect employees’ psychological well-being, can and often will detract from employees’ job performance, discourage employees from remaining on the job, or keep them from advancing in their careers’’ (Harris v Forklift Systems, Inc., 1993, p. 21). Although we cannot draw definitive causal conclusions from our correlational findings, our results are consistent with these sorts of effects. They support the possibility that ‘‘just’’ gender harassment can create a hostile environment that disadvantages women. Limitations
  • 99. As with any research, our studies have their limitations. All results were based on cross-sectional, correlational data, precluding strong temporal or causal inferences. That said, longitudinal studies of sexual harassment (e.g., Glomb, Munson, Hulin, Berman, & Drasgow, 1999; Sims et al., 2005) provide compelling evidence that our personal and professional outcomes follow, rather than precede, harassment experiences. These data were self-reported; because of this, common method variance or response set could potentially explain some of the significant findings. Surveys were designed to minimize some of these prob- lems: questions about mental health, physical health, and job attitudes were asked prior to and independently of the SEQ, so that responses about harassment did not bias reports of health and attitudes. Also, we were only able to use proxies for job and career turnover, with measures of turnover intentions rather than actual turnover rates. However, past research tells us that one of the best pre-
  • 100. dictors of actual turnover is thoughts of turnover (e.g., Griffeth, Hom, & Gaertner, 2000). Finally, while we did cross-validate results across two large samples that differed by ethnicity and socio-economic status, both samples came from male-dominated organizations. Gender parity has increased in both industries, but as of 2008, only 34% of lawyers were women (U.S. Department of Labor, 2009), and as of September 2009, only 14% of active-duty mili- tary personnel were women (Department of Defense, 2009). We should also emphasize that our data combine the experiences of hundreds of women, and just because gen- der harassment correlates with negative outcomes in these aggregate data does not mean that this behavior has neg- ative outcomes for every individual victim. We found that this is true on average. Whether it is true for any individual woman is a determination that must be made on a case-by- case basis. Our point is simply that courts and social sci -
  • 101. entists should not automatically assume that ‘‘just gender harassment’’ is, by definition, too trivial to create an abu- sive work environment. Future Directions Social scientists continue to focus on sexual ‘‘come-on’’ forms of sexual harassment (e.g., de Haas, Timmerman, & Höing, 2009; Woodzicka & LaFrance, 2005). When sur- veys do include questions about gender harassment, during the analysis of data these questions are often combined with questions about unwanted sexual advances (e.g., Fitzgerald, Drasgow et al., 1997; Sims et al., 2005). This happens even though there are major qualitative differ- ences across the experiences; for example, being sexually propositioned on one occasion is not the same as being targeted with demeaning anti-female remarks on a daily basis. Based on the current study, we recommend that more research parse out experiences of gender harassment from unwanted sexual attention/coercion, which will give rise to
  • 102. new avenues of inquiry. For example, proponents of the- ories as to why people harass others have generally looked for one unifying explanation (e.g., Bargh, Raymond, Pryor, & Strack, 1995; Berdahl, 2007b). It remains entirely pos- sible that different goals motivate the different subtypes of sex-based harassment. Regarding construct labels, we second Berdahl’s (2007b) recommendation that the term ‘‘sex-based harass- ment’’ be used in lieu of ‘‘sexual harassment.’’ Research on Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 37 123 lay perceptions suggests that the latter term conjures up narrow notions of unwanted sexual advances; it fails to include gender harassment in the minds of many (e.g., Fitzgerald & Ormerod, 1991; Loredo et al., 1995; Tang et al., 1995). The term ‘‘sex-based harassment’’ is broader, calling attention to both categories of behavior. It is also
