SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
A R T I C L E 
MOTHER–CHILD AND FATHER–CHILD INTERACTION WITH THEIR 24-MONTH-OLD 
CHILDREN DURING FEEDING, CONSIDERING PATERNAL INVOLVEMENT AND THE 
CHILD’S TEMPERAMENT IN A COMMUNITY SAMPLE 
LUCA CERNIGLIA 
International Telematic University Uninettuno 
SILVIA CIMINO AND GIULIA BALLAROTTO 
University of Rome, Sapienza 
ABSTRACT: The article aims to study mother–child and father–child interactions with 24-month-old children during feeding, considering the possible 
influence of time spent by the parent with the child, the infantile temperament, and the parental psychological profile. The families were recruited 
from 12 preschools in Italy (N = 77 families). Through an observation of the feeding [Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare (SVIA – 
Feeding Scale; I. Chatoor et al., 1997; L. Lucarelli et al., 2002)], self-reporting [Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R; L.R. Derogatis, 1994), 
and report-form questionnaires [Italian Questionnaires on Temperament (QUIT; G. Axia, 2002)], and information provided by the parents about 
the amount of time spent with their children, results showed that the overall quality of father–child interactions during feeding is lower than that 
of mother–child interactions. Fathers showed higher psychological symptoms than did mothers. No associations were found between the fathers’ 
psychopathological risk and the quality of interactions with their children during feeding. Mothers’ psychopathological risks predicted less contingent 
exchanges interactions with their children during feeding. Children’s temperaments significantly influence mother–child interactions, but no association 
exists between maternal involvement and the quality of interactions with their children. Paternal involvement predicts a better quality of father–infant 
interactions when associated with a child’s higher scores on Social Orientation. The quality of parents’ interactions with their children during feeding 
are impacted by different issues originating from the parent’s psychological profile, the degree of involvement, and from the child’s temperament. 
RESUMEN: El art´ıculo se propone estudiar las interacciones pap´a-hijo y mam´a-hijo con ni˜nos de 24 meses de edad durante el momento de alimentarse, 
considerando la posible influencia del tiempo que el progenitor pasa con el ni˜no, el temperamento infantil y el perfil sicol´ogico de los padres. Las 
familias fueron reclutadas en doce centros prescolares en Italia (N = 77 familias). Por medio de una observaci´on del momento de alimentaci´on 
(SVIA), el auto-reportaje (SCL-90-R) y cuestionarios en formularios de reporte (QUIT), e informaci´on ofrecida por los padres acerca de la cantidad 
de tiempo pasada con sus ni˜nos, los resultados mostraron que la calidad general de las interacciones pap´a-hijo durante el momento de alimentarse es 
m´as baja que las de mam´a-hijo. Los pap´as mostraron m´as altos s´ıntomas sicol´ogicos que las mam´as. No se encontr´o ninguna asociaci´on entre el riesgo 
sicopatol´ogico de los pap´as y la calidad de las interacciones con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on. Los riesgos sicopatol´ogicos de las madres predijeron 
menos interacciones de intercambios eventuales con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on. El temperamento de los ni˜nos significativamente influye en las 
interacciones mam´a-hijo, pero no existe asociaci´on entre el involucramiento materno y la calidad de las interacciones con sus ni˜nos. El involucramiento 
paterno predice una mejor calidad de interacciones pap´a-infante cuando est´a asociado con los m´as altos puntajes del ni˜no en la Orientaci´on Social. 
Diferentes asuntos impactan la calidad de las interacciones de los padres con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on, partiendo del perfil sicol´ogico de los 
padres, el grado de involucramiento y el temperamento del ni˜no. 
RE´ SUME´ : Cet article a pour but d’e´tudier les interactions pe`re-enfant et me`re-enfant avec leurs enfants de 24 mois durant l’alimentation, en conside´rant 
l’influence potentielle du temps pass´e par le parent avec l’enfant, le temp´erament de l’enfant et le profil psychologique parental. Les familles ont 
´et´e recrut´ees dans dix cr`eches italiennes (N = 77 familles). Au travers d’une observation de l’alimentation (SVIA), du questionnaire SCL-90-R, des 
questionnaires sous forme de rapport (QUIT), et les renseignements donn´es par les parents sur la quantit´e de temps pass´e avec leurs enfants, les r´esultats 
ont montr´e que la qualit´e g´en´erale des interactions p`ere-enfant durant l’alimentation est moins ´elev´ee que celle des interactions m`ere-enfant. Les p`eres 
ont fait preuve de plus de symptˆomes psychologiques que les m`eres. Aucun lien n’a ´et´e trouv´e entre le risque psychopathologique des p`eres et la 
qualit´e des interactions avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. Les risques psychopathologiques des m`eres ont pr´edit des ´echanges interactifs moins 
I, Luca Cerniglia, declare that no author has any conflict of interest. 
Direct correspondence to: Luca Cerniglia, Corso Vittorio Emanuele II, 39–00100–Rome, Italy; e-mail: l.cerniglia@uninettunouniversity.net. 
INFANT MENTAL HEALTH JOURNAL, Vol. 35(5), 473–481 (2014) 
C  
2014 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health 
View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com. 
DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21466 
473
474 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto 
contingents avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. Le temp´erament des enfants influencent fortement les interactions m`ere-enfant mais aucun lien 
n’existe entre l’engagement maternel et la qualit´e des interactions avec leurs enfants. L’engagement paternel pr´edit un meilleure qualit´e des interactions 
p`ere-b´eb´e lorsque li´ees `a des scores plus ´elev´es de l’enfant sur l’Orientation Sociale. Diff´erents probl`emes issus du profil psychologique du parent, du 
degr´e d’engagement et du temp´erament de l’enfant ont un impact sur la qualit´e des interactions des parents avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. 
ZUSAMMENFASSUNG: Der Artikel zielt darauf ab, Vater-Kind-und Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen bei 24 Monate alten Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung, 
in Anbetracht des m¨oglichen Einflusses der Zeit, die das Elternteil mit dem Kind verbringt, des kindlichen Temperaments und des psychologischen 
Profils der Eltern, zu untersuchen. Die Familien wurden aus zw¨olf Vorschulen in Italien (N = 77 Familien) rekrutiert. Anhand einer Beobachtung der 
F¨utterung (SVIA), Selbst-Berichten (SCL-90-R) und anderen Frageb¨ogen (QUIT) sowie Informationen von den Eltern ¨uber die Menge der Zeit, die 
sie mit ihren Kindern verbracht haben, zeigten die Ergebnisse, dass die Gesamtqualit¨at der Vater-Kind-Interaktionen w¨ahrend der F¨utterung niedriger 
ist als die der Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen. V¨ater zeigten h¨ohere psychische Symptome als M¨utter. Es wurden keine Zusammenh¨ange zwischen dem 
psychopathologischen Risiko der V¨ater und der Qualit¨at der Interaktion mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung gefunden. Die psychopathologischen 
Risiken der M¨utter sagten weniger zuf¨allige Austauschsinteraktionen mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung vorher. Die Temperamente der 
Kinder beeinflussen die Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen signifikant, es besteht jedoch kein Zusammenhang zwischen m¨utterlichem Engagement und der 
Interaktionsqualit¨at mit ihren Kindern. V¨aterliches Engagement sagt eine bessere Qualit¨at der Vater-Kind-Interaktionen vorher, wenn sie mit h¨oheren 
Werten f¨ur die soziale Orientierung des Kindes assoziiert wird. Die Interaktionsqualit¨at der Eltern mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung wird von 
unterschiedlichen Themen beeinflusst: dem psychologischen Profil der Eltern, dem Grad des Engagements und vom Temperament des Kindes. 
ABSTRACT: :
24			 
 !#12$%(N = 77 )(SVIA)(SCL-90-R)(QUIT)
'()%
	 
*+(	)%+,
-.	 
		
%*/-.	 
contingent exchanges interactions*)

	
	 
		
%*(
Social Orientation(-0#	1+( 
	
	
 

