SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 162
Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice
2000, Vol. 4. No. 1,27-43
Copyright 2000 by the Educational Publishing Foundation
1089-2699/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2699.4.1.27
Leadership Research and Theory: A Functional Integration
Martin M. Chemers
University of California, Santa Cruz
This historical overview of leadership theory and research with
an eye for commonali-
ties provides an opportunity for integration. Early unproductive
research focused on
personality traits and behaviors. A recognition of the more
complex nature of the
phenomenon resulted in the development of contingency
theories that examined leader
characteristics and behavior in the context of situational
parameters. The 1970s brought
an awareness that perceptions of leaders by followers and
others, and perceptions of
followers by leaders, were influenced by cognitive biases
arising from prior expecta-
tions and information-processing schema. Ironically, attention
was belatedly drawn to
the study of female leaders, who were often the victim of
cognitive biases and negative
assumptions. Recent research has reflected on the role of
cultural differences in
leadership processes and has been drawn again into the search
for outstanding leaders
with universally effective characteristics. The article concludes
with an integration of
current knowledge in leadership effectiveness.
For much of its history, leadership theory and
the empirical supporting research have been
regarded as a fractured and confusing set of
contradictory findings and assertions without
coherence or interpretability. In this article T
argue that a considerable commonality of
well-accepted findings points the way toward a
successful and useful integration of current
knowledge. That integration, based on the key
functions performed by effective leaders, begins
to answer the question of how good leaders
behave and raises intriguing questions about the
personal characteristics of leaders that facilitate
those behaviors.
This article takes an historical perspective,
and the analysis is divided into four periods: (a)
the period prior to the presentation of Fiedler's
(1964) contingency model; (b) the period from
1965 to 1975, focusing on the development and
elaboration of contingency theories; (c) the
period from 1975 to 1985, when cognitive
theories and concerns about gender differences
arose; and (d) the period since 1985, which has
most extensively focused on transformational
theories and cultural influences. The historical
analysis is followed by a presentation of an
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Martin M. Chemers, Division of Social Sciences,
117 Social Sciences I, University of California, 1156 High
Street, Santa Cruz, California 95064. Electronic mail may be
sent to [email protected]
integrative framework and a suggested direction
for future research.
In this analysis leadership is defined as "a
process of social influence in which one person
is able to enlist the aid and support of others in
the accomplishment of a common task." This
definition places the subject distinctly within the
purview of social psychology, and the analyses
that follow reveal how fully the leadership
literature is indebted to the field of social
psychology for its dominant paradigms and
central variables.
Before Contingency Theory:
Lost in the Wilderness
Social philosophers have had a long-standing
interest in both organizational and political
leadership. Western European philosophers,
embedded in a strongly individualistic cultural
milieu, looked primarily to the characteristics of
leaders for explanatory premises, For example,
Carlyle (1841/1907) proposed the great man
theory of leadership, which argued that success-
ful leaders possessed traits of personality and
character that set them apart from ordinary
followers. The interest in individual characteris-
tics of leaders was spurred by the emergence of
intelligence tests in the early 20th century.
Empirical psychology turned toward the study
of traits, and the nascent leadership field
followed suit.
27
28 CHEMERS
Traits
A sort of naive popularist psychology guided
the choice of traits considered for leadership
impact. Traits that were stereotypically associ-
ated with leadership, such as dominance,
assertiveness, intelligence, physical stature, so-
cial sensitivity, and many others, became causal
candidates. The typical research format for these
early studies was to identify a group with
leaders and followers and test for differences on
the selected trait measures. Stogdill (1948)
provided an extensive review of 30 years of the
trait studies. He reported that a few traits (most
notably intelligence) were sometimes associated
with reliable differences between leaders and
followers (i.e., about 35% of the time), but there
was no single variable or even cluster of
variables that was related to leadership across a
variety of situations. Stogdill concluded that
although individual differences were certainly
important in identifying emergent or effective
leaders, the great diversity of situations in which
leaders functioned made it unlikely that any one
trait would be a universal predictor. Although it
was not immediately recognized, StogduTs
analysis set the stage for theories of leadership
that were predicated on an interaction between
leader traits and situational contingencies.
Behaviors and Styles
Daunted by the failure of traits to predict
leadership, but unwilling to abandon individual-
istic explanations, researchers turned to the
study of leader behavior. Observations of the
effects of leadership style (i.e., autocratic vs.
democratic) on the atmosphere of small groups
(Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939), process
analyses of interactions in laboratory discussion
tasks (Bales & Slater, 1955), and reports of
industrial workers on the behavioral styles of
their supervisors (Kahn, 1951) sought to iden-
tify patterns of leader behavior associated with
high productivity or good morale.
The most extensive of these research pro-
grams and the one with the most enduring
impact on the field of leadership was the set of
studies surrounding the development of the
Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire
(LBDQ) at Ohio State University (Hemphill,
1950). This 150-item behavioral inventory was
used to collect ratings of military and industrial
leaders by supervisors, subordinates, and observ-
ers. Subsequent factor analyses revealed that a
major portion of the variability in leader
behavior could be explained by two major
clusters (Halpin & Winer, 1957). The most
prominent factor, labeled Consideration, in-
cluded behaviors such as showing concern for
the feelings of subordinates, making sure that
minority viewpoints were considered in deci-
sion making, and attempting to reduce conflict
in the work environment. These behaviors
seemed to reflect leader intentions to support
positive group morale and follower satisfaction.
A strong second factor, labeled Initiation of
Structure, included items measuring the leader's
use of standard operating procedures, criticism
of poor work, and emphasis on high levels of
performance. These behaviors appeared to be
related to a leader's focus on building a structure
for task accomplishment.
Although the LBDQ factors were found
reliably in ratings of leader behavior across a
wide range of settings, they were less than
completely successful at predicting the impor-
tant outcomes associated with leadership effec-
tiveness, that is, follower satisfaction and group
performance (Fleishmann & Harris, 1962; Kor-
man, 1966). Considerate leadership was often
related to follower satisfaction or morale, and
Consideration and Initiation of Structure were
sometimes but not always predictive of group
performance. The failure of this carefully
constructed and comprehensively researched
behavioral measure to predict leadership led
many researchers to throw up their hands in
frustration and seemed to be yet another
instance of leadership research leading to no
coherent conclusions.
Legitimacy
One of the brightest spots in the early
empirical work on leadership was the series of
studies conducted by Hollander, which illumi-
nated some of the facets of leadership status
accrual and legitimacy. In both laboratory and
field research venues, Hollander (1964; Hol-
lander & Julian, 1970) found that individuals in
groups gain status through the demonstration of
task-related competence and loyalty to group
values. Status acquisition is associated with the
accrual of so-called "idiosyncrasy credits,"
which can be thought of as units of group
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
29
acceptance that can be "spent" to influence
others and provide leeway from group norms to
allow for innovation in group processes and
views.
This pioneering work on how individuals
decide to follow those in leadership positions
has retained its currency to the present day, in
part because it embodies both cognitive and
behavioral elements in its approach. Contempo-
rary information-processing theories of leader-
ship posit leadership "prototypes" that are
characterized by the elements of competency
and trustworthiness that are the bases for the
accrual of idiosyncrasy credits. Recent work by
Hogg and his associates (Hogg, Hains, &
Mason, 1998) applying social identity theory to
leadership perception indicates that, despite a
tendency for followers to value leaders who
embody group values (the basis for perceptions
of trustworthiness in Hollander's model), they
also heavily weight task relevant competence in
leadership evaluation. Those basic determinants
of leadership status turn up in more recent
approaches to understanding leadership judg-
ments. A fuller discussion of the role of
perception in leadership process appears later.
The Mid-1960s to the Mid-1970s:
The Contingency Era
The Contingency Model
The study of leadership took a dramatic
change of direction with the publication of
Fiedler's first articles (1964) and subsequent
book (1967), which presented a new approach to
understanding leadership effectiveness. The
contingency model of leadership effectiveness
emerged as an answer to StogduTs (1948) call
for an approach based on the interaction of
leader traits with situational parameters, but it
did not start out that way. Early work (Cleven &
Fiedler, 1956; Fiedler, 1955, 1958) tested the
predictive validity of a leadership trait measure
on the basis of the leader's views of coworkers.
The measure, which eventually came to be
known as the Least Preferred Coworker (LPC)
scale, differentiated leaders who viewed poorly
performing coworkers in very negative terms
(thought to reveal a very strong concern with
effective task performance) from those who
viewed poorly performing coworkers in less
negative terms (hypothesized to reflect a greater
emphasis on interpersonal relationships).
Apparently promising early work found that
task-oriented leaders were more effective, but
subsequent studies yielded results showing that
relationship-oriented leaders had more effective
teams. Confused but not deterred by these
anomalous findings, Fiedler reanalyzed a large
number of studies—this time classifying the
group settings in terms of the degree of support
and cooperation offered by followers, the clarity
and structure of the group's task, and the
leader's formal authority to direct and reward
followers. These three variables were combined
into a dimension of "situational favorableness"
(Fiedler, 1967) or "situational control" (Fiedler,
Chemers, & Maher, 1976), thought to reflect the
degree to which the overall situation gave the
leader a feeling of certainty, predictability, and
control over group processes.
When the leader's orientation (i.e., LPC
score) was correlated with group performance
across the dimension of situational favorable-
ness, a reliable relationship was found. Specifi-
cally, groups led by task-oriented leaders
performed best in situations of high control and
predictability or very low control and predictabil-
ity, and groups led by relationship-oriented
leaders performed best in the situations of
moderate control or predictability. The explana-
tory rationale for these findings was that the
relatively more directive, task-focused leader-
ship style is most appropriate when an orderly
situation provides the leader with the clarity to
give directions and the follower support to be
sure of his or her performance, and the highly
volatile and unpredictable environment of the
very low control situation also requires the
steadying influence of clear directions and
structuring leader behavior. However, the more
interpersonally oriented, participative style of
leadership was thought to function most effec-
tively when the complexities of a moderate-
control situation required greater delicacy to
navigate a poorly understood task or to avoid the
dangers associated with uncertain follower
support.
The inductive method by which the contin-
gency model was constructed and the highly
complex nature of its predictions led to many
criticisms of the model during the 1970s
(Ashour, 1973; Graen, Alvarez, Orris, & Mar-
tella, 1970). However, subsequent research and
30 CHEMERS
extensive meta-analyses (Peters, Hartke, &
Pohlmann, 1983; Strube & Garcia, 1981)
provided strong support for the basic principles
of the model. (For a more complete discussion
of the development of the contingency model
and the controversy surrounding its validity, see
Chemers, 1997.) Another criticism of the
contingency model was its apparent assumption
that a leader could not choose to be both task
and relationship oriented when the situation
demanded it. Some other contingency ap-
proaches did not make that same assumption.
Normative Decision Theory
Energized by the potential of a contingency
approach to explain leadership performance, but
working from a more deductive theoretical base,
Vroom and Yetton (1973) offered a model of
decision-making effectiveness that integrated
leaders' decision strategy with situational fac-
tors. Leaders were conceived as having a range
of decision-making strategies available to them
that varied in degree of follower involvement in
the process—ranging from autocratic styles
(leader makes the decision with minimal fol-
lower input) to consultative styles (leader makes
the decision after getting follower opinion and
advice) to group or participative styles (leader
and group make decision together, with equal
weight).
The situational parameters included in the
model were represented as a series of questions
arranged to yield a decision tree. Leaders
seeking the most effective decision strategy
were asked to analyze situational factors that
included the clarity and structure of the task and
surrounding information, the degree of support
for the leader and the organization among the
followers, the degree of conflict among subordi-
nates, and the time urgency for a decision to be
made. The model specifies that when the task is
clear and the followers supportive, the leader
should use the more time-efficient autocratic
styles. If the task or information is unclear, using
the consultative strategies increases the informa-
tion yield and likelihood of a higher quality
decision. When the leader lacks follower
support, the participative strategy helps to
ensure follower commitment to the decision and
its implementation. Empirical research on the
normative decision model is not extensive but is
generally supportive of its basic premises (Field
& House, 1990).
The contingency model and normative deci-
sion theory have many features in common.
They are both focused on the leader as the
central actor in the group's efforts to interface
with the task environment. Both theories regard
the leader's task as to gain the group's support in
solving problems and implementing solutions
effectively. Also, they both hypothesize that
more directive approaches will be most effective
when a clear task and a supportive group give
the leader the certainty to take charge but that
more participative strategies will work better
when a less clear and orderly environment
argues against bold action and autocratic direc-
tion. The two theories part company in the
situation of very low control with the contin-
gency model more focused on immediate group
performance through leader direct action but
normative decision theory suggesting more
participative strategies to build a more support-
ive environment over the long run.
Path-Goal Theory
Contingency theories held the promise of
correcting the weaknesses of earlier approaches
to leadership effectiveness prediction. For ex-
ample, one promising but disappointing ap-
proach had been the attempt to relate leader
behavior (e.g., the LBDQ score) to organiza-
tional outcomes. R. J. House and his associates
(R. J. House, 1971; R. J. House & Dessler, 1974;
R. J. House & Mitchell, 1974) picked up that
gauntlet and attempted to merge traditional
behavioral approaches with emerging develop-
ments in the study of worker motivation to
understand the impact of the leader on the
motivation and performance of followers.
Path-goal theory argues that the leader's
main purpose is to motivate subordinates by
helping mem to see how their task-related
performance could help them to achieve their
personal goals. Research within the path-goal
framework attempted to understand how a
leader's directiveness (i.e., Initiation of Struc-
ture) or supportiveness (i.e., Consideration)
behaviors might affect subordinate motivation
and performance. Proceeding logically, the
theory predicted that a leader's structuring
behavior would be motivating to a subordinate
when the subordinate's task environment lacked
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
31
structure because of insufficient training or
experience or a highly complex task. However,
when a subordinate had sufficient structure,
leader directiveness would be regarded as overly
close monitoring or "pushing" and would have
negative effects. Consideration behavior was
seen to have its most positive effects when the
subordinate needed psychological or emotional
support to deal with an aversive work environ-
ment (made so by a boring or unpleasant task).
Consideration was viewed as superfluous in
situations that were engaging and intrinsically
interesting to the subordinate.
The typical research paradigm for path-goal
theory studies was to divide a group of
subordinates into situations of low clarity
(presumably interesting, but potentially frustrat-
ing because of lack of structure) and of very
high clarity-predictability (presumably boring
and uninvolving). Leader structuring behavior
was predicted to have positive effects on
subordinate motivation and performance in the
former situation but not the latter, whereas the
reverse was true for leader considerate behavior.
Path-goal theory generated a considerable body
of empirical research support for the basic
propositions. Considerate behavior, for ex-
ample, was usually related to positive subordi-
nate attitudes under boring or aversive task
situations but often had similarly positive effects
across all situations. Results regarding structur-
ing behavior were even less consistent.
A study by Griffin (1981) that included
measures of subordinates * ' * growth need
strength" (Hackman & Oldham, 1976) indicated
that the needs and expectations of subordinates
played a role in determining when subordinates
needed or wanted different types of leader
behavior. Growth-oriented, challenge-seeking
subordinates were quite comfortable with un-
structured and challenging tasks and were
therefore less receptive to directive leader
behavior under unstructured or structured condi-
tions. However, these growth-oriented subordi-
nates were very responsive to supportive
behavior when the task was boring. The more
change-averse, low-growth-need subordinates
were comfortable with leader structure across all
situations but needed less support when a task
was ostensibly boring. A reasonable conclusion
to be drawn from this literature is that leader
behavior that is seen as supportive by subordi-
nates is likely to lead to positive reactions and
higher motivation and that both characteristics
of the task and of the subordinate will contribute
to that receptiveness.
In an interesting extension of path-goal
theory, Kerr and Jermier (1978) argued that if
the leader's purpose is to supply missing
elements in the subordinate's job environment
(e.g., structure or support), then other sources of
those missing elements might make the leader's
behavior redundant and unnecessary. Their
"substitutes for leadership" theory predicted,
for example, that if a job provided plenty of
task-relevant feedback, leader structuring behav-
ior would be unnecessary, or if a compatible and
cohesive work group provided emotional sup-
port, leader consideration would be redundant.
Under such conditions, leader behaviors were
hypothesized to show minimal or even negative
relationships with subordinate motivation, satis-
faction, or performance. However, a review of a
number of studies of substitutes-for-leadership
hypotheses indicated very little support for the
theory's predictions (Podsakoff, Niehoff, Mac-
Kenzie, & Williams, 1993) and revealed that a
leader's behavior remains very important to
subordinates regardless of varying situational
conditions.
The research literature on the contingency
theories suggests that actions by a group's leader
can have strong effects on the motivational and
emotional states of followers and on the
successful accomplishment of the group's task.
The relationship of the specific leader actions to
those outcomes depends on the interaction of
those actions with relevant features of the
interpersonal and task environment.
The Mid-1970s to the Mid-1980s:
Cognitive Models and Gender Concerns
The growing influence of cognitive theories
in social psychology led to a similar interest
among leadership researchers. Two broad classes
of investigation were concerned with percep-
tions of leaders by others (i.e., followers,
superiors, and observers) and leaders' percep-
tions and evaluations of subordinates.
Leadership Perceptions
In the mid-1970s, studies involving ratings of
leader behavior began to reveal certain anoma-
lous findings. Eden and Leviatan (1975) re-
32 CHEMERS
ported that when research participants were
asked to make ratings of leader behavior by
simply imagining a leader, the resultant data
showed factor structures similar to those derived
from ratings of actual leaders. Staw (1975)
showed two sets of observers the same video-
tape of a group interaction but told the observers
that the group had either been very successful or
very unsuccessful on task performance. Ob-
server ratings of the "successful" leader were
higher on measures of both directive and
supportive leadership than were the ratings of
the "unsuccessful" leader.
That ratings of leaders might be strongly
biased created a problem on both theoretical and
methodological grounds. Leader legitimacy, a
central construct in understanding the bases of
leader influence, was based on follower percep-
tions. Furthermore, almost every research para-
digm in the leadership field depended on ratings
of leader behavior (Rush, Thomas, & Lord,
1977).
Attribution theory (Jones & Davis, 1965;
Kelley, 1967) provided a theoretical framework
for the investigation of leadership biases. Taking
a very strong position, Calder (1977) argued that
the very concept of leadership is rooted in
popular language and poorly articulated as a
scientific construct. He argued that with no way
of measuring leadership apart from social
perceptions, leadership exists primarily as an
attribution rather than a testable construct and
should, therefore, be abandoned as a subject of
scientific inquiry. Few researchers were willing
"to throw the baby out with the bathwater" and
began instead to make a systematic study of
leadership perceptions and the processes that
gave rise to them.
A useful model was provided by the research
on implicit personality theories, which Hastorf,
Schneider, and Polefka (1970) defined as a
structure of association about what traits or
characteristics are related that guides and
organizes perceptions, thoughts, and memories
about a phenomenon. Implicit theories of
leadership, then, would define the assumptions
that people held about what behaviors leaders
displayed and how those behaviors were associ-
ated with group and organizational outcomes.
An extensive research program by Lord and
his associates (Lord, 1985; Lord, Binning, Rush,
& Thomas, 1978; Lord & Maher, 1991) revealed
that leadership attributions were based on two
processes. Recognition processes determined
when an individual's behavior would result in
the perception of that person as a leader.
Observers were found to hold highly articulated
prototypes (Cantor & Mischel, 1979; Rosch,
1978) of leadership. When an actor's behavior
showed sufficient overlap with the prototypi-
cally driven expectations of observers, a leader-
ship attribution was made. Once an individual
was seen as a leader, selective attention and
memory reinforced that judgment.
Leadership judgments were also found to be
influenced by inferential processes. Because
implicit theories of leadership associate team
success with effective leadership, observers are
likely to infer the presence of good leadership
from evidence of group success (Phillips &
Lord, 1981). Thus, once a person is seen as a
leader, observer inferences are likely to rein-
force and enhance that perception. Of course, if
a person's characteristics, such as gender or
race, are inconsistent with observers' prototypic
expectations, then such a person is less likely to
be perceived as a legitimate or effective leader
despite any objective achievements.
The strength of common beliefs in the
importance of leadership for group outcomes
led Meindl (1990) to develop the "romance
of leadership" concept. In an ingenious series
of experiments and naturalistic observations,
Meindl showed that any remarkable group or
organizational outcome, whether highly positive
or highly negative, is likely to be attributed to
leadership effects, while other reasonable causes
are largely ignored.
Although the strong susceptibility to percep-
tual biases in the observations of leadership
might have constituted a problem for research
methodology, it opened a fascinating area for
theoretical development. If leadership is a
process of social influence, then factors that
affect the legitimacy, credibility, and influence
of leaders become a central aspect of leadership
function.
Another important component of the leader-
ship process involves the perception of follow-
ers by leaders. Almost every theory of leader-
ship posits that a central function of leadership
involves the direction of subordinates. The
follower-oriented contingency theories, such as
path-goal theory, maintain that it is the leader's
responsibility to provide the subordinate with
task-directed guidance or emotional support to
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
33
help them to be effective and satisfied. Implicit
in these premises is the expectation that leaders
are able to judge what kinds of behaviors on
their part are likely to have positive effects on
subordinates. In other words, leaders must
observe the actions and reactions of subordi-
nates to judge what is needed. This clearly
places attributionai processes at the center of the
relationship between leader and follower.
Mitchell and his associates (Green & Mitchell,
1979; Mitchell Larson, & Green, 1977; Mitchell
& Wood, 1980) applied Kelley's (1967) attribu-
tion model to leader evaluations of subordinates
and the effects that those evaluations have on
subsequent leader actions. That research re-
vealed that processes affecting attributions of
followers by leaders are consistent with earlier
attribution research. For example, head nurses
who were asked to make judgments about the
causes of poor performance by a floor nurse
integrated available information about the con-
sistency of the poor performance over time and
setting and how the performance compared with
that of other nurses. Also consistent with earlier
work was the finding that these judgments tend
to be susceptible to the fundamental attribution
error (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Ross, 1978), in
which performance is more likely to be ascribed
to internal, personal causes (such as motivation
or ability) over equally plausible external causes
(such as poor training or support). Furthermore,
the more extreme the consequences of the poor
performance (e.g., a patient injury), the greater
the tendency to ascribe cause to the individual.
Mitchell and Wood (1980) demonstrated that the
sort of attributions made by the supervisor (i.e.,
internal vs. external [to the subordinate]) had a
significant impact on the kinds of leadership
actions (e.g., training, punishment, termination)
that were likely to be used by the leader to
address the situation.
Brown (1984) made some insightful observa-
tions about leader attributions in real-world
work groups. Most attribution studies do not
involve any real or long-term involvement
between the observer and the actor. However, in
real work groups, leaders and followers are
bound together in a relationship of mutual
dependency; that is, when followers perform
poorly, leaders are usually held to account.
Furthermore, leader and follower are in a
relationship of reciprocal causality in that it is
the leader's responsibility to direct and support
the work of the subordinate. Subordinate failure
might be evidence of leadership failure. These
factors strengthen the tendency of leaders to
make ego-defensive attributions, blaming subor-
dinates for poor performance and possibly
taking personal credit for group success. Inaccu-
rate judgments that arise from these blind spots
can easily erode the working relationship and
bases of influence between the leader and
follower.
Gender Effects
Few careful, scientific studies of differences
between men and women in leadership effects
were done prior to the 1970s. Despite the lack of
scientific evidence on this issue, popular views
were widespread and strong. Bowman, Worthy,
and Greyser (1965) reported that surveys of
managers and business school students revealed
the strong belief that women were unsuited for
managerial roles and would make poor leaders.
Popular writers, such as Hennig and Jardim
(1977), offered quasi-theoretical justification for
such beliefs by proposing that women lacked the
skills and traits necessary for managerial
success. It is interesting that the 1980s brought a
rash of popular books, also with little empirical
basis, that proposed that feminine traits, such as
warmth, nurturance, and flexibility, made women
better leaders and managers than power-
oriented, controlling male leaders.
The questions that present themselves in this
area concern whether men and women actually
are different in their leadership orientations and
behaviors and whether such differences have an
effect on follower reactions and group or
organizational performance. Three theoretical
explanations exist for potential differences
between male and female leaders: (a) women
and men are biologically different (e.g., hor-
mones, temperament, etc.), (b) men and women
are culturally different (i.e., differentially social-
ized for gender roles), and (c) observed
differences between men and women and
reactions to those differences are structurally
determined (i.e., by differences between men's
and women's relative standing in organizational
structures).
One thing that is very clear is that the
leadership stereotypes held by the general public
about males and females are quite different. In
1971 Bass, Krusell, and Alexander reported an
34 CHEMERS
analysis of male managers' responses to a
survey of attitudes toward women at work
indicating that men felt that women lacked
career orientation, leadership potential, and
were undependable and emotionally unstable—
all of which made women unsuitable candidates
for management. Schein (1973,1975) found that
stereotypes of women, held by both men and
women, were very different from stereotypes of
men, with the latter being much closer than the
former to stereotypic perceptions about the
characteristics of a manager. As late as 1989
Heilman, Block, Martell, and Simon replicated
Schein's (1973) study and found little change in
these stereotypes. Clearly, then, the common
view was and may still be that women and men
are very different in their leadership style and
performance. How good is the evidence?
In a classic treatise on the subject of gender
differences, Deaux (1984) effectively dismissed
any biological bases for gender differences in
social behavior. The evidence simply does not
support such differences. But what about the
possibility that differences in socialization to