  • 103. closer to the original language of Title VII, which pro- hibited harassment ‘‘based on sex.’’ The current study focused on two male-dominated pro- fessions. Researchers have not yet determined how ‘‘just gender harassment’’ operates in gender-balanced and female-dominated industries. We would speculate that the behavior might be less common in more female-integrated contexts, where women are not perceived as ‘‘encroach- ing’’ on ‘‘men’s territory.’’ In those settings, anti-female hostility might also be less tolerated and more penalized, and thus experienced by victims as less threatening. These and other possibilities await future research. In closing, we emphasize the need for scholars of sex- based harassment, both in psychology and law, to continue the interdisciplinary exchange of ideas. Social scientists and legal theorists often struggle to converse, but consid- erable efforts have been made to bridge the disciplinary gap when it comes to this topic (for examples, see the
  • 104. special issue on ‘‘sexual harassment’’ in Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1999, and the special issue on ‘‘psychology, law, and the workplace’’ in Law and Human Behavior, 2004). The arguments of prominent legal scholars inspired the current psychological research. We hope that, in turn, our results can inform further evolution in legal thinking about harassment based on sex and gender. Acknowledgments We are grateful to Anna Kirkland, Abby Stewart, and members of our research lab at the University of Michigan for their valuable feedback on this research. Thanks also to the following individuals for their contributions to Study 2: Louise F. Fitzgerald, Leslie V. Freeman, Vicki J. Magley, Kimberly A. Lon- sway, Regina Day Langhout, Jill Hunter-Williams. This article is based on the master’s thesis of the first author, who presented parts of it in July 2008 at the Annual Meeting of the International Society for
  • 105. Political Psychology in Paris, France. References 1999 Survey of Active Duty Personnel [Data file and documentation on CD-ROM]. (2001). Arlington, VA: DMDC. Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power ? sex association and its consequences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 768–781. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.768. Barling, J., Rogers, A., & Kelloway, E. (2001). Behind closed doors: In-home workers’ experience of sexual harassment and work- place violence. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(3), 255–269. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.6.3.255. Barnes v. Costle, 561 F.2d 983 (D.C. Cir. 1977). Berdahl, J. L. (2007a). The sexual harassment of uppity women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 425–437. doi:10.1037/0021- 9010.92.2.425. Berdahl, J. L. (2007b). Harassment based on sex: Protecting social status in the context of gender hierarchy. Academy of Manage-
  • 106. ment Review, 32(2), 641–658. Berdahl, J. L., & Raver, J. L. (2010). Sexual harassment. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), Handbook of industrial/organizational psychology. Wash- ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Berg, S. H. (2006). Everyday sexism and post traumatic stress disorder in women: A correlational study. Violence Against Women, 12(10), 970–988. doi:10.1177/1077801206293082. Bergman, M., Langhout, R. D., Palmieri, P., Cortina, L. M., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). The (un)reasonableness of reporting: Antecedents and consequences of reporting sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 230–242. doi:10.1037/0021- 9010.87.2.230. Berwick, D. M., Murphy, J. M., Goldman, P. A., Ware, J. E., Barsky, A. J., & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance on a five-item mental health screening test. Medical Care, 29, 169–176. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008. Cortina, L. M., & Berdahl, J. L. (2008). Sexual harassment in organizations: A decade of research in review. In J. Barling & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of organizational
  • 107. behavior (pp. 469–497). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. L., Magley, V. J., Freeman, L. V., Collinsworth, L. L., Hunter, M., et al. (2002). What’s gender got to do with it? Incivility in the federal courts. Law and Social Inquiry, 27, 235–270. Culbertson, A., & Rosenfeld, P. (1994). Assessment of sexual harassment in the active-duty Navy. Military Psychology, 6(2), 69–93. doi:10.1207/s15327876mp0602_1. de Haas, S., Timmerman, G., & Höing, M. (2009). Sexual harassment and health among male and female police officers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 390–401. doi:10.1037/ a0017046. Department of Defense. (2009). Active duty military personnel by service by rank/grade. Retrieved December 14, 2009, from http://siadapp.dmdc.osd.mil/personnel/MILITARY/miltop.htm. Edwards, J. E., Elig, T. W., Edwards, D. L., & Riemer, R. A. (1997). The 1995 Armed Forces Sexual Harassment Survey: Adminis- tration, datasets, and codebook for Form B (report no. 95–015). Arlington, VA: DMDC.
