SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 7
Download to read offline
Evidence for the Effectiveness of Inquiry-Based, Particulate-Level
Instruction on Conceptions of the Particulate Nature of Matter
Chad A. Bridle*,†
and Ellen J. Yezierski‡
†
Science Department, Grandville High School, Grandville, Michigan 49418, United States
‡
Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Miami University, Oxford, Ohio 45056, United States
ABSTRACT: Research has shown that students in traditional college-preparatory
chemistry courses become masters of mathematical equations without an
understanding of the conceptual basis for the mathematical relationships. This
problem is rooted not only in what curriculum is presented to students, but also in
how it is experienced by the students. Ample evidence exists in support of both
inquiry-based instruction and the use of particulate-level models in instruction as a
means for improving students’ conceptual understandings in chemistry. Little
evidence exists, though, for the effectiveness of an instructional model that involves
the merger of these two methods. In an effort to address this gap, a series of
laboratory and classroom activities was created that blended guided inquiry-based
instructional practices with particulate-level modeling experiences. The content of
the curriculum focused exclusively on phases of matter and chemical versus
physical changes in matter. This research explores the novel curriculum’s effect on
student understanding of the particulate nature of matter in two sections of high school chemistry. Qualitative and quantitative
evidence supporting the curriculum is presented.
KEYWORDS: High School/Introductory Chemistry, Chemical Education Research, Hands-On Learning/Manipulatives,
Inquiry-Based/Discovery Learning, Constructivism
FEATURE: Chemical Education Research
■ INTRODUCTION
Chemistry students have long demonstrated their ability to
solve algebraic problems without a clear understanding of the
conceptual background of the problem.1−4
This lack of a
scientifically accurate conceptual framework for the very
foundations of chemistry is not only a problem by itself, but
its effects extend into all other aspects of student performance
in chemistry.5
Two different approaches to this issue have been
explored: curriculum focused on particulate-level representa-
tions of matter,6,7
and inquiry-based instructional models.8,9
This paper describes the implementation and effectiveness of a
novel curriculum that merges particulate-level modeling with
inquiry-based instruction.
■ BACKGROUND
Johnstone describes the three different levels of representation
in chemistry: macroscopic, submicroscopic, and symbolic.10
A
particulate understanding of atoms and their properties is
central to explaining any concept that is observed or described
on the macroscopic or symbolic levels.11
The importance of a
strong understanding of the particulate nature of matter must
be considered carefully. How do we appropriately represent
and describe the particulate level? What sort of prior knowledge
of the particulate level do students bring to the classroom?
How do instructors properly guide their students to an
appropriate understanding of the particulate nature of matter?
Because the particulate world of chemistry is invisible and
theoretical, descriptions of this level must include models. In
using models, the ability of the learner to comprehend the
meaning of the model must be considered. Harrison and
Treagust describe three different ability levels.12
At the most
basic level, learners interpret models as exact replicas of reality.
Thus, an atom that is represented as a red circle actually is a red
circle. As modeling ability increases, students become better at
differentiating between what a model is intended to show about
a situation and the limitations of the model. In a later study,
Harrison and Treagust describe a successful method for
employing atomic models in instruction.13
Their work showed
that part of the instructional process must involve exploring and
discussing the merits and limitations of a particular model.
Similarly, students bring varying degrees of understanding
about the basic behaviors of matter. As far as incorrect
conceptions about atoms and molecules, students frequently
struggle with size, shape, and composition.14
These mis-
conceptions include the assumption that one can see atoms and
molecules under a microscope, that their volume and weight
can change depending on conditions, and, for example, that
water molecules can contain atoms other than hydrogen and
oxygen. This lack of clarity leads to struggles with classification
of matter, such as being able to distinguish between compounds
and mixtures.15
Considering physical changes in matter,
Published: December 14, 2011
Article
pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc
© 2011 American Chemical Society and
Division of Chemical Education, Inc. 192 dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198
This is an open access article published under an ACS AuthorChoice License, which permits
copying and redistribution of the article or any adaptations for non-commercial purposes.
common misconceptions include the idea that boiling liquids
involves the expansion of molecules or even the breaking and
forming of chemical bonds within the molecules.16,17
Lastly,
these misconceptions lead to students’ inability to distinguish
clearly between physical and chemical changes in matter.18
This evidence suggests that appropriate instruction in this
field is a matter of constructing a correct conceptual framework
within the minds of the students and not simply memorizing
facts and processes. Using inquiry-based instructional ap-
proaches in chemistry has been proven effective. Hewson and
Hewson used inquiry instruction in the design and
implementation of curriculum regarding the concepts of
mass, volume, and density.19
They found a statistically
significant improvement not only in correct conceptual
understandings, but also in a reduced number of misconcep-
tions. Similar positive results were found by Lewis and Lewis
upon implementing a peer-led guided inquiry curriculum.9
The literature shows that both particulate-level instruction
and inquiry-based pedagogy improve students’ conceptual
understanding of chemistry. Gabel suggested both methods
for improving conceptual understanding, but little research
exists regarding the merger of these two instructional models.20
The evidence within the literature merits the exploration of the
fusion of the two methods in a novel inquiry-based, particulate-
level curriculum.
■ THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
The design of the intervention and the overall study is guided
by learning theories grounded in work by Piaget and more
recent constructivist points of view, including the conceptual
change model. DeBoer noted that the purpose of science
education goes beyond simply supplying students with facts
and figures.21
Students must be able to think like scientists:
effectively using their scientific knowledge, carefully studying
and observing their world, and developing new understandings
from these observations. Piaget’s theory of how humans
constructed knowledge caused educators to reconsider how
content was delivered. Prior models of instruction assumed that
the information presented by the teacher was simply transferred
to students, as though they were a blank sheet of paper waiting
to be filled. Piaget forced educators to consider learning from
the learner’s perspective. He believed that not only did each
learner construct his or her own understanding of a subject
individually, but also that any new knowledge had to be
integrated with the learner’s current framework of under-
standing.22
In regard to the particulate nature of matter,
students cannot simply be given information about the invisible
particulate universe and be expected to accept it. Instead,
students must explore and construct their own understandings
of the topic so that a specific representation of the atomic level
fits with their understandings of the world.23
This model for
instruction parallels not only Piaget’s theory of student
development, but also the philosophies of science research.24
Learners and researchers alike approach a new situation with
their current conceptual framework. Any new information or
findings may not fit with their current framework. Individuals
must then construct a new framework that accommodates the
new knowledge and can be accepted as reasonable and useful.
The process of constructing a true, correct understanding of the
nature of matter allows students to more accurately describe
any novel situations they may experience within and beyond
chemistry. This approach to chemistry instruction, grounded in
constructivist learning theories, necessitates the use of instruc-
tional and laboratory techniques that allow students to explore
and find meaning in a given situation.20
■ RESEARCH DESIGN
This pilot study aimed to investigate the effectiveness of
inquiry-based, particulate modeling experiences in improving
students’ conceptual understanding of chemistry. Using a quasi-
experimental, primarily quantitative study with a one-group
pretest−posttest design,25
the intervention engaged students in
inquiry-based activities with various particulate-level represen-
tations of matter early in the school year. The curriculum
implemented in the study was designed by a team of chemistry
educators as part of Grand Valley State University’s Target
Inquiry teacher professional development program. The
classroom activities were designed to be used in the first few
weeks of an introductory chemistry course. These laboratory
experiences were intended to provide students with a strong
particulate-level, conceptually based understanding of the basic
behaviors of matter, better equipping them for further learning
activities in chemistry.
■ CONTEXT
The novel curriculum was implemented in two high school
chemistry sections in a large, suburban high school in the
Midwest United States. One of the authors (Bridle) was the
instructor for the two chemistry sections that were studied.
Both sections were exposed to the same curriculum; data were
not compared to a control group or established norms. The
population in 3rd hour, n = 26, included 13 female and 13 male
participants, while the population in 4th hour, n = 28, consisted
of 13 female and 15 male participants. The participants were
15−17 years old, predominantly Caucasian and middle class.
The curriculum was implemented during five and one-half 60-
min instructional periods over a three-week period as described
in Table 1.
■ DESCRIPTION OF INTERVENTIONS
Describing and Categorizing Matter Using
Particulate-Level Models
The teacher guide and student guide for Putting the World in a
Box26
are available as cited through the Target Inquiry Web
Table 1. Summary of Inquiry Activities Implemented
Inquiry Activity Description
Instructional
Time, Min.
Putting the World in a
Box
Constructing an understanding of appropriate particulate-level modeling. Establishing particulate-level descriptions of
solids, liquids, gases, elements, compounds, and mixtures. Establishing appropriate methods for categorizing matter.
120
Change You Can
Believe In
Establishing clear, particulate-level definitions for physical changes and chemical changes. 90
The Only Thing
Constant in Life Is
Change
Establishing an appropriate connection between particulate-level definitions of physical and chemical changes, and
macroscopic observations.
120
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198193
site. This activity was designed to specifically target
misconceptions related to the description and categorization
of matter. The content addressed in this activity was the
students’ first exposure to particulate-level models in their
chemistry course. The literature describes students’ varying
abilities to appropriately interact with models and how this may
affect student learning regarding particulate-level instruc-
tion.12,13
As a result, a significant portion of the activity calls
for students to explore the usefulness and limitations of various
particulate-level representations of matter such that students
may construct an understanding of the particulate-level
behaviors of solids, liquids, and gases as they relate to
observable, macroscopic behaviors. Second, the students use a
similar approach to establish particulate- and macroscopic-level
knowledge of elements, compounds, and mixtures. Lastly, the
activity aims to merge these two knowledge structures in an
effort to establish appropriate procedures for describing and
classifying matter. Students explore the similarities and
differences between elements, compounds, and mixtures and
construct an appropriate classification scheme for particulate-
level representations of matter.
Constructing a Particulate-Level Conception of Physical
and Chemical Changes
The activity Change You Can Believe In27
is the first of two
activities, both available as cited through the Target Inquiry
Web site, that address the concepts of physical and chemical
changes. This activity uses cards that illustrate particulate-level
examples of physical and chemical changes. Figure 1 is a sample
card, representing the single replacement reaction between
magnesium metal and a hydrochloric acid solution.
Students were not given information about what substances
or processes were represented by each card. This was
