SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 8
Download to read offline
08 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
THE EVOLUTIONARY
ROLE OF CHILDHOOD
Children learn about the world just as scientists do – making predictions, conducting
experiments, analyzing statistics and forming theories. Drawing on her most recent research,
renowned scholar and best selling author, Alison Gopnik, demonstrates how cognitive abilities
of babies and young children far surpasses what adults normally attribute them with.
Illustration by Amruta Patil
Some of the research my colleagues
and I have done over the last
thirty years has completely
revolutionised the way we think about babies
and young children, and herefore the way we
think about basic human learning capacities.
What we have discovered is that even the very
youngest of babies and very young children
have capacities to learn that are greater than
those of the most illustrious of scientists.
Discovering these tremendous natural
capacities to learn ought to influence the way
we teach children.
If we think that education is all about trying to
encourage learning, then knowing something
about just how powerful these natural learning
capacities are, should help us to improve our
educational system. What I am going to do
today is talk about the fundamental science of
learning that has emerged, especially over the
past ten years.
Let’s start with one of those questions that you
think people would ask, but they rarely do:
“Why is it that we have a period of childhood
at all?” In fact, “why do we have children at
all?” If you think about it, at least from some
perspectives, you could argue that babies and
young children are useless after all.They don’t
go out and work; they don’t bring home the
bacon; in fact, in some ways, they are worse
than useless because we grown-ups have to
spend so much of our time and energy just
keeping them alive and taking care of them.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 2
2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 09WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
So, from a scientific perspective, one of
the questions we want to ask is, “Why,
from an evolutionary perspective, do we
actually have this period of childhood at
all?”; “Why does childhood even exist?”
It’s a particularly interesting question for
us because human beings have a much
longer period of childhood – a much
longer period of immaturity – than any
other species.
So, our babies and children are
dependent on us for much longer than
the children of any other species are
dependent on their grown-ups. My
oldest son is twenty two, and even now
we,as his parents,continue investing our
resources in taking care of him.
It turns out that when we look at
evolution, there is something of an
explanation for this. Consider two
animals. On the one hand, there is the
crow, in particular the new Caledonian
crow.Crows,ravens and other animals of
the Corvidae family are extremely
intelligent animals.The new Caledonian
crow was featured on the cover of
Science magazine because it has learned
how to use a tool, how to bend a wire in
order to be able to get at a piece of food.
Coincidently, crows have extremely long
periods of immaturity as well; crow
babies are immature, helpless and
dependent fledglings for as long as a year.
In the case of Caledonian crows, this
period lasts as long as two years, which
is a very, very long time in the life of a
crow. So these are animals that are
extremely intelligent, rely a lot on
learning, have relatively large brains, are
very flexible, and are “smart animals.”
On the other hand,we have the domestic
chicken. It represents chickens, turkeys,
geese, ducks and all of the birds in this
family. With apologies to all chicken
lovers, these birds are basically dumb.
Though these animals are extremely
good at pecking for grain, they are not
much good at doing anything else. Note
that chicken babies mature within a
space of few weeks or months.
What we observe is that, there seems to
be a correlation between the length of
the period of immaturity in the young
(how long the babies are dependent and
helpless) and the flexibility and
intelligence in them as adult animals.
Further, this seems true not only for
birds. Recent reports regarding
marsupials are echoing this observation.
Marsupials are animals like kangaroos
and wombats, found mostly in Australia
and New Zealand. Among marsupials,
the longer the babies live in the mother’s
pouch, the larger the size of the adult
brain.
There is another interesting aspect to this
story: when babies are immature for such
long periods, the adults have to put in
“greater parental investment.” So the
longer the period of immaturity, the
larger the brain and the better the adult
is at learning, the more the time and
energy that the parents invest in raising
those babies. This is once again evident
when you compare quokkas and
opossums.
Quokkas are marsupials with very large
brains and generally both the parent
quokkas are engaged in taking care of
just one baby. Opossums, on the other
hand, are much more small-brained
marsupials, and they often have many
more babies and there is very little
opportunity for parental investment.
The question that arises now is: “Why
would you see this correlation between
how smart the adult animal is,how much
the adult animal relies on learning, and
how long a period of immaturity the
adult undergoes as a baby?”
Animals use a number of strategies to
survive. Some animals, like the chicken
and the opossums,might be very good at
doing just one thing over and over again.
They are very well-suited for one
particular evolutionary task.
Other creatures like the crows and
human beings are not good at doing
anything in particular, but are adept at
learning to do new things, things that
will suit whatever environment they find
themselves in.
In this manner, we learn to be able to do
all the things that we need to do, to
survive as animals. The latter is a very
good strategy in a lot of ways. Very
characteristic of human beings, it is this
strategy that has allowed us to exist and
It’s a particularly
interesting question
for us because human
beings have a much longer
period of childhood – a
much longer period of
immaturity – than any
other species.
So the longer the
period of immaturity,
the larger the brain
and the better the
adult is at learning, the
more the time and
energy that the
parents invest in
raising those babies.
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 3
10 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
survive in more environments than any
other animal, including in outer space.
However, this strategy has one great
drawback: Until you do all of the
learning you have to do, you’re going to
be helpless. For example, suppose a
mastodon is charging at you. At that
moment, you cannot say to yourself,
“What shall I do about this? Should I
use a stick or a slingshot? What would
be more effective?.” Instead, you should
have already figured out how to deal with
the situation, before the mastodons
actually show up.
So, how and when do we learn all that
needs to be learned? Evolution seems to
have solved this problem by developing
a kind of division of labor.As a result,we
have the early protected period of
childhood (in which all we have to do is
to learn) and then we have adulthood
(when we take the things we learned as
very young children and put them to use,
to solve problems and do all the things
that we need to do as adults).
Our babies are like the research and
development division of the human
species, while adults are like the
production and marketing division. So,
there are the ones designed to focus on
learning, finding out, and exploring, and
there is us, the ones who have to take
what we learned in that early period and
use it in our everyday lives.
This division of labor seems to be the
reason we see this correlation between
immaturity in the young and intelligence
in the adult across so many different
species and kinds of animals.
Another way to think about it is using a
computer science analogy. Computer
scientists differentiate between systems
that explore (systems that can learn
easily,try out lot of different hypotheses)
