SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 110
International Journal
of Clinical and Health Psychology
www.elsevier.es/ijchp
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology (2013)
13, 91−100
1697-2600/$ - see front matter © 2012 Asociación Española de
Psicología Conductual. Published by Elsevier España, S.L. All
rights reserved.
International Journal of
Clinical and Health
Psychology
Publicación cuatrimestral / Four-monthly publication ISSN
1697-2600
Volumen 13, Número 2
Mayo - 2013
Volume 13, Number 2
May - 2013
Director / Editor:
Juan Carlos Sierra
Directores Asociados / Associate Editors:
Stephen N. Haynes
Michael W. Eysenck
Gualberto Buela-Casal
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in
children with autism undergoing intensive behavioral
intervention
Javier Virues-Ortega*,a, Víctor Rodríguezb, C.T. Yua
aUniversity of Manitoba and St. Amant Research Centre,
Canada
bFundación Planeta Imaginario, Spain
Received September 4, 2012; accepted March 18, 2013
*Corresponding author at: University of Manitoba, Psychology
Department, P518 Duff Roblin Bldg., 190 Dysart Road,
MB R3T Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada.
E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Virues-Ortega).
Abstract Outcome prediction is an important component of
treatment planning and prognosis.
However, reliable predictors of intensive behavioral
intervention (IBI) have not been clearly
established. IBI is an evidence-based approach to the systematic
teaching of academic, social,
verbal, and daily living skills to individuals with autism
spectrum disorder. Incorporating
longitudinal analysis to IBI outcome studies may help to
identify outcome predictors of clinical
value. Twenty-four children with autism underwent on average
two years of IBI and completed
language, daily living skills, cognitive, and motor assessments
(Early Learning Accomplishment
Profile and the Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic,
3rd edition) every six months. We
used multilevel analysis to identify potential longitudinal
predictors including gender, age,
intervention intensity, intervention duration, total intervention
time, and pre-intervention
functioning. Results indicated that total intervention time, pre-
intervention functioning, and
age caused the greatest increase in goodness-of-fit of the
longitudinal multilevel models.
Longitudinal analysis is a promising analytical strategy to
identify reliable predictors of the
clinical outcome of IBI.
© 2012 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual.
Published by Elsevier España, S.L.
All rights reserved.
KEYWORDS
Autism;
Predictors;
Applied behavior
analysis;
Quasi-experiment
(interrupted
time-series with
one group)
Resumen La predicción de resultados de tratamiento es un
componente importante de la
planificación clínica. No obstante, no se han hallado predictores
fiables de los efectos de la in-
tervención conductual intensiva en personas con trastorno del
espectro autista. La incorpora-
ción de análisis longitudinales a la investigación sobre
resultados de tratamiento en este área
puede contribuir a la identificación de predictores con valor
clínico. En el presente estudio se
evaluaron las habilidades verbales, cognitivas y de la vida
diaria (Early Learning Accomplish-
ment Profile y Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3ª
ed.) de 24 niños con trastorno del
espectro autista en un programa de intervención conductual
intensiva. Las evaluaciones se rea-
lizaron cada seis meses y durante un periodo medio de
intervención de dos años. Mediante
PALABRAS CLAVE
Autismo;
predictor
Análisis aplicado
de la conducta;
Cuasi-experimento
(serie temporal
interrumpida con
un grupo)
92 J. Virues-Ortega et al.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a pervasive developmental
disorder that affects 1 to 2.5% of children (Baio, 2012). A
number of comprehensive psychosocial interventions for
people with ASD have been developed for which preliminary
evidence exists. These include the Early Start Denver model
(ESDM, Dawson et al., 2010), the Treatment and Education
of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped
Children (TEACCH, Welterlin, Turner-Brown, Harris, Mesibov,
& Delmolino, 2012), and intensive behavioral intervention
based on the UCLA Young Autism Project model and applied
behavior analysis (IBI, Lovaas, 1987). Although there is no
single approach to treatment for all individuals with ASD,
IBI based on applied behavior analysis is among the few
approaches to treatment that have been tested extensively
using clinical trial methodology (Rogers & Vismara, 2008;
Virués-Ortega, 2010; Wetherby & Woods, 2006).
Applied behavior analysis is devoted to the experimental
study of socially significant behavior as a function of
environmental and social variables, and is the branch of
experimental psychology that supports the conceptual
framework of IBI (Luiselli, Russo, Christian, & Wilczynski,
2008). IBI is a comprehensive and evidence-based approach
to the systematic teaching of behavioral, verbal, cognitive,
and social repertoires to individuals diagnosed with ASD
(Howlin, Magiati, & Charman, 2009). Treatment typically
involves over 20 weekly hours of one-to-one teaching
incorporating multiple learning trails and specific programs
for targeted behavioral goals. Teachers program hundreds
of learning trials per day featuring discrimination training,
prompting, generalization, and other reinforcement-based
procedures known to facilitate the acquisition of new skills
in individuals with and without disabilities (Miltenberger,
2011). The IBI curriculum integrates complex sequences of
programs from basic attending or vocalizing skills, up to
complex verbal, social, and problem-solving skills (Lovaas,
2002).
Over 20 independent trials have been conducted which
jointly suggest that IBI has moderate to large effects on
daily living skills, cognitive functioning, language, and
social behavior (Foxx, 2008; Remington et al., 2007; Virués-
Ortega, 2010). The field of IBI has shown a considerable
growth as suggested by the increasing number of service
providers and certified professionals (Shook & Johnston,
2011).
Parents of children undergoing IBI and other evidence-
based interventions frequently want to know whether their
child will be able to attend school without special support,
what areas of behavioral functioning - whether motor,
social or cognitive - are likely to improve as a consequence
of treatment, and what intervention intensity and duration
may be optimal for their child. Until recently, outcome
research had been of little assistance to respond to these
and other questions pertaining to the longitudinal
progression of children undergoing treatment.
While the evidence available strongly suggests that some
individuals benefit significantly from IBI and other
approaches to treatment, participant and intervention
characteristics associated with greater intervention effects
are not well understood. The wider literature of treatment
outcomes in ASD has examined a range of mediating and
moderating factors that could, potentially, be established
as clinically valuable predictors. These include pre-
intervention IQ, treatment duration and intensity, family
characteristics, age at intervention onset, social initiation
skills, and structural dismorphologies of the central nervous
system. The scant literature available on these factors
have been reviewed by Rogers and Vismara (2008) who
concluded that “The current intervention research focus on
main effects models provides little information about who
does well in which treatments and why” (pp. 28-29).
Age, pre-intervention functioning, and intervention
intensity have been examined in the narrower literature of
IBI outcome predictors. Studies that have examined the
role of age at the onset of IBI have shown that the earlier
the intervention, the greater the intervention effect. For
instance, Granpeesheh, Dixon, Tarbox, Kaplan, and Wilke
(2009) found that children below seven years at treatment
onset mastered more behavioral objectives every month
than children who started IBI intervention above that age.
The studies that have examined pre-intervention
functioning as a predictor of treatment outcome have not
always been consistent in their findings. Perry et al. (2008)
examined progress of children with ASD that received IBI
services by comparing standardized assessments at the
beginning and end of the service. Children were classified
as having either higher, intermediate, or lower functioning
at intake based on their Vineland Adaptive Behavior
Composite score. The higher functioning group made
substantial gains (∼20 IQ increments) relative to the other
two groups. By contrast, Ben-Itzchak, Lahat, Burgin, and
Zachor (2008) reported that pre-intervention IQ (normal,
borderline, low) did not predict the IQ gains after a year of
IBI in a group of 81 young children with ASD and
developmental disabilities.
More evidence has been accrued on the effects of
intervention intensity. However, findings remain
inconsistent. Taking IQ as a prototypical outcome (Table 1),
Makrygianni and Reed (2010) in a correlational study did
not find any effects of intensity – similar results were found
by Sheinkopf and Siegel (1998). Virués-Ortega (2010)
análisis multinivel se examinaron posibles predictores
longitudinales incluyendo sexo, edad,
intensidad y duración de la intervención, tiempo total de
intervención y nivel de funcionamien-
to previo a la intervención. Los resultados indicaron que el
tiempo total de intervención, el
funcionamiento previo y la edad causaban los mayores
incrementos en bondad de ajuste de los
modelos longitudinales. El análisis longitudinal es una
estrategia analítica prometedora en la
identificación de predictores fiables de la efectividad de la
intervención conductual intensiva.
© 2012 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual.
Publicado por Elsevier España, S.L.
Todos los derechos reservados.
Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in
children 93
reported no effects of intensity on IQ in a pooled analysis
of 19 experimental IBI studies. Finally, Reed, Osborne, and
Corness (2007) established a moderate effect of intensity
in a small trial on IBI using the Psychoeducational Profile as
outcome. In summary, treatment intensity has not been
established as a consistent predictor of IBI intervention
effects.
Longitudinal modeling of intervention outcomes may
help to establish intervention predictors more firmly than
traditional pre-post assessments. Longitudinal analyses
are able to fit the mathematical functions followed by
outcome trajectories of individual clients over a period of
time. By doing so longitudinal analysis maximizes the
statistical power of regression models aiming at meaningful
outcome predictors. For instance, if IBI effects were to
follow a non-linear progression, rather than a linear
trajectory, it may be possible to establish the role of a
particular predictor more accurately through longitudinal
multi-level analyses suited to specific non-linear
mathematical functions (Singer & Willett, 2003).
Furthermore, predictors identified based on time-series
spanning the treatment duration, as opposed to pre-post
assessments, may strengthen the clinical utility of the
predictor. For example, pre-intervention functioning could
be a strong predictor of treatment outcomes during the
first year of treatment, but not during the second.
IBI operates through a package of systematic teaching
strategies which are expected to provide the individual
with an increasing set of cognitive and behavioral resources
that will in turn offset, to various extents, the behavioral
excesses and deficits that are characteristic of ASD and
other developmental disabilities. Being a training-based
and goal-directed approach to intervention, IBI may lead to
some degree of behavioral gains for as long as the
intervention is in place. Longitudinal analysis of IBI may
help to identify distinct treatment gain itineraries across
subjects and tie those to specific predictors. For instance,
it may be possible that individuals starting at a higher pre-
intervention level of functioning benefit more from IBI but
reach an asymptote (ceiling) sooner than individuals that
start at a lower level of functioning. The longitudinal
predictors of IBI effects shall be greatly informative, albeit,
they have been rarely explored in the literature. There are
several longitudinal analyses that feature patterns of
change in individuals with ASD (Dietz, Swinkels, Buitelaar,
van Daalen & van Engeland, 2007; Jonsdottir et al., 2007;
Magiati, Moss, Charman, & Howlin, 2011). Nonetheless,
these analyses are constrained by the number of longitudinal
assessments (three or less); the number of treatment
outcomes (e.g., Dietz et al. only reported IQ); and the data
analysis strategy (e.g., no multilevel analyses).
This article describes growth patterns of motor, cognitive,
verbal, daily-living, and social skills in a sample of children
with ASD admitted into a home-based IBI program managed
by trained behavior analysts and delivering 20 to 40 weekly
hours of intervention. We used the children’s performance
in standardized assessments conducted periodically to
longitudinally create curves charting the rates and
asymptotes of various behavioral repertoires. Subsequent
analyses were conducted to test the impact of several
personal and intervention-related predictors on the
longitudinal growth of IBI outcomes. The present analysis
may help to enhance the prognostic information available
to families and clinicians by determining the extent to
which specific client- and treatment-related variables more
closely predict treatment outcome over the duration of the
intervention.
Method
Participants
Twenty-four children diagnosed with ASD (Age: Mean =
50.05 months, SD = 28.3; Gender: 21 boys and 3 girls)
admitted to the IBI program of Fundación Planeta
Imaginario (Barcelona, Spain) participated in the study.
An a priori power analysis indicated that a total sample
size of 15 was required to detect large effects (Cohen
effect size = 1). Therefore, our sample would suffice to
identify moderate to large effect sizes. A priori power
analysis assumptions were based on the pooled effect
size of 20 trials on IBI using IQ reported by Virués-Ortega
(2010) (Pooled effect size = 1.19). Participants were
recruited consecutively and were not excluded based on
their age or pre-intervention functioning at the time of
referral. All participants received a diagnosis of ASD from
an external medical consultant based on the diagnostic
criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th edition text revised. Diagnosis was
supported by standardized assessments of autism
including either the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised
(ADI-R) or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-
Generic (ADOS-G) (Le Couteur, Haden, Hammal, &
McConachie, 2008). Further personal characteristics are
presented in Table 2.
Table 1 Effect of treatment intensity on IQ in intensive
behavioral intervention outcome studies.
Study Sample Intensity range Analysis Effect
sizea (h/week) size
Makrygianni & Reed (2010) 86 15-30 Correlational (Pearson r)
.22
Sheinkopf & Siegel (1998) 11 21-32 Correlational (Pearson r)
−.06
Virués-Ortega (2010) 340 12-45 Meta-regression .01
Note. Effects reported as Cohen d effect sizes. a Sample size of
the intervention group.
*All effect sizes were non-significant, p > .05.
94 J. Virues-Ortega et al.
Instruments
Fine and gross motor, cognitive, language, self-care and
social skills were assessed by means of the Early Learning
Accomplishment Profile (E-LAP; Glover, Priminger, &
Sanford, 1988; Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin, 2001) and the
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3rd edition,
(LAP-D; Hardin, Peisner-Feinberg, & Weeks, 2005). The
E-LAP and LAP-D scores are developmental age values
expressed in months. The score range is 0 to 36 for the
E-LAP and 36 to 72 for the LAP-D. If a participant achieved
the upper limit of the score range of E-LAP, the assessment
would be repeated with the LAP-D, which would then
continue to be used as the means of standardized assessment
every 6-month period until treatment was discontinued. In
order to control for potential ceiling effects in our data, if
a participant reached the LAP-D ceiling, assessment could
be repeated one additional time to inform maintenance
(provided that the individual would continue to receive
services through the program for the next six-month
period).
Both the E-LAP and the LAP-D have a high level of
inter-rater reliability, internal consistency, and
convergent validity with IQ (Fleming, 2000; Hardin et al.,
2005; Long, Blackman, Farrell, Smolkin, & Conaway,
2005; Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin, 2001). The test-retest
reliability of both instruments is reportedly excellent,
ranging between .93 and .99 (Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin,
2001, Hardin et al., 2005). Practice effects were unlikely,
as exposure to materials and tasks during the assessment
was minimal (few trials); and prompting, reinforcement,
and correction strategies were not present during the
assessment. The Spanish version of the E-LAP and the
LAP-D materials were used in the present study. The
LAP-D was validated in a representative sample of
Spanish-speaking children (Hardin et al., 2005). No
Spanish validation of the E-LAP is currently available.
Nonetheless, test scoring is performance-based - there
are no standard scores.
Both instruments have been used frequently as
standardized assessments in intervention studies with
individuals with ASD (e.g., Ganz, Simpson & Corbin-
Newsome, 2008). Moreover, the construct validity of E-LAP
and LAP-D is supported by items screening all diagnostic
areas of ASD (e.g., “initiates on play activities,” “responds
correctly when asked to show a toy,” “inflexible and rigid
in behavior”), items informing non-pathognomonic clinical
features of autism (e.g., motor functioning), and items
covering developmentally relevant skills (e.g., matching
skills). In summary, the E-LAP and LAP-D were considered
adequate for the present analysis due to their likely
resilience to practice effects; excellent stability; excellent
convergent validity with intellectual assessment measures;
and relevance to the clinical, adaptive, and behavioral
features of ASD.
Procedure
Participants were admitted consecutively to an IBI program
within the period May 2006 through January 2011. This
program was an official international replication site of the
UCLA Young Autism Project model and affiliated with the
Lovaas Institute (2011). At the onset of intervention,
participants received an average of 31.87 weekly hours (SD
= 10.11, range 15 -47.30) of home-based systematic
teaching following the UCLA young autism model of service
delivery and curriculum (Lovaas, 2002). Average treatment
duration was 21.87 months (SD = 14.38, range 5.33-58.57).
In keeping with all IBI bonafide programs, in addition to the
hours of formal intervention, incidental teaching and
practice goals were operating during most waking hours
(parents and caregivers acted as active co-therapists).
One-to-one teaching was delivered by trained tutors that
were supervised on a weekly basis by licensed psychologists
Table 2 Characteristics of the study sample.
Pre-test Post-test
(N=24) (N= 24)
Age in months, M±SD 51.91±27.31 69.46±27.26
Gender (male:female) 23:1
Ethnicity (% Caucasian) 100%
Social class,a % high 100%
IQ,b M±SD 74.50±13.98 91.50±16.86
Skills mastered in selected areas,c M±SD
Attending (max. 19) 13.04±4.34 19.16±3.05
Imitation (max. 27) 7.84±8.41 19.92±7.24
Matching (max. 13) 6.02±7.48 13.08±6.08
Basic labeling (max. 13) 12.44±5.33 31.21±19.88
Independent play (max. 15) 3.76±4.76 11.72±6.00
Interaction with peers/adults (max. 25) 2.28±3.82 11.60±9.06
Note. aEstimated by parental education and professional
background. bWechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
Intelligence, 3rd ed.;
Bailey Scales of Infant Development, and Merrill-Palmer Scales
of Mental Tests. cNumber of skills mastered by area (Lovaas
Institute
Midwest, 2010).
M = mean; SD = stardard deviation.
Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in
children 95
with a background in behavior analysis. Parents received
weekly or bi-weekly progress updates, and supervision and
specific routines that required their involvement in order
to ensure the consistency of the interventions across
contexts and caregivers. Intervention was individualized
and comprehensive; and targeted motor, behavioral, daily-
living, verbal, cognitive, and social skills. Goals were
informed by a standardized curriculum composed of over
850 skills organized in 45 broad clinical areas (e.g., reading,
self-control skills). These goals are informed by
developmental sequences of typically developing children
(Luiselli et al., 2008) and include skills that are instrumental
for the acquisition of more complex repertoires (e.g.,
matching skills, imitation). Teaching sessions were delivered
via one-to-one teaching with gradual transition to group
activities and natural contexts. Transition to natural social
contexts was emphasized after mastery in one-to-one
teaching format. Decision-making in terms of hour allocation
and treatment discontinuation weighted a number of
factors including availability of school support, progress
achieved, family priorities, and treatment costs. Typically,
individuals that showed a persistent asymptote in their
learning achievements or that became independent at
school were assigned a reduced number of hours in
preparation of service discontinuation (for details on the
IBI curriculum see Lovaas, 2002). The current program was
in line with the guidelines for responsible conduct published
by the Behavior Analyst Certification Board (2010).
All participants underwent standardized assessments
with the E-LAP or the LAP-D prior to the intervention and
approximately every six months into the program (average
data points per participant 3.8, range 2-6). The selection,
administration, and correction of instruments followed the
guidelines by Jurado and Pueyo (2012).The research
assistants conducting the standardized assessments were
not involved in the administration of treatment and were
not familiar with the hypotheses of the study.
Data analysis
Figure 1 shows the individual growth trajectories of
participants for the eight E-LAP and LAP-D outcomes. Visual
inspection of the data plots over time suggests that
trajectories accelerated away from the start point shortly
after the intervention commenced while progression
decelerated as the individual approached a personal or
scale ceiling. Therefore, individual trajectories did not
follow a linear progression but rather an exponential
negative growth. Exponential negative trajectories are
composed formally of a negatively accelerated curve,
Figure 1 Trajectories of Early Learning Accomplishment Profile
and Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic scores over
time.
Fitted exponential negative curves (solid black line) were
obtained for individuals above (dotted grey lines) and below
(solid grey
lines) the median of pre-intervention functioning at baseline in
each domain.
Gross Motor Fine Motor Pre-writing Cognitive
Receptive
80
80
60
60
40
40
20
20
0
0
0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60
Expressive Self-care Social
Intervention Time (Months)
Sc
al
e
sc
or
es
Sc
al
e
sc
or
es
96 J. Virues-Ortega et al.
ending in an upper asymptote. According to the formal
attributes of the data we selected a multilevel regression
model based on the following exponential negative
function:
Yij = αi – (αi – π0i) e –π TIMEij
Where αi represents the upper asymptote, π0i represents
the lower end of the trajectory, and π1i represents the
slope of the curve. Figure 2 illustrates different exponential
negative patterns of change over time for various parameter
values.
Multilevel models provide two distinct levels of analysis:
level-1 and level-2. The structural parts of the level-1
submodel contain two level-1 parameters and one within-
person variance component (εij). The first parameter, known
as intercept (π0i), represents the initial status of an
individual i in the population. The second parameter, known
as slope (π1i), represents the rate of change for the
individual i in the population by unit of time. Therefore,
