SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 46
Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Participation
After Stroke
Leeanne M. Carey, Thomas A. Matyas, Carolyn Baum
Leeanne M. Carey, PhD, BAppSc(OT), FAOTA,
FOTARA, is Professor of Occupational Therapy,
Discipline Lead, Occupational Therapy, School of Allied
Health, College of Science, Health, and Engineering, La
Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, and
Head, Neurorehabilitation and Recovery, Stroke Division,
Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health,
Heidelberg, Victoria, Australia; [email protected]
Thomas A. Matyas, PhD, is Adjunct Professor, School
of Allied Health and School of Psychology and Public
Health, College of Science, Health, and Engineering, La
Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, and
Honorary Professorial Fellow, Neurorehabilitation and
Recovery, Stroke Division, Florey Institute of Neuroscience
and Mental Health, Heidelberg, Victoria, Australia.
Carolyn Baum, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, is Professor,
Occupational Therapy and Neurology and Social Work,
Elias Michael Director, Program in Occupational Therapy,
Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO.
OBJECTIVE. Our objective was to determine the effect of loss
of body sensation on activity participation in
stroke survivors.
METHOD. Participants (N 5 268) were assessed at hospital
admission for somatosensory and motor
impairment using the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale.
Participation was assessed using the
Activity Card Sort (ACS) in the postacute phase. Between-
group differences in activity participation were
analyzed for participants with and without somatosensory
impairment and with or without paresis.
RESULTS. Somatosensory impairment was experienced in
33.6% of the sample and paresis in 42.9%. ACS
profiles were obtained at a median of 222 days poststroke.
Somatosensory loss alone (z 5 1.96, p 5 .048) and
paresis in upper and lower limbs without sensory loss (z 5 4.62,
p < .001) influenced activity participation.
CONCLUSION. Somatosensory impairment is associated with
reduced activity participation; however,
paresis of upper and lower limbs can mask the contribution of
sensory loss.
Carey, L. M., Matyas, T. A., & Baum, C. (2018). Effects of
somatosensory impairment on participation after stroke.
American
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 72, 7203205100.
https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2018.025114
One in 2 people experience loss of body sensation, or
somatosensation, afterstroke (Carey, 2012; Carey & Matyas,
2011; Winward, Halligan, & Wade,
2002). They typically have difficulty sensing touch, pressure,
and temperature;
perceiving own limb position; discriminating textures; and
recognizing objects
through the sense of touch (Carey, 1995). They may also
experience reduced or
altered pain sensation (Klit, Finnerup, Andersen, & Jensen,
2011). People who
experience somatosensory loss after stroke report problems such
as communi-
cating through a handshake, using utensils, dressing, and
holding objects
without crushing or dropping them. They also report problems
with personal,
family, and work roles. Despite the high prevalence and
apparent importance of
somatosensation in daily activities, there is limited empirical
evidence to
quantify the presence and nature of a relationship between
impaired somato-
sensation and participation in daily activities, as highlighted in
two recent re-
views (Carey, Lamp, & Turville, 2016; Meyer, Karttunen, Thijs,
Feys, &
Verheyden, 2014). The impact of somatosensory loss on
participation is
therefore critical to address to understand the factors that affect
participation
and to improve client-centered care planning.
Somatosensory impairment after stroke has been associated with
impaired
daily actions and activities, according to clinical and laboratory
studies (Borstad
& Nichols-Larsen, 2014; Carey et al., 2016; Kessner, Bingel, &
Thomalla,
2016; Meyer et al., 2014). Somatosensory loss reduces control
of hand
movements (Jeannerod, Michel, & Prablanc, 1984) and of the
fundamental
pinch-grip-lift-and-hold task (Blennerhassett, Matyas, & Carey,
2007) and
limits grip force control during object manipulation (Nowak &
Hermsdörfer,
2005). A relationship has also been demonstrated between
clinical measures of
7203205100p1 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3
mailto:[email protected]
https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2018.025114
hand function and dexterity (Blennerhassett, Carey, &
Matyas, 2008; Kong, Chua, & Lee, 2011; Park, Wolf,
Blanton, Winstein, & Nichols-Larsen, 2008).
Studies have demonstrated that the affected limb may
not be used spontaneously, despite adequate movement
abilities. This lack of use may contribute to a learned nonuse
of the limb and to further deterioration of motor function
after stroke (Carey, 1995; Dannenbaum & Dykes, 1988).
Moreover, stroke survivors have expressed uncertainty about
whether to use their sensory-impaired arm for task perfor-
mance (Connell, McMahon, & Adams, 2013). A predictive
relationship between somatosensory loss and independence
in activities of daily living (ADLs) and mobility has also
been demonstrated, for example, in a prospective sample of
102 stroke survivors (Tyson, Hanley, Chillala, Selley, &
Tallis, 2008) and using a life table analysis (N 5 95; Reding
& Potes, 1988). It is therefore plausible that sensory im-
pairment may also be associated with reduced participation
in daily activities and life roles; yet our understanding of its
impact on participation is limited.
The International Classification of Functioning, Dis-
ability and Health (ICF) defines participation as “a per-
son’s involvement in a life situation,” for example, in
leisure, social, and work activities (World Health Orga-
nization, 2001, p. 10). A major knowledge gap about the
impact of somatosensory impairment after stroke on par-
ticipation in life activities has been recently identified in a
scoping review (Carey et al., 2016) and in a systematic re-
view (Meyer et al., 2014). Hill, Fisher, Schmid, Crabtree,
and Page (2014) reported a good to excellent relationship
between performance of and satisfaction with valued ac-
tivities, assessed using the Canadian Occupational Per-
formance Measure (Law et al., 2014), and intact sensation
of the hand in 50 stroke survivors. Desrosiers, Noreau,
Rochette, Bravo, and Boutin (2002) found a low but
significant association between light touch of the upper
limb and proprioception assessed at discharge and ADLs
and social roles assessed 6 mo later (N 5 102).
Somatosensory impairment is commonly experienced
in association with motor impairment, and degree of
weakness has been significantly associated with sensory
impairment (Tyson et al., 2008). Upper limb motor
impairment is reported in 77% of stroke survivors
(Lawrence et al., 2001) and is likely to have an impact on
participation. Paresis may affect participation in its own
right and in addition to somatosensory impairment. We
therefore investigated the impact of somatosensory impair-
ment on participation in stroke survivors with and without
motor impairment. This approach is important because re-
habilitation traditionally targets motor problems, often
without attending to somatosensory deficits known to affect
performance (Carey, 2015; Kalra, 2010), despite the avail-
ability of interventions to remediate somatosensory deficits
(Carey et al., 2016; Carey, Macdonell, & Matyas, 2011).
Given the high prevalence of somatosensory loss in
stroke survivors and evidence of its relationship with daily
actions and activities, we investigated the effect of so-
matosensory impairment on activity participation in a
longitudinal stroke cohort. We were particularly interested
to learn whether the presence of sensory impairment soon
after stroke affected the number and type of activities
(instrumental, leisure, social) in which survivors retained
participation after they had returned home and completed
rehabilitation (at least 3 mo after stroke). Because sensory
impairment is often experienced concurrently with motor
impairment, which may have an effect on participation,
we investigated the impact of both motor and somato-
sensory impairment on activity participation in our analyses.
Specifically, our objectives were as follows:
• Characterize activity participation outcomes of stroke
survivors with and without somatosensory impairment
after stroke
• Determine whether sensory impairment, identified
during the acute stroke phase (at or within a few days
of hospital admission after stroke) using the National
Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS; Brott et al.,
1989), predicts activity participation in the postacute
phase, that is, after the survivor has returned home and
completed rehabilitation
• Determine the effects of somatosensory loss on partic-
ipation in stroke survivors with or without paresis
• Explore the effects of somatosensory loss on participation in
stroke survivors with and without paresis across the do-
mains of the Activity Card Sort (ACS; Baum & Edwards,
2001).
Method
Participants
The sample comprised a subset of participants from an
existing large stroke cohort (Wolf, Baum, & Connor, 2009).
Inclusion criteria were first clinical stroke; no history of
schizophrenia or dementia; and availability of required data,
which included the acute NIHSS individual item scores,
obtained on hospital admission, and the ACS construct
scores, collected after the participant had returned home
and completed rehabilitation.
A sample of 268 cases was obtained from a search
sample of 375 after an examination of missing data,
aphasia, neglect, pattern of paresis, and latency of the ACS
test. Cases with severe aphasia or neglect, defined as scores
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p2
greater than 1 on the relevant NIHSS scales, were excluded
because these conditions have the potential to confound
sensory deficit test results. Cases in which the ACS was
performed before 92 days poststroke were excluded. Because
sensory impairment of the upper limb is most likely associated
with activity participation restrictions, based on prior literature
(Desrosiers et al., 2002; Hill et al., 2014), and no separate
sensory assessment data were available for the lower limb, 36
cases with paresis in only the lower limb were also excluded.
Measures
The primary outcome was percentage of retained activity
participation, measured using the ACS. The ACS is a
reliable and valid instrument (Edwards, Hahn, Baum, &
Dromerick, 2006; Everard, Lach, Fisher, & Baum, 2000;
Tse, Douglas, Lentin, & Carey, 2013). A recent review
indicated that the ACS covers the most domains of the
ICF Activities and Participation domains, relative to
other measures, and meets the most psychometric prop-
erties (Tse et al., 2013). The version used to collect the
data in a telephone interview was the 55-item measure.
Participants reported on instrumental activities (e.g.,
shopping, household maintenance), social activities (e.g.,
visiting, studying), and high-demand (e.g., swimming,
running) and low-demand (e.g., reading, table games)
physical leisure activities. For each activity, participants re-
sponded to the following descriptors: never done, given up
due to stroke, doing less often due to stroke, and doing now.
The previous activity score was calculated by adding the
doing now, doing less, and given up scores. The current
activity score was the total of the doing now and doing less
scores. The retained participation score was obtained by
dividing the sum total of current activities by the total of
previous activities. Higher scores indicate higher retention
of prestroke activity. The primary score was the percentage
of retained overall activity participation, with additional
scores for the domains of Instrumental Activities, Social
Activities, High-Demand Leisure Activities, and Low-Demand
Leisure Activities.
The presence or absence of somatosensory impair-
ment and motor impairment was assessed by trained
nurses within a couple of days of hospital admission using
the NIHSS. The NIHSS is widely used early poststroke to
quantify the neurological impairment caused by stroke. It has
been repeatedly validated as a tool for assessing stroke severity
and as an excellent predictor for patient outcomes (Muir,
Weir, Murray, Povey, & Lees, 1996). Only somatosensory
loss attributed to stroke is scored as abnormal, and the ex-
aminer is required to test as many body areas (arms [not
hands], legs, trunk, face) as needed to accurately check for
hemisensory loss. Sensory loss is scored as 0 5 normal; no
sensory loss, 1 5 mild to moderate sensory loss, and 2 5 severe
to total sensory loss. Motor impairment is tested separately
for the arm and leg and is scored from 0 (no drift; limb
holds 90 [or 45] degrees for full 10 s) to 4 (no movement).
Procedure
Participants (N 5 268) were assessed at admission for
somatosensory and motor impairment using the NIHSS.
The activity participation profile was assessed using the
ACS at least 3 mo poststroke.
Data Analysis
The NIHSS somatosensory loss variable was dichotomized
(unimpaired, impaired). Because paresis is known to have
a major impact on functionality and may interact with
somatosensation, particularly in upper limb function, the
sample was also split according to presence and distribution
of paresis. Degree of paresis was dichotomized (unimpaired,
impaired). The ACS scores were obtained in the postacute
phase, that is, after participants had completed rehabili-
tation, returned home, and were experiencing the impact of
stroke on their activity participation. If participants were
tested on the ACS on more than one occasion, we selected
the ACS scores closest to 182 days (6 mo) poststroke for
analysis. Analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics
(Version 23; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).
Between-group differences in participation were
tested for participants with and without somatosensory
impairment and with or without paresis. The primary
analysis focused on the total retained activity participation
scores. A set of five pairwise comparisons was planned to
evaluate contrasts of interest across the six independent
groups (2 levels of sensory loss · 3 levels of paresis). A
primary family of three comparisons investigated the
impact of sensory loss in samples with different paresis
patterns (none, upper limb only, both upper and lower
limbs). To provide an interpretative framework for the
somatosensory loss effects, we used a secondary family of
two comparisons to investigate the effects of each of the
two paresis patterns in samples without sensory loss. The
ACS percentage of retained activity score was the out-
come variable. Because the total activity score is the total
of the four activity domains (Instrumental Activities,
Social Activities, High-Demand Leisure Activities, and
Low-Demand Leisure Activities), each of the four do-
mains of the ACS was used in exploratory secondary
analyses. The same set of comparisons as for the total
ACS retained activity score was used for each of these
secondary analyses.
7203205100p3 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3
Results
The 268 cases included yielded a sample with a gender
distribution that slightly favored women (53.5%). Age
was normally distributed with a mean (M) of 62.8 yr and
a standard deviation (SD) of 13.1 yr. There were 239
right dominant cases, 16 left dominant, and 3 ambi-
dextrous, with the remainder unknown. The median
NIHSS score for the sample was 3.00, with an
interquartile range of 1.00–5.00 (5th percentile 5 0.00;
95th percentile 5 9.95; range 5 0–15). The mean score
was 3.71 (SD 5 3.11). The distribution of somato-
sensory impairment and motor impairment in the
sample is summarized in Table 1. Activity participation
was measured at a median of 222 days poststroke. A
skewed distribution (M 5 279; median 5 222;
interquartile range 5 184–303 days) with wide dispersion
and long tail was noted for the latency of the ACS per-
formed closest to 182 days after admission.
Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Overall
Retained Activity Participation
Activity Participation Outcome With and Without
Somatosensory Impairment. Stroke survivors with somato-