  • 108. Epstein, L. B. (1998). What is a gender norm and why should we care? Implementing a new theory in sexual harassment law. Stanford Law Review, 51, 161–182. Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F., Hulin, C. L., Gelfand, M. J., & Magley, V. J. (1997). Antecedents and consequences of sexual harass- ment in organizations: A test of an integrated model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(4), 578–589. doi:10.1037/0021-9010. 82.4.578. Fitzgerald, L. F., Gelfand, M. J., & Drasgow, F. (1995). Measuring sexual harassment: Theoretical and psychometric advances. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 425–445. doi:10.1207/ s15324834basp1704_2. Fitzgerald, L. F., Magley, V. J., Drasgow, F., & Waldo, C. R. (1999). Measuring sexual harassment in the military: The Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ-DoD). Military Psychology, 11(3), 243–263. doi:10.1207/s15327876mp1103_3. Fitzgerald, L. F., & Ormerod, A. J. (1991). Perceptions of sexual harassment: The influence of gender and academic context.
  • 109. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 281–294. doi:10.1111/j. 1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x. Fitzgerald, L. F., Schullman, S. L., Bailey, N., Richards, M., Swecker, J., Gold, Y., et al. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of 38 Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 123 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.768 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.6.3.255 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.425 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.425 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077801206293082 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.230 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.230 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp0602_1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017046 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017046 http://siadapp.dmdc.osd.mil/personnel/MILITARY/miltop.htm http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.4.578 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.4.578 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1704_2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp1704_2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp1103_3 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00797.x sexual harassment in academia and the workplace. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 32, 152–175. doi:10.1016/0001-8791
  • 110. (88)90012-7. Fitzgerald, L. F., Swan, S., & Magley, V. J. (1997). But was it really sexual harassment? Legal, behavioral, and psychological defini - tions of the workplace victimization of women. In W. O’Donohue (Ed.), Sexual harassment: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 5–28). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Foote, W. E., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2005). Evaluating sexual harassment: Psychological social, and legal considerations in forensic evaluations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Franke, K. M. (1995). The central mistake of sex discrimination law: The disaggregation of sex from gender. University of Pennsyl - vania Law Review, 144, 1–99. Franke, K. M. (1997). What’s wrong with sexual harassme nt? Stanford Law Review, 49, 691–772. Franke, K. M. (2004). What’s wrong with sexual harassment? In C. A. MacKinnon & R. B. Siegel (Eds.), Directions in sexual harass- ment law (pp. 169–181). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Glomb, T. M., Munson, L. J., Hulin, C. L., Berman, M., & Drasgow,
  • 111. F. (1999). Structural equation models of sexual harassment: Longitudinal explorations and cross-sectional generalizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 14–28. doi:10.1037/0021- 9010.84.1.14. Griffeth, R. W., Hom, P. W., & Gaertner, S. (2000). A meta- analysis of antecedents and correlates of employee turnover: Update, moderator tests, and research implications for the next millen- nium. Journal of Management, 26, 463–488. doi:10.1177/014 920630002600305. Growe, J. D. (2007). Reform the EEOC guidelines: Protect employees from gender discrimination as mandated by Title VII. Journal of Law and Policy, 24, 275–300. Harris v. Forklift Sys (1993) 114 S.Ct. 367. Hartigan, J. A. (1975). Clustering algorithms. New York: Wiley. Hébert, C. (2005). The disparate impact of sexual harassment: Does motive matter? Kansas Law Review, 53, 341–395. Langhout, R. D., Bergman, M. E., Cortina, L. M., Fitzgerald, L. F., Drasgow, F., & Williams, J. H. (2005). Sexual harassment
  • 112. severity: Assessing situational and personal determinants and outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 975–1007. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02156.x. Lim, S., & Cortina, L. M. (2005). Interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace: The interface and impact of general incivility and sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 483– 496. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.483. Lipari, R. N., & Lancaster, A. R. (2003). Armed Forces 2002 Sexual Harassment Survey. DMDC Report No. 2003–026. Arlington, VA: Defense Manpower Data Center. Loredo, C., Reid, A., & Deaux, K. (1995). Judgments and definitions of sexual harassment by high school students. Sex Roles, 32, 29– 45. doi:10.1007/BF01544756. Meritor Savings Bank v. Vinson, 477 U.S. 57 (1986). Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. (1979). The measurement of organizational commitment. Journal of Voca- tional Behavior, 14, 224–247. doi:10.1016/0001-8791(79) 90072-1. Oncale v. Sundowner Offshore Services, 523 U.S. 75 (1998).
