SlideShare a Scribd company logo
RUNNING HEAD: THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS__ ____1
Psychology BS Research Paper: The Impact of Autism on Prosocial Behaviors
Sierra Lee, Winter 2016 – Fall 2016
A11299611
Earl Warren College
Faculty Advisor: Leslie Carver
_______________________________ ______________________________
Faculty/Advisor Signature Date
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 2
Abstract
Social cues are vital to engaging in pro-social behavior. In this study, we compared
typically developing children with children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) in
terms of engagement in prosocial behavior. In order to evaluate each participant’s level of
development at the time, we administered the Differential Abilities Scales to each child. We then
performed different tasks during which the child could perform prosocial behaviors. In each task,
we varied how explicit the provided cues were in order to indicate prosocial behaviors were
warranted. We expect typically developing children to pick up on the les explicit cues more
quickly than those with ASD, but expect that the children with ASD would engage at a similar
level to those typically developing when the cue to engage is explicit. At the time of writing this
paper, the study is ongoing and no results are available.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 3
The Impact of Autism on Prosocial Behaviors
Humans are social creatures, and in order to engage with those around us we use social
cues. We use these skills to engage with other people every day, using them to help us
communicate and make connections with each other. We start learning to pick up on and utilize
these social cues to our benefit when we are as young as 12 months, using facial cues to guide
behavior (Moore & Corkum, 1994). In this study, we looked at pro-social behaviors, which are
behaviors that do not benefit the actor, but rather are for the benefit of another individual, such as
comforting, helping, and sharing. These behaviors start to emerge in typically developing
children at around 1 to 2 years of age (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & Wagner, 1992).
Engaging in prosocial behavior can involve many subtle social cues, in which the actor has to
assume what the other person wants judging by observing behavior, along with the actor wanting
and being able to execute the prosocial behavior (Dunfield, Kuhlmeir, O’Connell, & Kelley
2011). For example, in this study the researcher doesn’t directly ask for help during many of the
tasks, but uses behavioral cues to ask for help such as looking at the child, stating what needs to
be accomplished, or stating what is upsetting her. Typically developing children use these cues
efficiently to engage in prosocial behavior. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)
have more difficulty engaging in these prosocial behaviors (Travis, Sigman, & Ruskin, 2001),
possibly as a result of not understanding when to engage in these behaviors. This deficit has
major consequences: prosocial behaviors are important in promoting social bonding and
adjustments (Coie et al., 1990; Eisenberg, Eggum, & Di Giunta, 2010), leaving those with ASD
at a disadvantage in our social world.
Current research on prosocial behaviors in people with ASD is limited. Travis et al.
(2001) found that children with ASD were less likely to engage in helping and sharing behavior
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 4
compared to children with developmental delays. Dunfield et al. (2011) found that children with
ASD were less likely to engage in sharing behaviors, but found no difference in helping or
comforting behaviors, when compared with typically developing children. Liebal et al. (2007)
found a slight tendency for children with ASD to help less than children with a developmental
delay, however it was not statistically significant. Jones and Fredrickson (2010) and Russell et al.
(2012) found that children with ASD had lower prosocial scores in comparison to typically
developing children using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997), as
reported caregivers. Lin, Tsai, Rangel, and Adolphs (2012) and Izuma, Matsumoto, Camer, and
Adolphs (2011) tested for differences in sharing in adults with ASD; both found those with ASD
were less likely to share, however the difference was not statistically significant. Sally and Hill
(2006) tested sharing in children, and found no difference in amount shared. However, a
limitation of their study questions these results. They collapsed age groups in the ASD category
but not in the typically developing group, even though their study and previous research
indicates prosocial behaviors are likely impacted with increased age (Fabes & Eisenberg, 1998).
In this study, we examined whether children with ASD have trouble engaging in
prosocial behaviors because they do not pick up on the cues, or if they do not understand
prosocial behavior itself. We administered prosocial tasks with cues ranging from less explicit to
more explicit to typically developing children and children diagnosed with ASD. We
hypothesize that children diagnosed with ASD will not pick up on the social cues given as
quickly as the developing children will, but that the children with ASD will engage a similar
amount when the cues given are explicit.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 5
Methods
Participants
Potential participants were recruited from a database of San Diego parents who indicated
they would be willing to participate in research studies at UC San Diego. At the time of writing
this paper, there were 19 total participants; 17 typically developing participants and 2
participants with ASD. Of all participants, 68% were male and 32% were female, with and
average age of 5.44 years. Both ASD participants were male. 58% of the participants were
identified by their caregiver as Caucasian, and 42% were identified as Hispanic, Latino, Asian,
or mixed ethnicity.
Materials
At the beginning of the study, each participant was administered the Differential Ability
Scales-Second Edition (Elliot, 2007) to determine and control for developmental level. Parents
