SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 37
Download to read offline
1
One must know when to stop: work stressors and individual
differences as predictors of psychological detachment from
work during short respite periods.
Goldsmiths, University of London
Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of an MSc in Occupational Psychology.
Student ID number: 33270657
Supervisor: Dr Jo Lloyd
Date: 16th September 2013
Word count: 8,604
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank my supervisor Dr Jo Lloyd for her patience, advice and support. She inspired,
encouraged, reassured and guided me throughout researching and writing the MSc dissertation. Thank
you for believing and motivating me to reach my potential.
Besides my supervisor I would like to thank my friends and family for having the confidence in me
throughout my MSc degree.
Thank you.
Executive Summary
1.1.Subject matter.
The aim of the current study was to investigate whether work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity
and individual differences: psychological flexibility (i.e. whether an individual has the ability to stay
focused on the present moment) and locus of control (i.e. whether an individual believes that they have
a direct control over their life) predict or hinder psychological detachment from work during short respite
periods (i.e. evenings). In relation to work stressors, it was proposed that individuals who experience
high levels of job demands and role ambiguity will also find it difficult to psychologically unwind from
work-related issues. In addition to individual differences, it was proposed that individuals who will report
high levels of psychological flexibility and strong believe that they have control over the environment
around them will also find it easier to mentally detach from work during off-job time.
1.2.Methods of analysis and findings.
The first step was to prepare the data by replacing participants’ responses with numerical equivalents,
investigating for missing values, reverse scored items of the scales where appropriate, and add all
scores for each factor. This was followed by analysing and exploring the data.
The results suggested that the psychological detachment, job demand, role ambiguity, psychological
flexibility and locus of control scales were consistent and reliable.
Analysis, which looked at the relationship between psychological detachment, job demands, role
ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control, suggested that individuals who reported high
levels of job demand and role ambiguity also showed low levels of psychological detachment from work
during non-work times. Furthermore, the results showed that individuals who showed high levels of job
demands also reported to have ambiguous roles. Moreover, employees who have ambiguous roles
also reported that they find it difficult to remain focused on the present moment and tend to believe that
they do not have direct control over environment around them. The results also implied that individuals
who are able to remain focused on the present moment also reported to have strong believe that they
control the environment around them.
However, further explorations and analysis of the data that looked at which factors predict
psychological detachment from work during non-work time, showed that only job demands emerged as
a strong predictor of psychological detachment.
1.3.Strengths and limitations of the current study.
There are a number of strengths of the study. Psychological detachment from work during non-work
time is a new phenomenon. In relation to individual differences, the current study looked at
psychological flexibility and locus of control, which have important implications for both individuals and
organisations, as these are factors that can be improved. However, previous studies focused on
individual differences such as personality, which is fixed and cannot be enhanced. Another, strength of
the study lies in having participants from a wide range of occupations, which suggests that the findings
can be generalized to the group of working adults, rather than only a specific group.
Having said that, the study is not without limitations. It can be concluded that, when employees
experience high levels of job demand their psychological detachment from work during off-job time
decreases. However, it cannot be said that low psychological detachment is caused by high job
demands. Another limitation of the study is using self-report measures. Thus, it cannot be stated
whether high job demands or individuals’ perception of high job demands is associated with low
psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
1.4.Recommendations.
There are a number of aspects for future research to consider. It is recommended that factors related to
employees’ personal life be further investigated, as these may distract employees and help them to
mentally detach from work related issues during short respite periods. Furthermore, future research
should re-examine psychological flexibility and locus of control in relation to psychological detachment
and investigate whether individual differences moderate the relationship between high job demands
and psychological detachment.
Job demand is an important factor in relation to the ability to psychologically unwind from work during
non-work time. Organisations should implement effective interventions and teach employees time
management skills so that they can organise their time for work tasks in a more efficient manner. This
will help employees to complete their duties more efficiently at work and detach mentally from work
related issues at home.
Abstract
This study used a cross-sectional design and examined the multivariate relationship between
psychological detachment and job stressors (job demands and role ambiguity) and individual
differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control). The objective was to investigate which of
these factors support or hinder psychological detachment from work during non-work time. In total, 202
working adults from a variety of professions took part in the study by completing a self-report online
survey. Regression analysis revealed that high job demand was the only significant predictor of low
psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Furthermore, the remaining job stressor,
role ambiguity, was a non-significant predictor of psychological detachment. Additionally, both of the
individual difference variables, psychological flexibility and locus of control, were non-significant
predictors of psychological detachment.
Contents
1.Introduction. ...............................................................................................................................................1
1.1.Psychological detachment from work during non-work time..................................................................1
1.2.The current study. .................................................................................................................................3
1.3.Predictors of psychological detachment: work stressors and individual differences..............................4
1.3.1.Work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity. ...........................................................................4
1.3.1.1.Job demands and psychological detachment from work during non-work time...........................4
1.3.1.2.Role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. .......................6
1.3.2.Individual differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control................................................7
1.3.2.1.Psychological flexibility................................................................................................................7
1.3.2.2.Locus of control...........................................................................................................................8
2.Method.......................................................................................................................................................10
2.1.Design.................................................................................................................................................10
2.2.Participants. ........................................................................................................................................10
2.3.Measures. ...........................................................................................................................................11
2.3.1.Demographics..............................................................................................................................11
2.3.2.Criterion variable measure...........................................................................................................11
2.3.2.1.Psychological detachment measure..........................................................................................11
2.3.3.Predictor variables measures.......................................................................................................12
2.3.3.1.Job demands measure..............................................................................................................12
2.3.3.2.Role ambiguity measure............................................................................................................12
2.3.3.3.Psychological flexibility measure...............................................................................................13
2.3.3.4.Locus of control measure..........................................................................................................13
2.4.Procedure............................................................................................................................................13
2.5.Ethics. .................................................................................................................................................14
3.Results ......................................................................................................................................................16
3.1.Data cleaning and screening...............................................................................................................16
3.2.Descriptive Statistics. ..........................................................................................................................16
3.3.Pearson’s correlation coefficient. ........................................................................................................17
3.4.Multiple Regression.............................................................................................................................18
3.4.1.Collinearity diagnostics. ...............................................................................................................18
3.5.Conclusion. .........................................................................................................................................19
4.Discussion................................................................................................................................................20
4.1.Job demand and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. ...................................20
4.2.Non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work during non-work time...................21
4.3.Additional significant results................................................................................................................22
4.4.Strengths of the current study. ............................................................................................................22
4.5.Limitations of the current study. ..........................................................................................................23
4.6.Recommendations for future research. ...............................................................................................23
4.7.Implications of the current study..........................................................................................................24
4.8.Conclusion. .........................................................................................................................................24
5.References................................................................................................................................................26
6.Appendices...............................................................................................................................................30
6.1.The online survey................................................................................................................................30
1
1. Introduction.
In recent years, there has been a growing body of literature surrounding the fairly new phenomenon of
psychological detachment (White, 2010). In today’s fast-paced, technologically developed, 24/7
economy, employees are largely still accessible to their managers while physically away from work.
Research indicates that this has resulted in a trend towards employees continuously facing work
demands, even during their non-work time (Etzion, Eden and Lapidot, 1998). As these demands are the
main sources of everyday stress, it is important that individuals detach from work, both physically and
psychologically, in order to keep stress levels at a manageable level (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005).
However, living in a technologically advanced society makes psychological detachment from work
during non-work time more difficult (Sonnentag, 2012). The current study seeks to explore some of the
variables that hinder or support psychological detachment.
1.1.Psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
Etzion, Eden and Lapidot (1998) define detachment as individuals being physically away from their
work situation and routine. They proposed that detachment from work becomes harder due to
developing technologies, which make , employees increasingly accessible to their managers as they
can be contacted and receive work related emails or phone calls even when they are physically away
from work. Furthermore, Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) emphasised the importance of not only being
physically detached from work, but also being mentally detached. Thus, the researchers highlighted the
psychological component in disengaging from the work environment as not thinking about work, work
related problems and activities during off-job time. In order for psychological detachment to occur, an
individual needs to physically leave the workplace, as well as refrain from reflecting on work-related
issues and engaging in work-related duties at home.
A study by Sonnentag, Kuttler and Fritz (2010) has demonstrated that employees who are mentally
detached from work-related duties during time off work reported higher levels of psychological well-
being, higher satisfaction with their life, experienced less emotional exhaustion, and showed fewer
symptoms of psychological strain (i.e. problems with sleep); whilst still remaining engaged at work
(Sonnentag, 2012). In a longitudinal study conducted by Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza (2010),
psychological detachment was found to be linked to changes in employees’ well-being over time.
2
Individuals who continued to be mentally preoccupied with work-related duties during off-hours reported
increased emotional exhaustion a year later. Moreover, employees who did not mentally disengage
from work during off-job time showed decreased energy levels that further add to burnout. Further
benefits of psychological detachment from work during time off work have been reported in a study that
looked at sabbatical leave. The results indicated that sabbatical leave decreased stress and burnout
and increased levels of life satisfaction and positive affect among individuals who psychologically
detached from work during their time off (Davidson, Eden, Westman, Cohen-Charash, Hammer, Kluger,
Krausz, Maslach, O’Driscoll, Perrewé, Quick, Rosenblatt and Spector, 2010).
Psychological detachment has also been found to have detrimental effects on individuals’ everyday
well-being. This has been illustrated in a study conducted by Sonnentag and Bayer (2005). The
researchers gathered questionnaires and daily survey measures over the course of three working days.
The results implied that employees who were psychologically detached from work during evening hours
experienced good moods and lower fatigue. In addition, Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza (2008)
demonstrated that psychological detachment from work during time after work is a powerful and
effective recovery experience. Over the course of one week, participants completed daily survey
measures. The researchers found that higher levels of fatigue and high negative activation the next
morning were reported amongst the individuals who did not psychologically detach from work in the
evening. Rothbard and Wilk (2011) argued for the importance of looking at employees’ mood at the
beginning of their working day. The researchers found that starting their working day in a good or a bad
mood had a significant influence on how employees perceived work events, how they related to these
events, and how well they performed during the day.
In order to maintain well-being at the workplace, it is essential for employees to have respite periods
away from work (Eden, 2001 cited in Sonnentag, 2003). Etzion, et al. (1998) looked at psychological
detachment in relation to longer respite periods, lasting two weeks or longer. The results from the
research indicated that along with mastery, control and relaxation, psychological detachment is one of
the main factors that lead to improved well-being when employees returned to work. Moreover,
Brosschot, Gerrin and Thayer (2006) argued that psychological detachment is the most important factor
for enhancing employees’ psychological well-being due to being the only recovery process that involves
mentally disengaging from work related duties (White, 2010). According to Westman and Eden (1997)
these beneficial effects of long respite periods became weaker three weeks after employees returned to
the workplace (Etzion, et al., 1998). In addition, Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) emphasised the
importance of researching psychological detachment from work in relation to short intervals.
Furthermore, based on research evidence Sonnentag (2003) reported that short respite periods such
3
as evening hours are also beneficial for employees’ well-being. Thus, daily recovery processes are
effective in supplementing the effects of long respite periods; indicating that employees do not need to
wait for a vacation in order to recover from work related stress.
1.2.The current study.
The current study aimed to predict the multivariate relationship between psychological detachment and
four predictor variables, including job stressors: job demand and role ambiguity, and individual
differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control, with the purpose of finding which of them
hinder or support psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings).
Living in a fast-paced 24/7 economy, employees are continuously facing demands of work, which are
the main source of everyday stress. This makes it difficult to psychologically detach from work during
non-work time (Sonnentag, 2012). The current study looks at psychological detachment as it has been
reported to have detrimental effects on individuals’ everyday well-being (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005;
Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2008). Psychological detachment will be considered in relation to
short respite periods (i.e. evenings), because, as reported by Westman and Eden (1997), the effects of
longer breaks, such as vacations, become weaker three weeks after employees return to work (Etzion,
Eden and Lapidot, 1998).
Therefore, it is useful to investigate which individual differences hinder or support psychological
detachment. The individual differences that have been chosen for investigation in this study are
psychological flexibility and locus of control. These individual differences are of particular interest, as
unlike personality, for example, psychological flexibility and locus of control can be enhanced, which
may lead to improved psychological detachment from work during off-work time.
Additionally, the study looks at job stressors in relation to psychological detachment from work during
non-work time. The work stressors chosen include job demands and role ambiguity. The importance of
investigating these stressors is twofold. Firstly, according to Zohar, Tzischinski and Epstein (2003),
employees who are challenged with job stressors have a greater need for recovery, which is the result
of using more effort and regulating emotions in order to face these job stressors (Sonnentag, Kuttler
and Fritz, 2010). Dealing with work stressors at the workplace will make it difficult for employees to
mentally detach from work at home, and consequently increases the need for recovery. Secondly,
understanding the job stressors that influence psychological detachment will have important
4
implications in the workplace, in terms of trying to minimise these and increase the well-being of
employees (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005).
1.3.Predictors of psychological detachment: work stressors and individual differences.
1.3.1. Work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity.
1.3.1.1. Job demands and psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
In relation to Karasek’s job control-demand model, the current study refers to job demands as a high
amount of complex work that an employee needs to accomplish, and dealing with incompatible
demands from others (Lavoie-Tremblay, Wright, Desforges, Gélinas Marchionni and Drevniok, 2008).
Occupational stress has a significant impact on organisations and employees’ physical and mental
health (Lu, 1999). In addition, Ganster, Fox and Dweyer (2001) argued that it is well established in the
scientific literature that job demands are one of the main factors that negatively affect health and well-
being (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). Geurts, Kompier, Roxburgh and Houtman (2003) suggested that
employees who experience a high workload will also report poor health and well-being as it limits their
opportunity for recovery (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). Moreover, in a 4-wave panel study including
668 employees, a significant relationship between high job demands and impaired mental health over
time was found (De Lange, Taris, Kompier, Houtman and Bongers, 2004).
McEwen (1998) argued that when individuals are exposed to high job demands, their physiological and
psychological systems become activated with the purpose of gathering the necessary energy to face
the job demands (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010). Thus, Meijman and Mulder (1998)
proposed that an individual’s physiological and psychological systems will neutralize during even a
short respite period, during which the individual can recuperate (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza,
2010). However, McEwen (1998) suggested that recovery will not occur when, during respite periods,
individuals are not exposed to job demands, yet, their physiological and psychological systems remain
activated. Moreover, it has been argued that experiencing high strain levels whilst away from job
demands could facilitate and lead to long term health issues (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010).
This implies that it is not sufficient for employees to be physically away from their work environment and
exposure to work-related demands. It is also essential for employees to be psychologically detached
from job demands during non-work time in order to reduce the high strain levels (Sonnentag, Binnewies
and Mojza, 2010). In addition, Brosschot, Pieper and Thayer (2005) argued that cognitive mechanisms
play a major role in physiological concomitants. Thus, the researchers argued that stressors do not lead
5
directly to prolonged activation. The constant and repeated activation of cognitive representations of
stress (i.e. preservative cognition) is a mediator between stressors (e.g. job demand) and prolonged
activation, which in turn will lead to maintained high strain levels. In respect to that, Sonnentag,
Binnewies and Mojza (2010) suggested that high strain levels caused by prolonged activation will
remain only when employees experience high job demands and lack of psychological detachment
during non-work hours.
Job demands have also been found to play an important role in psychological detachment from work
during non-work time. It has been established that employees who experience a high workload find it
difficult to mentally detach from work-related duties during off-work time, with work demands commonly
spilling over from work to home life (Sonnentag, Kuttler and Fritz, 2010). In a longitudinal experience-
sampling study, 106 working adults completed daily surveys: two at work, and one during the evening
at home. Their relatives were also interviewed every day over the phone. The results indicated that high
workload predicted negative affect at work. Furthermore, the researchers predicted that high workload
also extended and affected family life as it was associated with negative affect at home (Ilies, Schwind,
Wagner, Johnson, DeRue and Ilgen, 2007). Brosschot, Gerin and Thayer (2006) argued that
consequently, employees’ psychological and physiological systems will be activated and as a result
psychological detachment from work during non-work time will not occur (Sonnentag, et al., 2010). In
addition, Cropley and Millward Purvis (2003) conducted a diary study of school teachers, who made
hourly notes of their work-related thoughts over a work-day evening between 5 p.m. to 9p.m.
Individuals who reported high workload also showed low psychological detachment during the evenings
from work-related duties (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). The same finding emerged from a study
conducted by Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) based on daily survey measures during three working days.
The researchers reported that workload was negatively related to psychological detachment from work
during off-job time. A study by Sonnentag and Kruel (2006) revealed similar results, indicating that
employees who had a high workload demonstrated a low ability to mentally disengage from work during
non-work time, and continued to reflect on job-related issues in the evening.
In light of the previous research in the area, this study aims to further investigate the relationship
between job demands and psychological detachment during non-work time.
Hypothesis I: Individuals who experience high job demands will report low levels of psychological
detachment from work during non-work time.
6
1.3.1.2. Role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
In relation to organisational context, role ambiguity is defined as a lack of clear information about a role
and the role expectations (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). Thus, employees will experience role ambiguity