intentional, so as to prevent students from falling back on
their macroscopic understandings and experiences to describe
particulate-level situations. Students consider the cards with
only their prior knowledge as a basis and categorize the cards as
either physical or chemical changes. The student guides were
constructed in such a way that led to discussion and debate
between students. This process, guided by the instructor,
allowed students to modify and clarify their particulate-level
understanding of physical and chemical changes.
Constructing a Symbolic and Macroscopic Conception of
Physical and Chemical Changes
The Only Thing Constant in Life Is Change28
was the second
activity intended to address the concepts of physical and
chemical changes. Students performed actual macroscopic
examples of the processes represented by the physical and
chemical changes cards. A more traditional laboratory approach
would have students make macroscopic observations and then
ask them to relate these observations to the particulate level.
This second activity reversed this process. Students have
already categorized the laboratory activities as physical or
chemical changes in the previous activity. For example, the
students would have classified the process in Figure 1 as a
chemical change. When this reaction is performed in the
laboratory, one observation students would make would be the
evolution of a gas. Students then make macroscopic
observations of all of the processes illustrated in the cards
and analyze their observations to determine the usefulness of
each type of observation in defining an observed process as
physical or chemical. For example, students would consider
whether the evolution of a gas was a sign of a physical change, a
chemical change, both, or neither. In the case of gas evolution,
whether the production of steam, a gas, from boiling water fits
this same category caused students to consider limitations and
precise definitions for their macroscopic observations. This
alternative approach forced students to use macroscopic
observations as tools for constructing a model of particulate-
level behavior. A more traditional approach may have led
students to consider the production of a gas, for example, as a
definitive sign of a chemical change. The new approach results
in students concluding that particulate-level definitions of
physical and chemical changes are much more reliable
descriptors of macroscopic processes.
Instruments and Data Collection
In determining the effectiveness of the activities in bringing
about conceptual understanding, a published conceptual
chemistry instrument, Particulate Nature of Matter (ParNo-
MA), was used.29
The instrument is a 20-question, multiple-
choice test specifically focusing on students’ concepts at the
particulate level related to phases of matter and phase changes.
Each question addresses an aspect of the particulate behavior of
matter, with distracters being directly related to common
misconceptions. The authors of the instrument report 100%
validity of the appropriate correct responses when examined by
several of their colleagues as well as establishing the internal
consistency of the instrument when administered to an
introductory college chemistry section, N = 72, Cronbach’s α
= 0.78.
Students in this study were given the instrument both as a
pretest and posttest in order to measure any changes in their
conceptual framework. Student scores were reported as a
percent of the number of questions they correctly answered out
of 20. After obtaining parental consent and student assent
according to the permissions granted by the Human Research
Review Committee at Grand Valley State University, the
pretest was administered to students during the first few days of
the course. Students experienced the treatment curriculum
during the third through fifth weeks of the course, and the
posttest was then administered in a similar fashion during the
ninth week, at the end of the marking period. The significant
gap in time between the pretest, the treatment, and the posttest
limits any test−retest bias.
On the basis of the data from this instrument, structured
interviews were conducted with five students during the second
semester of the course, 4 months after the treatment and 3
months after the posttest, to probe specific concepts. The
significant time lapse from the treatment was intended with the
hope that the knowledge structures of the participants revealed
Figure 1. Sample activity card from Change You Can Believe In
depicting the reaction of Mg(s) with HCl(aq) at the particulate level.
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198194
in the interviews reflected their long-term understandings.
Following an analysis of student gains on the ParNoMA, five
students from both class periods were selected as potential
participants in the interview based on the gains they
experienced. One participant experienced no gain (0%) in his
ParNoMA score, three students experienced moderate gains
(15−20%), and one experienced large gains (50%).
The protocol began with participants being presented with a
clear liquid (water) boiling on a hot plate. The interview
protocol did not describe the substance as water to avoid any
prior knowledge bias of water’s behavior. The beaker contained
water, though, for practical purposes. A small plate of glass was
placed above the boiling liquid and condensation collected on
the glass. Students were then provided with a pictorial
representation of the situation that included several call-outs,
or zoomed-in views, of the situation (Figure 2). The three call-
outs referred to particulate behaviors within the beaker,
between the beaker and the glass, and at the surface of the
glass. Students were told that the substance in the beaker
consisted of a compound comprising two elements, A and B.
They were provided with small, blue triangles that represented
atoms of element A and yellow squares that represented atoms
of element B. The instructor constructed a particulate-level
representation of the compound, consisting of one of each
atom.
The participants were then asked to construct and describe
what they believed the particulate-level behavior of the
substance would be in each of the call-outs. The instructor
recorded video of the students modeling their ideas, including
audio of their descriptions. The protocol also asked students to
describe the changes the substance experienced and to identify
them as physical or chemical changes.
■ RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
ParNoMA Results
Statistical analyses using SPSS software were conducted to
determine whether any changes in student scores, reported as
percentages, between pre- and posttest occurred. While
itemized responses for each question were obtained, individual
questions on this survey have not been established as reliable
measures of individual concepts. Thus, probing individual
questions for further information about student understanding
was not pursued.
The data were collected from two different sections of the
course. The authors wished to consider both data sets together,
so the appropriateness of consolidation was considered first.
Statistical analyses were conducted on student gains between
pre- and posttest (posttest minus pretest). An Anderson−
Darling test for normality was performed on each section using
student gains. The test, which is optimal for small samples,
showed that results from both periods, 3rd hour with p =
0.0755, and 4th hour with p = 0.349, could be considered
normally distributed.
As shown in Table 2, mean student pre- and posttest scores
for each class do appear different. A test for equivalence was
carried out to determine whether a priori differences existed
between the two different class periods. The analysis of the
ParNoMA pretest scores was done using two, one-tailed t-tests
as prescribed by Lewis and Lewis.30
Results showed that the
two groups could not be considered statistically equivalent to
each other. The differences between these two populations may
lie in which students get scheduled in each hour. The
complexity of the high school scheduling process often allows
for honors or elective classes to only be offered during certain
hours. This often results in specific populations of students
unintentionally remaining grouped together in other classes.
This phenomenon may be at the root of the difference.
Considering each section individually, a paired samples t-test
was used to explore differences in student pre- and posttest
scores. As summarized in Table 3, students in 3rd hour did not
show a significant improvement in score, while 4th hour results
did show a statistically significant improvement, p = 0.011. The
standardized effect size index (Cohen’s d) for 4th hour was
0.51, indicating that the intervention produced a medium effect.
A power analysis using G*Power 3.1.2 revealed that the sample
size for each period was too small to detect a small effect. This
is important in drawing inferences from 3rd hour results. Figure
3 further illustrates student gains, showing the pretest and
posttest means and 95% confidence intervals for 3rd and 4th
hour results individually. This graph illustrates that both classes
achieved similar results on the posttest, despite 4th hour
students starting with a much lower mean pretest score.
The statistically significant difference between pre- and
posttest ParNoMA means for 4th hour warranted further
Figure 2. Diagram used during interviews for participants to illustrate
their particulate-level understanding of phenomena depicted: Water
boiling in a beaker, evaporating, and then condensing on a glass plate.
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for ParNoMA
Scores by Class Period
Pretest Scores, % Posttest Scores, %
Class Period (N) Mean SD Mean SD
third Period (26) 40.58 14.92 43.65 20.13
fourth Period (28) 34.82 15.30 44.46 23.43
Table 3. Student Gains by Percent: Paired Samples t-Test
Class Period
(N)
Mean
Difference SD 95% CI
F
Values
p Values (Two-
Tailed)
3rd Period
(26)
3.07 13.42 [2.34,
8.50]
1.17 0.253
4th Period
(28)
9.64 18.75 [2.37,
16.91]
2.72 0.011
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198195
considerations. A Pearson correlation between pre- and posttest
scores showed a significant moderate positive correlation,
0.601, p = 0.001. This relationship suggests that participants
who performed well on the pretest also performed well on the
posttest. This result shows some relationship between a
participant’s prior knowledge and their resulting knowledge
after the described treatment. A second Pearson correlation
between student pretest scores and student normalized gains
[(post − pre)/(100% − pre)] on the ParNoMA showed no
significant relationship, 0.036, p = 0.854. This suggests that the
amount of conceptual gain, as evidenced by the normalized gain
on the ParNoMA, was not dependent on the participant’s
pretest score, independent of the absolute magnitude of the
gain. Thus, students with a strong prior knowledge, while
performing at a higher level on the posttest, did not
demonstrate a greater gain in knowledge than students with
weak prior knowledge.
While the observed gains on the ParNoMA may seem
meager, one must consider them within the broader view of the
students’ educational experience. Prior to these students
beginning this course, they had a well-established conceptual
framework for how to describe and classify matter. To modify
any one piece of a student’s misconception about matter,
several steps must take place. Students must be presented with
a situation that runs counter to their current framework. They
must then be allowed an avenue to explore and make sense of
the new information and formulate a new, correct conception.
Such modification does not guarantee a correct conception, as
the student may establish a new, yet still incorrect, conception
that incorporates the dissonant situation.24
If one takes this
snapshot of the Piagetian learning process and expands it to all
of the concepts that fall into description and classification of
matter, the cognitive load on the student becomes immense.
Furthermore, students are not likely to have had any effective,
appropriate instruction in particulate-level behaviors prior to
their experience in this course.31
As a pilot study without a control group, the data describe
the effectiveness of the curriculum as it pertains to the sample
and the instructor of the course. Thus, these results do not
provide direct evidence for the novel curriculum over a
traditional model, nor do they speak to the effectiveness of the
curriculum in another setting. These results do, however, show
that the novel curriculum had a positive effect on student
conceptions regarding the particulate nature of matter as it
pertains to phases of matter and changes in matter for students
in the 4th hour class in our setting.
Structured Interview Results and Discussion
The researcher attempted to obtain as much of a representative
sample of the study population as possible through the
selection of participants with diverse conceptions about the
particulate nature of matter. Table 4 represents the participants’
pre- and posttest scores on the ParNoMA.
Audio and video recordings of participant responses were
collected. Analysis and coding of the data focused on
connecting participant responses to either correct conceptions
or to previously identified misconceptions. The video coding
software HyperRESEARCH32
was used to organize video clips
and look for patterns.
