SlideShare a Scribd company logo
http://jcc.sagepub.com
Psychology
Journal of Cross-Cultural
DOI: 10.1177/0022022194252002
1994; 25; 181 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
Deborah L. Best, Amy S. House, Anne E. Barnard and Brenda S.
Spicker
Effects of Gender and Culture
Parent-Child Interactions in France, Germany, and Italy: The
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/25/2/181
The online version of this article can be found at:
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology
at:
can be foundJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Additional
services and information for
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/25/2/181 Citations
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.iaccp.org/
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/25/2/181
http://jcc.sagepub.com
from the SAGE Social Science Collections. All Rights
Reserved.
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010
http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://jcc.sagepub.com
http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/01650254.html
Perspectives on gender development
Eleanor E. Maccoby
Stanford University, California, USA
Two traditional perspectives on gender development—the
socialisation and cognitive perspectives—
are reviewed. It is noted that although they deal quite well with
individual differences within each sex
with regard to degree of sex-typing, they do not offer
satisfactory explanations for some of the most
robust gender dimorphisms: namely, gender segregation and the
divergent patterns of interaction
within all-male as compared with all-female dyads or groups.
These patterns are brie�y summarised,
and their similarity to those found in nonhuman primates and
other mammals is noted. It is argued
that an ethological perspective, and its modern successor the
psychobiological perspective, are
needed, along with the more traditional perspectives, to provide
a comprehensive account of gender
development as it occurs in dyads and groups as well as within
individual children.
In the last several decades there have been important shifts in
psychologists’ thinking about gender development. There were
empirical questions �rst of all: In what ways, to what degree,
and how consistently, did boys and girls differ in the
developmental pathways typically taken? And there have been
notable changes in the points of view psychologists have
brought to bear in their efforts to understand and explain
whatever gender differentiation was thought to occur. Two
viewpoints about gender development were dominant for many
years: the socialisation perspective and the gender-cognitive
perspective. These perspectives are �rst described below, and
then their limitations are pointed out, stressing how narrow
these views were concerning the nature of the gender-
differentiated phenomena that need to be understood. The
paper turns then to considering how much an ethological
perspective, when added to the traditional pair, can contribute
towards achieving a more comprehensive view of gender
development. Finally, some more recent thinking from
psychobiology is brought to bear in the interests of moving
toward an integration of the several perspectives.
The direct socialisation perspective
At mid century, psychologists asked: By what processes do
children become ‘‘sex-typed?’’ By sex-typing, they usually
meant that children take on the attributes that are typical and/
or valued (expected, normative) for their own sex. In seeking
answers, they worked from the stimulus-response (S-R)
principles of the reinforcement learning theories that domi-
nated the �eld of psychology at that time. From this point of
view, sex-typed behaviours were a set of habits. Boys and girls
would develop different sex-typed habits if socialisation
agents—parents, teachers, older children—reinforced girls for
‘‘feminine’’ behaviours and provided negative consequences
when they displayed behaviours thought to be more appro-
priate for boys. Similarly, boys were thought to be ‘‘shaped’’
toward the version of ‘‘masculine’’ behaviour deemed proper
in the particular society where the children were growing up.
Implicit in this viewpoint is the idea that individual
differences within each sex, and mean differences between the
sexes on any given trait, are essentially re�ections of the same
processes. Thus, it would be assumed that if one aspect of
becoming masculine for a boy is to learn not to cry, then boys
on the average would be subject to more socialisation
pressures, that is, would be told, more often than girls, ‘‘That
didn’t really hurt’’, or ‘‘It’s only a scratch’’, or ‘‘Don’t be a
crybaby’’, or ‘‘Oh, toughen up!’’—and would develop stronger
inhibitions against crying then would girls. At the same time,
some boys would receive stronger, more consistent pressures of
this kind than others, and so some would develop stronger
crying inhibitions, and become more ‘‘masculine’’ than others.
Differential socialisation pressures on boys and girls could
take a variety of additional forms, beyond differential positive
and negative reinforcement: for example, via providing
different toys for children of the two sexes, or attributing
different characteristics to them. And by giving boys and girls
distinctive names and dressing them differently, their gender
was announced to allcomers with the message: ‘‘This child is a
boy/girl, to be treated accordingly.’’ Distinctive socialisation
pressures on boys and girls were thought to begin at birth, to be
fairly strong, and to be consistent in some respects throughout
a given culture.
A secondary perspective which in�uenced some of the
developmental research at mid century was derived from
psychodynamic theory. This theory was more developmental
than S-R learning theory, which did not specify particular
periods during growth that would be optimal for children’s
acquisition of sex-typed behaviours. Freud’s theory of psycho-
sexual development posited a transition at about the age of four
or �ve, when the resolution of Oedipal con�icts would involve
children’s identifying with their same-sex parent and thus
taking on the appropriate sex-typed characteristics of that
International Journal of Behavioral Development © 2000 The
International Society for the
2000, 24 (4), 398–406 Study of Behavioural Development
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Eleanor E. Maccoby,
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, 2130 Stanford,
CA
94305–2130, USA.
parent. This transition was thought to be more dif�cult for
boys, who had to break a prior identi�cation with their
mothers, and in becoming masculine would need to distance
themselves from all things feminine. Echoes of this theory are
found in the work of Chodorow (1978). In the 1950s and
1960s, Robert Sears and colleagues (Sears, Maccoby, & Levin,
1957; Sears, Rau, & Alpert, 1965) attempted to integrate S-R
and psychodynamic theories in their studies of the relation-
ships between parental child-rearing practices and the degree
of children’s sex-typing. The results failed to con�rm Freudian
theory. As Sears and colleagues wrote, in summarising their
�ndings: ‘‘The box score for primary identi�cation theory as
an
explanation of gender role is poor’’ (Sears et al., 1965, p. 194).
How well have the assumptions about children of the two
sexes being socialised differently stood up empirically? It is
certainly true that parents give their children sex-typed names,
dress them differently, and decorate their rooms differently.
Also, all languages of the world provide different ways of
speaking about male and female persons. In English and other
Indo-European languages, the sexes are distinguished by
pronouns—he, she, his, hers—which surely facilitates chil-
dren’s learning to code themselves and others as to gender. In
addition, there are distinctive ways of speaking to children that
emphasise stereotypical qualities, such as saying to a four-year-
old ‘‘That’s my sweet little girl’’ or ‘‘There’s my big strong
boy’’. Still, it has proved surprisingly dif�cult to document
differential treatment of boys and girls by their parents,
especially when children are young. Several reviews have
summarised child-rearing practices used by parents with sons
and daughters (mainly in modern Western societies). When it
comes to the traditional dimensions of child rearing (e.g.,
permissiveness, restrictiveness, monitoring, responsiveness,
warmth) few differences have been found in the way parents
deal with sons as compared with daughters (Huston, 1983;
Lytton & Romney, 1991; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Although
it is often assumed that parents react differently to assertive
behaviour by sons and daughters, and draw young daughters
into greater emotional closeness with themselves than they do
with young sons, evidence to date does not support these
assumptions. There are some ways in which parents do
consistently differentiate: they do more roughhousing with
sons, offer dolls more often to girls (and toy trucks to boys),
and talk about feelings more with girls (see Maccoby, 1998, for
a summary of studies). And fathers in particular show negative
reactions to any behaviour by their sons that seems effeminate.
Of course, when socialisation differences are found, the
ubiquitous issue of direction of effects arises. Do parents offer
dolls to girls, trucks to boys, because they want their children
to be appropriately ‘‘masculine’’ or ‘‘feminine’’, or because
they have discovered that these are the toys the children prefer?
If they roughhouse more with boys, is this because they have a
stereotypical view of what kind of play boys ought to like, or
because boys actually do like it more than girls and ‘‘train’’
their parents over time to play in ways that boys �nd most
enjoyable? There is good evidence that boys’ and girls’
different initiatives can indeed evoke different reactions from
their parents (see the summary in Maccoby, 1998). But, these
reactions, in their turn, can then in�uence the children, so the
existence of child-to-parent effects do not by any means
preclude parent-to-child effects (Ge et al., 1996; O’Connor,
Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & George, 1998).
Experimental studies have been done in which unfamiliar
children are given an arbitrary gender label. In such studies, an
infant is chosen whose sex cannot be easily identi�ed when the
child is dressed. Unfamiliar adults are then offered the
opportunity to interact with the infant, or view the infant on
videotape, the child having been introduced to some of the
subjects as a girl, to others as a boy. The adults’ reactions to the
child, and interpretations of the child’s behaviour, are then
recorded. Such studies control for the direction-of-effects
problem, because differential eliciting properties of male and
female infants are ruled out by design. Although early studies
suggested that adults’ reactions and interpretations were
indeed in�uenced by the child’s gender label, a review of 23
gender-labelling studies (Stern & Karraker, 1989) found that
overall effects were quite weak and quite inconsistent from
study to study.
No doubt there are subtle differences in parental treatment
that are not captured in the rather coarse-grained net that
researchers have cast, and certainly what seem to be rather
minor differences can accumulate over many repetitions into
signi�cant in�uences on children. And surely, socialisation
differences would be more apparent in traditional societies
where there are more rigid status differences between men and
women. Still, we would have to say that to date, the
socialisation theory that grew out of the S-R learning
perspective of the mid century and stressed the role of parents
as ‘‘shapers’’ of sex-typed behaviour rests on a weak empirical
foundation.
Of course, a socialisation perspective does not need to focus
so exclusively on parents as the developmental psychology of
the 1950s, 1960s, and 1970s tended to do. There were a
number of reasons for this focus. First of all, much of the
research at that time involved infants and children of preschool
age, in families in which few mothers worked outside the home.
Thus it was natural to assume that parents were indeed the
most in�uential socialisation agents. In addition, there was a
pervasive assumption that the early years were a time of great
plasticity, when children were especially subject to ‘‘shaping’’
with respect to characteristics thought to be pervasive and
long-lasting, such as the gendered aspects of the self. In recent
years much more attention has been given to the in�uence of
other socialisation agents, such as out-of-home caregivers,
peers, teachers, and coaches, whose positive or negative
reactions to children’s behaviour can provide additional
shaping for children’s sex-role development in ways that can
supplement—or sometimes even contradict—the in�uence of
parents.
In fact, to test a theory of direct socialisation adequately, it
would be necessary not only to demonstrate that socialisation
agents deal differentially with the two sexes, but that these
different socialisation experiences are related to any differences
in developmental trajectories that appear in male and female
children. Much depends, then, on what aspects of gender
enactment are chosen as ‘‘outcomes’’ in efforts to test
socialisation theory. This issue will be considered more fully
below.
The indirect socialisation perspective
Beginning in the 1960s, social learning theory added learning
by imitation (often called modelling) to reinforcement as a
powerful process involved in gender socialisation. In social
learning theory, children were still seen as being shaped by
direct positive and negative reinforcement. But it was also
shown that children could learn vicariously from seeing how
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL
DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 399
400 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER
DEVELOPMENT
other children’s gender-appropriate or gender-inappropriate
behaviour were reacted to by others (Bandura, 1965). Apart
from learning about the consequences of such behaviour,
children could also learn by observation what behaviours were
characteristic of each sex. And of course, they could learn these
things not only from what they observed within their families,
schools, and neighbourhoods, but from �lms or TV, or as
depicted in stories (Mischel, 1966). The introduction of
observational learning gave great added power to learning
theory. But it also brought some complications. In daily life,
boys and girls are exposed to models of both sexes. Both boys
and girls presumably would learn the same facts concerning
what behaviour is appropriate for boys, what for girls. The
theory called for selective imitation, such that boys would adopt
behaviour depicted by male models, or adapt their own
behaviour according to the reinforcement patterns they saw
being provided for male, rather than female, children. At the
least, this required that children would know their own gender
and that of the people whom they observed. Then too, it
required that children should be able to summarise and
generalise from multiple exemplars, and deal with exceptions.
In addition, it called for some motivation to adopt the
behaviour patterns of people who are ‘‘the same as me’’ with
respect to gender. Clearly, the theory called for the extensive
incorporation of cognitive elements in gender development.
The cognitive perspective
In the 1970s and 1980s, there was a strong surge of interest in
gender cognitions. Although a cognitive perspective on
developmental processes had been strongly anticipated in
Europe for many decades, it was the ‘‘cognitive revolution’’
that took place in American psychology beginning in the 1960s
that set the stage for an active revision of American views about
child development. Children were increasingly seen not as
passive recipients of socialisation pressures, but as active
selectors and users of information pertinent to their develop-
mental levels and personal goals. And children began to be
seen as developing the capacity to adopt standards, and
regulate their own behaviour in conformity to these standards,
thus contributing to their own socialisation.
Vast amounts of information are available to children
concerning the way gender is enacted in the world around
them. In the 1970s, 1980s, and continuing into the 1990s,
research focused heavily on how children acquire knowledge
and develop stereotypes and scripts concerning what is usual,
or considered ‘‘appropriate’’, for people of the two sexes. (See
Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a review.) It became evident that
gender is a highly salient category for children, perhaps because
it is neatly binary, because it is so heavily culturally
emphasised, and because socially ascribed sex and biological
sex are so completely redundant. The distinction, in other
words, is easy to make, and there is good evidence that children
do indeed make it very early in life. Gender categories, once
applied, have been shown to be a convenient hook on which
children can easily hang stereotypes about gender attributes
(Gelman, Coleman, & Maccoby, 1986) and assimilate new
incoming information to these stereotypes. Gender schema
theories introduced in the early 1980s held that children form
cognitive structures that organise their gender knowledge into a
set of expectations that guide and organise their social
perceptions (Bem, 1981; Martin & Halverson, 1981).
Knowledge about the characteristics of the two sexes, and
about the norms for their behaviour, is clearly necessary, but is
it suf�cient by itself to motivate children to adopt socially
prescribed roles and standards? Several hypotheses have been
advanced concerning the ways in which children’s knowledge
of their own gender identity and that of others could function
to set motivational processes in motion:
1. When observing the contingencies that are experienced
by other children, the observer is able to select the
experiences of children of their own sex as most relevant
to inferences about what might happen to themselves.
This allows observational learning to be focused
speci�cally on the acquisition of behaviours and stan-
dards that apply differentially to the child’s own sex.
2. Out of a need for cognitive consistency, children want to
adapt themselves (i.e., conform) to what they believe is
appropriate for their own sex. Kohlberg (1966) proposed
that this motivation would not appear until approxi-
mately the age of 5–7, when he thought children achieve
a �rm level of gender constancy. Because Kohlberg
urged the importance of gender constancy, evidence has
accumulated that the functional elements of gender
constancy (namely, identity and stability; see Maccoby,
1990; Ruble & Martin, 1998) are achieved at consider-
ably younger ages than Kohlberg believed.
3. When they have achieved a stable gender identity,
children classify themselves as members of a same-sex
group. They identify with this group, see members of the
other sex as belonging to an outgroup, and want to be
like members of their own-sex group.
These possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and may all
combine to generate the motivation for children to: (a)
selectively imitate same-sex models (if they are known to be
good exemplars of their gender category—see Perry & Bussey,
1979); (b) seek, select, and remember preferentially informa-
tion that is relevant to, and consistent with, children’s own-sex
schemas, and (c) reject, ignore, forget, or distort schema-
inconsistent material (see Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a
summary). With the acquisition of a stable gender identity,
children can also begin to monitor their own behaviour with
reference to a self-accepted standard of what is appropriate for
their own sex (Bussey & Bandura, 1992).
The socialisation and cognitive perspectives discussed so far
combine into a kind of social constructivist approach. Insofar
as children of the two sexes are found to differ in their
behaviour, interests and/or value, it is thought that this
differentiation comes about for three reasons: because adults
shape children in this way; because peers shape each other
according to the way they themselves have been socialised; and
because children—once they have established a �rm gender
identity—socialise themselves to conform to what they know to
be stereotypical for children of their own sex, within the limits
of what their own competencies permit (Bandura & Bussey,
1999).
Sometimes, the use of the word ‘‘stereotypes’’ is taken to
mean that attributes assimilated to social categories are
arbitrary, so that our concepts about the two sexes may be
quite arti�cial or distorted. How accurate are our gender
stereotypes? In their review, Deaux and LaFrance (1998) note
that it is hardly possible to check the reality base of some of our
stereotypes, because they refer to characteristics that are very
dif�cult to measure objectively. People’s beliefs about certain
attributes of the two sexes that can be objectively assessed,
however, turn out to be quite accurate (e.g., that women are
more involved than men in the care of young children). This
work has been done with adults, and we can only assume that
the accuracy of children’s gender stereotypes would improve
with age, as they accumulate information about a larger and
larger sample of exemplars.
At the end of the 20th century, then, a predominant
perspective on gender development is a dual one focusing on
individual differences. Its central themes are that children will
differ in the degree to which they become sex-typed as a result
of: (a) the strength of the socialisation pressures they have
experienced; and (b) the nature and coherence of their gender
schemas—their knowledge about the characteristics stereo-
typically associated with each sex, and about what the social
expectations are for persons of their own sex. Of course,
socialisation and cognitive factors in gender development are
not truly distinct. For example, socialisation pressures are one
source of information enabling children to develop their