sensory loss and no paresis showed a significantly reduced
percentage of retained activity participation compared with
those with no sensory loss (z 5 1.96, p 5 .048, two-tailed).
Compared with the median retained activity of 89.6%
observed in the comparison group without either paresis or
sensory impairments, the group with somatosensory im-
pairment uncomplicated by paresis had a lower median
retained activity of 81.6% (Figure 1). Because these samples
exhibited significant skew (skew parameters were 5.4 and
3.6 times their respective standard errors), Mann–Whitney
U tests were used to compare participation scores. These
tests revealed that the 78.0% greater activity loss in the
group with sensory impairment (18.4% loss from pre-
stroke) was significantly higher than that of the comparison
group (10.4% loss) without either sensory or motor im-
pairment. Thus, somatosensory impairment, identified at
hospital admission using the NIHSS, significantly predicts
overall percentage of retained activity participation at a
median of 222 days poststroke in stroke survivors with
somatosensory impairment but no paresis.
Activity Participation Outcome With Paresis. Both so-
matosensory loss and paresis appear to be associated with
lower total ACS retained activity scores (Figure 1), al-
though all subgroups demonstrated a wide distribution
of individual differences in retained activity scores. Despite
the wide dispersion, several trends can be identified. Paresis
had the typically expected marked effect on reducing
activity participation (Figure 1). Paresis of the upper limb
alone virtually doubled the median activity loss. Whereas
this difference was more than comparable to the effect of
sensory loss alone, the paresis effect only approached ac-
cepted standards of statistical significance (z 5 1.90, p 5
.057, two-tailed), a result that needs to be interpreted in
the context of the very small sample size. Paresis in both
upper and lower limbs (without sensory loss) approxi-
mately tripled the activity loss in groups without sensory
impairment. This effect was both large and statistically
significant (z 5 4.62, p < .001, two-tailed).
The effect of sensory loss in cases with upper limb
paresis was of smaller median magnitude (an additional
Table 1. Distribution of Paresis and Somatosensory Loss
Paresis
Somatosensory Loss
TotalUnimpaired Impaired
Unimpaired 119 34 153
Upper limb only 16 11 27
Upper and lower limbs 43 45 88
Total 178 90 268
Figure 1. Box plots showing overall percentage of retained
activity
scores poststroke as measured by the Activity Card Sort (ACS)
in
six groups of stroke survivors classified according to
somatosen-
sory impairment and paresis.
Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles (i.e., the
interquartile range
[IQR]). The median (50th percentile) is represented by a line
across and
within the box. The whiskers extend from the upper and lower
edges of
the box to the highest and lowest values that are not greater
than 1.5 times
the IQR above or below the box limits. Outliers, represented by
open circles,
locate cases with values that deviate more than 1.5 times the
IQR above or
below the box limits.
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p4
4.6% loss) compared with that observed in the group
without paresis (additional 7.1% loss) and estimated with
lower power from smaller samples. The effect was not
significant. The expected strong influence of paresis on
activity loss appeared to mask any additional effect from
the presence of sensory loss (Figure 1). The median ac-
tivity losses in the two subgroups with upper limb and
lower limb paresis were the strongest of all six subgroups
but comparable to each other (Figure 1) and their dif-
ference not statistically significant.
Figure 2. Box plots illustrating distributions of percentage of
retained activity scores of the Activity Card Sort (ACS)
domains.
Light gray bars represent groups without somatosensory loss;
dark gray bars indicate groups with sensory loss. Median values
are rounded to the nearest integer
value. Outliers, represented by open circles, indicate cases with
values that deviate from the median by more than 1.5 to 3 times
the IQR. Extreme outliers (values >3
times the IQR) are indicated by solid circles.
7203205100p5 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3
Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Activity
Card Sort Domains
Investigation of the four domains of the total ACS sug-
gested that the pattern revealed in the total score tended to
repeat within each domain (Figure 2), the most parsi-
monious hypothesis given that the four domains sum to
the total ACS. In the absence of paresis, the ACS scores
generally suggest that somatosensory impairment reduced
participation recovery. Paresis tended to reduce recovery
of participation, particularly for those with paresis in both
upper and lower limbs. The effect of paresis in both limbs
masked the effect of somatosensory loss. However, dif-
ferences in the dispersions within groups for some do-
mains, some variations in the magnitude of the median
differences, and the small group sizes for those with upper
limb paresis alone resulted in somewhat different results
for the formal pairwise comparison sets.
The adverse effect of somatosensory loss on recovery
of activity was statistically significant for participants
without paresis in low-demand leisure and social activities,
but not in the other two domains (Table 2). An effect of
somatosensory loss was not detected for samples with
upper limb paresis (Table 2) in any of the domains, de-
spite the suggestion of such an effect in Figure 2 for low-
demand leisure, instrumental, and social activities. However,
the sample sizes for these comparisons were small, re-
sulting in lower statistical power. Paresis of the upper
limb alone did not significantly lower activity recovery in
any of the domains (Table 2), despite the suggestion of
such an effect in Figure 2. The stronger adverse effect on
activity caused by paresis of both limbs emerged clearly in
all ACS domains, all of which showed statistically sig-
nificant effects (Table 2).
Comparability of Subgroups on Age, Gender,
and Handedness
Subgroups, divided according to paresis and somatosen-
sory loss, were compared on age, gender, and hemispheric
dominance. No statistically significant differences were
noted. In particular, cross-tabulation of gender distribu-
tion across sensory status revealed no statistically signifi-
cant difference for the subsamples without paresis (for
those with sensory loss, the female:male ratio was
50%:50%, whereas for those without sensory loss, the
ratio was 52.1%:47.9%). The overwhelming pre-
ponderance of right handers in the sample made domi-
nance an unlikely potential confounder of the sensory loss
effect found for cases without paresis. The obtained dis-
tributions were very skewed for both subsamples without
paresis (for those without sensory loss, the right:left
ratio was 92%:8%, whereas for those with sensory loss, it
was 97%:3%). Finally, age was very comparable for these
subgroups, with means of 63.8 yr (SD 5 12.8) and 61.9 yr
(SD 5 10.9), a difference that was not significant when
evaluated by a t test.
To further verify that age and gender did not con-
found the observed relationship between sensory loss and
recovery of participation, we conducted several multiple
regressions using age, gender, latency of ACS tests, and di-
chotomized sensory impairment scores as conjoint predictors
of participation recovery measured by the ACS. Because the
ACS distributions were significantly skewed, the IBM SPSS
Statistics Generalized Linear Models subroutine was used to
fit the data several times, making a variety of distributional
assumptions. The regression model demonstrated that in the
group without paresis, the association between somatosen-
sory loss and the ACS score was statistically significant (p 5
.016), even after statistically controlling for the influence
of age, gender, and latency of ACS. In contrast, multiple
Table 2. Effects of Somatosensory and Motor Impairment on
Activity Card Sort Domains Between Comparison Groups
Comparison Groups
Group A Group B
High-Demand
Leisure
Activities
Low-Demand
Leisure
Activities
Instrumental
Activities Social Activities
Paresis Sensory Loss Paresis Sensory Loss z p z p z p z p
No No No Yes 0.896 .371 2.567* .010* 1.564 .118 2.112* .035*
Upper limb only No Upper limb only Yes 0.645 .557 1.559 .134
0.901 .904 0.687 .716
Upper and lower limbs No Upper and lower limbs Yes 0.143
.886 0.806 .420 0.698 .485 0.205 .837
No No Upper limb only No 1.420 .155 0.388 .698 1.615 .106
1.500 .133
No No Upper and lower limbs No 4.410* .000* 4.600* .000*
4.230* .000* 4.020* .000*
Note. Summary of key statistics obtained from Mann–Whitney
pairwise tests conducted for four sets of five comparisons that
replicated the primary analysis for
each of the four domains of the ACS. The first three
comparisons present the effects of somatosensory loss for each
paretic status (no paresis, upper limb only,
upper and lower limbs). The last two comparisons present the
effects of paresis when there is no measured somatosensory
loss.
*p < .05.
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p6
regressions showed that the relationship between sensory loss
and ACS scores was not significant (p 5 .49) in the group
with paresis of an upper limb or in the group with paresis of
both upper and lower limbs (p 5 .89). These results support
the conclusions reached through nonparametric analyses,
reported earlier in this article.
Discussion
Somatosensory impairment at hospital admission after
stroke is associated with more activity participation loss
compared with survivors without somatosensory loss,
when there is no concomitant paresis. This predictive
association was identified in a longitudinal cohort (N 5
268) of stroke survivors with mild impairment (median
NIHSS score 5 3.00; interquartile range 5 1.00–5.00). It
is based on a dichotomous measure of somatosensory
impairment (NIHSS) obtained early poststroke, at the
time of hospitalization, and on quantitative measurement
of retained activity participation (ACS) in instrumental,
leisure, and social activities at a median time of 222 days
poststroke. This study addresses an identified gap in the
literature, highlighted recently in two reviews (Carey et al.,
2016; Meyer et al., 2014). Our finding is consistent with
and advances the current literature on the effect of somato-
sensory impairment on activity participation (Desrosiers
et al., 2002; Hill et al., 2014; Morris, van Wijck, Joice, &
Donaghy, 2013). It demonstrates a predictive relation-
ship between somatosensory loss identified during the
acute phase after stroke, using an acute screening mea-
sure, and actual retained activities during the postacute
phase, when people had completed rehabilitation, re-
turned home, and were experiencing the impact of stroke
on their activity participation (median 5 7.4 mo post-
stroke). Previous studies have been cross-sectional (Hill
et al., 2014) or have shown a predictive relationship from
2 to 4 wk after stroke or from hospital discharge to 6 mo
poststroke (Desrosiers et al., 2002; Morris et al., 2013).
Paresis affecting both upper and lower limbs, as
expected, also caused a major reduction in the percentage of
retained activity participation, an effect larger than that
resulting from somatosensory deficit alone. Paresis with the
addition of sensory deficit does not appear to further in-
crease loss in participation when there is paresis of both
upper and lower limbs, though a larger sample is needed to
clarify whether paresis of the upper limb only has as del-
eterious an effect on participation as when paresis is ac-
companied by sensory loss. In other words, the data suggest
that motor impairment in both upper and lower limbs is
sufficient to cause loss of activity participation over a wide
enough range of activities so that the addition of sensory
impairment may be insufficient to compromise additional
activity participation.
Results suggest that the presence of loss of activity
participation associated with paresis of both upper and
lower limbs can mask the contribution of somatosensory
loss and may promote an impression that somatosensory
loss can be left untreated. However, as the group with
sensory impairment unaccompanied by paresis shows,
sensory impairment alone does significantly affect par-
ticipation in everyday activities. Although an empirical
investigation using interventions to manipulate motor and
sensory impairments is ultimately necessary, the results
raise the risk that if motor impairment only, and not
sensory impairment, is addressed by intervention, the
reduction in activity participation as a result of sensory loss
will be unmasked. Addressing only motor impairment
may lead to suboptimal therapeutic effects in patients
whose somatosensory deficit has remained.
Unless motor rehabilitation training addresses sensory
deficits (indirectly), or perhaps the survivors’ confidence to
engage in activities, outcomes for stroke survivors will likely
be diminished. Historically, clinicians and researchers have
given precedence to the motor sequelae of stroke, ne-
glecting somatosensation (Kalra, 2010), but despite good
movement capacity, survivors with sensory loss learn to not
use their limb for task performance (Carey, 1995; Connell
et al., 2013; Dannenbaum & Dykes, 1988). Therefore,
according to our findings, sensory deficit alone has suffi-
cient impact on retained activity participation to include a
somatosensory intervention focus in therapeutic programs.
Exploration of the effect of somatosensory impair-
ment on the ACS domains revealed a significant impact on
low-demand leisure and social activities but not on high-
demand leisure and instrumental activities. The association
with social and low-demand leisure activities is consistent
with findings for social roles, as assessed with the Assess-
ment of Life Habits (Desrosiers et al., 2002) and the social
isolation and perceived physical activity subscales of the
Nottingham Health Profile (Morris et al., 2013). Although
the reduced statistical sensitivity resulting from small sample
sizes must be noted, we did not find a significant association
with instrumental activities, potentially at variance with the
low but significant association between somatosensation at
discharge and ADLs (Desrosiers et al., 2002).
The data confirm clinical impressions that the range of
individual differences in retained activity participation is large
after stroke, even when there is neither sensory nor paretic
impairment nor neglect or aphasia. Clearly, much remains to
be added to predict activity participation poststroke beyond
the measures used here. However, it is possible to anticipate
7203205100p7 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3
that these large individual differences cause strategic decisions
about approach to therapy (e.g., ignore somatosensory im-
pairment) difficult to make on the basis of clinical impres-
sions and individual assessment alone. Population-wide,
epidemiologically informed reasoning using empirical mea-
surement of impairment is indicated by these results.
Limitations and Recommendations for
Future Research
The sensory subscale of the NIHSS was used to identify
the presence of somatosensory loss. Although this tool is
commonly used to assess the presence and severity of acute
neurological impairment and involves standardized ad-
ministration, testing of somatosensation is not quantitative
or supported by psychometric data. Studies using quanti-
tative somatosensory measures with strong psychometric
properties to better assess the nature and extent of the
predictive relationship are indicated. A further benefit
would be assessment of the ability of this commonly used
clinical measure to predict quantitative somatosensory im-
pairment tests with evaluated reliability and validity.
The NIHSS was administered only once, at or within
a few days of hospital admission after stroke. Therefore,
when sensory loss, aphasia, neglect, or other NIHSS var-
iables were involved, the analyses indicate a predictive re-
lationship based on the patient status on these variables at
admission. The level of sensory loss, neglect, aphasia, and so
forth at the time of ACS administration is unknown.