gender roles carry over to behavior in the
workplace, so-called "gender role spillover"
(Nieva&Gutek, 1981)?
In the second edition of the Handbook of
Leadership, Bass (1981) reported that the
empirical evidence available at that time showed
no consistent pattern of differences between
men and women in supervisory style. However,
definitive analysis on this topic waited until
Eagly and her associates conducted a series of
meta-analyses on male-female differences in
leadership style (Eagly & Johnson, 1990),
leadership emergence (Eagly & Karau, 1991),
and leadership evaluation (Eagly, Makhijani, &
Klonsky, 1992). A careful reading of these
analyses suggests that to the extent that the
observations of leadership were taken in organi-
zational settings; using standard behavioral
measures; by observers, superiors, or subordi-
nates, the differences found between men and
women are so small as to be of little practical
significance. Women tend to emerge as leaders
about as often as men, and they tend to be
evaluated similarly to men when all other
variables are equal.
How about when other variables are not
equal? Women tend to emerge less frequently
and are evaluated less positively in situations
where followers are hostile to women in
leadership or when organizational settings are
not congenial to female leadership. In other
words, women show few differences from men
in actual leadership behavior but are still
susceptible to the impediments created by
negative stereotypes about female leadership.
This conclusion is quite compatible with Deaux's
(1984) view that gender is more important as a
social category than as a biological or cultural
characteristic. Negative views of women lead to
negative expectations that bias women's oppor-
tunities for achieving leadership roles and being
fairly evaluated in those roles. Research on the
structural approach supports this view.
J. House (1981) argued that actors in a social
structure are often strongly influenced by their
place in that structure. In a series of studies on
women and power in organizations, Ragins
(1989,1991; Ragins & Sundstrom, 1989) found
that women face a number of barriers along the
path to acquisition of status and power in
organizations. However, when male and female
managers are matched for level (i.e., power and
authority) within the organization almost no
differences are found in leader behavior, perfor-
mance, or acceptance by subordinates.
The conclusion that may be drawn from this
literature is that although few real differences in
leadership behavior or style exist between men
and women, false but persistent stereotypes
impede equal access and fair evaluation for
women in organizational leadership.
The Mid-1980s to the Mid-1990s:
Transformational Leadership and Cultural
Awareness Transformational Theories
A major shift of interest in leadership research
was sparked by the work of a political historian.
Burns's (1978) book on great leaders differenti-
ated transactional leaders, whose relationship to
followers was based on mutually beneficial
transactions, from transformational leaders,
who influence followers to transcend personal
interests and transform themselves into agents
of collective achievement. This was an exciting
perspective for a field locked in molecular
analyses of trait-situation interactions and
perceptual biases.
Anticipating this development by a year, R. J.
House (1977) published a theoretical analysis of
charismatic leadership in which he analyzed the
characteristics of historical leaders who elicited
SPECIAL ISSUE; LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
35
extraordinary levels of devotion and commit-
ment from followers—for example, Gandhi,
Martin Luther King, Jr., and others. House
identified three sets of characteristics that
typified charismatic leaders. Personal character-
istics included a strong belief in the moral
righteousness of one's beliefs, high levels of self
confidence, and a strong need to influence and
dominate others. Behaviors included dramatic
goal articulation, role modeling of desired
attitudes and behaviors, image building, exhibit-
ing high expectations of and confidence in
followers, and arousing follower motives that
were consistent with desired behavior (i.e.,
aggressive or altruistic motives). Finally, situ-
ational influences might include high levels of
environmental stress (e.g., economic crises,
social upheaval) or an opportunity to express
group goals in moralistic or spiritual terms.
The most careful, empirical analyses of
transformational leadership have been con-
ducted by Bass and his associates (Bass, 1985;
Bass & Avolio, 1990a, 1990b, 1993). Bass
started by interviewing managers about transfor-
mational leaders they had known. On the basis
of the interviews Bass built and validated a
questionnaire designed to measure transforma-
tional leadership: the Multi-Factor Leadership
Questionnaire (MLQ). Factor analysis of the
MLQ yielded seven factors, including three
"transactional" factors (Contingent Reward,
Management By Exception, and Laissez-Faire
Leadership) that were associated with moderate
to poor leadership effects and four transforma-
tional factors that were associated with high
levels of subordinate motivation and group or
organizational success. The transformational
factors included (a) Idealized Influence (cha-
risma), reflecting extremely high levels of leader
competency, trustworthiness, or both; (b) Inspi-
rational Motivation, involving the articulation of
the group's goals in emotional, moral, or
visionary terms; (c) Intellectual Stimulation,
entailing the encouragement of followers to
think independently and creatively and to move
away from past ideas or limitations; and (d)
Individualized Consideration, relating to the
leader's capacity to understand each follower's
personal needs and goals. Bass (1998) reported
data from many organizations in countries
around the world that indicate that leaders who
are rated highly on transformational leadership
characteristics by superiors, peers, or subordi-
nates are associated with high-performing teams
and organizations.
House and Shamir (1993) returned to the
study of transformational leadership, emphasiz-
ing the psychological processes of followers that
mediated the effects of charismatic or transfor-
mational leader actions. Weaving together path-
goal theory (with its emphasis on expectancy
motivation) with theories of intrinsic motivation
and self-concept, they argued that transforma-
tional leaders have several significant psycho-
logical effects on followers. By placing the
group's mission into moral or spiritual contexts,
such leaders raise the salience of collective
goals over personal or selfish interests of the
followers. Second, tying the follower's self-
concept to the group mission makes self-esteem
contingent on group success and fosters self-
motivation and self-regulation by followers.
Both the theoretical explication and perfor-
mance outcomes associated with transforma-
tional leadership make the construct quite
compelling but also leave the leadership scholar
with another conundrum. Transformational theo-
ries arc stated in terms of "universally"
effective leadership behavior—that is, for all
leaders in all situations. It is difficult to square
that idea with the equally compelling evidence
supporting various contingency theories that
show that effective leadership is the result of the
appropriateness or fit between particular behav-
iors and particular situations. Some recent
developments applying the concept of self-
efficacy (Bandura, 1982) to leadership offers a
potential resolution of this contradiction.
Leadership Efficacy
A number of contingency model studies
showing that in-match leaders felt more confi-
dent and in control (Chemers, Ayman, Sorod, &
Akimoto, 1991; Chemers, Hays, Rhodewalt, &
Wysocki, 1985) led me and my associates to
conduct a series of studies designed to assess the
role of leadership confidence or efficacy in
performance. Chemers, Watson, and May (in
press) reported concurrent, predictive, and
discriminant validity for a measure of leadership
efficacy in a longitudinal study of Reserve
Officer Training Corps cadets. Cadets filled out
a measure of self-perceived leadership ability
and were rated for military leadership potential
by their military science instructors. Several
36 CHEMERS
months later, the same cadets were followed up
through a 6-week U.S. Army-sponsored summer
leadership camp in which cadets rotated through
everyday leadership roles, participated in train-
ing, and were tested in realistic military
simulations. Ratings by course instructors,
training camp superiors, peer cadets, and
simulation exercise grading staff all revealed
dramatic effects indicating superior perfor-
mance of cadets who expressed greater confi-
dence in their leadership capability. By way of
discriminant validity, these performance ratings
were not predicted by general self-esteem, and
leadership efficacy did not predict nonleader-
ship performance (e.g., marksmanship, land
navigation).
Watson, Chemers, and Preiser (1996) re-
ported the results of leadership efficacy and
team collective efficacy on the success of small
college basketball teams (both men's and
women's). Before the start of the basketball
season, players were administered question-
naires assessing leadership efficacy and collec-
tive team efficacy. Teams were followed through
the season, and the team's win-loss record
provided the measure of performance. Path
analyses revealed clear and significant support
for the role of efficacy in team performance. The
strongest predictor of team success was team
collective efficacy, and team efficacy was, in
turn, most strongly predicted by leadership
efficacy self-ratings of the identified team leader
(usually the captain). Other factors potentially
related to team success (e.g., previous season
record, number of returning players, starters,
etc.) were controlled for in the analyses and did
not prove as predictive as the efficacy measures.
(No differences were found between men's and
women's teams.)
These findings on leadership efficacy provide
a possible resolution of the contradiction
between contingency theories, which make
situation-specific predictions of leadership suc-
cess and transformational theories, which make
universal predictions. The fit between the
leader's personal characteristics and situational
parameters is an important determinant of a
leader's confident and efficacious behavior—
behavior that is the basis for the critical
functional elements of leadership. That behav-
ior, in turn, gives rise to the effective group
processes and positive perceptions by observers
that constitute transformational leadership.
Transformational leadership measures leader-
ship at the outcome (i.e., dependent variable)
level, whereas the contingency theories tend to
place more focus on the leader characteristics
(i.e., independent variable) level. Leadership
efficacy may be the psychological link between
contingent fit and transformational behavior.
Later in the present article a functional integra-
tion of contemporary leadership theory will
elaborate these critical functions.
Cultural Differences
Two streams of thought on cultural difference
have had an influence on leadership theorizing.
One stream involved the work of social
psychologists who were interested in the effects
of culture on social processes but not necessarily
interested in leadership (e.g., Fiske, 1991;
Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1990,
1993). These theorists focused attention on the
dramatic value differences (i.e., individualism
vs. collectivism) among various national groups.
Briefly stated, individualistic cultures (such as
those of western Europe and English-speaking
countries) place a high value on personal
expression and achievement, whereas more
collectivist cultures (including most of the rest
of the world) are more concerned with group
harmony and collective success.
In a value-based approach more closely
focused on leadership and motivation, Hofstede
(1980, 1983) presented an analysis of four
dimensions of national values with profound
effects on organizational functioning. Power
distance refers to people's comfort with and
acceptance of large differences in power,
influence, and wealth among groups or classes
within the society. Uncertainty avoidance re-
flects the extent to which individuals in a society
resist risk and unexpected events by emphasiz-
ing rules, norms, and expertise. Hofstede's third
dimension was individualism-collectivism, and
the fourth was masculinity-femininity, which
differentiates cultures in which members value
stereotypically masculine pursuits such as
strength, competitiveness, and material achieve-
ment from those in which members are more
concerned with quality of life and concern for
others.
The basic thrust of the value theories is that
organizational processes in different cultures
will reflect what is considered appropriate and
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
37
important. For example, leaders should be seen
as aloof and powerful in high power distance
cultures; as expert, confident, and orderly in
high uncertainty avoidance cultures; as paternal-
istic and caring in collectivist cultures, and as
macho and competitive in masculine cultures.
Likewise, the needs of followers and the nature
of the leader-follower relationship—for ex-
ample, as manifested in a desire for structure
(uncertainty avoidance) or personal achieve-
ment (individualism)—would affect the ways
that leaders and followers interact in the
determination of follower satisfaction, morale,
and motivation.
A second, more focused and more empirical
stream of research involved testing various
leadership theories across different national
groups. In Japan, Misumi (1984; Misumi &
Peterson, 1985) conducted an extensive pro-
gram of laboratory and field research, influenced
by work in the United States on the LBDQ
factors Initiation of Structure and Consideration.
Misumi adapated the LBDQ to Japanese culture
by identifying two broad classes of leader
behavior: (a) behavior associated with work
accomplishment through direction and produc-
tivity emphasis (called Performance) and (b)
behavior intended to maintain high group
morale (called Maintenance). Misumi and Peter-
son (1985) reported that the most productive
work groups in Japanese organizations were led
by supervisors who were high on both Perfor-
mance and Maintenance behavior
Ayman and Chemers (1983) reported similar
results for Iranian managers. They factor
analyzed a Persian translation of the LBDQ to
which they added some probe items related to
the tendency for the worker to identify the
supervisor in fatherly terms (i.e., "My supervi-
sor is like a kind father to me"). Ayman and
Chemers found that structuring, consideration,
and the new items collapsed into a single factor,
which they labeled Benevolent Paternalism and
which was strongly associated with subordinate
satisfaction and performance ratings by superi-
ors. Ayman and Chemers concluded that subor-
dinates in highly collectivistic and power-
oriented cultures derive satisfaction from a
leader who is both directive and nurturant,
whereas subordinates in individualistic, low-
power cultures such as the United States are
more satisfied with a leader who provides
followers with autonomy and opportunities for
personal achievement. Here again we see that
the successful leader is the one who provides
subordinates with an atmosphere conducive to
the fulfillment of the followers' personal needs
and goals but that the nature of those needs and
goals is influenced by culturally socialized
values.
The 80 or 90 years of leadership research
briefly described in the preceding pages cover a
lot of territory. From contingency theories to
transformational leadership, and cognitive, gen-
der, and cultural factors, a complex pattern of
theoretical and empirical material has been
generated. The question remaining is whether a
coherent integration of these seemingly dispar-
ate findings is possible.
A Functional Integration
The apparent complexity of research findings
and theoretical perspectives in the field of
leadership might be reduced if one examined
this literature by focusing on the major functions
that leaders need to fulfill to be successful. I
believe that there are three such functions. A
leader must build credibility in the legitimacy of
his or her authority by projecting an image that
arouses feelings of trust in followers (image
management). A leader must develop relation-
ships with subordinates that enable those
subordinates to move toward individual and
collective goal attainment (relationship develop-
ment). Finally, leaders must effectively use l i e
knowledge, skills, and material resources pre-
sent within their group to accomplish the
group's mission {resource deployment).
Image Management
The definition of leadership provided earlier
stressed that social influence is at the core of the
leadership function. Influence depends on cred-
ibility. For followers to abdicate personal
autonomy and allow themselves to be led, they
must believe that the leader's authority is
legitimate. The information-processing models
of leadership make clear the central role of the
perceptions on which legitimacy is based.
Individuals who are seen as behaving in ways
that are consistent with observer-held leadership
prototypes are afforded authority, and subse-
quent perception, attention, and memory are
more likely to reaffirm the leader's legitimacy.
CHEMERS
We also know that the specific traits or
behaviors associated with leadership credibility
vary somewhat by leadership domain (e.g.,
political leadership vs. business leadership) and
across cultures (Lord, Foti, & De Vader, 1984;
Lord, Foti, & Phillips, 1982). Nonetheless, a
certain commonality exists across leadership
prototypes. As Hollander (1958, 1964) demon-
strated in his early work on leadership status
accrual, leaders must be seen as competent in
task-relevant abilities and as honest, trustwor-
thy, and loyal to group norms and values. This
makes a great deal of sense. Leaders need
task-relevant competencies to move the group
toward a goal, and they must be trustworthy to
ensure that the goal pursued is in the collective
interest.
The literature on charismatic and transforma-
tional leadership is consistent with this perspec-
tive. Bass's (1985) concept of idealized influ-
ence suggests that such leaders are seen as
having exceptional abilities. R. J. House's
(1977) discussion also addresses the extensive
efforts of charismatic leaders in demonstrating
their loyalty to the group cause—frequently by
taking great risks (e.g., Martin Luther King, Jr.)
or incurring significant hardship to follow the
path of the cause (e.g., Gandhi, Joan of Arc).
Relationship Development
Many leadership theories focus on the lead-
er's responsibility in motivating and guiding
followers to enable them to achieve task goals.
The work of Graen and his associates (Graen,
1976; Graen & Cashman, 1975; Graen, Cash-
man, Ginsburgh, & Schiemann, 1978; Graen &
Scandura, 1987) places the quality of the
leader-follower relationship at the center of
effective leadership.
The literature suggests that effective relation-
ships are based on the provision of levels of
coaching and guidance that are appropriate to
the follower's individual situation. The situation
is determined by the follower's task-relevant
skills and knowledge and personal values,
needs, and goals. Patterns of cultural socializa-
tion, personal histories, and contemporary fac-
tors might all contribute to the follower's
readiness for one or another type of treatment.
Effective leadership, then, depends on the
leader's ability to overcome potential egocentric
and defensive biases to make accurate judg-
ments about where the subordinate is and where
the subordinate needs to go next. At the highest
levels of leadership performance, these capabili-
ties are captured by Bass's (1985) concepts of
intellectual stimulation (i.e., delicately targeted
coaching and guidance that arouse intrinsic
motivation) and individualized consideration
(i.e., the subtle but comprehensive awareness of
the follower's situation).
Resource Deployment
Once a leader has established credibility and
mobilized follower motivation, the resultant
energies, knowledge, skills, and material re-
sources must be harnessed and directed to
achieve success in the group's mission. The
successful deployment of the group's resources
has two facets: first, the empowerment of the
individuals in the group, and second, the
effective interface of group processes with task
and environmental demands. Both facets are
influenced by contingency principles.
Individuals are not always able to make the
most effective use of their skills and abilities.
Contemporary theories of intelligence (e.g.,
Sternbcrg, 1988) suggest that effective interac-
tion with an individual's environment (i.e.,
successful utilization of personal resources) is
influenced by the fit between the actor's set of
skills and knowledge and the critical demands of
the challenge. Contingency theory research
conducted by me and my associates (Chemers &
Ayman, 1985; Chemers et al., 1985, 1991) has
found that leaders whose motivational orienta-
tion (LPC score) was in match with environmen-
tal factors (situational control) not only outper-
formed less well-matched leaders but also
showed higher levels of satisfaction, more
positive mood and confidence, and lower levels
of stress and stress-related illness. This pattern
of findings suggests that leaders who are in a
good "fit" with their leadership situation are
more confident, and more of them perform at
high levels.
My later research on leadership efficacy
(Watson et al., 1996) suggests that leadership
confidence (resulting from a good person-
situation match or as a dispositional characteris-
tic) is associated with high levels of team
performance and positive evaluations by follow-
ers and observers. Feelings of efficacy may be
one of the primary moderators of the effects of
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
39
contingency effects on leadership performance
at the personal level.
Both Fiedler's (1967; Fiedler & Chemers,
1974, 1984) contingency model and Vroom and
Yetton's (1973) normative decision model are
built around the notion that internal group
processes, such as decision-making processes,
must match with external task demands to
ensure high levels of group performance. For
example, overly centralized information-process-
ing strategies and autocratic decision structures
are likely to be more effective in highly routine
and predictable leadership situations than they
are in more ambiguous, less predictable situa-
tions requiring creative solutions to novel
problems. Effective leadership depends on
recognizing the nature of the group's environ-
ment and matching group process to external
demand.
On reflection, it appears that self-confidence
or self-efficacy might play an important role in
many aspects of leadership effectiveness. If one
examines the three functions just discussed, one
sees a role for self-confidence in each. Effective
image management depends on projecting the
appearance of competence. Confidence in one's
abilities provides a good marker for compe-
tence. When we observe competent people it is
natural to infer that their confidence is based on
some actual competence. R. J. House's (1977)
analysis of charismatic leadership emphasized
that outstanding leaders do indeed exhibit high
levels of self-confidence. Staw and Barsade
(1992) reported that MBA students with more
positive emotional dispositions were seen as
more appropriate for leadership roles by ob-
servers in an assessment center management
simulation.
Confidence may also play a role in relation-
ship development. Effective coaching and guid-
ance are dependent on accurate perceptions of
subordinates and cogent attributions about the
causes of their behavior and performance. The
major impediment to such perceptual accuracy
are the ego-defensive motivations created by the
leader's own concern for positive evaluations.
When a group performs poorly, the leader may
blame subordinates and be less attentive to
problems caused by other factors, such as the
support structure or his or her own shortcom-
ings. Confidence in one's own abilities might
allow leaders to be less concerned about such
judgments and allow for empathic relationships
with followers.
The deployment of personal and team re-
sources should be especially affected by confi-
dence. On the personal level, a voluminous
literature in social psychology tells us that
confident and optimistic people are better able to
cope with environmental demands (Scheier &
Carver, 1985) and stressful life events (Taylor &
Brown, 1988) and are more likely to take risks
(Isen, Nygren, & Ashby, 1988) and solve
problems creatively (Isen, Daubman, & Nan-
icki, 1987)—all of which are characteristics that
are related to effective leadership.
At the level of team deployment that requires
the ability to read the environment accurately
and respond flexibly, confidence should also
play a role. Staw and Barsade (1992) measured
management students for positive or negative
dispositional affect. More upbeat individuals
performed more effectively at a business deci-
sion task, integrating more information and
making better decisions. Experiments by Guzzo
(1986) and by Zaccaro, Peterson, Blair, and
Gilbert (1990) also show that collective efficacy
(i.e., the shared perception of group members of
the capability and effectiveness of the group)
has been positively related to group perfor-
mance in both experimental and organizational
settings.
We also know that when leaders feel that they
are in a congenial, accepting environment they
are more likely to act in a directive, "take-
charge" fashion (Eagly & Johnson, 1990) and
that directive leaders are more likely to make
effective use of their cognitive abilities (Fiedler
& Garcia, 1987).
Summary and a Few Conclusions
This historical overview of leadership re-
search reveals the extent to which this research
area, like many others, is influenced by periodic
fashions in research theory; for example, an
emphasis on traits at one time; on cognition at
another time, and so on. When we take a longer
view, we are able to find common findings and
streams of thought across theoretical perspec-
tives. The functional integration offered in this
article is an attempt to take such a perspective
driven especially by an emphasis on what
leaders must do to be effective, that is, to
influence followers toward goal attainment.
40 CHEMERS
My analysis argues that leaders must first
establish the legitimacy of their authority by
appearing competent and trustworthy to their
followers. When leaders are extremely effective
in image management they are seen as possess-
ing remarkable, charismatic levels of capability
and trust. Next, leaders must coach, guide, and
support their followers in a way that allows the
followers to contribute to group goal attainment
while satisfying their own personal needs and
goals. To do this, leaders must understand the
abilities, values, and personalities of their
subordinates, so they can provide the type of
coaching and support that will be most effective.
Sometimes leaders are so effective at creating a
motivational environment that followers merge
their personal goals with collective group goals
and are transformed in the process. Finally,
effective leaders must use the skills and abilities
possessed by themselves and their followers to
accomplish the group's mission. The first step in
utilizing these resources is creating a sense of
confidence and personal empowerment that
encourages each group member to release his or
her best efforts. The second step is focusing the
resultant resources on the task environment in a
way that provides the best fit between group
process and environmental demand. Sensitive
information processing and intelligent decision
making are the keys to the group environmental
interface.
In the final paragraphs of this article I raise
the hypothesis that leadership efficacy and
group collective efficacy may be the most
important contributors to each of the functional
necessities of leadership performance. Feelings
of efficacy in the leadership role are thought to
lead to calm decision making, sensitive interper-
sonal relations, ambitious goal setting, bold
action, and long-term perseverance that energize
and maintain the leader and the followers to
effective common effort.
References
Ashour, A. S. (1973). The contingency model of
leadership effectiveness: An evaluation. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Performance, 9,
339-355.
Ayman, R., & Chemers, M. M. (1983). The
relationship of supervisory behavior ratings to
work group effectiveness and subordinate satisfac-
tion among Iranian managers. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 68, 338-341.
Bales, R. E, & Slater, P. E. (1955). Role differentia-
tion in small decision-making groups. In T. Parsons
(Ed.), Family, socialization, and interaction pro-
cesses. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in
human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-
147.
Bass, B. M. (1981). Stogdill's handbook of leadership
(2nd ed.). New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance
beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M. (1998). Transformational leadership;
Industry, military, and educational impact. Mahwah,
NJ:Erlbaum.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1990a). The implications
of transactional and transformational leadership for
individual, team, and organizational development.
In R. W. Woodman & W. A. Passmore (Eds.),
Research in organizational change and develop-
ment. Greenwich, CT: JAJ Press.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1990b). Manual for the
multifactor leadership questionnaire. Palo Alto,
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1993). Transformational
leadership: A response to critiques. In M. M.
Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.). Leadership theory
and research: Perspectives and directions (pp.
49-80). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bass, B. M., Krusell, J., & Alexander, R. A. (1971).
Male managers' attitudes toward working women.
American Behavioral Scientist, 15, 221-236.
Bowman, G. W., Worthy, N. B., & Greyser, S. A.
(1965). Are women executives people? Harvard
Business Review, 43, 14-28.
Brown, K. A. (1984). Explaining group poor perfor-
mance: An attributional analysis. Academy of
Management Review, 9, 54-63.
Burns, J. M. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper &
Row.
Calder, B. J. (1977). An attribution theory of
leadership. In B. M. Staw & G. R. Salancik (Eds.),
New directions in organizational behavior. Chi-
cago: St. Clair Press.
Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in
person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12). New
York: Academic Press.
Carlyle, T. (1907). Heroes and hero worship. Boston:
Adams. (Original work published 1841)
Chemers, M. M. (1997). An integrative theory of
leadership. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chemers, M. M , & Ayman, R. (1985). Leadership
orientation as a moderator of the relationship
between performance and satisfaction of Mexican
managers. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, //, 359-367."
Chemers, M. M., Ayman, R., Sorod, B., & Akimoto,
S. (1991, July). Self-monitoring as a moderator of
SPECIAL ISSUE; LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
41
leader-follower relationships. Paper presented at
the International Congress of Psychology, Brus-
sels, Belgium.
Chemers, M. M , Hays, R., Rhodewalt, F., &
Wysocki, J. (1985). A person-environment analy-
sis of job stress: A contingency model explanation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49,
628-635.
Chemers, M. M., Watson, C. B., & May, S. (in press).
Dispositional affect and leadership performance: A
comparison of self-esteem, optimism, and efficacy.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
Cleven, W. A., & Fiedler, F. E. (1956). Interpersonal
perceptions of open-hearth foremen and steel
production. Journal of Applied Psychology, 40,
312-314.
Deaux, K. (1984). From individual differences to
social categories. American Psychologist, 39,
105-116.
Eagly, A. R , & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and
leadership style: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 108, 233-256.
Eagly, A. H., & KarauT S. J. (1991). Gender and the
emergence of leaders: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 60, 685-710.
Eagly, A. R , Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G.
(1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3-22.
Eden, D., & Leviatan, U. (1975). Implicit leadership
theory as a determinant of the factor structure