when they do not possess necessary information about what their work objectives are and how to
approach their work tasks, and when there is ambiguity about the priority of work that needs to be
accomplished.
Role ambiguity is a job stressor that has been studied extensively in the scientific literature, and
according to the empirical research, role ambiguity has detrimental effects on both employees and
organisations (Siegall, 2000). In addition, a sample of 15.256 men, who had no previous history of
mental disorders completed a self-reported survey of a number of different work stressors such as: role
ambiguity, workload and social support. During 5 years of follow up, the researchers kept a record of
participants’ who took sick leaves due to depressive disorders, which lasted 30 days or longer. The
results suggested that employees who experienced high levels of role ambiguity developed depressive
disorders and as a result they took 30 or more days of sick leave (Inoue, Kawakami, Haratani,
Kobayashi, Ishizaki, Hayashi, Fujota, Aizawa, Miyazaki, Hiro, Masumoto, Hashimoto and Araki, 2010).
However, not many studies have tested role ambiguity in relation to psychological detachment. The
studies that have looked at the correlation between role ambiguity and psychological detachment from
work during non-work time have indicated non-significant relationships between these variables
(Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006; Sonnentag and Fritz, 2007). When an individual experiences high role
ambiguity, they are more likely to be mentally preoccupied about it during time off work as they will lack
a clear understanding of what is expected of them and how to prioritise their job. As a result, they will
not be able to psychologically detach from work during respite periods. On the other hand, individuals
with low role ambiguity will have a clear understanding of their work objectives and roles and as a result
they may find it easier to mentally disengage from work after work hours (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006).
The current study will seek to add to the existing literature by investigating the relationship between role
ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
Hypothesis II: Individuals who experience high role ambiguity at work will report low levels of
psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
7
1.3.2. Individual differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control.
1.3.2.1. Psychological flexibility.
One of the individual difference variables included in the current study is psychological flexibility.
According to Hayes, Strosahl, Bunting, Twohig and Wilson (2004), Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy (ACT) is used in order to promote workplace mental health, and is based on mindfulness
strategies that have emerged from the Cognitive Behavioural Therapy movement. ACT promotes six
principles: acceptance, defusion, contact with the present moment, self-as-context, values, and
commitment action. These principles are interrelated and help to enhance psychological flexibility.
Psychological flexibility refers to the ability to remain focused on the present moment; being mindful
and aware of thoughts and feelings and yet having the ability to pursue goals and take actions
(Flaxman and Bond, 2010). It has been suggested that people who score high on psychological
flexibility will be able to psychologically detach themselves from work during time off work as they will
be able to focus on the present moment.
Psychological flexibility has been found to act as a mediator through which Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy can improve work-related stress. Fledderus, Bohlemijer and Pieterse (2010)
reported that psychological flexibility acts as a mediator in the relationship between coping styles and
emotional and psychological well-being (Fledderus, Bohlmeijer, Smit and Westerhof, 2010). In a study
conducted by Flaxman and Bond (2010), outcomes and processes of changes had been observed
across a three month period in working adults with above average levels of stress, who participated in
an ACT intervention. The researchers found that participants’ levels of psychological distress
decreased following the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy over the assessment period.
Furthermore, mediation effect analysis indicated that ACT was an effective intervention for mental
health improvement as a direct result of an increase in psychological flexibility. This is supported by
Lappalainen, Lehtonen, Skarp, Taubert, Ojanen and Hayes (2007), who found that client’s levels of
psychological flexibility elevated following Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Flaxman and Bond,
2010).
Enhanced psychological flexibility is also related to many health benefits. Bond and Bunce (2000)
conducted a study in which they compared a group of individuals who had been through an ACT
intervention with a control group, and a group of individuals who received training on how to reduce
stressors at their source. The researchers reported that general mental health was only improved for
the employees that participated in the brief ACT intervention. Furthermore, Bond and Bunce (2003)
proposed that individuals who report high levels of psychological flexibility experience better mental
8
health over time (Hayes and Strosahl, 2004). In an empirical literature review it has been concluded
that individuals who reported high psychological flexibility also reported fewer psychological problems
(Chawla and Ostafin, 2007). Moreover, Bond and Bunce (2003) found that psychological flexibility is
also related to enhanced quality of life, emotional well-being, and job satisfaction (Fledderus,
Bohlmeijer, Smit and Westerhof, 2010).
The current study suggests that psychological flexibility is an important element in allowing individuals
to be able to psychologically detach from work during off-work time. One of the main features of
psychological flexibility is having contact with the present moment. In addition, psychological flexibility
relates to being able to shift focus from one life domain (e.g. work) to another area of life (e.g. personal
life (Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010). Therefore, individuals who are not able to have contact with the
present moment (i.e. score low in psychological flexibility) also might be unable to psychologically
detach from work during off-work time. Psychological flexibility can be enhanced through Acceptance
and Commitment Therapy (Fledderus, 2010). This indicates that individuals’ psychological detachment
from work also could be enhanced.
In addition to previous research, the current study will seek to investigate the relationship between
psychological flexibility and detachment from work during non-work time.
Hypothesis III: Individuals who report high levels of psychological flexibility will also report high levels of
psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
1.3.2.2. Locus of control.
Rotter (1966) distinguished that individuals have either an internal or external locus of control.
Individuals with an internal locus of control try to control the environment around them in a confident,
alert and directive manner as they hold a strong belief that they have control over their life. Individuals
with external locus of control, on the contrary, believe that they do not have direct control over their life
and attribute outcomes to powerful others, fate, or luck (Ng, Sorensen and Eby, 2006).
Locus of control is a personality variable that has been studied at length in many different settings
(Spector, 1988). Judge and Bono (2001) argued that locus of control is a fundamental factor at work as
it is associated with numerous important work outcomes such as job satisfaction and job performance
(Ng, Sorensen and Eby, 2006). In addition, empirical research has shown that individuals who hold an
internal locus of control also display higher levels of physiological and psychological well-being.
9
Furthermore, internals manage both everyday stress and stress at the workplace better than externals
do (Spector, Cooper, Sanchez, O'Driscoll, Sparks, Bernin, Bussing, Dewe, Hart, Lu, Miller, De Moraes,
Ostrognay, Pagon, Pitariu, Poelmans, Radhakrishnan, Russinova, Salamatov, Salgado, Shima, Siu,
Stora, Teichmann, Theorell, Vlerick, Westman, Widerszal-Bazyl, Wong and Yu, 2002). In a recent
meta-analysis, researchers looked at the relationship between locus of control and a number of
different work outcomes. The results implied that locus of control was an important aspect related to
well-being and job-related affective reactions (Meier, Semmer, Elfering and Jacobshagen, 2008).
Kirkcaldy, Shephard and Furnham (2002) administered the pressure management indicator (PMI, which
measured job satisfaction, job stressors, physical and mental health, in addition to Type A behaviour
and locus of control) to a sample of 332 managers. The researchers reported that individuals with a
high internal locus of control perceive lower levels of stress and higher levels of health than individuals
with a high external locus of control.
In addition to the findings in the scientific literature, the current study proposes that individuals who
have high internal control beliefs (believe that they can control what is happening at work) will be able
to psychologically detach from work during off-work time. On the contrary, individuals who have high
external beliefs will be unable to psychologically detach from work during off-work time due to having
beliefs that things are beyond their control. They may also worry more and think about work during
respite periods.
In light of the previous literature discussed, the current study will investigate the relationship between
locus of control and psychological detachment.
Hypothesis IV: Individuals who report an internal locus of control will also report high levels of
psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
10
2. Method
2.1.Design.
This study made use of a cross-sectional design, with four predictor variables and one criterion
variable. The criterion variable in the study was psychological detachment and predictor variables
included: job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control. The data collection
procedure was standardized by providing all participants with the same instructions in relation to
responding to the survey. The data was analysed using correlation coefficient and multiple linear
regression analyses.
2.2.Participants.
The sampling method used in the study was opportunity and snowball sampling. The survey was
distributed via email to friends and family of the researcher, who farther distributed the survey to their
work colleagues, friends and family. Their participation in the study was voluntary. A total of 202
working adults participated in the study, of whom 71 (35.1%) were males and 131 (64.9%) were
females. One-hundred-and-ninety-four participants provided their age, which ranged from 18 years old
to 62 years old. The mean age was 34.83 and the standard deviation was 8.54. In terms of marital
status, 90 (44.6%) participants were single, divorced/ separated or widowed, whereas 111 (55%)
participants were in a relationship or married, and one person (0.5%) did not provide information about
their marital status. Fifty-eight (28.7%) participants reported having children, whereas 144 (71.3%)
stated that they do not have children. Among the 58 participants who have children, one (0.5%) did not
specify how many children they have, 21 (10.4%) participants reported having 1 child, 25 (12.4%) have
2 children, 6 (3%) participants have 3 children, 4 (2%) respondents have 4 children and one person
(0.5%) has 7 children. In relation to ethnicity, the vast majority of participants, 168 (83.2%) were White,
13 (6.4%) participants were Black or African American, 5 (2.5%) participants were Hispanic or Latino, 6
(3%) were Asian/Pacific Islanders. The remaining 10 (5%) of respondents were of other ethnicity.
Out of the people that participated in this study, 150 (74.3%) were full-time and 52 (325.7%) were part-
time working adults. Two participants (1%) had completed GCSE/ O’level as their highest level of
education, 8 (4%) completed A’level, 29 (14.4%) had a diploma, 59 (29.2%) had a bachelor’s degree,
80 (39.6%) had a master’s degree, 10 (5%) had a doctoral degree and remaining 14 (6.9%)
11
respondents had other qualifications. The participants of this study were working in various professions
such as psychologists, midwives, teachers/ lecturers, graphic designers, sales, catering, IT, and
medics.
2.3.Measures.
The online questionnaire (refer to appendix 6.1) consisted of six sections gathering information about
participants’ demographic characteristics and investigating the criterion variable (psychological
detachment) and predictor variables (job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of
control).
2.3.1. Demographics.
The first section consisted of 12 items regarding demographic characteristics. The participants were
asked to provide the following information about themselves: age in years, gender, ethnicity, marital
status, whether they have children, how many children they have, their highest level of educational
attainment , working regime, how many hours they work in a typical week, and their occupation.
2.3.2. Criterion variable measure.
2.3.2.1. Psychological detachment measure.
The second section was a subscale taken from The Recovery Experience Questionnaire by Sonnentag
and Fritz (2007), which consisted of four items: ‘I forget about work.’, ‘I don’t think about work.’, ‘I
distance myself from work.’, ‘I get a break from the demands of work.’ Participants were asked to
respond to the items in relation to their free evenings (as the study looked at short respite periods) and
indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with the statements. The items were rated on a 5-
point Likert scale as follows: I do not agree at all, I somewhat disagree, I neither agree nor disagree, I
somewhat agree, and I fully agree. In addition, the statement ‘I do not agree at all’ was scored as 1 and
the statement ‘I fully agree’ was scored as 5. Thus, the possible range of scores was from 4 to 20,
where low scores indicated low psychological detachment and high scores indicated high psychological
detachment. The scale is a self-report measure, which has been widely used by researchers in the field
12
of psychological detachment. The scale is consistent and reliable as indicated by high Cronbach’s α (α
= .855).
2.3.3. Predictor variables measures.
2.3.3.1. Job demands measure.
The third section was a subscale taken from Karasek’s (1985) Job Demand and Control Questionnaire,
which consisted of seven items. Sample items included: ‘My job requires working very fast.’, ‘My job
requires working very hard.’ and ‘I am not asked to do an excessive amount of work.’ Participants were
asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with the statements. The items were
rated on a 4-point Likert scale as follows: Strongly Agree, Agree, Disagree, and Strongly Disagree. In
addition, the statement ‘Strongly Agree’ was scored as 1, and the statement ‘Strongly Disagree’ was
scored as 4. Four items- 1, 2, 6 and 7- were reverse scored. Thus, the possible range of scores was
from 7 to 28. The scale is a self-report measure with high Cronbach’s α (α = .820), suggesting that the
scale was consistent and reliable.
2.3.3.2. Role ambiguity measure.
The fourth section included a work ambiguity subscale taken from the ‘Role conflict and ambiguity in
complex organizations’ by Rizzo, House and Lirtzman (1970), which consisted of 10 items. The sample
questions were: ‘I feel certain about how much authority I have.’, ‘I am able to act the same regardless
of the group I am with.’, ‘I feel certain how I will be evaluated for a raise or promotion.’ Respondents
were asked to rate how true each statement is for them. The items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale
as follow: Very false, Somewhat true, Slightly false, Neither false or true, Slightly true, Somewhat true
and Very true. In addition, the statement ‘Very false’ was scored as 1, and the statement ‘Very true’
was scored as 7. Thus, the possible range of scores was from 10 to 70. All 10 items were reverse
scored in order for low scores to indicate low levels of role ambiguity and high scores to represent high
levels of role ambiguity. The scale is a self-report measure, which is consistent and reliable as indicated
by high Cronbach’s α (α = .821).
13
2.3.3.3. Psychological flexibility measure.
The fifth section included the Work-related Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (WAAQ) by Bond,
Lloyd and Guenole (in press), which measured psychological flexibility. The scale consisted of 7 items
such as: ‘I am able to work effectively in spite of any personal worries that I have.’, ‘I can admit to my
mistakes at work and still be successful.’, ‘I can still work very effectively, even if I am nervous about
something.’ Participants were asked to rate how true each statement is for them. The items were rated
on a 7-point Likert scale as follow: Never true, Very seldom true, Seldom true, Sometimes true,
Frequently true, Almost always true and Always true. The statement ‘Never true’ was scored as 1, and
‘Always true’ was scored as 7. The possible range of scores was from 7 to 49, where low scores
indicated low levels of psychological flexibility and high scores represented high levels of psychological
flexibility. Cronbach’s α was very high (α = .911), suggesting that the scale was consistent and reliable.
2.3.3.4. Locus of control measure.
The sixth and final section consisted of the Development of the Work Locus of Control Scale by Spector
(1988), which included 16 items such as: ‘If employees are unhappy with a decision made by their
boss, they should do something about it.’, ‘Getting the job you want is mostly a matter of luck.’, ‘Making
money is primarily a matter of good fortune.’ Respondents were asked to indicate the extent to which
they agree or disagree with the statements. The items were rated on a 6-point Likert scale: Disagree
very much, Disagree moderately, Disagree slightly Agree slightly, Agree moderately and Agree very
much. The statement ‘Disagree very much’ was scored as 1, and ‘Agree very much’ was scored as 6.
As indicated by Spector (1988), eight items- 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, 11, 14 and 15- were reverse scored. The
possible range of scores was from 16 to 96, where low scores indicated internal locus of control and
high scores indicated external locus of control. The scale is a self-report measure, which is consistent
and reliable as indicated by high Cronbach’s α (α = .822).
2.4.Procedure.
The data was collected from participants using an online questionnaire (refer to appendix 6.1) created
on SurveyMonkey. The researcher emailed a brief description of the study with a link, which allowed
participants to access the questionnaire. The survey took approximately 10 minutes to complete and
14
participants were asked to distribute the link further to anyone who is 18 years old and over and
currently employed.
Once participants opened the link, they were presented with an introduction and briefing about the
study, which included information about the researcher and research supervisor, the rights of the
participant, and how to contact the researcher. The next page required participants to consent to take
part in the study and declare that they have read and understood the purpose of the study. Without
consent, participants would be unable to proceed to the study. Respondents were also asked to write
an anonymity number, which would enable the researcher to identify their responses if they wished to
withdraw from the study. Once giving their consent, participants could proceed with completing the
questionnaire. Before submitting their responses, participants were provided with a debriefing page,
which provided more detailed information about the aim of the study, references to studies that looked
at psychological detachment, and contact details if the participants had queries or if they wished to
receive results from the study.
The collected data was transferred to Excel format and then into SPSS and appropriate changes were
made such as coding the data, reversing values and computing scores of each variable. The data was
analysed using correlation coefficient and multiple linear regression analyses.
2.5.Ethics.
Before the researcher was allowed to proceed with the study, the ‘Psychology Department Ethical
Approval Form’ had to be completed and submitted for approval. The Departmental Ethical Committee
at the Institute of Management Studies at the Goldsmith’s University of London granted the ethical
approval. The BPS (British Psychology Society) guidelines were followed throughout the study.
All participants were presented with information about the aim of the study and what would be expected
of them; they were told that it would take approximately 10 minutes to complete the survey. Moreover,
participants were ensured about the confidentiality of the data they provided by explaining that the data
will be reported in aggregate format, questionnaires would be concealed, and no one other than the
researcher would have access to the data.
Furthermore, respondents were informed that participation in the study was entirely voluntary and that
they were free to withdraw from the study at any time and for any reason, and they do not need to
disclose this reason. Participants were asked to provide an anonymity number, in order to enable the
15
researcher to identify their responses should they wish to withdraw from the study. They were also
provided with the email address of the researcher in case they required more information about the
study and/or they wished to withdraw after completing the survey.
Participants were unable to proceed to the questionnaire without providing their consent to participate
in the study. Thus, they were required to tick a box next to the following statement ‘I confirm that I have
read and understand the purpose of this study and I give my consent to participate in the research
study.’
On the completion of the questionnaire participants were presented with a debriefing page. It included
more detailed information about the research, references of studies that looked at psychological
detachment and the email address to the researcher.
16
3. Results
3.1.Data cleaning and screening.
Once the data from SurveyMonkey was transferred to SPSS, participants’ responses were recorded
into numerical equivalent of their answers. Missing values were then recorded into SPSS into system
missing and missing observations were assumed to be missing completely at random (MCAR) as
Little’s MCAR test was non-significant. In addition to this result, the ‘Missing Value Analysis’ was used
in order to replace the 32 missing values. According to Field (2005) this method is better than using the
mean. The next step was to reverse scored four items- 1,2, 6,7- from the job demand scale, all of the
10 items from the role ambiguity measure and eight items- 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, 11, 14 and 15- from the locus of
control measure. Lastly, the scores for the criterion variable and for each of the predictor variables were
added by using the ‘compute’ command. Following that, the data was explored and analysed.
3.2.Descriptive Statistics.
Table 1. Means, standard deviation and Cronbach’s alpha for the criterion variable (psychological detachment)
and the predictor variables (job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control). (N=202).
M ± SD Cronbach’s alpha
Psychological Detachment 12.15 ± 4.35 .85
Job Demand 19.12 ± 3.53 .82
Role Ambiguity 28.45 ± 9.52 .82
Psychological Flexibility 34.84 ± 7.66 .91
Locus of Control 43.18 ± 10.00 .82
The table 1 represents Cronbach’s alpha values for each of the scale. The rule of thumb is that the
scale should have a minimum Cronbach’s alpha value of .7 to be considered consistent and reliable
(Brane, Kemp and Snelgar, 2009). In addition to this, all the scales showed internal reliability with
Cronbach’s α= .85 for psychological detachment, Cronbach’s α=.82 for job demand, role ambiguity and
locus of control and Cronbach’s α= .91 for locus of control.
The descriptive statistics of the criterion variable suggest that participants reported slightly above
moderate levels of psychological detachment (12.15 out of possible score of 20). Furthermore, the
17
results of predictor variables were as follow. In addition to job stressors, the results indicate that
participants scored slightly above moderate levels of job demand (19.12 out of possible score of 28)
and low scores of role ambiguity (28.45 out of possible score of 70). In relation to individual differences
participants reported high scores in psychological flexibility (34.84 out of possible score of 49) and
slightly below moderate levels of locus of control (43.18 out of possible score of 96).
3.3.Pearson’s correlation coefficient.
The bivariate correlations between the criterion variable and the predictor variables are presented in
table 2. According to the bivariate correlations, the criterion variable, psychological detachment, had
significant negative correlation with job demand (r = -.376, p < 0.001) and significant negative
relationship with role ambiguity (r = -.259, p < 0.001). Furthermore, according to the bivariate
correlations, the criterion variable did not have significant correlation with psychological flexibility (r =
.080, p= 0.260) and with locus of control (r = -.044, p= 0.531).
Furthermore, the table 2 represents significant correlations, which were not hypothesized in the study.
Job demand had positive significant relationship with role ambiguity (r = .337, p < 0.001). Moreover,
role ambiguity showed negative significant relationship with psychological flexibility (r = -.383, p <
0.001) and positive significant relationship with locus of control (r = .344, p < 0.001). Also, psychological
flexibility had negative significant relationship with locus of control (r = -.235, p < 0.001).
Table 2. The significance and correlation coefficient values of the bivariate correlations entered to the model
(Pearson’s r).
N=202
2 3 4 5
1 Psychological detachment -.376** -.259** .080 -.044
2 Job demand .337** -.134 .108
3 Role ambiguity -.383** .344**
4 Psychological flexibility -.235**
5 Locus of Control
*p < .05 ; **p < .001
18
3.4.Multiple Regression.
The data were analysed using the ‘Enter’ method and a multiple regression analysis was performed on
the raw data. The results indicate that the overall model was significant (F= 9.61, df= 4, p< 0.001). The
model explained 14.6% of the variance in psychological detachment from work during non-work time
(Adjusted R²= .146).
The information for the strength of the predictor variables entered into the model is provided in table 3.
Table 3. The unstandardized and standardised regression coefficient for the variables entered into the model.
Model B SE B β
Job demand -.399 .085 -.325**
Role ambiguity -.079 .035 -.173
Psychological flexibility -.011 .040 -.019
Locus of Control .020 .030 .046
*p < .05 ; **p < .001
Accordingly, job demand (β = -.325, p < 0.001) was significant predictor of psychological detachment
