SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 139
Chapter 2: DRAWING THE COLOR LINE
A black American writer, J. Saunders Redding, describes the
arrival of a ship in North
America in the year 1619:
Sails furled, flag drooping at her rounded stern, she rode the
tide in from the sea.
She was a strange ship, indeed, by all accounts, a frightening
ship, a ship of mystery.
Whether she was trader, privateer, or man-of-war no one knows.
Through her
bulwarks black-mouthed cannon yawned. The flag she flew was
Dutch; her crew a
motley. Her port of call, an English settlement, Jamestown, in
the colony of Virginia.
She came, she traded, and shortly afterwards was gone.
Probably no ship in modern
history has carried a more portentous freight. Her cargo?
Twenty slaves.
There is not a country in world history in which racism has been
more important, for so long a
time, as the United States. And the problem of "the color line,"
as W. E. B. Du Bois put it, is still
with us. So it is more than a purely historical question to ask:
How does it start?—and an even
more urgent question: How might it end? Or, to put it
differently: Is it possible for whites and
blacks to live together without hatred?
If history can help answer these questions, then the beginnings
of slavery in North America—
a continent where we can trace the coming of the first whites
and the first blacks—might supply
at least a few clues.
Some historians think those first blacks in Virginia were
considered as servants, like the white
indentured servants brought from Europe. But the strong
probability is that, even if they were
listed as "servants" (a more familiar category to the English),
they were viewed as being different
from white servants, were treated differently, and in fact were
slaves. In any case, slavery
developed quickly into a regular institution, into the normal
labor relation of blacks to whites in
the New World. With it developed that special racial feeling—
whether hatred, or contempt, or
pity, or patronization—that accompanied the inferior position of
blacks in America for the next
350 years —that combination of inferior status and derogatory
thought we call racism.
Everything in the experience of the first white settlers acted as
a pressure for the enslavement
of blacks.
The Virginians of 1619 were desperate for labor, to grow
enough food to stay alive. Among
them were survivors from the winter of 1609-1610, the
"starving time," when, crazed for want of
food, they roamed the woods for nuts and berries, dug up graves
to eat the corpses, and died in
batches until five hundred colonists were reduced to sixty.
In the Journals of the House of Burgesses of Virginia is a
document of 1619 which tells of
the first twelve years of the Jamestown colony. The first
settlement had a hundred persons, who
had one small ladle of barley per meal. When more people
arrived, there was even less food.
Many of the people lived in cavelike holes dug into the ground,
and in the winter of 1609-1610,
they were
...driven through insufferable hunger to eat those things which
nature most
abhorred, the flesh and excrements of man as well of our own
nation as of an
Indian, digged by some out of his grave after he had laid buried
there days and
wholly devoured him; others, envying the better state of body of
any whom hunger
has not yet so much wasted as their own, lay wait and
threatened to kill and eat
them; one among them slew his wife as she slept in his bosom,
cut her in pieces,
salted her and fed upon her till he had clean devoured all parts
saving her head...
A petition by thirty colonists to the House of Burgesses,
complaining against the twelve-year
governorship of Sir Thomas Smith, said:
In those 12 years of Sir Thomas Smith, his government, we aver
that the colony for
the most part remained in great want and misery under most
severe and cruel
laws... The allowance in those times for a man was only eight
ounces of meale and
half a pint of peas for a day... mouldy, rotten, full of cobwebs
and maggots,
loathsome to man and not fit for beasts, which forced many to
flee for relief to the
savage enemy, who being taken again were put to sundry deaths
as by hanging,
shooting and breaking upon the wheel... of whom one for
stealing two or three pints
of oatmeal had a bodkin thrust through his tongue and was tied
with a chain to a
tree until he starved...
The Virginians needed labor, to grow corn for subsistence, to
grow tobacco for export. They
had just figured out how to grow tobacco, and in 1617 they sent
off the first cargo to England.
Finding that, like all pleasureable drugs tainted with moral
disapproval, it brought a high price,
the planters, despite their high religious talk, were not going to
ask questions about something so
profitable.
They couldn't force the Indians to work for them, as Columbus
had done. They were
outnumbered, and while, with superior firearms, they could
massacre Indians, they would face
massacre in return. They could not capture them and keep them
enslaved; the Indians were
tough, resourceful, defiant, and at home in these woods, as the
transplanted Englishmen were
not.
White servants had not yet been brought over in sufficient
quantity. Besides, they did not
come out of slavery, and did not have to do more than contract
their labor for a few years to get
their passage and a start in the New World. As for the free
white settlers, many of them were
skilled craftsmen, or even men of leisure back in England, who
were so little inclined to work the
land that John Smith, in those early years, had to declare a kind
of martial law, organize them
into work gangs, and force them into the fields for survival.
There may have been a kind of frustrated rage at their own
ineptitude, at the Indian
superiority at taking care of themselves, that made the
Virginians especially ready to become the
masters of slaves. Edmund Morgan imagines their mood as he
writes in his book American
Slavery, American Freedom:
If you were a colonist, you knew that your technology was
superior to the Indians'.
You knew that you were civilized, and they were savages... But
your superior
technology had proved insufficient to extract anything. The
Indians, keeping to
themselves, laughed at your superior methods and lived from
the land more
abundantly and with less labor than you did... And when your
own people started
deserting in order to live with them, it was too much... So you
killed the Indians,
tortured them, burned their villages, burned their cornfields. It
proved your
superiority, in spite of your failures. And you gave similar
treatment to any of your
own people who succumbed to their savage ways of life. But
you still did not grow
much corn...
Black slaves were the answer. And it was natural to consider
imported blacks as slaves, even
if the institution of slavery would not be regularized and
legalized for several decades. Because,
by 1619, a million blacks had already been brought from Africa
to South America and the
Caribbean, to the Portuguese and Spanish colonies, to work as
slaves. Fifty years before
Columbus, the Portuguese took ten African blacks to Lisbon—
this was the start of a regular trade
in slaves. African blacks had been stamped as slave labor for a
hundred years. So it would have
been strange if those twenty blacks, forcibly transported to
Jamestown, and sold as objects to
settlers anxious for a steadfast source of labor, were considered
as anything but slaves.
Their helplessness made enslavement easier. The Indians were
on their own land. The whites
were in their own European culture. The blacks had been torn
from their land and culture, forced
into a situation where the heritage of language, dress, custom,
family relations, was bit by bit
obliterated except for remnants that blacks could hold on to by
sheer, extraordinary persistence.
Was their culture inferior—and so subject to easy destruction?
Inferior in military capability,
yes —vulnerable to whites with guns and ships. But in no other
way—except that cultures that
are different are often taken as inferior, especially when such a
judgment is practical and
profitable. Even militarily, while the Westerners could secure
forts on the African coast, they
were unable to subdue the interior and had to come to terms
with its chiefs.
The African civilization was as advanced in its own way as that
of Europe. In certain ways, it
was more admirable; but it also included cruelties, hierarchical
privilege, and the readiness to
sacrifice human lives for religion or profit. It was a civilization
of 100 million people, using iron
implements and skilled in farming. It had large urban centers
and remarkable achievements in
weaving, ceramics, sculpture.
European travelers in the sixteenth century were impressed with
the African kingdoms of
Timbuktu and Mali, already stable and organized at a time when
European states were just
beginning to develop into the modern nation. In 1563, Ramusio,
secretary to the rulers in Venice,
wrote to the Italian merchants: "Let them go and do business
with the King of Timbuktu and
Mali and there is no doubt that they will be well-received there
with their ships and their goods
and treated well, and granted the favours that they ask..."
A Dutch report, around 1602, on the West African kingdom of
Benin, said: "The Towne
seemeth to be very great, when you enter it. You go into a great
broad street, not paved, which
seemeth to be seven or eight times broader than the Warmoes
Street in Amsterdam. ...The
Houses in this Towne stand in good order, one close and even
with the other, as the Houses in
Holland stand."
The inhabitants of the Guinea Coast were described by one
traveler around 1680 as "very
civil and good-natured people, easy to be dealt with,
condescending to what Europeans require of
them in a civil way, and very ready to return double the presents
we make them."
Africa had a kind of feudalism, like Europe based on
agriculture, and with hierarchies of lords
and vassals. But African feudalism did not come, as di d
Europe's, out of the slave societies of
Greece and Rome, which had destroyed ancient tribal life. In
Africa, tribal life was still
powerful, and some of its better features—a communal spirit,
more kindness in law and
punishment—still existed. And because the lords did not have
the weapons that European lords
had, they could not command obedience as easily.
In his book The African Slave Trade, Basil Davidson contrasts
law in the Congo in the early
sixteenth century with law in Portugal and England. In those
European countries, where the idea
of private property was becoming powerful, theft was punished
brutally. In England, even as late
as 1740, a child could be hanged for stealing a rag of cotton.
But in the Congo, communal life
persisted, the idea of private property was a strange one, and
thefts were punished with fines or
various degrees of servitude. A Congolese leader, told of the
Portuguese legal codes, asked a
Portuguese once, teasingly: "What is the penalty in Portugal for
anyone who puts his feet on the
ground?"
Slavery existed in the African states, and it was sometimes used
by Europeans to justify their
own slave trade. But, as Davidson points out, the "slaves" of
Africa were more like the serfs of
Europe —in other words, like most of the population of Europe.
It was a harsh servitude, but
they had rights which slaves brought to America did not have,
and they were "altogether
different from the human cattle of the slave ships and the
American plantations." In the Ashanti
Kingdom of West Africa, one observer noted that "a slave might
marry; own property; himself
own a slave; swear an oath; be a competent witness and
ultimately become heir to his master...
An Ashanti slave, nine cases out of ten, possibly became an
adopted member of the family, and
in time his descendants so merged and intermarried with the
owner's kinsmen that only a few
would know their origin."
One slave trader, John Newton (who later became an antislavery
leader), wrote about the
people of what is now Sierra Leone:
The state of slavery, among these wild barbarous people, as we
esteem them, is
much milder than in our colonies. For as, on the one hand, they
have no land in high
cultivation, like our West India plantations, and therefore no
call for that excessive,
unintermitted labour, which exhausts our slaves: so, on the
other hand, no man is
permitted to draw blood even from a slave.
African slavery is hardly to be praised. But it was far different
from plantation or mining
slavery in the Americas, which was lifelong, morally crippling,
destructive of family ties,
without hope of any future. African slavery lacked two elements
that made American slavery the
most cruel form of slavery in history: the frenzy for limitless
profit that comes from capitalistic
agriculture; the reduction of the slave to less than human status
by the use of racial hatred, with
that relentless clarity based on color, where white was master,
black was slave.
In fact, it was because they came from a settled culture, of
tribal customs and family ties, of
communal life and traditional ritual, that African blacks found
themselves especially helpless
when removed from this. They were captured in the interior
(frequently by blacks caught up in
the slave trade themselves), sold on the coast, then shoved into
pens with blacks of other tribes,
often speaking different languages.
The conditions of capture and sale were crushing affirmations to
the black African of his
helplessness in the face of superior force. The marches to the
coast, sometimes for 1,000 miles,
with people shackled around the neck, under whip and gun,
were death marches, in which two of
every five blacks died. On the coast, they were kept in cages
until they were picked and sold.
One John Barbot, at the end of the seventeenth century,
described these cages on the Gold Coast:
As the slaves come down to Fida from the inland country, they
are put into a booth
or prison... near the beach, and when the Europeans are to
receive them, they are
brought out onto a large plain, where the ship's surgeons
examine every part of
everyone of them, to the smallest member, men and women
being stark naked...
Such as are allowed good and sound are set on one side...
marked on the breast with
a red- hot iron, imprinting the mark of the French, English or
Dutch companies...
The branded slaves after this are returned to their former booths
where they await
shipment, sometimes 10-15 days...
Then they were packed aboard the slave ships, in spaces not
much bigger than coffins,
chained together in the dark, wet slime of the ship's bottom,
choking in the stench of their own
excrement. Documents of the time describe the conditions:
The height, sometimes, between decks, was only eighteen
inches; so that the
unfortunate human beings could not turn around, or even on
their sides, the
elevation being less than the breadth of their shoulders; and
here they are usually
chained to the decks by the neck and legs. In such a place the
sense of misery and
suffocation is so great, that the Negroes... are driven to frenzy.
On one occasion, hearing a great noise from belowdecks where
the blacks were chained
together, the sailors opened the hatches and found the slaves in
different stages of suffocation,
many dead, some having killed others in desperate attempts to
breathe. Slaves often jumped
overboard to drown rather than continue their suffering. To one
observer a slave-deck was "so
covered with blood and mucus that it resembled a slaughter
house."
Under these conditions, perhaps one of every three blacks
transported overseas died, but the
huge profits (often double the investment on one trip) made it
worthwhile for the slave trader,
and so the blacks were packed into the holds like fish.
First the Dutch, then the English, dominated the slave trade. (By
1795 Liverpool had more
than a hundred ships carrying slaves and accounted for half of
all the European slave trade.)
Some Americans in New England entered the business, and in
1637 the first American slave
ship, the Desire, sailed from Marblehead. Its holds were
partitioned into racks, 2 feet by 6 feet,
with leg irons and bars.
By 1800, 10 to 15 million blacks had been transported as slaves
to the Americas, representing
perhaps one-third of those originally seized in Africa. It is
roughly estimated that Africa lost 50
million human beings to death and slavery in those centuries we
call the beginnings of modern
Western civilization, at the hands of slave traders and plantation
owners in Western Europe and
America, the countries deemed the most advanced in the world.
In the year 1610, a Catholic priest in the Americas named
Father Sandoval wrote back to a
church functionary in Europe to ask if the capture, transport,
and enslavement of African blacks
was legal by church doctrine. A letter dated March 12, 1610,
from Brother Luis Brandaon to
Father Sandoval gives the answer:
Your Reverence writes me that you would like to know whether
the Negroes who
are sent to your parts have been legally captured. To this I reply
that I think your
Reverence should have no scruples on this point, because this is
a matter which has
been questioned by the Board of Conscience in Lisbon, and all
its members are
learned and conscientious men. Nor did the bishops who were in
SaoThome, Cape
Verde, and here in Loando—all learned and virtuous men—find
fault with it. We
have been here ourselves for forty years and there have been
among us very learned
Fathers... never did they consider the trade as illicit. Therefore
we and the Fathers
of Brazil buy these slaves for our service without any scruple...
With all of this—the desperation of the Jamestown settlers for
labor, the impossibility of
using Indians and the difficulty of using whites, the availability
of blacks offered in greater and
greater numbers by profit-seeking dealers in human flesh, and
with such blacks possible to
control because they had just gone through an ordeal which if it
did not kill them must have left
them in a state of psychic and physical helplessness—is it any
wonder that such blacks were ripe
for enslavement?
And under these conditions, even if some blacks might have
been considered servants, would
blacks be treated the same as white servants?
The evidence, from the court records of colonial Virginia,
shows that in 1630 a white man
named Hugh Davis was ordered "to be soundly whipt... for
abusing himself... by defiling his
body in lying with a Negro." Ten years later, six servants and "a
negro of Mr. Reynolds" started
to run away. While the whites received lighter sentences,
"Emanuel the Negro to receive thirty
stripes and to be burnt in the cheek with the letter R, and to
work in shackle one year or more as
his master shall see cause."
Although slavery was not yet regularized or legalized in those
first years, the lists of servants
show blacks listed separately. A law passed in 1639 decreed
that "all persons except Negroes"
were to get arms and ammunition—probably to fight off
Indians. When in 1640 three servants
tried to run away, the two whites were punished with a
lengthening of their service. But, as the
court put it, "the third being a negro named John Punch shall
serve his master or his assigns for
the time of his natural life." Also in 1640, we have the case of a
Negro woman servant who begot
a child by Robert Sweat, a white man. The court ruled "that the
said negro woman shall be whipt
at the whipping post and the said Sweat shall tomorrow in the
forenoon do public penance for his
offense at James citychurch..."
This unequal treatment, this developing combination of
contempt and oppression, feeling and
action, which we call "racism"—was this the result of a
"natural" antipathy of white against
black? The question is important, not just as a matter of
historical accuracy, but because any
emphasis on "natural" racism lightens the responsibility of the
social system. If racism can't be
shown to be natural, then it is the result of certain conditions,
and we are impelled to eliminate
those conditions.
We have no way of testing the behavior of whites and blacks
toward one another under
favorable conditions—with no history of subordination, no
money incentive for exploitation and
enslavement, no desperation for survival requiring forced labor.
All the conditions for black and
white in seventeenth-century America were the opposite of that,
all powerfully directed toward
antagonism and mistreatment. Under such conditions even the
slightest display of humanity
between the races might be considered evidence of a basic
human drive toward community.
Sometimes it is noted that, even before 1600, when the slave
trade had just begun, before
Africans were stamped by it—literally and symbolically—the
color black was distasteful. In
England, before 1600, it meant, according to the Oxford English
Dictionary: "Deeply stained
with dirt; soiled, dirty, foul. Having dark or deadly purposes,
malignant; pertaining to or
involving death, deadly; baneful, disastrous, sinister. Foul,
iniquitous, atrocious, horribly wicked.
Indicating disgrace, censure, liability to punishment, etc." And
Elizabethan poetry often used the
color white in connection with beauty.
It may be that, in the absence of any other overriding factor,
darkness and blackness,
associated with night and unknown, would take on those
meanings. But the presence of another
human being is a powerful fact, and the conditions of that
presence are crucial in determining
whether an initial prejudice, against a mere color, divorced from
humankind, is turned into
brutality and hatred.
In spite of such preconceptions about blackness, in spite of
special subordination of blacks in
the Americas in the seventeenth century, there is evidence that
where whites and blacks found
themselves with common problems, common work, common
enemy in their master, they
behaved toward one another as equals. As one scholar of
slavery, Kenneth Stampp, has put it,
Negro and white servants of the seventeenth century were
"remarkably unconcerned about the
visible physical differences."
Black and white worked together, fraternized together. The very
fact that laws had to be
passed after a while to forbid such relations indicates the
strength of that tendency. In 1661 a law
was passed in Virginia that "in case any English servant shall
run away in company of any
Negroes" he would have to give special service for extra years
to the master of the runaway
Negro. In 1691, Virginia provided for the banishment of any
"white man or woman being free
who shall intermarry with a negro, mulatoo, or Indian man or
woman bond or free."
There is an enormous difference between a feeling of racial
strangeness, perhaps fear, and the
mass enslavement of millions of black people that took place in
the Americas. The transition
from one to the other cannot be explained easily by "natural"
tendencies. It is not hard to
understand as the outcome of historical conditions.
Slavery grew as the plantation system grew. The reason is easily
traceable to something other
than natural racial repugnance: the number of arriving whites,
whether free or indentured
servants (under four to seven years contract), was not enough to
meet the need of the plantations.
By 1700, in Virginia, there were 6,000 slaves, one-twelfth of
the population. By 1763, there were
170,000 slaves, about half the population.
Blacks were easier to enslave than whites or Indians. But they
were still not easy to enslave.
From the beginning, the imported black men and women
resisted their enslavement. Ultimately
their resistance was controlled, and slavery was established for
3 million blacks in the South.
Still, under the most difficult conditions, under pain of
mutilation and death, throughout their
two hundred years of enslavement in North America, these
Afro-Americans continued to rebel.
Only occasionally was there an organized insurrection. More
often they showed their refusal to
submit by running away. Even more often, they engaged in
sabotage, slowdowns, and subtle
forms of resistance which asserted, if only to themselves and
their brothers and sisters, their
dignity as human beings.
The refusal began in Africa. One slave trader reported that
Negroes were "so wilful and loth
to leave their own country, that they have often leap'd out of the
canoes, boat and ship into the
sea, and kept under water til they were drowned."
When the very first black slaves were brought into Hispaniola
in 1503, the Spanish governor
of Hispaniola complained to the Spanish court that fugitive
Negro slaves were teaching
disobedience to the Indians. In the 1520s and 1530s, there were
slave revolts in Hispaniola,
Puerto Rico, Santa Marta, and what is now Panama. Shortly
after those rebellions, the Spanish
established a special police for chasing fugitive slaves.
A Virginia statute of 1669 referred to "the obstinacy of many of
them," and in 1680 the
Assembly took note of slave meetings "under the pretense of
feasts and brawls" which they
considered of "dangerous consequence." In 1687, in the colony's
Northern Neck, a plot was
discovered in which slaves planned to kill all the whites in the
area and escape during a mass
funeral.
Gerald Mullin, who studied slave resistance in eighteenth-
century Virginia in his work Flight
and Rebellion, reports:
The available sources on slavery in 18th-century Virginia—
plantation and county
records, the newspaper advertisements for runaways—describe
rebellious slaves
and few others. The slaves described were lazy and thieving;
they feigned illnesses,
destroyed crops, stores, tools, and sometimes attacked or killed
overseers. They
operated blackmarkets in stolen goods. Runaways were defined
as various types,
they were truants (who usually returned voluntarily),
"outlaws"... and slaves who
were actually fugitives: men who visited relatives, went to town
to pass as free, or
tried to escape slavery completely, either by boarding ships and
leaving the colony,
or banding together in cooperative efforts to establish villages
or hide-outs in the
frontier. The commitment of another type of rebellious slave
was total; these men
became killers, arsonists, and insurrectionists.
Slaves recently from Africa, still holding on to the heritage of
their communal society, would
run away in groups and try to establish villages of runaways out
in the wilderness, on the
frontier. Slaves born in America, on the other hand, were more
likely to run off alone, and, with
the skills they had learned on the plantation, try to pass as free
men.
In the colonial papers of England, a 1729 report from the
lieutenant governor of Virginia to
the British Board of Trade tells how "a number of Negroes,
about fifteen... formed a design to
withdraw from their Master and to fix themselves in the
fastnesses of the neighboring
Mountains. They had found means to get into their possession
some Arms and Ammunition, and
they took along with them some Provisions, their Cloths,
bedding and working Tools... Tho' this
attempt has happily been defeated, it ought nevertheless to
awaken us into some effectual
measures..."
Slavery was immensely profitable to some masters. James
Madison told a British visitor
shortly after the American Revolution that he could make $257
on every Negro in a year, and
spend only $12 or $13 on his keep. Another viewpoint was of
slaveowner Landon Carter, writing
about fifty years earlier, complaining that his slaves so
neglected their work and were so
uncooperative ("either cannot or will not work") that he began
to wonder if keeping them was
worthwhile.
Some historians have painted a picture—based on the
infrequency of organized rebellions and
the ability of the South to maintain slavery for two hundred
years—of a slave population made
submissive by their condition; with their African heritage
destroyed, they were, as Stanley Elkins
said, made into "Sambos," "a society of helpless dependents."
Or as another historian, Ulrich
Phillips, said, "by racial quality submissive." But looking at the
totality of slave behavior, at the
resistance of everyday life, from quiet noncooperation in work
to running away, the picture
becomes different.
In 1710, warning the Virginia Assembly, Governor Alexander
Spotswood said:
...freedom wears a cap which can without a tongue, call together
all those who long
to shake off the fetters of slavery and as such an Insurrection
would surely be
attended with most dreadful consequences so I we cannot be too
early in providing
against it, both by putting our selves in a better posture of
defence and by making a
law to prevent the consultations of those Negroes.
Indeed, considering the harshness of punishment for running
away, that so many blacks did
run away must be a sign of a powerful rebelliousness. All
through the 1700s, the Virginia slave
code read:
Whereas many times slaves run away and lie hid and lurking in
swamps, woods,
and other obscure places, killing hogs, and commiting other
injuries to the
inhabitants... if the slave does not immediately return, anyone
whatsoever may kill
or destroy such slaves by such ways and means as he... shall
think fit... If the slave is
apprehended... it shall... be lawful for the county court, to order
such punishment
for the said slave, either by dismembering, or in any other
way... as they in their
discretion shall think fit, for the reclaiming any such
incorrigible slave, and
terrifying others from the like practices...
Mullin found newspaper advertisements between 1736 and 1801
for 1,138 men runaways,
and 141 women. One consistent reason for running away was to
find members of one's family—
showing that despite the attempts of the slave system to destroy
family ties by not allowing
marriages and by separating families, slaves would face death
and mutilation to get together.
In Maryland, where slaves were about one-third of the
population in 1750, slavery had been
written into law since the 1660s, and statutes for controlling
rebellious slaves were passed. There
were cases where slave women killed their masters, sometimes
by poisoning them, sometimes by
burning tobacco houses and homes. Punishment ranged from
whipping and branding to
execution, but the trouble continued. In 1742, seven slaves were
put to death for murdering their
master.
Fear of slave revolt seems to have been a permanent fact of
plantation life. William Byrd, a
wealthy Virginia slaveowner, wrote in 1736:
We have already at least 10,000 men of these descendants of
Ham, fit to bear arms,
and these numbers increase every day, as well by birth as by
importation. And in
case there should arise a man of desperate fortune, he might
with more advantage
than Cataline kindle a servile war... and tinge our rivers wide as
they are with
blood.
It was an intricate and powerful system of control that the
slaveowners developed to maintain
their labor supply and their way of life, a system both subtle
and crude, involving every device
that social orders employ for keeping power and wealth where it
is. As Kenneth Stampp puts it:
A wise master did not take seriously the belief that Negroes
were natural-born
slaves. He knew better. He knew that Negroes freshly imported
from Africa had to
be broken into bondage; that each succeeding generation had to
be carefully
trained. This was no easy task, for the bondsman rarely
submitted willingly.
Moreover, he rarely submitted completely. In most cases there
was no end to the
need for control—at least not until old age reduced the slave to
a condition of
helplessness.
The system was psychological and physical at the same time.
The slaves were taught
discipline, were impressed again and again with the idea of their
own inferiority to "know their
place," to see blackness as a sign of subordination, to be awed
by the power of the master, to
merge their interest with the master's, destroying their own
individual needs. To accomplish this
there was the discipline of hard labor, the breakup of the slave
family, the lulling effects of
religion (which sometimes led to "great mischief," as one
slaveholder reported), the creation of
disunity among slaves by separating them into field slaves and
more privileged house slaves, and
finally the power of law and the immediate power of the
overseer to invoke whipping, burning,
mutilation, and death. Dismemberment was provided for in the
Virginia Code of 1705. Maryland
passed a law in 1723 providing for cutting off the ears of blacks
who struck whites, and that for
certain serious crimes, slaves should be hanged and the body
quartered and exposed.
Still, rebellions took place—not many, but enough to create
constant fear among white
planters. The first large-scale revolt in the North American
colonies took place in New York in