')232*
/4( 
* * * 
It has been widely recognized that the quality of mother– 
infant interaction during a child’s first years of life has a profound 
influence on his or her psychological welfare and on the possi-ble 
onset of psychopathologies in infants (Ramchandani et al., 
2013). Nevertheless, family and ecological theories (Bronfen-brenner, 
1979; Minuchin, 1974) suggest that mother–infant and 
father–infant dyads are interconnected. Growing evidence has 
suggested that positive father–infant interactions can directly im-pact 
children’s cognitive and socioemotional development (Brown, 
McBride, Bost,  Shin, 2011) (e.g., engaging with their children, 
monitoring them, and setting limits and rules), and also indirectly 
impact such development (e.g., influencing mother–infant inter-actions, 
supporting mothers’ decisions and behaviors concerning 
their sons and daughters, and/or providing economic support to the 
family, which may contribute to the rearing and emotional health 
of the children) (Atzaba-Poria et al., 2010; Carlson, 2006; Cimino, 
Cerniglia, Paciello,  Sinesi, 2012). Lamb’s (2010) theoretical 
framework has conceptualized that fathers are unique kinds of in-teractional 
partners for their children, and are rather distinct from 
mothers; the paternal fashion of interactions differ from the ma-ternal 
fashion, being more characterized by physical contact and 
rough-and-tumble play, which seems to have a specific role in pro-moting 
a child’s emotional-regulation processes (Feldman, 2003). 
Consequently, in more recent years, research has concentrated on 
both mothers and fathers, specifically on the role of fathers as 
protectors, and on risk factors for the development of children’s 
psychological difficulties while considering triadic interactions to 
verify whether there are differences in parenting quality and chil-dren’s 
interactive behaviors with mothers and fathers in the dyadic 
context versus those in the triadic context (Di Folco  Zavattini, 
2014; Kwon, Jeon, Lewsader,  Elicker, 2012; Lamb  Lewis, 
2007). While it has been proposed that a combination of maternal 
and paternal psychopathologies may create a style of coparenting 
dominated by negative interactive cycles with children during play 
and feeding (Pinquart  Teubert, 2010), the specific impact that 
psychopathological symptoms in fathers might have on a child’s 
mental health has been only recently addressed, mostly investigat-ing 
the construct of involvement in community samples (Lamb, 
2010). Highly involved fathers (in terms of time spent with their 
children) seem likely to have more empathic sons and daughters 
who show a more internal locus of control (Cerniglia, Cimino, Bal-larotto, 
 Monniello, 2014; Pleck, 2010). With specific regard to 
the context of feeding, international research has mainly focused 
on mother–infant interactions because mothers are traditionally 
thought to take primary responsibility for feeding their children 
(Blissett, Meyer,  Haycraft, 2006) and are usually considered 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 475 
their primary caregiver (Campbell et al., 2010; Patrick, Nicklas, 
Hughes,  Morales, 2005). However, parents’ gender roles and re-sponsibilities 
have changed in the last few decades, above all as a 
result of increasing employment outside the home for mothers with 
young children. Research has been considering these changes, and 
recently has focused on fathers’ psychological characteristics and 
practices during feeding interactions with their children (Khand-pur, 
Blaine, Fisher,  Davison, 2014). Although Blissett and Hay-craft 
(2011) found no significant differences in feeding practices 
between mothers and fathers, in the same study fathers reported 
feeling personally responsible for organising meals at least half of 
the time, and deciding what kinds of foods their children should 
eat (Mallan et al., 2013). Hendy, Williams, Camise, Eckman, and 
Hedemann (2009) found that fathers were less likely to limit their 
children in consuming snacks and were less focused on letting 
them eat a variety of foods such as fruits and vegetables. Some 
studies have shown that fathers are more likely than are mothers 
to encourage children to eat using prompts (verbal or physical), 
reasoning, pressure, and incentives during feeding (Haycraft  
Blissett, 2008; Orrell-Valente et al., 2007). Nevertheless, threats to 
withdraw play privileges or access to food were not usually used 
by fathers, with no psychopathological risk (Haycraft  Blissett, 
2012; Tschann et al., 2013). Regarding the context of feeding, 
the ages of children have been linked to diverse father–child in-teractions. 
Fathers of older children reported lower use of food 
for emotional regulation than did fathers of younger children, and 
more restriction of food for older children if they were concerned 
about the child’s weight (Musher-Eizenman, de Lauzon-Guillain, 
Holub, Leporc,  Charles, 2009). Furthermore, parent–child inter-actions 
during feeding seem to be less problematic with toddlers 
than with infants, although this result seems to have been con-firmed 
only in low-risk samples whereas high-risk samples show a 
different trend. Psychopathological risk in mothers and in fathers 
as well as in low-income parents seems to be related to maladaptive 
parent–child interactions more frequently during toddlerhood than 
during childhood (Horodynski  Arndt, 2005). 
In fact, nonresponsive parenting characterizes families with 
psychopathological parents who frequently fail to respond to new 
skills and signals shown by toddlers, such as self-feeding, the use 
of utensils, and the use of language to request food and help (Birch, 
Fisher,  Davison, 2003). 
In clinical samples, paternal pressure to eat has been corre-lated 
with bulimia and restriction symptoms in fathers (Blissett 
et al., 2006) whereas eating disorders and depression in mothers 
proved to be the most significant psychopathology correlated with 
maladaptive patterns of interaction between mothers and infants 
(Cimino, Cerniglia,  Paciello 2014; Watkins, Cooper,  Lask, 
2012). Depressed mothers seem to display less positive engage-ment 
with their infants during feeding interactions and to show 
anxiety, anger, and intrusivity, having difficulties with empathi-cally 
recognizing and regulating the child’s affective states during 
mealtimes. Ammaniti, Lucarelli, Cimino, D’Olimpio, and Chatoor 
(2010) showed that these parental symptoms may foster feeding 
disorders in children. 
While earlier studies have aimed to consider the amount of 
time spent by fathers with their children as an indicator of in-volvement, 
more recent studies seem to have underlined the im-portance 
of the actual quality of interactions to specifically un-derstand 
why and how paternal involvement should be expected 
to have a positive effect on child development (Stueve  Pleck, 
2001). Lamb (2013) stressed the role of engagement accessibil-ity 
and sensitivity as well as the significance of paternal warmth 
and support during interactions. Attachment theory proposes that 
fathers’ involvement promotes child development because it fos-ters 
secure attachment, which is linked to positive child outcomes 
(Brown, McBride, Shin,  Bost, 2007). Nevertheless, little at-tention 
has been given in attachment literature to the quality of 
father–child interactions during feeding and to their links with 
adaptive or maladaptive outcomes in the emotional and behavioral 
development of children, which in turn may be bidirectionally 
linked to security or insecurity of attachment in infants and in their 
parents. 
Some research has suggested that temperamentally easy chil-dren 
are more likely to be involved in good-quality interactions 
with their fathers (Mehall, Spinrad, Eisenberg,  Gaertner, 2009). 
Children who show high levels of irritability, negative moods, and 
irregular behavioral and biological patterns seem to be involved 
in lower quality interactions with their mothers and fathers. On 
the other hand, Frodi et al. (1982) found that fathers were more 
involved with difficult sons (but not with difficult daughters), as 
recently confirmed by Leaper (2002) and Cabrera, Fagan, Wight, 
and Schadler (2011). 
To capture the bidirectional effect of child–father interaction, 
several studies have chosen to use observational methods, which 
take into consideration verbal and nonverbal contexts of behavioral 
dynamics (Hughes et al., 2010).Many of these studies have focused 
on play routines (both involving mothers and fathers) while only 
some of them have observed feeding interactions (Blissett, 2011). 
Yet, several studies have suggested that feeding interactions be-tween 
parents and their children constitute an important context in 
which infants and toddlers may learn to recognize and make sense 
of verbal and nonverbal communications and signals, thus build-ing 
a basis for emotional bonds and attachment quality that in turn 
forms essential roots for healthy emotional/behavioral functioning 
(Black  Aboud, 2011). 
Based on these theoretical and empirical premises, which 
include Lamb’s (2010) conceptual framework, and that no other 
study to our knowledge has addressed father–toddler interactions 
during feeding in nonclinical samples, we chose to study father– 
infant and mother–infant interactions during feeding at 24 months 
of age, considering the possible influence of time spent by the 
parent with the child, the infantile temperament, and the parental 
psychological profile. 
In particular, this study has the following objectives: 
• to verify whether the quality of mother–child and father– 
child interactions during feeding differ from each other; 
• to assess parental psychopathological risks; and 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
476 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto 
• to verify whether parental psychopathological risks affect 
the quality of parent–child interactions during feeding while 
considering child temperament and parental involvement. 
METHOD 
Participants 
Seventy-seven families were recruited from 12 preschools in Italy 
(N = 77). The children (45 girls, 32 boys) ranged from 23 to 26 
months of age (SD = 0.8), and their parents’ mean ages were 34.5 
(SD = 1.2) years for the fathers and 32.3 (SD = 0.9) years for 
the mothers. Eighty-nine per cent of the children were firstborn, 
and 73% of them had been breast-fed in their first year of life. 
Ninety-one per cent of the couples were married, and all parents 
were of middle-class socioeconomic status (SES; Hollingshead, 
1975). Ninety-four percent of the parents cohabited, three percent 
of the parents who cohabited were not married, and 82% of the 
families had double income. All partners of the mothers recruited 
for the present study were biological fathers of the children, and 
all parents were Caucasian. All parents included in the study, both 
mothers and fathers, were involved in caregiving practices with 
their children on a daily basis [average time spent weekly by the 
mothers with their children: 76.1 hr (SD = 2.3); average time spent 
weekly by the fathers with their children: 38.6 hr (SD = 4.1)]. 
Procedure 
Sample selection criteria were: (a) age of the children ranging 
between 23 and 26 months, (b) no reported mental or physical 
problems or disorders in either the parents or the children, and 
(c) both parents reporting frequently feeding the child without the 
presence of the other parent. A group of trained psychologists con-tacted 
12 preschools in Central Italy and presented the research 
rationale and objective. Families who agreed to be recruited for the 
study filled out an informed consent form. The order of the admin-istration 
of the measures (described later) was randomly selected. 
All parent–child pairs in the sample were observed at their homes 
in 20-min video-recordings during lunch at midday; the feeding 
interactions were part of one regular meal observed separately (on 
2 different days) for mother–child and father–child interactions. 
The videos were recorded by psychologists who were specifically 
trained in the use of this observational tool, and were coded by 
two trained independent raters who watched the videos and scored 
them on the basis of the manual (Lucarelli et al., 2002) using both 
a paper–pencil system and a coding software program designed for 
the computation of scores on each subscale. The whole observation 
was scheduled on the basis of a validated procedure for the Ital-ian 
population [Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare 
(SVIA) – Observational Scale for Mother-Infant Interaction dur-ing 
Feeding; Chatoor et al., 1997; Lucarelli et al., 2002; described 
later] and in connection with several previous studies (Ammaniti 
et al., 2010). This tool was chosen over others because it is the 
only procedure that specifically assesses parent–child interactions 
during feeding that are validated for the Italian population. Two 
instruments were given to each parent to be completed at their 
home and independently: a self-report measure for the assessment 
of their psychological symptom status, the Symptom Checklist- 
90-Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1994), which was filled out 
independently by each parent, and a report-form questionnaire to 
evaluate their child’s temperament: the Italian Questionnaires on 
Temperament (QUIT; Axia, 2002). Mothers and fathers indepen-dently 
filled out this questionnaire, but they obtained an interrater 
agreement of 89%. Consequently, we used a mean score. More-over, 
each parent filled out a form asking him or her to report the 
average number of hours spent with the child during the week. 
Measures 
Assessment of parents’ psychological symptom statuses. The 
SCL-90-R is a 90-item self-report symptom inventory designed 
to measure psychological symptoms and psychological distress 
(Derogatis, 1994). It is scored and interpreted in terms of 
nine primary subscales (Somatisation, Obsessive-Compulsivity, 
Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Hostility, Phobic 
Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychoticism) and three Global 
Indices of Distress (Global Severity Index, Positive Symptom 
Distress Index, and Positive Symptom Total). The SCL-90-R 
is rated on a Likert scale of 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) and 
asks participants to report if they have suffered in the past week 
from: Headaches (Somatisation scale), Trouble remembering 
things (Obsessive-Compulsivity scale), Feeling critical of others 
(Interpersonal Sensitivity scale), Blaming oneself for things (De-pression 
scale), Feeling fearful (Anxiety scale), Feeling watched 
or talked about by others (Paranoid Ideation scale), and The idea 
that something is wrong with one’s mind (Psychoticism scale). 
The SCL-90-R has been shown to have good internal coher-ence 
(α = 0.70–0.96) in adolescents and adults (Italian validated 
version: Prunas, Sarno, Preti, Madeddu,  Perugini, 2012). 
Assessment of child temperament. Participants’ answers on the 
QUIT (Axia, 2002) are rated on a Likert scale of 1 (almost never) 
to 6 (almost always). This questionnairewas validated for an Italian 
sample to measure child temperament from the first month after 
birth to 11 years of age, within four age groups: 1 to 12 months, 
13 to 36 months, 3 to 6 years, and 7 to 11 years. It is given to the 
parents and investigates six dimensions: the level of motor activity 
(e.g., The child grasps objects with rapid movements), attentional 
capacity (e.g., The child immediately turns his/her gaze towards 
the person by whom he or she is called), inhibition to novelty and 
availability to socialization (e.g., the child observes strangers for a 
long time), positive emotionality (e.g., the child maintains a smiling 
face when playing), and negative emotionality (e.g., the child cries 
for a long time when contradicted). The questionnaire for the age 
group 1 to 12 months showed good internal consistency (Cronbach 
αs = .59–.71), and the values of the Pearson correlations between 
the scales of the questionnaire completed separately by the mother 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 477 
and father, ranging between .32 and .62, show that both give a very 
similar assessment of the child (Axia, 2002). 
Assessment of parent–child feeding interactions. The Italian ver-sion 