Representation of States of Matter at the Particulate Level
The substance in the beaker and the condensate on the glass
should have each been identified and represented as a liquid.
Correct representations included particles placed in close
proximity to each other, but still allowing for freedom for
particle movement. The liquid should have also lacked any sort
of organized structure. With each of the five participants
providing either a correct or incorrect construction for the two
locations, 7 of the 10 models were deemed correct. Several
examples of correct participant descriptions are given below.
“It’s in the liquid form so it’d be close but still have enough
room to move around.”
(Allison)
“In here it’s going to be more of a consistently or evenly
spread out liquid.”
(Emily)
The three incorrect participant descriptions either involved
an incorrect identification of the phase of matter that was
observed or a description that did not clearly demonstrate that
the participant understood liquid behavior on the particulate
level.
The substance between the beaker and the glass plate should
have been correctly identified as a gas. Correct descriptions
needed to include the fact that the particles were spaced
significantly further apart than those of the liquid. Correct
participant models frequently accomplished this by using fewer
particles. Of the five potential participant responses, four were
identified as correct. The only participant response that was
Figure 3. Student pretest and posttest mean scores (%) and 95%
confidence intervals on the ParNoMA, broken down by class period.
Table 4. ParNoMA Results for Interview Participants
ParNoMA Score
Participant (pseudonym) Pretest Score, % Posttest Score, %
Nate 20 35
Emily 25 45
Kyle 35 50
Ryan 40 40
Allison 45 95
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198196
marked incorrect did not clearly distinguish between the
particle spacing of the liquid and the gas.
Representation of Changes at the Particulate Level
Each participant’s model gave insight into how he or she
thought the process he or she was observing at the macroscopic
level affected the connectivity of particles. In representing the
change that occurs between the heated liquid in the beaker and
the space above the liquid, all participants chose to break the
substance apart into its elements, which is incorrect and one of
the hallmarks of this particular misconception. Interestingly,
four of the five participants cited the heating process as the
cause of the decomposition of the compound.
“They undergo a chemical reaction because it’s heated.”
(Nate)
“They break down because of the heat and it makes the
atoms go faster and they break their bonds.”
(Kyle)
“They boil, which breaks the bonds to make them a gas.”
(Ryan)
All of the participants then oppositely showed the atoms
recombining to form the original compound when they cooled
and condensed on the glass. In each case, the elements were
combined such that they were identical to those on the beaker.
“Then the glass cools down the substance and they form back
together”
(Kyle)
Thus, participants seemed to recognize that the substance on
the glass and the substance in the beaker were identical.
Identification of Changes at the Particulate Level
Participants were also asked to describe each of the changes as
physical or chemical and provide a rationale for their decisions.
All participants decomposed the substance in the transition to
the gas phase and then reformed it on the glass surface. Thus,
all participant diagrams represented chemical changes. Four of
the five participants correctly identified and described their
model as a chemical change taking place. Participant responses
were identified as correct if they referenced bonds being broken
and formed or if they referenced the formation of new
substances.
“Chemical change because there’s a different substance
formed.”
(Nate)
“Chemical, because the bonds broke.”
(Kyle)
“It’d be a chemical change because it’s breaking up the
compound into two different things.”
(Allison)
Participants seem to have a clear understanding of how
physical and chemical changes differ on the particulate level,
even though their conception of what actually occurred with
the particles was incorrect.
■ CHANGES AT THE MACROSCOPIC LEVEL
Participants were asked to describe the macroscopic process of
the substance boiling (which was set up in front of them) as a
physical or chemical change. Four of the five participants
correctly identified the process as a physical change.
Appropriate justifications for such a response included
recognizing that simple phase changes were occurring.
“Physical, because it’s changing from a liquid to a gas and
then back to a liquid”
(Kyle)
“I think it would be a physical change because it’s going from
a liquid to a gas to a liquid again so it’s the physical
properties of the substance.”
(Allison)
Such a response was in conflict with the participants’
particulate models and appeared to cause some cognitive
dissonance for most of the participants.
“A physical change because...well...maybe a chemical change
because it’s being heated...so...well...actually I think it would
be a physical change because it’s just water evaporating it
looks like.”
(Nate)
Through their hesitant responses and body language,
participants seemed to recognize that their descriptions of the
changes on the macroscopic and particulate levels should have
matched, although none of them explicitly made that statement.
In summary, participants generally produced correct
descriptions of liquids and gases at the particulate level.
Participants could also clearly distinguish between chemical and
physical changes at the particulate level. When asked to identify
the macroscopic process as a physical or chemical change, all of
the participants seemed to recognize that their macroscopic and
particulate descriptions should have matched. Thus, it seems
the piece that the participants were missing was the difference
in strength between a chemical bond and intermolecular forces.
The implications of this misunderstanding are far-reaching in
an introductory chemistry course, as much of the traditional
curriculum involves processes in an aqueous solution. The
differentiation between the dissolution of a substance and any
chemical reactions it may participate in while in solution
requires a mental separation of processes involving intermo-
lecular forces and processes that involve the breaking and
forming of chemical bonds. Further consideration about how to
appropriately establish a correct understanding regarding this
conception is necessary. Furthermore, tools to assess the
student conceptions involved in the distinction would need to
be developed so that curricula could be evaluated appropriately.
■ CONCLUSIONS
Both the qualitative and quantitative results of this study
provide support for the positive effect of the novel curriculum
on developing students’ conceptual understanding of chemistry.
As a pilot study without a control group, the data describe the
effectiveness of the treatment curriculum as it pertains to the
selected population and the instructor of the course. These
results are not intended to prove superiority of the novel
curriculum over other models, but merely to demonstrate its
ability to positively affect student conceptions. As previously
mentioned, the uncontrollable aspects of the high school
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198197
schedule may have contributed to the differences between the
two sections. The scope of this study was to determine the
effectiveness of the curriculum and not necessarily how the
curriculum functioned within specific student populations.
Understanding the specific differences between the two
sections in this study may provide insight into how the
curriculum could be modified to be more effective for specific
student populations. Thus, further research into the effect of
the curriculum on different student populations is warranted.
The results of this study provide evidence for the
effectiveness of the curriculum within a specific population.
Further evidence would be required to make inferences about
the performance of the curriculum in comparison to other
curricula and in other school settings. First, a stronger argument
for the novel curriculum could be made if it were shown more
effective in concept building than a traditional model. This
would require implementing the curriculum described here
while simultaneously teaching a more traditional curriculum to
an equivalent student population. However, providing
instruction that may be inferior for the purpose of an
experimental control may raise ethical concerns surrounding
the teaching and learning beliefs of the instructor. Second,
while the researchers expect minimal variation among different
student groups, how an instructor implements the curriculum
has been previously shown to significantly influence the
potency of the curriculum.33−35
As such, an exploration of
how both different student populations and different
instructors affect outcomes due to the curriculum could
provide support for the broader use of the curriculum and
others designed around similar frameworks.
■ AUTHOR INFORMATION
Corresponding Author
*E-mail: cbridle1@gpsk12.net.
■ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We wish to thank the faculty and students of Grand Valley
State University’s Target Inquiry Program for their input and
collaboration in this effort. We are grateful for the willing
participation of the students of Grandville High School. This
material is based upon research and professional development
supported by the National Science Foundation (ESI-0553215)
and the Target Inquiry Program at Grand Valley State
University. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or
recommendations expressed in this publication are those of
the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the
National Science Foundation or Grand Valley State University.
■ REFERENCES
(1) Nurrenbern, S. C.; Pickering, M. J. Chem. Educ. 1987, 6, 508−
510.
(2) Sawrey, B. A. J. Chem. Educ. 1990, 3, 253−254.
(3) Sanger, M. J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 1, 131−134.
(4) Cracolice, M.; Deming, J.; Ehlert, B. J. Chem. Educ. 2008, 6, 873−
878.
(5) Gabel, D. L.; Samuel, K. V.; Hunn, D. J. Chem. Educ. 1987, 8,
695−697.
(6) Sanger, M. J. J. Chem. Educ. 2000, 6, 762−766.
(7) Ealy, J. B. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2004, 4, 461−471.
(8) Niaz, M.; Aguilera, D.; Maza, A.; Liendo, G. Sci. Educ. 2002, 4,
505−525.
(9) Lewis, S.; Lewis, J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 1, 135−139.
(10) Johnstone, A. H. J. Chem. Educ. 1993, 9, 701−705.
(11) Gabel, D. J. Chem. Educ. 1999, 4, 548−553.
(12) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sci. Educ. 1996, 5, 509−534.
(13) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sci. Educ. 2000, 3, 352−381.
(14) Griffiths, A.; Preston, K. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1992, 6, 611−628.
(15) Stains, M.; Talanquer, V. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2007, 5, 643−661.
(16) Garnett, P. J.; Garnett, P. J.; Hackling, M. W. Stud. Sci. Educ.
1995, 25, 69−95.
(17) Boz, Y. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2006, 2, 203−213.
(18) Ahtee, M.; Varjola, I. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 3, 305−316.
(19) Hewson, M. G.; Hewson, P. W. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1983, 8, 731−
744.
(20) Gabel, D. Educ. Quim. 2000, 2, 236−243.
(21) DeBoer, G. E. A History of Ideas in Science Education:
Implications for Practice; Teachers College Press: New York, 1991.
(22) Bodner, G. M. J. Chem. Educ. 1986, 10, 873−878.
(23) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sch. Sci. Math. 1998, 8, 420−
429.
(24) Posner, G. J.; Strike, K. A.; Hewson, P. W.; Gertzog, W. A. Sci.
Educ. 1982, 2, 211−227.
(25) Shadish, W.; Cook, T.; Campbell, D. Experimental and Quasi-
Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference; Houghton-
Mifflin Company: Boston, MA, 2002.
(26) Eizenga, D. Putting the World in a Box. Target Inquiry
Teaching Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (accessed
Nov 2011).
(27) Bridle, C. Change You Can Believe In. Target Inquiry Teaching
Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (accessed Nov 2011).
(28) Bridle, C. The Only Thing Constant in Life Is Change. Target
Inquiry Teaching Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (ac-
cessed Nov 2011).
(29) Yezierski, E. J.; Birk, J. P. J. Chem. Educ. 2006, 6, 954−960.
(30) Lewis, S.; Lewis, J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 9, 1408−1412.
(31) Montes, L. D.; Rockley, M. G. J. Chem. Educ. 2002, 2, 244−247.
(32) HyperRESEARCH Home Page. http://www.researchware.
com/products/hyperresearch.html (accessed Nov 2011).
(33) Simmons, P. E.; Emory, A.; Carter, T.; Coker, T.; Finnegan, B.;
Crockett, D.; Richardson, L.; Yager, R.; Craven, J.; Tillotson, J.;
Brunkhorst, H.; Twiest, M.; Hossain, K.; Gallagher, J.; Duggan-Haas,
D.; Parker, J.; Cajas, F.; Alshannag, Q.; McGlamery, S.; Krockover, J.;
Adams, P.; Spector, B.; LaPorta, T.; James, B.; Rearden, K.; Labuda, K.
J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1999, 8, 930−954.
(34) Roehrig, G. H.; Luft, J. A. J. Chem. Educ. 2004, 10, 1510−1516.
(35) Roehrig, G.; Garrow, S. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2007, 14, 1789−1811.
Journal of Chemical Education Article
dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198198