To protect against the possibility that an earlier ACS
may have influenced the data, we conducted a post hoc
analysis excluding participants from the sample with ACS
scores obtained in the 92–155 days range. Findings from
this trimmed sample (N 5 240) essentially remained the
same: There was a somatosensory loss effect in the ab-
sence of paresis (total retained activity participation: z 5
2.50, p 5 .012; low-demand leisure: z 5 2.90, p 5
.004; social: z 5 2.75, p 5 .006; instrumental z 5 2.04,
p 5 .042) and a highly significant effect of paresis in both
limbs for total retained activities and all subcategories
(z 5 3.88–4.26, p < .001).
Finally, use of existing data from the prospective
cohort limited choice of measures and the demographic and
organismic covariates that might be studied. Our initial
findings of a predictive association does however encourage
future studies to verify the observation. A design is rec-
ommended that uses superior measurement tools for so-
matosensory deficit and paresis and considers demographic
or organismic variables that might arguably influence
participation recovery or the strength of the relationship
between somatosensory impairment and participation in a
sufficiently large sample.
Implications for Occupational
Therapy Practice
Our findings have the following implications for occu-
pational therapy practice:
• Practitioners should assess for the presence of somato-
sensory impairment in stroke survivors, given its impact
on participation, especially in those without concomitant
paresis.
• Rehabilitation directed at upper limb function and
return to participation in previous life activities should
consider investigation of the effect of interventions
with a specific focus on somatosensory phenomena.
• Therapeutic program planning should consider that
the impact of somatosensory impairment may be un-
masked when rehabilitation has been directed only at
motor impairment recovery and the result has been
below expectation.
• Clinical evaluation and treatment in stroke have tradition-
ally focused on addressing motor impairment without
considering the impact of somatosensory functions. Our
findings suggest that additional gain may be obtained in
participation outcomes by augmenting motor-based inter-
ventions with a somatosensory rehabilitation component.
Investigations of this possibility are supported by the pre-
sent findings.
Conclusion
Somatosensory impairment is associated with reduced
activity participation. On the basis of evidence that so-
matosensory improvements are obtained, a focus on so-
matosensory intervention in rehabilitation is recommended
for people with somatosensory impairment (Carey et al.,
2011). Monitoring participation outcomes may yield evi-
dence of additional improvements in participation asso-
ciated with somatosensory intervention. Such investigation
will be important in furthering evidence not only of the
relationship between somatosensory impairment and par-
ticipation but also the potential impact of somatosensory-
focused interventions on participation outcomes. Our
findings also indicate that paresis of the upper and lower
limbs can mask the contribution of sensory loss. This
finding suggests that the potential impact of somatosensa-
tion on participation when movement returns should not be
ignored. s
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p8
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge financial support for this research from
the James S. McDonnell Foundation (JSMF Grants
21002032 and 220020087, awarded to CB) and National
Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Grants
307902 and 1022694 (awarded to LMC) and support
for analysis, write-up, and researchers from the James S.
McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative
in Cognitive Rehabilitation Collaborative Award (JSMF
Grant 220020413), the Victorian Government’s Opera-
tional Infrastructure Support Program, an Australian
Research Council Future Fellowship (FT0992299), and an
NHMRC Career Development Award (307905) awarded
to LMC. We acknowledge the contribution of Lisa Tabor
Connor to an earlier presentation of preliminary data re-
lated to this study and to Tamara Tse for assistance with
data compilation and checking.
References
Baum, C. M., & Edwards, D. F. (2001). Activity Card Sort
(ACS): Test manual. St. Louis: Program in Occupational
Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine.
Blennerhassett, J. M., Carey, L. M., & Matyas, T. A. (2008).
Clinical measures of handgrip limitation relate to impaired
pinch grip force control after stroke. Journal of Hand Ther-
apy, 21, 245–253. https://doi.org/10.1197/j.jht.2007.10.021
Blennerhassett, J. M., Matyas, T. A., & Carey, L. M. (2007).
Impaired discrimination of surface friction contributes
to pinch grip deficit after stroke. Neurorehabilitation
and Neural Repair, 21, 263–272. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1545968306295560
Borstad, A. L., & Nichols-Larsen, D. S. (2014). Assessing and
treating higher level somatosensory impairments post
stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 21, 290–295. https://
doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290
Brott, T., Adams, H. P., Jr., Olinger, C. P., Marler, J. R.,
Barsan, W. G., Biller, J., . . . Hertzberg, V. (1989). Mea-
surements of acute cerebral infarction: A clinical examina-
tion scale. Stroke, 20, 864–870. https://doi.org/10.1161/
01.STR.20.7.864
Carey, L. M. (1995). Somatosensory loss after stroke. Critical
Reviews in Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 7, 51–91.
https://doi.org/10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v7.i1.40
Carey, L. M. (2012). Touch and body sensations. In L. M.
Carey (Ed.), Stroke rehabilitation: Insights from neuroscience
and
imaging (pp. 157–172). New York: Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199797882.003.0012
Carey, L. M. (2015). Person factors. In C. H. Christiansen,
C. M. Baum, & J. Bass (Eds.), Occupational therapy: En-
abling performance, participation and well-being (4th ed.,
pp. 249–265). Thorofare, NJ: Slack.
Carey, L. M., Lamp, G., & Turville, M. (2016). The state-of-
the-science of somatosensory function and its impact on
daily life in adults and older adults, and following stroke: A
scoping review. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health,
36, 27S–41S. https://doi.org/10.1177/1539449216643941
Carey, L., Macdonell, R., & Matyas, T. A. (2011). SENSe:
Study of the Effectiveness of Neurorehabilitation on Sen-
sation: A randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabilitation
and Neural Repair, 25, 304–313. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1545968310397705
Carey, L. M., & Matyas, T. A. (2011). Frequency of discrim-
inative sensory loss in the hand after stroke in a rehabili-
tation setting. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43,
257–263. https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-0662
Connell, L. A., McMahon, N. E., & Adams, N. (2013). Stroke
survivors’ experiences of somatosensory impairment after
stroke: An interpretive phenomenological analysis. Physio-
therapy, 100, 150–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013.
09.003
Dannenbaum, R. M., & Dykes, R. W. (1988). Sensory loss
in the hand after sensory stroke: Therapeutic rationale.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 69,
833–839.
Desrosiers, J., Noreau, L., Rochette, A., Bravo, G., & Boutin,
C.
(2002). Predictors of handicap situations following post-
stroke rehabilitation. Disability and Rehabilitation, 24,
774–785. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638280210125814
Edwards, D. F., Hahn, M., Baum, C., & Dromerick, A. W.
(2006). The impact of mild stroke on meaningful activity and
life satisfaction. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular
Diseases,
15, 151–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.
2006.04.001
Everard, K. M., Lach, H. W., Fisher, E. B., & Baum, M. C.
(2000). Relationship of activity and social support to the
functional health of older adults. Journals of Gerontology,
Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55,
S208–S212. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.4.S208
Hill, V. A., Fisher, T., Schmid, A. A., Crabtree, J., & Page,
S. J. (2014). Relationship between touch sensation of the
affected hand and performance of valued activities in in-
dividuals with chronic stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilita-
tion, 21, 339–346. https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-339
Jeannerod, M., Michel, F., & Prablanc, C. (1984). The control
of hand movements in a case of hemianaesthesia following
a parietal lesion. Brain, 107, 899–920. https://doi.org/
10.1093/brain/107.3.899
Kalra, L. (2010). Stroke rehabilitation 2009: Old chestnuts and
new insights. Stroke, 41, e88–e90. https://doi.org/10.1161/
STROKEAHA.109.572297
Kessner, S. S., Bingel, U., & Thomalla, G. (2016). Somato-
sensory deficits after stroke: A scoping review. Topics in
Stroke Rehabilitation, 23, 136–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10749357.2015.1116822
Klit, H., Finnerup, N. B., Andersen, G., & Jensen, T. S. (2011).
Central poststroke pain: A population-based study. Pain,
152, 818–824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.030
Kong, K. H., Chua, K. S., & Lee, J. (2011). Recovery of upper
limb dexterity in patients more than 1 year after stroke: Fre-
quency, clinical correlates and predictors. NeuroRehabilita-
tion, 28, 105–111. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0639
7203205100p9 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3
https://doi.org/10.1197/j.jht.2007.10.021
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968306295560
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968306295560
https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290
https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864
https://doi.org/10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v7.i1.40
https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199797882.003.0012
https://doi.org/10.1177/1539449216643941
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968310397705
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968310397705
https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-0662
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013.09.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013.09.003
https://doi.org/10.1080/09638280210125814
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2006.04.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2006.04.001
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.4.S208
https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-339
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.3.899
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.3.899
https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.572297
https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.572297
https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2015.1116822
https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2015.1116822
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.030
https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0639
Law, M., Baptiste, S., Carswell, A., McColl, M. A., Polatajko,
H., & Pollock, N. (2014). Canadian Occupational Performance
Measure (5th ed.). Ottawa: CAOT Publications.
Lawrence, E. S., Coshall, C., Dundas, R., Stewart, J., Rudd,
A. G., Howard, R., & Wolfe, C. D. A. (2001). Estimates
of the prevalence of acute stroke impairments and disabil-
ity in a multiethnic population. Stroke, 32, 1279–1284.
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1279
Meyer, S., Karttunen, A. H., Thijs, V., Feys, H., & Verheyden,
G.
(2014). How do somatosensory deficits in the arm and hand
relate to upper limb impairment, activity, and participation
problems after stroke? A systematic review. Physical Therapy,
94, 1220–1231. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20130271
Morris, J. H., van Wijck, F., Joice, S., & Donaghy, M. (2013).
Predicting health related quality of life 6 months after
stroke: The role of anxiety and upper limb dysfunction.
Disability and Rehabilitation, 35, 291–299. https://doi.
org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942
Muir, K. W., Weir, C. J., Murray, G. D., Povey, C., & Lees,
K. R. (1996). Comparison of neurological scales and scor-
ing systems for acute stroke prognosis. Stroke, 27,
1817–1820. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.27.10.1817
Nowak, D. A., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). Grip force behavior
during object manipulation in neurological disorders: To-
ward an objective evaluation of manual performance def-
icits. Movement Disorders, 20, 11–25. https://doi.org/
10.1002/mds.20299
Park, S. W., Wolf, S. L., Blanton, S., Winstein, C., & Nichols-
Larsen, D. S. (2008). The EXCITE trial: Predicting a
clinically meaningful motor activity log outcome. Neuro-
rehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22, 486–493. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1545968308316906
Reding, M. J., & Potes, E. (1988). Rehabilitation outcome
following initial unilateral hemispheric stroke: Life table
analysis approach. Stroke, 19, 1354–1358. https://doi.org/
10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354
Tse, T., Douglas, J., Lentin, P., & Carey, L. (2013). Measuring
participation after stroke: A review of frequently used
tools. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 94,
177–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2012.09.002
Tyson, S. F., Hanley, M., Chillala, J., Selley, A. B., & Tallis,
R. C. (2008). Sensory loss in hospital-admitted people
with stroke: Characteristics, associated factors, and
relationship with function. Neurorehabilitation and
Neural Repair, 22, 166–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1545968307305523
Winward, C. E., Halligan, P. W., & Wade, D. T. (2002). The
Rivermead Assessment of Somatosensory Performance
(RASP): Standardization and reliability data. Clinical
Rehabilitation, 16, 523–533. https://doi.org/10.1191/
0269215502cr522oa
Wolf, T. J., Baum, C., & Connor, L. T. (2009). Changing face
of stroke: Implications for occupational therapy practice.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63, 621–625.
https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.63.5.621
World Health Organization. (2001). International classification
of functioning, disability and health. Geneva: Author.
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p10
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1279
https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20130271
https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942
https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.27.10.1817
https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20299
https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20299
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308316906
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308316906
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354
https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2012.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968307305523
https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968307305523
https://doi.org/10.1191/0269215502cr522oa
https://doi.org/10.1191/0269215502cr522oa
https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.63.5.621
Copyright of American Journal of Occupational Therapy is the
property of American
Occupational Therapy Association and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple
sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.
APA FOMRAT no plagiarizing original work only
#5
Identify the variables in the study. What is being examined or
measured? For the variables, provide an
operational definition, Identify the instruments or strategies
used to collect data on each of the variables, and identify the
scale of measurement for the data related to each variable under
examination.
#6 For each of the instruments, discuss the data related to the
validity and reliability of each. Compare the data found to the
concepts of validity/ reliability, are these instruments
considered valid and reliable why? Explain

