SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 21
Download to read offline
1
PRACTICAL PROJECT 2012/2013 SCHOOL OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES
Can Bumblebees Distinguish Between Different Types of Iridescence?
Cordelia Batt, 1007206, cb0200@my.bristol.ac.uk
For submission to: Animal Behaviour
Supervisor: Dr Heather Whitney
Word Count: 4,691
2
Iridescence, the change in hue corresponding to the viewing angle, is utilized1
by a large number of living organisms. Recently this optical phenomenon has2
been identified in the petals of many plant species, most likely as a signal to3
their specific pollinators. Bumblebees are able to distinguish iridescence from4
basic pigment colour, however there is more than one different type of5
iridescence; our experiment set out to discover if the bumblebee species,6
Bombus terrestris, can successfully discriminate between them. Our method7
involved training the bees’ to associate a certain type of iridescence with a8
reward, before adding different types of iridescence into the test environment.9
If the trained bee showed a preference towards the known, rewarding10
iridescence, it must, to a certain degree, be able to distinguish between them.11
We found that in most cases, this was true and bees were more likely to visit12
the known, rewarding iridescence. This infers that bees could be using not13
only pigment colour, but also structural colour to select rewarding plant14
species in their environment. Iridescence may therefore play an important role15
in improving foraging efficiency in bees.16
KEY WORDS17
Pollinator; pigment colour; structural colour; iridescence; multilayer;18
diffraction grating; colour constancy.19
20
The mutual relationship between plants and their pollinators is constantly evolving so21
that plants are more conspicuous to their pollinators. Similarly pollinators have22
become better equipped to receive visual signals from plants, which due to their lack23
of mobility often rely on insects for sexual reproduction. For this reason, plants24
display bright flowers, the colours produced by reflecting certain specific wavelengths25
of light which are perceptible to their pollinators. Colours can be produced by26
3
chemical pigments, or specific surface structures. Pigment colours are produced by27
chemicals which absorb most of the visible light spectrum; the wavelengths not28
absorbed determine the perceived colour of the object. Structural colours are those29
produced by means other than pigments, where certain light wavelengths of light are30
selectively reflected from a surface. The resulting colours are often more intense than31
those produced by pigment colours (Glover & Whitney, 2010).32
Structural colour has been recognised in nature for a long time, most notably in33
birds, insects and fish, however many reptiles and amphibians are also iridescent.34
The purpose of iridescence in many of these organisms includes species and sex35
recognition, as well mate choice and predator deterrence (Doucet & Meadows,36
2009). Recent studies have shown that many plant species also exhibit iridescence.37
Much of the iridescence occurs in the ultraviolet (UV) wavelengths, and unlike us,38
these signals are recognized by bumblebees, whose’ visual systems have high acuity39
in the blue and near-UV ranges (Whitney et al., 2009). Iridescence, like many other40
visual cues such as pigment colour (Gumbert, 2000), fluorescence (Gandía-Herrero,41
García-Carmona & Josefa Escribano, 2005) and patterns (Whitney, Kolle, Alvarez-42
Fernandez, Steiner & Glover, 2009), is thought to be used by plants as a means to43
attract pollinators. Bumblebees can not only detect iridescence independently of44
pigment colour (Whitney et al., 2009), but they are also more attracted to flowers that45
display it (Glover & Martin, 2002). This suggests an evolutionary advantage to46
structural colour in plants with regard to insect pollination. Any increase in the47
efficiency of pollination is not only relevant to ecosystems, but also to global food48
production, which relies heavily on insect pollination.49
There is no single means of producing iridescence. Diffraction gratings, multilayers50
and photic crystals are all mechanisms that produce structural colour (Glover &51
Whitney, 2010), allowing a variation in the optical properties of plants. Bumblebees52
are very specific in the flowers that they visit, each species favouring certain plant53
4
types (Goulson, 2013). It could therefore benefit bumblebees to be able to use these54
differences in iridescence to categorise particularly favourable plant species. We55
know that bumblebees can distinguish iridescence from simple pigment colour, but56
can they recognise the slight differences in structural colour produced by flowers of57
different species? More simply, can bumblebees discriminate between different58
iridescences? We tested to see if naïve bumblebees, Bombus terrestris, were able to59
discriminate between certain types of iridescence by using different mechanisms60
including diffraction gratings and multilayers to produce structural colour. If the bees61
could learn to categorise the different structural colours depending on whether or not62
they offered a reward, then we can conclude they are able to distinguish between the63
different kinds of iridescence.64
65
METHODS66
General Setup67
Bombus terrestris colonies were set up in a cardboard nest box which was attached68
to a glass flight arena 110cm in length, 70cm wide and 100cm high. The bees could69
enter the flight arena via a transparent pipe in which plastic partitions could be70
added, allowing the successful selection of a single bee. The lighting in the laboratory71
comprised six Sylvania Activa 172 Professional 36W fluorescent tubes.72
The bees were naïve with respect to foraging from natural flowers, and hadn’t been73
used for any previous experiments. They received half a pcr plate filled with a 30%74
sucrose solution daily. Over the weekend a gravity feeder was provided, filled with a75
solution mimicking that of the bees’ natural diet (Provided by Koppert Biological76
Systems, who also provided the bees.) Pollen was added to the colony on a weekly77
basis.78
5
How the Targets were Produced79
Targets were provided by Dr Heather Whitney, they were produced by adding80
different surfaces to artificial flowers constructed from Sterilin tubes 26mm diameter81
and 8cm high, with an inverted white lid 70mm diameter on top. The surfaces were82
circular plastic discs, with a diameter of 40mm. The inverted lid of a 0.5 ml Eppendorf83
tube placed in the middle of the target to contain the sucrose reward.84
There were 5 different target types categorised by the surface type on the lid. Two85
were non-iridescent; gloss a smooth moulded plastic and matt rough moulded plastic.86
Three of the targets were iridescent. Two of these were created using diffraction87
gratings; one being artificially produced using ordered rows of grooves on the88
surface; the other was other created using a mould of the adaxial petal surface89
whereby the grooves where much more variable. The third kind of iridescence was90
multilayer iridescence; thin films of optical material arranged in ordered layers. The91
targets were all maintained at room temperature. For ease I will refer to gloss surface92
as gloss; matt surface as matt; diffraction grating iridescence (artificial) as artificial;93
diffraction grating iridescence (natural) as floral and multilayer iridescence as94
multilayer.95
Marking the Bees96
The bees were distinguished using paint marks on their thorax. These were applied97
using a wooden skewer when the bees’ concentration was diverted by sucrose98
solution. We used between 1 and 4 dots of varying sizes and shapes (circles and99
lines for example) using the 5 different colours available to us. This enabled us to100
differentiate between large numbers of bees.101
102
103
6
Control Tests104
Before the test the bees were hungry having not been fed since the previous day.105
The test arena was cleared of all artificial flowers and any foraging bees, which were106
either encouraged back into the hive or captured under Sterilin tubes.107
Targets were placed randomly within the test arena. There were 3 of each kind of108
target, so 15 in total. They were free of any reward. Before starting the control we109
defined a ‘land’ to constitute of all 6 legs placed on the target. We used the same110
observer for all the tests to eliminate any potential differences in classification of a111
‘land.’112
One bee was released at a time. We allowed 20 lands, recording which targets113
were landed on. Once 20 lands were completed sucrose solution was added to the114
target the bee was situated on, and the bee was allowed to drink and return to the115
hive.116
The targets were removed, washed and cleansed with ethanol so that any remaining117
traces of sucrose were removed. Equally any signals that previous bees could be118
leaving would also be removed. The targets were then replaced and repositioned,119
and the experimental procedure repeated for a time with a different bee. A total of 20120
bees were tested.121
Training to Iridescence122
Firstly, we trained bees to multilayer iridescence. The test arena was cleared of any123
bees and 3 targets with multilayer iridescence were randomly placed inside. Each124
target had an approximately 1ml reward of 30% sucrose solution, which was refilled125
up once emptied. We tested one bee at a time allowing it to drink 20 times from any126
of the 3 targets before commencing with the experiment.127
7
All the multilayer targets were then removed, washed and cleansed with ethanol.128
They were returned to the test arena along with 3 of each of the other types of target,129
making a total of 15 targets. None of the targets had a reward. The trained bee was130
released, and we recorded which targets it landed on. A total of 20 bees were trained131
to multilayer and tested. This procedure was repeated to train the bees to floral.132
Different bees were used for each trial.133
We moved onto a second hive to train the bees to artificial. We completed a control134
test for this hive before obtaining results and used the same methods as previously135
described.136
137
Statistical Analysis138
We used a Chi squared X squared test to analyse the control, to see if the bee visited139
a certain kind of target more frequently than expected by chance. If yes, it would140
indicate that the bees have an innate preference to a certain target.141
We also used the Chi squared test when analysing our results with trained bees.142
This allowed us to see if after training, the bee visited a certain type of iridescence143
more times than expected by chance.144
145
RESULTS146
Control Experiments147
The control for hive 1 showed that there was no innate preference for a certain type148
of iridescence (Chi-square test: X10 = 8.55, P = 0.0734). This was also true for hive 2149
(Chi-square test: X10 = 3.95, P = 0.413).150
151
Results After Training152
After being trained to multilayer, the proportion of visits each bee made to the153
multilayer target increased significantly compared to the control (Chi-square test: X20154
8
= 111, P <0.001; Figure 1). This was also the case after training the bees to floral155
(Chi-square test: X20 = 56, P <0.001; Figure 1).156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
Figure 1166
Proportion of bees visiting each type of iridescence during the control, after training167
to multilayer and after training to floral.168
169
Training to artificial did not significantly increase the number of visits the bees made170
to the artificial disc compared to the other disc types (Chi-square test: X20 = 4, P =171
0.406).172
173
Extra Analysis174
Matt175