knowledge concerning the gendered norms that they are
expected to adopt. The direct socialisation experiences
children have, in other words, constitute a major source of
information upon which cognitive structures are built. Indeed,
the whole cultural milieu in which a child grows up presents to
children an array of cultural beliefs and practices concerning
gender, and when children draw on these to construct their
gender schemas, it can reasonably be said that they are being
socialised by the surrounding culture into becoming co-
practitioners of these cultural forms.
An emphasis on cognitive and socialisation factors by no
means precludes a recognition of possible biological in�uences
that may generate different predispositions in boys and girls.
Nowadays, there is widespread recognition of the importance
of biological factors. However, as biological sex and socially
ascribed gender are so completely redundant, it has proved
dif�cult to tease them apart. Something is known concerning
sex differences in brain structures and functions. For example,
in males, more functions are lateralised, so that they are
associated with activation primarily in one hemisphere of the
brain, whereas in females, the two hemispheres are more likely
to be both activated for a speci�c function. However, the
possible behavioural impacts of these structural differences are
far from being understood. A good deal is known concerning
the physiological events during gestation that differentiate the
genital structures of male and female fetuses. And we know,
too, something about the way in which prenatal hormones
organise the developing fetal brain so as to create different
propensities and sensitivities in the two sexes—tendencies
which will manifest themselves behaviourally at various times
during postnatal development, perhaps requiring either a
biological or environmental trigger for their activation.
As noted above, most accounts of gender development do
note possible biological underpinnings for some of the gender
differentiation that occurs. And there is considerable interest in
taking biological explanations one step backward to the genetic
factors that may control biological differentiation. But we are a
long way from having traced pathways from genes to the
behavioural attributes that typically differentiate the sexes. In
psychology, the great bulk of work has been concerned with the
social and cognitive factors that are thought to underlie this
differentiation, over and above what any biological predisposi-
tions may call for.
Limitations of these perspectives
In the 1980s and 1990s, it began to be evident that these
perspectives were not serving well, on the whole. First of all, it
was increasingly clear that sex differences in children’s
psychological attributes as usually measured were not sub-
stantial, and �ndings were inconsistent from one study to
another. Focusing on individual variation along dimensions
such as ‘‘masculinity’’, ‘‘femininity’’, ‘‘androgyny’’, or
‘‘degree
of sex-typing’’ was not turning out to be a strategy that
accounted for much variance in behavioural outcomes.
Although this might mean that gender simply is not an
important factor in children’s daily lives, it could also mean
that gender matters only in certain contexts, so that aggregat-
ing across contexts attenuates gender-related phenomena that
are in fact quite strong. If so, it would appear that research
should turn to studies of moderating contexts. And, in
addition, look for gender-related outcome variables that are
more robust than ‘‘sex-typing’’ as we have measured it with
toy- or activity-preference tests or clusters of personality traits.
The socialisation and cognitive perspectives have proved
disappointing in another respect: Empirical tests have failed to
give consistent support for the predicted connections between
processes and outcomes. As noted above, the similarities in the
ways socialisation agents treat boys and girls far outweigh the
differences. Still, some differences are found. Ruble and
Martin’s comprehensive review of studies examining connec-
tions between differential socialisation and sex-typed outcomes
shows very meagre relationships. They say: ‘‘Although adults
and peers treat boys and girls differently in many ways,
especially concerning activities and interests, the role of these
processes in children’s gender-related preferences and
behaviors
remains to be demonstrated’’ (Ruble & Martin, 1998, p. 982).
In a similar vein, they note that variations among children in
the level of their knowledge of gender stereotypes are generally
unconnected to individual differences in sex-typed behaviour
(e.g., Powlishta, 1995). This is by no means a fatal blow to
cognitive theories of gender development, because in many
respects gender cognitions are important in their own right,
regardless of how and whether they ‘‘drive’’ individual
differences in sex-typed behaviour. Furthermore, children’s
understanding of gender identity—their own and that of other
people—does appear to be somewhat connected to other
aspects of gender development early in life. However, under-
standing of gender identity is virtually complete in most
children by the age of about 3, and hence (as it varies so little)
cannot correlate with individual differences in sex-typing that
emerge after approximately the �rst three years of life.
Similarly, in some contexts and at some ages, there is very
little individual variation in the choice of same-sex playmates,
so that this aspect of sex-typing cannot be predicted from
individual cognitive or personality characteristics.
Clearly, a rethinking of the long-dominant perspectives
described above is in order, and has begun. A major
reorientation is a shift away from a focus on individual
differences in outcomes, and an increasing focus on some
robust outcomes for groups—gender effects which might
appear primarily, or only, in certain speci�c social contexts.
Robust gender phenomena in the context of groups
What might these outcomes and contexts be? Indications
began to emerge in the late 1970s that the answer could be
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL
DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 401
402 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER
DEVELOPMENT
found in the context of pairs or larger groups of children
engaged in social interaction. Jacklin and Maccoby (1978)
observed 92 pairs of previously unacquainted children, all close
to 33 months of age, as they interacted with each other and
with toys. Tallies were kept of the instances of social
behaviours (both positive and negative, verbal and nonverbal)
each child directed toward his/her partner. Some of the pairs
were composed of two boys, some of two girls, and some were
mixed-sex pairs. Results were that children of both sexes
directed about twice as much social behaviour toward partners
who were of their own sex as they did to other-sex partners.1 It
is notable that when data were computed without regard to the
sex of the child’s partner, boys and girls displayed virtually
identical levels of social behaviour. In other words, there was
no overall sex difference in a personality dimension that might
be called ‘‘sociability’’. This �nding underlines the fact that
analyses which look only at the behaviour of individual
children without regard to social context can totally obscure
powerful gender phenomena. It is no surprise, then, that many
simple comparisons between boys and girls have shown sex
differences to be weak or absent. Theorists who have
emphasised the importance of context, and variations in the
salience of gender from one context to another, might rightly
see the �ndings of the Jacklin and Maccoby study as a
vindication of their position. But there is something very
speci�c about the context that turned out to be important in
this work: It was the sex of a child’s interactive partner that
mattered, not context construed as environmental setting or
prior priming conditions. The study points to the importance
of group composition, and/or of relationships, in how gender is
enacted.
Astudy of preschoolers, conducted at about the same time,
helps us to understand the above �ndings. Wasserman and
Stern (1978) laid down a strip of carpet on a playroom �oor,
and asked a child (sometimes a boy, sometimes a girl) to stand
quietly at one end of it. Another child was then placed at the
‘‘starting’’ end of the strip, and asked to walk along it to the
standing child. It turned out that children would walk up quite
close to the standing child, facing directly forward, if the
standing child was of their own sex. Approaching an other-sex
child, however, both boys and girls would turn away as they
approached and stop sooner. Notably, this occurred whether
they were acquainted with the standing child or not, indicating
that we are seeing here a form of other-sex avoidance that is not
driven by previous experience with another child, but rather by
the other child’s membership in a gender category.
Perhaps we are seeing here an instance of the kind of self-
regulation pointed to by cognitive social learning theorists, in
which a child recognises the sex of another child, knows that it
is considered inappropriate to associate with children of the
other sex, and hence inhibits the approach to the other child. It
appears, though, that children begin to show cross-sex
avoidance at such a young age that they may not yet be able
to code their own gender identity and that of other children
reliably and explicitly. In traditional theories, there has never
been a claim that the cognitions involved are conscious or
deliberate, but still, gender-cognitive processes are assumed to
occur at some level. An alternative possibility is that there is
simply an uncognised raw emotion connected with gender
categories. The early paper by Zajonc: ‘‘Feeling and thinking:
Preferences need no inferences’’ (1980) presented evidence
that indeed preferences (liking/disliking, approach/avoidance)
can be primary, immediate, without (or prior to) activation of
related cognitions. Recent writings by Panksepp (1998), stress
not only the immediacy of emotional reactions, but their deep
mammalian origins. ‘‘Primal’’ emotions, including affective
reactions to certain characteristics of same- or other-sex
conspeci�cs, are claimed to be instinctive and species-wide.
Given a pattern of wariness toward children of the other sex,
and/or especial interest in, or compatibility with, own-sex other
children, it is predictable that when children have a choice of
playmates, they will congregate in same-sex pairs or groups. In
childhood, there is a clear pattern of cross-sex avoidance and/
or same-sex preference that begins in about the third year of
life and becomes progressively stronger though middle child-
hood. Children’s tendency to congregate socially with others of
their own sex has long been noted in the developmental
literature, and has been thoroughly documented, in a variety of
cultures and subcultures (see Hartup, 1983; Maccoby &
Jacklin, 1987; and Ruble & Martin, 1998, for reviews). In
modern Western societies, it is manifested most strongly in
situations not structured by adults, though in more traditional
societies the structures adults provide for children certainly
contribute to it. The phenomenon of gender segregation in
childhood is remarkably robust, with very little overlap, by the
age of 5 or 6, between the distributions of the two sexes with
respect to the gender of the other children with whom they
spend their free social time (Maccoby, 1998).
The students of gender segregation have been concerned
with the factors that bring it about (see Leaper, 1994) and with
the implications for how gender development should be
studied. Clearly, one implication of gender segregation is that
it is important to continue to study and understand the nature
of the group processes that occur in all-male as compared to
all-female dyads or groups, sustaining the progress that has
already been made in this work. There is now considerable
evidence that the groups or dyads formed by girls, as compared
with boys, differ with respect to the agendas they enact, and in
their prevailing interaction ‘‘styles’’. The nature of these
differences has been summarised elsewhere (Maccoby, 1998;
Ruble & Martin, 1998). Here it is suf�cient to note a few
dominant trends:
1. The themes that appear in boys’ fantasies, in the stories
they invent, the scenarios they enact when playing with
other boys, and the �ctional fare they prefer (books and
TV) involve danger, con�ict, destruction, heroic actions,
and trials of physical strength. Girls’ fantasy and play
themes tend to be oriented around domestic or romantic
scripts, portraying characters who are involved in social
relationships and depicting the maintenance or restora-
tion of order and safety.
2. Interaction among boys, more often than among girls,
involves rough-and-tumble play, competition, con�ict,
ego displays, risk-taking, and striving for dominance.
Girls, by contrast, are more responsive to the inputs of
their interactive partners, more likely to use suggestions
rather than imperative demands, and more likely to
construct collaborative scripts in which the actions of
play characters are reciprocal (see Leaper, 1991, on girls’
collaborative discourse style). This does not imply that
girls do not assert their own objectives, or that their
interactions are con�ict-free, only that they seek their
1 These results were replicated in a study of English children
ranging in age
from 19 to 39 months (Lloyd & Smith, 1986).
individual goals in the context of also striving to maintain
group harmony (see Sheldon, 1992).
3. Girls’ and boys’ friendships are qualitatively different,
girls’ friendships being more intimate in the sense that
friends share information about the details of their lives
and concerns, whereas boys typically know less about
their friends’ lives and base their friendships on shared
activities. The break-up of girls’ friendships is more
emotionally intense than for boys’ friendships.
By age 6, too, boys typically play in larger groups.
Benenson, Apostolaris, and Parnass (1997) showed, in a study
of same-sex six-child groups, that between the ages of 4 and 6,
boys greatly increased the time they spent in coordinated group
activities, so that by the age of 6, they were spending 74% of
their time in such activities. No such increase occurred for
girls, whose coordinated group activities dropped below 20%
of their time at age 6. And, girls have been found to show more
enjoyment than boys when engaged in dyadic interaction
(Benenson, 1993), whereas this differential is not found for
interaction in larger groups. Girls, too, are found to sustain
longer bouts of interaction in dyads than do boys. In other
words, girls actively seek, prefer, and elaborate dyadic
interactions, whereas boys do not appear to �nd such
interactions to be especially gratifying, and instead gravitate
toward coordinated activities carried out in larger groups.
There is evidence, too, that boys’ groups are not only larger,
but also stronger in some sense, that is, more cohesive, with
stronger ingroup identi�cation and stronger boundaries, in the
sense of more strongly excluding both girls and adults (see
Maccoby, 1998, for an elaboration of these processes).
The fact that boys congregate in larger groups has important
implications. When in dyads, children of both sexes are
relatively noncompetitive, and more emotionally supportive
of their interactive partners, than they are when participating in
larger groups (see Benenson, Nicholson, Waire, Roy, &
Simpson, in press), something that was noted many years
ago in interactions among adults (Bales & Borgotta, 1955).
Can it be, then, that the greater competitiveness and lesser
positive intimacy in male-male interaction can be accounted
for by boys’ being more often in larger play groups?Perhaps so,
but the fact that they congregate in larger groups may itself be a
re�ection of their preference for certain forms of competitive
but coordinated activity that can only be performed in larger
groups. Indeed, boys form coalitions to achieve group goals—
and gain group power thereby—to an extent that girls seldom
do.
The above account suggests that the two sexes are pursuing
different agendas in their same-sex groups. But children in
groups are not always engaged in enacting these differentiated
agendas, and when they are not, male-male and female-female
interactions can be much alike. It should be noted, too, that
researchers have not yet spelled out the developmental time-
line for these aspects of gender differentiation. Thus, although
there is reason to have a good deal of con�dence in their
occurrence, it is not yet clear how they wax and wane, and
whether there are privileged sequences such that some
processes need to occur before others can come into play.
The ethological perspective
Ethology originally referred to the study of animal behaviour,
and a guiding perspective in ethological research was an
evolutionary one. The principle of natural selection was
invoked to explain species-wide adaptive behaviours, such that
each animal species was equipped to survive in its particular
environmental niche. As the perspective began to be applied to
human behaviour, it was of great interest to trace similarities
between humans and nonhuman primates, their closest
relatives on the evolutionary tree. In the 1950s and 1960s,
John Bowlby (1969) drew on the work of Harlow (1961) and
Hinde (1966) to show striking resemblances between the
patterns of attachment behaviour in human infants or toddlers
and what was seen in young monkeys and apes. In the 1970s,
work by Blurton-Jones (1972) and Strayer (1977; Strayer &
Strayer, 1978) focused on patterns of children’s play, social
dominance, aggression, and peer af�liation, identifying further
parallels with the young of nonhuman primates.
In observing sex differences in behaviours of this kind,
ethologists do not look for generalised ‘‘trait’’ differences, but
rather for the situations under which a difference does or does
not appear, and the form the behaviour takes. It would not be
meaningful to an ethologist, for example, to ask whether male
ungulates are more aggressive than female ungulates. Rather,
they would note that both males and females attack predators,
and do so in the same way: with their hooves. Only males,
however, �ght over territory and mates, and they use their
horns, rather than their hooves, to attack or threaten other
males. In a similar way, in studying human children,
ethologists look for speci�c behavioural topography, and
speci�c contexts in which sex differentiation is seen. Striking
parallels between human children and the young of other
primates have indeed been found, and this fact has been
interpreted as pointing to evolved, genetically guided under-
pinnings for certain elements of human behaviour. The
evolutionary history of human development continues to be a
matter of great interest up to the present time (see Geary &
Bjorklund, 2000, for a review). Throughout, thinkers involved
in this work have been concerned with what adaptive purpose
an evolved behaviour pattern might serve.
In what way might an evolutionary perspective be pertinent
to gender development? Most evolutionary adaptations, after
all, are seen as species-wide, occurring in both sexes. However,
in bisexual species, the distinctive roles of the two sexes in
reproduction is thought to have produced different adaptive
behaviours (i.e., different strategies for mate selection, and
differential involvement in the rearing of young). With respect
to these domains, then, the two sexes are seen as distinct
subspecies.
Might evolution have anything to do with gender differ-
entiation that occurs in childhood, before the age when the
activities of mate selection and care of offspring emerge? The
fact that young monkeys and apes separate into same-sex
playgroups, and display some of the same sex-differentiated
playstyles as those seen in human children, strongly suggests
that there is indeed an evolutionary basis for these behaviours.
So does the fact that there appears to be substantial uniformity
in these patterns across human cultures. Geary and Bjorklund
(2000) say: ‘‘From an evolutionary perspective, these sex
differences are predicted to be a re�ection of and a preparation
for sex differences in adult reproductive activities’’ (p. 60).
Thus, boys’ competitiveness and dominance strivings are seen
as preparation for adult male competition over mates, whereas
girls’ greater social responsiveness and cooperativeness with
other girls can be seen as preparation for participation in the
kin-based social groups of females in which most rearing of
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL
DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 403
404 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER
DEVELOPMENT
the young occurs, in nonhuman primates as well as—
presumably—in the hunter-gatherer bands where some under-
pinnings of present-day human characteristics evolved.
In what possible way could sex segregation in childhood
serve an adaptive purpose, from the standpoint of the
subsequent successful reproductive activities as adults? One
hypothesis is: It functions to prevent incest, with its attendant
risks for expression, in offspring, of genetic defects carried on
recessive genes (see Maccoby, 1998, and Wolf, 1995, for
elaboration and evidence). According to this account, humans
are predisposed to lose sexual interest in anyone with whom
they have been closely associated in childhood, such as siblings
and other close relatives. Thus, paradoxical though it may
seem, cross-sex avoidance in childhood can be seen as a means
of fostering or safeguarding future heterosexual attraction.
It is more dif�cult to see how some of the other sex-
differentiated patterns we have noted might contribute to
individual reproductive success and viability of an individual’s
offspring. Rather, it would seem timely to expand the
ecological perspective to include group success, group viability.
Among chimpanzees, and in the human societies closest to the