More Related Content

What's hot

Online assignment shyma (1)
Online assignment shyma (1)Online assignment shyma (1)
Online assignment shyma (1)rencyrobert
 
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimCase study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimAkramEnglish
 
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar Najim
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar NajimA case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar Najim
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar NajimAkramEnglish
 
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curricula
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curriculaConceptual demand of practical work in science curricula
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curriculaSílvia Ferreira
 
An inquiry based cellulase laboratory
An inquiry based cellulase laboratoryAn inquiry based cellulase laboratory
An inquiry based cellulase laboratorykamegasilvia
 
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functions
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential FunctionsConstructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functions
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functionsinventionjournals
 
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...Alexander Decker
 
Chemical education material study
Chemical  education  material  studyChemical  education  material  study
Chemical education material studysarithaspr
 
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...iosrjce
 
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticle
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticlePenberthyMillarDisseminationArticle
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticleDebra Penberthy
 
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...Alexander Decker
 
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...IOSR Journals
 
What works in Remediation
What works in RemediationWhat works in Remediation
What works in RemediationAmilou Ace
 
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...theijes
 
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JR
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JRCOMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JR
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JRWELFREDO LUBRICO YU,JR.
 

What's hot (20)

Online assignment shyma (1)
Online assignment shyma (1)Online assignment shyma (1)
Online assignment shyma (1)
 
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimCase study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
 
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar Najim
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar NajimA case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar Najim
A case study of problem solving- Akram Jabar Najim
 
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curricula
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curriculaConceptual demand of practical work in science curricula
Conceptual demand of practical work in science curricula
 
abstract published
abstract publishedabstract published
abstract published
 
An inquiry based cellulase laboratory
An inquiry based cellulase laboratoryAn inquiry based cellulase laboratory
An inquiry based cellulase laboratory
 
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functions
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential FunctionsConstructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functions
Constructivist Approach Vs Expository Teaching: Exponential Functions
 
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...
The convergence of mayer’s model and constructivist model towards problem sol...
 
Chemical education material study
Chemical  education  material  studyChemical  education  material  study
Chemical education material study
 
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...
Correcting Students’ Chemical Misconceptions based on Two Conceptual change s...
 
Proposal
ProposalProposal
Proposal
 
10.00 canada
10.00 canada10.00 canada
10.00 canada
 
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticle
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticlePenberthyMillarDisseminationArticle
PenberthyMillarDisseminationArticle
 
A0350104
A0350104A0350104
A0350104
 
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...
11.the effectiveness of teaching physics through project method on academic a...
 
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...
Effects Of Using Gagne’s Learning Hierarchy On Chemistry Students’ Academic A...
 
What works in Remediation
What works in RemediationWhat works in Remediation
What works in Remediation
 
20
2020
20
 
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...
Different Approach in Teaching and Learning in Science for Mongolian Secondar...
 