underlying supervisory behavior scales. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 60, 736-741.
Fiedler, F. E. (1955). The influence of leader-keyman
relations on combat crew effectiveness. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 227-235.
Fiedler, F. E. (1958). Leader attitudes and group
effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fiedler, F. E. (1964). A contingency model of
leadership effectiveness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
1, pp. 149-190). New York: Academic Press.
Fiedler, F. E. (1967). A theory of leadership
effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fiedler, F. E., & Chemers, M. M. (1974). Leadership
and effective management. Glenview, IL: Scott,
Foresman.
Fiedler, F. E., & Chemers, M. M. (1984). Improving
leadership effectiveness: The Leader Match con-
cept (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Fiedler, F. E., Chemers, M. M., & Maher, L. (1976).
Improving leadership effectiveness: The Leader
Match concept. New York: Wiley.
Fiedler, F. E., & Garcia, J. E. (1987), New approaches
to effective leadership: Cognitive resources and
organizational performance. New York: Wiley.
Field, R. H. G., & House, R. J. (1990). A test of the
Vroom-Yetton model using manager and subordi-
nate reports. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75,
362-366.
Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four
elementary forms of human relations. New York:
Free Press.
Fleishmann, E. A., & Harris, E. F. (1962). Patterns of
leadership behavior related to employee greivances
and turnover. Personnel Psychology, 15, 43-54.
Graen, G. (1976). Role making processes within
complex organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.),
Handbook of industrial and organizational psychol-
ogy. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Graen, G., Alvarez, K., Orris, J. B., & MarteUa, S. A.
(1970). Contingency model of leadership effective-
ness: Antecedent and evidential results. Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 74,285-296.
Gracn, G., & Cashman, J, (1975). A role-making
model of leadership in formal organizations: A
developmental approach. In J. G. Hunt & L. L.
Larson (Eds.), Leadership frontiers. Kent, OH:
Kent State University Press.
Graen, G., Cashman, J. E, Ginsburgh, S., &
Schiemann, W. (1978). Effects of linking-pin
quality on the quality of working life of lower
participants: A longitudinal investigation of the
managerial understructure. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 22, 491-504.
Graen, G., & Scandura, T. A. (1987). Toward a
psychology of dyadic organizing. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 9, 175-208.
Green, S. G., & Mitchell, T. R. (1979). Attribution^
processes of leaders in leader-member interac-
tions. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 23, 429-458.
Griffin, R. N. (1981). Relationships among indi-
vidual, task design, and leader behavior variables.
Academy of Management Journal, 23, 665-683.
Guzzo, R. A. (1986). Group decision making and
group effectiveness in organizations. In P. Good-
man & Associates (Eds.), Designing effective work
groups (pp. 34-71). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation
through the design of work: Test of a theory.
Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 16, 250-279.
Halpin, A. W., & Winer, B. J. (1957). A factorial study
of the leader behavior descriptions. In R. M.
Stogdill & A. E. Coons (Eds.), Leader behavior: Its
description and measurement. Columbus: Ohio
State University Bureau of Business Research.
Hastorf, A. R , Schneider, D., & Polefka, J. (1970).
Person perception. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
Heilman, M. E., Block, C. J., Martell, R. F., & Simon,
M. C. (1989). Has anything changed? Current
characterizations of men, women, and managers.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 935-942.
42 CHEMERS
Hemphill, J. K. (1950). Leader behavior description.
Columbus: Ohio State University Personnel Re-
search Board.
Hennig, M., & Jardim, A. (1977). The managerial
woman. New York: Anchor Press.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences; Interna-
tional differences in work-related values. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Hofstede, G. (1983). Motivation, leadership, and
organization: Do American theories apply abroad?
Organizational Dynamics, 9, 42-63.
Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C , & Mason, I. (1998).
Identification and leadership in small groups:
Salience, frame of reference, and leader stereotypi-
cality effects on leadership evaluations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1248-
1263.
Hollander, E. P. (1958). Conformity, status, and
idiosyncrasy credit. Psychological Review, 65,
117-127.
Hollander, E. P. (1964). Leaders, groups, and
influence. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hollander, E. P., & Julian, J. W. (1970). Studies in
leader legitimacy, influence, and innovation. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press.
House, J. (1981). Social structure and personality. In
M. Rosenberg & R. Turner (Eds.), Social psychol-
ogy: Sociological perspectives (pp. 525-561). New
York: Basic Books.
House, R. J. (1971). Apath-goal theory of leadership.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321-338.
House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic
leadership. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.),
Leadership: The cutting edge. Carbondale: South-
ern Illinois University Press.
House, R. J., & Dessler, G. (1974). The path-goal
theory of leadership: Some post-hoc and a priori
tests. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.),
Contingency approaches to leadership. Carbon-
dale: Southern Illinois University Press.
House, R. J., & Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Path-goal
theory of leadership. Journal of Contemporary
Business, 3, 81-98.
House, R. J.. & Shamir, B. (1993). Integrating
transformational, charismatic, and visionary theo-
ries. In M. M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.),
Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and
directions (pp. 81-108). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nanicki, G. P. (1987).
Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
1122-1131.
Isen, A. M., Nygren, J. E., & Ashby, G. F. (1988). The
influence of positive affect on the subjective utility
of gains and losses: It's not worth the risk. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 710—
717.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to
dispositions: The attribution process in person
perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 2). New
York: Academic Press.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and
the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes
of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning
Press.
Kahn, R. L. (1951). An analysis of supervisory
practices and components of morale. In H.
Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and men.
Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press.
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social
psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska sympo-
sium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Ne-
braska Press.
Kerr, S., & Jermier, J. M. (1978). Substitutes for
leadership: Their meaning and measurement. Orga-
nizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22,
375-403.
Korman, A. K. (1966). "Consideration," "initiating
structure," and organizational criteria. Personnel
Psychology, 18, 349-360.
Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939).
Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally
created social climates. Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 10, 271-301.
Lord, R. G. (1985). An information processing
approach to social perceptions, leadership, and
behavioral measurement in organizations. In B. M.
Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in
organizational behavior (Vol. 7). Greenwich, CT:
JA1 Press.
Lord, R. G., Binning, J. F., Rush, M. C , & Thomas, J.
C. (1978). The effect of performance cues and
leader behavior on questionnaire ratings of leader-
ship behavior. Organizational Behavior and Hu-
man Performance, 21, 27-39.
Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & De Vader, C. (1984). A test
of leadership categorization theory: Internal struc-
ture, information processing, and leadership percep-
tions. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 34, 343-378.
Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & Phillips, i. S. (1982). A
theory of leadership categorization. In J. G. Hunt,
U. Sekaran, & C. Schriesheim (Eds.), Leadership:
Beyond establishment views. Carbondale: Southern
Illinois University Press.
Lord, R. G., & Maher, K. J. (1991). Leadership and
information processing: Linking perceptions and
performance. Boston: Unwin Hyman.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and
the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and
motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 224—253.
SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY
43
Meindl, J. R. (1990). On leadership: An alternative to
the conventional wisdom. In B. A. Staw (Ed.),
Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp.
159-203). New York: JAI Press.
Misumi, J. (1984). The behavioral science of
leadership (2nd ed.). Tokyo: Yuhikaku.
Misumi, J., & Peterson, M. F. (1985). The perfor-
mance-maintenance (PM) theory of leadership:
Review of a Japanese research program. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 30, 198-223.
Mitchell, T. R., Larson, J. R., & Green, S. G. (1977).
Leader behavior and situational moderators in
group performance: An attributional analysis.
Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 18, 254-268.
Mitchell, T. R., & Wood, R. E. (1980). Supervisors'
responses to subordinates' poor performance: A
test of an attribution model. Organizational Behav-
ior and Human Performance, 25, 123-138.
Nieva, V. R, & Gutek, B. A. (1981). Women and
work: A psychological perspective. New York:
Praeger.
Peters, L. H., Hartke, D. D., & Pohlmann, J. T.
(1983). Fiedler's contingency theory of leadership:
An application of the meta-analysis procedure of
Schmidt and Hunter. Psychological Bulletin, 97,
274-285.
Phillips, J. S., & Lord, R. G. (1981). Causal
attributions and perceptions of leadership. Organi-
zational Behavior and Human Performance, 28,
143-163.
Podsakoff, P. M., Niehoff, B. P., MacKenzie, S. B., &
Williams, M. L. (1993). Do substitutes for
leadership really substitute for leadership? An
empirical examination of Kerr and Jermier's
situational leadership model. Organizational Behav-
ior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 1—44.
Ragins, B. R. (1989). Power and gender congruency
effects in evaluations of male and female manag-
ers. Journal of Management, 15, 65-76.
Ragins, B. R. (1991). Gender effects in subordinate
evaluations of leaders: Real or artifact? Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 12, 258—268.
Ragins, B. R., & Sundstrom, E. (1989). Gender and
power in organizations: A longitudinal perspective.
Psychological Bulletin, 105, 51-88.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E.
Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and
categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ross, L. (1978). The intuitive psychologist and his
shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Cognitive
theories in social psychology. New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Rush, M. C , Thomas, J. C , & Lord, R. G. (1977).
Implicit leadership theory: A potential threat to the
internal validity of leader behavior questionnaires.
Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 20, 93-110.
Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism,
coping, and health: Assessment and implications of
generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol-
ogy, 4, 219-247.
Schein, V. E. (1973). The relationship of sex role
stereotypes and requisite management characteris-
tics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 95-100.
Schein, V. E. (1975). The relationship of sex role
stereotypes and requisite management characteris-
tics among female managers. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 60, 340-344.
Staw, B. M. (1975). Attribution of the "causes" of
performance: A general alternative interpretation of
cross-sectional research on organizations. Organi-
zational Behavior and Human Performance, 13,
414-432.
Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1992). Affect and
managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but-
wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypothesis. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 38, 304-331.
Stemberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new
theory of human intelligence. New York: Viking.
Stogdill, R. M. (1948). Personal factors associated
with leadership: A survey of the literature. Journal
of Psychology, 25, 35-71.
Strube, M. J., & Garcia, J. E. (1981). A meta-
analytical investigation of Fiedler's contingency
model of leadership effectiveness. Psychological
Bulletin, 90, 307-321.
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and
well-being: A psychological perspective on mental
health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of
individualism and collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 41-133).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Triandis, H. C. (1993). The contingency model in
cross-cultural perspective. In M. M. Chemers & R.
Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research:
Perspectives and directions (pp. 167-188). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Vroom, V. H., & Yetton, P. W. (1973). Leadership and
decision-making. Pittsburgh, PA: University of
Pittsburgh Press.
Watson, C. B., Chemers, M. M., & Preiser, N. (1996,
June). Collective efficacy: A multi-level analysis.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Psychological Society, San Francisco.
Zaccaro, S. J., Peterson, C , Blair, V., & Gilbert, J. A.
(1990, August). Some antecedents and conse-
quences of collective control. Paper presented at
the 98th Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association, Boston.
GENOGRAM ASSIGNMENT
CFD 135—Nory Behana & Heather Jaffe
TOTAL POINTS: 50
The goal of this assignment is to give you an opportunity to
learn more about your family, genetic predispositions,
environmental influences and apply knowledge you are gaining
in this course to the analysis of your family of origin and the
influences of various situations and experiences on you and
your family. (FOR ALL NAMES IN THIS ASSIGNMENT, USE
ONLY FIRST NAMES—EXCEPT FOR INCLUDING YOUR
FIRST AND LAST NAME ON THE COVER SHEET.)
This entire assignment will be completed in a Power Point file
and must be submitted in one file. It needs to be readable, so
carrying items from page to page to make print large enough to
read is often necessary and encouraged.
Part 1 – Genogram (10 points based on thoroughness and
following directions)
In Part 1, you will construct a Genogram of your family tracing
your family of origin on both the maternal and the paternal
sides back to your great grand-parents (i.e., 4 generations—you,
your parents, your grandparents, and your great-grandparents).
Use Power Point. Feel free to be creative! Photos are helpful.
You will not be sharing it in class, but you will be expected to
download the file to Blackboard to submit it. If you need help
with Power Point or Blackboard, see the help desk at the
library.
Use the symbols provided and display a symbol for each family
member. Number each symbol to correlate with the information
provided on the Factual Data Sheet (Part 2). This may take one
or more separate slides in Power Point.
DO NOT USE THE RELATIONSHIP DESCRIBING LINE
VARIATIONS EXPAINED IN THE TEXT ON PAGE 192 FOR
THIS ASSIGNMENT.
General Criteria:
· Include 4 generations representing both maternal and paternal
sides of the family.
· Draw men on the left and women on the right (except in
complex situations, then just explain)
· Draw siblings in chronological order with the first born on the
left.
· Use appropriate symbols for males and females.
· Use appropriate symbols for marriage, divorce, unmarried
couples, adoptions, etc.
· Start with yourself and highlight your symbol—you are to be
labeled #1.
· Numbers should coincide with the Factual Data Sheet which is
Part 3.
Genogram Symbols
x
x
MALE: FEMALE: DEATH:
|_________|
MARRIAGE: Husband on left, wife on right:
|_________|
CHILDREN: Listed in birth order, beginning on the left with
the oldest:
1 child: 3 children:
|_________| |_________|
| | | |
Twins M/F: Twins F/F:
|_________|
|_________|
__|_ _____|____
Current Pregnancy: Miscarriage or abortion:
|_________| |_________|
X
Living together: Marital separation:
|_ _ _ _ _ _ |
|_____/____|
/
Divorce: Adoptive or Foster Children:
|_____//___| |_________|
// | |
F
A
Part 2 –Factual Data Sheet (10 points based on thoroughness
and following directions)
The third part of your project is a factual data sheet that is to be
typed in chart form. You can use Word or some other typing
program for this part of the project. You must provide 10
columns with as many rows as numbered symbols on your
Genogram. The information provided in each column will
correspond to the family member represented by number on the
Genogram. A sample is given below, but you may find that
setting it up in Landscape instead of Portrait will give you more
space to use. The length will depend upon the number of family
members included and the amount of information about each
one. Feel free to extend some columns to allow for enough
space to accommodate your answers.
The 10 columns are:
1. Number (correlates to Genogram)
2. Name
3. Relationship to you
4. Date of birth/death
5. Approximate date of marriage/divorce/remarriage
6. Occupation/profession
7. Major life events
8. Physical/health problems
9. Personality traits
10. Rituals/traditions
Include a minimum of 10 family members including you. If you
cannot provide information for a column put in a “?” Do not
leave a column blank. Dates can be estimated.
This is intended to be an opportunity to connect with family
members, gather unknown information and share family stories.
If any part of this project creates a problem for you such as in
cases of adoption, foster care, divorces, estranged family
members, etc., please see me about finding enough family
members to use in the project and/or conduct an alternate
project based on those issues.
Feel free to have the columns extend to accommodate your
information, YOU ARE NOT LIMITED TO 10, THAT IS THE
MINIMUM.
#
Name
Rel
BD/DD
Mar/Div
Occupation
Life events
Health
Personality
Rituals/trad.
1
You
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
Part 3 – Family Profile (30 points based on thoroughness and
accuracy in applying concepts from the text to the family
dynamics and use of APA format along with correct grammar
and spelling.)
(Content to be equivalent to approximately 3-5 pages of
narrative text--double spaced, 12-point font if it were in a
regular paper. In Power point, you can single space to save
space. Don’t skimp.)
This part of your paper describes the characteristics and
composition of your family of origin. It should be written in
text (narrative) form and follow APA (American Psychological
Association) guidelines. In the narrative parts of your Family
Profile, cite the text with the correct citation format relating
text information to topics covered. Cite the text at least 10
times in this section. For example, when you discuss your
family’s SES, you could define it citing the text before
continuing with analysis of your family’s SES. A lot of the
grading will consist of analyzing the quality of the text material
chosen and how it is related to the family situation described.
You will be graded on both the quantity and the quality of the
citations. They are best done with an explanation of the term or
concept and then applying it to your narrative.
Your family profile should include the following sections:
1. Composition of your family of origin, including ages of all
members, gender and sibling order. Describe the personality of
each and analyze the influence the influence each has had upon
your development.
2. Occupations of parent(s), education achieved and SES—
Socioeconomic Status. Describe and analyze the influence the
influence each has had upon your development.
3. Size and type of community/communities in which your
family lived during your birth-18 years of age. Describe and
analyze the influence the influence each has had upon your
development.
4. Location and connections with extended family members.
Describe and analyze the influence the influence each has had
upon your development.
5. Health status of you and your immediate family. Describe and
analyze the influence each has had upon your development.
Family health and behavior patterns are often transmitted from
one generation to the next. For the closing of Part 3, identify
the generational transmissions in your family. Refer to your
completed Part 1 and Part 2 of your project for this information.
TO SUBMIT TO BLACKBOARD:
· Do all of the work in one Power Point file, even the grid and
narrative parts
· Submit it on the Blackboard site for this course using the
“Turnitin” link.
Part
GENOGRAM RUBRIC
Points
1
Part 1 – Genogram (10 points based on thoroughness and
following directions)
In Part 1, you will construct a Genogram of your family tracing
your family of origin on both the maternal and the paternal
sides back to your great grand-parents (i.e., 4 generations—you,
your parents, your grandparents, and your great-grandparents).
Use Power Point. Feel free to be creative! Photos are helpful.
Use the symbols provided and display a symbol for each family
member. Number each symbol to correlate with the information
provided on the Factual Data Sheet (Part 2).
DO NOT USE THE RELATIONSHIP DESCRIBING LINE
VARIATIONS THAT ARE EXPAINED IN THE TEXT ON
PAGE 192.
10
possible
2
Factual Data Sheet (10 points based on thoroughness and
following
directions)
The third part of your project is a factual data sheet that is to be
typed in chart form. You can use Word or some other typing
program for this part of the project. You must provide 10
columns with as many rows as numbered symbols on your
Genogram. The information provided in each column will
correspond to the family member represented by number on the
Genogram. A sample is given below, but you may find that
setting it up in Landscape instead of Portrait will give you more
space to use. The length will depend upon the number of family
members included and the amount of information about each
one. Feel free to extend some columns to allow for enough
space to accommodate your answers.
10 possible
3
Family Profile (30 points based on thoroughness and accuracy
in applying concepts from the text to the family dynamics and
use of APA format along with correct grammar and spelling.)
(Content to be equivalent to approximately 3-5 pages of
narrative text--double spaced, 12 point font if it were in a
regular paper. In Power point, you can single space to save
space. Don’t skimp.)
This part of your paper describes the characteristics and
composition of your family of origin. It should be written in
text (narrative) form and follow APA (American Psychological
Association) guidelines. In the narrative parts of your Family
Profile, cite the text with the correct citation format relating
text information to topics covered. Cite the text at least 10
times in this section. For example, when you discuss your
family’s SES, you could define it citing the text before
continuing with analysis of your family’s SES. A lot of the
grading will consist of analyzing the quality of the text material
chosen and how it is related to the family situation described.
30 possible
Total
50 possible
Comments
Your total
1
Promoting More Integrative Strategies for Leadership
Theory-Building
Bruce J. Avolio
University of Nebraska—Lincoln
The agenda for theory and research in the field of leader-
ship studies has evolved over the last 100 years from
focuses on the internal dispositions associated with effec-
tive leaders to broader inquiries that include emphases on
the cognitions, attributes, behaviors, and contexts in which
leaders and followers are dynamically embedded and in-
teract over time. Leadership theory and research has
reached a point in its development at which it needs to
move to the next level of integration— considering the
dynamic interplay between leaders and followers, taking
into account the prior, current, and emerging context—for
continued progress to be made in advancing both the
science and practice of leadership.
Keywords: authentic leadership development, integrative
theories, contingency theories
The field of leadership studies has frequently focusedon the
leader to the exclusion of other equally im-portant components
of the leadership process (Rost,
1991). Indeed, if the accumulated science of leadership had
produced a periodic chart of relevant elements analogous to
that in the field of chemistry, one might conclude that
leadership studies had traditionally focused too narrowly
on a limited set of elements, primarily highlighting the
leader yet overlooking many other potentially relevant el-
ements of leadership such as the follower and context.
Highlighting this issue, Zaccaro and Klimoski (2001)
noted,
Most theories of organizational leadership in the psychological
literature are largely context free. For example, leadership is
typically considered without adequate regard for the structural
contingencies that affect and moderate its conduct. We
maintain,
however, that organizational leadership cannot be modeled
effec-
tively without attending to such considerations. (p. 12)
Potential Benefits of Taking a More
Integrative Focus
By working toward identifying and integrating all of the
elements that constitute leadership, researchers can position
the field of leadership studies to better address questions such
as the degree to which leaders are born versus made; whether
what constitutes leadership effectiveness is more universal or
culturally specific; whether different forms of leadership, such
as charismatic or transformational, are more or less likely to
emerge on the basis of the stability or criticality of the context;
and whether one style of leadership is more or less effective
depending on the contingencies and demands facing leaders
and followers.
Addressing each of the above issues requires an ex-
amination of leadership that considers the relevant actors,
context (immediate, direct, indirect, etc.), time, history, and
how all of these interact with each other to create what is
eventually labeled leadership. This recommendation is in
line with suggestions made by Marion and Uhl-Bien
(2001), who criticized the field of leadership studies for its
tendency to follow a more reductionist strategy, concluding
that “leaders are one element of an interactive network that
is far bigger than they” (p. 414).
Moving toward more integrative strategies for theory-
building and testing has also been recommended by Lord,
Brown, Harvey, and Hall (2001) with respect to one of the
more traditionally leadercentric areas of leadership studies.
Lord et al. (2001) discussed what they called a connection-
ist-based model of leadership prototype generation to em-
phasize how perceptions of leadership are contingent on the
context and the dynamic states in which such mental rep-
resentations are created. They argued that one’s schema of
leadership can be a function of the culture, leader, follower,
task, or behavioral inputs and how they each interact to
form leadership categories and behavioral scripts. Lord et
al. (2001) emphasized that “leadership categories are gen-
erated on-the-fly to correspond to the requirements of dif-
ferent contexts, tasks, subordinates or maturational stages
of a group or organization” (p. 314) and that “leadership
perceptions are grounded within a larger social, cultural,
task and interpersonal environment” (p. 332). Their model
indicates that even one’s internal representation of how
leadership is mentally construed and how one makes sense
of situations appears to be a function of the proximal
(group or task) and distal (organizational or national cul-
ture) context in which those mental representations are
formed.
I begin the promotion of more integrative theories of
leadership by first discussing the importance of followers to
what constitutes leadership. I then examine how the inclu-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Bruce J.
Avolio, Gallup Leadership Institute, Department of
Management, College
of Business, University of Nebraska—Lincoln, CBA 114, P.O.
Box
880491, Lincoln, NE 68588-0491. E-mail: [email protected]
25January 2007 ● American Psychologist
Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association
0003-066X/07/$12.00
Vol. 62, No. 1, 25–33 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.1.25
sion of context found its way into the field of leadership
studies, highlighting early work on contingency theories of
leadership. This discussion is followed by one of more
recent work on leadership, leadership development, and
strategic leadership, demonstrating the necessity of advo-
cating more integrative strategies to advance the science
and practice of leadership.
Examining a Follower Focus
Grint (2000) described the field of leadership studies as
being theoretically inadequate from its inception because it
primarily excluded followers when explaining what consti-
tuted leadership. Grint (2005) stated that “it only requires
the good follower to do nothing for leadership to fail” (p.
133) and that it is the followers who teach leadership to
leaders. Howell and Shamir (2005) concluded that “follow-
ers also play a more active role in constructing the leader-
ship relationship, empowering the leader and influencing
his or her behavior, and ultimately determining the conse-
quences of the leadership relationship” (p. 97). Lord,
Brown, and Frieberg (1999) asserted, “the follower remains
an unexplored source of variance in understanding leader-
ship processes” (p. 167).
After reviewing the accumulated research on transfor-
mational leadership theory, which has been the most fre-
quently researched leadership theory over the last two
decades, I could only find three published studies that
specifically focused on how follower characteristics mod-
erated the effects of leadership on work outcomes (i.e.,
Dvir & Shamir, 2003; Ehrhart & Klein, 2001; Wofford,
Whittington, & Goodwin, 2001). For example, Ehrhart and
Klein reported that followers scoring high in achievement
orientation, self-esteem, and risk-taking were more likely
to be drawn to transformational leaders.
What this research suggests is that a follower’s deci-
sion to follow a leader may be a more active process, based
on the extent to which the leader is perceived as represent-
ing the follower’s values and identity (Howell & Shamir,
2005). Unfortunately, most leadership research has consid-
ered the follower a passive or nonexistent element when
examining what constitutes leadership. An exception to this
conclusion is the work that has been done on relational
models of leadership, such as the vertical dyad linkage
(Dansereau, Graen, & Haga, 1975) or leader–member ex-
change theory (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). The main gist of
leader–member exchange theory is that the quality of the
exchange relationship between leaders and followers will
determine the qualities of leadership and outcomes
achieved.
Early Beginnings of Exploring the
Context
Although leadership studies dating back to the early 20th
century focused more on the leader than on the context of
leadership in which it was observed (Ayman, 2003; Avolio,
Sosik, Jung, & Berson, 2003; Day, 2000), there have been
some important inquiries into what constitutes leadership
throughout human history that included reference to the
context. For example, Plato’s philosophical discussions of
the moral and ethical purpose of leadership highlighted the
relevance of the context.
Similar to discussions of ethical leadership, early writ-
ings on what constituted charismatic leadership also fo-
cused on the context. Weber (1924/1947) recognized that
there were certain unique qualities of leaders that differen-
tiated the bureaucratic from the charismatic leader. Weber
argued that a social crisis was necessary to promote the
emergence of charismatic leaders. Although subsequent
research on the emergence of charismatic leadership has
challenged Weber’s base assumption (Bass, 1990), the
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx
Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx

More Related Content

Similar to Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx

202004300445589829d981da.pdf
202004300445589829d981da.pdf202004300445589829d981da.pdf
202004300445589829d981da.pdfAmnajabbar13
 
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And Conflict
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And ConflictA Literature Review On Leadership Styles And Conflict
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And ConflictJeff Brooks
 
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)BHUOnlineDepartment
 
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docx
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docxResearching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docx
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docxronak56
 
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...Claudia Acosta
 
Leadership effectiveness a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...
Leadership effectiveness  a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...Leadership effectiveness  a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...
Leadership effectiveness a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...jameskandi
 
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership Theories
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership TheoriesA Systematic Review Of Various Leadership Theories
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership TheoriesSandra Long
 
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...inventionjournals
 
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docx
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docxOverall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docx
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docxgerardkortney
 
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docx
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docxA Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docx
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docxransayo
 
The role of the situation in leadership
The role of the situation in leadershipThe role of the situation in leadership
The role of the situation in leadershipJovenary Muta
 
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docx
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docxEthics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docx
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docxhumphrieskalyn
 
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docx
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docxPersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docx
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docxmattjtoni51554
 
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docx
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docxBriefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docx
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docxjasoninnes20
 
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docx
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docxLesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docx
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docxsmile790243
 
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxBusiness Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxRAHUL126667
 
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxBusiness Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxfelicidaddinwoodie
 

Similar to Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx (20)

202004300445589829d981da.pdf
202004300445589829d981da.pdf202004300445589829d981da.pdf
202004300445589829d981da.pdf
 
Leadership
LeadershipLeadership
Leadership
 
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And Conflict
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And ConflictA Literature Review On Leadership Styles And Conflict
A Literature Review On Leadership Styles And Conflict
 
Leadership theories
Leadership theoriesLeadership theories
Leadership theories
 
Leadership
LeadershipLeadership
Leadership
 
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)
LS 300 Global Leadership Research (Week 1)
 
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docx
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docxResearching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docx
Researching Leadership TheoryIn researching leadership theor.docx
 
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...
A Study Of The Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Leadership Abi...
 