and those that exploit (systems that can
take one thing and do it extremely well).
Computer scientists have discovered that
both exploiting and exploring are crucial
for a system that is going to be able to
learn effectively. In fact, in machine
learning, for example in a computer
learning system, in the beginning the
system starts out by being in the explore
mode, where it tries lots and lots of
different things, exploring lots of
possibilities, and even very unlikely
options. After a while,however,it moves
into the exploit mode, where it chooses
to do the things that are the best and
most effective for itself. Now if this
evolutionary picture is true,I believe that
children are designed to be in this
explore mode.This implies that children
are literally for learning.
The reason why we have children is so
that they can explore the world and find
out all the things they need to do, that
they can then use and exploit as adults.
Now if this were true, we should not be
thinking about children as being sort of
defective grown-ups, or as grown-ups
that do not have some of the wonderful
capacities we have as grown-ups.
A more appropriate way would be to
think of children as being in a specific
developmental phase of the species, that
is different from the phase of being an
adult. To clarify, the difference between
children and adults is rather like the
difference between caterpillars and
butterflies.
In our case, however, the children would
be the butterflies who are fluttering
around, exploring and discovering the
world, while we would be the adult
caterpillars who are humping along our
particular narrow adult path.
Now, if childhood is a different phase in
the developmental history of a species,
we might expect to see some signs of this
difference reflected in the brain
structures of children and adults. It turns
out that we do!
When we look at the development of
neuro connections in the brain,there are a
couple of interesting things that happen
as children grow older. First of all, when
you look at the brain in the early period
when a child is very young, you can see
that many, many new neuro connections
are being formed across the brain.
As the child grows older, in the later
periods of a child’s life, the connections
that are effective continue to be used and
maintained.However,there is a bunch of
other neuro connections called “prude.”
These are neuro connections that are
being or have been dropped off.
Our babies are like
the research and
development division of
the human species, while
adults are like the
production and
marketing division.
The reason
why we have
children is so that they
can explore the world
and find out all the
things they need to do,
that they can then
use and exploit
as adults.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 4
2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 11WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
Further, if you were to plot these
different kinds of neuro connections
along a curve, you will see there is an
early period, when many, many new
connections are being formed,and a later
period, when this formation drops off.
This process of forming and dropping
neuro connections reflects the kinds of
learning that children are doing at
different phases. For example, the
connections in the visual cortex, are
formed by the time children are about a
year old,implying that by then the visual
system has already been established.
Various parts of the brain establish
themselves at different times. For
example,the process of connecting in the
auditory cortex, which is responsible for
language,isn’t done until the children are
five years old, when they learn to speak.
Connections in the prefrontal cortex,the
part of the brain that is like the chief
executive office of the brain, responsible
for things like planning and control, are
not finished and fully matured until the
mid-20s, well into adulthood.
So there is an early period when the
brain seems to be trying out lots of new
things, where there is a great deal of
flexibility.Then gradually, as children get
older, the flexibility decreases as the
brain gets better and better at doing one
thing in particular,till it is not as good at
being flexible and becomes more plastic
at changing or managing to learn new
things.
This evolutionary picture of the brain
reiterates that, especially in the early
period, children are really designed to
explore and to learn. We don’t have to
teach them how to learn or make them
learn; they are designed to learn
spontaneously by themselves.
So,if this is the case,when do we see this
kind of learning? Over the last 15 years
or so, we have discovered that even very
young children are capable of using
complicated statistical patterns to infer
how the world works, to discover the
causal structure of the world.
What’s even more impressive is that
children usually do a set of natural
experiments that allow them to learn
more about how the world actually
works.The way that they do these natural
experiments is by doing what we grown-
ups think of as “just playing around.”
Now it’s been an idea that’s been around
in developmental psychology and in
education, I think intuitively, for a long
time – that children are actually learning
through play; that by playing, by just
exploring the world around them, they
are actually figuring out how the
world works.
Children Are Learning Through Play
Until recently,however,we didn’t actually
have very good systematic scientific
evidence to show that children were
actually learning just through their
spontaneous play. Over the past five or
six years, though, an increasing number
of studies have demonstrated that what
just looks like children’s spontaneous play
is really a kind of research strategy that’s
helps them figure out how the world
around them works.
The study by Christine Legare at the
University of Texas, Austin, is an
example. Christine gave children a
particular kind of problem to solve and
then asked them to explain how that
particular system worked. The system
consisted of a machine made of blocks.
When presented to a little boy, he saw
that some blocks made this machine go,
while others did not. Then he noticed
that the red ones made it move while the
yellow ones didn’t.Then he saw that one
of the yellow ones did not help the
machine move.
So Christine asked him “Can you figure
out why that block didn’t work and can
you figure out why the machine goes the
way it does?” In answer, the boy began
manipulating the blocks systematically,
one after another, observing all the
changes each manipulation resulted in,
and figured out that some machines
needed two blocks to light up while
others needed four!
When the same experiment was repeated
with several other children, all aged
between four and five, they all followed
the same general pattern of
experimentation and manipulation to
reach their conclusions.
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
...there is an early
period when the brain
seems to be trying out
lots of new things, where
there is a great deal of
flexibility. Then gradually,
as children get older,
the flexibility
decreases
...children are actually
learning through play;
that by playing, by just
exploring the world
around them, they are
actually figuring out
how theworld works.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 5
12 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
Thus, when you give the children a real
problem to solve with real objects,
and particularly when you give
them what Christine called an “anomaly”
(something unexpected or unusual) and
ask them to explain it,they produce these
kinds of experimental behaviors in the
very course of their play.
In the experiment, the little boy actually
tested five different hypotheses about
how that block could work in the course
of two minutes of spontaneous play! So,
by giving children a real system,one that
you really had to understand and explain,
or by giving them something
that didn’t quite fit (didn’t suit what they
already believed), you could provoke the
children to produce extremely intelligent
exploratory kinds of behaviors.
In Christine’s experiments, this was true
not just of particularly bright and