from work during non-work time as the predictor variable had the greatest beta loading and the highest
significance. The other variables, psychological flexibility and locus of control did not emerge as
significant predictors. Furthermore, the table 3 shows that role ambiguity lost significance. The
hypothesis was rejected.
3.4.1. Collinearity diagnostics.
Collinearity diagnostics were requested in order to investigate the assumption of no multicollinearity.
The tolerance values indicated that correlation between predictor variables, job demand, role ambiguity,
psychological flexibility and locus of control, varied between 0 and 1 indicating independence of
variance of each variable. Furthermore, variance inflation factor (VIF) was not greater than 10 and the
average VIF was not substantially greater than 1 indicating that the regression was not biased by
multicollinearity (Field, 2005). Taking all into account assumption of non-multicollinearity was met.
19
3.5.Conclusion.
To conclude, a multiple regression analysis was performed between psychological detachment as the
criterion variable and four predictor variables: job stressors including job demand and role ambiguity,
and individual differences including psychological flexibility and locus of control. Using the ‘Enter’
method, a significant model emerged. The model was of weak strength explaining 14.6% of the
variance. Furthermore, the only significant predictor of psychological detachment from work during non-
work time was job demand.
Moreover, bivariate correlations revealed that that there was a significant correlation between
psychological detachment and role ambiguity. However, under multiple regression analysis role
ambiguity became non-significant predictor of psychological detachment. Also, the predictor variables
correlated with each other. The results indicated significant correlation between job demand and role
ambiguity. Role ambiguity showed significant correlation with psychological flexibility and locus of
control. Lastly, there was significant correlation between psychological flexibility and locus of control.
20
4. Discussion
The purpose of the study was to investigate work stressors (job demand and role ambiguity) and
individual differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control) in order to explain which factors
hinder or support psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). In
addition to the literature review, there were four proposed hypotheses. Job demand, role ambiguity and
locus of control were hypothesised to be negatively associated with psychological detachment from
work during non-work time, whereas psychological flexibility was hypothesised to be a positive
predictor. Following data exploration and analysis, the results revealed that job demand was the only
significant predictor of psychological detachment. The remaining predictor variables (role ambiguity,
psychological flexibility and locus of control) were non- significant predictors of psychological
detachment from work.
The relationship between the criterion variable (psychological detachment from work during non-work
time) and the significant predictor variable, job demand, will now be explored further. Thereafter, the
results of the non-significant predictor variables (role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of
control) will be discussed. This will be followed by reporting other significant findings in the study. The
limitations and strengths of the current study will then be deliberated, before providing
recommendations for future research and practical implications of the current study.
4.1.Job demand and psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
With respect to job demand, the job stressor has been found to be a significant negative predictor of
psychological detachment from work during non-work time. This result was expected and is in line with
previous research that revealed that employees who experience high job demands will find it difficult to
mentally detach from work during non-work time (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005; Sonnentag and Kruel,
2006; Sonnentag, et al., 2010). Indeed, individuals show low psychological detachment from work when
they ponder upon and anticipate job demands (i.e. chronic time pressure), which are going to continue
in the following days (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). In addition, McEwen (1998) explained that
employees, who have been exposed to high job demands during the day, will have activated both their
physical and psychological systems in order to collect energy, which will be used to deal with the
demands. However, individuals will not be able to recover when these systems are still activated during
non-work time, in which they do not have to deal with these job demands (Sonnentag, Binnewies and
21
Mojza, 2010). Thus, it is plausible to suggest that nowadays, employees take a lot of work home with
them in order to complete tasks or prepare for the next working day during non-work time. Additionally,
they will be preoccupied with job demands physically and/or psychologically, thus, making it less likely
that they will psychologically unwind from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings).
4.2.Non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
Analyses showed that role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant
predictors of psychological detachment.
In relation to job stressors, the results indicate that the model revealed a complex relationship with role
ambiguity. Under bivariate correlation, role ambiguity had significant negative relationship with
psychological detachment. This suggests that individuals who reported high levels of role ambiguity
also showed lower levels of psychological detachment from work during off-job time. However, the
variable became non-significant under the multiple regression analysis. The results imply that job
demand is more important in relation to being able to mentally detach from work during non-work time
than role ambiguity, which is in line with previous research (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). In addition to
that, there are a number of reasons why job demand has been found to be a significant predictor of low
psychological detachment, whereas role ambiguity has not. Firstly, participants have scored higher on
job demand compared to role ambiguity. This might indicate that job demands are perceived as more
stressful for employees than ambiguous roles are. Also, individuals might be preoccupied with work
related tasks during non-work time when faced with a high workload rather than when faced with role
ambiguity (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006).
In relation to individual differences, both psychological flexibility and locus of control have been found to
be non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work. These two variables, however,
were correlated with role ambiguity and also indicated a relationship with each other. An explanation as
to why these individual differences did not emerge as significant predictors of the criterion variable
might simply be that psychological flexibility and locus of control do not affect psychological detachment
from work during off-job time. However, previous research into psychological detachment did not
investigate the relationship between psychological flexibility and locus of control. To the researchers’
knowledge, this is the first study that investigated these individual differences in relation to
psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). These were of
particular interest because these specific individual differences can be enhanced.
22
4.3.Additional significant results.
The results from the correlation coefficients showed that role ambiguity had significant negative
relationship with psychological detachment and psychological flexibility; and significant positive
relationship with job demand and locus of control. The results also showed that psychological flexibility
had significant negative relationship with locus of control.
Individuals who reported having ambiguous roles also reported low psychological flexibility and an
external locus of control. This implies that individuals who do not have a clear understanding of their job
responsibilities are also less likely to be able to remain focused on the present moment, are unable to
be mindful of their thoughts and feelings, and still be able to pursue goals and take actions.
Furthermore, individuals who experience high levels of role ambiguity are more likely to believe that
they do not have direct control over the environment around them. The correlation coefficients results
further showed that individuals who experience high role ambiguity reported high levels of job demand,
which might suggest that ambiguous roles are perceived as more demanding. Moreover, the individual
differences showed significant negative relationships with each other. Thus, individuals who reported
high levels of psychological flexibility also reported having an internal locus of control; suggesting that
those who are likely to be able to remain focused at the present moment, be mindful of their thoughts
and feelings, and still be able to pursue goals and take actions, also have a strong belief that they can
control the environment around them.
4.4.Strengths of the current study.
The study looked at psychological detachment, which is a new phenomenon in the scientific literature.
Previous research looked at job stressors, workload and role ambiguity (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005;
Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006) and individual differences such as personality (Sonnentag and Fritz, 2007)
in relation to psychological detachment from work during non-work time. However, the current study
looked at individual differences including psychological flexibility and locus of control, which can be
enhanced, unlike personality. In addition, this could have important implications for both individuals and
organisations in terms of improving employees psychological detachment. The results indicated that
psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant predictors of psychological
detachment, yet, the correlation coefficients results indicated some relationships.
23
The sample in the study included employees from a wide range of professions such as: midwifes,
psychologists, teachers, medics, individuals working in retail, catering, IT, etc. This indicates that the
findings can be generalized to the group of working adults, rather than only a specific group.
4.5.Limitations of the current study.
Conclusions about causality cannot be drawn due to use of the multiple regression method. Thus, a
significant outcome does not imply causation or direction. In addition, it can be concluded that, when
employees experience high job demand their psychological detachment from work during off-job time
decreases. However, it cannot be said that low psychological detachment is caused by high job
demands. In terms of directing future research, this limitation can be used to inform future studies,
where other methodologies such as experimental designs may be used in order to confirm the causality
and direction of this finding.
Self-report measures have been used in the study. Consequently, there is a lack of a comprehensive
answer as to whether high job demands or individuals’ perception of high job demands is associated
with low psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Future research should consider
using objective measures in order to clarify this (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006).
4.6.Recommendations for future research.
The current study controlled for demographic characteristics such as marital status, whether
participants had children, and if so how many. However, future research should look into other factors
related to employees’ personal life, which might serve as a distraction and help employees to detach
from work-related issues during short respite periods. Sonnentag and Kruel (2006) suggested that
positive (i.e. pleasant evening) as well as negative (i.e. argument with a partner) experiences at home
might help individuals to mentally unwind from work during off-job time.
It is suggested that the importance of both of the individual differences in relation to psychological
detachment from work during non-work time is reinvestigated. The researcher proposes that mainly it is
important to look at psychological flexibility as the particular individual difference can be enhanced
through Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). For instance, it would be recommended that a
study with a control group and a group of employees who will receive ACT, which enhances
psychological flexibility, be conducted in order to investigate whether employees, who receive ACT will
24
also report higher levels of psychological detachment following the session. Additionally, psychological
detachment should be measured before and after the intervention, and once more a few weeks later in
order to investigate how long the effects last.
A further recommendation is to investigate whether psychological flexibility and/ or locus of control
serve as moderators between high job demands and psychological detachment.
4.7.Implications of the current study.
As established by the current study and previous research, job demands play a major role in
employees’ ability to mentally detach from work-related issues during short respite periods (i.e.
evening).
In order to improve employees’ psychological detachment from work during off-job time it is essential
for organisations to address the issue of high job demands and introduce efficient time management
interventions. This can be done by teaching employees how to more efficiently manage their time at
work. In addition, it is proposed that time management interventions be implemented in the workplace.
A review of time management literature suggested that there are a number of benefits of implementing
such an intervention. The research indicated that following a time management intervention, employees
learned how to manage their time more effectively, worried less, and reduced procrastination
(Claessens, Van Eerde, Rutte and Roe, 2007). Consequently, employees should complete their work-
related tasks in a shorter time frame, thus allowing them to mentally unwind from these demands during
non-work time (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006).
4.8.Conclusion.
The purpose of the study was to investigate whether work stressors (job demand and role ambiguity)
and individual differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control) hinder or support psychological
detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evening). Job demand was found to be a
significant negative predictor of psychological detachment from work during non-work time, whereas
role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant predictors of the
criterion variable. However, the correlation coefficients results suggested that psychological
detachment from work had a significant negative relationship with both of the job stressors. Moreover,
role ambiguity had significant positive relationship with job demand and locus of control and a
25
significant negative relationship with psychological flexibility. Furthermore, the individual differences
showed a significant negative relationship with each other.
It was the first study that looked at psychological flexibility and locus of control in relation to
psychological detachment. These particular individual differences were included as they can be
enhanced, which could have important implications for both individuals and organisations in terms of
improving employees psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Even though, they
were non-significant, the correlation coefficients results indicated some relationships. In addition, future
research should consider reinvestigating the importance of both of the individual differences in relation
to psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
26
5. References:
Bond, F.W. & Bunce, D. (2003) cited in Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit, F., & Westerhof, G. J.
(2010). Mental health promotion as a new goal in public mental health care: A randomized
controlled trial of an intervention enhancing psychological flexibility. Journal
Information, 100(12).
Bond, F. W. & Bunce, D. (2000) cited in Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New York: KA/PP.
Bond, F. W. & Bunce, D. (2003) cited in Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New York: KA/PP.
Bond, F. W., Lloyd, J., & Guenole, N. (in press). The work‐related acceptance and action questionnaire:
initial psychometric findings and their implications for measuring psychological flexibility in
specific contexts. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology.
Brane, N.Kemp, R. & Snelgar, R. (2009). SPSS for Psychologists (4th ed.). Palgrave Macmillan.
Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006) cited in Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010).
Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the
benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365.
Brosschot, J.F., Gerrin, W. & Thayer, J.F. (2006) cited in White, E. (2010). Helping to promote
psychological well-being at work: the role of work engagement, work stress and psychological
detachment using the job demand-resources model. The Plymouth Student Scientist, 4(2),
155-180.
Brosschot, J. F., Pieper, S., & Thayer, J. F. (2005). Expanding stress theory: prolonged activation and
perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 1043-1049.
Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to
psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(9), 871-890.
Claessens, B. J., Van Eerde, W., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A. (2007). A review of the time management
literature. Personnel Review, 36(2), 255-276.
Cropley, M. & Millward Purvis, L.J. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological
detachment from work during off-job time: The role of job stressors, job involvement, and
recovery-related self-efficacy. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(2),
197-217.
Davidson, O. B., Eden, D., Westman, M., Cohen-Charash, Y., Hammer, L. B., Kluger, A. N., Krausz, M.,
Maslach, C., O’Driscoll, M., Perrewé, P. L., Quick, J. C., Rosenblatt, Z., & Spector, P. E.
(2010). Sabbatical leave: who gains and how much?. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5),
953.
27
De Lange, A. H., Taris, T. W., Kompier, M. A., Houtman, I. L., & Bongers, P. M. (2004). The
relationships between work characteristics and mental health: examining normal, reversed and
reciprocal relationships in a 4-wave study. Work & Stress, 18(2), 149-166.
Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors and burnout: Reserve service as a
respite. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(4), 577-585.
Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (2nd ed.). London:SAGE.
Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and
commitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8),
816-820.
Fledderus, M., Bohlemijer, E.T. & Pieterse, M.E. (2010) cited in Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit,
F., & Westerhof, G. J. (2010). Mental health promotion as a new goal in public mental health
care: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention enhancing psychological flexibility. Journal
Information, 100(12).
Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit, F., & Westerhof, G. J. (2010). Mental health promotion as a new
goal in public mental health care: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention enhancing
psychological flexibility. Journal Information, 100(12).
Ganster, D.C., Fox, M.L., & Dweyer, M.J. (2001) cited in Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005).
Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of psychological detachment from work
during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10(4), 393.
Geurts, S. A. E., Kompier, M. A. J., Roxburgh, S., & Houtman, I. L. D. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., &
Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of psychological
detachment from work during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10(4),
393.
Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New
York: KA/PP.
Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K.D., Bunting, K., Twohig, M. & Wilson, K. (2004) cited in Flaxman, P. E., &
Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and commitment
therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8), 816-820.
Ilies, R., Schwind, K. M., Wagner, D. T., Johnson, M. D., DeRue, D. S., & Ilgen, D. R. (2007). When can
employees have a family life? The effects of daily workload and affect on work-family conflict
and social behavior at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1368–1379.
Inoue, A., Kawakami, N., Haratani, T., Kobayashi, F., Ishizaki, M., Hayashi, T., Fujota, O., Aizawa, Y.,
Miyazaki, S., Hiro, H., Masumoto, T., Hashimoto & Araki, S. (2010). Job stressors and long
term sick leave due to depressive disorders among Japanese male employees: findings from
the Japan Work Stress and Health Cohort study. Journal of epidemiology and community
health, 64(3), 229-235.
28
Judge, T.A. & Bono, J.E. (2001) cited in Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of
control at work: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087.
Karasek R. (1985). Job Content Instrument Questionnaire and User’s Guide, Version 1.1. Los Angeles:
Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Southern California.
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of
health. Clinical psychology review, 30(7), 865-878.
Kirkcaldy, B. D., Shephard, R. J., & Furnham, A. F. (2002). The influence of type A behaviour and locus
of control upon job satisfaction and occupational health. Personality and Individual
Differences, 33(8), 1361-1371.
Lappalainen, R., Lehtonen, T., Skarp, E., Taubert, E., Ojanen, M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007) cited in Fla
man, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and co
mitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8),
816-820.
Lavoie-Tremblay, M., Wright, D., Desforges, N., Gélinas, C., Marchionni, C. & Drevniok, U. (2008).
Creating a Healthy Workplacefor New-Generation Nurses. Journal of Nursing Scholarship,
40(3), 290-297.
Lu, L. (1999). Work Motivation, Job Stress and Employees’ Well-being. Journal of Applied Management
Studies, 8(1), 61-72.
McEwen, B.S. (1998) cited in Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and
engaged when demands are high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 95(5), 965.
Meier, L. L., Semmer, N. K., Elfering, A., & Jacobshagen, N. (2008). The double meaning of control:
Three-way interactions between internal resources, job control, and stressors at work. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology,13(3), 244.
Meijman, T.F. & Mulder, G. (1998) cited in Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying
well and engaged when demands are high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 95(5), 965.
Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a meta‐analysis. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087.
Rizzo, J.R, House, R.J. & Lirtzman, S,I. (1970). Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 15(2), 150-163.
Rothbard, N. P., & Wilk, S. L. (2011). Waking up on the right or wrong side of the bed: Start-of-workday
mood, work events, employee affect, and performance. Academy of Management
Journal, 54(5), 959-980.
29
Rotter, J.B. (1966) cited in Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a
meta‐analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087.
Siegall, M. (2000). Putting the stress back into role stress: Improving the measurement of role conflict
and role ambiguity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 15(5), 427-435.
Sonnentag, S. (2003). Recovery, work engagement, and proactive behaviour: a new look at the
interface between nonwork and work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 518.
Sonnentag, S. (2012). Psychological Detachment From Work During Leisure Time The Benefits of
Mentally Disengaging From Work. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 114-118.
Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of
psychological detachment from work during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 10(4), 393.
Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2008). " Did you have a nice evening?" A day-level study
on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(3), 674.
Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and engaged when demands are
high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 965.
Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The recovery experience questionnaire: Development and validation
of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 12(3), 204.
Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment from work during off-job time: The role of
job stressors, job involvement, and recovery-related self-efficacy. European Journal of Work
and Organizational Psychology, 15(2), 197-217.
Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for
recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365.
Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010) cited in Sonnentag, S. (2012). Psychological Detachment
From Work During Leisure Time The Benefits of Mentally Disengaging From Work. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 114-118.
Sonnentag, S., Mojza, E. J., Binnewies, C., & Scholl, A. (2008). Being engaged at work and detached
at home: A week-level study on work engagement, psychological detachment, and affect. Work
& Stress, 22(3), 257-276.
Spector, P. E. (1988). Development of the work locus of control scale. Journal of occupational
psychology, 61(4), 335-340.
Spector, P. E., Cooper, C. L., Sanchez, J. I., O'Driscoll, M., Sparks, K., Bernin, P., Bussing, A., Dewe,
P., Hart, P., Lu, L., Miller, K., De Moraes, L.R., Ostrognay, G.M., Pagon, M., Pitariu, H.D.,
Poelmans, S.A.Y., Radhakrishnan, P., Russinova, V., Salamatov, V., Salgado, J.F., Shima, S.,
30
Siu, O., Stora, J.B., Teichmann, M., Theorell, T., Vlerick, P., Westman, M., Widerszal-Bazyl, M., Wong,
P.T.P. & Yu, S. (2002). Locus of control and well-being at work: how generalizable are western
findings?. Academy of Management Journal, 45(2), 453-466.
Westman & Eden (1997) cited in Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors
and burnout: Reserve service as a respite. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(4), 577-585.
White, E. (2010). Helping to promote psychological well-being at work: the role of work engagement,
work stress and psychological detachment using the job demand-resources model. The
Plymouth Student Scientist, 4(2), 155-180.
Zohar, D., Tzischinski, O. & Epstein, R. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job
stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of
psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365.
6. Appendices
6.1.The online survey.