level-1 establishes individual change overtime. By contrast,
the parameters at level-2 do not represent individual
variation, but average level of the outcome in the
population. Specifically, the parameters at level-2 represent
the average outcome level in the population corresponding
to the intercept and slope values at level-1. At level-2, the
pattern of change is not examined in terms of time, as is
the case at level-1, but rather, in terms of a predictor. In
summary, there are four parameters at level-2: γ00 is the
population average of level-1 intercept with level-2
predictor value of 0, γ01 is the population average difference
in level-1 intercept for a 1-unit variation in the predictor,
γ10 is the population average of the level-1 slope when the
predictor equals 0, and finally, γ11 is the population average
difference when the predictor equals 1. γ00 and γ10 are
baseline parameters while γ01 and γ11 estimate the association
of the predictor with the initial status and the rate of
change of the longitudinal progression, respectively. The
model also provides a residual variance value for the
intercept (σ02), the slope (σ12), and the covariance among
these two (σ01). For multilevel models incorporating two
predictors we will also report γ12 and γ12, which represents
the population’s average variation in the outcome level for
a one-unit increment in the predictors 1 and 2 (level-1),
respectively (for more details in multilevel analysis refer to
Singer & Willet, 2003). The estimation of the predictor
coefficients at level-2 is presented formally below:
π0i = ϒ00 + ϒ01 (PREDICTORi – PREDICTOR) + ξ0i
π0i = ϒ10 + ϒ11 (PREDICTORi – PREDICTOR) + ξ1i
According to this model, individual growth parameters
(π0i, π1i) across children will be a function of population
average values (γ00, γ10), and population variance components
(ξ0i, ξ1i) represented by residual variances (σ02, σ12) and
covariance (σ01).
We estimated a series of multilevel models using different
sets of predictors in order to select models that would
maximize goodness-of-fit for a given outcome when
compared with an unconditional baseline model (model
with no predictors). This was accomplished in two sequential
multilevel analyses. In the first sets of models we examined
the impact of time-based predictors (intervention duration
in weeks, total hours of intervention - weekly hours of
interventions multiplied by weeks of intervention - and age
in months). We would then select the model incorporating
the single time-based predictor with best goodness-of-fit
for each of the eight outcomes under analysis. Subsequently,
we calculated a new set of two-predictor models
incorporating the predictor previously selected and a
specific personal factor that, when added, resulted in
further increases in goodness-of-fit. The personal factors
examined for each of the eight outcomes were age (if not
selected in the preceding step), gender, and pre-intervention
functioning. Two levels of pre-intervention functioning
were established using the median value at baseline as cut-
off point. The rationale for selecting these predictors is
twofold: a) they are all common individual/treatment
characteristics readily accessible to the clinician, and b)
they have been examined in previous IBI studies although
not in the context of a longitudinal analysis. Longitudinal
predictors that changed overtime (intervention duration,
total intervention duration, age) were re-calculated each
time an individual was assessed.
The Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian
information criterion (BIC) were computed as goodness-of-
fit parameters for all one- and two-predictor models. Lower
AIC and BIC values are indicative of better fitting. The best
100
75
50
25
0
E(Y)
Time
0 2 4 6 8 10
α = 100
π1 = .3
π1 = .2
π1 = .1
π0 = 15
Figure 2 Exponential patterns of change based on different
parameter values.
Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in
children 97
fitting two-predictor model was selected for each outcome
and was fully reported. All analyses were conducted with
STATA version 11 (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX)
and its GLAMM program for multi-level analysis. A .05 level
of significance was used throughout. Results have been
reported according to the guidelines by Hartley (2012).
By comparing the goodness-of-fit of one- and two-
predictor models with an unconditional model, we aimed
to establish which factors would better explain the
longitudinal variation in our data. This analysis will help to
determine prominent trajectories of intervention outcomes
based on specific predictors. This strategy also serves the
purpose of suggesting causality in the absence of a control
group, similar to the way in which dose-response relations
inform causation (see a discussion relevant to this point in
Arjas and Parner, 2004). Namely, the causation inference
would be supported if intervention intensity (e.g., total
intervention hours at each time of assessment) is indeed
superior in its ability to increase the fit of the model
relative to an arbitrary time-dependent predictor
(individuals’ age).
Results
The examination of the goodness-of-fit parameters of
multilevel regression models showed that one-predictor
and two-predictor models had a superior fit than
unconditional models for every domain of the E-LAP and
the LAP-D. AIC and BIC goodness-of-fit parameters of all
models are reported in Table 3. Total intervention time
(hours per week multiplied by weeks of intervention) was
the single predictor with the highest favorable impact on
goodness-of-fit for all E-LAP and LAP-D outcomes. Other
time-based predictors including individuals’ age and
intervention duration in months had a positive impact in
the model’s fit, but did so to a lesser extent than total
intervention time in all eight outcomes.
Further improvements in goodness-of-fit were achieved
in two-predictor models. Keeping total intervention time as
the first factor, we examined the fit of regression models
incorporating age, gender, or pre-intervention level as a
second predictor. Age was the second most efficient
predictor in terms of improving fit of the regression models
for gross motor function, receptive language, self-care,
and social behavior; while pre-intervention level was the
second most efficient predictor for regression models using
fine motor function, prewriting, cognitive, and expressive
language (Table 3). The regression models of domains
assessing motor, daily living, and social skills (gross motor
function, fine motor function, self-care and social behavior)
achieved better fitting than regression models of language-
related domains (prewriting, receptive language, expressive
language, cognitive).
Table 4 presents the best fitting two-predictor multilevel
model for each E-LAP and LAP-D outcome. Both predictors
were statistically significant (p < .001) for every outcome.
Rate of change attributable to total intervention time in
hours (γ11) ranged from .004 to .009 (outcome average
increase by predictor unit). Coefficient magnitudes for age
in months (γ12) as a predictor ranged from .391 to .514.
Finally, coefficients for the dichotomous variable pre-
Table 3 Goodness-of-fit parameters of all one- and two-
predictor multilevel models of change for Early Learning
Accomplishment Profile and Learning Accomplishment Profile-
Diagnostic scores.
Goodness-of-fit (AIC, BIC)
GMF FMF PWR COG RLG ELG SFC SBH
Unconditional model 761.56 774.54 761.31 774.29 809.53
822.50 782.89 795.87
800.47 813.44 761.91 774.88 755.07 768.05 747.29 760.26
One-predictor models
Intervention duration 715.86 721.17 779.29 746.27 763.16
732.19 706.67 706.42
731.43 736.70 794.86 761.84 778.73 747.77 722.24 721.99
Total intervention time 693.33 691.12 744.67 720.03 737.42
712.48 677.15 688.53
708.59 706.38 759.93 735.29 752.68 727.74 692.41 703.79
Age 716.29 731.43 776.85 761.27 769.47 740.37 708.01 721.45
731.86 747 792.42 776.84 785.03 755.94 723.58 737.02
Two-predictor models
Age 664.01 673.20 727.92 710.50 724.67 702.71 651.35 676.02
681.81 691 745.72 728.30 742.47 720.52 669.16 693.82
Gender 691.93 690.19 745.08 719.15 735.78 711.83 675.18
687.74
709.73 707.99 762.88 736.95 753.59 729.63 692.99 705.55
Pre-intervention level 675.54 661.39 715.56 705.10 726.15
698.89 661.74 676.27
693.35 679.20 733.36 722.90 743.96 716.70 679.55 694.07
Note. AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian
information criterion; COG = Cognitive; ELG = Expressive
language; FMF = Fine
motor function; GMF = Gross motor function; PWR =
Prewriting; RLG = Receptive language; SBH = Social behavior;
SFC = Self-care.
Intercept constant; slope is established as the intervention
duration in months. Level-2 and level-3 best fitting models by
outcome are
highlighted.
98 J. Virues-Ortega et al.
intervention level (γ12) ranged from 22.971 to 35.669. Figure
1 portrays fitted curves based on an exponential negative
growth of subsamples above and below the median value of
pre-intervention level for each of the eight standardized
outcomes.
Discussion
Multilevel regression analyses based on an exponential
negative growth trajectory indicated that total intervention
duration in hours was the single predictor with the highest
contribution to the model fit for all outcomes when
compared with unconditional models. This finding suggests
that a subtle characteristic of the intervention – a
combination of both treatment intensity (weekly hours)
and treatment duration (total weeks of treatment) –
optimizes the fitting of individual trajectories to a specific
mathematical function for the duration of the intervention
and across a range of standardized outcomes. Improvements
in model fitting caused by duration alone did not improve
goodness-of-fit to the extent achieved by total intervention
time as a single predictor (Table 3). Therefore, our data
suggest that both intensity and duration, as represented by
total intervention time, remained important factors of
intervention gains regardless of pre-intervention functioning
or age. Finally, total intervention time remained significant
(p < .001) in all final two-predictor multilevel models (Table
4). When used in one-predictor models, pre-intervention
functioning was inferior to total intervention time in terms
of improving goodness-of-fit for all outcomes.
We tested the impact of pre-intervention functioning in
the goodness-of-fit of multilevel models incorporating two
predictors. Including pre-intervention level as a second
predictor, improved goodness-of-fit for all outcomes in the
two-predictor models (Table 3). For four of the eight
standardized outcomes examined (fine motor, pre-writing,
cognitive, expressive language), pre-intervention level was
the personal characteristic (above age and gender) that
generated the greatest improvement in model fit. Pre-
intervention level was a significant factor (p < .001) in the
final two-predictor models for fine motor, pre-writing,
cognitive and expressive language domains (Table 4).
Interestingly, these outcomes involved more complex
cognitive abilities relative to the remainder of E-LAP and
LAP-D outcomes (e.g., fine vs. gross motor; expressive vs.
receptive language; cognitive vs. self-care).
Our results suggest that individuals starting intervention
at a lower level in a given outcome were more likely to
follow an asymptotical growth as opposed to individuals
that initiated treatment with a higher level of performance
(cf. fitted curves on Fig. 1). The visual inspection of the
individual longitudinal trajectories in our sample suggests
that pre-intervention level is a plausible predictor of
individuals’ performance over the course of the intervention
to the extent that a bimodal pattern seems obvious in most
of the outcomes (e.g., Cognitive, Social). Bimodal
trajectories in our dataset are consistent with the distinction
between most and least positive responders to IBI discussed
by Remington et al. (2007). The visual examination of
individual trajectories on Figure 1 suggests that the pre-
intervention median is an acceptable cut-off point as
attested by the predictors significance and fit gains in
models that incorporated this factor. A more sophisticated
strategy to determine the cut-off point would have required
asymmetrical assignment of participants above and below
the cut-off points, which may have harmed statistical
power and increase the potential for type II error. Therefore,
future analyses would benefit from samples sizes larger
than ours.
Learning processes have been found to accommodate
well to exponential negative or logistic patterns of change
(e.g., Hicklin, 1976). The possibility remains, however, that
non-linear patterns of growth found in the present study
may have been caused by measurement-dependent factors,
like inadequate scaling assumptions or excessive ceiling
Table 4 Multilevel models for Early Learning Accomplishment
Profile and Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic scores
change over the duration of intensive behavioral intervention.
Goodness-of-fit (AIC, BIC)
GMF FMF PWR COG RLG ELG SFC SBH
Fixed effects
Intercept (γ00) 7.44 13.91** 10.97* 8.87 −3.00 5.46 -4.45 .52
Intervention, hours (γ11) .00** .01** .01** .01** .01** .01**
.01** .01**
Age, months (γ12) .51** – – – .51** – .57** .39**
Pre-intervention levela (γ12) – 28.43** 35.67** 24.27** –
22.97** – –
Variance components
Level-1: Within-person (σε2) 25.32 23.32 47.64 32.91 47.79
28.39 16.54 18.90
Level-2: Intercept (σ02) 89.91 60.71 158.79 123.89 204.10
122.62 155.74 159.19
Level-2: Slope (σ12) .07 .19 .25 .33 4.37 .40 .10 .32
Level-2: Covariance (σ01) .46 2.37 -4.16 2.76 .11 .98 2.81 3.92
Note. COG = Cognitive; ELG = Expressive language; FMF =
Fine motor function; GMF = Gross motor function; PWR =
Prewriting; RLG =
Receptive language; SBH = Social behavior; SFC = Self-care.
Goodness-of-fit parameters and domain abbreviations in Table
3. *p < .01;
**p < .001. aPre-intervention levels above and below the
median at pre-test.
Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in
children 99
effects in the psychometric instrument used to establish
treatment outcomes. These potential shortcomings,
however, may have had little impact on the validity of the
predictors, which is independent from the specific shape of
the longitudinal growth.
The contributions of our study are primarily methodological
and to a lesser extent practical. As discussed in our
introduction, the literature on the effect of intensity and
other predictors on the outcome of IBI have yielded
inconsistent results. This inconsistency may be explained,
at least to some extent, by non-linear variations of the
predictor and the outcome overtime. Therefore, longitudinal
studies may enhance our ability to examine outcome
predictors with sufficient statistical power. Our results
provide evidence in this direction being the first study to
use this methodology in the context of IBI intervention.
In terms of the applied relevance of our findings, future
longitudinal studies expanding the present analysis could
eventually provide the basis for evidence-informed clinical
decision-making. Namely, clinicians could combine various
predictors available at the beginning of the intervention
(e.g., pre-intervention functioning in an specific area, age,
expected treatment intensity and duration) to estimate the
progress of the client over the next years, which could in
turn inform the decision-making of family, caregivers and
health decision-makers in terms of treatment planning and
resource allocation.
In summary, the present analysis helps to identify the
general features of the longitudinal progression of children
with autism undergoing IBI. Our results suggest that
increased intervention time, lower age at intervention
onset, and higher pre-intervention functioning might be
associated with greater IBI outcomes for intervention
programs of up to four years in duration. The present study
provides the methodological basis for predictor identification
in the longitudinal analysis of IBI.
References
Arjas, E., & Parner, J. (2004). Causal reasoning from
longitudinal
data. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 31, 171-187.
Baio, J. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders:
Autism
and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites,
United States, 2008. MMWR Surveillance Summaries,
61(SS03),
1-19.
Behavior Analyst Certification Board (2010). BACB Guidelines
for
responsible conduct for behavior analysts. Available from:
http://www.bacb.com/index.php?page=57 [retrieved 23 Jan
2013].
Ben-Itzchak, E., Lahat, E., Burgin, E., & Zachor, A. D. (2008).
Cognitive, behavior and intervention in young children with
autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 447-458.
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J.,
Greenson,
J., Donaldson, A., & Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled
trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start
Denver model. Pediatrics, 125, 17-23.
Dietz, C., Swinkels, S. H., Buitelaar, J. K., van Daalen, E., &
van
Engeland, H. (2007). Stability and change of IQ scores in
preschool children diagnosed with autistic spectrum disorder.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 405-410.
Fleming, J.A. (2000). An examination of inter-rater reliability
of
the E-LAP. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University.
Foxx, R.M. (2008). Applied behavior analysis treatment of
autism.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17,
821-834.
Ganz, J. B., Simpson, R. L., & Corbin-Newsome, J. (2008). The
impact of the picture exchange communication system on
requesting and speech development in preschoolers with autism
spectrum disorders and similar characteristics. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 157-169.
Glover, M. E., Priminger, J. L. & Sanford, A. R. (1988). Early
learning
accomplishments profile. Winston-Salem, NC: Kaplan.
Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., &
Wilke,
A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treatment intensity on
behavioral intervention outcomes for children with autism
spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3,
1014-1022.
Hardin, B. J., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Weeks, S. W. (2005).
The
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic (LAP-D), third
edition. Lewisville, NC: Kaplan Early Learning.
Hartley, J. (2012). New ways of making academic articles easier
to
read. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology,
12, 143-160.
Hicklin, W. J. (1976). A model for mastery learning based on
dynamic equilibrium theory. Journal of Mathematical
Psychology, 13, 79-88.
Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic
review of
early intensive behavioral interventions for children with
autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities,114, 23-41.
Jónsdóttir, S. L., Saemundsen, E., Asmundsdóttir, G.,
Hjartardóttir,
S., Asgeirsdóttir, B. B., Smáradóttir, H. H., Sigurdardóttir, S.,
&
Smaári, J. (2007). Follow-up of children diagnosed with
pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 37, 1361-1374.
Jurado, M. A., & Pueyo, R. (2012). Doing and reporting a
neuropsychological assessment. International Journal of
Clinical and Health Psychology, 12, 123-141.
Le Couteur, A., Haden, G., Hammal, D., & McConachie, H.
(2008).
Diagnosing autism spectrum disorders in pre-school children
using two standardised assessment instruments: The ADI-R and
the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38,
362-372.
Long, C. E., Blackman, J. A., Farrell, W. J., Smolkin, M. E. &
Conaway, M. R. (2005). A comparison of developmental versus
functional assessment in the rehabilitation of young children.
Pediatric Rehabilitation, 8, 151-161.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal
educational
and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3-9.
Lovaas, O. I. (2002). Teaching developmentally disable
children:
The ME Book. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.
Lovaas Institute (2011). NIMH Replication Sites. Available
from:
http://www.lovaas.com/contact.php [retrieved 1 Sep 2012].
Lovaas Institute Midwest (2010). Matrix curriculum and clinical
goal areas. Minneapolis, MN: Author.
Luiselli, J. K., Russo, D. C., Christian, W. P., & Wilczynski, S.
M.
(2008). Effective practices for children with autism. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Magiati, I., Moss, J., Charman, T., & Howlin, P. (2011).
Patterns of
change in children with autism spectrum disorders who received
community based comprehensive interventions in their pre-
school years: A seven year follow-up study. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1016-1027.
Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). Factors impacting on
the
outcomes of Greek intervention programmes for children with
autistic spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 4, 697-708.
Miltenberger, R. G. (2011). Behavior modification: Principles
and
procedures (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage.
100 J. Virues-Ortega et al.
Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Hardin, B. J. (2001). The early
learning
accomplishment profile edition examiner’s manual and
technical report. New York: Kaplan.
Perry, A., Cummings, A., Geier, J. D., Freeman, N. L., Hughes,
S.,
LaRose, L., Managan, T., Reitzel, J. A., & Williams, J. (2008).
Effectiveness of intensive behavioral intervention in a large,
community-based program. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 2, 621-642.
Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Corness, M. (2007). Relative
effectiveness
of different home-based behavioral approaches to early
teaching intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 37, 1815-1821.
Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., degli Espinosa,
F.,
Jahr, E., Brown, T., Alsford, P., Lemaic, M., & Ward, N.
(2007).
Early intensive behavioral intervention. American Journal on
Mental Retardation, 112, 418-438.
Rogers, S. J., & Vismara, L. A. (2008). Evidence-based
comprehensive
treatments for early autism. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology,37, 8-38.
Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behavioral
treatment of young children with autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 28, 15-23.
Shook, G. L., & Johnston, J. M. (2011). Training and
professional
certification. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane
(Eds.),
Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 498-510). New
York: Guilford Press.
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data
analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.
Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic intervention
for autism in early childhood. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,
387-399.
Welterlin, A., Turner-Brown, L. M., Harris, S., Mesibov, G., &
Delmolino,
L. (2012). The home TEACCHing program for toddlers with
autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1827-1835.
Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social
interaction
project for children with autism spectrum disorders beginning
in the second year of life: A preliminary study. Topics in Early
Childhood Special Education, 26, 67-82.
O R I G I N A L P A P E R
Savant Syndrome: Realities, Myths and Misconceptions
Darold A. Treffert
Published online: 6 August 2013
� Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
Abstract It was 126 years ago that Down first described
savant syndrome as a specific condition and 70 years ago
that Kanner first described Early Infantile Autism. While as
many as one in ten autistic persons have savant abilities,
such special skills occur in other CNS conditions as well
such that approximately 50 % of cases of savant syndrome
have autism as the underlying developmental disability and
50 % are associated with other disabilities. This paper sorts
out realities from myths and misconceptions about both
savant syndrome and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) that
have developed through the years. The reality is that low
IQ is not necessarily an accompaniment of savant syn-
drome; in some cases IQ can be superior. Also, savants can
be creative, rather than just duplicative, and the skills
increase over time on a continuum from duplication, to
improvisation to creation, rather than diminishing or sud-
denly disappearing. Genius and prodigy exist separate from