More Related Content

Similar to Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on ParticipationAfter St.docx

PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after TraumaPreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
Elizabeth York
 
AllenKelly_Thesis
AllenKelly_ThesisAllenKelly_Thesis
AllenKelly_Thesis
Kelly Allen
 
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
Younis I Munshi
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ADDY50
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
SHIVA101531
 
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docxFamily-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
mglenn3
 
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. BDementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
hannah06
 
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
tamicawaysmith
 
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
aulasnilda
 
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docxText dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
mattinsonjanel
 
Nam assignment finishedlll
Nam assignment finishedlllNam assignment finishedlll
Nam assignment finishedlll
Laura Dunn
 
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docxThe  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
randymartin91030
 

Similar to Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on ParticipationAfter St.docx (20)

Self-Care Booklet PDF
Self-Care Booklet PDFSelf-Care Booklet PDF
Self-Care Booklet PDF
 
Tb ihomeless final
Tb ihomeless finalTb ihomeless final
Tb ihomeless final
 
PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after TraumaPreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
PreDataCollectionCall-ERYORK-RTW after Trauma
 
AllenKelly_Thesis
AllenKelly_ThesisAllenKelly_Thesis
AllenKelly_Thesis
 
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
The good life --assessing the relative importance of physical, psychological,...
 
Lit Review
Lit ReviewLit Review
Lit Review
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
 
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docxFamily-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
Family-Centered Care for Military and Veteran Families Affecte.docx
 
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. BDementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
Dementia And Aggression Psy 492 M7a2 Levea. B
 
Impact of exercise program on functional status among post lumbar laminectom...
Impact of exercise program on functional status among post  lumbar laminectom...Impact of exercise program on functional status among post  lumbar laminectom...
Impact of exercise program on functional status among post lumbar laminectom...
 