More Related Content

What's hot

Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...
Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...
Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...inventionjournals
 
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...QUESTJOURNAL
 
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work PlaceJob Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Placeinventionjournals
 
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective Workforce
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective WorkforceMindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective Workforce
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective WorkforceKendra I. Reece
 
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performanceAlexander Decker
 
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...Alexander Decker
 
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...Chris Rossiter
 
ORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter twoORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter twoClassic Tech
 
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)Santhosh Nair
 
Stress management final
Stress management finalStress management final
Stress management finalJiten Menghani
 
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...IJERA Editor
 
a-study-on-stress-management
a-study-on-stress-managementa-study-on-stress-management
a-study-on-stress-managementShibin Vincent
 
Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance
	Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance	Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance
Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performanceinventionjournals
 
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employees
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employeesRole of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employees
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employeesdeshwal852
 
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in India
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in IndiaOccupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in India
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in Indiaiosrjce
 
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-report
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-reportWorkplace bullying-in-australia-final-report
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-reportFlint Wilkes
 

What's hot (20)

Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...
Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...
Organizational Stress, Job Satisfaction and Employee Mental Health: A Compara...
 
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...
Stress and Emotional Bases, investigating the Effectiveness of Emotional Inte...
 
KARTHIK.MBAPRJ
KARTHIK.MBAPRJKARTHIK.MBAPRJ
KARTHIK.MBAPRJ
 
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work PlaceJob Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
 
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective Workforce
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective WorkforceMindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective Workforce
Mindfulness in the Workplace-Cultivation of Engaged and Effective Workforce
 
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance
11.factors causing stress and impact on job performance
 
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...
A study on the effect of stress on performance of employees in commercial ban...
 
Stress management report
Stress management reportStress management report
Stress management report
 
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...
Psychological Assessment of Adults with Specific Performance Difficulties at ...
 
ORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter twoORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter two
 
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)
Santhosh Nair Project (Stress Management)
 
Project Report on Stress
Project Report on Stress Project Report on Stress
Project Report on Stress
 
Stress management final
Stress management finalStress management final
Stress management final
 
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...
The Relationship between Psychological Health, Self-Confidence and Locus of C...
 
a-study-on-stress-management
a-study-on-stress-managementa-study-on-stress-management
a-study-on-stress-management
 
Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance
	Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance	Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance
Impact of stress antecedents on work stress and employees performance
 
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employees
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employeesRole of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employees
Role of stress management courses: A case study of banking sector employees
 
stress research
stress researchstress research
stress research
 
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in India
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in IndiaOccupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in India
Occupational Stress at Workplace: Study of the Corporate Sector in India
 
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-report
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-reportWorkplace bullying-in-australia-final-report
Workplace bullying-in-australia-final-report
 

Viewers also liked

Fetchh_Pitch_Pioneers
Fetchh_Pitch_PioneersFetchh_Pitch_Pioneers
Fetchh_Pitch_PioneersWill Jensen
 
Resume terron jackson2 (murriata)
Resume   terron jackson2 (murriata)Resume   terron jackson2 (murriata)
Resume terron jackson2 (murriata)Terron Jackson
 
Psychological consideration in acne treatment
Psychological consideration in acne treatmentPsychological consideration in acne treatment
Psychological consideration in acne treatmentDr. Faramarz Didar
 
The agile and Lean mindset
The agile and Lean mindsetThe agile and Lean mindset
The agile and Lean mindsetYves Hanoulle
 
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016Paty Cj
 
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3Alfredo Carrato
 
Кобилдър България
Кобилдър БългарияКобилдър България
Кобилдър БългарияMariela Daskalova
 
Introduction of shoping mall (2)
Introduction of shoping mall (2)Introduction of shoping mall (2)
Introduction of shoping mall (2)Ar. Sahid Akhtar
 

Viewers also liked (11)

Fetchh_Pitch_Pioneers
Fetchh_Pitch_PioneersFetchh_Pitch_Pioneers
Fetchh_Pitch_Pioneers
 
Dandruff
DandruffDandruff
Dandruff
 
Resume terron jackson2 (murriata)
Resume   terron jackson2 (murriata)Resume   terron jackson2 (murriata)
Resume terron jackson2 (murriata)
 
Psychological consideration in acne treatment
Psychological consideration in acne treatmentPsychological consideration in acne treatment
Psychological consideration in acne treatment
 
Resume
ResumeResume
Resume
 
The agile and Lean mindset
The agile and Lean mindsetThe agile and Lean mindset
The agile and Lean mindset
 
Biologia
BiologiaBiologia
Biologia
 
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016
Catálogo yves rocher campaña 15, 2016
 
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3
BIM Project Execution Plans and Open BIM via IFC2x3
 
Кобилдър България
Кобилдър БългарияКобилдър България
Кобилдър България
 
Introduction of shoping mall (2)
Introduction of shoping mall (2)Introduction of shoping mall (2)
Introduction of shoping mall (2)
 

Similar to One must know when to stop

3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx
3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx
3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptxssuser0bcda8
 
Work place stress preventive and curative measures
Work place stress preventive and curative measuresWork place stress preventive and curative measures
Work place stress preventive and curative measuresIJMER
 
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docxRunning head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docxsusanschei
 
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)Kajal Jain
 
Attitudes & Job Satisfaction
Attitudes & Job SatisfactionAttitudes & Job Satisfaction
Attitudes & Job SatisfactionGULFAMSHAHZADA
 
The Science Behind PI-1
The Science Behind PI-1The Science Behind PI-1
The Science Behind PI-1John Bowser
 
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-report
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-reportshraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-report
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-reportbzdbsfdz
 
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFEMAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFEAnant Kumar
 
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docxdrennanmicah
 
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and ValuesLesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and ValuesMervyn Maico Aldana
 
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCY
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCYWHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCY
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCYMike Hamerly
 
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORS
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORSA STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORS
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORSIAEME Publication
 
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)Indus University
 
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxRunning head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxjeanettehully
 
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxRunning head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxwlynn1
 

Similar to One must know when to stop (20)

3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx
3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx
3. Summary of 8 Topics - M. Athar Jamil (Assignment#2).pptx
 
Work place stress preventive and curative measures
Work place stress preventive and curative measuresWork place stress preventive and curative measures
Work place stress preventive and curative measures
 
The Science Behind PI
The Science Behind PIThe Science Behind PI
The Science Behind PI
 
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docxRunning head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
 
Ob1 unit 3 chapter - 9 - attitude
Ob1   unit 3 chapter - 9 - attitudeOb1   unit 3 chapter - 9 - attitude
Ob1 unit 3 chapter - 9 - attitude
 
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)
Stress at the Workplace: Managing Job & Work Stress (MBA Project)
 
Attitudes & Job Satisfaction
Attitudes & Job SatisfactionAttitudes & Job Satisfaction
Attitudes & Job Satisfaction
 
The Science Behind PI-1
The Science Behind PI-1The Science Behind PI-1
The Science Behind PI-1
 
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-report
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-reportshraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-report
shraddha mishra-mba-hr-project-report
 
E0321030034
E0321030034E0321030034
E0321030034
 
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFEMAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
MAPPING INDIVIDUAL AND COLLECTIVE REGRET IN ORGANIZATIONAL LIFE
 
OVERALL JOB SATISFACTION
OVERALL JOB SATISFACTIONOVERALL JOB SATISFACTION
OVERALL JOB SATISFACTION
 
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
 
Main
MainMain
Main
 
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and ValuesLesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values
Lesson 2 - Individual Behavior, Personality, and Values
 
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCY
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCYWHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCY
WHITE PAPER ON CORPORATE CHAPLAINCY
 
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORS
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORSA STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORS
A STUDY ON EMPLOYEES ATTITUDE TOWARDS VARIOUS LEVELS OF STRESSORS
 
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)
Attitude and job satisfactio (Report)
 
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxRunning head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
 
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docxRunning head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
Running head GOAL SETTING AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION 1GOA.docx
 