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JR
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JRCOMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JR
COMPETENCY BASED LESSON GUIDE -WELFREDO L. YU JR
 

Similar to Evidence for the effectiveness of inquiry based, particulate-level

Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_c
Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_cBridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_c
Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_cLeoVega10
 
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...AJHSSR Journal
 
Thesis on chem 95.1
Thesis on chem 95.1Thesis on chem 95.1
Thesis on chem 95.1merogim1
 
C ience and pedagogy working togetherer
C ience and pedagogy working togethererC ience and pedagogy working togetherer
C ience and pedagogy working togethererGladys Rivera
 
Research trends in science education
Research trends in science educationResearch trends in science education
Research trends in science educationgarimatandon10
 
Sem interipaiv redhana
Sem interipaiv redhanaSem interipaiv redhana
Sem interipaiv redhanaiwayanredhana
 
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytd
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytdstudent related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytd
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytdSirajudinAkmel1
 
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instruction
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology InstructionUsing Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instruction
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instructionijtsrd
 
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...ijejournal
 
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...Journal of Education and Learning (EduLearn)
 
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptx
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptxKELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptx
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptxchaniapitrisia21
 
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...Alexander Decker
 
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students Attitudes ...
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students  Attitudes ...An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students  Attitudes ...
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students Attitudes ...Jeff Nelson
 
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistry
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School ChemistryContemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistry
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistrytheijes
 
Application of calculas in mdc
Application of calculas in mdcApplication of calculas in mdc
Application of calculas in mdcMohamed Sameer
 
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...ijtsrd
 

Similar to Evidence for the effectiveness of inquiry based, particulate-level (20)

Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_c
Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_cBridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_c
Bridging the gaps_how_students_seek_disciplinary_c
 
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...
Effects of Inquiry-Based Learning Strategies on Chemistry Students’ Conceptio...
 
The learners’ conceptual understanding: Literature review of vapor-pressure ...
The learners’ conceptual understanding: Literature review of  vapor-pressure ...The learners’ conceptual understanding: Literature review of  vapor-pressure ...
The learners’ conceptual understanding: Literature review of vapor-pressure ...
 
A mismatch
A mismatchA mismatch
A mismatch
 
Thesis on chem 95.1
Thesis on chem 95.1Thesis on chem 95.1
Thesis on chem 95.1
 
C ience and pedagogy working togetherer
C ience and pedagogy working togethererC ience and pedagogy working togetherer
C ience and pedagogy working togetherer
 
Research trends in science education
Research trends in science educationResearch trends in science education
Research trends in science education
 
Sem interipaiv redhana
Sem interipaiv redhanaSem interipaiv redhana
Sem interipaiv redhana
 
Effects of technology-integrated formative assessment on students' conceptual...
Effects of technology-integrated formative assessment on students' conceptual...Effects of technology-integrated formative assessment on students' conceptual...
Effects of technology-integrated formative assessment on students' conceptual...
 
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytd
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytdstudent related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytd
student related.pdfutdkuydluyfdkuydjtysdjytd
 
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instruction
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology InstructionUsing Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instruction
Using Transdisciplinary Approach in Biology Instruction
 
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...
SPIRAL PROGRESSION APPROACH IN THE K TO 12 SCIENCE CURRICULUM: A LITERATURE R...
 
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...
The Influence of Biology Learning Using Concept Attainment Model on Student’s...
 
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptx
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptxKELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptx
KELOMPOK 14_PPT REVIEW JURNAL INTERNASIONAL_KHPPF.pptx
 
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...
The effect of project based learning and the arcs motivational model on stude...
 
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students Attitudes ...
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students  Attitudes ...An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students  Attitudes ...
An Academic Survey Concerning High School And University Students Attitudes ...
 
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistry
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School ChemistryContemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistry
Contemporary Teaching Methods in Mongolian Secondary School Chemistry
 
Application of calculas in mdc
Application of calculas in mdcApplication of calculas in mdc
Application of calculas in mdc
 
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...
Levels of Conceptual Understanding and Problem Solving Skills of Physics Teac...
 
Vista pbl
Vista pblVista pbl
Vista pbl
 

Recently uploaded

Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxabhijeetpadhi001
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupJonathanParaisoCruz
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxUnboundStockton
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 