articulate children, but about children
in general.
Spontaneous Exploratory Play Is
Actually A Very Systematic Research
Program
Another experiment was done by Laura
Schulz and her colleagues at the
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
(MIT).They wanted to try and test more
systematically the idea that when
children play, they are spontaneously
exploring and finding evidence that
would be relevant to the problems they
are trying to solve.
The team was interested in problems
about causes, problems about what kind
of things make what other things
happen. In their experiments they used
the same block and lighting up of the
machine – the Blicket Detector – that
was used in Christine’s experiment. The
Blicket Detector was invented in my lab
a few years ago and it has turned out to
be incredibly valuable for a very simple
and inexpensive little piece of equipment.
Laura Schulz and her colleagues tried to
see if four year old children would play
differently, would manipulate a toy
differently, depending on what its causal
structure is like. In the first part of the
experiment,they showed the children an
object – a toy – in one of two different
conditions. What the children saw were
a pair of little beads, those that you can
put together to make a single pair.
In the first condition, the children saw
that the two beads can be put together
and when the two beads are together,
they always made the toy go.
They also saw that each of the individual
beads could make the toy go. In the
second condition, the children saw that
some of the individual beads made the
toy go but some individual beads didn’t
make the toy go.
Finally, the children were all given a pair
of beads, and these beads were either
glued together or were two beads that
they could pull apart from each other.
They were then left to play with the toy.
The inherent assumptions about the
causal structure were as follows. If you
had seen that all of the beads always
make the toy go, then you already know
that both of the beads you now have in
the pair are likely to make it go. But if
you had seen that only some of the beads
make it go and others don’t make it go,
then there might be an interesting
question to ask:“Which of the beads will
make the toy go?”
What was discovered was that the
children were much more unlikely to pull
the beads apart in the case where the
causal assumption was that all the beads
made the toy go. When all of the beads
made it go, they simply put the
combined glued pair onto the detector
and did nothing more.
However, in the case where the
assumption was that only some of the
beads made the toy go, the children
pulled the toy apart and then tried one
bead after another on it. This was easily
possible in the case where the children
were presented with two beads that
could be separated.
In the case where the beads were actually
glued together so you couldn’t separate
them, the children used a strategy of
holding and twisting the glued pair so
that one part (one bead) was in touch
with the toy at first, and then the other.
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
They
wanted to try
and test more
systematically the idea
that when children play,
they are spontaneously
exploring and finding
evidence that would be
relevant to the problems
they are trying to
solve.
...when
you give the
children a real problem
to solve with real objects,
and particularly when you
give them what Christine
called an“anomaly”...and ask
them to explain it, they
produce these kinds of
experimental behaviors
in the very course of
their play.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 6
2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 13WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
So even when they couldn’t solve the
problem by pulling the beads apart, they
solved it by moving the beads in one
direction or another.They never did that
when all of the beads had made the toy
go, because there was no need to know
which bead made it go and which bead
did not.
These experimental scenarios clearly
show that what looks like just
spontaneous exploratory play is actually a
very systematic research program that
these very young children are using, to
try to solve problems about the causal
structure of the world around them, to
try and figure out how the world around
them actually works.
These experiments, along with a number
of other recent experiments, show that
even five-year-olds are using their play to
learn and that the method they use looks
a lot like the one that the very brightest,
most intelligent, and most illustrious
scientists use to learn.
If children are learning so much through
their spontaneous enquiries,explorations,
and free play, what does this exploratory
enquiry learning look like when we sit
down to teach children about the world?
In the past few years, we have started
looking much more systematically at the
relationship between learning and
teaching. To illustrate, let us look at a
couple more experiments that have
pitted this kind of spontaneous
exploratory learning against teaching, a
situation where one has an instructor
telling you what you are supposed to do.
Good teachers encourage exploration
anyway but here we are considering the
more common kind of teaching, where
the teacher knows what the right answer
is and is telling the child this before the
child begins the exploration at all.
In the first experiment, conducted in
Laura Shultz’s lab at MIT, they showed
the children a novel toy.This toy has four
interesting properties. The first is that it
has a little squeaker , which if pushed
makes a squeak. Second, it has a light
that lights up when you look in it.Third,
it has a mirror at the end of one of the
tubes in the toy where you can see
reflections.Fourth,there is another piece
that you can push to make music.
So there are four novel things that this
toy can do,but none of them are obvious.
One has to explore a bit to find out that
the toy can do all of these four things.
These toys were given to the children in
two different conditions. In the first
condition, while giving the child the toy
to play with,the experimenter would sort
of accidentally bump into the toy in
a way that one of the four things were
set off.
For example, the toy would squeak. The
experimenter would then say something
like,“Gee! Wonder why that happened!”
Then bump into something else,setting a
second interesting aspect of the
toy off.
Then, the experimenter would simply
leave the toy with the child, to see what
the child would do with it. It was found
that the child would then play around
with the toy and eventually end up
finding the different things the toy
could do, in the course of their
spontaneous play.
The second condition had almost
everything that was the same as the
first, except that this time the
experimenter showed the child the
toy and said, “This is my toy. I’m
going to show you how it works.”
Then the experimenter would show
the child only one of the things that
the toy does and would then leave
the child alone.
In these cases, the experimenters
found that the child did not explore
the toy and find the other things
that the toy does. Instead, the child
would keep doing what the
experimenter had showed him.
From consistent responses to these
scenarios, the experimenters
discovered that when you give the
children the answer, they never
really discover the other
possibilities within the learning
situation. The implications of this
are multiple.
On the one hand,direct instructions and
teaching is a good way of giving
children a single right answer, and
children seem to be sensitive to that.
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
These
experiments,
along with a number
of other recent
experiments, show that
even five-year-olds are using
their play to learn and that the
method they use looks a lot
like the one that the very
brightest, most intelligent,
and most illustrious
scientists use to
learn.
From consistent
responses to these
scenarios, the
experimenters
discovered that when
you give the children
the answer, they never
really discover the
other possibilities
within the learning
situation.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 7
14 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