One must know when to stop

  • 1. 1 One must know when to stop: work stressors and individual differences as predictors of psychological detachment from work during short respite periods. Goldsmiths, University of London Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of an MSc in Occupational Psychology. Student ID number: 33270657 Supervisor: Dr Jo Lloyd Date: 16th September 2013 Word count: 8,604
  • 2. Acknowledgements I would like to thank my supervisor Dr Jo Lloyd for her patience, advice and support. She inspired, encouraged, reassured and guided me throughout researching and writing the MSc dissertation. Thank you for believing and motivating me to reach my potential. Besides my supervisor I would like to thank my friends and family for having the confidence in me throughout my MSc degree. Thank you.
  • 3. Executive Summary 1.1.Subject matter. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity and individual differences: psychological flexibility (i.e. whether an individual has the ability to stay focused on the present moment) and locus of control (i.e. whether an individual believes that they have a direct control over their life) predict or hinder psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). In relation to work stressors, it was proposed that individuals who experience high levels of job demands and role ambiguity will also find it difficult to psychologically unwind from work-related issues. In addition to individual differences, it was proposed that individuals who will report high levels of psychological flexibility and strong believe that they have control over the environment around them will also find it easier to mentally detach from work during off-job time. 1.2.Methods of analysis and findings. The first step was to prepare the data by replacing participants’ responses with numerical equivalents, investigating for missing values, reverse scored items of the scales where appropriate, and add all scores for each factor. This was followed by analysing and exploring the data. The results suggested that the psychological detachment, job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control scales were consistent and reliable. Analysis, which looked at the relationship between psychological detachment, job demands, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control, suggested that individuals who reported high levels of job demand and role ambiguity also showed low levels of psychological detachment from work during non-work times. Furthermore, the results showed that individuals who showed high levels of job demands also reported to have ambiguous roles. Moreover, employees who have ambiguous roles also reported that they find it difficult to remain focused on the present moment and tend to believe that they do not have direct control over environment around them. The results also implied that individuals who are able to remain focused on the present moment also reported to have strong believe that they control the environment around them. However, further explorations and analysis of the data that looked at which factors predict psychological detachment from work during non-work time, showed that only job demands emerged as a strong predictor of psychological detachment.
  • 4. 1.3.Strengths and limitations of the current study. There are a number of strengths of the study. Psychological detachment from work during non-work time is a new phenomenon. In relation to individual differences, the current study looked at psychological flexibility and locus of control, which have important implications for both individuals and organisations, as these are factors that can be improved. However, previous studies focused on individual differences such as personality, which is fixed and cannot be enhanced. Another, strength of the study lies in having participants from a wide range of occupations, which suggests that the findings can be generalized to the group of working adults, rather than only a specific group. Having said that, the study is not without limitations. It can be concluded that, when employees experience high levels of job demand their psychological detachment from work during off-job time decreases. However, it cannot be said that low psychological detachment is caused by high job demands. Another limitation of the study is using self-report measures. Thus, it cannot be stated whether high job demands or individuals’ perception of high job demands is associated with low psychological detachment from work during non-work time. 1.4.Recommendations. There are a number of aspects for future research to consider. It is recommended that factors related to employees’ personal life be further investigated, as these may distract employees and help them to mentally detach from work related issues during short respite periods. Furthermore, future research should re-examine psychological flexibility and locus of control in relation to psychological detachment and investigate whether individual differences moderate the relationship between high job demands and psychological detachment. Job demand is an important factor in relation to the ability to psychologically unwind from work during non-work time. Organisations should implement effective interventions and teach employees time management skills so that they can organise their time for work tasks in a more efficient manner. This will help employees to complete their duties more efficiently at work and detach mentally from work related issues at home.
  • 5. Abstract This study used a cross-sectional design and examined the multivariate relationship between psychological detachment and job stressors (job demands and role ambiguity) and individual differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control). The objective was to investigate which of these factors support or hinder psychological detachment from work during non-work time. In total, 202 working adults from a variety of professions took part in the study by completing a self-report online survey. Regression analysis revealed that high job demand was the only significant predictor of low psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Furthermore, the remaining job stressor, role ambiguity, was a non-significant predictor of psychological detachment. Additionally, both of the individual difference variables, psychological flexibility and locus of control, were non-significant predictors of psychological detachment.
  • 6. Contents 1.Introduction. ...............................................................................................................................................1 1.1.Psychological detachment from work during non-work time..................................................................1 1.2.The current study. .................................................................................................................................3 1.3.Predictors of psychological detachment: work stressors and individual differences..............................4 1.3.1.Work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity. ...........................................................................4 1.3.1.1.Job demands and psychological detachment from work during non-work time...........................4 1.3.1.2.Role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. .......................6 1.3.2.Individual differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control................................................7 1.3.2.1.Psychological flexibility................................................................................................................7 1.3.2.2.Locus of control...........................................................................................................................8 2.Method.......................................................................................................................................................10 2.1.Design.................................................................................................................................................10 2.2.Participants. ........................................................................................................................................10 2.3.Measures. ...........................................................................................................................................11 2.3.1.Demographics..............................................................................................................................11 2.3.2.Criterion variable measure...........................................................................................................11 2.3.2.1.Psychological detachment measure..........................................................................................11 2.3.3.Predictor variables measures.......................................................................................................12 2.3.3.1.Job demands measure..............................................................................................................12 2.3.3.2.Role ambiguity measure............................................................................................................12 2.3.3.3.Psychological flexibility measure...............................................................................................13 2.3.3.4.Locus of control measure..........................................................................................................13 2.4.Procedure............................................................................................................................................13 2.5.Ethics. .................................................................................................................................................14 3.Results ......................................................................................................................................................16 3.1.Data cleaning and screening...............................................................................................................16 3.2.Descriptive Statistics. ..........................................................................................................................16 3.3.Pearson’s correlation coefficient. ........................................................................................................17 3.4.Multiple Regression.............................................................................................................................18 3.4.1.Collinearity diagnostics. ...............................................................................................................18 3.5.Conclusion. .........................................................................................................................................19
  • 7. 4.Discussion................................................................................................................................................20 4.1.Job demand and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. ...................................20 4.2.Non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work during non-work time...................21 4.3.Additional significant results................................................................................................................22 4.4.Strengths of the current study. ............................................................................................................22 4.5.Limitations of the current study. ..........................................................................................................23 4.6.Recommendations for future research. ...............................................................................................23 4.7.Implications of the current study..........................................................................................................24 4.8.Conclusion. .........................................................................................................................................24 5.References................................................................................................................................................26 6.Appendices...............................................................................................................................................30 6.1.The online survey................................................................................................................................30
  • 8. 1 1. Introduction. In recent years, there has been a growing body of literature surrounding the fairly new phenomenon of psychological detachment (White, 2010). In today’s fast-paced, technologically developed, 24/7 economy, employees are largely still accessible to their managers while physically away from work. Research indicates that this has resulted in a trend towards employees continuously facing work demands, even during their non-work time (Etzion, Eden and Lapidot, 1998). As these demands are the main sources of everyday stress, it is important that individuals detach from work, both physically and psychologically, in order to keep stress levels at a manageable level (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). However, living in a technologically advanced society makes psychological detachment from work during non-work time more difficult (Sonnentag, 2012). The current study seeks to explore some of the variables that hinder or support psychological detachment. 1.1.Psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Etzion, Eden and Lapidot (1998) define detachment as individuals being physically away from their work situation and routine. They proposed that detachment from work becomes harder due to developing technologies, which make , employees increasingly accessible to their managers as they can be contacted and receive work related emails or phone calls even when they are physically away from work. Furthermore, Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) emphasised the importance of not only being physically detached from work, but also being mentally detached. Thus, the researchers highlighted the psychological component in disengaging from the work environment as not thinking about work, work related problems and activities during off-job time. In order for psychological detachment to occur, an individual needs to physically leave the workplace, as well as refrain from reflecting on work-related issues and engaging in work-related duties at home. A study by Sonnentag, Kuttler and Fritz (2010) has demonstrated that employees who are mentally detached from work-related duties during time off work reported higher levels of psychological well- being, higher satisfaction with their life, experienced less emotional exhaustion, and showed fewer symptoms of psychological strain (i.e. problems with sleep); whilst still remaining engaged at work (Sonnentag, 2012). In a longitudinal study conducted by Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza (2010), psychological detachment was found to be linked to changes in employees’ well-being over time.
  • 9. 2 Individuals who continued to be mentally preoccupied with work-related duties during off-hours reported increased emotional exhaustion a year later. Moreover, employees who did not mentally disengage from work during off-job time showed decreased energy levels that further add to burnout. Further benefits of psychological detachment from work during time off work have been reported in a study that looked at sabbatical leave. The results indicated that sabbatical leave decreased stress and burnout and increased levels of life satisfaction and positive affect among individuals who psychologically detached from work during their time off (Davidson, Eden, Westman, Cohen-Charash, Hammer, Kluger, Krausz, Maslach, O’Driscoll, Perrewé, Quick, Rosenblatt and Spector, 2010). Psychological detachment has also been found to have detrimental effects on individuals’ everyday well-being. This has been illustrated in a study conducted by Sonnentag and Bayer (2005). The researchers gathered questionnaires and daily survey measures over the course of three working days. The results implied that employees who were psychologically detached from work during evening hours experienced good moods and lower fatigue. In addition, Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza (2008) demonstrated that psychological detachment from work during time after work is a powerful and effective recovery experience. Over the course of one week, participants completed daily survey measures. The researchers found that higher levels of fatigue and high negative activation the next morning were reported amongst the individuals who did not psychologically detach from work in the evening. Rothbard and Wilk (2011) argued for the importance of looking at employees’ mood at the beginning of their working day. The researchers found that starting their working day in a good or a bad mood had a significant influence on how employees perceived work events, how they related to these events, and how well they performed during the day. In order to maintain well-being at the workplace, it is essential for employees to have respite periods away from work (Eden, 2001 cited in Sonnentag, 2003). Etzion, et al. (1998) looked at psychological detachment in relation to longer respite periods, lasting two weeks or longer. The results from the research indicated that along with mastery, control and relaxation, psychological detachment is one of the main factors that lead to improved well-being when employees returned to work. Moreover, Brosschot, Gerrin and Thayer (2006) argued that psychological detachment is the most important factor for enhancing employees’ psychological well-being due to being the only recovery process that involves mentally disengaging from work related duties (White, 2010). According to Westman and Eden (1997) these beneficial effects of long respite periods became weaker three weeks after employees returned to the workplace (Etzion, et al., 1998). In addition, Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) emphasised the importance of researching psychological detachment from work in relation to short intervals. Furthermore, based on research evidence Sonnentag (2003) reported that short respite periods such
  • 10. 3 as evening hours are also beneficial for employees’ well-being. Thus, daily recovery processes are effective in supplementing the effects of long respite periods; indicating that employees do not need to wait for a vacation in order to recover from work related stress. 1.2.The current study. The current study aimed to predict the multivariate relationship between psychological detachment and four predictor variables, including job stressors: job demand and role ambiguity, and individual differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control, with the purpose of finding which of them hinder or support psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). Living in a fast-paced 24/7 economy, employees are continuously facing demands of work, which are the main source of everyday stress. This makes it difficult to psychologically detach from work during non-work time (Sonnentag, 2012). The current study looks at psychological detachment as it has been reported to have detrimental effects on individuals’ everyday well-being (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005; Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2008). Psychological detachment will be considered in relation to short respite periods (i.e. evenings), because, as reported by Westman and Eden (1997), the effects of longer breaks, such as vacations, become weaker three weeks after employees return to work (Etzion, Eden and Lapidot, 1998). Therefore, it is useful to investigate which individual differences hinder or support psychological detachment. The individual differences that have been chosen for investigation in this study are psychological flexibility and locus of control. These individual differences are of particular interest, as unlike personality, for example, psychological flexibility and locus of control can be enhanced, which may lead to improved psychological detachment from work during off-work time. Additionally, the study looks at job stressors in relation to psychological detachment from work during non-work time. The work stressors chosen include job demands and role ambiguity. The importance of investigating these stressors is twofold. Firstly, according to Zohar, Tzischinski and Epstein (2003), employees who are challenged with job stressors have a greater need for recovery, which is the result of using more effort and regulating emotions in order to face these job stressors (Sonnentag, Kuttler and Fritz, 2010). Dealing with work stressors at the workplace will make it difficult for employees to mentally detach from work at home, and consequently increases the need for recovery. Secondly, understanding the job stressors that influence psychological detachment will have important
  • 11. 4 implications in the workplace, in terms of trying to minimise these and increase the well-being of employees (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). 1.3.Predictors of psychological detachment: work stressors and individual differences. 1.3.1. Work stressors: job demand and role ambiguity. 1.3.1.1. Job demands and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. In relation to Karasek’s job control-demand model, the current study refers to job demands as a high amount of complex work that an employee needs to accomplish, and dealing with incompatible demands from others (Lavoie-Tremblay, Wright, Desforges, Gélinas Marchionni and Drevniok, 2008). Occupational stress has a significant impact on organisations and employees’ physical and mental health (Lu, 1999). In addition, Ganster, Fox and Dweyer (2001) argued that it is well established in the scientific literature that job demands are one of the main factors that negatively affect health and well- being (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). Geurts, Kompier, Roxburgh and Houtman (2003) suggested that employees who experience a high workload will also report poor health and well-being as it limits their opportunity for recovery (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). Moreover, in a 4-wave panel study including 668 employees, a significant relationship between high job demands and impaired mental health over time was found (De Lange, Taris, Kompier, Houtman and Bongers, 2004). McEwen (1998) argued that when individuals are exposed to high job demands, their physiological and psychological systems become activated with the purpose of gathering the necessary energy to face the job demands (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010). Thus, Meijman and Mulder (1998) proposed that an individual’s physiological and psychological systems will neutralize during even a short respite period, during which the individual can recuperate (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010). However, McEwen (1998) suggested that recovery will not occur when, during respite periods, individuals are not exposed to job demands, yet, their physiological and psychological systems remain activated. Moreover, it has been argued that experiencing high strain levels whilst away from job demands could facilitate and lead to long term health issues (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010). This implies that it is not sufficient for employees to be physically away from their work environment and exposure to work-related demands. It is also essential for employees to be psychologically detached from job demands during non-work time in order to reduce the high strain levels (Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza, 2010). In addition, Brosschot, Pieper and Thayer (2005) argued that cognitive mechanisms play a major role in physiological concomitants. Thus, the researchers argued that stressors do not lead
  • 12. 5 directly to prolonged activation. The constant and repeated activation of cognitive representations of stress (i.e. preservative cognition) is a mediator between stressors (e.g. job demand) and prolonged activation, which in turn will lead to maintained high strain levels. In respect to that, Sonnentag, Binnewies and Mojza (2010) suggested that high strain levels caused by prolonged activation will remain only when employees experience high job demands and lack of psychological detachment during non-work hours. Job demands have also been found to play an important role in psychological detachment from work during non-work time. It has been established that employees who experience a high workload find it difficult to mentally detach from work-related duties during off-work time, with work demands commonly spilling over from work to home life (Sonnentag, Kuttler and Fritz, 2010). In a longitudinal experience- sampling study, 106 working adults completed daily surveys: two at work, and one during the evening at home. Their relatives were also interviewed every day over the phone. The results indicated that high workload predicted negative affect at work. Furthermore, the researchers predicted that high workload also extended and affected family life as it was associated with negative affect at home (Ilies, Schwind, Wagner, Johnson, DeRue and Ilgen, 2007). Brosschot, Gerin and Thayer (2006) argued that consequently, employees’ psychological and physiological systems will be activated and as a result psychological detachment from work during non-work time will not occur (Sonnentag, et al., 2010). In addition, Cropley and Millward Purvis (2003) conducted a diary study of school teachers, who made hourly notes of their work-related thoughts over a work-day evening between 5 p.m. to 9p.m. Individuals who reported high workload also showed low psychological detachment during the evenings from work-related duties (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). The same finding emerged from a study conducted by Sonnentag and Bayer (2005) based on daily survey measures during three working days. The researchers reported that workload was negatively related to psychological detachment from work during off-job time. A study by Sonnentag and Kruel (2006) revealed similar results, indicating that employees who had a high workload demonstrated a low ability to mentally disengage from work during non-work time, and continued to reflect on job-related issues in the evening. In light of the previous research in the area, this study aims to further investigate the relationship between job demands and psychological detachment during non-work time. Hypothesis I: Individuals who experience high job demands will report low levels of psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