1712. In New York, slaves were 10 percent of the population,
the highest proportion in the
northern states, where economic conditions usually did not
require large numbers of field slaves.
About twenty- five blacks and two Indians set fire to a building,
then killed nine whites who
came on the scene. They were captured by soldiers, put on trial,
and twenty-one were executed.
The governor's report to England said: "Some were burnt, others
were hanged, one broke on the
wheel, and one hung alive in chains in the town..." One had
been burned over a slow fire for
eight to ten hours—all this to serve notice to other slaves.
A letter to London from South Carolina in 1720 reports:
I am now to acquaint you that very lately we have had a very
wicked and barbarous
plot of the designe of the negroes rising with a designe to
destroy all the white people
in the country and then to take Charles Town in full body but it
pleased God it was
discovered and many of them taken prisoners and some burnt
and some hang'd and
some banish'd.
Around this time there were a number of fires in Boston and
New Haven, suspected to be the
work of Negro slaves. As a result, one Negro was executed in
Boston, and the Boston Council
ruled that any slaves who on their own gathered in groups of
two or more were to be punished by
whipping.
At Stono, South Carolina, in 1739, about twenty slaves rebelled,
killed two warehouse guards,
stole guns and gunpowder, and headed south, killing people in
their way, and burning buildings.
They were joined by others, until there were perhaps eighty
slaves in all and, according to one
account of the time, "they called out Liberty, marched on with
Colours displayed, and two
Drums beating." The militia found and attacked them. In the
ensuing battle perhaps fifty slaves
and twenty-five whites were killed before the uprising was
crushed.
Herbert Aptheker, who did detailed research on slave resistance
in North America for his
book American Negro Slave Revolts, found about 250 instances
where a minimum of ten slaves
joined in a revolt or conspiracy.
From time to time, whites were involved in the slave resistance.
As early as 1663, indentured
white servants and black slaves in Gloucester County, Virginia,
formed a conspiracy to rebel and
gain their freedom. The plot was betrayed, and ended with
executions. Mullin reports that the
newspaper notices of runaways in Virginia often warned "ill -
disposed" whites about harboring
fugitives. Sometimes slaves and free men ran off together, or
cooperated in crimes together.
Sometimes, black male slaves ran off and joined white women.
From time to time, white ship
captains and watermen dealt with runaways, perhaps making the
slave a part of the crew.
In New York in 1741, there were ten thousand whites in the city
and two thousand black
slaves. It had been a hard winter and the poor—slave and free—
had suffered greatly. When
mysterious fires broke out, blacks and whites were accused of
conspiring together. Mass hysteria
developed against the accused. After a trial full of lurid
accusations by informers, and forced
confessions, two white men and two white women were
executed, eighteen slaves were hanged,
and thirteen slaves were burned alive.
Only one fear was greater than the fear of black rebellion in the
new American colonies. That
was the fear that discontented whites would join black slaves to
overthrow the existing order. In
the early years of slavery, especially, before racism as a way of
thinking was firmly ingrained,
while white indentured servants were often treated as badly as
black slaves, there was a
possibility of cooperation. As Edmund Morgan sees it:
There are hints that the two despised groups initially saw each
other as sharing the
same predicament. It was common, for example, for servants
and slaves to run away
together, steal hogs together, get drunk together. It was not
uncommon for them to
make love together. In Bacon's Rebellion, one of the last gr oups
to surrender was a
mixed band of eighty negroes and twenty English servants.
As Morgan says, masters, "initially at least, perceived slaves in
much the same way they had
always perceived servants... shiftless, irresponsible, unfaithful,
ungrateful, dishonest..." And "if
freemen with disappointed hopes should make common cause
with slaves of desperate hope, the
results might be worse than anything Bacon had done."
And so, measures were taken. About the same time that slave
codes, involving discipline and
punishment, were passed by the Virginia Assembly,
Virginia's ruling class, having proclaimed that all white men
were superior to black,
went on to offer their social (but white) inferiors a number of
benefits previously
denied them. In 1705 a law was passed requiring masters to
provide white servants
whose indenture time was up with ten bushels of corn, thirty
shillings, and a gun,
while women servants were to get 15 bushels of corn and forty
shillings. Also, the
newly freed servants were to get 50 acres of land.
Morgan concludes: "Once the small planter felt less exploited
by taxation and began to
prosper a little, he became less turbulent, less dangerous, more
respectable. He could begin to see
his big neighbor not as an extortionist but as a powerful
protector of their common interests."
We see now a complex web of historical threads to ensnare
blacks for slavery in America: the
desperation of starving settlers, the special helplessness of the
displaced African, the powerful
incentive of profit for slave trader and planter, the temptation of
superior status for poor whites,
the elaborate controls against escape and rebellion, the legal
and social punishment of black and
white collaboration.
The point is that the elements of this web are historical, not
"natural." This does not mean that
they are easily disentangled, dismantled. It means only that
there is a possibility for something
else, under historical conditions not yet realized. And one of
these conditions would be the
elimination of that class exploitation which has made poor
whites desperate for small gifts of
status, and has prevented that unity of black and white
necessary for joint rebellion and
reconstruction.
Around 1700, the Virginia House of Burgesses declared:
The Christian Servants in this country for the most part consists
of the Worser Sort
of the people of Europe. And since... such numbers of Irish and
other Nations have
been brought in of which a great many have been soldiers in the
late warrs that
according to our present Circumstances we can hardly governe
them and if they
were fitted with Armes and had the Opertunity of meeting
together by Musters we
have just reason to fears they may rise upon us.
It was a kind of class consciousness, a class fear. There were
things happening in early
Virginia, and in the other colonies, to warrant it.
Chapter 2: DRAWING THE COLOR LINE
10
Diversity and Assessment
Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruyama, Ashley Elefant,
and Bruce Bongar
Palo Alto University, California, USA
Background and History of Multicultural
Personality Assessment
Multicultural research in psychology has increased dramatically
in recent years, with
growing evidence that cultural factors impact many aspects of
psychology including
symptom presentation, diagnosis, treatment, and assessment
(Dana, 2000; Church,
2001; Leong, Leung, & Cheung, 2010). Within this cultural
research, studies on
culturally competent assessment have been less developed than
other areas such as
diagnosis and treatment (Dana, 2000). It has been recognized
for some time that
standard personality assessments carry some cultural bias and
are affected by cultural
influence. Yet, only recently has research begun to examine
systematically the effects
of culture to incorporate culturally competent assessment into
standard personality
assessment (Dana, 2000; Flaugher, 1978).
The need for culturally competent psychological and personality
assessment is
evident when examining the changing demographics of the
United States. In 2010,
36.3% of the US population identified as part of an ethnic
minority group (Center
for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014a), and this percentage
is expected to
grow significantly in the coming years. The Latino population
alone, for example, is
estimated to comprise 31% of the US population by 2060
(Center for Disease
Control and Prevention, 20146). These changing demographics
demonstrate the
clear importance of developing culturally competent personality
assessments.
Historically, culture has been neglected in the development of
personality assess-
ments (Dana, 2000; Hall, Bansal, & Lopez, 1999; Laher, 2007).
One key example
of this lack of integration of culture into personality assessment
is evident with the
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). The
MMPI was first intro-
duced in the 1940s by Hathaway and McKinley to evaluate
personality, and it quickly
became one of the most used clinical instruments for the
assessment of personality
(Butcher, 2004; Hall et al., 1999; Hill, Pace, & Robbins, 2010).
However, this
measure used a standardization sample that did not include
ethnic minorities, and
The Wiley Handbook ofPersonality Assessment, First Edition.
Edited by Updesh Kumar.
© 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Published 2016 by John Wiley
& Sons, Ltd.
135 Diversity and Assessment
was criticized by many researchers for being racially biased
(Butcher, 2005; Dana,
2000; Pritchard & Rosenblatt, 1980). Subsequently, the
instrument was redeveloped
in 1989 (the MMPI-2) with the goals ofrevising test items that
may have contained
racial bias, and restandardizing population norms of the testing
instrument to be
more representative of ethnic minorities in the population (Hall
et al., 1999). The
MMPI -2 was also translated into several different languages,
such as Spanish and
Chinese (Butcher, 2004, 2005). These attempts to make the
MMPI-2 more cultur-
ally appropriate represented important advances with regards to
the development
of culturally competent personality assessments.
Although multicultural personality assessment has witnessed
important improve-
ments over the past several decades, its development is still
nascent with several
challenges to the creation of reliable and valid personality
assessments for diverse
populations (Church, 2001; Dana, 2000). One source of these
difficulties lies in a lack
of diversity and assessment research, with limited generativity
and dissemination.
Second, the cultural assessment literature has been marked by a
constricted focus on
culturally adjusted norms and language translation as a means
of developing cultur-
ally competent multicultural personality assessments. Although
culturally adjusted
norms and language translation represent an important part of
the process, this
narrow focus ignores other important cultural factors related to
administration,
interpretation, and validity of personality assessment
instruments (Dana, 2000;
Leong et al., 2010). Finally, there has been little theoretical
guidance about the main
ways or domains in which culture can affect assessment, and
how assessment instru -
ments should be developed, administered, and interpreted to
account for such
domains of cultural influence.
The purpose of this chapter is to conduct a literature review of
current research
on multicultural personality assessment, to establish the five
main domains or ways
in which culture informs assessment. Together, these domains
constitute a solid
foundation for culturally competent assessment to guide future
advances in the
research and practice of personality assessment with diverse
populations. These five
domains of culturally competent assessment include: ( 1) a need
for differential
norms that represent diverse populations; (2) assessment tools
should represent
culturally valid representations of the construct; (3) assessment
scales should be
constructed and interpreted to account for cultural idioms of
distress and reporting
style; ( 4) consideration of the cultural and technical context of
the testing process;
and (5) assessment feedback should be modified to account for
culturally informed
responses and expectations.
Differential Norms
Differential norms can play an important role in the
interpretation of personality
assessment results. Normative data provide a wealth of
information by allowing
researchers to compare an individual's raw scores to a
population of similar individ-
uals. Raw scores for assessment measures derive their meaning
from standardization
or normative samples with which they are developed and tested
(Geisinger, 1994).
136 Joyce P. Chu et al.
Without a sufficient degree of congruence between the
standardization sample and
the individual being tested, raw scores become meaningless.
Thus, when using an
assessment measure on a particular population, it is important to
ensure the measure
was normed on a population similar to that being tested.
Church (2001) described sample bias as occurring when
normative samples of an
assessment tool do not match respondent characteristics. These
characteristics can
include a range of cultural identities such as region of origin,
ethnicity, language, and
sexual orientation. Yet many factors and characteristics are
involved in ensuring an
adequate match between the test subject and the normative
sample. With numbers of
diverse groups in the US growing rapidly, it is critical for
psychologists to appreciate
the complex nature of culture. It is unfeasible to assume that
two individuals from the
same ethnic background will adhere to the same cultural
standards, as differences
within cultural groups are often greater than differences
between cultural groups.
Acculturation has received particular research attention as a
demographic char-
acteristic that can affect outcomes of personality assessment
and the validity of
assessment norms (Vijver & Phalet, 2004). Research suggests
that acculturation
can affect basic personality characteristics such as extraversion,
emotional stability,
social desirability, or acquiescence ( e.g., Grimm & Church,
1999; Ward & Kennedy,
1993). Thus, while many researchers simply gloss over issues of
acculturation in
assessment, Vijver and Phalet (2004) assert that the concurrent
use of an accultur-
ation measure can strengthen the validity of multicultural
assessment through the
use of differential cutoff scores, criterion data, or even
statistical manipulation.
Using acculturation scores as a covariate in regression models
may also help to
account for the role of acculturation in multicultural personality
assessment (Vijver &
Phalet, 2004). The use of acculturative information could prove
invaluable in
providing culturally competent personality assessment services.
When it is determined that the target population differs from the
normative
sample in critical areas or demographic characteristics (whether
due to acculturation,
age, gender, education, or others), a different set of norms may
be necessary before
the assessment results can be interpreted meaningfully. Indeed,
research has high-
lighted the need for different norms with numerous well-known
personality
assessment measures. For example, on the MMPI, some cultural
minority groups
consistently demonstrate elevated scores on scales 2, 8, and 9
relative to the norma-
tive sample, indicative of the need for shifted norms rather than
true elevations in
pathology (Butcher, 1996). Because of notable differences
between adolescent and
adult populations, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Inventory (MMPI) -
Adolescent was developed specifically for use with adolescent
populations ( Geisinger,
1994). The TEMAS version ofthe Tell-Me-A-Story projective
personality assessment
was developed as a specialized Thematic Apperception Test
(TAT) for urban minority
children (Constantino & Malgady, 2000). The developers found
that tailoring
the TEMAS to situations applicable to the target population
yielded more accurate
and significant results. More accurate results, however, does not
imply that cultural
differences were fully accounted for. Ambiguity and
inconsistent interpretations of
projective tests of personality make it difficult to discern the
appropriateness of these
tests for use with diverse populations.
137 Diversity and Assessment
Several challenges inherent to the creation of differential norms
preclude its
role as a universal or comprehensive solution to the need for
culturally competent
personality assessment. First, the empirical task of creating
separate norms for differ-
ent cultural identity groups can be a time-consuming and
resource-heavy endeavor.
Financial and practical constraints make it unlikely that
specialized norms or versions
of test instruments can be developed for multitudes of cultural
subgroups. Second,
because within-group variability is often greater than between-
group variability,
differential norms often represent an oversimplified solution
inapplicable to the
various response styles present within any one cultural group.
Finally, norms based
on a small subset of demographic characteristics become
untenable as mixed race
and multiple intersecting identities become increasingly
common ( e.g., which gender,
age, or ethnic norms would one choose for a 68-year-old
African-American trans-
gender individual?). Despite these challenges, creation of
differential norms has
provided important advancements in accounting for cultural
variations in assessment
responses across diverse groups.
Cultural Validity of the Construct
Cultural variations in personality construct validity
A second domain in which culture affects personality
assessment relates to construct
validity - whether the assessment actually measures what it is
supposed to measure
(Geisinger, 1994). Construct bias is a term used to describe
incongruence in con-
ceptualization of a construct between cultural groups (Vijver &
Phalet, 2004).
A similar term, conceptual equivalence, refers to whether or not
a construct assessed
by an assessment instrument has the same meaning in different
cultures (Dana, 2000;
Leong et al., 2010).
Indeed, research suggests that validity of personality constructs
can be inconsis-
tent depending on one's cultural identity or group membership.
For example,
assessing personality based on trait-level differences may not be
ideal in cross-
cultural assessment, as collectivist cultures tend to be less
familiar with Western
practices of introspection and self-assessment (Church, 2001).
These essential
differences can lead to a host of issues that may serve to
invalidate the results of
personality assessments. When assessing an individual from a
collectivist background,
measures may require adaptations in wording to reflect a more
relationally oriented
version of personality, since individuals from collectivist
cultures are more likely to
act based on social roles rather than individual trait
characteristics ( Church, 2001).
For example, in Western cultures, an individual may attribute
his or her actions of
caring for elderly parents to personality constructs such as
conscientiousness,
whereas in collectivist cultures, caring for one's elderly parents
is an expected social
role for children.
The most popular and well-researched model of personality -
the extroversion,
agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness to experience, and
neuroticism factors of
the Five-Factor Model of personality (McCrae & Costa, 1987) -
has itself been
138 Joyce P. Chu et al.
questioned for its validity across cultures. Factor analysis
research comparing Western
measures of personality with measurements in non-Western
cultures has revealed
a potential sixth personality factor, interpersonal relatedness,
suggesting that the-
ories of personality may not be equally applicable or stable
across cultures ( Cheung,
Cheung, Leung, Ward & Leong, 2003 ). Additionally,
personality may be mercurial
in nature, changing across the life span as a function of life
experience.
A need for measures that assess culturally valid
representations of personality constructs
Given the culturally variant nature of personality constructs,
assessment tools should
be evaluated and/or modified to ensure that they embody
culturally valid representa-
tions of the constructs they assess. As establishing conceptual
equivalence has proven
to be a complex task, researchers have proffered different ways
to evaluate con-
struct validity in assessment. Factor analysis has commonly
been used to ensure
construct validity across cultures (Church, 2001). Factor-
analytic techniques allow
researchers to not only assess the validity of constructs between
differing cultural
groups, but also provide information on how some constructs
may present differently
in other cultures by looking at specific factor loadings (
Geisinger, 1994). Alternatively,
Geisinger ( 1994) suggested the use of an editorial board to
assess and make necessary
changes to establish conceptual equivalence.
Several personality assessment tools have been criticized for
potential difficulties
with conceptual non-equivalence. Projective measures of
personality like the
Rorschach and the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) have
been discussed as being
based in culturally insensitive interpretations of personality.
Although versions of
these widely used projective tests have been standardized and
normed with various
cultures, it remains uncertain whether the Western
psychoanalytic theories that
the TAT and Rorschach are based on can apply to other cultures
(Church, 2001).
Geisinger ( 1994) observed that a personality assessment, such
as the MMPI-A, that
asks questions about going to a school dance or the movies may
not make any sense
to someone from a developing country. Although these
questions may represent
one construct in the US, this construct may not be the same
across all cultures, and
adaptations must be made to accurately examine conceptual
equivalence.
In some cases when constructs are determined to be strongly
culturally variant,
entirely new "indigenous" measures have been created to fit the
cultural needs of
a population. Pursuant to a factor analysis showing a non-
Western personality
factor of interpersonal relatedness, the Cross-Cultural
Personality Inventory-2
(CPAI-2) was developed indigenously for use in non-Western
cultures (Cheung
et al., 2003). With increasing diversity across the globe,
however, there are practical
limitations to developing indigenous or specific measures of
personality that will
apply to every cultural group. The growing number
ofindividuals with intersecting
identities confers additional challenges; straddling multiple
cultural identities can
lead to a variety of presentations that would require
increasingly specialized levels
of analysis. As a compromise, researchers in cross-cultural
personality assessment have
begun to find utility in integrating both universally endorsed
personality constructs
139 Diversity and Assessment
and culture-specific personality constructs to develop a more
encompassing
personality assessment for diverse groups (Leong et al., 2010).
Cultural Idioms of Distress and Reporting
and Response Style
The third domain of intersection of culture with personality
assessment states that
assessment scales should be constructed and interpreted to
account for cultural
idioms of distress and reporting and response style. Idioms of
distress refer to the
various ways in which members of different cultural groups
express distress and
psychological symptoms (Nichter, 1981). These idioms of
distress affect how people
from cultural groups present their symptoms, what symptoms
people feel, and how
they report these symptoms to mental healthcare providers.
For example, individuals from non-Western cultures are more
likely to experience
mental health problems somatically rather than psychologically
(Mak & Zane, 2004;
Ryder et al., 2008 ). This idiom of distress can be seen
throughout non-Western cul-
tures, but is particularly salient for those from traditional Asian
cultures. Assessment
instruments have been modified to incorporate these cultural
idioms of distress;
somatic symptoms are added as one of three main factors
screened in the Vietnamese
Depression Scale (VDS; Dinh, Yamada, & Yee, 2009). An
understanding of idioms
of distress and the successful incorporation of these cultural
differences into
psychological tests is crucial for culturally competent
assessment.
When considering the development ofculturally competent
personality assessment,
cultural variations in reporting style should also be taken into
consideration (Laher,
2007). Reporting style refers to how an individual reports his or
her symptoms and
how much an individual reports ( e.g., over- or under-reporting)
(Heiervang,
Goodman, & Goodman, 2008), and response style refers to the
way an individual
responds to survey questions regardless of the content of the
questions (Van
Vaerenbergh & Thomas, 2013).
Culture can impact the validity of test results in many ways due
to cultural differ-
ences in reporting and response style. Those from Western
cultures ( e.g., the US) are
more likely to engage in self-enhancement behaviors than
individuals from Eastern
cultures ( e.g., Japan), which can impact reporting style
(Norasakkunkit & Kalick,
2002). Questionnaires may show those from Eastern cultures as
having more severe
symptoms, based on their lack of comparable engagement in
self-enhancement
behaviors, than their Western peers. This difference in
assessment scores would be
indicative of a difference in reporting style rather than a true
difference in severity of
symptoms (Norasakkunkit & Kalick, 2002; Leong et al., 2010).
Hamamura, Heine, and Paulhus (2008) also noted that Asian-
Americans have
a central tendency bias to report more moderate symptoms,
regardless of the severity
of the actual symptoms, than their Caucasian peers. Asian-
Americans may therefore
under-report the severity of their symptoms based on
differences in cultural values
alone. Finally, forced-choice responses - those that require
either a "true" or "false"
response - potentially limit the cultural validity of
measurement. Having to choose
140 Joyce P Chu et al.
between a "true" or "false" response may misrepresent
responses from some cultures
by not allowing for contextual,. situational, and cultural factors
to be addressed in
the response (Laher, 2007). Laher (2007) suggested that in order
to obtain the
most accurate and culturally competent assessment of
personality, a battery of tests
combined with client interview would be most prudent for
examination of all of the
responses within the client's cultural context.
Overall, cultural variations in idioms of distress and reporting
and response styles
can result in an over- or under-estimation of true scores on an
assessment instrument.
It will be important for assessment practitioners to assess and
note such cultural factors,
and adjust score interpretation accordingly. Utilization of
multiple modes of assessment
can help to detect cultural differences due to reporting or
response style, and all
assessment results should be understood within the cultural
context of the clients' lives.
Cultural and Technical Context of the Testing Process
In creating a culturally competent personality assessment
administration, literature
suggests that one must consider the cultural and technical
context of the testing pro-
cess. It is a common assumption that assessment measures yield
consistent responses
from the same respondent regardless of the type of room,
examiner, or method of
administration. However, one important factor in considering
equivalence of assess-
ments is the cultural and technical context in which the
assessment is given (Flaugher,
1978), as people from different cultural backgrounds react
differentially to several
aspects of the testing environment.
First, the language ( e.g., wording choice of the instrumen t, as
well as test
administration in an individual's preferred language) in which
assessments are given can
affect the validity of one's test responses. Linguistic
equivalence, also known as transla-
tional equivalence, is focused on the accuracy of translation
oftest items. Many poten-
tial problems arise as the result of translating personality
assessments from one language
to another, such as differences in the meaning ofwords,
differences in connotation, and
masculine bias of wording. Furthermore, regional differences in
word usage make it
difficult to create linguistically equivalent personality
assessments. Brislin ( 199 3) rec-
ommended a translation followed by back-translation procedure
(i.e., translating back
to the original language and examining for parity with the
original version) to help
ameliorate non-equivalence issues. Geisinger (1994)
recommended taking this process
one step further, through the use of an editorial board who
would back-translate the
document as a group in order to discuss any possible differences
in opinion or concerns
about the translation. When working with more than one
linguistically equivalent ver-
sion of an assessment tool, one must be aware that issues of
metric equivalence can still
exist. Lopez and Romero (1988), for example, found that
although there are many
similarities between the English and Spanish versions ofthe
Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale (WAIS), there are important psychometric differences that
preclude direct
comparison between English WAIS and Spanish WAIS scores.
Second, characteristics or behaviors of the examiner or
environment can elicit
different responses that reflect cultural influence rather than the
true construct in
141 Diversity and Assessment
question. Stereotype threat, for instance, occurs when the tester
or testing environment
activates a negative stereotype about one's cultural identity
group; research shows
that stereotype threat can negatively affect one's test
performance ( e.g., Steele &
Aronson, 1995). Stereotypes can be activated by minute aspects
of the testing envi-
ronment or materials (Thames et al., 2013). For example,
discomfort by the exam-
inee with the tester (i.e., because of previous negative
experiences with others of a
similar gender or race, or because of the tester's interpersonal
mannerisms) may
discourage versus facilitate open, introspective, and/or honest
responses on assess-
ment instruments (Thames et al., 2013).
Third, the method of test administration - termed technical
equivalence - can also
affect one's assessment responses. Ryder et al. (2008), for
example, found that the
same respondents reported different severity levels of
symptomatology depending on
method of test administration via spontaneous report, self-report
questionnaire, versus
in-person clinical interview. Specifically, Chinese participants
reported more somatic
symptoms than their Euro-Canadian counterparts when queried
directly via clinical
interview or spontaneous report. These group differences in
somatization were not
apparent when assessed via self-report questionnaire. Morrison
and Downey (2000)
found similarly that ethnic minority clients at a counseling
center were less likely than
non-minority peers to disclose suicidal ideation on self-report; a
greater level of ide-
ation became evident only after direct suicide risk assessment
by a provider.
The mediating reasons why different methods of test
administration or testing
environment yield different responses in ethnic minority
individuals is still largely
unknown, though a cadre of cultural factors may account for
such effects. Social
desirability, cultural mistrust of healthcare professionals,
cultural practices of expres-
sivity versus inhibition, stigma about psychology, and so on,
may represent cultural
variables that could affect testing response and are deserving of
further study. Clearly,
research is needed to further quantify the effects of the cultural
and technical con-
text of the testing environment on personality assessment
responses, and to identify
the cultural factors that mediate differential testing responses.
When conducting assessment, it is essential to remember that
many factors beyond
the assessment items themselves, such as the cultural and
technical context of the
testing process, may affect the equivalence and validity of
results (Dana, 2000).
Although some factors may be out of the control of the test
administrator, efforts
should be directed towards minimizing extraneous factors,
adapting the test envi-
ronment to maximize open responding depending on the client's
cultural
background, and contextualizing one's interpretation oftest
responses within salient
cultural influences in the testing environment.