way our ancestors probably lived (Collier & Risaldo, 1981),
males form coalitions to engage in cooperative group hunting
and group warfare,2 enterprises not directly related to
individual reproductive success, but relevant to survival of
the troupe. Some of the processes seen in boys’ groups, then,
may have the function of regulating hostility and competition
among group members in the interests of allowing cooperative
group enterprises to emerge.
The psychobiological perspective
The ethological perspective largely describes instinctive ele-
ments in a species’ behavioural repertoire (i.e., behavioural
dispositions assumed to be governed directly by evolved
genetic programmes). From this perspective, individual life
experience and environmental contexts have little in�uence,
other than to provide the innate environmental triggers
required to ‘‘release’’ an instinctive behaviour. In fact,
however, there are many aspects of the environment, other
than innate releasers, that function jointly with genetic factors
to in�uence behavioural development. Many years of research
have revealed that the way genetic instructions are carried out
depends on environmental inputs at every stage of develop-
ment from conception to maturity.
The modern psychobiological view is that genes (G) and
environment (E) have a bidirectional, reciprocal relationship,
and cannot properly be understood as separate components
whose effects can be independently estimated and then
compared or summed (Gottlieb, Wahlsten, and Lickliter,
1998). G E interactions are widespread, and recent work has
begun to �ll in the gap between genes and behavioural
outcomes, by showing how both environment (e.g., conditions
of rearing) and an allele of a given gene can affect a speci�c
intervening biochemical process, which then in�uences beha-
vioural outcomes (Anisman, Zaharia, Meany, & Merali, 1998;
Suomi, in press; and see Maccoby, 2000, for a summary). The
role of environment, as it interacts with genetic predispositions,
is much more complex than simply providing a releaser for an
instinctive response. In both rodents and primates, it has been
shown that animals coming from different genetic strains will
manifest their different predispositions only under certain early
rearing conditions (i.e., being deprived of contact with
maternal animals or peers—see Hood & Cairns, 1989; Suomi,
1997). And for some songbirds, a young male will not acquire
the species-speci�c male courting song unless he is reared in
the company of older females, who ‘‘train’’ him by responding
selectively to the elements of song that females of the species
�nd most compelling (West & King, in press).
Conditions of rearing matter, too, in how different the two
sexes become. Wallen (1996) has summarised a series of
studies with rhesus monkeys, showing that males display
elevated levels of threat/aggression toward other animals if they
were earlier raised with only limited access to peers, whereas
such limited access increases the amount of submissive, not
aggressive, behaviour in females. For males, the amount of
rough play is also affected by the social conditions of rearing,
although for females, the level of such play remains low
regardless of conditions of rearing. When females are
prenatally exposed to androgens (late in gestation, or over an
extended period), they subsequently show elevated levels of
rough play, although there is no such effect for males. Clearly,
both biology and conditions of rearing are important, but
differently for the two sexes. Wallen notes that it is the sex-
dimorphic behaviours most strongly affected by prenatal
hormones in a given sex that show the least effects of rearing
conditions for that sex. And he concludes: ‘‘These studies
demonstrate that the expression of consistent juvenile beha-
vioral sex differences results from hormonally induced predis-
positions to engage in speci�c patterns of juvenile behavior
whose expression is shaped by the speci�c social environment
experienced by the developing monkey’’ (Wallen, 1996,
p. 364).
Humans are very different from birds, mice, and monkeys,
but nevertheless there are useful parallels here. We see that
whatever differential predispositions boys and girls may have, it
is likely that the way they are enacted will depend greatly on
the
social conditions provided by the adults and peers with whom
they interact. Societies differ with respect to how much time
children of each sex spend with adults, with peers of their own
sex, and with peers or siblings of the other sex. They also differ
with respect to how much autonomy peer groups have at what
developmental periods. These cultural variations may be
expected to produce variations in the degree and kind of sex
differentiation that appears as children grow up in different
societies.
Integrating perspectives
The above account is meant to show that there is a good case
for including the ethological and psychobiological perspectives
in any attempt to understand gender differentiation in child-
hood. These perspectives are not meant to replace the
socialisation and cognitive perspectives, only to enrich them
by expanding our view of how biological and experiential and
cognitive factors work together when it comes to the enactment
of gender.
The two biological perspectives are especially useful in
helping us to understand any characteristics where between-
sex differences are robust and consistent across cultures and
even across species. However, there is considerable variation
within each sex and among cultural groups in the nature of
2 For information on male hunting and warfare in chimpanzees,
see: Boesch
and Boesch, 1989; McGrew, Marchant, and Nishida, 1996; and
Stanford,
Wallis, Matama, and Goddall, 1994.
gendered behaviour displayed and the contexts in which it
appears. Although some boys, for example, establish a network
of good male friends, and participate actively in male group
activities, others are loners or peripheral ‘‘hangers-on’’ to these
groups, and still others are the victims of teasing and
humiliation by other boys. Some children join peer groups
that are basically prosocial, others associated mainly with
same-sex peers who engage in risky, antisocial behaviour.
Among girls, too, there is variation in how fully they participate
in ‘‘girl culture’’, and in how much interest they have in less
‘‘feminine’’ activities such as team sports. There is evidence
that these individual differences re�ect developed differences
in competencies or vulnerabilities acquired at earlier periods of
development—differences which in their turn undoubtedly
re�ect both within-sex genetic variability and individual
socialisation histories. Thus, the socialisation and cognitive
perspectives should be especially pertinent to the under-
standing of such within-sex variation.
In the last century, there have been substantial changes in
gender roles and the relationships between the sexes, changes
that have occurred much too rapidly to be explained in genetic
terms. These changes underscore how large the social factors
are in such matters as the relative dominance and power of the
two sexes, and make it clear that these matters are indeed open
to change and not built in to human nature as a result of our
species evolution. The above review has suggested that male
power in society stems in no small degree from male groups
and male alliances, despite the internal con�icts and competi-
tion that characterise male interaction. The paradox is this: We
have seen that girls and women have especially strong
interactive skills that support collaboration and cooperation
in their close relationships. Yet, women’s social groupings do
not appear to yield power in out-of-home contexts to the extent
that men’s do. Perhaps it will be possible to gain insight into
this issue with more focused developmental research on the
way power is exercised within same-sex groups and between
the sexes.
The fuller incorporation of the ethological and psychobio-
logical perspectives on gender into our existing frameworks
should enrich the research agenda of students of social
development. More detailed observation of children in groups
is called for, with more attention to the agendas that children of
the two sexes seem motivated to enact, and more careful
delineation of how gender-related patterns change with age. Of
especial interest are the changes that occur in gender
enactment during the transition into adolescence. Useful cues
for how such work can be carried out comes from pioneers in
neighbouring disciplines (e.g., the sociologists Maltz & Borker,
1982 and Barrie Thorne, 1986; and sociolinguist Penelope
Eckert, 1996, 2000).
Developmental psychology is in the process of building a
coherent social psychology of childhood. Considerable atten-
tion has been focused on individual children as members of
groups—their acceptance or rejection by peers, their group
entry skills—and the causes and consequences of individual
variation in these things. In addition, there has been some work
on the nature of groups, apart from the individuals who make
them up. In their comprehensive review, Rubin, Bukowski, and
Parker (1998) distinguish group processes from the interac-
tions that occur within dyadic relationships, noting that groups
can have properties that individuals and dyads cannot have
(e.g., hierarchies, density of relationships, norms). Studies of
‘‘crowds’’ and ‘‘cliques’’ (e.g., Brown, 1990) underscore the
importance of shared norms in the in�uence peer groups have
on their members, and Kinderman (1993) has shown that a
group can have its own identity over and above the identity of
its members, that is, a group norm persists throughout a school
year despite considerable turnover in the membership of the
group. It is time for gender to take a more central place than it
has occupied so far in such work, with more consistent
attention to the gender composition of dyads and groups. Only
with this knowledge in hand will we be able to understand the
role of peers as gender-socialisation agents, and the way in
which children build shared gender cognitions that can serve
either to amplify or dampen the gender differentiation of roles
and status.
Manuscript received June 2000
Revised manuscript received June 2000
References
Anisman, H., Zaharia, M.D., Meany, M.J., & Merali, Z. (1998).
Do early life
events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to
stressors?
International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 149–
164.
Bales, R.F., & Borgatta, E.F. (1955). Size of group as a factor
in the interaction
pro�le. In A.P. Hare, E.F. Borgatta & R.F. Bales (Eds.), Small
groups: Studies
in social interaction (pp. 496–512). Toronto: Random House.
Bandura, A. (1965). In�uence of models’ reinforcement
contingencies on the
acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,
1, 589–595.
Bem, S.L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account
of sex typing.
Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.
Benenson, J.F. (1993). Greater preference among females than
males for dyadic
interaction in early childhood. Child Development, 64, 544–
555.
Benenson, J.F., Apostolaris, N.H., & Parnass, J. (1997). Age
and sex differences
in dyadic and group interaction. Developmental Psychology, 33,
538–543.
Benenson, J.F., Nicholson, C., Waire, A., Roy, R., &Simpson,
A. (in press). The
in�uence of size of social context on children’s competitive
behaviour. Child
Development.
Blurton-Jones, N. (Ed.) (1972). Ethological studies of child
behaviour. London:
Cambridge University Press.
Boesch, C., & Boesch H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild
chimpanzees in the
Tai National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
78, 547–573.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Brown, B.B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S.S.
Feldman & G.R.
Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA:
Harvard
University Press.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1992). Self-regulatory mechanisms
governing gender
development. Child Development, 63, 1236–1250.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social-cognitive theory of
gender develop-
ment and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.
Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering:
Psychoanalysis and the
sociology of gender. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Collier, J., & Risaldo, S. (1981). Politics and gender in simple
societies. In S.B.
Ortner & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Sexual meanings. New York:
Cambridge
University Press.
Deaux, K. & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T.
Fiske, &
G. Lindsey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1).
New York: McGraw
Hill.
Eckert, P. (1996). Vowels and nailpolish: The emergence of
linguistic style in the
preadolescent heterosexual marketplace. In J. Ahlers, L. Bilmes,
M. Chen,
M. Oliver, N. Warner, & S. Werhsteim (Eds.), Gender and
belief systems.
Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group.
Eckert, P. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice.
Cambridge, UK:
Blackwell.
Ge, X., Conger, R., Cadoret, R., Niederhiser, J., Yates, W., et
al. (1996). The
developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual
in�uence
model of child anti-social behavior and parent behavior.
Developmental
Psychology, 32, 574–589.
Geary, D.C., & Bjorklund, D.F. (2000). Evolutionary
developmental psychol-
ogy. Child Development, 71, 57–65.
Gelman, S.A., Coleman, P., & Maccoby, E.E. (1986). Inferring
properties from
categories, versus inferring categories from properties: The case
of gender.
Child Development, 57, 396–404.
Gottleib, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (1998). The signi�
cance of biology
for human development: A developmental psychobiological
systems view. In
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL
DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 405
406 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER
DEVELOPMENT
W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 1.
Theoretical models of human development. (5th ed.), New
York: Wiley.
Harlow, H.F. (1961). The development of affectional patterns in
infant
monkeys. In B.M. Foss (Ed.), The determinants of infant
behaviour (Vol. 1).
London: Methuen.
Hartup, W. (1983). Peer relations. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.)
and E.M.
Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4.
Socialization,
personality, and social development (4th ed.). New York:
Wiley.
Hinde, R.A. (1966). Animal behaviour: A synthesis of ethology
and comparative
psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hood, K.E., & Cairns, R.B. (1989). A developmental genetic
analysis of
aggressive behavior in mice: 4. Genotype-environment
interaction. Aggressive
Behavior, 15, 361–380.
Huston, A.C. (1983). Sex typing. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.)
and E.M.
Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4.
Socialization,
personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 387–467).
New York: Wiley.
Jacklin, C.N., & Maccoby, E.E. (1978). Social behavior at 33
months in same-
sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557–569.
Kinderman, T.A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for
individual
development: The case of children’s motivation in school.
Developmental
Psychology, 29, 970–977.
Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of
children’s sex-role
concepts and attitudes. In E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development
of sex differences.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Leaper, C. (1991). In�uence and involvement in children’s
discourse: Age,
gender and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797–811.
Leaper, C. (1994). Childhood gender segregation: Causes and
consequences. New
Directions for Child Development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-
Bass.
Lloyd, B., & Smith, C. (1986). The effects of age and gender on
social behaviour
in very young children. British Journal of Social Psychology,
25, 33–41.
Lytton, H., & Romney, D.M. (1991). Parents’ differential
socialization of boys
and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–
296.
Maccoby, E.E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender
constancy in sex-
differentiated development. In D. Schrader (Ed.), The legacy of
Lawrence
Kohlberg: No. 47. New directions for child development. San
Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Maccoby, E.E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart,
coming together.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Maccoby, E.E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On
reading and
misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology,
51, 1–27.
Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex
differences. Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press.
Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1987). Gender segregation in
childhood. In
H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child behavior and development.
New York:
Academic Press.
Maltz, D.N. & Borker, R.A. (1982). A cultural approach to
male-female
miscommunication. In J.A. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social
identity (pp.
195–216). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Martin, C.L., & Halverson, C.F. (1981). A schematic processing
model of sex
typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 54,
563–574.
McGrew, W.C., Marchant, L.F., & Nichida, T. (1996). Great ape
societies. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Mischel, W. (1966). Asocial-learning view of sex differences in
behavior. In E.E.
Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford,
CA: Stanford
University Press.
O’Connor, T.G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter, M., &
Plomin, R.
(1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late childhood
and early
adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive
parenting. Develop-
mental Psychology, 34, 970–981.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of
human and animal
emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Perry, D.G., & Bussey, K. (1979). The social-learning theory of
sex differences:
Imitation is alive and well. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 37,
1699–1712.
Powlishta, K.K. (1995). Intergroup processes in childhood:
Social categorization
and sex-role development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 781–
788.
Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J.G. (1998). Peer
interactions, relation-
ships, and groups. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),
Handbook of child
psychology (5th ed.), (Vol. 3, pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.
Ruble, D.N., & Martin, C.L. (1998). Gender Development. In
W. Damon &
N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of chlid psychology (5th ed.),
(Vol. 3, pp. 933–
1016). New York: Wiley.
Sears, R.R., Maccoby, E.E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of
child rearing.
Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Sears, R.R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1965). Identi�cation and
child rearing.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Sheldon, A. (1992). Con�ict talk: Sociolinguistic challenges to
self-assertion and
how young girls meet them. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 95–
117.
Stanford, C.B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., & Goddall, J. (1994).
Patterns of
predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe
National Park,
1982–1991. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 94,
213–228.
Stern, M., & Karraker, K.H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants:
A review of
gender labeling studies. Sex Roles, 20, 501–522.
Strayer, F.F. (1977). Peer attachment and af�liative subgroups.
In F.F. Strayer
(Ed.), Ethological perspectives on preschool social
organization. Research memo,
No. 5. Department of Psychologie, University of Quebec.
Strayer, F., & Strayer, F.F. (1978). Social aggressive and power
relations among
preschool children. Aggressive Behavior, 4, 173–182.
Suomi, S.J. (1997). Long-term effects of different early rearing
experiences on
social, emotional and physiological development in non-human
primates. In
M.S. Keshevan & R.M. Murra (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental
models of adult
psychopathology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Suomi, S.J. (in press). A biobehavioral perspective on
develpmental psycho-
pathology: Excessive aggression and serotonergic dysfunction
in monkeys. In
A.J. Samaroff, M. Lewis, & S. Miller (Eds.), Handbok of
developmental
psychopathology. New York: Plenum.
Thorne, B. (1986). Boys and girls together . . . but mostly apart:
Gender
arrangements in elementary schools. In W.H. Hartup & Z. Rubin
(Eds.),
Relationships and development. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
Wallen, K. (1996). Nature needs nurture: The interaction of
hormonal and
social in�uences on the development of behavioral sex
differences in rhesus
monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 364–378.
Wasserman, G.A., & Stern, D.N. (1978). An early manifestation
of differential
behavior toward children of the same and opposite sex. Journal
of Genetic
Psychology, 133, 129–137.
West, M.J., & King, A.P. (in press). Science lies its way to the
truth—really. In
E. Blass (Ed.), Developmental psychobiology, developmental
neurobiology, and
behavioral ecology: Mechanisms and early principles: Vol. 12.
Handbook of
behavioral neurobiology. New York: Plenum Press.
Wolf, A. (1995). Sexual attraction and childhood association: A
Chinese brief for
Edward Westermark. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
inferences.
American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
Copyright of International Journal of Behavioral Development
is the property of Sage
Publications Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed
to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