savant syndrome and not all such highly gifted persons
have Asperger’s Disorder. This paper also emphasizes the
critical importance of separating ‘autistic-like’ symptoms
from ASD especially in children when the savant ability
presents as hyperlexia (children who read early) or as
Einstein syndrome (children who speak late), or have
impaired vision (Blindisms) because prognosis and out-
come are very different when that careful distinction is
made. In those cases the term ‘outgrowing autism’ might
be mistakenly applied when in fact the child did not have
ASD in the first place.
Keywords Savant syndrome � Autism � Autism
spectrum disorder � Hyperlexia � Einstein syndrome
Realities
Savant Syndrome Defined
Savant syndrome is a rare but spectacular condition in
which persons with developmental disabilities, including
but not limited to autism, or other CNS disorders or disease
have some spectacular ‘islands of genius’ that stand in
jarring juxtaposition to overall limitations. (Treffert 2010)
The condition can be present from birth and evident in
early childhood (congenital) or develop later in life after
CNS injury or disease (acquired). It affects males 4–6 times
more frequently than females. Typically the skills occur in
five general areas—music, art, calendar calculating,
mathematics or mechanical/visual-spatial skills. Other
skills occur less frequently including language (polyglot),
unusual sensory discrimination, athletics or outstanding
knowledge in specific fields such as neurophysiology, sta-
tistics, navigation or computers, for example. Skills are
usually single skills, but multiple skills can occur as well.
Whatever the skill it is always associated with massive
memory of a habit or procedural type—very narrow but
exceedingly deep within the confines of the special skill. In
some cases massive memory is the special skill.
Savant Syndrome is Not a New Disorder (Nor is
Autism)
It is over 200 years ago since the first case of savant syn-
drome appeared in a scientific journal in Germany (Moritz
1783). And it is 126 years since Dr. J. Langdon Down first
D. A. Treffert (&)
Agnesian HealthCare, 430 East Division Street, Fond du Lac,
WI 54935, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
123
J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571
DOI 10.1007/s10803-013-1906-8
described savant syndrome as a distinct condition. (Down
1887). In his 1887 lectures Down described ten cases of
savant syndrome, including a boy who had memorized The
Rise and Fall of the Roman Empire verbatim and could
recite it backward or forward. Interestingly, in those same
lectures Down described a form of mental retardation later
named Down’s syndrome, and he described as well a form
of ‘‘developmental retardation’’ that unmistakably con-
sisted of cases of what we now would term early onset and
late onset autistic disorder. (Treffert 2006) And in Kanner’s
description of early infantile autism cases in 1944 there are
several individuals who would now be considered cases of
savant syndrome.
Down coined the term ‘idiot savant’. He did not intend
that term be degrading or insulting. At the time ‘idiot’ was
an accepted scientific word for persons with an IQ below
25 and ‘savant’ was derived from the French word savoir,
meaning ‘to know’. Because of its pejorative connotation, a
1988 paper suggested it was time to discard that archaic
term and substitute ‘savant syndrome’ instead. (Treffert
1988) Then in 1989 the movie Rain Man made the term
‘‘autistic savant’’ household words.
Not All Savants are Autistic, and Not All Autistic
Persons are Savants
Rain Man was a marvelous movie. It was accurately and
sensitively done. Yet some persons came away from the
movie assuming that, like Raymond Babbitt, all savants are
autistic. Not so. Approximately one in ten persons with
autism has savant skills; so nine out of ten do not.
Approximately 1 out of 1,400 persons with mental retar-
dation or CNS deficits other than autism do have savant
skills so such abilities are not limited to autistic disorder.
(Saloviita 2000) Hence not all autistic persons are savants,
and not all savants are autistic.
Savant Skills Represent a Spectrum of Abilities
While admittedly a subjective scale at this point, savant
skills lie on a spectrum of abilities. Most common are
splinter skill savants who have obsessive preoccupation
with and memorization of music & sports trivia, birthdays,
license plate numbers, historical facts, train or bus sched-
ules, navigation abilities, or maps for example. Talented
savants are those in whom musical, art or other special
abilities are more conspicuous not only in contrast to
individual limitations, but also in contrast to peer group
abilities whether disabled or not. And prodigious savant is
an extremely high threshold term reserved for those
extraordinarily rare individuals in whom the special skill is
so outstanding that were it to be seen in a non-impaired
person such a person would be termed a ‘‘prodigy’’ or
‘‘genius’’.
The Acquired Savant: ‘‘Accidental Genius’’
In 1923 Minogue reported a case in which musical genius
appeared in a three-year old child following meningitis. In
1980 Brink described the case of Mr. Z who demonstrated
savant mechanical skills and traits at age nine after a bullet
wound to the left brain produced muteness, deafness and
left sided paralysis, but precipitated the newly surfaced
savant skills. Dorman in 1991 published a case in which an
8 year old boy began to show exceptional calendar calcu-
lating skills following a left hemispherectomy.
But it was Miller’s reports on 12 individuals with fronto-
temporal dementia who developed exceptional savant art
and musical skills that really brought the ‘acquired savant’
to prominent attention. (Miller et al. 1996, 1998, 2000)
Miller had done SPECT imaging on these 12 patients, and
he also did SPECT imaging on a 9 year old autistic, artistic
savant. In these instances there was left anterior temporal
dysfunction and evidence of what Kapur called ‘‘para-
doxical functional facilitation’’–dysfunction in one area of
the brain which uncovered, or facilitated ‘paradoxical’
function in some other area of still intact brain capacity
(Kapur 1996).
Since that time there have been numerous reports of
what might be called ‘‘acquired savant syndrome’’ fol-
lowing a cerebral insult from stroke, a blow to the head,
dementia or other CNS disease or injury accompanied by
the emergence of savant skills, sometimes at a prodigious
level (Treffert 2010). In most of these cases there was some
sort of ‘trade-off’ of cognitive or other abilities for the new
found savant skills. Yet in other cases, more aptly called
‘accidental genius’ (following being struck by lightning in
one instance) there has been no trade off at all with the
emergence of new found skills. These instances raise many
interesting questions about dormant capacity within us all,
and raise the even more challenging question of how to tap
those buried abilities without enduring some CNS catas-
trophe. These cases of acquired savant syndrome are pre-
sented in much more detail on the savant syndrome web
site at www.savantsyndrome.com.
The Most Important Question of All: How do They
do It?
There have been many theories put forth to try to explain
savant syndrome ranging from early heredity theories to
present day Quantum theory. (Treffert 2010). Some of the
neuropsychological theories such as weak central coher-
ence, mind blindedness are interesting as they apply to the
autistic savant. But 50 % of savants are not autistic. The
J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 565
123
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
role of heredity is no doubt a contributor and the search for
a savant ‘gene’ is underway, one study finding such a gene,
but another not confirming that finding. Compensatory
learning, reinforcement and repetition-compulsion may
also play a role, but then, if those dynamics produce savant
syndrome, why wouldn’t that apply to all persons with
autism or other CNS limitations?
The theory I favor is that what I have come to call the
‘‘three R’s’’ and reflects the process Kapur termed ‘‘para-
doxical functional facilitation’’ in 1996 in which one area
of the brain in released from the inhibiting influence of
some other brain area. In the case of savants, both con-
genital and acquired, there is brain damage in one area,
frequently the left hemisphere, with recruitment of still
intact brain tissue in another area of the brain, rewiring of
circuitry to that new area, and release of dormant capacity,
through a disinhibiting process, of information and skills
already stored in that newly recruited area.
It is genetic memory—the genetic transfer of knowledge
and skills—that accounts for the already stored dormant
capacity tapped by the recruitment, rewiring and release. I
address genetic memory much more fully in Islands of
Genius as well, and on the savant syndrome web site at
www.savantsyndrome.com. Genetic memory is based on
the fact that some savants, particularly those severely
limited in other ways, clearly ‘‘know things they never
learned’’. The only possible way to know things one never
learned—sometimes at complex levels—is for that
knowledge to be factory installed, genetically transmitted.
But there is one other important element that contributes
to the ‘‘how do they do it?’’ question. That is the role of the
family or other caregivers, teachers or mentors in first
discovering the special gift in the savant, then tenderly
nurturing and encouraging that gift, and supporting and
reinforcing it by praise coupled with copious unconditional
love.
Some Mysteries Remain
There are many scientific mysteries still about savant
syndrome. But two are especially intriguing. First is the
conspicuous regularity in which the triad of mental
impairment (often from autism) ? impaired vision ?
musical genius occurs. Savant syndrome is rare but the
frequency of this triad is very conspicuous and dispropor-
tionate throughout the history of savant cases combining
blindness and mental disability with prodigious musical
skills. The first of such persons was Thomas Bethune,
better known as ‘‘Blind Tom’’, who was born in 1850 and
gave his first public piano concert at age 8. From then on he
became an international celebrity and was the most cele-
brated black concert artist of the nineteenth century. His
repertoire was in the thousands of pieces including many of
his own compositions, the first of which he created at age
five.
Since that time a number of such cases with this triad are
glaringly over-represented in savant syndrome, in itself a
rare condition. Leslie Lemke in the United States, Derek,
Paravicini in the United Kingdom and Hikari Oe in Japan
are present day representatives of this amazing combina-
tion of ability and disability. In addition to those examples,
a number of other such cases are documented in detail in
Islands of Genius. The savant syndrome web site provides
further video evidence and documentation of this extraor-
dinary triad of mental impairment ? blindness ? musical
genius.
A second mystery is the why calendar calculating, an
obscure skill in neurotypical persons, is seemingly almost
universally present in persons with savant syndrome. This
ability is a clear example of how savants, sometimes
severely impaired, innately ‘‘know things they never
learned’’. Yes, there are formulas for calendar calculating.
And yes, if any person puts his or her mind to it, he or she
can learn (laboriously) how to calendar calculate. But
savants seem to have this algorithm or formula ‘uncon-
sciously’ inscribed or inculcated in their brain and in most
such individuals there simply has not been any ‘study’ of
the calendar nor the ‘learning’ of any formula. Why cal-
endar calculating? And why is that so prominent in savant
syndrome but generally not seen in other brain diseases or
disorders? Some imaging studies are underway with cal-
endar calculating savants, comparing them to neurotypical
‘expert’ calendar calculators and control groups.
Myths and Misconceptions
Savants are Not ‘Creative’
Some observers, while extolling the eidetic-like ability and
memory of savants, point out that in contrast to such
astonishing imitative ability, savants, as a group are not
very creative. In fact I was one of those observers who
wrote just that in the original 1988 version of Extraordi-
nary People:Understanding Savant Syndrome. I raised the
question there ‘‘Is the savant creative?’’ I answered it this
way: ‘‘In my experience, not very’’.
I was wrong and have corrected that perception in my
later writings. What changed my mind? Some additional
years of observation. There is always a tremendous
advantage in having a longitudinal view of a patient and his
or her ‘natural history’ of illness or disorder, compared to a
one-time, snapshot consultation. And now, having the
opportunity to observe the ‘natural history’ of how the
savant skills emerge and develop over many years, I have
noted predictable and replicable sequence of steps that
566 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571
123
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
progresses from imitation, to improvisation, to creation in
savant syndrome. Let me expand on that, using Leslie
Lemke as an example.
Leslie Lemke’s ability to store and replicate music, even
after only a single hearing, was spectacular. Indeed at age
14 he was able to play back Tchaikovsky’s First Piano
concerto flawlessly having heard it for the first time as a
theme song to a television movie.
Over time, however, Leslie began to show improvisa-
tional skills in a addition to replication abilities. For
example at a 1989 concert a young girl came up to the
stage in the challenge portion of the concert and played
‘‘Mississippi Hotdog’’. Leslie listened and then, when
asked, dutifully played back the piece exactly as he had
heard it. But toward the end of the piece he began to look a
bit restless, and seemed more excited and more eager to
play. After the initial playback of Mississippi Hotdog was
completed, flawlessly as usual, Leslie then launched into a
5 min improvisation which could be called ‘‘Variations on
a Theme of Mississippi Hotdog.’’ He changed pitch,
changed tempo and demonstrated convincingly that he
does indeed have innate access to the ‘rules of music’,
confirmed by a number of professional musicians who have
observed him.
Adding to the improvisation skill now is creation and
composing of entirely new pieces. One such song he calls
‘‘Down Home on the Farm in Arpin’’, and another he
names ‘‘Bird Song’’. In that latter piece he duplicates, by
whistling softly as he plays his new tune, the bird songs he
hears as he sits for hours outside his farm home.
That same sequence from replication to improvisation to
creation occurs in other savants whether musicians or
artists. The artists begin their ‘career’ with striking replicas
of what they have seen and stored, usually requiring no
model or constant reference piece. Then some improvisa-
tion begins to appear—a telephone pole deleted here, or a
new tree there—slightly different from the original. Then
comes creation of entirely new pieces, maybe now free-
form or in an entirely different art style.
So the savant can be creative. Some savants prefer to
stay with replication, but many have gone beyond literal
copying, as stunning as that can be, to improvisation and
then creation of something entirely new.
These clinical impressions regarding creativity in the
savant have been bolstered by several formal research
projects. A 1987 study by Hermelin, O’Connor and Lee
looked at musical inventiveness in five musical savants
compared to six non-savant children who had musical
training over a period of 2 years but who had not been
exposed to compositional or improvisational instruction.
Five tasks were used to grade for ‘‘musical inventiveness’’.
On those tests the savant group was superior to the control
group. Similarly, on tests of musical competence—timing,
balance and complexity—the savants (with a mean IQ of
59) were also superior to the control group.
Hermelin and her co-workers indicated this study was
consistent with earlier findings—that a series of separate
intelligences, of which music is but one, exist in each
person rather than a single, consistent intelligence that
permeates all the skills and abilities of each person. With
respect to music, they concluded that savants were able to
show some creativity and improvisation in addition to
mimicry.
Hermelin et al. (1989) conducted a study of improvi-
sations by Leslie Lemke compared to a professional, non-
savant musician after each had heard the same musical
pieces, one lyric (Grieg) and one a-tonal (Bartok). Leslie’s
improvisations were described as ‘‘virtuoso embellish-
ments with a considerable degree of musical inventiveness
and pianistic virtuosity.’’ That study concluded that ‘‘both
subjects’ attempts at improvisation show a high degree of
generative musical ability, and what distinguishes them
from each other is not so much a differential degree of
musicianship but rather their own, different musical pref-
erences as well as their respective personality characteris-
tics.’’ In improvisational style on the Bartok, a-tonal piece,
both musicians resembled each other as well.
In summary, savants can be creative. Most savants travel
along a route of first replication, then improvisation, and
finally creation. As we learn more about the brain from the
study of savants, we may also learn much more about talent
and creativity itself through the unique window into the
brain and special skills savant syndrome provides.
The ‘‘Nadia’’ Effect and the ‘‘Dreaded Trade-Off’’
In 1977 Dr. Lorna Selfe described the case of Nadia, a
prolific childhood artist, whose special abilities disap-
peared after she was sent away to school to increase lan-
guage acquisition, socialization abilities and daily living
skills. With the publication of Dr. Selfe’s 2011 book—
Nadia Revisited—we now have the benefit of long term
follow-up on Nadia. Selfe describes the loss of skills this
way: ‘‘In the years following my first study, and throughout
her school days, Nadia was given intensive help with lan-
guage development and her ability to communicate
improved with the production of two/three word sentences.
She also started to draw like an infant so that, for a period,
two styles coexisted and sometimes on the same piece of
paper. Gradually and inexorably she lost the ability to draw
realistically. Unlike some savant artists such as Stephen
Wiltshire, who has maintained the strength of his drawing
ability, Nadia’s ability appeared to peter out. She is now
middle-aged and lives in a specialist care home but for
many years she has simply refused to draw.’’ But, in spite
of the loss of art skills, Selfe points out, importantly, that
J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 567
123
even though now Nadia is not interested in art and is totally
dependent on others in that supervised setting there is also
the ‘‘optimistic story of the love and care of the family that
raised her and of the people who now care for Nadia. She is
in the safe and competent hands of dedicated staff who
devote themselves to the care of people who are unable to
look after themselves.’’
We don’t seem to know exactly what did happen with
Nadia and why those special skills disappeared. But what I
do know is that in the many, many savants with whom I
have worked, or know about, such a ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’,
or loss of skills, does not occur as the savant gets older or
when exposed to more formal education and training. To
the contrary, in my experience vigorously ‘‘training the
talent’’, whatever that special skill is, leads, in and of itself,
to increased language, social and daily living skills without
any ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’ of special skills. So Nadia’s
experience is the exception, not the rule.
Putting aside the fear of a ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’ is
important because parents, teachers or therapists are
sometimes reluctant to venture forth with more formal
education or training efforts lest the ‘‘Nadia’’ effect occur.
The good news is that such a fear is, in my experience,
unfounded and should not prevent presenting the savant
with more formal education and training within his or her
area of specialty, as well as in a more general educational
sense. That being the case, parents and teachers can con-
tinue not only to applaud and reinforce the special skills as
they surface, but can confidently add teaching and training
in a more formal sense as well without fear of loss of talent,
ingenuity or enthusiasm on the part of the savant.
Savant Syndrome is Always Associated with Low IQ
Perhaps stemming from Down’s original description
regarding low IQ and the presence of savant skills, a
misconception continues that low IQ is a necessary
accompaniment of savant syndrome. That in fact is not the
case. While it is true that most savants have measured IQ’s
between 50 and 70, in some instances IQ can be as high as
125, or even higher. Thus an IQ level above 70 does not
‘disqualify’ someone from having savant syndrome.
One reason that many savants, or many autistic persons,
have IQ scores below 70 is that IQ measurement depends
so heavily on verbal scales, and many autistic individuals,
including those with savant syndrome, have language
(verbal) deficits as an intrinsic part of the underlying
disorder.
A second reason for low IQ scores among savants is the
fact that IQ tests measure only one facet of ‘‘intelligence’’,
something termed ‘‘IQ’’. Savants tend to do poorly on that
particular measure of ‘‘intelligence’’. But savants point out
forcefully that there are multiple forms of ‘‘intelligence’’
and IQ measures only one such ‘‘intelligence’’. IQ tests do
measure something defined as ‘‘IQ’’. But IQ tests fail to
measure some of the other forms of ‘‘intelligence’’ that
savants possess in greater or lesser measure as well. Some
of the savants are profoundly disabled in capacities as
measured by IQ, but yet they are astoundingly ‘intelligent’
within their ‘‘island of genius’’.
There is much debate among psychologists regarding
single v. multiple intelligence theories. But savant syn-
drome, with sometimes extraordinary ability co-existing
with profound disability in the same individual argues
forcefully for the concept of multiple intelligence. And the
fact of multiple intelligences has profound implications not
just for better understanding and approaching savant syn-
drome, but also for implementing more effective, individ-
ualized and targeted education efforts for all segments of
the population.
Thirdly, in all developmental disabilities, and savant
syndrome, one has to make a distinction between ‘‘actual
retardation’’, as classified by IQ scores, from ‘‘functional
retardation’’—instances in which persons with presumably
normal or high IQ (if it could be accurately measured)
function at levels more consistent with sub-normal IQ. In
such instances, either the language and verbal deficits, or
behavioral traits and symptoms, prevent accurate mea-
surement of ‘‘IQ’’. These individuals, whether savants or
not, ‘‘function’’ as if ‘‘retarded’’, but their abilities in cer-
tain other areas of function belie a below average IQ score.
That is termed ‘‘functional retardation.’’
Leslie Lemke provides an example of how misleading
IQ levels can be as a single measure of intelligence. Leslie
has a measured IQ of 58 on the WAIS-R test, based solely
on verbal scores; performance tests were not done because
such testing relies heavily on vision, and Leslie is blind.
Other tests were carried out as well including the 4th edi-
tion of the Stanford-Binet; the Tactual Performance Test;
the American Association for Mental Deficiency Adaptive
Behavioral Scale; and the Animal List Selective Remind-
ing Test. By looking at the scores on these tests as a whole,
the neuropsychologist concluded Leslie was functioning in
the moderately retarded range of intelligence, defined as an
IQ level between 35 and 55.
Yet a videotape of one of Leslie’s concerts challenges
the accuracy of such a low level of intelligence figures. At
this particular concert Leslie was asked to play a piece he
had never heard before with the other pianist, rather than
waiting for the piece to conclude and then play it back after