Culturally Informed Responses to Feedback
Providing feedback about test results to clients is an integral
part of the assessment
process, with the field of psychology moving towards a more
comprehensive model
of feedback provision (Finn & Tonsager, 1992 ). In fact,
research indicates that
providing assessment feedback to clients in a professional and
compassionate
142 Joyce P. Chu et al.
manner can, in and of itself, serve as a clinical intervention. A
series of experi-
mental studies found a number of positive consequences of
providing empathetic
feedback to college students after completing the MMPI-2 (Finn
& Tonsager,
1992; Newman & Greenway, 1997). Compared with control
participants who only
received examiner attention, participants in the feedback
condition demonstrated a
significant decrease in symptomatology that persisted at follow-
up two weeks later.
Additionally, participants in the experimental condition
evidenced greater levels of
hopefulness and self-esteem compared with the control group
immediately and two
weeks following feedback.
Although few dispute the importance of giving assessment
feedback to clients,
there is a dearth of research investigating the effects of
different approaches to client
feedback, and even fewer investigations offeedback as
influenced by issues of cultural
diversity. There is a need for psychologists to understand
potential issues that culture
can present when providing feedback to clients.
For instance, some of the individual difference characteristics
responsible for the
effectiveness of assessment feedback may be culturally variant.
In Finn and Tonsager's
(1992) study, authors found that participants who were more
self-reflective at
intake were generally more likely to show positive reactions to
MMPI-2 feedback.
Self-reflection varies depending on cultural identity such as age
( e.g., Haga, Kraft, &
Corby, 2009), highlighting that one must account for cultural
variation in self-
reflection or other emotion regulation tendencies when tailoring
one's approach
towards assessment feedback.
Client response to the mode of assessment feedback may also be
culturally deter-
mined. Feedback may be delivered by the provider via several
modes: in person versus
in a written report. Some providers may even decide it is
prudent not to provide
feedback at all. Most models of feedback provision have been
based on person-to-
person provision of feedback, emphasizing a humanistic
component that engages
the client and allows them to feel more involved (Finn &
Tonsager, 1992). Yet, just
as the technical context or mode of assessment administration
can elicit different
responses from different cultural individuals, mode of feedback
can also elicit
difference responses. For example, providing a written report of
assessment results to
a cultural minority client with a limited familiarity with or
understanding of
psychological and medical jargon may serve to alienate the
client or decrease effec-
tiveness of the assessment feedback.
A last example of cultural influence on assessment feedback
involves expectations
of involvement by the client, the assessor, and the client's
therapist in the feedback
process. According to Kreilkamp (1989) active participation by
the client is a basic
and important component of providing assessment feedback that
motivates clients
to take ownership of their results. The recommendation may be
less applicable for
clients from collectivistic cultures who may prefer not to
collaborate as an active
participant or advocate in the process of feedback provision.
Cultures that value
respect for authority figures may view client participation in the
feedback provision
as disrespectful or unwanted.
Providing feedback to clients about their personality assessment
results can be
a challenging, yet rewarding experience. The challenges become
increasingly more
143 Diversity and Assessment
complicated with the need for incorporation of diversity and
cultural considerations.
Clearly, flexibility and careful consideration of cultural aspects
will be an integral part
of competently providing feedback to clients. Additional
research is needed to inves-
tigate recommended models of feedback provision with diverse
populations.
Conclusion
The literature to date concerning multicultural personality
assessment is rich with
recommendations for practice and further research. As the field
of personality
assessment moves forward, it is essential that findings from the
literature be
incorporated into practice. Without awareness and knowledge of
cultural differences,
personality assessment can be misleading or inaccurate.
Competence in personality
assessment, as with psychological assessment in general,
requires administrators to be
knowledgeable about the appropriateness and limits of the test
they are using,
including cultural variables that can impact the testing process
and test performance.
The current literature analysis indicated that development,
administration, inter-
pretation, and client feedback of results from assessment
measures should incorporate
cultural influences in five main domains: differential norms,
incorporation of cultur-
ally valid representations of the construct, cultural idioms of
distress and reporting
and response style, the cultural and technical context of the
testing process, and
culturally informed responses to assessment feedback. By
accounting for these
cultural domains, personality assessment will have a better
chance at creating a
testing process with utility in a variety of contexts and diverse
populations.
The future of personality assessment is ripe with potential. With
accumulating
research on personality across cultures, assessment is becoming
more advanced.
Further research investigating personality differences between
cultural groups, within
cultural groups, and across cultures is needed to enhance the
validity of existing per-
sonality constructs and theories. Studies are needed to further
identify cultural variables
that may interfere with the substantive validity of true
assessment results, to examine
the ideal testing environment and assessment feedback
conditions depending on
cultural background, and to provide guidelines for incorporating
idioms of distress and
reporting and response style into culturally competent
interpretation of test results.
Research is also needed to investigate solutions to appropriately
assess personality
constructs in diverse groups without creating individualized
assessments for the infinite
numbers ofcultural identity subgroups. The field ofdiversity and
personality assessment
is in a nascent stage with considerable potential for
advancement. The five domains in
this paper provide a basis to conceptualize the effects of culture
on assessment, and
provide guidelines for practice and future research in diversity
and assessment.
References
Brislin, R. W. (1993). Understanding Culture"s Influence on
Behavior. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Butcher, J.N. (1996). International Adaptations ofthe MMPI-2:
Research and Clinical Appli-
cations. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
144 Joyce P. Chu et al.
Butcher, J.N. (2004). Personality assessment without borders:
Adaptation of the MMPI-2
across cultures. Journal ofPersonality Arsessment, 83 (2 ), 90-
104.
Butcher, J.N. (2005). MMPI-2: A Practitioner's Guide.
Washington, DC: American Psychi-
atric Association.
Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014a). Racial &
ethnic minority populations.
Retrieved from
http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/remp.html
Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (20146 ). Hispanic
or Latino Populations. Retrieved
from
http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/REMP/hispanic.
html
Cheung, F.M., Cheung, S.F., Leung, K., Ward, C., & Leong, F.
(2003). The English version
of the Chinese Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology,
34 (4), 433-452.
Church, A. (2001 ). Personality measurement in cross-cultural
perspective. Journal ofPerson-
ality, 69 (6), 979-1006.
Constantino, G., & Malgady, R.G. (2000). Multicultural and
cross-cultural utility of the
TEMAS (Tell-Me-A-Story) test. In R.H. Dana (Ed.), Handbook
of Cross-cultural and
Multicultural Personality Arsessment (pp. 481-513). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Dana, R.H. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of Cross-cultural and
Multicultural Personality
Arsessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Dinh, T.Q., Yamada, A.M., & Yee, B.W.K. (2009). A culturally
relevant conceptualization
of depression: an empirical examination of the factorial
structure of the Vietnamese
Depression Scale. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 55
( 6 ), 496-505.
Finn, S.E., & Tonsager, M.E. (1992). Therapeutic effects
ofproviding MMPI-2 test feedback
to college students awaiting therapy. Psychological Assessment,
4 ( 3 ), 2 78-28 7.
Flaugher, R.L. ( 1978 ). The many definitions oftest bias.
American Psychowgist, 33 (7), 671-679.
Geisinger, K.F. (1994). Cross-cultural normative assessment:
Translation and adaptation
issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment
instruments. Psychological
Arsessment, 6(4), 304-312.
Grimm, S.D., & Church, A.T. (1999). A cross-cultural study
ofresponse biases in personality
measures. Journal ofResearch in Personality, 33 (4), 415-441.
Haga, S.M., Kraft, P., & Corby, E.K. (2009). Emotion
regulation: Antecedents and well-being
outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in
cross-cultural samples.
Journal ofHappiness Studies, 10 (3), 271-291.
Hall, G.C.N., Bansal, A., & Lopez, I.R. (1999). Ethnicity and
psychopathology: A meta-
analytic review of 31 years of comparative MMPI/MMPI-2
Research. Journal of
Psychological.Arsessment, 11 (2), 186-197.
Hamamura, T., Heine, S.J., & Paulhus, D.L. (2008). Cultural
differences in response
styles: The role of dialectical thinking. Personality and
Individual Differences, 44 ( 4 ),
932-942.
Heiervang, E., Goodman, A., & Goodman, R. (2008). The
Nordic advantage in child mental
health: separating health differences from reporting style in a
cross-cultural comparison
of psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 49 (6), 678-685.
Hill, J.S., Pace, T.M., & Robbins, R.R. (2010). Decolonizing
personality assessment and
honoring indigenous voices: A critical examination of the
MMPI-2. Cultural Diversity
and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16 (1), 16-25.
Kreilkamp, T. (1989). Time-limited, Intermittent Therapy with
Children and Families.
New York: Brunner/Maze!.
Laher, S. (2007). Personality re-conceptualized: Amillonian
approach. South African Journal
of Psychology, 37 (1 ), 82-95.
http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/REMP
http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/remp.html
145 Diversity and Assessment
Leong, F.L., Leung, K, & Cheung, F.M. (2010). Integrating
cross-cultural psychology research
methods into ethnic minority psychology. Cultural Diversity and
Ethnic Minority Psy-
chology, 16 (4), 590-597.
Lopez, S., & Romero, A. (1988). Assessing the intellectual
functioning of Spanish-speaking
adults: Comparison of the EIWA and the WAIS. Journal of
Processional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 19 (3), 263-270.
Mak, W.W., & Zane, N.W. (2004). The phenomenon of
somatization among community
Chinese Americans. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 39 ( 12), 967-974.
McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T. (1987). Validation of the five-
factor model of personality across
instruments and observers. Journal ofPersonality and Social
Psychology, 52 ( 1), 81-90.
Morrison, L.L., & Downey, D.L. (2000). Racial differences in
self-disclosure of suicidal
ideation and reasons for living: implications for training.
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Minority Psychology, 6 (4), 374.
Newman, M.L., & Greenway, P. (1997). Therapeutic effects of
providing MMPI-2 test
feedback to clients at a university counseling service: A
collaborative approach.
Psychological Assessment, 9 (2), 122-131.
Nichter, M. ( 1981 ). Idioms of distress: Alternatives in the
expression of psychosocial distress:
A case study from South India. Culture, Medicine and
Psychiatry, 5 (4), 379-408.
Norasakkunkit, V., & Kalick, S.M. (2002). Culture, ethnicity,
and emotional distress measures:
The role of self-construal and self-enhancement. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology,
33 (1 ), 56-70.
Pritchard, D.A., & Rosenblatt, A. ( 1980). Racial bias in the
MMPI: A methodological review.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48 (2), 263-267.
Ryder, A.G., Yang, J., Zhu, X., Yao, S., Yi, J., Heine, S.J., &
Bagby, M. (2008). The cultural
shaping of depression: Somatic symptoms in China,
psychological symptoms in North
America. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 117 (2), 300-313.
Steele, C.M., & Aronson, J. (1995 ). Stereotype threat and the
intellectual test performance of
African Americans. Journal ofPersonality and Social
Psychology, 69 (5), 797-811.
Thames, A.D., Hinkin, C.H., Byrd, D.A., Bilder, R.M., Duff,
KJ., Mindt, M.R., & ... Streiff,
V. (2013). Effects of stereotype threat, perceived
discrimination, and examiner race on
neuropsychological performance: Simple as Black and White?
Journal ofthe International
Neuropsychological Society, 19 (5), 583-593.
Van Vaerenbergh, Y., & Thomas, T.D. (2013). Response styles
in survey research: A literature
review of antecedents, consequences, and remedies.
International Journal of Public
Opinion Research, 25 (2), 195-217.
Vijver, F., & Phalet, K (2004 ). Assessment in multicultural
groups: The role of acculturation.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53 (2), 215-236.
Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1993). Where's the 'culture' in cross-
cultural transition? Compara-
tive studies of sojourner adjustment. Journal ofCross-Cultural
Psychology, 24 (2 ), 221-249.
M i n d s e t Wo r k s
®
E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t
Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights
reserved.
M
in
d
s
e
t
W
o
rk
s
®
E
d
u
c
a
to
rK
it
MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE TOOL
For the Teacher: Using the Mindset Assessment Profile
This is a tool to get a quick assessment of your students’
mindsets—their beliefs about the malleability
of intelligence, the relative importance of learning and perfect
performance, and their attitudes toward
effort and mistakes.
It’s important that students not feel labeled by this tool. The
MAP categories just represent the way
they are thinking and feeling about these questions at the
present time. They can change these beliefs,
and they may feel differently on different days.
You can use this assessment tool in a number of ways. For
example, you can use it as an:
1) Individual assessment, scored by the teacher (with the result
not shared with the student)
2) Individual assessment, scored by the teacher (with the result
shared with the student)
3) Individual assessment, scored by the student
4) Individual assessment, scored by a peer
Once students have completed the assessment, you can follow
up with discussions
or activities to explore the issues raised. For example, you can:
discuss 1:1
to select the statement where they had a Profile
number of 1-3
(the “fixed mindset” range) and write or talk about it.
the MAP
description fits them.
their profiles and discuss
their beliefs.
category to the
class.
Here are some questions that you might explore in any of the
above formats:
l confident that
you can learn and
do well?
that you can’t do
any better?
really hard? How
did you learn it?
you be willing to work hard to achieve if you
knew it was
possible?
effort, what goals
would you set for yourself?
M i n d s e t Wo r k s
®
E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t
Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights
reserved.
M
in
d
s
e
t
W
o
rk
s
®
E
d
u
c
a
to
rK
it
Mindset Assessment Profile Tool
Description: Survey for getting a quick assessment of your
students’ mindsets
Objective: Students will complete survey and (optional)
reflection
Timeline: 10-30 minutes
Instructions:
survey is a tool to
gather information—in this case, your opinions about
intelligence, performance,
learning, effort, and challenges. It may look like a test or quiz,
but in fact it is
not! Answer honestly and say what you believe. There will be
no grade attached
to the survey and the “score” you receive is not a percentage
correct. Afterwards,
we will discuss the questions and the different ways that people
think about
them.
anonymously if
desired.
-score, or not.
writing or discussion.
After th e surve y
Make sure that you emphasize that the survey is a gauge (like
taking a temperature with a
thermometer) of their thinking right now. As we learn new
things, our thinking changes.
The survey is not intended to be a way to label students, but
rather to get to the core of their
thinking so that new learning can occur.
Option al Ref lecti on/D iscu ssio n
Debrief with your class after they complete the survey. Ask:
asked?
not?
M i n d s e t Wo r k s
®
E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t
Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights
reserved.
M
in
d
s
e
t
W
o
rk
s
®
E
d
u
c
a
to
rK
it
MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE Name:
_____________________________________
This is NOT a test! It is an opinion survey about beliefs and
goals regarding ability and performance. It is
very important that you give your honest opinion, not what you
believe someone else would think best.
Read each statement, decide how much you agree or disagree
with the statement, and circle your
answer.
Do you Agree or Disagree? Disagree
A Lot
Disagree Disagree
A Little
Agree
A Little
Agree Agree
A Lot
Profile
Number
1. No matter how much intelligence
you have, you can always change it
a good deal.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
2. You can learn new things, but you
cannot really change your basic
level of intelligence.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
3. I like my work best when
it makes me think hard.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
4. I like my work best when I can
do it really well without too much
trouble.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
5. I like work that I'll learn
from even if I make a lot of mistakes.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
6. I like my work best when I can
do it perfectly without any mistakes.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
7. When something is hard, it just
makes me want to work more on it,
not less.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
8. To tell the truth, when I work hard,
it makes me feel as though I'm not
very smart.
1 2 2 3 4 5 6
MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE NUMBER
M i n d s e t Wo r k s
®
E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t
Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights
reserved.
M
in
d
s
e
t
W
o
rk
s
®
E
d
u
c
a
to
rK
it
Creating Your Mindset Assessment Profile
1. First, determine your Profile Number for each question.
7), write the number
of your answer into the boxes in the right
column.
below to fill in the gray boxes in the right
column.
If you chose this answer: Then write this number in the gray
box on the right (Profile Number).
Disagree A Lot (1) 6
Disagree (2) 5
Disagree A Little (3) 4
Agree A Little (4) 3
Agree (5) 2
Agree A Lot (6) 1
2. Now, add up all your Profile numbers.
he right,
and write the total in the last box in the bottom
right corner.
3. What does your Mindset Profile Number mean?
and circle it.
If your profile
number falls
into this range:
Then your MAP (Mindset
Assessment Profile) group is:
People in this MAP group usually believe the
following things:
8-12
F5 You strongly believe that your intelligence is fixed—it
doesn’t change much. If you can’t perform perfectly
you would rather not do something. You think smart
people don’t have to work hard.
13-16 F4
17-20
F3 You lean toward thinking that your intelligence doesn’t
change much. You prefer not to make mistakes if you
can help it and you also don’t really like to put in a lot
of work. You may think that learning should be easy.
21-24 F2
25-28
F1 You are unsure about whether you can change your
intelligence. You care about your performance and you
also want to learn, but you don’t really want to have to
work too hard for it.
29-32 G1
33-36
G2 You believe that your intelligence is something that you
can increase. You care about learning and you’re willing
to work hard. You do want to do well, but you think it’s
more important to learn than to always perform well.
37-40 G3
41-44
G4 You really feel sure that you can increase your
intelligence by learning and you like a challenge. You
believe that the best way to learn is to work hard, and
you don’t mind making mistakes while you do it.
45-48 G5
4. Do you think the description under your MAP group matches
the way you think and feel about
your school work? Which parts are true for you and which are
not?
Howard Zinn – A people’s history of the United States
Chapter 1: COLUMBUS, THE INDIANS, AND HUMAN
PROGRESS
Arawak men and women, naked, tawny, and full of wonder,
emerged from their villages onto
the island's beaches and swam out to get a closer look at the
strange big boat. When Columbus
and his sailors came ashore, carrying swords, speaking oddly,
the Arawaks ran to greet them,
brought them food, water, gifts. He later wrote of this in his
log:
They ... brought us parrots and balls of cotton and spears and
many other things,
which they exchanged for the glass beads and hawks' bells.
They willingly traded
everything they owned... . They were well-built, with good
bodies and handsome
features.... They do not bear arms, and do not know them, for I
showed them a
sword, they took it by the edge and cut themselves out of
ignorance. They have no
iron. Their spears are made of cane... . They would make fine
servants.... With fifty
men we could subjugate them all and make them do whatever
we want.
These Arawaks of the Bahama Islands were much like Indians
on the mainland, who were
remarkable (European observers were to say again and again)
for their hospitality, their belief in
sharing. These traits did not stand out in the Europe of the
Renaissance, dominated as it was by
the religion of popes, the government of kings, the frenzy for
money that marked Western
civilization and its first messenger to the Americas, Christopher
Columbus.
Columbus wrote:
As soon as I arrived in the Indies, on the first Isl and which I
found, I took some of
the natives by force in order that they might learn and might
give me information of
whatever there is in these parts.
The information that Columbus wanted most was: Where is the
gold? He had persuaded the king
and queen of Spain to finance an expedition to the lands, the
wealth, he expected would be on the
other side of the Atlantic-the Indies and Asia, gold and spices.
For, like other informed people of
his time, he knew the world was round and he could sail west in
order to get to the Far East.
Spain was recently unified, one of the new modern nation-
states, like France, England, and
Portugal. Its population, mostly poor peasants, worked for the
nobility, who were 2 percent of
the population and owned 95 percent of the land. Spain had tied
itself to the Catholic Church,
expelled all the Jews, driven out the Moors. Like other states of
the modern world, Spain sought
gold, which was becoming the new mark of wealth, more useful
than land because it could buy
anything.
There was gold in Asia, it was thought, and certainly silks and
spices, for Marco Polo and
others had brought back marvelous things from their overland
expeditions centuries before. Now
that the Turks had conquered Constantinople and the eastern
Mediterranean, and controlled the
land routes to Asia, a sea route was needed. Portuguese sailors
were working their way around
the southern tip of Africa. Spain decided to gamble on a long
sail across an unknown ocean.
In return for bringing back gold and spices, they promised
Columbus 10 percent of the profits,
governorship over new-found lands, and the fame that would go
with a new title: Admiral of the
Ocean Sea. He was a merchant's clerk from the Italian city of
Genoa, part-time weaver (the son
of a skilled weaver), and expert sailor. He set out with three
sailing ships, the largest of which
was the Santa Maria, perhaps 100 feet long, and thirty-nine
crew members.
Columbus would never have made it to Asia, which was
thousands of miles farther away than
he had calculated, imagining a smaller world. He would have
been doomed by that great expanse
of sea. But he was lucky. One-fourth of the way there he came
upon an unknown, uncharted land
that lay between Europe and Asia-the Americas. It was early
October 1492, and thirty-three days
since he and his crew had left the Canary Islands, off the
Atlantic coast of Africa. Now they saw
branches and sticks floating in the water. They saw flocks of
birds.
These were signs of land. Then, on October 12, a sailor called
Rodrigo saw the early morning
moon shining on white sands, and cried out. It was an island in
the Bahamas, the Caribbean sea.
The first man to sight land was supposed to get a yearly pension
of 10,000 maravedis for life, but
Rodrigo never got it. Columbus claimed he had seen a light the
evening before. He got the
reward.
So, approaching land, they were met by the Arawak Indians,
who swam out to greet them.
The Arawaks lived in village communes, had a developed
agriculture of corn, yams, cassava.
They could spin and weave, but they had no horses or work
animals. They had no iron, but they
wore tiny gold ornaments in their ears.
This was to have enormous consequences: it led Columbus to
take some of them aboard ship
as prisoners because he insisted that they guide him to the
source of the gold. He then sailed to
what is now Cuba, then to Hispaniola (the island which today
consists of Haiti and the
Dominican Republic). There, bits of visible gold in the rivers,
and a gold mask presented to
Columbus by a local Indian chief, led to wild visions of gold
fields.
On Hispaniola, out of timbers from the Santa Maria, which had
run aground, Columbus built
a fort, the first European military base in the Western
Hemisphere. He called it Navidad
(Christmas) and left thirty-nine crewmembers there, with
instructions to find and store the gold.
He took more Indian prisoners and put them aboard his two
remaining ships. At one part of the
island he got into a fight with Indians who refused to trade as
many bows and arrows as he and
his men wanted. Two were run through with swords and bled to
death. Then the Nina and
the Pinta set sail for the Azores and Spain. When the weather
turned cold, the Indian prisoners
began to die.
Columbus's report to the Court in Madrid was extravagant. He
insisted he had reached Asia (it
was Cuba) and an island off the coast of China (Hispaniola).
His descriptions were part fact, part
fiction:
Hispaniola is a miracle. Mountains and hills, plains and
pastures, are both fertile
and beautiful ... the harbors are unbelievably good and there are
many wide rivers
of which the majority contain gold. . . . There are many spices,
and great mines of
gold and other metals....
The Indians, Columbus reported, "are so naive and so free with
their possessions that no one
who has not witnessed them would believe it. When you ask for
something they have, they never
say no. To the contrary, they offer to share with anyone...." He
concluded his report by asking for
a little help from their Majesties, and in return he would bring
them from his next voyage "as
much gold as they need ... and as many slaves as they ask." He
was full of religious talk: "Thus
the eternal God, our Lord, gives victory to those who follow His
way over apparent
impossibilities."
Because of Columbus's exaggerated report and promises, his
second expedition was given
seventeen ships and more than twelve hundred men. The aim
was clear: slaves and gold. They
went from island to island in the Caribbean, taking Indians as
captives. But as word spread of the
Europeans' intent they found more and more empty villages. On
Haiti, they found that the sailors
left behind at Fort Navidad had been killed in a battle with the
Indians, after they had roamed the
island in gangs looking for gold, taking women and children as
slaves for sex and labor.
Now, from his base on Haiti, Columbus sent expedition after
expedition into the interior.
They found no gold fields, but had to fill up the ships returning
to Spain with some kind of
dividend. In the year 1495, they went on a great slave raid,
rounded up fifteen hundred Arawak
men, women, and children, put them in pens guarded by
Spaniards and dogs, then picked the five
hundred best specimens to load onto ships. Of those five
hundred, two hundred died en route.
The rest arrived alive in Spain and were put up for sale by the
archdeacon of the town, who
reported that, although the slaves were "naked as the day they
were born," they showed "no more
embarrassment than animals." Columbus later wrote: "Let us in
the name of the Holy Trinity go
on sending all the slaves that can be sold."
But too many of the slaves died in captivity. And so Columbus,
desperate to pay back
dividends to those who had invested, had to make good his
promise to fill the ships with gold. In
the province of Cicao on Haiti, where he and his men imagined
huge gold fields to exist, they
ordered all persons fourteen years or older to collect a certain
quantity of gold every three
months. When they brought it, they were given copper tokens to
hang around their necks. Indians
found without a copper token had their hands cut off and bled to
death.
The Indians had been given an impossible task. The only gold
around was bits of dust
garnered from the streams. So they fled, were hunted down with
dogs, and were killed.
Trying to put together an army of resistance, the Arawaks faced
Spaniards who had armor,
muskets, swords, horses. When the Spaniards took prisoners
they hanged them or burned them to
death. Among the Arawaks, mass suicides began, with cassava
poison. Infants were killed to
save them from the Spaniards. In two years, through murder,
mutilation, or suicide, half of the
250,000 Indians on Haiti were dead.
When it became clear that there was no gold left, the Indians
were taken as slave labor on
huge estates, known later as encomiendas. They were worked at
a ferocious pace, and died by
the thousands. By the year 1515, there were perhaps fifty
thousand Indians left. By 1550, there
were five hundred. A report of the year 1650 shows none of the
original Arawaks or their
descendants left on the island.
The chief source-and, on many matters the only source-of
information about what happened
on the islands after Columbus came is Bartolome de las Casas,
who, as a young priest,
participated in the conquest of Cuba. For a time he owned a
plantation on which Indian slaves
worked, but he gave that up and became a vehement critic of
Spanish cruelty. Las Casas
transcribed Columbus's journal and, in his fifties, began a
multivolume History of the Indies. In
it, he describes the Indians. They are agile, he says, and can
swim long distances, especially the
women. They are not completely peaceful, because they do
battle from time to time with other
tribes, but their casualties seem small, and they fight when they
are individually moved to do so
because of some grievance, not on the orders of captains or
kings.
Women in Indian society were treated so well as to startle the
Spaniards. Las Casas describes
sex relations:
Marriage laws are non-existent men and women alike choose
their mates and leave them
as they please, without offense, jealousy or anger. They
multiply in great abundance;
pregnant women work to the last minute and give birth almost
painlessly; up the next
day, they bathe in the river and are as clean and healthy as
before giving birth. If they
tire of their men, they give themselves abortions with herbs that
force stillbirths,
covering their shameful parts with leaves or cotton cloth;
although on the whole, Indian
men and women look upon total nakedness with as much
casualness as we look upon a
man's head or at his hands.
The Indians, Las Casas says, have no religion, at least no
temples. They live in
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write
Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write