More Related Content

Similar to httpjcc.sagepub.comPsychology Journal of Cross-Cultur.docx

Article
ArticleArticle
Article
Akhilasasi
 
Cognitivist Approach on Gender
Cognitivist Approach on GenderCognitivist Approach on Gender
Cognitivist Approach on Gender
Ghulam Ghaus
 
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docxchapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
tiffanyd4
 
Ppt ch11 parenting_socialization
Ppt ch11 parenting_socializationPpt ch11 parenting_socialization
Ppt ch11 parenting_socializationAnna Montes
 
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is importantEssay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
Erin Anderson
 
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docxHi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
pooleavelina
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
simonithomas47935
 
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wiley
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wileypsy216_final_project_paper_jamie wiley
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wileyjamie wiley
 
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docxCognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
mary772
 
Gender and Society 1-1.pdf
Gender and Society 1-1.pdfGender and Society 1-1.pdf
Gender and Society 1-1.pdf
MelverADangpilen
 
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docxCopyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
dickonsondorris
 
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docxCopyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
bobbywlane695641
 
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdfADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
CatalinaAguiar2
 
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
SONU61709
 

Similar to httpjcc.sagepub.comPsychology Journal of Cross-Cultur.docx (14)

Article
ArticleArticle
Article
 
Cognitivist Approach on Gender
Cognitivist Approach on GenderCognitivist Approach on Gender
Cognitivist Approach on Gender
 
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docxchapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
chapter 8 Emotional and Social Development in Early ChildhoodDur.docx
 
Ppt ch11 parenting_socialization
Ppt ch11 parenting_socializationPpt ch11 parenting_socialization
Ppt ch11 parenting_socialization
 
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is importantEssay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
Essay On School Lunches. Why School Lunch is important
 