hearing it as he usually does. The other pianist began
playing. Leslie waited about 3 seconds and then did indeed
play the piece with the other pianist, separated only by
those 3 seconds. In that three second delay Leslie was
taking in what he heard, processing it, and simultaneously
outputting the music as he played along with the other
568 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571
123
pianist. Leslie was parallel processing, just as some very
intelligent, but rare, interpreters are able to translate what a
speaker is saying into another language simultaneously,
rather than having the speaker pause from time to time
while the interpreter ‘catches up’.
Leslie was parallel processing. That would not be pos-
sible if the IQ level of 35–55 was an accurate barometer of
his over-all intelligence. He exceeds that level by far with
the parallel processing of music which signals that more
than a single ‘intelligence’ was at work during that com-
plex performance.
In summary, measured IQ levels in savants can range
from sub-normal to exceptional and low IQ is not a pre-
requisite to being classified as a ‘savant’. While many
savants have measured IQ levels below 70, some have
measured IQ’s above normal which can range as high as
125 or above. In assessing IQ scores, one has to differen-
tiate’actual’ retardation from ‘functional’ retardation
All ‘‘Geniuses’’ and ‘‘Prodigies’’, Past and Present are
Really ‘‘Aspies’’
With increased interest in autism and Asperger’s, and
especially with the visibility given to the extraordinary skills
seen in savant syndrome, it seems popular these days to
apply the diagnosis of Asperger’s disorder particularly to
anyone considered to be a ‘genius’ or ‘prodigy’ past or
present. Names such as Einstein, Rembrandt, Mozart,
Jefferson and many others are bandied about in such dis-
cussions. It is difficult enough to make accurate diagnoses of
autism or Asperger’s disorder in real life, with face-to-face
interviews and comprehensive testing, let alone trying to
apply post-mortem diagnoses, sight unseen. Retrospective
medical diagnoses are always problematical and suspect.
And then there are present-day prodigies and geniuses.
Some, outrageously bright, but not autistic children, have
composed multiple symphonies by age seven, or have
mastered instruments, sometimes multiple instruments, by
age three. Others show astonishing artistic, mathematical,
prose or poetry skills well beyond their years. If children,
we call them prodigies. They are neither autistic, nor
Aspergers. If adults, we call them geniuses. They also are
neither Asperger’s nor autistic. Prodigies and geniuses
have special, spectacular abilities in absence of any
underlying disability. Typically, rather than there being
simply one ‘‘island’’of genius as is often the case with
savants, whatever the skills of the prodigy or genius, they
are associated with a high measured IQ in all areas of
functioning.
In short, not every gifted child, nor every ‘absent
minded professor’, has Asperger’s disorder. Instead,
‘‘prodigy’’ and ‘‘genius’’ do exist as independent conditions
separate from any underlying disability or disorder The
temptation to classify all prodigies and geniuses as having
autism or Asperger’s seems to be part of the disease de jour
phenomenon quite rampant these days and needs to be
resisted in favor of careful analysis lest continued ‘diag-
nosis creep’ deletes all meaningful classification, all the
disorders lose their specificity, and the ‘spectrum’ engulfs
us all.
The beginning of wisdom is to call things by their right
name. Asperger’s, autism, and savant syndrome surely do
exist. But so do the categories of ‘‘normal’’, ‘‘gifted’’,
‘‘prodigy’’ and ‘‘genius’’. The important thing is to know
the difference lest every parent of a gifted child for
example, whether mildly gifted or profoundly gifted, fear
their child is autistic.
‘‘Outgrowing’’ Autism: Separating ‘Autistic-Like’
Traits from Autistic Disorder in Children Who Read
Early, Speak Late, or are Blind
I get many ‘‘I’ve got a son or daughter who……..’’ emails
from the savant syndrome web site in which parents describe
various accelerated skills in their children and inquire whe-
ther those might be forms of savant syndrome, and if so, how
should those special skills, and that child, be approached
educationally and otherwise. Among those many inquiries
are children who read early (hyperlexia) or speak late
(Einstein Syndrome). Often children in both those groups are
automatically, and mistakenly, assumed to be autistic when
in fact they only have ‘autistic-like’ behaviors and traits with
very different causes and outcomes than Autistic Disorder.
They tend to ‘outgrow’ their autism (their choice of terms),
which was not autism in the first place.
Based on a number of such cases brought to my atten-
tion separating ‘‘autistic-like’’ behaviors and traits from
‘‘Autistic Disorder’’ in children who read early, or speak
late, or who are blind, is a critical differential diagnosis
with vast causal, treatment and outcome ramifications.
Hyperlexia I, II and III
Some neurotypical children simply read early. They may
be reading, instead of the teacher, to their nursery school
class, or reading at a 7th grade level at age 3 for example.
There are no associated autistic or autistic-like traits or
behaviors. They are entirely ‘normal’ (neurotypical) chil-
dren. Eventually their classmates catch up with reading
skills but in the meantime the advanced, precocious read-
ing ability at such an early age draws considerable atten-
tion. I describe several such examples on the ‘‘Hyperlexia’’
posting on the savant syndrome web site at www.
savantsyndrome.com I refer to this type of early reading
ability as Hyperlexia I.
J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 569
123
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
Hyperlexia II is when early reading ability presents as a
‘splinter skill’ as part of an Autistic Spectrum Disorder.
These children read voraciously along with astonishing
memory for what they read. They often have other mem-
orization abilities sometimes linked with fascination with
numbers or calendar calculating skills. These children
show other characteristic language, social and behavioral
symptoms seen in autistic spectrum disorder, including
traits such as withdrawal, poor eye contact, lack of interest
in seeking or giving affection, insistence on sameness, and
obsessive compulsive behavior, for example. They usually
carry a formal diagnosis of Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s
Disorder, or pervasive developmental disorder (PPD/NOS)
with intense fascination with words and numbers present-
ing as a ‘splinter skill’.
Hyperlexia III is a less frequently recognized form of
early reading ability. It is not an autistic spectrum disorder
even though there are some ‘‘autistic-like’’ traits and
behaviors that gradually fade as the child gets older. Some
times this is referred to as ‘‘outgrowing autism’’. These
children read early and have striking memorization abilities
sometimes coupled with precocious abilities in other areas
as well. They may show unusual sensory sensitivity, ech-
olalia, pronoun reversals, intense need for sameness, spe-
cific fears or phobias, have lining/stacking rituals and
demonstrate strong visual and auditory memory. Unlike
children with ASD, however, they are often very outgoing
and affectionate with family, even though reserved and
distant with peers and would be playmates. They do make
eye contact and can be very interactive with persons close
to them, especially adults. These children present as being
very bright, inquisitive and precocious overall. Indeed
these ‘autistic-like’ traits and behaviors do fade as the child
gets older, but in the meantime parents are exposed to
unnecessary fear and dread because the diagnosis of
‘‘autism’’ has been prematurely and inappropriately applied
without ‘‘hyperlexia III’’ being considered in the differen-
tial diagnosis. Space precludes an extensive discussion of
Hyperlexia III, but the savant syndrome website as www.
savantsyndrome.com provides a number of example of
such cases with respect to characteristics and outcome.
Einstein Syndrome: Children Who Speak Late
In his 1997 book Late Talking Children Thomas Sowell
pointed out how often ‘‘autistic-like’’ symptoms, as
opposed to Autistic Disorder itself, appeared in children
with delayed speech based on parental reports in a group of
46 such children. In a follow-up book 4 years later—The
Einstein Syndrome:Bright Children Who Talk Late—
Sowell expanded that group to 239 late talking children
who were exceptionally late in beginning to speak but were
also exceptionally bright. (Sowell 2001). His book is
replete with examples. As with Hyperlexia III, Sowell
found in his correspondence with parents that many of the
children with delayed speech had been given a diagnosis of
ASD along the way but that the ‘‘autistic-like’’ symptoms
in these children were transient, and like with the Hyper-
lexia III children, those traits and behaviors faded over
time. He recommended careful professional evaluation for
children who speak late by clinicians familiar with the
various parameters and conditions involved with such
children, sparing parents unnecessary worry, concern
and pessimism that always accompanies a diagnosis of
‘‘autism’’. This condition is also discussed in more detail
on the savant syndrome website.
Blindisms
Teachers and parents of visually impaired children often
refer to what are called ‘‘blindisms’’ in such children. Ek
and co-workers point out that ‘‘blindisms’’—stereotypical
movements, language problems and certain other behav-
iors—are common in children with congenital or other
types of blindness. (Ek et al. 1998). Hobson described the
similarities in development during pre-school age
(3–4 years) between blind children and those with autism
(Hobson 1993). In both groups impairments in symbolic
play, confusion in the use of language and stereotypes were
frequent. Many of the autistic features in the young, blind
child without cerebral damage disappeared with age. As the
child acquired a better understanding of the surrounding
world, and with the development of language, a basis for
sharing experiences and feelings with other people devel-
oped. Hobson noted ‘‘blindness seems to delay rather than
prevent development in these respects.’’ In 2010 Hobson
and Lee did an 8 year follow-up study on nine congenitally
blind and seven sighted children who met formal diag-
nostic criteria for autism. Follow-up of the nine congeni-
tally blind children with ‘autism’ revealed that, in
adolescence, only one such child satisfied the criteria for
that disorder. In contrast, all of the seven sighted children
still did meet the Autistic Disorder criteria. For the group
with what turned out to be autistic-like symptoms, in the
title of his report Hobson uses the interesting term
‘‘reversible autism’’.
Autism, autistic-like symptoms and blindisms can be
confused with each other in visually impaired children. But
just as with children who read early or speak late, differ-
entiation between Autistic Disorder and ‘‘autistic-like’’
symptoms is critical with these children if parents are to be
spared unnecessary distress from an autism diagnosis
improperly applied and, equally important, if the right
treatment is to be applied to the right patient.
570 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571
123
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
http://www.savantsyndrome.com
Summary
With all the emphasis by some on the autism ‘epidemic’
and with it the need for early identification and a prolif-
eration of programs, it is important to remember that not
every child who reads at 18 months, draws at 2 years,
hums back all the melodies he or she hears, or likes to line
up railroad cars, resists certain foods, insists on routine,
memorizes license plates and birthdays, has certain fear
and phobias or is very late in speaking is on the autistic
spectrum. If one looks up ‘‘hyperlexia’’ on the internet,
though, most often the site links hyperlexia to autism. That
simply is not so in all cases as pointed out above. That
same link to autism is likewise often made for children who
speak late, or are blind.
Again, not so in all cases. While early identification of
autism in affected children is important, those efforts need
to be balanced with sensible caution lest parents be
unnecessarily frightened and overwhelmed by premature,
and erroneous, diagnoses. Except in truly ‘classic’ cases,
often some time of watchful observation needs to elapse
until the ‘natural history of the disorder’ reveals the real
diagnosis. Such ‘watchful observation’, diagnostic caution
and separation of ‘autistic-like’ behaviors from Autistic
Disorder can provide example of what some have called
‘‘reversible autism’’.
References
Brink, T. L. (1980). Idiot savant with unusual mechanical
ability: An
organic explanation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137,
250–251.
Dorman, C. (1991) Exceptional calendar calculating ability after
early
left hemispherectomy. Brain and Cognition 15, 26–36 London:
Churchill.
Down, J.L. (1887). On some of the mental affections of
childhood and
youth. London, Churchill.
Ek, U., Fernello, E., Jacobson, L., & Gillberg, C. (1998).
Relation
between blindness due to retinopathy and autistic spectrum
disorders: A population study. Developmental Medicine and
Child Neurology, 40, 297–301.
Hermelin, B., O’Connor, N., & Lee, S. (1987). Musical
inventiveness
of five idiot-savants. Psychological Medicine, 17, 685–694.
Hermelin, B., O’Connor, N., Lee, S., & Treffert, D. A. (1989).
Intelligence level and musical improvisational ability. Psycho-
logical Medicine, 19, 447–457.
Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the development of the mind.
Hove:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hobson, R. P. (2010). Reversible autism in congenitally blind
children? A controlled study Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 51(11), 1235–1241.
Kanner, L.:217-25055-58 (1944) Early infantile Autism Journal
of
Pediatrics 25, 200–217.
Kapur, N. (1996). Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-
behavior research. Brain, 119, 1775–1790.
Miller, B., et al. (1996). Enhanced artistic creativity with
temporal
lobe degeneration. Lancet, 348, 1744–1745.
Miller, B., et al. (1998). Emergence of artistic talent in
frontotemporal
dementia. Neurology, 51, 978–982.
Miller, B., et al. (2000). Functional correlates of musical and
visual
ability in frontotemporal dementia. British Journal of Psychia-
try, 176, 458–463.
Minogue, B. M. (1923). A case of secondary mental deficiency
with
musical talent. Journal of Applied Psychology, 7, 349–357.
Mortiz, K. P. (1783). Gnothi Sauton oder magazin der
erfahrungs-
seelenkunde als ein lesebuch fur gelehrte and ungelehrte.
Berlin,
Germany: Mylius.
Selfe, L. (1977). Nadia: A case of extraordinary drawing ability
in an
Autistic child. London: Academic Press.
Selfe, L. (2011). Nadia revisited: A longitudinal study of an
Autistic
Savant. New York: Psychology Press.
Sowell, T. (1997). Late talking children. New York: Basic
Books.
Sowell, T. (2001). The Einstein syndrome: Bright children who
talk
late. New York: Basic books.
Treffert, D.A. (1988). The idiot savant: A review of the
syndrome.
American Journal of Psychiatry 145563–145572.
Treffert, D.A. (1989). Extraordinary people: Understanding
savant
syndrome. Lincoln, Nebraska: iUniverse.com.
Treffert, D. A. (2006). Dr. Down and ‘‘developmental
disorders’’.
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 36, 965–966.
Treffert, D. A. (2010). Islands of genius: The bountiful mind of
the
Autistic acquired and sudden savant. London: Jessica Kingsley.
J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 571
123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without
permission.
c.10803_2013_Article_1906.pdfSavant Syndrome: Realities,
Myths and MisconceptionsAbstractRealitiesSavant Syndrome
DefinedSavant Syndrome is Not a New Disorder (Nor is
Autism)Not All Savants are Autistic, and Not All Autistic
Persons are SavantsSavant Skills Represent a Spectrum of
AbilitiesThe Acquired Savant: ‘‘Accidental Genius’’The Most
Important Question of All: How do They do It?Some Mysteries
RemainMyths and MisconceptionsSavants are Not ‘Creative’The
‘‘Nadia’’ Effect and the ‘‘Dreaded Trade-Off’’Savant Syndrome
is Always Associated with Low IQAll ‘‘Geniuses’’ and
‘‘Prodigies’’, Past and Present are Really
‘‘Aspies’’‘‘Outgrowing’’ Autism: Separating ‘Autistic-Like’
Traits from Autistic Disorder in Children Who Read Early,
Speak Late, or are BlindHyperlexia I, II and IIIEinstein
Syndrome: Children Who Speak
LateBlindismsSummaryReferences
DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY
REVIEW
The genetic landscape of autism spectrum disorders
RASIM O ROSTI1 | ABDELRAHIM A SADEK2 | KEITH K
VAUX1 | JOSEPH G GLEESON1
1 Department of Neurosciences and Pediatrics, Howard Hughes
Medical Institute, University of California, San Diego, CA,
USA. 2 Pediatric Neurology Unit,
Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, Sohag
University, Sohag, Egypt.
Correspondence to Joseph G Gleeson at University of California
San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive M/C 0665, La Jolla, CA, USA.
E-mail: [email protected]
PUBLICATION DATA
Accepted for publication 22nd July 2013.
Published online 1st October 2013.
ABBREVIATIONS
ASD Autism spectrum disorders
CGH Comparative genomic
hybridization
CNV Copy number variants
Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of heterogeneous
neurodevelopmental disor-
ders that show impaired communication and socialization,
restricted interests, and stereotypi-
cal behavioral patterns. Recent advances in molecular medicine
and high throughput
screenings, such as array comparative genomic hybridization
(CGH) and exome and whole
genome sequencing, have revealed both novel insights and new
questions about the nature
of this spectrum of disorders. What has emerged is a better
understanding about the genetic
architecture of various genetic subtypes of ASD and
correlations of genetic mutations with
specific autism subtypes. Based on this new information, we
outline a strategy for advancing
diagnosis, prognosis, and counseling for patients and families.
Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of com-
plex neurodevelopmental disabilities that affect social inter-
action and communication skills. The prevalence of ASDs
appears to be constantly and gradually increasing, but it is
not clear if this is because of clarification of diagnostic cri-
teria or an actual increase in the number of cases. Most
recent estimates find that the median of prevalence esti-
mates of ASD is 62 out of 10 000.1 Those with molecu-
larly defined causes make up roughly 20% of the cases, but
the heritability has been estimated to be 90%, suggesting
as yet undiscovered causes. However, there are new reports
suggesting that the previous estimate of heritability was
too high and may need to be adjusted downwards.2
The diagnostic criteria have evolved with increasing clin-
ical and molecular understanding of this umbrella term.
This aspect makes the diagnosis more challenging as the
clinical spectrum is highly variable and the etiological sub-
grouping tends to change with the ever-growing molecular
data now fed by high throughput techniques such as array
comparative genomic hybridization (CGH), whole exome
and whole genome sequencing. The judgment of the
physician, critical in achieving accurate prognosis and
genetic counseling, requires a systematic approach. By
incorporating these techniques along with careful clinical
and neuropsychological assessment, a more accurate
diagnosis of an ASD disorder can be achieved.
Distinguishing between essential autism and complex
(syndromic) autism might be considered the starting point
of this systematic approach.3 Of all individuals meeting cri-
teria for autism, essential autism makes up approximately
75% of the cases. Although essentially a diagnosis of exclu-
sion, the main characteristics are the lack of dysmorphic
features, higher male to female ratio (6:1), higher sibling
recurrence risk (up to 35%), and positive family history
(up to 20%). Syndromic autism, on the other hand, is
characterized by accompanying recognizable patterns of
dysmorphology, a reduced male to female ratio (3.5:1),
lower recurrence risk (4%–6%), and family history to a
lesser extent (up to 9%).1
Clinical recognition of well-known phenotypes leading
to a targeted molecular testing approach can strengthen
the hand of the clinician in answering additional questions
about the recurrence risk and prognosis according to the
molecular basis identified by targeted testing. However, for
most forms of essential autism, there is no familiar pheno-
type that points to one particular genetic cause or another.
In the absence of a specific genetic test option after clinical
evaluation, array CGH testing is recommended as it has
been reported to provide the highest diagnostic yield in
cases with ASD – ranging from 10%4 to 18%.5 Once such
a genetic alteration is identified in an affected individual,
more specific information about diagnosis, prognosis,
recurrence risks, and possible targeted therapy options can
be conveyed to the family.
ASD phenotypes caused by genetic alterations can be
divided into three subgroups: cytogenetic alterations that
can be detected by conventional karyotyping (not the
primary focus of this paper). These comprise classic chro-
mosomal syndromes such as Turner and Down syndrome,
and large segmental cytogenetic alterations that have been
associated with autism.6 Other subgroups are single gene
disorders (detected in <5%) and copy number variants
(detected in 10%–35% of the cases1).
NON-SYNDROMIC AND POSSIBLE TREATABLE
FORMS OF ASD: SINGLE GENE DISORDERS
Single gene disorders are mainly composed of metabolic
conditions in which accompanying ASD features are
12 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12278 © 2013 Mac Keith Press
reported. Among these, phenylketonuria is common
enough to be included in newborn screening programs in
most countries. Comorbidity of ASD and phenylketonuria
was consistently described in the literature in up to 6% of
patients.7 The fact that none of the 62 patients with phen-
ylketonuria who were treated with the phenylketonuria diet
early in life met ASD diagnostic criteria, whereas 5.7% (2
out of 35) of individuals with late diagnosis met diagnostic
criteria, underlines the importance of early diagnosis in
successful treatment in such metabolic conditions.8 Condi-
tions such as mitochondrial disorders, adenylsuccinate lyase
and creatine deficiencies may also phenocopy ASD.
Accompanying autistic features in these disorders range
from 0.4% to 80%.1,9,10 Although mitochondrial disorders
can present with autistic features, atypical findings of hypo-
tonia, fatigue with activity, failure to thrive, intermittent
episodes of regression, especially those after fever and ele-
vated plasma lactate concentrations, make diagnosis of the
condition more straightforward. Non-specific features such
as epilepsy and intellectual disability that accompany
autism in the setting of mitochondrial disorders on the
other hand, can make the diagnosis more challenging.
A recent addition to this group of disorders was made
by Novarino et al.11 who reported mutations in the
BCKDK gene in the affected individuals from three consan-
guineous families who had epilepsy, autistic features, and
intellectual disability. The encoded protein is responsible
for phosphorylation-mediated inactivation of the E1a sub-
unit of branched-chain ketoacid dehydrogenase enzyme.
Patients with BCKDK mutations displayed reductions in
BCKDK messenger RNA and protein, E1a phosphoryla-
tion, and plasma branched-chain amino acids. Bckdk knock-
out mice showed abnormal brain amino acid profiles and
neurobehavioral deficits that respond to dietary supple-
mentation. By supplementing the diet of humans with
branched-chain amino acids, the authors were able to nor-
malize plasma branched-chain amino acids levels, but the
degree to which neurocognitive changes are treatable or
reversible remains to be determined.
ASD ASSOCIATED WITH RECOGNIZABLE PATTERNS
OF MALFORMATIONS CAUSED BY SINGLE GENE
DISORDERS
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx
International Journal  of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx

More Related Content

Similar to International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx

Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010
Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010
Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010Simon Bignell
 
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docx
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docxApplication of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docx
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docxarmitageclaire49
 
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptx
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptxDietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptx
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptxArif Majeed
 
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi Alexander Decker
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity Giuseppe Fattori
 
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...ijtsrd
 
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdf
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdfDelay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdf
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdfsdfghj21
 
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhd
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhdEmotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhd
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhdRosa Vera Garcia
 
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With Autism
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With AutismA Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With Autism
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With AutismLori Moore
 
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...Mari Inkinen
 
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...Arun Varghese
 
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDA
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDAEficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDA
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDAPaula Maciel
 

Similar to International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx (20)

1 LITERATURE
1 LITERATURE1 LITERATURE
1 LITERATURE
 
1 LITERATURE
1 LITERATURE1 LITERATURE
1 LITERATURE
 
Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010
Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010
Topic 9 - Treatment For Autism 2010
 
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docx
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docxApplication of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docx
Application of Applied Behavior Analysis to Mental Health Issu.docx
 
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptx
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptxDietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptx
Dietary paterrns of autistic chidren - Copy.pptx
 
AOTA poster
AOTA posterAOTA poster
AOTA poster
 
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi
Development of an instrument for measuring women’s multi
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
 
E03403019026
E03403019026E03403019026
E03403019026
 
SIRS conference 2018
SIRS conference 2018SIRS conference 2018
SIRS conference 2018
 
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...
“A Study on Mental Health Problems of Adolescent” with Special Reference to C...
 
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdf
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdfDelay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdf
Delay in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children.pdf
 
2013 Up Coming SUNLOWS
2013 Up Coming SUNLOWS2013 Up Coming SUNLOWS
2013 Up Coming SUNLOWS
 
Asd research
Asd researchAsd research
Asd research
 
979 2563-1-pb
979 2563-1-pb979 2563-1-pb
979 2563-1-pb
 
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhd
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhdEmotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhd
Emotional intelligence-as-an-evolutive-factor-on-adult-with-adhd
 
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With Autism
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With AutismA Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With Autism
A Literature Analysis On The Quality Of Life In Adults With Autism
 
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...
A socio-ecological approach to physical activity interventions in childcare, ...
 
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...
Impacts of Mentor’s Strategic Communication on Adjustment Problems among Adol...
 
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDA
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDAEficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDA
Eficácia do metilfenidato e atomoxetina em crianças PHDA
 

More from normanibarber20063

Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docx
Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docxAssist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docx
Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docx
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docxAssistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docx
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docxnormanibarber20063
 
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docx
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docxassingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docx
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docx
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docxAssimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docx
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docx
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docxAssignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docx
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docx
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docxAssignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docx
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docx
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docxAssignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docx
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docx
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docxAssignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docx
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docx
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docxAssignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docx
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docx
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docxAssignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docx
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docx
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docxAssignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docx
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docx
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docxAssignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docx
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docx
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docxAssignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docx
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docx
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docxAssignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docx
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docx
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docxAssignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docx
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docx
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docxAssignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docx
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docx
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docxAssignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docx
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docxAssignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docx
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docxAssignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docx
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docxnormanibarber20063
 
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docxAssignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docxnormanibarber20063
 

More from normanibarber20063 (20)

Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docx
Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docxAssist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docx
Assist with first annotated bibliography.  Assist with f.docx
 
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docx
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docxAssistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docx
Assistance needed with SQL commandsI need assistance with the quer.docx
 
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docx
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docxassingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docx
assingment Assignment Agenda Comparison Grid and Fact Sheet or .docx
 
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docx
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docxAssimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docx
Assimilate the lessons learned from the dream sequences in Defense o.docx
 
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docx
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docxAssignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docx
Assignmnt-500 words with 2 referencesRecognizing the fa.docx
 
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docx
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docxAssignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docx
Assignmnt-700 words with 3 referencesToday, there is a crisi.docx
 
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docx
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docxAssignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docx
Assignment  For Paper #2, you will pick two poems on a similar th.docx
 
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docx
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docxAssignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docx
Assignment Write an essay comparingcontrasting two thingspeople.docx
 
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docx
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docxAssignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docx
Assignment Travel Journal to Points of Interest from the Early Midd.docx
 
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docx
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docxAssignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docx
Assignment What are the factors that influence the selection of .docx
 
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docx
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docxAssignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docx
Assignment Write a research paper that contains the following.docx
 
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docx
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docxAssignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docx
Assignment Thinking about Managers and Leaders· Identifya man.docx
 
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docx
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docxAssignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docx
Assignment Talk to friends, family, potential beneficiaries abou.docx
 
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docx
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docxAssignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docx
Assignment The objective of assignment is to provide a Power .docx
 
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docx
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docxAssignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docx
Assignment During the on-ground, residency portion of Skill.docx
 
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docx
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docxAssignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docx
Assignment PurposeThe first part of this assignment will assist.docx
 
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docx
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docxAssignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docx
Assignment PowerPoint Based on what you have learned so .docx
 
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docxAssignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following quest.docx
 
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docx
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docxAssignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docx
Assignment NameUnit 2 Discussion BoardDeliverable Length150-.docx
 
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docxAssignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docx
Assignment In essay format, please answer the following questions.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinRaunakKeshri1
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 

