SlideShare a Scribd company logo
This article was downloaded by: [Texas Woman's University]
On: 30 September 2014, At: 06:11
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered
Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T
3JH, UK
Early Child Development and Care
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gecd20
Comparisons of levels and predictors of
mothers' and fathers' engagement with
their preschool-aged children
Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan a , Letitia E. Kotila a , Rongfang Jia
a ,
Sarah N. Lang a & Daniel J. Bower a
a Human Development and Family Science , The Ohio State
University , Columbus , OH , USA
Published online: 03 Sep 2012.
To cite this article: Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan , Letitia E. Kotila
, Rongfang Jia , Sarah N. Lang &
Daniel J. Bower (2013) Comparisons of levels and predictors of
mothers' and fathers' engagement
with their preschool-aged children, Early Child Development
and Care, 183:3-4, 498-514, DOI:
10.1080/03004430.2012.711596
To link to this article:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.711596
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of
all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.
However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or
warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and
views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The
accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified
with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any
losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other
liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection
with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private
study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling,
loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is
expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Comparisons of levels and predictors of mothers’ and fathers’
engagement with their preschool-aged children
Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan∗ , Letitia E. Kotila, Rongfang Jia,
Sarah N. Lang and
Daniel J. Bower
Human Development and Family Science, The Ohio State
University, Columbus, OH, USA
(Received 9 December 2011; final version received 30 March
2012)
Self-report data from 112 two-parent families were used to
compare levels and
predictors of four types of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement
with their
preschool-aged children: socialisation, didactic, caregiving, and
physical play.
Mothers were more involved than fathers in socialisation,
didactic, and
caregiving, whereas fathers were more involved than mothers in
physical play.
Mothers’ greatest engagement was in caregiving, whereas
fathers were about
equally engaged in didactic, caregiving, and physical play.
Mothers who
contributed more to family income were less engaged in
socialisation and
caregiving, whereas fathers with non-traditional beliefs about
their roles were
more engaged in didactic and caregiving. Children with greater
temperamental
effortful control received more didactic and physical play
engagement from
mothers. Fathers were more likely to engage in socialisation
activities with
earlier-born children, whereas mothers were more likely to
engage in
socialisation with girls high in effortful control. Mothers were
more likely to
engage in physical play with boys and with later-born children.
Keywords: father engagement; mother engagement; child
temperament; child
gender; birth order; caregiving; physical play
Developmentally appropriate engagement by mothers and
fathers has been linked to
positive outcomes for children (Sarkadi, Kristiansson,
Oberklaid, & Bremberg,
2008). In the past 30 – 40 years fathers’ engagement with their
children has increased
(Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004); however, there is still significant
variability in fathers’
engagement with young children, even in families headed by
married parents
(Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, & Hofferth, 2001). Fathers are
no less capable than
mothers at providing children with sensitive, responsive care
(Lamb, 1997), yet
mothers are most often the primary caregivers for children
regardless of employment
status (Bianchi, Robinson, & Milkie, 2006).
This distinction has shaped the ways in which research has
examined mothers’ and
fathers’ parenting. Whereas research on mothers’ parenting has
primarily focused on
the quality of maternal behaviour, research on fathers’ parenting
has focused more
on the quantity of involvement (Adamsons & Buehler, 2007).
Relatively rare are quan-
titative comparisons of maternal and paternal engagement, using
parents from the same
families (LaFlamme, Pomerleau, & Malcuit, 2002; McBride &
Mills, 1993), even
# 2013 Taylor & Francis
∗ Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Early Child Development and Care, 2013
Vol. 183, Nos. 3 – 4, 498 – 514,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.711596
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
though fathers’ and mothers’ interactions with children are
necessarily intertwined
(Pleck & Hofferth, 2008).
The use of qualitatively distinct conceptualisations and
measures of maternal and
paternal parenting has made comparisons within and across
studies difficult. As a
result, we have a limited understanding of the ways in which
mothers and fathers inter-
act with their children, and the extent to which patterns and
predictors of engagement
are similar or different. Understanding the patterns and
predictors of mothers’ and
fathers’ engagement with their children is critical to informing
the continued develop-
ment of theoretical models about parenting and fathering, and
important to the design of
educational programmes intended to promote positive parental
engagement. Thus, the
purpose of this study was to compare mothers’ and fathers’
involvement in four types of
engagement with their preschool-aged children: socialisation,
didactic, caregiving, and
physical play, as well as to examine parent and child
characteristics as predictors of
mothers’ and fathers’ engagement in these domains.
Conceptualisation of parental engagement
Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, and Levine (1985) identified three
domains of involvement:
engagement, responsibility, and accessibility. Engagement is
most often a focus of
research due to its strong associations with child outcomes
(Tamis-LeMonda,
Shannon, Cabrera, & Lamb, 2004). In their research on non-
resident father involve-
ment, Cabrera et al. (2004) distinguished four subtypes of
meaningful engagement
activities: socialisation (e.g. taking the child to family,
community, or social events),
didactic (e.g. reading or singing with the child), caregiving (e.g.
assisting the child
with eating or other physical care activities), and physical play
(e.g. playing outside
with the child).
The socialisation aspect of engagement involves the roles of
parents in linking chil-
dren with the outside world (Parke & Ladd, 1992). Some have
argued that fathers may
play a special role in child socialisation (Paquette, 2004).
Fathers provide children with
new experiences, companionship during these experiences, and
knowledge and advice
about them (Hewlett, 1992). In families with infants, however,
Laflamme et al. (2002)
found that mothers were more involved than fathers in taking
children on ‘outings’ in
the neighbourhood and community. Nonetheless, comparisons of
parental participation
in family and community activities with preschool children are
unknown.
Didactic interactions are those in which parents encourage
children to engage in and
understand their environments, as well as providing the
opportunity to observe, learn,
and imitate behaviours (Bornstein, 2002). Although didactic
interactions between
father and child have shown the strongest links with positive
child development
(Bianchi et al., 2006; Yeung et al., 2001), most research has
focused on didactic inter-
actions between mother and child, and studies comparing
parents’ involvement in this
domain are rare. In an exception, Yeung et al. (2001) compared
mothers’ and fathers’
time with children in various activities and found that fathers
spent 1/3 of the time that
mothers did in didactic activities such as reading, educational
play, and studying with
children ages 0 – 9.
Engagement in childcare activities is somewhat different from
the other types of
engagement, because these interactions may be more task- and
routine-oriented, and
are arguably less ‘elective’ than other forms of parent
engagement. Yet, even routine
childcare tasks performed by parents are important for children.
When fathers are
more involved in caregiving activities such as feeding, bathing,
and putting their
Early Child Development and Care 499
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
child to bed, they have more opportunities to get to know their
children and develop
confidence in their parenting (Barry, Smith, Deutsch, & Perry-
Jenkins, 2011),
thereby promoting the formation and maintenance of the close
relationships with chil-
dren that facilitate their socio-emotional development.
Whereas mothers are believed to specialise in caregiving,
popular notions suggest
that fathers spend the majority of their time in ‘play’ activities
with their children, and
most findings indicate that fathers do spend a greater proportion
of time in physical play
with their children than mothers (Paquette, 2004; Yeung et al.,
2001). Parent – child
physical play peaks during the preschool years, a period during
which most children
rapidly develop perceptual, motor, cognitive, emotional, and
social functioning
(Paquette, Carbonneau, Dubeau, Bigras, & Tremblay, 2003),
and provides important
opportunities for children to build skills important for peer
competence (Carson &
Parke, 1996; MacDonald & Parke, 1984).
Predictors of parental engagement
Two frameworks were used to identify key potential predictors
of mothers’ and fathers’
engagement with their children: the Lamb – Pleck model of the
determinants of father
involvement (Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, & Levine, 1987) and
Belsky’s (1984) process
model of parenting. The Lamb – Pleck model proposed four
categories of influence
on father involvement: (1) motivation (e.g. fathers’ desire for
involvement), (2) skills
and self-confidence (e.g. parenting self-efficacy), (3) social
supports and stresses
(e.g. marital relationships), and (4) institutional factors (e.g.
workplace policies).
Belsky’s (1984) process model described three categories of
influence on parenting
behaviours: (1) parent characteristics (e.g. personality), (2)
child characteristics (e.g.
temperament, age, and gender), and (3) contextual sources of
stress and support (e.g.
marital relationships). In this study, we focused on parent and
child characteristics –
specifically, mothers’ contributions to family income and
parents’ beliefs about
fathers’ roles, couple relationship satisfaction, and child
effortful control, gender, and
birth order.
Parent characteristics
According to a set of perspectives described as the ‘relative
resources hypothesis’ (Col-
trane, 2000, p. 1214), partners use resources that they bring to
the relationship to nego-
tiate lighter workloads at home. This hypothesis has been
supported by research on
household labour (Coltrane, 2000); when women contribute
more to family income
their divisions of household labour with their partners are more
equal. Part of the
move towards equality when mothers contribute greater
resources is due to greater
father involvement in childrearing (Pleck & Masciadrelli,
2004). But, it is not necess-
arily clear that mothers with greater relative earnings reduce
their involvement in child-
rearing, as they may be unable or unwilling to do so (Craig &
Mullan, 2011).
Parents’ beliefs about fathers’ roles, a key component of the
motivation factor of the
Lamb – Pleck (Lamb et al., 1987) model, have also been linked
to levels of paternal
involvement with children. Fathers with more non-traditional
beliefs about their roles
are more engaged with their children (Beitel & Parke, 1998;
Freeman, Newland, &
Coyl, 2008). Moreover, mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about
fathers’ roles have
been linked to greater paternal engagement as well (Fagan &
Barnett, 2003),
500 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
whereas mothers who view the paternal role as surrogate to the
maternal role have part-
ners who are less involved (Beitel & Parke, 1998).
Fathers’ parenting has been described as especially sensitive to
the family context
(Doherty, Kouneski, & Erickson, 1998), and in particular
affected by the quality of the
father’s relationship with the child’s mother. However, the
quality of the marital
relationship also figures prominently in Belsky’s (1984) model,
which was developed
primarily based on research with mothers. Indeed, greater
marital satisfaction has been
linked to mother as well as to father involvement (Carlson,
Pilkauskas, McLanahan, &
Brooks-Gunn, 2011; Mehall, Spinrad, Eisenberg, & Gaertner,
2009).
Child characteristics
Effortful control is the ability to voluntarily focus and shift
attention and to initiate or
inhibit behaviours that emerges in infancy, develops rapidly
during the toddler years,
and plays an important role in the regulation and control of
early emotional reactions
and behaviours (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Children with higher
levels of effortful
control, who can better control their emotions and behaviour,
may make parental
engagement more rewarding, thereby strengthening parents’
motivation for further
engagement. To our knowledge, studies of child effortful
control and parental engage-
ment are absent in the literature, but a few relevant studies have
found prospective
associations between poorer child attentional and behavioural
regulation and poorer
parenting quality (Bridgett et al., 2009; Eisenberg et al., 1999).
Research has also suggested associations of child birth order
and child gender with
parental involvement. When differences are found, parent
involvement is greater with
first-born than with later-born children (Bègue & Roché, 2005;
Price, 2008). Differ-
ences by child gender are not always found, but when they are,
child gender is associ-
ated more closely with father than with mother involvement,
and fathers of sons are
more involved than fathers of daughters (Lundberg, McLanahan,
& Rose, 2007;
Raley & Bianchi, 2006). Child gender and birth order may also
interact. Yoshida
(2012) reported that fathers were more involved in physical care
when there was a
young male child (less than five years old) in the family, but
that the presence of
older, school-aged children reduced fathers’ involvement.
The present study
The data used were drawn from a study of preschool-aged
children in 112 two-parent
families. Our first goal was to examine mothers’ and fathers’
involvement in four types
of engagement: socialisation, didactic, caregiving, and physical
play. Consistent with
research reviewed above, we hypothesised that mothers would
be more frequently
engaged in didactic and caregiving activities with children than
fathers, whereas
fathers would be more frequently engaged in physical play
activities with children
than mothers. We did not advance a hypothesis with respect to
parent gender differ-
ences in socialisation. We further expected that relative to their
engagement in other
domains, mothers would be most frequently engaged in
caregiving activities and
least frequently engaged in physical play, whereas fathers
would be more frequently
engaged in physical play relative to other types of engagement.
Our second goal was to examine the contributions of parent and
child characteristics
to maternal and paternal engagement. Consistent with theory
and prior research, we
expected that when mothers made a greater contribution to
family income they
Early Child Development and Care 501
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
would be less frequently engaged in caregiving (but not
necessarily other) activities but
fathers would be more frequently engaged in caregiving. When
mothers and fathers
held more non-traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles, we
anticipated that fathers
would be more frequently engaged in activities with their
children – especially in car-
egiving, which has been traditionally associated with the
maternal role. We also
hypothesised that both fathers and mothers would be more
engaged across domains
when couples reported greater relationship satisfaction.
With respect to child characteristics, we anticipated that parents
would be more fre-
quently engaged in activities with their children – especially
socialisation, didactic, and
physical play, which are arguably more ‘elective’ than
caregiving – when children had
higher temperamental effortful control. Consistent with the vast
majority of the litera-
ture, we also expected that parents would be more frequently
engaged in activities with
first-born children than with later-born children, and that child
gender would be associ-
ated with fathers’ but not mothers’ engagement, with
differences in father engagement
in favour of sons.
Finally, we also tested child gender as a moderator of
associations of other parent
and child characteristics with parental engagement. Consistent
with the notion that
father involvement is especially sensitive to contextual factors
(Doherty et al., 1998),
McBride, Schoppe, and Rane (2002) found that less sociable
female children had
less involved fathers, but sociability was not associated with
father involvement with
sons. In contrast, mothers’ involvement was not associated with
child temperament
or gender. Thus, we expected that father engagement with sons
would be more
robust to contextual influences than father engagement with
daughters, but that there
would be no moderating effects of child gender for mothers.
Method
Participants
One-hundred and twelve families consisting of mothers, fathers,
and a preschool-aged focal
child (M ¼ 4.12 years; SD ¼ 0.52; 58 boys, 54 girls) were
recruited to participate through
local preschools, advertisements, and participant referrals in a
large Midwestern metropo-
litan area. Mothers’ average age was 36.03 years (SD ¼ 5.26),
and fathers’ average age
was 37.73 years (SD ¼ 5.74). Eighty-three percent of mothers
and 81% percent of
fathers had obtained at least a college degree (Range ¼ some
high school to Ph.D.).
Family income ranged from less than $10,000 to over $100,000
per year (Mdn ¼