Evidence for the effectiveness of inquiry based, particulate-level

  • 1. Evidence for the Effectiveness of Inquiry-Based, Particulate-Level Instruction on Conceptions of the Particulate Nature of Matter Chad A. Bridle*,† and Ellen J. Yezierski‡ † Science Department, Grandville High School, Grandville, Michigan 49418, United States ‡ Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, Miami University, Oxford, Ohio 45056, United States ABSTRACT: Research has shown that students in traditional college-preparatory chemistry courses become masters of mathematical equations without an understanding of the conceptual basis for the mathematical relationships. This problem is rooted not only in what curriculum is presented to students, but also in how it is experienced by the students. Ample evidence exists in support of both inquiry-based instruction and the use of particulate-level models in instruction as a means for improving students’ conceptual understandings in chemistry. Little evidence exists, though, for the effectiveness of an instructional model that involves the merger of these two methods. In an effort to address this gap, a series of laboratory and classroom activities was created that blended guided inquiry-based instructional practices with particulate-level modeling experiences. The content of the curriculum focused exclusively on phases of matter and chemical versus physical changes in matter. This research explores the novel curriculum’s effect on student understanding of the particulate nature of matter in two sections of high school chemistry. Qualitative and quantitative evidence supporting the curriculum is presented. KEYWORDS: High School/Introductory Chemistry, Chemical Education Research, Hands-On Learning/Manipulatives, Inquiry-Based/Discovery Learning, Constructivism FEATURE: Chemical Education Research ■ INTRODUCTION Chemistry students have long demonstrated their ability to solve algebraic problems without a clear understanding of the conceptual background of the problem.1−4 This lack of a scientifically accurate conceptual framework for the very foundations of chemistry is not only a problem by itself, but its effects extend into all other aspects of student performance in chemistry.5 Two different approaches to this issue have been explored: curriculum focused on particulate-level representa- tions of matter,6,7 and inquiry-based instructional models.8,9 This paper describes the implementation and effectiveness of a novel curriculum that merges particulate-level modeling with inquiry-based instruction. ■ BACKGROUND Johnstone describes the three different levels of representation in chemistry: macroscopic, submicroscopic, and symbolic.10 A particulate understanding of atoms and their properties is central to explaining any concept that is observed or described on the macroscopic or symbolic levels.11 The importance of a strong understanding of the particulate nature of matter must be considered carefully. How do we appropriately represent and describe the particulate level? What sort of prior knowledge of the particulate level do students bring to the classroom? How do instructors properly guide their students to an appropriate understanding of the particulate nature of matter? Because the particulate world of chemistry is invisible and theoretical, descriptions of this level must include models. In using models, the ability of the learner to comprehend the meaning of the model must be considered. Harrison and Treagust describe three different ability levels.12 At the most basic level, learners interpret models as exact replicas of reality. Thus, an atom that is represented as a red circle actually is a red circle. As modeling ability increases, students become better at differentiating between what a model is intended to show about a situation and the limitations of the model. In a later study, Harrison and Treagust describe a successful method for employing atomic models in instruction.13 Their work showed that part of the instructional process must involve exploring and discussing the merits and limitations of a particular model. Similarly, students bring varying degrees of understanding about the basic behaviors of matter. As far as incorrect conceptions about atoms and molecules, students frequently struggle with size, shape, and composition.14 These mis- conceptions include the assumption that one can see atoms and molecules under a microscope, that their volume and weight can change depending on conditions, and, for example, that water molecules can contain atoms other than hydrogen and oxygen. This lack of clarity leads to struggles with classification of matter, such as being able to distinguish between compounds and mixtures.15 Considering physical changes in matter, Published: December 14, 2011 Article pubs.acs.org/jchemeduc © 2011 American Chemical Society and Division of Chemical Education, Inc. 192 dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198 This is an open access article published under an ACS AuthorChoice License, which permits copying and redistribution of the article or any adaptations for non-commercial purposes.
  • 2. common misconceptions include the idea that boiling liquids involves the expansion of molecules or even the breaking and forming of chemical bonds within the molecules.16,17 Lastly, these misconceptions lead to students’ inability to distinguish clearly between physical and chemical changes in matter.18 This evidence suggests that appropriate instruction in this field is a matter of constructing a correct conceptual framework within the minds of the students and not simply memorizing facts and processes. Using inquiry-based instructional ap- proaches in chemistry has been proven effective. Hewson and Hewson used inquiry instruction in the design and implementation of curriculum regarding the concepts of mass, volume, and density.19 They found a statistically significant improvement not only in correct conceptual understandings, but also in a reduced number of misconcep- tions. Similar positive results were found by Lewis and Lewis upon implementing a peer-led guided inquiry curriculum.9 The literature shows that both particulate-level instruction and inquiry-based pedagogy improve students’ conceptual understanding of chemistry. Gabel suggested both methods for improving conceptual understanding, but little research exists regarding the merger of these two instructional models.20 The evidence within the literature merits the exploration of the fusion of the two methods in a novel inquiry-based, particulate- level curriculum. ■ THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK The design of the intervention and the overall study is guided by learning theories grounded in work by Piaget and more recent constructivist points of view, including the conceptual change model. DeBoer noted that the purpose of science education goes beyond simply supplying students with facts and figures.21 Students must be able to think like scientists: effectively using their scientific knowledge, carefully studying and observing their world, and developing new understandings from these observations. Piaget’s theory of how humans constructed knowledge caused educators to reconsider how content was delivered. Prior models of instruction assumed that the information presented by the teacher was simply transferred to students, as though they were a blank sheet of paper waiting to be filled. Piaget forced educators to consider learning from the learner’s perspective. He believed that not only did each learner construct his or her own understanding of a subject individually, but also that any new knowledge had to be integrated with the learner’s current framework of under- standing.22 In regard to the particulate nature of matter, students cannot simply be given information about the invisible particulate universe and be expected to accept it. Instead, students must explore and construct their own understandings of the topic so that a specific representation of the atomic level fits with their understandings of the world.23 This model for instruction parallels not only Piaget’s theory of student development, but also the philosophies of science research.24 Learners and researchers alike approach a new situation with their current conceptual framework. Any new information or findings may not fit with their current framework. Individuals must then construct a new framework that accommodates the new knowledge and can be accepted as reasonable and useful. The process of constructing a true, correct understanding of the nature of matter allows students to more accurately describe any novel situations they may experience within and beyond chemistry. This approach to chemistry instruction, grounded in constructivist learning theories, necessitates the use of instruc- tional and laboratory techniques that allow students to explore and find meaning in a given situation.20 ■ RESEARCH DESIGN This pilot study aimed to investigate the effectiveness of inquiry-based, particulate modeling experiences in improving students’ conceptual understanding of chemistry. Using a quasi- experimental, primarily quantitative study with a one-group pretest−posttest design,25 the intervention engaged students in inquiry-based activities with various particulate-level represen- tations of matter early in the school year. The curriculum implemented in the study was designed by a team of chemistry educators as part of Grand Valley State University’s Target Inquiry teacher professional development program. The classroom activities were designed to be used in the first few weeks of an introductory chemistry course. These laboratory experiences were intended to provide students with a strong particulate-level, conceptually based understanding of the basic behaviors of matter, better equipping them for further learning activities in chemistry. ■ CONTEXT The novel curriculum was implemented in two high school chemistry sections in a large, suburban high school in the Midwest United States. One of the authors (Bridle) was the instructor for the two chemistry sections that were studied. Both sections were exposed to the same curriculum; data were not compared to a control group or established norms. The population in 3rd hour, n = 26, included 13 female and 13 male participants, while the population in 4th hour, n = 28, consisted of 13 female and 15 male participants. The participants were 15−17 years old, predominantly Caucasian and middle class. The curriculum was implemented during five and one-half 60- min instructional periods over a three-week period as described in Table 1. ■ DESCRIPTION OF INTERVENTIONS Describing and Categorizing Matter Using Particulate-Level Models The teacher guide and student guide for Putting the World in a Box26 are available as cited through the Target Inquiry Web Table 1. Summary of Inquiry Activities Implemented Inquiry Activity Description Instructional Time, Min. Putting the World in a Box Constructing an understanding of appropriate particulate-level modeling. Establishing particulate-level descriptions of solids, liquids, gases, elements, compounds, and mixtures. Establishing appropriate methods for categorizing matter. 120 Change You Can Believe In Establishing clear, particulate-level definitions for physical changes and chemical changes. 90 The Only Thing Constant in Life Is Change Establishing an appropriate connection between particulate-level definitions of physical and chemical changes, and macroscopic observations. 120 Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198193