More Related Content

Similar to Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write

American History Chapter 2
American History Chapter 2American History Chapter 2
American History Chapter 2hngreenwave
 
Hogan's History- Tensions Over Slavery
Hogan's History- Tensions Over SlaveryHogan's History- Tensions Over Slavery
Hogan's History- Tensions Over SlaveryWilliam Hogan
 
Was Virginia The Mother Of Slavery
Was Virginia The Mother Of SlaveryWas Virginia The Mother Of Slavery
Was Virginia The Mother Of SlaveryStratalum
 
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018Rasta101
 
The French in North America
The French in North AmericaThe French in North America
The French in North AmericaAlex
 
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2Jose rizo theme 4 part 2
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2jrizo87
 
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupole
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupoleIn the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupole
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupoleEwald Erubaar Kuupole
 
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?historyteacher38668
 

Similar to Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write (8)

American History Chapter 2
American History Chapter 2American History Chapter 2
American History Chapter 2
 
Hogan's History- Tensions Over Slavery
Hogan's History- Tensions Over SlaveryHogan's History- Tensions Over Slavery
Hogan's History- Tensions Over Slavery
 
Was Virginia The Mother Of Slavery
Was Virginia The Mother Of SlaveryWas Virginia The Mother Of Slavery
Was Virginia The Mother Of Slavery
 
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018
Economics Vestiges of Enslavement darc 2018
 
The French in North America
The French in North AmericaThe French in North America
The French in North America
 
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2Jose rizo theme 4 part 2
Jose rizo theme 4 part 2
 
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupole
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupoleIn the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupole
In the name of revenge:Ewald E kuupole
 
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?
HIS 2213 LU7 Did Racism Cause Slavery?
 