$71,000 – 80,000). Seventy-seven percent of children were
White, non-Hispanic, 9%
African American, 1% Hispanic, 1% Asian, and 13%
multiracial/multiethnic. Sixty
percent of focal children were first-born, 27% were second-
born, and 14% were third- or
later-born. Families had 2.23 children on average (SD ¼ 0.98;
Range ¼ 1 – 7). Thirty-six
percent of mothers were working full time (31+ hours per
week), 23% were working
part time (,10 – 30 hours per week), and 42% were not
employed outside the home.
Eighty-seven percent of fathers were working full time, 8% part
time, and 6% were not
employed. On average, mothers contributed 25.51% of the
family income (SD ¼ 27.71%).
Measures
Mothers and fathers each reported their own level of
engagement with their preschool
age child using the Activities with Child Scale created for the
Early Head Start
502 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Research and Evaluation Project and the Early Childhood
Longitudinal Birth Cohort
(Cabrera et al., 2004). Parents indicated the frequency of their
involvement (1 ¼ not
at all; 6 ¼ more than once a day) in 30 activities with their
child within the last
month. These items can be divided into 5 subscales (see Cabrera
& Mitchell, 2009):
socialisation (9 items; e.g. ‘Visit friends with child’; a ¼ 0.64
for mothers and a ¼
0.65 for fathers), management (3 items; e.g. ‘Take child to
doctor’; a ¼ 0.55 for
mothers and a ¼ 0.38 for fathers), didactic (5 items; e.g. ‘Play
with building toys
with child’; a ¼ 0.71 for both mothers and fathers), physical
play (6 items; e.g.
‘Play chasing games with child’; a ¼ 0.68 for mothers and a ¼
0.71 for fathers),
and caregiving (7 items; e.g. ‘Put child to bed’; a ¼ 0.63 for
mothers and a ¼ 0.70
for fathers). Because of very low reliability, the management
component of parental
engagement was not considered further.
Each parent completed the What Is a Father? (WIAF)
questionnaire (Schoppe,
2001), a 15-item measure adapted from Palkovitz’s (1984) Role
of the Father Question-
naire. Items on the WIAF ask respondents to rate statements
concerning fathers and
fathering from 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 5 ¼ strongly agree. Five
items tap non-tra-
ditional beliefs (e.g. ‘Fathers are just as sensitive in caring for
children as mothers
are’), and five items tap traditional beliefs (e.g. ‘The father’s
role is to provide for
his family, not babysit the children’). Ratings on the five
traditional items were
reverse-scored and averaged with ratings on the five non-
traditional items. Thus,
higher scores indicated the presence of more non-traditional
beliefs about fathers and
the absence of more traditional beliefs. Cronbach’s alpha for
mothers was 0.66 and
for fathers 0.60.
Mothers and fathers reported on their relationship satisfaction
using the 32-item
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976), a widely-used,
reliable, and valid measure
of relationship satisfaction. Summary scores were created for
mothers (a ¼ 0.92)
and fathers (a ¼ 0.91), and a couple-level score was created by
averaging parents’
reports, which were strongly correlated, r ¼ 0.59, p , 0.01.
Parents reported on their child’s temperament using the 36-item
Very Short Form of
the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, appropriate for child
children aged 3 – 8
(Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). The questionnaire contains
statements about children
that parents rate on a scale from 1 ¼ extremely untrue of my
child to 7 ¼ extremely
true of my child. Parents’ ratings of child effortful control (12
items; e.g. ‘When
drawing or coloring in a book, shows strong concentration’ ‘Is
good at following
instructions’) were the focus of this study (a ¼ 0.73 for mothers
and a ¼ 0.72 for
fathers). Parents’ ratings were significantly and moderately
correlated, r ¼ 0.42, p ,
0.01, and thus averaged.
Mothers and fathers reported demographic information for
themselves and their
families on a questionnaire created for the study. We focused on
mothers’ reports of
their percent contribution to the family income, child gender (0
¼ boy; 1 ¼ girl),
and child birth order (higher scores indicated that the focal
child was later-born).
Results
Comparisons of mothers’ vs. fathers’ engagement
Mean levels of the four aspects of engagement for mothers and
fathers are reported in
Table 1. Parents reported engaging in socialisation activities
approximately a few times
per month, didactic activities a few times a week to about once
a day, caregiving
Early Child Development and Care 503
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
activities a few times a week to about once a day, and physical
play a few times a month
to a few times a week. Paired samples t-tests revealed that
mothers and fathers differed
significantly on all four aspects of engagement (Figure 1).
Mothers engaged in greater
socialisation, t(110) ¼ 8.19, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.78,
didactic, t(110) ¼ 4.37, p
, 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.41, and caregiving, t(110) ¼ 8.08, p ,
0.01, Cohen’s d ¼
0.77, than fathers. Fathers engaged in greater physical play than
mothers, t(110) ¼
24.64, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.44.
Figure 1. Comparisons of mothers’ vs. fathers’ mean levels of
engagement.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of study variables.
M SD Range
Parental engagement
M Socialisation 3.20 0.42 2.00 – 4.44
F Socialisation 2.81 0.42 1.25 – 3.78
M Didactic 4.25 0.75 2.60 – 5.80
F Didactic 3.83 0.76 1.80 – 5.60
M Caregiving 4.67 0.61 2.71 – 5.86
F Caregiving 4.04 0.64 2.14 – 5.57
M Physical Play 3.41 0.71 1.83 – 5.33
F Physical Play 3.79 0.69 2.33 – 5.50
Predictors
M Income 25.51 27.71 0.00 – 100.00
Birth order 1.59 0.91 1.00 – 6.00
Effortful control 5.39 0.57 3.25 – 6.55
M non-traditional 3.56 0.48 2.00 – 4.90
F non-traditional 3.50 0.43 2.20 – 4.70
Relationship satisfaction 3.58 0.38 2.39 – 4.52
Note: M, Mother; F, Father.
504 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
No aspects of mothers’ or fathers’ engagement differed by child
gender. Corre-
lations among the four aspects of engagement for mothers and
fathers (see Table 2)
revealed that when parents were more involved in one aspect of
engagement, they
tended to be more involved in the other aspects as well. Within
families, mothers’
and fathers’ engagement in socialisation and physical play were
significantly and posi-
tively correlated, but there were no significant correlations for
didactic or caregiving.
A within-subjects ANOVA with parent gender and engagement
type as within-sub-
jects factors was used to further test whether the distributions of
mothers’ and fathers’
involvement across the four types of engagement differed.
There was a main effect of
parent gender, F(1, 110) ¼ 21.98, p , 0.01, and of engagement
type, F(1, 108) ¼
258.42, p , 0.01. These main effects were qualified by a
significant parent gender X
engagement type interaction, F(1, 108) ¼ 53.81, p , 0.01. Thus,
the patterns of
mothers’ and fathers’ involvement across engagement types
differed, such that
mothers were most frequently engaged in caregiving activities,
with the least frequent
engagement in physical play, whereas fathers were about
equally involved in didactic,
caregiving, and physical play.
Predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement
Correlations among the predictor variables and between the
predictors and the four
aspects of engagement for mothers and fathers are shown in
Table 2. Hierarchical
regression analyses were used to further examine the relative
contributions of parent
and child predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement in
these four types of activi-
ties. Eight equations were computed – four predicting the types
of engagement for
mothers and four predicting the types of engagement for fathers.
In each equation,
mothers’ contributions to the family income and the
corresponding type of involvement
by the other parent were entered together on the first step,
followed by the three child
predictors on the second step: child gender, birth order, and
effortful control. Mothers’
and fathers’ progressive and traditional beliefs about fathers’
roles and relationship sat-
isfaction were entered together on the third step. On the fourth
step, the birth order X
child gender and effortful control X child gender interactions
were entered. Finally,
on the fifth step, the three remaining two-way interactions
between child gender and
each of the parent predictors (mothers’ and fathers’ non-
traditional beliefs and relation-
ship satisfaction) were entered. Continuous variables were
mean-centered prior to the
computation of product terms. Results for mothers and fathers
are shown in Tables 3
and 4, respectively. Significant interactions were probed using
an SPSS macro
created by Hayes and Matthes (2009).
Mothers’ engagement
The more mothers contributed to family income, the less
involved they were in socia-
lisation activities with their children, b ¼ 20.22, p , 0.05.
Mothers were more
engaged in socialisation when fathers were more engaged in
socialisation, b ¼ 0.36,
p , 0.01. None of the child or parent characteristics explained
significant variance
in mothers’ engagement in socialisation on steps 2 or 3.
However, the interaction
between effortful control and child gender (b ¼ 0.37, p , 0.01)
was significant on
step 4. Post hoc probing of this interaction revealed that greater
effortful control was
associated with greater maternal engagement in socialisation for
mothers of daughters,
b ¼ 0.39, p , 0.01, but not for mothers of sons, b ¼ 20.05, p ¼
0.57. None of the
Early Child Development and Care 505
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Table 2. Correlations between parental engagement and parent
and child characteristics.
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14.
1. M Socialisation 1.00 0.32∗ ∗ 0.46∗ ∗ 20.04 0.10 20.06
0.42∗ ∗ 0.04 20.20∗ 0.05 0.16 20.13 20.06 0.03
2. F Socialisation 1.00 0.11 0.35∗ ∗ 20.04 0.23∗ 0.08 0.29∗ ∗
0.07 20.25∗ ∗ 0.21∗ 20.03 0.07 0.14
3. M Didactic 1.00 0.09 0.24∗ ∗ 0.10 0.54∗ ∗ 0.23∗ 20.07
20.02 0.31∗ ∗ 0.02 20.02 0.13
4. F Didactic 1.00 0.01 0.41∗ ∗ 20.02 0.58∗ ∗ 0.16 20.17 0.14
0.06 0.23∗ 0.09
5. M Caregiving 1.00 0.12 0.30∗ ∗ 0.04 20.21∗ 20.01 0.10
20.10 20.08 20.06
6. F Caregiving 1.00 0.08 0.33∗ ∗ 0.06 20.19∗ 20.06 0.17
0.25∗ ∗ 0.03
7. M Physical Play 1.00 0.26∗ ∗ 20.05 0.19∗ 0.25∗ ∗ 20.14
20.07 0.03
8. F Physical Play 1.00 0.07 20.05 0.07 20.08 0.01 20.01
9. M income 1.00 20.15 0.10 0.32∗ ∗ 0.16 20.12
10. Birth order 1.00 20.07 20.10 20.07 0.09
11. Effortful control 1.00 0.02 0.06 0.15
12. M non-traditional 1.00 0.48∗ ∗ 0.00
13. F non-traditional 1.00 0.03
14. Relationship
satisfaction
1.00
Note: M, Mother; F, Father.
∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01.
5
0
6
S
.J.
S
ch
o
p
p
e-S
u
lliva
n
et
al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Table 3. Hierarchical regression predicting mothers’
engagement.
Mother socialisation Mother didactic Mother Caregiving Mother
physical play
Predictor b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF
Step 1
% Income by M 20.22∗ 0.17 10.35∗ ∗ 20.08 0.01 0.63 20.22∗
0.06 3.15∗ 20.07 0.05 2.87†
F Engagement 0.36∗ ∗ 0.08 0.10 0.22∗
Step 2
Child gender 0.00 0.04 1.49 20.13 0.11 4.18∗ ∗ 20.08 0.02 0.73
20.20∗ 0.14 6.08∗ ∗
Birthord 0.09 0.01 20.01 0.24∗
Effortful 0.14 0.36∗ ∗ 0.15 0.31∗ ∗
Step 3
M NTrad 20.02 0.00 0.10 0.13 0.02 0.69 20.01 0.03 0.91 0.03
0.02 1.03
F NTrad 20.04 20.09 20.06 20.16
Satisf 20.08 0.08 20.15 20.06
Step 4
BirthOrd × Gen 0.15 0.08 5.26∗ ∗ 0.20† 0.03 1.75 20.04 0.00
0.10 0.03 0.01 0.81
Effortful × Gen 0.37∗ ∗ 0.11 20.04 0.16
Step 5
Satisf × Gen 0.13 0.03 1.07 20.22 0.03 1.35 0.04 0.08 3.12∗
0.19 0.02 0.68
M NTrad × Gen 0.05 0.19 20.27† 0.01
F NTrad × Gen 0.09 0.00 0.52∗ ∗ 0.08
Note: M, Mother; F, Father; BirthOrd, Birth order; Effortful,
Child’s effortful control; NTrad, Non-traditional beliefs
regarding fathers; Satisf, Couple relationship satisfaction;
Gen, Child gender.
†p , 0.10; ∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01.
E
a
rly
C
h
ild
D
evelo
p
m
en
t
a
n
d
C
a
re
5
0
7
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Table 4. Hierarchical regression predicting fathers’ engagement.
Predictor
Father socialisation Father didactic Father caregiving Father
physical play
b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF
Step 1
% Income by M 0.15 0.14 8.29∗ ∗ 0.16 0.03 1.63 0.08 0.02 0.79
0.08 0.05 2.96†
M Engagement 0.37∗ ∗ 0.08 0.11 0.22∗
Step 2
Child Gender 20.12 0.06 2.64† 0.13 0.05 1.82 20.04 0.04 1.35
20.03 0.01 0.17
BirthOrd 20.20∗ 20.15 20.16 20.06
Effortful 0.15 0.07 20.10 0.00
Step 3
M NTrad 20.06 0.04 1.64 20.12 0.05 1.92 0.07 0.08 2.97∗
20.11 0.01 0.36
F NTrad 0.08 0.24∗ 0.25∗ 0.09
Satisf 0.18∗ 0.08 0.01 0.00
Step 4
BirthOrd × Gen 0.17 0.02 1.35 0.06 0.03 1.49 20.05 0.01 0.36
20.10 0.02 0.81
Effortful × Gen 20.05 20.21 0.09 20.14
Step 5
Satisf × Gen 0.18 0.01 0.52 20.02 0.01 0.29 20.10 0.02 0.72
20.24 0.03 0.87
M NTrad × Gen 0.02 0.10 0.13 0.07
F NTrad × Gen 20.03 20.15 20.23 20.07
Note: M, Mother; F, Father; BirthOrd, Birth order; Effortful,
Child’s effortful control; NTrad, Non-traditional beliefs
regarding fathers; Satisf, Couple relationship satisfaction;
Gen, Child gender.
†p , 0.10; ∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01.
5
0
8
S
.J.
S
ch
o
p
p
e-S
u
lliva
n
et
al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
interactions between parent characteristics and child gender
entered on step 5 reached
statistical significance.
Mothers were more involved in didactic activities when the
focal child was higher
on effortful control, b ¼ 0.36, p , 0.01. None of the other
predictors explained signifi-
cant variance in mothers’ engagement in didactic activities.
Mothers reported greater
engagement in caregiving activities when they made less of a
contribution to family
income, b ¼ 20.22, p , 0.05. There were no significant simple
effects of child or
parent characteristics, nor were interactions among child
characteristics significant.
However, on step 5, the interaction between fathers’ non-
traditional beliefs and child
gender explained significant variance in mothers’ engagement
in caregiving, b ¼
0.52, p , 0.01. This interaction indicated that mothers of boys, b
¼ 20.64, p ,
0.05, but not mothers of girls, b ¼ 0.30, p ¼ 0.14, were less
engaged in caregiving
when their partners held more progressive beliefs about fathers’
roles.
With respect to physical play, mothers were more involved in
this aspect of engage-
ment when their partners were more involved in physical play, b
¼ 0.22, p , 0.05, and
when they were the parents of boys, b ¼ 20.20, p , 0.05, later-
born children, b ¼
0.24, p , 0.05, and/or children with high levels of effortful
control, b ¼ 0.31, p ,
0.01. No parent characteristics or interactions of child and
parent characteristics with
child gender explained significant variance in mothers’
engagement in physical play.
Fathers’ engagement
When predicting fathers’ engagement in socialisation activities,
mothers’ engagement
was a significant predictor, b ¼ 0.37, p , 0.01. On the second
step, child birth order
was a significant predictor, b ¼ 20.20, p , 0.05, indicating that
fathers were more
involved in socialisation activities with earlier-born children.
Fathers were also more
involved in socialisation activities when parents reported higher
levels of relationship
satisfaction, b ¼ 0.18, p , 0.05 (although the F-change
associated with this step was
not statistically significant). None of the interactions between
parent or child character-
istics and child gender was associated with fathers’ involvement
in socialisation.
The analysis of predictors of fathers’ engagement in didactic
and caregiving inter-
actions revealed only one significant predictor in both equations
– fathers’ non-tra-
ditional beliefs about fathers’ roles, b ¼ 0.24, p , 0.05 for
didactic and b ¼ 0.25, p
, 0.05 for caregiving. In other words, fathers were more engaged
in didactic and car-
egiving with their children when they held more non-traditional
beliefs about fathers’
roles. With the exception of mothers’ involvement in physical
play, b ¼ 0.22, p ,
0.05, no other predictors explained significant variance in
fathers’ engagement in phys-
ical play.
Discussion
Overall, the parents in this study were highly engaged with their
children. But, even
among a group of families with highly motivated fathers,
mothers were still more
involved than fathers in three out of the four aspects of
engagement examined in this
study: socialisation, didactic, and caregiving. Differences for
socialisation and caregiv-
ing in favour of mothers were medium to large. Fathers only
reported more frequent
engagement than mothers in physical play. These differences
are particularly striking,
given that mothers and fathers each reported on their own
involvement, which reduced
the bias toward finding differences in favour of mothers that
may be problematic in
Early Child Development and Care 509
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
studies that have relied solely on mothers’ reports of parental
involvement (Coley &
Morris, 2004). The distributions of mothers’ and fathers’
engagement also differed,
with mothers most frequently engaged in caregiving activities
and least frequently
engaged in physical play, and with fathers about equally
engaged in didactic, caregiv-
ing, and physical play. These results were generally consistent
with our expectations
and with prior research (Laflamme et al., 2002; McBride &
Mills, 1993; Yeung
et al., 2001).
Next, we attempted to understand individual differences among
mothers and fathers
in the four types of engagement. Parent characteristics played
an important role. When
mothers contributed more to the family’s income they were less
frequently engaged in
socialisation and caregiving activities. This is consistent with
the relative resources
hypothesis that women who contribute more to family income
have the power to
‘buy out’ of family work (Coltrane, 2000). However, mothers’
contributions to
family income were not associated with their engagement in
didactic or physical
play, indicating that regardless of their resources, mothers did
not use them to ‘buy
out’ of what they may have perceived as the most important
aspects of their engagement
(Craig & Mullan, 2011). Moreover, mothers’ contributions to
family income were not
associated with any aspect of fathers’ engagement. Thus, in this
study, it does not
appear that fathers compensated for lower levels of involvement
in socialisation and
caregiving by mothers who made a greater contribution to