We noticed the bees visited matt very infrequently during our test experiments. We176
tested the significance of this and indeed the bees did visit matt less than any of the177
other targets after training (Chi-square test: X80 = 38.58, P <0.001; Table 1).178
Secondary Preferences179
9
We looked to see if the bees showed any obvious secondary preferences. After180
training to floral and multilayer, the bees’ preferentially visited the target they were181
trained to, but gloss was the second preference (Table 1; Figure 2; Figure 3).182
We also noticed that the bee visited multilayer and gloss a similar number of times in183
each experiment. This is evident from Table 1.184
185
186
187
Table 1 The visits made to each target type when trained to multilayer, floral and188
artificial iridescences, ordered from the highest to the lowest number of visits. The189
number of visits made to multilayer and gloss have been highlighted in red.190
Trained to
multilayer
Multilayer Gloss Floral Artificial Matt
No. of visits 160 82 65 51 42
Trained to
floral
Floral Gloss Multilayer Artificial Matt
No. of visits 130 91 69 67 41
Trained to
Artificial
Artificial Floral Gloss Multilayer Matt
No. of visits 93 82 81 75 69
10
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
Figure 2202
The number of visits made by the bee to each target type after training to the203
multilayer target had taken place.204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
Figure 3216
The number of visits made by the bee to each target type after training to the floral217
target had taken place.218
11
219
Spatial Distribution220
We noticed that the spatial distribution of the targets had an effect on the first target221
visited by the bee. The nearest target to the hive was often the first landed on. This222
prompted us to record which target was nearest, before seeing if the proportion of223
visits made by bees on their first landing to this particular target was higher than224
expected by chance. In most instances this was the case. When the multilayer target225
was nearest the hive exit, the bees’ primary landing was mostly on the multilayer226
target (Chi-square test: X14 = 24.57, P < 0.001). This was also the case for matt227
(Chi-square test: X21 = 30.7, P = <0.001), floral (Chi-square test: X15 = 16, P =228
0.03), and gloss (Chi-square test: X16 = 18.4, P = 0.001). The result when the229
artificial target was nearest the hive was non-significant (Chi-square test: X14 = 5.29,230
P = 0.259).231
232
DISCUSSION233
Our results show that bumblebees visit multilayer iridescence more often compared234
to other iridescences once a reward association had been learnt. The same is true235
for floral iridescence. This suggests that bees can discriminate between different236
types of iridescence and may use this information to improve foraging efficiency.237
238
Iridescence in Nature239
Colour in nature can be produced by chemical pigments or surface structures.240
Pigment colour is the complementary colour that remains after all other wavelengths241
have been removed by a chemical pigment. Structural colour occurs when an objects242
surface reflects certain wavelengths of light, thus producing an iridescent effect243
(Glover & Whitney, 2010). Iridescence, from the Greek ‘iris’ meaning rainbow, can be244
described as a change in colour depending on viewing geometry (Meadows et al.,245
2009). Despite only being reproduced by humans relatively recently, iridescence has246
12
been present in nature for millions of years. Recent research has suggested that247
many of the shells of animals from the Burgess Shale, British Columbia, where in fact248
iridescent. These animals are thought to have lived around 515 million years ago in249
the Cambrian period, a time of rapid evolutionary change. The evolution of250
iridescence, alongside that of more advanced visual systems capable of detecting251
structural colour, may have played a role in this explosion of metazoan evolution.252
Iridescence is seen in a broad range of taxa in nature, from algae to birds of253
paradise. Some examples of its uses are mate recognition in Helioconius butterflies254
(Sweeney, Jiggins & Johnsen, 2003), seed dispersal in Elaeopcarpus trees (Lee,255
1991) and indications of quality in many bird species (Fitzpatrick, 1998).256
There are different ways in which plants and animals can produce iridescence. The257
most common seen in nature are diffraction gratings and multilayers. Diffraction258
grating iridescence is produced by grooves or depressions on a surface that disperse259
different wavelengths of light in different directions. A wide range of arthropods make260
use of diffraction gratings. It was first noticed in scarab beetles, Serica serica (Seago,261
Brady, Vigneron & Schultz, 2009) and has later been noted in many other species,262
including the spider Cosmophasis thalassina (Parker & Hegedus, 2003). Multilayer263
iridescence is caused by the layering of optical materials; but again light is reflected264
differently depending on its wavelength. A striking example of multilayer iridescence265
in nature is that of the Morpho butterfly. The vivid blue colouration of its wings can be266
seen from great distances despite them containing no blue pigments (Glover &267
Whitney, 2010).268
Structural colours are often brighter than pigment colours; this is because they are269
more specific in the bandwidths they reflect. Pigment colours reflect a broader270
spectrum of light, which contains a mixture of colours, resulting in a less intense271
colour (Glover & Whitney, 2010). Photic crystals are another, less common way of272
producing structural colour. This kind of iridescence is produced by a tetrahedral273
microstructure that diffracts light at different angles, reflecting bright colour over a274
13
broad angle. Photic crystals are seen in the butterfly species Parides sesostris275
(Vukusic & Sambles, 2003).276
277
Plant Use of Iridescence278
Around 100 million years ago, a mutualistic relationship between plants and insects279
was developing. In the early stages insects seeking protein rich pollen had to find280
brown or green flowers amongst the foliage. It wasn’t long before more conspicuous281
flowers evolved, attracting more insects and increasing the efficiency of insect282
pollination. Water lilies and magnolias were the first plants to evolve petals; they283
were white and conspicuous against the dull background (Goulson, 2013.)284
Fluorescence, contrasting colour patterns, and we now think iridescence evolved in285
some plants to further advertise their position to pollinators. Often more than one286
pollination signal is compiled, creating an even stronger basis for discrimination. For287
example Hibiscus trionum has a dark base on each of its petals produced by purple288
pigmentation. This presumably acts like a target, guiding the pollinator to the nectar289
reward. This guide is made even more eye-catching to the bee by the presence of an290
overlying iridescence (Whitney, Kolle & Alvarez-Fernandez et al., 2009).291
Iridescence is utilised by plants for diverse reasons, including predation avoidance,292
defence against high light levels, fruit dispersal and even non-communicational roles293
such as integument strengthening and water repellence (Doucet & Meadows, 2009).294
Multilayer iridescence can sometimes be seen in the leaves of plants which inhabit295
shaded areas, where it is thought to increase the absorption of wavelengths more296
abundant in this low light environment. For example-species in the genus Selaginella297
exhibit iridescence when growing in the shade, but not when growing in more direct298
light (Hébant & Lee, 1984).299
More recently, diffraction gratings have been shown in petals of plant species300
including Tulipa humilis, Hibiscus trionum and Mentzelia lindleyi. Although not visible301
to the human eye, the cuticular striations seen from a scanning electron microscope302
14
enable scientists to infer iridescence in the UV spectrum. This iridescence, which303
although invisible to the human eye, is very obvious to bees, is likely to be used in304
the attraction of pollinators. In some plants iridescence is used to aid mimicry. For305
example iridescence on an enlarged petal known as the labellum in the306
pseudocopulatory orchid Ophrys, helps with the deception of male wasps and solitary307
bees. They are tricked into thinking that the labellum is the iridescent female of their308
species (Whitney, Kolle & Alvarez-Fernandez et al., 2009). So far 10 angiosperm309
families have been identified to contain at least one species with iridescent petals.310
Bearing in mind that over 50% of angiosperms exhibit a textured petal surface, it is311
highly likely that a much higher number of flowers than currently described are312
iridescent (Whitney, Kolle, Andrew et al., 2009).313
314
Bumblebee Foraging Behaviour315
Bumblebees, like their relatives the honey bees, feed on the nectar and pollen316
produced by many plants. This process results in the pollination of a vast proportion317
of many fruit and vegetable crops globally. Therefore it is extremely important not just318
regarding ecosystem functioning, but also to the economy and food industry. There319
are over 250 species of bumblebee, mostly present in the northern latitudes, each320
species favour certain flower types. Specialised anatomy helps them to exploit their321
preferred species, for example the garden bumblebees, Bombus hortorum, are one322
of the few species with a tongue long enough to reach the nectar in foxgloves323
(Goulson, 2013). Bumblebees are highly reliant on visual cues to find specific flower324
species, and hence have evolved highly sensitive compound eyes made up of many325
ommatidia.326
Bumblebees are trichromatic; their 3 photoreceptors produce sensitivity peaks in the327
UV, blue and green regions of the visual spectrum (Skorupski, Döring & Chittka,328
2007). Slight sensitivity differences in the photoreceptors of different species have329
fine-tuned bumblebee colour vision towards their preferred plant species, improving330
15
foraging efficiency (Chittka & Menzel, 1992). The fact that visual cues are so331
important in flower identification is reflected by the fact that mainly the larger bees,332
which have increased visual capacity, forage for the hive (Goulson, 2013). Evidence333
that bumblebees can distinguish structural colour was provided in experiments where334
bumblebees where trained to targets displaying artificial iridescence. The bees’ were335
later capable of correctly identifying and choosing this target type over non-iridescent336
targets displaying the same pigment colour. This shows that bees’ can successfully337
distinguish structural colour from pigment colour. The bees’ could also distinguish the338
more variable floral iridescence, which would be present in nature (Whitney, Kolle,339
Andrew et al., 2009).340
Bumblebee flower choice reflects innate preferences as well as learnt associations341
between certain colours and a reward (Gumbert, 2000). Although naive bumblebees342
showed no preference towards structural colour in our experiments, they successfully343
learnt associations between certain iridescences and a reward. Alongside innate and344
acquired preferences towards certain pigment colours, this ability to discriminate345
between different structural colours could offer bees’ a powerful means to categorise346
different flowers in their environment. Experiments on wild populations of Antirrhinum347
majus show that this may be the case. A mutant line of these flowers lacking348
iridescence were visited significantly less than their iridescent counterparts by bee349
pollinators (Glover & Martin, 2002).350
Our results showed that as well as distinguishing iridescence from pigment colour,351
