SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 118
@charset "utf-8";
@font-face {
font-family: Champagne;
src: url(cac_champagne.woff) format('woff'),
url(cac_champagne.ttf) format('ttf');
}
/*
New Perspectives on HTML5 and CSS3, 7th Edition
Tutorial 3
Review Assignment
Typographical Style Sheet for the Pandaisia Chocolates
Monthly Specials
Filename: pc_styles4.css
*/
html {
background-color: rgb(186, 136, 81);
}
/* Page Body Styles */
body {
background-color: rgb(138, 97, 51);
font-family: Verdana, Geneva, Arial, sans-serif;
}
/* Horizontal Navigation List Styles */
nav.horizontal a {
background-color: rgb(113, 86, 39);
font-family: Champagne, cursive;
font-size: 1.4em;
line-height: 1.8em;
text-align: center;
}
nav.horizontal a:link,
nav.horizontal a:visited {
color: rgb(239, 220, 156);
}
nav.horizontal a:hover,
nav.horizontal a:active {
background-color: rgb(71, 52, 29);
}
/* Article Styles */
article > h1 {
font-family: 'Times New Roman', Times, serif;
font-size: 2.2em;
font-weight: normal;
margin: 0.5em 0;
}
article p {
line-height: 1.2;
margin-bottom: 2em;
}
/* Specials Styles */
div.specials {
background-color: rgba(255, 255, 255, 0.15);
}
div.specials h1 {
font-family: Champagne, cursive;
font-size: 1.8em;
font-weight: normal;
margin: 0.3em 0;
text-align: center;
}
div.specials p {
font-size: 0.8em;
margin-bottom: 1em;
line-height: 1.2;
}
div.specials a {
color: rgb(186, 186, 156);
}
/* Award Styles */
div#awardList {
background-color: rgb(172, 116, 30);
margin-top: 20px;
}
div#awardList h1 {
font-family: Champagne, cursive;
font-size: 2.5em;
font-weight: normal;
text-align: center;
}
div.awards p {
font-size: 0.8em;
text-align: center;
margin: 0.3em 0;
}
div.awards img {
display: block;
width: 100%;
}
/* Footer Styles */
footer {
color: rgb(186, 186, 156);
background-color: rgb(71, 52, 29);
font-size: 0.9em;
padding: 10px 0px;
text-align: center;
}
Identifying and Training
Adaptive Cross-Cultural
Management Skills:
The Crucial Role of
Cultural Metacognition
SHIRA MOR
Erasmus University
MICHAEL MORRIS
Columbia University
JOHANN JOH
London Business School
For managers, intercultural effectiveness requires forging close
working relationships
with people from different cultural backgrounds (Black,
Mendenhall, & Oddou, 1991).
Recent research with executives has found that higher cultural
metacognition is
associated with affective closeness and creative collaboration in
intercultural
relationships (Chua, Morris, & Mor, & 2012). However, little is
known about the social
cognitive mechanisms that facilitate the performance of
individuals who score high on
cultural metacognition. We propose that one important question
for cross-cultural
research and training is identifying which metacognitive
strategies enable successful
intercultural collaborations. We suggest that one such strategy
is “cultural perspective
taking”— considering how another’s cultural background shapes
their behavior in a
given context. We hypothesized that cultural perspective taking
facilitates intercultural
coordination and cooperation, and that a manipulation that
boosts cultural perspective
taking would be especially beneficial for individuals who score
low in dispositional
cultural metacognition. We found support for the above
hypotheses in five studies using
both quasi-field and experimental approaches. We discuss the
implications of these
findings for literatures on expatriate managers, cross-cultural
training, cultural
intelligence, and intercultural negotiations.
...............................................................................................
.........................................................................
“For him to have understood me would have
meant reorganizing his thinking . . . . giving
up his intellectual ballast, and few people are
willing to risk such a radical move.”
–Edward T. Hall
The globalization of business presses managers to
communicate, negotiate, and collaborate across
cultures (Adler & Gundersen, 2008; Boyacigiller &
Adler, 1991). Firms depend on informal coordina-
tion between managers located in different parts
We thank Adam Galinsky and Modupe Akinola for their help
with data collection for Study 3A. We are grateful to Daniel
Ames, E. Tory Higgins, Shirley Kopelman, Roy Chua, Krishna
Savani, and Aaron Wallen for providing their comments on
earlier drafts of this manuscript.
� Academy of Management Learning & Education, 2013, Vol.
12, No. 3, 453–475. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amle.2012.0202
...............................................................................................
.........................................................................
453
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved.
Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or
otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s
express written permission. Users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use only.
of the organization (Barnard, 1968). In global firms,
this requires intercultural collaboration. While ex-
patriate assignments can facilitate the transfer of
knowledge across borders, expatriates often face
difficulties in adjusting to the foreign culture
(Caruso, Epley, & Bazerman, 2006), which can result
in early termination of assignments (Graham,
1985). Other mechanisms for coordination in global
organizations are multinational teams and inter-
national alliances (Adler & Gundersen, 2008; By-
rne, 1993; Manz & Sims, 1987). Yet such structures
often run aground on the failure of managers from
different cultures and countries to work effectively
with one another (Earley & Gibson, 2002; Hagel &
Brown, 2005).
Firms have long sought employees with cross-
cultural capabilities but have not known how best
to select for them or develop them. Recently, man-
agement researchers integrated disparate insights
about relevant characteristics and capabilities un-
der the rubric of cultural intelligence (CQ; Earley &
Ang, 2003; Earley & Peterson, 2004; Ng, Van Dyne, &
Ang, 2009). Earley and Ang (2003) define cultural
intelligence as the capability of an individual to
function effectively in culturally diverse settings.
We suggest that cultural intelligence is an individ-
ual difference that can inform the field’s renewed
interest in cross-cultural research and training
programs (Black & Mendenhall, 1990; Mendenhall,
Stahl, Ehnert, Oddou, Osland, & Kuhlmann, 2004;
Rehg, Gundlach, & Grigorian, 2012). More specifi-
cally, the present research aims to bridge research
and practice by identifying specific social cogni-
tive strategies associated with cultural intelli-
gence, strategies that might be targeted by inter-
cultural training programs. We first highlight the
contribution of our approach to cross-cultural
training and past and ongoing research.
NOVEL APPROACH TO CROSS-CULTURAL
TRAINING AND RESEARCH
Our research offers a novel approach to cross-
cultural research and training by examining an
individual difference measure associated with in-
tercultural success and its associated social cog-
nitive mechanisms. We propose that this approach
can contribute to researchers and practitioners’ un-
derstanding of cross-cultural competency and
training in a number of ways. First, we look for
trainable strategies through a descriptive ap-
proach of identifying thought patterns associated
with an individual difference dimension that is
known to correlate with intercultural effectiveness.
Second, we examine social cognitive mechanisms
associated with intercultural performance. Third,
we examine intercultural performance in the con-
text of working in the home country, not just in
overseas assignments. Notably, in contrast to prior
training programs focused on teaching managers
culture-specific knowledge (Fowler & Blohm, 2004;
Mendenhall et al., 2004), we focus on general strat-
egies for dealing with cultural differences, not on
training specific to particular cultures. We intro-
duce our approach and hypotheses by first review-
ing the literature on cultural intelligence and the
specific role of cultural metacognition in promot-
ing intercultural performance.
CULTURAL METACOGNITION
Ang and Van Dyne (2008) have proposed four cen-
tral dimensions that foster cultural intelligence:
metacognitive, cognitive, motivational, and behav-
ioral. Our focus is on the first of these dimensions–
cultural metacognition. Metacognition is defined
as thinking about thinking, comprising the pro-
cesses of monitoring and adjusting one’s thoughts
and strategies as one learns new skills (Triandis,
1995). Cultural metacognition refers to an individ-
ual’s level of conscious cultural awareness and
executive processing during cross-cultural interac-
tions (Ang & Van Dyne, 2008). Thomas et al. (2008)
propose that this dimension is the core of cultural
intelligence.
Past researchers have found that intercultural
success is correlated with mindfulness and self-
awareness about cultural assumptions (Johnson,
Cullen, Sakano, & Takenouchi, 1996; LaBahn &
Harich, 1997). Those with high metacognitive cul-
tural intelligence (CQ) are consciously aware of
others’ cultural preferences before and during in-
teractions. They also question cultural assump-
tions and adjust their mental models during and
after interactions (Brislin, Worthley, & MacNab,
2006; Triandis, 2006). Other theorists of intercul-
tural competence have proposed similar infer-
ences as the signature of cultural metacognition
We suggest that cultural intelligence is
an individual difference that can inform
the field’s renewed interest in cross-
cultural research and training programs.
454 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
(Klafehn, Banerjee, & Chiu, 2008; Thomas, 2006).
Evidence suggests that cultural metacognition de-
velops through reflection during intercultural ex-
periences (Ng et al., 2009), consistent with evidence
that reflection helps people fine-tune their as-
sumptions after experiences (Kolb & Kolb, 2005).
Although the construct of metacognition is new
in management research, it follows a tradition em-
phasizing the importance of self-awareness and
sensitivity toward others when adjusting to new
environments (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985). Cogni-
tive psychologists characterize metacognition as
monitoring and adjusting one’s thoughts and strat-
egies as one learns new skills (Flavell, 1979). Ex-
panding this line of theorizing, Ang et al. (2007)
defined cultural metacognition as mental pro-
cesses directed at acquiring, comprehending, and
calibrating cultural knowledge. Researchers
(Klafehn, Banerjee, & Chiu, 2008) suggest that
metacognition in cultural domains increases inter-
cultural effectiveness by promoting (1) contextual-
ized thinking (i.e., heightened sensitivity to the fact
that individuals’ motivations and behaviors are
invariably shaped by the cultural contexts in
which they are embedded), and (2) cognitive flexi-
bility (i.e., discriminative use of mental schemas
and behavioral scripts when interacting across
cultures). The ability to think contextually and flex-
ibly about issues from a foreign counterpart’s per-
spective has been termed cultural perspective tak-
ing in the social psychology and negotiations
literature (Lee, Adair, & Seo, 2011). Thus, cultural
perspective taking may play a central role in the
inferences that enable individuals high in cultural
metacognition to interact effectively across
cultures.
IDENTIFYING AND TRAINING A
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGY
Recent research examining the role of cultural
metacognition in cross-cultural performance has
found that cultural metacognition is associated
with greater success in collaborating in intercul-
tural (but not intracultural) relationships (Chua et
al., 2012). However, past research has yet to iden-
tify which metacognitive strategies may facilitate
intercultural coordination. We propose that one
metacognitive strategy facilitating intercultural
coordination is considering how another person’s
cultural background may affect their response to a
situation—“cultural perspective taking” (Lee et al.,
2011). This thought process, while not foolproof,
generally heightens the accuracy of people’s ex-
pectations about a foreign counterpart’s intentions
and behaviors. Of course, it could be ineffective
when a counterpart’s actual intentions are oppo-
site to those typical of his group, but this is by
definition atypical. We argue that examining cog-
nitive processes that facilitate interpersonal accu-
racy among American managers is important to
investigate, as past research reveals that people
from individualistic cultures, compared to those
from collectivistic cultures, are less accurate in
assessing their counterparts’ interests (Gelfand &
Christakopoulou, 1999).
We explore whether inducing cultural perspec-
tive taking can improve effectiveness in working
with individuals from different cultures, critical to
organizational success. We expected that this in-
tervention would be particularly helpful for man-
agers who are low in cultural metacognition, ei-
ther by disposition or habit. We propose that
cultural perspective taking works like other forms
of perspective taking, in that imagining the world
from another’s person’s perspective (under specific
conditions) has been found to reduce some prob-
lematic patterns in social cognition, such as con-
firmation bias and stereotyping (Galinsky, 2002;
Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). Perspective taking
brings one’s self-experience closer to one’s repre-
sentation of the other person (Davis, Conklin,
Smith, & Luce, 1996). Hence, perspective takers are
more likely to see themselves as similar to the
other person (Galinsky, Ku, & Wang, 2005; Todd &
Burgmer, 2013). Related to this, perspective taking
can lead to mimicking the other person’s observed
or expected behavior (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows,
1996). In this way, perspective taking can help in
meshing or coordinating with another person (Ga-
linsky et al., 2005) and offers a unique advantage in
interpersonal coordination in comparison with
affective-based mechanisms such as empathy (Ga-
linsky, Maddux, Gilin, & White, 2008).
In line with past research and the propositions
we put forth above, we propose the following:
Hypothesis 1a: Cultural metacognition will be pos-
itively associated with intercul-
tural cooperation.
Hypothesis 1b: The relationship between cultural
metacognition and intercultural
cooperation will be mediated by
cultural perspective taking.
How would cultural perspective taking change a
manager’s negotiation strategy? Consider the case
of an American facing a counterpart from a col-
2013 455Mor, Morris, and Joh
lectivistic culture. Cross-cultural research finds
that collectivists favor cooperative, relationship-
preserving tactics, whereas individualists favor
competitive tactics and emphasize their own needs
(Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Culture influences nor-
mative beliefs about what is appropriate, what
others will do, and what others expect (Gelfand &
Dyer, 2000; Lytle, Brett, Barsness, Tinsley, & Jans-
sens, 1995). When Americans reflect on a collectiv-
istic counterpart’s beliefs and practices, they
should become more likely to adopt relational
goals and cooperative tactics themselves. Simi-
larly, prior research on perspective taking has
found that when taking the perspective of an out-
group member, individuals subsequently act more
consistently with the other group’s norms (Ku,
Wang & Galinsky, 2010). Thus, we propose the fol-
lowing predictions:
Hypothesis 2a: An intervention inducing cultural
perspective taking will promote in-
tercultural cooperation with collec-
tivistic counterparts.
Hypothesis 2b: The relationship between cultural
perspective taking and intercul-
tural cooperation will be mediated
by one’s relational goals toward
collectivistic counterparts.
Hypothesis 2c: The relationship between cultural
perspective taking and intercul-
tural cooperation will be mediated
by expectations about the rela-
tional goals of collectivistic
counterparts.
Extending past theory on cultural intelligence
and cultural metacognition, we further propose
that a cultural perspective-taking intervention
would have a larger positive impact on intercul-
tural cooperation decisions of individuals low on
cultural metacognitive tendencies.
Hypothesis 2d: Inducing cultural perspective tak-
ing would promote intercultural co-
operation more for individuals with
low habitual cultural metacogni-
tion than those with high habitual
cultural metacognition.
The Present Research
Although past research has examined perspective
taking broadly, few studies have experimentally
examined the role of cultural perspective taking in
promoting cross-cultural management skills, such
as facilitating intercultural collaboration and co-
operation in international teams, cross-cultural ne-
gotiations, or mixed-motive tasks. In extending
past research on cultural intelligence and cross-
cultural training, we contend that one important
cognitive tendency that managers with high levels
of metacognitive tendencies (high meta-CQ) en-
gage in is cultural perspective taking prior to and
during an intercultural interaction. More impor-
tant, we suggest that cultural perspective taking
tendencies can be temporarily heightened, and
thus, can be especially beneficial for managers
chronically low on cultural metacognitive
tendencies.
Methodological Approach
The methodological approach we employ to test
our hypotheses is adopted from social psychology
research and involves experimental inductions to
manipulate social cognitive processes, such as
cultural perspective taking. To examine the causal
relationships between our variables of interest, we
use an experimental design with samples of MBA
students and crowdsourcing participants by way
of Mechanical Turk. We chose to use Mechanical
Turk participants for our experimental designs
rather than undergraduate student samples to test
our hypotheses for a number of reasons. First, re-
cent research reveals that Mechanical Turk partic-
ipants are significantly more diverse than typical
American college samples (Buhrmester, Kwang, &
Gosling, 2011). Moreover, compared with a tradi-
tional university participant pool, crowdsourcing
respondents have more work experience, are often
older, and are more ethnically diverse (Behrend,
Sharek, Meade, & Wiebe, 2011). Thus, by examin-
ing our hypotheses using both individuals with
professional experience and MBA students’ sam-
ples, we believe that the social cognitive mecha-
nisms examined are shared among lay people as
well as managers, and thus, can be used to train
individuals in a diverse array of professional
settings.
OVERVIEW OF STUDIES
We examined our hypotheses in five studies,
which span from quasi-field settings to controlled
laboratory experiments. In the first, we examine
whether cultural metacognition among American
MBA students was associated with greater levels
of intercultural cooperation with different-culture
peers working together in international teams and
456 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
whether this effect is explained by cultural
perspective-taking habits (Hyp 1a and Hyp 1b). In
our pilot study, we tested a cultural perspective-
taking (CPT) intervention—thinking about a Chi-
nese counterpart’s cultural values and beliefs—
prior to making a business decision to cooperate in
a mixed-motive business scenario. We examined
whether this intervention heightened individuals’
intercultural cooperation due to heightening inten-
tions to adopt a relational orientation (associated
with Chinese cultural values; Hyp 2a and Hyp 2b).
In Study 2, we investigate whether a cultural
perspective-taking intervention promoted Ameri-
can MBA students’ intercultural cooperation in the
same business scenario as the pilot study, but we
further examined whether this effect could also be
explained by expectations about the counterpart’s
relational goals (Hyp 2c). In Study 3A, we examine
whether inducing CPT in American MBAs prior to
engaging in intercultural negotiation with a Japa-
nese counterpart heightened MBA students’ expec-
tations that their Japanese counterpart would be
cooperative in an upcoming negotiation. We fur-
ther examined whether this intervention was more
beneficial for MBAs low on cultural metacognition
than high on cultural metacognition (Hyp 2d). In
Study 3B we provide a comprehensive test of Hy-
potheses 2c-2d in a unified design.
STUDY 1: CULTURAL METACOGNITION AND
INTERCULTURAL COOPERATION
The goal of Study 1 is to examine Hypotheses H1a
and H1b using a sample of American MBA students
studying in an American business school where
50% of the student population is composed of
international students. In this MBA program, inter-
cultural cooperation is needed to complete study-
related tasks, such as papers and class assign-
ments. To examine our predictions, we surveyed
entering students about their cultural metacogni-
tive tendencies. Two months subsequent to evalu-
ating students’ cultural metacognition tendencies,
we evaluated students’ cultural perspective-taking
and cooperation levels using ratings made by their
international peers who had been working with
them in multinational student teams for 2 months.
We hypothesized that MBA students’ cultural meta-
cognition would be positively associated with
different-culture peers’ evaluation of their cooper-
ative tendencies and that this effect would be ex-
plained by heightened cultural perspective-taking
tendencies.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Two hundred American MBA students (Males �
58.5%; Mean Age � 27; 61.3% had managed at least
one employee in their previous jobs) were recruited
to fill out an on-line survey as a voluntary part of
their pre-MBA assignments (73% of incoming stu-
dents completed the survey). Students were pro-
vided a link to an on-line survey that asked about
their past work experiences abroad and their ex-
periences working in multicultural work environ-
ments—an environment where foreign national
students are equally represented as American
ones. The cultural intelligence measure was col-
lected as part of this survey. Upon arrival, students
were assigned to multinational learning teams of
5– 6 students. Teams were created to maximize
their cultural diversity and typically comprised of
three American students, one European student,
and two students from other world regions such as
Africa, South America, the Middle East, and East
Asia or South Asia. These teams assemble in MBA
student orientation and students spend the major-
ity of orientation activities and their first year of
classes working in teams. After 2 months in their
international teams, 305 international student
peers (from non-U.S. nationalities), representing 45
nationalities evaluated target American students
on a host of leadership-related measures as part of
a 360 leadership assessment. Each student was
evaluated by between 1 and 4 different-culture
peers with whom they worked. Student peers were
asked to anonymously appraise their team mem-
bers’ levels of cooperation and perspective-taking
tendencies when working in these teams as part of
their class assignment. Let us now describe the
measures in the study in more detail.
Measures
Cultural Metacognition
Incoming MBA students reported their cultural
metacognition tendencies using a 6-item scale de-
veloped by Van Dyne, Ang, Ng, Rockstuhl, Tan, and
Koh (2012). The items tap (1) cultural awareness
(e.g., “I am aware of how to use my cultural knowl-
edge when interacting with people from different
cultures”); (2) adjustment during intercultural inter-
actions (“I adjust my cultural knowledge while in-
teracting with people from a new or an unfamiliar
culture”); and (3) planning before intercultural in-
teractions (e.g., “I develop action plans for interact-
2013 457Mor, Morris, and Joh
ing with people from a different culture”; Scale
reliability: � � .82). We averaged students’ self-
reports on the six items to create a cultural meta-
cognition score for each student.
Cultural Perspective Taking
After working 2 months in their international teams,
participants were evaluated by classmates from
other cultures with regard to their perspective-
taking habits. The items were the following: “S/he
is able to empathize and understand someone
else’s perspective”; “S/he misjudges people’s per-
sonality and character” [Reversed]; “S/he fails to
realize the impact of what s/he says and does on
others” [Reversed]; “S/he is good at assessing other
