SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 20
Distrust in American Society
Alyssa Volkmann
SOC 3930
Fall 2014
1
Introduction
In a recent study by Morris Berman (2006), it has been found that distrust is largely
increasing at a great cost to American society. Trust exists in all social interactions and social
systems and can be defined as “a generalized expectancy held by an individual that the word,
promise, oral or written statement of another individual or group can be relied on” (Smith,
2010; 455). As trust can be seen as positive expectations in regards to others actions, words
and decisions, distrust is seen as negative expectations. Both trust and mistrust involve
expectations; however, distrust is the expectation of things feared as opposed to trust as the
hope of things hoped for (Lewicki et al, 1998). Although a general culture of trust is more
likely to occur in democratic political systems like that of the United States, as our
democracy is corrupted with inequality, political and social distrust is continuing to increase
in American society (Sztompka, 1998; Lenard, 2010). This general culture of distrust is
affecting the country’s overall social capital. As social capital decreases, society becomes less
equipped to deal with important societal concerns such as crime, poverty and unemployment
because social capital is a key instrument in dealing with these issues (Smith, 2010; Berman,
2006). As distrust continues to become more prevalent in society, it leads one to wonder
what the causes of individuals and groups being distrustful are. The purpose of this study is
to explore what causes people to become distrustful of other people and institutions in
American society.
2
Literature Review
Studying trust within groups has revealed that variables such as race, gender,
education, socioeconomic status, political affiliation and income, determine degrees of trust
allocated by individuals (Simpson et al, 2007; Binning, 2007). Individuals who experience
inequality based on their identities, such as racial, class, and educational privilege, are more
likely to be distrusting because they experience the negative sides of inequality, as opposed to
the positive side of equality (Simpson et all, 2007).
There is strong evidence that the lack of trust understood by minority groups in the
United States, particularly African-Americans, is greater than that of white identified people
(Smith, 2010). This distrust and lack of confidence African Americans feel towards
institutions, such as the health care system, are deeply rooted in the historical health issues
such as the Tuskegee Syphilis experiments and the AIDS outbreak, which have exploited
and endangered the African American community (Schoff and Yang, 2012; Simmonds,
2008). In a survey conducted in 2006, 767 African Americans and 1267 White Americans
were asked about discrimination and access to health care, as well as sociodemographic
features and racial segregation in their residential areas. The results showed that more
discrimination is experienced among African Americans than White Americans, resulting in
higher amounts of distrust of the health care system (Armstrong et all, 2013). According to
Schoff and Yang (2012), because African-Americans tend to distrust the health care system
so much due to past experiences, it is also more common for them to distrust and lack
confidence in other institutions in society as well. Several studies argue that the only way to
increase the trust and confidence of minority groups toward institutions such as health care,
3
and government in general, is to eradicate racial discrimination, and work to create a culture
of trust by continuing to create and maintain democratic institutions (Armstrong et all, 2013;
Simmonds, 2008; Schoff and Yang, 2012; Sztompka, 1998).
Though out-group trust needs to be repaired, according to Alberto Voci (2006),
people are more likely to trust people who share similar group identities with them. Due to
in-group trust being consistently stronger than out-group trust, it is likely that people are
more likely to trust others who identify within the same racial group as them (Simpson et al,
2007). When examining African-Americans’ distrust of institutions in the United States such
as the police force, it has been concluded that distrust stems from not only negative past
interactions with the police, but also a race-gap within police force. Whites are much less
likely to say that they distrust the police, and they do not share the same racial history with
the police force, or the same racial disparities as African Americans do with the police (Sharp
and Johnson, 2009).
Due to the intersectionalities of inequality, race is not the only factor that causes
distrust. When looking at the differences between racial groups, there are many intersections
to take into account, such as differences in gender, income and education levels. These
variances also impact individuals’ willingness to trust (Austin and Dodge, 1992; Simpson et
all, 2007; Smith, 2010). White Americans largely attain higher levels of education and higher
socioeconomic status than African Americans, and they report higher levels of general trust
(Smith, 2010). Additionally, African-American women struggle with feelings of distrust and
discontentment due to discrimination, expressing the highest amount of discontentment
among Black and White woman and men, while Black men express the second highest
4
amount; Black men experiencing more discrimination and distrust than White women shows
that gender is a less important factor of discrimination than race (Austin and Dodge, 1992).
According to Lewicki et al (1998), to begin to reestablish trust among American
citizens, particularly African Americans, social and political trust must begin to be restored
for all people. Individuals and institutions must recognize the intersectionalities of identities
that cause individuals to be more cautious and less trusting. As discrimination and
marginalized identities are explored in the context of distrust, the following four hypotheses
will be tested:
H1: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, confidence
in institutions will decrease;
H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, trust will
decrease;
H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, closeness to that
racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is not apart
of;
H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial in-group
closeness will not be affected by sex.
5
Methods
This study used data from General Social Survey (GSS) from 2012 with the program
Statistical Program for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Distrust was measured by using several
variables in this study; and an index was created with the following indicators: race, sex and
class. Race is an element that impacts marginalized groups, as non-whites, specifically
African-Americans and Latinos, experience racial discrimination, which leads to distrust
(Smith, 2012). Sex will be understood as a factor that marginalizes the non-dominant sex
(Austin and Dodge, 1992). Class is another variable, as historically it has impacted the
accessibility to resources such as education, that continues to impact socioeconomic status
of marginalized people today (Austin and Dodge, 1992). Together, these three variables will
create a Marginalized Identity Index. The most marginalized identity within the index is
poor, Black women while the least marginalized identity within the index is upper class,
white men.
For H1, I used the following GSS questions to create an index of confidence in
American institutions:
“I am going to name some institutions in this country. As far as the people running these
institutions are concerned, would you say you have a great deal of confidence, only some
confidence, or hardly any confidence at all in them? a. Banks and financial institutions
(CONFINAN); b. Major companies (CONBUS); c. Organized religion (CONCLERG); d.
Education (CONEDUC); e. Executive branch of the federal government (CONFED); g.
Press (CONPRESS); h. Medicine (CONMEDIC).”
6
I created an Institution Confidence Index, using a-h of the above GSS question as
indicators. The values of a-h are: A Great Deal, Only Some, Hardly Any. Scores ranging
from 7-12 on the index will indicate a great deal of confidence in these institutions and are
recoded as 1, scores ranging from 13-16 will indicate some confidence in institutions and are
recoded as 2, and lastly, scores ranging from 17-21 will indicate hardly any confidence in
these institutions and are recoded as 3. The listed values for the Institution Confidence
Index are: Highly Confident, Somewhat Confident, Not At All Confident. This index was
along with the marginalized identity index to test the relationship of marginalized identities
and confidence in institutions.
For H2, the following GSS questions was used to understand the impact of peoples’
own identities and the relationship these identities have with their aptitude to trust others:
“Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you can't be too
careful in life? (TRUST)” and “Do you think most people would try to take advantage of