filled out the Autism Spectrum Quotient (Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, & Wheelwright,
2006), the Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (Constintino & Gruber, 2012), and the
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) to evaluate levels of Autistic traits
and social behaviors from the perspective of the parents. This was done to determine how
accurate the data collected during the experiment was compared to normal behavior reported
from the parents. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (Lord et al. 2012) was
administered to confirm diagnosis of the participants with ASD.
Procedure
After explaining and detailing the consent form, the parent was given the questionnaires
to fill out while the child began the experiment.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 6
First, the experimenter administered the DAS to the child in the first examination room.
This included the following sections: verbal comprehension, matrices, naming vocabulary,
pattern construction, copying, and picture similarities. Once complete, the child was given three
miniature toy monkeys and told that every child that gets through this room receives the
monkeys as a prize and that they are theirs to keep and take home with them. The child then was
escorted to the next room and was encouraged to bring the monkeys with them.
In the next room, the experimenter turned on the camera and started removing previously
placed crumpled up paper balls from the table. This task (to be discussed later) was the first to be
done in this room. Then, the following tasks were randomized for each participant: the
experimenter spilled a cup of pens, “hit” her foot under the table, participant was asked to engage
in the sticker task, play with blocks, and asked to play with monkeys (awarded in the previous
room). All the tasks except for sticker-sharing were evaluated through a cue-level system,
described in the next paragraph. Each task evaluated one of three types of prosocial behaviors:
sharing, helping, or comforting. Sharing was evaluated by the sticker task, the block task, and the
monkey-sharing task. Helping was evaluated by the paper ball task and the pen-spilling task.
Comforting was evaluated by the experimenter pretending to get hurt and recording the child’s
reaction.
To determine the child’s cue level, the child would be given a score of one, two, or three
depending on when they started to engage in or recognize the cue for pro-social behavior with
the experimenter. After that, the experimenter makes the cues more and more explicit. For the
first ten seconds, the experimenter stared at the stimulus (cue level 1); the next ten seconds, the
experimenter alternated her gaze between the stimulus and the child (cue level 2); and for the
final ten seconds, the experimenter vocalizes that the experimenter needs something to be done
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 7
or that something happened (cue level 3). This process stops whenever the participant either
acknowledged the cue to engage or engaged in prosocial behavior. The tasks that had a cue level
system were the paper balls, the comforting task, block sharing, pen helping, and monkey
sharing. The helping tasks (paper balls and pens) were preceded by the experimenter starting to
clean and then giving the cues. The experimenter also kept track of how many items the child
shared or helped to pick up, and what the child said to the experimenter when she got “hurt.”
The child was given an explicit option to share in the sticker-sharing task, and was given
less explicit cues to share in the block and monkey sharing tasks. The explicit option of sharing
in the sticker task would eliminate the possible hurdle of deciphering a less explicit cue. This
would be used as the measure of how much a participant would share when explicitly asked to,
therefore measuring the willingness to engage in prosocial behavior. In the monkey task, the
experimenter told the child that the monkeys they received in the previous room were the last
monkeys available, and the experimenter had wanted to have a monkey. In the block task, the
experimenter looked at her empty toy box and vocalized that she had no more blocks and wants
to make her structure bigger. For both of these tasks, the child has to realize that the
experimenter is subtlety asking for the child to share.
The sticker-sharing task did not involve a cue system, but rather gave the child a chance
to share on their own volition. First, the child was asked to write their name on an envelope.
Then the experimenter showed the child pictures of three different children of similar age and
gender to the child’s. The child was told that these were the experimenter’s three friends that
“could not be there, but really love stickers.” The experimenter then counted out seven stickers
and laid them out in front of the child. The experimenter explained to the child that these stickers
were the child’s, and that they can share none, a portion, or all of them with the “friend,” and that
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 8
they can put the stickers they want to keep in their envelope, and the stickers they want to share
in the “friend’s” envelope. Then the experimenter turned around and let the child put however
many sticker in each envelope. This was done three times. In order to make it clear that the child
would do this task more than once, the upcoming envelopes with children’s pictures and the
remaining stickers (seven per “friend”) were placed on the table off to the side.
In the helping tasks, the experimenter does not vocally ask the child for help and simply
starts cleaning up the paper balls or pens. Only in cue level two does the experimenter look at the
child to nonverbally ask for assistance, and then at cue level three vocalizes the need to clean up.
Comforting was evaluated by the experimenter pretending to hurt herself and vocalizing
that she was hurt. The experimenter looked at the child for comfort at level two and vocalized
again that she was hurt at level three. Acknowledgment of the injury was enough to complete the
task.