family income. Perhaps
others (e.g. child care providers and grandparents) compensated
for lower frequency
of engagement by mothers who contributed more to family
income, and future research
should investigate this possibility.
Fathers’, but not mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about fathers’
roles were also
related to several aspects of parental engagement. When fathers
held more non-tra-
ditional beliefs, they were more frequently engaged in didactic
and caregiving activities
with their children. This is consistent with the Lamb – Pleck
model (Lamb et al., 1987)
and with a number of other studies (Beitel & Parke, 1998;
Freeman et al., 2008). In con-
trast, mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about fathers were not
associated with fathers’
engagement, nor were mothers’ beliefs about fathers associated
with lower engagement
on the part of mothers. Fathers’ greater non-traditional beliefs
were associated with less
frequent maternal engagement in caregiving, but only in
families with sons. Thus, in
families in which fathers have non-traditional beliefs, fathers
are more likely to
engage in caregiving of boys and girls, whereas mothers of boys
were less likely to
engage in caregiving. Perhaps mothers are willing to reduce
their involvement in car-
egiving for boys when their partner is more involved, but not
willing to reduce their
involvement in caregiving for girls. Or, caregiving of
preschool-aged girls may
involve activities (e.g. bathing) that parents do not feel
comfortable assigning to the
male parent (Yoshida, 2012).
Child characteristics also proved to be important for individual
differences in par-
ental engagement – especially for mothers. Thus, our hypothesis
that contextual factors
would be more closely related to fathers’ involvement with girls
than with boys (and
that child gender should not moderate associations between
predictors and maternal
involvement) was not supported. For mothers, greater child
effortful control was associ-
ated with greater engagement in didactic and physical play, and
also greater engage-
ment in socialisation, if the focal child was female. Mothers
may enjoy these types
of activities more (and choose to engage in them more often)
when their children are
higher in effortful control, or better able to control their
emotions and behaviour. For
socialisation activities, which involve interactions in contexts
outside of the home,
510 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
mothers may have higher expectations for their daughters’
regulatory abilities or antici-
pate that others in social settings may have higher expectations
for girls. The maxim
‘boys will be boys’ may excuse the behaviour of preschool-aged
boys at church or
at a museum, but girls’ out of control behaviour may not be so
readily overlooked.
Mothers’ engagement in caregiving, however, was not
associated with child effortful
control. Thus, as expected, effortful control appeared to matter
for parents’ involvement
in arguably more ‘elective’ aspects of parenting.
Child birth order and child gender were also relevant to
understanding parental
engagement. Fathers were more likely to engage in socialisation
activities with
earlier-born children. Some have argued that it is the father’s
role to introduce the
child to the outside world (Paquette, 2004), and previous
research has suggested
higher levels of father involvement for first-born children
(Bègue & Roché, 2005;
Price, 2008); thus, fathers may be especially likely to provide
opportunities for their
earlier-born children to engage in social interactions in contexts
outside the home.
Mothers were more likely to engage in physical play with boys
and with later-born chil-
dren. Mothers may show more frequent engagement with later-
born children, because if
they have more children, they may engage in play with children
as a group. That
mothers were also more likely to engage in physical play with
boys may be due to gen-
dered expectations for children.
Another important predictor of parent engagement was the
engagement of the other
parent, at least for socialisation and physical play forms of
engagement. These forms of
engagement may be more likely to involve both parents
simultaneously, or parents may
model each other’s involvement (Pleck & Hofferth, 2008),
whereas parent engagement
in caregiving and didactic activities may be complementary.
Besides mothers’ engage-
ment in physical play, none of the other parent and child
variables that we considered
predicted fathers’ engagement in physical play. Although we
recognise that con-
clusions drawn from null findings must be tentative, one
possible interpretation of
our lack of success in predicting this aspect of engagement is
that physical play is
ingrained in the father role (Paquette, 2004), and thus is most
robust to contextual influ-
ences. However, it is certainly possible that other factors that
we did not examine in this
study explain individual differences in engagement in physical
play – for instance,
father characteristics such as personality or subjective health
and well-being. Future
research considering these factors and others will be necessary
to understand correlates
of fathers’ engagement in physical play.
Couple relationship satisfaction was less important than
anticipated in explaining
parents’ engagement with their children – greater satisfaction
was only associated
with greater father involvement in socialisation activities. These
results may seem sur-
prising, given that prior research has found associations
between marital satisfaction
and mother as well as father involvement (Carlson et al., 2011;
Mehall et al., 2009).
It is true that the couples in the present study were on average
quite satisfied in their
relationships. Thus, a lack of variability in marital satisfaction
may explain these null
findings. However, it is also possible that marital adjustment is
a less important predic-
tor of parental engagement for families headed by married
parents with preschool age
children, especially when compared with other factors such as
children’s own
characteristics.
Limitations of this research should be acknowledged. This was a
cross-sectional
study, which makes it especially difficult to argue that certain
parent and child
factors caused individual differences in parental engagement.
For instance, although
we have argued that children’s temperamental effortful control
affects the extent of
Early Child Development and Care 511
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
maternal engagement, it is equally possible that greater
maternal engagement has
helped children learn to better regulate their emotions and
behaviour. Longitudinal
research is needed to uncover the likely reciprocal patterns of
those associations. Our
sample was mostly white, highly educated, relatively wealthy,
and married, and thus
our results may not apply to other populations of mothers and
fathers. Moreover,
self-report frequency measures may not be the best way to tap
parental engagement.
Time use measures such as time diaries may provide more
objective and accurate infor-
mation regarding parental engagement (Yeung et al., 2001).
Finally, our focus in this
study was on quantity of parental engagement in
developmentally appropriate activi-
ties; however, the quality of parental engagement may be just as
if not more important
for child development. Future studies would do well to include
observational measures
of the quality of parental engagement in addition to quantitative
measures.
As the benefits of involved parenting continue to mount,
practitioners’ and policy-
makers’ interest in fostering parental engagement will endure,
and as expectations for
fathers’ involvement in parenting increase, parents will keep on
struggling to meet these
expectations. Headway in encouraging and supporting parental
engagement –
especially that of fathers – can only be made with a thorough
understanding of
where we are at and where we need to go and the multiple
factors that underlie individ-
ual differences in maternal and paternal engagement.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by an NICHD grant (R03
HD050235) to the first author. We
express our gratitude to the parents and children who
participated in this study and the students
who assisted with data collection, especially Catherine Buckley.
Notes on contributors
Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan is an associate professor of human
development and family science at
The Ohio State University. Her research interests include co-
parenting relationships, fathering
behaviour, and the transition to parenthood.
Letitia E. Kotila is a doctoral student in human development
and family science at The Ohio
State University. Her research focuses on fathering, especially
among unmarried, at-risk men.
Rongfang Jia is a doctoral student in human development and
family science at The Ohio State
University. Her research focuses on the development of mother
– and father – child relationships
and implications for child functioning.
Sarah N. Lang is a doctoral student in human development and
family science at The Ohio State
University. Her research interests include caregiver – child
relationships in the home and in
childcare contexts.
Daniel J. Bower is a doctoral student in human development and
family science at The Ohio
State University. His research interests include the effects of
individual psychological character-
istics on couple relationships.
References
Adamsons, K., & Buehler, C. (2007). Mothering versus
fathering versus parenting:
Measurement equivalence in parenting measures. Parenting:
Science and Practice, 7,
271 – 303.
512 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Barry, A.A., Smith, J.Z., Deutsch, F.M., & Perry-Jenkins, M.
(2011). Fathers’ involvement in
child care and perceptions of parenting skill over the transition
to parenthood. Journal of
Family Issues, 32, 1500 – 1521.
Bègue, L., & Roché, S. (2005). Birth order and youth delinquent
behavior: Testing the differ-
ential parental control hypothesis in a French representative
sample. Psychology, Crime
& Law, 11, 73 – 85.
Beitel, A.H., & Parke, R.D. (1998). Paternal involvement in
infancy: The role of maternal and
paternal attitudes. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 268 – 288.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process
model. Child Development, 55,
83 – 96. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1129836
Bianchi, S.M., Robinson, J.P., & Milkie, M.A. (2006). Changing
rhythms of American family
life. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Bornstein, M. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting, Vol. 1:
Children and parenting (2nd ed.).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bridgett, D.J., Gartstein, M.A., Putnam, S.P., McKay, T.,
Iddins, E., Robertson, C., . . .
Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and
the effect of temperament
development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant
Behavior and
Development, 32, 103 – 116.
Cabrera, N.J., & Mitchell, S. (2009). An exploratory study of
fathers’ parenting stress and tod-
dlers’ social development in low-income African American
families. Fathering, 7,
201 – 225.
Cabrera, N.J., Ryan, R.M., Shannon, J.D., Brooks-Gunn, J.,
Vogel, C., Raikes, H., & Cohen, R.
(2004). Low-income fathers’ involvement in their toddlers’
lives: Biological fathers from the
early head start research and evaluation study. Fathering, 2, 5 –
36.
Carlson, M.J., Pilkauskas, N.V., McLanahan, S.S., & Brooks-
Gunn, J. (2011). Couples as part-
ners and parents over children’s early years. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 73, 317 – 334.
Carson, J., & Parke, R.D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in
parent child interactions and chil-
dren’s peer competency. Child Development, 67, 22 – 26.
Coley, R.L., & Morris, J.E. (2004). Comparing father and
mother reports of father involvement
among low-income minority families. Journal of Marriage and
Family, 64, 982 – 997.
Coltrane, S. (2000). Research on household labor: Modeling and
measuring the social embedd-
edness of routine family work. Journal of Marriage and Family,
62, 1208 – 1233. Retrieved
from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1566732
Craig, L., & Mullan, K. (2011). How mothers and fathers share
childcare: A cross-national time-
use comparison. American Sociological Review, 76, 834 – 861.
Doherty, W.J., Kouneski, E.F., & Erickson, M.F. (1998).
Responsible fathering: An overview
and conceptual framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
60, 277 – 292. Retrieved
from http://www.jstor.org/stable/353848
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Shepard, S.A., Guthrie, I.K.,
Murphy, B.C., & Reiser, M. (1999).
Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal
relations to quality of chil-
dren’s social functioning. Child Development, 70, 513 – 534.
Fagan, J., & Barnett, M. (2003). The relationship between
maternal gatekeeping, paternal com-
petence, mothers’ attitudes about the father role, and father
involvement. Journal of Family
Issues, 24, 1020 – 1043.
Freeman, H., Newland, L.A., & Coyl, D.D. (2008). Father
beliefs as a mediator between con-
textual barriers and father involvement. Early Child
Development and Care, 178, 803 – 819.
Hayes, A.F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures
for probing interactions in OLS
and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations.
Behavior Research Methods, 41,
924 – 936.
Hewlett, B. (Ed.). (1992). Father – child relations: Cultural and
biosocial contexts. New York:
Aldine de Gruyter.
Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., & Malcuit, G. (2002). A
comparison of fathers’ and mothers’
involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free-
play with their infants at
9 and 15 months. Sex Roles, 47, 507 – 518.
Lamb, M. (Ed.). (1997). The role of the father in child
development (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Lamb, M.E., Pleck, J.H., Charnov, E.L., & Levine, J.A. (1985).
Paternal behavior in humans.
American Zoologist, 25, 883 – 894. Retrieved from
http://www.jstor.org/stable/3883043
Early Child Development and Care 513
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4
Lamb, M.E., Pleck, J.H., Charnov, E.L., & Levine, J.A. (1987).
A biosocial perspective on
paternal behavior and involvement. In J.B. Lancaster, J.
Altman, A.S. Rossi, & L.R.
Sherroa (Eds.), Parenting across the lifespan: Biosocial
dimensions (pp. 111 – 142).
New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Lundberg, S., McLanahan, S., & Rose, E. (2007). Child gender
and father involvement in fragile
families. Demography, 44, 79 – 92.
MacDonald, K., & Parke, R.D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent
child play interaction and peer
interactive competence. Child Development, 55, 1265 – 1277.
Retrieved from http://www.
jstor.org/stable/1129996
McBride, B.A., & Mills, G. (1993). A comparison of mother and
father involvement with their
preschool age children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8,
457 – 477.
McBride, B.A., Schoppe, S.J., & Rane, T.R. (2002). Child
characteristics, parenting stress, and
parental involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 64,
998 – 1011.
Mehall, K.G., Spinrad, T.L., Eisenberg, N., & Gaertner, B.M.
(2009). Examining the relations of
infant temperament and couples’ marital satisfaction to mother
and father involvement: A
longitudinal study. Fathering, 7, 23 – 48.
Palkovitz, R. (1984). Parental attitudes and fathers’ interactions
with their 5-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychology, 20, 1054 – 1060.
Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father – child relationship:
Mechanisms and developmental
outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193 – 219.
Paquette, D., Carbonneau, R., Dubeau, D., Bigras, M., &
Tremblay, R.E. (2003). Prevalence of
father – child rough-and-tumble-play and physical aggression in
preschool children.
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 18, 171 – 189.
Parke, R.D., & Ladd, G.W. (1992). Family – peer relations:
Modes of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Pleck, J.H., & Hofferth, S.L. (2008). Mother involvement as an
influence on father involvement
with early adolescents. Fathering, 6, 267 – 286.
Pleck, J.H., & Masciadrelli, B.P. (2004). Parental involvement
by U.S. residential fathers:
Levels, sources, and consequences. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The
role of the father in child devel-
opment (4th ed., pp. 222 – 271). New York: Wiley.
Price, J. (2008). Parent – child quality time: Does birth order
matter? Journal of Human
Resources, 43, 240 – 265.
Putnam, S.P., & Rothbart, M.K. (2006). Development of short
and very short forms of the
Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 87, 102 – 112.
Raley, S., & Bianchi, S. (2006). Sons, daughters, and family
processes: Does gender of
children matter? Annual Review of Sociology, 32, 401 – 421.
Rothbart, M.K., & Bates, J.E. (2006). Temperament. In N.
Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R.M.
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3. Social,
emotional, and personality
development (pp. 99 – 166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., & Bremberg, S.
(2008). Fathers’ involvement and
children’s developmental outcomes: A systematic review of
longitudinal studies. Acta
Paediatrica, 97, 153 – 158.
Schoppe, S.J. (2001). What is a father? Unpublished
manuscript, University of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign.
Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales
for assessing the quality of mar-
riage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38,
15 – 28. Retrieved from
http://www.jstor.org/stable/350547
Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Shannon, J.D., Cabrera, N.J., & Lamb,
M.E. (2004). Fathers and
mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year olds: Contributions to
language and cognitive devel-
opment. Child Development, 75, 1806 – 1820.
Yeung, W.J., Sandberg, J.F., Davis-Kean, P.A., & Hofferth,
S.A. (2001). Children’s time with
fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63,
136 – 154.
Yoshida, A. (2012). Dads who do diapers: Factors affecting care
of young children by fathers.
Journal of Family Issues, 33, 451 – 477.
514 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
by
[T
ex
as
W
om
an
's
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
] a
t 0
6:
12
3
0
Se
pt
em
be
r 2
01
4