Children seem to know that if the
teacher tells you “this is the way the toy
works,” then that is the way the toy
works and that’s all there is to the way
the toy works.
On the other hand, it seems that in
this circumstance the children did not
do the kind of wide ranging
exploration that would give them other
answers, that would let them find a
more diverse range of answers for the
same problem.
The experimenters, Laura and Liz,
titled their paper reporting this
experiment “The ‘double-edged sword’
of Pedagogy”.The idea is that pedagogy
is sort of a double-edged sword.On the
one hand, it lets you get a piece of
information across to a child very
effectively. On the other hand, it may
limit the kind of spontaneous
exploration children are capable of.
Children Are Good at Learning
from Complicated Statistical
Patterns.
Another experiment which makes the
same kind of point was done in my lab.
It shows that, in addition to having
capacities for experimentation in
exploration that lets them learn,
children are also very good at learning
from statistical patterns, even quite
complicated kinds of statistical patterns.
This experiment was also to try and see
if children could figure out what it is
that caused what. In this experiment
with four-year-olds we gave them a
musical toy.
The toy has lots of things that you
could do to it – handles and rings to
pull, shake the toy, squash the toy, etc.
In addition, sometimes the toy plays
music, and sometimes it doesn’t.
When the experimenter gave the toy
to the child, she would produce three
different actions. For example, she
would pull the handle,and then squash
the toy, and then shake the toy. When
she did this, sometimes the machine
would play music and sometimes it
wouldn’t. The children saw ten
sequences like this; ten combinations
of different actions that sometimes led
to an answer and sometimes didn’t. At
the end of it, the experimenter would
give the child the toy and simply ask
the child to have a go at the toy itself.
What the experimenters were looking
to record was what the child would
decide to do. Which actions would
the child produce to make the toy
actually go?
Here are the different kind of patterns
of actions and outcomes the children
got to see.If you were a statistician,you
would see that only the last two
actions, the B and C actions, were
necessary to make the toy go in the BC
Condition.
You would conclude that the most
likely alternative is that only the last
two actions were necessary to make the
machine go. If you were looking at the
other patterns of results that we saw,
you’d draw different conclusions.
If for instance,you saw that a particular
sequence of the three actions always
made it go, you might conclude that
you needed all three actions to make it
go. Or, if you saw that only the last
action made it go, the sensible thing
would be to conclude that, in C
Condition, only the last action was
necessary to make it go.
The question that we were asking
ourselves was,“Would the children use
this statistical pattern to figure out a
really intelligent solution to this
problem?” The most intelligent
solution was to produce just the two
actions by themselves, even though
they had seen that demonstrated by
the experimenter.
What we observed was that when a
child is shown a complicated pattern
of actions, one which made the toy
play music and another which resulted
in no music, they pulled out the right
solution, which was that only the last
two actions were necessary to produce
the music.
This was a sign of that children,
spontaneously behaving in this
incredibly intelligent way, are able to
analyze data that even adults have a
hard time doing, in some
circumstances.
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
The idea is that
pedagogy is sort of a
double-edged sword. On
the one hand, it lets you get
a piece of information across
to a child very effectively. On
the other hand, it may limit
the kind of spontaneous
exploration children
are capable of.
...in
addition to
having capacities for
experimentation in
exploration that lets them
learn, children are also very
good at learning from
statistical patterns, even
quite complicated
kinds of statistical
patterns.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 8
2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 15WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN
Educator
Then we tried to do a somewhat different
version of the experiment. This time,
when the experimenter gave the child
the musical toy, she would say, “This is
my toy and I don’t even know how it
works…”
Then in another version, we did the
experiment exactly the same way except
this time we told the child, “This is my
toy and I’m going to show you how it
works.”Whenever we did it this way,the
children always just imitated exactly
what the experimenter did and did no
exploration.
They did exactly the same set of three
actions that the experimenter had
performed, but they literally never
produced the more intelligent solution,
of just using the last two actions, by
themselves. Yet again, there seems to be
a kind of double-edged sword regarding
teaching or directly instructing a child.
Directly instructing a child may get them
to one solution, the one answer that you
think is the right answer, but it may
actually prevent them from spontaneously
getting to the really intelligent solutions,
which they would get to if just you let
them exercise their own natural
spontaneous learning abilities.
For people who are teaching, this
suggests that we have a great
opportunity, because children (even the
very young ones) are equipped by
evolution with these extremely powerful
learning abilities. In fact, that is what
children are for: children are for learning.
It is not just a nice coincidence that they
have these wonderful learning abilities.
The very reason they are children is to
give them a chance to have these
powerful learning abilities. What we
would suggest for schools, is to
encourage and allow these learning
abilities to blossom, to get children to
apply these learning abilities to a very
wide range of subjects and to a wide
range of domains.However,what we end
up doing is the opposite – squelching
those learning abilities.
This is not to say that there isn’t a place
for giving children direct instructions.In
fact one of the things that our
experiments show is that children as
young as four and five are already
sensitive to the fact that a teacher knows
something that they may not know, and
that they should learn differently when
they are learning from a teacher.
However, if that be the only way of
teaching children, it is not really taking
advantage of these extremely powerful,
inbuilt, evolutionarily determined brain
mechanisms that are there, that are
allowing them to learn as much as they
actually do. If we are going to teach
children the way that they are designed
to learn,what we need to do is give them
opportunities to explore,to enquire,to do
the sort of research and development
that they were actually designed for. n
Adapted from a talk by Alison Gopnik at the XSEED
School of Tomorrow Conference 2011
SCHOOL OF
TOMORROW
November 2011
2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m.
The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education
Special Feature
Directly instructing a
child may get them to
one solution, the one
answer that you think
is the right answer, but
it may actually prevent
them from
spontaneously getting
to the really intelligent
solutions
ALISOn GOPnIk is a professor of
psychology and an affiliate
professor of philosophy at the
University of California, Berkeley.
An internationally recognized
leader in the study of children’s
learning and development, she was
the first to argue that children’s
minds could help us understand
deep philosophical questions.
She is the author of over a hundred
journal articles and of several
books, including the best-selling
and critically acclaimed books, “The
Scientist in the Crib” and “The
Philosophical Baby.”
She has also written widely about
cognitive science and psychology
for Science, The Times Literary
Supplement, The New York Review of
Books, and The New York Times.
MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 9