  • 113. Perry, E., Kulik, C., & Schmidtke, J. (1998). Individual differences in the effectiveness of sexual harassment awareness training. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(8), 698–723. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01727.x. Pryor, J. B. (1987). Sexual harassment proclivities in men. Sex Roles, 269–290. doi:10.1007/BF00288453. Richman, J. A., Shinsako, S. A., Rospenda, K. M., Flaherty, J. A., & Freels, S. (2002). Workplace harassment/abuse and alcohol - related outcomes: The mediating role of psychological distress. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(4), 412–419. Rotundo, M., Nguyen, D., & Sackett, P. R. (2001). A meta- analytic review of gender differences in perceptions of sexual harass - ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 914–922. doi:10.1037/ 0021-9010.86.5.914. Schneider, K. T., Swan, S., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1997). Job- related psychological effects of sexual harassment in the workplace: Empirical evidence from two organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(3), 401–415. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.401.
  • 114. Schneider, K. T., Tomaka, J., & Palacois, R. (2001). Women’s cognitive, affective, and physiological reactions to a male coworker’s sexist behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychol - ogy, 31, 1995–2018. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00161.x. Schultz, V. (1998). Reconceptualizing sexual harassment. Yale Law Journal, 107, 1683–1732. Schultz, V. (2003). The sanitized workplace. Yale Law Journal, 112(8), 2061–2197. Schultz, V. (2006). Understanding sexual harassment law in action: What has gone wrong and what we can do about it. Thomas Jefferson Law Review, 29, 1–53. Sims, C. S., Drasgow, F., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2005). The effects of sexual harassment on turnover in the military: Time-dependent modeling. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6), 1141–1152. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1141. Spector, P. E. (1997). Job satisfaction: Application, assessment, cause, and consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Stark, S., Chernyshenko, O. S., Lancaster, A. R., Drasgow, F., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). Toward standardized measurement of
  • 115. sexual harassment: Shortening the SEQ-DoD using item response theory. Military Psychology, 14, 49–72. doi:10.1207/ S15327876MP1401_03. Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.69.5.797. Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 31–53. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00200. Tang, C. S.-K., Yik, M. S. M., Cheung, F. M. C., & Choi, P.-K. (1995). How do Chinese college students define sexual harass - ment? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 503–515. doi: 10.1177/088626095010004008. Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, 42 U.S.C. § 2000e (1994). Retrieved May 15, 2009, from http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/ vii.html. U.S. Department of Labor. (2009). Women in the labor force: A
  • 116. databook. Retrieved December 14, 2009, from http://www. bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf. U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board. (1994). Sexual harassment in the federal workplace: Trends, progress, continuing challenges. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Viet, C. T., & Ware, J. E., Jr. (1983). The structure of psychological distress and well-being in general populations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 730–742. doi:10.1037/ 0022-006X.51.5.730. Ware, J. E., & Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-item short form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care, 30, 473–483. doi:10.1097/00005650- 199206000-00002. Williams v. General Motors Corp., 187 F.3d 553 (6th Cir. 1999). Woodzicka, J., & LaFrance, M. (2005). The effects of subtle sexual harassment on women’s performance in a job interview. Sex Roles, 53(1), 67–77. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-4279-4. Law Hum Behav (2011) 35:25–39 39 123