International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology.docx

  • 1. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology www.elsevier.es/ijchp International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology (2013) 13, 91−100 1697-2600/$ - see front matter © 2012 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual. Published by Elsevier España, S.L. All rights reserved. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology Publicación cuatrimestral / Four-monthly publication ISSN 1697-2600 Volumen 13, Número 2 Mayo - 2013 Volume 13, Number 2 May - 2013 Director / Editor: Juan Carlos Sierra Directores Asociados / Associate Editors: Stephen N. Haynes Michael W. Eysenck
  • 2. Gualberto Buela-Casal ORIGINAL ARTICLE Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children with autism undergoing intensive behavioral intervention Javier Virues-Ortega*,a, Víctor Rodríguezb, C.T. Yua aUniversity of Manitoba and St. Amant Research Centre, Canada bFundación Planeta Imaginario, Spain Received September 4, 2012; accepted March 18, 2013 *Corresponding author at: University of Manitoba, Psychology Department, P518 Duff Roblin Bldg., 190 Dysart Road, MB R3T Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada. E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Virues-Ortega). Abstract Outcome prediction is an important component of treatment planning and prognosis. However, reliable predictors of intensive behavioral intervention (IBI) have not been clearly established. IBI is an evidence-based approach to the systematic teaching of academic, social, verbal, and daily living skills to individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Incorporating longitudinal analysis to IBI outcome studies may help to identify outcome predictors of clinical value. Twenty-four children with autism underwent on average two years of IBI and completed language, daily living skills, cognitive, and motor assessments (Early Learning Accomplishment
  • 3. Profile and the Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3rd edition) every six months. We used multilevel analysis to identify potential longitudinal predictors including gender, age, intervention intensity, intervention duration, total intervention time, and pre-intervention functioning. Results indicated that total intervention time, pre- intervention functioning, and age caused the greatest increase in goodness-of-fit of the longitudinal multilevel models. Longitudinal analysis is a promising analytical strategy to identify reliable predictors of the clinical outcome of IBI. © 2012 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual. Published by Elsevier España, S.L. All rights reserved. KEYWORDS Autism; Predictors; Applied behavior analysis; Quasi-experiment (interrupted time-series with one group) Resumen La predicción de resultados de tratamiento es un componente importante de la planificación clínica. No obstante, no se han hallado predictores fiables de los efectos de la in- tervención conductual intensiva en personas con trastorno del espectro autista. La incorpora- ción de análisis longitudinales a la investigación sobre resultados de tratamiento en este área puede contribuir a la identificación de predictores con valor
  • 4. clínico. En el presente estudio se evaluaron las habilidades verbales, cognitivas y de la vida diaria (Early Learning Accomplish- ment Profile y Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3ª ed.) de 24 niños con trastorno del espectro autista en un programa de intervención conductual intensiva. Las evaluaciones se rea- lizaron cada seis meses y durante un periodo medio de intervención de dos años. Mediante PALABRAS CLAVE Autismo; predictor Análisis aplicado de la conducta; Cuasi-experimento (serie temporal interrumpida con un grupo) 92 J. Virues-Ortega et al. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a pervasive developmental disorder that affects 1 to 2.5% of children (Baio, 2012). A number of comprehensive psychosocial interventions for people with ASD have been developed for which preliminary evidence exists. These include the Early Start Denver model (ESDM, Dawson et al., 2010), the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH, Welterlin, Turner-Brown, Harris, Mesibov, & Delmolino, 2012), and intensive behavioral intervention based on the UCLA Young Autism Project model and applied behavior analysis (IBI, Lovaas, 1987). Although there is no single approach to treatment for all individuals with ASD,
  • 5. IBI based on applied behavior analysis is among the few approaches to treatment that have been tested extensively using clinical trial methodology (Rogers & Vismara, 2008; Virués-Ortega, 2010; Wetherby & Woods, 2006). Applied behavior analysis is devoted to the experimental study of socially significant behavior as a function of environmental and social variables, and is the branch of experimental psychology that supports the conceptual framework of IBI (Luiselli, Russo, Christian, & Wilczynski, 2008). IBI is a comprehensive and evidence-based approach to the systematic teaching of behavioral, verbal, cognitive, and social repertoires to individuals diagnosed with ASD (Howlin, Magiati, & Charman, 2009). Treatment typically involves over 20 weekly hours of one-to-one teaching incorporating multiple learning trails and specific programs for targeted behavioral goals. Teachers program hundreds of learning trials per day featuring discrimination training, prompting, generalization, and other reinforcement-based procedures known to facilitate the acquisition of new skills in individuals with and without disabilities (Miltenberger, 2011). The IBI curriculum integrates complex sequences of programs from basic attending or vocalizing skills, up to complex verbal, social, and problem-solving skills (Lovaas, 2002). Over 20 independent trials have been conducted which jointly suggest that IBI has moderate to large effects on daily living skills, cognitive functioning, language, and social behavior (Foxx, 2008; Remington et al., 2007; Virués- Ortega, 2010). The field of IBI has shown a considerable growth as suggested by the increasing number of service providers and certified professionals (Shook & Johnston, 2011). Parents of children undergoing IBI and other evidence-
  • 6. based interventions frequently want to know whether their child will be able to attend school without special support, what areas of behavioral functioning - whether motor, social or cognitive - are likely to improve as a consequence of treatment, and what intervention intensity and duration may be optimal for their child. Until recently, outcome research had been of little assistance to respond to these and other questions pertaining to the longitudinal progression of children undergoing treatment. While the evidence available strongly suggests that some individuals benefit significantly from IBI and other approaches to treatment, participant and intervention characteristics associated with greater intervention effects are not well understood. The wider literature of treatment outcomes in ASD has examined a range of mediating and moderating factors that could, potentially, be established as clinically valuable predictors. These include pre- intervention IQ, treatment duration and intensity, family characteristics, age at intervention onset, social initiation skills, and structural dismorphologies of the central nervous system. The scant literature available on these factors have been reviewed by Rogers and Vismara (2008) who concluded that “The current intervention research focus on main effects models provides little information about who does well in which treatments and why” (pp. 28-29). Age, pre-intervention functioning, and intervention intensity have been examined in the narrower literature of IBI outcome predictors. Studies that have examined the role of age at the onset of IBI have shown that the earlier the intervention, the greater the intervention effect. For instance, Granpeesheh, Dixon, Tarbox, Kaplan, and Wilke (2009) found that children below seven years at treatment onset mastered more behavioral objectives every month
  • 7. than children who started IBI intervention above that age. The studies that have examined pre-intervention functioning as a predictor of treatment outcome have not always been consistent in their findings. Perry et al. (2008) examined progress of children with ASD that received IBI services by comparing standardized assessments at the beginning and end of the service. Children were classified as having either higher, intermediate, or lower functioning at intake based on their Vineland Adaptive Behavior Composite score. The higher functioning group made substantial gains (∼20 IQ increments) relative to the other two groups. By contrast, Ben-Itzchak, Lahat, Burgin, and Zachor (2008) reported that pre-intervention IQ (normal, borderline, low) did not predict the IQ gains after a year of IBI in a group of 81 young children with ASD and developmental disabilities. More evidence has been accrued on the effects of intervention intensity. However, findings remain inconsistent. Taking IQ as a prototypical outcome (Table 1), Makrygianni and Reed (2010) in a correlational study did not find any effects of intensity – similar results were found by Sheinkopf and Siegel (1998). Virués-Ortega (2010) análisis multinivel se examinaron posibles predictores longitudinales incluyendo sexo, edad, intensidad y duración de la intervención, tiempo total de intervención y nivel de funcionamien- to previo a la intervención. Los resultados indicaron que el tiempo total de intervención, el funcionamiento previo y la edad causaban los mayores incrementos en bondad de ajuste de los modelos longitudinales. El análisis longitudinal es una estrategia analítica prometedora en la identificación de predictores fiables de la efectividad de la
  • 8. intervención conductual intensiva. © 2012 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual. Publicado por Elsevier España, S.L. Todos los derechos reservados. Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children 93 reported no effects of intensity on IQ in a pooled analysis of 19 experimental IBI studies. Finally, Reed, Osborne, and Corness (2007) established a moderate effect of intensity in a small trial on IBI using the Psychoeducational Profile as outcome. In summary, treatment intensity has not been established as a consistent predictor of IBI intervention effects. Longitudinal modeling of intervention outcomes may help to establish intervention predictors more firmly than traditional pre-post assessments. Longitudinal analyses are able to fit the mathematical functions followed by outcome trajectories of individual clients over a period of time. By doing so longitudinal analysis maximizes the statistical power of regression models aiming at meaningful outcome predictors. For instance, if IBI effects were to follow a non-linear progression, rather than a linear trajectory, it may be possible to establish the role of a particular predictor more accurately through longitudinal multi-level analyses suited to specific non-linear mathematical functions (Singer & Willett, 2003). Furthermore, predictors identified based on time-series spanning the treatment duration, as opposed to pre-post assessments, may strengthen the clinical utility of the predictor. For example, pre-intervention functioning could be a strong predictor of treatment outcomes during the
  • 9. first year of treatment, but not during the second. IBI operates through a package of systematic teaching strategies which are expected to provide the individual with an increasing set of cognitive and behavioral resources that will in turn offset, to various extents, the behavioral excesses and deficits that are characteristic of ASD and other developmental disabilities. Being a training-based and goal-directed approach to intervention, IBI may lead to some degree of behavioral gains for as long as the intervention is in place. Longitudinal analysis of IBI may help to identify distinct treatment gain itineraries across subjects and tie those to specific predictors. For instance, it may be possible that individuals starting at a higher pre- intervention level of functioning benefit more from IBI but reach an asymptote (ceiling) sooner than individuals that start at a lower level of functioning. The longitudinal predictors of IBI effects shall be greatly informative, albeit, they have been rarely explored in the literature. There are several longitudinal analyses that feature patterns of change in individuals with ASD (Dietz, Swinkels, Buitelaar, van Daalen & van Engeland, 2007; Jonsdottir et al., 2007; Magiati, Moss, Charman, & Howlin, 2011). Nonetheless, these analyses are constrained by the number of longitudinal assessments (three or less); the number of treatment outcomes (e.g., Dietz et al. only reported IQ); and the data analysis strategy (e.g., no multilevel analyses). This article describes growth patterns of motor, cognitive, verbal, daily-living, and social skills in a sample of children with ASD admitted into a home-based IBI program managed by trained behavior analysts and delivering 20 to 40 weekly hours of intervention. We used the children’s performance in standardized assessments conducted periodically to longitudinally create curves charting the rates and
  • 10. asymptotes of various behavioral repertoires. Subsequent analyses were conducted to test the impact of several personal and intervention-related predictors on the longitudinal growth of IBI outcomes. The present analysis may help to enhance the prognostic information available to families and clinicians by determining the extent to which specific client- and treatment-related variables more closely predict treatment outcome over the duration of the intervention. Method Participants Twenty-four children diagnosed with ASD (Age: Mean = 50.05 months, SD = 28.3; Gender: 21 boys and 3 girls) admitted to the IBI program of Fundación Planeta Imaginario (Barcelona, Spain) participated in the study. An a priori power analysis indicated that a total sample size of 15 was required to detect large effects (Cohen effect size = 1). Therefore, our sample would suffice to identify moderate to large effect sizes. A priori power analysis assumptions were based on the pooled effect size of 20 trials on IBI using IQ reported by Virués-Ortega (2010) (Pooled effect size = 1.19). Participants were recruited consecutively and were not excluded based on their age or pre-intervention functioning at the time of referral. All participants received a diagnosis of ASD from an external medical consultant based on the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition text revised. Diagnosis was supported by standardized assessments of autism including either the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- Generic (ADOS-G) (Le Couteur, Haden, Hammal, & McConachie, 2008). Further personal characteristics are
  • 11. presented in Table 2. Table 1 Effect of treatment intensity on IQ in intensive behavioral intervention outcome studies. Study Sample Intensity range Analysis Effect sizea (h/week) size Makrygianni & Reed (2010) 86 15-30 Correlational (Pearson r) .22 Sheinkopf & Siegel (1998) 11 21-32 Correlational (Pearson r) −.06 Virués-Ortega (2010) 340 12-45 Meta-regression .01 Note. Effects reported as Cohen d effect sizes. a Sample size of the intervention group. *All effect sizes were non-significant, p > .05. 94 J. Virues-Ortega et al. Instruments Fine and gross motor, cognitive, language, self-care and social skills were assessed by means of the Early Learning Accomplishment Profile (E-LAP; Glover, Priminger, & Sanford, 1988; Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin, 2001) and the Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic, 3rd edition, (LAP-D; Hardin, Peisner-Feinberg, & Weeks, 2005). The E-LAP and LAP-D scores are developmental age values expressed in months. The score range is 0 to 36 for the E-LAP and 36 to 72 for the LAP-D. If a participant achieved the upper limit of the score range of E-LAP, the assessment would be repeated with the LAP-D, which would then continue to be used as the means of standardized assessment
  • 12. every 6-month period until treatment was discontinued. In order to control for potential ceiling effects in our data, if a participant reached the LAP-D ceiling, assessment could be repeated one additional time to inform maintenance (provided that the individual would continue to receive services through the program for the next six-month period). Both the E-LAP and the LAP-D have a high level of inter-rater reliability, internal consistency, and convergent validity with IQ (Fleming, 2000; Hardin et al., 2005; Long, Blackman, Farrell, Smolkin, & Conaway, 2005; Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin, 2001). The test-retest reliability of both instruments is reportedly excellent, ranging between .93 and .99 (Peisner-Feinberg & Hardin, 2001, Hardin et al., 2005). Practice effects were unlikely, as exposure to materials and tasks during the assessment was minimal (few trials); and prompting, reinforcement, and correction strategies were not present during the assessment. The Spanish version of the E-LAP and the LAP-D materials were used in the present study. The LAP-D was validated in a representative sample of Spanish-speaking children (Hardin et al., 2005). No Spanish validation of the E-LAP is currently available. Nonetheless, test scoring is performance-based - there are no standard scores. Both instruments have been used frequently as standardized assessments in intervention studies with individuals with ASD (e.g., Ganz, Simpson & Corbin- Newsome, 2008). Moreover, the construct validity of E-LAP and LAP-D is supported by items screening all diagnostic areas of ASD (e.g., “initiates on play activities,” “responds correctly when asked to show a toy,” “inflexible and rigid in behavior”), items informing non-pathognomonic clinical
  • 13. features of autism (e.g., motor functioning), and items covering developmentally relevant skills (e.g., matching skills). In summary, the E-LAP and LAP-D were considered adequate for the present analysis due to their likely resilience to practice effects; excellent stability; excellent convergent validity with intellectual assessment measures; and relevance to the clinical, adaptive, and behavioral features of ASD. Procedure Participants were admitted consecutively to an IBI program within the period May 2006 through January 2011. This program was an official international replication site of the UCLA Young Autism Project model and affiliated with the Lovaas Institute (2011). At the onset of intervention, participants received an average of 31.87 weekly hours (SD = 10.11, range 15 -47.30) of home-based systematic teaching following the UCLA young autism model of service delivery and curriculum (Lovaas, 2002). Average treatment duration was 21.87 months (SD = 14.38, range 5.33-58.57). In keeping with all IBI bonafide programs, in addition to the hours of formal intervention, incidental teaching and practice goals were operating during most waking hours (parents and caregivers acted as active co-therapists). One-to-one teaching was delivered by trained tutors that were supervised on a weekly basis by licensed psychologists Table 2 Characteristics of the study sample. Pre-test Post-test (N=24) (N= 24) Age in months, M±SD 51.91±27.31 69.46±27.26 Gender (male:female) 23:1 Ethnicity (% Caucasian) 100%
  • 14. Social class,a % high 100% IQ,b M±SD 74.50±13.98 91.50±16.86 Skills mastered in selected areas,c M±SD Attending (max. 19) 13.04±4.34 19.16±3.05 Imitation (max. 27) 7.84±8.41 19.92±7.24 Matching (max. 13) 6.02±7.48 13.08±6.08 Basic labeling (max. 13) 12.44±5.33 31.21±19.88 Independent play (max. 15) 3.76±4.76 11.72±6.00 Interaction with peers/adults (max. 25) 2.28±3.82 11.60±9.06 Note. aEstimated by parental education and professional background. bWechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 3rd ed.; Bailey Scales of Infant Development, and Merrill-Palmer Scales of Mental Tests. cNumber of skills mastered by area (Lovaas Institute Midwest, 2010). M = mean; SD = stardard deviation. Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children 95 with a background in behavior analysis. Parents received weekly or bi-weekly progress updates, and supervision and specific routines that required their involvement in order to ensure the consistency of the interventions across contexts and caregivers. Intervention was individualized and comprehensive; and targeted motor, behavioral, daily- living, verbal, cognitive, and social skills. Goals were informed by a standardized curriculum composed of over 850 skills organized in 45 broad clinical areas (e.g., reading, self-control skills). These goals are informed by developmental sequences of typically developing children (Luiselli et al., 2008) and include skills that are instrumental
  • 15. for the acquisition of more complex repertoires (e.g., matching skills, imitation). Teaching sessions were delivered via one-to-one teaching with gradual transition to group activities and natural contexts. Transition to natural social contexts was emphasized after mastery in one-to-one teaching format. Decision-making in terms of hour allocation and treatment discontinuation weighted a number of factors including availability of school support, progress achieved, family priorities, and treatment costs. Typically, individuals that showed a persistent asymptote in their learning achievements or that became independent at school were assigned a reduced number of hours in preparation of service discontinuation (for details on the IBI curriculum see Lovaas, 2002). The current program was in line with the guidelines for responsible conduct published by the Behavior Analyst Certification Board (2010). All participants underwent standardized assessments with the E-LAP or the LAP-D prior to the intervention and approximately every six months into the program (average data points per participant 3.8, range 2-6). The selection, administration, and correction of instruments followed the guidelines by Jurado and Pueyo (2012).The research assistants conducting the standardized assessments were not involved in the administration of treatment and were not familiar with the hypotheses of the study. Data analysis Figure 1 shows the individual growth trajectories of participants for the eight E-LAP and LAP-D outcomes. Visual inspection of the data plots over time suggests that trajectories accelerated away from the start point shortly after the intervention commenced while progression decelerated as the individual approached a personal or
  • 16. scale ceiling. Therefore, individual trajectories did not follow a linear progression but rather an exponential negative growth. Exponential negative trajectories are composed formally of a negatively accelerated curve, Figure 1 Trajectories of Early Learning Accomplishment Profile and Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic scores over time. Fitted exponential negative curves (solid black line) were obtained for individuals above (dotted grey lines) and below (solid grey lines) the median of pre-intervention functioning at baseline in each domain. Gross Motor Fine Motor Pre-writing Cognitive Receptive 80 80 60 60 40 40 20 20 0
  • 17. 0 0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60 0 20 40 60 Expressive Self-care Social Intervention Time (Months) Sc al e sc or es Sc al e sc or es 96 J. Virues-Ortega et al. ending in an upper asymptote. According to the formal attributes of the data we selected a multilevel regression model based on the following exponential negative function: Yij = αi – (αi – π0i) e –π TIMEij
  • 18. Where αi represents the upper asymptote, π0i represents the lower end of the trajectory, and π1i represents the slope of the curve. Figure 2 illustrates different exponential negative patterns of change over time for various parameter values. Multilevel models provide two distinct levels of analysis: level-1 and level-2. The structural parts of the level-1 submodel contain two level-1 parameters and one within- person variance component (εij). The first parameter, known as intercept (π0i), represents the initial status of an individual i in the population. The second parameter, known as slope (π1i), represents the rate of change for the individual i in the population by unit of time. Therefore, level-1 establishes individual change overtime. By contrast, the parameters at level-2 do not represent individual variation, but average level of the outcome in the population. Specifically, the parameters at level-2 represent the average outcome level in the population corresponding to the intercept and slope values at level-1. At level-2, the pattern of change is not examined in terms of time, as is the case at level-1, but rather, in terms of a predictor. In summary, there are four parameters at level-2: γ00 is the population average of level-1 intercept with level-2 predictor value of 0, γ01 is the population average difference in level-1 intercept for a 1-unit variation in the predictor, γ10 is the population average of the level-1 slope when the predictor equals 0, and finally, γ11 is the population average difference when the predictor equals 1. γ00 and γ10 are baseline parameters while γ01 and γ11 estimate the association of the predictor with the initial status and the rate of change of the longitudinal progression, respectively. The model also provides a residual variance value for the intercept (σ02), the slope (σ12), and the covariance among
  • 19. these two (σ01). For multilevel models incorporating two predictors we will also report γ12 and γ12, which represents the population’s average variation in the outcome level for a one-unit increment in the predictors 1 and 2 (level-1), respectively (for more details in multilevel analysis refer to Singer & Willet, 2003). The estimation of the predictor coefficients at level-2 is presented formally below: π0i = ϒ00 + ϒ01 (PREDICTORi – PREDICTOR) + ξ0i π0i = ϒ10 + ϒ11 (PREDICTORi – PREDICTOR) + ξ1i According to this model, individual growth parameters (π0i, π1i) across children will be a function of population average values (γ00, γ10), and population variance components (ξ0i, ξ1i) represented by residual variances (σ02, σ12) and covariance (σ01). We estimated a series of multilevel models using different sets of predictors in order to select models that would maximize goodness-of-fit for a given outcome when compared with an unconditional baseline model (model with no predictors). This was accomplished in two sequential multilevel analyses. In the first sets of models we examined the impact of time-based predictors (intervention duration in weeks, total hours of intervention - weekly hours of interventions multiplied by weeks of intervention - and age in months). We would then select the model incorporating the single time-based predictor with best goodness-of-fit for each of the eight outcomes under analysis. Subsequently, we calculated a new set of two-predictor models incorporating the predictor previously selected and a specific personal factor that, when added, resulted in further increases in goodness-of-fit. The personal factors examined for each of the eight outcomes were age (if not selected in the preceding step), gender, and pre-intervention functioning. Two levels of pre-intervention functioning
  • 20. were established using the median value at baseline as cut- off point. The rationale for selecting these predictors is twofold: a) they are all common individual/treatment characteristics readily accessible to the clinician, and b) they have been examined in previous IBI studies although not in the context of a longitudinal analysis. Longitudinal predictors that changed overtime (intervention duration, total intervention duration, age) were re-calculated each time an individual was assessed. The Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) were computed as goodness-of- fit parameters for all one- and two-predictor models. Lower AIC and BIC values are indicative of better fitting. The best 100 75 50 25 0 E(Y) Time 0 2 4 6 8 10 α = 100 π1 = .3 π1 = .2
  • 21. π1 = .1 π0 = 15 Figure 2 Exponential patterns of change based on different parameter values. Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children 97 fitting two-predictor model was selected for each outcome and was fully reported. All analyses were conducted with STATA version 11 (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX) and its GLAMM program for multi-level analysis. A .05 level of significance was used throughout. Results have been reported according to the guidelines by Hartley (2012). By comparing the goodness-of-fit of one- and two- predictor models with an unconditional model, we aimed to establish which factors would better explain the longitudinal variation in our data. This analysis will help to determine prominent trajectories of intervention outcomes based on specific predictors. This strategy also serves the purpose of suggesting causality in the absence of a control group, similar to the way in which dose-response relations inform causation (see a discussion relevant to this point in Arjas and Parner, 2004). Namely, the causation inference would be supported if intervention intensity (e.g., total intervention hours at each time of assessment) is indeed superior in its ability to increase the fit of the model relative to an arbitrary time-dependent predictor (individuals’ age).
  • 22. Results The examination of the goodness-of-fit parameters of multilevel regression models showed that one-predictor and two-predictor models had a superior fit than unconditional models for every domain of the E-LAP and the LAP-D. AIC and BIC goodness-of-fit parameters of all models are reported in Table 3. Total intervention time (hours per week multiplied by weeks of intervention) was the single predictor with the highest favorable impact on goodness-of-fit for all E-LAP and LAP-D outcomes. Other time-based predictors including individuals’ age and intervention duration in months had a positive impact in the model’s fit, but did so to a lesser extent than total intervention time in all eight outcomes. Further improvements in goodness-of-fit were achieved in two-predictor models. Keeping total intervention time as the first factor, we examined the fit of regression models incorporating age, gender, or pre-intervention level as a second predictor. Age was the second most efficient predictor in terms of improving fit of the regression models for gross motor function, receptive language, self-care, and social behavior; while pre-intervention level was the second most efficient predictor for regression models using fine motor function, prewriting, cognitive, and expressive language (Table 3). The regression models of domains assessing motor, daily living, and social skills (gross motor function, fine motor function, self-care and social behavior) achieved better fitting than regression models of language- related domains (prewriting, receptive language, expressive language, cognitive). Table 4 presents the best fitting two-predictor multilevel model for each E-LAP and LAP-D outcome. Both predictors