people’s strengths and weaknesses”; “S/he is good
at sensing what other people are thinking and
feeling” (Scale reliability: � � .83). We averaged
each rater’s evaluation of each target student on
these five items to create each rater’s cultural
perspective-taking score for the target student.
Intercultural Cooperation
Peers also evaluated target students on three
items that assessed American MBA students’ abil-
ity to work effectively in their international teams.
The items were the following: “She/he is able to
build effective working relationships with others
who have different opinions or interests” and
“She/he is able to build coalitions to get things
done” (Scale reliability: � � .65). We averaged each
rater’s evaluation of each target student on these
two items to create each rater’s intercultural coop-
eration score for the target student.
We conducted confirmatory factor analyses
(CFA) using structural equation modeling (LISREL
8.80) to ensure that the mediator and outcome vari-
able (cultural perspective taking and intercultural
cooperation) are distinct factors. Results indicate
that a 2-factor model where cultural perspective
taking and intercultural cooperation items load
into respective separate factors has a better fit to
the data (�2 � 35.15, df � 13, RMSEA � 0.08) than a
1-factor model (�2 � 35.16, df � 14, RMSEA � 0.09).
Control Variables
Dimensions of cultural intelligence other than cul-
tural metacognition, namely cognitive, motiva-
tional, and behavioral CQ, have been found to
predict intercultural cooperation tendencies (Imai
& Gelfand, 2010). As a result, we included these
scales’ control variables: cognitive CQ (� � .84),
motivational CQ (� � .86), and behavioral CQ
(� � .86).
Additionally, since student peer ratings of target
students’ cooperation levels may be influenced by
their levels of acquaintance with target students
outside of class assignments, we included raters’
familiarity with the target student as an additional
control variable (“how well do you know this per-
son?”, 1 � Not at all to 4 � extremely well).
Results and Discussion
Hypothesis 1a predicted that cultural metacogni-
tion would be positively associated with intercul-
tural cooperation. To test this hypothesis, we used
a hierarchical linear model (HLM) software to carry
out our analyses (Raudenbush, 2004). We ran a
linear HLM treating different-culture student peers
as nested within target American students, with
student-peer ratings as the dependent variable (at
Level 1) and cultural metacognition and the control
variables as Level 2 predictors. Table 1 reports the
results from the hierarchical linear model analy-
ses. Model 1 contains the control variables;
whereas Model 2 adds the predictor of cultural
metacognition. Analyses revealed that cultural
TABLE 1
Hierarchical Linear Model Regression on Student
Peers’ Ratings of Intercultural Cooperation
(Study 1)
Intercultural cooperation
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Key predictors
Cultural metacognition — 0.20* 0.06
(�0.09) (�0.07)
Cultural perspective
taking (mediator)
0.67**
(�0.04)
Control variables
Cognitive CQ 0.05 �0.01 0.01
(�0.07) (�0.08) (�0.06)
Motivational CQ 0.14� 0.09 0.12�
(�0.08) (�0.09) (�0.07)
Behavioral CQ �0.04 �0.07 �0.08
(�0.07) (�0.07) (�0.05)
Student peers familiarity 0.23** 0.26** 0.07
(0.10) (0.10) (0.08)
Intercept 4.23** 3.91** 1.10**
(0.46) (0.48) (0.41)
Note. CQ � cultural intelligence. Numbers in parenthesis are
standard errors. �p � .10, **p � .01, *p � .05.
458 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
metacognition was positively associated with
peers’ evaluation of target students’ levels of coop-
eration in international teams, � � .20, SE � .09,
t(195) � 2.22, p � .05. Thus, the results supported
Hypothesis 1a.
Next, we examined whether cultural perspective-
taking tendencies mediated the relationship be-
tween cultural metacognition and intercultural
cooperation. To conduct mediational analyses (in
this study and follow-up studies), we followed
Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures as well as
carried out a bootstrapping test with 20,000 re-
samples using percentile bootstraps (Hayes, 2009).
Analyses revealed that cultural metacognition
(Level 2 predictor) was positively associated with
student peers’ evaluation of target students’ cul-
tural perspective-taking tendencies (Level 1 de-
pendent variable), � � .20, SE � .09, t(195) � 2.26,
p � .05. Second, when cultural metacognition and
cultural perspective taking (mediator) were both
entered into the HLM model as Level 2 predictors
(see Model 3), the effect of cultural metacognition
turned statistically nonsignificant, � � .06,
SE � .07, t � .90, p � .37; whereas the effect of
cultural perspective taking on intercultural coop-
eration remained statistically significant, � � .67,
SE � .04, t � 15.01, p � .001, suggesting mediation
(see Figure 1 for the full mediation model). A boot-
strapping test confirmed a positive indirect effect
of cultural metacognition on intercultural cooper-
ation by way of cultural perspective taking (95% CI
[.02, 25]). These results provide support for Hypoth-
esis 1b.
In summary, the results from Study 1 provide
evidence supporting our hypotheses that chroni-
cally high cultural perspective-taking tendencies
among American MBA students explain the rela-
tionship between metacognitive habits and suc-
cessful intercultural collaboration with different-
culture counterparts.
STUDY 2: CULTURAL
PERSPECTIVE-TAKING INTERVENTION
Although Study 1 provides the first empirical evi-
dence that the relationship between cultural meta-
cognition and intercultural cooperation is ex-
plained by cultural perspective-taking tendencies,
in Study 2, we examined one intervention for pro-
moting cultural perspective taking—reflecting
how the cultural background of one’s counterparts
may affect their approach to a mixed-motive con-
flict. This cognitively oriented intervention is dis-
tinct from affectively oriented interventions in past
work on intercultural collaboration (Chua et al.,
2012). We tested whether this cultural perspective-
taking intervention would shift participants’ deci-
sions, in this case, toward a relational, harmonious
approach congruent with Chinese norms (Hyp 2b)
in a Prisoners’ Dilemma scenario that was adapted
into a decision-making task in a business setting.
PILOT STUDY
Method
Participants and Procedures
For an initial test of the cultural perspective-taking
intervention, we recruited 107 American adults
(81.3% � White/Caucasian; 7.5% � Asian; 6.5% �
β =.20* β = .67***
β = .20*
(.06)
Cultural
Metacognition
Intercultural
Cooperation
Cultural
Perspective
Taking
FIGURE 1
Mediation Model Showing Cultural Perspective Taking
Mediates the Relationship Between Cultural
Metacognition and Intercultural Cooperation (Evaluated by
Different-Culture Peers; Study 1).
Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. *
� p � .05, *** � p � .001.
2013 459Mor, Morris, and Joh
African American; 3.7% � Hispanic; 9% � Native
American; Female � 51%; Mean Age � 34, 28.1%
college students, 70.1% currently working) via Me-
chanical Turk for a study on problem solving. Par-
ticipants were randomly assigned to a cultural
perspective-taking condition or control condition.
Materials
Mixed Motive Conflict. Participants were asked to
read a scenario, a Prisoners’ Dilemma task pre-
sented as a conflict on an advertising campaign
between an American product manager and a Chi-
nese product manager, adapted from past research
(Greenhalgh & Bazerman, 2004; Ku et al., 2010).
Participants were first presented with the follow-
ing prompt about their role and objectives as
American managers:
You are Mr./Ms. Graham, the head of a prod-
uct management group in a consumer prod-
ucts firm based in the U.S., handling the mar-
keting of a new liquid dishwasher detergent.
As a manager, you are faced with a recurring
decision as to whether you should put on an
advertising campaign during the next sales
period. This campaign would provide con-
sumers with comparative information about
your Chinese competitor’s product: Li Hong.
The comparative advertising campaign will
describe the destructive impact of your com-
petitor’s product on a dishwasher’s motor,
spots left on the dishes, and/or its high cost.
Your objective is to maximize your profit.
This is also the objective of your Chinese
competitor, Li Hong. Profitability will also be
used as a way of measuring your success as a
manager. The profitability of your product de-
pends not only on your decision but also on
the decision of Li Hong. Specifically, if neither
you nor Li put on a derogatory advertising
campaign, each company will make $1 mil-
lion for the sales period. If one of you puts on
a comparative advertising campaign but the
competing company does not, then the com-
pany that advertises will have a profit of
$2 million for the sales period and the com-
petitor will lose $2 million. If both companies
advertise the deficiencies of their competi-
tor’s product, then total sales of liquid dish-
washer detergent will fall and both compa-
nies will lose $1 million for the sales period.
Participants were asked to decide whether to
embark on an advertising campaign disparaging a
Chinese product manager, Li Hong, who is selling
a similar product. Advertising was equivalent to
an aggressive, competitive response; whereas not
advertising was equivalent to cooperating. Partic-
ipants’ decision not to advertise was used as a
measure of intercultural cooperation.
Manipulation. Next, participants in the CPT condi-
tion were asked to think about their counterpart’s
culture before making their decision. Below is the
prompt they received:
Before you go on to make your decision, we
would like you to do the following: Please
write down a few sentences describing Li’s
(Chinese manager’s) interests and concerns
as a person living and working in China. . .
How would Li’s cultural values and beliefs
guide his behavior and decision in this
situation?
Business Decision. Next, participants were asked
to make their decision in the decision-making sce-
nario (advertise or not advertise).
Goals. After making their decision, participants
indicated in an open-ended response format what
guided their decision-making in the scenario. They
were presented with the following prompt:
Please write down a few sentences explain-
ing what guided your decision to advertise or
not advertise in the business case.
Two research assistants blind to the hypotheses
rated participants’ open-ended responses. Raters
evaluated participants’ decisions on the following
items: “The participant was interested in promot-
ing future business relations with Li”; “The partic-
ipant was interested in cooperating with Li”; “The
participant was interested in achieving the busi-
ness goal of maximizing profit/minimizing profit
loss” (Reversed); (1 � strongly disagree, 7 �
strongly agree; ICC � .80). The 3 items were aver-
aged to create a score for each participant (� � .89).
Manipulation Check. After making their decision,
participants indicated whether they thought about
Chinese cultural values while evaluating the sce-
nario. They rated their thought process on the fol-
lowing three items: “I tried to think what a Chinese
manager would do in this case”; “I thought about
Chinese business norms when making my deci-
sion”; “I thought about Chinese cultural values
460 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
when making my decision.” Items averaged to cre-
ate a score for each participant (1 � not at all; 7 �
very much; � �. 87).
Control Variables. At the end of the study, partici-
pants reported their cultural intelligence (cultural
metacognition Van Dyne et al., 2012): � �.87; Cog-
nitive CQ: � �.80; Motivational CQ: � �. 80; Behav-
ioral CQ: � �. 87. As in Study 1, these four cultural
intelligence factors were included as control vari-
ables in our analyses.
Results
To test whether the CPT manipulation induced cul-
tural perspective taking in participants, we ran an
ANCOVA (between subjects factor: experimental
condition; covariates: 4 CQ factors) on the cultural
perspective-taking scores and found that partici-
pants reported thinking more about Chinese cul-
tural values and beliefs in the CPT condition
(M � 4.58) than the control condition (M � 3.32),
F(1,150) � 18.84, p � .001.
Hypothesis 2a proposed that cultural perspective
taking would promote cooperation with a different-
culture colleague. To test this, we regressed par-
ticipants’ choices to cooperate (advertise � com-
pete � 0; not advertise � cooperate � 1) on the
experimental condition (0 � Control, 1 � CPT),
controlling for the 4 CQ factors. Analyses revealed
a main positive effect of the manipulation on
choosing a cooperative strategy with a Chinese
counterpart, � � .93, SE � .48, Wald (1) � 3.73,
p � .05. Thus, the results supported Hypothesis 2a.
To examine whether the effect of the CPT inter-
vention on decisions runs through goals, we fol-
lowed Baron and Kenny’s (1986) steps for testing
mediation as well as a bootstrapping test (Hayes,
2009). We first examined the relationship between
the experimental condition and goals (controlling
for cultural intelligence factors) using multiple re-
gression analysis and found them to be positively
associated, � � .90, SE � .26, t(101) � 3.45, p � .01.
Then we conducted binary logistic analysis and
entered both the experimental condition and stu-
dents’ relational orientation scores as predictors of
choice to cooperate, controlling for cultural intelli-
gence factors. Analyses revealed that the effect of
the condition turned statistically nonsignificant,
� � �.58, SE � .78, Wald (1) � .55, p � .46; whereas
the effect of students’ relational scores remained a
significant predictor of choice to cooperate with Li,
� � 2.13, SE � .44, Wald (1) � 23.47, p � .001 sug-
gesting mediation (see Figure 2). A bootstrapping
test with 20,000 resamples confirmed a positive
indirect effect between CPT on students’ choice to
cooperate by way of intentions about adopting a
relational business orientation (95% CI [.31, 3.27]).
Thus, the results above provided support for Hy-
pothesis 2b. In sum, our pilot study revealed that a
cultural perspective-taking intervention increased
participants’ relational orientation toward a Chi-
nese counterpart and explained their decision to
cooperate with him. However, the question still
remains whether this heightened relational orien-
tation was the result of an increase in other–self
overlap associated with collectivistic cultural
values.
β = .90** β = 2.13**
β = .93*
(–.58)
Experimental
Condition
(0=Control;
1=CPT)
Intercultural
Cooperation
Relational Goals
FIGURE 2
Mediation Model Showing the Positive Casual Effect of
Cultural Perspective Taking on Intercultural
Cooperation with a Chinese Counterpart Is Mediated by One’s
Relational Goals (Study 2, Pilot Study).
Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. *
� p � .05, ** � p � .01.
2013 461Mor, Morris, and Joh
STUDY 2: CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE-TAKING
INTERVENTION WITH MBA STUDENTS
In Study 2 we shifted to test our CPT intervention
with MBA students whose demographic character-
istics are more closely aligned with those of man-
agers. Moreover, we also explored participants’ ex-
pectations about their counterpart’s decision in the
mixed-motive conflict task, since much of past re-
search finds that cooperation in mixed-motive con-
flicts hinges on expectancies of the counterpart’s
cooperation (Wong & Hong, 2005). Accordingly, we
examined whether the association between cul-
tural perspective taking and cooperation hinges on
heightened expectancies of the Chinese counter-
part’s goal to cooperate (Hyp 2a and Hyp 2b).
Method
Participants and Procedures
Fifty-seven (Non-Chinese) MBAs (61.4% American1;
Males � 65%; Mean Age � 28) completed the study
as part of an in-class exercise. Seventy-two per-
cent of students identified as White/Caucasian,
21.1% Asian (non-Chinese), 3.5% Latin/Hispanic,
1.8% as African-American, and 3.5% as other eth-
nicities. Students were randomly assigned to a
CPT condition or control condition.
Materials
Mixed-Motive Conflict
Students were asked to read a business scenario,
which was a Prisoner’s Dilemma task presented as
an advertising task. The instructions and task
were presented in the same manner as in the pi-
lot study.
Manipulation
Next, participants in the CPT condition were asked
to think about their Chinese counterpart’s cultural
values before making their decision. The prompt
presented to MBA students was the same prompt
used in the pilot study.
Business Decision
Next, participants were asked to make their deci-
sion in the case (to advertise or not advertise).
Manipulation Check
After making their decision, participants indicated
whether they thought about Chinese cultural val-
ues while evaluating the case, which served as a
manipulation check using the following three
items: “I tried to think what a Chinese manager
would do in this case”; “I thought about Chinese
business norms when making my decision”; “I
thought about Chinese cultural values when mak-
ing my decision.” Items were averaged to create a
score for each participant (1 � not at all; 7 � very
much; � � .89).
Expectancies of Counterpart’s Cooperativeness
Next, participants rated their confidence that Li
would not advertise (i.e., cooperate) using a 7-
point scale (1 � not at all; 7 � very much; Wong &
Hong, 2005).
Control Variables
At the end of the study, participants completed a
cultural intelligence assessment (Cultural meta-
cognition; Van Dyne and colleagues, 2012): � � .73;
Cognitive CQ: � � .90; Motivational CQ: � � .79;
Behavioral CQ: � � .88.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
To test whether the cultural perspective-taking
manipulation induced cultural perspective taking
in MBA students, we ran an ANCOVA (between
subjects factor: experimental condition; covariates:
4 CQ factors) on the cultural perspective-taking
scores and found that students reported more
thoughts about counterparts’ cultural values and
beliefs in the cultural perspective-taking condition
(M � 5.49) than the control condition (M � 4.17), F
(1,57) � 14.27, p � .001.
Hypothesis 2a suggested that inducing cultural
perspective taking would promote cooperation
with a counterpart from a culture known for coop-
erative relational norms. To test this prediction, we
conducted a binary logistic regression regressing
students’ choice to cooperate (advertise � com-
pete � 0; not advertise � cooperate � 1) on the
1 Preliminary analysis revealed that students’ cultural back-
grounds (American vs. non-American students) did not reveal
differential effects on their responses to the manipulation and
their decision to cooperate in the case, and thus, we retained
both American and non-American students in the sample.
462 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
experimental condition (0 � Control, 1 � CPT),
controlling for cultural intelligence factors. Analy-
ses revealed a positive main effect of CPT on in-
tercultural cooperation, � � 1.43, SE � .63, Wald (1)
� 5.23, p � .05. These results supported Hypothesis
2a, replicating the results from the pilot study with
MBA students and revealing that cultural perspec-
tive taking had a direct positive effect on intercul-
tural cooperation.
Hypothesis 2b further suggested that the rela-
tionship between CPT and intercultural coopera-
tion would be mediated by expectations about the
counterpart’s cooperativeness. To test this predic-
tion, we first examined the relationship between
the experimental condition and expectations about
the counterpart’s cooperativeness (the mediator).
Multiple regression analysis revealed that the CPT
condition had a positive main effect on expecta-
tions that the counterpart would cooperate
(� � 1.37, SE � .52, t � 2.62, p � .05). When both
experimental condition and expectations about the
counterpart’s cooperativeness were entered as pre-
dictors of cooperation in a logistic regression
model, the relationship between the experimental
condition and intercultural cooperation turned sta-
tistically nonsignificant (� � .95, SE � .69, Wald (1)
� 1.86, p � .17), while expectations about Li’s co-
operation remained a significant predictor of inter-
cultural cooperation (� � .47, SE � .18, Wald (1)
� 7.19, p � .01), suggesting mediation (see Figure
3). A bootstrapping test confirmed there was a pos-
itive indirect effect between CPT on intercultural
cooperation via expectations about Li’s coopera-
tiveness (95% CI [.01, 1.84]).
These results reveal that the cultural perspective-
taking manipulation increased MBA students’ co-
operation with a Chinese counterpart, and this ef-
fect was explained by their heightened
expectations that their counterpart—Li Hong—
would cooperate as well. Thus, the results sup-
ported Hypotheses H2a and H2b and suggest that
one useful intervention for promoting managers’
cross-cultural working relations with collectivistic
counterparts is asking them to reflect about their
counterpart’s values and beliefs prior to meeting
them. Moreover, we find that CPT also increases
expectations that one’s counterpart holds rela-
tional, cooperative goals when making his or her
decision. Overall, it appears that reflection prior to
intercultural interaction facilitates overlap be-
tween MBA students’ and their counterparts’ cul-
tural values and goals.
STUDY 3A: CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE TAKING
AND INTERCULTURAL NEGOTIATIONS
Although Study 2 revealed a causal positive rela-
tionship between cultural perspective taking and
intercultural cooperation, in Study 3A, we exam-
ined this intervention as a preparation for an in-
ternational negotiation between an American
β = 1.37*
β = 1.43*
β = .47**
(.95)
Experimental
Condition
(0=Control;
1=CPT)
Intercultural
Cooperation
Expectation
about
counterpart’s
relational goals
FIGURE 3
Mediation Model Showing the Positive Effect of Cultural
Perspective-Taking Condition on Intercultural
Cooperation Is Mediated by Expectations That the Chinese
Counterpart Holds Relational Goals (Study 2).
Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. *
� p � .05; ** � p � .01
Overall, it appears that reflection prior to
intercultural interaction facilitates
overlap between MBA students’ and their
counterparts’ cultural values and goals.
2013 463Mor, Morris, and Joh
manager and a Japanese counterpart. In this con-
text, we examined American MBA students’ expec-
tations about their counterpart’s cooperative ver-
sus competitive orientation in an upcoming
negotiation. Expectations about the counterpart’s
orientation should be associated with students’ de-
cisions to cooperate, as past research on negotia-
tions has found that expectations greatly influence
strategic decisions about concessions to make and
information to provide or withhold from one’s coun-
terpart (Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). We also fur-
ther explored whether a cultural perspective inter-
vention produced greater benefits (e.g., increased
expectation about counterpart’s cooperativeness)
for MBA students low on metacognition than those
high metacognition individuals. In other words, we
expected that culture-specific cues would lead in-
dividuals low on cultural metacognition to modify
their expectations about their counterpart’s goals,
leading to greater levels of intercultural coopera-
tion in this task.
Method
Procedure
Seventy-six American MBA students (Males � 56%;
Mean Age � 27) participated in a negotiation ex-
ercise in their first semester of school as part of an
in-class exercise in a leadership class. Upon be-
ginning their MBA program, the American MBA
students reported their cultural metacognition lev-
els in an orientation survey (using the same
method as in Study 1). Two months later (as part of
an in-class exercise) the students were asked to
complete a prenegotiation survey prior to a nego-
tiation with a Japanese counterpart over the man-
ufacturing of “mini” excavators (Patel & Brett,
2007). Participants were not aware of any cultural
elements in the exercise or the fact that the prene-
gotiation survey was intended to assess their pro-
clivity toward working with a different-culture
counterpart. Since students later carried out the
negotiation in teams, we only examined their ex-
pectations about their counterpart’s cooperation
prior to the negotiation. Students were randomly
assigned to either a cultural perspective-taking
manipulation or a control condition.
Materials
Cultural Intelligence
Two months prior to the negotiation exercise, stu-