(SVIA – Feeding Scale; Chatoor et al., 1997; Lucarelli et al., 
2002) of the observational scale has 41 items rated on a Likert 
scale of 0 (confirm, none) to 3 (confirm, many) and four subscales 
coding dyadic mother–child and father–child interactions sepa-rately: 
Affective State of the Parent (e.g., the parent appears sad 
during feeding), Interactional Conflict (e.g., the parent forces food 
into the child’s mouth), Food Refusal Behaviors of the Child (e.g., 
the child refuses to open his/her mouth), and Affective State of 
the Dyad (e.g., the parent and the child show joy during feeding). 
Higher scores on the Affective State of the Parent refer to greater 
difficulties of the caregiver in showing positive affects and to a 
higher frequency of negative affects such as sadness or distress. 
The subscale Interactional Conflict evaluates both the presence 
and the intensity of conflictual exchanges within the dyad (e.g., 
the parent directs the meal according to his or her own emotions 
and intentions rather than following the signals from the child). 
The subscale Food Refusal Behaviors of the Child explores the 
behavioral and emotional characteristics of the feeding patterns 
of the child (e.g., being easily distracted, or showing opposition 
or negativity). Higher scores on the Affective State of the Dyad 
refer to the difficulties of the caregiver in supporting autonomous 
initiatives of the child (by means of requests, insistent orders, and 
criticism) while the child demonstrates distress and is generally 
oppositional. In the Italian version that was validated for mother– 
infant interactions (Lucarelli et al., 2002), the discriminant analysis 
showed correct group classification ranging from 82 to 92%, and 
construct validity for the tool has been proven. Interrater reliability 
ranged between 0.82 and 0.92 (intraclass correlation coefficients). 
DATA ANALYSES 
A preliminary screening of the data showed few data missing for 
each instrument (4% for each instrument). Missing data were cor-rected 
using multiple imputation in SPSS software (Version 18.0). 
To examine the quality of the interactional patterns between fa-thers 
and children and mothers and children during feeding, we 
carried out multivariate analyses of variances (MANOVAs) on the 
SVIA dimensions (to respond to Objective a). In all MANOVAs, 
univariate analyses were then conducted on significant effects, and 
the Duncan test (Bonferroni correction) was used for contrasts. To 
respond to Objective b, we investigated the maternal and paternal 
psychological status through a series of MANOVAs that were car-ried 
out on the scores (transformed in their square root whenever 
the distribution deviated from normality) obtained from the SCL- 
90-R (the Global Severity Index and, subsequently, the subscales). 
In MANOVAs regarding the SVIA procedure and the SCL-90-R 
questionnaires, we considered the group of mothers and the group 
of fathers as criterion variables while mothers’ and fathers’ scores 
on the measures constituted dependent variables. As a final step 
(to respond to Objective c), two hierarchical regression analyses 
were conducted to investigate the influence of specific SCL-90-R 
subscales, temperament subscales, and parental involvement on 
the relational dimensions of the Feeding Scale in mother–infant 
and father–infant interactions. In all the analyses we conducted, 
the child’s gender showed no significant effect on the variables. 
All analyses were performed with SPSS software (Version 
18.0). 
RESULTS 
Quality of Mother–Child and Father–Child Interactions During 
Feeding 
MANOVA analyses of the observational scale during feeding 
showed that fathers’ scores were significantly higher than were 
mothers’ scores in all four subscales, p  .01 (see Table 1). No 
participant exceeded the cutoffs for the clinical population of the 
Italian version of the SVIA (Lucarelli et al., 2002). 
Parental Psychopathological Risk Assessment 
With respect to the assessment of mothers’ and fathers’ psycho-logical 
symptoms, the analyses showed that fathers scored higher 
than did mothers on the Global Severity Index and in all SCL-90- 
R subscales, p  .01 (see Table 2). Furthermore, 3 mothers and 4 
fathers obtained scores higher than the cutoffs of the clinical range 
(Prunas et al., 2012). 
Quality of Parent–Child Interactions During Feeding Considering 
Parental Psychopathological Risk, Child Temperament, and 
Parental Involvement 
Two regression analyses were conducted separately for mothers 
and fathers to investigate the influence of all nine SCL-90-R sub-scales, 
all five temperament subscales, and parental involvement 
(in terms of hours spent by the parent with the child) on all four 
of the relational dimensions of the SVIA in mother–infant and 
father–infant interactions, which showed higher maternal scores 
on Phobic Anxiety predicting mothers’ Affective State of the Par-ent, 
p  .05, in the direction of more negative affects. Higher 
children’s Negative Emotionality scores in the QUIT, p  .001, 
predicted the Affective State of the Dyad in the direction of more 
maladaptive mother–infant interactions during feeding. Maternal 
psychological symptoms and involvement of the mother (in terms 
of time spent with the child weekly) did not predict the quality of 
mother–infant interactions during feeding. 
Regarding fathers, the regression analyses showed the only 
significant results: Higher paternal involvement (in terms of the 
number of hours spent with the child) predicts lower scores (i.e., a 
better quality of father–child interactions) on three of the sub-scales 
of the SVIA only when considered in association with 
higher children’s scores on Social Orientation (in particular, In-teractional 
Conflict, p  .05; Food Refusal of the Child, p  
.05; and Affective State of the Dyad, p  .05). No association or 
prediction was found between fathers’ psychopathological risks 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
478 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto 
TABLE 1. Means (SDs), F, and P Values of Mothers’ and Fathers’ Scores on SVIA Subscales 
Mothers Fathers Fisher F Test p 
Affective State of the Parent 5.27 (3.33) 12.46 (5.35) F(1, 153) = 100,29 .001 
Interactional Conflict 4.64 (3.85) 11.29 (6.2) F(1, 153) = 63,91 .001 
Food Refusal of the Child 2.52 (1.94) 6.42 (3.36) F(1, 153) = 77,86 .001 
Affective State of the Dyad 2.79 (2.25) 6.55 (4.27) F(1, 153) = 46,61 .001 
SVIA = Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare. 
TABLE 2. Means (SDs) F, and P Values of Mothers’ and Fathers’ Scores on SCL-90-R Subscales 
Mothers Fathers Fisher F Test p 
Somatization 0.15 (0.11) 0.41 (0.44) F(1, 153) = 24,93 .001 
Obsessive-Compulsive 0.11 (0.11) 0.71 (1.08) F(1, 153) = 23,7 .001 
Interpersonal Sensitivity 0.1 (0.1) 0.49 (0.61) F(1, 153) = 30,45 .001 
Depression 0.12 (0.11) 0.41 (0.42) F(1, 153) = 34,49 .001 
Anxiety 0.1 (0.12) 0.75 (1.02) F(1, 153) = 30,02 .001 
Hostility 0.1 (0.14) 0.41 (0.40) F(1, 153) = 41,98 .001 
Phobic Anxiety 0.12 (0.13) 0.48 (0.5) F(1, 153) = 36,98 .001 
Paranoid Ideation 0.07 (0.11) 0.41 (0.46) F(1, 153) = 39,67 .001 
Psycoticism 0.14 (0.12) 0.44 (0.4) F(1, 153) = 39,49 .001 
Global Severity Index 0.11 (0.05) 0.5 (0.54) F(1, 153) = 39,14 .001 
SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-90-Revised. 
TABLE 3. Results and Values of the Regression Analyses 
SVIA QUIT Negative Emotionality SCL-90-R Phobic Anxiety 
Mother R2 β t p R2 β t p 
Affective State n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. .239 .365 2.232 .029∗ 
Affective State of the Dyad .001 .342 2.749 .001∗∗ n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. 
Involvement 
Father R2 β t p 
Interactional Conflict • Social Orientation .121 .348 3.214 .002∗ 
Food Refusal of the Child • Social Orientation .062 .249 2.227 .029∗ 
Affective State of the Dyad • Social Orientation .112 .334 3.073 .003∗ 
Note. The subscales that are not shown in this table are not statistically significant. n.s. = not significant; SVIA = Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare; 
QUIT = [Italian Questionnaires on Temperament Questionnaire; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-90-Revised; • = Association with. 
∗p  .05. ∗∗p  .01. 
and the quality of interactions with their children during feeding. 
The results and values of the regression analyses are shown in 
Table 3. 
DISCUSSION 
This study aimed to examine specific characteristics of mother– 
infant and father–infant interactions during feeding, taking into 
account the possible influence of parental psychological profiles, 
children’s temperaments, and parental involvement. 
To assess the quality of parent–child interactions during feed-ing, 
we chose to use an observational procedure (SVIA). Our re-sults 
indicated that mothers’ and fathers’ interactions with their 
children differ significantly. In particular, father–child interactions 
are significantly more maladaptive. Despite this, no participant 
(mothers or fathers) exceeded the clinical cutoffs in the Italian pop-ulation. 
To verify the possible weight of parental psychopathologi-cal 
risk, child temperament, and parental involvement on the qual-ity 
of parent–child interactions during feeding (assessed through 
the SVIA), we chose to use a self-report questionnaire (for parental 
psychopathological risk; SCL-90-R), a report-form measure (for 
child’s temperament; QUIT), and information provided by parents 
about the time spent with their children during the week (parental 
involvement). We found that mothers’ psychopathological risks 
(viz., phobic anxiety) predict the general quality of their interac-tions 
with their children during feeding in the direction of less 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 479 
contingent and less sensitive exchanges. Nevertheless, no predic-tion 
was found between mothers’ psychological profiles or risks 
and conflictual interactions (in terms of mothers’ controlling prac-tices 
and/or nonemphatic behaviors). Children’s temperaments ap-pear 
to significantly influence mother–infant interactions. Nega-tive 
emotionality in the child predicts difficulties for the mother in 
supporting the autonomous initiatives of the child (by means of re-quests, 
insistent orders, and criticism) while the child demonstrates 
distress and is generally oppositional during feeding. Mothers’ in-volvement 
seems to be unrelated to the quality of interactions with 
their children. No association or prediction was found between 
fathers’ psychological profiles or psychopathological risks and the 
quality of interactions with their children during feeding. More-over, 
in agreement with Kim and Kochanska (2012), our results 
show that the quantity of paternal involvement is related to better 
quality of interactions with their children only in cases where the 
children had higher scores on Social Orientation (a subdimension 
of children’s temperament). 
Although Braungart-Rieker, Courtney, and Garwood (1999) 
suggested that mothers and fathers do not differ in their level of 
sensitiveness and contingency toward their children, nor do they 
show differences in recognizing emotional and behavioral signals 
from children and adjusting to them (Notaro  Volling, 1999), our 
results are in line with those of Shoppe-Sullivan, Brown, Cannon, 
Mangelsdorf, and Sokolowski (2008) and Lamb (2010). According 
to these authors, fathers displayed a poorer ability to recognize 
distress cues shown by their childrenwhen compared with mothers. 
They also found that fathers were less sensitive and attuned to 
their children in the first years of life. It also has been suggested 
that mothers tend to stimulate children with more rhythmical and 
containing gestures and speeches whereas fathers are more prone 
to sudden and unpredictable stimuli and rough-and-tumble play 
(Lamb, 2010). 
Our results show that mothers’ psychopathological risks have 
an influence on the quality of dyadic exchanges with their children 
during feeding, which has not proven true in the case of fathers. 
This result needs further investigation; however, Borke, 
Lamm, Eickhorst, and Keller (2007) stated that fathers are more 
likely to engage in “distal” interactions whereas mothers show 
more “proximal” ones, evenwhen the overall quality of interactions 
was good for both parents. Thus, the difference in the observed 
interactions could be related to a different style of reciprocal com-munication 
between fathers and children, and not to an impaired 
or compromised pattern of behavioral and emotional exchanges. 
Lamb and Lewis (2010) emphasized that these hypotheses must 
be reconsidered once the involvement of the father is controlled, 
as lower attunement could derive from a smaller amount of time 
spent by fathers with their children when compared to mothers, 
who generally interact more with infants in their first years of life 
due to maternity leave. 
We tried to respond to this suggestion, and our findings 
state that father–child interactions during feeding are not im-proved 
by paternal involvement, except in the case of the child’s 
temperamental-specific characteristics (high scores on Social Ori-entation). 
This indicates that fathers are more strictly influenced 
by children’s behaviors rather than the emotional content in the 
context of feeding, so that even when psychopathologically at risk, 
fathers succeed in having moments of good quality with their sons 
and daughters if the moment of feeding is not compromised by 
the oppositional behaviors of the children. It must be stressed that 
this article presents a community sample in which no participants 
had a referred diagnosis that could substantially impair parent– 
infant interactions. The present study, thus, supports previous re-sults 
(Sarkadi, Kristiansson, Oberklaid,  Bremberg, 2008) that 
have stated that father–child interactions are more simply related 
to the child’s behaviors while maternal dyadic exchanges may be 
related to a wide-ranging spectrum of factors related to both the 
child’s and the mother’s characteristics. 
Limitations 
This study has limitations. First, we did not control or investi-gate 
the emotional/adaptive profiles of the children, except regard-ing 
their temperaments. Second, no attachment security/insecurity 
measure was used to assess the quality of attachment between the 
children and their parents. Furthermore, we used report-form tools 
to assess the parents’ psychological profiles and the children’s 
temperaments, although it could have been informative to have an 
objective assessment administered by a professional psychologist. 
The homogeneity of the sample, in terms of cultural, geographi-cal, 
and SES, limits replication of the study in other countries or 
cultures. 
Conclusion and Future Directions 
Nevertheless, we think this article has several strengths. To our 
knowledge, no other study has addressed father–child interac-tions 
during feeding in nonclinical samples. We used widely used 
and validated tools regarding the infants’ temperaments and the 
parents’ psychological profiles. In addition, we chose to use an 
observational and naturalistic method to investigate parent–infant 
interactions during feeding whereas most studies have used report-form 
questionnaires filled out by parents. We intend to further 
our research with a larger sample and to fix the aforementioned 
limitations within a longitudinal study, which should consider sta-bility 
and changes in the specific patterns of interaction between 
mothers, fathers, and their infants over time. 
REFERENCES 
Ammaniti, M., Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., D’Olimpio, F.,  Chatoor, I. 
(2010). Maternal psychopathology and child risk factors in infantile 
anorexia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43(3), 233–240. 
Atzaba-Poria, N., Meiri, G., Millikovsky, M., Barkai, A., Dunaevsky- 
Idan, M.,  Yerushalmi, B. (2010). Father–child and mother–child 
interaction in families with a child feeding disorder: The role of 
paternal involvement. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31(6), 682–698. 
Axia, G. (2002). QUIT. Questionari italiani del temperamento [Italian 
Questionnairs for child’s temperament]. Trento, Italy: Erickson. 
Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Brain gym 3 anys
Brain gym 3 anysBrain gym 3 anys
Brain gym 3 anysCEIPSAUBA
 