More Related Content

Similar to THE EVOLUTIONARY ROLE OF CHILDHOOD

Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental Psych
Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental PsychChapter 4 ap psych- Developmental Psych
Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental PsychDr. J's AP Psych Class
 
Unleash the genius in your child
Unleash the genius in your childUnleash the genius in your child
Unleash the genius in your childshiva lal
 
Development_ppt2.ppt
Development_ppt2.pptDevelopment_ppt2.ppt
Development_ppt2.pptMusaargungu
 
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docx
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docxWeek One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docx
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docxtroutmanboris
 
Science for young children ppt
Science for young children pptScience for young children ppt
Science for young children pptirishedu
 
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)Roger Yates
 
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For College
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For CollegeCaptivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For College
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For CollegeNat Rice
 
Piaget's theory of cognitive development
Piaget's theory of cognitive developmentPiaget's theory of cognitive development
Piaget's theory of cognitive developmentDrGMSunagar1
 
1 piaget's theary of cognitive development
1 piaget's theary of cognitive development1 piaget's theary of cognitive development
1 piaget's theary of cognitive developmentDrGMSunagar
 
The Universe of Babies by Caleb Gattegno
The Universe of Babies by Caleb GattegnoThe Universe of Babies by Caleb Gattegno
The Universe of Babies by Caleb GattegnoEducational Solutions
 

Similar to THE EVOLUTIONARY ROLE OF CHILDHOOD (16)

kgavura 5 development
kgavura 5 developmentkgavura 5 development
kgavura 5 development
 
Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental Psych
Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental PsychChapter 4 ap psych- Developmental Psych
Chapter 4 ap psych- Developmental Psych
 
Unleash the genius in your child
Unleash the genius in your childUnleash the genius in your child
Unleash the genius in your child
 
Development_ppt2.ppt
Development_ppt2.pptDevelopment_ppt2.ppt
Development_ppt2.ppt
 
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docx
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docxWeek One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docx
Week One Learning ResourcesThe following are required readings .docx
 
Science for young children ppt
Science for young children pptScience for young children ppt
Science for young children ppt
 
science of people
science of peoplescience of people
science of people
 
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)
An introduction to Animal Education Outreach (Ireland)
 
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For College
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For CollegeCaptivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For College
Captivating Problem Solution Essay Topics For College
 
An Essay On Science
An Essay On ScienceAn Essay On Science
An Essay On Science
 
An Essay On Science
An Essay On ScienceAn Essay On Science
An Essay On Science
 
Chapter4
Chapter4Chapter4
Chapter4
 
Ghulam abass jamali
Ghulam abass jamaliGhulam abass jamali
Ghulam abass jamali
 
Piaget's theory of cognitive development
Piaget's theory of cognitive developmentPiaget's theory of cognitive development
Piaget's theory of cognitive development
 
1 piaget's theary of cognitive development
1 piaget's theary of cognitive development1 piaget's theary of cognitive development
1 piaget's theary of cognitive development
 
The Universe of Babies by Caleb Gattegno
The Universe of Babies by Caleb GattegnoThe Universe of Babies by Caleb Gattegno
The Universe of Babies by Caleb Gattegno
 

Recently uploaded

Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Shubhangi Sonawane
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxNikitaBankoti2
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docxPoojaSen20
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptRamjanShidvankar
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 