bees could also discriminate between different kinds of iridescence. This was true for352
floral and multilayer iridescence (Figure 1); unfortunately our results when training to353
artificial iridescence were non-significant. This is contrary to what we expected, and354
previous findings by other researchers. This contradictory result may have been355
caused by experimental procedure; we could have used a larger sample of bees356
thereby increasing the significance of our results. We could also have increased the357
effectiveness of our training method by using differential conditioning as opposed to358
16
operant conditioning. This would involve the use of a quinine forfeit for when the bee359
selected an incorrect target type. It has been proved that differential conditioning360
does in fact improve bees’ ability to discriminate between different colours (Dyer &361
Chittka, 2004). There was also a clear difference in the hive quality. Our training to362
artificial commenced in a second hive, where the bees were acting irrationally, not363
returning to the hive from the test arena, so it is possible the queen had lost control of364
her workers.365
The significance of our results could have been improved with respect to spatial366
design. As noted in the results section, bees had a tendency to land on the nearest367
target. This is likely because naïve bumblebee foragers use movement rules when368
foraging. These involve short movements to begin with; therefore it is likely that the369
bees’ first land will be on the closest target (Burns & Thomson 2005). Furthermore370
with respect to spatial distribution, assigning coordinates to the test arena and using371
a random number generator to determine the target positions in each test would have372
removed any bias. Despite thinking that we were placing the targets randomly,373
there’s a possibility that we may have been subconsciously placing them in certain374
areas. Bumblebees have extremely good spatial memory, so if we had been placing375
the targets non-randomly, this could have affected our results.376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
17
The Blue Tulip Colour Hexagon387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
Figure 4: The Blue Tulip Colour Hexagon402
A plot of photoreceptor excitation for the trichromatic colour vision system of Bombus403
terrestris. The points represent the level of stimulation each photoreceptor (UV, Blue404
and Green) incurs whilst the viewing angle of each surface was altered (Whitney &405
Sandbach, unpublished). As the distance between 2 points increases, so does the406
bees’ ability to discriminate between the 2 surfaces (Chittka, 1992).407
The Bee Colour Hexagon (Figure 4) has been created with respect to the different408
target types used in our experiment. From this particular model we can infer, how409
easy or difficult it is for the bee to distinguish between matt, gloss, multilayer, artificial410
and floral surfaces. For the iridescent targets there is quite a large spread of points411
across the visual spectrum due to the reflection of a multitude of different412
wavelengths by the iridescent surface depending on the angle of approach. This413
18
causes the excitement of a larger number of photoreceptors and is why iridescent414
surfaces appear different depending on the angle of approach. This could have415
negatively affected the bees’ ability to learn the iridescent targets; however this didn’t416
seem to be the case. Perhaps the bees have a mechanism allowing them to417
compensate for this visual phenomenon. A colour constancy mechanism is seen in418
the honeybee with regards to pigment colour when illumination changes (Faruq,419
Mcowan & Chittka, 2013), and it is reasonable to assume that a similar mechanism420
allows bees to categorise structural colour, despite the changes in spectral421
reflectance with viewing geometry. The mechanisms involved in helping the bee to422
categorise different structural colours are as yet unknown. Adaptation of423
photoreceptors could play a key role, or higher input may be needed, from the central424
nervous system for instance.425
The non-iridescent targets, matt and gloss, occupy much smaller areas on the426
visual spectrum, as very little scattering of light occurs on reflection. A small amount427
of scattering occurs when light hits the matt target due to specular reflection,428
whereby small reflective facets dispersed randomly on the surface cause a small429
amount of light scattering (Torrance & Sparrow, 1967). It could be assumed that the430
smaller range of the non-iridescent targets may increase the ease of discrimination431
for bees. This seems to be true for matt, which the bees visit less often once trained432
to the iridescent targets; showing that they can readily distinguish it as non-rewarding433
(Table 1). However gloss also has a small range, but is still visited at a high434
frequency, similar to multilayer (Figure 2; Table 1) which has a much larger range.435
The fact that gloss and multilayer are visited at similar frequencies is interesting, as436
they occupy similar areas in the bees’ visual spectrum (Figure 3), meaning they437
excite a similar repertoire of photoreceptors. This suggests an overlap of visual438
ranges may make it harder for bees to distinguish between different surfaces.439
19
The different surfaces used in the experiment where added to a white lid leaving a440
visible rim around the perimeter of the target. This may be significant as bees are441
often attracted to contrasting colours when foraging (Lunau, Wacht & Chittka, 1996).442
We do not know the spectral properties of the white lid, however if it contrasted more443
greatly with certain surfaces used in our test, this may have increased its attraction to444
the bees and could have affected our results.445
446
Conclusion447
Our experiments and previous research conclude that bumblebees’ can successfully448
distinguish different kinds of iridescence from simple pigment colour, and each other.449
They achieve this despite the significant changes in appearance produced by450
different viewing angles and changes in the spectral composition of the illuminant.451
Ecological studies of bumblebees have shown that they usually exhibit constancy in452
the flowers that they visit, often detecting favorable species using visual signals.453
Categorising flowers using differences in their structural colour as well as pigment454
colour is likely to improve the foraging efficiency of bumblebees, thereby increasing455
their chances of survival.456
It would be interesting to find out exactly how important structural colour is in457
comparison to other more widely known visual cues. Bearing in mind the importance458
of bumblebee pollination to the economy, even a slight advantage incurred by the459
use of structural colour could have huge implications to not just bumblebee460
populations, but also the human race. It is also important to know whether the461
bumblebee’s ability to categorise colour is altered by changes in the light462
environment. If so, this could be relevant in areas affected by smog and other463
anthropogenic pollutants which may reduce the efficiency of pollination, ultimately464
affecting the survival chances of the hive. In a time when bee diversity has declined465
20
markedly, any information concerning their ecology could be used to aid bumblebee466
conservation, and hopefully reverse this decline.467
468
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS469
Thanks are due to my supervisor Dr Heather Whitney, who provided valuable470
information on laboratory conduct when working with bumblebees’ and helped us471
greatly with our experimental procedure. Also thanks to Lucy Sandbach who472
provided us with the bee colour hexagon and was available to answer any queries on473
its production. A final thanks to my project partner, without whom I couldn’t have474
carried out any of the experiments.475
476
REFERENCES477
478
 Burns,J.G., & Thomson, J.D. (2005). A test of spatial memory and movement479
patterns of bumblebees at multiple spatial and temporal scales. Behavioral480
Ecology, 17, 48-55481
 Chittka, L. (1992). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 170, 533-543482
 Chittka,L,. & Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours483
and the insect pollinators’ colour vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,484
171, 171-181485
 Doucet, S.M., & Meadows, M.G. (2009). Iridescence: a functional perspective.486
Journal of the Royal Society, 6, 115-132487
 Dyer, A.G., & Chittka, L. (2004). Fine colour discrimination requires488
differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften, 91, 224-227489
 Faruq, S., McOwan, P.W., & Chittka, L. (2013). The biological significance of490
color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers491
under varied illumination. Journal of Vision, 13, 1–14492
 Fitzpatrick, S. (1998). Colour schemes for birds: structural colouration and493
signals of quality in feathers. Acta Zoologica Fennica, 35, 67-77494
 Gandía-Herrero,F., García-Carmona,F., & Escribano, J. (2005). Botany:495
Floral fluorescence effect. Nature, 437, 334496
 Glover, B.J., & Martin, C. (2002). The role of petal cell shape and497
pigmentation in pollination success in Antirrhinum majus. Heredity, 80, 778–498
784499
21
 Glover, B.J., & Whitney, H.M. (2010). Structural colour and iridescence in500
plants: the poorly studied relations of pigment colour. Annuals of Botany, 105,501
505-511502
 Goulson, D. (2013). A Sting in the Tale. London, UK: Johnathon Cape.503
 Gumbert, A. (2000). Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris):504
innate preferences and generalization after learning. Behavioral Ecology and505
Sociobiology, 48, 36-43506
 Hébant,C., & Lee, D.W. (1984). Ultrastructural Basis and Developmental507
Control of Blue Iridescence in Selaginella Leaves. American Journal of508
Botany, 71, 216-219509
 Lunau, K., Wacht, S., & Chittka, L. (1996). Colour choices of naive bumble510
bees and their implications for colour perception. Journal of Comparative511
Physiology A, 178, 477-489512
 Lee, D. (1991). Ultrastructural basis and function of iridescent blue colour in513
fruits of Elaoecarpus. Nature, 349, 260-262514
 Meadows, M.G., Butler, M.W., Morehouse, N.I., Taylor, L.A., Toomey, M.B., &515
McGraw, K.J., et al. (2009). Iridescence: views from many angles. Journal of516
the Royal Society, 6, 107-113517
 Parker, A.R., & Hegedus, Z. (2003). Journal of Optics A: Pure and Applied518
Optics, 5,111519
 Seago, A.E., Brady, P., Vigneron,J.., & Schultz, T.D. (2009). Gold bugs and520
beyond: a review of iridescence and structural colour mechanisms in beetles521
(Coleoptera). Journal of the Royal Society, 6, 165-184522
 Skorupski, P., Döring, T.F., & Chittka, L. (2007). Photoreceptor spectral523
sensitivity in island and mainland populations of the bumblebee, Bombus524
terrestris. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 193, 485–494525
 Sweeney, A., Jiggins, C., & Johnsen, S. (2003). Insect communication:526
Polarized light as a butterfly mating signal. Nature, 423, 31-32527
 Torrance, K.E., & Sparrow, E.M. (1967). Theory for Off-Specular Reflection528
From Roughened Surfaces. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57,529
1105-1112530
 Vukusic,P., & Sambles, J.R. (2003). Photonic structures in biology. Nature531
424, 852-855532
 Whitney, H.M., Kolle, M., Andrew, P., Chittka,L., Steiner, U., & Glover, B.J.533
(2009). Floral Iridescence, Produced by Diffractive Optics, Acts As a Cue for534
Animal Pollinators. Science, 323, 130-133535
 Whitney, H.M., Kolle, M., Alvarez-Fernandez, R., Steiner, U., & Glover, B.J.536
(2009). Contributions of iridescence to floral patterning. Communicative &537
Integrative Biology, 2, 230-232538