More from EstelaJeffery653

Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docx
Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docxIndividual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docx
Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docx
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docxIndividual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docx
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docx
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docxIndividual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docx
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docx
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docxIndividual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docx
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docx
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docxIndividual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docx
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docx
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docxIndividual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docx
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docx
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docxIndividual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docx
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docx
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docxIndividual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docx
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docx
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docxIndividual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docx
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more »Expand view.docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more  »Expand view.docxIndividual Expanded Website PlanView more  »Expand view.docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more »Expand view.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docxIndividual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docx
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docxIndividual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docx
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docx
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docxIndividual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docx
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docx
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docxIndividual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docx
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docx
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docxInclude LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docx
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docx
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docxInclude the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docx
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docxEstelaJeffery653
 
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docx
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docxinclude as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docx
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docx
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docxIncorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docx
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docx
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docxinal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docx
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docxEstelaJeffery653
 
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docx
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docxinclude 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docx
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docxEstelaJeffery653
 

More from EstelaJeffery653 (20)

Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docx
Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docxIndividual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docx
Individual ProjectMedical TechnologyWed, 9617Num.docx
 
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docx
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docxIndividual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docx
Individual ProjectThe Post-Watergate EraWed, 3817Numeric.docx
 
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docx
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docxIndividual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docx
Individual ProjectArticulating the Integrated PlanWed, 31.docx
 
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docx
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docxIndividual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docx
Individual Multilingualism Guidelines1)Where did the a.docx
 
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docx
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docxIndividual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docx
Individual Implementation Strategiesno new messagesObjectives.docx
 
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docx
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docxIndividual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docx
Individual Refine and Finalize WebsiteDueJul 02View m.docx
 
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docx
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docxIndividual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docx
Individual Cultural Communication Written Assignment  (Worth 20 of .docx
 
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docx
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docxIndividual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docx
Individual ProjectThe Basic Marketing PlanWed, 3117N.docx
 
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docx
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docxIndividual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docx
Individual ProjectFinancial Procedures in a Health Care Organiza.docx
 
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more »Expand view.docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more  »Expand view.docxIndividual Expanded Website PlanView more  »Expand view.docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanView more »Expand view.docx
 
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docxIndividual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docx
Individual Expanded Website PlanDueJul 02View more .docx
 
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docx
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docxIndividual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docx
Individual Communicating to Management Concerning Information Syste.docx
 
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docx
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docxIndividual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docx
Individual Case Analysis-MatavIn max 4 single-spaced total pag.docx
 
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docx
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docxIndividual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docx
Individual Assignment Report Format• Report should contain not m.docx
 
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docx
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docxInclude LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docx
Include LOCO api that allows user to key in an address and get the d.docx
 
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docx
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docxInclude the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docx
Include the title, the name of the composer (if known) and of the .docx
 
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docx
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docxinclude as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docx
include as many events as possible to support your explanation of th.docx
 
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docx
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docxIncorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docx
Incorporate the suggestions that were provided by your fellow projec.docx
 
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docx
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docxinal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docx
inal ProjectDUE Jun 25, 2017 1155 PMGrade DetailsGradeNA.docx
 
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docx
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docxinclude 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docx
include 1page proposal- short introduction to research paper and yo.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 