THE EVOLUTIONARY ROLE OF CHILDHOOD

  • 1. 08 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature THE EVOLUTIONARY ROLE OF CHILDHOOD Children learn about the world just as scientists do – making predictions, conducting experiments, analyzing statistics and forming theories. Drawing on her most recent research, renowned scholar and best selling author, Alison Gopnik, demonstrates how cognitive abilities of babies and young children far surpasses what adults normally attribute them with. Illustration by Amruta Patil Some of the research my colleagues and I have done over the last thirty years has completely revolutionised the way we think about babies and young children, and herefore the way we think about basic human learning capacities. What we have discovered is that even the very youngest of babies and very young children have capacities to learn that are greater than those of the most illustrious of scientists. Discovering these tremendous natural capacities to learn ought to influence the way we teach children. If we think that education is all about trying to encourage learning, then knowing something about just how powerful these natural learning capacities are, should help us to improve our educational system. What I am going to do today is talk about the fundamental science of learning that has emerged, especially over the past ten years. Let’s start with one of those questions that you think people would ask, but they rarely do: “Why is it that we have a period of childhood at all?” In fact, “why do we have children at all?” If you think about it, at least from some perspectives, you could argue that babies and young children are useless after all.They don’t go out and work; they don’t bring home the bacon; in fact, in some ways, they are worse than useless because we grown-ups have to spend so much of our time and energy just keeping them alive and taking care of them. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 2
  • 2. 2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 09WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator So, from a scientific perspective, one of the questions we want to ask is, “Why, from an evolutionary perspective, do we actually have this period of childhood at all?”; “Why does childhood even exist?” It’s a particularly interesting question for us because human beings have a much longer period of childhood – a much longer period of immaturity – than any other species. So, our babies and children are dependent on us for much longer than the children of any other species are dependent on their grown-ups. My oldest son is twenty two, and even now we,as his parents,continue investing our resources in taking care of him. It turns out that when we look at evolution, there is something of an explanation for this. Consider two animals. On the one hand, there is the crow, in particular the new Caledonian crow.Crows,ravens and other animals of the Corvidae family are extremely intelligent animals.The new Caledonian crow was featured on the cover of Science magazine because it has learned how to use a tool, how to bend a wire in order to be able to get at a piece of food. Coincidently, crows have extremely long periods of immaturity as well; crow babies are immature, helpless and dependent fledglings for as long as a year. In the case of Caledonian crows, this period lasts as long as two years, which is a very, very long time in the life of a crow. So these are animals that are extremely intelligent, rely a lot on learning, have relatively large brains, are very flexible, and are “smart animals.” On the other hand,we have the domestic chicken. It represents chickens, turkeys, geese, ducks and all of the birds in this family. With apologies to all chicken lovers, these birds are basically dumb. Though these animals are extremely good at pecking for grain, they are not much good at doing anything else. Note that chicken babies mature within a space of few weeks or months. What we observe is that, there seems to be a correlation between the length of the period of immaturity in the young (how long the babies are dependent and helpless) and the flexibility and intelligence in them as adult animals. Further, this seems true not only for birds. Recent reports regarding marsupials are echoing this observation. Marsupials are animals like kangaroos and wombats, found mostly in Australia and New Zealand. Among marsupials, the longer the babies live in the mother’s pouch, the larger the size of the adult brain. There is another interesting aspect to this story: when babies are immature for such long periods, the adults have to put in “greater parental investment.” So the longer the period of immaturity, the larger the brain and the better the adult is at learning, the more the time and energy that the parents invest in raising those babies. This is once again evident when you compare quokkas and opossums. Quokkas are marsupials with very large brains and generally both the parent quokkas are engaged in taking care of just one baby. Opossums, on the other hand, are much more small-brained marsupials, and they often have many more babies and there is very little opportunity for parental investment. The question that arises now is: “Why would you see this correlation between how smart the adult animal is,how much the adult animal relies on learning, and how long a period of immaturity the adult undergoes as a baby?” Animals use a number of strategies to survive. Some animals, like the chicken and the opossums,might be very good at doing just one thing over and over again. They are very well-suited for one particular evolutionary task. Other creatures like the crows and human beings are not good at doing anything in particular, but are adept at learning to do new things, things that will suit whatever environment they find themselves in. In this manner, we learn to be able to do all the things that we need to do, to survive as animals. The latter is a very good strategy in a lot of ways. Very characteristic of human beings, it is this strategy that has allowed us to exist and It’s a particularly interesting question for us because human beings have a much longer period of childhood – a much longer period of immaturity – than any other species. So the longer the period of immaturity, the larger the brain and the better the adult is at learning, the more the time and energy that the parents invest in raising those babies. SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 3
  • 3. 10 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature survive in more environments than any other animal, including in outer space. However, this strategy has one great drawback: Until you do all of the learning you have to do, you’re going to be helpless. For example, suppose a mastodon is charging at you. At that moment, you cannot say to yourself, “What shall I do about this? Should I use a stick or a slingshot? What would be more effective?.” Instead, you should have already figured out how to deal with the situation, before the mastodons actually show up. So, how and when do we learn all that needs to be learned? Evolution seems to have solved this problem by developing a kind of division of labor.As a result,we have the early protected period of childhood (in which all we have to do is to learn) and then we have adulthood (when we take the things we learned as very young children and put them to use, to solve problems and do all the things that we need to do as adults). Our babies are like the research and development division of the human species, while adults are like the production and marketing division. So, there are the ones designed to focus on learning, finding out, and exploring, and there is us, the ones who have to take what we learned in that early period and use it in our everyday lives. This division of labor seems to be the reason we see this correlation between immaturity in the young and intelligence in the adult across so many different species and kinds of animals. Another way to think about it is using a computer science analogy. Computer scientists differentiate between systems that explore (systems that can learn easily,try out lot of different hypotheses) and those that exploit (systems that can take one thing and do it extremely well). Computer scientists have discovered that both exploiting and exploring are crucial for a system that is going to be able to learn effectively. In fact, in machine learning, for example in a computer learning system, in the beginning the system starts out by being in the explore mode, where it tries lots and lots of different things, exploring lots of possibilities, and even very unlikely options. After a while,however,it moves into the exploit mode, where it chooses to do the things that are the best and most effective for itself. Now if this evolutionary picture is true,I believe that children are designed to be in this explore mode.This implies that children are literally for learning. The reason why we have children is so that they can explore the world and find out all the things they need to do, that they can then use and exploit as adults. Now if this were true, we should not be thinking about children as being sort of defective grown-ups, or as grown-ups that do not have some of the wonderful capacities we have as grown-ups. A more appropriate way would be to think of children as being in a specific developmental phase of the species, that is different from the phase of being an adult. To clarify, the difference between children and adults is rather like the difference between caterpillars and butterflies. In our case, however, the children would be the butterflies who are fluttering around, exploring and discovering the world, while we would be the adult caterpillars who are humping along our particular narrow adult path. Now, if childhood is a different phase in the developmental history of a species, we might expect to see some signs of this difference reflected in the brain structures of children and adults. It turns out that we do! When we look at the development of neuro connections in the brain,there are a couple of interesting things that happen as children grow older. First of all, when you look at the brain in the early period when a child is very young, you can see that many, many new neuro connections are being formed across the brain. As the child grows older, in the later periods of a child’s life, the connections that are effective continue to be used and maintained.However,there is a bunch of other neuro connections called “prude.” These are neuro connections that are being or have been dropped off. Our babies are like the research and development division of the human species, while adults are like the production and marketing division. The reason why we have children is so that they can explore the world and find out all the things they need to do, that they can then use and exploit as adults. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 4