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
392018 OwlSearchhttpeds.b.ep.ezproxy.harford.edueds.docx
 
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
174 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 9, Number 4,.docx
 
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docxText dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
Text dataTable 4-7Child Mortality, Female Literacy Rate,Per Capita.docx
 
Confabulation what is associated with its rise and fall a study in brain injury
Confabulation what is associated with its rise and fall a study in brain injuryConfabulation what is associated with its rise and fall a study in brain injury
Confabulation what is associated with its rise and fall a study in brain injury
 
Aging Issues Following Brain Injury
Aging Issues Following Brain InjuryAging Issues Following Brain Injury
Aging Issues Following Brain Injury
 
Neuropsychologic function and level of caregiver supervision
Neuropsychologic function and level of caregiver supervisionNeuropsychologic function and level of caregiver supervision
Neuropsychologic function and level of caregiver supervision
 
Clinical issues: Psychological impact of Stroke
Clinical issues: Psychological impact of StrokeClinical issues: Psychological impact of Stroke
Clinical issues: Psychological impact of Stroke
 
Nam assignment finishedlll
Nam assignment finishedlllNam assignment finishedlll
Nam assignment finishedlll
 
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docxThe  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
The  business direction for HR is to be more strategic in their fu.docx
 

More from toltonkendal

Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docxElementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docxElementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of MusicPitch- relative highness or lowness that we .docx
Elements of MusicPitch-  relative highness or lowness that we .docxElements of MusicPitch-  relative highness or lowness that we .docx
Elements of MusicPitch- relative highness or lowness that we .docx
toltonkendal
 
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docxElevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docxElev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm the flow of music in te.docx
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm  the flow of music in te.docxElements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm  the flow of music in te.docx
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm the flow of music in te.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docxElements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docxElements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docxElements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docxElements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
toltonkendal
 
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docxElements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
toltonkendal
 
ELEG 421 Control Systems Transient and Steady State .docx
ELEG 421 Control Systems  Transient and Steady State .docxELEG 421 Control Systems  Transient and Steady State .docx
ELEG 421 Control Systems Transient and Steady State .docx
toltonkendal
 
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docxElement 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
toltonkendal
 
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docxELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
toltonkendal
 

More from toltonkendal (20)

Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docxElementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
Elementary CurriculaBoth articles highlight the fact that middle.docx
 
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docxElementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
Elementary Statistics (MATH220)Assignment Statistic.docx
 
Elements of Religious Traditions PaperWritea 700- to 1,050-word .docx
Elements of Religious Traditions PaperWritea 700- to 1,050-word .docxElements of Religious Traditions PaperWritea 700- to 1,050-word .docx
Elements of Religious Traditions PaperWritea 700- to 1,050-word .docx
 
Elements of MusicPitch- relative highness or lowness that we .docx
Elements of MusicPitch-  relative highness or lowness that we .docxElements of MusicPitch-  relative highness or lowness that we .docx
Elements of MusicPitch- relative highness or lowness that we .docx
 
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docxElevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children AssociatedWith the Fl.docx
 
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docxElev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
Elev ent h EDIT IONREAL ESTATE PRINCIPLESCHARLES F. .docx
 
Elements of the Communication ProcessIn Chapter One, we learne.docx
Elements of the Communication ProcessIn Chapter One, we learne.docxElements of the Communication ProcessIn Chapter One, we learne.docx
Elements of the Communication ProcessIn Chapter One, we learne.docx
 
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm the flow of music in te.docx
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm  the flow of music in te.docxElements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm  the flow of music in te.docx
Elements of Music #1 Handout1. Rhythm the flow of music in te.docx
 
Elements of Music Report InstrumentsFor the assignment on the el.docx
Elements of Music Report InstrumentsFor the assignment on the el.docxElements of Music Report InstrumentsFor the assignment on the el.docx
Elements of Music Report InstrumentsFor the assignment on the el.docx
 
Elements of GenreAfter watching three of the five .docx
Elements of GenreAfter watching three of the five .docxElements of GenreAfter watching three of the five .docx
Elements of GenreAfter watching three of the five .docx
 
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docxElements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and designing .docx
 
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docxElements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
Elements of Critical Thinking [WLOs 2, 3, 4] [CLOs 2, 3, 4]P.docx
 
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docxElements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
Elements of DesignDuring the process of envisioning and design.docx
 
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docxElements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
Elements of a contact due 16 OctRead the Case Campbell Soup Co. v..docx
 
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docxElements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
Elements for analyzing mise en sceneIdentify the components of.docx
 
Elements in the same row have the same number of () levelsWhi.docx
Elements in the same row have the same number of () levelsWhi.docxElements in the same row have the same number of () levelsWhi.docx
Elements in the same row have the same number of () levelsWhi.docx
 
ELEG 421 Control Systems Transient and Steady State .docx
ELEG 421 Control Systems  Transient and Steady State .docxELEG 421 Control Systems  Transient and Steady State .docx
ELEG 421 Control Systems Transient and Steady State .docx
 
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docxElement 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
Element 010 ASSIGNMENT 3000 WORDS (100)Task Individual assign.docx
 
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docxELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
ELEG 320L – Signals & Systems Laboratory Dr. Jibran Khan Yous.docx
 
Electronic Media PresentationChoose two of the following.docx
Electronic Media PresentationChoose two of the following.docxElectronic Media PresentationChoose two of the following.docx
Electronic Media PresentationChoose two of the following.docx
 

Recently uploaded

1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
QucHHunhnh
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
MateoGardella
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
PECB
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Chris Hunter
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
SanaAli374401
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
ciinovamais
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
Gardella_Mateo_IntellectualProperty.pdf.
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
 
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 

Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on ParticipationAfter St.docx