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Raising the bar new responses to marketing law firms
Raising the bar   new responses to marketing law firmsRaising the bar   new responses to marketing law firms
Raising the bar new responses to marketing law firms
Sarah Bagnall
 
035
035035
035
JIuc
 
032
032032
032
JIuc
 
Fc 118 termination packet termination questionnaire and reporting forms
Fc 118 termination packet  termination questionnaire and reporting formsFc 118 termination packet  termination questionnaire and reporting forms
Fc 118 termination packet termination questionnaire and reporting forms
screaminc
 
031
031031
031
JIuc
 
Como quitar una verruga
Como quitar una verrugaComo quitar una verruga
Como quitar una verruga
aturaleqasopl
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Raising the bar new responses to marketing law firms
Raising the bar   new responses to marketing law firmsRaising the bar   new responses to marketing law firms
Raising the bar new responses to marketing law firms
 
Unit 2: Sensitivity
Unit 2: SensitivityUnit 2: Sensitivity
Unit 2: Sensitivity
 
Mediul
Mediul Mediul
Mediul
 
Roger figueira-Matematica3
Roger figueira-Matematica3Roger figueira-Matematica3
Roger figueira-Matematica3
 
moodle_Admin teacher
moodle_Admin teachermoodle_Admin teacher
moodle_Admin teacher
 
Consuntivo 2014 dell'economia regionale
Consuntivo 2014 dell'economia regionaleConsuntivo 2014 dell'economia regionale
Consuntivo 2014 dell'economia regionale
 
Bab3 klsx
Bab3 klsxBab3 klsx
Bab3 klsx
 
035
035035
035
 
032
032032
032
 
Ortopedicos kleyber
Ortopedicos kleyberOrtopedicos kleyber
Ortopedicos kleyber
 
Fc 118 termination packet termination questionnaire and reporting forms
Fc 118 termination packet  termination questionnaire and reporting formsFc 118 termination packet  termination questionnaire and reporting forms
Fc 118 termination packet termination questionnaire and reporting forms
 
Animales
AnimalesAnimales
Animales
 
031
031031
031
 
Como quitar una verruga
Como quitar una verrugaComo quitar una verruga
Como quitar una verruga
 
Soal UN fisika SMA 2013 Elektrostatika
Soal UN fisika SMA 2013 ElektrostatikaSoal UN fisika SMA 2013 Elektrostatika
Soal UN fisika SMA 2013 Elektrostatika
 
Еліктеу сөздің түрлері
Еліктеу сөздің түрлеріЕліктеу сөздің түрлері
Еліктеу сөздің түрлері
 
Sena 2014 jhon
Sena 2014 jhonSena 2014 jhon
Sena 2014 jhon
 
[Interbrand] Best Global Brand 2014 report
[Interbrand] Best Global Brand 2014 report[Interbrand] Best Global Brand 2014 report
[Interbrand] Best Global Brand 2014 report
 
Sena 2014 jhon
Sena 2014 jhonSena 2014 jhon
Sena 2014 jhon
 
The State of Open Research Data
The State of Open Research DataThe State of Open Research Data
The State of Open Research Data
 

Similar to Can Bumblebees Distinguish Between Different Types of Iridescence

Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
Innspub Net
 
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docxBehavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
AASTHA76
 
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from 13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from  13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from  13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from 13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
BIOLOGICAL FORUM
 
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing SystemMortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
paperpublications3
 

Similar to Can Bumblebees Distinguish Between Different Types of Iridescence (20)

Studies on Prevalence of Ixodid Ticks Infesting Cattle and Their Control by P...
Studies on Prevalence of Ixodid Ticks Infesting Cattle and Their Control by P...Studies on Prevalence of Ixodid Ticks Infesting Cattle and Their Control by P...
Studies on Prevalence of Ixodid Ticks Infesting Cattle and Their Control by P...
 