  • 4. 2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 11WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator Further, if you were to plot these different kinds of neuro connections along a curve, you will see there is an early period, when many, many new connections are being formed,and a later period, when this formation drops off. This process of forming and dropping neuro connections reflects the kinds of learning that children are doing at different phases. For example, the connections in the visual cortex, are formed by the time children are about a year old,implying that by then the visual system has already been established. Various parts of the brain establish themselves at different times. For example,the process of connecting in the auditory cortex, which is responsible for language,isn’t done until the children are five years old, when they learn to speak. Connections in the prefrontal cortex,the part of the brain that is like the chief executive office of the brain, responsible for things like planning and control, are not finished and fully matured until the mid-20s, well into adulthood. So there is an early period when the brain seems to be trying out lots of new things, where there is a great deal of flexibility.Then gradually, as children get older, the flexibility decreases as the brain gets better and better at doing one thing in particular,till it is not as good at being flexible and becomes more plastic at changing or managing to learn new things. This evolutionary picture of the brain reiterates that, especially in the early period, children are really designed to explore and to learn. We don’t have to teach them how to learn or make them learn; they are designed to learn spontaneously by themselves. So,if this is the case,when do we see this kind of learning? Over the last 15 years or so, we have discovered that even very young children are capable of using complicated statistical patterns to infer how the world works, to discover the causal structure of the world. What’s even more impressive is that children usually do a set of natural experiments that allow them to learn more about how the world actually works.The way that they do these natural experiments is by doing what we grown- ups think of as “just playing around.” Now it’s been an idea that’s been around in developmental psychology and in education, I think intuitively, for a long time – that children are actually learning through play; that by playing, by just exploring the world around them, they are actually figuring out how the world works. Children Are Learning Through Play Until recently,however,we didn’t actually have very good systematic scientific evidence to show that children were actually learning just through their spontaneous play. Over the past five or six years, though, an increasing number of studies have demonstrated that what just looks like children’s spontaneous play is really a kind of research strategy that’s helps them figure out how the world around them works. The study by Christine Legare at the University of Texas, Austin, is an example. Christine gave children a particular kind of problem to solve and then asked them to explain how that particular system worked. The system consisted of a machine made of blocks. When presented to a little boy, he saw that some blocks made this machine go, while others did not. Then he noticed that the red ones made it move while the yellow ones didn’t.Then he saw that one of the yellow ones did not help the machine move. So Christine asked him “Can you figure out why that block didn’t work and can you figure out why the machine goes the way it does?” In answer, the boy began manipulating the blocks systematically, one after another, observing all the changes each manipulation resulted in, and figured out that some machines needed two blocks to light up while others needed four! When the same experiment was repeated with several other children, all aged between four and five, they all followed the same general pattern of experimentation and manipulation to reach their conclusions. SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature ...there is an early period when the brain seems to be trying out lots of new things, where there is a great deal of flexibility. Then gradually, as children get older, the flexibility decreases ...children are actually learning through play; that by playing, by just exploring the world around them, they are actually figuring out how theworld works. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 5
  • 5. 12 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator Thus, when you give the children a real problem to solve with real objects, and particularly when you give them what Christine called an “anomaly” (something unexpected or unusual) and ask them to explain it,they produce these kinds of experimental behaviors in the very course of their play. In the experiment, the little boy actually tested five different hypotheses about how that block could work in the course of two minutes of spontaneous play! So, by giving children a real system,one that you really had to understand and explain, or by giving them something that didn’t quite fit (didn’t suit what they already believed), you could provoke the children to produce extremely intelligent exploratory kinds of behaviors. In Christine’s experiments, this was true not just of particularly bright and articulate children, but about children in general. Spontaneous Exploratory Play Is Actually A Very Systematic Research Program Another experiment was done by Laura Schulz and her colleagues at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT).They wanted to try and test more systematically the idea that when children play, they are spontaneously exploring and finding evidence that would be relevant to the problems they are trying to solve. The team was interested in problems about causes, problems about what kind of things make what other things happen. In their experiments they used the same block and lighting up of the machine – the Blicket Detector – that was used in Christine’s experiment. The Blicket Detector was invented in my lab a few years ago and it has turned out to be incredibly valuable for a very simple and inexpensive little piece of equipment. Laura Schulz and her colleagues tried to see if four year old children would play differently, would manipulate a toy differently, depending on what its causal structure is like. In the first part of the experiment,they showed the children an object – a toy – in one of two different conditions. What the children saw were a pair of little beads, those that you can put together to make a single pair. In the first condition, the children saw that the two beads can be put together and when the two beads are together, they always made the toy go. They also saw that each of the individual beads could make the toy go. In the second condition, the children saw that some of the individual beads made the toy go but some individual beads didn’t make the toy go. Finally, the children were all given a pair of beads, and these beads were either glued together or were two beads that they could pull apart from each other. They were then left to play with the toy. The inherent assumptions about the causal structure were as follows. If you had seen that all of the beads always make the toy go, then you already know that both of the beads you now have in the pair are likely to make it go. But if you had seen that only some of the beads make it go and others don’t make it go, then there might be an interesting question to ask:“Which of the beads will make the toy go?” What was discovered was that the children were much more unlikely to pull the beads apart in the case where the causal assumption was that all the beads made the toy go. When all of the beads made it go, they simply put the combined glued pair onto the detector and did nothing more. However, in the case where the assumption was that only some of the beads made the toy go, the children pulled the toy apart and then tried one bead after another on it. This was easily possible in the case where the children were presented with two beads that could be separated. In the case where the beads were actually glued together so you couldn’t separate them, the children used a strategy of holding and twisting the glued pair so that one part (one bead) was in touch with the toy at first, and then the other. SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature They wanted to try and test more systematically the idea that when children play, they are spontaneously exploring and finding evidence that would be relevant to the problems they are trying to solve. ...when you give the children a real problem to solve with real objects, and particularly when you give them what Christine called an“anomaly”...and ask them to explain it, they produce these kinds of experimental behaviors in the very course of their play. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 6
  • 6. 2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 13WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator So even when they couldn’t solve the problem by pulling the beads apart, they solved it by moving the beads in one direction or another.They never did that when all of the beads had made the toy go, because there was no need to know which bead made it go and which bead did not. These experimental scenarios clearly show that what looks like just spontaneous exploratory play is actually a very systematic research program that these very young children are using, to try to solve problems about the causal structure of the world around them, to try and figure out how the world around them actually works. These experiments, along with a number of other recent experiments, show that even five-year-olds are using their play to learn and that the method they use looks a lot like the one that the very brightest, most intelligent, and most illustrious scientists use to learn. If children are learning so much through their spontaneous enquiries,explorations, and free play, what does this exploratory enquiry learning look like when we sit down to teach children about the world? In the past few years, we have started looking much more systematically at the relationship between learning and teaching. To illustrate, let us look at a couple more experiments that have pitted this kind of spontaneous exploratory learning against teaching, a situation where one has an instructor telling you what you are supposed to do. Good teachers encourage exploration anyway but here we are considering the more common kind of teaching, where the teacher knows what the right answer is and is telling the child this before the child begins the exploration at all. In the first experiment, conducted in Laura Shultz’s lab at MIT, they