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docxHi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
Hi Jason,Thank you for submitting your unit 2 paper. SafeAssign .docx
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
 
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wiley
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wileypsy216_final_project_paper_jamie wiley
psy216_final_project_paper_jamie wiley
 
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docxCognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
Cognitive Development 21 (2006) 420–440Heterosexual adoles.docx
 
Gender and Society 1-1.pdf
Gender and Society 1-1.pdfGender and Society 1-1.pdf
Gender and Society 1-1.pdf
 
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docxCopyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
 
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docxCopyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
Copyright © 2002 American Psychological SocietyCURRENT D.docx
 
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdfADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
ADOLESCENT FRIENDSHIPS AND PEER.pdf
 
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
1. As a child did you eat food items with spokes-characters If so.docx
 

More from wellesleyterresa

Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docxHw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docxHW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docxHW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docxHW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docxHW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docxHW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docxHW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii) .docx
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii)        .docxHW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii)        .docx
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii) .docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docxHW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docxHW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docxHussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docxhw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docxHUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docxHW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docxHW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docxHunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HW 2 - SQL The database you will use for this assignme.docx
HW 2 - SQL   The database you will use for this assignme.docxHW 2 - SQL   The database you will use for this assignme.docx
HW 2 - SQL The database you will use for this assignme.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docxHumanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
wellesleyterresa
 
HURRICANE KATRINA A NATION STILL UNPREPARED .docx
HURRICANE KATRINA  A NATION STILL UNPREPARED   .docxHURRICANE KATRINA  A NATION STILL UNPREPARED   .docx
HURRICANE KATRINA A NATION STILL UNPREPARED .docx
wellesleyterresa
 
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docxHumanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
wellesleyterresa
 

More from wellesleyterresa (20)

Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docxHw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
Hw059f6dbf-250a-4d74-8f5e-f28f14227edc.jpg__MACOSXHw._059.docx
 
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docxHW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
HW in teams of 3 studentsAn oil remanufacturing company uses c.docx
 
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docxHW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
HW 5.docxAssignment 5 – Currency riskYou may do this assig.docx
 
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docxHW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
HW#3 – Spring 20181. Giulia is traveling from Italy to China. .docx
 
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docxHW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
HW 2Due July 1 by 500 PM.docx
 
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docxHW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
HW 4 Gung Ho Commentary DUE Thursday, April 20 at 505 PM on.docx
 
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docxHW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
HW 5 Math 405. Due beginning of class – Monday, 10 Oct 2016.docx
 
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii) .docx
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii)        .docxHW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii)        .docx
HW 5-RSAascii2str.mfunction str = ascii2str(ascii) .docx
 
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docxHW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
HW 3 Project Control• Status meeting agenda – shows time, date .docx
 
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docxHW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
HW 1January 19 2017Due back Jan 26, in class.1. (T.docx
 
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docxHussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
Hussam Malibari Heckman MAT 242 Spring 2017Assignment Chapte.docx
 
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docxhw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
hw1.docxCS 211 Homework #1Please complete the homework problem.docx
 
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docxHUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
HUS 335 Interpersonal Helping SkillsCase Assessment FormatT.docx
 
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docxHW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
HW #1Tech Alert on IT & Strategy (Ch 3-5Ch 3 -5 IT Strategy opt.docx
 
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docxHW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
HW 2 (1) Visit Monsanto (httpwww.monsanto.com) again and Goog.docx
 
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docxHunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
Hunters Son Dialogue Activity1. Please write 1-2 sentences for e.docx
 
HW 2 - SQL The database you will use for this assignme.docx
HW 2 - SQL   The database you will use for this assignme.docxHW 2 - SQL   The database you will use for this assignme.docx
HW 2 - SQL The database you will use for this assignme.docx
 
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docxHumanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
Humanities Commons Learning Goals1. Write about primary and seco.docx
 
HURRICANE KATRINA A NATION STILL UNPREPARED .docx
HURRICANE KATRINA  A NATION STILL UNPREPARED   .docxHURRICANE KATRINA  A NATION STILL UNPREPARED   .docx
HURRICANE KATRINA A NATION STILL UNPREPARED .docx
 
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docxHumanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
Humanities 115Short Essay Grading CriteriaExcellentPassing.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Celine George
 
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdfspecial B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
Special education needs
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Jean Carlos Nunes Paixão
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
Peter Windle
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Po-Chuan Chen
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
Delapenabediema
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Tamralipta Mahavidyalaya
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
heathfieldcps1
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
JosvitaDsouza2
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Balvir Singh
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
EugeneSaldivar
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech RepublicPolish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Anna Sz.
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
camakaiclarkmusic
 
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Jheel Barad
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
 
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdfspecial B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
special B.ed 2nd year old paper_20240531.pdf
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 5pptx.pptx
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
 
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech RepublicPolish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
 
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptxInstructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
Instructions for Submissions thorugh G- Classroom.pptx
 