you if they got a chance, or would they try to be fair? (FAIR).” The values for TRUST are:
Can Trust, Cannot Trust, Depends. The values for FAIR are: Take Advantage, Fair,
Depends. The marginalized identity index was used with this question to determine how
marginalized identities impact individuals’ general trust in others. The index was also used to
understand the relationship between marginalized identities and the belief that others are
more likely to take advantage of them. Trust and the belief of fairness are inherently related
because in a culture of trust, people would be less inclined to believe that others would take
advantage of them. This data is crucial in understanding the relationship of personal
identities and general trust.
7
For H3, instead of using the marginality index, race was used as the independent
variable with the following GSS questions about closeness to particular racial groups: “In
general, how close do you feel to blacks? (CLOSEBLK)” and “And in general, how close do
you feel to whites? (CLOSEWHT).” These two questions are important to gauge the general
sense of comfort, which is inherently linked to trust, among white and black racial groups.
The values of CLOSEBLK and CLOSEWHT are: Very Close, Somewhat Close, Not Close
At All. The relationship among in-group and out-group comfort can be understood by using
race as the independent variable to see how the relationship of ones own racial identity
impacts their closeness to their in and out racial group.
The data from H3 was used as the original table. H4 was tested using race as the
independent variable and CLOSEBLK and CLOSEWHT as the dependent variable with sex
as the control variable. The purpose of this table was to determine whether or not sex
impacts in-group and out-group closeness.
Analysis
Analyses of data were completed using cross tabulations, in addition to calculating the
Chi-Square values and statistical significance of each table. Table 1 refers to testing for H1:
As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, confidence in
institutions will decrease. There is no statistically significant relationship (P=0.622) between
marginalized identities and confidence in institutions. The marginality index is a
measurement of racial, gender and class inequality, which impacts how people view
institutions. As race has served as a large influence of inequality, which is linked to distrust;
Simpson et al also found that factors such as gender and education also have an influence
8
inequality (2007). While the marginality index measures race, sex and class, rather than
education level, these are all indicators of degrees of inequality. Smith found that class is
positively correlated with trust, as it impacts education. On average, minority groups are less
wealthy and educated than whites; this inequality could account for a gap in trust (2010).
Though the most marginalized group was the least confident within the “highly confident”
category with a nearly 6% difference between the most and least marginalized identified
people. However, overall the table is not a statistically significant. In short, this table did not
show a significant relationship between marginalized identities and confidence in
institutions, so H1 cannot be supported.
Table 1: Institution Confidence by Marginalized Identity Index, Year 2012
Chi-Square=2.629, P=0.622
Table 2 tests H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized
identities, trust will decrease. This data is statistically significant (P=0.000) and shows a
correlation between marginality and distrust. One interesting point about this question is its
phrasing. Respondents are asked about their general trust in “most people,” and it has been
found that regardless of the respondent’s race, the phrase “most people” is synonymous to
“white people” (Simpson et all, 2007). Perhaps this has an influence on the results, as
9
respondents among least marginalized (48.8%) and somewhat marginalized (40.4%) group
are significantly more trusting than those who are the most marginalized (18.5%). There is a
30% difference among belief in trusting most people between the most and least
marginalized people. Another interesting aspect of this table is the relationship between the
most marginalized people and distrust, as over 75% of the most marginalized people believe
people cannot be trusted and over half of somewhat marginalized (56.7%) and most
marginalized (76.7%) people believe people cannot be trusted, and only 45.5% of the least
marginalized group believes most people cannot be trusted. Overall, as people identify as
more marginalized, their sense of trust decreases; therefore, table 2 supports H2.
Table 2: Trust Level by Marginality Index, Year 2012
Chi-Square=86.715, P=0.000
Table 3 also tests H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized
identities, trust will decrease. However, this table uses FAIR instead of trust. This
relationship is also statistically significant (P=0.000) and shows a correlation between
marginality and a belief of most people being fair or taking advantage of you. This question
can be examined in terms of trust by understanding that a general belief in people taking
advantage or being fair is linked to a culture of trust. As individuals believe that people are
10
fair, there is ‘a confident positive expectation in other’s conduct;’ while people who believe
that others will take advantage of them, there is ‘a confident negative expectation in other’s
conduct’ (Lewicki et all, 1998). This table shows that as people identify as being more
marginalized, they have a higher belief that most people will take advantage and a lower
belief that most people are fair. Over 59% of the least and somewhat marginalized groups
believe that most people are fair, while only 39% of the most marginalized group believes
the same. This question, like the previous, asks respondents again to answer if “most
people” are fair or take advantage of. Again, it is important to be aware that “most people”
usually leads people to think of white people, regardless of their own race (Smith et all,
2007). This conceivable dual meaning of “most people” for “white people” could
potentially influence marginalized peoples’ belief of fairness, as the question could be
interpreted in terms of how fair respondents believe white people to be. Like table 2, there is
neither a huge difference nor a trend among those who chose “depends” across the
marginality index. However, perhaps one could infer that the reason for the most
marginalized people being the highest value of “depends” could attribute to their belief of
fairness depending on what their definition of “most people.” In short, this table supports
11
H2; because there is a relationship between the most marginalized groups and a low belief in
fairness and a high belief being taken advantage of.
Table 3: Belief in Fairness by Marginality Index, Year 2012
Chi-Square=49.976, P=0.000
Table 4 tests H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group,
closeness to that racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is
not apart of. This relationship is statistically significant (P=0.000) and there is a strong
correlation between race and closeness to white people. 67.1% of white people are identified
with being “very close” to other white people, this being the highest value in that row.
Simpson et all found that trust is fundamentally stronger within racial groups, rather than
among out-groups (2007). Trust is inherently related to closeness, as trust cannot be
established among people without a general sense of closeness or comfort (Sztompka, 1998).
Conversely, 41.9% of Black respondents identified as “somewhat close” to white people,
meaning that there is about an 11% difference between Black and White people identifying
as somewhat close to white people. One might infer that the high number of Black
respondents identifying with being “somewhat close” to white people could be attributed to
assimilating to the white majority. There is a very low percentage of white people who
12
identify as “not close at all” to other white people (2.2%), while there is a higher percent of
Black respondents (8.1%) who identify as “not close at all” to white people.
Table 4: Closeness to White People by Race, Year 2012
Chi-Square=31.791, P=0.000
Table 5 tests H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group,
closeness to that racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is
not apart of. This relationship is statistically significant (P=0.000) and like the previous
table, shows a positive correlation between race and closeness to black people. 76.6% of
Black respondents identify as “very close” to other Black people. This is significantly higher
than the 67.1% of white people who identify as “very close” to other white people in table 4.
Black respondents high percentage of in-group closeness (“very close”) and low percentage
of in-group alienation (“not close at all”) could be attributed to “historical and contemporary
experiences of discrimination, neighborhood and community context, and ethnoracial
socialization” (Smith, 2011; 457). Less than half of white respondents (47%) identify as