Staff Presentation
Staff Presentation Staff Presentation
Staff Presentation caitlino
 
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of deviance
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of devianceIntegrative theory of crime and other forms of deviance
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of devianceshtatspb
 
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος δεκεμβριος 2013
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος   δεκεμβριος 201346 σποριαδες νοεμβριος   δεκεμβριος 2013
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος δεκεμβριος 2013poimenikos
 
Email marketing for charities and non-profits
Email marketing for charities and non-profitsEmail marketing for charities and non-profits
Email marketing for charities and non-profitsMailNinja
 
Presentation1
Presentation1Presentation1
Presentation1Theologi
 
Jet Press 720S technical developments
Jet Press 720S technical developmentsJet Press 720S technical developments
Jet Press 720S technical developmentsFujifilmPrint
 
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscape
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscapeIT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscape
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscapeVietnamBusinessTV
 

Viewers also liked (11)

skydrive_ppt_doc
skydrive_ppt_docskydrive_ppt_doc
skydrive_ppt_doc
 
Brain gym 3 anys
Brain gym 3 anysBrain gym 3 anys
Brain gym 3 anys
 
Staff Presentation
Staff Presentation Staff Presentation
Staff Presentation
 
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of deviance
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of devianceIntegrative theory of crime and other forms of deviance
Integrative theory of crime and other forms of deviance
 
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος δεκεμβριος 2013
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος   δεκεμβριος 201346 σποριαδες νοεμβριος   δεκεμβριος 2013
46 σποριαδες νοεμβριος δεκεμβριος 2013
 
Email marketing for charities and non-profits
Email marketing for charities and non-profitsEmail marketing for charities and non-profits
Email marketing for charities and non-profits
 
Satish.S -Resume.
Satish.S -Resume.Satish.S -Resume.
Satish.S -Resume.
 
Presentation1
Presentation1Presentation1
Presentation1
 
papua
papuapapua
papua
 
Jet Press 720S technical developments
Jet Press 720S technical developmentsJet Press 720S technical developments
Jet Press 720S technical developments
 
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscape
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscapeIT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscape
IT, Media, Marketing - A regultatory lanscape
 

Similar to Mother-infant and father infant interactions

Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Felicia Nicole Ghrist
 
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosiMothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosilucacerniglia
 
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfJIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfAGNESSANTOS19
 
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docx
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docxArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docx
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docxfredharris32
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective studylucacerniglia
 
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)Nighat Shah
 
Writ 109SS Research Paper
Writ 109SS Research PaperWrit 109SS Research Paper
Writ 109SS Research PaperKatharine Ross
 
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docx
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docxEarly Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docx
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docxsagarlesley
 
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...ijtsrd
 
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018Francesca Vescia (she/her)
 
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, Mullins
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, MullinsCPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, Mullins
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, MullinsShawna Scott
 
MizzouResearchPoster (2)
MizzouResearchPoster (2)MizzouResearchPoster (2)
MizzouResearchPoster (2)Sami Kurkowski
 
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docx
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docxSBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docx
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docxJose Colon
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFMarissa Schneider
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...lucacerniglia
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...lucacerniglia
 

Similar to Mother-infant and father infant interactions (20)

Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
 
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosiMothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
 
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfJIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
 
Article Review.docx
Article Review.docxArticle Review.docx
Article Review.docx
 
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docx
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docxArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docx
ArticleEffect of a Paraprofessional Home-Visiting Interven.docx
 
Fpsyg 06-01210
Fpsyg 06-01210Fpsyg 06-01210
Fpsyg 06-01210
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective study
 
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)
Paretalstyles [autosaved] (1)
 
Writ 109SS Research Paper
Writ 109SS Research PaperWrit 109SS Research Paper
Writ 109SS Research Paper
 
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docx
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docxEarly Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docx
Early Head Start Relationships Associationwith Program Outc.docx
 
Cowenprutttart
CowenprutttartCowenprutttart
Cowenprutttart
 
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
Rethinking Peer Apprenticeship Through the Acquisition of Compliance Skills i...
 
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018
Strengthening Families in Drug Treatment Court: Tompkins County, 2014-2018
 
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, Mullins
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, MullinsCPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, Mullins
CPA 2011 Scott, Hakim-Larson, Babb, Mullins
 
MizzouResearchPoster (2)
MizzouResearchPoster (2)MizzouResearchPoster (2)
MizzouResearchPoster (2)
 
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docx
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docxSBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docx
SBPS 6340 Evaluation Section_DRAFT.docx
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
 
Dissertation to print
Dissertation to printDissertation to print
Dissertation to print
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
 

Recently uploaded

Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patna
Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service PatnaLow Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patna
Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patnamakika9823
 
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...Miss joya
 
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...narwatsonia7
 
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Delivery
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on DeliveryCall Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Delivery
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Deliverynehamumbai
 
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...astropune
 
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel room
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel roomLucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel room
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel roomdiscovermytutordmt
 
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...Miss joya
 
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...Miss joya
 
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...Miss joya
 
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safe
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% SafeBangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safe
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safenarwatsonia7
 
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...indiancallgirl4rent
 
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escorts
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore EscortsCall Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escorts
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escortsvidya singh
 
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoy
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night EnjoyCall Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoy
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoybabeytanya
 
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual Needs
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual NeedsBangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual Needs
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual NeedsGfnyt
 
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...astropune
 
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋 9256729539 🚀 Indore Escorts
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋  9256729539 🚀 Indore EscortsVIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋  9256729539 🚀 Indore Escorts
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋 9256729539 🚀 Indore Escortsaditipandeya
 
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Call Girls in Nagpur High Profile
 
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️ 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatore
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️  8250192130 Independent Escort Service CoimbatoreCall Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️  8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatore
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️ 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatorenarwatsonia7
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patna
Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service PatnaLow Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patna
Low Rate Call Girls Patna Anika 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Patna
 
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Tanvi 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call g...
 
Russian Call Girls in Delhi Tanvi ➡️ 9711199012 💋📞 Independent Escort Service...
Russian Call Girls in Delhi Tanvi ➡️ 9711199012 💋📞 Independent Escort Service...Russian Call Girls in Delhi Tanvi ➡️ 9711199012 💋📞 Independent Escort Service...
Russian Call Girls in Delhi Tanvi ➡️ 9711199012 💋📞 Independent Escort Service...
 
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...
VIP Call Girls Tirunelveli Aaradhya 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Tir...
 
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Delivery
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on DeliveryCall Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Delivery
Call Girls Colaba Mumbai ❤️ 9920874524 👈 Cash on Delivery
 
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
♛VVIP Hyderabad Call Girls Chintalkunta🖕7001035870🖕Riya Kappor Top Call Girl ...
 
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel room
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel roomLucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel room
Lucknow Call girls - 8800925952 - 24x7 service with hotel room
 
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...
Low Rate Call Girls Pune Esha 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girl...
 
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...
Russian Call Girls in Pune Riya 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call gi...
 
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...
VIP Call Girls Pune Vrinda 9907093804 Short 1500 Night 6000 Best call girls S...
 
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safe
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% SafeBangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safe
Bangalore Call Girls Majestic 📞 9907093804 High Profile Service 100% Safe
 
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...
(Rocky) Jaipur Call Girl - 9521753030 Escorts Service 50% Off with Cash ON De...
 
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escorts
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore EscortsCall Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escorts
Call Girls Horamavu WhatsApp Number 7001035870 Meeting With Bangalore Escorts
 
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoy
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night EnjoyCall Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoy
Call Girl Number in Panvel Mumbai📲 9833363713 💞 Full Night Enjoy
 
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual Needs
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual NeedsBangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual Needs
Bangalore Call Girl Whatsapp Number 100% Complete Your Sexual Needs
 
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
Best Rate (Hyderabad) Call Girls Jahanuma ⟟ 8250192130 ⟟ High Class Call Girl...
 