dents completed the 4-factor cultural intelligence
assessment described in the previous studies (cul-
tural metacognition: � � .80; Cognitive CQ: � � .80;
Motivational CQ: � � .85; Behavioral CQ: � � .86).
The four CQ factors were included as control vari-
ables in all of the following analyses.
Negotiation Exercise
Upon arriving to class, students received their ne-
gotiation roles and were asked to read the materi-
als prior to completing their prenegotiation sur-
veys. The negotiation case involved a negotiation
between two companies, Abhas and Bussan (rep-
resented by Sato-san), over the manufacturing of
“mini” excavators, and the case materials in-
cluded five issues. All American students were
assigned to the Abhas role. The case contained
background information about the Japanese com-
pany and Sato-san (the Japanese negotiator).
Cultural Perspective-Taking Manipulation
Prior to the negotiation, students were provided a
link to an on-line survey where they were asked to
predict their Japanese counterpart’s orientation in
the upcoming negotiations. Students in the CPT
condition received the following information be-
fore making their predictions about their Japanese
counterpart (Students in the control received no
prompt).
Before you go on to guess Sato-san’s (Japa-
nese counterpart from the Bussan company)
priorities and goals in the negotiations, we
would like you to do the following: Please
take a few moments to think about the per-
spective of Sato-san negotiating on behalf of
a company operating in Japan. Try to imagine
what Sato-san would be thinking and what
may be her/his interests and concerns based
on the fact that she/he is negotiating on be-
half of a company operating in Japan. Try
In other words, we expected that culture-
specific cues would lead individuals low
on cultural metacognition to modify their
expectations about their counterpart’s
goals, leading to greater levels of
intercultural cooperation in this task.
464 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
your best to put yourself in Sato-san’s shoes.
Please write down a few sentences describ-
ing Sato-san’s interests and concerns about
the five case issues as a Japanese negotiator.
Expectancy of Counterpart’s Cooperativeness
Next, students were asked to predict the orienta-
tion of their Japanese counterpart during the up-
coming negotiation using the following categories:
(1) Cooperative, (2) Competitive, or (3) Don’t Know.
All other responses (competitive or don’t know)
were coded as 0.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
We conducted a logistic regression analysis exam-
ining the effects of the experimental condition on
MBA students’ expectations that their Japanese
counterpart would be cooperative in the upcoming
negotiation. Analyses revealed no main effect of
condition, � � .27, SE � .26, Wald (1) � 1.11, p � .29.
Next, we examined whether the intervention had a
stronger effect for individuals with a low baseline
proclivity toward cultural metacognition. To test
this prediction, we used binary logistic regression
and tested for an interaction between cultural
metacognition and the experimental condition on
perceptions of cooperativeness. Analyses revealed
a significant 2-way interaction between cultural
metacognition and the experimental condition on
expectation that Japanese counterpart would coop-
erate, � � �0.84, SE � .39, Wald (1) � 4.71, p � .05.
Simple slope analysis following procedures by Ai-
ken, West, and Reno (1991) revealed that MBAs low
on cultural metacognition when asked to reflect on
their Japanese counterpart’s values and behaviors
were more likely to expect their Japanese counter-
part would be cooperative (CPT condition) than not
(control condition), � � 1.80, SE � .80, Wald (1)
� 5.10, p � .05 (see Figure 4). At the same time, the
CPT for MBAs high on cultural metacognition
did not alter their (already moderately high) expec-
tations about their Japanese counterpart’s cooper-
ativeness, � � �0.75, SE � .73, Wald (1) � 1.07,
p � .30 (see Figure 4). The patterns depicted in
Figure 4 further suggest that individuals low on
cultural metacognition did not simply regress to-
ward the mean in their expectations about their
partner, but rather more closely matched the pre-
dictions of individuals high on cultural
metacognition.
Although the manipulation did not have a main
effect on expectancies, there was an interaction
effect: Individuals habitually low in cultural meta-
cognition were prompted by the manipulation to
think about their Japanese counterpart’s coopera-
tiveness more than they did in the control condi-
tion; whereas individuals high in cultural meta-
cognition were not pushed to think more than they
did in the control condition (already a considerable
amount). Hence this is an intervention that works
most for individuals not culturally minded in that it
helps them take useful cultural information into
account. To help students who are already cultur-
ally minded make even more finely calibrated de-
cisions, another kind of manipulation may be
needed.
FIGURE 4
Graph Depicting the Interaction Between Experimental
Condition and Cultural Metacognition on
Expectations That Japanese Counterpart Would Be Cooperative
in an Upcoming Negotiation (Study 3A).
2013 465Mor, Morris, and Joh
STUDY 3B
Although Study 3A revealed that offering a cultural
perspective-taking intervention to individuals low
on cultural metacognition enhanced their expecta-
tion that their different-culture counterpart would
cooperate in an upcoming international negotia-
tion, we did not examine whether this intervention
also directly increased intercultural cooperation
decisions of MBAs low on cultural metacognition.
Thus, the aim of Study 3B was to examine Hypoth-
eses 2b and 2c in a unified study design.
Method
Participants and Procedures
Seventy-six American adults (100% White/Cauca-
sian, Female � 52%; Mean Age � 34, 26% college
students, 62% currently working) were recruited by
way of Mechanical Turk and were asked to com-
plete a survey on decision making. The procedure
was the same design as Study 2. Participants were
randomly assigned to a cultural perspective-
taking or control condition.
Mixed-Motive Conflict
Participants were asked to read a scenario which
was a Prisoner’s Dilemma task presented as an
advertising task. The task presented was the same
task presented in Study 2.
Manipulation
Next, participants in the cultural perspective-
taking condition were asked to think about their
Chinese counterpart’s cultural values before mak-
ing their decision in the business scenario (same
task as Study 2).
Business Decision
Next, participants were asked to make their deci-
sion in the case (to advertise/not advertise).
Manipulation Check
After making their decision, participants indicated
whether they thought about Chinese cultural val-
ues while evaluating the case which served as a
manipulation check using the following three
items (as in Study 2): “I tried to think what a Chi-
nese manager would do in this case”; “I thought
about Chinese business norms when making my
decision”; “I thought about Chinese cultural val-
ues when making my decision.” Items were aver-
aged to create a score for each participant (1 � not
at all; 7 � very much; � �. 83).
Expectancies
We also evaluated participants’ inferences about
their Chinese counterpart’s long-term relational
goals using the following statements: “I thought Li
would care about our long-term relationship”; “I
thought Li would be most concerned about making
profit” [Reversed]). The two items were averaged to
create a score for each participant (� � .87).
Cultural Intelligence Measure
At the end of the study, participants completed the
usual cultural intelligence assessment (cultural
metacognition: � �.87; Cognitive CQ: � � .86; Mo-
tivational CQ: � � .89; Behavioral CQ: � � .91). As
in the previous studies, these four cultural intelli-
gence factors were included as control variables in
our analyses.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
To test whether the CPT manipulation induced cul-
tural perspective taking, we ran an ANCOVA (be-
tween subjects factor: experimental condition; co-
variates: 4 CQ factors) on the cultural perspective-
taking scores and found that participants reported
greater levels of cultural perspective taking in the
CPT condition (M � 4.72) than the control condition
(M � 3.55), F(1,76) � 11.77, p � .01.
We first tested Hypothesis 2a, which suggested
that cultural perspective taking would promote in-
tercultural cooperation. To do so we conducted a
binary logistic regression using experimental con-
dition as a predictor variable on participants’ de-
cision to cooperate. Consistent with Hypothesis 2a
(and results from studies 2A and 2B), the CPT con-
dition had a positive effect on choosing a cooper-
ative strategy with Li, � � 1.06, SE � .51, Wald (1)
� 4.30, p � .05.
Next, we conducted a binary logistic regression
analysis to examine whether expectations about
the Chinese counterpart’s strategy explained the
relationship between the experimental condition
and decision to cooperate. When both experimen-
tal condition and expectations about counterpart’s
cooperation were entered into the binary logistic
466 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
regression model, the effect of CPT turned statisti-
cally nonsignificant, � � .74, SE � .55, Wald (1)
� 1.80, p � .18, while expectations about Li’s rela-
tional goals remained a significant positive pre-
dictor of cooperation, indicating mediation, � � .66,
SE � .21, Wald (1) � 9.60, p � .01 (see Figure 5). A
bootstrapping test confirmed there was a positive
indirect effect between CPT on intercultural coop-
eration by way of expectations about counterpart’s
relational goals (95% CI [.03, 1.59]). The results
above provide additional support for Hypotheses
2a and 2b by revealing that the CPT manipulation
increased the likelihood of choosing a cooperative
strategy with a Chinese counterpart by heighten-
ing expectations that the Chinese counterpart
would chose a cooperative strategy as well.
Next, we examined Hypothesis 2d and tested
whether inducing CPT had a greater positive effect
on decisions to cooperate for individuals low on
cultural metacognition. To test this hypothesis, we
conducted a binary logistic regression analysis
and tested for an interaction between the experi-
mental manipulation and cultural metacognition
on decision to cooperate. Analyses revealed a sig-
nificant interaction, � � �1.47, SE � .63, Wald (1)
� 5.55, p � .05. To probe the interaction, we con-
ducted simple slope analysis (Aiken et al., 1991),
examining the effect of the experimental condition
in individuals low on cultural metacognition ver-
sus those high in cultural metacognition. Consis-
tent with our predictions, analysis revealed that
individuals low on cultural metacognition were
significantly more cooperative in the CPT condi-
tion than the control condition, � � 1.39, SE � .48,
Wald (1) � 8.41, p � .01. At the same time, the CPT
manipulation did not enhance cooperation levels
for individuals high on cultural metacognition,
� � �.08, SE � .35, Wald (1) � .05, p � .82.
DISCUSSION
In five studies we provided a comprehensive test of
our hypotheses positing that a metacognitive ten-
dency, namely— cultural perspective taking— can
promote intercultural cooperation and be espe-
cially useful for promoting intercultural coopera-
tion among American MBA students and working
adults low on cultural metacognition. Our research
contributes to ongoing research in management
education and learning by providing a novel
framework utilizing both an individual difference
approach and situational priming geared toward
identifying and developing individuals and man-
agers’ cross-cultural management skills.
Theoretical Implications
While much of past research on cultural intelli-
gence (CQ) has focused on identifying the link
between cultural intelligence and managerial per-
formance measures (for a review see Ang, Van
Dyne, & Tan, 2011), our research is the first to iden-
tify a cognitive-based mechanism associated with
one of the factors of cultural intelligence. More-
over, we provide the first empirical evidence that a
cognitive mechanism— cultural perspective tak-
ing— has a direct positive effect on behavioral
measures associated with managerial perfor-
mance in culturally diverse settings, spanning in-
tercultural collaboration in international teams,
(.74)
Experimental
Condition
(0=Control;
1=CPT)
Intercultural
Cooperation
Expectation
about
counterpart’s
relational goals
β = .68*
β = 1.06*
β = .66**
FIGURE 5
Mediation Model Showing the Positive Effect of Cultural
Perspective-Taking Condition on Intercultural
Cooperation Is Mediated by Expectations That the Chinese
Counterpart Holds Relational Goals (Study 3B).
Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. *
� p � .05; ** � p � .01.
2013 467Mor, Morris, and Joh
decision making in mixed-motive conflicts, and
preparation for international negotiations.
Interestingly, while past CQ research and theory
has suggested that cultural knowledge (cognitive
CQ) should lead individuals to have more accurate
understanding of foreign cultures (Ang et al., 2007),
the results of Study 1 do not support these assump-
tions, nor does recent research revealing no asso-
ciation between self-reported cognitive CQ and
cross-cultural accuracy (Mor, Ames, & Joh, 2013).
The results revealed that only metacognitive CQ,
and not cognitive CQ, was associated with cul-
tural perspective taking. These results suggest
that the ability to accurately detect culture-specific
congruent or incongruent norms may require the
development of metacognitive habits in tandem
with foreign cultural knowledge.
Cultural Intelligence and
Intercultural Relationships
Our findings contribute to the growing body of
research on how dimensions of cultural intelli-
gence (CQ) affect people’s abilities to manage var-
ious forms interdependence with counterparts
from different cultures. Recent research by Chua,
Morris, and Mor (2012) focused on collaborations
within the context of professional networks. They
found that executives with a higher proclivity to-
ward cultural metacognition attain more creative
collaboration success in their intercultural ties, as
they develop more affect-based trust in these ties,
relative to those lower in cultural metacognition.
In the context of intercultural negotiations, Imai
and Gelfand (2010) have found that motivational
CQ (the motivation and efficacy to engage cultur-
ally different others) predicted integrative behav-
iors, resulting in higher joint gains and that behav-
ioral CQ (behavioral flexibility during intercultural
interactions), but not other dimensions of CQ, pre-
dicted sequences of cooperative strategies. Our
research adds to this stream of research by (1)
identifying strategies for using cultural knowledge
that are associated with cultural metacognition,
such as cultural perspective taking, and (2) exam-
ining whether metacognitive strategies can be in-
duced through an intervention guiding perspective
taking. Our research also reveals the discriminate
validity of the cultural metacognition factor from
the three other CQ factors, a query recently advo-
cated by Van Dyne et al. (2012).
Our findings further extend past research exam-
ining intercultural trust and collaboration (Chua et
al., 2012; Takahashi, Yamagishi, Liu, Wang, Lin, &
Yu, 2008) by examining the role of cognitive-based
tendencies rather than affective mechanisms lead-
ing to intercultural trust and collaboration. Past
research finds that trust leads to better rapport,
which increases willingness to cooperate with oth-
ers in mixed-motive conflicts (Drolet & Morris,
2000). Trust can arise by way of two distinct psy-
chological processes: a cognitive evaluation of the
other party’s competence and reliability, or an af-
fective experience of liking and rapport (McAllis-
ter, 1995). The former is based on expectations of
task-related competence and involves an analytic
and utilitarian assessment of the other party; the
latter is closely linked to empathy and rapport, and
arises from emotional closeness. These two types
of trust closely resemble two central dimensions of
social perception: warmth and competence (Fiske,
Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). Affect-based trust is strongly
linked to perceived warmth, cognition-based trust
to perceived competence. We argue that cognitive-
based mechanisms leading to intercultural trust
and cooperation are important to examine for a
number of reasons. First, McAllister (1995) notes
that some level of cognition-based trust is neces-
sary for affect-based trust to develop. Second, ne-
gotiations scholars have found that differences in
cognitive schema impede the building of trust in
intercultural negotiations (Brett & Okumura, 1998;
Jang & Chua, 2011). When negotiating across cul-
tures, negotiation counterparts do not always
share the same implicit scripts, norms, and as-
sumptions. We argue that matching the cognitive
schemas of counterparts, for example, by engaging
in cultural perspective taking, can facilitate bridg-
ing across differences in cognitive schema. At the
same time, future research should continue to in-
vestigate differential effects of cognitive- versus
affective-based mechanisms in intercultural
exchanges.
Also important, our research makes a contribu-
tion to the broader literature on cross-cultural or-
ganizational behavior by empirically examining
intercultural interactions. While much of the orga-
nizational behavior literature discusses cross-
cultural interactions, very few studies empirically
investigate these interactions (Gelfand, Erez, & Ay-
can, 2007). Studies that do examine cross-cultural
interactions and adaptation tend to take a long-
term orientation (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985), and
relatively few examine discreet, individual inter-
actions that cross cultural boundaries (Adair, Oku-
mura, & Brett, 2001; LaBahn & Harich, 1997; Molin-
468 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
sky, 2007). By examining an individual’s behavior
as a function of both their levels of cultural intel-
ligence, as well as the culture of a relevant other,
we extend prior work on cross-cultural interactions
and provide a framework with which future re-
searchers can further extend the body of knowl-
edge on cross-cultural organizational behavior.
Mindfulness in Management
Previous research has argued that mindfulness
training may ameliorate the potential for emo-
tions, fears, prejudices or biases to “hijack”
thought and action (Langer, 2000). A psychological
definition of mindfulness is bringing one’s com-
plete attention to the experiences occurring in the
present moment, in a nonjudgmental and accept-
ing manner (Baer, 2003; Brown & Ryan, 2003; Kabat-
Zinn, 1982). Differently put, it is the “simple act of
drawing novel distinctions” helping us have a
“greater sensitivity to context” and overcome, or
not form, mind-sets that may limit our thinking
(Langer, 2000). It is also suggested that the tech-
niques for cultivating mindfulness all rely upon
slowing down the onrush of mental activity and
trying to focus attention on the world of sensations
in itself rather than “jumping on the first interpre-
tation that comes along” (Claxton, 1997: 183). Along
the same vein, our research provides empirical
evidence suggesting that being mindful of
different-culture counterparts’ thoughts and feel-
ings (by engaging in cultural perspective taking
examined in Study 1) as well as being aware of
different-culture counterparts’ values and beliefs
(Studies 2 and 3) can facilitate intercultural coop-
eration associated with cross-cultural managerial
performance. Similarly, scholars have noted that a
mindful manager—a manager who adopts a posi-
tive nonjudgmental and reflective stance—might
be more likely to engender enhanced empathy and
positive regard that influences task (e.g., financial)
and relational (e.g., long-term business relation-
ship) instrumental outcomes, as well as well-being
(Davidson et al., 2003; Kopelman, Avi-Yonah, & Var-
ghese, 2011). A recent review of mindfulness and
management research suggests that mindfulness
practices can enhance task performance (Dane,
2011). Consistent with these claims, our research
offers an intervention that is associated with en-
gaging in mindfulness about a counterpart’s cul-
tural background and has a direct positive effect
on relational organizational outcomes, such as col-
laboration in international teams (Study 1) and
decision making in mixed-motive conflicts (Studies
2 and 3). Thus, future research should continue
developing interventions focused on making man-
agers mindful of cultural cues in organizational
contexts and examine their effects on managers’
cross-cultural management skills.
Practical Implications
Cross-Cultural Training
To date, one of the most common formats for con-
ducting cross-cultural training programs includes
brief lectures that provide basic information about
the history and socioeconomic situation of a target
or foreign culture (Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000; Earley,
Ang, & Tan, 2006). However, the field has long rec-
ognized that this training approach is problematic
for a number of reasons. First, such training
does not adjust for individual differences in capa-
bility across cognitive, metacognitive, motiva-
tional, and behavioral skills (Earley & Peterson,
2004). Second, such training fails to consider the
nature of the target culture and the work to be
performed in terms of intensity, duration, and na-
ture (Earley et al., 2006). Third, the knowledge pro-
vided to managers in culture-specific training pro-
grams is not transferable across cultural domains
(Earley & Peterson, 2004).
Increasingly, managers take on more frequent
and shorter offshore assignments. As a result, de-
veloping metacognitive tendencies could be more
advantageous than previous frameworks for cross-
cultural training (Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000). As Ear-
ley and Peterson note (2004), career trends have
managers working in more countries and spending
shorter periods in any single country. Thus, it ap-
pears that cross-cultural training should shift to
harness managers’ metacognitive strategies in
tandem with providing in-depth analysis of cul-
tural differences. Developing metacognitive skills
are also important even for managers who don’t
leave their native country but work in internation-
ally diverse teams whose members follow a myr-
iad of country-specific cultural norms (Earley &
Mosakowski, 2000). In instances such as these,
training managers with country-specific knowl-
edge is less practical than equipping global man-
agers with metacognitive skills. With regard to
developing cultural metacognition, Tan and Chua
(2003) acknowledged that while CQ may be par-
tially determined by basic intellectual ability, an
individual’s CQ can still be improved through
2013 469Mor, Morris, and Joh
training. In fact, recent research finds that cross-
cultural management training can enhance cul-
tural intelligence dimensions (Eisenberg et al.,
2013; Rehg et al., 2012). In conclusion, the research
and training approach examined in this paper is
compatible with ongoing structural changes to in-
ternational assignments, and thus, is more practi-
cal and cost effective than training managers with
country-specific knowledge.
Training and Developing Cultural Metacognition
Researchers and practitioners alike may find that
training and harnessing metacognitive habits
among students and managers could benefit both
domestic and expatriate performance. For exam-
ple, teachers may decide to assign MBA students
who score higher on cultural metacognition to ne-
gotiate or work on class assignments with students
scoring low on cultural metacognition, as prior re-
search reveals that in dyads, it is the individual
with the higher metacognitive CQ that drives in-
tercultural success (Chua et al., 2012). Performance
feedback is also an important component for de-
veloping awareness and planning habits among
students. For example, teachers can provide stu-
dents with feedback from self- and peer-ratings, in
order to show them areas where their metacogni-
tive strategies could benefit from development.
Last, cultural simulator exercises (Triandis, 1995)
as well as research websites providing culture-
specific knowledge in business can be integrated
into class assignments prior to intercultural nego-