Chapter 2 DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American write

  • 1. Chapter 2: DRAWING THE COLOR LINE A black American writer, J. Saunders Redding, describes the arrival of a ship in North America in the year 1619: Sails furled, flag drooping at her rounded stern, she rode the tide in from the sea. She was a strange ship, indeed, by all accounts, a frightening ship, a ship of mystery. Whether she was trader, privateer, or man-of-war no one knows. Through her bulwarks black-mouthed cannon yawned. The flag she flew was Dutch; her crew a motley. Her port of call, an English settlement, Jamestown, in the colony of Virginia. She came, she traded, and shortly afterwards was gone. Probably no ship in modern history has carried a more portentous freight. Her cargo? Twenty slaves. There is not a country in world history in which racism has been more important, for so long a
  • 2. time, as the United States. And the problem of "the color line," as W. E. B. Du Bois put it, is still with us. So it is more than a purely historical question to ask: How does it start?—and an even more urgent question: How might it end? Or, to put it differently: Is it possible for whites and blacks to live together without hatred? If history can help answer these questions, then the beginnings of slavery in North America— a continent where we can trace the coming of the first whites and the first blacks—might supply at least a few clues. Some historians think those first blacks in Virginia were considered as servants, like the white indentured servants brought from Europe. But the strong probability is that, even if they were listed as "servants" (a more familiar category to the English), they were viewed as being different from white servants, were treated differently, and in fact were slaves. In any case, slavery developed quickly into a regular institution, into the normal labor relation of blacks to whites in the New World. With it developed that special racial feeling—
  • 3. whether hatred, or contempt, or pity, or patronization—that accompanied the inferior position of blacks in America for the next 350 years —that combination of inferior status and derogatory thought we call racism. Everything in the experience of the first white settlers acted as a pressure for the enslavement of blacks. The Virginians of 1619 were desperate for labor, to grow enough food to stay alive. Among them were survivors from the winter of 1609-1610, the "starving time," when, crazed for want of food, they roamed the woods for nuts and berries, dug up graves to eat the corpses, and died in batches until five hundred colonists were reduced to sixty. In the Journals of the House of Burgesses of Virginia is a document of 1619 which tells of the first twelve years of the Jamestown colony. The first settlement had a hundred persons, who had one small ladle of barley per meal. When more people arrived, there was even less food. Many of the people lived in cavelike holes dug into the ground, and in the winter of 1609-1610,
  • 4. they were ...driven through insufferable hunger to eat those things which nature most abhorred, the flesh and excrements of man as well of our own nation as of an Indian, digged by some out of his grave after he had laid buried there days and wholly devoured him; others, envying the better state of body of any whom hunger has not yet so much wasted as their own, lay wait and threatened to kill and eat them; one among them slew his wife as she slept in his bosom, cut her in pieces, salted her and fed upon her till he had clean devoured all parts saving her head... A petition by thirty colonists to the House of Burgesses, complaining against the twelve-year governorship of Sir Thomas Smith, said: In those 12 years of Sir Thomas Smith, his government, we aver that the colony for the most part remained in great want and misery under most severe and cruel
  • 5. laws... The allowance in those times for a man was only eight ounces of meale and half a pint of peas for a day... mouldy, rotten, full of cobwebs and maggots, loathsome to man and not fit for beasts, which forced many to flee for relief to the savage enemy, who being taken again were put to sundry deaths as by hanging, shooting and breaking upon the wheel... of whom one for stealing two or three pints of oatmeal had a bodkin thrust through his tongue and was tied with a chain to a tree until he starved... The Virginians needed labor, to grow corn for subsistence, to grow tobacco for export. They had just figured out how to grow tobacco, and in 1617 they sent off the first cargo to England. Finding that, like all pleasureable drugs tainted with moral disapproval, it brought a high price, the planters, despite their high religious talk, were not going to ask questions about something so profitable.
  • 6. They couldn't force the Indians to work for them, as Columbus had done. They were outnumbered, and while, with superior firearms, they could massacre Indians, they would face massacre in return. They could not capture them and keep them enslaved; the Indians were tough, resourceful, defiant, and at home in these woods, as the transplanted Englishmen were not. White servants had not yet been brought over in sufficient quantity. Besides, they did not come out of slavery, and did not have to do more than contract their labor for a few years to get their passage and a start in the New World. As for the free white settlers, many of them were skilled craftsmen, or even men of leisure back in England, who were so little inclined to work the land that John Smith, in those early years, had to declare a kind of martial law, organize them into work gangs, and force them into the fields for survival. There may have been a kind of frustrated rage at their own ineptitude, at the Indian superiority at taking care of themselves, that made the Virginians especially ready to become the
  • 7. masters of slaves. Edmund Morgan imagines their mood as he writes in his book American Slavery, American Freedom: If you were a colonist, you knew that your technology was superior to the Indians'. You knew that you were civilized, and they were savages... But your superior technology had proved insufficient to extract anything. The Indians, keeping to themselves, laughed at your superior methods and lived from the land more abundantly and with less labor than you did... And when your own people started deserting in order to live with them, it was too much... So you killed the Indians, tortured them, burned their villages, burned their cornfields. It proved your superiority, in spite of your failures. And you gave similar treatment to any of your own people who succumbed to their savage ways of life. But you still did not grow much corn...
  • 8. Black slaves were the answer. And it was natural to consider imported blacks as slaves, even if the institution of slavery would not be regularized and legalized for several decades. Because, by 1619, a million blacks had already been brought from Africa to South America and the Caribbean, to the Portuguese and Spanish colonies, to work as slaves. Fifty years before Columbus, the Portuguese took ten African blacks to Lisbon— this was the start of a regular trade in slaves. African blacks had been stamped as slave labor for a hundred years. So it would have been strange if those twenty blacks, forcibly transported to Jamestown, and sold as objects to settlers anxious for a steadfast source of labor, were considered as anything but slaves. Their helplessness made enslavement easier. The Indians were on their own land. The whites were in their own European culture. The blacks had been torn from their land and culture, forced into a situation where the heritage of language, dress, custom, family relations, was bit by bit obliterated except for remnants that blacks could hold on to by
  • 9. sheer, extraordinary persistence. Was their culture inferior—and so subject to easy destruction? Inferior in military capability, yes —vulnerable to whites with guns and ships. But in no other way—except that cultures that are different are often taken as inferior, especially when such a judgment is practical and profitable. Even militarily, while the Westerners could secure forts on the African coast, they were unable to subdue the interior and had to come to terms with its chiefs. The African civilization was as advanced in its own way as that of Europe. In certain ways, it was more admirable; but it also included cruelties, hierarchical privilege, and the readiness to sacrifice human lives for religion or profit. It was a civilization of 100 million people, using iron implements and skilled in farming. It had large urban centers and remarkable achievements in weaving, ceramics, sculpture. European travelers in the sixteenth century were impressed with the African kingdoms of Timbuktu and Mali, already stable and organized at a time when European states were just
  • 10. beginning to develop into the modern nation. In 1563, Ramusio, secretary to the rulers in Venice, wrote to the Italian merchants: "Let them go and do business with the King of Timbuktu and Mali and there is no doubt that they will be well-received there with their ships and their goods and treated well, and granted the favours that they ask..." A Dutch report, around 1602, on the West African kingdom of Benin, said: "The Towne seemeth to be very great, when you enter it. You go into a great broad street, not paved, which seemeth to be seven or eight times broader than the Warmoes Street in Amsterdam. ...The Houses in this Towne stand in good order, one close and even with the other, as the Houses in Holland stand." The inhabitants of the Guinea Coast were described by one traveler around 1680 as "very civil and good-natured people, easy to be dealt with, condescending to what Europeans require of them in a civil way, and very ready to return double the presents we make them."
  • 11. Africa had a kind of feudalism, like Europe based on agriculture, and with hierarchies of lords and vassals. But African feudalism did not come, as di d Europe's, out of the slave societies of Greece and Rome, which had destroyed ancient tribal life. In Africa, tribal life was still powerful, and some of its better features—a communal spirit, more kindness in law and punishment—still existed. And because the lords did not have the weapons that European lords had, they could not command obedience as easily. In his book The African Slave Trade, Basil Davidson contrasts law in the Congo in the early sixteenth century with law in Portugal and England. In those European countries, where the idea of private property was becoming powerful, theft was punished brutally. In England, even as late as 1740, a child could be hanged for stealing a rag of cotton. But in the Congo, communal life persisted, the idea of private property was a strange one, and thefts were punished with fines or various degrees of servitude. A Congolese leader, told of the Portuguese legal codes, asked a
  • 12. Portuguese once, teasingly: "What is the penalty in Portugal for anyone who puts his feet on the ground?" Slavery existed in the African states, and it was sometimes used by Europeans to justify their own slave trade. But, as Davidson points out, the "slaves" of Africa were more like the serfs of Europe —in other words, like most of the population of Europe. It was a harsh servitude, but they had rights which slaves brought to America did not have, and they were "altogether different from the human cattle of the slave ships and the American plantations." In the Ashanti Kingdom of West Africa, one observer noted that "a slave might marry; own property; himself own a slave; swear an oath; be a competent witness and ultimately become heir to his master... An Ashanti slave, nine cases out of ten, possibly became an adopted member of the family, and in time his descendants so merged and intermarried with the owner's kinsmen that only a few would know their origin." One slave trader, John Newton (who later became an antislavery leader), wrote about the
  • 13. people of what is now Sierra Leone: The state of slavery, among these wild barbarous people, as we esteem them, is much milder than in our colonies. For as, on the one hand, they have no land in high cultivation, like our West India plantations, and therefore no call for that excessive, unintermitted labour, which exhausts our slaves: so, on the other hand, no man is permitted to draw blood even from a slave. African slavery is hardly to be praised. But it was far different from plantation or mining slavery in the Americas, which was lifelong, morally crippling, destructive of family ties, without hope of any future. African slavery lacked two elements that made American slavery the most cruel form of slavery in history: the frenzy for limitless profit that comes from capitalistic agriculture; the reduction of the slave to less than human status by the use of racial hatred, with that relentless clarity based on color, where white was master,
  • 14. black was slave. In fact, it was because they came from a settled culture, of tribal customs and family ties, of communal life and traditional ritual, that African blacks found themselves especially helpless when removed from this. They were captured in the interior (frequently by blacks caught up in the slave trade themselves), sold on the coast, then shoved into pens with blacks of other tribes, often speaking different languages. The conditions of capture and sale were crushing affirmations to the black African of his helplessness in the face of superior force. The marches to the coast, sometimes for 1,000 miles, with people shackled around the neck, under whip and gun, were death marches, in which two of every five blacks died. On the coast, they were kept in cages until they were picked and sold. One John Barbot, at the end of the seventeenth century, described these cages on the Gold Coast: As the slaves come down to Fida from the inland country, they are put into a booth or prison... near the beach, and when the Europeans are to receive them, they are
  • 15. brought out onto a large plain, where the ship's surgeons examine every part of everyone of them, to the smallest member, men and women being stark naked... Such as are allowed good and sound are set on one side... marked on the breast with a red- hot iron, imprinting the mark of the French, English or Dutch companies... The branded slaves after this are returned to their former booths where they await shipment, sometimes 10-15 days... Then they were packed aboard the slave ships, in spaces not much bigger than coffins, chained together in the dark, wet slime of the ship's bottom, choking in the stench of their own excrement. Documents of the time describe the conditions: The height, sometimes, between decks, was only eighteen inches; so that the unfortunate human beings could not turn around, or even on their sides, the elevation being less than the breadth of their shoulders; and here they are usually
  • 16. chained to the decks by the neck and legs. In such a place the sense of misery and suffocation is so great, that the Negroes... are driven to frenzy. On one occasion, hearing a great noise from belowdecks where the blacks were chained together, the sailors opened the hatches and found the slaves in different stages of suffocation, many dead, some having killed others in desperate attempts to breathe. Slaves often jumped overboard to drown rather than continue their suffering. To one observer a slave-deck was "so covered with blood and mucus that it resembled a slaughter house." Under these conditions, perhaps one of every three blacks transported overseas died, but the huge profits (often double the investment on one trip) made it worthwhile for the slave trader, and so the blacks were packed into the holds like fish. First the Dutch, then the English, dominated the slave trade. (By 1795 Liverpool had more than a hundred ships carrying slaves and accounted for half of all the European slave trade.) Some Americans in New England entered the business, and in
  • 17. 1637 the first American slave ship, the Desire, sailed from Marblehead. Its holds were partitioned into racks, 2 feet by 6 feet, with leg irons and bars. By 1800, 10 to 15 million blacks had been transported as slaves to the Americas, representing perhaps one-third of those originally seized in Africa. It is roughly estimated that Africa lost 50 million human beings to death and slavery in those centuries we call the beginnings of modern Western civilization, at the hands of slave traders and plantation owners in Western Europe and America, the countries deemed the most advanced in the world. In the year 1610, a Catholic priest in the Americas named Father Sandoval wrote back to a church functionary in Europe to ask if the capture, transport, and enslavement of African blacks was legal by church doctrine. A letter dated March 12, 1610, from Brother Luis Brandaon to Father Sandoval gives the answer: Your Reverence writes me that you would like to know whether the Negroes who
  • 18. are sent to your parts have been legally captured. To this I reply that I think your Reverence should have no scruples on this point, because this is a matter which has been questioned by the Board of Conscience in Lisbon, and all its members are learned and conscientious men. Nor did the bishops who were in SaoThome, Cape Verde, and here in Loando—all learned and virtuous men—find fault with it. We have been here ourselves for forty years and there have been among us very learned Fathers... never did they consider the trade as illicit. Therefore we and the Fathers of Brazil buy these slaves for our service without any scruple... With all of this—the desperation of the Jamestown settlers for labor, the impossibility of using Indians and the difficulty of using whites, the availability of blacks offered in greater and greater numbers by profit-seeking dealers in human flesh, and with such blacks possible to control because they had just gone through an ordeal which if it did not kill them must have left
  • 19. them in a state of psychic and physical helplessness—is it any wonder that such blacks were ripe for enslavement? And under these conditions, even if some blacks might have been considered servants, would blacks be treated the same as white servants? The evidence, from the court records of colonial Virginia, shows that in 1630 a white man named Hugh Davis was ordered "to be soundly whipt... for abusing himself... by defiling his body in lying with a Negro." Ten years later, six servants and "a negro of Mr. Reynolds" started to run away. While the whites received lighter sentences, "Emanuel the Negro to receive thirty stripes and to be burnt in the cheek with the letter R, and to work in shackle one year or more as his master shall see cause." Although slavery was not yet regularized or legalized in those first years, the lists of servants show blacks listed separately. A law passed in 1639 decreed that "all persons except Negroes" were to get arms and ammunition—probably to fight off Indians. When in 1640 three servants
  • 20. tried to run away, the two whites were punished with a lengthening of their service. But, as the court put it, "the third being a negro named John Punch shall serve his master or his assigns for the time of his natural life." Also in 1640, we have the case of a Negro woman servant who begot a child by Robert Sweat, a white man. The court ruled "that the said negro woman shall be whipt at the whipping post and the said Sweat shall tomorrow in the forenoon do public penance for his offense at James citychurch..." This unequal treatment, this developing combination of contempt and oppression, feeling and action, which we call "racism"—was this the result of a "natural" antipathy of white against black? The question is important, not just as a matter of historical accuracy, but because any emphasis on "natural" racism lightens the responsibility of the social system. If racism can't be shown to be natural, then it is the result of certain conditions, and we are impelled to eliminate those conditions.
  • 21. We have no way of testing the behavior of whites and blacks toward one another under favorable conditions—with no history of subordination, no money incentive for exploitation and enslavement, no desperation for survival requiring forced labor. All the conditions for black and white in seventeenth-century America were the opposite of that, all powerfully directed toward antagonism and mistreatment. Under such conditions even the slightest display of humanity between the races might be considered evidence of a basic human drive toward community. Sometimes it is noted that, even before 1600, when the slave trade had just begun, before Africans were stamped by it—literally and symbolically—the color black was distasteful. In England, before 1600, it meant, according to the Oxford English Dictionary: "Deeply stained with dirt; soiled, dirty, foul. Having dark or deadly purposes, malignant; pertaining to or involving death, deadly; baneful, disastrous, sinister. Foul, iniquitous, atrocious, horribly wicked. Indicating disgrace, censure, liability to punishment, etc." And Elizabethan poetry often used the
  • 22. color white in connection with beauty. It may be that, in the absence of any other overriding factor, darkness and blackness, associated with night and unknown, would take on those meanings. But the presence of another human being is a powerful fact, and the conditions of that presence are crucial in determining whether an initial prejudice, against a mere color, divorced from humankind, is turned into brutality and hatred. In spite of such preconceptions about blackness, in spite of special subordination of blacks in the Americas in the seventeenth century, there is evidence that where whites and blacks found themselves with common problems, common work, common enemy in their master, they behaved toward one another as equals. As one scholar of slavery, Kenneth Stampp, has put it, Negro and white servants of the seventeenth century were "remarkably unconcerned about the visible physical differences." Black and white worked together, fraternized together. The very fact that laws had to be
  • 23. passed after a while to forbid such relations indicates the strength of that tendency. In 1661 a law was passed in Virginia that "in case any English servant shall run away in company of any Negroes" he would have to give special service for extra years to the master of the runaway Negro. In 1691, Virginia provided for the banishment of any "white man or woman being free who shall intermarry with a negro, mulatoo, or Indian man or woman bond or free." There is an enormous difference between a feeling of racial strangeness, perhaps fear, and the mass enslavement of millions of black people that took place in the Americas. The transition from one to the other cannot be explained easily by "natural" tendencies. It is not hard to understand as the outcome of historical conditions. Slavery grew as the plantation system grew. The reason is easily traceable to something other than natural racial repugnance: the number of arriving whites, whether free or indentured servants (under four to seven years contract), was not enough to
  • 24. meet the need of the plantations. By 1700, in Virginia, there were 6,000 slaves, one-twelfth of the population. By 1763, there were 170,000 slaves, about half the population. Blacks were easier to enslave than whites or Indians. But they were still not easy to enslave. From the beginning, the imported black men and women resisted their enslavement. Ultimately their resistance was controlled, and slavery was established for 3 million blacks in the South. Still, under the most difficult conditions, under pain of mutilation and death, throughout their two hundred years of enslavement in North America, these Afro-Americans continued to rebel. Only occasionally was there an organized insurrection. More often they showed their refusal to submit by running away. Even more often, they engaged in sabotage, slowdowns, and subtle forms of resistance which asserted, if only to themselves and their brothers and sisters, their dignity as human beings. The refusal began in Africa. One slave trader reported that Negroes were "so wilful and loth
  • 25. to leave their own country, that they have often leap'd out of the canoes, boat and ship into the sea, and kept under water til they were drowned." When the very first black slaves were brought into Hispaniola in 1503, the Spanish governor of Hispaniola complained to the Spanish court that fugitive Negro slaves were teaching disobedience to the Indians. In the 1520s and 1530s, there were slave revolts in Hispaniola, Puerto Rico, Santa Marta, and what is now Panama. Shortly after those rebellions, the Spanish established a special police for chasing fugitive slaves. A Virginia statute of 1669 referred to "the obstinacy of many of them," and in 1680 the Assembly took note of slave meetings "under the pretense of feasts and brawls" which they considered of "dangerous consequence." In 1687, in the colony's Northern Neck, a plot was discovered in which slaves planned to kill all the whites in the area and escape during a mass funeral. Gerald Mullin, who studied slave resistance in eighteenth- century Virginia in his work Flight
  • 26. and Rebellion, reports: The available sources on slavery in 18th-century Virginia— plantation and county records, the newspaper advertisements for runaways—describe rebellious slaves and few others. The slaves described were lazy and thieving; they feigned illnesses, destroyed crops, stores, tools, and sometimes attacked or killed overseers. They operated blackmarkets in stolen goods. Runaways were defined as various types, they were truants (who usually returned voluntarily), "outlaws"... and slaves who were actually fugitives: men who visited relatives, went to town to pass as free, or tried to escape slavery completely, either by boarding ships and leaving the colony, or banding together in cooperative efforts to establish villages or hide-outs in the frontier. The commitment of another type of rebellious slave was total; these men became killers, arsonists, and insurrectionists.
  • 27. Slaves recently from Africa, still holding on to the heritage of their communal society, would run away in groups and try to establish villages of runaways out in the wilderness, on the frontier. Slaves born in America, on the other hand, were more likely to run off alone, and, with the skills they had learned on the plantation, try to pass as free men. In the colonial papers of England, a 1729 report from the lieutenant governor of Virginia to the British Board of Trade tells how "a number of Negroes, about fifteen... formed a design to withdraw from their Master and to fix themselves in the fastnesses of the neighboring Mountains. They had found means to get into their possession some Arms and Ammunition, and they took along with them some Provisions, their Cloths, bedding and working Tools... Tho' this attempt has happily been defeated, it ought nevertheless to awaken us into some effectual measures..." Slavery was immensely profitable to some masters. James Madison told a British visitor
  • 28. shortly after the American Revolution that he could make $257 on every Negro in a year, and spend only $12 or $13 on his keep. Another viewpoint was of slaveowner Landon Carter, writing about fifty years earlier, complaining that his slaves so neglected their work and were so uncooperative ("either cannot or will not work") that he began to wonder if keeping them was worthwhile. Some historians have painted a picture—based on the infrequency of organized rebellions and the ability of the South to maintain slavery for two hundred years—of a slave population made submissive by their condition; with their African heritage destroyed, they were, as Stanley Elkins said, made into "Sambos," "a society of helpless dependents." Or as another historian, Ulrich Phillips, said, "by racial quality submissive." But looking at the totality of slave behavior, at the resistance of everyday life, from quiet noncooperation in work to running away, the picture becomes different. In 1710, warning the Virginia Assembly, Governor Alexander Spotswood said:
  • 29. ...freedom wears a cap which can without a tongue, call together all those who long to shake off the fetters of slavery and as such an Insurrection would surely be attended with most dreadful consequences so I we cannot be too early in providing against it, both by putting our selves in a better posture of defence and by making a law to prevent the consultations of those Negroes. Indeed, considering the harshness of punishment for running away, that so many blacks did run away must be a sign of a powerful rebelliousness. All through the 1700s, the Virginia slave code read: Whereas many times slaves run away and lie hid and lurking in swamps, woods, and other obscure places, killing hogs, and commiting other injuries to the inhabitants... if the slave does not immediately return, anyone whatsoever may kill or destroy such slaves by such ways and means as he... shall
  • 30. think fit... If the slave is apprehended... it shall... be lawful for the county court, to order such punishment for the said slave, either by dismembering, or in any other way... as they in their discretion shall think fit, for the reclaiming any such incorrigible slave, and terrifying others from the like practices... Mullin found newspaper advertisements between 1736 and 1801 for 1,138 men runaways, and 141 women. One consistent reason for running away was to find members of one's family— showing that despite the attempts of the slave system to destroy family ties by not allowing marriages and by separating families, slaves would face death and mutilation to get together. In Maryland, where slaves were about one-third of the population in 1750, slavery had been written into law since the 1660s, and statutes for controlling rebellious slaves were passed. There were cases where slave women killed their masters, sometimes by poisoning them, sometimes by burning tobacco houses and homes. Punishment ranged from
  • 31. whipping and branding to execution, but the trouble continued. In 1742, seven slaves were put to death for murdering their master. Fear of slave revolt seems to have been a permanent fact of plantation life. William Byrd, a wealthy Virginia slaveowner, wrote in 1736: We have already at least 10,000 men of these descendants of Ham, fit to bear arms, and these numbers increase every day, as well by birth as by importation. And in case there should arise a man of desperate fortune, he might with more advantage than Cataline kindle a servile war... and tinge our rivers wide as they are with blood. It was an intricate and powerful system of control that the slaveowners developed to maintain their labor supply and their way of life, a system both subtle and crude, involving every device that social orders employ for keeping power and wealth where it is. As Kenneth Stampp puts it:
  • 32. A wise master did not take seriously the belief that Negroes were natural-born slaves. He knew better. He knew that Negroes freshly imported from Africa had to be broken into bondage; that each succeeding generation had to be carefully trained. This was no easy task, for the bondsman rarely submitted willingly. Moreover, he rarely submitted completely. In most cases there was no end to the need for control—at least not until old age reduced the slave to a condition of helplessness. The system was psychological and physical at the same time. The slaves were taught discipline, were impressed again and again with the idea of their own inferiority to "know their place," to see blackness as a sign of subordination, to be awed by the power of the master, to merge their interest with the master's, destroying their own individual needs. To accomplish this there was the discipline of hard labor, the breakup of the slave family, the lulling effects of
  • 33. religion (which sometimes led to "great mischief," as one slaveholder reported), the creation of disunity among slaves by separating them into field slaves and more privileged house slaves, and finally the power of law and the immediate power of the overseer to invoke whipping, burning, mutilation, and death. Dismemberment was provided for in the Virginia Code of 1705. Maryland passed a law in 1723 providing for cutting off the ears of blacks who struck whites, and that for certain serious crimes, slaves should be hanged and the body quartered and exposed. Still, rebellions took place—not many, but enough to create constant fear among white planters. The first large-scale revolt in the North American colonies took place in New York in 1712. In New York, slaves were 10 percent of the population, the highest proportion in the northern states, where economic conditions usually did not require large numbers of field slaves. About twenty- five blacks and two Indians set fire to a building, then killed nine whites who came on the scene. They were captured by soldiers, put on trial,