“somewhat close” to Black people, while 10.3% identify as “not close at all,” which is higher
than the percent of Black respondents who identified as not close at all to white people. This
could potentially be attributed to white people being the majority; therefore, they are less
13
likely to be put into social situations with black people unless they choose to be in said
spaces, whereas black folks find themselves interacting with white people more regularly due
to being a minority racial group in majority white society. In conclusion, both table 4 and
table 5 supports H3, because they show that in-group closeness is stronger than in-group
alienation or out-group closeness.
Table 5: Closeness to Black People by Race, Year 2012
Chi-Square=76.053, P=0.000
Table 6 tests H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial
in-group closeness will not be affected by sex. This table displays the relationship between
closeness to white people by race and is controlled for gender, and the data is statistically
significant (male P=0.005, female P=0.000) as well. Both white males (66.7%) and white
females (67.3%) feel nearly equally close to other white people, while black males (8.3%) and
black females (7.9%) feel almost equally “not close at all” to white people. This leads one to
infer that sex is less of a factor of closeness than race. This is true because racial socialization
is stronger and harder to disassociate from than gender socialization (Simpson et all, 2007).
Another interesting aspect of this data is that it can be seen that black males feel generally
closer to white people (58.3%) than black females do (43.9%). This could potentially be
14
attributed to living a patriarchal society, giving black men more privilege than black women,
allowing for black men to connect with white people on the basis on male privilege in a
patriarchal society. Though the out-group closeness to white people from black men and
women change according to sex, the in-group closeness of white people is not affected by
sex; therefore, H4 is supported.
Table 6: Closeness White People by Race Controlled for Sex, Year 2012
MALE: Chi-Square=10.712, P=0.005, FEMALE: Chi-Square=25.823, P=0.000
Table 7 tests H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial
in-group closeness will not be affected by sex. The data is statistically significant (male
P=0.000, female P=0.000) and shows the relationship between closeness to black people by
race and is controlled for gender. Like table 6, there the closeness among men and women
within their racial group does not change significantly because of gender. Fascinatingly, over
75% of black males and females feel very close to their racial group, which is higher than the
percent of white people who felt “very” close to their racial group in table 6. As stated in
the original table, a higher percent of white people feel “not close at all” to black people,
15
than black people who feel not close at all to white people. Conversely, less than 1% of black
women identify as “not close at all” to other black folks, as opposed to 4.8% of black men
identifying as “not close at all” to black people. As previously stated, this could potentially
be due to the patriarchic society we live in, which gives men privilege and power over
women, potentially making it easier for black men to branch out from their racial group.
When looking at closeness between black and white people to black people, there is a less
than 1% difference among closeness by race for each sex. In conclusion, table 7 supports H4
because there is no statistically significant difference between the sexes and their closeness
within and outside their racial category.
Table 7: Closeness Black People by Race Controlled for Sex, Year 2012
MALE: Chi-Square=30.903, P=0.000, FEMALE: Chi-Square=46.246, P=0.000
Conclusion
In short, all hypotheses were supported with the exception of H1. There was not a
statistically significant relationship between marginalized identities and confidence in
16
institutions; however, this could be due to measuring too many institutions in the institution
confidence index. Perhaps if confidence in an institution known for being discriminatory,
such as medical and police institution(s) were measured alone, a relationship between
marginalized identities could be found. From previous research, it has been found that past
experiences of discrimination cause higher levels of distrust; therefore, there must be effort
to eliminate the discrimination experienced by groups who are marginalized due to their
race, sex, class, education, and so on (Armstrong et all, 2013; Smith, 2010). Further research
should be completed to find out the relationship of intersecting marginalized identities and
confidence in particular institutions in America.
Being a member of marginalized identity groups has a strong impact on both the
general sense of trust in most people and the belief that most people are fair. The more
marginalized ones identity is, the less trusting of most people they become. As a culture of
distrust is created, cultural capital sacrificed (Sztompka, 1998). This loss of cultural capital
manifests in the deterioration of relationships and trust among family, friends, and social
structures and is harmful for society (Berman, 2006). This sense of distrust alienates people,
as they learn to be skeptical of other groups within society. According to Simpson et all, the
United States is separated by race more than any other identity and this was supported
through the research (2007). Individuals identified as being closer to people within their
racial group than outside their racial group. It was found that that sex does not have a strong
influence on how close people feel to their racial group. Perhaps more research could be
done using age as a factor of in-group racial closeness.
17
In closing, the marginalization of people in society, which is linked distrust, is causing
a decline in social capital. Further research is needed to understand how to prevent
discrimination and treat individuals of all identities equally in society. As long as the United
States continues to marginalize certain racial, sexual, class, educational and class, there will be
in and out-group separation. This alienation and distrust among groups within our society is
harmful to the country’s social capital as a whole. Without a sense of trust and high social
capital, the United States will continue to be ill equipped to deal with issues of inequality,
which perpetuates the marginalization of certain identities (Berman, 2006). This leaves one
with a question that needs extensive research, how can we begin to reverse this culture of
distrust, which perpetuates inequality through marginalizing particular identities?
18
References
Armstrong et all. 2013. “Prior Experiences of Racial Discrimination and Racial Differences
in Health Care System Distrust.” Medical Care 51(2): 144-149.
doi:10.1097/MLR.0b013e31827310a1.
Austin, Roy L. and Hiroko Hayama Dodge. 1992. “Despair, Distrust and Dissatisfaction
among Blacks and Women, 1973-1987.” The Sociological Quarterly 33(4): 579-598.
(http://jstor.org/stable/4121397).
Berman, Morris. 2006. Dark Ages America: The Final Phase of an Empire. New York,
NY: W.W. Norton & Company.
Binning, Kevin R. 2007. “It’s Us Against the World: How Distrust in Americans versus
People-In-General Shapes Competitive Foreign Policy Preferences.” 2007. Political Psychology
28(6): 777-799. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221
Lenard, Patti Tamara. 2010. “Rebuilding Trust in an Era of Widening Wealth Inequality.”
Journal of Social Philosophy 41(1): 73-91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9833.2009.01479.
Lewicki et all. 1998. “Trust and Distrust: New Relationships and Realities.” The Academy of
Management Review 23(3): 438-458 (http://jstor.org/stable/259288).
Schoff, Carla and Tse-Chuan Yang. 2012. “Untangling the associations among distrust, race,
and neighborhood social environment: A social disorganization perspective.” Social Science
and Medicine 74: 1342-1352. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.01.012.
Sharp, Elaine B. and Paul E. Johnson. 2009. "Accounting for Variation in Distrust of Local
Police." Justice Quarterly 26(1):157-182
(http://search.proquest.com/docview/61782576?accountid=28109).
Simmonds, Gwenneth, M.S.N., C.N.M. 2008. "African American Participation in Public
Health Research." ABNF Journal 19(2):69-72
(http://search.proquest.com/docview/218900418?accountid=28109).
Simpson, Brent et all. 2007. “Are Blacks Really Less Trusting Than Whites? Revisiting the
Race and Trust Question.” Oxford University Press 86(2): 525-552.
(http://jstor.org/stable/20430752).
Smith, Sandra Susan. 2010. “Race and Trust.” Annual Review of Sociology 36:453-475. doi:
10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102526.
Sztompka, Piotr. 1998. “Trust, Distrust and Two Paradoxes of Democracy.” European Journal
of Social Theory 1(1): 19-32. doi: 10.1177/136843198001001003.
19
Voci, Alberto. 2006. “The link between identification and in-group favouritism: Effects of
threat to social identity and trust-related emotions.” British Journal of Social Psychology 45:
265-284. doi:10.1348/014466605X52245.