showed the children a novel toy.This toy has four interesting properties. The first is that it has a little squeaker , which if pushed makes a squeak. Second, it has a light that lights up when you look in it.Third, it has a mirror at the end of one of the tubes in the toy where you can see reflections.Fourth,there is another piece that you can push to make music. So there are four novel things that this toy can do,but none of them are obvious. One has to explore a bit to find out that the toy can do all of these four things. These toys were given to the children in two different conditions. In the first condition, while giving the child the toy to play with,the experimenter would sort of accidentally bump into the toy in a way that one of the four things were set off. For example, the toy would squeak. The experimenter would then say something like,“Gee! Wonder why that happened!” Then bump into something else,setting a second interesting aspect of the toy off. Then, the experimenter would simply leave the toy with the child, to see what the child would do with it. It was found that the child would then play around with the toy and eventually end up finding the different things the toy could do, in the course of their spontaneous play. The second condition had almost everything that was the same as the first, except that this time the experimenter showed the child the toy and said, “This is my toy. I’m going to show you how it works.” Then the experimenter would show the child only one of the things that the toy does and would then leave the child alone. In these cases, the experimenters found that the child did not explore the toy and find the other things that the toy does. Instead, the child would keep doing what the experimenter had showed him. From consistent responses to these scenarios, the experimenters discovered that when you give the children the answer, they never really discover the other possibilities within the learning situation. The implications of this are multiple. On the one hand,direct instructions and teaching is a good way of giving children a single right answer, and children seem to be sensitive to that. SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature These experiments, along with a number of other recent experiments, show that even five-year-olds are using their play to learn and that the method they use looks a lot like the one that the very brightest, most intelligent, and most illustrious scientists use to learn. From consistent responses to these scenarios, the experimenters discovered that when you give the children the answer, they never really discover the other possibilities within the learning situation. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 7
  • 7. 14 MINDFIELDS | JUNE 2012 WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator Children seem to know that if the teacher tells you “this is the way the toy works,” then that is the way the toy works and that’s all there is to the way the toy works. On the other hand, it seems that in this circumstance the children did not do the kind of wide ranging exploration that would give them other answers, that would let them find a more diverse range of answers for the same problem. The experimenters, Laura and Liz, titled their paper reporting this experiment “The ‘double-edged sword’ of Pedagogy”.The idea is that pedagogy is sort of a double-edged sword.On the one hand, it lets you get a piece of information across to a child very effectively. On the other hand, it may limit the kind of spontaneous exploration children are capable of. Children Are Good at Learning from Complicated Statistical Patterns. Another experiment which makes the same kind of point was done in my lab. It shows that, in addition to having capacities for experimentation in exploration that lets them learn, children are also very good at learning from statistical patterns, even quite complicated kinds of statistical patterns. This experiment was also to try and see if children could figure out what it is that caused what. In this experiment with four-year-olds we gave them a musical toy. The toy has lots of things that you could do to it – handles and rings to pull, shake the toy, squash the toy, etc. In addition, sometimes the toy plays music, and sometimes it doesn’t. When the experimenter gave the toy to the child, she would produce three different actions. For example, she would pull the handle,and then squash the toy, and then shake the toy. When she did this, sometimes the machine would play music and sometimes it wouldn’t. The children saw ten sequences like this; ten combinations of different actions that sometimes led to an answer and sometimes didn’t. At the end of it, the experimenter would give the child the toy and simply ask the child to have a go at the toy itself. What the experimenters were looking to record was what the child would decide to do. Which actions would the child produce to make the toy actually go? Here are the different kind of patterns of actions and outcomes the children got to see.If you were a statistician,you would see that only the last two actions, the B and C actions, were necessary to make the toy go in the BC Condition. You would conclude that the most likely alternative is that only the last two actions were necessary to make the machine go. If you were looking at the other patterns of results that we saw, you’d draw different conclusions. If for instance,you saw that a particular sequence of the three actions always made it go, you might conclude that you needed all three actions to make it go. Or, if you saw that only the last action made it go, the sensible thing would be to conclude that, in C Condition, only the last action was necessary to make it go. The question that we were asking ourselves was,“Would the children use this statistical pattern to figure out a really intelligent solution to this problem?” The most intelligent solution was to produce just the two actions by themselves, even though they had seen that demonstrated by the experimenter. What we observed was that when a child is shown a complicated pattern of actions, one which made the toy play music and another which resulted in no music, they pulled out the right solution, which was that only the last two actions were necessary to produce the music. This was a sign of that children, spontaneously behaving in this incredibly intelligent way, are able to analyze data that even adults have a hard time doing, in some circumstances. SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature The idea is that pedagogy is sort of a double-edged sword. On the one hand, it lets you get a piece of information across to a child very effectively. On the other hand, it may limit the kind of spontaneous exploration children are capable of. ...in addition to having capacities for experimentation in exploration that lets them learn, children are also very good at learning from statistical patterns, even quite complicated kinds of statistical patterns. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 8
  • 8. 2012 JUNE | MINDFIELDS 15WWW.MINDFIELDS.IN Educator Then we tried to do a somewhat different version of the experiment. This time, when the experimenter gave the child the musical toy, she would say, “This is my toy and I don’t even know how it works…” Then in another version, we did the experiment exactly the same way except this time we told the child, “This is my toy and I’m going to show you how it works.”Whenever we did it this way,the children always just imitated exactly what the experimenter did and did no exploration. They did exactly the same set of three actions that the experimenter had performed, but they literally never produced the more intelligent solution, of just using the last two actions, by themselves. Yet again, there seems to be a kind of double-edged sword regarding teaching or directly instructing a child. Directly instructing a child may get them to one solution, the one answer that you think is the right answer, but it may actually prevent them from spontaneously getting to the really intelligent solutions, which they would get to if just you let them exercise their own natural spontaneous learning abilities. For people who are teaching, this suggests that we have a great opportunity, because children (even the very young ones) are equipped by evolution with these extremely powerful learning abilities. In fact, that is what children are for: children are for learning. It is not just a nice coincidence that they have these wonderful learning abilities. The very reason they are children is to give them a chance to have these powerful learning abilities. What we would suggest for schools, is to encourage and allow these learning abilities to blossom, to get children to apply these learning abilities to a very wide range of subjects and to a wide range of domains.However,what we end up doing is the opposite – squelching those learning abilities. This is not to say that there isn’t a place for giving children direct instructions.In fact one of the things that our experiments show is that children as young as four and five are already sensitive to the fact that a teacher knows something that they may not know, and that they should learn differently when they are learning from a teacher. However, if that be the only way of teaching children, it is not really taking advantage of these extremely powerful, inbuilt, evolutionarily determined brain mechanisms that are there, that are allowing them to learn as much as they actually do. If we are going to teach children the way that they are designed to learn,what we need to do is give them opportunities to explore,to enquire,to do the sort of research and development that they were actually designed for. n Adapted from a talk by Alison Gopnik at the XSEED School of Tomorrow Conference 2011 SCHOOL OF TOMORROW November 2011 2.00 p.m. to 9.00 p.m. The XSEED Conference on the Future of School Education Special Feature Directly instructing a child may get them to one solution, the one answer that you think is the right answer, but it may actually prevent them from spontaneously getting to the really intelligent solutions ALISOn GOPnIk is a professor of psychology and an affiliate professor of philosophy at the University of California, Berkeley. An internationally recognized leader in the study of children’s learning and development, she was the first to argue that children’s minds could help us understand deep philosophical questions. She is the author of over a hundred journal articles and of several books, including the best-selling and critically acclaimed books, “The Scientist in the Crib” and “The Philosophical Baby.” She has also written widely about cognitive science and psychology for Science, The Times Literary Supplement, The New York Review of Books, and The New York Times. MF16 Pg 08-15 Gopnik:Layout 2 11/07/12 9:36 AM Page 9