Methods of marking insects
Methods of marking insectsMethods of marking insects
Methods of marking insects
 
Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
Prevelance of Lyperosomum longicauda Rudolphi, 1809 (Dicrocoeliioidae: Tremat...
 
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docxBehavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
Behavioural Processes 109 (2014) 157–163Contents lists ava.docx
 
Critique Assignment
Critique AssignmentCritique Assignment
Critique Assignment
 
Critique: Multiple Routes of Pesticide Exposure for Honey Bees Living Near Ag...
Critique: Multiple Routes of Pesticide Exposure for Honey Bees Living Near Ag...Critique: Multiple Routes of Pesticide Exposure for Honey Bees Living Near Ag...
Critique: Multiple Routes of Pesticide Exposure for Honey Bees Living Near Ag...
 
Biol108 chp12-pt2-pp-spr12-120417073821-phpapp02
Biol108 chp12-pt2-pp-spr12-120417073821-phpapp02Biol108 chp12-pt2-pp-spr12-120417073821-phpapp02
Biol108 chp12-pt2-pp-spr12-120417073821-phpapp02
 
29.1
29.129.1
29.1
 
1-s2.0-S0376635705001087-main.pdf
1-s2.0-S0376635705001087-main.pdf1-s2.0-S0376635705001087-main.pdf
1-s2.0-S0376635705001087-main.pdf
 
Anther culture
Anther cultureAnther culture
Anther culture
 
Cryptolaemus monrouzeiri
Cryptolaemus monrouzeiriCryptolaemus monrouzeiri
Cryptolaemus monrouzeiri
 
production of double haploid in crops
production of double haploid in cropsproduction of double haploid in crops
production of double haploid in crops
 
DETERMINATIONS OF IMIDACLOPRID AND NANO- IMIDACLOPRID ON SOME TOMATO SERIOUS ...
DETERMINATIONS OF IMIDACLOPRID AND NANO- IMIDACLOPRID ON SOME TOMATO SERIOUS ...DETERMINATIONS OF IMIDACLOPRID AND NANO- IMIDACLOPRID ON SOME TOMATO SERIOUS ...
DETERMINATIONS OF IMIDACLOPRID AND NANO- IMIDACLOPRID ON SOME TOMATO SERIOUS ...
 
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from 13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from  13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from  13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
13 isolation and identification of endophytic fungi from 13 ijtas 93-2018-hu...
 
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing SystemMortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
Mortality of Fayoumi and Sonali Chicks in Scavenging Rearing System
 
Evolution of plant animal interaction via receiver bias
Evolution of plant animal interaction via receiver biasEvolution of plant animal interaction via receiver bias
Evolution of plant animal interaction via receiver bias
 
Reproductive Biology of Some Tropical Forest Species : Vikas kumar, vkskumar4...
Reproductive Biology of Some Tropical Forest Species : Vikas kumar, vkskumar4...Reproductive Biology of Some Tropical Forest Species : Vikas kumar, vkskumar4...
Reproductive Biology of Some Tropical Forest Species : Vikas kumar, vkskumar4...
 
Oriculture
OricultureOriculture
Oriculture
 
Efficacy of some Plant Essential Oils as Green Insecticides to Control Whitef...
Efficacy of some Plant Essential Oils as Green Insecticides to Control Whitef...Efficacy of some Plant Essential Oils as Green Insecticides to Control Whitef...
Efficacy of some Plant Essential Oils as Green Insecticides to Control Whitef...
 
Lab Work Protocol on Nematology
Lab Work Protocol on Nematology Lab Work Protocol on Nematology
Lab Work Protocol on Nematology
 