More Related Content

What's hot

Violence bymaleathletes
Violence bymaleathletesViolence bymaleathletes
Violence bymaleathleteslovneetbangar
 
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological IdentitiesBlack Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological IdentitiesJonathan Dunnemann
 
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015Cameron Turner
 
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 upmpeffl
 
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social Issue
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social IssueGender Inequality as a Worldwide Social Issue
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social IssueJames O'Banion
 
Homosexual
HomosexualHomosexual
HomosexualAM Oh
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaAlejandro Luna
 
Women of Color Politics New Media PDF
Women of Color Politics New Media PDFWomen of Color Politics New Media PDF
Women of Color Politics New Media PDFNicole Madera
 
JuniorSenior Seminar Paper
JuniorSenior Seminar PaperJuniorSenior Seminar Paper
JuniorSenior Seminar PaperRachel Kunz
 
The homosexual community
The homosexual communityThe homosexual community
The homosexual communityHmicBrown77
 
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential ElectionRace and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Electionjdubrow2000
 

What's hot (19)

gender final
gender finalgender final
gender final
 
Violence bymaleathletes
Violence bymaleathletesViolence bymaleathletes
Violence bymaleathletes
 
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological IdentitiesBlack Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
Black Males, Social Imagery, and the Disruption of Pathological Identities
 
Race 2009
Race 2009Race 2009
Race 2009
 
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015
MaleSurvivors Lit Review and interviews- March 18 2015
 
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up
475 why americans hate the media II 2012 up
 
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
 
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social Issue
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social IssueGender Inequality as a Worldwide Social Issue
Gender Inequality as a Worldwide Social Issue
 
Homosexual
HomosexualHomosexual
Homosexual
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
 
poster
posterposter
poster
 
Reaction paper social problems
Reaction paper social problemsReaction paper social problems
Reaction paper social problems
 
Take Home Exam
Take Home ExamTake Home Exam
Take Home Exam
 
Women of Color Politics New Media PDF
Women of Color Politics New Media PDFWomen of Color Politics New Media PDF
Women of Color Politics New Media PDF
 
JuniorSenior Seminar Paper
JuniorSenior Seminar PaperJuniorSenior Seminar Paper
JuniorSenior Seminar Paper
 
The homosexual community
The homosexual communityThe homosexual community
The homosexual community
 
Final Proposal
Final Proposal Final Proposal
Final Proposal
 
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn LiteracyPanel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
Panel: Healthy Sexuality, Pornography, and Porn Literacy
 
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential ElectionRace and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election
Race and the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election
 

Viewers also liked

Viewers also liked (10)

TUP Diploma
TUP DiplomaTUP Diploma
TUP Diploma
 
Teori bahasa dan otomata geral
Teori bahasa dan otomata geralTeori bahasa dan otomata geral
Teori bahasa dan otomata geral
 
1.0
1.01.0
1.0
 
fazendo convites em ingles (making invittiond)
fazendo convites em ingles (making invittiond)fazendo convites em ingles (making invittiond)
fazendo convites em ingles (making invittiond)
 
Nuevas siete maravillas del mundo moderno
Nuevas siete maravillas del mundo modernoNuevas siete maravillas del mundo moderno
Nuevas siete maravillas del mundo moderno
 
Rubrica 2
Rubrica 2Rubrica 2
Rubrica 2
 
Problemas y políticas de la educación básica 1
Problemas y políticas de la educación básica 1Problemas y políticas de la educación básica 1
Problemas y políticas de la educación básica 1
 
Diseño y planificacion de puentes
Diseño y planificacion de puentesDiseño y planificacion de puentes
Diseño y planificacion de puentes
 
Wikis
Wikis Wikis
Wikis
 
Aprendizaje basado en problemas (ABP)
Aprendizaje basado en problemas (ABP)Aprendizaje basado en problemas (ABP)
Aprendizaje basado en problemas (ABP)
 

Similar to DISTRUST-IN-AMERICAN-SOCIETY

Research methods final project
Research methods final projectResearch methods final project
Research methods final projectHayoung Cho
 