  • 34. and twenty-one were executed. The governor's report to England said: "Some were burnt, others were hanged, one broke on the wheel, and one hung alive in chains in the town..." One had been burned over a slow fire for eight to ten hours—all this to serve notice to other slaves. A letter to London from South Carolina in 1720 reports: I am now to acquaint you that very lately we have had a very wicked and barbarous plot of the designe of the negroes rising with a designe to destroy all the white people in the country and then to take Charles Town in full body but it pleased God it was discovered and many of them taken prisoners and some burnt and some hang'd and some banish'd. Around this time there were a number of fires in Boston and New Haven, suspected to be the work of Negro slaves. As a result, one Negro was executed in Boston, and the Boston Council ruled that any slaves who on their own gathered in groups of two or more were to be punished by
  • 35. whipping. At Stono, South Carolina, in 1739, about twenty slaves rebelled, killed two warehouse guards, stole guns and gunpowder, and headed south, killing people in their way, and burning buildings. They were joined by others, until there were perhaps eighty slaves in all and, according to one account of the time, "they called out Liberty, marched on with Colours displayed, and two Drums beating." The militia found and attacked them. In the ensuing battle perhaps fifty slaves and twenty-five whites were killed before the uprising was crushed. Herbert Aptheker, who did detailed research on slave resistance in North America for his book American Negro Slave Revolts, found about 250 instances where a minimum of ten slaves joined in a revolt or conspiracy. From time to time, whites were involved in the slave resistance. As early as 1663, indentured white servants and black slaves in Gloucester County, Virginia, formed a conspiracy to rebel and gain their freedom. The plot was betrayed, and ended with executions. Mullin reports that the
  • 36. newspaper notices of runaways in Virginia often warned "ill - disposed" whites about harboring fugitives. Sometimes slaves and free men ran off together, or cooperated in crimes together. Sometimes, black male slaves ran off and joined white women. From time to time, white ship captains and watermen dealt with runaways, perhaps making the slave a part of the crew. In New York in 1741, there were ten thousand whites in the city and two thousand black slaves. It had been a hard winter and the poor—slave and free— had suffered greatly. When mysterious fires broke out, blacks and whites were accused of conspiring together. Mass hysteria developed against the accused. After a trial full of lurid accusations by informers, and forced confessions, two white men and two white women were executed, eighteen slaves were hanged, and thirteen slaves were burned alive. Only one fear was greater than the fear of black rebellion in the new American colonies. That was the fear that discontented whites would join black slaves to
  • 37. overthrow the existing order. In the early years of slavery, especially, before racism as a way of thinking was firmly ingrained, while white indentured servants were often treated as badly as black slaves, there was a possibility of cooperation. As Edmund Morgan sees it: There are hints that the two despised groups initially saw each other as sharing the same predicament. It was common, for example, for servants and slaves to run away together, steal hogs together, get drunk together. It was not uncommon for them to make love together. In Bacon's Rebellion, one of the last gr oups to surrender was a mixed band of eighty negroes and twenty English servants. As Morgan says, masters, "initially at least, perceived slaves in much the same way they had always perceived servants... shiftless, irresponsible, unfaithful, ungrateful, dishonest..." And "if freemen with disappointed hopes should make common cause with slaves of desperate hope, the results might be worse than anything Bacon had done."
  • 38. And so, measures were taken. About the same time that slave codes, involving discipline and punishment, were passed by the Virginia Assembly, Virginia's ruling class, having proclaimed that all white men were superior to black, went on to offer their social (but white) inferiors a number of benefits previously denied them. In 1705 a law was passed requiring masters to provide white servants whose indenture time was up with ten bushels of corn, thirty shillings, and a gun, while women servants were to get 15 bushels of corn and forty shillings. Also, the newly freed servants were to get 50 acres of land. Morgan concludes: "Once the small planter felt less exploited by taxation and began to prosper a little, he became less turbulent, less dangerous, more respectable. He could begin to see his big neighbor not as an extortionist but as a powerful protector of their common interests." We see now a complex web of historical threads to ensnare blacks for slavery in America: the desperation of starving settlers, the special helplessness of the
  • 39. displaced African, the powerful incentive of profit for slave trader and planter, the temptation of superior status for poor whites, the elaborate controls against escape and rebellion, the legal and social punishment of black and white collaboration. The point is that the elements of this web are historical, not "natural." This does not mean that they are easily disentangled, dismantled. It means only that there is a possibility for something else, under historical conditions not yet realized. And one of these conditions would be the elimination of that class exploitation which has made poor whites desperate for small gifts of status, and has prevented that unity of black and white necessary for joint rebellion and reconstruction. Around 1700, the Virginia House of Burgesses declared: The Christian Servants in this country for the most part consists of the Worser Sort of the people of Europe. And since... such numbers of Irish and other Nations have
  • 40. been brought in of which a great many have been soldiers in the late warrs that according to our present Circumstances we can hardly governe them and if they were fitted with Armes and had the Opertunity of meeting together by Musters we have just reason to fears they may rise upon us. It was a kind of class consciousness, a class fear. There were things happening in early Virginia, and in the other colonies, to warrant it. Chapter 2: DRAWING THE COLOR LINE 10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruyama, Ashley Elefant, and Bruce Bongar Palo Alto University, California, USA Background and History of Multicultural Personality Assessment Multicultural research in psychology has increased dramatically in recent years, with
  • 41. growing evidence that cultural factors impact many aspects of psychology including symptom presentation, diagnosis, treatment, and assessment (Dana, 2000; Church, 2001; Leong, Leung, & Cheung, 2010). Within this cultural research, studies on culturally competent assessment have been less developed than other areas such as diagnosis and treatment (Dana, 2000). It has been recognized for some time that standard personality assessments carry some cultural bias and are affected by cultural influence. Yet, only recently has research begun to examine systematically the effects of culture to incorporate culturally competent assessment into standard personality assessment (Dana, 2000; Flaugher, 1978). The need for culturally competent psychological and personality assessment is evident when examining the changing demographics of the United States. In 2010, 36.3% of the US population identified as part of an ethnic minority group (Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014a), and this percentage is expected to grow significantly in the coming years. The Latino population alone, for example, is estimated to comprise 31% of the US population by 2060 (Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 20146). These changing demographics demonstrate the clear importance of developing culturally competent personality assessments. Historically, culture has been neglected in the development of
  • 42. personality assess- ments (Dana, 2000; Hall, Bansal, & Lopez, 1999; Laher, 2007). One key example of this lack of integration of culture into personality assessment is evident with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). The MMPI was first intro- duced in the 1940s by Hathaway and McKinley to evaluate personality, and it quickly became one of the most used clinical instruments for the assessment of personality (Butcher, 2004; Hall et al., 1999; Hill, Pace, & Robbins, 2010). However, this measure used a standardization sample that did not include ethnic minorities, and The Wiley Handbook ofPersonality Assessment, First Edition. Edited by Updesh Kumar. © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Published 2016 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 135 Diversity and Assessment was criticized by many researchers for being racially biased (Butcher, 2005; Dana, 2000; Pritchard & Rosenblatt, 1980). Subsequently, the instrument was redeveloped in 1989 (the MMPI-2) with the goals ofrevising test items that may have contained racial bias, and restandardizing population norms of the testing instrument to be more representative of ethnic minorities in the population (Hall et al., 1999). The MMPI -2 was also translated into several different languages,
  • 43. such as Spanish and Chinese (Butcher, 2004, 2005). These attempts to make the MMPI-2 more cultur- ally appropriate represented important advances with regards to the development of culturally competent personality assessments. Although multicultural personality assessment has witnessed important improve- ments over the past several decades, its development is still nascent with several challenges to the creation of reliable and valid personality assessments for diverse populations (Church, 2001; Dana, 2000). One source of these difficulties lies in a lack of diversity and assessment research, with limited generativity and dissemination. Second, the cultural assessment literature has been marked by a constricted focus on culturally adjusted norms and language translation as a means of developing cultur- ally competent multicultural personality assessments. Although culturally adjusted norms and language translation represent an important part of the process, this narrow focus ignores other important cultural factors related to administration, interpretation, and validity of personality assessment instruments (Dana, 2000; Leong et al., 2010). Finally, there has been little theoretical guidance about the main ways or domains in which culture can affect assessment, and how assessment instru - ments should be developed, administered, and interpreted to account for such domains of cultural influence.
  • 44. The purpose of this chapter is to conduct a literature review of current research on multicultural personality assessment, to establish the five main domains or ways in which culture informs assessment. Together, these domains constitute a solid foundation for culturally competent assessment to guide future advances in the research and practice of personality assessment with diverse populations. These five domains of culturally competent assessment include: ( 1) a need for differential norms that represent diverse populations; (2) assessment tools should represent culturally valid representations of the construct; (3) assessment scales should be constructed and interpreted to account for cultural idioms of distress and reporting style; ( 4) consideration of the cultural and technical context of the testing process; and (5) assessment feedback should be modified to account for culturally informed responses and expectations. Differential Norms Differential norms can play an important role in the interpretation of personality assessment results. Normative data provide a wealth of information by allowing researchers to compare an individual's raw scores to a population of similar individ- uals. Raw scores for assessment measures derive their meaning from standardization or normative samples with which they are developed and tested
  • 45. (Geisinger, 1994). 136 Joyce P. Chu et al. Without a sufficient degree of congruence between the standardization sample and the individual being tested, raw scores become meaningless. Thus, when using an assessment measure on a particular population, it is important to ensure the measure was normed on a population similar to that being tested. Church (2001) described sample bias as occurring when normative samples of an assessment tool do not match respondent characteristics. These characteristics can include a range of cultural identities such as region of origin, ethnicity, language, and sexual orientation. Yet many factors and characteristics are involved in ensuring an adequate match between the test subject and the normative sample. With numbers of diverse groups in the US growing rapidly, it is critical for psychologists to appreciate the complex nature of culture. It is unfeasible to assume that two individuals from the same ethnic background will adhere to the same cultural standards, as differences within cultural groups are often greater than differences between cultural groups. Acculturation has received particular research attention as a demographic char- acteristic that can affect outcomes of personality assessment
  • 46. and the validity of assessment norms (Vijver & Phalet, 2004). Research suggests that acculturation can affect basic personality characteristics such as extraversion, emotional stability, social desirability, or acquiescence ( e.g., Grimm & Church, 1999; Ward & Kennedy, 1993). Thus, while many researchers simply gloss over issues of acculturation in assessment, Vijver and Phalet (2004) assert that the concurrent use of an accultur- ation measure can strengthen the validity of multicultural assessment through the use of differential cutoff scores, criterion data, or even statistical manipulation. Using acculturation scores as a covariate in regression models may also help to account for the role of acculturation in multicultural personality assessment (Vijver & Phalet, 2004). The use of acculturative information could prove invaluable in providing culturally competent personality assessment services. When it is determined that the target population differs from the normative sample in critical areas or demographic characteristics (whether due to acculturation, age, gender, education, or others), a different set of norms may be necessary before the assessment results can be interpreted meaningfully. Indeed, research has high- lighted the need for different norms with numerous well-known personality assessment measures. For example, on the MMPI, some cultural minority groups consistently demonstrate elevated scores on scales 2, 8, and 9
  • 47. relative to the norma- tive sample, indicative of the need for shifted norms rather than true elevations in pathology (Butcher, 1996). Because of notable differences between adolescent and adult populations, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) - Adolescent was developed specifically for use with adolescent populations ( Geisinger, 1994). The TEMAS version ofthe Tell-Me-A-Story projective personality assessment was developed as a specialized Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) for urban minority children (Constantino & Malgady, 2000). The developers found that tailoring the TEMAS to situations applicable to the target population yielded more accurate and significant results. More accurate results, however, does not imply that cultural differences were fully accounted for. Ambiguity and inconsistent interpretations of projective tests of personality make it difficult to discern the appropriateness of these tests for use with diverse populations. 137 Diversity and Assessment Several challenges inherent to the creation of differential norms preclude its role as a universal or comprehensive solution to the need for culturally competent personality assessment. First, the empirical task of creating separate norms for differ- ent cultural identity groups can be a time-consuming and
  • 48. resource-heavy endeavor. Financial and practical constraints make it unlikely that specialized norms or versions of test instruments can be developed for multitudes of cultural subgroups. Second, because within-group variability is often greater than between- group variability, differential norms often represent an oversimplified solution inapplicable to the various response styles present within any one cultural group. Finally, norms based on a small subset of demographic characteristics become untenable as mixed race and multiple intersecting identities become increasingly common ( e.g., which gender, age, or ethnic norms would one choose for a 68-year-old African-American trans- gender individual?). Despite these challenges, creation of differential norms has provided important advancements in accounting for cultural variations in assessment responses across diverse groups. Cultural Validity of the Construct Cultural variations in personality construct validity A second domain in which culture affects personality assessment relates to construct validity - whether the assessment actually measures what it is supposed to measure (Geisinger, 1994). Construct bias is a term used to describe incongruence in con- ceptualization of a construct between cultural groups (Vijver & Phalet, 2004). A similar term, conceptual equivalence, refers to whether or not
  • 49. a construct assessed by an assessment instrument has the same meaning in different cultures (Dana, 2000; Leong et al., 2010). Indeed, research suggests that validity of personality constructs can be inconsis- tent depending on one's cultural identity or group membership. For example, assessing personality based on trait-level differences may not be ideal in cross- cultural assessment, as collectivist cultures tend to be less familiar with Western practices of introspection and self-assessment (Church, 2001). These essential differences can lead to a host of issues that may serve to invalidate the results of personality assessments. When assessing an individual from a collectivist background, measures may require adaptations in wording to reflect a more relationally oriented version of personality, since individuals from collectivist cultures are more likely to act based on social roles rather than individual trait characteristics ( Church, 2001). For example, in Western cultures, an individual may attribute his or her actions of caring for elderly parents to personality constructs such as conscientiousness, whereas in collectivist cultures, caring for one's elderly parents is an expected social role for children. The most popular and well-researched model of personality - the extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness to experience, and
  • 50. neuroticism factors of the Five-Factor Model of personality (McCrae & Costa, 1987) - has itself been 138 Joyce P. Chu et al. questioned for its validity across cultures. Factor analysis research comparing Western measures of personality with measurements in non-Western cultures has revealed a potential sixth personality factor, interpersonal relatedness, suggesting that the- ories of personality may not be equally applicable or stable across cultures ( Cheung, Cheung, Leung, Ward & Leong, 2003 ). Additionally, personality may be mercurial in nature, changing across the life span as a function of life experience. A need for measures that assess culturally valid representations of personality constructs Given the culturally variant nature of personality constructs, assessment tools should be evaluated and/or modified to ensure that they embody culturally valid representa- tions of the constructs they assess. As establishing conceptual equivalence has proven to be a complex task, researchers have proffered different ways to evaluate con- struct validity in assessment. Factor analysis has commonly been used to ensure construct validity across cultures (Church, 2001). Factor- analytic techniques allow
  • 51. researchers to not only assess the validity of constructs between differing cultural groups, but also provide information on how some constructs may present differently in other cultures by looking at specific factor loadings ( Geisinger, 1994). Alternatively, Geisinger ( 1994) suggested the use of an editorial board to assess and make necessary changes to establish conceptual equivalence. Several personality assessment tools have been criticized for potential difficulties with conceptual non-equivalence. Projective measures of personality like the Rorschach and the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) have been discussed as being based in culturally insensitive interpretations of personality. Although versions of these widely used projective tests have been standardized and normed with various cultures, it remains uncertain whether the Western psychoanalytic theories that the TAT and Rorschach are based on can apply to other cultures (Church, 2001). Geisinger ( 1994) observed that a personality assessment, such as the MMPI-A, that asks questions about going to a school dance or the movies may not make any sense to someone from a developing country. Although these questions may represent one construct in the US, this construct may not be the same across all cultures, and adaptations must be made to accurately examine conceptual equivalence. In some cases when constructs are determined to be strongly
  • 52. culturally variant, entirely new "indigenous" measures have been created to fit the cultural needs of a population. Pursuant to a factor analysis showing a non- Western personality factor of interpersonal relatedness, the Cross-Cultural Personality Inventory-2 (CPAI-2) was developed indigenously for use in non-Western cultures (Cheung et al., 2003). With increasing diversity across the globe, however, there are practical limitations to developing indigenous or specific measures of personality that will apply to every cultural group. The growing number ofindividuals with intersecting identities confers additional challenges; straddling multiple cultural identities can lead to a variety of presentations that would require increasingly specialized levels of analysis. As a compromise, researchers in cross-cultural personality assessment have begun to find utility in integrating both universally endorsed personality constructs 139 Diversity and Assessment and culture-specific personality constructs to develop a more encompassing personality assessment for diverse groups (Leong et al., 2010). Cultural Idioms of Distress and Reporting and Response Style The third domain of intersection of culture with personality
  • 53. assessment states that assessment scales should be constructed and interpreted to account for cultural idioms of distress and reporting and response style. Idioms of distress refer to the various ways in which members of different cultural groups express distress and psychological symptoms (Nichter, 1981). These idioms of distress affect how people from cultural groups present their symptoms, what symptoms people feel, and how they report these symptoms to mental healthcare providers. For example, individuals from non-Western cultures are more likely to experience mental health problems somatically rather than psychologically (Mak & Zane, 2004; Ryder et al., 2008 ). This idiom of distress can be seen throughout non-Western cul- tures, but is particularly salient for those from traditional Asian cultures. Assessment instruments have been modified to incorporate these cultural idioms of distress; somatic symptoms are added as one of three main factors screened in the Vietnamese Depression Scale (VDS; Dinh, Yamada, & Yee, 2009). An understanding of idioms of distress and the successful incorporation of these cultural differences into psychological tests is crucial for culturally competent assessment. When considering the development ofculturally competent personality assessment, cultural variations in reporting style should also be taken into consideration (Laher,
  • 54. 2007). Reporting style refers to how an individual reports his or her symptoms and how much an individual reports ( e.g., over- or under-reporting) (Heiervang, Goodman, & Goodman, 2008), and response style refers to the way an individual responds to survey questions regardless of the content of the questions (Van Vaerenbergh & Thomas, 2013). Culture can impact the validity of test results in many ways due to cultural differ- ences in reporting and response style. Those from Western cultures ( e.g., the US) are more likely to engage in self-enhancement behaviors than individuals from Eastern cultures ( e.g., Japan), which can impact reporting style (Norasakkunkit & Kalick, 2002). Questionnaires may show those from Eastern cultures as having more severe symptoms, based on their lack of comparable engagement in self-enhancement behaviors, than their Western peers. This difference in assessment scores would be indicative of a difference in reporting style rather than a true difference in severity of symptoms (Norasakkunkit & Kalick, 2002; Leong et al., 2010). Hamamura, Heine, and Paulhus (2008) also noted that Asian- Americans have a central tendency bias to report more moderate symptoms, regardless of the severity of the actual symptoms, than their Caucasian peers. Asian- Americans may therefore under-report the severity of their symptoms based on differences in cultural values
  • 55. alone. Finally, forced-choice responses - those that require either a "true" or "false" response - potentially limit the cultural validity of measurement. Having to choose 140 Joyce P Chu et al. between a "true" or "false" response may misrepresent responses from some cultures by not allowing for contextual,. situational, and cultural factors to be addressed in the response (Laher, 2007). Laher (2007) suggested that in order to obtain the most accurate and culturally competent assessment of personality, a battery of tests combined with client interview would be most prudent for examination of all of the responses within the client's cultural context. Overall, cultural variations in idioms of distress and reporting and response styles can result in an over- or under-estimation of true scores on an assessment instrument. It will be important for assessment practitioners to assess and note such cultural factors, and adjust score interpretation accordingly. Utilization of multiple modes of assessment can help to detect cultural differences due to reporting or response style, and all assessment results should be understood within the cultural context of the clients' lives. Cultural and Technical Context of the Testing Process
  • 56. In creating a culturally competent personality assessment administration, literature suggests that one must consider the cultural and technical context of the testing pro- cess. It is a common assumption that assessment measures yield consistent responses from the same respondent regardless of the type of room, examiner, or method of administration. However, one important factor in considering equivalence of assess- ments is the cultural and technical context in which the assessment is given (Flaugher, 1978), as people from different cultural backgrounds react differentially to several aspects of the testing environment. First, the language ( e.g., wording choice of the instrumen t, as well as test administration in an individual's preferred language) in which assessments are given can affect the validity of one's test responses. Linguistic equivalence, also known as transla- tional equivalence, is focused on the accuracy of translation oftest items. Many poten- tial problems arise as the result of translating personality assessments from one language to another, such as differences in the meaning ofwords, differences in connotation, and masculine bias of wording. Furthermore, regional differences in word usage make it difficult to create linguistically equivalent personality assessments. Brislin ( 199 3) rec- ommended a translation followed by back-translation procedure (i.e., translating back to the original language and examining for parity with the original version) to help
  • 57. ameliorate non-equivalence issues. Geisinger (1994) recommended taking this process one step further, through the use of an editorial board who would back-translate the document as a group in order to discuss any possible differences in opinion or concerns about the translation. When working with more than one linguistically equivalent ver- sion of an assessment tool, one must be aware that issues of metric equivalence can still exist. Lopez and Romero (1988), for example, found that although there are many similarities between the English and Spanish versions ofthe Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), there are important psychometric differences that preclude direct comparison between English WAIS and Spanish WAIS scores. Second, characteristics or behaviors of the examiner or environment can elicit different responses that reflect cultural influence rather than the true construct in 141 Diversity and Assessment question. Stereotype threat, for instance, occurs when the tester or testing environment activates a negative stereotype about one's cultural identity group; research shows that stereotype threat can negatively affect one's test performance ( e.g., Steele & Aronson, 1995). Stereotypes can be activated by minute aspects of the testing envi- ronment or materials (Thames et al., 2013). For example,
  • 58. discomfort by the exam- inee with the tester (i.e., because of previous negative experiences with others of a similar gender or race, or because of the tester's interpersonal mannerisms) may discourage versus facilitate open, introspective, and/or honest responses on assess- ment instruments (Thames et al., 2013). Third, the method of test administration - termed technical equivalence - can also affect one's assessment responses. Ryder et al. (2008), for example, found that the same respondents reported different severity levels of symptomatology depending on method of test administration via spontaneous report, self-report questionnaire, versus in-person clinical interview. Specifically, Chinese participants reported more somatic symptoms than their Euro-Canadian counterparts when queried directly via clinical interview or spontaneous report. These group differences in somatization were not apparent when assessed via self-report questionnaire. Morrison and Downey (2000) found similarly that ethnic minority clients at a counseling center were less likely than non-minority peers to disclose suicidal ideation on self-report; a greater level of ide- ation became evident only after direct suicide risk assessment by a provider. The mediating reasons why different methods of test administration or testing environment yield different responses in ethnic minority individuals is still largely
  • 59. unknown, though a cadre of cultural factors may account for such effects. Social desirability, cultural mistrust of healthcare professionals, cultural practices of expres- sivity versus inhibition, stigma about psychology, and so on, may represent cultural variables that could affect testing response and are deserving of further study. Clearly, research is needed to further quantify the effects of the cultural and technical con- text of the testing environment on personality assessment responses, and to identify the cultural factors that mediate differential testing responses. When conducting assessment, it is essential to remember that many factors beyond the assessment items themselves, such as the cultural and technical context of the testing process, may affect the equivalence and validity of results (Dana, 2000). Although some factors may be out of the control of the test administrator, efforts should be directed towards minimizing extraneous factors, adapting the test envi- ronment to maximize open responding depending on the client's cultural background, and contextualizing one's interpretation oftest responses within salient cultural influences in the testing environment. Culturally Informed Responses to Feedback Providing feedback about test results to clients is an integral part of the assessment process, with the field of psychology moving towards a more comprehensive model