Can Bumblebees Distinguish Between Different Types of Iridescence

  • 1. 1 PRACTICAL PROJECT 2012/2013 SCHOOL OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES Can Bumblebees Distinguish Between Different Types of Iridescence? Cordelia Batt, 1007206, cb0200@my.bristol.ac.uk For submission to: Animal Behaviour Supervisor: Dr Heather Whitney Word Count: 4,691
  • 2. 2 Iridescence, the change in hue corresponding to the viewing angle, is utilized1 by a large number of living organisms. Recently this optical phenomenon has2 been identified in the petals of many plant species, most likely as a signal to3 their specific pollinators. Bumblebees are able to distinguish iridescence from4 basic pigment colour, however there is more than one different type of5 iridescence; our experiment set out to discover if the bumblebee species,6 Bombus terrestris, can successfully discriminate between them. Our method7 involved training the bees’ to associate a certain type of iridescence with a8 reward, before adding different types of iridescence into the test environment.9 If the trained bee showed a preference towards the known, rewarding10 iridescence, it must, to a certain degree, be able to distinguish between them.11 We found that in most cases, this was true and bees were more likely to visit12 the known, rewarding iridescence. This infers that bees could be using not13 only pigment colour, but also structural colour to select rewarding plant14 species in their environment. Iridescence may therefore play an important role15 in improving foraging efficiency in bees.16 KEY WORDS17 Pollinator; pigment colour; structural colour; iridescence; multilayer;18 diffraction grating; colour constancy.19 20 The mutual relationship between plants and their pollinators is constantly evolving so21 that plants are more conspicuous to their pollinators. Similarly pollinators have22 become better equipped to receive visual signals from plants, which due to their lack23 of mobility often rely on insects for sexual reproduction. For this reason, plants24 display bright flowers, the colours produced by reflecting certain specific wavelengths25 of light which are perceptible to their pollinators. Colours can be produced by26
  • 3. 3 chemical pigments, or specific surface structures. Pigment colours are produced by27 chemicals which absorb most of the visible light spectrum; the wavelengths not28 absorbed determine the perceived colour of the object. Structural colours are those29 produced by means other than pigments, where certain light wavelengths of light are30 selectively reflected from a surface. The resulting colours are often more intense than31 those produced by pigment colours (Glover & Whitney, 2010).32 Structural colour has been recognised in nature for a long time, most notably in33 birds, insects and fish, however many reptiles and amphibians are also iridescent.34 The purpose of iridescence in many of these organisms includes species and sex35 recognition, as well mate choice and predator deterrence (Doucet & Meadows,36 2009). Recent studies have shown that many plant species also exhibit iridescence.37 Much of the iridescence occurs in the ultraviolet (UV) wavelengths, and unlike us,38 these signals are recognized by bumblebees, whose’ visual systems have high acuity39 in the blue and near-UV ranges (Whitney et al., 2009). Iridescence, like many other40 visual cues such as pigment colour (Gumbert, 2000), fluorescence (Gandía-Herrero,41 García-Carmona & Josefa Escribano, 2005) and patterns (Whitney, Kolle, Alvarez-42 Fernandez, Steiner & Glover, 2009), is thought to be used by plants as a means to43 attract pollinators. Bumblebees can not only detect iridescence independently of44 pigment colour (Whitney et al., 2009), but they are also more attracted to flowers that45 display it (Glover & Martin, 2002). This suggests an evolutionary advantage to46 structural colour in plants with regard to insect pollination. Any increase in the47 efficiency of pollination is not only relevant to ecosystems, but also to global food48 production, which relies heavily on insect pollination.49 There is no single means of producing iridescence. Diffraction gratings, multilayers50 and photic crystals are all mechanisms that produce structural colour (Glover &51 Whitney, 2010), allowing a variation in the optical properties of plants. Bumblebees52 are very specific in the flowers that they visit, each species favouring certain plant53
  • 4. 4 types (Goulson, 2013). It could therefore benefit bumblebees to be able to use these54 differences in iridescence to categorise particularly favourable plant species. We55 know that bumblebees can distinguish iridescence from simple pigment colour, but56 can they recognise the slight differences in structural colour produced by flowers of57 different species? More simply, can bumblebees discriminate between different58 iridescences? We tested to see if naïve bumblebees, Bombus terrestris, were able to59 discriminate between certain types of iridescence by using different mechanisms60 including diffraction gratings and multilayers to produce structural colour. If the bees61 could learn to categorise the different structural colours depending on whether or not62 they offered a reward, then we can conclude they are able to distinguish between the63 different kinds of iridescence.64 65 METHODS66 General Setup67 Bombus terrestris colonies were set up in a cardboard nest box which was attached68 to a glass flight arena 110cm in length, 70cm wide and 100cm high. The bees could69 enter the flight arena via a transparent pipe in which plastic partitions could be70 added, allowing the successful selection of a single bee. The lighting in the laboratory71 comprised six Sylvania Activa 172 Professional 36W fluorescent tubes.72 The bees were naïve with respect to foraging from natural flowers, and hadn’t been73 used for any previous experiments. They received half a pcr plate filled with a 30%74 sucrose solution daily. Over the weekend a gravity feeder was provided, filled with a75 solution mimicking that of the bees’ natural diet (Provided by Koppert Biological76 Systems, who also provided the bees.) Pollen was added to the colony on a weekly77 basis.78
  • 5. 5 How the Targets were Produced79 Targets were provided by Dr Heather Whitney, they were produced by adding80 different surfaces to artificial flowers constructed from Sterilin tubes 26mm diameter81 and 8cm high, with an inverted white lid 70mm diameter on top. The surfaces were82 circular plastic discs, with a diameter of 40mm. The inverted lid of a 0.5 ml Eppendorf83 tube placed in the middle of the target to contain the sucrose reward.84 There were 5 different target types categorised by the surface type on the lid. Two85 were non-iridescent; gloss a smooth moulded plastic and matt rough moulded plastic.86 Three of the targets were iridescent. Two of these were created using diffraction87 gratings; one being artificially produced using ordered rows of grooves on the88 surface; the other was other created using a mould of the adaxial petal surface89 whereby the grooves where much more variable. The third kind of iridescence was90 multilayer iridescence; thin films of optical material arranged in ordered layers. The91 targets were all maintained at room temperature. For ease I will refer to gloss surface92 as gloss; matt surface as matt; diffraction grating iridescence (artificial) as artificial;93 diffraction grating iridescence (natural) as floral and multilayer iridescence as94 multilayer.95 Marking the Bees96 The bees were distinguished using paint marks on their thorax. These were applied97 using a wooden skewer when the bees’ concentration was diverted by sucrose98 solution. We used between 1 and 4 dots of varying sizes and shapes (circles and99 lines for example) using the 5 different colours available to us. This enabled us to100 differentiate between large numbers of bees.101 102 103
  • 6. 6 Control Tests104 Before the test the bees were hungry having not been fed since the previous day.105 The test arena was cleared of all artificial flowers and any foraging bees, which were106 either encouraged back into the hive or captured under Sterilin tubes.107 Targets were placed randomly within the test arena. There were 3 of each kind of108 target, so 15 in total. They were free of any reward. Before starting the control we109 defined a ‘land’ to constitute of all 6 legs placed on the target. We used the same110 observer for all the tests to eliminate any potential differences in classification of a111 ‘land.’112 One bee was released at a time. We allowed 20 lands, recording which targets113 were landed on. Once 20 lands were completed sucrose solution was added to the114 target the bee was situated on, and the bee was allowed to drink and return to the115 hive.116 The targets were removed, washed and cleansed with ethanol so that any remaining117 traces of sucrose were removed. Equally any signals that previous bees could be118 leaving would also be removed. The targets were then replaced and repositioned,119 and the experimental procedure repeated for a time with a different bee. A total of 20120 bees were tested.121 Training to Iridescence122 Firstly, we trained bees to multilayer iridescence. The test arena was cleared of any123 bees and 3 targets with multilayer iridescence were randomly placed inside. Each124 target had an approximately 1ml reward of 30% sucrose solution, which was refilled125 up once emptied. We tested one bee at a time allowing it to drink 20 times from any126 of the 3 targets before commencing with the experiment.127
  • 7. 7 All the multilayer targets were then removed, washed and cleansed with ethanol.128 They were returned to the test arena along with 3 of each of the other types of target,129 making a total of 15 targets. None of the targets had a reward. The trained bee was130 released, and we recorded which targets it landed on. A total of 20 bees were trained131 to multilayer and tested. This procedure was repeated to train the bees to floral.132 Different bees were used for each trial.133 We moved onto a second hive to train the bees to artificial. We completed a control134 test for this hive before obtaining results and used the same methods as previously135 described.136 137 Statistical Analysis138 We used a Chi squared X squared test to analyse the control, to see if the bee visited139 a certain kind of target more frequently than expected by chance. If yes, it would140 indicate that the bees have an innate preference to a certain target.141 We also used the Chi squared test when analysing our results with trained bees.142 This allowed us to see if after training, the bee visited a certain type of iridescence143 more times than expected by chance.144 145 RESULTS146 Control Experiments147 The control for hive 1 showed that there was no innate preference for a certain type148 of iridescence (Chi-square test: X10 = 8.55, P = 0.0734). This was also true for hive 2149 (Chi-square test: X10 = 3.95, P = 0.413).150 151 Results After Training152 After being trained to multilayer, the proportion of visits each bee made to the153 multilayer target increased significantly compared to the control (Chi-square test: X20154
  • 8. 8 = 111, P <0.001; Figure 1). This was also the case after training the bees to floral155 (Chi-square test: X20 = 56, P <0.001; Figure 1).156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 Figure 1166 Proportion of bees visiting each type of iridescence during the control, after training167 to multilayer and after training to floral.168 169 Training to artificial did not significantly increase the number of visits the bees made170 to the artificial disc compared to the other disc types (Chi-square test: X20 = 4, P =171 0.406).172 173 Extra Analysis174 Matt175 We noticed the bees visited matt very infrequently during our test experiments. We176 tested the significance of this and indeed the bees did visit matt less than any of the177 other targets after training (Chi-square test: X80 = 38.58, P <0.001; Table 1).178 Secondary Preferences179