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docx
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docxRunning Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docx
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docxtoltonkendal
 
Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this
 Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this  Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this
Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this MoseStaton39
 
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docx
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docxResearch is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docx
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docxwrite4
 
Research Paper - Wealth Inequality
Research Paper - Wealth InequalityResearch Paper - Wealth Inequality
Research Paper - Wealth InequalityJordan Chapman
 
Marital Infidelity: Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...
Marital Infidelity:  Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...Marital Infidelity:  Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...
Marital Infidelity: Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...Rachel Woodward
 
U.s. government lecture#5
U.s. government lecture#5U.s. government lecture#5
U.s. government lecture#5Amanda Bartley
 
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docx
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docxResponse one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docx
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docxronak56
 
Political Ideological Divides and Actual Views
Political Ideological Divides and Actual ViewsPolitical Ideological Divides and Actual Views
Political Ideological Divides and Actual ViewsMichael Silverman
 
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docx
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docxRunning head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docx
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docxcharisellington63520
 
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALFINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALMargaret O'Brien
 
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docx
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docxReligion has had its ups and Now that.docx
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docxwrite30
 
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdf
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdfTowards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdf
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdfStatsCommunications
 
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The Satisfact
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The SatisfactSatisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The Satisfact
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The SatisfactKatie Harris
 
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society Report
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society ReportIPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society Report
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society ReportOlivia Kresic
 
America_Divided_Guide-4
America_Divided_Guide-4America_Divided_Guide-4
America_Divided_Guide-4Sara Wicht
 
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...Elizabeth Montes
 
PLAP 4150 Research Report
PLAP 4150 Research ReportPLAP 4150 Research Report
PLAP 4150 Research ReportLauren Bryant
 
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docx
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docxYour friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docx
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docxdanielfoster65629
 

Similar to DISTRUST-IN-AMERICAN-SOCIETY (20)

Research methods final project
Research methods final projectResearch methods final project
Research methods final project
 
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docx
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docxRunning Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docx
Running Head WEALTH INEQULITIES AND DEMOCRACY1WEALTH INEQULI.docx
 
Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this
 Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this  Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this
Report #3 Changing Public Opinion Before beginning this
 
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docx
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docxResearch is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docx
Research is so important to psychology because it helps researchers.docx
 
Research Paper - Wealth Inequality
Research Paper - Wealth InequalityResearch Paper - Wealth Inequality
Research Paper - Wealth Inequality
 
The Changing Jury Pool: Gen Y'ers
The Changing Jury Pool:  Gen Y'ersThe Changing Jury Pool:  Gen Y'ers
The Changing Jury Pool: Gen Y'ers
 
Marital Infidelity: Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...
Marital Infidelity:  Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...Marital Infidelity:  Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...
Marital Infidelity: Beliefs and Practices Changing America’s Most Famous Ins...
 
U.s. government lecture#5
U.s. government lecture#5U.s. government lecture#5
U.s. government lecture#5
 
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docx
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docxResponse one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docx
Response one pold-01How Diversity Affects Knowledge and Politica.docx
 
Political Ideological Divides and Actual Views
Political Ideological Divides and Actual ViewsPolitical Ideological Divides and Actual Views
Political Ideological Divides and Actual Views
 
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docx
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docxRunning head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docx
Running head FAMILY CHANGES SINCE 1960FAMILY CHANGES SINCE .docx
 
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALFINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
 
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docx
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docxReligion has had its ups and Now that.docx
Religion has had its ups and Now that.docx
 
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdf
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdfTowards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdf
Towards a more comprehensive measure of eudaimonia_Carol Graham.pdf
 
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The Satisfact
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The SatisfactSatisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The Satisfact
Satisfaction From Helping Others Essay. The Satisfact
 
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society Report
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society ReportIPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society Report
IPR Leger 2023 Disinformation in Society Report
 
America_Divided_Guide-4
America_Divided_Guide-4America_Divided_Guide-4
America_Divided_Guide-4
 
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...
Essay Divorce. Narrative essay divorce. Attitudes Toward Marriage and Divorce...
 
PLAP 4150 Research Report
PLAP 4150 Research ReportPLAP 4150 Research Report
PLAP 4150 Research Report
 
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docx
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docxYour friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docx
Your friend remarked, A company will never drop a product from it.docx
 