  • 1. Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Participation After Stroke Leeanne M. Carey, Thomas A. Matyas, Carolyn Baum Leeanne M. Carey, PhD, BAppSc(OT), FAOTA, FOTARA, is Professor of Occupational Therapy, Discipline Lead, Occupational Therapy, School of Allied Health, College of Science, Health, and Engineering, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, and Head, Neurorehabilitation and Recovery, Stroke Division, Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Heidelberg, Victoria, Australia; [email protected] Thomas A. Matyas, PhD, is Adjunct Professor, School of Allied Health and School of Psychology and Public Health, College of Science, Health, and Engineering, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, and Honorary Professorial Fellow, Neurorehabilitation and Recovery, Stroke Division, Florey Institute of Neuroscience
  • 2. and Mental Health, Heidelberg, Victoria, Australia. Carolyn Baum, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, is Professor, Occupational Therapy and Neurology and Social Work, Elias Michael Director, Program in Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO. OBJECTIVE. Our objective was to determine the effect of loss of body sensation on activity participation in stroke survivors. METHOD. Participants (N 5 268) were assessed at hospital admission for somatosensory and motor impairment using the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale. Participation was assessed using the Activity Card Sort (ACS) in the postacute phase. Between- group differences in activity participation were analyzed for participants with and without somatosensory impairment and with or without paresis. RESULTS. Somatosensory impairment was experienced in 33.6% of the sample and paresis in 42.9%. ACS profiles were obtained at a median of 222 days poststroke. Somatosensory loss alone (z 5 1.96, p 5 .048) and paresis in upper and lower limbs without sensory loss (z 5 4.62, p < .001) influenced activity participation. CONCLUSION. Somatosensory impairment is associated with reduced activity participation; however, paresis of upper and lower limbs can mask the contribution of
  • 3. sensory loss. Carey, L. M., Matyas, T. A., & Baum, C. (2018). Effects of somatosensory impairment on participation after stroke. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 72, 7203205100. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2018.025114 One in 2 people experience loss of body sensation, or somatosensation, afterstroke (Carey, 2012; Carey & Matyas, 2011; Winward, Halligan, & Wade, 2002). They typically have difficulty sensing touch, pressure, and temperature; perceiving own limb position; discriminating textures; and recognizing objects through the sense of touch (Carey, 1995). They may also experience reduced or altered pain sensation (Klit, Finnerup, Andersen, & Jensen, 2011). People who experience somatosensory loss after stroke report problems such as communi- cating through a handshake, using utensils, dressing, and holding objects without crushing or dropping them. They also report problems with personal, family, and work roles. Despite the high prevalence and apparent importance of
  • 4. somatosensation in daily activities, there is limited empirical evidence to quantify the presence and nature of a relationship between impaired somato- sensation and participation in daily activities, as highlighted in two recent re- views (Carey, Lamp, & Turville, 2016; Meyer, Karttunen, Thijs, Feys, & Verheyden, 2014). The impact of somatosensory loss on participation is therefore critical to address to understand the factors that affect participation and to improve client-centered care planning. Somatosensory impairment after stroke has been associated with impaired daily actions and activities, according to clinical and laboratory studies (Borstad & Nichols-Larsen, 2014; Carey et al., 2016; Kessner, Bingel, & Thomalla, 2016; Meyer et al., 2014). Somatosensory loss reduces control of hand movements (Jeannerod, Michel, & Prablanc, 1984) and of the fundamental pinch-grip-lift-and-hold task (Blennerhassett, Matyas, & Carey,
  • 5. 2007) and limits grip force control during object manipulation (Nowak & Hermsdörfer, 2005). A relationship has also been demonstrated between clinical measures of 7203205100p1 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3 mailto:[email protected] https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2018.025114 hand function and dexterity (Blennerhassett, Carey, & Matyas, 2008; Kong, Chua, & Lee, 2011; Park, Wolf, Blanton, Winstein, & Nichols-Larsen, 2008). Studies have demonstrated that the affected limb may not be used spontaneously, despite adequate movement abilities. This lack of use may contribute to a learned nonuse of the limb and to further deterioration of motor function after stroke (Carey, 1995; Dannenbaum & Dykes, 1988). Moreover, stroke survivors have expressed uncertainty about whether to use their sensory-impaired arm for task perfor- mance (Connell, McMahon, & Adams, 2013). A predictive
  • 6. relationship between somatosensory loss and independence in activities of daily living (ADLs) and mobility has also been demonstrated, for example, in a prospective sample of 102 stroke survivors (Tyson, Hanley, Chillala, Selley, & Tallis, 2008) and using a life table analysis (N 5 95; Reding & Potes, 1988). It is therefore plausible that sensory im- pairment may also be associated with reduced participation in daily activities and life roles; yet our understanding of its impact on participation is limited. The International Classification of Functioning, Dis- ability and Health (ICF) defines participation as “a per- son’s involvement in a life situation,” for example, in leisure, social, and work activities (World Health Orga- nization, 2001, p. 10). A major knowledge gap about the impact of somatosensory impairment after stroke on par- ticipation in life activities has been recently identified in a scoping review (Carey et al., 2016) and in a systematic re- view (Meyer et al., 2014). Hill, Fisher, Schmid, Crabtree, and Page (2014) reported a good to excellent relationship between performance of and satisfaction with valued ac-
  • 7. tivities, assessed using the Canadian Occupational Per- formance Measure (Law et al., 2014), and intact sensation of the hand in 50 stroke survivors. Desrosiers, Noreau, Rochette, Bravo, and Boutin (2002) found a low but significant association between light touch of the upper limb and proprioception assessed at discharge and ADLs and social roles assessed 6 mo later (N 5 102). Somatosensory impairment is commonly experienced in association with motor impairment, and degree of weakness has been significantly associated with sensory impairment (Tyson et al., 2008). Upper limb motor impairment is reported in 77% of stroke survivors (Lawrence et al., 2001) and is likely to have an impact on participation. Paresis may affect participation in its own right and in addition to somatosensory impairment. We therefore investigated the impact of somatosensory impair- ment on participation in stroke survivors with and without motor impairment. This approach is important because re-
  • 8. habilitation traditionally targets motor problems, often without attending to somatosensory deficits known to affect performance (Carey, 2015; Kalra, 2010), despite the avail- ability of interventions to remediate somatosensory deficits (Carey et al., 2016; Carey, Macdonell, & Matyas, 2011). Given the high prevalence of somatosensory loss in stroke survivors and evidence of its relationship with daily actions and activities, we investigated the effect of so- matosensory impairment on activity participation in a longitudinal stroke cohort. We were particularly interested to learn whether the presence of sensory impairment soon after stroke affected the number and type of activities (instrumental, leisure, social) in which survivors retained participation after they had returned home and completed rehabilitation (at least 3 mo after stroke). Because sensory impairment is often experienced concurrently with motor impairment, which may have an effect on participation, we investigated the impact of both motor and somato-
  • 9. sensory impairment on activity participation in our analyses. Specifically, our objectives were as follows: • Characterize activity participation outcomes of stroke survivors with and without somatosensory impairment after stroke • Determine whether sensory impairment, identified during the acute stroke phase (at or within a few days of hospital admission after stroke) using the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS; Brott et al., 1989), predicts activity participation in the postacute phase, that is, after the survivor has returned home and completed rehabilitation • Determine the effects of somatosensory loss on partic- ipation in stroke survivors with or without paresis • Explore the effects of somatosensory loss on participation in stroke survivors with and without paresis across the do- mains of the Activity Card Sort (ACS; Baum & Edwards, 2001). Method Participants
  • 10. The sample comprised a subset of participants from an existing large stroke cohort (Wolf, Baum, & Connor, 2009). Inclusion criteria were first clinical stroke; no history of schizophrenia or dementia; and availability of required data, which included the acute NIHSS individual item scores, obtained on hospital admission, and the ACS construct scores, collected after the participant had returned home and completed rehabilitation. A sample of 268 cases was obtained from a search sample of 375 after an examination of missing data, aphasia, neglect, pattern of paresis, and latency of the ACS test. Cases with severe aphasia or neglect, defined as scores The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p2 greater than 1 on the relevant NIHSS scales, were excluded because these conditions have the potential to confound sensory deficit test results. Cases in which the ACS was performed before 92 days poststroke were excluded. Because
  • 11. sensory impairment of the upper limb is most likely associated with activity participation restrictions, based on prior literature (Desrosiers et al., 2002; Hill et al., 2014), and no separate sensory assessment data were available for the lower limb, 36 cases with paresis in only the lower limb were also excluded. Measures The primary outcome was percentage of retained activity participation, measured using the ACS. The ACS is a reliable and valid instrument (Edwards, Hahn, Baum, & Dromerick, 2006; Everard, Lach, Fisher, & Baum, 2000; Tse, Douglas, Lentin, & Carey, 2013). A recent review indicated that the ACS covers the most domains of the ICF Activities and Participation domains, relative to other measures, and meets the most psychometric prop- erties (Tse et al., 2013). The version used to collect the data in a telephone interview was the 55-item measure. Participants reported on instrumental activities (e.g., shopping, household maintenance), social activities (e.g.,
  • 12. visiting, studying), and high-demand (e.g., swimming, running) and low-demand (e.g., reading, table games) physical leisure activities. For each activity, participants re- sponded to the following descriptors: never done, given up due to stroke, doing less often due to stroke, and doing now. The previous activity score was calculated by adding the doing now, doing less, and given up scores. The current activity score was the total of the doing now and doing less scores. The retained participation score was obtained by dividing the sum total of current activities by the total of previous activities. Higher scores indicate higher retention of prestroke activity. The primary score was the percentage of retained overall activity participation, with additional scores for the domains of Instrumental Activities, Social Activities, High-Demand Leisure Activities, and Low-Demand Leisure Activities. The presence or absence of somatosensory impair- ment and motor impairment was assessed by trained nurses within a couple of days of hospital admission using
  • 13. the NIHSS. The NIHSS is widely used early poststroke to quantify the neurological impairment caused by stroke. It has been repeatedly validated as a tool for assessing stroke severity and as an excellent predictor for patient outcomes (Muir, Weir, Murray, Povey, & Lees, 1996). Only somatosensory loss attributed to stroke is scored as abnormal, and the ex- aminer is required to test as many body areas (arms [not hands], legs, trunk, face) as needed to accurately check for hemisensory loss. Sensory loss is scored as 0 5 normal; no sensory loss, 1 5 mild to moderate sensory loss, and 2 5 severe to total sensory loss. Motor impairment is tested separately for the arm and leg and is scored from 0 (no drift; limb holds 90 [or 45] degrees for full 10 s) to 4 (no movement). Procedure Participants (N 5 268) were assessed at admission for somatosensory and motor impairment using the NIHSS. The activity participation profile was assessed using the ACS at least 3 mo poststroke. Data Analysis The NIHSS somatosensory loss variable was dichotomized
  • 14. (unimpaired, impaired). Because paresis is known to have a major impact on functionality and may interact with somatosensation, particularly in upper limb function, the sample was also split according to presence and distribution of paresis. Degree of paresis was dichotomized (unimpaired, impaired). The ACS scores were obtained in the postacute phase, that is, after participants had completed rehabili- tation, returned home, and were experiencing the impact of stroke on their activity participation. If participants were tested on the ACS on more than one occasion, we selected the ACS scores closest to 182 days (6 mo) poststroke for analysis. Analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 23; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Between-group differences in participation were tested for participants with and without somatosensory impairment and with or without paresis. The primary analysis focused on the total retained activity participation scores. A set of five pairwise comparisons was planned to
  • 15. evaluate contrasts of interest across the six independent groups (2 levels of sensory loss · 3 levels of paresis). A primary family of three comparisons investigated the impact of sensory loss in samples with different paresis patterns (none, upper limb only, both upper and lower limbs). To provide an interpretative framework for the somatosensory loss effects, we used a secondary family of two comparisons to investigate the effects of each of the two paresis patterns in samples without sensory loss. The ACS percentage of retained activity score was the out- come variable. Because the total activity score is the total of the four activity domains (Instrumental Activities, Social Activities, High-Demand Leisure Activities, and Low-Demand Leisure Activities), each of the four do- mains of the ACS was used in exploratory secondary analyses. The same set of comparisons as for the total ACS retained activity score was used for each of these secondary analyses.
  • 16. 7203205100p3 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3 Results The 268 cases included yielded a sample with a gender distribution that slightly favored women (53.5%). Age was normally distributed with a mean (M) of 62.8 yr and a standard deviation (SD) of 13.1 yr. There were 239 right dominant cases, 16 left dominant, and 3 ambi- dextrous, with the remainder unknown. The median NIHSS score for the sample was 3.00, with an interquartile range of 1.00–5.00 (5th percentile 5 0.00; 95th percentile 5 9.95; range 5 0–15). The mean score was 3.71 (SD 5 3.11). The distribution of somato- sensory impairment and motor impairment in the sample is summarized in Table 1. Activity participation was measured at a median of 222 days poststroke. A skewed distribution (M 5 279; median 5 222; interquartile range 5 184–303 days) with wide dispersion and long tail was noted for the latency of the ACS per- formed closest to 182 days after admission. Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Overall Retained Activity Participation