Leadership effectiveness a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...
Leadership effectiveness  a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...Leadership effectiveness  a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...
Leadership effectiveness a multi-factorial model dr. m. roussety mba, m led,...
 
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership Theories
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership TheoriesA Systematic Review Of Various Leadership Theories
A Systematic Review Of Various Leadership Theories
 
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...
Implicit Leadership Perception at Context of; Intergovernmental and Nongovern...
 
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docx
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docxOverall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docx
Overall great paper. To get even more points I would recommend dig.docx
 
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docx
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docxA Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docx
A Test of Excellent Leadership in Public Relations KeyQuali.docx
 
The role of the situation in leadership
The role of the situation in leadershipThe role of the situation in leadership
The role of the situation in leadership
 
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docx
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docxEthics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docx
Ethics Matter Moderating Leaders’ Power Use and Followers’C.docx
 
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docx
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docxPersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docx
PersonalityLeadership LinkageThere is a link between an individ.docx
 
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docx
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docxBriefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docx
Briefly described your healthcare organization, including its cult.docx
 
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docx
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docxLesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docx
Lesson Four Leadership Behaviors and their Ethical Implications.docx
 
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxBusiness Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
 
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docxBusiness Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
Business Executives’ Perceptions of Ethical Leadershipand It.docx
 

More from whittemorelucilla

Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxDatabase reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxDataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxDataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxDataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxDataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxDataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxDataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxDataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxDatabase Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docxDatabases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxDatabase SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxDATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxDatabase Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxDatabase Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxDatabase Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxDatabase Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxDatabase Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docxDatabase Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxDataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxwhittemorelucilla
 

More from whittemorelucilla (20)

Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxDatabase reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
 
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxDataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
 
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxDataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
 
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxDataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
 
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxDataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
 
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxDataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
 
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxDataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
 
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxDataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
 
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxDatabase Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
 
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docxDatabases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
 
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxDatabase SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
 
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxDATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
 
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxDatabase Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
 
Data.docx
Data.docxData.docx
Data.docx
 
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxDatabase Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
 
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxDatabase Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
 
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxDatabase Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
 
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxDatabase Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
 
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docxDatabase Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
 
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxDataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersChitralekhaTherkar
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 

Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice2000, Vol. 4. .docx

  • 1. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 2000, Vol. 4. No. 1,27-43 Copyright 2000 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 1089-2699/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2699.4.1.27 Leadership Research and Theory: A Functional Integration Martin M. Chemers University of California, Santa Cruz This historical overview of leadership theory and research with an eye for commonali- ties provides an opportunity for integration. Early unproductive research focused on personality traits and behaviors. A recognition of the more complex nature of the phenomenon resulted in the development of contingency theories that examined leader characteristics and behavior in the context of situational parameters. The 1970s brought an awareness that perceptions of leaders by followers and others, and perceptions of followers by leaders, were influenced by cognitive biases arising from prior expecta- tions and information-processing schema. Ironically, attention was belatedly drawn to the study of female leaders, who were often the victim of cognitive biases and negative assumptions. Recent research has reflected on the role of cultural differences in leadership processes and has been drawn again into the search
  • 2. for outstanding leaders with universally effective characteristics. The article concludes with an integration of current knowledge in leadership effectiveness. For much of its history, leadership theory and the empirical supporting research have been regarded as a fractured and confusing set of contradictory findings and assertions without coherence or interpretability. In this article T argue that a considerable commonality of well-accepted findings points the way toward a successful and useful integration of current knowledge. That integration, based on the key functions performed by effective leaders, begins to answer the question of how good leaders behave and raises intriguing questions about the personal characteristics of leaders that facilitate those behaviors. This article takes an historical perspective, and the analysis is divided into four periods: (a) the period prior to the presentation of Fiedler's (1964) contingency model; (b) the period from 1965 to 1975, focusing on the development and elaboration of contingency theories; (c) the period from 1975 to 1985, when cognitive theories and concerns about gender differences arose; and (d) the period since 1985, which has most extensively focused on transformational theories and cultural influences. The historical analysis is followed by a presentation of an Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- dressed to Martin M. Chemers, Division of Social Sciences, 117 Social Sciences I, University of California, 1156 High
  • 3. Street, Santa Cruz, California 95064. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected] integrative framework and a suggested direction for future research. In this analysis leadership is defined as "a process of social influence in which one person is able to enlist the aid and support of others in the accomplishment of a common task." This definition places the subject distinctly within the purview of social psychology, and the analyses that follow reveal how fully the leadership literature is indebted to the field of social psychology for its dominant paradigms and central variables. Before Contingency Theory: Lost in the Wilderness Social philosophers have had a long-standing interest in both organizational and political leadership. Western European philosophers, embedded in a strongly individualistic cultural milieu, looked primarily to the characteristics of leaders for explanatory premises, For example, Carlyle (1841/1907) proposed the great man theory of leadership, which argued that success- ful leaders possessed traits of personality and character that set them apart from ordinary followers. The interest in individual characteris- tics of leaders was spurred by the emergence of intelligence tests in the early 20th century. Empirical psychology turned toward the study of traits, and the nascent leadership field followed suit.
  • 4. 27 28 CHEMERS Traits A sort of naive popularist psychology guided the choice of traits considered for leadership impact. Traits that were stereotypically associ- ated with leadership, such as dominance, assertiveness, intelligence, physical stature, so- cial sensitivity, and many others, became causal candidates. The typical research format for these early studies was to identify a group with leaders and followers and test for differences on the selected trait measures. Stogdill (1948) provided an extensive review of 30 years of the trait studies. He reported that a few traits (most notably intelligence) were sometimes associated with reliable differences between leaders and followers (i.e., about 35% of the time), but there was no single variable or even cluster of variables that was related to leadership across a variety of situations. Stogdill concluded that although individual differences were certainly important in identifying emergent or effective leaders, the great diversity of situations in which leaders functioned made it unlikely that any one trait would be a universal predictor. Although it was not immediately recognized, StogduTs analysis set the stage for theories of leadership that were predicated on an interaction between leader traits and situational contingencies.
  • 5. Behaviors and Styles Daunted by the failure of traits to predict leadership, but unwilling to abandon individual- istic explanations, researchers turned to the study of leader behavior. Observations of the effects of leadership style (i.e., autocratic vs. democratic) on the atmosphere of small groups (Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939), process analyses of interactions in laboratory discussion tasks (Bales & Slater, 1955), and reports of industrial workers on the behavioral styles of their supervisors (Kahn, 1951) sought to iden- tify patterns of leader behavior associated with high productivity or good morale. The most extensive of these research pro- grams and the one with the most enduring impact on the field of leadership was the set of studies surrounding the development of the Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire (LBDQ) at Ohio State University (Hemphill, 1950). This 150-item behavioral inventory was used to collect ratings of military and industrial leaders by supervisors, subordinates, and observ- ers. Subsequent factor analyses revealed that a major portion of the variability in leader behavior could be explained by two major clusters (Halpin & Winer, 1957). The most prominent factor, labeled Consideration, in- cluded behaviors such as showing concern for the feelings of subordinates, making sure that minority viewpoints were considered in deci- sion making, and attempting to reduce conflict in the work environment. These behaviors
  • 6. seemed to reflect leader intentions to support positive group morale and follower satisfaction. A strong second factor, labeled Initiation of Structure, included items measuring the leader's use of standard operating procedures, criticism of poor work, and emphasis on high levels of performance. These behaviors appeared to be related to a leader's focus on building a structure for task accomplishment. Although the LBDQ factors were found reliably in ratings of leader behavior across a wide range of settings, they were less than completely successful at predicting the impor- tant outcomes associated with leadership effec- tiveness, that is, follower satisfaction and group performance (Fleishmann & Harris, 1962; Kor- man, 1966). Considerate leadership was often related to follower satisfaction or morale, and Consideration and Initiation of Structure were sometimes but not always predictive of group performance. The failure of this carefully constructed and comprehensively researched behavioral measure to predict leadership led many researchers to throw up their hands in frustration and seemed to be yet another instance of leadership research leading to no coherent conclusions. Legitimacy One of the brightest spots in the early empirical work on leadership was the series of studies conducted by Hollander, which illumi- nated some of the facets of leadership status accrual and legitimacy. In both laboratory and
  • 7. field research venues, Hollander (1964; Hol- lander & Julian, 1970) found that individuals in groups gain status through the demonstration of task-related competence and loyalty to group values. Status acquisition is associated with the accrual of so-called "idiosyncrasy credits," which can be thought of as units of group SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 29 acceptance that can be "spent" to influence others and provide leeway from group norms to allow for innovation in group processes and views. This pioneering work on how individuals decide to follow those in leadership positions has retained its currency to the present day, in part because it embodies both cognitive and behavioral elements in its approach. Contempo- rary information-processing theories of leader- ship posit leadership "prototypes" that are characterized by the elements of competency and trustworthiness that are the bases for the accrual of idiosyncrasy credits. Recent work by Hogg and his associates (Hogg, Hains, & Mason, 1998) applying social identity theory to leadership perception indicates that, despite a tendency for followers to value leaders who embody group values (the basis for perceptions of trustworthiness in Hollander's model), they also heavily weight task relevant competence in leadership evaluation. Those basic determinants
  • 8. of leadership status turn up in more recent approaches to understanding leadership judg- ments. A fuller discussion of the role of perception in leadership process appears later. The Mid-1960s to the Mid-1970s: The Contingency Era The Contingency Model The study of leadership took a dramatic change of direction with the publication of Fiedler's first articles (1964) and subsequent book (1967), which presented a new approach to understanding leadership effectiveness. The contingency model of leadership effectiveness emerged as an answer to StogduTs (1948) call for an approach based on the interaction of leader traits with situational parameters, but it did not start out that way. Early work (Cleven & Fiedler, 1956; Fiedler, 1955, 1958) tested the predictive validity of a leadership trait measure on the basis of the leader's views of coworkers. The measure, which eventually came to be known as the Least Preferred Coworker (LPC) scale, differentiated leaders who viewed poorly performing coworkers in very negative terms (thought to reveal a very strong concern with effective task performance) from those who viewed poorly performing coworkers in less negative terms (hypothesized to reflect a greater emphasis on interpersonal relationships). Apparently promising early work found that task-oriented leaders were more effective, but
  • 9. subsequent studies yielded results showing that relationship-oriented leaders had more effective teams. Confused but not deterred by these anomalous findings, Fiedler reanalyzed a large number of studies—this time classifying the group settings in terms of the degree of support and cooperation offered by followers, the clarity and structure of the group's task, and the leader's formal authority to direct and reward followers. These three variables were combined into a dimension of "situational favorableness" (Fiedler, 1967) or "situational control" (Fiedler, Chemers, & Maher, 1976), thought to reflect the degree to which the overall situation gave the leader a feeling of certainty, predictability, and control over group processes. When the leader's orientation (i.e., LPC score) was correlated with group performance across the dimension of situational favorable- ness, a reliable relationship was found. Specifi- cally, groups led by task-oriented leaders performed best in situations of high control and predictability or very low control and predictabil- ity, and groups led by relationship-oriented leaders performed best in the situations of moderate control or predictability. The explana- tory rationale for these findings was that the relatively more directive, task-focused leader- ship style is most appropriate when an orderly situation provides the leader with the clarity to give directions and the follower support to be sure of his or her performance, and the highly volatile and unpredictable environment of the very low control situation also requires the steadying influence of clear directions and
  • 10. structuring leader behavior. However, the more interpersonally oriented, participative style of leadership was thought to function most effec- tively when the complexities of a moderate- control situation required greater delicacy to navigate a poorly understood task or to avoid the dangers associated with uncertain follower support. The inductive method by which the contin- gency model was constructed and the highly complex nature of its predictions led to many criticisms of the model during the 1970s (Ashour, 1973; Graen, Alvarez, Orris, & Mar- tella, 1970). However, subsequent research and 30 CHEMERS extensive meta-analyses (Peters, Hartke, & Pohlmann, 1983; Strube & Garcia, 1981) provided strong support for the basic principles of the model. (For a more complete discussion of the development of the contingency model and the controversy surrounding its validity, see Chemers, 1997.) Another criticism of the contingency model was its apparent assumption that a leader could not choose to be both task and relationship oriented when the situation demanded it. Some other contingency ap- proaches did not make that same assumption. Normative Decision Theory Energized by the potential of a contingency
  • 11. approach to explain leadership performance, but working from a more deductive theoretical base, Vroom and Yetton (1973) offered a model of decision-making effectiveness that integrated leaders' decision strategy with situational fac- tors. Leaders were conceived as having a range of decision-making strategies available to them that varied in degree of follower involvement in the process—ranging from autocratic styles (leader makes the decision with minimal fol- lower input) to consultative styles (leader makes the decision after getting follower opinion and advice) to group or participative styles (leader and group make decision together, with equal weight). The situational parameters included in the model were represented as a series of questions arranged to yield a decision tree. Leaders seeking the most effective decision strategy were asked to analyze situational factors that included the clarity and structure of the task and surrounding information, the degree of support for the leader and the organization among the followers, the degree of conflict among subordi- nates, and the time urgency for a decision to be made. The model specifies that when the task is clear and the followers supportive, the leader should use the more time-efficient autocratic styles. If the task or information is unclear, using the consultative strategies increases the informa- tion yield and likelihood of a higher quality decision. When the leader lacks follower support, the participative strategy helps to ensure follower commitment to the decision and its implementation. Empirical research on the
  • 12. normative decision model is not extensive but is generally supportive of its basic premises (Field & House, 1990). The contingency model and normative deci- sion theory have many features in common. They are both focused on the leader as the central actor in the group's efforts to interface with the task environment. Both theories regard the leader's task as to gain the group's support in solving problems and implementing solutions effectively. Also, they both hypothesize that more directive approaches will be most effective when a clear task and a supportive group give the leader the certainty to take charge but that more participative strategies will work better when a less clear and orderly environment argues against bold action and autocratic direc- tion. The two theories part company in the situation of very low control with the contin- gency model more focused on immediate group performance through leader direct action but normative decision theory suggesting more participative strategies to build a more support- ive environment over the long run. Path-Goal Theory Contingency theories held the promise of correcting the weaknesses of earlier approaches to leadership effectiveness prediction. For ex- ample, one promising but disappointing ap- proach had been the attempt to relate leader behavior (e.g., the LBDQ score) to organiza- tional outcomes. R. J. House and his associates
  • 13. (R. J. House, 1971; R. J. House & Dessler, 1974; R. J. House & Mitchell, 1974) picked up that gauntlet and attempted to merge traditional behavioral approaches with emerging develop- ments in the study of worker motivation to understand the impact of the leader on the motivation and performance of followers. Path-goal theory argues that the leader's main purpose is to motivate subordinates by helping mem to see how their task-related performance could help them to achieve their personal goals. Research within the path-goal framework attempted to understand how a leader's directiveness (i.e., Initiation of Struc- ture) or supportiveness (i.e., Consideration) behaviors might affect subordinate motivation and performance. Proceeding logically, the theory predicted that a leader's structuring behavior would be motivating to a subordinate when the subordinate's task environment lacked SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 31 structure because of insufficient training or experience or a highly complex task. However, when a subordinate had sufficient structure, leader directiveness would be regarded as overly close monitoring or "pushing" and would have negative effects. Consideration behavior was seen to have its most positive effects when the subordinate needed psychological or emotional support to deal with an aversive work environ-
  • 14. ment (made so by a boring or unpleasant task). Consideration was viewed as superfluous in situations that were engaging and intrinsically interesting to the subordinate. The typical research paradigm for path-goal theory studies was to divide a group of subordinates into situations of low clarity (presumably interesting, but potentially frustrat- ing because of lack of structure) and of very high clarity-predictability (presumably boring and uninvolving). Leader structuring behavior was predicted to have positive effects on subordinate motivation and performance in the former situation but not the latter, whereas the reverse was true for leader considerate behavior. Path-goal theory generated a considerable body of empirical research support for the basic propositions. Considerate behavior, for ex- ample, was usually related to positive subordi- nate attitudes under boring or aversive task situations but often had similarly positive effects across all situations. Results regarding structur- ing behavior were even less consistent. A study by Griffin (1981) that included measures of subordinates * ' * growth need strength" (Hackman & Oldham, 1976) indicated that the needs and expectations of subordinates played a role in determining when subordinates needed or wanted different types of leader behavior. Growth-oriented, challenge-seeking subordinates were quite comfortable with un- structured and challenging tasks and were therefore less receptive to directive leader behavior under unstructured or structured condi-
  • 15. tions. However, these growth-oriented subordi- nates were very responsive to supportive behavior when the task was boring. The more change-averse, low-growth-need subordinates were comfortable with leader structure across all situations but needed less support when a task was ostensibly boring. A reasonable conclusion to be drawn from this literature is that leader behavior that is seen as supportive by subordi- nates is likely to lead to positive reactions and higher motivation and that both characteristics of the task and of the subordinate will contribute to that receptiveness. In an interesting extension of path-goal theory, Kerr and Jermier (1978) argued that if the leader's purpose is to supply missing elements in the subordinate's job environment (e.g., structure or support), then other sources of those missing elements might make the leader's behavior redundant and unnecessary. Their "substitutes for leadership" theory predicted, for example, that if a job provided plenty of task-relevant feedback, leader structuring behav- ior would be unnecessary, or if a compatible and cohesive work group provided emotional sup- port, leader consideration would be redundant. Under such conditions, leader behaviors were hypothesized to show minimal or even negative relationships with subordinate motivation, satis- faction, or performance. However, a review of a number of studies of substitutes-for-leadership hypotheses indicated very little support for the theory's predictions (Podsakoff, Niehoff, Mac- Kenzie, & Williams, 1993) and revealed that a
  • 16. leader's behavior remains very important to subordinates regardless of varying situational conditions. The research literature on the contingency theories suggests that actions by a group's leader can have strong effects on the motivational and emotional states of followers and on the successful accomplishment of the group's task. The relationship of the specific leader actions to those outcomes depends on the interaction of those actions with relevant features of the interpersonal and task environment. The Mid-1970s to the Mid-1980s: Cognitive Models and Gender Concerns The growing influence of cognitive theories in social psychology led to a similar interest among leadership researchers. Two broad classes of investigation were concerned with percep- tions of leaders by others (i.e., followers, superiors, and observers) and leaders' percep- tions and evaluations of subordinates. Leadership Perceptions In the mid-1970s, studies involving ratings of leader behavior began to reveal certain anoma- lous findings. Eden and Leviatan (1975) re- 32 CHEMERS ported that when research participants were
  • 17. asked to make ratings of leader behavior by simply imagining a leader, the resultant data showed factor structures similar to those derived from ratings of actual leaders. Staw (1975) showed two sets of observers the same video- tape of a group interaction but told the observers that the group had either been very successful or very unsuccessful on task performance. Ob- server ratings of the "successful" leader were higher on measures of both directive and supportive leadership than were the ratings of the "unsuccessful" leader. That ratings of leaders might be strongly biased created a problem on both theoretical and methodological grounds. Leader legitimacy, a central construct in understanding the bases of leader influence, was based on follower percep- tions. Furthermore, almost every research para- digm in the leadership field depended on ratings of leader behavior (Rush, Thomas, & Lord, 1977). Attribution theory (Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967) provided a theoretical framework for the investigation of leadership biases. Taking a very strong position, Calder (1977) argued that the very concept of leadership is rooted in popular language and poorly articulated as a scientific construct. He argued that with no way of measuring leadership apart from social perceptions, leadership exists primarily as an attribution rather than a testable construct and should, therefore, be abandoned as a subject of scientific inquiry. Few researchers were willing "to throw the baby out with the bathwater" and
  • 18. began instead to make a systematic study of leadership perceptions and the processes that gave rise to them. A useful model was provided by the research on implicit personality theories, which Hastorf, Schneider, and Polefka (1970) defined as a structure of association about what traits or characteristics are related that guides and organizes perceptions, thoughts, and memories about a phenomenon. Implicit theories of leadership, then, would define the assumptions that people held about what behaviors leaders displayed and how those behaviors were associ- ated with group and organizational outcomes. An extensive research program by Lord and his associates (Lord, 1985; Lord, Binning, Rush, & Thomas, 1978; Lord & Maher, 1991) revealed that leadership attributions were based on two processes. Recognition processes determined when an individual's behavior would result in the perception of that person as a leader. Observers were found to hold highly articulated prototypes (Cantor & Mischel, 1979; Rosch, 1978) of leadership. When an actor's behavior showed sufficient overlap with the prototypi- cally driven expectations of observers, a leader- ship attribution was made. Once an individual was seen as a leader, selective attention and memory reinforced that judgment. Leadership judgments were also found to be influenced by inferential processes. Because implicit theories of leadership associate team
  • 19. success with effective leadership, observers are likely to infer the presence of good leadership from evidence of group success (Phillips & Lord, 1981). Thus, once a person is seen as a leader, observer inferences are likely to rein- force and enhance that perception. Of course, if a person's characteristics, such as gender or race, are inconsistent with observers' prototypic expectations, then such a person is less likely to be perceived as a legitimate or effective leader despite any objective achievements. The strength of common beliefs in the importance of leadership for group outcomes led Meindl (1990) to develop the "romance of leadership" concept. In an ingenious series of experiments and naturalistic observations, Meindl showed that any remarkable group or organizational outcome, whether highly positive or highly negative, is likely to be attributed to leadership effects, while other reasonable causes are largely ignored. Although the strong susceptibility to percep- tual biases in the observations of leadership might have constituted a problem for research methodology, it opened a fascinating area for theoretical development. If leadership is a process of social influence, then factors that affect the legitimacy, credibility, and influence of leaders become a central aspect of leadership function. Another important component of the leader- ship process involves the perception of follow- ers by leaders. Almost every theory of leader-
  • 20. ship posits that a central function of leadership involves the direction of subordinates. The follower-oriented contingency theories, such as path-goal theory, maintain that it is the leader's responsibility to provide the subordinate with task-directed guidance or emotional support to SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 33 help them to be effective and satisfied. Implicit in these premises is the expectation that leaders are able to judge what kinds of behaviors on their part are likely to have positive effects on subordinates. In other words, leaders must observe the actions and reactions of subordi- nates to judge what is needed. This clearly places attributionai processes at the center of the relationship between leader and follower. Mitchell and his associates (Green & Mitchell, 1979; Mitchell Larson, & Green, 1977; Mitchell & Wood, 1980) applied Kelley's (1967) attribu- tion model to leader evaluations of subordinates and the effects that those evaluations have on subsequent leader actions. That research re- vealed that processes affecting attributions of followers by leaders are consistent with earlier attribution research. For example, head nurses who were asked to make judgments about the causes of poor performance by a floor nurse integrated available information about the con- sistency of the poor performance over time and setting and how the performance compared with
  • 21. that of other nurses. Also consistent with earlier work was the finding that these judgments tend to be susceptible to the fundamental attribution error (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Ross, 1978), in which performance is more likely to be ascribed to internal, personal causes (such as motivation or ability) over equally plausible external causes (such as poor training or support). Furthermore, the more extreme the consequences of the poor performance (e.g., a patient injury), the greater the tendency to ascribe cause to the individual. Mitchell and Wood (1980) demonstrated that the sort of attributions made by the supervisor (i.e., internal vs. external [to the subordinate]) had a significant impact on the kinds of leadership actions (e.g., training, punishment, termination) that were likely to be used by the leader to address the situation. Brown (1984) made some insightful observa- tions about leader attributions in real-world work groups. Most attribution studies do not involve any real or long-term involvement between the observer and the actor. However, in real work groups, leaders and followers are bound together in a relationship of mutual dependency; that is, when followers perform poorly, leaders are usually held to account. Furthermore, leader and follower are in a relationship of reciprocal causality in that it is the leader's responsibility to direct and support the work of the subordinate. Subordinate failure might be evidence of leadership failure. These factors strengthen the tendency of leaders to make ego-defensive attributions, blaming subor-
  • 22. dinates for poor performance and possibly taking personal credit for group success. Inaccu- rate judgments that arise from these blind spots can easily erode the working relationship and bases of influence between the leader and follower. Gender Effects Few careful, scientific studies of differences between men and women in leadership effects were done prior to the 1970s. Despite the lack of scientific evidence on this issue, popular views were widespread and strong. Bowman, Worthy, and Greyser (1965) reported that surveys of managers and business school students revealed the strong belief that women were unsuited for managerial roles and would make poor leaders. Popular writers, such as Hennig and Jardim (1977), offered quasi-theoretical justification for such beliefs by proposing that women lacked the skills and traits necessary for managerial success. It is interesting that the 1980s brought a rash of popular books, also with little empirical basis, that proposed that feminine traits, such as warmth, nurturance, and flexibility, made women better leaders and managers than power- oriented, controlling male leaders. The questions that present themselves in this area concern whether men and women actually are different in their leadership orientations and behaviors and whether such differences have an effect on follower reactions and group or organizational performance. Three theoretical explanations exist for potential differences
  • 23. between male and female leaders: (a) women and men are biologically different (e.g., hor- mones, temperament, etc.), (b) men and women are culturally different (i.e., differentially social- ized for gender roles), and (c) observed differences between men and women and reactions to those differences are structurally determined (i.e., by differences between men's and women's relative standing in organizational structures). One thing that is very clear is that the leadership stereotypes held by the general public about males and females are quite different. In 1971 Bass, Krusell, and Alexander reported an 34 CHEMERS analysis of male managers' responses to a survey of attitudes toward women at work indicating that men felt that women lacked career orientation, leadership potential, and were undependable and emotionally unstable— all of which made women unsuitable candidates for management. Schein (1973,1975) found that stereotypes of women, held by both men and women, were very different from stereotypes of men, with the latter being much closer than the former to stereotypic perceptions about the characteristics of a manager. As late as 1989 Heilman, Block, Martell, and Simon replicated Schein's (1973) study and found little change in these stereotypes. Clearly, then, the common view was and may still be that women and men