More Related Content

Similar to This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx

Annotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyAnnotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyLaurie Roberts
 
Criminal Justice
Criminal JusticeCriminal Justice
Criminal Justice
Lanate Drummond
 
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
Elton John Embodo
 
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docxLiterature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
croysierkathey
 
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docxLiterature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
jeremylockett77
 
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docxRunning head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
toltonkendal
 
Knipp E week6_discussion6
Knipp E week6_discussion6Knipp E week6_discussion6
Knipp E week6_discussion6
Eknipp2
 
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptxI-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
LorenaCantong2
 
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docxSoc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
whitneyleman54422
 
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14Katherine Mileto
 
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)Tim Jahnke
 
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
Scott Donald
 
Fatherhood figures
Fatherhood figuresFatherhood figures
Fatherhood figuresmacshare66
 
Peer Involvement in Social Interaction
Peer Involvement in Social InteractionPeer Involvement in Social Interaction
Peer Involvement in Social Interactionadaccardi
 
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptxParental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
MarilynClaudineBambi2
 
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their ChildrenChallenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
paperpublications3
 
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in BrazilFamily Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
annisamedika
 
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of SocietySingle Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
JudithLhamon
 

Similar to This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx (20)

Annotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyAnnotated bibliography
Annotated bibliography
 
Criminal Justice
Criminal JusticeCriminal Justice
Criminal Justice
 
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
The Level of Influence of Family-related factors on the Selected Tangub City ...
 