  • 17. Activity Participation Outcome With and Without Somatosensory Impairment. Stroke survivors with somato- sensory loss and no paresis showed a significantly reduced percentage of retained activity participation compared with those with no sensory loss (z 5 1.96, p 5 .048, two-tailed). Compared with the median retained activity of 89.6% observed in the comparison group without either paresis or sensory impairments, the group with somatosensory im- pairment uncomplicated by paresis had a lower median retained activity of 81.6% (Figure 1). Because these samples exhibited significant skew (skew parameters were 5.4 and 3.6 times their respective standard errors), Mann–Whitney U tests were used to compare participation scores. These tests revealed that the 78.0% greater activity loss in the group with sensory impairment (18.4% loss from pre- stroke) was significantly higher than that of the comparison group (10.4% loss) without either sensory or motor im- pairment. Thus, somatosensory impairment, identified at hospital admission using the NIHSS, significantly predicts overall percentage of retained activity participation at a
  • 18. median of 222 days poststroke in stroke survivors with somatosensory impairment but no paresis. Activity Participation Outcome With Paresis. Both so- matosensory loss and paresis appear to be associated with lower total ACS retained activity scores (Figure 1), al- though all subgroups demonstrated a wide distribution of individual differences in retained activity scores. Despite the wide dispersion, several trends can be identified. Paresis had the typically expected marked effect on reducing activity participation (Figure 1). Paresis of the upper limb alone virtually doubled the median activity loss. Whereas this difference was more than comparable to the effect of sensory loss alone, the paresis effect only approached ac- cepted standards of statistical significance (z 5 1.90, p 5 .057, two-tailed), a result that needs to be interpreted in the context of the very small sample size. Paresis in both upper and lower limbs (without sensory loss) approxi- mately tripled the activity loss in groups without sensory
  • 19. impairment. This effect was both large and statistically significant (z 5 4.62, p < .001, two-tailed). The effect of sensory loss in cases with upper limb paresis was of smaller median magnitude (an additional Table 1. Distribution of Paresis and Somatosensory Loss Paresis Somatosensory Loss TotalUnimpaired Impaired Unimpaired 119 34 153 Upper limb only 16 11 27 Upper and lower limbs 43 45 88 Total 178 90 268 Figure 1. Box plots showing overall percentage of retained activity scores poststroke as measured by the Activity Card Sort (ACS) in six groups of stroke survivors classified according to somatosen- sory impairment and paresis. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles (i.e., the interquartile range [IQR]). The median (50th percentile) is represented by a line across and within the box. The whiskers extend from the upper and lower edges of
  • 20. the box to the highest and lowest values that are not greater than 1.5 times the IQR above or below the box limits. Outliers, represented by open circles, locate cases with values that deviate more than 1.5 times the IQR above or below the box limits. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p4 4.6% loss) compared with that observed in the group without paresis (additional 7.1% loss) and estimated with lower power from smaller samples. The effect was not significant. The expected strong influence of paresis on activity loss appeared to mask any additional effect from the presence of sensory loss (Figure 1). The median ac- tivity losses in the two subgroups with upper limb and lower limb paresis were the strongest of all six subgroups but comparable to each other (Figure 1) and their dif- ference not statistically significant. Figure 2. Box plots illustrating distributions of percentage of retained activity scores of the Activity Card Sort (ACS) domains. Light gray bars represent groups without somatosensory loss;
  • 21. dark gray bars indicate groups with sensory loss. Median values are rounded to the nearest integer value. Outliers, represented by open circles, indicate cases with values that deviate from the median by more than 1.5 to 3 times the IQR. Extreme outliers (values >3 times the IQR) are indicated by solid circles. 7203205100p5 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3 Effects of Somatosensory Impairment on Activity Card Sort Domains Investigation of the four domains of the total ACS sug- gested that the pattern revealed in the total score tended to repeat within each domain (Figure 2), the most parsi- monious hypothesis given that the four domains sum to the total ACS. In the absence of paresis, the ACS scores generally suggest that somatosensory impairment reduced participation recovery. Paresis tended to reduce recovery of participation, particularly for those with paresis in both upper and lower limbs. The effect of paresis in both limbs masked the effect of somatosensory loss. However, dif- ferences in the dispersions within groups for some do- mains, some variations in the magnitude of the median
  • 22. differences, and the small group sizes for those with upper limb paresis alone resulted in somewhat different results for the formal pairwise comparison sets. The adverse effect of somatosensory loss on recovery of activity was statistically significant for participants without paresis in low-demand leisure and social activities, but not in the other two domains (Table 2). An effect of somatosensory loss was not detected for samples with upper limb paresis (Table 2) in any of the domains, de- spite the suggestion of such an effect in Figure 2 for low- demand leisure, instrumental, and social activities. However, the sample sizes for these comparisons were small, re- sulting in lower statistical power. Paresis of the upper limb alone did not significantly lower activity recovery in any of the domains (Table 2), despite the suggestion of such an effect in Figure 2. The stronger adverse effect on activity caused by paresis of both limbs emerged clearly in all ACS domains, all of which showed statistically sig-
  • 23. nificant effects (Table 2). Comparability of Subgroups on Age, Gender, and Handedness Subgroups, divided according to paresis and somatosen- sory loss, were compared on age, gender, and hemispheric dominance. No statistically significant differences were noted. In particular, cross-tabulation of gender distribu- tion across sensory status revealed no statistically signifi- cant difference for the subsamples without paresis (for those with sensory loss, the female:male ratio was 50%:50%, whereas for those without sensory loss, the ratio was 52.1%:47.9%). The overwhelming pre- ponderance of right handers in the sample made domi- nance an unlikely potential confounder of the sensory loss effect found for cases without paresis. The obtained dis- tributions were very skewed for both subsamples without paresis (for those without sensory loss, the right:left ratio was 92%:8%, whereas for those with sensory loss, it
  • 24. was 97%:3%). Finally, age was very comparable for these subgroups, with means of 63.8 yr (SD 5 12.8) and 61.9 yr (SD 5 10.9), a difference that was not significant when evaluated by a t test. To further verify that age and gender did not con- found the observed relationship between sensory loss and recovery of participation, we conducted several multiple regressions using age, gender, latency of ACS tests, and di- chotomized sensory impairment scores as conjoint predictors of participation recovery measured by the ACS. Because the ACS distributions were significantly skewed, the IBM SPSS Statistics Generalized Linear Models subroutine was used to fit the data several times, making a variety of distributional assumptions. The regression model demonstrated that in the group without paresis, the association between somatosen- sory loss and the ACS score was statistically significant (p 5 .016), even after statistically controlling for the influence of age, gender, and latency of ACS. In contrast, multiple Table 2. Effects of Somatosensory and Motor Impairment on
  • 25. Activity Card Sort Domains Between Comparison Groups Comparison Groups Group A Group B High-Demand Leisure Activities Low-Demand Leisure Activities Instrumental Activities Social Activities Paresis Sensory Loss Paresis Sensory Loss z p z p z p z p No No No Yes 0.896 .371 2.567* .010* 1.564 .118 2.112* .035* Upper limb only No Upper limb only Yes 0.645 .557 1.559 .134 0.901 .904 0.687 .716 Upper and lower limbs No Upper and lower limbs Yes 0.143 .886 0.806 .420 0.698 .485 0.205 .837 No No Upper limb only No 1.420 .155 0.388 .698 1.615 .106 1.500 .133 No No Upper and lower limbs No 4.410* .000* 4.600* .000* 4.230* .000* 4.020* .000* Note. Summary of key statistics obtained from Mann–Whitney pairwise tests conducted for four sets of five comparisons that replicated the primary analysis for
  • 26. each of the four domains of the ACS. The first three comparisons present the effects of somatosensory loss for each paretic status (no paresis, upper limb only, upper and lower limbs). The last two comparisons present the effects of paresis when there is no measured somatosensory loss. *p < .05. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p6 regressions showed that the relationship between sensory loss and ACS scores was not significant (p 5 .49) in the group with paresis of an upper limb or in the group with paresis of both upper and lower limbs (p 5 .89). These results support the conclusions reached through nonparametric analyses, reported earlier in this article. Discussion Somatosensory impairment at hospital admission after stroke is associated with more activity participation loss compared with survivors without somatosensory loss, when there is no concomitant paresis. This predictive association was identified in a longitudinal cohort (N 5 268) of stroke survivors with mild impairment (median
  • 27. NIHSS score 5 3.00; interquartile range 5 1.00–5.00). It is based on a dichotomous measure of somatosensory impairment (NIHSS) obtained early poststroke, at the time of hospitalization, and on quantitative measurement of retained activity participation (ACS) in instrumental, leisure, and social activities at a median time of 222 days poststroke. This study addresses an identified gap in the literature, highlighted recently in two reviews (Carey et al., 2016; Meyer et al., 2014). Our finding is consistent with and advances the current literature on the effect of somato- sensory impairment on activity participation (Desrosiers et al., 2002; Hill et al., 2014; Morris, van Wijck, Joice, & Donaghy, 2013). It demonstrates a predictive relation- ship between somatosensory loss identified during the acute phase after stroke, using an acute screening mea- sure, and actual retained activities during the postacute phase, when people had completed rehabilitation, re- turned home, and were experiencing the impact of stroke
  • 28. on their activity participation (median 5 7.4 mo post- stroke). Previous studies have been cross-sectional (Hill et al., 2014) or have shown a predictive relationship from 2 to 4 wk after stroke or from hospital discharge to 6 mo poststroke (Desrosiers et al., 2002; Morris et al., 2013). Paresis affecting both upper and lower limbs, as expected, also caused a major reduction in the percentage of retained activity participation, an effect larger than that resulting from somatosensory deficit alone. Paresis with the addition of sensory deficit does not appear to further in- crease loss in participation when there is paresis of both upper and lower limbs, though a larger sample is needed to clarify whether paresis of the upper limb only has as del- eterious an effect on participation as when paresis is ac- companied by sensory loss. In other words, the data suggest that motor impairment in both upper and lower limbs is sufficient to cause loss of activity participation over a wide enough range of activities so that the addition of sensory impairment may be insufficient to compromise additional
  • 29. activity participation. Results suggest that the presence of loss of activity participation associated with paresis of both upper and lower limbs can mask the contribution of somatosensory loss and may promote an impression that somatosensory loss can be left untreated. However, as the group with sensory impairment unaccompanied by paresis shows, sensory impairment alone does significantly affect par- ticipation in everyday activities. Although an empirical investigation using interventions to manipulate motor and sensory impairments is ultimately necessary, the results raise the risk that if motor impairment only, and not sensory impairment, is addressed by intervention, the reduction in activity participation as a result of sensory loss will be unmasked. Addressing only motor impairment may lead to suboptimal therapeutic effects in patients whose somatosensory deficit has remained. Unless motor rehabilitation training addresses sensory
  • 30. deficits (indirectly), or perhaps the survivors’ confidence to engage in activities, outcomes for stroke survivors will likely be diminished. Historically, clinicians and researchers have given precedence to the motor sequelae of stroke, ne- glecting somatosensation (Kalra, 2010), but despite good movement capacity, survivors with sensory loss learn to not use their limb for task performance (Carey, 1995; Connell et al., 2013; Dannenbaum & Dykes, 1988). Therefore, according to our findings, sensory deficit alone has suffi- cient impact on retained activity participation to include a somatosensory intervention focus in therapeutic programs. Exploration of the effect of somatosensory impair- ment on the ACS domains revealed a significant impact on low-demand leisure and social activities but not on high- demand leisure and instrumental activities. The association with social and low-demand leisure activities is consistent with findings for social roles, as assessed with the Assess- ment of Life Habits (Desrosiers et al., 2002) and the social
  • 31. isolation and perceived physical activity subscales of the Nottingham Health Profile (Morris et al., 2013). Although the reduced statistical sensitivity resulting from small sample sizes must be noted, we did not find a significant association with instrumental activities, potentially at variance with the low but significant association between somatosensation at discharge and ADLs (Desrosiers et al., 2002). The data confirm clinical impressions that the range of individual differences in retained activity participation is large after stroke, even when there is neither sensory nor paretic impairment nor neglect or aphasia. Clearly, much remains to be added to predict activity participation poststroke beyond the measures used here. However, it is possible to anticipate 7203205100p7 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3 that these large individual differences cause strategic decisions about approach to therapy (e.g., ignore somatosensory im- pairment) difficult to make on the basis of clinical impres-
  • 32. sions and individual assessment alone. Population-wide, epidemiologically informed reasoning using empirical mea- surement of impairment is indicated by these results. Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research The sensory subscale of the NIHSS was used to identify the presence of somatosensory loss. Although this tool is commonly used to assess the presence and severity of acute neurological impairment and involves standardized ad- ministration, testing of somatosensation is not quantitative or supported by psychometric data. Studies using quanti- tative somatosensory measures with strong psychometric properties to better assess the nature and extent of the predictive relationship are indicated. A further benefit would be assessment of the ability of this commonly used clinical measure to predict quantitative somatosensory im- pairment tests with evaluated reliability and validity. The NIHSS was administered only once, at or within