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋 9256729539 🚀 Indore Escorts
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋  9256729539 🚀 Indore EscortsVIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋  9256729539 🚀 Indore Escorts
VIP Call Girls Indore Kirti 💚😋 9256729539 🚀 Indore Escorts
 
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
Book Paid Powai Call Girls Mumbai 𖠋 9930245274 𖠋Low Budget Full Independent H...
 
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️ 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatore
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️  8250192130 Independent Escort Service CoimbatoreCall Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️  8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatore
Call Girl Coimbatore Prisha☎️ 8250192130 Independent Escort Service Coimbatore
 
Escort Service Call Girls In Sarita Vihar,, 99530°56974 Delhi NCR
Escort Service Call Girls In Sarita Vihar,, 99530°56974 Delhi NCREscort Service Call Girls In Sarita Vihar,, 99530°56974 Delhi NCR
Escort Service Call Girls In Sarita Vihar,, 99530°56974 Delhi NCR
 

Mother-infant and father infant interactions

  • 1. A R T I C L E MOTHER–CHILD AND FATHER–CHILD INTERACTION WITH THEIR 24-MONTH-OLD CHILDREN DURING FEEDING, CONSIDERING PATERNAL INVOLVEMENT AND THE CHILD’S TEMPERAMENT IN A COMMUNITY SAMPLE LUCA CERNIGLIA International Telematic University Uninettuno SILVIA CIMINO AND GIULIA BALLAROTTO University of Rome, Sapienza ABSTRACT: The article aims to study mother–child and father–child interactions with 24-month-old children during feeding, considering the possible influence of time spent by the parent with the child, the infantile temperament, and the parental psychological profile. The families were recruited from 12 preschools in Italy (N = 77 families). Through an observation of the feeding [Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare (SVIA – Feeding Scale; I. Chatoor et al., 1997; L. Lucarelli et al., 2002)], self-reporting [Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R; L.R. Derogatis, 1994), and report-form questionnaires [Italian Questionnaires on Temperament (QUIT; G. Axia, 2002)], and information provided by the parents about the amount of time spent with their children, results showed that the overall quality of father–child interactions during feeding is lower than that of mother–child interactions. Fathers showed higher psychological symptoms than did mothers. No associations were found between the fathers’ psychopathological risk and the quality of interactions with their children during feeding. Mothers’ psychopathological risks predicted less contingent exchanges interactions with their children during feeding. Children’s temperaments significantly influence mother–child interactions, but no association exists between maternal involvement and the quality of interactions with their children. Paternal involvement predicts a better quality of father–infant interactions when associated with a child’s higher scores on Social Orientation. The quality of parents’ interactions with their children during feeding are impacted by different issues originating from the parent’s psychological profile, the degree of involvement, and from the child’s temperament. RESUMEN: El art´ıculo se propone estudiar las interacciones pap´a-hijo y mam´a-hijo con ni˜nos de 24 meses de edad durante el momento de alimentarse, considerando la posible influencia del tiempo que el progenitor pasa con el ni˜no, el temperamento infantil y el perfil sicol´ogico de los padres. Las familias fueron reclutadas en doce centros prescolares en Italia (N = 77 familias). Por medio de una observaci´on del momento de alimentaci´on (SVIA), el auto-reportaje (SCL-90-R) y cuestionarios en formularios de reporte (QUIT), e informaci´on ofrecida por los padres acerca de la cantidad de tiempo pasada con sus ni˜nos, los resultados mostraron que la calidad general de las interacciones pap´a-hijo durante el momento de alimentarse es m´as baja que las de mam´a-hijo. Los pap´as mostraron m´as altos s´ıntomas sicol´ogicos que las mam´as. No se encontr´o ninguna asociaci´on entre el riesgo sicopatol´ogico de los pap´as y la calidad de las interacciones con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on. Los riesgos sicopatol´ogicos de las madres predijeron menos interacciones de intercambios eventuales con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on. El temperamento de los ni˜nos significativamente influye en las interacciones mam´a-hijo, pero no existe asociaci´on entre el involucramiento materno y la calidad de las interacciones con sus ni˜nos. El involucramiento paterno predice una mejor calidad de interacciones pap´a-infante cuando est´a asociado con los m´as altos puntajes del ni˜no en la Orientaci´on Social. Diferentes asuntos impactan la calidad de las interacciones de los padres con sus ni˜nos durante la alimentaci´on, partiendo del perfil sicol´ogico de los padres, el grado de involucramiento y el temperamento del ni˜no. RE´ SUME´ : Cet article a pour but d’e´tudier les interactions pe`re-enfant et me`re-enfant avec leurs enfants de 24 mois durant l’alimentation, en conside´rant l’influence potentielle du temps pass´e par le parent avec l’enfant, le temp´erament de l’enfant et le profil psychologique parental. Les familles ont ´et´e recrut´ees dans dix cr`eches italiennes (N = 77 familles). Au travers d’une observation de l’alimentation (SVIA), du questionnaire SCL-90-R, des questionnaires sous forme de rapport (QUIT), et les renseignements donn´es par les parents sur la quantit´e de temps pass´e avec leurs enfants, les r´esultats ont montr´e que la qualit´e g´en´erale des interactions p`ere-enfant durant l’alimentation est moins ´elev´ee que celle des interactions m`ere-enfant. Les p`eres ont fait preuve de plus de symptˆomes psychologiques que les m`eres. Aucun lien n’a ´et´e trouv´e entre le risque psychopathologique des p`eres et la qualit´e des interactions avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. Les risques psychopathologiques des m`eres ont pr´edit des ´echanges interactifs moins I, Luca Cerniglia, declare that no author has any conflict of interest. Direct correspondence to: Luca Cerniglia, Corso Vittorio Emanuele II, 39–00100–Rome, Italy; e-mail: l.cerniglia@uninettunouniversity.net. INFANT MENTAL HEALTH JOURNAL, Vol. 35(5), 473–481 (2014) C 2014 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com. DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21466 473
  • 2. 474 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto contingents avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. Le temp´erament des enfants influencent fortement les interactions m`ere-enfant mais aucun lien n’existe entre l’engagement maternel et la qualit´e des interactions avec leurs enfants. L’engagement paternel pr´edit un meilleure qualit´e des interactions p`ere-b´eb´e lorsque li´ees `a des scores plus ´elev´es de l’enfant sur l’Orientation Sociale. Diff´erents probl`emes issus du profil psychologique du parent, du degr´e d’engagement et du temp´erament de l’enfant ont un impact sur la qualit´e des interactions des parents avec leurs enfants durant l’alimentation. ZUSAMMENFASSUNG: Der Artikel zielt darauf ab, Vater-Kind-und Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen bei 24 Monate alten Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung, in Anbetracht des m¨oglichen Einflusses der Zeit, die das Elternteil mit dem Kind verbringt, des kindlichen Temperaments und des psychologischen Profils der Eltern, zu untersuchen. Die Familien wurden aus zw¨olf Vorschulen in Italien (N = 77 Familien) rekrutiert. Anhand einer Beobachtung der F¨utterung (SVIA), Selbst-Berichten (SCL-90-R) und anderen Frageb¨ogen (QUIT) sowie Informationen von den Eltern ¨uber die Menge der Zeit, die sie mit ihren Kindern verbracht haben, zeigten die Ergebnisse, dass die Gesamtqualit¨at der Vater-Kind-Interaktionen w¨ahrend der F¨utterung niedriger ist als die der Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen. V¨ater zeigten h¨ohere psychische Symptome als M¨utter. Es wurden keine Zusammenh¨ange zwischen dem psychopathologischen Risiko der V¨ater und der Qualit¨at der Interaktion mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung gefunden. Die psychopathologischen Risiken der M¨utter sagten weniger zuf¨allige Austauschsinteraktionen mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung vorher. Die Temperamente der Kinder beeinflussen die Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen signifikant, es besteht jedoch kein Zusammenhang zwischen m¨utterlichem Engagement und der Interaktionsqualit¨at mit ihren Kindern. V¨aterliches Engagement sagt eine bessere Qualit¨at der Vater-Kind-Interaktionen vorher, wenn sie mit h¨oheren Werten f¨ur die soziale Orientierung des Kindes assoziiert wird. Die Interaktionsqualit¨at der Eltern mit ihren Kindern w¨ahrend der F¨utterung wird von unterschiedlichen Themen beeinflusst: dem psychologischen Profil der Eltern, dem Grad des Engagements und vom Temperament des Kindes. ABSTRACT: :
  • 3. 24 !#12$%(N = 77 )(SVIA)(SCL-90-R)(QUIT)
  • 4. '()% *+( )%+, -. %*/-. contingent exchanges interactions*) %*( Social Orientation(-0# 1+( ')232* /4( * * * It has been widely recognized that the quality of mother– infant interaction during a child’s first years of life has a profound influence on his or her psychological welfare and on the possi-ble onset of psychopathologies in infants (Ramchandani et al., 2013). Nevertheless, family and ecological theories (Bronfen-brenner, 1979; Minuchin, 1974) suggest that mother–infant and father–infant dyads are interconnected. Growing evidence has suggested that positive father–infant interactions can directly im-pact children’s cognitive and socioemotional development (Brown, McBride, Bost, Shin, 2011) (e.g., engaging with their children, monitoring them, and setting limits and rules), and also indirectly impact such development (e.g., influencing mother–infant inter-actions, supporting mothers’ decisions and behaviors concerning their sons and daughters, and/or providing economic support to the family, which may contribute to the rearing and emotional health of the children) (Atzaba-Poria et al., 2010; Carlson, 2006; Cimino, Cerniglia, Paciello, Sinesi, 2012). Lamb’s (2010) theoretical framework has conceptualized that fathers are unique kinds of in-teractional partners for their children, and are rather distinct from mothers; the paternal fashion of interactions differ from the ma-ternal fashion, being more characterized by physical contact and rough-and-tumble play, which seems to have a specific role in pro-moting a child’s emotional-regulation processes (Feldman, 2003). Consequently, in more recent years, research has concentrated on both mothers and fathers, specifically on the role of fathers as protectors, and on risk factors for the development of children’s psychological difficulties while considering triadic interactions to verify whether there are differences in parenting quality and chil-dren’s interactive behaviors with mothers and fathers in the dyadic context versus those in the triadic context (Di Folco Zavattini, 2014; Kwon, Jeon, Lewsader, Elicker, 2012; Lamb Lewis, 2007). While it has been proposed that a combination of maternal and paternal psychopathologies may create a style of coparenting dominated by negative interactive cycles with children during play and feeding (Pinquart Teubert, 2010), the specific impact that psychopathological symptoms in fathers might have on a child’s mental health has been only recently addressed, mostly investigat-ing the construct of involvement in community samples (Lamb, 2010). Highly involved fathers (in terms of time spent with their children) seem likely to have more empathic sons and daughters who show a more internal locus of control (Cerniglia, Cimino, Bal-larotto, Monniello, 2014; Pleck, 2010). With specific regard to the context of feeding, international research has mainly focused on mother–infant interactions because mothers are traditionally thought to take primary responsibility for feeding their children (Blissett, Meyer, Haycraft, 2006) and are usually considered Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 5. Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 475 their primary caregiver (Campbell et al., 2010; Patrick, Nicklas, Hughes, Morales, 2005). However, parents’ gender roles and re-sponsibilities have changed in the last few decades, above all as a result of increasing employment outside the home for mothers with young children. Research has been considering these changes, and recently has focused on fathers’ psychological characteristics and practices during feeding interactions with their children (Khand-pur, Blaine, Fisher, Davison, 2014). Although Blissett and Hay-craft (2011) found no significant differences in feeding practices between mothers and fathers, in the same study fathers reported feeling personally responsible for organising meals at least half of the time, and deciding what kinds of foods their children should eat (Mallan et al., 2013). Hendy, Williams, Camise, Eckman, and Hedemann (2009) found that fathers were less likely to limit their children in consuming snacks and were less focused on letting them eat a variety of foods such as fruits and vegetables. Some studies have shown that fathers are more likely than are mothers to encourage children to eat using prompts (verbal or physical), reasoning, pressure, and incentives during feeding (Haycraft Blissett, 2008; Orrell-Valente et al., 2007). Nevertheless, threats to withdraw play privileges or access to food were not usually used by fathers, with no psychopathological risk (Haycraft Blissett, 2012; Tschann et al., 2013). Regarding the context of feeding, the ages of children have been linked to diverse father–child in-teractions. Fathers of older children reported lower use of food for emotional regulation than did fathers of younger children, and more restriction of food for older children if they were concerned about the child’s weight (Musher-Eizenman, de Lauzon-Guillain, Holub, Leporc, Charles, 2009). Furthermore, parent–child inter-actions during feeding seem to be less problematic with toddlers than with infants, although this result seems to have been con-firmed only in low-risk samples whereas high-risk samples show a different trend. Psychopathological risk in mothers and in fathers as well as in low-income parents seems to be related to maladaptive parent–child interactions more frequently during toddlerhood than during childhood (Horodynski Arndt, 2005). In fact, nonresponsive parenting characterizes families with psychopathological parents who frequently fail to respond to new skills and signals shown by toddlers, such as self-feeding, the use of utensils, and the use of language to request food and help (Birch, Fisher, Davison, 2003). In clinical samples, paternal pressure to eat has been corre-lated with bulimia and restriction symptoms in fathers (Blissett et al., 2006) whereas eating disorders and depression in mothers proved to be the most significant psychopathology correlated with maladaptive patterns of interaction between mothers and infants (Cimino, Cerniglia, Paciello 2014; Watkins, Cooper, Lask, 2012). Depressed mothers seem to display less positive engage-ment with their infants during feeding interactions and to show anxiety, anger, and intrusivity, having difficulties with empathi-cally recognizing and regulating the child’s affective states during mealtimes. Ammaniti, Lucarelli, Cimino, D’Olimpio, and Chatoor (2010) showed that these parental symptoms may foster feeding disorders in children. While earlier studies have aimed to consider the amount of time spent by fathers with their children as an indicator of in-volvement, more recent studies seem to have underlined the im-portance of the actual quality of interactions to specifically un-derstand why and how paternal involvement should be expected to have a positive effect on child development (Stueve Pleck, 2001). Lamb (2013) stressed the role of engagement accessibil-ity and sensitivity as well as the significance of paternal warmth and support during interactions. Attachment theory proposes that fathers’ involvement promotes child development because it fos-ters secure attachment, which is linked to positive child outcomes (Brown, McBride, Shin, Bost, 2007). Nevertheless, little at-tention has been given in attachment literature to the quality of father–child interactions during feeding and to their links with adaptive or maladaptive outcomes in the emotional and behavioral development of children, which in turn may be bidirectionally linked to security or insecurity of attachment in infants and in their parents. Some research has suggested that temperamentally easy chil-dren are more likely to be involved in good-quality interactions with their fathers (Mehall, Spinrad, Eisenberg, Gaertner, 2009). Children who show high levels of irritability, negative moods, and irregular behavioral and biological patterns seem to be involved in lower quality interactions with their mothers and fathers. On the other hand, Frodi et al. (1982) found that fathers were more involved with difficult sons (but not with difficult daughters), as recently confirmed by Leaper (2002) and Cabrera, Fagan, Wight, and Schadler (2011). To capture the bidirectional effect of child–father interaction, several studies have chosen to use observational methods, which take into consideration verbal and nonverbal contexts of behavioral dynamics (Hughes et al., 2010).Many of these studies have focused on play routines (both involving mothers and fathers) while only some of them have observed feeding interactions (Blissett, 2011). Yet, several studies have suggested that feeding interactions be-tween parents and their children constitute an important context in which infants and toddlers may learn to recognize and make sense of verbal and nonverbal communications and signals, thus build-ing a basis for emotional bonds and attachment quality that in turn forms essential roots for healthy emotional/behavioral functioning (Black Aboud, 2011). Based on these theoretical and empirical premises, which include Lamb’s (2010) conceptual framework, and that no other study to our knowledge has addressed father–toddler interactions during feeding in nonclinical samples, we chose to study father– infant and mother–infant interactions during feeding at 24 months of age, considering the possible influence of time spent by the parent with the child, the infantile temperament, and the parental psychological profile. In particular, this study has the following objectives: • to verify whether the quality of mother–child and father– child interactions during feeding differ from each other; • to assess parental psychopathological risks; and Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 6. 476 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto • to verify whether parental psychopathological risks affect the quality of parent–child interactions during feeding while considering child temperament and parental involvement. METHOD Participants Seventy-seven families were recruited from 12 preschools in Italy (N = 77). The children (45 girls, 32 boys) ranged from 23 to 26 months of age (SD = 0.8), and their parents’ mean ages were 34.5 (SD = 1.2) years for the fathers and 32.3 (SD = 0.9) years for the mothers. Eighty-nine per cent of the children were firstborn, and 73% of them had been breast-fed in their first year of life. Ninety-one per cent of the couples were married, and all parents were of middle-class socioeconomic status (SES; Hollingshead, 1975). Ninety-four percent of the parents cohabited, three percent of the parents who cohabited were not married, and 82% of the families had double income. All partners of the mothers recruited for the present study were biological fathers of the children, and all parents were Caucasian. All parents included in the study, both mothers and fathers, were involved in caregiving practices with their children on a daily basis [average time spent weekly by the mothers with their children: 76.1 hr (SD = 2.3); average time spent weekly by the fathers with their children: 38.6 hr (SD = 4.1)]. Procedure Sample selection criteria were: (a) age of the children ranging between 23 and 26 months, (b) no reported mental or physical problems or disorders in either the parents or the children, and (c) both parents reporting frequently feeding the child without the presence of the other parent. A group of trained psychologists con-tacted 12 preschools in Central Italy and presented the research rationale and objective. Families who agreed to be recruited for the study filled out an informed consent form. The order of the admin-istration of the measures (described later) was randomly selected. All parent–child pairs in the sample were observed at their homes in 20-min video-recordings during lunch at midday; the feeding interactions were part of one regular meal observed separately (on 2 different days) for mother–child and father–child interactions. The videos were recorded by psychologists who were specifically trained in the use of this observational tool, and were coded by two trained independent raters who watched the videos and scored them on the basis of the manual (Lucarelli et al., 2002) using both a paper–pencil system and a coding software program designed for the computation of scores on each subscale. The whole observation was scheduled on the basis of a validated procedure for the Ital-ian population [Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare (SVIA) – Observational Scale for Mother-Infant Interaction dur-ing Feeding; Chatoor et al., 1997; Lucarelli et al., 2002; described later] and in connection with several previous studies (Ammaniti et al., 2010). This tool was chosen over others because it is the only procedure that specifically assesses parent–child interactions during feeding that are validated for the Italian population. Two instruments were given to each parent to be completed at their home and independently: a self-report measure for the assessment of their psychological symptom status, the Symptom Checklist- 90-Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1994), which was filled out independently by each parent, and a report-form questionnaire to evaluate their child’s temperament: the Italian Questionnaires on Temperament (QUIT; Axia, 2002). Mothers and fathers indepen-dently filled out this questionnaire, but they obtained an interrater agreement of 89%. Consequently, we used a mean score. More-over, each parent filled out a form asking him or her to report the average number of hours spent with the child during the week. Measures Assessment of parents’ psychological symptom statuses. The SCL-90-R is a 90-item self-report symptom inventory designed to measure psychological symptoms and psychological distress (Derogatis, 1994). It is scored and interpreted in terms of nine primary subscales (Somatisation, Obsessive-Compulsivity, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Hostility, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychoticism) and three Global Indices of Distress (Global Severity Index, Positive Symptom Distress Index, and Positive Symptom Total). The SCL-90-R is rated on a Likert scale of 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) and asks participants to report if they have suffered in the past week from: Headaches (Somatisation scale), Trouble remembering things (Obsessive-Compulsivity scale), Feeling critical of others (Interpersonal Sensitivity scale), Blaming oneself for things (De-pression scale), Feeling fearful (Anxiety scale), Feeling watched or talked about by others (Paranoid Ideation scale), and The idea that something is wrong with one’s mind (Psychoticism scale). The SCL-90-R has been shown to have good internal coher-ence (α = 0.70–0.96) in adolescents and adults (Italian validated version: Prunas, Sarno, Preti, Madeddu, Perugini, 2012). Assessment of child temperament. Participants’ answers on the QUIT (Axia, 2002) are rated on a Likert scale of 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always). This questionnairewas validated for an Italian sample to measure child temperament from the first month after birth to 11 years of age, within four age groups: 1 to 12 months, 13 to 36 months, 3 to 6 years, and 7 to 11 years. It is given to the parents and investigates six dimensions: the level of motor activity (e.g., The child grasps objects with rapid movements), attentional capacity (e.g., The child immediately turns his/her gaze towards the person by whom he or she is called), inhibition to novelty and availability to socialization (e.g., the child observes strangers for a long time), positive emotionality (e.g., the child maintains a smiling face when playing), and negative emotionality (e.g., the child cries for a long time when contradicted). The questionnaire for the age group 1 to 12 months showed good internal consistency (Cronbach αs = .59–.71), and the values of the Pearson correlations between the scales of the questionnaire completed separately by the mother Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 7. Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 477 and father, ranging between .32 and .62, show that both give a very similar assessment of the child (Axia, 2002). Assessment of parent–child feeding interactions. The Italian ver-sion (SVIA – Feeding Scale; Chatoor et al., 1997; Lucarelli et al., 2002) of the observational scale has 41 items rated on a Likert scale of 0 (confirm, none) to 3 (confirm, many) and four subscales coding dyadic mother–child and father–child interactions sepa-rately: Affective State of the Parent (e.g., the parent appears sad during feeding), Interactional Conflict (e.g., the parent forces food into the child’s mouth), Food Refusal Behaviors of the Child (e.g., the child refuses to open his/her mouth), and Affective State of the Dyad (e.g., the parent and the child show joy during feeding). Higher scores on the Affective State of the Parent refer to greater difficulties of the caregiver in showing positive affects and to a higher frequency of negative affects such as sadness or distress. The subscale Interactional Conflict evaluates both the presence and the intensity of conflictual exchanges within the dyad (e.g., the parent directs the meal according to his or her own emotions and intentions rather than following the signals from the child). The subscale Food Refusal Behaviors of the Child explores the behavioral and emotional characteristics of the feeding patterns of the child (e.g., being easily distracted, or showing opposition or negativity). Higher scores on the Affective State of the Dyad refer to the difficulties of the caregiver in supporting autonomous initiatives of the child (by means of requests, insistent orders, and criticism) while the child demonstrates distress and is generally oppositional. In the Italian version that was validated for mother– infant interactions (Lucarelli et al., 2002), the discriminant analysis showed correct group classification ranging from 82 to 92%, and construct validity for the tool has been proven. Interrater reliability ranged between 0.82 and 0.92 (intraclass correlation coefficients). DATA ANALYSES A preliminary screening of the data showed few data missing for each instrument (4% for each instrument). Missing data were cor-rected using multiple imputation in SPSS software (Version 18.0). To examine the quality of the interactional patterns between fa-thers and children and mothers and children during feeding, we carried out multivariate analyses of variances (MANOVAs) on the SVIA dimensions (to respond to Objective a). In all MANOVAs, univariate analyses were then conducted on significant effects, and the Duncan test (Bonferroni correction) was used for contrasts. To respond to Objective b, we investigated the maternal and paternal psychological status through a series of MANOVAs that were car-ried out on the scores (transformed in their square root whenever the distribution deviated from normality) obtained from the SCL- 90-R (the Global Severity Index and, subsequently, the subscales). In MANOVAs regarding the SVIA procedure and the SCL-90-R questionnaires, we considered the group of mothers and the group of fathers as criterion variables while mothers’ and fathers’ scores on the measures constituted dependent variables. As a final step (to respond to Objective c), two hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to investigate the influence of specific SCL-90-R subscales, temperament subscales, and parental involvement on the relational dimensions of the Feeding Scale in mother–infant and father–infant interactions. In all the analyses we conducted, the child’s gender showed no significant effect on the variables. All analyses were performed with SPSS software (Version 18.0). RESULTS Quality of Mother–Child and Father–Child Interactions During Feeding MANOVA analyses of the observational scale during feeding showed that fathers’ scores were significantly higher than were mothers’ scores in all four subscales, p .01 (see Table 1). No participant exceeded the cutoffs for the clinical population of the Italian version of the SVIA (Lucarelli et al., 2002). Parental Psychopathological Risk Assessment With respect to the assessment of mothers’ and fathers’ psycho-logical symptoms, the analyses showed that fathers scored higher than did mothers on the Global Severity Index and in all SCL-90- R subscales, p .01 (see Table 2). Furthermore, 3 mothers and 4 fathers obtained scores higher than the cutoffs of the clinical range (Prunas et al., 2012). Quality of Parent–Child Interactions During Feeding Considering Parental Psychopathological Risk, Child Temperament, and Parental Involvement Two regression analyses were conducted separately for mothers and fathers to investigate the influence of all nine SCL-90-R sub-scales, all five temperament subscales, and parental involvement (in terms of hours spent by the parent with the child) on all four of the relational dimensions of the SVIA in mother–infant and father–infant interactions, which showed higher maternal scores on Phobic Anxiety predicting mothers’ Affective State of the Par-ent, p .05, in the direction of more negative affects. Higher children’s Negative Emotionality scores in the QUIT, p .001, predicted the Affective State of the Dyad in the direction of more maladaptive mother–infant interactions during feeding. Maternal psychological symptoms and involvement of the mother (in terms of time spent with the child weekly) did not predict the quality of mother–infant interactions during feeding. Regarding fathers, the regression analyses showed the only significant results: Higher paternal involvement (in terms of the number of hours spent with the child) predicts lower scores (i.e., a better quality of father–child interactions) on three of the sub-scales of the SVIA only when considered in association with higher children’s scores on Social Orientation (in particular, In-teractional Conflict, p .05; Food Refusal of the Child, p .05; and Affective State of the Dyad, p .05). No association or prediction was found between fathers’ psychopathological risks Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 8. 478 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto TABLE 1. Means (SDs), F, and P Values of Mothers’ and Fathers’ Scores on SVIA Subscales Mothers Fathers Fisher F Test p Affective State of the Parent 5.27 (3.33) 12.46 (5.35) F(1, 153) = 100,29 .001 Interactional Conflict 4.64 (3.85) 11.29 (6.2) F(1, 153) = 63,91 .001 Food Refusal of the Child 2.52 (1.94) 6.42 (3.36) F(1, 153) = 77,86 .001 Affective State of the Dyad 2.79 (2.25) 6.55 (4.27) F(1, 153) = 46,61 .001 SVIA = Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare. TABLE 2. Means (SDs) F, and P Values of Mothers’ and Fathers’ Scores on SCL-90-R Subscales Mothers Fathers Fisher F Test p Somatization 0.15 (0.11) 0.41 (0.44) F(1, 153) = 24,93 .001 Obsessive-Compulsive 0.11 (0.11) 0.71 (1.08) F(1, 153) = 23,7 .001 Interpersonal Sensitivity 0.1 (0.1) 0.49 (0.61) F(1, 153) = 30,45 .001 Depression 0.12 (0.11) 0.41 (0.42) F(1, 153) = 34,49 .001 Anxiety 0.1 (0.12) 0.75 (1.02) F(1, 153) = 30,02 .001 Hostility 0.1 (0.14) 0.41 (0.40) F(1, 153) = 41,98 .001 Phobic Anxiety 0.12 (0.13) 0.48 (0.5) F(1, 153) = 36,98 .001 Paranoid Ideation 0.07 (0.11) 0.41 (0.46) F(1, 153) = 39,67 .001 Psycoticism 0.14 (0.12) 0.44 (0.4) F(1, 153) = 39,49 .001 Global Severity Index 0.11 (0.05) 0.5 (0.54) F(1, 153) = 39,14 .001 SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-90-Revised. TABLE 3. Results and Values of the Regression Analyses SVIA QUIT Negative Emotionality SCL-90-R Phobic Anxiety Mother R2 β t p R2 β t p Affective State n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. .239 .365 2.232 .029∗ Affective State of the Dyad .001 .342 2.749 .001∗∗ n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Involvement Father R2 β t p Interactional Conflict • Social Orientation .121 .348 3.214 .002∗ Food Refusal of the Child • Social Orientation .062 .249 2.227 .029∗ Affective State of the Dyad • Social Orientation .112 .334 3.073 .003∗ Note. The subscales that are not shown in this table are not statistically significant. n.s. = not significant; SVIA = Scala di Valutazione dell’Interazione Alimentare; QUIT = [Italian Questionnaires on Temperament Questionnaire; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-90-Revised; • = Association with. ∗p .05. ∗∗p .01. and the quality of interactions with their children during feeding. The results and values of the regression analyses are shown in Table 3. DISCUSSION This study aimed to examine specific characteristics of mother– infant and father–infant interactions during feeding, taking into account the possible influence of parental psychological profiles, children’s temperaments, and parental involvement. To assess the quality of parent–child interactions during feed-ing, we chose to use an observational procedure (SVIA). Our re-sults indicated that mothers’ and fathers’ interactions with their children differ significantly. In particular, father–child interactions are significantly more maladaptive. Despite this, no participant (mothers or fathers) exceeded the clinical cutoffs in the Italian pop-ulation. To verify the possible weight of parental psychopathologi-cal risk, child temperament, and parental involvement on the qual-ity of parent–child interactions during feeding (assessed through the SVIA), we chose to use a self-report questionnaire (for parental psychopathological risk; SCL-90-R), a report-form measure (for child’s temperament; QUIT), and information provided by parents about the time spent with their children during the week (parental involvement). We found that mothers’ psychopathological risks (viz., phobic anxiety) predict the general quality of their interac-tions with their children during feeding in the direction of less Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 9. Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 479 contingent and less sensitive exchanges. Nevertheless, no predic-tion was found between mothers’ psychological profiles or risks and conflictual interactions (in terms of mothers’ controlling prac-tices and/or nonemphatic behaviors). Children’s temperaments ap-pear to significantly influence mother–infant interactions. Nega-tive emotionality in the child predicts difficulties for the mother in supporting the autonomous initiatives of the child (by means of re-quests, insistent orders, and criticism) while the child demonstrates distress and is generally oppositional during feeding. Mothers’ in-volvement seems to be unrelated to the quality of interactions with their children. No association or prediction was found between fathers’ psychological profiles or psychopathological risks and the quality of interactions with their children during feeding. More-over, in agreement with Kim and Kochanska (2012), our results show that the quantity of paternal involvement is related to better quality of interactions with their children only in cases where the children had higher scores on Social Orientation (a subdimension of children’s temperament). Although Braungart-Rieker, Courtney, and Garwood (1999) suggested that mothers and fathers do not differ in their level of sensitiveness and contingency toward their children, nor do they show differences in recognizing emotional and behavioral signals from children and adjusting to them (Notaro Volling, 1999), our results are in line with those of Shoppe-Sullivan, Brown, Cannon, Mangelsdorf, and Sokolowski (2008) and Lamb (2010). According to these authors, fathers displayed a poorer ability to recognize distress cues shown by their childrenwhen compared with mothers. They also found that fathers were less sensitive and attuned to their children in the first years of life. It also has been suggested that mothers tend to stimulate children with more rhythmical and containing gestures and speeches whereas fathers are more prone to sudden and unpredictable stimuli and rough-and-tumble play (Lamb, 2010). Our results show that mothers’ psychopathological risks have an influence on the quality of dyadic exchanges with their children during feeding, which has not proven true in the case of fathers. This result needs further investigation; however, Borke, Lamm, Eickhorst, and Keller (2007) stated that fathers are more likely to engage in “distal” interactions whereas mothers show more “proximal” ones, evenwhen the overall quality of interactions was good for both parents. Thus, the difference in the observed interactions could be related to a different style of reciprocal com-munication between fathers and children, and not to an impaired or compromised pattern of behavioral and emotional exchanges. Lamb and Lewis (2010) emphasized that these hypotheses must be reconsidered once the involvement of the father is controlled, as lower attunement could derive from a smaller amount of time spent by fathers with their children when compared to mothers, who generally interact more with infants in their first years of life due to maternity leave. We tried to respond to this suggestion, and our findings state that father–child interactions during feeding are not im-proved by paternal involvement, except in the case of the child’s temperamental-specific characteristics (high scores on Social Ori-entation). This indicates that fathers are more strictly influenced by children’s behaviors rather than the emotional content in the context of feeding, so that even when psychopathologically at risk, fathers succeed in having moments of good quality with their sons and daughters if the moment of feeding is not compromised by the oppositional behaviors of the children. It must be stressed that this article presents a community sample in which no participants