  • 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(88)90012-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(88)90012-7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.1.14 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.1.14 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014920630002600305 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014920630002600305 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02156.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.483 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01544756 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(79)90072-1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(79)90072-1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01727.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00288453 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.914 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.914 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.82.3.401 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb00161.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1141 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327876MP1401_03 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327876MP1401_03 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00200 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/088626095010004008 http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/vii.html http://www.eeoc.gov/policy/vii.html http://www.bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf http://www.bls.gov/cps/wlf-databook-2009.pdf http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.730 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.51.5.730 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-4279-4
  • 118. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. c.10979_2010_Article_9241.pdfGender Harassment: Broadening Our Understanding of Sex-Based Harassment at WorkAbstractCentral ConstructsLegal Perspectives on Sexual HarassmentPsychological Research on Sexual HarassmentIf It’s ‘‘Just’’ Gender Harassment, Why Should We Care?HypothesesStudy 1: The Military SurveyParticipants and ProcedureMeasuresSex-Based HarassmentPsychological Well- BeingPerformance Decline Due to Poor Emotional HealthPerformance Decline Due to Poor Physical HealthGeneral HealthWork AttitudesTurnover IntentionsControl VariablesResultsProfiles of Sex-Based HarassmentOutcomes of Sex-Based HarassmentStudy 2: The Attorney SurveyParticipants and ProcedureMeasuresSex-Based HarassmentJob-Related OutcomesControl VariablesResultsProfiles of Sex-Based HarassmentOutcomes of Sex-Based HarassmentGeneral DiscussionKey FindingsImplications for Sexual Harassment JurisprudenceLimitationsFuture DirectionsAcknowledgmentsReferences How Should We Respond to Sexual Harassment? Taub, Amanda . New York Times (Online) , New York: New York Times Company. Nov 29, 2017. ProQuest document link ABSTRACT (ENGLISH) The legal system and social science offer principles to evaluate each case and to explain why this moment is so
  • 119. disorienting. FULL TEXT As accusations of sexual misconduct against famous men accumulate , the sheer quantity of dispiriting news is starting to create a confusing blur. The task of responding to sexual harassment and assault feels simultaneously more urgent and more daunting than ever. Society is out of practice at this task; the same culture of silence that protected harassers also suppressed the public response to their crimes. Many people struggle even to know which questions to ask, and worry that if they ask the wrong ones, they might become part of the problem. There is a temptation to simplify matters by viewing all harassers and their offenses as equally awful, or, alternatively, as equally misunderstood. But to be fair and effective, any system needs to make distinctions: to sort Harvey Weinstein from Roy Moore; and Louis C.K. and Matt Lauer from Al Franken. The legal system, while quite different from the court of public opinion, offers principles and reasoning that we can use to evaluate each case as it flares. Slippery slopes and consequences
  • 120. Until recently, all of those accused, no matter the severity of their offenses, faced the same consequences: generally none. Protected by their power and authority, they kept their careers and reputations intact. As that begins to change, some worry that we might bungle the job. “Taking harassment seriously also requires making serious distinctions,” Jonah Goldberg, a conservative columnist, wrote recently for The Los Angeles Times. “And yet Franken’s name is routinely listed alongside Moore’s and Weinstein’s.” Masha Gessen, writing in The New Yorker , worried we may be on the verge of a “sex panic.” Jane Curtin, a comedian who is a friend and former colleague of Mr. Franken’s, compared the current atmosphere to McCarthyism. “It’s just like the red menace,” she said in an interview with The Times. “You don’t know who’s going to be next.” Many of those accused have lost their jobs, but for the most part, they are not facing legal consequences. Yet principles borrowed from criminal law can help us analyze whether our response to their actions is just and fair. Criminal punishment tends to rest on two broad principles: the seriousness of the wrongdoing, and the perpetrator’s intent in committing the crime.