  • 24. are very different in their leadership style and performance. How good is the evidence? In a classic treatise on the subject of gender differences, Deaux (1984) effectively dismissed any biological bases for gender differences in social behavior. The evidence simply does not support such differences. But what about the possibility that differences in socialization to gender roles carry over to behavior in the workplace, so-called "gender role spillover" (Nieva&Gutek, 1981)? In the second edition of the Handbook of Leadership, Bass (1981) reported that the empirical evidence available at that time showed no consistent pattern of differences between men and women in supervisory style. However, definitive analysis on this topic waited until Eagly and her associates conducted a series of meta-analyses on male-female differences in leadership style (Eagly & Johnson, 1990), leadership emergence (Eagly & Karau, 1991), and leadership evaluation (Eagly, Makhijani, & Klonsky, 1992). A careful reading of these analyses suggests that to the extent that the observations of leadership were taken in organi- zational settings; using standard behavioral measures; by observers, superiors, or subordi- nates, the differences found between men and women are so small as to be of little practical significance. Women tend to emerge as leaders about as often as men, and they tend to be evaluated similarly to men when all other variables are equal.
  • 25. How about when other variables are not equal? Women tend to emerge less frequently and are evaluated less positively in situations where followers are hostile to women in leadership or when organizational settings are not congenial to female leadership. In other words, women show few differences from men in actual leadership behavior but are still susceptible to the impediments created by negative stereotypes about female leadership. This conclusion is quite compatible with Deaux's (1984) view that gender is more important as a social category than as a biological or cultural characteristic. Negative views of women lead to negative expectations that bias women's oppor- tunities for achieving leadership roles and being fairly evaluated in those roles. Research on the structural approach supports this view. J. House (1981) argued that actors in a social structure are often strongly influenced by their place in that structure. In a series of studies on women and power in organizations, Ragins (1989,1991; Ragins & Sundstrom, 1989) found that women face a number of barriers along the path to acquisition of status and power in organizations. However, when male and female managers are matched for level (i.e., power and authority) within the organization almost no differences are found in leader behavior, perfor- mance, or acceptance by subordinates. The conclusion that may be drawn from this literature is that although few real differences in leadership behavior or style exist between men
  • 26. and women, false but persistent stereotypes impede equal access and fair evaluation for women in organizational leadership. The Mid-1980s to the Mid-1990s: Transformational Leadership and Cultural Awareness Transformational Theories A major shift of interest in leadership research was sparked by the work of a political historian. Burns's (1978) book on great leaders differenti- ated transactional leaders, whose relationship to followers was based on mutually beneficial transactions, from transformational leaders, who influence followers to transcend personal interests and transform themselves into agents of collective achievement. This was an exciting perspective for a field locked in molecular analyses of trait-situation interactions and perceptual biases. Anticipating this development by a year, R. J. House (1977) published a theoretical analysis of charismatic leadership in which he analyzed the characteristics of historical leaders who elicited SPECIAL ISSUE; LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 35 extraordinary levels of devotion and commit- ment from followers—for example, Gandhi, Martin Luther King, Jr., and others. House identified three sets of characteristics that
  • 27. typified charismatic leaders. Personal character- istics included a strong belief in the moral righteousness of one's beliefs, high levels of self confidence, and a strong need to influence and dominate others. Behaviors included dramatic goal articulation, role modeling of desired attitudes and behaviors, image building, exhibit- ing high expectations of and confidence in followers, and arousing follower motives that were consistent with desired behavior (i.e., aggressive or altruistic motives). Finally, situ- ational influences might include high levels of environmental stress (e.g., economic crises, social upheaval) or an opportunity to express group goals in moralistic or spiritual terms. The most careful, empirical analyses of transformational leadership have been con- ducted by Bass and his associates (Bass, 1985; Bass & Avolio, 1990a, 1990b, 1993). Bass started by interviewing managers about transfor- mational leaders they had known. On the basis of the interviews Bass built and validated a questionnaire designed to measure transforma- tional leadership: the Multi-Factor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ). Factor analysis of the MLQ yielded seven factors, including three "transactional" factors (Contingent Reward, Management By Exception, and Laissez-Faire Leadership) that were associated with moderate to poor leadership effects and four transforma- tional factors that were associated with high levels of subordinate motivation and group or organizational success. The transformational factors included (a) Idealized Influence (cha- risma), reflecting extremely high levels of leader
  • 28. competency, trustworthiness, or both; (b) Inspi- rational Motivation, involving the articulation of the group's goals in emotional, moral, or visionary terms; (c) Intellectual Stimulation, entailing the encouragement of followers to think independently and creatively and to move away from past ideas or limitations; and (d) Individualized Consideration, relating to the leader's capacity to understand each follower's personal needs and goals. Bass (1998) reported data from many organizations in countries around the world that indicate that leaders who are rated highly on transformational leadership characteristics by superiors, peers, or subordi- nates are associated with high-performing teams and organizations. House and Shamir (1993) returned to the study of transformational leadership, emphasiz- ing the psychological processes of followers that mediated the effects of charismatic or transfor- mational leader actions. Weaving together path- goal theory (with its emphasis on expectancy motivation) with theories of intrinsic motivation and self-concept, they argued that transforma- tional leaders have several significant psycho- logical effects on followers. By placing the group's mission into moral or spiritual contexts, such leaders raise the salience of collective goals over personal or selfish interests of the followers. Second, tying the follower's self- concept to the group mission makes self-esteem contingent on group success and fosters self- motivation and self-regulation by followers.
  • 29. Both the theoretical explication and perfor- mance outcomes associated with transforma- tional leadership make the construct quite compelling but also leave the leadership scholar with another conundrum. Transformational theo- ries arc stated in terms of "universally" effective leadership behavior—that is, for all leaders in all situations. It is difficult to square that idea with the equally compelling evidence supporting various contingency theories that show that effective leadership is the result of the appropriateness or fit between particular behav- iors and particular situations. Some recent developments applying the concept of self- efficacy (Bandura, 1982) to leadership offers a potential resolution of this contradiction. Leadership Efficacy A number of contingency model studies showing that in-match leaders felt more confi- dent and in control (Chemers, Ayman, Sorod, & Akimoto, 1991; Chemers, Hays, Rhodewalt, & Wysocki, 1985) led me and my associates to conduct a series of studies designed to assess the role of leadership confidence or efficacy in performance. Chemers, Watson, and May (in press) reported concurrent, predictive, and discriminant validity for a measure of leadership efficacy in a longitudinal study of Reserve Officer Training Corps cadets. Cadets filled out a measure of self-perceived leadership ability and were rated for military leadership potential by their military science instructors. Several
  • 30. 36 CHEMERS months later, the same cadets were followed up through a 6-week U.S. Army-sponsored summer leadership camp in which cadets rotated through everyday leadership roles, participated in train- ing, and were tested in realistic military simulations. Ratings by course instructors, training camp superiors, peer cadets, and simulation exercise grading staff all revealed dramatic effects indicating superior perfor- mance of cadets who expressed greater confi- dence in their leadership capability. By way of discriminant validity, these performance ratings were not predicted by general self-esteem, and leadership efficacy did not predict nonleader- ship performance (e.g., marksmanship, land navigation). Watson, Chemers, and Preiser (1996) re- ported the results of leadership efficacy and team collective efficacy on the success of small college basketball teams (both men's and women's). Before the start of the basketball season, players were administered question- naires assessing leadership efficacy and collec- tive team efficacy. Teams were followed through the season, and the team's win-loss record provided the measure of performance. Path analyses revealed clear and significant support for the role of efficacy in team performance. The strongest predictor of team success was team collective efficacy, and team efficacy was, in turn, most strongly predicted by leadership efficacy self-ratings of the identified team leader
  • 31. (usually the captain). Other factors potentially related to team success (e.g., previous season record, number of returning players, starters, etc.) were controlled for in the analyses and did not prove as predictive as the efficacy measures. (No differences were found between men's and women's teams.) These findings on leadership efficacy provide a possible resolution of the contradiction between contingency theories, which make situation-specific predictions of leadership suc- cess and transformational theories, which make universal predictions. The fit between the leader's personal characteristics and situational parameters is an important determinant of a leader's confident and efficacious behavior— behavior that is the basis for the critical functional elements of leadership. That behav- ior, in turn, gives rise to the effective group processes and positive perceptions by observers that constitute transformational leadership. Transformational leadership measures leader- ship at the outcome (i.e., dependent variable) level, whereas the contingency theories tend to place more focus on the leader characteristics (i.e., independent variable) level. Leadership efficacy may be the psychological link between contingent fit and transformational behavior. Later in the present article a functional integra- tion of contemporary leadership theory will elaborate these critical functions. Cultural Differences
  • 32. Two streams of thought on cultural difference have had an influence on leadership theorizing. One stream involved the work of social psychologists who were interested in the effects of culture on social processes but not necessarily interested in leadership (e.g., Fiske, 1991; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1990, 1993). These theorists focused attention on the dramatic value differences (i.e., individualism vs. collectivism) among various national groups. Briefly stated, individualistic cultures (such as those of western Europe and English-speaking countries) place a high value on personal expression and achievement, whereas more collectivist cultures (including most of the rest of the world) are more concerned with group harmony and collective success. In a value-based approach more closely focused on leadership and motivation, Hofstede (1980, 1983) presented an analysis of four dimensions of national values with profound effects on organizational functioning. Power distance refers to people's comfort with and acceptance of large differences in power, influence, and wealth among groups or classes within the society. Uncertainty avoidance re- flects the extent to which individuals in a society resist risk and unexpected events by emphasiz- ing rules, norms, and expertise. Hofstede's third dimension was individualism-collectivism, and the fourth was masculinity-femininity, which differentiates cultures in which members value stereotypically masculine pursuits such as strength, competitiveness, and material achieve- ment from those in which members are more
  • 33. concerned with quality of life and concern for others. The basic thrust of the value theories is that organizational processes in different cultures will reflect what is considered appropriate and SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 37 important. For example, leaders should be seen as aloof and powerful in high power distance cultures; as expert, confident, and orderly in high uncertainty avoidance cultures; as paternal- istic and caring in collectivist cultures, and as macho and competitive in masculine cultures. Likewise, the needs of followers and the nature of the leader-follower relationship—for ex- ample, as manifested in a desire for structure (uncertainty avoidance) or personal achieve- ment (individualism)—would affect the ways that leaders and followers interact in the determination of follower satisfaction, morale, and motivation. A second, more focused and more empirical stream of research involved testing various leadership theories across different national groups. In Japan, Misumi (1984; Misumi & Peterson, 1985) conducted an extensive pro- gram of laboratory and field research, influenced by work in the United States on the LBDQ factors Initiation of Structure and Consideration. Misumi adapated the LBDQ to Japanese culture
  • 34. by identifying two broad classes of leader behavior: (a) behavior associated with work accomplishment through direction and produc- tivity emphasis (called Performance) and (b) behavior intended to maintain high group morale (called Maintenance). Misumi and Peter- son (1985) reported that the most productive work groups in Japanese organizations were led by supervisors who were high on both Perfor- mance and Maintenance behavior Ayman and Chemers (1983) reported similar results for Iranian managers. They factor analyzed a Persian translation of the LBDQ to which they added some probe items related to the tendency for the worker to identify the supervisor in fatherly terms (i.e., "My supervi- sor is like a kind father to me"). Ayman and Chemers found that structuring, consideration, and the new items collapsed into a single factor, which they labeled Benevolent Paternalism and which was strongly associated with subordinate satisfaction and performance ratings by superi- ors. Ayman and Chemers concluded that subor- dinates in highly collectivistic and power- oriented cultures derive satisfaction from a leader who is both directive and nurturant, whereas subordinates in individualistic, low- power cultures such as the United States are more satisfied with a leader who provides followers with autonomy and opportunities for personal achievement. Here again we see that the successful leader is the one who provides subordinates with an atmosphere conducive to the fulfillment of the followers' personal needs
  • 35. and goals but that the nature of those needs and goals is influenced by culturally socialized values. The 80 or 90 years of leadership research briefly described in the preceding pages cover a lot of territory. From contingency theories to transformational leadership, and cognitive, gen- der, and cultural factors, a complex pattern of theoretical and empirical material has been generated. The question remaining is whether a coherent integration of these seemingly dispar- ate findings is possible. A Functional Integration The apparent complexity of research findings and theoretical perspectives in the field of leadership might be reduced if one examined this literature by focusing on the major functions that leaders need to fulfill to be successful. I believe that there are three such functions. A leader must build credibility in the legitimacy of his or her authority by projecting an image that arouses feelings of trust in followers (image management). A leader must develop relation- ships with subordinates that enable those subordinates to move toward individual and collective goal attainment (relationship develop- ment). Finally, leaders must effectively use l i e knowledge, skills, and material resources pre- sent within their group to accomplish the group's mission {resource deployment). Image Management
  • 36. The definition of leadership provided earlier stressed that social influence is at the core of the leadership function. Influence depends on cred- ibility. For followers to abdicate personal autonomy and allow themselves to be led, they must believe that the leader's authority is legitimate. The information-processing models of leadership make clear the central role of the perceptions on which legitimacy is based. Individuals who are seen as behaving in ways that are consistent with observer-held leadership prototypes are afforded authority, and subse- quent perception, attention, and memory are more likely to reaffirm the leader's legitimacy. CHEMERS We also know that the specific traits or behaviors associated with leadership credibility vary somewhat by leadership domain (e.g., political leadership vs. business leadership) and across cultures (Lord, Foti, & De Vader, 1984; Lord, Foti, & Phillips, 1982). Nonetheless, a certain commonality exists across leadership prototypes. As Hollander (1958, 1964) demon- strated in his early work on leadership status accrual, leaders must be seen as competent in task-relevant abilities and as honest, trustwor- thy, and loyal to group norms and values. This makes a great deal of sense. Leaders need task-relevant competencies to move the group toward a goal, and they must be trustworthy to ensure that the goal pursued is in the collective interest.
  • 37. The literature on charismatic and transforma- tional leadership is consistent with this perspec- tive. Bass's (1985) concept of idealized influ- ence suggests that such leaders are seen as having exceptional abilities. R. J. House's (1977) discussion also addresses the extensive efforts of charismatic leaders in demonstrating their loyalty to the group cause—frequently by taking great risks (e.g., Martin Luther King, Jr.) or incurring significant hardship to follow the path of the cause (e.g., Gandhi, Joan of Arc). Relationship Development Many leadership theories focus on the lead- er's responsibility in motivating and guiding followers to enable them to achieve task goals. The work of Graen and his associates (Graen, 1976; Graen & Cashman, 1975; Graen, Cash- man, Ginsburgh, & Schiemann, 1978; Graen & Scandura, 1987) places the quality of the leader-follower relationship at the center of effective leadership. The literature suggests that effective relation- ships are based on the provision of levels of coaching and guidance that are appropriate to the follower's individual situation. The situation is determined by the follower's task-relevant skills and knowledge and personal values, needs, and goals. Patterns of cultural socializa- tion, personal histories, and contemporary fac- tors might all contribute to the follower's readiness for one or another type of treatment. Effective leadership, then, depends on the
  • 38. leader's ability to overcome potential egocentric and defensive biases to make accurate judg- ments about where the subordinate is and where the subordinate needs to go next. At the highest levels of leadership performance, these capabili- ties are captured by Bass's (1985) concepts of intellectual stimulation (i.e., delicately targeted coaching and guidance that arouse intrinsic motivation) and individualized consideration (i.e., the subtle but comprehensive awareness of the follower's situation). Resource Deployment Once a leader has established credibility and mobilized follower motivation, the resultant energies, knowledge, skills, and material re- sources must be harnessed and directed to achieve success in the group's mission. The successful deployment of the group's resources has two facets: first, the empowerment of the individuals in the group, and second, the effective interface of group processes with task and environmental demands. Both facets are influenced by contingency principles. Individuals are not always able to make the most effective use of their skills and abilities. Contemporary theories of intelligence (e.g., Sternbcrg, 1988) suggest that effective interac- tion with an individual's environment (i.e., successful utilization of personal resources) is influenced by the fit between the actor's set of skills and knowledge and the critical demands of the challenge. Contingency theory research
  • 39. conducted by me and my associates (Chemers & Ayman, 1985; Chemers et al., 1985, 1991) has found that leaders whose motivational orienta- tion (LPC score) was in match with environmen- tal factors (situational control) not only outper- formed less well-matched leaders but also showed higher levels of satisfaction, more positive mood and confidence, and lower levels of stress and stress-related illness. This pattern of findings suggests that leaders who are in a good "fit" with their leadership situation are more confident, and more of them perform at high levels. My later research on leadership efficacy (Watson et al., 1996) suggests that leadership confidence (resulting from a good person- situation match or as a dispositional characteris- tic) is associated with high levels of team performance and positive evaluations by follow- ers and observers. Feelings of efficacy may be one of the primary moderators of the effects of SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 39 contingency effects on leadership performance at the personal level. Both Fiedler's (1967; Fiedler & Chemers, 1974, 1984) contingency model and Vroom and Yetton's (1973) normative decision model are built around the notion that internal group processes, such as decision-making processes,
  • 40. must match with external task demands to ensure high levels of group performance. For example, overly centralized information-process- ing strategies and autocratic decision structures are likely to be more effective in highly routine and predictable leadership situations than they are in more ambiguous, less predictable situa- tions requiring creative solutions to novel problems. Effective leadership depends on recognizing the nature of the group's environ- ment and matching group process to external demand. On reflection, it appears that self-confidence or self-efficacy might play an important role in many aspects of leadership effectiveness. If one examines the three functions just discussed, one sees a role for self-confidence in each. Effective image management depends on projecting the appearance of competence. Confidence in one's abilities provides a good marker for compe- tence. When we observe competent people it is natural to infer that their confidence is based on some actual competence. R. J. House's (1977) analysis of charismatic leadership emphasized that outstanding leaders do indeed exhibit high levels of self-confidence. Staw and Barsade (1992) reported that MBA students with more positive emotional dispositions were seen as more appropriate for leadership roles by ob- servers in an assessment center management simulation. Confidence may also play a role in relation- ship development. Effective coaching and guid- ance are dependent on accurate perceptions of
  • 41. subordinates and cogent attributions about the causes of their behavior and performance. The major impediment to such perceptual accuracy are the ego-defensive motivations created by the leader's own concern for positive evaluations. When a group performs poorly, the leader may blame subordinates and be less attentive to problems caused by other factors, such as the support structure or his or her own shortcom- ings. Confidence in one's own abilities might allow leaders to be less concerned about such judgments and allow for empathic relationships with followers. The deployment of personal and team re- sources should be especially affected by confi- dence. On the personal level, a voluminous literature in social psychology tells us that confident and optimistic people are better able to cope with environmental demands (Scheier & Carver, 1985) and stressful life events (Taylor & Brown, 1988) and are more likely to take risks (Isen, Nygren, & Ashby, 1988) and solve problems creatively (Isen, Daubman, & Nan- icki, 1987)—all of which are characteristics that are related to effective leadership. At the level of team deployment that requires the ability to read the environment accurately and respond flexibly, confidence should also play a role. Staw and Barsade (1992) measured management students for positive or negative dispositional affect. More upbeat individuals performed more effectively at a business deci- sion task, integrating more information and
  • 42. making better decisions. Experiments by Guzzo (1986) and by Zaccaro, Peterson, Blair, and Gilbert (1990) also show that collective efficacy (i.e., the shared perception of group members of the capability and effectiveness of the group) has been positively related to group perfor- mance in both experimental and organizational settings. We also know that when leaders feel that they are in a congenial, accepting environment they are more likely to act in a directive, "take- charge" fashion (Eagly & Johnson, 1990) and that directive leaders are more likely to make effective use of their cognitive abilities (Fiedler & Garcia, 1987). Summary and a Few Conclusions This historical overview of leadership re- search reveals the extent to which this research area, like many others, is influenced by periodic fashions in research theory; for example, an emphasis on traits at one time; on cognition at another time, and so on. When we take a longer view, we are able to find common findings and streams of thought across theoretical perspec- tives. The functional integration offered in this article is an attempt to take such a perspective driven especially by an emphasis on what leaders must do to be effective, that is, to influence followers toward goal attainment. 40 CHEMERS
  • 43. My analysis argues that leaders must first establish the legitimacy of their authority by appearing competent and trustworthy to their followers. When leaders are extremely effective in image management they are seen as possess- ing remarkable, charismatic levels of capability and trust. Next, leaders must coach, guide, and support their followers in a way that allows the followers to contribute to group goal attainment while satisfying their own personal needs and goals. To do this, leaders must understand the abilities, values, and personalities of their subordinates, so they can provide the type of coaching and support that will be most effective. Sometimes leaders are so effective at creating a motivational environment that followers merge their personal goals with collective group goals and are transformed in the process. Finally, effective leaders must use the skills and abilities possessed by themselves and their followers to accomplish the group's mission. The first step in utilizing these resources is creating a sense of confidence and personal empowerment that encourages each group member to release his or her best efforts. The second step is focusing the resultant resources on the task environment in a way that provides the best fit between group process and environmental demand. Sensitive information processing and intelligent decision making are the keys to the group environmental interface. In the final paragraphs of this article I raise the hypothesis that leadership efficacy and group collective efficacy may be the most
  • 44. important contributors to each of the functional necessities of leadership performance. Feelings of efficacy in the leadership role are thought to lead to calm decision making, sensitive interper- sonal relations, ambitious goal setting, bold action, and long-term perseverance that energize and maintain the leader and the followers to effective common effort. References Ashour, A. S. (1973). The contingency model of leadership effectiveness: An evaluation. Organiza- tional Behavior and Human Performance, 9, 339-355. Ayman, R., & Chemers, M. M. (1983). The relationship of supervisory behavior ratings to work group effectiveness and subordinate satisfac- tion among Iranian managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 338-341. Bales, R. E, & Slater, P. E. (1955). Role differentia- tion in small decision-making groups. In T. Parsons (Ed.), Family, socialization, and interaction pro- cesses. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122- 147. Bass, B. M. (1981). Stogdill's handbook of leadership (2nd ed.). New York: Free Press. Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.
  • 45. Bass, B. M. (1998). Transformational leadership; Industry, military, and educational impact. Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum. Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1990a). The implications of transactional and transformational leadership for individual, team, and organizational development. In R. W. Woodman & W. A. Passmore (Eds.), Research in organizational change and develop- ment. Greenwich, CT: JAJ Press. Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1990b). Manual for the multifactor leadership questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1993). Transformational leadership: A response to critiques. In M. M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.). Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and directions (pp. 49-80). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Bass, B. M., Krusell, J., & Alexander, R. A. (1971). Male managers' attitudes toward working women. American Behavioral Scientist, 15, 221-236. Bowman, G. W., Worthy, N. B., & Greyser, S. A. (1965). Are women executives people? Harvard Business Review, 43, 14-28. Brown, K. A. (1984). Explaining group poor perfor- mance: An attributional analysis. Academy of Management Review, 9, 54-63. Burns, J. M. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper & Row.
  • 46. Calder, B. J. (1977). An attribution theory of leadership. In B. M. Staw & G. R. Salancik (Eds.), New directions in organizational behavior. Chi- cago: St. Clair Press. Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12). New York: Academic Press. Carlyle, T. (1907). Heroes and hero worship. Boston: Adams. (Original work published 1841) Chemers, M. M. (1997). An integrative theory of leadership. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chemers, M. M , & Ayman, R. (1985). Leadership orientation as a moderator of the relationship between performance and satisfaction of Mexican managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, //, 359-367." Chemers, M. M., Ayman, R., Sorod, B., & Akimoto, S. (1991, July). Self-monitoring as a moderator of SPECIAL ISSUE; LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 41 leader-follower relationships. Paper presented at the International Congress of Psychology, Brus- sels, Belgium. Chemers, M. M , Hays, R., Rhodewalt, F., &