  • 23. were statistically significant (p < .001) for every outcome. Rate of change attributable to total intervention time in hours (γ11) ranged from .004 to .009 (outcome average increase by predictor unit). Coefficient magnitudes for age in months (γ12) as a predictor ranged from .391 to .514. Finally, coefficients for the dichotomous variable pre- Table 3 Goodness-of-fit parameters of all one- and two- predictor multilevel models of change for Early Learning Accomplishment Profile and Learning Accomplishment Profile- Diagnostic scores. Goodness-of-fit (AIC, BIC) GMF FMF PWR COG RLG ELG SFC SBH Unconditional model 761.56 774.54 761.31 774.29 809.53 822.50 782.89 795.87 800.47 813.44 761.91 774.88 755.07 768.05 747.29 760.26 One-predictor models Intervention duration 715.86 721.17 779.29 746.27 763.16 732.19 706.67 706.42 731.43 736.70 794.86 761.84 778.73 747.77 722.24 721.99 Total intervention time 693.33 691.12 744.67 720.03 737.42 712.48 677.15 688.53 708.59 706.38 759.93 735.29 752.68 727.74 692.41 703.79 Age 716.29 731.43 776.85 761.27 769.47 740.37 708.01 721.45 731.86 747 792.42 776.84 785.03 755.94 723.58 737.02 Two-predictor models Age 664.01 673.20 727.92 710.50 724.67 702.71 651.35 676.02 681.81 691 745.72 728.30 742.47 720.52 669.16 693.82 Gender 691.93 690.19 745.08 719.15 735.78 711.83 675.18 687.74 709.73 707.99 762.88 736.95 753.59 729.63 692.99 705.55 Pre-intervention level 675.54 661.39 715.56 705.10 726.15 698.89 661.74 676.27
  • 24. 693.35 679.20 733.36 722.90 743.96 716.70 679.55 694.07 Note. AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; COG = Cognitive; ELG = Expressive language; FMF = Fine motor function; GMF = Gross motor function; PWR = Prewriting; RLG = Receptive language; SBH = Social behavior; SFC = Self-care. Intercept constant; slope is established as the intervention duration in months. Level-2 and level-3 best fitting models by outcome are highlighted. 98 J. Virues-Ortega et al. intervention level (γ12) ranged from 22.971 to 35.669. Figure 1 portrays fitted curves based on an exponential negative growth of subsamples above and below the median value of pre-intervention level for each of the eight standardized outcomes. Discussion Multilevel regression analyses based on an exponential negative growth trajectory indicated that total intervention duration in hours was the single predictor with the highest contribution to the model fit for all outcomes when compared with unconditional models. This finding suggests that a subtle characteristic of the intervention – a combination of both treatment intensity (weekly hours) and treatment duration (total weeks of treatment) – optimizes the fitting of individual trajectories to a specific mathematical function for the duration of the intervention and across a range of standardized outcomes. Improvements
  • 25. in model fitting caused by duration alone did not improve goodness-of-fit to the extent achieved by total intervention time as a single predictor (Table 3). Therefore, our data suggest that both intensity and duration, as represented by total intervention time, remained important factors of intervention gains regardless of pre-intervention functioning or age. Finally, total intervention time remained significant (p < .001) in all final two-predictor multilevel models (Table 4). When used in one-predictor models, pre-intervention functioning was inferior to total intervention time in terms of improving goodness-of-fit for all outcomes. We tested the impact of pre-intervention functioning in the goodness-of-fit of multilevel models incorporating two predictors. Including pre-intervention level as a second predictor, improved goodness-of-fit for all outcomes in the two-predictor models (Table 3). For four of the eight standardized outcomes examined (fine motor, pre-writing, cognitive, expressive language), pre-intervention level was the personal characteristic (above age and gender) that generated the greatest improvement in model fit. Pre- intervention level was a significant factor (p < .001) in the final two-predictor models for fine motor, pre-writing, cognitive and expressive language domains (Table 4). Interestingly, these outcomes involved more complex cognitive abilities relative to the remainder of E-LAP and LAP-D outcomes (e.g., fine vs. gross motor; expressive vs. receptive language; cognitive vs. self-care). Our results suggest that individuals starting intervention at a lower level in a given outcome were more likely to follow an asymptotical growth as opposed to individuals that initiated treatment with a higher level of performance (cf. fitted curves on Fig. 1). The visual inspection of the individual longitudinal trajectories in our sample suggests
  • 26. that pre-intervention level is a plausible predictor of individuals’ performance over the course of the intervention to the extent that a bimodal pattern seems obvious in most of the outcomes (e.g., Cognitive, Social). Bimodal trajectories in our dataset are consistent with the distinction between most and least positive responders to IBI discussed by Remington et al. (2007). The visual examination of individual trajectories on Figure 1 suggests that the pre- intervention median is an acceptable cut-off point as attested by the predictors significance and fit gains in models that incorporated this factor. A more sophisticated strategy to determine the cut-off point would have required asymmetrical assignment of participants above and below the cut-off points, which may have harmed statistical power and increase the potential for type II error. Therefore, future analyses would benefit from samples sizes larger than ours. Learning processes have been found to accommodate well to exponential negative or logistic patterns of change (e.g., Hicklin, 1976). The possibility remains, however, that non-linear patterns of growth found in the present study may have been caused by measurement-dependent factors, like inadequate scaling assumptions or excessive ceiling Table 4 Multilevel models for Early Learning Accomplishment Profile and Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic scores change over the duration of intensive behavioral intervention. Goodness-of-fit (AIC, BIC) GMF FMF PWR COG RLG ELG SFC SBH Fixed effects Intercept (γ00) 7.44 13.91** 10.97* 8.87 −3.00 5.46 -4.45 .52 Intervention, hours (γ11) .00** .01** .01** .01** .01** .01**
  • 27. .01** .01** Age, months (γ12) .51** – – – .51** – .57** .39** Pre-intervention levela (γ12) – 28.43** 35.67** 24.27** – 22.97** – – Variance components Level-1: Within-person (σε2) 25.32 23.32 47.64 32.91 47.79 28.39 16.54 18.90 Level-2: Intercept (σ02) 89.91 60.71 158.79 123.89 204.10 122.62 155.74 159.19 Level-2: Slope (σ12) .07 .19 .25 .33 4.37 .40 .10 .32 Level-2: Covariance (σ01) .46 2.37 -4.16 2.76 .11 .98 2.81 3.92 Note. COG = Cognitive; ELG = Expressive language; FMF = Fine motor function; GMF = Gross motor function; PWR = Prewriting; RLG = Receptive language; SBH = Social behavior; SFC = Self-care. Goodness-of-fit parameters and domain abbreviations in Table 3. *p < .01; **p < .001. aPre-intervention levels above and below the median at pre-test. Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children 99 effects in the psychometric instrument used to establish treatment outcomes. These potential shortcomings, however, may have had little impact on the validity of the predictors, which is independent from the specific shape of the longitudinal growth. The contributions of our study are primarily methodological and to a lesser extent practical. As discussed in our
  • 28. introduction, the literature on the effect of intensity and other predictors on the outcome of IBI have yielded inconsistent results. This inconsistency may be explained, at least to some extent, by non-linear variations of the predictor and the outcome overtime. Therefore, longitudinal studies may enhance our ability to examine outcome predictors with sufficient statistical power. Our results provide evidence in this direction being the first study to use this methodology in the context of IBI intervention. In terms of the applied relevance of our findings, future longitudinal studies expanding the present analysis could eventually provide the basis for evidence-informed clinical decision-making. Namely, clinicians could combine various predictors available at the beginning of the intervention (e.g., pre-intervention functioning in an specific area, age, expected treatment intensity and duration) to estimate the progress of the client over the next years, which could in turn inform the decision-making of family, caregivers and health decision-makers in terms of treatment planning and resource allocation. In summary, the present analysis helps to identify the general features of the longitudinal progression of children with autism undergoing IBI. Our results suggest that increased intervention time, lower age at intervention onset, and higher pre-intervention functioning might be associated with greater IBI outcomes for intervention programs of up to four years in duration. The present study provides the methodological basis for predictor identification in the longitudinal analysis of IBI. References Arjas, E., & Parner, J. (2004). Causal reasoning from longitudinal
  • 29. data. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 31, 171-187. Baio, J. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 61(SS03), 1-19. Behavior Analyst Certification Board (2010). BACB Guidelines for responsible conduct for behavior analysts. Available from: http://www.bacb.com/index.php?page=57 [retrieved 23 Jan 2013]. Ben-Itzchak, E., Lahat, E., Burgin, E., & Zachor, A. D. (2008). Cognitive, behavior and intervention in young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 447-458. Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J., Donaldson, A., & Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver model. Pediatrics, 125, 17-23. Dietz, C., Swinkels, S. H., Buitelaar, J. K., van Daalen, E., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Stability and change of IQ scores in preschool children diagnosed with autistic spectrum disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 405-410. Fleming, J.A. (2000). An examination of inter-rater reliability of the E-LAP. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University. Foxx, R.M. (2008). Applied behavior analysis treatment of
  • 30. autism. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17, 821-834. Ganz, J. B., Simpson, R. L., & Corbin-Newsome, J. (2008). The impact of the picture exchange communication system on requesting and speech development in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders and similar characteristics. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 157-169. Glover, M. E., Priminger, J. L. & Sanford, A. R. (1988). Early learning accomplishments profile. Winston-Salem, NC: Kaplan. Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., & Wilke, A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treatment intensity on behavioral intervention outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 1014-1022. Hardin, B. J., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Weeks, S. W. (2005). The Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic (LAP-D), third edition. Lewisville, NC: Kaplan Early Learning. Hartley, J. (2012). New ways of making academic articles easier to read. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 12, 143-160. Hicklin, W. J. (1976). A model for mastery learning based on dynamic equilibrium theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 13, 79-88. Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic
  • 31. review of early intensive behavioral interventions for children with autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,114, 23-41. Jónsdóttir, S. L., Saemundsen, E., Asmundsdóttir, G., Hjartardóttir, S., Asgeirsdóttir, B. B., Smáradóttir, H. H., Sigurdardóttir, S., & Smaári, J. (2007). Follow-up of children diagnosed with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1361-1374. Jurado, M. A., & Pueyo, R. (2012). Doing and reporting a neuropsychological assessment. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 12, 123-141. Le Couteur, A., Haden, G., Hammal, D., & McConachie, H. (2008). Diagnosing autism spectrum disorders in pre-school children using two standardised assessment instruments: The ADI-R and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 362-372. Long, C. E., Blackman, J. A., Farrell, W. J., Smolkin, M. E. & Conaway, M. R. (2005). A comparison of developmental versus functional assessment in the rehabilitation of young children. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 8, 151-161. Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3-9. Lovaas, O. I. (2002). Teaching developmentally disable children:
  • 32. The ME Book. Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Lovaas Institute (2011). NIMH Replication Sites. Available from: http://www.lovaas.com/contact.php [retrieved 1 Sep 2012]. Lovaas Institute Midwest (2010). Matrix curriculum and clinical goal areas. Minneapolis, MN: Author. Luiselli, J. K., Russo, D. C., Christian, W. P., & Wilczynski, S. M. (2008). Effective practices for children with autism. New York: Oxford University Press. Magiati, I., Moss, J., Charman, T., & Howlin, P. (2011). Patterns of change in children with autism spectrum disorders who received community based comprehensive interventions in their pre- school years: A seven year follow-up study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1016-1027. Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). Factors impacting on the outcomes of Greek intervention programmes for children with autistic spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 697-708. Miltenberger, R. G. (2011). Behavior modification: Principles and procedures (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage. 100 J. Virues-Ortega et al. Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Hardin, B. J. (2001). The early
  • 33. learning accomplishment profile edition examiner’s manual and technical report. New York: Kaplan. Perry, A., Cummings, A., Geier, J. D., Freeman, N. L., Hughes, S., LaRose, L., Managan, T., Reitzel, J. A., & Williams, J. (2008). Effectiveness of intensive behavioral intervention in a large, community-based program. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 621-642. Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Corness, M. (2007). Relative effectiveness of different home-based behavioral approaches to early teaching intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1815-1821. Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., degli Espinosa, F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., Alsford, P., Lemaic, M., & Ward, N. (2007). Early intensive behavioral intervention. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 418-438. Rogers, S. J., & Vismara, L. A. (2008). Evidence-based comprehensive treatments for early autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,37, 8-38. Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behavioral treatment of young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 15-23. Shook, G. L., & Johnston, J. M. (2011). Training and professional certification. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane
  • 34. (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 498-510). New York: Guilford Press. Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis. New York: Oxford University Press. Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic intervention for autism in early childhood. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 387-399. Welterlin, A., Turner-Brown, L. M., Harris, S., Mesibov, G., & Delmolino, L. (2012). The home TEACCHing program for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1827-1835. Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social interaction project for children with autism spectrum disorders beginning in the second year of life: A preliminary study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26, 67-82. O R I G I N A L P A P E R Savant Syndrome: Realities, Myths and Misconceptions Darold A. Treffert Published online: 6 August 2013 � Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
  • 35. Abstract It was 126 years ago that Down first described savant syndrome as a specific condition and 70 years ago that Kanner first described Early Infantile Autism. While as many as one in ten autistic persons have savant abilities, such special skills occur in other CNS conditions as well such that approximately 50 % of cases of savant syndrome have autism as the underlying developmental disability and 50 % are associated with other disabilities. This paper sorts out realities from myths and misconceptions about both savant syndrome and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) that have developed through the years. The reality is that low IQ is not necessarily an accompaniment of savant syn- drome; in some cases IQ can be superior. Also, savants can be creative, rather than just duplicative, and the skills increase over time on a continuum from duplication, to improvisation to creation, rather than diminishing or sud- denly disappearing. Genius and prodigy exist separate from savant syndrome and not all such highly gifted persons
  • 36. have Asperger’s Disorder. This paper also emphasizes the critical importance of separating ‘autistic-like’ symptoms from ASD especially in children when the savant ability presents as hyperlexia (children who read early) or as Einstein syndrome (children who speak late), or have impaired vision (Blindisms) because prognosis and out- come are very different when that careful distinction is made. In those cases the term ‘outgrowing autism’ might be mistakenly applied when in fact the child did not have ASD in the first place. Keywords Savant syndrome � Autism � Autism spectrum disorder � Hyperlexia � Einstein syndrome Realities Savant Syndrome Defined Savant syndrome is a rare but spectacular condition in which persons with developmental disabilities, including but not limited to autism, or other CNS disorders or disease have some spectacular ‘islands of genius’ that stand in jarring juxtaposition to overall limitations. (Treffert 2010)
  • 37. The condition can be present from birth and evident in early childhood (congenital) or develop later in life after CNS injury or disease (acquired). It affects males 4–6 times more frequently than females. Typically the skills occur in five general areas—music, art, calendar calculating, mathematics or mechanical/visual-spatial skills. Other skills occur less frequently including language (polyglot), unusual sensory discrimination, athletics or outstanding knowledge in specific fields such as neurophysiology, sta- tistics, navigation or computers, for example. Skills are usually single skills, but multiple skills can occur as well. Whatever the skill it is always associated with massive memory of a habit or procedural type—very narrow but exceedingly deep within the confines of the special skill. In some cases massive memory is the special skill. Savant Syndrome is Not a New Disorder (Nor is Autism) It is over 200 years ago since the first case of savant syn-
  • 38. drome appeared in a scientific journal in Germany (Moritz 1783). And it is 126 years since Dr. J. Langdon Down first D. A. Treffert (&) Agnesian HealthCare, 430 East Division Street, Fond du Lac, WI 54935, USA e-mail: [email protected] 123 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 DOI 10.1007/s10803-013-1906-8 described savant syndrome as a distinct condition. (Down 1887). In his 1887 lectures Down described ten cases of savant syndrome, including a boy who had memorized The Rise and Fall of the Roman Empire verbatim and could recite it backward or forward. Interestingly, in those same lectures Down described a form of mental retardation later named Down’s syndrome, and he described as well a form of ‘‘developmental retardation’’ that unmistakably con- sisted of cases of what we now would term early onset and
  • 39. late onset autistic disorder. (Treffert 2006) And in Kanner’s description of early infantile autism cases in 1944 there are several individuals who would now be considered cases of savant syndrome. Down coined the term ‘idiot savant’. He did not intend that term be degrading or insulting. At the time ‘idiot’ was an accepted scientific word for persons with an IQ below 25 and ‘savant’ was derived from the French word savoir, meaning ‘to know’. Because of its pejorative connotation, a 1988 paper suggested it was time to discard that archaic term and substitute ‘savant syndrome’ instead. (Treffert 1988) Then in 1989 the movie Rain Man made the term ‘‘autistic savant’’ household words. Not All Savants are Autistic, and Not All Autistic Persons are Savants Rain Man was a marvelous movie. It was accurately and sensitively done. Yet some persons came away from the movie assuming that, like Raymond Babbitt, all savants are
  • 40. autistic. Not so. Approximately one in ten persons with autism has savant skills; so nine out of ten do not. Approximately 1 out of 1,400 persons with mental retar- dation or CNS deficits other than autism do have savant skills so such abilities are not limited to autistic disorder. (Saloviita 2000) Hence not all autistic persons are savants, and not all savants are autistic. Savant Skills Represent a Spectrum of Abilities While admittedly a subjective scale at this point, savant skills lie on a spectrum of abilities. Most common are splinter skill savants who have obsessive preoccupation with and memorization of music & sports trivia, birthdays, license plate numbers, historical facts, train or bus sched- ules, navigation abilities, or maps for example. Talented savants are those in whom musical, art or other special abilities are more conspicuous not only in contrast to individual limitations, but also in contrast to peer group abilities whether disabled or not. And prodigious savant is
  • 41. an extremely high threshold term reserved for those extraordinarily rare individuals in whom the special skill is so outstanding that were it to be seen in a non-impaired person such a person would be termed a ‘‘prodigy’’ or ‘‘genius’’. The Acquired Savant: ‘‘Accidental Genius’’ In 1923 Minogue reported a case in which musical genius appeared in a three-year old child following meningitis. In 1980 Brink described the case of Mr. Z who demonstrated savant mechanical skills and traits at age nine after a bullet wound to the left brain produced muteness, deafness and left sided paralysis, but precipitated the newly surfaced savant skills. Dorman in 1991 published a case in which an 8 year old boy began to show exceptional calendar calcu- lating skills following a left hemispherectomy. But it was Miller’s reports on 12 individuals with fronto- temporal dementia who developed exceptional savant art and musical skills that really brought the ‘acquired savant’
  • 42. to prominent attention. (Miller et al. 1996, 1998, 2000) Miller had done SPECT imaging on these 12 patients, and he also did SPECT imaging on a 9 year old autistic, artistic savant. In these instances there was left anterior temporal dysfunction and evidence of what Kapur called ‘‘para- doxical functional facilitation’’–dysfunction in one area of the brain which uncovered, or facilitated ‘paradoxical’ function in some other area of still intact brain capacity (Kapur 1996). Since that time there have been numerous reports of what might be called ‘‘acquired savant syndrome’’ fol- lowing a cerebral insult from stroke, a blow to the head, dementia or other CNS disease or injury accompanied by the emergence of savant skills, sometimes at a prodigious level (Treffert 2010). In most of these cases there was some sort of ‘trade-off’ of cognitive or other abilities for the new found savant skills. Yet in other cases, more aptly called ‘accidental genius’ (following being struck by lightning in
  • 43. one instance) there has been no trade off at all with the emergence of new found skills. These instances raise many interesting questions about dormant capacity within us all, and raise the even more challenging question of how to tap those buried abilities without enduring some CNS catas- trophe. These cases of acquired savant syndrome are pre- sented in much more detail on the savant syndrome web site at www.savantsyndrome.com. The Most Important Question of All: How do They do It? There have been many theories put forth to try to explain savant syndrome ranging from early heredity theories to present day Quantum theory. (Treffert 2010). Some of the neuropsychological theories such as weak central coher- ence, mind blindedness are interesting as they apply to the autistic savant. But 50 % of savants are not autistic. The J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 565 123
  • 44. http://www.savantsyndrome.com role of heredity is no doubt a contributor and the search for a savant ‘gene’ is underway, one study finding such a gene, but another not confirming that finding. Compensatory learning, reinforcement and repetition-compulsion may also play a role, but then, if those dynamics produce savant syndrome, why wouldn’t that apply to all persons with autism or other CNS limitations? The theory I favor is that what I have come to call the ‘‘three R’s’’ and reflects the process Kapur termed ‘‘para- doxical functional facilitation’’ in 1996 in which one area of the brain in released from the inhibiting influence of some other brain area. In the case of savants, both con- genital and acquired, there is brain damage in one area, frequently the left hemisphere, with recruitment of still intact brain tissue in another area of the brain, rewiring of circuitry to that new area, and release of dormant capacity,
  • 45. through a disinhibiting process, of information and skills already stored in that newly recruited area. It is genetic memory—the genetic transfer of knowledge and skills—that accounts for the already stored dormant capacity tapped by the recruitment, rewiring and release. I address genetic memory much more fully in Islands of Genius as well, and on the savant syndrome web site at www.savantsyndrome.com. Genetic memory is based on the fact that some savants, particularly those severely limited in other ways, clearly ‘‘know things they never learned’’. The only possible way to know things one never learned—sometimes at complex levels—is for that knowledge to be factory installed, genetically transmitted. But there is one other important element that contributes to the ‘‘how do they do it?’’ question. That is the role of the family or other caregivers, teachers or mentors in first discovering the special gift in the savant, then tenderly nurturing and encouraging that gift, and supporting and
  • 46. reinforcing it by praise coupled with copious unconditional love. Some Mysteries Remain There are many scientific mysteries still about savant syndrome. But two are especially intriguing. First is the conspicuous regularity in which the triad of mental impairment (often from autism) ? impaired vision ? musical genius occurs. Savant syndrome is rare but the frequency of this triad is very conspicuous and dispropor- tionate throughout the history of savant cases combining blindness and mental disability with prodigious musical skills. The first of such persons was Thomas Bethune, better known as ‘‘Blind Tom’’, who was born in 1850 and gave his first public piano concert at age 8. From then on he became an international celebrity and was the most cele- brated black concert artist of the nineteenth century. His repertoire was in the thousands of pieces including many of his own compositions, the first of which he created at age
  • 47. five. Since that time a number of such cases with this triad are glaringly over-represented in savant syndrome, in itself a rare condition. Leslie Lemke in the United States, Derek, Paravicini in the United Kingdom and Hikari Oe in Japan are present day representatives of this amazing combina- tion of ability and disability. In addition to those examples, a number of other such cases are documented in detail in Islands of Genius. The savant syndrome web site provides further video evidence and documentation of this extraor- dinary triad of mental impairment ? blindness ? musical genius. A second mystery is the why calendar calculating, an obscure skill in neurotypical persons, is seemingly almost universally present in persons with savant syndrome. This ability is a clear example of how savants, sometimes severely impaired, innately ‘‘know things they never learned’’. Yes, there are formulas for calendar calculating.
  • 48. And yes, if any person puts his or her mind to it, he or she can learn (laboriously) how to calendar calculate. But savants seem to have this algorithm or formula ‘uncon- sciously’ inscribed or inculcated in their brain and in most such individuals there simply has not been any ‘study’ of the calendar nor the ‘learning’ of any formula. Why cal- endar calculating? And why is that so prominent in savant syndrome but generally not seen in other brain diseases or disorders? Some imaging studies are underway with cal- endar calculating savants, comparing them to neurotypical ‘expert’ calendar calculators and control groups. Myths and Misconceptions Savants are Not ‘Creative’ Some observers, while extolling the eidetic-like ability and memory of savants, point out that in contrast to such astonishing imitative ability, savants, as a group are not very creative. In fact I was one of those observers who wrote just that in the original 1988 version of Extraordi-