tiations or international teamwork assignments to
help students develop habits of planning and
awareness associated with higher levels of cul-
tural metacognition.
Working Effectively in Global Teams
Developing metacognitive habits in managers is of
particularly high importance for the success of
multinational teams (Earley & Peterson, 2004).
Global teams often face the challenge of getting
members from different cultures and countries to
work effectively with one another (Earley & Gib-
son, 2002; Hagel & Brown, 2005). Establishing com-
mon goals, clear roles, and consensual norms of
conduct is made difficult by cultural differences
(Earley & Gibson, 2002; Earley & Mosakowski, 2000).
As a result, global teams often search for common-
alities to create a hybrid culture (Adler & Bar-
tholomew, 1992; Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). One
way in which a hybrid culture can develop is by
establishing shared schemas for work tasks. Ac-
cording to Earley and Peterson (2004), metacogni-
tion is critical for developing such shared sche-
mas. Thus, future research should examine
whether a cultural perspective-taking intervention
may enable managers to bridge cultural differ-
ences in global teams.
Developing Cross-Cultural Negotiation Skills
Intercultural negotiations often fail (Brett & Oku-
mura, 1998; Graham, 1985), and thus far manage-
ment education has developed few tools for im-
proving cross-cultural negotiation skills and
outcomes (Adair et al., 2001). A review of negotia-
tion simulations designed to teach cross-cultural
negotiations revealed that most either prepare stu-
dents for particular cultural preferences or partic-
ular cultural styles of communication (Adair, 2008).
In contrast to these types of interventions, a
metacognitive-strategy intervention may instill
general skills for facing differences (Brett, 2007).
Our proposed intervention for cross-cultural nego-
tiation training is consistent with Adler’s (1997)
proposition that cultural adaptation in negotia-
tions (such as matching your counterpart’s strat-
egy) may increase the chances of positive negoti-
ation outcomes. Moreover, the argument that CPT
may promote intercultural negotiation outcomes is
also consistent with past research findings that
general perspective taking in negotiation—the ac-
tive consideration of the other party’s alternatives
and interests prior to negotiation—aids negotia-
tors in both claiming and creating value (Galinsky
et al., 2008; Kemp & Smith, 1994; Neale & Bazer-
man, 1982).
Future Directions
One question that arises from our findings is how
individuals high on cultural perspective taking
adapt to situations in which different-culture coun-
terparts disconfirm culture-based expectations.
Recent research reveals that individuals high on
cultural metacognition are more apt at adjusting
their expectations when a different-culture coun-
terpart disconfirms culture-specific behaviors (Mor
& Morris, 2013). At the same time, congruent with
our findings, individuals high on cultural metacog-
nition are also more likely to apply culture-based
assumptions when a counterpart confirms culture-
based expectancies, such a Chinese person behav-
470 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
ing in a way that confirms a preexisting assump-
tion that she would behave in accordance with
collectivistic values. This agility in applying and
updating assumptions seems like an important
cognitive capability, where globalization forces
may be reducing conformity to culture-based val-
ues and behaviors among some individuals (Fu,
Morris, Lee, Chao, Chiu, & Hong, 2007), and thus,
individuals may need to engage in contingent ap-
plication of culture-based knowledge to arrive at
more coordinated and successful interpersonal
outcomes.
Limitations
The present research has several limitations wor-
thy of note. First, the cultural metacognition scale
is vulnerable to the limits in people’s ability to
reflect upon their mental processes (Nisbett & Wil-
son, 1977). At the same time, past research finds
convergent validity between self-reports and ob-
server reports, suggesting that metacognition is
rated from overt words and behaviors rather than
introspection (Kim & Van Dyne, 2012; Van Dyne,
Ang, & Koh, 2008). Another limitation of the study is
that we examined a cultural perspective-taking
intervention with collectivistic but not individual-
istic counterparts. Also our samples included
mostly Americans. We focused on collectivistic
counterparts when testing the CPT intervention be-
cause of the greater cultural distance between
American culture (individualistic) and Chinese
and Japanese cultures (Oyserman, Coon, & Kem-
melmeier, 2002). At the same time, it is important to
note that past research has found that general
perspective taking can enhance a competitive ori-
entation in competitive contexts but less so in co-
operative situations (Epley, Caruso, & Bazerman,
2005). These findings suggest that cultural per-
spective taking may facilitate cooperation and
joint gains more with collectivistic than individu-
alistic counterparts. Indeed, recent research finds
that East Asian negotiators taking the perspective
of North American negotiators were more self-
interested (than other oriented) and claimed more
value in the negotiations (Lee et al., 2011). None-
theless, we claim that cultural perspective taking
may provide more realistic expectations about
counterparts’ goals and behavior which may facil-
itate social coordination. Consistent with this
claim, prior research found that people from indi-
vidualistic cultures display less cooperative be-
havior in a Prisoner’s Dilemma task than collectiv-
istic individuals (Cox, Lobel, & McLeod, 1991).
Third, while in our studies we did not collect
information about participants’ past experiences
or knowledge with the specific foreign culture of
one’s counterpart, we did assess participants’ for-
eign cultural knowledge by way of the cultural
intelligence assessment and used participants’
levels of foreign cultural knowledge (cognitive CQ)
as a control variable in all of our analyses. The
results revealed that participants self-reported
cognitive CQ did not explain or change our effects.
At the same time, we do recognize that future stud-
ies should examine whether general knowledge of
counterpart’s culture is associated with cultural
perspective-taking tendencies. Another limitation
of the present research is that we have tested some
of our hypotheses using simulated cases rather
than real-life interactions. While recognizing the
limitations of Studies 2 and 3B using a simulated
scenario, we found that the findings from these
studies are consistent with studies examining be-
havioral tendencies by peers (Study 1) and a nego-
tiation exercise that had taken place with a real
counterpart (Study 3A). Along the same vein, much
of past research using economic games relies on
simulated cases or interactions to infer actual real-
world behaviors in real-life interactions and has
found convergent validity across these methods
with field studies (Fein & Spencer, 1997; Landis,
Brislin, & Hulgus, 1985).
CONCLUSIONS
In this article we have focused on identifying and
training a cognitive habit of managers highly ef-
fective at intercultural collaboration: high cultural
metacognitive habits. Future research should con-
tinue examining adaptive cognitive- and affective-
based psychological mechanisms utilized by man-
agers who effectively collaborate with different-
culture counterparts. Notably, the findings and
approach put forth here can provide management
and education scholars with novel insights about
developing interventions and tools for global man-
agers who need to successfully master intercul-
tural collaborations across a wide range of
cultures.
REFERENCES
Adair, W. L. 2008. Go-Go global: Teaching what we know of
culture and the negotiation dance. Negotiation and Conflict
Management Research, 1: 353–370.
2013 471Mor, Morris, and Joh
Adair, W. L., Okumura, T., & Brett, J. M. 2001. Negotiation
behav-
ior when cultures collide: The United States and Japan.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 371–385.
Adler, N. J. 1997. International dimensions of organizational
behaviour south, vol. 164: 101–114. Cincinnati, Ohio: West-
ern College.
Adler, N. J., & Bartholomew, S. 1992. Managing globally
compe-
tent people. The Executive, 6: 52– 65.
Adler, N. J., & Gundersen, A. 2008. International dimensions of
organizational behavior. S Mason, OH: Thomson South
Western.
Aiken., L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. 1991. Multiple
regression:
Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. 2008. Conceptualization of cultural
intelligence: Definition, distinctiveness, and nomological
network. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne, (Eds.), Handbook of
cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applica-
tions: 3–15. New York: M. E. Sharpe.
Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., Koh, C., Ng, K. Y., Templer, K. J., Tay,
C., &
Chandrasekar, N. A. 2007. Cultural intelligence: Its mea-
surement and effects on cultural judgment and decision
making, cultural adaptation and task performance. Man-
agement and Organization Review, 3: 335–371.
Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Tan, M. L. 2011. Cultural
intelligence.
Cambridge Handbook of intelligence: 582– 602. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Baer, R. A. 2003. Mindfulness training as a clinical
intervention:
A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 10: 125–143.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. 1996. Automaticity of
social
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype
activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 71: 230 –244.
Barnard, C. I. 1968. The functions of the executive. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. 1986. The moderator–mediator
variable distinction in social psychological research: Con-
ceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 1173–1182.
Behrend, T. S., Sharek, D. J., Meade, A. W., & Wiebe, E. N.
2011.
The viability of crowdsourcing for survey research. Behav-
ior Research Methods, 43: 800 – 813.
Bhawuk, D., & Brislin, R. 2000. Cross-cultural training: A
review.
Applied Psychology, 49: 162–191.
Black, J. S., & Mendenhall, M. 1990. Cross-cultural training ef-
fectiveness: A review and a theoretical framework for fu-
ture research. Academy of Management Review, 15: 113–
136.
Black, J. S., Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. 1991. Toward a
com-
prehensive model of international adjustment: An integra-
tion of multiple theoretical perspectives. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 16: 291–317.
Boyacigiller, N. A., & Adler, N. J. 1991. The parochial
dinosaur:
Organizational science in a global context. Academy of
Management Review, 16: 262–290.
Brett, J. M. 2007. Negotiating globally: How to negotiate deals,
resolve disputes, and make decisions across cultural bound-
aries. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Brett, J. M., & Okumura, T. 1998. Inter- and intracultural
negoti-
ation: US and Japanese negotiators. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 41: 495–510.
Brislin, R., Worthley, R., & MacNab, B. 2006. Cultural intelli-
gence: Understanding behaviors that serve people’s goals.
Group and Organization Management, 31: 40 –55.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. 2003. The benefits of being
present:
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84: 822– 848.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. 2011. Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-
quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6: 3–5.
Byrne, J. A. 1993. The horizontal corporation. Business Week,
20:
76 – 81.
Caruso, E. M., Epley, N., & Bazerman, M. H. 2006. The good,
the
bad, and the ugly of perspective taking in groups. In E. A.
Mannix, M. A. Neale, & A. E. Tenbrunsel, Eds. Research on
managing groups and teams: Ethics and groups (Vol. 8).
London: Elsevier.
Chua, R. Y. J., Morris, M. W., & Mor, S. 2012. Collaborating
across
cultures: Cultural metacognition and affect-based trust in
creative collaboration. Organizational Behavior and Hu-
man Decision Processes, 118: 179 –188.
Claxton, G. 1997. Hare brain tortoise mind: Why intelligence
increases when you think less. London: Fourth Estate.
Cox, T. H., Lobel, S. A., & McLeod, P. L. 1991. Effects of
ethnic
group cultural differences on cooperative and competitive
behavior on a group task. Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 34: 827– 847.
Dane, E. 2011. Paying attention to mindfulness and its effects
on
task performance in the workplace. Journal of Manage-
ment, 37: 997–1018.
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz,
M.,
Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A.,
Bonus, K., & Sheridan, J. F. 2003. Alterations in brain and
immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psy-
chosomatic Medicine, 65: 564 –570.
Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. 1996. Effect
of
perspective taking on the cognitive representation of per-
sons: A merging of self and other. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 70: 713–726.
Drolet, A. L., & Morris, M. W. 2000. Rapport in conflict
resolution:
Accounting for how face-to-face contact fosters mutual co-
operation in mixed-motive conflicts. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 36: 26 –50.
Earley, P. C., & Ang, S. 2003. Cultural intelligence: Individual
interactions across cultures. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univer-
sity Press.
Earley, P. C., Ang, S., & Tan, J. S. 2006. CQ: Developing
cultural
intelligence at work. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Earley, P. C., & Gibson, C. B. 2002. Multinational work teams:
A
new perspective. Lawrence Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
472 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. 2000. Creating hybrid team
cultures: An empirical test of transnational team function-
ing. Academy of Management Journal, 43: 26 – 49.
Earley, P. C., & Peterson, R. S. 2004. The elusive cultural
chame-
leon: Cultural intelligence as a new approach to intercul-
tural training for the global manager. Academy of Manage-
ment Learning and Education, 3: 100 –115.
Eisenberg, J., Lee, H.-J., Brück, F., Brenner, B., Claes, M.-T.,
Mi-
ronski, J., & Bell, R. In press. Can business schools make
students culturally competent? Academy of Management
Learning and Education.
Epley, N., Caruso, E., & Bazerman, M. H. 2006. When
perspective
taking increases taking: Reactive egoism in social interac-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91: 872–
889.
Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. 1997. Prejudice as self-image mainte-
nance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73: 31– 44.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. 2007. Universal
dimensions
of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 11: 77– 83.
Flavell, J. H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring.
American Psychologist, 34: 906 –911.
Fowler, S. M., & Blohm, J. M. 2004. An analysis of methods for
intercultural training. In D. Landis & J. M. Bennett (Eds.),
Handbook of intercultural training, (3rd ed.: 37– 84). Thou-
sand Oaks. CA: Sage.
Fu, J. H., Morris, M. W., Lee, S. L., Chao, M., Chiu, C. Y., &
Hong,
Y. Y. 2007. Epistemic motives and cultural conformity: Need
for closure, culture, and context as determinants of conflict
judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
92: 191–207.
Galinsky, A. D. 2002. Creating and reducing intergroup
conflict:
The role of perspective-taking in affecting out-group eval-
uations. Research on Managing Groups and Teams, 4: 85–
113.
Galinsky, A. D., Ku, G., & Wang, C. S. 2005. Perspective-
taking
and self-other overlap: Fostering social bonds and facilitat-
ing social coordination. Group Processes and Intergroup
Relations, 8: 109.
Galinsky, A. D., Maddux, W. W., Gilin, D., & White, J. B.
2008.
Why it pays to get inside the head of your opponent. Psy-
chological Science, 19: 378 –384.
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. 2000. Perspective-taking:
Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility,
and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78: 708 –724.
Gelfand, M. J., & Christakopoulou, S. 1999. Culture and negoti-
ator cognition: Judgment accuracy and negotiation pro-
cesses in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Orga-
nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79: 248 –
269.
Gelfand, M. J., Erez, M., & Aycan, Z. 2007. Cross-cultural
organi-
zational behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 479 –
514.
Gelfand, M., & Dyer, N. 2000. A cultural perspective on negoti-
ation: Progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Applied Psychology,
49: 62–99.
Graham, J. L. 1985. The influence of culture on the process of
business negotiations: An exploratory study. Journal of In-
ternational Business Studies, 16: 81–96.
Greenhalgh, L., & Bazerman, M. 2004. Comparative
advertising.
Creative Concensus, Inc. Available from www.
Creativeconsensus-Inc.Com.
Hagel, J., & Brown, J. S. 2005. The only sustainable edge: Why
business strategy depends on productive friction and dy-
namic specialization. Boston, MA: Harvard Business Press.
Hayes, A. F. 2009. Beyond baron and Kenny: Statistical media-
tion analysis in the new millennium. Communication
Monographs, 76: 408 – 420.
Imai, L., & Gelfand, M. J. 2010. The culturally intelligent
negoti-
ator: The impact of cultural intelligence (CQ) on negotiation
sequences and outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Hu-
man Decision Processes, 112: 83–98.
Jang, S., & Chua, R. 2011. Building 16 Intercultural Trust at the
negotiating table. In Negotiation excellence: Successful
Deal making. Singapore: World Scientific.
Johnson, J. L., Cullen, J. B., Sakano, T., & Takenouchi, H.
1996.
Setting the stage for trust and strategic integration in Jap-
anese-US cooperative alliances. Journal of International
Business Studies, 27: 981–1004.
Kabat-Zinn, J. 1982. An outpatient program in behavioral medi-
cine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of
mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and
preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4: 33– 47.
Kemp, K. E., & Smith, W. P. 1994. Information exchange,
tough-
ness, and integrative bargaining: The roles of explicit cues
and perspective-taking. International Journal of Conflict
Management, 5: 5–21.
Kim, Y. J., & Van Dyne, L. 2012. Cultural intelligence and
inter-
national leadership potential: The importance of contact for
members of the majority. Applied Psychology, 61: 272–294.
Klafehn, J., Banerjee, P., & Chiu, C.-Y. 2008. Navigating
cultures:
The role of metacognitive cultural intelligence. In S. Ang &
L. Van Dyne (Eds.), Handbook of cultural intelligence: The-
ory, measurement, and applications: 318 –331. New York:
M. E. Sharpe.
Kolb, A. Y., & Kolb, D. A. 2005. Learning styles and learning
spaces: Enhancing experiential learning in higher educa-
tion. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 4:
193–212.
Kopelman, S., Avi-Yonah, O., & Varghese, A. 2011. The
mindful
negotiator: Strategic emotion management and wellbeing.
Paper Presented at the IACM Annual Conference, Istanbul,
Turkey.
Ku, G., Wang, C. S., & Galinsky, A. D. 2010. Perception
through
a perspective-taking lens: Differential effects on judgment
and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
46: 792–798.
LaBahn, D. W., & Harich, K. R. 1997. Sensitivity to national
business culture: Effects on US-Mexican channel relation-
2013 473Mor, Morris, and Joh
ship performance. Journal of International Marketing, 5:
29 –51.
Landis, D., Brislin, R. W., & Hulgus, J. F. 1985. Attributional
training versus contact in acculturative learning: A, Labo-
ratory Study 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15:
466 – 482.
Langer, E. J. 2000. Mindful learning. Current Directions in Psy-
chological Science, 9: 220 –223.
Lee, S., Adair, W. L., & Seo, S.-J. 2011. Cultural Perspective
Taking in Cross-Cultural Negotiation. Group Decision and
Negotiation: 1–17.
Lytle, A., Brett, J., Barsness, Z., Tinsley, C., & Janssens, M.
1995. A
paradigm for confirmatory cross-cultural research in orga-
nizational-behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior,
17: 167–214.
Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P., Jr.. 1987. Leading workers to lead
themselves: The external leadership of self-managing work
teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32: 106 –129.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. 1991. Culture and the self:
Impli-
cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychologi-
cal Review, 98: 224.
McAllister, D. J. 1995. Affect- and cognition-based trust as
foun-
dations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations.
Academy of Management Journal, 38: 24 –59.
Mendenhall, M. E., Stahl, G. K., Ehnert, I., Oddou, G., Osland,
J. S., & Kuhlmann, T. 2004. Evaluation studies of cross-
cultural training programs. In D. Landis, J. M. Bennett, &
M. J. Bennett (Eds.). Handbook of intercultural training (3rd
ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. 1985. The dimensions of
expatriate
acculturation: A review. Academy of Management Review,
10: 39 – 47.
Molinsky, A. 2007. Cross-cultural code-switching: The psycho-
logical challenges of adapting behavior in foreign cultural
interactions. Academy of Management Review, 32: 622– 640.
Mor, S., Ames, D. R., & Joh, J. 2013. Cultural metacognition
and
calibration in cross-cultural knowledge. Unpublished man-
uscript.
Mor, S., & Morris, M. W. 2013. The role of cultural
metacognition
in applying and updating cultural stereotypes in intercul-
tural interaction. Paper presented at Academy of Manage-
ment Annual Conference, Orlando, Florida.
Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. 1982. Role of perspective-
taking
ability in negotiating under different forms of arbitration.
Industrial and Labour Relations Review, 36: 378.
Ng, K. Y., Van Dyne, L., & Ang, S. 2009. From experience to
experiential learning: Cultural intelligence as a learning
capability for global leader development. Academy of Man-
agement Learning and Education Archive, 8: 511–526.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. 1977. Telling more than we can
know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological
Review, 84: 231–259.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. 2002.
Rethinking
individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical
assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin,
128: 3–72.
Patel, A., & Brett, J. 2007. Abhas-Bussan exercise. Evanston,
IL:
DRRC, Northwestern University.
Raudenbush, S. W. 2004. HLM, vol. 6: Hierarchical linear and
nonlinear modeling. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software Inter-
national.
Rehg, M. T., Gundlach, M. J., & Grigorian, R. A. 2012.
Examining
the influence of cross-cultural training on cultural intelli-
gence and specific self-efficacy. Cross cultural manage-
ment: An International Journal, 19: 215–232.
Takahashi, C., Yamagishi, T., Liu, J. H., Wang, F., Lin, Y., &
Yu,
S. 2008. The intercultural trust paradigm: Studying joint
cultural interaction and social exchange in real time over
the internet. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,
32: 215–228.
Tan, J. S., & Chua, R. Y. 2003. Training and developing
cultural
intelligence. In Cultural intelligence: Individual interac-
tions across Cultures, 258 –303. Stanford, CA: Stanford Uni-
versity Press.
Thomas, D. C. 2006. Domain and development of cultural intel-
ligence: The importance of mindfulness. Group & Organi-
zation Management, 31: 78 –99.
Thomas, D. C., Elron, E., Stahl, G., Ekelund, B. Z., Ravlin, E.
C.,
Cerdin, J.-L., Poelmans, S., Brislin, R., Pekerti, A., & Aycan,
Z.
2008. Cultural intelligence domain and assessment. Inter-
national Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 8: 123–143.
Todd, A. R., & Burgmer, P. 2013. Perspective taking and auto-
matic intergroup evaluation change: Testing an associative
self-anchoring account. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 104: 786 – 802.
Triandis, H. C. 1995. Culture specific assimilators. Intercultural
Sourcebook: Cross-Cultural Training Methods, 1: 179 –186.
Triandis, H. C. 2006. Cultural intelligence in organizations.
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx

More Related Content

Similar to @charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx

The cross cultural training for the global workforce and
The cross cultural training for the global workforce andThe cross cultural training for the global workforce and
The cross cultural training for the global workforce andsudeeppanicker
 
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more dept
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more deptFor your response posts, do the following demonstrate more dept
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more deptarnit1
 
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate Setting
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate SettingCulture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate Setting
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate SettingCatherine (Cass) Mercer Bing
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity
Developig itercultural sensitivityDevelopig itercultural sensitivity
Developig itercultural sensitivityzhoujia
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxcheryllwashburn
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxfathwaitewalter
 
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transferLTGnetrade
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
LCO Partners introduction
LCO Partners introductionLCO Partners introduction
LCO Partners introductionFernando Lanzer
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改zhoujia
 
cultural aspects of information systems development
cultural aspects of information systems developmentcultural aspects of information systems development
cultural aspects of information systems developmentJan Pawlowski
 
Converging on IHRM Best Practices • 123CONVERGING .docx
Converging on IHRM Best Practices     •     123CONVERGING .docxConverging on IHRM Best Practices     •     123CONVERGING .docx
Converging on IHRM Best Practices • 123CONVERGING .docxdickonsondorris
 
research on Psychology
research on Psychologyresearch on Psychology
research on Psychologybiola1
 

Similar to @charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx (20)

The cross cultural training for the global workforce and
The cross cultural training for the global workforce andThe cross cultural training for the global workforce and
The cross cultural training for the global workforce and
 
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more dept
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more deptFor your response posts, do the following demonstrate more dept
For your response posts, do the following demonstrate more dept
 
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate Setting
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate SettingCulture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate Setting
Culture, Power and Applied Anthropology in a Corporate Setting
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity
Developig itercultural sensitivityDevelopig itercultural sensitivity
Developig itercultural sensitivity
 
Sample
SampleSample
Sample
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
 
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docxRunning Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
Running Head THESIS 1THESIS2Comment by Author I thi.docx
 
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer
22 cross bordner-knowledge-transfer
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
LCO Partners introduction
LCO Partners introductionLCO Partners introduction
LCO Partners introduction
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
Developig itercultural sensitivity修改
 
cultural aspects of information systems development
cultural aspects of information systems developmentcultural aspects of information systems development
cultural aspects of information systems development
 
Converging on IHRM Best Practices • 123CONVERGING .docx
Converging on IHRM Best Practices     •     123CONVERGING .docxConverging on IHRM Best Practices     •     123CONVERGING .docx
Converging on IHRM Best Practices • 123CONVERGING .docx
 
Culture Design 101
Culture Design 101Culture Design 101
Culture Design 101
 
research on Psychology
research on Psychologyresearch on Psychology
research on Psychology
 

More from ransayo

Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docx
Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docxZoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docx
Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docxransayo
 
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docx
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docxZlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docx
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docxransayo
 
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docx
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docxZia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docx
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docxransayo
 
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docx
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docxZiyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docx
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docxransayo
 
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docx
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docxZiyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docx
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docxransayo
 
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docx
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docxZhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docx
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docxransayo
 
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docx
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docxZichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docx
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docxransayo
 
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docx
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docxZheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docx
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docxransayo
 
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docx
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docxZhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docx
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docxransayo
 
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docx
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docxZhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docx
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docxransayo
 
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docx
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docxZhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docx
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docxransayo
 
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docx
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docxZero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docx
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docxransayo
 
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docxZero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docxransayo
 
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docxZero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docxransayo
 
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docx
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docxZero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docx
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docxransayo
 
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docx
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docxZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docx
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docxransayo
 
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docx
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docxzctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docx
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docxransayo
 
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docx
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docxZeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docx
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docxransayo
 
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docx
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docxzClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docx
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docxransayo
 
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docx
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docxzClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docx
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docxransayo
 

More from ransayo (20)

Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docx
Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docxZoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docx
Zoe is a second grader with autism spectrum disorders. Zoe’s father .docx
 
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docx
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docxZlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docx
Zlatan Ibrahimović – Sports PsychologyOutlineIntroduction .docx
 
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docx
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docxZia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docx
Zia 2Do You Choose to AcceptYour mission, should you choose.docx
 
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docx
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docxZiyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docx
Ziyao LiIAS 3753Dr. Manata HashemiWorking Title The Edu.docx
 
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docx
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docxZiyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docx
Ziyan Huang (Jerry)Assignment 4Brand PositioningProfessor .docx
 
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docx
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docxZhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docx
Zhtavius Moye04192019BUSA 4126SWOT AnalysisDr. Setliff.docx
 
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docx
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docxZichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docx
Zichun Gao Professor Karen Accounting 1AIBM FInancial Stat.docx
 
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docx
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docxZheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docx
Zheng Hes Inscription This inscription was carved on a stele erec.docx
 
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docx
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docxZhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docx
Zhou 1Time and Memory in Two Portal Fantasies An Analys.docx
 
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docx
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docxZhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docx
Zhang 1Yixiang ZhangTamara KuzmenkovEnglish 101.docx
 
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docx
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docxZhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docx
Zhang 1Nick ZhangMr. BetheaLyric Peotry13.docx
 
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docx
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docxZero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docx
Zero trust is a security stance for networking based on not trusting.docx
 
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docxZero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesNature offers many examples of sp.docx
 
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docxZero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docx
Zero plagiarism4 referencesLearning ObjectivesStudents w.docx
 
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docx
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docxZero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docx
Zero Plagiarism or receive a grade of a 0.Choose one important p.docx
 
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docx
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docxZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docx
ZACHARY SHEMTOB AND DAVID LATZachary Shemtob, formerly editor in.docx
 
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docx
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docxzctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docx
zctnoFrl+.1Affid ow9iar!(al+{FJr.docx
 
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docx
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docxZeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docx
Zeng Jiawen ZengChenxia Zhu English 3001-015292017Refl.docx
 
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docx
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docxzClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docx
zClass 44.8.19§ Announcements§ Go over quiz #1.docx
 
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docx
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docxzClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docx
zClass 185.13.19§ Announcements§ Review of last .docx
 

Recently uploaded

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 

Recently uploaded (20)