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docxLiterature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
 
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docxLiterature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
Literature ReviewAn essential concern in special education is th.docx
 
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docxRunning head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
Running head PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY1PARENTAL RESPONSIBILITY.docx
 
Knipp E week6_discussion6
Knipp E week6_discussion6Knipp E week6_discussion6
Knipp E week6_discussion6
 
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptxI-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
I-SEARCH_LLCedited.pptx
 
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docxSoc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
Soc Psychol Educ (2014) 171–17DOI 10.1007s11218-013-9237-3.docx
 
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14
KMiletoActionResearchProjectFinalWriteUp12.19.14
 
Cowenprutttart
CowenprutttartCowenprutttart
Cowenprutttart
 
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)
Ashley-Tim-Manuscript (3)
 
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
Academic Achievement In Grade 11 And 12 In K12 High School Students In Public...
 
Fatherhood figures
Fatherhood figuresFatherhood figures
Fatherhood figures
 
Peer Involvement in Social Interaction
Peer Involvement in Social InteractionPeer Involvement in Social Interaction
Peer Involvement in Social Interaction
 
Brittany Poster-2
Brittany Poster-2Brittany Poster-2
Brittany Poster-2
 
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptxParental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
Parental Support and Academic Performance.pptx
 
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their ChildrenChallenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
Challenges Parents Face While Participating in the Education of Their Children
 
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in BrazilFamily Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
Family Size, Gender, and Birth Order in Brazil
 
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of SocietySingle Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
Single Fathers Rearing Successful Children and Productive Members of Society
 

More from christalgrieg

Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docxPlease read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docxPlease read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docxPlease read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docxPlease read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docxPlease read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docxPlease read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docxPlease Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docxPlease read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
christalgrieg
 
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docxPLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docxPlease read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docxPlease Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docxPlease react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docxPlease provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docxPlease proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
christalgrieg
 
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docxPlease prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docxPlease prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docxPlease prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
christalgrieg
 
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docxPlease provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docxPlease provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
christalgrieg
 
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docxPlease provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
christalgrieg
 

More from christalgrieg (20)

Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docxPlease read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
Please read the case  Fraud at WorldCom in the book provided below  .docx
 
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docxPlease read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
Please read the below two discussion posts and provide the response .docx
 
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docxPlease read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
Please read the below discussion post and provide response in 75 to .docx
 
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docxPlease read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
Please read the assignment content throughly Internet Resources .docx
 
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docxPlease read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
Please read the article by Peterson (2004). Your responses to th.docx
 
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docxPlease read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
Please read the article which appears below. Write and submit an.docx
 
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docxPlease Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
Please Read instructions Role Model LeadersChoose one • 1 .docx
 
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docxPlease read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
Please read each attachment for instructions, please answer each q.docx
 
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docxPLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
PLEASE READ BEFORE STARTING! 500 WORD PAPER ONLY USING THE NOTES I.docx
 
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docxPlease read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
Please read Patricia Benners Five Stages of Proficiency. Explai.docx
 
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docxPlease Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
Please Read Instructions OBJECTIV.docx
 
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docxPlease react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
Please react to this student post. remember references and plarigari.docx
 
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docxPlease provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
Please provide the following information about your culture which is.docx
 
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docxPlease proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
Please proof the paper attached and complete question 6 and 7..docx
 
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docxPlease prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
Please prepare PPT( 5 Slides and 1 citation slide) and also explain .docx
 
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docxPlease prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
Please prepare a one-pageProject Idea that includes the .docx
 
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docxPlease prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
Please prepare at least in 275 to 300 words with APA references and .docx
 
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docxPlease provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
Please provide references for your original postings in APA form.docx
 
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docxPlease provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
Please provide an update to include information about methodology, n.docx
 
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docxPlease provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
Please provide an evaluation of the Path to Competitive Advantage an.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
MysoreMuleSoftMeetup
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
JosvitaDsouza2
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
Sandy Millin
 
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp NetworkIntroduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
TechSoup
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
EugeneSaldivar
 
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
CarlosHernanMontoyab2
 
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe..."Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
SACHIN R KONDAGURI
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Po-Chuan Chen
 
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with MechanismOverview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
DeeptiGupta154
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Atul Kumar Singh
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
Vivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
Celine George
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
EduSkills OECD
 
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Jheel Barad
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
Levi Shapiro
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
camakaiclarkmusic
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Mohd Adib Abd Muin, Senior Lecturer at Universiti Utara Malaysia
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
Jisc
 
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9  .docxAcetabularia Information For Class 9  .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
vaibhavrinwa19
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
 
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp NetworkIntroduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
Introduction to AI for Nonprofits with Tapp Network
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
 
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
678020731-Sumas-y-Restas-Para-Colorear.pdf
 
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe..."Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
 
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with MechanismOverview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
 
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
 
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9  .docxAcetabularia Information For Class 9  .docx
Acetabularia Information For Class 9 .docx
 