Results
At the writing of this paper, the study is ongoing and no results are available. We expect
that the children diagnosed with ASD will pick up on cues significantly later than the typically
developing children, but that children diagnosed with ASD will still engage in prosocial behavior
when explicitly prompted to do so in the sticker-sharing task.
Discussion
This study examined why children with ASD have deficits in prosocial behaviors. We
hope to understand whether children with ASD are not picking up the cues for engaging in
prosocial behavior, or if they simply do not understand or are not motivated to engage in
prosocial behavior. We hypothesize that picking up on the less explicit cues to engage in
prosocial behavior are what makes engagement difficult for those with ASD, and not prosocial
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 9
behavior as a whole. We expect that those with ASD will not pick up on the less explicit cues to
engage in prosocial behavior in most of the tasks. However, we believe that they will engage in
prosocial behavior if given more explicit cues to engage. We tested this by giving the child a
more explicit cue to engage during the sticker sharing task, and less explicit cues to engage
during the various other tasks. If our hypothesis is correct, professionals working with children
with ASD may be able to create or modify current therapy techniques to specifically target
developing the skills needed to recognize less explicit cues, helping children with ASD better
engage in our social world in general. If this is not the case, the scientific community will still
gain more knowledge about this topic, specifically that it may be more likely that those with
ASD do not want to engage in or understand the concept of prosocial behaviors. Either way,
more research should be done on the subject to get reliable and generalizable results.
There were limitations for this study. We tested a greater amount of males because of the
likelihood of having mostly male ASD participants, which could make the results less applicable
to females. We were conducting this study in an affluent area of San Diego, possibly skewing the
results. In addition, one of the participants with ASD was administered the tasks in their own
home rather than in the lab for convenience reasons, which could have changed their likelihood
of engaging in the prosocial behaviors. We tried to make this experiment as naturalistic as
possible, however there may have been some variables that were not adequately controlled for
(such as the amount of time the participant spent with the experimenter(s), which could alter how
willing they were to engage in prosocial behaviors with the experimenter). Future studies should
account for these flaws, and possibly also come up with a reliable way to measure whether the
participants with ASD understood the aspects of their role (for example, that they get to keep
what is given to them), and possibly measure how they feel about prosocial behavior in general.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 10
Hopefully, from this study in combination with future studies, the scientific community can give
those with ASD more tools to engage in the social world.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 11
References
Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R.A., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S. (2006). The Autism
Spectrum Quotient (AQ) — Adolescent Version Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 36:343-350
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). 2 Peer group behavior and social
status. Peer rejection in childhood, 17.
Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition. Los
Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the Diversity of
Prosocial Behavior: Helping, Sharing, and Comforting in Infancy. Infancy, 16(3), 227–247.
Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., & Di Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-Related Responding:
Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Social Issues
and Policy Review, 4(1), 143–180.
Elliott, C. D. (2007). Differential ability scales – second edition: Administration and scoring
manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.
Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1998). Meta-analyses of age and sex differences in children’s and
adolescents’ prosocial behavior. Handbook of Child Psychology, 3.
Goodman, R. (1997) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A Research Note. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586.
Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social
reputation in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 17302–
17307.
THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 12
Jones, A. P., & Frederickson, N. (2010). Multi-Informant Predictors of Social Inclusion for
Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Attending Mainstream School. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1094–1103. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0957-
3
Liebal, K., Colombi, C., Rogers, S. J., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and
Cooperation in Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
38(2), 224–238.
Lin, A., Tsai, K., Rangel, A., & Adolphs, R. (2012). Reduced social preferences in autism:
evidence from charitable donations. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders, 4(8).
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P.C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S.L. (2012). ADOS-2:
autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life.
Developmental Review, 14(4), 349–372.
Sally, D., & Hill, E. (2006). The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind,
cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 73–97.
Travis, L., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (2001). Links Between Social Understanding and Social
Behavior in Verbally Able Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 31(2), 119–130.
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of
concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136.