DISTRUST-IN-AMERICAN-SOCIETY

  • 1. Distrust in American Society Alyssa Volkmann SOC 3930 Fall 2014
  • 2. 1 Introduction In a recent study by Morris Berman (2006), it has been found that distrust is largely increasing at a great cost to American society. Trust exists in all social interactions and social systems and can be defined as “a generalized expectancy held by an individual that the word, promise, oral or written statement of another individual or group can be relied on” (Smith, 2010; 455). As trust can be seen as positive expectations in regards to others actions, words and decisions, distrust is seen as negative expectations. Both trust and mistrust involve expectations; however, distrust is the expectation of things feared as opposed to trust as the hope of things hoped for (Lewicki et al, 1998). Although a general culture of trust is more likely to occur in democratic political systems like that of the United States, as our democracy is corrupted with inequality, political and social distrust is continuing to increase in American society (Sztompka, 1998; Lenard, 2010). This general culture of distrust is affecting the country’s overall social capital. As social capital decreases, society becomes less equipped to deal with important societal concerns such as crime, poverty and unemployment because social capital is a key instrument in dealing with these issues (Smith, 2010; Berman, 2006). As distrust continues to become more prevalent in society, it leads one to wonder what the causes of individuals and groups being distrustful are. The purpose of this study is to explore what causes people to become distrustful of other people and institutions in American society.
  • 3. 2 Literature Review Studying trust within groups has revealed that variables such as race, gender, education, socioeconomic status, political affiliation and income, determine degrees of trust allocated by individuals (Simpson et al, 2007; Binning, 2007). Individuals who experience inequality based on their identities, such as racial, class, and educational privilege, are more likely to be distrusting because they experience the negative sides of inequality, as opposed to the positive side of equality (Simpson et all, 2007). There is strong evidence that the lack of trust understood by minority groups in the United States, particularly African-Americans, is greater than that of white identified people (Smith, 2010). This distrust and lack of confidence African Americans feel towards institutions, such as the health care system, are deeply rooted in the historical health issues such as the Tuskegee Syphilis experiments and the AIDS outbreak, which have exploited and endangered the African American community (Schoff and Yang, 2012; Simmonds, 2008). In a survey conducted in 2006, 767 African Americans and 1267 White Americans were asked about discrimination and access to health care, as well as sociodemographic features and racial segregation in their residential areas. The results showed that more discrimination is experienced among African Americans than White Americans, resulting in higher amounts of distrust of the health care system (Armstrong et all, 2013). According to Schoff and Yang (2012), because African-Americans tend to distrust the health care system so much due to past experiences, it is also more common for them to distrust and lack confidence in other institutions in society as well. Several studies argue that the only way to increase the trust and confidence of minority groups toward institutions such as health care,
  • 4. 3 and government in general, is to eradicate racial discrimination, and work to create a culture of trust by continuing to create and maintain democratic institutions (Armstrong et all, 2013; Simmonds, 2008; Schoff and Yang, 2012; Sztompka, 1998). Though out-group trust needs to be repaired, according to Alberto Voci (2006), people are more likely to trust people who share similar group identities with them. Due to in-group trust being consistently stronger than out-group trust, it is likely that people are more likely to trust others who identify within the same racial group as them (Simpson et al, 2007). When examining African-Americans’ distrust of institutions in the United States such as the police force, it has been concluded that distrust stems from not only negative past interactions with the police, but also a race-gap within police force. Whites are much less likely to say that they distrust the police, and they do not share the same racial history with the police force, or the same racial disparities as African Americans do with the police (Sharp and Johnson, 2009). Due to the intersectionalities of inequality, race is not the only factor that causes distrust. When looking at the differences between racial groups, there are many intersections to take into account, such as differences in gender, income and education levels. These variances also impact individuals’ willingness to trust (Austin and Dodge, 1992; Simpson et all, 2007; Smith, 2010). White Americans largely attain higher levels of education and higher socioeconomic status than African Americans, and they report higher levels of general trust (Smith, 2010). Additionally, African-American women struggle with feelings of distrust and discontentment due to discrimination, expressing the highest amount of discontentment among Black and White woman and men, while Black men express the second highest
  • 5. 4 amount; Black men experiencing more discrimination and distrust than White women shows that gender is a less important factor of discrimination than race (Austin and Dodge, 1992). According to Lewicki et al (1998), to begin to reestablish trust among American citizens, particularly African Americans, social and political trust must begin to be restored for all people. Individuals and institutions must recognize the intersectionalities of identities that cause individuals to be more cautious and less trusting. As discrimination and marginalized identities are explored in the context of distrust, the following four hypotheses will be tested: H1: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, confidence in institutions will decrease; H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, trust will decrease; H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, closeness to that racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is not apart of; H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial in-group closeness will not be affected by sex.
  • 6. 5 Methods This study used data from General Social Survey (GSS) from 2012 with the program Statistical Program for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Distrust was measured by using several variables in this study; and an index was created with the following indicators: race, sex and class. Race is an element that impacts marginalized groups, as non-whites, specifically African-Americans and Latinos, experience racial discrimination, which leads to distrust (Smith, 2012). Sex will be understood as a factor that marginalizes the non-dominant sex (Austin and Dodge, 1992). Class is another variable, as historically it has impacted the accessibility to resources such as education, that continues to impact socioeconomic status of marginalized people today (Austin and Dodge, 1992). Together, these three variables will create a Marginalized Identity Index. The most marginalized identity within the index is poor, Black women while the least marginalized identity within the index is upper class, white men. For H1, I used the following GSS questions to create an index of confidence in American institutions: “I am going to name some institutions in this country. As far as the people running these institutions are concerned, would you say you have a great deal of confidence, only some confidence, or hardly any confidence at all in them? a. Banks and financial institutions (CONFINAN); b. Major companies (CONBUS); c. Organized religion (CONCLERG); d. Education (CONEDUC); e. Executive branch of the federal government (CONFED); g. Press (CONPRESS); h. Medicine (CONMEDIC).”
  • 7. 6 I created an Institution Confidence Index, using a-h of the above GSS question as indicators. The values of a-h are: A Great Deal, Only Some, Hardly Any. Scores ranging from 7-12 on the index will indicate a great deal of confidence in these institutions and are recoded as 1, scores ranging from 13-16 will indicate some confidence in institutions and are recoded as 2, and lastly, scores ranging from 17-21 will indicate hardly any confidence in these institutions and are recoded as 3. The listed values for the Institution Confidence Index are: Highly Confident, Somewhat Confident, Not At All Confident. This index was along with the marginalized identity index to test the relationship of marginalized identities and confidence in institutions. For H2, the following GSS questions was used to understand the impact of peoples’ own identities and the relationship these identities have with their aptitude to trust others: “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you can't be too careful in life? (TRUST)” and “Do you think most people would try to take advantage of you if they got a chance, or would they try to be fair? (FAIR).” The values for TRUST are: Can Trust, Cannot Trust, Depends. The values for FAIR are: Take Advantage, Fair, Depends. The marginalized identity index was used with this question to determine how marginalized identities impact individuals’ general trust in others. The index was also used to understand the relationship between marginalized identities and the belief that others are more likely to take advantage of them. Trust and the belief of fairness are inherently related because in a culture of trust, people would be less inclined to believe that others would take advantage of them. This data is crucial in understanding the relationship of personal identities and general trust.