  • 3. site. This activity was designed to specifically target misconceptions related to the description and categorization of matter. The content addressed in this activity was the students’ first exposure to particulate-level models in their chemistry course. The literature describes students’ varying abilities to appropriately interact with models and how this may affect student learning regarding particulate-level instruc- tion.12,13 As a result, a significant portion of the activity calls for students to explore the usefulness and limitations of various particulate-level representations of matter such that students may construct an understanding of the particulate-level behaviors of solids, liquids, and gases as they relate to observable, macroscopic behaviors. Second, the students use a similar approach to establish particulate- and macroscopic-level knowledge of elements, compounds, and mixtures. Lastly, the activity aims to merge these two knowledge structures in an effort to establish appropriate procedures for describing and classifying matter. Students explore the similarities and differences between elements, compounds, and mixtures and construct an appropriate classification scheme for particulate- level representations of matter. Constructing a Particulate-Level Conception of Physical and Chemical Changes The activity Change You Can Believe In27 is the first of two activities, both available as cited through the Target Inquiry Web site, that address the concepts of physical and chemical changes. This activity uses cards that illustrate particulate-level examples of physical and chemical changes. Figure 1 is a sample card, representing the single replacement reaction between magnesium metal and a hydrochloric acid solution. Students were not given information about what substances or processes were represented by each card. This was intentional, so as to prevent students from falling back on their macroscopic understandings and experiences to describe particulate-level situations. Students consider the cards with only their prior knowledge as a basis and categorize the cards as either physical or chemical changes. The student guides were constructed in such a way that led to discussion and debate between students. This process, guided by the instructor, allowed students to modify and clarify their particulate-level understanding of physical and chemical changes. Constructing a Symbolic and Macroscopic Conception of Physical and Chemical Changes The Only Thing Constant in Life Is Change28 was the second activity intended to address the concepts of physical and chemical changes. Students performed actual macroscopic examples of the processes represented by the physical and chemical changes cards. A more traditional laboratory approach would have students make macroscopic observations and then ask them to relate these observations to the particulate level. This second activity reversed this process. Students have already categorized the laboratory activities as physical or chemical changes in the previous activity. For example, the students would have classified the process in Figure 1 as a chemical change. When this reaction is performed in the laboratory, one observation students would make would be the evolution of a gas. Students then make macroscopic observations of all of the processes illustrated in the cards and analyze their observations to determine the usefulness of each type of observation in defining an observed process as physical or chemical. For example, students would consider whether the evolution of a gas was a sign of a physical change, a chemical change, both, or neither. In the case of gas evolution, whether the production of steam, a gas, from boiling water fits this same category caused students to consider limitations and precise definitions for their macroscopic observations. This alternative approach forced students to use macroscopic observations as tools for constructing a model of particulate- level behavior. A more traditional approach may have led students to consider the production of a gas, for example, as a definitive sign of a chemical change. The new approach results in students concluding that particulate-level definitions of physical and chemical changes are much more reliable descriptors of macroscopic processes. Instruments and Data Collection In determining the effectiveness of the activities in bringing about conceptual understanding, a published conceptual chemistry instrument, Particulate Nature of Matter (ParNo- MA), was used.29 The instrument is a 20-question, multiple- choice test specifically focusing on students’ concepts at the particulate level related to phases of matter and phase changes. Each question addresses an aspect of the particulate behavior of matter, with distracters being directly related to common misconceptions. The authors of the instrument report 100% validity of the appropriate correct responses when examined by several of their colleagues as well as establishing the internal consistency of the instrument when administered to an introductory college chemistry section, N = 72, Cronbach’s α = 0.78. Students in this study were given the instrument both as a pretest and posttest in order to measure any changes in their conceptual framework. Student scores were reported as a percent of the number of questions they correctly answered out of 20. After obtaining parental consent and student assent according to the permissions granted by the Human Research Review Committee at Grand Valley State University, the pretest was administered to students during the first few days of the course. Students experienced the treatment curriculum during the third through fifth weeks of the course, and the posttest was then administered in a similar fashion during the ninth week, at the end of the marking period. The significant gap in time between the pretest, the treatment, and the posttest limits any test−retest bias. On the basis of the data from this instrument, structured interviews were conducted with five students during the second semester of the course, 4 months after the treatment and 3 months after the posttest, to probe specific concepts. The significant time lapse from the treatment was intended with the hope that the knowledge structures of the participants revealed Figure 1. Sample activity card from Change You Can Believe In depicting the reaction of Mg(s) with HCl(aq) at the particulate level. Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198194
  • 4. in the interviews reflected their long-term understandings. Following an analysis of student gains on the ParNoMA, five students from both class periods were selected as potential participants in the interview based on the gains they experienced. One participant experienced no gain (0%) in his ParNoMA score, three students experienced moderate gains (15−20%), and one experienced large gains (50%). The protocol began with participants being presented with a clear liquid (water) boiling on a hot plate. The interview protocol did not describe the substance as water to avoid any prior knowledge bias of water’s behavior. The beaker contained water, though, for practical purposes. A small plate of glass was placed above the boiling liquid and condensation collected on the glass. Students were then provided with a pictorial representation of the situation that included several call-outs, or zoomed-in views, of the situation (Figure 2). The three call- outs referred to particulate behaviors within the beaker, between the beaker and the glass, and at the surface of the glass. Students were told that the substance in the beaker consisted of a compound comprising two elements, A and B. They were provided with small, blue triangles that represented atoms of element A and yellow squares that represented atoms of element B. The instructor constructed a particulate-level representation of the compound, consisting of one of each atom. The participants were then asked to construct and describe what they believed the particulate-level behavior of the substance would be in each of the call-outs. The instructor recorded video of the students modeling their ideas, including audio of their descriptions. The protocol also asked students to describe the changes the substance experienced and to identify them as physical or chemical changes. ■ RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ParNoMA Results Statistical analyses using SPSS software were conducted to determine whether any changes in student scores, reported as percentages, between pre- and posttest occurred. While itemized responses for each question were obtained, individual questions on this survey have not been established as reliable measures of individual concepts. Thus, probing individual questions for further information about student understanding was not pursued. The data were collected from two different sections of the course. The authors wished to consider both data sets together, so the appropriateness of consolidation was considered first. Statistical analyses were conducted on student gains between pre- and posttest (posttest minus pretest). An Anderson− Darling test for normality was performed on each section using student gains. The test, which is optimal for small samples, showed that results from both periods, 3rd hour with p = 0.0755, and 4th hour with p = 0.349, could be considered normally distributed. As shown in Table 2, mean student pre- and posttest scores for each class do appear different. A test for equivalence was carried out to determine whether a priori differences existed between the two different class periods. The analysis of the ParNoMA pretest scores was done using two, one-tailed t-tests as prescribed by Lewis and Lewis.30 Results showed that the two groups could not be considered statistically equivalent to each other. The differences between these two populations may lie in which students get scheduled in each hour. The complexity of the high school scheduling process often allows for honors or elective classes to only be offered during certain hours. This often results in specific populations of students unintentionally remaining grouped together in other classes. This phenomenon may be at the root of the difference. Considering each section individually, a paired samples t-test was used to explore differences in student pre- and posttest scores. As summarized in Table 3, students in 3rd hour did not show a significant improvement in score, while 4th hour results did show a statistically significant improvement, p = 0.011. The standardized effect size index (Cohen’s d) for 4th hour was 0.51, indicating that the intervention produced a medium effect. A power analysis using G*Power 3.1.2 revealed that the sample size for each period was too small to detect a small effect. This is important in drawing inferences from 3rd hour results. Figure 3 further illustrates student gains, showing the pretest and posttest means and 95% confidence intervals for 3rd and 4th hour results individually. This graph illustrates that both classes achieved similar results on the posttest, despite 4th hour students starting with a much lower mean pretest score. The statistically significant difference between pre- and posttest ParNoMA means for 4th hour warranted further Figure 2. Diagram used during interviews for participants to illustrate their particulate-level understanding of phenomena depicted: Water boiling in a beaker, evaporating, and then condensing on a glass plate. Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for ParNoMA Scores by Class Period Pretest Scores, % Posttest Scores, % Class Period (N) Mean SD Mean SD third Period (26) 40.58 14.92 43.65 20.13 fourth Period (28) 34.82 15.30 44.46 23.43 Table 3. Student Gains by Percent: Paired Samples t-Test Class Period (N) Mean Difference SD 95% CI F Values p Values (Two- Tailed) 3rd Period (26) 3.07 13.42 [2.34, 8.50] 1.17 0.253 4th Period (28) 9.64 18.75 [2.37, 16.91] 2.72 0.011 Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198195
  • 5. considerations. A Pearson correlation between pre- and posttest scores showed a significant moderate positive correlation, 0.601, p = 0.001. This relationship suggests that participants who performed well on the pretest also performed well on the posttest. This result shows some relationship between a participant’s prior knowledge and their resulting knowledge after the described treatment. A second Pearson correlation between student pretest scores and student normalized gains [(post − pre)/(100% − pre)] on the ParNoMA showed no significant relationship, 0.036, p = 0.854. This suggests that the amount of conceptual gain, as evidenced by the normalized gain on the ParNoMA, was not dependent on the participant’s pretest score, independent of the absolute magnitude of the gain. Thus, students with a strong prior knowledge, while performing at a higher level on the posttest, did not demonstrate a greater gain in knowledge than students with weak prior knowledge. While the observed gains on the ParNoMA may seem meager, one must consider them within the broader view of the students’ educational experience. Prior to these students beginning this course, they had a well-established conceptual framework for how to describe and classify matter. To modify any one piece of a student’s misconception about matter, several steps must take place. Students must be presented with a situation that runs counter to their current framework. They must then be allowed an avenue to explore and make sense of the new information and formulate a new, correct conception. Such modification does not guarantee a correct conception, as the student may establish a new, yet still incorrect, conception that incorporates the dissonant situation.24 If one takes this snapshot of the Piagetian learning process and expands it to all of the concepts that fall into description and classification of matter, the cognitive load on the student becomes immense. Furthermore, students are not likely to have had any effective, appropriate instruction in particulate-level behaviors prior to their experience in this course.31 As a pilot study without a control group, the data describe the effectiveness of the curriculum as it pertains to the sample and the instructor of the course. Thus, these results do not provide direct evidence for the novel curriculum over a traditional model, nor do they speak to the effectiveness of the curriculum in another setting. These results do, however, show that the novel curriculum had a positive effect on student conceptions regarding the particulate nature of matter as it pertains to phases of matter and changes in matter for students in the 4th hour class in our setting. Structured Interview Results and Discussion The researcher attempted to obtain as much of a representative sample of the study population as possible through the selection of participants with diverse conceptions about the particulate nature of matter. Table 4 represents the participants’ pre- and posttest scores on the ParNoMA. Audio and video recordings of participant responses were collected. Analysis and coding of the data focused on connecting participant responses to either correct conceptions or to previously identified misconceptions. The video coding software HyperRESEARCH32 was used to organize video clips and look for patterns. Representation of States of Matter at the Particulate Level The substance in the beaker and the condensate on the glass should have each been identified and represented as a liquid. Correct representations included particles placed in close proximity to each other, but still allowing for freedom for particle movement. The liquid should have also lacked any sort of organized structure. With each of the five participants providing either a correct or incorrect construction for the two locations, 7 of the 10 models were deemed correct. Several examples of correct participant descriptions are given below. “It’s in the liquid form so it’d be close but still have enough room to move around.” (Allison) “In here it’s going to be more of a consistently or evenly spread out liquid.” (Emily) The three incorrect participant descriptions either involved an incorrect identification of the phase of matter that was observed or a description that did not clearly demonstrate that the participant understood liquid behavior on the particulate level. The substance between the beaker and the glass plate should have been correctly identified as a gas. Correct descriptions needed to include the fact that the particles were spaced significantly further apart than those of the liquid. Correct participant models frequently accomplished this by using fewer particles. Of the five potential participant responses, four were identified as correct. The only participant response that was Figure 3. Student pretest and posttest mean scores (%) and 95% confidence intervals on the ParNoMA, broken down by class period. Table 4. ParNoMA Results for Interview Participants ParNoMA Score Participant (pseudonym) Pretest Score, % Posttest Score, % Nate 20 35 Emily 25 45 Kyle 35 50 Ryan 40 40 Allison 45 95 Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198196