  • 13. 6 1.3.1.2. Role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. In relation to organisational context, role ambiguity is defined as a lack of clear information about a role and the role expectations (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). Thus, employees will experience role ambiguity when they do not possess necessary information about what their work objectives are and how to approach their work tasks, and when there is ambiguity about the priority of work that needs to be accomplished. Role ambiguity is a job stressor that has been studied extensively in the scientific literature, and according to the empirical research, role ambiguity has detrimental effects on both employees and organisations (Siegall, 2000). In addition, a sample of 15.256 men, who had no previous history of mental disorders completed a self-reported survey of a number of different work stressors such as: role ambiguity, workload and social support. During 5 years of follow up, the researchers kept a record of participants’ who took sick leaves due to depressive disorders, which lasted 30 days or longer. The results suggested that employees who experienced high levels of role ambiguity developed depressive disorders and as a result they took 30 or more days of sick leave (Inoue, Kawakami, Haratani, Kobayashi, Ishizaki, Hayashi, Fujota, Aizawa, Miyazaki, Hiro, Masumoto, Hashimoto and Araki, 2010). However, not many studies have tested role ambiguity in relation to psychological detachment. The studies that have looked at the correlation between role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time have indicated non-significant relationships between these variables (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006; Sonnentag and Fritz, 2007). When an individual experiences high role ambiguity, they are more likely to be mentally preoccupied about it during time off work as they will lack a clear understanding of what is expected of them and how to prioritise their job. As a result, they will not be able to psychologically detach from work during respite periods. On the other hand, individuals with low role ambiguity will have a clear understanding of their work objectives and roles and as a result they may find it easier to mentally disengage from work after work hours (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). The current study will seek to add to the existing literature by investigating the relationship between role ambiguity and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Hypothesis II: Individuals who experience high role ambiguity at work will report low levels of psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
  • 14. 7 1.3.2. Individual differences: psychological flexibility and locus of control. 1.3.2.1. Psychological flexibility. One of the individual difference variables included in the current study is psychological flexibility. According to Hayes, Strosahl, Bunting, Twohig and Wilson (2004), Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) is used in order to promote workplace mental health, and is based on mindfulness strategies that have emerged from the Cognitive Behavioural Therapy movement. ACT promotes six principles: acceptance, defusion, contact with the present moment, self-as-context, values, and commitment action. These principles are interrelated and help to enhance psychological flexibility. Psychological flexibility refers to the ability to remain focused on the present moment; being mindful and aware of thoughts and feelings and yet having the ability to pursue goals and take actions (Flaxman and Bond, 2010). It has been suggested that people who score high on psychological flexibility will be able to psychologically detach themselves from work during time off work as they will be able to focus on the present moment. Psychological flexibility has been found to act as a mediator through which Acceptance and Commitment Therapy can improve work-related stress. Fledderus, Bohlemijer and Pieterse (2010) reported that psychological flexibility acts as a mediator in the relationship between coping styles and emotional and psychological well-being (Fledderus, Bohlmeijer, Smit and Westerhof, 2010). In a study conducted by Flaxman and Bond (2010), outcomes and processes of changes had been observed across a three month period in working adults with above average levels of stress, who participated in an ACT intervention. The researchers found that participants’ levels of psychological distress decreased following the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy over the assessment period. Furthermore, mediation effect analysis indicated that ACT was an effective intervention for mental health improvement as a direct result of an increase in psychological flexibility. This is supported by Lappalainen, Lehtonen, Skarp, Taubert, Ojanen and Hayes (2007), who found that client’s levels of psychological flexibility elevated following Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Flaxman and Bond, 2010). Enhanced psychological flexibility is also related to many health benefits. Bond and Bunce (2000) conducted a study in which they compared a group of individuals who had been through an ACT intervention with a control group, and a group of individuals who received training on how to reduce stressors at their source. The researchers reported that general mental health was only improved for the employees that participated in the brief ACT intervention. Furthermore, Bond and Bunce (2003) proposed that individuals who report high levels of psychological flexibility experience better mental
  • 15. 8 health over time (Hayes and Strosahl, 2004). In an empirical literature review it has been concluded that individuals who reported high psychological flexibility also reported fewer psychological problems (Chawla and Ostafin, 2007). Moreover, Bond and Bunce (2003) found that psychological flexibility is also related to enhanced quality of life, emotional well-being, and job satisfaction (Fledderus, Bohlmeijer, Smit and Westerhof, 2010). The current study suggests that psychological flexibility is an important element in allowing individuals to be able to psychologically detach from work during off-work time. One of the main features of psychological flexibility is having contact with the present moment. In addition, psychological flexibility relates to being able to shift focus from one life domain (e.g. work) to another area of life (e.g. personal life (Kashdan and Rottenberg, 2010). Therefore, individuals who are not able to have contact with the present moment (i.e. score low in psychological flexibility) also might be unable to psychologically detach from work during off-work time. Psychological flexibility can be enhanced through Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Fledderus, 2010). This indicates that individuals’ psychological detachment from work also could be enhanced. In addition to previous research, the current study will seek to investigate the relationship between psychological flexibility and detachment from work during non-work time. Hypothesis III: Individuals who report high levels of psychological flexibility will also report high levels of psychological detachment from work during non-work time. 1.3.2.2. Locus of control. Rotter (1966) distinguished that individuals have either an internal or external locus of control. Individuals with an internal locus of control try to control the environment around them in a confident, alert and directive manner as they hold a strong belief that they have control over their life. Individuals with external locus of control, on the contrary, believe that they do not have direct control over their life and attribute outcomes to powerful others, fate, or luck (Ng, Sorensen and Eby, 2006). Locus of control is a personality variable that has been studied at length in many different settings (Spector, 1988). Judge and Bono (2001) argued that locus of control is a fundamental factor at work as it is associated with numerous important work outcomes such as job satisfaction and job performance (Ng, Sorensen and Eby, 2006). In addition, empirical research has shown that individuals who hold an internal locus of control also display higher levels of physiological and psychological well-being.
  • 16. 9 Furthermore, internals manage both everyday stress and stress at the workplace better than externals do (Spector, Cooper, Sanchez, O'Driscoll, Sparks, Bernin, Bussing, Dewe, Hart, Lu, Miller, De Moraes, Ostrognay, Pagon, Pitariu, Poelmans, Radhakrishnan, Russinova, Salamatov, Salgado, Shima, Siu, Stora, Teichmann, Theorell, Vlerick, Westman, Widerszal-Bazyl, Wong and Yu, 2002). In a recent meta-analysis, researchers looked at the relationship between locus of control and a number of different work outcomes. The results implied that locus of control was an important aspect related to well-being and job-related affective reactions (Meier, Semmer, Elfering and Jacobshagen, 2008). Kirkcaldy, Shephard and Furnham (2002) administered the pressure management indicator (PMI, which measured job satisfaction, job stressors, physical and mental health, in addition to Type A behaviour and locus of control) to a sample of 332 managers. The researchers reported that individuals with a high internal locus of control perceive lower levels of stress and higher levels of health than individuals with a high external locus of control. In addition to the findings in the scientific literature, the current study proposes that individuals who have high internal control beliefs (believe that they can control what is happening at work) will be able to psychologically detach from work during off-work time. On the contrary, individuals who have high external beliefs will be unable to psychologically detach from work during off-work time due to having beliefs that things are beyond their control. They may also worry more and think about work during respite periods. In light of the previous literature discussed, the current study will investigate the relationship between locus of control and psychological detachment. Hypothesis IV: Individuals who report an internal locus of control will also report high levels of psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
  • 17. 10 2. Method 2.1.Design. This study made use of a cross-sectional design, with four predictor variables and one criterion variable. The criterion variable in the study was psychological detachment and predictor variables included: job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control. The data collection procedure was standardized by providing all participants with the same instructions in relation to responding to the survey. The data was analysed using correlation coefficient and multiple linear regression analyses. 2.2.Participants. The sampling method used in the study was opportunity and snowball sampling. The survey was distributed via email to friends and family of the researcher, who farther distributed the survey to their work colleagues, friends and family. Their participation in the study was voluntary. A total of 202 working adults participated in the study, of whom 71 (35.1%) were males and 131 (64.9%) were females. One-hundred-and-ninety-four participants provided their age, which ranged from 18 years old to 62 years old. The mean age was 34.83 and the standard deviation was 8.54. In terms of marital status, 90 (44.6%) participants were single, divorced/ separated or widowed, whereas 111 (55%) participants were in a relationship or married, and one person (0.5%) did not provide information about their marital status. Fifty-eight (28.7%) participants reported having children, whereas 144 (71.3%) stated that they do not have children. Among the 58 participants who have children, one (0.5%) did not specify how many children they have, 21 (10.4%) participants reported having 1 child, 25 (12.4%) have 2 children, 6 (3%) participants have 3 children, 4 (2%) respondents have 4 children and one person (0.5%) has 7 children. In relation to ethnicity, the vast majority of participants, 168 (83.2%) were White, 13 (6.4%) participants were Black or African American, 5 (2.5%) participants were Hispanic or Latino, 6 (3%) were Asian/Pacific Islanders. The remaining 10 (5%) of respondents were of other ethnicity. Out of the people that participated in this study, 150 (74.3%) were full-time and 52 (325.7%) were part- time working adults. Two participants (1%) had completed GCSE/ O’level as their highest level of education, 8 (4%) completed A’level, 29 (14.4%) had a diploma, 59 (29.2%) had a bachelor’s degree, 80 (39.6%) had a master’s degree, 10 (5%) had a doctoral degree and remaining 14 (6.9%)
  • 18. 11 respondents had other qualifications. The participants of this study were working in various professions such as psychologists, midwives, teachers/ lecturers, graphic designers, sales, catering, IT, and medics. 2.3.Measures. The online questionnaire (refer to appendix 6.1) consisted of six sections gathering information about participants’ demographic characteristics and investigating the criterion variable (psychological detachment) and predictor variables (job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control). 2.3.1. Demographics. The first section consisted of 12 items regarding demographic characteristics. The participants were asked to provide the following information about themselves: age in years, gender, ethnicity, marital status, whether they have children, how many children they have, their highest level of educational attainment , working regime, how many hours they work in a typical week, and their occupation. 2.3.2. Criterion variable measure. 2.3.2.1. Psychological detachment measure. The second section was a subscale taken from The Recovery Experience Questionnaire by Sonnentag and Fritz (2007), which consisted of four items: ‘I forget about work.’, ‘I don’t think about work.’, ‘I distance myself from work.’, ‘I get a break from the demands of work.’ Participants were asked to respond to the items in relation to their free evenings (as the study looked at short respite periods) and indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with the statements. The items were rated on a 5- point Likert scale as follows: I do not agree at all, I somewhat disagree, I neither agree nor disagree, I somewhat agree, and I fully agree. In addition, the statement ‘I do not agree at all’ was scored as 1 and the statement ‘I fully agree’ was scored as 5. Thus, the possible range of scores was from 4 to 20, where low scores indicated low psychological detachment and high scores indicated high psychological detachment. The scale is a self-report measure, which has been widely used by researchers in the field
  • 19. 12 of psychological detachment. The scale is consistent and reliable as indicated by high Cronbach’s α (α = .855). 2.3.3. Predictor variables measures. 2.3.3.1. Job demands measure. The third section was a subscale taken from Karasek’s (1985) Job Demand and Control Questionnaire, which consisted of seven items. Sample items included: ‘My job requires working very fast.’, ‘My job requires working very hard.’ and ‘I am not asked to do an excessive amount of work.’ Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with the statements. The items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale as follows: Strongly Agree, Agree, Disagree, and Strongly Disagree. In addition, the statement ‘Strongly Agree’ was scored as 1, and the statement ‘Strongly Disagree’ was scored as 4. Four items- 1, 2, 6 and 7- were reverse scored. Thus, the possible range of scores was from 7 to 28. The scale is a self-report measure with high Cronbach’s α (α = .820), suggesting that the scale was consistent and reliable. 2.3.3.2. Role ambiguity measure. The fourth section included a work ambiguity subscale taken from the ‘Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations’ by Rizzo, House and Lirtzman (1970), which consisted of 10 items. The sample questions were: ‘I feel certain about how much authority I have.’, ‘I am able to act the same regardless of the group I am with.’, ‘I feel certain how I will be evaluated for a raise or promotion.’ Respondents were asked to rate how true each statement is for them. The items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale as follow: Very false, Somewhat true, Slightly false, Neither false or true, Slightly true, Somewhat true and Very true. In addition, the statement ‘Very false’ was scored as 1, and the statement ‘Very true’ was scored as 7. Thus, the possible range of scores was from 10 to 70. All 10 items were reverse scored in order for low scores to indicate low levels of role ambiguity and high scores to represent high levels of role ambiguity. The scale is a self-report measure, which is consistent and reliable as indicated by high Cronbach’s α (α = .821).
  • 20. 13 2.3.3.3. Psychological flexibility measure. The fifth section included the Work-related Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (WAAQ) by Bond, Lloyd and Guenole (in press), which measured psychological flexibility. The scale consisted of 7 items such as: ‘I am able to work effectively in spite of any personal worries that I have.’, ‘I can admit to my mistakes at work and still be successful.’, ‘I can still work very effectively, even if I am nervous about something.’ Participants were asked to rate how true each statement is for them. The items were rated on a 7-point Likert scale as follow: Never true, Very seldom true, Seldom true, Sometimes true, Frequently true, Almost always true and Always true. The statement ‘Never true’ was scored as 1, and ‘Always true’ was scored as 7. The possible range of scores was from 7 to 49, where low scores indicated low levels of psychological flexibility and high scores represented high levels of psychological flexibility. Cronbach’s α was very high (α = .911), suggesting that the scale was consistent and reliable. 2.3.3.4. Locus of control measure. The sixth and final section consisted of the Development of the Work Locus of Control Scale by Spector (1988), which included 16 items such as: ‘If employees are unhappy with a decision made by their boss, they should do something about it.’, ‘Getting the job you want is mostly a matter of luck.’, ‘Making money is primarily a matter of good fortune.’ Respondents were asked to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree with the statements. The items were rated on a 6-point Likert scale: Disagree very much, Disagree moderately, Disagree slightly Agree slightly, Agree moderately and Agree very much. The statement ‘Disagree very much’ was scored as 1, and ‘Agree very much’ was scored as 6. As indicated by Spector (1988), eight items- 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, 11, 14 and 15- were reverse scored. The possible range of scores was from 16 to 96, where low scores indicated internal locus of control and high scores indicated external locus of control. The scale is a self-report measure, which is consistent and reliable as indicated by high Cronbach’s α (α = .822). 2.4.Procedure. The data was collected from participants using an online questionnaire (refer to appendix 6.1) created on SurveyMonkey. The researcher emailed a brief description of the study with a link, which allowed participants to access the questionnaire. The survey took approximately 10 minutes to complete and
  • 21. 14 participants were asked to distribute the link further to anyone who is 18 years old and over and currently employed. Once participants opened the link, they were presented with an introduction and briefing about the study, which included information about the researcher and research supervisor, the rights of the participant, and how to contact the researcher. The next page required participants to consent to take part in the study and declare that they have read and understood the purpose of the study. Without consent, participants would be unable to proceed to the study. Respondents were also asked to write an anonymity number, which would enable the researcher to identify their responses if they wished to withdraw from the study. Once giving their consent, participants could proceed with completing the questionnaire. Before submitting their responses, participants were provided with a debriefing page, which provided more detailed information about the aim of the study, references to studies that looked at psychological detachment, and contact details if the participants had queries or if they wished to receive results from the study. The collected data was transferred to Excel format and then into SPSS and appropriate changes were made such as coding the data, reversing values and computing scores of each variable. The data was analysed using correlation coefficient and multiple linear regression analyses. 2.5.Ethics. Before the researcher was allowed to proceed with the study, the ‘Psychology Department Ethical Approval Form’ had to be completed and submitted for approval. The Departmental Ethical Committee at the Institute of Management Studies at the Goldsmith’s University of London granted the ethical approval. The BPS (British Psychology Society) guidelines were followed throughout the study. All participants were presented with information about the aim of the study and what would be expected of them; they were told that it would take approximately 10 minutes to complete the survey. Moreover, participants were ensured about the confidentiality of the data they provided by explaining that the data will be reported in aggregate format, questionnaires would be concealed, and no one other than the researcher would have access to the data. Furthermore, respondents were informed that participation in the study was entirely voluntary and that they were free to withdraw from the study at any time and for any reason, and they do not need to disclose this reason. Participants were asked to provide an anonymity number, in order to enable the
  • 22. 15 researcher to identify their responses should they wish to withdraw from the study. They were also provided with the email address of the researcher in case they required more information about the study and/or they wished to withdraw after completing the survey. Participants were unable to proceed to the questionnaire without providing their consent to participate in the study. Thus, they were required to tick a box next to the following statement ‘I confirm that I have read and understand the purpose of this study and I give my consent to participate in the research study.’ On the completion of the questionnaire participants were presented with a debriefing page. It included more detailed information about the research, references of studies that looked at psychological detachment and the email address to the researcher.
  • 23. 16 3. Results 3.1.Data cleaning and screening. Once the data from SurveyMonkey was transferred to SPSS, participants’ responses were recorded into numerical equivalent of their answers. Missing values were then recorded into SPSS into system missing and missing observations were assumed to be missing completely at random (MCAR) as Little’s MCAR test was non-significant. In addition to this result, the ‘Missing Value Analysis’ was used in order to replace the 32 missing values. According to Field (2005) this method is better than using the mean. The next step was to reverse scored four items- 1,2, 6,7- from the job demand scale, all of the 10 items from the role ambiguity measure and eight items- 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, 11, 14 and 15- from the locus of control measure. Lastly, the scores for the criterion variable and for each of the predictor variables were added by using the ‘compute’ command. Following that, the data was explored and analysed. 3.2.Descriptive Statistics. Table 1. Means, standard deviation and Cronbach’s alpha for the criterion variable (psychological detachment) and the predictor variables (job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control). (N=202). M ± SD Cronbach’s alpha Psychological Detachment 12.15 ± 4.35 .85 Job Demand 19.12 ± 3.53 .82 Role Ambiguity 28.45 ± 9.52 .82 Psychological Flexibility 34.84 ± 7.66 .91 Locus of Control 43.18 ± 10.00 .82 The table 1 represents Cronbach’s alpha values for each of the scale. The rule of thumb is that the scale should have a minimum Cronbach’s alpha value of .7 to be considered consistent and reliable (Brane, Kemp and Snelgar, 2009). In addition to this, all the scales showed internal reliability with Cronbach’s α= .85 for psychological detachment, Cronbach’s α=.82 for job demand, role ambiguity and locus of control and Cronbach’s α= .91 for locus of control. The descriptive statistics of the criterion variable suggest that participants reported slightly above moderate levels of psychological detachment (12.15 out of possible score of 20). Furthermore, the
  • 24. 17 results of predictor variables were as follow. In addition to job stressors, the results indicate that participants scored slightly above moderate levels of job demand (19.12 out of possible score of 28) and low scores of role ambiguity (28.45 out of possible score of 70). In relation to individual differences participants reported high scores in psychological flexibility (34.84 out of possible score of 49) and slightly below moderate levels of locus of control (43.18 out of possible score of 96). 3.3.Pearson’s correlation coefficient. The bivariate correlations between the criterion variable and the predictor variables are presented in table 2. According to the bivariate correlations, the criterion variable, psychological detachment, had significant negative correlation with job demand (r = -.376, p < 0.001) and significant negative relationship with role ambiguity (r = -.259, p < 0.001). Furthermore, according to the bivariate correlations, the criterion variable did not have significant correlation with psychological flexibility (r = .080, p= 0.260) and with locus of control (r = -.044, p= 0.531). Furthermore, the table 2 represents significant correlations, which were not hypothesized in the study. Job demand had positive significant relationship with role ambiguity (r = .337, p < 0.001). Moreover, role ambiguity showed negative significant relationship with psychological flexibility (r = -.383, p < 0.001) and positive significant relationship with locus of control (r = .344, p < 0.001). Also, psychological flexibility had negative significant relationship with locus of control (r = -.235, p < 0.001). Table 2. The significance and correlation coefficient values of the bivariate correlations entered to the model (Pearson’s r). N=202 2 3 4 5 1 Psychological detachment -.376** -.259** .080 -.044 2 Job demand .337** -.134 .108 3 Role ambiguity -.383** .344** 4 Psychological flexibility -.235** 5 Locus of Control *p < .05 ; **p < .001