  • 33. a few days of hospital admission after stroke. Therefore, when sensory loss, aphasia, neglect, or other NIHSS var- iables were involved, the analyses indicate a predictive re- lationship based on the patient status on these variables at admission. The level of sensory loss, neglect, aphasia, and so forth at the time of ACS administration is unknown. To protect against the possibility that an earlier ACS may have influenced the data, we conducted a post hoc analysis excluding participants from the sample with ACS scores obtained in the 92–155 days range. Findings from this trimmed sample (N 5 240) essentially remained the same: There was a somatosensory loss effect in the ab- sence of paresis (total retained activity participation: z 5 2.50, p 5 .012; low-demand leisure: z 5 2.90, p 5 .004; social: z 5 2.75, p 5 .006; instrumental z 5 2.04, p 5 .042) and a highly significant effect of paresis in both limbs for total retained activities and all subcategories (z 5 3.88–4.26, p < .001). Finally, use of existing data from the prospective
  • 34. cohort limited choice of measures and the demographic and organismic covariates that might be studied. Our initial findings of a predictive association does however encourage future studies to verify the observation. A design is rec- ommended that uses superior measurement tools for so- matosensory deficit and paresis and considers demographic or organismic variables that might arguably influence participation recovery or the strength of the relationship between somatosensory impairment and participation in a sufficiently large sample. Implications for Occupational Therapy Practice Our findings have the following implications for occu- pational therapy practice: • Practitioners should assess for the presence of somato- sensory impairment in stroke survivors, given its impact on participation, especially in those without concomitant paresis. • Rehabilitation directed at upper limb function and
  • 35. return to participation in previous life activities should consider investigation of the effect of interventions with a specific focus on somatosensory phenomena. • Therapeutic program planning should consider that the impact of somatosensory impairment may be un- masked when rehabilitation has been directed only at motor impairment recovery and the result has been below expectation. • Clinical evaluation and treatment in stroke have tradition- ally focused on addressing motor impairment without considering the impact of somatosensory functions. Our findings suggest that additional gain may be obtained in participation outcomes by augmenting motor-based inter- ventions with a somatosensory rehabilitation component. Investigations of this possibility are supported by the pre- sent findings. Conclusion Somatosensory impairment is associated with reduced activity participation. On the basis of evidence that so-
  • 36. matosensory improvements are obtained, a focus on so- matosensory intervention in rehabilitation is recommended for people with somatosensory impairment (Carey et al., 2011). Monitoring participation outcomes may yield evi- dence of additional improvements in participation asso- ciated with somatosensory intervention. Such investigation will be important in furthering evidence not only of the relationship between somatosensory impairment and par- ticipation but also the potential impact of somatosensory- focused interventions on participation outcomes. Our findings also indicate that paresis of the upper and lower limbs can mask the contribution of sensory loss. This finding suggests that the potential impact of somatosensa- tion on participation when movement returns should not be ignored. s The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p8 Acknowledgments
  • 37. We acknowledge financial support for this research from the James S. McDonnell Foundation (JSMF Grants 21002032 and 220020087, awarded to CB) and National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Grants 307902 and 1022694 (awarded to LMC) and support for analysis, write-up, and researchers from the James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Cognitive Rehabilitation Collaborative Award (JSMF Grant 220020413), the Victorian Government’s Opera- tional Infrastructure Support Program, an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (FT0992299), and an NHMRC Career Development Award (307905) awarded to LMC. We acknowledge the contribution of Lisa Tabor Connor to an earlier presentation of preliminary data re- lated to this study and to Tamara Tse for assistance with data compilation and checking. References Baum, C. M., & Edwards, D. F. (2001). Activity Card Sort (ACS): Test manual. St. Louis: Program in Occupational
  • 38. Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine. Blennerhassett, J. M., Carey, L. M., & Matyas, T. A. (2008). Clinical measures of handgrip limitation relate to impaired pinch grip force control after stroke. Journal of Hand Ther- apy, 21, 245–253. https://doi.org/10.1197/j.jht.2007.10.021 Blennerhassett, J. M., Matyas, T. A., & Carey, L. M. (2007). Impaired discrimination of surface friction contributes to pinch grip deficit after stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 21, 263–272. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1545968306295560 Borstad, A. L., & Nichols-Larsen, D. S. (2014). Assessing and treating higher level somatosensory impairments post stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 21, 290–295. https:// doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290 Brott, T., Adams, H. P., Jr., Olinger, C. P., Marler, J. R., Barsan, W. G., Biller, J., . . . Hertzberg, V. (1989). Mea- surements of acute cerebral infarction: A clinical examina- tion scale. Stroke, 20, 864–870. https://doi.org/10.1161/ 01.STR.20.7.864 Carey, L. M. (1995). Somatosensory loss after stroke. Critical Reviews in Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 7, 51–91. https://doi.org/10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v7.i1.40
  • 39. Carey, L. M. (2012). Touch and body sensations. In L. M. Carey (Ed.), Stroke rehabilitation: Insights from neuroscience and imaging (pp. 157–172). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199797882.003.0012 Carey, L. M. (2015). Person factors. In C. H. Christiansen, C. M. Baum, & J. Bass (Eds.), Occupational therapy: En- abling performance, participation and well-being (4th ed., pp. 249–265). Thorofare, NJ: Slack. Carey, L. M., Lamp, G., & Turville, M. (2016). The state-of- the-science of somatosensory function and its impact on daily life in adults and older adults, and following stroke: A scoping review. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health, 36, 27S–41S. https://doi.org/10.1177/1539449216643941 Carey, L., Macdonell, R., & Matyas, T. A. (2011). SENSe: Study of the Effectiveness of Neurorehabilitation on Sen- sation: A randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 25, 304–313. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1545968310397705 Carey, L. M., & Matyas, T. A. (2011). Frequency of discrim- inative sensory loss in the hand after stroke in a rehabili- tation setting. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 43,
  • 40. 257–263. https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-0662 Connell, L. A., McMahon, N. E., & Adams, N. (2013). Stroke survivors’ experiences of somatosensory impairment after stroke: An interpretive phenomenological analysis. Physio- therapy, 100, 150–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013. 09.003 Dannenbaum, R. M., & Dykes, R. W. (1988). Sensory loss in the hand after sensory stroke: Therapeutic rationale. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 69, 833–839. Desrosiers, J., Noreau, L., Rochette, A., Bravo, G., & Boutin, C. (2002). Predictors of handicap situations following post- stroke rehabilitation. Disability and Rehabilitation, 24, 774–785. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638280210125814 Edwards, D. F., Hahn, M., Baum, C., & Dromerick, A. W. (2006). The impact of mild stroke on meaningful activity and life satisfaction. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases, 15, 151–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis. 2006.04.001 Everard, K. M., Lach, H. W., Fisher, E. B., & Baum, M. C.
  • 41. (2000). Relationship of activity and social support to the functional health of older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 55, S208–S212. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.4.S208 Hill, V. A., Fisher, T., Schmid, A. A., Crabtree, J., & Page, S. J. (2014). Relationship between touch sensation of the affected hand and performance of valued activities in in- dividuals with chronic stroke. Topics in Stroke Rehabilita- tion, 21, 339–346. https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-339 Jeannerod, M., Michel, F., & Prablanc, C. (1984). The control of hand movements in a case of hemianaesthesia following a parietal lesion. Brain, 107, 899–920. https://doi.org/ 10.1093/brain/107.3.899 Kalra, L. (2010). Stroke rehabilitation 2009: Old chestnuts and new insights. Stroke, 41, e88–e90. https://doi.org/10.1161/ STROKEAHA.109.572297 Kessner, S. S., Bingel, U., & Thomalla, G. (2016). Somato- sensory deficits after stroke: A scoping review. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 23, 136–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 10749357.2015.1116822 Klit, H., Finnerup, N. B., Andersen, G., & Jensen, T. S. (2011). Central poststroke pain: A population-based study. Pain,
  • 42. 152, 818–824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.030 Kong, K. H., Chua, K. S., & Lee, J. (2011). Recovery of upper limb dexterity in patients more than 1 year after stroke: Fre- quency, clinical correlates and predictors. NeuroRehabilita- tion, 28, 105–111. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0639 7203205100p9 May/June 2018, Volume 72, Number 3 https://doi.org/10.1197/j.jht.2007.10.021 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968306295560 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968306295560 https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290 https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-290 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.20.7.864 https://doi.org/10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v7.i1.40 https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199797882.003.0012 https://doi.org/10.1177/1539449216643941 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968310397705 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968310397705 https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-0662 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013.09.003 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physio.2013.09.003 https://doi.org/10.1080/09638280210125814 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2006.04.001 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2006.04.001 https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.4.S208 https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr2104-339 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.3.899 https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/107.3.899 https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.572297 https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.572297 https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2015.1116822
  • 43. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2015.1116822 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.030 https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-2011-0639 Law, M., Baptiste, S., Carswell, A., McColl, M. A., Polatajko, H., & Pollock, N. (2014). Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (5th ed.). Ottawa: CAOT Publications. Lawrence, E. S., Coshall, C., Dundas, R., Stewart, J., Rudd, A. G., Howard, R., & Wolfe, C. D. A. (2001). Estimates of the prevalence of acute stroke impairments and disabil- ity in a multiethnic population. Stroke, 32, 1279–1284. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1279 Meyer, S., Karttunen, A. H., Thijs, V., Feys, H., & Verheyden, G. (2014). How do somatosensory deficits in the arm and hand relate to upper limb impairment, activity, and participation problems after stroke? A systematic review. Physical Therapy, 94, 1220–1231. https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20130271 Morris, J. H., van Wijck, F., Joice, S., & Donaghy, M. (2013). Predicting health related quality of life 6 months after stroke: The role of anxiety and upper limb dysfunction. Disability and Rehabilitation, 35, 291–299. https://doi. org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942 Muir, K. W., Weir, C. J., Murray, G. D., Povey, C., & Lees, K. R. (1996). Comparison of neurological scales and scor- ing systems for acute stroke prognosis. Stroke, 27, 1817–1820. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.27.10.1817 Nowak, D. A., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). Grip force behavior during object manipulation in neurological disorders: To- ward an objective evaluation of manual performance def-
  • 44. icits. Movement Disorders, 20, 11–25. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/mds.20299 Park, S. W., Wolf, S. L., Blanton, S., Winstein, C., & Nichols- Larsen, D. S. (2008). The EXCITE trial: Predicting a clinically meaningful motor activity log outcome. Neuro- rehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22, 486–493. https://doi. org/10.1177/1545968308316906 Reding, M. J., & Potes, E. (1988). Rehabilitation outcome following initial unilateral hemispheric stroke: Life table analysis approach. Stroke, 19, 1354–1358. https://doi.org/ 10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354 Tse, T., Douglas, J., Lentin, P., & Carey, L. (2013). Measuring participation after stroke: A review of frequently used tools. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 94, 177–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2012.09.002 Tyson, S. F., Hanley, M., Chillala, J., Selley, A. B., & Tallis, R. C. (2008). Sensory loss in hospital-admitted people with stroke: Characteristics, associated factors, and relationship with function. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 22, 166–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1545968307305523 Winward, C. E., Halligan, P. W., & Wade, D. T. (2002). The
  • 45. Rivermead Assessment of Somatosensory Performance (RASP): Standardization and reliability data. Clinical Rehabilitation, 16, 523–533. https://doi.org/10.1191/ 0269215502cr522oa Wolf, T. J., Baum, C., & Connor, L. T. (2009). Changing face of stroke: Implications for occupational therapy practice. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63, 621–625. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.63.5.621 World Health Organization. (2001). International classification of functioning, disability and health. Geneva: Author. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 7203205100p10 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.32.6.1279 https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20130271 https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942 https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2012.691942 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.27.10.1817 https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20299 https://doi.org/10.1002/mds.20299 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308316906 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308316906 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354 https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.19.11.1354 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2012.09.002 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968307305523 https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968307305523 https://doi.org/10.1191/0269215502cr522oa https://doi.org/10.1191/0269215502cr522oa https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.63.5.621
  • 46. Copyright of American Journal of Occupational Therapy is the property of American Occupational Therapy Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. APA FOMRAT no plagiarizing original work only #5 Identify the variables in the study. What is being examined or measured? For the variables, provide an operational definition, Identify the instruments or strategies used to collect data on each of the variables, and identify the scale of measurement for the data related to each variable under examination. #6 For each of the instruments, discuss the data related to the validity and reliability of each. Compare the data found to the concepts of validity/ reliability, are these instruments considered valid and reliable why? Explain