More Related Content

What's hot

Early Childhood Final Presentation
Early Childhood Final PresentationEarly Childhood Final Presentation
Early Childhood Final PresentationHaleyParrish
 
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...Renzhie Katigbak
 
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...ijtsrd
 
Erik Erikson's theory of Psychosocial Development
Erik Erikson's theory of  Psychosocial DevelopmentErik Erikson's theory of  Psychosocial Development
Erik Erikson's theory of Psychosocial DevelopmentDrGMSunagar1
 
Adolescent and Adult Learning
Adolescent and Adult LearningAdolescent and Adult Learning
Adolescent and Adult LearningLovely Centizas
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peersgaz12000
 
Attachment & Autism
Attachment & AutismAttachment & Autism
Attachment & AutismSean Bryan
 
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlers
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlersSocio emotional development of infants and toddlers
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlersShanelou Pading Ü
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)MarioBuzz1
 
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?buck86
 
Applications of the whole child concept
Applications of the whole child conceptApplications of the whole child concept
Applications of the whole child conceptJaya Saini
 
Social-Emotional Development in Preschool
Social-Emotional Development in PreschoolSocial-Emotional Development in Preschool
Social-Emotional Development in PreschoolHatch Early Learning
 
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on Learning
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on LearningA Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on Learning
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on LearningBetty Wakia (白丽)
 
Social development
Social developmentSocial development
Social developmentAmna Abid
 
Formation of Identity
Formation of IdentityFormation of Identity
Formation of IdentityDrGMSunagar1
 
Lifespan psychology lecture 3.2
Lifespan psychology   lecture 3.2Lifespan psychology   lecture 3.2
Lifespan psychology lecture 3.2kclancy
 

What's hot (20)

Early Childhood Final Presentation
Early Childhood Final PresentationEarly Childhood Final Presentation
Early Childhood Final Presentation
 
Child Psychology
Child PsychologyChild Psychology
Child Psychology
 
The Late Childhood
The Late ChildhoodThe Late Childhood
The Late Childhood
 
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...
attitudes of a student as affected by the guidance provided by the parents wh...
 
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...
Attachment Security and Perceived Parental Psychological Control as Parameter...
 
Social development
Social developmentSocial development
Social development
 
Erik Erikson's theory of Psychosocial Development
Erik Erikson's theory of  Psychosocial DevelopmentErik Erikson's theory of  Psychosocial Development
Erik Erikson's theory of Psychosocial Development
 
Adolescent and Adult Learning
Adolescent and Adult LearningAdolescent and Adult Learning
Adolescent and Adult Learning
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
 
Attachment
AttachmentAttachment
Attachment
 
Attachment & Autism
Attachment & AutismAttachment & Autism
Attachment & Autism
 
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlers
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlersSocio emotional development of infants and toddlers
Socio emotional development of infants and toddlers
 
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
Impact of parental styles (ejop daniela)
 
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?
Authoritative Parenting and Children’s Behavior: Is it the Best Way to Parent?
 