  • 60. of feedback provision (Finn & Tonsager, 1992 ). In fact, research indicates that providing assessment feedback to clients in a professional and compassionate 142 Joyce P. Chu et al. manner can, in and of itself, serve as a clinical intervention. A series of experi- mental studies found a number of positive consequences of providing empathetic feedback to college students after completing the MMPI-2 (Finn & Tonsager, 1992; Newman & Greenway, 1997). Compared with control participants who only received examiner attention, participants in the feedback condition demonstrated a significant decrease in symptomatology that persisted at follow- up two weeks later. Additionally, participants in the experimental condition evidenced greater levels of hopefulness and self-esteem compared with the control group immediately and two weeks following feedback. Although few dispute the importance of giving assessment feedback to clients, there is a dearth of research investigating the effects of different approaches to client feedback, and even fewer investigations offeedback as influenced by issues of cultural diversity. There is a need for psychologists to understand potential issues that culture can present when providing feedback to clients.
  • 61. For instance, some of the individual difference characteristics responsible for the effectiveness of assessment feedback may be culturally variant. In Finn and Tonsager's (1992) study, authors found that participants who were more self-reflective at intake were generally more likely to show positive reactions to MMPI-2 feedback. Self-reflection varies depending on cultural identity such as age ( e.g., Haga, Kraft, & Corby, 2009), highlighting that one must account for cultural variation in self- reflection or other emotion regulation tendencies when tailoring one's approach towards assessment feedback. Client response to the mode of assessment feedback may also be culturally deter- mined. Feedback may be delivered by the provider via several modes: in person versus in a written report. Some providers may even decide it is prudent not to provide feedback at all. Most models of feedback provision have been based on person-to- person provision of feedback, emphasizing a humanistic component that engages the client and allows them to feel more involved (Finn & Tonsager, 1992). Yet, just as the technical context or mode of assessment administration can elicit different responses from different cultural individuals, mode of feedback can also elicit difference responses. For example, providing a written report of assessment results to a cultural minority client with a limited familiarity with or
  • 62. understanding of psychological and medical jargon may serve to alienate the client or decrease effec- tiveness of the assessment feedback. A last example of cultural influence on assessment feedback involves expectations of involvement by the client, the assessor, and the client's therapist in the feedback process. According to Kreilkamp (1989) active participation by the client is a basic and important component of providing assessment feedback that motivates clients to take ownership of their results. The recommendation may be less applicable for clients from collectivistic cultures who may prefer not to collaborate as an active participant or advocate in the process of feedback provision. Cultures that value respect for authority figures may view client participation in the feedback provision as disrespectful or unwanted. Providing feedback to clients about their personality assessment results can be a challenging, yet rewarding experience. The challenges become increasingly more 143 Diversity and Assessment complicated with the need for incorporation of diversity and cultural considerations. Clearly, flexibility and careful consideration of cultural aspects will be an integral part
  • 63. of competently providing feedback to clients. Additional research is needed to inves- tigate recommended models of feedback provision with diverse populations. Conclusion The literature to date concerning multicultural personality assessment is rich with recommendations for practice and further research. As the field of personality assessment moves forward, it is essential that findings from the literature be incorporated into practice. Without awareness and knowledge of cultural differences, personality assessment can be misleading or inaccurate. Competence in personality assessment, as with psychological assessment in general, requires administrators to be knowledgeable about the appropriateness and limits of the test they are using, including cultural variables that can impact the testing process and test performance. The current literature analysis indicated that development, administration, inter- pretation, and client feedback of results from assessment measures should incorporate cultural influences in five main domains: differential norms, incorporation of cultur- ally valid representations of the construct, cultural idioms of distress and reporting and response style, the cultural and technical context of the testing process, and culturally informed responses to assessment feedback. By accounting for these
  • 64. cultural domains, personality assessment will have a better chance at creating a testing process with utility in a variety of contexts and diverse populations. The future of personality assessment is ripe with potential. With accumulating research on personality across cultures, assessment is becoming more advanced. Further research investigating personality differences between cultural groups, within cultural groups, and across cultures is needed to enhance the validity of existing per- sonality constructs and theories. Studies are needed to further identify cultural variables that may interfere with the substantive validity of true assessment results, to examine the ideal testing environment and assessment feedback conditions depending on cultural background, and to provide guidelines for incorporating idioms of distress and reporting and response style into culturally competent interpretation of test results. Research is also needed to investigate solutions to appropriately assess personality constructs in diverse groups without creating individualized assessments for the infinite numbers ofcultural identity subgroups. The field ofdiversity and personality assessment is in a nascent stage with considerable potential for advancement. The five domains in this paper provide a basis to conceptualize the effects of culture on assessment, and provide guidelines for practice and future research in diversity and assessment.
  • 65. References Brislin, R. W. (1993). Understanding Culture"s Influence on Behavior. New York: Harcourt Brace. Butcher, J.N. (1996). International Adaptations ofthe MMPI-2: Research and Clinical Appli- cations. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. 144 Joyce P. Chu et al. Butcher, J.N. (2004). Personality assessment without borders: Adaptation of the MMPI-2 across cultures. Journal ofPersonality Arsessment, 83 (2 ), 90- 104. Butcher, J.N. (2005). MMPI-2: A Practitioner's Guide. Washington, DC: American Psychi- atric Association. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014a). Racial & ethnic minority populations. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/remp.html Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (20146 ). Hispanic or Latino Populations. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/REMP/hispanic. html Cheung, F.M., Cheung, S.F., Leung, K., Ward, C., & Leong, F. (2003). The English version of the Chinese Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of
  • 66. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34 (4), 433-452. Church, A. (2001 ). Personality measurement in cross-cultural perspective. Journal ofPerson- ality, 69 (6), 979-1006. Constantino, G., & Malgady, R.G. (2000). Multicultural and cross-cultural utility of the TEMAS (Tell-Me-A-Story) test. In R.H. Dana (Ed.), Handbook of Cross-cultural and Multicultural Personality Arsessment (pp. 481-513). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Dana, R.H. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of Cross-cultural and Multicultural Personality Arsessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Dinh, T.Q., Yamada, A.M., & Yee, B.W.K. (2009). A culturally relevant conceptualization of depression: an empirical examination of the factorial structure of the Vietnamese Depression Scale. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 55 ( 6 ), 496-505. Finn, S.E., & Tonsager, M.E. (1992). Therapeutic effects ofproviding MMPI-2 test feedback to college students awaiting therapy. Psychological Assessment, 4 ( 3 ), 2 78-28 7. Flaugher, R.L. ( 1978 ). The many definitions oftest bias. American Psychowgist, 33 (7), 671-679. Geisinger, K.F. (1994). Cross-cultural normative assessment: Translation and adaptation
  • 67. issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. Psychological Arsessment, 6(4), 304-312. Grimm, S.D., & Church, A.T. (1999). A cross-cultural study ofresponse biases in personality measures. Journal ofResearch in Personality, 33 (4), 415-441. Haga, S.M., Kraft, P., & Corby, E.K. (2009). Emotion regulation: Antecedents and well-being outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in cross-cultural samples. Journal ofHappiness Studies, 10 (3), 271-291. Hall, G.C.N., Bansal, A., & Lopez, I.R. (1999). Ethnicity and psychopathology: A meta- analytic review of 31 years of comparative MMPI/MMPI-2 Research. Journal of Psychological.Arsessment, 11 (2), 186-197. Hamamura, T., Heine, S.J., & Paulhus, D.L. (2008). Cultural differences in response styles: The role of dialectical thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 44 ( 4 ), 932-942. Heiervang, E., Goodman, A., & Goodman, R. (2008). The Nordic advantage in child mental health: separating health differences from reporting style in a cross-cultural comparison of psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49 (6), 678-685. Hill, J.S., Pace, T.M., & Robbins, R.R. (2010). Decolonizing personality assessment and honoring indigenous voices: A critical examination of the
  • 68. MMPI-2. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16 (1), 16-25. Kreilkamp, T. (1989). Time-limited, Intermittent Therapy with Children and Families. New York: Brunner/Maze!. Laher, S. (2007). Personality re-conceptualized: Amillonian approach. South African Journal of Psychology, 37 (1 ), 82-95. http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/REMP http://www.cdc.gov/minorityhealth/populations/remp.html 145 Diversity and Assessment Leong, F.L., Leung, K, & Cheung, F.M. (2010). Integrating cross-cultural psychology research methods into ethnic minority psychology. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psy- chology, 16 (4), 590-597. Lopez, S., & Romero, A. (1988). Assessing the intellectual functioning of Spanish-speaking adults: Comparison of the EIWA and the WAIS. Journal of Processional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19 (3), 263-270. Mak, W.W., & Zane, N.W. (2004). The phenomenon of somatization among community Chinese Americans. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39 ( 12), 967-974. McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T. (1987). Validation of the five- factor model of personality across
  • 69. instruments and observers. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 52 ( 1), 81-90. Morrison, L.L., & Downey, D.L. (2000). Racial differences in self-disclosure of suicidal ideation and reasons for living: implications for training. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6 (4), 374. Newman, M.L., & Greenway, P. (1997). Therapeutic effects of providing MMPI-2 test feedback to clients at a university counseling service: A collaborative approach. Psychological Assessment, 9 (2), 122-131. Nichter, M. ( 1981 ). Idioms of distress: Alternatives in the expression of psychosocial distress: A case study from South India. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 5 (4), 379-408. Norasakkunkit, V., & Kalick, S.M. (2002). Culture, ethnicity, and emotional distress measures: The role of self-construal and self-enhancement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (1 ), 56-70. Pritchard, D.A., & Rosenblatt, A. ( 1980). Racial bias in the MMPI: A methodological review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48 (2), 263-267. Ryder, A.G., Yang, J., Zhu, X., Yao, S., Yi, J., Heine, S.J., & Bagby, M. (2008). The cultural shaping of depression: Somatic symptoms in China, psychological symptoms in North America. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 117 (2), 300-313.
  • 70. Steele, C.M., & Aronson, J. (1995 ). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 69 (5), 797-811. Thames, A.D., Hinkin, C.H., Byrd, D.A., Bilder, R.M., Duff, KJ., Mindt, M.R., & ... Streiff, V. (2013). Effects of stereotype threat, perceived discrimination, and examiner race on neuropsychological performance: Simple as Black and White? Journal ofthe International Neuropsychological Society, 19 (5), 583-593. Van Vaerenbergh, Y., & Thomas, T.D. (2013). Response styles in survey research: A literature review of antecedents, consequences, and remedies. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 25 (2), 195-217. Vijver, F., & Phalet, K (2004 ). Assessment in multicultural groups: The role of acculturation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53 (2), 215-236. Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1993). Where's the 'culture' in cross- cultural transition? Compara- tive studies of sojourner adjustment. Journal ofCross-Cultural Psychology, 24 (2 ), 221-249. M i n d s e t Wo r k s ® E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t
  • 71. Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights reserved. M in d s e t W o rk s ® E d u c a to rK it MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE TOOL
  • 72. For the Teacher: Using the Mindset Assessment Profile This is a tool to get a quick assessment of your students’ mindsets—their beliefs about the malleability of intelligence, the relative importance of learning and perfect performance, and their attitudes toward effort and mistakes. It’s important that students not feel labeled by this tool. The MAP categories just represent the way they are thinking and feeling about these questions at the present time. They can change these beliefs, and they may feel differently on different days. You can use this assessment tool in a number of ways. For example, you can use it as an: 1) Individual assessment, scored by the teacher (with the result not shared with the student) 2) Individual assessment, scored by the teacher (with the result shared with the student) 3) Individual assessment, scored by the student 4) Individual assessment, scored by a peer Once students have completed the assessment, you can follow up with discussions
  • 73. or activities to explore the issues raised. For example, you can: discuss 1:1 to select the statement where they had a Profile number of 1-3 (the “fixed mindset” range) and write or talk about it. the MAP description fits them. their profiles and discuss their beliefs. category to the class. Here are some questions that you might explore in any of the above formats: l confident that you can learn and do well? that you can’t do any better?
  • 74. really hard? How did you learn it? you be willing to work hard to achieve if you knew it was possible? effort, what goals would you set for yourself? M i n d s e t Wo r k s ® E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights reserved. M in d s e t
  • 75. W o rk s ® E d u c a to rK it Mindset Assessment Profile Tool Description: Survey for getting a quick assessment of your students’ mindsets Objective: Students will complete survey and (optional) reflection Timeline: 10-30 minutes Instructions:
  • 76. survey is a tool to gather information—in this case, your opinions about intelligence, performance, learning, effort, and challenges. It may look like a test or quiz, but in fact it is not! Answer honestly and say what you believe. There will be no grade attached to the survey and the “score” you receive is not a percentage correct. Afterwards, we will discuss the questions and the different ways that people think about them. anonymously if desired. -score, or not. writing or discussion. After th e surve y Make sure that you emphasize that the survey is a gauge (like taking a temperature with a
  • 77. thermometer) of their thinking right now. As we learn new things, our thinking changes. The survey is not intended to be a way to label students, but rather to get to the core of their thinking so that new learning can occur. Option al Ref lecti on/D iscu ssio n Debrief with your class after they complete the survey. Ask: asked? not? M i n d s e t Wo r k s ® E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights
  • 78. reserved. M in d s e t W o rk s ® E d u c a to rK it MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE Name: _____________________________________ This is NOT a test! It is an opinion survey about beliefs and
  • 79. goals regarding ability and performance. It is very important that you give your honest opinion, not what you believe someone else would think best. Read each statement, decide how much you agree or disagree with the statement, and circle your answer. Do you Agree or Disagree? Disagree A Lot Disagree Disagree A Little Agree A Little Agree Agree A Lot Profile Number 1. No matter how much intelligence you have, you can always change it a good deal. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6
  • 80. 2. You can learn new things, but you cannot really change your basic level of intelligence. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 3. I like my work best when it makes me think hard. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 4. I like my work best when I can do it really well without too much trouble. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 5. I like work that I'll learn from even if I make a lot of mistakes. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 6. I like my work best when I can do it perfectly without any mistakes. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6
  • 81. 7. When something is hard, it just makes me want to work more on it, not less. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 8. To tell the truth, when I work hard, it makes me feel as though I'm not very smart. 1 2 2 3 4 5 6 MINDSET ASSESSMENT PROFILE NUMBER M i n d s e t Wo r k s ® E d u c a t o r K i t - M o du l e 1 T oo l k i t Copyright © 2002-2012 Mindset Works, Inc. All rights reserved. M in d s e
  • 82. t W o rk s ® E d u c a to rK it Creating Your Mindset Assessment Profile 1. First, determine your Profile Number for each question. 7), write the number of your answer into the boxes in the right column. below to fill in the gray boxes in the right column. If you chose this answer: Then write this number in the gray box on the right (Profile Number).
  • 83. Disagree A Lot (1) 6 Disagree (2) 5 Disagree A Little (3) 4 Agree A Little (4) 3 Agree (5) 2 Agree A Lot (6) 1 2. Now, add up all your Profile numbers. he right, and write the total in the last box in the bottom right corner. 3. What does your Mindset Profile Number mean? and circle it. If your profile number falls into this range: Then your MAP (Mindset Assessment Profile) group is: People in this MAP group usually believe the following things:
  • 84. 8-12 F5 You strongly believe that your intelligence is fixed—it doesn’t change much. If you can’t perform perfectly you would rather not do something. You think smart people don’t have to work hard. 13-16 F4 17-20 F3 You lean toward thinking that your intelligence doesn’t change much. You prefer not to make mistakes if you can help it and you also don’t really like to put in a lot of work. You may think that learning should be easy. 21-24 F2 25-28 F1 You are unsure about whether you can change your intelligence. You care about your performance and you also want to learn, but you don’t really want to have to work too hard for it. 29-32 G1 33-36 G2 You believe that your intelligence is something that you can increase. You care about learning and you’re willing
  • 85. to work hard. You do want to do well, but you think it’s more important to learn than to always perform well. 37-40 G3 41-44 G4 You really feel sure that you can increase your intelligence by learning and you like a challenge. You believe that the best way to learn is to work hard, and you don’t mind making mistakes while you do it. 45-48 G5 4. Do you think the description under your MAP group matches the way you think and feel about your school work? Which parts are true for you and which are not? Howard Zinn – A people’s history of the United States Chapter 1: COLUMBUS, THE INDIANS, AND HUMAN PROGRESS Arawak men and women, naked, tawny, and full of wonder, emerged from their villages onto the island's beaches and swam out to get a closer look at the strange big boat. When Columbus
  • 86. and his sailors came ashore, carrying swords, speaking oddly, the Arawaks ran to greet them, brought them food, water, gifts. He later wrote of this in his log: They ... brought us parrots and balls of cotton and spears and many other things, which they exchanged for the glass beads and hawks' bells. They willingly traded everything they owned... . They were well-built, with good bodies and handsome features.... They do not bear arms, and do not know them, for I showed them a sword, they took it by the edge and cut themselves out of ignorance. They have no iron. Their spears are made of cane... . They would make fine servants.... With fifty men we could subjugate them all and make them do whatever we want. These Arawaks of the Bahama Islands were much like Indians on the mainland, who were remarkable (European observers were to say again and again) for their hospitality, their belief in sharing. These traits did not stand out in the Europe of the
  • 87. Renaissance, dominated as it was by the religion of popes, the government of kings, the frenzy for money that marked Western civilization and its first messenger to the Americas, Christopher Columbus. Columbus wrote: As soon as I arrived in the Indies, on the first Isl and which I found, I took some of the natives by force in order that they might learn and might give me information of whatever there is in these parts. The information that Columbus wanted most was: Where is the gold? He had persuaded the king and queen of Spain to finance an expedition to the lands, the wealth, he expected would be on the other side of the Atlantic-the Indies and Asia, gold and spices. For, like other informed people of his time, he knew the world was round and he could sail west in order to get to the Far East. Spain was recently unified, one of the new modern nation- states, like France, England, and Portugal. Its population, mostly poor peasants, worked for the nobility, who were 2 percent of
  • 88. the population and owned 95 percent of the land. Spain had tied itself to the Catholic Church, expelled all the Jews, driven out the Moors. Like other states of the modern world, Spain sought gold, which was becoming the new mark of wealth, more useful than land because it could buy anything. There was gold in Asia, it was thought, and certainly silks and spices, for Marco Polo and others had brought back marvelous things from their overland expeditions centuries before. Now that the Turks had conquered Constantinople and the eastern Mediterranean, and controlled the land routes to Asia, a sea route was needed. Portuguese sailors were working their way around the southern tip of Africa. Spain decided to gamble on a long sail across an unknown ocean. In return for bringing back gold and spices, they promised Columbus 10 percent of the profits, governorship over new-found lands, and the fame that would go with a new title: Admiral of the Ocean Sea. He was a merchant's clerk from the Italian city of
  • 89. Genoa, part-time weaver (the son of a skilled weaver), and expert sailor. He set out with three sailing ships, the largest of which was the Santa Maria, perhaps 100 feet long, and thirty-nine crew members. Columbus would never have made it to Asia, which was thousands of miles farther away than he had calculated, imagining a smaller world. He would have been doomed by that great expanse of sea. But he was lucky. One-fourth of the way there he came upon an unknown, uncharted land that lay between Europe and Asia-the Americas. It was early October 1492, and thirty-three days since he and his crew had left the Canary Islands, off the Atlantic coast of Africa. Now they saw branches and sticks floating in the water. They saw flocks of birds. These were signs of land. Then, on October 12, a sailor called Rodrigo saw the early morning moon shining on white sands, and cried out. It was an island in the Bahamas, the Caribbean sea. The first man to sight land was supposed to get a yearly pension of 10,000 maravedis for life, but Rodrigo never got it. Columbus claimed he had seen a light the
  • 90. evening before. He got the reward. So, approaching land, they were met by the Arawak Indians, who swam out to greet them. The Arawaks lived in village communes, had a developed agriculture of corn, yams, cassava. They could spin and weave, but they had no horses or work animals. They had no iron, but they wore tiny gold ornaments in their ears. This was to have enormous consequences: it led Columbus to take some of them aboard ship as prisoners because he insisted that they guide him to the source of the gold. He then sailed to what is now Cuba, then to Hispaniola (the island which today consists of Haiti and the Dominican Republic). There, bits of visible gold in the rivers, and a gold mask presented to Columbus by a local Indian chief, led to wild visions of gold fields. On Hispaniola, out of timbers from the Santa Maria, which had run aground, Columbus built a fort, the first European military base in the Western Hemisphere. He called it Navidad
  • 91. (Christmas) and left thirty-nine crewmembers there, with instructions to find and store the gold. He took more Indian prisoners and put them aboard his two remaining ships. At one part of the island he got into a fight with Indians who refused to trade as many bows and arrows as he and his men wanted. Two were run through with swords and bled to death. Then the Nina and the Pinta set sail for the Azores and Spain. When the weather turned cold, the Indian prisoners began to die. Columbus's report to the Court in Madrid was extravagant. He insisted he had reached Asia (it was Cuba) and an island off the coast of China (Hispaniola). His descriptions were part fact, part fiction: Hispaniola is a miracle. Mountains and hills, plains and pastures, are both fertile and beautiful ... the harbors are unbelievably good and there are many wide rivers of which the majority contain gold. . . . There are many spices, and great mines of gold and other metals....
  • 92. The Indians, Columbus reported, "are so naive and so free with their possessions that no one who has not witnessed them would believe it. When you ask for something they have, they never say no. To the contrary, they offer to share with anyone...." He concluded his report by asking for a little help from their Majesties, and in return he would bring them from his next voyage "as much gold as they need ... and as many slaves as they ask." He was full of religious talk: "Thus the eternal God, our Lord, gives victory to those who follow His way over apparent impossibilities." Because of Columbus's exaggerated report and promises, his second expedition was given seventeen ships and more than twelve hundred men. The aim was clear: slaves and gold. They went from island to island in the Caribbean, taking Indians as captives. But as word spread of the Europeans' intent they found more and more empty villages. On Haiti, they found that the sailors left behind at Fort Navidad had been killed in a battle with the
  • 93. Indians, after they had roamed the island in gangs looking for gold, taking women and children as slaves for sex and labor. Now, from his base on Haiti, Columbus sent expedition after expedition into the interior. They found no gold fields, but had to fill up the ships returning to Spain with some kind of dividend. In the year 1495, they went on a great slave raid, rounded up fifteen hundred Arawak men, women, and children, put them in pens guarded by Spaniards and dogs, then picked the five hundred best specimens to load onto ships. Of those five hundred, two hundred died en route. The rest arrived alive in Spain and were put up for sale by the archdeacon of the town, who reported that, although the slaves were "naked as the day they were born," they showed "no more embarrassment than animals." Columbus later wrote: "Let us in the name of the Holy Trinity go on sending all the slaves that can be sold." But too many of the slaves died in captivity. And so Columbus, desperate to pay back dividends to those who had invested, had to make good his promise to fill the ships with gold. In
  • 94. the province of Cicao on Haiti, where he and his men imagined huge gold fields to exist, they ordered all persons fourteen years or older to collect a certain quantity of gold every three months. When they brought it, they were given copper tokens to hang around their necks. Indians found without a copper token had their hands cut off and bled to death. The Indians had been given an impossible task. The only gold around was bits of dust garnered from the streams. So they fled, were hunted down with dogs, and were killed. Trying to put together an army of resistance, the Arawaks faced Spaniards who had armor, muskets, swords, horses. When the Spaniards took prisoners they hanged them or burned them to death. Among the Arawaks, mass suicides began, with cassava poison. Infants were killed to save them from the Spaniards. In two years, through murder, mutilation, or suicide, half of the 250,000 Indians on Haiti were dead. When it became clear that there was no gold left, the Indians were taken as slave labor on
  • 95. huge estates, known later as encomiendas. They were worked at a ferocious pace, and died by the thousands. By the year 1515, there were perhaps fifty thousand Indians left. By 1550, there were five hundred. A report of the year 1650 shows none of the original Arawaks or their descendants left on the island. The chief source-and, on many matters the only source-of information about what happened on the islands after Columbus came is Bartolome de las Casas, who, as a young priest, participated in the conquest of Cuba. For a time he owned a plantation on which Indian slaves worked, but he gave that up and became a vehement critic of Spanish cruelty. Las Casas transcribed Columbus's journal and, in his fifties, began a multivolume History of the Indies. In it, he describes the Indians. They are agile, he says, and can swim long distances, especially the women. They are not completely peaceful, because they do battle from time to time with other tribes, but their casualties seem small, and they fight when they are individually moved to do so
  • 96. because of some grievance, not on the orders of captains or kings. Women in Indian society were treated so well as to startle the Spaniards. Las Casas describes sex relations: Marriage laws are non-existent men and women alike choose their mates and leave them as they please, without offense, jealousy or anger. They multiply in great abundance; pregnant women work to the last minute and give birth almost painlessly; up the next day, they bathe in the river and are as clean and healthy as before giving birth. If they tire of their men, they give themselves abortions with herbs that force stillbirths, covering their shameful parts with leaves or cotton cloth; although on the whole, Indian men and women look upon total nakedness with as much casualness as we look upon a man's head or at his hands. The Indians, Las Casas says, have no religion, at least no temples. They live in