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 

@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx

  • 1. @charset "utf-8"; @font-face { font-family: Champagne; src: url(cac_champagne.woff) format('woff'), url(cac_champagne.ttf) format('ttf'); } /* New Perspectives on HTML5 and CSS3, 7th Edition Tutorial 3 Review Assignment Typographical Style Sheet for the Pandaisia Chocolates Monthly Specials Filename: pc_styles4.css
  • 2. */ html { background-color: rgb(186, 136, 81); } /* Page Body Styles */ body { background-color: rgb(138, 97, 51); font-family: Verdana, Geneva, Arial, sans-serif; } /* Horizontal Navigation List Styles */ nav.horizontal a { background-color: rgb(113, 86, 39);
  • 3. font-family: Champagne, cursive; font-size: 1.4em; line-height: 1.8em; text-align: center; } nav.horizontal a:link, nav.horizontal a:visited { color: rgb(239, 220, 156); } nav.horizontal a:hover, nav.horizontal a:active { background-color: rgb(71, 52, 29); } /* Article Styles */
  • 4. article > h1 { font-family: 'Times New Roman', Times, serif; font-size: 2.2em; font-weight: normal; margin: 0.5em 0; } article p { line-height: 1.2; margin-bottom: 2em; } /* Specials Styles */ div.specials { background-color: rgba(255, 255, 255, 0.15); }
  • 5. div.specials h1 { font-family: Champagne, cursive; font-size: 1.8em; font-weight: normal; margin: 0.3em 0; text-align: center; } div.specials p { font-size: 0.8em; margin-bottom: 1em; line-height: 1.2; } div.specials a { color: rgb(186, 186, 156);
  • 6. } /* Award Styles */ div#awardList { background-color: rgb(172, 116, 30); margin-top: 20px; } div#awardList h1 { font-family: Champagne, cursive; font-size: 2.5em; font-weight: normal; text-align: center; }
  • 7. div.awards p { font-size: 0.8em; text-align: center; margin: 0.3em 0; } div.awards img { display: block; width: 100%; } /* Footer Styles */ footer { color: rgb(186, 186, 156); background-color: rgb(71, 52, 29);
  • 8. font-size: 0.9em; padding: 10px 0px; text-align: center; } Identifying and Training Adaptive Cross-Cultural Management Skills: The Crucial Role of Cultural Metacognition SHIRA MOR Erasmus University MICHAEL MORRIS Columbia University JOHANN JOH London Business School For managers, intercultural effectiveness requires forging close working relationships with people from different cultural backgrounds (Black, Mendenhall, & Oddou, 1991). Recent research with executives has found that higher cultural metacognition is associated with affective closeness and creative collaboration in intercultural
  • 9. relationships (Chua, Morris, & Mor, & 2012). However, little is known about the social cognitive mechanisms that facilitate the performance of individuals who score high on cultural metacognition. We propose that one important question for cross-cultural research and training is identifying which metacognitive strategies enable successful intercultural collaborations. We suggest that one such strategy is “cultural perspective taking”— considering how another’s cultural background shapes their behavior in a given context. We hypothesized that cultural perspective taking facilitates intercultural coordination and cooperation, and that a manipulation that boosts cultural perspective taking would be especially beneficial for individuals who score low in dispositional cultural metacognition. We found support for the above hypotheses in five studies using both quasi-field and experimental approaches. We discuss the implications of these findings for literatures on expatriate managers, cross-cultural training, cultural intelligence, and intercultural negotiations. ............................................................................................... ......................................................................... “For him to have understood me would have meant reorganizing his thinking . . . . giving up his intellectual ballast, and few people are willing to risk such a radical move.” –Edward T. Hall
  • 10. The globalization of business presses managers to communicate, negotiate, and collaborate across cultures (Adler & Gundersen, 2008; Boyacigiller & Adler, 1991). Firms depend on informal coordina- tion between managers located in different parts We thank Adam Galinsky and Modupe Akinola for their help with data collection for Study 3A. We are grateful to Daniel Ames, E. Tory Higgins, Shirley Kopelman, Roy Chua, Krishna Savani, and Aaron Wallen for providing their comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. � Academy of Management Learning & Education, 2013, Vol. 12, No. 3, 453–475. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amle.2012.0202 ............................................................................................... ......................................................................... 453 Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only. of the organization (Barnard, 1968). In global firms, this requires intercultural collaboration. While ex- patriate assignments can facilitate the transfer of knowledge across borders, expatriates often face difficulties in adjusting to the foreign culture (Caruso, Epley, & Bazerman, 2006), which can result in early termination of assignments (Graham, 1985). Other mechanisms for coordination in global
  • 11. organizations are multinational teams and inter- national alliances (Adler & Gundersen, 2008; By- rne, 1993; Manz & Sims, 1987). Yet such structures often run aground on the failure of managers from different cultures and countries to work effectively with one another (Earley & Gibson, 2002; Hagel & Brown, 2005). Firms have long sought employees with cross- cultural capabilities but have not known how best to select for them or develop them. Recently, man- agement researchers integrated disparate insights about relevant characteristics and capabilities un- der the rubric of cultural intelligence (CQ; Earley & Ang, 2003; Earley & Peterson, 2004; Ng, Van Dyne, & Ang, 2009). Earley and Ang (2003) define cultural intelligence as the capability of an individual to function effectively in culturally diverse settings. We suggest that cultural intelligence is an individ- ual difference that can inform the field’s renewed interest in cross-cultural research and training programs (Black & Mendenhall, 1990; Mendenhall, Stahl, Ehnert, Oddou, Osland, & Kuhlmann, 2004; Rehg, Gundlach, & Grigorian, 2012). More specifi- cally, the present research aims to bridge research and practice by identifying specific social cogni- tive strategies associated with cultural intelli- gence, strategies that might be targeted by inter- cultural training programs. We first highlight the contribution of our approach to cross-cultural training and past and ongoing research. NOVEL APPROACH TO CROSS-CULTURAL TRAINING AND RESEARCH Our research offers a novel approach to cross-
  • 12. cultural research and training by examining an individual difference measure associated with in- tercultural success and its associated social cog- nitive mechanisms. We propose that this approach can contribute to researchers and practitioners’ un- derstanding of cross-cultural competency and training in a number of ways. First, we look for trainable strategies through a descriptive ap- proach of identifying thought patterns associated with an individual difference dimension that is known to correlate with intercultural effectiveness. Second, we examine social cognitive mechanisms associated with intercultural performance. Third, we examine intercultural performance in the con- text of working in the home country, not just in overseas assignments. Notably, in contrast to prior training programs focused on teaching managers culture-specific knowledge (Fowler & Blohm, 2004; Mendenhall et al., 2004), we focus on general strat- egies for dealing with cultural differences, not on training specific to particular cultures. We intro- duce our approach and hypotheses by first review- ing the literature on cultural intelligence and the specific role of cultural metacognition in promot- ing intercultural performance. CULTURAL METACOGNITION Ang and Van Dyne (2008) have proposed four cen- tral dimensions that foster cultural intelligence: metacognitive, cognitive, motivational, and behav- ioral. Our focus is on the first of these dimensions– cultural metacognition. Metacognition is defined as thinking about thinking, comprising the pro- cesses of monitoring and adjusting one’s thoughts
  • 13. and strategies as one learns new skills (Triandis, 1995). Cultural metacognition refers to an individ- ual’s level of conscious cultural awareness and executive processing during cross-cultural interac- tions (Ang & Van Dyne, 2008). Thomas et al. (2008) propose that this dimension is the core of cultural intelligence. Past researchers have found that intercultural success is correlated with mindfulness and self- awareness about cultural assumptions (Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, & Takenouchi, 1996; LaBahn & Harich, 1997). Those with high metacognitive cul- tural intelligence (CQ) are consciously aware of others’ cultural preferences before and during in- teractions. They also question cultural assump- tions and adjust their mental models during and after interactions (Brislin, Worthley, & MacNab, 2006; Triandis, 2006). Other theorists of intercul- tural competence have proposed similar infer- ences as the signature of cultural metacognition We suggest that cultural intelligence is an individual difference that can inform the field’s renewed interest in cross- cultural research and training programs. 454 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education (Klafehn, Banerjee, & Chiu, 2008; Thomas, 2006). Evidence suggests that cultural metacognition de- velops through reflection during intercultural ex- periences (Ng et al., 2009), consistent with evidence that reflection helps people fine-tune their as-
  • 14. sumptions after experiences (Kolb & Kolb, 2005). Although the construct of metacognition is new in management research, it follows a tradition em- phasizing the importance of self-awareness and sensitivity toward others when adjusting to new environments (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985). Cogni- tive psychologists characterize metacognition as monitoring and adjusting one’s thoughts and strat- egies as one learns new skills (Flavell, 1979). Ex- panding this line of theorizing, Ang et al. (2007) defined cultural metacognition as mental pro- cesses directed at acquiring, comprehending, and calibrating cultural knowledge. Researchers (Klafehn, Banerjee, & Chiu, 2008) suggest that metacognition in cultural domains increases inter- cultural effectiveness by promoting (1) contextual- ized thinking (i.e., heightened sensitivity to the fact that individuals’ motivations and behaviors are invariably shaped by the cultural contexts in which they are embedded), and (2) cognitive flexi- bility (i.e., discriminative use of mental schemas and behavioral scripts when interacting across cultures). The ability to think contextually and flex- ibly about issues from a foreign counterpart’s per- spective has been termed cultural perspective tak- ing in the social psychology and negotiations literature (Lee, Adair, & Seo, 2011). Thus, cultural perspective taking may play a central role in the inferences that enable individuals high in cultural metacognition to interact effectively across cultures. IDENTIFYING AND TRAINING A METACOGNITIVE STRATEGY
  • 15. Recent research examining the role of cultural metacognition in cross-cultural performance has found that cultural metacognition is associated with greater success in collaborating in intercul- tural (but not intracultural) relationships (Chua et al., 2012). However, past research has yet to iden- tify which metacognitive strategies may facilitate intercultural coordination. We propose that one metacognitive strategy facilitating intercultural coordination is considering how another person’s cultural background may affect their response to a situation—“cultural perspective taking” (Lee et al., 2011). This thought process, while not foolproof, generally heightens the accuracy of people’s ex- pectations about a foreign counterpart’s intentions and behaviors. Of course, it could be ineffective when a counterpart’s actual intentions are oppo- site to those typical of his group, but this is by definition atypical. We argue that examining cog- nitive processes that facilitate interpersonal accu- racy among American managers is important to investigate, as past research reveals that people from individualistic cultures, compared to those from collectivistic cultures, are less accurate in assessing their counterparts’ interests (Gelfand & Christakopoulou, 1999). We explore whether inducing cultural perspec- tive taking can improve effectiveness in working with individuals from different cultures, critical to organizational success. We expected that this in- tervention would be particularly helpful for man- agers who are low in cultural metacognition, ei- ther by disposition or habit. We propose that cultural perspective taking works like other forms
  • 16. of perspective taking, in that imagining the world from another’s person’s perspective (under specific conditions) has been found to reduce some prob- lematic patterns in social cognition, such as con- firmation bias and stereotyping (Galinsky, 2002; Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). Perspective taking brings one’s self-experience closer to one’s repre- sentation of the other person (Davis, Conklin, Smith, & Luce, 1996). Hence, perspective takers are more likely to see themselves as similar to the other person (Galinsky, Ku, & Wang, 2005; Todd & Burgmer, 2013). Related to this, perspective taking can lead to mimicking the other person’s observed or expected behavior (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996). In this way, perspective taking can help in meshing or coordinating with another person (Ga- linsky et al., 2005) and offers a unique advantage in interpersonal coordination in comparison with affective-based mechanisms such as empathy (Ga- linsky, Maddux, Gilin, & White, 2008). In line with past research and the propositions we put forth above, we propose the following: Hypothesis 1a: Cultural metacognition will be pos- itively associated with intercul- tural cooperation. Hypothesis 1b: The relationship between cultural metacognition and intercultural cooperation will be mediated by cultural perspective taking. How would cultural perspective taking change a manager’s negotiation strategy? Consider the case of an American facing a counterpart from a col-
  • 17. 2013 455Mor, Morris, and Joh lectivistic culture. Cross-cultural research finds that collectivists favor cooperative, relationship- preserving tactics, whereas individualists favor competitive tactics and emphasize their own needs (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Culture influences nor- mative beliefs about what is appropriate, what others will do, and what others expect (Gelfand & Dyer, 2000; Lytle, Brett, Barsness, Tinsley, & Jans- sens, 1995). When Americans reflect on a collectiv- istic counterpart’s beliefs and practices, they should become more likely to adopt relational goals and cooperative tactics themselves. Simi- larly, prior research on perspective taking has found that when taking the perspective of an out- group member, individuals subsequently act more consistently with the other group’s norms (Ku, Wang & Galinsky, 2010). Thus, we propose the fol- lowing predictions: Hypothesis 2a: An intervention inducing cultural perspective taking will promote in- tercultural cooperation with collec- tivistic counterparts. Hypothesis 2b: The relationship between cultural perspective taking and intercul- tural cooperation will be mediated by one’s relational goals toward collectivistic counterparts. Hypothesis 2c: The relationship between cultural
  • 18. perspective taking and intercul- tural cooperation will be mediated by expectations about the rela- tional goals of collectivistic counterparts. Extending past theory on cultural intelligence and cultural metacognition, we further propose that a cultural perspective-taking intervention would have a larger positive impact on intercul- tural cooperation decisions of individuals low on cultural metacognitive tendencies. Hypothesis 2d: Inducing cultural perspective tak- ing would promote intercultural co- operation more for individuals with low habitual cultural metacogni- tion than those with high habitual cultural metacognition. The Present Research Although past research has examined perspective taking broadly, few studies have experimentally examined the role of cultural perspective taking in promoting cross-cultural management skills, such as facilitating intercultural collaboration and co- operation in international teams, cross-cultural ne- gotiations, or mixed-motive tasks. In extending past research on cultural intelligence and cross- cultural training, we contend that one important cognitive tendency that managers with high levels of metacognitive tendencies (high meta-CQ) en- gage in is cultural perspective taking prior to and during an intercultural interaction. More impor-
  • 19. tant, we suggest that cultural perspective taking tendencies can be temporarily heightened, and thus, can be especially beneficial for managers chronically low on cultural metacognitive tendencies. Methodological Approach The methodological approach we employ to test our hypotheses is adopted from social psychology research and involves experimental inductions to manipulate social cognitive processes, such as cultural perspective taking. To examine the causal relationships between our variables of interest, we use an experimental design with samples of MBA students and crowdsourcing participants by way of Mechanical Turk. We chose to use Mechanical Turk participants for our experimental designs rather than undergraduate student samples to test our hypotheses for a number of reasons. First, re- cent research reveals that Mechanical Turk partic- ipants are significantly more diverse than typical American college samples (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Moreover, compared with a tradi- tional university participant pool, crowdsourcing respondents have more work experience, are often older, and are more ethnically diverse (Behrend, Sharek, Meade, & Wiebe, 2011). Thus, by examin- ing our hypotheses using both individuals with professional experience and MBA students’ sam- ples, we believe that the social cognitive mecha- nisms examined are shared among lay people as well as managers, and thus, can be used to train individuals in a diverse array of professional settings.
  • 20. OVERVIEW OF STUDIES We examined our hypotheses in five studies, which span from quasi-field settings to controlled laboratory experiments. In the first, we examine whether cultural metacognition among American MBA students was associated with greater levels of intercultural cooperation with different-culture peers working together in international teams and 456 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education whether this effect is explained by cultural perspective-taking habits (Hyp 1a and Hyp 1b). In our pilot study, we tested a cultural perspective- taking (CPT) intervention—thinking about a Chi- nese counterpart’s cultural values and beliefs— prior to making a business decision to cooperate in a mixed-motive business scenario. We examined whether this intervention heightened individuals’ intercultural cooperation due to heightening inten- tions to adopt a relational orientation (associated with Chinese cultural values; Hyp 2a and Hyp 2b). In Study 2, we investigate whether a cultural perspective-taking intervention promoted Ameri- can MBA students’ intercultural cooperation in the same business scenario as the pilot study, but we further examined whether this effect could also be explained by expectations about the counterpart’s relational goals (Hyp 2c). In Study 3A, we examine whether inducing CPT in American MBAs prior to engaging in intercultural negotiation with a Japa- nese counterpart heightened MBA students’ expec- tations that their Japanese counterpart would be
  • 21. cooperative in an upcoming negotiation. We fur- ther examined whether this intervention was more beneficial for MBAs low on cultural metacognition than high on cultural metacognition (Hyp 2d). In Study 3B we provide a comprehensive test of Hy- potheses 2c-2d in a unified design. STUDY 1: CULTURAL METACOGNITION AND INTERCULTURAL COOPERATION The goal of Study 1 is to examine Hypotheses H1a and H1b using a sample of American MBA students studying in an American business school where 50% of the student population is composed of international students. In this MBA program, inter- cultural cooperation is needed to complete study- related tasks, such as papers and class assign- ments. To examine our predictions, we surveyed entering students about their cultural metacogni- tive tendencies. Two months subsequent to evalu- ating students’ cultural metacognition tendencies, we evaluated students’ cultural perspective-taking and cooperation levels using ratings made by their international peers who had been working with them in multinational student teams for 2 months. We hypothesized that MBA students’ cultural meta- cognition would be positively associated with different-culture peers’ evaluation of their cooper- ative tendencies and that this effect would be ex- plained by heightened cultural perspective-taking tendencies. Method Participants and Procedure
  • 22. Two hundred American MBA students (Males � 58.5%; Mean Age � 27; 61.3% had managed at least one employee in their previous jobs) were recruited to fill out an on-line survey as a voluntary part of their pre-MBA assignments (73% of incoming stu- dents completed the survey). Students were pro- vided a link to an on-line survey that asked about their past work experiences abroad and their ex- periences working in multicultural work environ- ments—an environment where foreign national students are equally represented as American ones. The cultural intelligence measure was col- lected as part of this survey. Upon arrival, students were assigned to multinational learning teams of 5– 6 students. Teams were created to maximize their cultural diversity and typically comprised of three American students, one European student, and two students from other world regions such as Africa, South America, the Middle East, and East Asia or South Asia. These teams assemble in MBA student orientation and students spend the major- ity of orientation activities and their first year of classes working in teams. After 2 months in their international teams, 305 international student peers (from non-U.S. nationalities), representing 45 nationalities evaluated target American students on a host of leadership-related measures as part of a 360 leadership assessment. Each student was evaluated by between 1 and 4 different-culture peers with whom they worked. Student peers were asked to anonymously appraise their team mem- bers’ levels of cooperation and perspective-taking tendencies when working in these teams as part of their class assignment. Let us now describe the measures in the study in more detail.
  • 23. Measures Cultural Metacognition Incoming MBA students reported their cultural metacognition tendencies using a 6-item scale de- veloped by Van Dyne, Ang, Ng, Rockstuhl, Tan, and Koh (2012). The items tap (1) cultural awareness (e.g., “I am aware of how to use my cultural knowl- edge when interacting with people from different cultures”); (2) adjustment during intercultural inter- actions (“I adjust my cultural knowledge while in- teracting with people from a new or an unfamiliar culture”); and (3) planning before intercultural in- teractions (e.g., “I develop action plans for interact- 2013 457Mor, Morris, and Joh ing with people from a different culture”; Scale reliability: � � .82). We averaged students’ self- reports on the six items to create a cultural meta- cognition score for each student. Cultural Perspective Taking After working 2 months in their international teams, participants were evaluated by classmates from other cultures with regard to their perspective- taking habits. The items were the following: “S/he is able to empathize and understand someone else’s perspective”; “S/he misjudges people’s per- sonality and character” [Reversed]; “S/he fails to realize the impact of what s/he says and does on others” [Reversed]; “S/he is good at assessing other
  • 24. people’s strengths and weaknesses”; “S/he is good at sensing what other people are thinking and feeling” (Scale reliability: � � .83). We averaged each rater’s evaluation of each target student on these five items to create each rater’s cultural perspective-taking score for the target student. Intercultural Cooperation Peers also evaluated target students on three items that assessed American MBA students’ abil- ity to work effectively in their international teams. The items were the following: “She/he is able to build effective working relationships with others who have different opinions or interests” and “She/he is able to build coalitions to get things done” (Scale reliability: � � .65). We averaged each rater’s evaluation of each target student on these two items to create each rater’s intercultural coop- eration score for the target student. We conducted confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) using structural equation modeling (LISREL 8.80) to ensure that the mediator and outcome vari- able (cultural perspective taking and intercultural cooperation) are distinct factors. Results indicate that a 2-factor model where cultural perspective taking and intercultural cooperation items load into respective separate factors has a better fit to the data (�2 � 35.15, df � 13, RMSEA � 0.08) than a 1-factor model (�2 � 35.16, df � 14, RMSEA � 0.09). Control Variables Dimensions of cultural intelligence other than cul- tural metacognition, namely cognitive, motiva-
  • 25. tional, and behavioral CQ, have been found to predict intercultural cooperation tendencies (Imai & Gelfand, 2010). As a result, we included these scales’ control variables: cognitive CQ (� � .84), motivational CQ (� � .86), and behavioral CQ (� � .86). Additionally, since student peer ratings of target students’ cooperation levels may be influenced by their levels of acquaintance with target students outside of class assignments, we included raters’ familiarity with the target student as an additional control variable (“how well do you know this per- son?”, 1 � Not at all to 4 � extremely well). Results and Discussion Hypothesis 1a predicted that cultural metacogni- tion would be positively associated with intercul- tural cooperation. To test this hypothesis, we used a hierarchical linear model (HLM) software to carry out our analyses (Raudenbush, 2004). We ran a linear HLM treating different-culture student peers as nested within target American students, with student-peer ratings as the dependent variable (at Level 1) and cultural metacognition and the control variables as Level 2 predictors. Table 1 reports the results from the hierarchical linear model analy- ses. Model 1 contains the control variables; whereas Model 2 adds the predictor of cultural metacognition. Analyses revealed that cultural TABLE 1 Hierarchical Linear Model Regression on Student
  • 26. Peers’ Ratings of Intercultural Cooperation (Study 1) Intercultural cooperation Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Key predictors Cultural metacognition — 0.20* 0.06 (�0.09) (�0.07) Cultural perspective taking (mediator) 0.67** (�0.04) Control variables Cognitive CQ 0.05 �0.01 0.01 (�0.07) (�0.08) (�0.06) Motivational CQ 0.14� 0.09 0.12� (�0.08) (�0.09) (�0.07) Behavioral CQ �0.04 �0.07 �0.08 (�0.07) (�0.07) (�0.05) Student peers familiarity 0.23** 0.26** 0.07 (0.10) (0.10) (0.08) Intercept 4.23** 3.91** 1.10** (0.46) (0.48) (0.41) Note. CQ � cultural intelligence. Numbers in parenthesis are standard errors. �p � .10, **p � .01, *p � .05.
  • 27. 458 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education metacognition was positively associated with peers’ evaluation of target students’ levels of coop- eration in international teams, � � .20, SE � .09, t(195) � 2.22, p � .05. Thus, the results supported Hypothesis 1a. Next, we examined whether cultural perspective- taking tendencies mediated the relationship be- tween cultural metacognition and intercultural cooperation. To conduct mediational analyses (in this study and follow-up studies), we followed Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures as well as carried out a bootstrapping test with 20,000 re- samples using percentile bootstraps (Hayes, 2009). Analyses revealed that cultural metacognition (Level 2 predictor) was positively associated with student peers’ evaluation of target students’ cul- tural perspective-taking tendencies (Level 1 de- pendent variable), � � .20, SE � .09, t(195) � 2.26, p � .05. Second, when cultural metacognition and cultural perspective taking (mediator) were both entered into the HLM model as Level 2 predictors (see Model 3), the effect of cultural metacognition turned statistically nonsignificant, � � .06, SE � .07, t � .90, p � .37; whereas the effect of cultural perspective taking on intercultural coop- eration remained statistically significant, � � .67, SE � .04, t � 15.01, p � .001, suggesting mediation (see Figure 1 for the full mediation model). A boot- strapping test confirmed a positive indirect effect
  • 28. of cultural metacognition on intercultural cooper- ation by way of cultural perspective taking (95% CI [.02, 25]). These results provide support for Hypoth- esis 1b. In summary, the results from Study 1 provide evidence supporting our hypotheses that chroni- cally high cultural perspective-taking tendencies among American MBA students explain the rela- tionship between metacognitive habits and suc- cessful intercultural collaboration with different- culture counterparts. STUDY 2: CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE-TAKING INTERVENTION Although Study 1 provides the first empirical evi- dence that the relationship between cultural meta- cognition and intercultural cooperation is ex- plained by cultural perspective-taking tendencies, in Study 2, we examined one intervention for pro- moting cultural perspective taking—reflecting how the cultural background of one’s counterparts may affect their approach to a mixed-motive con- flict. This cognitively oriented intervention is dis- tinct from affectively oriented interventions in past work on intercultural collaboration (Chua et al., 2012). We tested whether this cultural perspective- taking intervention would shift participants’ deci- sions, in this case, toward a relational, harmonious approach congruent with Chinese norms (Hyp 2b) in a Prisoners’ Dilemma scenario that was adapted into a decision-making task in a business setting. PILOT STUDY
  • 29. Method Participants and Procedures For an initial test of the cultural perspective-taking intervention, we recruited 107 American adults (81.3% � White/Caucasian; 7.5% � Asian; 6.5% � β =.20* β = .67*** β = .20* (.06) Cultural Metacognition Intercultural Cooperation Cultural Perspective Taking FIGURE 1 Mediation Model Showing Cultural Perspective Taking Mediates the Relationship Between Cultural Metacognition and Intercultural Cooperation (Evaluated by Different-Culture Peers; Study 1). Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. * � p � .05, *** � p � .001. 2013 459Mor, Morris, and Joh
  • 30. African American; 3.7% � Hispanic; 9% � Native American; Female � 51%; Mean Age � 34, 28.1% college students, 70.1% currently working) via Me- chanical Turk for a study on problem solving. Par- ticipants were randomly assigned to a cultural perspective-taking condition or control condition. Materials Mixed Motive Conflict. Participants were asked to read a scenario, a Prisoners’ Dilemma task pre- sented as a conflict on an advertising campaign between an American product manager and a Chi- nese product manager, adapted from past research (Greenhalgh & Bazerman, 2004; Ku et al., 2010). Participants were first presented with the follow- ing prompt about their role and objectives as American managers: You are Mr./Ms. Graham, the head of a prod- uct management group in a consumer prod- ucts firm based in the U.S., handling the mar- keting of a new liquid dishwasher detergent. As a manager, you are faced with a recurring decision as to whether you should put on an advertising campaign during the next sales period. This campaign would provide con- sumers with comparative information about your Chinese competitor’s product: Li Hong. The comparative advertising campaign will describe the destructive impact of your com- petitor’s product on a dishwasher’s motor, spots left on the dishes, and/or its high cost.