Applications of the whole child concept
Applications of the whole child conceptApplications of the whole child concept
Applications of the whole child concept
 
Social-Emotional Development in Preschool
Social-Emotional Development in PreschoolSocial-Emotional Development in Preschool
Social-Emotional Development in Preschool
 
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on Learning
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on LearningA Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on Learning
A Critical Analysis of the Principles of Nurture and its Impact on Learning
 
Social development
Social developmentSocial development
Social development
 
Formation of Identity
Formation of IdentityFormation of Identity
Formation of Identity
 
Lifespan psychology lecture 3.2
Lifespan psychology   lecture 3.2Lifespan psychology   lecture 3.2
Lifespan psychology lecture 3.2
 

Similar to Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxcroysierkathey
 
Jameel2015GreatExpectations
Jameel2015GreatExpectationsJameel2015GreatExpectations
Jameel2015GreatExpectationsLeila Jameel
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFMarissa Schneider
 
Development ofemotionrecognition
Development ofemotionrecognitionDevelopment ofemotionrecognition
Development ofemotionrecognitionBrittany Bergstrom
 
Problem Solving
Problem SolvingProblem Solving
Problem Solvingmcneeljc86
 
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docxStudent Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docxemelyvalg9
 
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-mainssuser49876c
 
Social cognitive theory power point
Social cognitive theory power pointSocial cognitive theory power point
Social cognitive theory power pointabonica
 
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...rsiehs
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxcroysierkathey
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLkarenahmanny4c
 
Autism Spectrum Disability Study
Autism Spectrum Disability StudyAutism Spectrum Disability Study
Autism Spectrum Disability StudyStefanie Yang
 
Stereotypes 2018
Stereotypes 2018Stereotypes 2018
Stereotypes 2018abonica
 
Avaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseAvaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseglaucohp
 
Down syndrome presentation
Down syndrome   presentationDown syndrome   presentation
Down syndrome presentationEuridiki
 

Similar to Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper (20)

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISREDUCING BEHAVIOR PROB.docx
 
Jameel2015GreatExpectations
Jameel2015GreatExpectationsJameel2015GreatExpectations
Jameel2015GreatExpectations
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
 
JEIBI
JEIBI                                                         JEIBI
JEIBI
 
Study
StudyStudy
Study
 
Development ofemotionrecognition
Development ofemotionrecognitionDevelopment ofemotionrecognition
Development ofemotionrecognition
 
Problem Solving
Problem SolvingProblem Solving
Problem Solving
 
senior sem paper
senior sem papersenior sem paper
senior sem paper
 
DahlmanResearchPaper
DahlmanResearchPaperDahlmanResearchPaper
DahlmanResearchPaper
 
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docxStudent Name  Annotated Bibliography  Bares, D.S., T.docx
Student Name Annotated Bibliography Bares, D.S., T.docx
 
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
1 s2.0-s0074775000800129-main
 
Social cognitive theory power point
Social cognitive theory power pointSocial cognitive theory power point
Social cognitive theory power point
 
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...
The Development of Antisocial Personality Disorder Over the Lifespan: A Psych...
 
Spring2013V7N1P1
Spring2013V7N1P1Spring2013V7N1P1
Spring2013V7N1P1
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
 
Autism Spectrum Disability Study
Autism Spectrum Disability StudyAutism Spectrum Disability Study
Autism Spectrum Disability Study
 
Stereotypes 2018
Stereotypes 2018Stereotypes 2018
Stereotypes 2018
 
Avaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunriseAvaliando programa sunrise
Avaliando programa sunrise
 
Down syndrome presentation
Down syndrome   presentationDown syndrome   presentation
Down syndrome presentation
 