httpjcc.sagepub.comPsychology Journal of Cross-Cultur.docx

  • 1. http://jcc.sagepub.com Psychology Journal of Cross-Cultural DOI: 10.1177/0022022194252002 1994; 25; 181 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Deborah L. Best, Amy S. House, Anne E. Barnard and Brenda S. Spicker Effects of Gender and Culture Parent-Child Interactions in France, Germany, and Italy: The http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/25/2/181 The online version of this article can be found at: Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology at: can be foundJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Additional services and information for http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:
  • 2. http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions: http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/25/2/181 Citations at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://www.iaccp.org/ http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/25/2/181 http://jcc.sagepub.com from the SAGE Social Science Collections. All Rights Reserved. at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 3. http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 4. http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com at WALDEN UNIVERSITY on June 8, 2010 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://jcc.sagepub.com http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/01650254.html Perspectives on gender development Eleanor E. Maccoby Stanford University, California, USA
  • 5. Two traditional perspectives on gender development—the socialisation and cognitive perspectives— are reviewed. It is noted that although they deal quite well with individual differences within each sex with regard to degree of sex-typing, they do not offer satisfactory explanations for some of the most robust gender dimorphisms: namely, gender segregation and the divergent patterns of interaction within all-male as compared with all-female dyads or groups. These patterns are brie�y summarised, and their similarity to those found in nonhuman primates and other mammals is noted. It is argued that an ethological perspective, and its modern successor the psychobiological perspective, are needed, along with the more traditional perspectives, to provide a comprehensive account of gender development as it occurs in dyads and groups as well as within individual children. In the last several decades there have been important shifts in psychologists’ thinking about gender development. There were empirical questions �rst of all: In what ways, to what degree, and how consistently, did boys and girls differ in the developmental pathways typically taken? And there have been notable changes in the points of view psychologists have brought to bear in their efforts to understand and explain whatever gender differentiation was thought to occur. Two viewpoints about gender development were dominant for many years: the socialisation perspective and the gender-cognitive perspective. These perspectives are �rst described below, and then their limitations are pointed out, stressing how narrow these views were concerning the nature of the gender- differentiated phenomena that need to be understood. The paper turns then to considering how much an ethological perspective, when added to the traditional pair, can contribute towards achieving a more comprehensive view of gender
  • 6. development. Finally, some more recent thinking from psychobiology is brought to bear in the interests of moving toward an integration of the several perspectives. The direct socialisation perspective At mid century, psychologists asked: By what processes do children become ‘‘sex-typed?’’ By sex-typing, they usually meant that children take on the attributes that are typical and/ or valued (expected, normative) for their own sex. In seeking answers, they worked from the stimulus-response (S-R) principles of the reinforcement learning theories that domi- nated the �eld of psychology at that time. From this point of view, sex-typed behaviours were a set of habits. Boys and girls would develop different sex-typed habits if socialisation agents—parents, teachers, older children—reinforced girls for ‘‘feminine’’ behaviours and provided negative consequences when they displayed behaviours thought to be more appro- priate for boys. Similarly, boys were thought to be ‘‘shaped’’ toward the version of ‘‘masculine’’ behaviour deemed proper in the particular society where the children were growing up. Implicit in this viewpoint is the idea that individual differences within each sex, and mean differences between the sexes on any given trait, are essentially re�ections of the same processes. Thus, it would be assumed that if one aspect of becoming masculine for a boy is to learn not to cry, then boys on the average would be subject to more socialisation pressures, that is, would be told, more often than girls, ‘‘That didn’t really hurt’’, or ‘‘It’s only a scratch’’, or ‘‘Don’t be a crybaby’’, or ‘‘Oh, toughen up!’’—and would develop stronger inhibitions against crying then would girls. At the same time, some boys would receive stronger, more consistent pressures of this kind than others, and so some would develop stronger crying inhibitions, and become more ‘‘masculine’’ than others.
  • 7. Differential socialisation pressures on boys and girls could take a variety of additional forms, beyond differential positive and negative reinforcement: for example, via providing different toys for children of the two sexes, or attributing different characteristics to them. And by giving boys and girls distinctive names and dressing them differently, their gender was announced to allcomers with the message: ‘‘This child is a boy/girl, to be treated accordingly.’’ Distinctive socialisation pressures on boys and girls were thought to begin at birth, to be fairly strong, and to be consistent in some respects throughout a given culture. A secondary perspective which in�uenced some of the developmental research at mid century was derived from psychodynamic theory. This theory was more developmental than S-R learning theory, which did not specify particular periods during growth that would be optimal for children’s acquisition of sex-typed behaviours. Freud’s theory of psycho- sexual development posited a transition at about the age of four or �ve, when the resolution of Oedipal con�icts would involve children’s identifying with their same-sex parent and thus taking on the appropriate sex-typed characteristics of that International Journal of Behavioral Development © 2000 The International Society for the 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 Study of Behavioural Development Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Eleanor E. Maccoby, Department of Psychology, Stanford University, 2130 Stanford, CA 94305–2130, USA. parent. This transition was thought to be more dif�cult for boys, who had to break a prior identi�cation with their
  • 8. mothers, and in becoming masculine would need to distance themselves from all things feminine. Echoes of this theory are found in the work of Chodorow (1978). In the 1950s and 1960s, Robert Sears and colleagues (Sears, Maccoby, & Levin, 1957; Sears, Rau, & Alpert, 1965) attempted to integrate S-R and psychodynamic theories in their studies of the relation- ships between parental child-rearing practices and the degree of children’s sex-typing. The results failed to con�rm Freudian theory. As Sears and colleagues wrote, in summarising their �ndings: ‘‘The box score for primary identi�cation theory as an explanation of gender role is poor’’ (Sears et al., 1965, p. 194). How well have the assumptions about children of the two sexes being socialised differently stood up empirically? It is certainly true that parents give their children sex-typed names, dress them differently, and decorate their rooms differently. Also, all languages of the world provide different ways of speaking about male and female persons. In English and other Indo-European languages, the sexes are distinguished by pronouns—he, she, his, hers—which surely facilitates chil- dren’s learning to code themselves and others as to gender. In addition, there are distinctive ways of speaking to children that emphasise stereotypical qualities, such as saying to a four-year- old ‘‘That’s my sweet little girl’’ or ‘‘There’s my big strong boy’’. Still, it has proved surprisingly dif�cult to document differential treatment of boys and girls by their parents, especially when children are young. Several reviews have summarised child-rearing practices used by parents with sons and daughters (mainly in modern Western societies). When it comes to the traditional dimensions of child rearing (e.g., permissiveness, restrictiveness, monitoring, responsiveness, warmth) few differences have been found in the way parents deal with sons as compared with daughters (Huston, 1983; Lytton & Romney, 1991; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Although it is often assumed that parents react differently to assertive
  • 9. behaviour by sons and daughters, and draw young daughters into greater emotional closeness with themselves than they do with young sons, evidence to date does not support these assumptions. There are some ways in which parents do consistently differentiate: they do more roughhousing with sons, offer dolls more often to girls (and toy trucks to boys), and talk about feelings more with girls (see Maccoby, 1998, for a summary of studies). And fathers in particular show negative reactions to any behaviour by their sons that seems effeminate. Of course, when socialisation differences are found, the ubiquitous issue of direction of effects arises. Do parents offer dolls to girls, trucks to boys, because they want their children to be appropriately ‘‘masculine’’ or ‘‘feminine’’, or because they have discovered that these are the toys the children prefer? If they roughhouse more with boys, is this because they have a stereotypical view of what kind of play boys ought to like, or because boys actually do like it more than girls and ‘‘train’’ their parents over time to play in ways that boys �nd most enjoyable? There is good evidence that boys’ and girls’ different initiatives can indeed evoke different reactions from their parents (see the summary in Maccoby, 1998). But, these reactions, in their turn, can then in�uence the children, so the existence of child-to-parent effects do not by any means preclude parent-to-child effects (Ge et al., 1996; O’Connor, Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & George, 1998). Experimental studies have been done in which unfamiliar children are given an arbitrary gender label. In such studies, an infant is chosen whose sex cannot be easily identi�ed when the child is dressed. Unfamiliar adults are then offered the opportunity to interact with the infant, or view the infant on videotape, the child having been introduced to some of the subjects as a girl, to others as a boy. The adults’ reactions to the child, and interpretations of the child’s behaviour, are then
  • 10. recorded. Such studies control for the direction-of-effects problem, because differential eliciting properties of male and female infants are ruled out by design. Although early studies suggested that adults’ reactions and interpretations were indeed in�uenced by the child’s gender label, a review of 23 gender-labelling studies (Stern & Karraker, 1989) found that overall effects were quite weak and quite inconsistent from study to study. No doubt there are subtle differences in parental treatment that are not captured in the rather coarse-grained net that researchers have cast, and certainly what seem to be rather minor differences can accumulate over many repetitions into signi�cant in�uences on children. And surely, socialisation differences would be more apparent in traditional societies where there are more rigid status differences between men and women. Still, we would have to say that to date, the socialisation theory that grew out of the S-R learning perspective of the mid century and stressed the role of parents as ‘‘shapers’’ of sex-typed behaviour rests on a weak empirical foundation. Of course, a socialisation perspective does not need to focus so exclusively on parents as the developmental psychology of the 1950s, 1960s, and 1970s tended to do. There were a number of reasons for this focus. First of all, much of the research at that time involved infants and children of preschool age, in families in which few mothers worked outside the home. Thus it was natural to assume that parents were indeed the most in�uential socialisation agents. In addition, there was a pervasive assumption that the early years were a time of great plasticity, when children were especially subject to ‘‘shaping’’ with respect to characteristics thought to be pervasive and long-lasting, such as the gendered aspects of the self. In recent years much more attention has been given to the in�uence of other socialisation agents, such as out-of-home caregivers,
  • 11. peers, teachers, and coaches, whose positive or negative reactions to children’s behaviour can provide additional shaping for children’s sex-role development in ways that can supplement—or sometimes even contradict—the in�uence of parents. In fact, to test a theory of direct socialisation adequately, it would be necessary not only to demonstrate that socialisation agents deal differentially with the two sexes, but that these different socialisation experiences are related to any differences in developmental trajectories that appear in male and female children. Much depends, then, on what aspects of gender enactment are chosen as ‘‘outcomes’’ in efforts to test socialisation theory. This issue will be considered more fully below. The indirect socialisation perspective Beginning in the 1960s, social learning theory added learning by imitation (often called modelling) to reinforcement as a powerful process involved in gender socialisation. In social learning theory, children were still seen as being shaped by direct positive and negative reinforcement. But it was also shown that children could learn vicariously from seeing how INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 399 400 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT other children’s gender-appropriate or gender-inappropriate behaviour were reacted to by others (Bandura, 1965). Apart from learning about the consequences of such behaviour, children could also learn by observation what behaviours were
  • 12. characteristic of each sex. And of course, they could learn these things not only from what they observed within their families, schools, and neighbourhoods, but from �lms or TV, or as depicted in stories (Mischel, 1966). The introduction of observational learning gave great added power to learning theory. But it also brought some complications. In daily life, boys and girls are exposed to models of both sexes. Both boys and girls presumably would learn the same facts concerning what behaviour is appropriate for boys, what for girls. The theory called for selective imitation, such that boys would adopt behaviour depicted by male models, or adapt their own behaviour according to the reinforcement patterns they saw being provided for male, rather than female, children. At the least, this required that children would know their own gender and that of the people whom they observed. Then too, it required that children should be able to summarise and generalise from multiple exemplars, and deal with exceptions. In addition, it called for some motivation to adopt the behaviour patterns of people who are ‘‘the same as me’’ with respect to gender. Clearly, the theory called for the extensive incorporation of cognitive elements in gender development. The cognitive perspective In the 1970s and 1980s, there was a strong surge of interest in gender cognitions. Although a cognitive perspective on developmental processes had been strongly anticipated in Europe for many decades, it was the ‘‘cognitive revolution’’ that took place in American psychology beginning in the 1960s that set the stage for an active revision of American views about child development. Children were increasingly seen not as passive recipients of socialisation pressures, but as active selectors and users of information pertinent to their develop- mental levels and personal goals. And children began to be seen as developing the capacity to adopt standards, and regulate their own behaviour in conformity to these standards, thus contributing to their own socialisation.
  • 13. Vast amounts of information are available to children concerning the way gender is enacted in the world around them. In the 1970s, 1980s, and continuing into the 1990s, research focused heavily on how children acquire knowledge and develop stereotypes and scripts concerning what is usual, or considered ‘‘appropriate’’, for people of the two sexes. (See Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a review.) It became evident that gender is a highly salient category for children, perhaps because it is neatly binary, because it is so heavily culturally emphasised, and because socially ascribed sex and biological sex are so completely redundant. The distinction, in other words, is easy to make, and there is good evidence that children do indeed make it very early in life. Gender categories, once applied, have been shown to be a convenient hook on which children can easily hang stereotypes about gender attributes (Gelman, Coleman, & Maccoby, 1986) and assimilate new incoming information to these stereotypes. Gender schema theories introduced in the early 1980s held that children form cognitive structures that organise their gender knowledge into a set of expectations that guide and organise their social perceptions (Bem, 1981; Martin & Halverson, 1981). Knowledge about the characteristics of the two sexes, and about the norms for their behaviour, is clearly necessary, but is it suf�cient by itself to motivate children to adopt socially prescribed roles and standards? Several hypotheses have been advanced concerning the ways in which children’s knowledge of their own gender identity and that of others could function to set motivational processes in motion: 1. When observing the contingencies that are experienced by other children, the observer is able to select the experiences of children of their own sex as most relevant to inferences about what might happen to themselves. This allows observational learning to be focused
  • 14. speci�cally on the acquisition of behaviours and stan- dards that apply differentially to the child’s own sex. 2. Out of a need for cognitive consistency, children want to adapt themselves (i.e., conform) to what they believe is appropriate for their own sex. Kohlberg (1966) proposed that this motivation would not appear until approxi- mately the age of 5–7, when he thought children achieve a �rm level of gender constancy. Because Kohlberg urged the importance of gender constancy, evidence has accumulated that the functional elements of gender constancy (namely, identity and stability; see Maccoby, 1990; Ruble & Martin, 1998) are achieved at consider- ably younger ages than Kohlberg believed. 3. When they have achieved a stable gender identity, children classify themselves as members of a same-sex group. They identify with this group, see members of the other sex as belonging to an outgroup, and want to be like members of their own-sex group. These possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and may all combine to generate the motivation for children to: (a) selectively imitate same-sex models (if they are known to be good exemplars of their gender category—see Perry & Bussey, 1979); (b) seek, select, and remember preferentially informa- tion that is relevant to, and consistent with, children’s own-sex schemas, and (c) reject, ignore, forget, or distort schema- inconsistent material (see Ruble & Martin, 1998, for a summary). With the acquisition of a stable gender identity, children can also begin to monitor their own behaviour with reference to a self-accepted standard of what is appropriate for their own sex (Bussey & Bandura, 1992). The socialisation and cognitive perspectives discussed so far combine into a kind of social constructivist approach. Insofar
  • 15. as children of the two sexes are found to differ in their behaviour, interests and/or value, it is thought that this differentiation comes about for three reasons: because adults shape children in this way; because peers shape each other according to the way they themselves have been socialised; and because children—once they have established a �rm gender identity—socialise themselves to conform to what they know to be stereotypical for children of their own sex, within the limits of what their own competencies permit (Bandura & Bussey, 1999). Sometimes, the use of the word ‘‘stereotypes’’ is taken to mean that attributes assimilated to social categories are arbitrary, so that our concepts about the two sexes may be quite arti�cial or distorted. How accurate are our gender stereotypes? In their review, Deaux and LaFrance (1998) note that it is hardly possible to check the reality base of some of our stereotypes, because they refer to characteristics that are very dif�cult to measure objectively. People’s beliefs about certain attributes of the two sexes that can be objectively assessed, however, turn out to be quite accurate (e.g., that women are more involved than men in the care of young children). This work has been done with adults, and we can only assume that the accuracy of children’s gender stereotypes would improve with age, as they accumulate information about a larger and larger sample of exemplars. At the end of the 20th century, then, a predominant perspective on gender development is a dual one focusing on individual differences. Its central themes are that children will differ in the degree to which they become sex-typed as a result of: (a) the strength of the socialisation pressures they have experienced; and (b) the nature and coherence of their gender