  • 6. marked incorrect did not clearly distinguish between the particle spacing of the liquid and the gas. Representation of Changes at the Particulate Level Each participant’s model gave insight into how he or she thought the process he or she was observing at the macroscopic level affected the connectivity of particles. In representing the change that occurs between the heated liquid in the beaker and the space above the liquid, all participants chose to break the substance apart into its elements, which is incorrect and one of the hallmarks of this particular misconception. Interestingly, four of the five participants cited the heating process as the cause of the decomposition of the compound. “They undergo a chemical reaction because it’s heated.” (Nate) “They break down because of the heat and it makes the atoms go faster and they break their bonds.” (Kyle) “They boil, which breaks the bonds to make them a gas.” (Ryan) All of the participants then oppositely showed the atoms recombining to form the original compound when they cooled and condensed on the glass. In each case, the elements were combined such that they were identical to those on the beaker. “Then the glass cools down the substance and they form back together” (Kyle) Thus, participants seemed to recognize that the substance on the glass and the substance in the beaker were identical. Identification of Changes at the Particulate Level Participants were also asked to describe each of the changes as physical or chemical and provide a rationale for their decisions. All participants decomposed the substance in the transition to the gas phase and then reformed it on the glass surface. Thus, all participant diagrams represented chemical changes. Four of the five participants correctly identified and described their model as a chemical change taking place. Participant responses were identified as correct if they referenced bonds being broken and formed or if they referenced the formation of new substances. “Chemical change because there’s a different substance formed.” (Nate) “Chemical, because the bonds broke.” (Kyle) “It’d be a chemical change because it’s breaking up the compound into two different things.” (Allison) Participants seem to have a clear understanding of how physical and chemical changes differ on the particulate level, even though their conception of what actually occurred with the particles was incorrect. ■ CHANGES AT THE MACROSCOPIC LEVEL Participants were asked to describe the macroscopic process of the substance boiling (which was set up in front of them) as a physical or chemical change. Four of the five participants correctly identified the process as a physical change. Appropriate justifications for such a response included recognizing that simple phase changes were occurring. “Physical, because it’s changing from a liquid to a gas and then back to a liquid” (Kyle) “I think it would be a physical change because it’s going from a liquid to a gas to a liquid again so it’s the physical properties of the substance.” (Allison) Such a response was in conflict with the participants’ particulate models and appeared to cause some cognitive dissonance for most of the participants. “A physical change because...well...maybe a chemical change because it’s being heated...so...well...actually I think it would be a physical change because it’s just water evaporating it looks like.” (Nate) Through their hesitant responses and body language, participants seemed to recognize that their descriptions of the changes on the macroscopic and particulate levels should have matched, although none of them explicitly made that statement. In summary, participants generally produced correct descriptions of liquids and gases at the particulate level. Participants could also clearly distinguish between chemical and physical changes at the particulate level. When asked to identify the macroscopic process as a physical or chemical change, all of the participants seemed to recognize that their macroscopic and particulate descriptions should have matched. Thus, it seems the piece that the participants were missing was the difference in strength between a chemical bond and intermolecular forces. The implications of this misunderstanding are far-reaching in an introductory chemistry course, as much of the traditional curriculum involves processes in an aqueous solution. The differentiation between the dissolution of a substance and any chemical reactions it may participate in while in solution requires a mental separation of processes involving intermo- lecular forces and processes that involve the breaking and forming of chemical bonds. Further consideration about how to appropriately establish a correct understanding regarding this conception is necessary. Furthermore, tools to assess the student conceptions involved in the distinction would need to be developed so that curricula could be evaluated appropriately. ■ CONCLUSIONS Both the qualitative and quantitative results of this study provide support for the positive effect of the novel curriculum on developing students’ conceptual understanding of chemistry. As a pilot study without a control group, the data describe the effectiveness of the treatment curriculum as it pertains to the selected population and the instructor of the course. These results are not intended to prove superiority of the novel curriculum over other models, but merely to demonstrate its ability to positively affect student conceptions. As previously mentioned, the uncontrollable aspects of the high school Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198197
  • 7. schedule may have contributed to the differences between the two sections. The scope of this study was to determine the effectiveness of the curriculum and not necessarily how the curriculum functioned within specific student populations. Understanding the specific differences between the two sections in this study may provide insight into how the curriculum could be modified to be more effective for specific student populations. Thus, further research into the effect of the curriculum on different student populations is warranted. The results of this study provide evidence for the effectiveness of the curriculum within a specific population. Further evidence would be required to make inferences about the performance of the curriculum in comparison to other curricula and in other school settings. First, a stronger argument for the novel curriculum could be made if it were shown more effective in concept building than a traditional model. This would require implementing the curriculum described here while simultaneously teaching a more traditional curriculum to an equivalent student population. However, providing instruction that may be inferior for the purpose of an experimental control may raise ethical concerns surrounding the teaching and learning beliefs of the instructor. Second, while the researchers expect minimal variation among different student groups, how an instructor implements the curriculum has been previously shown to significantly influence the potency of the curriculum.33−35 As such, an exploration of how both different student populations and different instructors affect outcomes due to the curriculum could provide support for the broader use of the curriculum and others designed around similar frameworks. ■ AUTHOR INFORMATION Corresponding Author *E-mail: cbridle1@gpsk12.net. ■ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We wish to thank the faculty and students of Grand Valley State University’s Target Inquiry Program for their input and collaboration in this effort. We are grateful for the willing participation of the students of Grandville High School. This material is based upon research and professional development supported by the National Science Foundation (ESI-0553215) and the Target Inquiry Program at Grand Valley State University. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation or Grand Valley State University. ■ REFERENCES (1) Nurrenbern, S. C.; Pickering, M. J. Chem. Educ. 1987, 6, 508− 510. (2) Sawrey, B. A. J. Chem. Educ. 1990, 3, 253−254. (3) Sanger, M. J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 1, 131−134. (4) Cracolice, M.; Deming, J.; Ehlert, B. J. Chem. Educ. 2008, 6, 873− 878. (5) Gabel, D. L.; Samuel, K. V.; Hunn, D. J. Chem. Educ. 1987, 8, 695−697. (6) Sanger, M. J. J. Chem. Educ. 2000, 6, 762−766. (7) Ealy, J. B. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2004, 4, 461−471. (8) Niaz, M.; Aguilera, D.; Maza, A.; Liendo, G. Sci. Educ. 2002, 4, 505−525. (9) Lewis, S.; Lewis, J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 1, 135−139. (10) Johnstone, A. H. J. Chem. Educ. 1993, 9, 701−705. (11) Gabel, D. J. Chem. Educ. 1999, 4, 548−553. (12) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sci. Educ. 1996, 5, 509−534. (13) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sci. Educ. 2000, 3, 352−381. (14) Griffiths, A.; Preston, K. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1992, 6, 611−628. (15) Stains, M.; Talanquer, V. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2007, 5, 643−661. (16) Garnett, P. J.; Garnett, P. J.; Hackling, M. W. Stud. Sci. Educ. 1995, 25, 69−95. (17) Boz, Y. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2006, 2, 203−213. (18) Ahtee, M.; Varjola, I. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 3, 305−316. (19) Hewson, M. G.; Hewson, P. W. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1983, 8, 731− 744. (20) Gabel, D. Educ. Quim. 2000, 2, 236−243. (21) DeBoer, G. E. A History of Ideas in Science Education: Implications for Practice; Teachers College Press: New York, 1991. (22) Bodner, G. M. J. Chem. Educ. 1986, 10, 873−878. (23) Harrison, A. G.; Treagust, D. F. Sch. Sci. Math. 1998, 8, 420− 429. (24) Posner, G. J.; Strike, K. A.; Hewson, P. W.; Gertzog, W. A. Sci. Educ. 1982, 2, 211−227. (25) Shadish, W.; Cook, T.; Campbell, D. Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference; Houghton- Mifflin Company: Boston, MA, 2002. (26) Eizenga, D. Putting the World in a Box. Target Inquiry Teaching Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (accessed Nov 2011). (27) Bridle, C. Change You Can Believe In. Target Inquiry Teaching Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (accessed Nov 2011). (28) Bridle, C. The Only Thing Constant in Life Is Change. Target Inquiry Teaching Materials. http://www.gvsu.edu/targetinquiry (ac- cessed Nov 2011). (29) Yezierski, E. J.; Birk, J. P. J. Chem. Educ. 2006, 6, 954−960. (30) Lewis, S.; Lewis, J. J. Chem. Educ. 2005, 9, 1408−1412. (31) Montes, L. D.; Rockley, M. G. J. Chem. Educ. 2002, 2, 244−247. (32) HyperRESEARCH Home Page. http://www.researchware. com/products/hyperresearch.html (accessed Nov 2011). (33) Simmons, P. E.; Emory, A.; Carter, T.; Coker, T.; Finnegan, B.; Crockett, D.; Richardson, L.; Yager, R.; Craven, J.; Tillotson, J.; Brunkhorst, H.; Twiest, M.; Hossain, K.; Gallagher, J.; Duggan-Haas, D.; Parker, J.; Cajas, F.; Alshannag, Q.; McGlamery, S.; Krockover, J.; Adams, P.; Spector, B.; LaPorta, T.; James, B.; Rearden, K.; Labuda, K. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1999, 8, 930−954. (34) Roehrig, G. H.; Luft, J. A. J. Chem. Educ. 2004, 10, 1510−1516. (35) Roehrig, G.; Garrow, S. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2007, 14, 1789−1811. Journal of Chemical Education Article dx.doi.org/10.1021/ed100735u | J. Chem. Educ. 2012, 89, 192−198198