  • 31. Your objective is to maximize your profit. This is also the objective of your Chinese competitor, Li Hong. Profitability will also be used as a way of measuring your success as a manager. The profitability of your product de- pends not only on your decision but also on the decision of Li Hong. Specifically, if neither you nor Li put on a derogatory advertising campaign, each company will make $1 mil- lion for the sales period. If one of you puts on a comparative advertising campaign but the competing company does not, then the com- pany that advertises will have a profit of $2 million for the sales period and the com- petitor will lose $2 million. If both companies advertise the deficiencies of their competi- tor’s product, then total sales of liquid dish- washer detergent will fall and both compa- nies will lose $1 million for the sales period. Participants were asked to decide whether to embark on an advertising campaign disparaging a Chinese product manager, Li Hong, who is selling a similar product. Advertising was equivalent to an aggressive, competitive response; whereas not advertising was equivalent to cooperating. Partic- ipants’ decision not to advertise was used as a measure of intercultural cooperation. Manipulation. Next, participants in the CPT condi- tion were asked to think about their counterpart’s culture before making their decision. Below is the prompt they received: Before you go on to make your decision, we would like you to do the following: Please write down a few sentences describing Li’s
  • 32. (Chinese manager’s) interests and concerns as a person living and working in China. . . How would Li’s cultural values and beliefs guide his behavior and decision in this situation? Business Decision. Next, participants were asked to make their decision in the decision-making sce- nario (advertise or not advertise). Goals. After making their decision, participants indicated in an open-ended response format what guided their decision-making in the scenario. They were presented with the following prompt: Please write down a few sentences explain- ing what guided your decision to advertise or not advertise in the business case. Two research assistants blind to the hypotheses rated participants’ open-ended responses. Raters evaluated participants’ decisions on the following items: “The participant was interested in promot- ing future business relations with Li”; “The partic- ipant was interested in cooperating with Li”; “The participant was interested in achieving the busi- ness goal of maximizing profit/minimizing profit loss” (Reversed); (1 � strongly disagree, 7 � strongly agree; ICC � .80). The 3 items were aver- aged to create a score for each participant (� � .89). Manipulation Check. After making their decision, participants indicated whether they thought about Chinese cultural values while evaluating the sce- nario. They rated their thought process on the fol- lowing three items: “I tried to think what a Chinese manager would do in this case”; “I thought about Chinese business norms when making my deci-
  • 33. sion”; “I thought about Chinese cultural values 460 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education when making my decision.” Items averaged to cre- ate a score for each participant (1 � not at all; 7 � very much; � �. 87). Control Variables. At the end of the study, partici- pants reported their cultural intelligence (cultural metacognition Van Dyne et al., 2012): � �.87; Cog- nitive CQ: � �.80; Motivational CQ: � �. 80; Behav- ioral CQ: � �. 87. As in Study 1, these four cultural intelligence factors were included as control vari- ables in our analyses. Results To test whether the CPT manipulation induced cul- tural perspective taking in participants, we ran an ANCOVA (between subjects factor: experimental condition; covariates: 4 CQ factors) on the cultural perspective-taking scores and found that partici- pants reported thinking more about Chinese cul- tural values and beliefs in the CPT condition (M � 4.58) than the control condition (M � 3.32), F(1,150) � 18.84, p � .001. Hypothesis 2a proposed that cultural perspective taking would promote cooperation with a different- culture colleague. To test this, we regressed par- ticipants’ choices to cooperate (advertise � com- pete � 0; not advertise � cooperate � 1) on the experimental condition (0 � Control, 1 � CPT), controlling for the 4 CQ factors. Analyses revealed
  • 34. a main positive effect of the manipulation on choosing a cooperative strategy with a Chinese counterpart, � � .93, SE � .48, Wald (1) � 3.73, p � .05. Thus, the results supported Hypothesis 2a. To examine whether the effect of the CPT inter- vention on decisions runs through goals, we fol- lowed Baron and Kenny’s (1986) steps for testing mediation as well as a bootstrapping test (Hayes, 2009). We first examined the relationship between the experimental condition and goals (controlling for cultural intelligence factors) using multiple re- gression analysis and found them to be positively associated, � � .90, SE � .26, t(101) � 3.45, p � .01. Then we conducted binary logistic analysis and entered both the experimental condition and stu- dents’ relational orientation scores as predictors of choice to cooperate, controlling for cultural intelli- gence factors. Analyses revealed that the effect of the condition turned statistically nonsignificant, � � �.58, SE � .78, Wald (1) � .55, p � .46; whereas the effect of students’ relational scores remained a significant predictor of choice to cooperate with Li, � � 2.13, SE � .44, Wald (1) � 23.47, p � .001 sug- gesting mediation (see Figure 2). A bootstrapping test with 20,000 resamples confirmed a positive indirect effect between CPT on students’ choice to cooperate by way of intentions about adopting a relational business orientation (95% CI [.31, 3.27]). Thus, the results above provided support for Hy- pothesis 2b. In sum, our pilot study revealed that a cultural perspective-taking intervention increased participants’ relational orientation toward a Chi- nese counterpart and explained their decision to cooperate with him. However, the question still remains whether this heightened relational orien-
  • 35. tation was the result of an increase in other–self overlap associated with collectivistic cultural values. β = .90** β = 2.13** β = .93* (–.58) Experimental Condition (0=Control; 1=CPT) Intercultural Cooperation Relational Goals FIGURE 2 Mediation Model Showing the Positive Casual Effect of Cultural Perspective Taking on Intercultural Cooperation with a Chinese Counterpart Is Mediated by One’s Relational Goals (Study 2, Pilot Study). Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. * � p � .05, ** � p � .01. 2013 461Mor, Morris, and Joh STUDY 2: CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE-TAKING INTERVENTION WITH MBA STUDENTS
  • 36. In Study 2 we shifted to test our CPT intervention with MBA students whose demographic character- istics are more closely aligned with those of man- agers. Moreover, we also explored participants’ ex- pectations about their counterpart’s decision in the mixed-motive conflict task, since much of past re- search finds that cooperation in mixed-motive con- flicts hinges on expectancies of the counterpart’s cooperation (Wong & Hong, 2005). Accordingly, we examined whether the association between cul- tural perspective taking and cooperation hinges on heightened expectancies of the Chinese counter- part’s goal to cooperate (Hyp 2a and Hyp 2b). Method Participants and Procedures Fifty-seven (Non-Chinese) MBAs (61.4% American1; Males � 65%; Mean Age � 28) completed the study as part of an in-class exercise. Seventy-two per- cent of students identified as White/Caucasian, 21.1% Asian (non-Chinese), 3.5% Latin/Hispanic, 1.8% as African-American, and 3.5% as other eth- nicities. Students were randomly assigned to a CPT condition or control condition. Materials Mixed-Motive Conflict Students were asked to read a business scenario, which was a Prisoner’s Dilemma task presented as an advertising task. The instructions and task were presented in the same manner as in the pi- lot study.
  • 37. Manipulation Next, participants in the CPT condition were asked to think about their Chinese counterpart’s cultural values before making their decision. The prompt presented to MBA students was the same prompt used in the pilot study. Business Decision Next, participants were asked to make their deci- sion in the case (to advertise or not advertise). Manipulation Check After making their decision, participants indicated whether they thought about Chinese cultural val- ues while evaluating the case, which served as a manipulation check using the following three items: “I tried to think what a Chinese manager would do in this case”; “I thought about Chinese business norms when making my decision”; “I thought about Chinese cultural values when mak- ing my decision.” Items were averaged to create a score for each participant (1 � not at all; 7 � very much; � � .89). Expectancies of Counterpart’s Cooperativeness Next, participants rated their confidence that Li would not advertise (i.e., cooperate) using a 7- point scale (1 � not at all; 7 � very much; Wong & Hong, 2005). Control Variables
  • 38. At the end of the study, participants completed a cultural intelligence assessment (Cultural meta- cognition; Van Dyne and colleagues, 2012): � � .73; Cognitive CQ: � � .90; Motivational CQ: � � .79; Behavioral CQ: � � .88. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION To test whether the cultural perspective-taking manipulation induced cultural perspective taking in MBA students, we ran an ANCOVA (between subjects factor: experimental condition; covariates: 4 CQ factors) on the cultural perspective-taking scores and found that students reported more thoughts about counterparts’ cultural values and beliefs in the cultural perspective-taking condition (M � 5.49) than the control condition (M � 4.17), F (1,57) � 14.27, p � .001. Hypothesis 2a suggested that inducing cultural perspective taking would promote cooperation with a counterpart from a culture known for coop- erative relational norms. To test this prediction, we conducted a binary logistic regression regressing students’ choice to cooperate (advertise � com- pete � 0; not advertise � cooperate � 1) on the 1 Preliminary analysis revealed that students’ cultural back- grounds (American vs. non-American students) did not reveal differential effects on their responses to the manipulation and their decision to cooperate in the case, and thus, we retained both American and non-American students in the sample. 462 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
  • 39. experimental condition (0 � Control, 1 � CPT), controlling for cultural intelligence factors. Analy- ses revealed a positive main effect of CPT on in- tercultural cooperation, � � 1.43, SE � .63, Wald (1) � 5.23, p � .05. These results supported Hypothesis 2a, replicating the results from the pilot study with MBA students and revealing that cultural perspec- tive taking had a direct positive effect on intercul- tural cooperation. Hypothesis 2b further suggested that the rela- tionship between CPT and intercultural coopera- tion would be mediated by expectations about the counterpart’s cooperativeness. To test this predic- tion, we first examined the relationship between the experimental condition and expectations about the counterpart’s cooperativeness (the mediator). Multiple regression analysis revealed that the CPT condition had a positive main effect on expecta- tions that the counterpart would cooperate (� � 1.37, SE � .52, t � 2.62, p � .05). When both experimental condition and expectations about the counterpart’s cooperativeness were entered as pre- dictors of cooperation in a logistic regression model, the relationship between the experimental condition and intercultural cooperation turned sta- tistically nonsignificant (� � .95, SE � .69, Wald (1) � 1.86, p � .17), while expectations about Li’s co- operation remained a significant predictor of inter- cultural cooperation (� � .47, SE � .18, Wald (1) � 7.19, p � .01), suggesting mediation (see Figure 3). A bootstrapping test confirmed there was a pos- itive indirect effect between CPT on intercultural cooperation via expectations about Li’s coopera-
  • 40. tiveness (95% CI [.01, 1.84]). These results reveal that the cultural perspective- taking manipulation increased MBA students’ co- operation with a Chinese counterpart, and this ef- fect was explained by their heightened expectations that their counterpart—Li Hong— would cooperate as well. Thus, the results sup- ported Hypotheses H2a and H2b and suggest that one useful intervention for promoting managers’ cross-cultural working relations with collectivistic counterparts is asking them to reflect about their counterpart’s values and beliefs prior to meeting them. Moreover, we find that CPT also increases expectations that one’s counterpart holds rela- tional, cooperative goals when making his or her decision. Overall, it appears that reflection prior to intercultural interaction facilitates overlap be- tween MBA students’ and their counterparts’ cul- tural values and goals. STUDY 3A: CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE TAKING AND INTERCULTURAL NEGOTIATIONS Although Study 2 revealed a causal positive rela- tionship between cultural perspective taking and intercultural cooperation, in Study 3A, we exam- ined this intervention as a preparation for an in- ternational negotiation between an American β = 1.37* β = 1.43* β = .47**
  • 41. (.95) Experimental Condition (0=Control; 1=CPT) Intercultural Cooperation Expectation about counterpart’s relational goals FIGURE 3 Mediation Model Showing the Positive Effect of Cultural Perspective-Taking Condition on Intercultural Cooperation Is Mediated by Expectations That the Chinese Counterpart Holds Relational Goals (Study 2). Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. * � p � .05; ** � p � .01 Overall, it appears that reflection prior to intercultural interaction facilitates overlap between MBA students’ and their counterparts’ cultural values and goals. 2013 463Mor, Morris, and Joh manager and a Japanese counterpart. In this con-
  • 42. text, we examined American MBA students’ expec- tations about their counterpart’s cooperative ver- sus competitive orientation in an upcoming negotiation. Expectations about the counterpart’s orientation should be associated with students’ de- cisions to cooperate, as past research on negotia- tions has found that expectations greatly influence strategic decisions about concessions to make and information to provide or withhold from one’s coun- terpart (Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). We also fur- ther explored whether a cultural perspective inter- vention produced greater benefits (e.g., increased expectation about counterpart’s cooperativeness) for MBA students low on metacognition than those high metacognition individuals. In other words, we expected that culture-specific cues would lead in- dividuals low on cultural metacognition to modify their expectations about their counterpart’s goals, leading to greater levels of intercultural coopera- tion in this task. Method Procedure Seventy-six American MBA students (Males � 56%; Mean Age � 27) participated in a negotiation ex- ercise in their first semester of school as part of an in-class exercise in a leadership class. Upon be- ginning their MBA program, the American MBA students reported their cultural metacognition lev- els in an orientation survey (using the same method as in Study 1). Two months later (as part of an in-class exercise) the students were asked to complete a prenegotiation survey prior to a nego- tiation with a Japanese counterpart over the man-
  • 43. ufacturing of “mini” excavators (Patel & Brett, 2007). Participants were not aware of any cultural elements in the exercise or the fact that the prene- gotiation survey was intended to assess their pro- clivity toward working with a different-culture counterpart. Since students later carried out the negotiation in teams, we only examined their ex- pectations about their counterpart’s cooperation prior to the negotiation. Students were randomly assigned to either a cultural perspective-taking manipulation or a control condition. Materials Cultural Intelligence Two months prior to the negotiation exercise, stu- dents completed the 4-factor cultural intelligence assessment described in the previous studies (cul- tural metacognition: � � .80; Cognitive CQ: � � .80; Motivational CQ: � � .85; Behavioral CQ: � � .86). The four CQ factors were included as control vari- ables in all of the following analyses. Negotiation Exercise Upon arriving to class, students received their ne- gotiation roles and were asked to read the materi- als prior to completing their prenegotiation sur- veys. The negotiation case involved a negotiation between two companies, Abhas and Bussan (rep- resented by Sato-san), over the manufacturing of “mini” excavators, and the case materials in- cluded five issues. All American students were assigned to the Abhas role. The case contained
  • 44. background information about the Japanese com- pany and Sato-san (the Japanese negotiator). Cultural Perspective-Taking Manipulation Prior to the negotiation, students were provided a link to an on-line survey where they were asked to predict their Japanese counterpart’s orientation in the upcoming negotiations. Students in the CPT condition received the following information be- fore making their predictions about their Japanese counterpart (Students in the control received no prompt). Before you go on to guess Sato-san’s (Japa- nese counterpart from the Bussan company) priorities and goals in the negotiations, we would like you to do the following: Please take a few moments to think about the per- spective of Sato-san negotiating on behalf of a company operating in Japan. Try to imagine what Sato-san would be thinking and what may be her/his interests and concerns based on the fact that she/he is negotiating on be- half of a company operating in Japan. Try In other words, we expected that culture- specific cues would lead individuals low on cultural metacognition to modify their expectations about their counterpart’s goals, leading to greater levels of intercultural cooperation in this task. 464 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education
  • 45. your best to put yourself in Sato-san’s shoes. Please write down a few sentences describ- ing Sato-san’s interests and concerns about the five case issues as a Japanese negotiator. Expectancy of Counterpart’s Cooperativeness Next, students were asked to predict the orienta- tion of their Japanese counterpart during the up- coming negotiation using the following categories: (1) Cooperative, (2) Competitive, or (3) Don’t Know. All other responses (competitive or don’t know) were coded as 0. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION We conducted a logistic regression analysis exam- ining the effects of the experimental condition on MBA students’ expectations that their Japanese counterpart would be cooperative in the upcoming negotiation. Analyses revealed no main effect of condition, � � .27, SE � .26, Wald (1) � 1.11, p � .29. Next, we examined whether the intervention had a stronger effect for individuals with a low baseline proclivity toward cultural metacognition. To test this prediction, we used binary logistic regression and tested for an interaction between cultural metacognition and the experimental condition on perceptions of cooperativeness. Analyses revealed a significant 2-way interaction between cultural metacognition and the experimental condition on expectation that Japanese counterpart would coop- erate, � � �0.84, SE � .39, Wald (1) � 4.71, p � .05. Simple slope analysis following procedures by Ai-
  • 46. ken, West, and Reno (1991) revealed that MBAs low on cultural metacognition when asked to reflect on their Japanese counterpart’s values and behaviors were more likely to expect their Japanese counter- part would be cooperative (CPT condition) than not (control condition), � � 1.80, SE � .80, Wald (1) � 5.10, p � .05 (see Figure 4). At the same time, the CPT for MBAs high on cultural metacognition did not alter their (already moderately high) expec- tations about their Japanese counterpart’s cooper- ativeness, � � �0.75, SE � .73, Wald (1) � 1.07, p � .30 (see Figure 4). The patterns depicted in Figure 4 further suggest that individuals low on cultural metacognition did not simply regress to- ward the mean in their expectations about their partner, but rather more closely matched the pre- dictions of individuals high on cultural metacognition. Although the manipulation did not have a main effect on expectancies, there was an interaction effect: Individuals habitually low in cultural meta- cognition were prompted by the manipulation to think about their Japanese counterpart’s coopera- tiveness more than they did in the control condi- tion; whereas individuals high in cultural meta- cognition were not pushed to think more than they did in the control condition (already a considerable amount). Hence this is an intervention that works most for individuals not culturally minded in that it helps them take useful cultural information into account. To help students who are already cultur- ally minded make even more finely calibrated de- cisions, another kind of manipulation may be needed.
  • 47. FIGURE 4 Graph Depicting the Interaction Between Experimental Condition and Cultural Metacognition on Expectations That Japanese Counterpart Would Be Cooperative in an Upcoming Negotiation (Study 3A). 2013 465Mor, Morris, and Joh STUDY 3B Although Study 3A revealed that offering a cultural perspective-taking intervention to individuals low on cultural metacognition enhanced their expecta- tion that their different-culture counterpart would cooperate in an upcoming international negotia- tion, we did not examine whether this intervention also directly increased intercultural cooperation decisions of MBAs low on cultural metacognition. Thus, the aim of Study 3B was to examine Hypoth- eses 2b and 2c in a unified study design. Method Participants and Procedures Seventy-six American adults (100% White/Cauca- sian, Female � 52%; Mean Age � 34, 26% college students, 62% currently working) were recruited by way of Mechanical Turk and were asked to com- plete a survey on decision making. The procedure was the same design as Study 2. Participants were randomly assigned to a cultural perspective- taking or control condition.
  • 48. Mixed-Motive Conflict Participants were asked to read a scenario which was a Prisoner’s Dilemma task presented as an advertising task. The task presented was the same task presented in Study 2. Manipulation Next, participants in the cultural perspective- taking condition were asked to think about their Chinese counterpart’s cultural values before mak- ing their decision in the business scenario (same task as Study 2). Business Decision Next, participants were asked to make their deci- sion in the case (to advertise/not advertise). Manipulation Check After making their decision, participants indicated whether they thought about Chinese cultural val- ues while evaluating the case which served as a manipulation check using the following three items (as in Study 2): “I tried to think what a Chi- nese manager would do in this case”; “I thought about Chinese business norms when making my decision”; “I thought about Chinese cultural val- ues when making my decision.” Items were aver- aged to create a score for each participant (1 � not at all; 7 � very much; � �. 83).
  • 49. Expectancies We also evaluated participants’ inferences about their Chinese counterpart’s long-term relational goals using the following statements: “I thought Li would care about our long-term relationship”; “I thought Li would be most concerned about making profit” [Reversed]). The two items were averaged to create a score for each participant (� � .87). Cultural Intelligence Measure At the end of the study, participants completed the usual cultural intelligence assessment (cultural metacognition: � �.87; Cognitive CQ: � � .86; Mo- tivational CQ: � � .89; Behavioral CQ: � � .91). As in the previous studies, these four cultural intelli- gence factors were included as control variables in our analyses. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION To test whether the CPT manipulation induced cul- tural perspective taking, we ran an ANCOVA (be- tween subjects factor: experimental condition; co- variates: 4 CQ factors) on the cultural perspective- taking scores and found that participants reported greater levels of cultural perspective taking in the CPT condition (M � 4.72) than the control condition (M � 3.55), F(1,76) � 11.77, p � .01. We first tested Hypothesis 2a, which suggested that cultural perspective taking would promote in- tercultural cooperation. To do so we conducted a binary logistic regression using experimental con- dition as a predictor variable on participants’ de-
  • 50. cision to cooperate. Consistent with Hypothesis 2a (and results from studies 2A and 2B), the CPT con- dition had a positive effect on choosing a cooper- ative strategy with Li, � � 1.06, SE � .51, Wald (1) � 4.30, p � .05. Next, we conducted a binary logistic regression analysis to examine whether expectations about the Chinese counterpart’s strategy explained the relationship between the experimental condition and decision to cooperate. When both experimen- tal condition and expectations about counterpart’s cooperation were entered into the binary logistic 466 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education regression model, the effect of CPT turned statisti- cally nonsignificant, � � .74, SE � .55, Wald (1) � 1.80, p � .18, while expectations about Li’s rela- tional goals remained a significant positive pre- dictor of cooperation, indicating mediation, � � .66, SE � .21, Wald (1) � 9.60, p � .01 (see Figure 5). A bootstrapping test confirmed there was a positive indirect effect between CPT on intercultural coop- eration by way of expectations about counterpart’s relational goals (95% CI [.03, 1.59]). The results above provide additional support for Hypotheses 2a and 2b by revealing that the CPT manipulation increased the likelihood of choosing a cooperative strategy with a Chinese counterpart by heighten- ing expectations that the Chinese counterpart would chose a cooperative strategy as well. Next, we examined Hypothesis 2d and tested
  • 51. whether inducing CPT had a greater positive effect on decisions to cooperate for individuals low on cultural metacognition. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a binary logistic regression analysis and tested for an interaction between the experi- mental manipulation and cultural metacognition on decision to cooperate. Analyses revealed a sig- nificant interaction, � � �1.47, SE � .63, Wald (1) � 5.55, p � .05. To probe the interaction, we con- ducted simple slope analysis (Aiken et al., 1991), examining the effect of the experimental condition in individuals low on cultural metacognition ver- sus those high in cultural metacognition. Consis- tent with our predictions, analysis revealed that individuals low on cultural metacognition were significantly more cooperative in the CPT condi- tion than the control condition, � � 1.39, SE � .48, Wald (1) � 8.41, p � .01. At the same time, the CPT manipulation did not enhance cooperation levels for individuals high on cultural metacognition, � � �.08, SE � .35, Wald (1) � .05, p � .82. DISCUSSION In five studies we provided a comprehensive test of our hypotheses positing that a metacognitive ten- dency, namely— cultural perspective taking— can promote intercultural cooperation and be espe- cially useful for promoting intercultural coopera- tion among American MBA students and working adults low on cultural metacognition. Our research contributes to ongoing research in management education and learning by providing a novel framework utilizing both an individual difference approach and situational priming geared toward
  • 52. identifying and developing individuals and man- agers’ cross-cultural management skills. Theoretical Implications While much of past research on cultural intelli- gence (CQ) has focused on identifying the link between cultural intelligence and managerial per- formance measures (for a review see Ang, Van Dyne, & Tan, 2011), our research is the first to iden- tify a cognitive-based mechanism associated with one of the factors of cultural intelligence. More- over, we provide the first empirical evidence that a cognitive mechanism— cultural perspective tak- ing— has a direct positive effect on behavioral measures associated with managerial perfor- mance in culturally diverse settings, spanning in- tercultural collaboration in international teams, (.74) Experimental Condition (0=Control; 1=CPT) Intercultural Cooperation Expectation about counterpart’s relational goals
  • 53. β = .68* β = 1.06* β = .66** FIGURE 5 Mediation Model Showing the Positive Effect of Cultural Perspective-Taking Condition on Intercultural Cooperation Is Mediated by Expectations That the Chinese Counterpart Holds Relational Goals (Study 3B). Note. Regression results are reported in unstandardized betas. * � p � .05; ** � p � .01. 2013 467Mor, Morris, and Joh decision making in mixed-motive conflicts, and preparation for international negotiations. Interestingly, while past CQ research and theory has suggested that cultural knowledge (cognitive CQ) should lead individuals to have more accurate understanding of foreign cultures (Ang et al., 2007), the results of Study 1 do not support these assump- tions, nor does recent research revealing no asso- ciation between self-reported cognitive CQ and cross-cultural accuracy (Mor, Ames, & Joh, 2013). The results revealed that only metacognitive CQ, and not cognitive CQ, was associated with cul- tural perspective taking. These results suggest that the ability to accurately detect culture-specific congruent or incongruent norms may require the development of metacognitive habits in tandem
  • 54. with foreign cultural knowledge. Cultural Intelligence and Intercultural Relationships Our findings contribute to the growing body of research on how dimensions of cultural intelli- gence (CQ) affect people’s abilities to manage var- ious forms interdependence with counterparts from different cultures. Recent research by Chua, Morris, and Mor (2012) focused on collaborations within the context of professional networks. They found that executives with a higher proclivity to- ward cultural metacognition attain more creative collaboration success in their intercultural ties, as they develop more affect-based trust in these ties, relative to those lower in cultural metacognition. In the context of intercultural negotiations, Imai and Gelfand (2010) have found that motivational CQ (the motivation and efficacy to engage cultur- ally different others) predicted integrative behav- iors, resulting in higher joint gains and that behav- ioral CQ (behavioral flexibility during intercultural interactions), but not other dimensions of CQ, pre- dicted sequences of cooperative strategies. Our research adds to this stream of research by (1) identifying strategies for using cultural knowledge that are associated with cultural metacognition, such as cultural perspective taking, and (2) exam- ining whether metacognitive strategies can be in- duced through an intervention guiding perspective taking. Our research also reveals the discriminate validity of the cultural metacognition factor from the three other CQ factors, a query recently advo- cated by Van Dyne et al. (2012).