  • 9. 9 We looked to see if the bees showed any obvious secondary preferences. After180 training to floral and multilayer, the bees’ preferentially visited the target they were181 trained to, but gloss was the second preference (Table 1; Figure 2; Figure 3).182 We also noticed that the bee visited multilayer and gloss a similar number of times in183 each experiment. This is evident from Table 1.184 185 186 187 Table 1 The visits made to each target type when trained to multilayer, floral and188 artificial iridescences, ordered from the highest to the lowest number of visits. The189 number of visits made to multilayer and gloss have been highlighted in red.190 Trained to multilayer Multilayer Gloss Floral Artificial Matt No. of visits 160 82 65 51 42 Trained to floral Floral Gloss Multilayer Artificial Matt No. of visits 130 91 69 67 41 Trained to Artificial Artificial Floral Gloss Multilayer Matt No. of visits 93 82 81 75 69
  • 10. 10 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 Figure 2202 The number of visits made by the bee to each target type after training to the203 multilayer target had taken place.204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 Figure 3216 The number of visits made by the bee to each target type after training to the floral217 target had taken place.218
  • 11. 11 219 Spatial Distribution220 We noticed that the spatial distribution of the targets had an effect on the first target221 visited by the bee. The nearest target to the hive was often the first landed on. This222 prompted us to record which target was nearest, before seeing if the proportion of223 visits made by bees on their first landing to this particular target was higher than224 expected by chance. In most instances this was the case. When the multilayer target225 was nearest the hive exit, the bees’ primary landing was mostly on the multilayer226 target (Chi-square test: X14 = 24.57, P < 0.001). This was also the case for matt227 (Chi-square test: X21 = 30.7, P = <0.001), floral (Chi-square test: X15 = 16, P =228 0.03), and gloss (Chi-square test: X16 = 18.4, P = 0.001). The result when the229 artificial target was nearest the hive was non-significant (Chi-square test: X14 = 5.29,230 P = 0.259).231 232 DISCUSSION233 Our results show that bumblebees visit multilayer iridescence more often compared234 to other iridescences once a reward association had been learnt. The same is true235 for floral iridescence. This suggests that bees can discriminate between different236 types of iridescence and may use this information to improve foraging efficiency.237 238 Iridescence in Nature239 Colour in nature can be produced by chemical pigments or surface structures.240 Pigment colour is the complementary colour that remains after all other wavelengths241 have been removed by a chemical pigment. Structural colour occurs when an objects242 surface reflects certain wavelengths of light, thus producing an iridescent effect243 (Glover & Whitney, 2010). Iridescence, from the Greek ‘iris’ meaning rainbow, can be244 described as a change in colour depending on viewing geometry (Meadows et al.,245 2009). Despite only being reproduced by humans relatively recently, iridescence has246
  • 12. 12 been present in nature for millions of years. Recent research has suggested that247 many of the shells of animals from the Burgess Shale, British Columbia, where in fact248 iridescent. These animals are thought to have lived around 515 million years ago in249 the Cambrian period, a time of rapid evolutionary change. The evolution of250 iridescence, alongside that of more advanced visual systems capable of detecting251 structural colour, may have played a role in this explosion of metazoan evolution.252 Iridescence is seen in a broad range of taxa in nature, from algae to birds of253 paradise. Some examples of its uses are mate recognition in Helioconius butterflies254 (Sweeney, Jiggins & Johnsen, 2003), seed dispersal in Elaeopcarpus trees (Lee,255 1991) and indications of quality in many bird species (Fitzpatrick, 1998).256 There are different ways in which plants and animals can produce iridescence. The257 most common seen in nature are diffraction gratings and multilayers. Diffraction258 grating iridescence is produced by grooves or depressions on a surface that disperse259 different wavelengths of light in different directions. A wide range of arthropods make260 use of diffraction gratings. It was first noticed in scarab beetles, Serica serica (Seago,261 Brady, Vigneron & Schultz, 2009) and has later been noted in many other species,262 including the spider Cosmophasis thalassina (Parker & Hegedus, 2003). Multilayer263 iridescence is caused by the layering of optical materials; but again light is reflected264 differently depending on its wavelength. A striking example of multilayer iridescence265 in nature is that of the Morpho butterfly. The vivid blue colouration of its wings can be266 seen from great distances despite them containing no blue pigments (Glover &267 Whitney, 2010).268 Structural colours are often brighter than pigment colours; this is because they are269 more specific in the bandwidths they reflect. Pigment colours reflect a broader270 spectrum of light, which contains a mixture of colours, resulting in a less intense271 colour (Glover & Whitney, 2010). Photic crystals are another, less common way of272 producing structural colour. This kind of iridescence is produced by a tetrahedral273 microstructure that diffracts light at different angles, reflecting bright colour over a274
  • 13. 13 broad angle. Photic crystals are seen in the butterfly species Parides sesostris275 (Vukusic & Sambles, 2003).276 277 Plant Use of Iridescence278 Around 100 million years ago, a mutualistic relationship between plants and insects279 was developing. In the early stages insects seeking protein rich pollen had to find280 brown or green flowers amongst the foliage. It wasn’t long before more conspicuous281 flowers evolved, attracting more insects and increasing the efficiency of insect282 pollination. Water lilies and magnolias were the first plants to evolve petals; they283 were white and conspicuous against the dull background (Goulson, 2013.)284 Fluorescence, contrasting colour patterns, and we now think iridescence evolved in285 some plants to further advertise their position to pollinators. Often more than one286 pollination signal is compiled, creating an even stronger basis for discrimination. For287 example Hibiscus trionum has a dark base on each of its petals produced by purple288 pigmentation. This presumably acts like a target, guiding the pollinator to the nectar289 reward. This guide is made even more eye-catching to the bee by the presence of an290 overlying iridescence (Whitney, Kolle & Alvarez-Fernandez et al., 2009).291 Iridescence is utilised by plants for diverse reasons, including predation avoidance,292 defence against high light levels, fruit dispersal and even non-communicational roles293 such as integument strengthening and water repellence (Doucet & Meadows, 2009).294 Multilayer iridescence can sometimes be seen in the leaves of plants which inhabit295 shaded areas, where it is thought to increase the absorption of wavelengths more296 abundant in this low light environment. For example-species in the genus Selaginella297 exhibit iridescence when growing in the shade, but not when growing in more direct298 light (Hébant & Lee, 1984).299 More recently, diffraction gratings have been shown in petals of plant species300 including Tulipa humilis, Hibiscus trionum and Mentzelia lindleyi. Although not visible301 to the human eye, the cuticular striations seen from a scanning electron microscope302
  • 14. 14 enable scientists to infer iridescence in the UV spectrum. This iridescence, which303 although invisible to the human eye, is very obvious to bees, is likely to be used in304 the attraction of pollinators. In some plants iridescence is used to aid mimicry. For305 example iridescence on an enlarged petal known as the labellum in the306 pseudocopulatory orchid Ophrys, helps with the deception of male wasps and solitary307 bees. They are tricked into thinking that the labellum is the iridescent female of their308 species (Whitney, Kolle & Alvarez-Fernandez et al., 2009). So far 10 angiosperm309 families have been identified to contain at least one species with iridescent petals.310 Bearing in mind that over 50% of angiosperms exhibit a textured petal surface, it is311 highly likely that a much higher number of flowers than currently described are312 iridescent (Whitney, Kolle, Andrew et al., 2009).313 314 Bumblebee Foraging Behaviour315 Bumblebees, like their relatives the honey bees, feed on the nectar and pollen316 produced by many plants. This process results in the pollination of a vast proportion317 of many fruit and vegetable crops globally. Therefore it is extremely important not just318 regarding ecosystem functioning, but also to the economy and food industry. There319 are over 250 species of bumblebee, mostly present in the northern latitudes, each320 species favour certain flower types. Specialised anatomy helps them to exploit their321 preferred species, for example the garden bumblebees, Bombus hortorum, are one322 of the few species with a tongue long enough to reach the nectar in foxgloves323 (Goulson, 2013). Bumblebees are highly reliant on visual cues to find specific flower324 species, and hence have evolved highly sensitive compound eyes made up of many325 ommatidia.326 Bumblebees are trichromatic; their 3 photoreceptors produce sensitivity peaks in the327 UV, blue and green regions of the visual spectrum (Skorupski, Döring & Chittka,328 2007). Slight sensitivity differences in the photoreceptors of different species have329 fine-tuned bumblebee colour vision towards their preferred plant species, improving330
  • 15. 15 foraging efficiency (Chittka & Menzel, 1992). The fact that visual cues are so331 important in flower identification is reflected by the fact that mainly the larger bees,332 which have increased visual capacity, forage for the hive (Goulson, 2013). Evidence333 that bumblebees can distinguish structural colour was provided in experiments where334 bumblebees where trained to targets displaying artificial iridescence. The bees’ were335 later capable of correctly identifying and choosing this target type over non-iridescent336 targets displaying the same pigment colour. This shows that bees’ can successfully337 distinguish structural colour from pigment colour. The bees’ could also distinguish the338 more variable floral iridescence, which would be present in nature (Whitney, Kolle,339 Andrew et al., 2009).340 Bumblebee flower choice reflects innate preferences as well as learnt associations341 between certain colours and a reward (Gumbert, 2000). Although naive bumblebees342 showed no preference towards structural colour in our experiments, they successfully343 learnt associations between certain iridescences and a reward. Alongside innate and344 acquired preferences towards certain pigment colours, this ability to discriminate345 between different structural colours could offer bees’ a powerful means to categorise346 different flowers in their environment. Experiments on wild populations of Antirrhinum347 majus show that this may be the case. A mutant line of these flowers lacking348 iridescence were visited significantly less than their iridescent counterparts by bee349 pollinators (Glover & Martin, 2002).350 Our results showed that as well as distinguishing iridescence from pigment colour,351 bees could also discriminate between different kinds of iridescence. This was true for352 floral and multilayer iridescence (Figure 1); unfortunately our results when training to353 artificial iridescence were non-significant. This is contrary to what we expected, and354 previous findings by other researchers. This contradictory result may have been355 caused by experimental procedure; we could have used a larger sample of bees356 thereby increasing the significance of our results. We could also have increased the357 effectiveness of our training method by using differential conditioning as opposed to358