  • 16. schemas—their knowledge about the characteristics stereo- typically associated with each sex, and about what the social expectations are for persons of their own sex. Of course, socialisation and cognitive factors in gender development are not truly distinct. For example, socialisation pressures are one source of information enabling children to develop their knowledge concerning the gendered norms that they are expected to adopt. The direct socialisation experiences children have, in other words, constitute a major source of information upon which cognitive structures are built. Indeed, the whole cultural milieu in which a child grows up presents to children an array of cultural beliefs and practices concerning gender, and when children draw on these to construct their gender schemas, it can reasonably be said that they are being socialised by the surrounding culture into becoming co- practitioners of these cultural forms. An emphasis on cognitive and socialisation factors by no means precludes a recognition of possible biological in�uences that may generate different predispositions in boys and girls. Nowadays, there is widespread recognition of the importance of biological factors. However, as biological sex and socially ascribed gender are so completely redundant, it has proved dif�cult to tease them apart. Something is known concerning sex differences in brain structures and functions. For example, in males, more functions are lateralised, so that they are associated with activation primarily in one hemisphere of the brain, whereas in females, the two hemispheres are more likely to be both activated for a speci�c function. However, the possible behavioural impacts of these structural differences are far from being understood. A good deal is known concerning the physiological events during gestation that differentiate the genital structures of male and female fetuses. And we know, too, something about the way in which prenatal hormones organise the developing fetal brain so as to create different propensities and sensitivities in the two sexes—tendencies
  • 17. which will manifest themselves behaviourally at various times during postnatal development, perhaps requiring either a biological or environmental trigger for their activation. As noted above, most accounts of gender development do note possible biological underpinnings for some of the gender differentiation that occurs. And there is considerable interest in taking biological explanations one step backward to the genetic factors that may control biological differentiation. But we are a long way from having traced pathways from genes to the behavioural attributes that typically differentiate the sexes. In psychology, the great bulk of work has been concerned with the social and cognitive factors that are thought to underlie this differentiation, over and above what any biological predisposi- tions may call for. Limitations of these perspectives In the 1980s and 1990s, it began to be evident that these perspectives were not serving well, on the whole. First of all, it was increasingly clear that sex differences in children’s psychological attributes as usually measured were not sub- stantial, and �ndings were inconsistent from one study to another. Focusing on individual variation along dimensions such as ‘‘masculinity’’, ‘‘femininity’’, ‘‘androgyny’’, or ‘‘degree of sex-typing’’ was not turning out to be a strategy that accounted for much variance in behavioural outcomes. Although this might mean that gender simply is not an important factor in children’s daily lives, it could also mean that gender matters only in certain contexts, so that aggregat- ing across contexts attenuates gender-related phenomena that are in fact quite strong. If so, it would appear that research should turn to studies of moderating contexts. And, in addition, look for gender-related outcome variables that are more robust than ‘‘sex-typing’’ as we have measured it with toy- or activity-preference tests or clusters of personality traits.
  • 18. The socialisation and cognitive perspectives have proved disappointing in another respect: Empirical tests have failed to give consistent support for the predicted connections between processes and outcomes. As noted above, the similarities in the ways socialisation agents treat boys and girls far outweigh the differences. Still, some differences are found. Ruble and Martin’s comprehensive review of studies examining connec- tions between differential socialisation and sex-typed outcomes shows very meagre relationships. They say: ‘‘Although adults and peers treat boys and girls differently in many ways, especially concerning activities and interests, the role of these processes in children’s gender-related preferences and behaviors remains to be demonstrated’’ (Ruble & Martin, 1998, p. 982). In a similar vein, they note that variations among children in the level of their knowledge of gender stereotypes are generally unconnected to individual differences in sex-typed behaviour (e.g., Powlishta, 1995). This is by no means a fatal blow to cognitive theories of gender development, because in many respects gender cognitions are important in their own right, regardless of how and whether they ‘‘drive’’ individual differences in sex-typed behaviour. Furthermore, children’s understanding of gender identity—their own and that of other people—does appear to be somewhat connected to other aspects of gender development early in life. However, under- standing of gender identity is virtually complete in most children by the age of about 3, and hence (as it varies so little) cannot correlate with individual differences in sex-typing that emerge after approximately the �rst three years of life. Similarly, in some contexts and at some ages, there is very little individual variation in the choice of same-sex playmates, so that this aspect of sex-typing cannot be predicted from individual cognitive or personality characteristics.
  • 19. Clearly, a rethinking of the long-dominant perspectives described above is in order, and has begun. A major reorientation is a shift away from a focus on individual differences in outcomes, and an increasing focus on some robust outcomes for groups—gender effects which might appear primarily, or only, in certain speci�c social contexts. Robust gender phenomena in the context of groups What might these outcomes and contexts be? Indications began to emerge in the late 1970s that the answer could be INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 401 402 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT found in the context of pairs or larger groups of children engaged in social interaction. Jacklin and Maccoby (1978) observed 92 pairs of previously unacquainted children, all close to 33 months of age, as they interacted with each other and with toys. Tallies were kept of the instances of social behaviours (both positive and negative, verbal and nonverbal) each child directed toward his/her partner. Some of the pairs were composed of two boys, some of two girls, and some were mixed-sex pairs. Results were that children of both sexes directed about twice as much social behaviour toward partners who were of their own sex as they did to other-sex partners.1 It is notable that when data were computed without regard to the sex of the child’s partner, boys and girls displayed virtually identical levels of social behaviour. In other words, there was no overall sex difference in a personality dimension that might be called ‘‘sociability’’. This �nding underlines the fact that analyses which look only at the behaviour of individual
  • 20. children without regard to social context can totally obscure powerful gender phenomena. It is no surprise, then, that many simple comparisons between boys and girls have shown sex differences to be weak or absent. Theorists who have emphasised the importance of context, and variations in the salience of gender from one context to another, might rightly see the �ndings of the Jacklin and Maccoby study as a vindication of their position. But there is something very speci�c about the context that turned out to be important in this work: It was the sex of a child’s interactive partner that mattered, not context construed as environmental setting or prior priming conditions. The study points to the importance of group composition, and/or of relationships, in how gender is enacted. Astudy of preschoolers, conducted at about the same time, helps us to understand the above �ndings. Wasserman and Stern (1978) laid down a strip of carpet on a playroom �oor, and asked a child (sometimes a boy, sometimes a girl) to stand quietly at one end of it. Another child was then placed at the ‘‘starting’’ end of the strip, and asked to walk along it to the standing child. It turned out that children would walk up quite close to the standing child, facing directly forward, if the standing child was of their own sex. Approaching an other-sex child, however, both boys and girls would turn away as they approached and stop sooner. Notably, this occurred whether they were acquainted with the standing child or not, indicating that we are seeing here a form of other-sex avoidance that is not driven by previous experience with another child, but rather by the other child’s membership in a gender category. Perhaps we are seeing here an instance of the kind of self- regulation pointed to by cognitive social learning theorists, in which a child recognises the sex of another child, knows that it is considered inappropriate to associate with children of the other sex, and hence inhibits the approach to the other child. It
  • 21. appears, though, that children begin to show cross-sex avoidance at such a young age that they may not yet be able to code their own gender identity and that of other children reliably and explicitly. In traditional theories, there has never been a claim that the cognitions involved are conscious or deliberate, but still, gender-cognitive processes are assumed to occur at some level. An alternative possibility is that there is simply an uncognised raw emotion connected with gender categories. The early paper by Zajonc: ‘‘Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences’’ (1980) presented evidence that indeed preferences (liking/disliking, approach/avoidance) can be primary, immediate, without (or prior to) activation of related cognitions. Recent writings by Panksepp (1998), stress not only the immediacy of emotional reactions, but their deep mammalian origins. ‘‘Primal’’ emotions, including affective reactions to certain characteristics of same- or other-sex conspeci�cs, are claimed to be instinctive and species-wide. Given a pattern of wariness toward children of the other sex, and/or especial interest in, or compatibility with, own-sex other children, it is predictable that when children have a choice of playmates, they will congregate in same-sex pairs or groups. In childhood, there is a clear pattern of cross-sex avoidance and/ or same-sex preference that begins in about the third year of life and becomes progressively stronger though middle child- hood. Children’s tendency to congregate socially with others of their own sex has long been noted in the developmental literature, and has been thoroughly documented, in a variety of cultures and subcultures (see Hartup, 1983; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1987; and Ruble & Martin, 1998, for reviews). In modern Western societies, it is manifested most strongly in situations not structured by adults, though in more traditional societies the structures adults provide for children certainly contribute to it. The phenomenon of gender segregation in childhood is remarkably robust, with very little overlap, by the
  • 22. age of 5 or 6, between the distributions of the two sexes with respect to the gender of the other children with whom they spend their free social time (Maccoby, 1998). The students of gender segregation have been concerned with the factors that bring it about (see Leaper, 1994) and with the implications for how gender development should be studied. Clearly, one implication of gender segregation is that it is important to continue to study and understand the nature of the group processes that occur in all-male as compared to all-female dyads or groups, sustaining the progress that has already been made in this work. There is now considerable evidence that the groups or dyads formed by girls, as compared with boys, differ with respect to the agendas they enact, and in their prevailing interaction ‘‘styles’’. The nature of these differences has been summarised elsewhere (Maccoby, 1998; Ruble & Martin, 1998). Here it is suf�cient to note a few dominant trends: 1. The themes that appear in boys’ fantasies, in the stories they invent, the scenarios they enact when playing with other boys, and the �ctional fare they prefer (books and TV) involve danger, con�ict, destruction, heroic actions, and trials of physical strength. Girls’ fantasy and play themes tend to be oriented around domestic or romantic scripts, portraying characters who are involved in social relationships and depicting the maintenance or restora- tion of order and safety. 2. Interaction among boys, more often than among girls, involves rough-and-tumble play, competition, con�ict, ego displays, risk-taking, and striving for dominance. Girls, by contrast, are more responsive to the inputs of their interactive partners, more likely to use suggestions rather than imperative demands, and more likely to construct collaborative scripts in which the actions of
  • 23. play characters are reciprocal (see Leaper, 1991, on girls’ collaborative discourse style). This does not imply that girls do not assert their own objectives, or that their interactions are con�ict-free, only that they seek their 1 These results were replicated in a study of English children ranging in age from 19 to 39 months (Lloyd & Smith, 1986). individual goals in the context of also striving to maintain group harmony (see Sheldon, 1992). 3. Girls’ and boys’ friendships are qualitatively different, girls’ friendships being more intimate in the sense that friends share information about the details of their lives and concerns, whereas boys typically know less about their friends’ lives and base their friendships on shared activities. The break-up of girls’ friendships is more emotionally intense than for boys’ friendships. By age 6, too, boys typically play in larger groups. Benenson, Apostolaris, and Parnass (1997) showed, in a study of same-sex six-child groups, that between the ages of 4 and 6, boys greatly increased the time they spent in coordinated group activities, so that by the age of 6, they were spending 74% of their time in such activities. No such increase occurred for girls, whose coordinated group activities dropped below 20% of their time at age 6. And, girls have been found to show more enjoyment than boys when engaged in dyadic interaction (Benenson, 1993), whereas this differential is not found for interaction in larger groups. Girls, too, are found to sustain longer bouts of interaction in dyads than do boys. In other words, girls actively seek, prefer, and elaborate dyadic interactions, whereas boys do not appear to �nd such
  • 24. interactions to be especially gratifying, and instead gravitate toward coordinated activities carried out in larger groups. There is evidence, too, that boys’ groups are not only larger, but also stronger in some sense, that is, more cohesive, with stronger ingroup identi�cation and stronger boundaries, in the sense of more strongly excluding both girls and adults (see Maccoby, 1998, for an elaboration of these processes). The fact that boys congregate in larger groups has important implications. When in dyads, children of both sexes are relatively noncompetitive, and more emotionally supportive of their interactive partners, than they are when participating in larger groups (see Benenson, Nicholson, Waire, Roy, & Simpson, in press), something that was noted many years ago in interactions among adults (Bales & Borgotta, 1955). Can it be, then, that the greater competitiveness and lesser positive intimacy in male-male interaction can be accounted for by boys’ being more often in larger play groups?Perhaps so, but the fact that they congregate in larger groups may itself be a re�ection of their preference for certain forms of competitive but coordinated activity that can only be performed in larger groups. Indeed, boys form coalitions to achieve group goals— and gain group power thereby—to an extent that girls seldom do. The above account suggests that the two sexes are pursuing different agendas in their same-sex groups. But children in groups are not always engaged in enacting these differentiated agendas, and when they are not, male-male and female-female interactions can be much alike. It should be noted, too, that researchers have not yet spelled out the developmental time- line for these aspects of gender differentiation. Thus, although there is reason to have a good deal of con�dence in their occurrence, it is not yet clear how they wax and wane, and whether there are privileged sequences such that some processes need to occur before others can come into play.
  • 25. The ethological perspective Ethology originally referred to the study of animal behaviour, and a guiding perspective in ethological research was an evolutionary one. The principle of natural selection was invoked to explain species-wide adaptive behaviours, such that each animal species was equipped to survive in its particular environmental niche. As the perspective began to be applied to human behaviour, it was of great interest to trace similarities between humans and nonhuman primates, their closest relatives on the evolutionary tree. In the 1950s and 1960s, John Bowlby (1969) drew on the work of Harlow (1961) and Hinde (1966) to show striking resemblances between the patterns of attachment behaviour in human infants or toddlers and what was seen in young monkeys and apes. In the 1970s, work by Blurton-Jones (1972) and Strayer (1977; Strayer & Strayer, 1978) focused on patterns of children’s play, social dominance, aggression, and peer af�liation, identifying further parallels with the young of nonhuman primates. In observing sex differences in behaviours of this kind, ethologists do not look for generalised ‘‘trait’’ differences, but rather for the situations under which a difference does or does not appear, and the form the behaviour takes. It would not be meaningful to an ethologist, for example, to ask whether male ungulates are more aggressive than female ungulates. Rather, they would note that both males and females attack predators, and do so in the same way: with their hooves. Only males, however, �ght over territory and mates, and they use their horns, rather than their hooves, to attack or threaten other males. In a similar way, in studying human children, ethologists look for speci�c behavioural topography, and speci�c contexts in which sex differentiation is seen. Striking parallels between human children and the young of other primates have indeed been found, and this fact has been
  • 26. interpreted as pointing to evolved, genetically guided under- pinnings for certain elements of human behaviour. The evolutionary history of human development continues to be a matter of great interest up to the present time (see Geary & Bjorklund, 2000, for a review). Throughout, thinkers involved in this work have been concerned with what adaptive purpose an evolved behaviour pattern might serve. In what way might an evolutionary perspective be pertinent to gender development? Most evolutionary adaptations, after all, are seen as species-wide, occurring in both sexes. However, in bisexual species, the distinctive roles of the two sexes in reproduction is thought to have produced different adaptive behaviours (i.e., different strategies for mate selection, and differential involvement in the rearing of young). With respect to these domains, then, the two sexes are seen as distinct subspecies. Might evolution have anything to do with gender differ- entiation that occurs in childhood, before the age when the activities of mate selection and care of offspring emerge? The fact that young monkeys and apes separate into same-sex playgroups, and display some of the same sex-differentiated playstyles as those seen in human children, strongly suggests that there is indeed an evolutionary basis for these behaviours. So does the fact that there appears to be substantial uniformity in these patterns across human cultures. Geary and Bjorklund (2000) say: ‘‘From an evolutionary perspective, these sex differences are predicted to be a re�ection of and a preparation for sex differences in adult reproductive activities’’ (p. 60). Thus, boys’ competitiveness and dominance strivings are seen as preparation for adult male competition over mates, whereas girls’ greater social responsiveness and cooperativeness with other girls can be seen as preparation for participation in the kin-based social groups of females in which most rearing of