Lee, Sierra - Psychology BS Research Paper

  • 1. RUNNING HEAD: THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS__ ____1 Psychology BS Research Paper: The Impact of Autism on Prosocial Behaviors Sierra Lee, Winter 2016 – Fall 2016 A11299611 Earl Warren College Faculty Advisor: Leslie Carver _______________________________ ______________________________ Faculty/Advisor Signature Date
  • 2. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 2 Abstract Social cues are vital to engaging in pro-social behavior. In this study, we compared typically developing children with children diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) in terms of engagement in prosocial behavior. In order to evaluate each participant’s level of development at the time, we administered the Differential Abilities Scales to each child. We then performed different tasks during which the child could perform prosocial behaviors. In each task, we varied how explicit the provided cues were in order to indicate prosocial behaviors were warranted. We expect typically developing children to pick up on the les explicit cues more quickly than those with ASD, but expect that the children with ASD would engage at a similar level to those typically developing when the cue to engage is explicit. At the time of writing this paper, the study is ongoing and no results are available.
  • 3. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 3 The Impact of Autism on Prosocial Behaviors Humans are social creatures, and in order to engage with those around us we use social cues. We use these skills to engage with other people every day, using them to help us communicate and make connections with each other. We start learning to pick up on and utilize these social cues to our benefit when we are as young as 12 months, using facial cues to guide behavior (Moore & Corkum, 1994). In this study, we looked at pro-social behaviors, which are behaviors that do not benefit the actor, but rather are for the benefit of another individual, such as comforting, helping, and sharing. These behaviors start to emerge in typically developing children at around 1 to 2 years of age (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & Wagner, 1992). Engaging in prosocial behavior can involve many subtle social cues, in which the actor has to assume what the other person wants judging by observing behavior, along with the actor wanting and being able to execute the prosocial behavior (Dunfield, Kuhlmeir, O’Connell, & Kelley 2011). For example, in this study the researcher doesn’t directly ask for help during many of the tasks, but uses behavioral cues to ask for help such as looking at the child, stating what needs to be accomplished, or stating what is upsetting her. Typically developing children use these cues efficiently to engage in prosocial behavior. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) have more difficulty engaging in these prosocial behaviors (Travis, Sigman, & Ruskin, 2001), possibly as a result of not understanding when to engage in these behaviors. This deficit has major consequences: prosocial behaviors are important in promoting social bonding and adjustments (Coie et al., 1990; Eisenberg, Eggum, & Di Giunta, 2010), leaving those with ASD at a disadvantage in our social world. Current research on prosocial behaviors in people with ASD is limited. Travis et al. (2001) found that children with ASD were less likely to engage in helping and sharing behavior
  • 4. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 4 compared to children with developmental delays. Dunfield et al. (2011) found that children with ASD were less likely to engage in sharing behaviors, but found no difference in helping or comforting behaviors, when compared with typically developing children. Liebal et al. (2007) found a slight tendency for children with ASD to help less than children with a developmental delay, however it was not statistically significant. Jones and Fredrickson (2010) and Russell et al. (2012) found that children with ASD had lower prosocial scores in comparison to typically developing children using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997), as reported caregivers. Lin, Tsai, Rangel, and Adolphs (2012) and Izuma, Matsumoto, Camer, and Adolphs (2011) tested for differences in sharing in adults with ASD; both found those with ASD were less likely to share, however the difference was not statistically significant. Sally and Hill (2006) tested sharing in children, and found no difference in amount shared. However, a limitation of their study questions these results. They collapsed age groups in the ASD category but not in the typically developing group, even though their study and previous research indicates prosocial behaviors are likely impacted with increased age (Fabes & Eisenberg, 1998). In this study, we examined whether children with ASD have trouble engaging in prosocial behaviors because they do not pick up on the cues, or if they do not understand prosocial behavior itself. We administered prosocial tasks with cues ranging from less explicit to more explicit to typically developing children and children diagnosed with ASD. We hypothesize that children diagnosed with ASD will not pick up on the social cues given as quickly as the developing children will, but that the children with ASD will engage a similar amount when the cues given are explicit.