  • 16. 16 operant conditioning. This would involve the use of a quinine forfeit for when the bee359 selected an incorrect target type. It has been proved that differential conditioning360 does in fact improve bees’ ability to discriminate between different colours (Dyer &361 Chittka, 2004). There was also a clear difference in the hive quality. Our training to362 artificial commenced in a second hive, where the bees were acting irrationally, not363 returning to the hive from the test arena, so it is possible the queen had lost control of364 her workers.365 The significance of our results could have been improved with respect to spatial366 design. As noted in the results section, bees had a tendency to land on the nearest367 target. This is likely because naïve bumblebee foragers use movement rules when368 foraging. These involve short movements to begin with; therefore it is likely that the369 bees’ first land will be on the closest target (Burns & Thomson 2005). Furthermore370 with respect to spatial distribution, assigning coordinates to the test arena and using371 a random number generator to determine the target positions in each test would have372 removed any bias. Despite thinking that we were placing the targets randomly,373 there’s a possibility that we may have been subconsciously placing them in certain374 areas. Bumblebees have extremely good spatial memory, so if we had been placing375 the targets non-randomly, this could have affected our results.376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386
  • 17. 17 The Blue Tulip Colour Hexagon387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 Figure 4: The Blue Tulip Colour Hexagon402 A plot of photoreceptor excitation for the trichromatic colour vision system of Bombus403 terrestris. The points represent the level of stimulation each photoreceptor (UV, Blue404 and Green) incurs whilst the viewing angle of each surface was altered (Whitney &405 Sandbach, unpublished). As the distance between 2 points increases, so does the406 bees’ ability to discriminate between the 2 surfaces (Chittka, 1992).407 The Bee Colour Hexagon (Figure 4) has been created with respect to the different408 target types used in our experiment. From this particular model we can infer, how409 easy or difficult it is for the bee to distinguish between matt, gloss, multilayer, artificial410 and floral surfaces. For the iridescent targets there is quite a large spread of points411 across the visual spectrum due to the reflection of a multitude of different412 wavelengths by the iridescent surface depending on the angle of approach. This413
  • 18. 18 causes the excitement of a larger number of photoreceptors and is why iridescent414 surfaces appear different depending on the angle of approach. This could have415 negatively affected the bees’ ability to learn the iridescent targets; however this didn’t416 seem to be the case. Perhaps the bees have a mechanism allowing them to417 compensate for this visual phenomenon. A colour constancy mechanism is seen in418 the honeybee with regards to pigment colour when illumination changes (Faruq,419 Mcowan & Chittka, 2013), and it is reasonable to assume that a similar mechanism420 allows bees to categorise structural colour, despite the changes in spectral421 reflectance with viewing geometry. The mechanisms involved in helping the bee to422 categorise different structural colours are as yet unknown. Adaptation of423 photoreceptors could play a key role, or higher input may be needed, from the central424 nervous system for instance.425 The non-iridescent targets, matt and gloss, occupy much smaller areas on the426 visual spectrum, as very little scattering of light occurs on reflection. A small amount427 of scattering occurs when light hits the matt target due to specular reflection,428 whereby small reflective facets dispersed randomly on the surface cause a small429 amount of light scattering (Torrance & Sparrow, 1967). It could be assumed that the430 smaller range of the non-iridescent targets may increase the ease of discrimination431 for bees. This seems to be true for matt, which the bees visit less often once trained432 to the iridescent targets; showing that they can readily distinguish it as non-rewarding433 (Table 1). However gloss also has a small range, but is still visited at a high434 frequency, similar to multilayer (Figure 2; Table 1) which has a much larger range.435 The fact that gloss and multilayer are visited at similar frequencies is interesting, as436 they occupy similar areas in the bees’ visual spectrum (Figure 3), meaning they437 excite a similar repertoire of photoreceptors. This suggests an overlap of visual438 ranges may make it harder for bees to distinguish between different surfaces.439
  • 19. 19 The different surfaces used in the experiment where added to a white lid leaving a440 visible rim around the perimeter of the target. This may be significant as bees are441 often attracted to contrasting colours when foraging (Lunau, Wacht & Chittka, 1996).442 We do not know the spectral properties of the white lid, however if it contrasted more443 greatly with certain surfaces used in our test, this may have increased its attraction to444 the bees and could have affected our results.445 446 Conclusion447 Our experiments and previous research conclude that bumblebees’ can successfully448 distinguish different kinds of iridescence from simple pigment colour, and each other.449 They achieve this despite the significant changes in appearance produced by450 different viewing angles and changes in the spectral composition of the illuminant.451 Ecological studies of bumblebees have shown that they usually exhibit constancy in452 the flowers that they visit, often detecting favorable species using visual signals.453 Categorising flowers using differences in their structural colour as well as pigment454 colour is likely to improve the foraging efficiency of bumblebees, thereby increasing455 their chances of survival.456 It would be interesting to find out exactly how important structural colour is in457 comparison to other more widely known visual cues. Bearing in mind the importance458 of bumblebee pollination to the economy, even a slight advantage incurred by the459 use of structural colour could have huge implications to not just bumblebee460 populations, but also the human race. It is also important to know whether the461 bumblebee’s ability to categorise colour is altered by changes in the light462 environment. If so, this could be relevant in areas affected by smog and other463 anthropogenic pollutants which may reduce the efficiency of pollination, ultimately464 affecting the survival chances of the hive. In a time when bee diversity has declined465
  • 20. 20 markedly, any information concerning their ecology could be used to aid bumblebee466 conservation, and hopefully reverse this decline.467 468 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS469 Thanks are due to my supervisor Dr Heather Whitney, who provided valuable470 information on laboratory conduct when working with bumblebees’ and helped us471 greatly with our experimental procedure. Also thanks to Lucy Sandbach who472 provided us with the bee colour hexagon and was available to answer any queries on473 its production. A final thanks to my project partner, without whom I couldn’t have474 carried out any of the experiments.475 476 REFERENCES477 478  Burns,J.G., & Thomson, J.D. (2005). A test of spatial memory and movement479 patterns of bumblebees at multiple spatial and temporal scales. Behavioral480 Ecology, 17, 48-55481  Chittka, L. (1992). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 170, 533-543482  Chittka,L,. & Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours483 and the insect pollinators’ colour vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,484 171, 171-181485  Doucet, S.M., & Meadows, M.G. (2009). Iridescence: a functional perspective.486 Journal of the Royal Society, 6, 115-132487  Dyer, A.G., & Chittka, L. (2004). Fine colour discrimination requires488 differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften, 91, 224-227489  Faruq, S., McOwan, P.W., & Chittka, L. (2013). The biological significance of490 color constancy: An agent-based model with bees foraging from flowers491 under varied illumination. Journal of Vision, 13, 1–14492  Fitzpatrick, S. (1998). Colour schemes for birds: structural colouration and493 signals of quality in feathers. Acta Zoologica Fennica, 35, 67-77494  Gandía-Herrero,F., García-Carmona,F., & Escribano, J. (2005). Botany:495 Floral fluorescence effect. Nature, 437, 334496  Glover, B.J., & Martin, C. (2002). The role of petal cell shape and497 pigmentation in pollination success in Antirrhinum majus. Heredity, 80, 778–498 784499
  • 21. 21  Glover, B.J., & Whitney, H.M. (2010). Structural colour and iridescence in500 plants: the poorly studied relations of pigment colour. Annuals of Botany, 105,501 505-511502  Goulson, D. (2013). A Sting in the Tale. London, UK: Johnathon Cape.503  Gumbert, A. (2000). Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris):504 innate preferences and generalization after learning. Behavioral Ecology and505 Sociobiology, 48, 36-43506  Hébant,C., & Lee, D.W. (1984). Ultrastructural Basis and Developmental507 Control of Blue Iridescence in Selaginella Leaves. American Journal of508 Botany, 71, 216-219509  Lunau, K., Wacht, S., & Chittka, L. (1996). Colour choices of naive bumble510 bees and their implications for colour perception. Journal of Comparative511 Physiology A, 178, 477-489512  Lee, D. (1991). Ultrastructural basis and function of iridescent blue colour in513 fruits of Elaoecarpus. Nature, 349, 260-262514  Meadows, M.G., Butler, M.W., Morehouse, N.I., Taylor, L.A., Toomey, M.B., &515 McGraw, K.J., et al. (2009). Iridescence: views from many angles. Journal of516 the Royal Society, 6, 107-113517  Parker, A.R., & Hegedus, Z. (2003). Journal of Optics A: Pure and Applied518 Optics, 5,111519  Seago, A.E., Brady, P., Vigneron,J.., & Schultz, T.D. (2009). Gold bugs and520 beyond: a review of iridescence and structural colour mechanisms in beetles521 (Coleoptera). Journal of the Royal Society, 6, 165-184522  Skorupski, P., Döring, T.F., & Chittka, L. (2007). Photoreceptor spectral523 sensitivity in island and mainland populations of the bumblebee, Bombus524 terrestris. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 193, 485–494525  Sweeney, A., Jiggins, C., & Johnsen, S. (2003). Insect communication:526 Polarized light as a butterfly mating signal. Nature, 423, 31-32527  Torrance, K.E., & Sparrow, E.M. (1967). Theory for Off-Specular Reflection528 From Roughened Surfaces. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57,529 1105-1112530  Vukusic,P., & Sambles, J.R. (2003). Photonic structures in biology. Nature531 424, 852-855532  Whitney, H.M., Kolle, M., Andrew, P., Chittka,L., Steiner, U., & Glover, B.J.533 (2009). Floral Iridescence, Produced by Diffractive Optics, Acts As a Cue for534 Animal Pollinators. Science, 323, 130-133535  Whitney, H.M., Kolle, M., Alvarez-Fernandez, R., Steiner, U., & Glover, B.J.536 (2009). Contributions of iridescence to floral patterning. Communicative &537 Integrative Biology, 2, 230-232538