  • 121. Viewed through that lens, the accusations against Mr. Weinstein, which include rape, and Mr. Moore, who is accused of molesting teenage girls, are clearer-cut cases for punishment than those against, say, Louis C.K., who masturbated in front of adult women but did not touch them. It’s also important that courts do not consider only the moment of the crime itself in determining punishment. Our system also punishes defendants who threaten witnesses or obstruct justice, as well as others who help them do so. Here again, the accusations against Mr. Weinstein are especially extreme. According to a report by Ronan Farrow in The New Yorker, he hired ex-Israeli intelligence agents to intimidate victims and journalists into silence. Dana Min Goodman and Julia Wolov, two of the women who have accused Louis C.K. of misconduct, have said they stayed silent for years in part because of pressure from Dave Becky, Louis C.K.’s manager. Mr. Becky has https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how- should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se- 2?accountid=28844 https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how- should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se- 2?accountid=28844 denied threatening them. But the women have said they feared
  • 122. that speaking would bring retribution. Accountability for harm The question of punishment is merely one factor in considering these cases. The wave of accusations has also led to demands that society recognize and repair the harm of sexual misconduct. Caroline Framke, a culture critic for Vox, called for an accounting of the “graveyard of potential cut short by careless cruelty.” The principles of civil law, which are intended to make victims whole and ensure that no one profits from wrongdoing, can offer useful guidance about what is fair, and what is necessary. A central principle is that the person at fault, not the victim, should bear the cost of the harms of wrongdoing. In law school, budding attorneys learn the “eggshell plaintiff” rule, which says that defendants are responsible for all of the harm they cause, even if the injuries were made more extreme because, say, the victim’s skull was as thin as an eggshell. Otherwise, the reasoning goes, the costs will fall on the victims —a more unfair outcome than holding perpetrators responsible for unexpectedly severe damages. It is now becoming clear that there is not a one-to-one
  • 123. correlation between the objective egregiousness of sexual misconduct and the damage it can cause. Louis C.K.’s actions may have been less extreme than Mr. Weinstein’s. But Ms. Goodman and Ms. Wolov have said they felt they could no longer work on projects involving him or his manager —a category that grew to include a large chunk of the comedy industry as Louis C.K.’s career took off. And the Emmy-award-winning writer Kater Gordon told The Information that when Matthew Weiner, her boss on the show “Mad Men,” told her that he “deserved to see her naked,” he undermined her confidence and ambition. (Mr. Weiner has said that he does not remember making that comment, and would not speak that way to a colleague.) Held up next to the allegations against Mr. Weinstein or Mr. Moore, those words may seem like a misdemeanor. But the harm was nevertheless severe, Ms. Gordon says, because she left the television industry, abandoning a promising career. Women are often told to grow a “thicker skin” and become less sensitive to harassment. But the eggshell plaintiff rule suggests a different conclusion: that the harassers should bear the costs of the harm they impose, even on
  • 124. “thin-skinned” victims. We must also consider harms that go beyond the immediate victims. Less diverse workplaces offer women fewer opportunities to find mentorship and achieve success; research suggests such workplaces are also less profitable. Holding particular harassers responsible for harms suffered by an entire industry or gender is difficult; there are too many contributing factors for it to be easy to apportion blame. Collective harm may be more suited to government- or society-level responses. But again, the harm is there. The question is who ought to bear the cost. Why it’s hard to think through these accusations As more men are tarred as bad actors, and once-cherished public figures become pariahs, imposing responsibility can feel uncomfortable, even alarming. People worry that we are sliding down a slippery slope to neo- puritanism, or in the throes of a witch hunt for sexual impropriety. Perhaps it will turn out that we are. But social science research suggests that this discomfort is a natural consequence of shifting social norms, not necessarily a sign that the changes are going too far. Humans are wired to conform to group judgments. Dan Kahan, a professor at Yale Law School, argued in an