  • 5. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 5 Methods Participants Potential participants were recruited from a database of San Diego parents who indicated they would be willing to participate in research studies at UC San Diego. At the time of writing this paper, there were 19 total participants; 17 typically developing participants and 2 participants with ASD. Of all participants, 68% were male and 32% were female, with and average age of 5.44 years. Both ASD participants were male. 58% of the participants were identified by their caregiver as Caucasian, and 42% were identified as Hispanic, Latino, Asian, or mixed ethnicity. Materials At the beginning of the study, each participant was administered the Differential Ability Scales-Second Edition (Elliot, 2007) to determine and control for developmental level. Parents filled out the Autism Spectrum Quotient (Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, & Wheelwright, 2006), the Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (Constintino & Gruber, 2012), and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) to evaluate levels of Autistic traits and social behaviors from the perspective of the parents. This was done to determine how accurate the data collected during the experiment was compared to normal behavior reported from the parents. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (Lord et al. 2012) was administered to confirm diagnosis of the participants with ASD. Procedure After explaining and detailing the consent form, the parent was given the questionnaires to fill out while the child began the experiment.
  • 6. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 6 First, the experimenter administered the DAS to the child in the first examination room. This included the following sections: verbal comprehension, matrices, naming vocabulary, pattern construction, copying, and picture similarities. Once complete, the child was given three miniature toy monkeys and told that every child that gets through this room receives the monkeys as a prize and that they are theirs to keep and take home with them. The child then was escorted to the next room and was encouraged to bring the monkeys with them. In the next room, the experimenter turned on the camera and started removing previously placed crumpled up paper balls from the table. This task (to be discussed later) was the first to be done in this room. Then, the following tasks were randomized for each participant: the experimenter spilled a cup of pens, “hit” her foot under the table, participant was asked to engage in the sticker task, play with blocks, and asked to play with monkeys (awarded in the previous room). All the tasks except for sticker-sharing were evaluated through a cue-level system, described in the next paragraph. Each task evaluated one of three types of prosocial behaviors: sharing, helping, or comforting. Sharing was evaluated by the sticker task, the block task, and the monkey-sharing task. Helping was evaluated by the paper ball task and the pen-spilling task. Comforting was evaluated by the experimenter pretending to get hurt and recording the child’s reaction. To determine the child’s cue level, the child would be given a score of one, two, or three depending on when they started to engage in or recognize the cue for pro-social behavior with the experimenter. After that, the experimenter makes the cues more and more explicit. For the first ten seconds, the experimenter stared at the stimulus (cue level 1); the next ten seconds, the experimenter alternated her gaze between the stimulus and the child (cue level 2); and for the final ten seconds, the experimenter vocalizes that the experimenter needs something to be done
  • 7. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 7 or that something happened (cue level 3). This process stops whenever the participant either acknowledged the cue to engage or engaged in prosocial behavior. The tasks that had a cue level system were the paper balls, the comforting task, block sharing, pen helping, and monkey sharing. The helping tasks (paper balls and pens) were preceded by the experimenter starting to clean and then giving the cues. The experimenter also kept track of how many items the child shared or helped to pick up, and what the child said to the experimenter when she got “hurt.” The child was given an explicit option to share in the sticker-sharing task, and was given less explicit cues to share in the block and monkey sharing tasks. The explicit option of sharing in the sticker task would eliminate the possible hurdle of deciphering a less explicit cue. This would be used as the measure of how much a participant would share when explicitly asked to, therefore measuring the willingness to engage in prosocial behavior. In the monkey task, the experimenter told the child that the monkeys they received in the previous room were the last monkeys available, and the experimenter had wanted to have a monkey. In the block task, the experimenter looked at her empty toy box and vocalized that she had no more blocks and wants to make her structure bigger. For both of these tasks, the child has to realize that the experimenter is subtlety asking for the child to share. The sticker-sharing task did not involve a cue system, but rather gave the child a chance to share on their own volition. First, the child was asked to write their name on an envelope. Then the experimenter showed the child pictures of three different children of similar age and gender to the child’s. The child was told that these were the experimenter’s three friends that “could not be there, but really love stickers.” The experimenter then counted out seven stickers and laid them out in front of the child. The experimenter explained to the child that these stickers were the child’s, and that they can share none, a portion, or all of them with the “friend,” and that