This article was downloaded by [Texas Womans University]On.docx

  • 1. This article was downloaded by: [Texas Woman's University] On: 30 September 2014, At: 06:11 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Early Child Development and Care Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gecd20 Comparisons of levels and predictors of mothers' and fathers' engagement with their preschool-aged children Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan a , Letitia E. Kotila a , Rongfang Jia a , Sarah N. Lang a & Daniel J. Bower a a Human Development and Family Science , The Ohio State University , Columbus , OH , USA Published online: 03 Sep 2012. To cite this article: Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan , Letitia E. Kotila , Rongfang Jia , Sarah N. Lang & Daniel J. Bower (2013) Comparisons of levels and predictors of mothers' and fathers' engagement with their preschool-aged children, Early Child Development and Care, 183:3-4, 498-514, DOI: 10.1080/03004430.2012.711596 To link to this article:
  • 2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.711596 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions
  • 3. Comparisons of levels and predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement with their preschool-aged children Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan∗ , Letitia E. Kotila, Rongfang Jia, Sarah N. Lang and Daniel J. Bower Human Development and Family Science, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA (Received 9 December 2011; final version received 30 March 2012) Self-report data from 112 two-parent families were used to compare levels and predictors of four types of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement with their preschool-aged children: socialisation, didactic, caregiving, and physical play. Mothers were more involved than fathers in socialisation, didactic, and caregiving, whereas fathers were more involved than mothers in physical play. Mothers’ greatest engagement was in caregiving, whereas fathers were about equally engaged in didactic, caregiving, and physical play. Mothers who contributed more to family income were less engaged in socialisation and caregiving, whereas fathers with non-traditional beliefs about their roles were more engaged in didactic and caregiving. Children with greater temperamental effortful control received more didactic and physical play
  • 4. engagement from mothers. Fathers were more likely to engage in socialisation activities with earlier-born children, whereas mothers were more likely to engage in socialisation with girls high in effortful control. Mothers were more likely to engage in physical play with boys and with later-born children. Keywords: father engagement; mother engagement; child temperament; child gender; birth order; caregiving; physical play Developmentally appropriate engagement by mothers and fathers has been linked to positive outcomes for children (Sarkadi, Kristiansson, Oberklaid, & Bremberg, 2008). In the past 30 – 40 years fathers’ engagement with their children has increased (Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004); however, there is still significant variability in fathers’ engagement with young children, even in families headed by married parents (Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, & Hofferth, 2001). Fathers are no less capable than mothers at providing children with sensitive, responsive care (Lamb, 1997), yet mothers are most often the primary caregivers for children regardless of employment status (Bianchi, Robinson, & Milkie, 2006). This distinction has shaped the ways in which research has examined mothers’ and fathers’ parenting. Whereas research on mothers’ parenting has primarily focused on the quality of maternal behaviour, research on fathers’ parenting
  • 5. has focused more on the quantity of involvement (Adamsons & Buehler, 2007). Relatively rare are quan- titative comparisons of maternal and paternal engagement, using parents from the same families (LaFlamme, Pomerleau, & Malcuit, 2002; McBride & Mills, 1993), even # 2013 Taylor & Francis ∗ Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] Early Child Development and Care, 2013 Vol. 183, Nos. 3 – 4, 498 – 514, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.711596 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an 's
  • 6. U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 though fathers’ and mothers’ interactions with children are necessarily intertwined (Pleck & Hofferth, 2008). The use of qualitatively distinct conceptualisations and measures of maternal and paternal parenting has made comparisons within and across
  • 7. studies difficult. As a result, we have a limited understanding of the ways in which mothers and fathers inter- act with their children, and the extent to which patterns and predictors of engagement are similar or different. Understanding the patterns and predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement with their children is critical to informing the continued develop- ment of theoretical models about parenting and fathering, and important to the design of educational programmes intended to promote positive parental engagement. Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare mothers’ and fathers’ involvement in four types of engagement with their preschool-aged children: socialisation, didactic, caregiving, and physical play, as well as to examine parent and child characteristics as predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement in these domains. Conceptualisation of parental engagement Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, and Levine (1985) identified three domains of involvement: engagement, responsibility, and accessibility. Engagement is most often a focus of research due to its strong associations with child outcomes (Tamis-LeMonda, Shannon, Cabrera, & Lamb, 2004). In their research on non- resident father involve- ment, Cabrera et al. (2004) distinguished four subtypes of meaningful engagement activities: socialisation (e.g. taking the child to family, community, or social events), didactic (e.g. reading or singing with the child), caregiving (e.g.
  • 8. assisting the child with eating or other physical care activities), and physical play (e.g. playing outside with the child). The socialisation aspect of engagement involves the roles of parents in linking chil- dren with the outside world (Parke & Ladd, 1992). Some have argued that fathers may play a special role in child socialisation (Paquette, 2004). Fathers provide children with new experiences, companionship during these experiences, and knowledge and advice about them (Hewlett, 1992). In families with infants, however, Laflamme et al. (2002) found that mothers were more involved than fathers in taking children on ‘outings’ in the neighbourhood and community. Nonetheless, comparisons of parental participation in family and community activities with preschool children are unknown. Didactic interactions are those in which parents encourage children to engage in and understand their environments, as well as providing the opportunity to observe, learn, and imitate behaviours (Bornstein, 2002). Although didactic interactions between father and child have shown the strongest links with positive child development (Bianchi et al., 2006; Yeung et al., 2001), most research has focused on didactic inter- actions between mother and child, and studies comparing parents’ involvement in this domain are rare. In an exception, Yeung et al. (2001) compared mothers’ and fathers’
  • 9. time with children in various activities and found that fathers spent 1/3 of the time that mothers did in didactic activities such as reading, educational play, and studying with children ages 0 – 9. Engagement in childcare activities is somewhat different from the other types of engagement, because these interactions may be more task- and routine-oriented, and are arguably less ‘elective’ than other forms of parent engagement. Yet, even routine childcare tasks performed by parents are important for children. When fathers are more involved in caregiving activities such as feeding, bathing, and putting their Early Child Development and Care 499 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om
  • 10. an 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 child to bed, they have more opportunities to get to know their children and develop confidence in their parenting (Barry, Smith, Deutsch, & Perry-
  • 11. Jenkins, 2011), thereby promoting the formation and maintenance of the close relationships with chil- dren that facilitate their socio-emotional development. Whereas mothers are believed to specialise in caregiving, popular notions suggest that fathers spend the majority of their time in ‘play’ activities with their children, and most findings indicate that fathers do spend a greater proportion of time in physical play with their children than mothers (Paquette, 2004; Yeung et al., 2001). Parent – child physical play peaks during the preschool years, a period during which most children rapidly develop perceptual, motor, cognitive, emotional, and social functioning (Paquette, Carbonneau, Dubeau, Bigras, & Tremblay, 2003), and provides important opportunities for children to build skills important for peer competence (Carson & Parke, 1996; MacDonald & Parke, 1984). Predictors of parental engagement Two frameworks were used to identify key potential predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement with their children: the Lamb – Pleck model of the determinants of father involvement (Lamb, Pleck, Charnov, & Levine, 1987) and Belsky’s (1984) process model of parenting. The Lamb – Pleck model proposed four categories of influence on father involvement: (1) motivation (e.g. fathers’ desire for involvement), (2) skills and self-confidence (e.g. parenting self-efficacy), (3) social
  • 12. supports and stresses (e.g. marital relationships), and (4) institutional factors (e.g. workplace policies). Belsky’s (1984) process model described three categories of influence on parenting behaviours: (1) parent characteristics (e.g. personality), (2) child characteristics (e.g. temperament, age, and gender), and (3) contextual sources of stress and support (e.g. marital relationships). In this study, we focused on parent and child characteristics – specifically, mothers’ contributions to family income and parents’ beliefs about fathers’ roles, couple relationship satisfaction, and child effortful control, gender, and birth order. Parent characteristics According to a set of perspectives described as the ‘relative resources hypothesis’ (Col- trane, 2000, p. 1214), partners use resources that they bring to the relationship to nego- tiate lighter workloads at home. This hypothesis has been supported by research on household labour (Coltrane, 2000); when women contribute more to family income their divisions of household labour with their partners are more equal. Part of the move towards equality when mothers contribute greater resources is due to greater father involvement in childrearing (Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004). But, it is not necess- arily clear that mothers with greater relative earnings reduce their involvement in child- rearing, as they may be unable or unwilling to do so (Craig &
  • 13. Mullan, 2011). Parents’ beliefs about fathers’ roles, a key component of the motivation factor of the Lamb – Pleck (Lamb et al., 1987) model, have also been linked to levels of paternal involvement with children. Fathers with more non-traditional beliefs about their roles are more engaged with their children (Beitel & Parke, 1998; Freeman, Newland, & Coyl, 2008). Moreover, mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles have been linked to greater paternal engagement as well (Fagan & Barnett, 2003), 500 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al. D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an
  • 14. 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 whereas mothers who view the paternal role as surrogate to the maternal role have part- ners who are less involved (Beitel & Parke, 1998). Fathers’ parenting has been described as especially sensitive to
  • 15. the family context (Doherty, Kouneski, & Erickson, 1998), and in particular affected by the quality of the father’s relationship with the child’s mother. However, the quality of the marital relationship also figures prominently in Belsky’s (1984) model, which was developed primarily based on research with mothers. Indeed, greater marital satisfaction has been linked to mother as well as to father involvement (Carlson, Pilkauskas, McLanahan, & Brooks-Gunn, 2011; Mehall, Spinrad, Eisenberg, & Gaertner, 2009). Child characteristics Effortful control is the ability to voluntarily focus and shift attention and to initiate or inhibit behaviours that emerges in infancy, develops rapidly during the toddler years, and plays an important role in the regulation and control of early emotional reactions and behaviours (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Children with higher levels of effortful control, who can better control their emotions and behaviour, may make parental engagement more rewarding, thereby strengthening parents’ motivation for further engagement. To our knowledge, studies of child effortful control and parental engage- ment are absent in the literature, but a few relevant studies have found prospective associations between poorer child attentional and behavioural regulation and poorer parenting quality (Bridgett et al., 2009; Eisenberg et al., 1999).
  • 16. Research has also suggested associations of child birth order and child gender with parental involvement. When differences are found, parent involvement is greater with first-born than with later-born children (Bègue & Roché, 2005; Price, 2008). Differ- ences by child gender are not always found, but when they are, child gender is associ- ated more closely with father than with mother involvement, and fathers of sons are more involved than fathers of daughters (Lundberg, McLanahan, & Rose, 2007; Raley & Bianchi, 2006). Child gender and birth order may also interact. Yoshida (2012) reported that fathers were more involved in physical care when there was a young male child (less than five years old) in the family, but that the presence of older, school-aged children reduced fathers’ involvement. The present study The data used were drawn from a study of preschool-aged children in 112 two-parent families. Our first goal was to examine mothers’ and fathers’ involvement in four types of engagement: socialisation, didactic, caregiving, and physical play. Consistent with research reviewed above, we hypothesised that mothers would be more frequently engaged in didactic and caregiving activities with children than fathers, whereas fathers would be more frequently engaged in physical play activities with children than mothers. We did not advance a hypothesis with respect to parent gender differ-
  • 17. ences in socialisation. We further expected that relative to their engagement in other domains, mothers would be most frequently engaged in caregiving activities and least frequently engaged in physical play, whereas fathers would be more frequently engaged in physical play relative to other types of engagement. Our second goal was to examine the contributions of parent and child characteristics to maternal and paternal engagement. Consistent with theory and prior research, we expected that when mothers made a greater contribution to family income they Early Child Development and Care 501 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an
  • 18. 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 would be less frequently engaged in caregiving (but not necessarily other) activities but fathers would be more frequently engaged in caregiving. When mothers and fathers held more non-traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles, we
  • 19. anticipated that fathers would be more frequently engaged in activities with their children – especially in car- egiving, which has been traditionally associated with the maternal role. We also hypothesised that both fathers and mothers would be more engaged across domains when couples reported greater relationship satisfaction. With respect to child characteristics, we anticipated that parents would be more fre- quently engaged in activities with their children – especially socialisation, didactic, and physical play, which are arguably more ‘elective’ than caregiving – when children had higher temperamental effortful control. Consistent with the vast majority of the litera- ture, we also expected that parents would be more frequently engaged in activities with first-born children than with later-born children, and that child gender would be associ- ated with fathers’ but not mothers’ engagement, with differences in father engagement in favour of sons. Finally, we also tested child gender as a moderator of associations of other parent and child characteristics with parental engagement. Consistent with the notion that father involvement is especially sensitive to contextual factors (Doherty et al., 1998), McBride, Schoppe, and Rane (2002) found that less sociable female children had less involved fathers, but sociability was not associated with father involvement with sons. In contrast, mothers’ involvement was not associated with
  • 20. child temperament or gender. Thus, we expected that father engagement with sons would be more robust to contextual influences than father engagement with daughters, but that there would be no moderating effects of child gender for mothers. Method Participants One-hundred and twelve families consisting of mothers, fathers, and a preschool-aged focal child (M ¼ 4.12 years; SD ¼ 0.52; 58 boys, 54 girls) were recruited to participate through local preschools, advertisements, and participant referrals in a large Midwestern metropo- litan area. Mothers’ average age was 36.03 years (SD ¼ 5.26), and fathers’ average age was 37.73 years (SD ¼ 5.74). Eighty-three percent of mothers and 81% percent of fathers had obtained at least a college degree (Range ¼ some high school to Ph.D.). Family income ranged from less than $10,000 to over $100,000 per year (Mdn ¼ $71,000 – 80,000). Seventy-seven percent of children were White, non-Hispanic, 9% African American, 1% Hispanic, 1% Asian, and 13% multiracial/multiethnic. Sixty percent of focal children were first-born, 27% were second- born, and 14% were third- or later-born. Families had 2.23 children on average (SD ¼ 0.98; Range ¼ 1 – 7). Thirty-six percent of mothers were working full time (31+ hours per week), 23% were working part time (,10 – 30 hours per week), and 42% were not