  • 125. influential paper that we rely more on our peers’ opinions than on actual laws to determine what behavior is right or wrong. In the famous “conformity study ”by the researcher Solomon Asch, a majority of participants chose to select a clearly incorrect answer to a question rather than defy the group and cease being a peer in good standing. Partisanship was a crucial element in the issue of sexual assault during the 2016 presidential campaign, when Donald J. Trump was heard on tape boasting about grabbing women’s genitals. The ensuing public debate led many women to discuss their experiences for the first time. That was a shift away from the previous rules, in which victims stayed silent. But the partisan aspect of the episode meant that the new conversation about assault was still a form of group morality and a way to conform to group judgments. Opposing sexual assault became a way to call Mr. Trump unfit for office, and so it fit within the familiar context of partisan rivalry. But the more recent accusations —affecting Democrats as well as Republicans —have scrambled that partisan logic and made such group moral decision-making more
  • 126. difficult. Meanwhile, the old norms of gender roles and hierarchies have not disappeared, and may conflict with new demands for accountability. There is no safe harbor of conformity to be had. It would be convenient if doing the right thing were easy. But bringing long-hidden harms to the surface cannot help disturbing the status quo. Accounting for years of wrongdoing is costly, and dismantling hierarchies that fostered harm can lead, in the short term, to chaos. Now society must decide how many of those costs it is willing to bear. Source URL: https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/29/upshot/sexual- harassment-response-legal-system- guidelines.html?partner=bloomberg Credit: AMANDA TAUB DETAILS Subject: Principles; Law schools; Sexual harassment; Society; Criminal law; Costs; Morality; Sex crimes; Partisanship; Accountability
  • 127. People: Goldberg, Jonah CK, Louis Curtin, Jane Company / organization: Name: New Yorker Magazine Inc; NAICS: 511120 Identifier / keyword: Sexual Harassment Suits and Litigation (Civil) Women and Girls Weinstein, Harvey C K, Louis Franken, Al Trump, Donald J URL: https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/29/upshot/sexual- harassment-response-legal- system-guidelines.html?partner=bloomberg Publication title: New York Times (Online); New York Publication year: 2017 Publication date: Nov 29, 2017 Section: upshot Publisher: New York Times Company Place of publication: New York Country of publication: United States, New York Publication subject: General Interest Periodicals--United States Source type: Blogs, Podcasts, &Websites Language of publication: English
  • 128. LINKS Linking Service Terms and Conditions Contact ProQuest Document type: News ProQuest document ID: 1969786545 Document URL: https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts- websites/how-should-we-respond- sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se-2?accountid=28844 Copyright: Copyright 2017 The New York Times Company Last updated: 2017-11-30 Database: ProQuest One Academic https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how- should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se- 2?accountid=28844 https://search.proquest.com/blogs-podcasts-websites/how- should-we-respond-sexual-harassment/docview/1969786545/se- 2?accountid=28844 http://YV9QF3BQ4D.search.serialssolutions.com?ctx_ver=Z39. 88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF- 8&rfr_id=info:sid/ProQ:nytimes&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev: mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.jtitle=New%20York%20Time s%20(Online)&rft.atitle=How%20Should%20We%20Respond% 20to%20Sexual%20Harassment?&rft.au=Taub,%20Amanda&rft.
  • 129. aulast=Taub&rft.aufirst=Amanda&rft.date=2017-11- 29&rft.volume=&rft.issue=&rft.spage=&rft.isbn=&rft.btitle=&r ft.title=New%20York%20Times%20(Online)&rft.issn=&rft_id=i nfo:doi/ https://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditions http://about.proquest.com/go/pqissupportcontactHow Should We Respond to Sexual Harassment? 4/10/2021 Sexual Harassment | U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission https://www.eeoc.gov/sexual-harassment 1/2 U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission Sexual Harassment It is unlawful to harass a person (an applicant or employee) because of that person's sex. Harassment can include "sexual harassment" or unwelcome sexual advances, requests for sexual favors, and other verbal or physical harassment of a sexual nature. Harassment does not have to be of a sexual nature, however, and can include o�ensive remarks about a person's sex. For example, it is illegal to harass a woman by making o�ensive comments about women in general. Both victim and the harasser can be either a woman or a man, and the victim and harasser can be the same sex.
  • 130. Although the law doesn't prohibit simple teasing, o�hand comments, or isolated incidents that are not very serious, harassment is illegal when it is so frequent or severe that it creates a hostile or o�ensive work environment or when it results in an adverse employment decision (such as the victim being fired or demoted). The harasser can be the victim's supervisor, a supervisor in another area, a co- worker, or someone who is not an employee of the employer, such as a client or customer. Employer Coverage 15 or more employees Time Limits https://www.eeoc.gov/ 4/10/2021 Sexual Harassment | U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission https://www.eeoc.gov/sexual-harassment 2/2 180 days to file a charge (https://www.eeoc.gov/employees/charge.cfm) (may be extended by state laws) Federal employees have 45 days to contact an EEO Counselor (https://www.eeoc.gov/federal/fed_employees/complaint_overvi