  • 8. 7 For H3, instead of using the marginality index, race was used as the independent variable with the following GSS questions about closeness to particular racial groups: “In general, how close do you feel to blacks? (CLOSEBLK)” and “And in general, how close do you feel to whites? (CLOSEWHT).” These two questions are important to gauge the general sense of comfort, which is inherently linked to trust, among white and black racial groups. The values of CLOSEBLK and CLOSEWHT are: Very Close, Somewhat Close, Not Close At All. The relationship among in-group and out-group comfort can be understood by using race as the independent variable to see how the relationship of ones own racial identity impacts their closeness to their in and out racial group. The data from H3 was used as the original table. H4 was tested using race as the independent variable and CLOSEBLK and CLOSEWHT as the dependent variable with sex as the control variable. The purpose of this table was to determine whether or not sex impacts in-group and out-group closeness. Analysis Analyses of data were completed using cross tabulations, in addition to calculating the Chi-Square values and statistical significance of each table. Table 1 refers to testing for H1: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, confidence in institutions will decrease. There is no statistically significant relationship (P=0.622) between marginalized identities and confidence in institutions. The marginality index is a measurement of racial, gender and class inequality, which impacts how people view institutions. As race has served as a large influence of inequality, which is linked to distrust; Simpson et al also found that factors such as gender and education also have an influence
  • 9. 8 inequality (2007). While the marginality index measures race, sex and class, rather than education level, these are all indicators of degrees of inequality. Smith found that class is positively correlated with trust, as it impacts education. On average, minority groups are less wealthy and educated than whites; this inequality could account for a gap in trust (2010). Though the most marginalized group was the least confident within the “highly confident” category with a nearly 6% difference between the most and least marginalized identified people. However, overall the table is not a statistically significant. In short, this table did not show a significant relationship between marginalized identities and confidence in institutions, so H1 cannot be supported. Table 1: Institution Confidence by Marginalized Identity Index, Year 2012 Chi-Square=2.629, P=0.622 Table 2 tests H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, trust will decrease. This data is statistically significant (P=0.000) and shows a correlation between marginality and distrust. One interesting point about this question is its phrasing. Respondents are asked about their general trust in “most people,” and it has been found that regardless of the respondent’s race, the phrase “most people” is synonymous to “white people” (Simpson et all, 2007). Perhaps this has an influence on the results, as
  • 10. 9 respondents among least marginalized (48.8%) and somewhat marginalized (40.4%) group are significantly more trusting than those who are the most marginalized (18.5%). There is a 30% difference among belief in trusting most people between the most and least marginalized people. Another interesting aspect of this table is the relationship between the most marginalized people and distrust, as over 75% of the most marginalized people believe people cannot be trusted and over half of somewhat marginalized (56.7%) and most marginalized (76.7%) people believe people cannot be trusted, and only 45.5% of the least marginalized group believes most people cannot be trusted. Overall, as people identify as more marginalized, their sense of trust decreases; therefore, table 2 supports H2. Table 2: Trust Level by Marginality Index, Year 2012 Chi-Square=86.715, P=0.000 Table 3 also tests H2: As individuals express more characteristics of marginalized identities, trust will decrease. However, this table uses FAIR instead of trust. This relationship is also statistically significant (P=0.000) and shows a correlation between marginality and a belief of most people being fair or taking advantage of you. This question can be examined in terms of trust by understanding that a general belief in people taking advantage or being fair is linked to a culture of trust. As individuals believe that people are
  • 11. 10 fair, there is ‘a confident positive expectation in other’s conduct;’ while people who believe that others will take advantage of them, there is ‘a confident negative expectation in other’s conduct’ (Lewicki et all, 1998). This table shows that as people identify as being more marginalized, they have a higher belief that most people will take advantage and a lower belief that most people are fair. Over 59% of the least and somewhat marginalized groups believe that most people are fair, while only 39% of the most marginalized group believes the same. This question, like the previous, asks respondents again to answer if “most people” are fair or take advantage of. Again, it is important to be aware that “most people” usually leads people to think of white people, regardless of their own race (Smith et all, 2007). This conceivable dual meaning of “most people” for “white people” could potentially influence marginalized peoples’ belief of fairness, as the question could be interpreted in terms of how fair respondents believe white people to be. Like table 2, there is neither a huge difference nor a trend among those who chose “depends” across the marginality index. However, perhaps one could infer that the reason for the most marginalized people being the highest value of “depends” could attribute to their belief of fairness depending on what their definition of “most people.” In short, this table supports
  • 12. 11 H2; because there is a relationship between the most marginalized groups and a low belief in fairness and a high belief being taken advantage of. Table 3: Belief in Fairness by Marginality Index, Year 2012 Chi-Square=49.976, P=0.000 Table 4 tests H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, closeness to that racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is not apart of. This relationship is statistically significant (P=0.000) and there is a strong correlation between race and closeness to white people. 67.1% of white people are identified with being “very close” to other white people, this being the highest value in that row. Simpson et all found that trust is fundamentally stronger within racial groups, rather than among out-groups (2007). Trust is inherently related to closeness, as trust cannot be established among people without a general sense of closeness or comfort (Sztompka, 1998). Conversely, 41.9% of Black respondents identified as “somewhat close” to white people, meaning that there is about an 11% difference between Black and White people identifying as somewhat close to white people. One might infer that the high number of Black respondents identifying with being “somewhat close” to white people could be attributed to assimilating to the white majority. There is a very low percentage of white people who
  • 13. 12 identify as “not close at all” to other white people (2.2%), while there is a higher percent of Black respondents (8.1%) who identify as “not close at all” to white people. Table 4: Closeness to White People by Race, Year 2012 Chi-Square=31.791, P=0.000 Table 5 tests H3: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, closeness to that racial group will be greater than closeness to racial groups the individual is not apart of. This relationship is statistically significant (P=0.000) and like the previous table, shows a positive correlation between race and closeness to black people. 76.6% of Black respondents identify as “very close” to other Black people. This is significantly higher than the 67.1% of white people who identify as “very close” to other white people in table 4. Black respondents high percentage of in-group closeness (“very close”) and low percentage of in-group alienation (“not close at all”) could be attributed to “historical and contemporary experiences of discrimination, neighborhood and community context, and ethnoracial socialization” (Smith, 2011; 457). Less than half of white respondents (47%) identify as “somewhat close” to Black people, while 10.3% identify as “not close at all,” which is higher than the percent of Black respondents who identified as not close at all to white people. This could potentially be attributed to white people being the majority; therefore, they are less