  • 47. Wysocki, J. (1985). A person-environment analy- sis of job stress: A contingency model explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 628-635. Chemers, M. M., Watson, C. B., & May, S. (in press). Dispositional affect and leadership performance: A comparison of self-esteem, optimism, and efficacy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Cleven, W. A., & Fiedler, F. E. (1956). Interpersonal perceptions of open-hearth foremen and steel production. Journal of Applied Psychology, 40, 312-314. Deaux, K. (1984). From individual differences to social categories. American Psychologist, 39, 105-116. Eagly, A. R , & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233-256. Eagly, A. H., & KarauT S. J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60, 685-710. Eagly, A. R , Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3-22. Eden, D., & Leviatan, U. (1975). Implicit leadership theory as a determinant of the factor structure underlying supervisory behavior scales. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 736-741.
  • 48. Fiedler, F. E. (1955). The influence of leader-keyman relations on combat crew effectiveness. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 227-235. Fiedler, F. E. (1958). Leader attitudes and group effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fiedler, F. E. (1964). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 149-190). New York: Academic Press. Fiedler, F. E. (1967). A theory of leadership effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fiedler, F. E., & Chemers, M. M. (1974). Leadership and effective management. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Fiedler, F. E., & Chemers, M. M. (1984). Improving leadership effectiveness: The Leader Match con- cept (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Fiedler, F. E., Chemers, M. M., & Maher, L. (1976). Improving leadership effectiveness: The Leader Match concept. New York: Wiley. Fiedler, F. E., & Garcia, J. E. (1987), New approaches to effective leadership: Cognitive resources and organizational performance. New York: Wiley. Field, R. H. G., & House, R. J. (1990). A test of the Vroom-Yetton model using manager and subordi- nate reports. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 362-366.
  • 49. Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of human relations. New York: Free Press. Fleishmann, E. A., & Harris, E. F. (1962). Patterns of leadership behavior related to employee greivances and turnover. Personnel Psychology, 15, 43-54. Graen, G. (1976). Role making processes within complex organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychol- ogy. Chicago: Rand McNally. Graen, G., Alvarez, K., Orris, J. B., & MarteUa, S. A. (1970). Contingency model of leadership effective- ness: Antecedent and evidential results. Psychologi- cal Bulletin, 74,285-296. Gracn, G., & Cashman, J, (1975). A role-making model of leadership in formal organizations: A developmental approach. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.), Leadership frontiers. Kent, OH: Kent State University Press. Graen, G., Cashman, J. E, Ginsburgh, S., & Schiemann, W. (1978). Effects of linking-pin quality on the quality of working life of lower participants: A longitudinal investigation of the managerial understructure. Administrative Science Quarterly, 22, 491-504. Graen, G., & Scandura, T. A. (1987). Toward a psychology of dyadic organizing. Research in Organizational Behavior, 9, 175-208.
  • 50. Green, S. G., & Mitchell, T. R. (1979). Attribution^ processes of leaders in leader-member interac- tions. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- mance, 23, 429-458. Griffin, R. N. (1981). Relationships among indi- vidual, task design, and leader behavior variables. Academy of Management Journal, 23, 665-683. Guzzo, R. A. (1986). Group decision making and group effectiveness in organizations. In P. Good- man & Associates (Eds.), Designing effective work groups (pp. 34-71). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- mance, 16, 250-279. Halpin, A. W., & Winer, B. J. (1957). A factorial study of the leader behavior descriptions. In R. M. Stogdill & A. E. Coons (Eds.), Leader behavior: Its description and measurement. Columbus: Ohio State University Bureau of Business Research. Hastorf, A. R , Schneider, D., & Polefka, J. (1970). Person perception. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley. Heilman, M. E., Block, C. J., Martell, R. F., & Simon, M. C. (1989). Has anything changed? Current characterizations of men, women, and managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 935-942.
  • 51. 42 CHEMERS Hemphill, J. K. (1950). Leader behavior description. Columbus: Ohio State University Personnel Re- search Board. Hennig, M., & Jardim, A. (1977). The managerial woman. New York: Anchor Press. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences; Interna- tional differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hofstede, G. (1983). Motivation, leadership, and organization: Do American theories apply abroad? Organizational Dynamics, 9, 42-63. Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C , & Mason, I. (1998). Identification and leadership in small groups: Salience, frame of reference, and leader stereotypi- cality effects on leadership evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1248- 1263. Hollander, E. P. (1958). Conformity, status, and idiosyncrasy credit. Psychological Review, 65, 117-127. Hollander, E. P. (1964). Leaders, groups, and influence. New York: Oxford University Press. Hollander, E. P., & Julian, J. W. (1970). Studies in leader legitimacy, influence, and innovation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press.
  • 52. House, J. (1981). Social structure and personality. In M. Rosenberg & R. Turner (Eds.), Social psychol- ogy: Sociological perspectives (pp. 525-561). New York: Basic Books. House, R. J. (1971). Apath-goal theory of leadership. Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321-338. House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.), Leadership: The cutting edge. Carbondale: South- ern Illinois University Press. House, R. J., & Dessler, G. (1974). The path-goal theory of leadership: Some post-hoc and a priori tests. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.), Contingency approaches to leadership. Carbon- dale: Southern Illinois University Press. House, R. J., & Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Path-goal theory of leadership. Journal of Contemporary Business, 3, 81-98. House, R. J.. & Shamir, B. (1993). Integrating transformational, charismatic, and visionary theo- ries. In M. M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and directions (pp. 81-108). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nanicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1122-1131. Isen, A. M., Nygren, J. E., & Ashby, G. F. (1988). The
  • 53. influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: It's not worth the risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 710— 717. Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press. Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Kahn, R. L. (1951). An analysis of supervisory practices and components of morale. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and men. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska sympo- sium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Ne- braska Press. Kerr, S., & Jermier, J. M. (1978). Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement. Orga- nizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22, 375-403. Korman, A. K. (1966). "Consideration," "initiating structure," and organizational criteria. Personnel Psychology, 18, 349-360.
  • 54. Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally created social climates. Journal of Social Psychol- ogy, 10, 271-301. Lord, R. G. (1985). An information processing approach to social perceptions, leadership, and behavioral measurement in organizations. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 7). Greenwich, CT: JA1 Press. Lord, R. G., Binning, J. F., Rush, M. C , & Thomas, J. C. (1978). The effect of performance cues and leader behavior on questionnaire ratings of leader- ship behavior. Organizational Behavior and Hu- man Performance, 21, 27-39. Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & De Vader, C. (1984). A test of leadership categorization theory: Internal struc- ture, information processing, and leadership percep- tions. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- mance, 34, 343-378. Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & Phillips, i. S. (1982). A theory of leadership categorization. In J. G. Hunt, U. Sekaran, & C. Schriesheim (Eds.), Leadership: Beyond establishment views. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Lord, R. G., & Maher, K. J. (1991). Leadership and information processing: Linking perceptions and performance. Boston: Unwin Hyman. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and
  • 55. motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 224—253. SPECIAL ISSUE: LEADERSHIP RESEARCH AND THEORY 43 Meindl, J. R. (1990). On leadership: An alternative to the conventional wisdom. In B. A. Staw (Ed.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 159-203). New York: JAI Press. Misumi, J. (1984). The behavioral science of leadership (2nd ed.). Tokyo: Yuhikaku. Misumi, J., & Peterson, M. F. (1985). The perfor- mance-maintenance (PM) theory of leadership: Review of a Japanese research program. Adminis- trative Science Quarterly, 30, 198-223. Mitchell, T. R., Larson, J. R., & Green, S. G. (1977). Leader behavior and situational moderators in group performance: An attributional analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- mance, 18, 254-268. Mitchell, T. R., & Wood, R. E. (1980). Supervisors' responses to subordinates' poor performance: A test of an attribution model. Organizational Behav- ior and Human Performance, 25, 123-138. Nieva, V. R, & Gutek, B. A. (1981). Women and work: A psychological perspective. New York: Praeger. Peters, L. H., Hartke, D. D., & Pohlmann, J. T.
  • 56. (1983). Fiedler's contingency theory of leadership: An application of the meta-analysis procedure of Schmidt and Hunter. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 274-285. Phillips, J. S., & Lord, R. G. (1981). Causal attributions and perceptions of leadership. Organi- zational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 143-163. Podsakoff, P. M., Niehoff, B. P., MacKenzie, S. B., & Williams, M. L. (1993). Do substitutes for leadership really substitute for leadership? An empirical examination of Kerr and Jermier's situational leadership model. Organizational Behav- ior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 1—44. Ragins, B. R. (1989). Power and gender congruency effects in evaluations of male and female manag- ers. Journal of Management, 15, 65-76. Ragins, B. R. (1991). Gender effects in subordinate evaluations of leaders: Real or artifact? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12, 258—268. Ragins, B. R., & Sundstrom, E. (1989). Gender and power in organizations: A longitudinal perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 51-88. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ross, L. (1978). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Cognitive theories in social psychology. New York: Aca-
  • 57. demic Press. Rush, M. C , Thomas, J. C , & Lord, R. G. (1977). Implicit leadership theory: A potential threat to the internal validity of leader behavior questionnaires. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor- mance, 20, 93-110. Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol- ogy, 4, 219-247. Schein, V. E. (1973). The relationship of sex role stereotypes and requisite management characteris- tics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57, 95-100. Schein, V. E. (1975). The relationship of sex role stereotypes and requisite management characteris- tics among female managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 340-344. Staw, B. M. (1975). Attribution of the "causes" of performance: A general alternative interpretation of cross-sectional research on organizations. Organi- zational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 414-432. Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1992). Affect and managerial performance: A test of the sadder-but- wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypothesis. Adminis- trative Science Quarterly, 38, 304-331. Stemberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new theory of human intelligence. New York: Viking.
  • 58. Stogdill, R. M. (1948). Personal factors associated with leadership: A survey of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 25, 35-71. Strube, M. J., & Garcia, J. E. (1981). A meta- analytical investigation of Fiedler's contingency model of leadership effectiveness. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 307-321. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210. Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 41-133). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Triandis, H. C. (1993). The contingency model in cross-cultural perspective. In M. M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and directions (pp. 167-188). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Vroom, V. H., & Yetton, P. W. (1973). Leadership and decision-making. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press. Watson, C. B., Chemers, M. M., & Preiser, N. (1996, June). Collective efficacy: A multi-level analysis. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Society, San Francisco. Zaccaro, S. J., Peterson, C , Blair, V., & Gilbert, J. A. (1990, August). Some antecedents and conse-
  • 59. quences of collective control. Paper presented at the 98th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston. GENOGRAM ASSIGNMENT CFD 135—Nory Behana & Heather Jaffe TOTAL POINTS: 50 The goal of this assignment is to give you an opportunity to learn more about your family, genetic predispositions, environmental influences and apply knowledge you are gaining in this course to the analysis of your family of origin and the influences of various situations and experiences on you and your family. (FOR ALL NAMES IN THIS ASSIGNMENT, USE ONLY FIRST NAMES—EXCEPT FOR INCLUDING YOUR FIRST AND LAST NAME ON THE COVER SHEET.) This entire assignment will be completed in a Power Point file and must be submitted in one file. It needs to be readable, so carrying items from page to page to make print large enough to read is often necessary and encouraged. Part 1 – Genogram (10 points based on thoroughness and following directions) In Part 1, you will construct a Genogram of your family tracing your family of origin on both the maternal and the paternal sides back to your great grand-parents (i.e., 4 generations—you,
  • 60. your parents, your grandparents, and your great-grandparents). Use Power Point. Feel free to be creative! Photos are helpful. You will not be sharing it in class, but you will be expected to download the file to Blackboard to submit it. If you need help with Power Point or Blackboard, see the help desk at the library. Use the symbols provided and display a symbol for each family member. Number each symbol to correlate with the information provided on the Factual Data Sheet (Part 2). This may take one or more separate slides in Power Point. DO NOT USE THE RELATIONSHIP DESCRIBING LINE VARIATIONS EXPAINED IN THE TEXT ON PAGE 192 FOR THIS ASSIGNMENT. General Criteria: · Include 4 generations representing both maternal and paternal sides of the family. · Draw men on the left and women on the right (except in complex situations, then just explain) · Draw siblings in chronological order with the first born on the left. · Use appropriate symbols for males and females. · Use appropriate symbols for marriage, divorce, unmarried couples, adoptions, etc. · Start with yourself and highlight your symbol—you are to be labeled #1. · Numbers should coincide with the Factual Data Sheet which is Part 3. Genogram Symbols x
  • 61. x MALE: FEMALE: DEATH: |_________| MARRIAGE: Husband on left, wife on right: |_________| CHILDREN: Listed in birth order, beginning on the left with the oldest: 1 child: 3 children: |_________| |_________| | | | | Twins M/F: Twins F/F: |_________| |_________| __|_ _____|____ Current Pregnancy: Miscarriage or abortion: |_________| |_________| X Living together: Marital separation:
  • 62. |_ _ _ _ _ _ | |_____/____| / Divorce: Adoptive or Foster Children: |_____//___| |_________| // | | F A Part 2 –Factual Data Sheet (10 points based on thoroughness and following directions) The third part of your project is a factual data sheet that is to be typed in chart form. You can use Word or some other typing program for this part of the project. You must provide 10 columns with as many rows as numbered symbols on your Genogram. The information provided in each column will correspond to the family member represented by number on the Genogram. A sample is given below, but you may find that setting it up in Landscape instead of Portrait will give you more space to use. The length will depend upon the number of family members included and the amount of information about each one. Feel free to extend some columns to allow for enough space to accommodate your answers. The 10 columns are: 1. Number (correlates to Genogram) 2. Name 3. Relationship to you
  • 63. 4. Date of birth/death 5. Approximate date of marriage/divorce/remarriage 6. Occupation/profession 7. Major life events 8. Physical/health problems 9. Personality traits 10. Rituals/traditions Include a minimum of 10 family members including you. If you cannot provide information for a column put in a “?” Do not leave a column blank. Dates can be estimated. This is intended to be an opportunity to connect with family members, gather unknown information and share family stories. If any part of this project creates a problem for you such as in cases of adoption, foster care, divorces, estranged family members, etc., please see me about finding enough family members to use in the project and/or conduct an alternate project based on those issues. Feel free to have the columns extend to accommodate your information, YOU ARE NOT LIMITED TO 10, THAT IS THE MINIMUM. # Name Rel BD/DD Mar/Div Occupation Life events Health Personality Rituals/trad. 1 You
  • 64. 2 3 4
  • 66. 9 10 Part 3 – Family Profile (30 points based on thoroughness and accuracy in applying concepts from the text to the family
  • 67. dynamics and use of APA format along with correct grammar and spelling.) (Content to be equivalent to approximately 3-5 pages of narrative text--double spaced, 12-point font if it were in a regular paper. In Power point, you can single space to save space. Don’t skimp.) This part of your paper describes the characteristics and composition of your family of origin. It should be written in text (narrative) form and follow APA (American Psychological Association) guidelines. In the narrative parts of your Family Profile, cite the text with the correct citation format relating text information to topics covered. Cite the text at least 10 times in this section. For example, when you discuss your family’s SES, you could define it citing the text before continuing with analysis of your family’s SES. A lot of the grading will consist of analyzing the quality of the text material chosen and how it is related to the family situation described. You will be graded on both the quantity and the quality of the citations. They are best done with an explanation of the term or concept and then applying it to your narrative. Your family profile should include the following sections: 1. Composition of your family of origin, including ages of all members, gender and sibling order. Describe the personality of each and analyze the influence the influence each has had upon your development. 2. Occupations of parent(s), education achieved and SES— Socioeconomic Status. Describe and analyze the influence the influence each has had upon your development. 3. Size and type of community/communities in which your family lived during your birth-18 years of age. Describe and analyze the influence the influence each has had upon your development.
  • 68. 4. Location and connections with extended family members. Describe and analyze the influence the influence each has had upon your development. 5. Health status of you and your immediate family. Describe and analyze the influence each has had upon your development. Family health and behavior patterns are often transmitted from one generation to the next. For the closing of Part 3, identify the generational transmissions in your family. Refer to your completed Part 1 and Part 2 of your project for this information. TO SUBMIT TO BLACKBOARD: · Do all of the work in one Power Point file, even the grid and narrative parts · Submit it on the Blackboard site for this course using the “Turnitin” link. Part GENOGRAM RUBRIC Points 1 Part 1 – Genogram (10 points based on thoroughness and following directions) In Part 1, you will construct a Genogram of your family tracing your family of origin on both the maternal and the paternal sides back to your great grand-parents (i.e., 4 generations—you, your parents, your grandparents, and your great-grandparents). Use Power Point. Feel free to be creative! Photos are helpful. Use the symbols provided and display a symbol for each family member. Number each symbol to correlate with the information provided on the Factual Data Sheet (Part 2). DO NOT USE THE RELATIONSHIP DESCRIBING LINE VARIATIONS THAT ARE EXPAINED IN THE TEXT ON PAGE 192.
  • 69. 10 possible 2 Factual Data Sheet (10 points based on thoroughness and following directions) The third part of your project is a factual data sheet that is to be typed in chart form. You can use Word or some other typing program for this part of the project. You must provide 10 columns with as many rows as numbered symbols on your Genogram. The information provided in each column will correspond to the family member represented by number on the Genogram. A sample is given below, but you may find that setting it up in Landscape instead of Portrait will give you more space to use. The length will depend upon the number of family members included and the amount of information about each one. Feel free to extend some columns to allow for enough space to accommodate your answers. 10 possible 3 Family Profile (30 points based on thoroughness and accuracy in applying concepts from the text to the family dynamics and use of APA format along with correct grammar and spelling.) (Content to be equivalent to approximately 3-5 pages of narrative text--double spaced, 12 point font if it were in a regular paper. In Power point, you can single space to save space. Don’t skimp.) This part of your paper describes the characteristics and composition of your family of origin. It should be written in text (narrative) form and follow APA (American Psychological Association) guidelines. In the narrative parts of your Family Profile, cite the text with the correct citation format relating text information to topics covered. Cite the text at least 10 times in this section. For example, when you discuss your family’s SES, you could define it citing the text before
  • 70. continuing with analysis of your family’s SES. A lot of the grading will consist of analyzing the quality of the text material chosen and how it is related to the family situation described. 30 possible Total 50 possible Comments Your total 1 Promoting More Integrative Strategies for Leadership Theory-Building Bruce J. Avolio University of Nebraska—Lincoln The agenda for theory and research in the field of leader- ship studies has evolved over the last 100 years from
  • 71. focuses on the internal dispositions associated with effec- tive leaders to broader inquiries that include emphases on the cognitions, attributes, behaviors, and contexts in which leaders and followers are dynamically embedded and in- teract over time. Leadership theory and research has reached a point in its development at which it needs to move to the next level of integration— considering the dynamic interplay between leaders and followers, taking into account the prior, current, and emerging context—for continued progress to be made in advancing both the science and practice of leadership. Keywords: authentic leadership development, integrative theories, contingency theories The field of leadership studies has frequently focusedon the leader to the exclusion of other equally im-portant components of the leadership process (Rost, 1991). Indeed, if the accumulated science of leadership had produced a periodic chart of relevant elements analogous to that in the field of chemistry, one might conclude that leadership studies had traditionally focused too narrowly on a limited set of elements, primarily highlighting the leader yet overlooking many other potentially relevant el- ements of leadership such as the follower and context. Highlighting this issue, Zaccaro and Klimoski (2001) noted, Most theories of organizational leadership in the psychological literature are largely context free. For example, leadership is typically considered without adequate regard for the structural contingencies that affect and moderate its conduct. We maintain, however, that organizational leadership cannot be modeled effec- tively without attending to such considerations. (p. 12)
  • 72. Potential Benefits of Taking a More Integrative Focus By working toward identifying and integrating all of the elements that constitute leadership, researchers can position the field of leadership studies to better address questions such as the degree to which leaders are born versus made; whether what constitutes leadership effectiveness is more universal or culturally specific; whether different forms of leadership, such as charismatic or transformational, are more or less likely to emerge on the basis of the stability or criticality of the context; and whether one style of leadership is more or less effective depending on the contingencies and demands facing leaders and followers. Addressing each of the above issues requires an ex- amination of leadership that considers the relevant actors, context (immediate, direct, indirect, etc.), time, history, and how all of these interact with each other to create what is eventually labeled leadership. This recommendation is in line with suggestions made by Marion and Uhl-Bien (2001), who criticized the field of leadership studies for its tendency to follow a more reductionist strategy, concluding that “leaders are one element of an interactive network that is far bigger than they” (p. 414). Moving toward more integrative strategies for theory- building and testing has also been recommended by Lord, Brown, Harvey, and Hall (2001) with respect to one of the more traditionally leadercentric areas of leadership studies. Lord et al. (2001) discussed what they called a connection- ist-based model of leadership prototype generation to em- phasize how perceptions of leadership are contingent on the context and the dynamic states in which such mental rep-
  • 73. resentations are created. They argued that one’s schema of leadership can be a function of the culture, leader, follower, task, or behavioral inputs and how they each interact to form leadership categories and behavioral scripts. Lord et al. (2001) emphasized that “leadership categories are gen- erated on-the-fly to correspond to the requirements of dif- ferent contexts, tasks, subordinates or maturational stages of a group or organization” (p. 314) and that “leadership perceptions are grounded within a larger social, cultural, task and interpersonal environment” (p. 332). Their model indicates that even one’s internal representation of how leadership is mentally construed and how one makes sense of situations appears to be a function of the proximal (group or task) and distal (organizational or national cul- ture) context in which those mental representations are formed. I begin the promotion of more integrative theories of leadership by first discussing the importance of followers to what constitutes leadership. I then examine how the inclu- Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Bruce J. Avolio, Gallup Leadership Institute, Department of Management, College of Business, University of Nebraska—Lincoln, CBA 114, P.O. Box 880491, Lincoln, NE 68588-0491. E-mail: [email protected] 25January 2007 ● American Psychologist Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0003-066X/07/$12.00 Vol. 62, No. 1, 25–33 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.1.25 sion of context found its way into the field of leadership
  • 74. studies, highlighting early work on contingency theories of leadership. This discussion is followed by one of more recent work on leadership, leadership development, and strategic leadership, demonstrating the necessity of advo- cating more integrative strategies to advance the science and practice of leadership. Examining a Follower Focus Grint (2000) described the field of leadership studies as being theoretically inadequate from its inception because it primarily excluded followers when explaining what consti- tuted leadership. Grint (2005) stated that “it only requires the good follower to do nothing for leadership to fail” (p. 133) and that it is the followers who teach leadership to leaders. Howell and Shamir (2005) concluded that “follow- ers also play a more active role in constructing the leader- ship relationship, empowering the leader and influencing his or her behavior, and ultimately determining the conse- quences of the leadership relationship” (p. 97). Lord, Brown, and Frieberg (1999) asserted, “the follower remains an unexplored source of variance in understanding leader- ship processes” (p. 167). After reviewing the accumulated research on transfor- mational leadership theory, which has been the most fre- quently researched leadership theory over the last two decades, I could only find three published studies that specifically focused on how follower characteristics mod- erated the effects of leadership on work outcomes (i.e., Dvir & Shamir, 2003; Ehrhart & Klein, 2001; Wofford, Whittington, & Goodwin, 2001). For example, Ehrhart and Klein reported that followers scoring high in achievement orientation, self-esteem, and risk-taking were more likely to be drawn to transformational leaders. What this research suggests is that a follower’s deci-
  • 75. sion to follow a leader may be a more active process, based on the extent to which the leader is perceived as represent- ing the follower’s values and identity (Howell & Shamir, 2005). Unfortunately, most leadership research has consid- ered the follower a passive or nonexistent element when examining what constitutes leadership. An exception to this conclusion is the work that has been done on relational models of leadership, such as the vertical dyad linkage (Dansereau, Graen, & Haga, 1975) or leader–member ex- change theory (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). The main gist of leader–member exchange theory is that the quality of the exchange relationship between leaders and followers will determine the qualities of leadership and outcomes achieved. Early Beginnings of Exploring the Context Although leadership studies dating back to the early 20th century focused more on the leader than on the context of leadership in which it was observed (Ayman, 2003; Avolio, Sosik, Jung, & Berson, 2003; Day, 2000), there have been some important inquiries into what constitutes leadership throughout human history that included reference to the context. For example, Plato’s philosophical discussions of the moral and ethical purpose of leadership highlighted the relevance of the context. Similar to discussions of ethical leadership, early writ- ings on what constituted charismatic leadership also fo- cused on the context. Weber (1924/1947) recognized that there were certain unique qualities of leaders that differen- tiated the bureaucratic from the charismatic leader. Weber argued that a social crisis was necessary to promote the emergence of charismatic leaders. Although subsequent research on the emergence of charismatic leadership has challenged Weber’s base assumption (Bass, 1990), the