  • 27. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 403 404 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT the young occurs, in nonhuman primates as well as— presumably—in the hunter-gatherer bands where some under- pinnings of present-day human characteristics evolved. In what possible way could sex segregation in childhood serve an adaptive purpose, from the standpoint of the subsequent successful reproductive activities as adults? One hypothesis is: It functions to prevent incest, with its attendant risks for expression, in offspring, of genetic defects carried on recessive genes (see Maccoby, 1998, and Wolf, 1995, for elaboration and evidence). According to this account, humans are predisposed to lose sexual interest in anyone with whom they have been closely associated in childhood, such as siblings and other close relatives. Thus, paradoxical though it may seem, cross-sex avoidance in childhood can be seen as a means of fostering or safeguarding future heterosexual attraction. It is more dif�cult to see how some of the other sex- differentiated patterns we have noted might contribute to individual reproductive success and viability of an individual’s offspring. Rather, it would seem timely to expand the ecological perspective to include group success, group viability. Among chimpanzees, and in the human societies closest to the way our ancestors probably lived (Collier & Risaldo, 1981), males form coalitions to engage in cooperative group hunting and group warfare,2 enterprises not directly related to individual reproductive success, but relevant to survival of the troupe. Some of the processes seen in boys’ groups, then,
  • 28. may have the function of regulating hostility and competition among group members in the interests of allowing cooperative group enterprises to emerge. The psychobiological perspective The ethological perspective largely describes instinctive ele- ments in a species’ behavioural repertoire (i.e., behavioural dispositions assumed to be governed directly by evolved genetic programmes). From this perspective, individual life experience and environmental contexts have little in�uence, other than to provide the innate environmental triggers required to ‘‘release’’ an instinctive behaviour. In fact, however, there are many aspects of the environment, other than innate releasers, that function jointly with genetic factors to in�uence behavioural development. Many years of research have revealed that the way genetic instructions are carried out depends on environmental inputs at every stage of develop- ment from conception to maturity. The modern psychobiological view is that genes (G) and environment (E) have a bidirectional, reciprocal relationship, and cannot properly be understood as separate components whose effects can be independently estimated and then compared or summed (Gottlieb, Wahlsten, and Lickliter, 1998). G E interactions are widespread, and recent work has begun to �ll in the gap between genes and behavioural outcomes, by showing how both environment (e.g., conditions of rearing) and an allele of a given gene can affect a speci�c intervening biochemical process, which then in�uences beha- vioural outcomes (Anisman, Zaharia, Meany, & Merali, 1998; Suomi, in press; and see Maccoby, 2000, for a summary). The role of environment, as it interacts with genetic predispositions, is much more complex than simply providing a releaser for an instinctive response. In both rodents and primates, it has been shown that animals coming from different genetic strains will
  • 29. manifest their different predispositions only under certain early rearing conditions (i.e., being deprived of contact with maternal animals or peers—see Hood & Cairns, 1989; Suomi, 1997). And for some songbirds, a young male will not acquire the species-speci�c male courting song unless he is reared in the company of older females, who ‘‘train’’ him by responding selectively to the elements of song that females of the species �nd most compelling (West & King, in press). Conditions of rearing matter, too, in how different the two sexes become. Wallen (1996) has summarised a series of studies with rhesus monkeys, showing that males display elevated levels of threat/aggression toward other animals if they were earlier raised with only limited access to peers, whereas such limited access increases the amount of submissive, not aggressive, behaviour in females. For males, the amount of rough play is also affected by the social conditions of rearing, although for females, the level of such play remains low regardless of conditions of rearing. When females are prenatally exposed to androgens (late in gestation, or over an extended period), they subsequently show elevated levels of rough play, although there is no such effect for males. Clearly, both biology and conditions of rearing are important, but differently for the two sexes. Wallen notes that it is the sex- dimorphic behaviours most strongly affected by prenatal hormones in a given sex that show the least effects of rearing conditions for that sex. And he concludes: ‘‘These studies demonstrate that the expression of consistent juvenile beha- vioral sex differences results from hormonally induced predis- positions to engage in speci�c patterns of juvenile behavior whose expression is shaped by the speci�c social environment experienced by the developing monkey’’ (Wallen, 1996, p. 364). Humans are very different from birds, mice, and monkeys, but nevertheless there are useful parallels here. We see that
  • 30. whatever differential predispositions boys and girls may have, it is likely that the way they are enacted will depend greatly on the social conditions provided by the adults and peers with whom they interact. Societies differ with respect to how much time children of each sex spend with adults, with peers of their own sex, and with peers or siblings of the other sex. They also differ with respect to how much autonomy peer groups have at what developmental periods. These cultural variations may be expected to produce variations in the degree and kind of sex differentiation that appears as children grow up in different societies. Integrating perspectives The above account is meant to show that there is a good case for including the ethological and psychobiological perspectives in any attempt to understand gender differentiation in child- hood. These perspectives are not meant to replace the socialisation and cognitive perspectives, only to enrich them by expanding our view of how biological and experiential and cognitive factors work together when it comes to the enactment of gender. The two biological perspectives are especially useful in helping us to understand any characteristics where between- sex differences are robust and consistent across cultures and even across species. However, there is considerable variation within each sex and among cultural groups in the nature of 2 For information on male hunting and warfare in chimpanzees, see: Boesch and Boesch, 1989; McGrew, Marchant, and Nishida, 1996; and Stanford, Wallis, Matama, and Goddall, 1994.
  • 31. gendered behaviour displayed and the contexts in which it appears. Although some boys, for example, establish a network of good male friends, and participate actively in male group activities, others are loners or peripheral ‘‘hangers-on’’ to these groups, and still others are the victims of teasing and humiliation by other boys. Some children join peer groups that are basically prosocial, others associated mainly with same-sex peers who engage in risky, antisocial behaviour. Among girls, too, there is variation in how fully they participate in ‘‘girl culture’’, and in how much interest they have in less ‘‘feminine’’ activities such as team sports. There is evidence that these individual differences re�ect developed differences in competencies or vulnerabilities acquired at earlier periods of development—differences which in their turn undoubtedly re�ect both within-sex genetic variability and individual socialisation histories. Thus, the socialisation and cognitive perspectives should be especially pertinent to the under- standing of such within-sex variation. In the last century, there have been substantial changes in gender roles and the relationships between the sexes, changes that have occurred much too rapidly to be explained in genetic terms. These changes underscore how large the social factors are in such matters as the relative dominance and power of the two sexes, and make it clear that these matters are indeed open to change and not built in to human nature as a result of our species evolution. The above review has suggested that male power in society stems in no small degree from male groups and male alliances, despite the internal con�icts and competi- tion that characterise male interaction. The paradox is this: We have seen that girls and women have especially strong interactive skills that support collaboration and cooperation in their close relationships. Yet, women’s social groupings do not appear to yield power in out-of-home contexts to the extent that men’s do. Perhaps it will be possible to gain insight into
  • 32. this issue with more focused developmental research on the way power is exercised within same-sex groups and between the sexes. The fuller incorporation of the ethological and psychobio- logical perspectives on gender into our existing frameworks should enrich the research agenda of students of social development. More detailed observation of children in groups is called for, with more attention to the agendas that children of the two sexes seem motivated to enact, and more careful delineation of how gender-related patterns change with age. Of especial interest are the changes that occur in gender enactment during the transition into adolescence. Useful cues for how such work can be carried out comes from pioneers in neighbouring disciplines (e.g., the sociologists Maltz & Borker, 1982 and Barrie Thorne, 1986; and sociolinguist Penelope Eckert, 1996, 2000). Developmental psychology is in the process of building a coherent social psychology of childhood. Considerable atten- tion has been focused on individual children as members of groups—their acceptance or rejection by peers, their group entry skills—and the causes and consequences of individual variation in these things. In addition, there has been some work on the nature of groups, apart from the individuals who make them up. In their comprehensive review, Rubin, Bukowski, and Parker (1998) distinguish group processes from the interac- tions that occur within dyadic relationships, noting that groups can have properties that individuals and dyads cannot have (e.g., hierarchies, density of relationships, norms). Studies of ‘‘crowds’’ and ‘‘cliques’’ (e.g., Brown, 1990) underscore the importance of shared norms in the in�uence peer groups have on their members, and Kinderman (1993) has shown that a group can have its own identity over and above the identity of its members, that is, a group norm persists throughout a school
  • 33. year despite considerable turnover in the membership of the group. It is time for gender to take a more central place than it has occupied so far in such work, with more consistent attention to the gender composition of dyads and groups. Only with this knowledge in hand will we be able to understand the role of peers as gender-socialisation agents, and the way in which children build shared gender cognitions that can serve either to amplify or dampen the gender differentiation of roles and status. Manuscript received June 2000 Revised manuscript received June 2000 References Anisman, H., Zaharia, M.D., Meany, M.J., & Merali, Z. (1998). Do early life events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 149– 164. Bales, R.F., & Borgatta, E.F. (1955). Size of group as a factor in the interaction pro�le. In A.P. Hare, E.F. Borgatta & R.F. Bales (Eds.), Small groups: Studies in social interaction (pp. 496–512). Toronto: Random House. Bandura, A. (1965). In�uence of models’ reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 589–595. Bem, S.L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing.
  • 34. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364. Benenson, J.F. (1993). Greater preference among females than males for dyadic interaction in early childhood. Child Development, 64, 544– 555. Benenson, J.F., Apostolaris, N.H., & Parnass, J. (1997). Age and sex differences in dyadic and group interaction. Developmental Psychology, 33, 538–543. Benenson, J.F., Nicholson, C., Waire, A., Roy, R., &Simpson, A. (in press). The in�uence of size of social context on children’s competitive behaviour. Child Development. Blurton-Jones, N. (Ed.) (1972). Ethological studies of child behaviour. London: Cambridge University Press. Boesch, C., & Boesch H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Tai National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547–573. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Brown, B.B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S.S. Feldman & G.R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1992). Self-regulatory mechanisms
  • 35. governing gender development. Child Development, 63, 1236–1250. Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social-cognitive theory of gender develop- ment and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713. Chodorow, N. (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis and the sociology of gender. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Collier, J., & Risaldo, S. (1981). Politics and gender in simple societies. In S.B. Ortner & H. Whitehead (Eds.), Sexual meanings. New York: Cambridge University Press. Deaux, K. & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindsey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1). New York: McGraw Hill. Eckert, P. (1996). Vowels and nailpolish: The emergence of linguistic style in the preadolescent heterosexual marketplace. In J. Ahlers, L. Bilmes, M. Chen, M. Oliver, N. Warner, & S. Werhsteim (Eds.), Gender and belief systems. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Women and Language Group. Eckert, P. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice. Cambridge, UK: Blackwell.
  • 36. Ge, X., Conger, R., Cadoret, R., Niederhiser, J., Yates, W., et al. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual in�uence model of child anti-social behavior and parent behavior. Developmental Psychology, 32, 574–589. Geary, D.C., & Bjorklund, D.F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychol- ogy. Child Development, 71, 57–65. Gelman, S.A., Coleman, P., & Maccoby, E.E. (1986). Inferring properties from categories, versus inferring categories from properties: The case of gender. Child Development, 57, 396–404. Gottleib, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (1998). The signi� cance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2000, 24 (4), 398–406 405 406 MACCOBY / PERSPECTIVES ON GENDER DEVELOPMENT W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development. (5th ed.), New York: Wiley.
  • 37. Harlow, H.F. (1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In B.M. Foss (Ed.), The determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 1). London: Methuen. Hartup, W. (1983). Peer relations. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.) and E.M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed.). New York: Wiley. Hinde, R.A. (1966). Animal behaviour: A synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hood, K.E., & Cairns, R.B. (1989). A developmental genetic analysis of aggressive behavior in mice: 4. Genotype-environment interaction. Aggressive Behavior, 15, 361–380. Huston, A.C. (1983). Sex typing. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.) and E.M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 387–467). New York: Wiley. Jacklin, C.N., & Maccoby, E.E. (1978). Social behavior at 33 months in same- sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557–569. Kinderman, T.A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for individual
  • 38. development: The case of children’s motivation in school. Developmental Psychology, 29, 970–977. Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Leaper, C. (1991). In�uence and involvement in children’s discourse: Age, gender and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797–811. Leaper, C. (1994). Childhood gender segregation: Causes and consequences. New Directions for Child Development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey- Bass. Lloyd, B., & Smith, C. (1986). The effects of age and gender on social behaviour in very young children. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 33–41. Lytton, H., & Romney, D.M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267– 296. Maccoby, E.E. (1990). The role of gender identity and gender constancy in sex- differentiated development. In D. Schrader (Ed.), The legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg: No. 47. New directions for child development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
  • 39. Maccoby, E.E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Maccoby, E.E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On reading and misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 1–27. Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child behavior and development. New York: Academic Press. Maltz, D.N. & Borker, R.A. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J.A. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. 195–216). New York: Cambridge University Press. Martin, C.L., & Halverson, C.F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 54, 563–574. McGrew, W.C., Marchant, L.F., & Nichida, T. (1996). Great ape societies. New York: Cambridge University Press. Mischel, W. (1966). Asocial-learning view of sex differences in
  • 40. behavior. In E.E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. O’Connor, T.G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. (1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parenting. Develop- mental Psychology, 34, 970–981. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. Perry, D.G., & Bussey, K. (1979). The social-learning theory of sex differences: Imitation is alive and well. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1699–1712. Powlishta, K.K. (1995). Intergroup processes in childhood: Social categorization and sex-role development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 781– 788. Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J.G. (1998). Peer interactions, relation- ships, and groups. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.), (Vol. 3, pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley. Ruble, D.N., & Martin, C.L. (1998). Gender Development. In
  • 41. W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of chlid psychology (5th ed.), (Vol. 3, pp. 933– 1016). New York: Wiley. Sears, R.R., Maccoby, E.E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson. Sears, R.R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1965). Identi�cation and child rearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Sheldon, A. (1992). Con�ict talk: Sociolinguistic challenges to self-assertion and how young girls meet them. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 95– 117. Stanford, C.B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., & Goddall, J. (1994). Patterns of predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park, 1982–1991. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 94, 213–228. Stern, M., & Karraker, K.H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: A review of gender labeling studies. Sex Roles, 20, 501–522. Strayer, F.F. (1977). Peer attachment and af�liative subgroups. In F.F. Strayer (Ed.), Ethological perspectives on preschool social organization. Research memo, No. 5. Department of Psychologie, University of Quebec. Strayer, F., & Strayer, F.F. (1978). Social aggressive and power
  • 42. relations among preschool children. Aggressive Behavior, 4, 173–182. Suomi, S.J. (1997). Long-term effects of different early rearing experiences on social, emotional and physiological development in non-human primates. In M.S. Keshevan & R.M. Murra (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental models of adult psychopathology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Suomi, S.J. (in press). A biobehavioral perspective on develpmental psycho- pathology: Excessive aggression and serotonergic dysfunction in monkeys. In A.J. Samaroff, M. Lewis, & S. Miller (Eds.), Handbok of developmental psychopathology. New York: Plenum. Thorne, B. (1986). Boys and girls together . . . but mostly apart: Gender arrangements in elementary schools. In W.H. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and development. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum. Wallen, K. (1996). Nature needs nurture: The interaction of hormonal and social in�uences on the development of behavioral sex differences in rhesus monkeys. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 364–378. Wasserman, G.A., & Stern, D.N. (1978). An early manifestation of differential behavior toward children of the same and opposite sex. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 133, 129–137.
  • 43. West, M.J., & King, A.P. (in press). Science lies its way to the truth—really. In E. Blass (Ed.), Developmental psychobiology, developmental neurobiology, and behavioral ecology: Mechanisms and early principles: Vol. 12. Handbook of behavioral neurobiology. New York: Plenum Press. Wolf, A. (1995). Sexual attraction and childhood association: A Chinese brief for Edward Westermark. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175. Copyright of International Journal of Behavioral Development is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.