  • 8. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 8 they can put the stickers they want to keep in their envelope, and the stickers they want to share in the “friend’s” envelope. Then the experimenter turned around and let the child put however many sticker in each envelope. This was done three times. In order to make it clear that the child would do this task more than once, the upcoming envelopes with children’s pictures and the remaining stickers (seven per “friend”) were placed on the table off to the side. In the helping tasks, the experimenter does not vocally ask the child for help and simply starts cleaning up the paper balls or pens. Only in cue level two does the experimenter look at the child to nonverbally ask for assistance, and then at cue level three vocalizes the need to clean up. Comforting was evaluated by the experimenter pretending to hurt herself and vocalizing that she was hurt. The experimenter looked at the child for comfort at level two and vocalized again that she was hurt at level three. Acknowledgment of the injury was enough to complete the task. Results At the writing of this paper, the study is ongoing and no results are available. We expect that the children diagnosed with ASD will pick up on cues significantly later than the typically developing children, but that children diagnosed with ASD will still engage in prosocial behavior when explicitly prompted to do so in the sticker-sharing task. Discussion This study examined why children with ASD have deficits in prosocial behaviors. We hope to understand whether children with ASD are not picking up the cues for engaging in prosocial behavior, or if they simply do not understand or are not motivated to engage in prosocial behavior. We hypothesize that picking up on the less explicit cues to engage in prosocial behavior are what makes engagement difficult for those with ASD, and not prosocial
  • 9. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 9 behavior as a whole. We expect that those with ASD will not pick up on the less explicit cues to engage in prosocial behavior in most of the tasks. However, we believe that they will engage in prosocial behavior if given more explicit cues to engage. We tested this by giving the child a more explicit cue to engage during the sticker sharing task, and less explicit cues to engage during the various other tasks. If our hypothesis is correct, professionals working with children with ASD may be able to create or modify current therapy techniques to specifically target developing the skills needed to recognize less explicit cues, helping children with ASD better engage in our social world in general. If this is not the case, the scientific community will still gain more knowledge about this topic, specifically that it may be more likely that those with ASD do not want to engage in or understand the concept of prosocial behaviors. Either way, more research should be done on the subject to get reliable and generalizable results. There were limitations for this study. We tested a greater amount of males because of the likelihood of having mostly male ASD participants, which could make the results less applicable to females. We were conducting this study in an affluent area of San Diego, possibly skewing the results. In addition, one of the participants with ASD was administered the tasks in their own home rather than in the lab for convenience reasons, which could have changed their likelihood of engaging in the prosocial behaviors. We tried to make this experiment as naturalistic as possible, however there may have been some variables that were not adequately controlled for (such as the amount of time the participant spent with the experimenter(s), which could alter how willing they were to engage in prosocial behaviors with the experimenter). Future studies should account for these flaws, and possibly also come up with a reliable way to measure whether the participants with ASD understood the aspects of their role (for example, that they get to keep what is given to them), and possibly measure how they feel about prosocial behavior in general.
  • 10. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 10 Hopefully, from this study in combination with future studies, the scientific community can give those with ASD more tools to engage in the social world.
  • 11. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 11 References Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R.A., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S. (2006). The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) — Adolescent Version Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36:343-350 Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1990). 2 Peer group behavior and social status. Peer rejection in childhood, 17. Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the Diversity of Prosocial Behavior: Helping, Sharing, and Comforting in Infancy. Infancy, 16(3), 227–247. Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., & Di Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-Related Responding: Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 4(1), 143–180. Elliott, C. D. (2007). Differential ability scales – second edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc. Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1998). Meta-analyses of age and sex differences in children’s and adolescents’ prosocial behavior. Handbook of Child Psychology, 3. Goodman, R. (1997) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A Research Note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586. Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 17302– 17307.
  • 12. THE IMPACT OF AUTISM ON PROSOCIAL BEHAVIORS 12 Jones, A. P., & Frederickson, N. (2010). Multi-Informant Predictors of Social Inclusion for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Attending Mainstream School. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1094–1103. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0957- 3 Liebal, K., Colombi, C., Rogers, S. J., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and Cooperation in Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 224–238. Lin, A., Tsai, K., Rangel, A., & Adolphs, R. (2012). Reduced social preferences in autism: evidence from charitable donations. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders, 4(8). Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P.C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S.L. (2012). ADOS-2: autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life. Developmental Review, 14(4), 349–372. Sally, D., & Hill, E. (2006). The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 73–97. Travis, L., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (2001). Links Between Social Understanding and Social Behavior in Verbally Able Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 119–130. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136.