had a referred diagnosis that could substantially impair parent– infant interactions. The present study, thus, supports previous re-sults (Sarkadi, Kristiansson, Oberklaid, Bremberg, 2008) that have stated that father–child interactions are more simply related to the child’s behaviors while maternal dyadic exchanges may be related to a wide-ranging spectrum of factors related to both the child’s and the mother’s characteristics. Limitations This study has limitations. First, we did not control or investi-gate the emotional/adaptive profiles of the children, except regard-ing their temperaments. Second, no attachment security/insecurity measure was used to assess the quality of attachment between the children and their parents. Furthermore, we used report-form tools to assess the parents’ psychological profiles and the children’s temperaments, although it could have been informative to have an objective assessment administered by a professional psychologist. The homogeneity of the sample, in terms of cultural, geographi-cal, and SES, limits replication of the study in other countries or cultures. Conclusion and Future Directions Nevertheless, we think this article has several strengths. To our knowledge, no other study has addressed father–child interac-tions during feeding in nonclinical samples. We used widely used and validated tools regarding the infants’ temperaments and the parents’ psychological profiles. In addition, we chose to use an observational and naturalistic method to investigate parent–infant interactions during feeding whereas most studies have used report-form questionnaires filled out by parents. We intend to further our research with a larger sample and to fix the aforementioned limitations within a longitudinal study, which should consider sta-bility and changes in the specific patterns of interaction between mothers, fathers, and their infants over time. REFERENCES Ammaniti, M., Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., D’Olimpio, F., Chatoor, I. (2010). Maternal psychopathology and child risk factors in infantile anorexia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43(3), 233–240. Atzaba-Poria, N., Meiri, G., Millikovsky, M., Barkai, A., Dunaevsky- Idan, M., Yerushalmi, B. (2010). Father–child and mother–child interaction in families with a child feeding disorder: The role of paternal involvement. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31(6), 682–698. Axia, G. (2002). QUIT. Questionari italiani del temperamento [Italian Questionnairs for child’s temperament]. Trento, Italy: Erickson. Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 10. 480 • L. Cerniglia, S. Cimino, and G. Ballarotto Birch, L.L., Fisher, J.O., Davison, K.K. (2003). Learning to overeat: Maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 78(2), 215–220. Black,M.M., Aboud, F.E. (2011). Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. Journal of Nutrition, 141(3), 490–494. doi:10.3945/jn.110.129973 Blissett, J. (2011). Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite, 57, 826–831. Blissett, J., Haycraft, E. (2011). Parental eating disorder symptoms and observations of mealtime interactions with children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 70(4), 368–371. Blissett, J., Meyer, C., Haycraft, E. (2006). Maternal and paternal con-trolling feeding practices with male and female children. Appetite, 47(2), 212–219. Borke, J., Lamm, B., Eickhorst, A., Keller, H. (2007). Father-infant interaction, paternal ideas about early child care, and their conse-quences for the development of children’s self-recognition. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 168(4), 365–379. Braungart-Rieker, J.M., Courtney, S., Garwood, M.M. (1999).Mother– and father–infant attachment: Families in context. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 535–555. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Exper-iments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Brown, G.L., McBride, B.A., Bost, K.K., Shin, N. (2011). Parental in-volvement, child temperament, and parents’ work hours: Differential relations for mothers and fathers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 313–322. Brown, G.L., McBride, B.A., Shin, N., Bost, K.K. (2007). Parenting predictors of father-child attachment security: Interactive effects of father involvement and fathering quality. Fathering, 5, 197–219. Cabrera, N.J., Fagan, J., Wight, V., Schadler, C. (2011). Influence of mother, father, and child risk on parenting and children’s cognitive and social behaviors. Child Development, 82(6), 1985–2005. Campbell, K., Andrianopoulos, N., Hesketh, K., Ball, K., Crawford, D., Brennan, L. et al. (2010). Parental use of restrictive feeding practices and child BMI z-score. A 3-year prospective cohort study. Appetite, 55, 84–8. Carlson, M. (2006). Family structure, father involvement, and adolescent behavioral outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 137–154. Cerniglia, L., Cimino, S., Ballarotto, G., Monniello, G. (2014). Parental loss during childhood and outcomes on adolescents’ psy-chological profiles: A longitudinal study. Current Psychology. doi:10.1007/s12144-014-9228-3 Chatoor, I., Getson, P.,Menvielle, E., Brasseaux, C.,O’Donnell, R.,Rivera Y, Mrazek, D.A. (1997). A feeding scale for research and clinical practice to assess mother-infant interactions in the first three years of life. Infant Mental Health Journal, 18(1), 76–91. Cimino, S., Cerniglia, L., Paciello, M. (2014). Mothers with depres-sion, anxiety or eating disorders: Outcomes on their children and the role of paternal psychological profiles. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. doi:10.1007/s10578-014-0462-6 Cimino, S., Cerniglia, L., Paciello, M., Sinesi, S. (2012). A six-year prospective study on children of mothers with eating disorders: The role of paternal psychological profiles. European Eating Disorders Review, 21(3), 238–246. Derogatis, L.R. (1994). SCL-90-R Symptom Checklist-90-R administra-tion, scoring and procedures manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Di Folco, S., Zavattini, G.C. (2014). Father-child attachment relation-ship: A review of the literature. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 41, 1 (in press). Feldman, R. (2003). Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 1–23. Frodi, A., Lamb, M., Frodi, M., Hwang, P., Forsstrom, B., Corry, T. (1982). Stability and change in parental attitudes following an infant’s birth into traditional and nontraditonal Swedish families. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 23, 53–62. Haycraft, E., Blissett, J. (2008).Maternal and paternal controlling feed-ing practices: Reliability and relationships with BMI. Obesity, 16, 1552–1558. doi:10.1038/oby.2008.238 Haycraft, E., Blissett, J. (2012). Predictors of paternal and maternal controlling feeding practices with 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 44(5), 390–397. Hendy, H.M., Williams, K.E., Camise, T.S., Eckman, N., Hedemann, A. (2009). The Parent Mealtime Action Scale (PMAS). Development and association with children’s diet and weight. Appetite, 52(2), 328. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.003 Hollingshead,A.B. (1975). Four factors index of social status. NewHaven, CT: Yale University Department of Sociology. Horodynski, A., Arndt, M.J. (2005). “Eating-together” mealtimes with African-American fathers and their toodlers. Applied Nursing Re-search, 18(2), 106–109. Hughes, S.O., Frankel, L.A., Beltran, A., Hodges, E., Hoerr, S., Lu-meng, J. et al. (2010). Food parenting measurement issues: Work-ing Group Consensus Report. Childhood Obesity, 9(1), S95–S102. doi:10.1089/chi.2013.0032 Khandpur,N., Blaine, R.E., Fisher, J.O.,Davison, K.K. (2014). Fathers’ child feeding practices: A review of the evidence. Appetite, 78, 110– 121. Kim, S., Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83(4), 1275–1289. Kwon, K.A., Jeon, H.J., Lewsader, J.T., Elicker, J. (2012). Mothers’ and fathers’ parenting quality and toddlers’ interactive behaviours in dyadic and triadic family contexts. Infant and Child Development, 21, 356–373. doi:10.1002/icd.1746 Lamb, M.E. (2010). The role of the father in child development (5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Lamb, M.E. (Ed.). (2013). The father’s role: Cross cultural perspectives. London: Routledge. Lamb, M.E., Lewis, C. (2007). Understanding fatherhood. A review of recent research. New York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
  • 11. Parent–Child Interaction During Feeding • 481 Lamb, M.E., Lewis, C. (2010). The development and significance of father-child relationships in two-parent families. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (5th ed., pp. 94–153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Leaper, C. (2002). Parenting girls and boys. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 189–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., Perucchini, P., Speranza, A.M., Ammaniti, M., Ercolani, A.P. (2002). I disturbi alimentari nella prima infanzia: Validazione di uno strumento osservativo dell’interazione madre-bambino [Feeding disorders in infancy; validation of an observational tool of mother-infant interaction]. Infanzia e Adolescenza, 1, 113– 124. Mallan, K.M., Nothard, M., Thorpe, K., Nicholson, J.M., Wilson, A., Scuffham, P.A., Daniels, L.A. (2013). The role of fathers in child feeding: Perceived responsibility and predictors of partic-ipation. Child: Care, Health and Development. doi:10.1111/cch. 12088 Mehall, K.G., Spinrad, T.L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B.M. (2009). Examining the relations of infant temperament and couples’ marital satisfaction to mother and father involvement: A longitudinal study. Fathering, 7, 23–48. Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. Cambridge, MA: Har-vard University Press. Musher-Eizenman, D., de Lauzon-Guillain, B., Holub, S., Leporc, E., Charles, M. (2009). Child and parent characteristics related to parental feeding practices. A cross-cultural examination in the US and France. Appetite, 52(1), 89–95. Notaro, P.C., Volling, B.L. (1999). Parental responsiveness and infant– parent attachment: A replication study with fathers and mothers. Infant Behavior Development, 22, 345–352. Orrell-Valente, J.K., Hill, L.G., Brechwald, W.A., Dodge, K.A., Pettit, G.S., Bates, J.E. (2007). “Just three more bites”: An observational analysis of parents’ socialization of children’s eating at mealtime. Appetite, 48(1), 37–45. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.06.006 Patrick, H., Nicklas, T.A., Hughes, S.O., Morales, M. (2005). The benefits of authoritative feeding style: Caregiver feeding styles and children’s food consumption patterns. Appetite, 44(2), 243–250. Pinquart, M., Teubert, D. (2010). A meta-analytic study of couple interventions during the transition to parenthood. Family Relations, 59, 221–231. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00597.x Pleck, J. (2010).Why could father involvement benefit children? Theoret-ical perspectives. Applied Developmental Science, 11(4), 196–202. doi:10.1080/10888690701762068 Prunas, A., Sarno, I., Preti, E., Madeddu, F., Perugini, M. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the SCL-90-R: A study of a large community sample. European Psychiatry, 27(8), 591–597. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.12.006 Ramchandani, P.G., Domoney, J., Sethna, V., Lamprini, P., Vlachos, H., Murray L. (2013). Do early father–infant interactions predict the onset of externalising behaviours in young children? Findings from a longitudinal cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-atry, 54(1), 56–64. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02583 Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., Bremberg, S. (2008). Fa-thers’ involvement and children’s developmental outcomes: A sys-tematic review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatrica, 97(2), 153– 158. Schoppe-Sullivan, S.J., Brown, G.L., Cannon, E.A.,Mangelsdorf, S.C., Sokolowski,M.S. (2008).Maternal gatekeeping, coparenting quality, and fathering behavior in families with infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(3), 389–398. Stueve, J.L., Pleck, J.H. (2001). “Parenting voice”: Solo parent identity and co-parent identity in married parents’ narratives of meaningful parenting experiences. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 691–708. Tschann, J.M., Gregorich, S.E., Penilla, C., Pasch, L.A., de Groat, C.L., Flores, E. et al. (2013). Parental feeding practices in Mexican American families: Initial test of an expanded measure. Interna-tional Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 10(6). doi:10.1186/1479-5868-10-6 Watkins, B., Cooper, P.J., Lask, B. (2012). History of eating disorder in mothers of children with early onset eating disorder or disturbance. European Eating Disorder Review, 20(2), 121–125. Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.