  • 21. employed outside the home. Eighty-seven percent of fathers were working full time, 8% part time, and 6% were not employed. On average, mothers contributed 25.51% of the family income (SD ¼ 27.71%). Measures Mothers and fathers each reported their own level of engagement with their preschool age child using the Activities with Child Scale created for the Early Head Start 502 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al. D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an 's U
  • 22. ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 Research and Evaluation Project and the Early Childhood Longitudinal Birth Cohort (Cabrera et al., 2004). Parents indicated the frequency of their involvement (1 ¼ not at all; 6 ¼ more than once a day) in 30 activities with their child within the last month. These items can be divided into 5 subscales (see Cabrera
  • 23. & Mitchell, 2009): socialisation (9 items; e.g. ‘Visit friends with child’; a ¼ 0.64 for mothers and a ¼ 0.65 for fathers), management (3 items; e.g. ‘Take child to doctor’; a ¼ 0.55 for mothers and a ¼ 0.38 for fathers), didactic (5 items; e.g. ‘Play with building toys with child’; a ¼ 0.71 for both mothers and fathers), physical play (6 items; e.g. ‘Play chasing games with child’; a ¼ 0.68 for mothers and a ¼ 0.71 for fathers), and caregiving (7 items; e.g. ‘Put child to bed’; a ¼ 0.63 for mothers and a ¼ 0.70 for fathers). Because of very low reliability, the management component of parental engagement was not considered further. Each parent completed the What Is a Father? (WIAF) questionnaire (Schoppe, 2001), a 15-item measure adapted from Palkovitz’s (1984) Role of the Father Question- naire. Items on the WIAF ask respondents to rate statements concerning fathers and fathering from 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 5 ¼ strongly agree. Five items tap non-tra- ditional beliefs (e.g. ‘Fathers are just as sensitive in caring for children as mothers are’), and five items tap traditional beliefs (e.g. ‘The father’s role is to provide for his family, not babysit the children’). Ratings on the five traditional items were reverse-scored and averaged with ratings on the five non- traditional items. Thus, higher scores indicated the presence of more non-traditional beliefs about fathers and the absence of more traditional beliefs. Cronbach’s alpha for
  • 24. mothers was 0.66 and for fathers 0.60. Mothers and fathers reported on their relationship satisfaction using the 32-item Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976), a widely-used, reliable, and valid measure of relationship satisfaction. Summary scores were created for mothers (a ¼ 0.92) and fathers (a ¼ 0.91), and a couple-level score was created by averaging parents’ reports, which were strongly correlated, r ¼ 0.59, p , 0.01. Parents reported on their child’s temperament using the 36-item Very Short Form of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire, appropriate for child children aged 3 – 8 (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). The questionnaire contains statements about children that parents rate on a scale from 1 ¼ extremely untrue of my child to 7 ¼ extremely true of my child. Parents’ ratings of child effortful control (12 items; e.g. ‘When drawing or coloring in a book, shows strong concentration’ ‘Is good at following instructions’) were the focus of this study (a ¼ 0.73 for mothers and a ¼ 0.72 for fathers). Parents’ ratings were significantly and moderately correlated, r ¼ 0.42, p , 0.01, and thus averaged. Mothers and fathers reported demographic information for themselves and their families on a questionnaire created for the study. We focused on mothers’ reports of their percent contribution to the family income, child gender (0
  • 25. ¼ boy; 1 ¼ girl), and child birth order (higher scores indicated that the focal child was later-born). Results Comparisons of mothers’ vs. fathers’ engagement Mean levels of the four aspects of engagement for mothers and fathers are reported in Table 1. Parents reported engaging in socialisation activities approximately a few times per month, didactic activities a few times a week to about once a day, caregiving Early Child Development and Care 503 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an
  • 26. 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 activities a few times a week to about once a day, and physical play a few times a month to a few times a week. Paired samples t-tests revealed that mothers and fathers differed significantly on all four aspects of engagement (Figure 1).
  • 27. Mothers engaged in greater socialisation, t(110) ¼ 8.19, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.78, didactic, t(110) ¼ 4.37, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.41, and caregiving, t(110) ¼ 8.08, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.77, than fathers. Fathers engaged in greater physical play than mothers, t(110) ¼ 24.64, p , 0.01, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.44. Figure 1. Comparisons of mothers’ vs. fathers’ mean levels of engagement. Table 1. Means and standard deviations of study variables. M SD Range Parental engagement M Socialisation 3.20 0.42 2.00 – 4.44 F Socialisation 2.81 0.42 1.25 – 3.78 M Didactic 4.25 0.75 2.60 – 5.80 F Didactic 3.83 0.76 1.80 – 5.60 M Caregiving 4.67 0.61 2.71 – 5.86 F Caregiving 4.04 0.64 2.14 – 5.57 M Physical Play 3.41 0.71 1.83 – 5.33 F Physical Play 3.79 0.69 2.33 – 5.50 Predictors M Income 25.51 27.71 0.00 – 100.00 Birth order 1.59 0.91 1.00 – 6.00 Effortful control 5.39 0.57 3.25 – 6.55 M non-traditional 3.56 0.48 2.00 – 4.90 F non-traditional 3.50 0.43 2.20 – 4.70 Relationship satisfaction 3.58 0.38 2.39 – 4.52 Note: M, Mother; F, Father. 504 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al.
  • 29. 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 No aspects of mothers’ or fathers’ engagement differed by child gender. Corre- lations among the four aspects of engagement for mothers and fathers (see Table 2) revealed that when parents were more involved in one aspect of engagement, they tended to be more involved in the other aspects as well. Within families, mothers’ and fathers’ engagement in socialisation and physical play were significantly and posi- tively correlated, but there were no significant correlations for didactic or caregiving. A within-subjects ANOVA with parent gender and engagement type as within-sub- jects factors was used to further test whether the distributions of mothers’ and fathers’ involvement across the four types of engagement differed. There was a main effect of parent gender, F(1, 110) ¼ 21.98, p , 0.01, and of engagement type, F(1, 108) ¼
  • 30. 258.42, p , 0.01. These main effects were qualified by a significant parent gender X engagement type interaction, F(1, 108) ¼ 53.81, p , 0.01. Thus, the patterns of mothers’ and fathers’ involvement across engagement types differed, such that mothers were most frequently engaged in caregiving activities, with the least frequent engagement in physical play, whereas fathers were about equally involved in didactic, caregiving, and physical play. Predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement Correlations among the predictor variables and between the predictors and the four aspects of engagement for mothers and fathers are shown in Table 2. Hierarchical regression analyses were used to further examine the relative contributions of parent and child predictors of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement in these four types of activi- ties. Eight equations were computed – four predicting the types of engagement for mothers and four predicting the types of engagement for fathers. In each equation, mothers’ contributions to the family income and the corresponding type of involvement by the other parent were entered together on the first step, followed by the three child predictors on the second step: child gender, birth order, and effortful control. Mothers’ and fathers’ progressive and traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles and relationship sat- isfaction were entered together on the third step. On the fourth step, the birth order X
  • 31. child gender and effortful control X child gender interactions were entered. Finally, on the fifth step, the three remaining two-way interactions between child gender and each of the parent predictors (mothers’ and fathers’ non- traditional beliefs and relation- ship satisfaction) were entered. Continuous variables were mean-centered prior to the computation of product terms. Results for mothers and fathers are shown in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. Significant interactions were probed using an SPSS macro created by Hayes and Matthes (2009). Mothers’ engagement The more mothers contributed to family income, the less involved they were in socia- lisation activities with their children, b ¼ 20.22, p , 0.05. Mothers were more engaged in socialisation when fathers were more engaged in socialisation, b ¼ 0.36, p , 0.01. None of the child or parent characteristics explained significant variance in mothers’ engagement in socialisation on steps 2 or 3. However, the interaction between effortful control and child gender (b ¼ 0.37, p , 0.01) was significant on step 4. Post hoc probing of this interaction revealed that greater effortful control was associated with greater maternal engagement in socialisation for mothers of daughters, b ¼ 0.39, p , 0.01, but not for mothers of sons, b ¼ 20.05, p ¼ 0.57. None of the Early Child Development and Care 505
  • 33. 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 Table 2. Correlations between parental engagement and parent and child characteristics. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 1. M Socialisation 1.00 0.32∗ ∗ 0.46∗ ∗ 20.04 0.10 20.06 0.42∗ ∗ 0.04 20.20∗ 0.05 0.16 20.13 20.06 0.03 2. F Socialisation 1.00 0.11 0.35∗ ∗ 20.04 0.23∗ 0.08 0.29∗ ∗ 0.07 20.25∗ ∗ 0.21∗ 20.03 0.07 0.14 3. M Didactic 1.00 0.09 0.24∗ ∗ 0.10 0.54∗ ∗ 0.23∗ 20.07 20.02 0.31∗ ∗ 0.02 20.02 0.13 4. F Didactic 1.00 0.01 0.41∗ ∗ 20.02 0.58∗ ∗ 0.16 20.17 0.14 0.06 0.23∗ 0.09 5. M Caregiving 1.00 0.12 0.30∗ ∗ 0.04 20.21∗ 20.01 0.10 20.10 20.08 20.06 6. F Caregiving 1.00 0.08 0.33∗ ∗ 0.06 20.19∗ 20.06 0.17 0.25∗ ∗ 0.03 7. M Physical Play 1.00 0.26∗ ∗ 20.05 0.19∗ 0.25∗ ∗ 20.14 20.07 0.03 8. F Physical Play 1.00 0.07 20.05 0.07 20.08 0.01 20.01 9. M income 1.00 20.15 0.10 0.32∗ ∗ 0.16 20.12
  • 34. 10. Birth order 1.00 20.07 20.10 20.07 0.09 11. Effortful control 1.00 0.02 0.06 0.15 12. M non-traditional 1.00 0.48∗ ∗ 0.00 13. F non-traditional 1.00 0.03 14. Relationship satisfaction 1.00 Note: M, Mother; F, Father. ∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01. 5 0 6 S .J. S ch o p p e-S u lliva n et al.
  • 36. 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 Table 3. Hierarchical regression predicting mothers’ engagement. Mother socialisation Mother didactic Mother Caregiving Mother physical play Predictor b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF Step 1 % Income by M 20.22∗ 0.17 10.35∗ ∗ 20.08 0.01 0.63 20.22∗ 0.06 3.15∗ 20.07 0.05 2.87† F Engagement 0.36∗ ∗ 0.08 0.10 0.22∗ Step 2 Child gender 0.00 0.04 1.49 20.13 0.11 4.18∗ ∗ 20.08 0.02 0.73 20.20∗ 0.14 6.08∗ ∗ Birthord 0.09 0.01 20.01 0.24∗ Effortful 0.14 0.36∗ ∗ 0.15 0.31∗ ∗
  • 37. Step 3 M NTrad 20.02 0.00 0.10 0.13 0.02 0.69 20.01 0.03 0.91 0.03 0.02 1.03 F NTrad 20.04 20.09 20.06 20.16 Satisf 20.08 0.08 20.15 20.06 Step 4 BirthOrd × Gen 0.15 0.08 5.26∗ ∗ 0.20† 0.03 1.75 20.04 0.00 0.10 0.03 0.01 0.81 Effortful × Gen 0.37∗ ∗ 0.11 20.04 0.16 Step 5 Satisf × Gen 0.13 0.03 1.07 20.22 0.03 1.35 0.04 0.08 3.12∗ 0.19 0.02 0.68 M NTrad × Gen 0.05 0.19 20.27† 0.01 F NTrad × Gen 0.09 0.00 0.52∗ ∗ 0.08 Note: M, Mother; F, Father; BirthOrd, Birth order; Effortful, Child’s effortful control; NTrad, Non-traditional beliefs regarding fathers; Satisf, Couple relationship satisfaction; Gen, Child gender. †p , 0.10; ∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01. E a rly C h ild D evelo p m en
  • 39. ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 Table 4. Hierarchical regression predicting fathers’ engagement. Predictor Father socialisation Father didactic Father caregiving Father physical play b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF b DR2 DF
  • 40. Step 1 % Income by M 0.15 0.14 8.29∗ ∗ 0.16 0.03 1.63 0.08 0.02 0.79 0.08 0.05 2.96† M Engagement 0.37∗ ∗ 0.08 0.11 0.22∗ Step 2 Child Gender 20.12 0.06 2.64† 0.13 0.05 1.82 20.04 0.04 1.35 20.03 0.01 0.17 BirthOrd 20.20∗ 20.15 20.16 20.06 Effortful 0.15 0.07 20.10 0.00 Step 3 M NTrad 20.06 0.04 1.64 20.12 0.05 1.92 0.07 0.08 2.97∗ 20.11 0.01 0.36 F NTrad 0.08 0.24∗ 0.25∗ 0.09 Satisf 0.18∗ 0.08 0.01 0.00 Step 4 BirthOrd × Gen 0.17 0.02 1.35 0.06 0.03 1.49 20.05 0.01 0.36 20.10 0.02 0.81 Effortful × Gen 20.05 20.21 0.09 20.14 Step 5 Satisf × Gen 0.18 0.01 0.52 20.02 0.01 0.29 20.10 0.02 0.72 20.24 0.03 0.87 M NTrad × Gen 0.02 0.10 0.13 0.07 F NTrad × Gen 20.03 20.15 20.23 20.07 Note: M, Mother; F, Father; BirthOrd, Birth order; Effortful, Child’s effortful control; NTrad, Non-traditional beliefs regarding fathers; Satisf, Couple relationship satisfaction; Gen, Child gender. †p , 0.10; ∗ p , 0.05; ∗ ∗ p , 0.01. 5 0 8
  • 42. om an 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 interactions between parent characteristics and child gender entered on step 5 reached
  • 43. statistical significance. Mothers were more involved in didactic activities when the focal child was higher on effortful control, b ¼ 0.36, p , 0.01. None of the other predictors explained signifi- cant variance in mothers’ engagement in didactic activities. Mothers reported greater engagement in caregiving activities when they made less of a contribution to family income, b ¼ 20.22, p , 0.05. There were no significant simple effects of child or parent characteristics, nor were interactions among child characteristics significant. However, on step 5, the interaction between fathers’ non- traditional beliefs and child gender explained significant variance in mothers’ engagement in caregiving, b ¼ 0.52, p , 0.01. This interaction indicated that mothers of boys, b ¼ 20.64, p , 0.05, but not mothers of girls, b ¼ 0.30, p ¼ 0.14, were less engaged in caregiving when their partners held more progressive beliefs about fathers’ roles. With respect to physical play, mothers were more involved in this aspect of engage- ment when their partners were more involved in physical play, b ¼ 0.22, p , 0.05, and when they were the parents of boys, b ¼ 20.20, p , 0.05, later- born children, b ¼ 0.24, p , 0.05, and/or children with high levels of effortful control, b ¼ 0.31, p , 0.01. No parent characteristics or interactions of child and parent characteristics with child gender explained significant variance in mothers’
  • 44. engagement in physical play. Fathers’ engagement When predicting fathers’ engagement in socialisation activities, mothers’ engagement was a significant predictor, b ¼ 0.37, p , 0.01. On the second step, child birth order was a significant predictor, b ¼ 20.20, p , 0.05, indicating that fathers were more involved in socialisation activities with earlier-born children. Fathers were also more involved in socialisation activities when parents reported higher levels of relationship satisfaction, b ¼ 0.18, p , 0.05 (although the F-change associated with this step was not statistically significant). None of the interactions between parent or child character- istics and child gender was associated with fathers’ involvement in socialisation. The analysis of predictors of fathers’ engagement in didactic and caregiving inter- actions revealed only one significant predictor in both equations – fathers’ non-tra- ditional beliefs about fathers’ roles, b ¼ 0.24, p , 0.05 for didactic and b ¼ 0.25, p , 0.05 for caregiving. In other words, fathers were more engaged in didactic and car- egiving with their children when they held more non-traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles. With the exception of mothers’ involvement in physical play, b ¼ 0.22, p , 0.05, no other predictors explained significant variance in fathers’ engagement in phys- ical play.
  • 45. Discussion Overall, the parents in this study were highly engaged with their children. But, even among a group of families with highly motivated fathers, mothers were still more involved than fathers in three out of the four aspects of engagement examined in this study: socialisation, didactic, and caregiving. Differences for socialisation and caregiv- ing in favour of mothers were medium to large. Fathers only reported more frequent engagement than mothers in physical play. These differences are particularly striking, given that mothers and fathers each reported on their own involvement, which reduced the bias toward finding differences in favour of mothers that may be problematic in Early Child Development and Care 509 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as
  • 47. studies that have relied solely on mothers’ reports of parental involvement (Coley & Morris, 2004). The distributions of mothers’ and fathers’ engagement also differed, with mothers most frequently engaged in caregiving activities and least frequently engaged in physical play, and with fathers about equally engaged in didactic, caregiv- ing, and physical play. These results were generally consistent with our expectations and with prior research (Laflamme et al., 2002; McBride & Mills, 1993; Yeung et al., 2001). Next, we attempted to understand individual differences among mothers and fathers in the four types of engagement. Parent characteristics played an important role. When mothers contributed more to the family’s income they were less frequently engaged in socialisation and caregiving activities. This is consistent with the relative resources hypothesis that women who contribute more to family income have the power to ‘buy out’ of family work (Coltrane, 2000). However, mothers’ contributions to family income were not associated with their engagement in didactic or physical play, indicating that regardless of their resources, mothers did not use them to ‘buy out’ of what they may have perceived as the most important aspects of their engagement (Craig & Mullan, 2011). Moreover, mothers’ contributions to family income were not associated with any aspect of fathers’ engagement. Thus, in this study, it does not