  • 14. 13 likely to be put into social situations with black people unless they choose to be in said spaces, whereas black folks find themselves interacting with white people more regularly due to being a minority racial group in majority white society. In conclusion, both table 4 and table 5 supports H3, because they show that in-group closeness is stronger than in-group alienation or out-group closeness. Table 5: Closeness to Black People by Race, Year 2012 Chi-Square=76.053, P=0.000 Table 6 tests H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial in-group closeness will not be affected by sex. This table displays the relationship between closeness to white people by race and is controlled for gender, and the data is statistically significant (male P=0.005, female P=0.000) as well. Both white males (66.7%) and white females (67.3%) feel nearly equally close to other white people, while black males (8.3%) and black females (7.9%) feel almost equally “not close at all” to white people. This leads one to infer that sex is less of a factor of closeness than race. This is true because racial socialization is stronger and harder to disassociate from than gender socialization (Simpson et all, 2007). Another interesting aspect of this data is that it can be seen that black males feel generally closer to white people (58.3%) than black females do (43.9%). This could potentially be
  • 15. 14 attributed to living a patriarchal society, giving black men more privilege than black women, allowing for black men to connect with white people on the basis on male privilege in a patriarchal society. Though the out-group closeness to white people from black men and women change according to sex, the in-group closeness of white people is not affected by sex; therefore, H4 is supported. Table 6: Closeness White People by Race Controlled for Sex, Year 2012 MALE: Chi-Square=10.712, P=0.005, FEMALE: Chi-Square=25.823, P=0.000 Table 7 tests H4: As individuals share membership to a particular racial group, racial in-group closeness will not be affected by sex. The data is statistically significant (male P=0.000, female P=0.000) and shows the relationship between closeness to black people by race and is controlled for gender. Like table 6, there the closeness among men and women within their racial group does not change significantly because of gender. Fascinatingly, over 75% of black males and females feel very close to their racial group, which is higher than the percent of white people who felt “very” close to their racial group in table 6. As stated in the original table, a higher percent of white people feel “not close at all” to black people,
  • 16. 15 than black people who feel not close at all to white people. Conversely, less than 1% of black women identify as “not close at all” to other black folks, as opposed to 4.8% of black men identifying as “not close at all” to black people. As previously stated, this could potentially be due to the patriarchic society we live in, which gives men privilege and power over women, potentially making it easier for black men to branch out from their racial group. When looking at closeness between black and white people to black people, there is a less than 1% difference among closeness by race for each sex. In conclusion, table 7 supports H4 because there is no statistically significant difference between the sexes and their closeness within and outside their racial category. Table 7: Closeness Black People by Race Controlled for Sex, Year 2012 MALE: Chi-Square=30.903, P=0.000, FEMALE: Chi-Square=46.246, P=0.000 Conclusion In short, all hypotheses were supported with the exception of H1. There was not a statistically significant relationship between marginalized identities and confidence in
  • 17. 16 institutions; however, this could be due to measuring too many institutions in the institution confidence index. Perhaps if confidence in an institution known for being discriminatory, such as medical and police institution(s) were measured alone, a relationship between marginalized identities could be found. From previous research, it has been found that past experiences of discrimination cause higher levels of distrust; therefore, there must be effort to eliminate the discrimination experienced by groups who are marginalized due to their race, sex, class, education, and so on (Armstrong et all, 2013; Smith, 2010). Further research should be completed to find out the relationship of intersecting marginalized identities and confidence in particular institutions in America. Being a member of marginalized identity groups has a strong impact on both the general sense of trust in most people and the belief that most people are fair. The more marginalized ones identity is, the less trusting of most people they become. As a culture of distrust is created, cultural capital sacrificed (Sztompka, 1998). This loss of cultural capital manifests in the deterioration of relationships and trust among family, friends, and social structures and is harmful for society (Berman, 2006). This sense of distrust alienates people, as they learn to be skeptical of other groups within society. According to Simpson et all, the United States is separated by race more than any other identity and this was supported through the research (2007). Individuals identified as being closer to people within their racial group than outside their racial group. It was found that that sex does not have a strong influence on how close people feel to their racial group. Perhaps more research could be done using age as a factor of in-group racial closeness.
  • 18. 17 In closing, the marginalization of people in society, which is linked distrust, is causing a decline in social capital. Further research is needed to understand how to prevent discrimination and treat individuals of all identities equally in society. As long as the United States continues to marginalize certain racial, sexual, class, educational and class, there will be in and out-group separation. This alienation and distrust among groups within our society is harmful to the country’s social capital as a whole. Without a sense of trust and high social capital, the United States will continue to be ill equipped to deal with issues of inequality, which perpetuates the marginalization of certain identities (Berman, 2006). This leaves one with a question that needs extensive research, how can we begin to reverse this culture of distrust, which perpetuates inequality through marginalizing particular identities?
  • 19. 18 References Armstrong et all. 2013. “Prior Experiences of Racial Discrimination and Racial Differences in Health Care System Distrust.” Medical Care 51(2): 144-149. doi:10.1097/MLR.0b013e31827310a1. Austin, Roy L. and Hiroko Hayama Dodge. 1992. “Despair, Distrust and Dissatisfaction among Blacks and Women, 1973-1987.” The Sociological Quarterly 33(4): 579-598. (http://jstor.org/stable/4121397). Berman, Morris. 2006. Dark Ages America: The Final Phase of an Empire. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company. Binning, Kevin R. 2007. “It’s Us Against the World: How Distrust in Americans versus People-In-General Shapes Competitive Foreign Policy Preferences.” 2007. Political Psychology 28(6): 777-799. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221 Lenard, Patti Tamara. 2010. “Rebuilding Trust in an Era of Widening Wealth Inequality.” Journal of Social Philosophy 41(1): 73-91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9833.2009.01479. Lewicki et all. 1998. “Trust and Distrust: New Relationships and Realities.” The Academy of Management Review 23(3): 438-458 (http://jstor.org/stable/259288). Schoff, Carla and Tse-Chuan Yang. 2012. “Untangling the associations among distrust, race, and neighborhood social environment: A social disorganization perspective.” Social Science and Medicine 74: 1342-1352. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.01.012. Sharp, Elaine B. and Paul E. Johnson. 2009. "Accounting for Variation in Distrust of Local Police." Justice Quarterly 26(1):157-182 (http://search.proquest.com/docview/61782576?accountid=28109). Simmonds, Gwenneth, M.S.N., C.N.M. 2008. "African American Participation in Public Health Research." ABNF Journal 19(2):69-72 (http://search.proquest.com/docview/218900418?accountid=28109). Simpson, Brent et all. 2007. “Are Blacks Really Less Trusting Than Whites? Revisiting the Race and Trust Question.” Oxford University Press 86(2): 525-552. (http://jstor.org/stable/20430752). Smith, Sandra Susan. 2010. “Race and Trust.” Annual Review of Sociology 36:453-475. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102526. Sztompka, Piotr. 1998. “Trust, Distrust and Two Paradoxes of Democracy.” European Journal of Social Theory 1(1): 19-32. doi: 10.1177/136843198001001003.
  • 20. 19 Voci, Alberto. 2006. “The link between identification and in-group favouritism: Effects of threat to social identity and trust-related emotions.” British Journal of Social Psychology 45: 265-284. doi:10.1348/014466605X52245.