  • 55. Our findings further extend past research exam- ining intercultural trust and collaboration (Chua et al., 2012; Takahashi, Yamagishi, Liu, Wang, Lin, & Yu, 2008) by examining the role of cognitive-based tendencies rather than affective mechanisms lead- ing to intercultural trust and collaboration. Past research finds that trust leads to better rapport, which increases willingness to cooperate with oth- ers in mixed-motive conflicts (Drolet & Morris, 2000). Trust can arise by way of two distinct psy- chological processes: a cognitive evaluation of the other party’s competence and reliability, or an af- fective experience of liking and rapport (McAllis- ter, 1995). The former is based on expectations of task-related competence and involves an analytic and utilitarian assessment of the other party; the latter is closely linked to empathy and rapport, and arises from emotional closeness. These two types of trust closely resemble two central dimensions of social perception: warmth and competence (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). Affect-based trust is strongly linked to perceived warmth, cognition-based trust to perceived competence. We argue that cognitive- based mechanisms leading to intercultural trust and cooperation are important to examine for a number of reasons. First, McAllister (1995) notes that some level of cognition-based trust is neces- sary for affect-based trust to develop. Second, ne- gotiations scholars have found that differences in cognitive schema impede the building of trust in intercultural negotiations (Brett & Okumura, 1998; Jang & Chua, 2011). When negotiating across cul- tures, negotiation counterparts do not always share the same implicit scripts, norms, and as- sumptions. We argue that matching the cognitive
  • 56. schemas of counterparts, for example, by engaging in cultural perspective taking, can facilitate bridg- ing across differences in cognitive schema. At the same time, future research should continue to in- vestigate differential effects of cognitive- versus affective-based mechanisms in intercultural exchanges. Also important, our research makes a contribu- tion to the broader literature on cross-cultural or- ganizational behavior by empirically examining intercultural interactions. While much of the orga- nizational behavior literature discusses cross- cultural interactions, very few studies empirically investigate these interactions (Gelfand, Erez, & Ay- can, 2007). Studies that do examine cross-cultural interactions and adaptation tend to take a long- term orientation (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985), and relatively few examine discreet, individual inter- actions that cross cultural boundaries (Adair, Oku- mura, & Brett, 2001; LaBahn & Harich, 1997; Molin- 468 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education sky, 2007). By examining an individual’s behavior as a function of both their levels of cultural intel- ligence, as well as the culture of a relevant other, we extend prior work on cross-cultural interactions and provide a framework with which future re- searchers can further extend the body of knowl- edge on cross-cultural organizational behavior. Mindfulness in Management
  • 57. Previous research has argued that mindfulness training may ameliorate the potential for emo- tions, fears, prejudices or biases to “hijack” thought and action (Langer, 2000). A psychological definition of mindfulness is bringing one’s com- plete attention to the experiences occurring in the present moment, in a nonjudgmental and accept- ing manner (Baer, 2003; Brown & Ryan, 2003; Kabat- Zinn, 1982). Differently put, it is the “simple act of drawing novel distinctions” helping us have a “greater sensitivity to context” and overcome, or not form, mind-sets that may limit our thinking (Langer, 2000). It is also suggested that the tech- niques for cultivating mindfulness all rely upon slowing down the onrush of mental activity and trying to focus attention on the world of sensations in itself rather than “jumping on the first interpre- tation that comes along” (Claxton, 1997: 183). Along the same vein, our research provides empirical evidence suggesting that being mindful of different-culture counterparts’ thoughts and feel- ings (by engaging in cultural perspective taking examined in Study 1) as well as being aware of different-culture counterparts’ values and beliefs (Studies 2 and 3) can facilitate intercultural coop- eration associated with cross-cultural managerial performance. Similarly, scholars have noted that a mindful manager—a manager who adopts a posi- tive nonjudgmental and reflective stance—might be more likely to engender enhanced empathy and positive regard that influences task (e.g., financial) and relational (e.g., long-term business relation- ship) instrumental outcomes, as well as well-being (Davidson et al., 2003; Kopelman, Avi-Yonah, & Var- ghese, 2011). A recent review of mindfulness and management research suggests that mindfulness
  • 58. practices can enhance task performance (Dane, 2011). Consistent with these claims, our research offers an intervention that is associated with en- gaging in mindfulness about a counterpart’s cul- tural background and has a direct positive effect on relational organizational outcomes, such as col- laboration in international teams (Study 1) and decision making in mixed-motive conflicts (Studies 2 and 3). Thus, future research should continue developing interventions focused on making man- agers mindful of cultural cues in organizational contexts and examine their effects on managers’ cross-cultural management skills. Practical Implications Cross-Cultural Training To date, one of the most common formats for con- ducting cross-cultural training programs includes brief lectures that provide basic information about the history and socioeconomic situation of a target or foreign culture (Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000; Earley, Ang, & Tan, 2006). However, the field has long rec- ognized that this training approach is problematic for a number of reasons. First, such training does not adjust for individual differences in capa- bility across cognitive, metacognitive, motiva- tional, and behavioral skills (Earley & Peterson, 2004). Second, such training fails to consider the nature of the target culture and the work to be performed in terms of intensity, duration, and na- ture (Earley et al., 2006). Third, the knowledge pro- vided to managers in culture-specific training pro- grams is not transferable across cultural domains
  • 59. (Earley & Peterson, 2004). Increasingly, managers take on more frequent and shorter offshore assignments. As a result, de- veloping metacognitive tendencies could be more advantageous than previous frameworks for cross- cultural training (Bhawuk & Brislin, 2000). As Ear- ley and Peterson note (2004), career trends have managers working in more countries and spending shorter periods in any single country. Thus, it ap- pears that cross-cultural training should shift to harness managers’ metacognitive strategies in tandem with providing in-depth analysis of cul- tural differences. Developing metacognitive skills are also important even for managers who don’t leave their native country but work in internation- ally diverse teams whose members follow a myr- iad of country-specific cultural norms (Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). In instances such as these, training managers with country-specific knowl- edge is less practical than equipping global man- agers with metacognitive skills. With regard to developing cultural metacognition, Tan and Chua (2003) acknowledged that while CQ may be par- tially determined by basic intellectual ability, an individual’s CQ can still be improved through 2013 469Mor, Morris, and Joh training. In fact, recent research finds that cross- cultural management training can enhance cul- tural intelligence dimensions (Eisenberg et al., 2013; Rehg et al., 2012). In conclusion, the research and training approach examined in this paper is
  • 60. compatible with ongoing structural changes to in- ternational assignments, and thus, is more practi- cal and cost effective than training managers with country-specific knowledge. Training and Developing Cultural Metacognition Researchers and practitioners alike may find that training and harnessing metacognitive habits among students and managers could benefit both domestic and expatriate performance. For exam- ple, teachers may decide to assign MBA students who score higher on cultural metacognition to ne- gotiate or work on class assignments with students scoring low on cultural metacognition, as prior re- search reveals that in dyads, it is the individual with the higher metacognitive CQ that drives in- tercultural success (Chua et al., 2012). Performance feedback is also an important component for de- veloping awareness and planning habits among students. For example, teachers can provide stu- dents with feedback from self- and peer-ratings, in order to show them areas where their metacogni- tive strategies could benefit from development. Last, cultural simulator exercises (Triandis, 1995) as well as research websites providing culture- specific knowledge in business can be integrated into class assignments prior to intercultural nego- tiations or international teamwork assignments to help students develop habits of planning and awareness associated with higher levels of cul- tural metacognition. Working Effectively in Global Teams Developing metacognitive habits in managers is of
  • 61. particularly high importance for the success of multinational teams (Earley & Peterson, 2004). Global teams often face the challenge of getting members from different cultures and countries to work effectively with one another (Earley & Gib- son, 2002; Hagel & Brown, 2005). Establishing com- mon goals, clear roles, and consensual norms of conduct is made difficult by cultural differences (Earley & Gibson, 2002; Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). As a result, global teams often search for common- alities to create a hybrid culture (Adler & Bar- tholomew, 1992; Earley & Mosakowski, 2000). One way in which a hybrid culture can develop is by establishing shared schemas for work tasks. Ac- cording to Earley and Peterson (2004), metacogni- tion is critical for developing such shared sche- mas. Thus, future research should examine whether a cultural perspective-taking intervention may enable managers to bridge cultural differ- ences in global teams. Developing Cross-Cultural Negotiation Skills Intercultural negotiations often fail (Brett & Oku- mura, 1998; Graham, 1985), and thus far manage- ment education has developed few tools for im- proving cross-cultural negotiation skills and outcomes (Adair et al., 2001). A review of negotia- tion simulations designed to teach cross-cultural negotiations revealed that most either prepare stu- dents for particular cultural preferences or partic- ular cultural styles of communication (Adair, 2008). In contrast to these types of interventions, a metacognitive-strategy intervention may instill general skills for facing differences (Brett, 2007).
  • 62. Our proposed intervention for cross-cultural nego- tiation training is consistent with Adler’s (1997) proposition that cultural adaptation in negotia- tions (such as matching your counterpart’s strat- egy) may increase the chances of positive negoti- ation outcomes. Moreover, the argument that CPT may promote intercultural negotiation outcomes is also consistent with past research findings that general perspective taking in negotiation—the ac- tive consideration of the other party’s alternatives and interests prior to negotiation—aids negotia- tors in both claiming and creating value (Galinsky et al., 2008; Kemp & Smith, 1994; Neale & Bazer- man, 1982). Future Directions One question that arises from our findings is how individuals high on cultural perspective taking adapt to situations in which different-culture coun- terparts disconfirm culture-based expectations. Recent research reveals that individuals high on cultural metacognition are more apt at adjusting their expectations when a different-culture coun- terpart disconfirms culture-specific behaviors (Mor & Morris, 2013). At the same time, congruent with our findings, individuals high on cultural metacog- nition are also more likely to apply culture-based assumptions when a counterpart confirms culture- based expectancies, such a Chinese person behav- 470 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education ing in a way that confirms a preexisting assump-
  • 63. tion that she would behave in accordance with collectivistic values. This agility in applying and updating assumptions seems like an important cognitive capability, where globalization forces may be reducing conformity to culture-based val- ues and behaviors among some individuals (Fu, Morris, Lee, Chao, Chiu, & Hong, 2007), and thus, individuals may need to engage in contingent ap- plication of culture-based knowledge to arrive at more coordinated and successful interpersonal outcomes. Limitations The present research has several limitations wor- thy of note. First, the cultural metacognition scale is vulnerable to the limits in people’s ability to reflect upon their mental processes (Nisbett & Wil- son, 1977). At the same time, past research finds convergent validity between self-reports and ob- server reports, suggesting that metacognition is rated from overt words and behaviors rather than introspection (Kim & Van Dyne, 2012; Van Dyne, Ang, & Koh, 2008). Another limitation of the study is that we examined a cultural perspective-taking intervention with collectivistic but not individual- istic counterparts. Also our samples included mostly Americans. We focused on collectivistic counterparts when testing the CPT intervention be- cause of the greater cultural distance between American culture (individualistic) and Chinese and Japanese cultures (Oyserman, Coon, & Kem- melmeier, 2002). At the same time, it is important to note that past research has found that general perspective taking can enhance a competitive ori- entation in competitive contexts but less so in co-
  • 64. operative situations (Epley, Caruso, & Bazerman, 2005). These findings suggest that cultural per- spective taking may facilitate cooperation and joint gains more with collectivistic than individu- alistic counterparts. Indeed, recent research finds that East Asian negotiators taking the perspective of North American negotiators were more self- interested (than other oriented) and claimed more value in the negotiations (Lee et al., 2011). None- theless, we claim that cultural perspective taking may provide more realistic expectations about counterparts’ goals and behavior which may facil- itate social coordination. Consistent with this claim, prior research found that people from indi- vidualistic cultures display less cooperative be- havior in a Prisoner’s Dilemma task than collectiv- istic individuals (Cox, Lobel, & McLeod, 1991). Third, while in our studies we did not collect information about participants’ past experiences or knowledge with the specific foreign culture of one’s counterpart, we did assess participants’ for- eign cultural knowledge by way of the cultural intelligence assessment and used participants’ levels of foreign cultural knowledge (cognitive CQ) as a control variable in all of our analyses. The results revealed that participants self-reported cognitive CQ did not explain or change our effects. At the same time, we do recognize that future stud- ies should examine whether general knowledge of counterpart’s culture is associated with cultural perspective-taking tendencies. Another limitation of the present research is that we have tested some of our hypotheses using simulated cases rather than real-life interactions. While recognizing the
  • 65. limitations of Studies 2 and 3B using a simulated scenario, we found that the findings from these studies are consistent with studies examining be- havioral tendencies by peers (Study 1) and a nego- tiation exercise that had taken place with a real counterpart (Study 3A). Along the same vein, much of past research using economic games relies on simulated cases or interactions to infer actual real- world behaviors in real-life interactions and has found convergent validity across these methods with field studies (Fein & Spencer, 1997; Landis, Brislin, & Hulgus, 1985). CONCLUSIONS In this article we have focused on identifying and training a cognitive habit of managers highly ef- fective at intercultural collaboration: high cultural metacognitive habits. Future research should con- tinue examining adaptive cognitive- and affective- based psychological mechanisms utilized by man- agers who effectively collaborate with different- culture counterparts. Notably, the findings and approach put forth here can provide management and education scholars with novel insights about developing interventions and tools for global man- agers who need to successfully master intercul- tural collaborations across a wide range of cultures. REFERENCES Adair, W. L. 2008. Go-Go global: Teaching what we know of culture and the negotiation dance. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 1: 353–370.
  • 66. 2013 471Mor, Morris, and Joh Adair, W. L., Okumura, T., & Brett, J. M. 2001. Negotiation behav- ior when cultures collide: The United States and Japan. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 371–385. Adler, N. J. 1997. International dimensions of organizational behaviour south, vol. 164: 101–114. Cincinnati, Ohio: West- ern College. Adler, N. J., & Bartholomew, S. 1992. Managing globally compe- tent people. The Executive, 6: 52– 65. Adler, N. J., & Gundersen, A. 2008. International dimensions of organizational behavior. S Mason, OH: Thomson South Western. Aiken., L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. 1991. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. 2008. Conceptualization of cultural intelligence: Definition, distinctiveness, and nomological network. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne, (Eds.), Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applica- tions: 3–15. New York: M. E. Sharpe. Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., Koh, C., Ng, K. Y., Templer, K. J., Tay, C., & Chandrasekar, N. A. 2007. Cultural intelligence: Its mea- surement and effects on cultural judgment and decision
  • 67. making, cultural adaptation and task performance. Man- agement and Organization Review, 3: 335–371. Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Tan, M. L. 2011. Cultural intelligence. Cambridge Handbook of intelligence: 582– 602. New York: Cambridge University Press. Baer, R. A. 2003. Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10: 125–143. Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. 1996. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 71: 230 –244. Barnard, C. I. 1968. The functions of the executive. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. 1986. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Con- ceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51: 1173–1182. Behrend, T. S., Sharek, D. J., Meade, A. W., & Wiebe, E. N. 2011. The viability of crowdsourcing for survey research. Behav- ior Research Methods, 43: 800 – 813. Bhawuk, D., & Brislin, R. 2000. Cross-cultural training: A review. Applied Psychology, 49: 162–191.
  • 68. Black, J. S., & Mendenhall, M. 1990. Cross-cultural training ef- fectiveness: A review and a theoretical framework for fu- ture research. Academy of Management Review, 15: 113– 136. Black, J. S., Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. 1991. Toward a com- prehensive model of international adjustment: An integra- tion of multiple theoretical perspectives. Academy of Man- agement Review, 16: 291–317. Boyacigiller, N. A., & Adler, N. J. 1991. The parochial dinosaur: Organizational science in a global context. Academy of Management Review, 16: 262–290. Brett, J. M. 2007. Negotiating globally: How to negotiate deals, resolve disputes, and make decisions across cultural bound- aries. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Brett, J. M., & Okumura, T. 1998. Inter- and intracultural negoti- ation: US and Japanese negotiators. Academy of Manage- ment Journal, 41: 495–510. Brislin, R., Worthley, R., & MacNab, B. 2006. Cultural intelli- gence: Understanding behaviors that serve people’s goals. Group and Organization Management, 31: 40 –55. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. 2003. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84: 822– 848. Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. 2011. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-
  • 69. quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6: 3–5. Byrne, J. A. 1993. The horizontal corporation. Business Week, 20: 76 – 81. Caruso, E. M., Epley, N., & Bazerman, M. H. 2006. The good, the bad, and the ugly of perspective taking in groups. In E. A. Mannix, M. A. Neale, & A. E. Tenbrunsel, Eds. Research on managing groups and teams: Ethics and groups (Vol. 8). London: Elsevier. Chua, R. Y. J., Morris, M. W., & Mor, S. 2012. Collaborating across cultures: Cultural metacognition and affect-based trust in creative collaboration. Organizational Behavior and Hu- man Decision Processes, 118: 179 –188. Claxton, G. 1997. Hare brain tortoise mind: Why intelligence increases when you think less. London: Fourth Estate. Cox, T. H., Lobel, S. A., & McLeod, P. L. 1991. Effects of ethnic group cultural differences on cooperative and competitive behavior on a group task. Academy of Management Jour- nal, 34: 827– 847. Dane, E. 2011. Paying attention to mindfulness and its effects on task performance in the workplace. Journal of Manage- ment, 37: 997–1018. Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A.,
  • 70. Bonus, K., & Sheridan, J. F. 2003. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psy- chosomatic Medicine, 65: 564 –570. Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. 1996. Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of per- sons: A merging of self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70: 713–726. Drolet, A. L., & Morris, M. W. 2000. Rapport in conflict resolution: Accounting for how face-to-face contact fosters mutual co- operation in mixed-motive conflicts. Journal of Experimen- tal Social Psychology, 36: 26 –50. Earley, P. C., & Ang, S. 2003. Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univer- sity Press. Earley, P. C., Ang, S., & Tan, J. S. 2006. CQ: Developing cultural intelligence at work. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Earley, P. C., & Gibson, C. B. 2002. Multinational work teams: A new perspective. Lawrence Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 472 SeptemberAcademy of Management Learning & Education Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. 2000. Creating hybrid team cultures: An empirical test of transnational team function- ing. Academy of Management Journal, 43: 26 – 49.
  • 71. Earley, P. C., & Peterson, R. S. 2004. The elusive cultural chame- leon: Cultural intelligence as a new approach to intercul- tural training for the global manager. Academy of Manage- ment Learning and Education, 3: 100 –115. Eisenberg, J., Lee, H.-J., Brück, F., Brenner, B., Claes, M.-T., Mi- ronski, J., & Bell, R. In press. Can business schools make students culturally competent? Academy of Management Learning and Education. Epley, N., Caruso, E., & Bazerman, M. H. 2006. When perspective taking increases taking: Reactive egoism in social interac- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91: 872– 889. Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. 1997. Prejudice as self-image mainte- nance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73: 31– 44. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. 2007. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11: 77– 83. Flavell, J. H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34: 906 –911. Fowler, S. M., & Blohm, J. M. 2004. An analysis of methods for intercultural training. In D. Landis & J. M. Bennett (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training, (3rd ed.: 37– 84). Thou- sand Oaks. CA: Sage.
  • 72. Fu, J. H., Morris, M. W., Lee, S. L., Chao, M., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. 2007. Epistemic motives and cultural conformity: Need for closure, culture, and context as determinants of conflict judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92: 191–207. Galinsky, A. D. 2002. Creating and reducing intergroup conflict: The role of perspective-taking in affecting out-group eval- uations. Research on Managing Groups and Teams, 4: 85– 113. Galinsky, A. D., Ku, G., & Wang, C. S. 2005. Perspective- taking and self-other overlap: Fostering social bonds and facilitat- ing social coordination. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8: 109. Galinsky, A. D., Maddux, W. W., Gilin, D., & White, J. B. 2008. Why it pays to get inside the head of your opponent. Psy- chological Science, 19: 378 –384. Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. 2000. Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78: 708 –724. Gelfand, M. J., & Christakopoulou, S. 1999. Culture and negoti- ator cognition: Judgment accuracy and negotiation pro- cesses in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Orga- nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79: 248 – 269. Gelfand, M. J., Erez, M., & Aycan, Z. 2007. Cross-cultural
  • 73. organi- zational behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 479 – 514. Gelfand, M., & Dyer, N. 2000. A cultural perspective on negoti- ation: Progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Applied Psychology, 49: 62–99. Graham, J. L. 1985. The influence of culture on the process of business negotiations: An exploratory study. Journal of In- ternational Business Studies, 16: 81–96. Greenhalgh, L., & Bazerman, M. 2004. Comparative advertising. Creative Concensus, Inc. Available from www. Creativeconsensus-Inc.Com. Hagel, J., & Brown, J. S. 2005. The only sustainable edge: Why business strategy depends on productive friction and dy- namic specialization. Boston, MA: Harvard Business Press. Hayes, A. F. 2009. Beyond baron and Kenny: Statistical media- tion analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76: 408 – 420. Imai, L., & Gelfand, M. J. 2010. The culturally intelligent negoti- ator: The impact of cultural intelligence (CQ) on negotiation sequences and outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Hu- man Decision Processes, 112: 83–98. Jang, S., & Chua, R. 2011. Building 16 Intercultural Trust at the negotiating table. In Negotiation excellence: Successful Deal making. Singapore: World Scientific. Johnson, J. L., Cullen, J. B., Sakano, T., & Takenouchi, H.
  • 74. 1996. Setting the stage for trust and strategic integration in Jap- anese-US cooperative alliances. Journal of International Business Studies, 27: 981–1004. Kabat-Zinn, J. 1982. An outpatient program in behavioral medi- cine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4: 33– 47. Kemp, K. E., & Smith, W. P. 1994. Information exchange, tough- ness, and integrative bargaining: The roles of explicit cues and perspective-taking. International Journal of Conflict Management, 5: 5–21. Kim, Y. J., & Van Dyne, L. 2012. Cultural intelligence and inter- national leadership potential: The importance of contact for members of the majority. Applied Psychology, 61: 272–294. Klafehn, J., Banerjee, P., & Chiu, C.-Y. 2008. Navigating cultures: The role of metacognitive cultural intelligence. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (Eds.), Handbook of cultural intelligence: The- ory, measurement, and applications: 318 –331. New York: M. E. Sharpe. Kolb, A. Y., & Kolb, D. A. 2005. Learning styles and learning spaces: Enhancing experiential learning in higher educa- tion. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 4: 193–212. Kopelman, S., Avi-Yonah, O., & Varghese, A. 2011. The mindful negotiator: Strategic emotion management and wellbeing.
  • 75. Paper Presented at the IACM Annual Conference, Istanbul, Turkey. Ku, G., Wang, C. S., & Galinsky, A. D. 2010. Perception through a perspective-taking lens: Differential effects on judgment and behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46: 792–798. LaBahn, D. W., & Harich, K. R. 1997. Sensitivity to national business culture: Effects on US-Mexican channel relation- 2013 473Mor, Morris, and Joh ship performance. Journal of International Marketing, 5: 29 –51. Landis, D., Brislin, R. W., & Hulgus, J. F. 1985. Attributional training versus contact in acculturative learning: A, Labo- ratory Study 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15: 466 – 482. Langer, E. J. 2000. Mindful learning. Current Directions in Psy- chological Science, 9: 220 –223. Lee, S., Adair, W. L., & Seo, S.-J. 2011. Cultural Perspective Taking in Cross-Cultural Negotiation. Group Decision and Negotiation: 1–17. Lytle, A., Brett, J., Barsness, Z., Tinsley, C., & Janssens, M. 1995. A paradigm for confirmatory cross-cultural research in orga- nizational-behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 17: 167–214.
  • 76. Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P., Jr.. 1987. Leading workers to lead themselves: The external leadership of self-managing work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32: 106 –129. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. 1991. Culture and the self: Impli- cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychologi- cal Review, 98: 224. McAllister, D. J. 1995. Affect- and cognition-based trust as foun- dations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 24 –59. Mendenhall, M. E., Stahl, G. K., Ehnert, I., Oddou, G., Osland, J. S., & Kuhlmann, T. 2004. Evaluation studies of cross- cultural training programs. In D. Landis, J. M. Bennett, & M. J. Bennett (Eds.). Handbook of intercultural training (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. 1985. The dimensions of expatriate acculturation: A review. Academy of Management Review, 10: 39 – 47. Molinsky, A. 2007. Cross-cultural code-switching: The psycho- logical challenges of adapting behavior in foreign cultural interactions. Academy of Management Review, 32: 622– 640. Mor, S., Ames, D. R., & Joh, J. 2013. Cultural metacognition and calibration in cross-cultural knowledge. Unpublished man- uscript. Mor, S., & Morris, M. W. 2013. The role of cultural
  • 77. metacognition in applying and updating cultural stereotypes in intercul- tural interaction. Paper presented at Academy of Manage- ment Annual Conference, Orlando, Florida. Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. 1982. Role of perspective- taking ability in negotiating under different forms of arbitration. Industrial and Labour Relations Review, 36: 378. Ng, K. Y., Van Dyne, L., & Ang, S. 2009. From experience to experiential learning: Cultural intelligence as a learning capability for global leader development. Academy of Man- agement Learning and Education Archive, 8: 511–526. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84: 231–259. Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. 2002. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128: 3–72. Patel, A., & Brett, J. 2007. Abhas-Bussan exercise. Evanston, IL: DRRC, Northwestern University. Raudenbush, S. W. 2004. HLM, vol. 6: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software Inter- national. Rehg, M. T., Gundlach, M. J., & Grigorian, R. A. 2012. Examining the influence of cross-cultural training on cultural intelli-
  • 78. gence and specific self-efficacy. Cross cultural manage- ment: An International Journal, 19: 215–232. Takahashi, C., Yamagishi, T., Liu, J. H., Wang, F., Lin, Y., & Yu, S. 2008. The intercultural trust paradigm: Studying joint cultural interaction and social exchange in real time over the internet. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32: 215–228. Tan, J. S., & Chua, R. Y. 2003. Training and developing cultural intelligence. In Cultural intelligence: Individual interac- tions across Cultures, 258 –303. Stanford, CA: Stanford Uni- versity Press. Thomas, D. C. 2006. Domain and development of cultural intel- ligence: The importance of mindfulness. Group & Organi- zation Management, 31: 78 –99. Thomas, D. C., Elron, E., Stahl, G., Ekelund, B. Z., Ravlin, E. C., Cerdin, J.-L., Poelmans, S., Brislin, R., Pekerti, A., & Aycan, Z. 2008. Cultural intelligence domain and assessment. Inter- national Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 8: 123–143. Todd, A. R., & Burgmer, P. 2013. Perspective taking and auto- matic intergroup evaluation change: Testing an associative self-anchoring account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104: 786 – 802. Triandis, H. C. 1995. Culture specific assimilators. Intercultural Sourcebook: Cross-Cultural Training Methods, 1: 179 –186. Triandis, H. C. 2006. Cultural intelligence in organizations.