  • 48. appear that fathers compensated for lower levels of involvement in socialisation and caregiving by mothers who made a greater contribution to family income. Perhaps others (e.g. child care providers and grandparents) compensated for lower frequency of engagement by mothers who contributed more to family income, and future research should investigate this possibility. Fathers’, but not mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about fathers’ roles were also related to several aspects of parental engagement. When fathers held more non-tra- ditional beliefs, they were more frequently engaged in didactic and caregiving activities with their children. This is consistent with the Lamb – Pleck model (Lamb et al., 1987) and with a number of other studies (Beitel & Parke, 1998; Freeman et al., 2008). In con- trast, mothers’ non-traditional beliefs about fathers were not associated with fathers’ engagement, nor were mothers’ beliefs about fathers associated with lower engagement on the part of mothers. Fathers’ greater non-traditional beliefs were associated with less frequent maternal engagement in caregiving, but only in families with sons. Thus, in families in which fathers have non-traditional beliefs, fathers are more likely to engage in caregiving of boys and girls, whereas mothers of boys were less likely to engage in caregiving. Perhaps mothers are willing to reduce their involvement in car- egiving for boys when their partner is more involved, but not willing to reduce their
  • 49. involvement in caregiving for girls. Or, caregiving of preschool-aged girls may involve activities (e.g. bathing) that parents do not feel comfortable assigning to the male parent (Yoshida, 2012). Child characteristics also proved to be important for individual differences in par- ental engagement – especially for mothers. Thus, our hypothesis that contextual factors would be more closely related to fathers’ involvement with girls than with boys (and that child gender should not moderate associations between predictors and maternal involvement) was not supported. For mothers, greater child effortful control was associ- ated with greater engagement in didactic and physical play, and also greater engage- ment in socialisation, if the focal child was female. Mothers may enjoy these types of activities more (and choose to engage in them more often) when their children are higher in effortful control, or better able to control their emotions and behaviour. For socialisation activities, which involve interactions in contexts outside of the home, 510 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al. D ow nl oa de
  • 51. r 2 01 4 mothers may have higher expectations for their daughters’ regulatory abilities or antici- pate that others in social settings may have higher expectations for girls. The maxim ‘boys will be boys’ may excuse the behaviour of preschool-aged boys at church or at a museum, but girls’ out of control behaviour may not be so readily overlooked. Mothers’ engagement in caregiving, however, was not associated with child effortful control. Thus, as expected, effortful control appeared to matter for parents’ involvement in arguably more ‘elective’ aspects of parenting. Child birth order and child gender were also relevant to understanding parental engagement. Fathers were more likely to engage in socialisation activities with earlier-born children. Some have argued that it is the father’s role to introduce the child to the outside world (Paquette, 2004), and previous research has suggested higher levels of father involvement for first-born children (Bègue & Roché, 2005; Price, 2008); thus, fathers may be especially likely to provide opportunities for their earlier-born children to engage in social interactions in contexts outside the home. Mothers were more likely to engage in physical play with boys
  • 52. and with later-born chil- dren. Mothers may show more frequent engagement with later- born children, because if they have more children, they may engage in play with children as a group. That mothers were also more likely to engage in physical play with boys may be due to gen- dered expectations for children. Another important predictor of parent engagement was the engagement of the other parent, at least for socialisation and physical play forms of engagement. These forms of engagement may be more likely to involve both parents simultaneously, or parents may model each other’s involvement (Pleck & Hofferth, 2008), whereas parent engagement in caregiving and didactic activities may be complementary. Besides mothers’ engage- ment in physical play, none of the other parent and child variables that we considered predicted fathers’ engagement in physical play. Although we recognise that con- clusions drawn from null findings must be tentative, one possible interpretation of our lack of success in predicting this aspect of engagement is that physical play is ingrained in the father role (Paquette, 2004), and thus is most robust to contextual influ- ences. However, it is certainly possible that other factors that we did not examine in this study explain individual differences in engagement in physical play – for instance, father characteristics such as personality or subjective health and well-being. Future research considering these factors and others will be necessary
  • 53. to understand correlates of fathers’ engagement in physical play. Couple relationship satisfaction was less important than anticipated in explaining parents’ engagement with their children – greater satisfaction was only associated with greater father involvement in socialisation activities. These results may seem sur- prising, given that prior research has found associations between marital satisfaction and mother as well as father involvement (Carlson et al., 2011; Mehall et al., 2009). It is true that the couples in the present study were on average quite satisfied in their relationships. Thus, a lack of variability in marital satisfaction may explain these null findings. However, it is also possible that marital adjustment is a less important predic- tor of parental engagement for families headed by married parents with preschool age children, especially when compared with other factors such as children’s own characteristics. Limitations of this research should be acknowledged. This was a cross-sectional study, which makes it especially difficult to argue that certain parent and child factors caused individual differences in parental engagement. For instance, although we have argued that children’s temperamental effortful control affects the extent of Early Child Development and Care 511 D
  • 55. Se pt em be r 2 01 4 maternal engagement, it is equally possible that greater maternal engagement has helped children learn to better regulate their emotions and behaviour. Longitudinal research is needed to uncover the likely reciprocal patterns of those associations. Our sample was mostly white, highly educated, relatively wealthy, and married, and thus our results may not apply to other populations of mothers and fathers. Moreover, self-report frequency measures may not be the best way to tap parental engagement. Time use measures such as time diaries may provide more objective and accurate infor- mation regarding parental engagement (Yeung et al., 2001). Finally, our focus in this study was on quantity of parental engagement in developmentally appropriate activi- ties; however, the quality of parental engagement may be just as if not more important for child development. Future studies would do well to include observational measures
  • 56. of the quality of parental engagement in addition to quantitative measures. As the benefits of involved parenting continue to mount, practitioners’ and policy- makers’ interest in fostering parental engagement will endure, and as expectations for fathers’ involvement in parenting increase, parents will keep on struggling to meet these expectations. Headway in encouraging and supporting parental engagement – especially that of fathers – can only be made with a thorough understanding of where we are at and where we need to go and the multiple factors that underlie individ- ual differences in maternal and paternal engagement. Acknowledgements This research was supported by an NICHD grant (R03 HD050235) to the first author. We express our gratitude to the parents and children who participated in this study and the students who assisted with data collection, especially Catherine Buckley. Notes on contributors Sarah J. Schoppe-Sullivan is an associate professor of human development and family science at The Ohio State University. Her research interests include co- parenting relationships, fathering behaviour, and the transition to parenthood. Letitia E. Kotila is a doctoral student in human development and family science at The Ohio State University. Her research focuses on fathering, especially among unmarried, at-risk men.
  • 57. Rongfang Jia is a doctoral student in human development and family science at The Ohio State University. Her research focuses on the development of mother – and father – child relationships and implications for child functioning. Sarah N. Lang is a doctoral student in human development and family science at The Ohio State University. Her research interests include caregiver – child relationships in the home and in childcare contexts. Daniel J. Bower is a doctoral student in human development and family science at The Ohio State University. His research interests include the effects of individual psychological character- istics on couple relationships. References Adamsons, K., & Buehler, C. (2007). Mothering versus fathering versus parenting: Measurement equivalence in parenting measures. Parenting: Science and Practice, 7, 271 – 303. 512 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al. D ow nl oa de d
  • 59. 01 4 Barry, A.A., Smith, J.Z., Deutsch, F.M., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (2011). Fathers’ involvement in child care and perceptions of parenting skill over the transition to parenthood. Journal of Family Issues, 32, 1500 – 1521. Bègue, L., & Roché, S. (2005). Birth order and youth delinquent behavior: Testing the differ- ential parental control hypothesis in a French representative sample. Psychology, Crime & Law, 11, 73 – 85. Beitel, A.H., & Parke, R.D. (1998). Paternal involvement in infancy: The role of maternal and paternal attitudes. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 268 – 288. Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83 – 96. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1129836 Bianchi, S.M., Robinson, J.P., & Milkie, M.A. (2006). Changing rhythms of American family life. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Bornstein, M. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of parenting, Vol. 1: Children and parenting (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Bridgett, D.J., Gartstein, M.A., Putnam, S.P., McKay, T., Iddins, E., Robertson, C., . . .
  • 60. Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 103 – 116. Cabrera, N.J., & Mitchell, S. (2009). An exploratory study of fathers’ parenting stress and tod- dlers’ social development in low-income African American families. Fathering, 7, 201 – 225. Cabrera, N.J., Ryan, R.M., Shannon, J.D., Brooks-Gunn, J., Vogel, C., Raikes, H., & Cohen, R. (2004). Low-income fathers’ involvement in their toddlers’ lives: Biological fathers from the early head start research and evaluation study. Fathering, 2, 5 – 36. Carlson, M.J., Pilkauskas, N.V., McLanahan, S.S., & Brooks- Gunn, J. (2011). Couples as part- ners and parents over children’s early years. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73, 317 – 334. Carson, J., & Parke, R.D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent child interactions and chil- dren’s peer competency. Child Development, 67, 22 – 26. Coley, R.L., & Morris, J.E. (2004). Comparing father and mother reports of father involvement among low-income minority families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 982 – 997. Coltrane, S. (2000). Research on household labor: Modeling and measuring the social embedd- edness of routine family work. Journal of Marriage and Family,
  • 61. 62, 1208 – 1233. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1566732 Craig, L., & Mullan, K. (2011). How mothers and fathers share childcare: A cross-national time- use comparison. American Sociological Review, 76, 834 – 861. Doherty, W.J., Kouneski, E.F., & Erickson, M.F. (1998). Responsible fathering: An overview and conceptual framework. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 277 – 292. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/353848 Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Shepard, S.A., Guthrie, I.K., Murphy, B.C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of chil- dren’s social functioning. Child Development, 70, 513 – 534. Fagan, J., & Barnett, M. (2003). The relationship between maternal gatekeeping, paternal com- petence, mothers’ attitudes about the father role, and father involvement. Journal of Family Issues, 24, 1020 – 1043. Freeman, H., Newland, L.A., & Coyl, D.D. (2008). Father beliefs as a mediator between con- textual barriers and father involvement. Early Child Development and Care, 178, 803 – 819. Hayes, A.F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924 – 936.
  • 62. Hewlett, B. (Ed.). (1992). Father – child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., & Malcuit, G. (2002). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free- play with their infants at 9 and 15 months. Sex Roles, 47, 507 – 518. Lamb, M. (Ed.). (1997). The role of the father in child development (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley. Lamb, M.E., Pleck, J.H., Charnov, E.L., & Levine, J.A. (1985). Paternal behavior in humans. American Zoologist, 25, 883 – 894. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3883043 Early Child Development and Care 513 D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W
  • 63. om an 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0 6: 12 3 0 Se pt em be r 2 01 4 Lamb, M.E., Pleck, J.H., Charnov, E.L., & Levine, J.A. (1987). A biosocial perspective on
  • 64. paternal behavior and involvement. In J.B. Lancaster, J. Altman, A.S. Rossi, & L.R. Sherroa (Eds.), Parenting across the lifespan: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 111 – 142). New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Lundberg, S., McLanahan, S., & Rose, E. (2007). Child gender and father involvement in fragile families. Demography, 44, 79 – 92. MacDonald, K., & Parke, R.D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent child play interaction and peer interactive competence. Child Development, 55, 1265 – 1277. Retrieved from http://www. jstor.org/stable/1129996 McBride, B.A., & Mills, G. (1993). A comparison of mother and father involvement with their preschool age children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 457 – 477. McBride, B.A., Schoppe, S.J., & Rane, T.R. (2002). Child characteristics, parenting stress, and parental involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 998 – 1011. Mehall, K.G., Spinrad, T.L., Eisenberg, N., & Gaertner, B.M. (2009). Examining the relations of infant temperament and couples’ marital satisfaction to mother and father involvement: A longitudinal study. Fathering, 7, 23 – 48. Palkovitz, R. (1984). Parental attitudes and fathers’ interactions with their 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1054 – 1060.
  • 65. Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father – child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193 – 219. Paquette, D., Carbonneau, R., Dubeau, D., Bigras, M., & Tremblay, R.E. (2003). Prevalence of father – child rough-and-tumble-play and physical aggression in preschool children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 18, 171 – 189. Parke, R.D., & Ladd, G.W. (1992). Family – peer relations: Modes of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pleck, J.H., & Hofferth, S.L. (2008). Mother involvement as an influence on father involvement with early adolescents. Fathering, 6, 267 – 286. Pleck, J.H., & Masciadrelli, B.P. (2004). Parental involvement by U.S. residential fathers: Levels, sources, and consequences. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child devel- opment (4th ed., pp. 222 – 271). New York: Wiley. Price, J. (2008). Parent – child quality time: Does birth order matter? Journal of Human Resources, 43, 240 – 265. Putnam, S.P., & Rothbart, M.K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 102 – 112. Raley, S., & Bianchi, S. (2006). Sons, daughters, and family processes: Does gender of
  • 66. children matter? Annual Review of Sociology, 32, 401 – 421. Rothbart, M.K., & Bates, J.E. (2006). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 99 – 166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Fathers’ involvement and children’s developmental outcomes: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatrica, 97, 153 – 158. Schoppe, S.J. (2001). What is a father? Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of mar- riage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15 – 28. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/350547 Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Shannon, J.D., Cabrera, N.J., & Lamb, M.E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year olds: Contributions to language and cognitive devel- opment. Child Development, 75, 1806 – 1820. Yeung, W.J., Sandberg, J.F., Davis-Kean, P.A., & Hofferth, S.A. (2001). Children’s time with fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 136 – 154. Yoshida, A. (2012). Dads who do diapers: Factors affecting care
  • 67. of young children by fathers. Journal of Family Issues, 33, 451 – 477. 514 S.J. Schoppe-Sullivan et al. D ow nl oa de d by [T ex as W om an 's U ni ve rs ity ] a t 0