  • 25. 18 3.4.Multiple Regression. The data were analysed using the ‘Enter’ method and a multiple regression analysis was performed on the raw data. The results indicate that the overall model was significant (F= 9.61, df= 4, p< 0.001). The model explained 14.6% of the variance in psychological detachment from work during non-work time (Adjusted R²= .146). The information for the strength of the predictor variables entered into the model is provided in table 3. Table 3. The unstandardized and standardised regression coefficient for the variables entered into the model. Model B SE B β Job demand -.399 .085 -.325** Role ambiguity -.079 .035 -.173 Psychological flexibility -.011 .040 -.019 Locus of Control .020 .030 .046 *p < .05 ; **p < .001 Accordingly, job demand (β = -.325, p < 0.001) was significant predictor of psychological detachment from work during non-work time as the predictor variable had the greatest beta loading and the highest significance. The other variables, psychological flexibility and locus of control did not emerge as significant predictors. Furthermore, the table 3 shows that role ambiguity lost significance. The hypothesis was rejected. 3.4.1. Collinearity diagnostics. Collinearity diagnostics were requested in order to investigate the assumption of no multicollinearity. The tolerance values indicated that correlation between predictor variables, job demand, role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control, varied between 0 and 1 indicating independence of variance of each variable. Furthermore, variance inflation factor (VIF) was not greater than 10 and the average VIF was not substantially greater than 1 indicating that the regression was not biased by multicollinearity (Field, 2005). Taking all into account assumption of non-multicollinearity was met.
  • 26. 19 3.5.Conclusion. To conclude, a multiple regression analysis was performed between psychological detachment as the criterion variable and four predictor variables: job stressors including job demand and role ambiguity, and individual differences including psychological flexibility and locus of control. Using the ‘Enter’ method, a significant model emerged. The model was of weak strength explaining 14.6% of the variance. Furthermore, the only significant predictor of psychological detachment from work during non- work time was job demand. Moreover, bivariate correlations revealed that that there was a significant correlation between psychological detachment and role ambiguity. However, under multiple regression analysis role ambiguity became non-significant predictor of psychological detachment. Also, the predictor variables correlated with each other. The results indicated significant correlation between job demand and role ambiguity. Role ambiguity showed significant correlation with psychological flexibility and locus of control. Lastly, there was significant correlation between psychological flexibility and locus of control.
  • 27. 20 4. Discussion The purpose of the study was to investigate work stressors (job demand and role ambiguity) and individual differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control) in order to explain which factors hinder or support psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). In addition to the literature review, there were four proposed hypotheses. Job demand, role ambiguity and locus of control were hypothesised to be negatively associated with psychological detachment from work during non-work time, whereas psychological flexibility was hypothesised to be a positive predictor. Following data exploration and analysis, the results revealed that job demand was the only significant predictor of psychological detachment. The remaining predictor variables (role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control) were non- significant predictors of psychological detachment from work. The relationship between the criterion variable (psychological detachment from work during non-work time) and the significant predictor variable, job demand, will now be explored further. Thereafter, the results of the non-significant predictor variables (role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control) will be discussed. This will be followed by reporting other significant findings in the study. The limitations and strengths of the current study will then be deliberated, before providing recommendations for future research and practical implications of the current study. 4.1.Job demand and psychological detachment from work during non-work time. With respect to job demand, the job stressor has been found to be a significant negative predictor of psychological detachment from work during non-work time. This result was expected and is in line with previous research that revealed that employees who experience high job demands will find it difficult to mentally detach from work during non-work time (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005; Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006; Sonnentag, et al., 2010). Indeed, individuals show low psychological detachment from work when they ponder upon and anticipate job demands (i.e. chronic time pressure), which are going to continue in the following days (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005). In addition, McEwen (1998) explained that employees, who have been exposed to high job demands during the day, will have activated both their physical and psychological systems in order to collect energy, which will be used to deal with the demands. However, individuals will not be able to recover when these systems are still activated during non-work time, in which they do not have to deal with these job demands (Sonnentag, Binnewies and
  • 28. 21 Mojza, 2010). Thus, it is plausible to suggest that nowadays, employees take a lot of work home with them in order to complete tasks or prepare for the next working day during non-work time. Additionally, they will be preoccupied with job demands physically and/or psychologically, thus, making it less likely that they will psychologically unwind from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). 4.2.Non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Analyses showed that role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant predictors of psychological detachment. In relation to job stressors, the results indicate that the model revealed a complex relationship with role ambiguity. Under bivariate correlation, role ambiguity had significant negative relationship with psychological detachment. This suggests that individuals who reported high levels of role ambiguity also showed lower levels of psychological detachment from work during off-job time. However, the variable became non-significant under the multiple regression analysis. The results imply that job demand is more important in relation to being able to mentally detach from work during non-work time than role ambiguity, which is in line with previous research (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). In addition to that, there are a number of reasons why job demand has been found to be a significant predictor of low psychological detachment, whereas role ambiguity has not. Firstly, participants have scored higher on job demand compared to role ambiguity. This might indicate that job demands are perceived as more stressful for employees than ambiguous roles are. Also, individuals might be preoccupied with work related tasks during non-work time when faced with a high workload rather than when faced with role ambiguity (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). In relation to individual differences, both psychological flexibility and locus of control have been found to be non-significant predictors of psychological detachment from work. These two variables, however, were correlated with role ambiguity and also indicated a relationship with each other. An explanation as to why these individual differences did not emerge as significant predictors of the criterion variable might simply be that psychological flexibility and locus of control do not affect psychological detachment from work during off-job time. However, previous research into psychological detachment did not investigate the relationship between psychological flexibility and locus of control. To the researchers’ knowledge, this is the first study that investigated these individual differences in relation to psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evenings). These were of particular interest because these specific individual differences can be enhanced.
  • 29. 22 4.3.Additional significant results. The results from the correlation coefficients showed that role ambiguity had significant negative relationship with psychological detachment and psychological flexibility; and significant positive relationship with job demand and locus of control. The results also showed that psychological flexibility had significant negative relationship with locus of control. Individuals who reported having ambiguous roles also reported low psychological flexibility and an external locus of control. This implies that individuals who do not have a clear understanding of their job responsibilities are also less likely to be able to remain focused on the present moment, are unable to be mindful of their thoughts and feelings, and still be able to pursue goals and take actions. Furthermore, individuals who experience high levels of role ambiguity are more likely to believe that they do not have direct control over the environment around them. The correlation coefficients results further showed that individuals who experience high role ambiguity reported high levels of job demand, which might suggest that ambiguous roles are perceived as more demanding. Moreover, the individual differences showed significant negative relationships with each other. Thus, individuals who reported high levels of psychological flexibility also reported having an internal locus of control; suggesting that those who are likely to be able to remain focused at the present moment, be mindful of their thoughts and feelings, and still be able to pursue goals and take actions, also have a strong belief that they can control the environment around them. 4.4.Strengths of the current study. The study looked at psychological detachment, which is a new phenomenon in the scientific literature. Previous research looked at job stressors, workload and role ambiguity (Sonnentag and Bayer, 2005; Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006) and individual differences such as personality (Sonnentag and Fritz, 2007) in relation to psychological detachment from work during non-work time. However, the current study looked at individual differences including psychological flexibility and locus of control, which can be enhanced, unlike personality. In addition, this could have important implications for both individuals and organisations in terms of improving employees psychological detachment. The results indicated that psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant predictors of psychological detachment, yet, the correlation coefficients results indicated some relationships.
  • 30. 23 The sample in the study included employees from a wide range of professions such as: midwifes, psychologists, teachers, medics, individuals working in retail, catering, IT, etc. This indicates that the findings can be generalized to the group of working adults, rather than only a specific group. 4.5.Limitations of the current study. Conclusions about causality cannot be drawn due to use of the multiple regression method. Thus, a significant outcome does not imply causation or direction. In addition, it can be concluded that, when employees experience high job demand their psychological detachment from work during off-job time decreases. However, it cannot be said that low psychological detachment is caused by high job demands. In terms of directing future research, this limitation can be used to inform future studies, where other methodologies such as experimental designs may be used in order to confirm the causality and direction of this finding. Self-report measures have been used in the study. Consequently, there is a lack of a comprehensive answer as to whether high job demands or individuals’ perception of high job demands is associated with low psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Future research should consider using objective measures in order to clarify this (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). 4.6.Recommendations for future research. The current study controlled for demographic characteristics such as marital status, whether participants had children, and if so how many. However, future research should look into other factors related to employees’ personal life, which might serve as a distraction and help employees to detach from work-related issues during short respite periods. Sonnentag and Kruel (2006) suggested that positive (i.e. pleasant evening) as well as negative (i.e. argument with a partner) experiences at home might help individuals to mentally unwind from work during off-job time. It is suggested that the importance of both of the individual differences in relation to psychological detachment from work during non-work time is reinvestigated. The researcher proposes that mainly it is important to look at psychological flexibility as the particular individual difference can be enhanced through Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT). For instance, it would be recommended that a study with a control group and a group of employees who will receive ACT, which enhances psychological flexibility, be conducted in order to investigate whether employees, who receive ACT will
  • 31. 24 also report higher levels of psychological detachment following the session. Additionally, psychological detachment should be measured before and after the intervention, and once more a few weeks later in order to investigate how long the effects last. A further recommendation is to investigate whether psychological flexibility and/ or locus of control serve as moderators between high job demands and psychological detachment. 4.7.Implications of the current study. As established by the current study and previous research, job demands play a major role in employees’ ability to mentally detach from work-related issues during short respite periods (i.e. evening). In order to improve employees’ psychological detachment from work during off-job time it is essential for organisations to address the issue of high job demands and introduce efficient time management interventions. This can be done by teaching employees how to more efficiently manage their time at work. In addition, it is proposed that time management interventions be implemented in the workplace. A review of time management literature suggested that there are a number of benefits of implementing such an intervention. The research indicated that following a time management intervention, employees learned how to manage their time more effectively, worried less, and reduced procrastination (Claessens, Van Eerde, Rutte and Roe, 2007). Consequently, employees should complete their work- related tasks in a shorter time frame, thus allowing them to mentally unwind from these demands during non-work time (Sonnentag and Kruel, 2006). 4.8.Conclusion. The purpose of the study was to investigate whether work stressors (job demand and role ambiguity) and individual differences (psychological flexibility and locus of control) hinder or support psychological detachment from work during short respite periods (i.e. evening). Job demand was found to be a significant negative predictor of psychological detachment from work during non-work time, whereas role ambiguity, psychological flexibility and locus of control were non-significant predictors of the criterion variable. However, the correlation coefficients results suggested that psychological detachment from work had a significant negative relationship with both of the job stressors. Moreover, role ambiguity had significant positive relationship with job demand and locus of control and a
  • 32. 25 significant negative relationship with psychological flexibility. Furthermore, the individual differences showed a significant negative relationship with each other. It was the first study that looked at psychological flexibility and locus of control in relation to psychological detachment. These particular individual differences were included as they can be enhanced, which could have important implications for both individuals and organisations in terms of improving employees psychological detachment from work during non-work time. Even though, they were non-significant, the correlation coefficients results indicated some relationships. In addition, future research should consider reinvestigating the importance of both of the individual differences in relation to psychological detachment from work during non-work time.
  • 33. 26 5. References: Bond, F.W. & Bunce, D. (2003) cited in Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit, F., & Westerhof, G. J. (2010). Mental health promotion as a new goal in public mental health care: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention enhancing psychological flexibility. Journal Information, 100(12). Bond, F. W. & Bunce, D. (2000) cited in Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New York: KA/PP. Bond, F. W. & Bunce, D. (2003) cited in Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New York: KA/PP. Bond, F. W., Lloyd, J., & Guenole, N. (in press). The work‐related acceptance and action questionnaire: initial psychometric findings and their implications for measuring psychological flexibility in specific contexts. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology. Brane, N.Kemp, R. & Snelgar, R. (2009). SPSS for Psychologists (4th ed.). Palgrave Macmillan. Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006) cited in Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365. Brosschot, J.F., Gerrin, W. & Thayer, J.F. (2006) cited in White, E. (2010). Helping to promote psychological well-being at work: the role of work engagement, work stress and psychological detachment using the job demand-resources model. The Plymouth Student Scientist, 4(2), 155-180. Brosschot, J. F., Pieper, S., & Thayer, J. F. (2005). Expanding stress theory: prolonged activation and perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 1043-1049. Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(9), 871-890. Claessens, B. J., Van Eerde, W., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A. (2007). A review of the time management literature. Personnel Review, 36(2), 255-276. Cropley, M. & Millward Purvis, L.J. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment from work during off-job time: The role of job stressors, job involvement, and recovery-related self-efficacy. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(2), 197-217. Davidson, O. B., Eden, D., Westman, M., Cohen-Charash, Y., Hammer, L. B., Kluger, A. N., Krausz, M., Maslach, C., O’Driscoll, M., Perrewé, P. L., Quick, J. C., Rosenblatt, Z., & Spector, P. E. (2010). Sabbatical leave: who gains and how much?. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 953.
  • 34. 27 De Lange, A. H., Taris, T. W., Kompier, M. A., Houtman, I. L., & Bongers, P. M. (2004). The relationships between work characteristics and mental health: examining normal, reversed and reciprocal relationships in a 4-wave study. Work & Stress, 18(2), 149-166. Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors and burnout: Reserve service as a respite. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(4), 577-585. Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (2nd ed.). London:SAGE. Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and commitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8), 816-820. Fledderus, M., Bohlemijer, E.T. & Pieterse, M.E. (2010) cited in Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit, F., & Westerhof, G. J. (2010). Mental health promotion as a new goal in public mental health care: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention enhancing psychological flexibility. Journal Information, 100(12). Fledderus, M., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Smit, F., & Westerhof, G. J. (2010). Mental health promotion as a new goal in public mental health care: A randomized controlled trial of an intervention enhancing psychological flexibility. Journal Information, 100(12). Ganster, D.C., Fox, M.L., & Dweyer, M.J. (2001) cited in Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of psychological detachment from work during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10(4), 393. Geurts, S. A. E., Kompier, M. A. J., Roxburgh, S., & Houtman, I. L. D. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of psychological detachment from work during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10(4), 393. Hayes, S.C. & Strosahl, K.D. (2004). A Clinician’s Guide to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. New York: KA/PP. Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K.D., Bunting, K., Twohig, M. & Wilson, K. (2004) cited in Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and commitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8), 816-820. Ilies, R., Schwind, K. M., Wagner, D. T., Johnson, M. D., DeRue, D. S., & Ilgen, D. R. (2007). When can employees have a family life? The effects of daily workload and affect on work-family conflict and social behavior at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1368–1379. Inoue, A., Kawakami, N., Haratani, T., Kobayashi, F., Ishizaki, M., Hayashi, T., Fujota, O., Aizawa, Y., Miyazaki, S., Hiro, H., Masumoto, T., Hashimoto & Araki, S. (2010). Job stressors and long term sick leave due to depressive disorders among Japanese male employees: findings from the Japan Work Stress and Health Cohort study. Journal of epidemiology and community health, 64(3), 229-235.
  • 35. 28 Judge, T.A. & Bono, J.E. (2001) cited in Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087. Karasek R. (1985). Job Content Instrument Questionnaire and User’s Guide, Version 1.1. Los Angeles: Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering, University of Southern California. Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical psychology review, 30(7), 865-878. Kirkcaldy, B. D., Shephard, R. J., & Furnham, A. F. (2002). The influence of type A behaviour and locus of control upon job satisfaction and occupational health. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(8), 1361-1371. Lappalainen, R., Lehtonen, T., Skarp, E., Taubert, E., Ojanen, M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007) cited in Fla man, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance and co mitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behaviour research and therapy, 48(8), 816-820. Lavoie-Tremblay, M., Wright, D., Desforges, N., Gélinas, C., Marchionni, C. & Drevniok, U. (2008). Creating a Healthy Workplacefor New-Generation Nurses. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 40(3), 290-297. Lu, L. (1999). Work Motivation, Job Stress and Employees’ Well-being. Journal of Applied Management Studies, 8(1), 61-72. McEwen, B.S. (1998) cited in Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and engaged when demands are high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 965. Meier, L. L., Semmer, N. K., Elfering, A., & Jacobshagen, N. (2008). The double meaning of control: Three-way interactions between internal resources, job control, and stressors at work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,13(3), 244. Meijman, T.F. & Mulder, G. (1998) cited in Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and engaged when demands are high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 965. Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087. Rizzo, J.R, House, R.J. & Lirtzman, S,I. (1970). Role conflict and ambiguity in complex organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 15(2), 150-163. Rothbard, N. P., & Wilk, S. L. (2011). Waking up on the right or wrong side of the bed: Start-of-workday mood, work events, employee affect, and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 54(5), 959-980.
  • 36. 29 Rotter, J.B. (1966) cited in Ng, T. W., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057-1087. Siegall, M. (2000). Putting the stress back into role stress: Improving the measurement of role conflict and role ambiguity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 15(5), 427-435. Sonnentag, S. (2003). Recovery, work engagement, and proactive behaviour: a new look at the interface between nonwork and work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 518. Sonnentag, S. (2012). Psychological Detachment From Work During Leisure Time The Benefits of Mentally Disengaging From Work. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 114-118. Sonnentag, S., & Bayer, U. V. (2005). Switching off mentally: Predictors and consequences of psychological detachment from work during off-job time.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10(4), 393. Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2008). " Did you have a nice evening?" A day-level study on recovery experiences, sleep, and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(3), 674. Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and engaged when demands are high: the role of psychological detachment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 965. Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The recovery experience questionnaire: Development and validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(3), 204. Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment from work during off-job time: The role of job stressors, job involvement, and recovery-related self-efficacy. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(2), 197-217. Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365. Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010) cited in Sonnentag, S. (2012). Psychological Detachment From Work During Leisure Time The Benefits of Mentally Disengaging From Work. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 114-118. Sonnentag, S., Mojza, E. J., Binnewies, C., & Scholl, A. (2008). Being engaged at work and detached at home: A week-level study on work engagement, psychological detachment, and affect. Work & Stress, 22(3), 257-276. Spector, P. E. (1988). Development of the work locus of control scale. Journal of occupational psychology, 61(4), 335-340. Spector, P. E., Cooper, C. L., Sanchez, J. I., O'Driscoll, M., Sparks, K., Bernin, P., Bussing, A., Dewe, P., Hart, P., Lu, L., Miller, K., De Moraes, L.R., Ostrognay, G.M., Pagon, M., Pitariu, H.D., Poelmans, S.A.Y., Radhakrishnan, P., Russinova, V., Salamatov, V., Salgado, J.F., Shima, S.,
  • 37. 30 Siu, O., Stora, J.B., Teichmann, M., Theorell, T., Vlerick, P., Westman, M., Widerszal-Bazyl, M., Wong, P.T.P. & Yu, S. (2002). Locus of control and well-being at work: how generalizable are western findings?. Academy of Management Journal, 45(2), 453-466. Westman & Eden (1997) cited in Etzion, D., Eden, D., & Lapidot, Y. (1998). Relief from job stressors and burnout: Reserve service as a respite. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(4), 577-585. White, E. (2010). Helping to promote psychological well-being at work: the role of work engagement, work stress and psychological detachment using the job demand-resources model. The Plymouth Student Scientist, 4(2), 155-180. Zohar, D., Tzischinski, O. & Epstein, R. (2003) cited in Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(3), 355-365. 6. Appendices 6.1.The online survey.