  • 49. nary People:Understanding Savant Syndrome. I raised the question there ‘‘Is the savant creative?’’ I answered it this way: ‘‘In my experience, not very’’. I was wrong and have corrected that perception in my later writings. What changed my mind? Some additional years of observation. There is always a tremendous advantage in having a longitudinal view of a patient and his or her ‘natural history’ of illness or disorder, compared to a one-time, snapshot consultation. And now, having the opportunity to observe the ‘natural history’ of how the savant skills emerge and develop over many years, I have noted predictable and replicable sequence of steps that 566 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 123 http://www.savantsyndrome.com progresses from imitation, to improvisation, to creation in savant syndrome. Let me expand on that, using Leslie Lemke as an example.
  • 50. Leslie Lemke’s ability to store and replicate music, even after only a single hearing, was spectacular. Indeed at age 14 he was able to play back Tchaikovsky’s First Piano concerto flawlessly having heard it for the first time as a theme song to a television movie. Over time, however, Leslie began to show improvisa- tional skills in a addition to replication abilities. For example at a 1989 concert a young girl came up to the stage in the challenge portion of the concert and played ‘‘Mississippi Hotdog’’. Leslie listened and then, when asked, dutifully played back the piece exactly as he had heard it. But toward the end of the piece he began to look a bit restless, and seemed more excited and more eager to play. After the initial playback of Mississippi Hotdog was completed, flawlessly as usual, Leslie then launched into a 5 min improvisation which could be called ‘‘Variations on a Theme of Mississippi Hotdog.’’ He changed pitch, changed tempo and demonstrated convincingly that he
  • 51. does indeed have innate access to the ‘rules of music’, confirmed by a number of professional musicians who have observed him. Adding to the improvisation skill now is creation and composing of entirely new pieces. One such song he calls ‘‘Down Home on the Farm in Arpin’’, and another he names ‘‘Bird Song’’. In that latter piece he duplicates, by whistling softly as he plays his new tune, the bird songs he hears as he sits for hours outside his farm home. That same sequence from replication to improvisation to creation occurs in other savants whether musicians or artists. The artists begin their ‘career’ with striking replicas of what they have seen and stored, usually requiring no model or constant reference piece. Then some improvisa- tion begins to appear—a telephone pole deleted here, or a new tree there—slightly different from the original. Then comes creation of entirely new pieces, maybe now free- form or in an entirely different art style.
  • 52. So the savant can be creative. Some savants prefer to stay with replication, but many have gone beyond literal copying, as stunning as that can be, to improvisation and then creation of something entirely new. These clinical impressions regarding creativity in the savant have been bolstered by several formal research projects. A 1987 study by Hermelin, O’Connor and Lee looked at musical inventiveness in five musical savants compared to six non-savant children who had musical training over a period of 2 years but who had not been exposed to compositional or improvisational instruction. Five tasks were used to grade for ‘‘musical inventiveness’’. On those tests the savant group was superior to the control group. Similarly, on tests of musical competence—timing, balance and complexity—the savants (with a mean IQ of 59) were also superior to the control group. Hermelin and her co-workers indicated this study was consistent with earlier findings—that a series of separate
  • 53. intelligences, of which music is but one, exist in each person rather than a single, consistent intelligence that permeates all the skills and abilities of each person. With respect to music, they concluded that savants were able to show some creativity and improvisation in addition to mimicry. Hermelin et al. (1989) conducted a study of improvi- sations by Leslie Lemke compared to a professional, non- savant musician after each had heard the same musical pieces, one lyric (Grieg) and one a-tonal (Bartok). Leslie’s improvisations were described as ‘‘virtuoso embellish- ments with a considerable degree of musical inventiveness and pianistic virtuosity.’’ That study concluded that ‘‘both subjects’ attempts at improvisation show a high degree of generative musical ability, and what distinguishes them from each other is not so much a differential degree of musicianship but rather their own, different musical pref- erences as well as their respective personality characteris-
  • 54. tics.’’ In improvisational style on the Bartok, a-tonal piece, both musicians resembled each other as well. In summary, savants can be creative. Most savants travel along a route of first replication, then improvisation, and finally creation. As we learn more about the brain from the study of savants, we may also learn much more about talent and creativity itself through the unique window into the brain and special skills savant syndrome provides. The ‘‘Nadia’’ Effect and the ‘‘Dreaded Trade-Off’’ In 1977 Dr. Lorna Selfe described the case of Nadia, a prolific childhood artist, whose special abilities disap- peared after she was sent away to school to increase lan- guage acquisition, socialization abilities and daily living skills. With the publication of Dr. Selfe’s 2011 book— Nadia Revisited—we now have the benefit of long term follow-up on Nadia. Selfe describes the loss of skills this way: ‘‘In the years following my first study, and throughout her school days, Nadia was given intensive help with lan-
  • 55. guage development and her ability to communicate improved with the production of two/three word sentences. She also started to draw like an infant so that, for a period, two styles coexisted and sometimes on the same piece of paper. Gradually and inexorably she lost the ability to draw realistically. Unlike some savant artists such as Stephen Wiltshire, who has maintained the strength of his drawing ability, Nadia’s ability appeared to peter out. She is now middle-aged and lives in a specialist care home but for many years she has simply refused to draw.’’ But, in spite of the loss of art skills, Selfe points out, importantly, that J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 567 123 even though now Nadia is not interested in art and is totally dependent on others in that supervised setting there is also the ‘‘optimistic story of the love and care of the family that raised her and of the people who now care for Nadia. She is
  • 56. in the safe and competent hands of dedicated staff who devote themselves to the care of people who are unable to look after themselves.’’ We don’t seem to know exactly what did happen with Nadia and why those special skills disappeared. But what I do know is that in the many, many savants with whom I have worked, or know about, such a ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’, or loss of skills, does not occur as the savant gets older or when exposed to more formal education and training. To the contrary, in my experience vigorously ‘‘training the talent’’, whatever that special skill is, leads, in and of itself, to increased language, social and daily living skills without any ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’ of special skills. So Nadia’s experience is the exception, not the rule. Putting aside the fear of a ‘‘dreaded trade-off’’ is important because parents, teachers or therapists are sometimes reluctant to venture forth with more formal education or training efforts lest the ‘‘Nadia’’ effect occur.
  • 57. The good news is that such a fear is, in my experience, unfounded and should not prevent presenting the savant with more formal education and training within his or her area of specialty, as well as in a more general educational sense. That being the case, parents and teachers can con- tinue not only to applaud and reinforce the special skills as they surface, but can confidently add teaching and training in a more formal sense as well without fear of loss of talent, ingenuity or enthusiasm on the part of the savant. Savant Syndrome is Always Associated with Low IQ Perhaps stemming from Down’s original description regarding low IQ and the presence of savant skills, a misconception continues that low IQ is a necessary accompaniment of savant syndrome. That in fact is not the case. While it is true that most savants have measured IQ’s between 50 and 70, in some instances IQ can be as high as 125, or even higher. Thus an IQ level above 70 does not ‘disqualify’ someone from having savant syndrome.
  • 58. One reason that many savants, or many autistic persons, have IQ scores below 70 is that IQ measurement depends so heavily on verbal scales, and many autistic individuals, including those with savant syndrome, have language (verbal) deficits as an intrinsic part of the underlying disorder. A second reason for low IQ scores among savants is the fact that IQ tests measure only one facet of ‘‘intelligence’’, something termed ‘‘IQ’’. Savants tend to do poorly on that particular measure of ‘‘intelligence’’. But savants point out forcefully that there are multiple forms of ‘‘intelligence’’ and IQ measures only one such ‘‘intelligence’’. IQ tests do measure something defined as ‘‘IQ’’. But IQ tests fail to measure some of the other forms of ‘‘intelligence’’ that savants possess in greater or lesser measure as well. Some of the savants are profoundly disabled in capacities as measured by IQ, but yet they are astoundingly ‘intelligent’ within their ‘‘island of genius’’.
  • 59. There is much debate among psychologists regarding single v. multiple intelligence theories. But savant syn- drome, with sometimes extraordinary ability co-existing with profound disability in the same individual argues forcefully for the concept of multiple intelligence. And the fact of multiple intelligences has profound implications not just for better understanding and approaching savant syn- drome, but also for implementing more effective, individ- ualized and targeted education efforts for all segments of the population. Thirdly, in all developmental disabilities, and savant syndrome, one has to make a distinction between ‘‘actual retardation’’, as classified by IQ scores, from ‘‘functional retardation’’—instances in which persons with presumably normal or high IQ (if it could be accurately measured) function at levels more consistent with sub-normal IQ. In such instances, either the language and verbal deficits, or behavioral traits and symptoms, prevent accurate mea-
  • 60. surement of ‘‘IQ’’. These individuals, whether savants or not, ‘‘function’’ as if ‘‘retarded’’, but their abilities in cer- tain other areas of function belie a below average IQ score. That is termed ‘‘functional retardation.’’ Leslie Lemke provides an example of how misleading IQ levels can be as a single measure of intelligence. Leslie has a measured IQ of 58 on the WAIS-R test, based solely on verbal scores; performance tests were not done because such testing relies heavily on vision, and Leslie is blind. Other tests were carried out as well including the 4th edi- tion of the Stanford-Binet; the Tactual Performance Test; the American Association for Mental Deficiency Adaptive Behavioral Scale; and the Animal List Selective Remind- ing Test. By looking at the scores on these tests as a whole, the neuropsychologist concluded Leslie was functioning in the moderately retarded range of intelligence, defined as an IQ level between 35 and 55. Yet a videotape of one of Leslie’s concerts challenges
  • 61. the accuracy of such a low level of intelligence figures. At this particular concert Leslie was asked to play a piece he had never heard before with the other pianist, rather than waiting for the piece to conclude and then play it back after hearing it as he usually does. The other pianist began playing. Leslie waited about 3 seconds and then did indeed play the piece with the other pianist, separated only by those 3 seconds. In that three second delay Leslie was taking in what he heard, processing it, and simultaneously outputting the music as he played along with the other 568 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 123 pianist. Leslie was parallel processing, just as some very intelligent, but rare, interpreters are able to translate what a speaker is saying into another language simultaneously, rather than having the speaker pause from time to time while the interpreter ‘catches up’.
  • 62. Leslie was parallel processing. That would not be pos- sible if the IQ level of 35–55 was an accurate barometer of his over-all intelligence. He exceeds that level by far with the parallel processing of music which signals that more than a single ‘intelligence’ was at work during that com- plex performance. In summary, measured IQ levels in savants can range from sub-normal to exceptional and low IQ is not a pre- requisite to being classified as a ‘savant’. While many savants have measured IQ levels below 70, some have measured IQ’s above normal which can range as high as 125 or above. In assessing IQ scores, one has to differen- tiate’actual’ retardation from ‘functional’ retardation All ‘‘Geniuses’’ and ‘‘Prodigies’’, Past and Present are Really ‘‘Aspies’’ With increased interest in autism and Asperger’s, and especially with the visibility given to the extraordinary skills seen in savant syndrome, it seems popular these days to
  • 63. apply the diagnosis of Asperger’s disorder particularly to anyone considered to be a ‘genius’ or ‘prodigy’ past or present. Names such as Einstein, Rembrandt, Mozart, Jefferson and many others are bandied about in such dis- cussions. It is difficult enough to make accurate diagnoses of autism or Asperger’s disorder in real life, with face-to-face interviews and comprehensive testing, let alone trying to apply post-mortem diagnoses, sight unseen. Retrospective medical diagnoses are always problematical and suspect. And then there are present-day prodigies and geniuses. Some, outrageously bright, but not autistic children, have composed multiple symphonies by age seven, or have mastered instruments, sometimes multiple instruments, by age three. Others show astonishing artistic, mathematical, prose or poetry skills well beyond their years. If children, we call them prodigies. They are neither autistic, nor Aspergers. If adults, we call them geniuses. They also are neither Asperger’s nor autistic. Prodigies and geniuses
  • 64. have special, spectacular abilities in absence of any underlying disability. Typically, rather than there being simply one ‘‘island’’of genius as is often the case with savants, whatever the skills of the prodigy or genius, they are associated with a high measured IQ in all areas of functioning. In short, not every gifted child, nor every ‘absent minded professor’, has Asperger’s disorder. Instead, ‘‘prodigy’’ and ‘‘genius’’ do exist as independent conditions separate from any underlying disability or disorder The temptation to classify all prodigies and geniuses as having autism or Asperger’s seems to be part of the disease de jour phenomenon quite rampant these days and needs to be resisted in favor of careful analysis lest continued ‘diag- nosis creep’ deletes all meaningful classification, all the disorders lose their specificity, and the ‘spectrum’ engulfs us all. The beginning of wisdom is to call things by their right
  • 65. name. Asperger’s, autism, and savant syndrome surely do exist. But so do the categories of ‘‘normal’’, ‘‘gifted’’, ‘‘prodigy’’ and ‘‘genius’’. The important thing is to know the difference lest every parent of a gifted child for example, whether mildly gifted or profoundly gifted, fear their child is autistic. ‘‘Outgrowing’’ Autism: Separating ‘Autistic-Like’ Traits from Autistic Disorder in Children Who Read Early, Speak Late, or are Blind I get many ‘‘I’ve got a son or daughter who……..’’ emails from the savant syndrome web site in which parents describe various accelerated skills in their children and inquire whe- ther those might be forms of savant syndrome, and if so, how should those special skills, and that child, be approached educationally and otherwise. Among those many inquiries are children who read early (hyperlexia) or speak late (Einstein Syndrome). Often children in both those groups are automatically, and mistakenly, assumed to be autistic when
  • 66. in fact they only have ‘autistic-like’ behaviors and traits with very different causes and outcomes than Autistic Disorder. They tend to ‘outgrow’ their autism (their choice of terms), which was not autism in the first place. Based on a number of such cases brought to my atten- tion separating ‘‘autistic-like’’ behaviors and traits from ‘‘Autistic Disorder’’ in children who read early, or speak late, or who are blind, is a critical differential diagnosis with vast causal, treatment and outcome ramifications. Hyperlexia I, II and III Some neurotypical children simply read early. They may be reading, instead of the teacher, to their nursery school class, or reading at a 7th grade level at age 3 for example. There are no associated autistic or autistic-like traits or behaviors. They are entirely ‘normal’ (neurotypical) chil- dren. Eventually their classmates catch up with reading skills but in the meantime the advanced, precocious read- ing ability at such an early age draws considerable atten-
  • 67. tion. I describe several such examples on the ‘‘Hyperlexia’’ posting on the savant syndrome web site at www. savantsyndrome.com I refer to this type of early reading ability as Hyperlexia I. J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 569 123 http://www.savantsyndrome.com http://www.savantsyndrome.com Hyperlexia II is when early reading ability presents as a ‘splinter skill’ as part of an Autistic Spectrum Disorder. These children read voraciously along with astonishing memory for what they read. They often have other mem- orization abilities sometimes linked with fascination with numbers or calendar calculating skills. These children show other characteristic language, social and behavioral symptoms seen in autistic spectrum disorder, including traits such as withdrawal, poor eye contact, lack of interest in seeking or giving affection, insistence on sameness, and
  • 68. obsessive compulsive behavior, for example. They usually carry a formal diagnosis of Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder, or pervasive developmental disorder (PPD/NOS) with intense fascination with words and numbers present- ing as a ‘splinter skill’. Hyperlexia III is a less frequently recognized form of early reading ability. It is not an autistic spectrum disorder even though there are some ‘‘autistic-like’’ traits and behaviors that gradually fade as the child gets older. Some times this is referred to as ‘‘outgrowing autism’’. These children read early and have striking memorization abilities sometimes coupled with precocious abilities in other areas as well. They may show unusual sensory sensitivity, ech- olalia, pronoun reversals, intense need for sameness, spe- cific fears or phobias, have lining/stacking rituals and demonstrate strong visual and auditory memory. Unlike children with ASD, however, they are often very outgoing and affectionate with family, even though reserved and
  • 69. distant with peers and would be playmates. They do make eye contact and can be very interactive with persons close to them, especially adults. These children present as being very bright, inquisitive and precocious overall. Indeed these ‘autistic-like’ traits and behaviors do fade as the child gets older, but in the meantime parents are exposed to unnecessary fear and dread because the diagnosis of ‘‘autism’’ has been prematurely and inappropriately applied without ‘‘hyperlexia III’’ being considered in the differen- tial diagnosis. Space precludes an extensive discussion of Hyperlexia III, but the savant syndrome website as www. savantsyndrome.com provides a number of example of such cases with respect to characteristics and outcome. Einstein Syndrome: Children Who Speak Late In his 1997 book Late Talking Children Thomas Sowell pointed out how often ‘‘autistic-like’’ symptoms, as opposed to Autistic Disorder itself, appeared in children with delayed speech based on parental reports in a group of
  • 70. 46 such children. In a follow-up book 4 years later—The Einstein Syndrome:Bright Children Who Talk Late— Sowell expanded that group to 239 late talking children who were exceptionally late in beginning to speak but were also exceptionally bright. (Sowell 2001). His book is replete with examples. As with Hyperlexia III, Sowell found in his correspondence with parents that many of the children with delayed speech had been given a diagnosis of ASD along the way but that the ‘‘autistic-like’’ symptoms in these children were transient, and like with the Hyper- lexia III children, those traits and behaviors faded over time. He recommended careful professional evaluation for children who speak late by clinicians familiar with the various parameters and conditions involved with such children, sparing parents unnecessary worry, concern and pessimism that always accompanies a diagnosis of ‘‘autism’’. This condition is also discussed in more detail on the savant syndrome website.
  • 71. Blindisms Teachers and parents of visually impaired children often refer to what are called ‘‘blindisms’’ in such children. Ek and co-workers point out that ‘‘blindisms’’—stereotypical movements, language problems and certain other behav- iors—are common in children with congenital or other types of blindness. (Ek et al. 1998). Hobson described the similarities in development during pre-school age (3–4 years) between blind children and those with autism (Hobson 1993). In both groups impairments in symbolic play, confusion in the use of language and stereotypes were frequent. Many of the autistic features in the young, blind child without cerebral damage disappeared with age. As the child acquired a better understanding of the surrounding world, and with the development of language, a basis for sharing experiences and feelings with other people devel- oped. Hobson noted ‘‘blindness seems to delay rather than prevent development in these respects.’’ In 2010 Hobson
  • 72. and Lee did an 8 year follow-up study on nine congenitally blind and seven sighted children who met formal diag- nostic criteria for autism. Follow-up of the nine congeni- tally blind children with ‘autism’ revealed that, in adolescence, only one such child satisfied the criteria for that disorder. In contrast, all of the seven sighted children still did meet the Autistic Disorder criteria. For the group with what turned out to be autistic-like symptoms, in the title of his report Hobson uses the interesting term ‘‘reversible autism’’. Autism, autistic-like symptoms and blindisms can be confused with each other in visually impaired children. But just as with children who read early or speak late, differ- entiation between Autistic Disorder and ‘‘autistic-like’’ symptoms is critical with these children if parents are to be spared unnecessary distress from an autism diagnosis improperly applied and, equally important, if the right treatment is to be applied to the right patient.
  • 73. 570 J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 123 http://www.savantsyndrome.com http://www.savantsyndrome.com Summary With all the emphasis by some on the autism ‘epidemic’ and with it the need for early identification and a prolif- eration of programs, it is important to remember that not every child who reads at 18 months, draws at 2 years, hums back all the melodies he or she hears, or likes to line up railroad cars, resists certain foods, insists on routine, memorizes license plates and birthdays, has certain fear and phobias or is very late in speaking is on the autistic spectrum. If one looks up ‘‘hyperlexia’’ on the internet, though, most often the site links hyperlexia to autism. That simply is not so in all cases as pointed out above. That same link to autism is likewise often made for children who speak late, or are blind.
  • 74. Again, not so in all cases. While early identification of autism in affected children is important, those efforts need to be balanced with sensible caution lest parents be unnecessarily frightened and overwhelmed by premature, and erroneous, diagnoses. Except in truly ‘classic’ cases, often some time of watchful observation needs to elapse until the ‘natural history of the disorder’ reveals the real diagnosis. Such ‘watchful observation’, diagnostic caution and separation of ‘autistic-like’ behaviors from Autistic Disorder can provide example of what some have called ‘‘reversible autism’’. References Brink, T. L. (1980). Idiot savant with unusual mechanical ability: An organic explanation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 250–251. Dorman, C. (1991) Exceptional calendar calculating ability after early left hemispherectomy. Brain and Cognition 15, 26–36 London:
  • 75. Churchill. Down, J.L. (1887). On some of the mental affections of childhood and youth. London, Churchill. Ek, U., Fernello, E., Jacobson, L., & Gillberg, C. (1998). Relation between blindness due to retinopathy and autistic spectrum disorders: A population study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 297–301. Hermelin, B., O’Connor, N., & Lee, S. (1987). Musical inventiveness of five idiot-savants. Psychological Medicine, 17, 685–694. Hermelin, B., O’Connor, N., Lee, S., & Treffert, D. A. (1989). Intelligence level and musical improvisational ability. Psycho- logical Medicine, 19, 447–457. Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the development of the mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hobson, R. P. (2010). Reversible autism in congenitally blind children? A controlled study Journal of Child Psychology and
  • 76. Psychiatry, 51(11), 1235–1241. Kanner, L.:217-25055-58 (1944) Early infantile Autism Journal of Pediatrics 25, 200–217. Kapur, N. (1996). Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain- behavior research. Brain, 119, 1775–1790. Miller, B., et al. (1996). Enhanced artistic creativity with temporal lobe degeneration. Lancet, 348, 1744–1745. Miller, B., et al. (1998). Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 51, 978–982. Miller, B., et al. (2000). Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia. British Journal of Psychia- try, 176, 458–463. Minogue, B. M. (1923). A case of secondary mental deficiency with musical talent. Journal of Applied Psychology, 7, 349–357. Mortiz, K. P. (1783). Gnothi Sauton oder magazin der erfahrungs-
  • 77. seelenkunde als ein lesebuch fur gelehrte and ungelehrte. Berlin, Germany: Mylius. Selfe, L. (1977). Nadia: A case of extraordinary drawing ability in an Autistic child. London: Academic Press. Selfe, L. (2011). Nadia revisited: A longitudinal study of an Autistic Savant. New York: Psychology Press. Sowell, T. (1997). Late talking children. New York: Basic Books. Sowell, T. (2001). The Einstein syndrome: Bright children who talk late. New York: Basic books. Treffert, D.A. (1988). The idiot savant: A review of the syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry 145563–145572. Treffert, D.A. (1989). Extraordinary people: Understanding savant syndrome. Lincoln, Nebraska: iUniverse.com. Treffert, D. A. (2006). Dr. Down and ‘‘developmental disorders’’.
  • 78. Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 36, 965–966. Treffert, D. A. (2010). Islands of genius: The bountiful mind of the Autistic acquired and sudden savant. London: Jessica Kingsley. J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:564–571 571 123 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. c.10803_2013_Article_1906.pdfSavant Syndrome: Realities, Myths and MisconceptionsAbstractRealitiesSavant Syndrome DefinedSavant Syndrome is Not a New Disorder (Nor is Autism)Not All Savants are Autistic, and Not All Autistic Persons are SavantsSavant Skills Represent a Spectrum of AbilitiesThe Acquired Savant: ‘‘Accidental Genius’’The Most Important Question of All: How do They do It?Some Mysteries RemainMyths and MisconceptionsSavants are Not ‘Creative’The ‘‘Nadia’’ Effect and the ‘‘Dreaded Trade-Off’’Savant Syndrome is Always Associated with Low IQAll ‘‘Geniuses’’ and ‘‘Prodigies’’, Past and Present are Really ‘‘Aspies’’‘‘Outgrowing’’ Autism: Separating ‘Autistic-Like’ Traits from Autistic Disorder in Children Who Read Early, Speak Late, or are BlindHyperlexia I, II and IIIEinstein Syndrome: Children Who Speak LateBlindismsSummaryReferences DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY
  • 79. REVIEW The genetic landscape of autism spectrum disorders RASIM O ROSTI1 | ABDELRAHIM A SADEK2 | KEITH K VAUX1 | JOSEPH G GLEESON1 1 Department of Neurosciences and Pediatrics, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA. 2 Pediatric Neurology Unit, Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, Sohag University, Sohag, Egypt. Correspondence to Joseph G Gleeson at University of California San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive M/C 0665, La Jolla, CA, USA. E-mail: [email protected] PUBLICATION DATA Accepted for publication 22nd July 2013. Published online 1st October 2013. ABBREVIATIONS ASD Autism spectrum disorders CGH Comparative genomic hybridization CNV Copy number variants Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disor- ders that show impaired communication and socialization,
  • 80. restricted interests, and stereotypi- cal behavioral patterns. Recent advances in molecular medicine and high throughput screenings, such as array comparative genomic hybridization (CGH) and exome and whole genome sequencing, have revealed both novel insights and new questions about the nature of this spectrum of disorders. What has emerged is a better understanding about the genetic architecture of various genetic subtypes of ASD and correlations of genetic mutations with specific autism subtypes. Based on this new information, we outline a strategy for advancing diagnosis, prognosis, and counseling for patients and families. Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of com- plex neurodevelopmental disabilities that affect social inter- action and communication skills. The prevalence of ASDs appears to be constantly and gradually increasing, but it is not clear if this is because of clarification of diagnostic cri- teria or an actual increase in the number of cases. Most recent estimates find that the median of prevalence esti- mates of ASD is 62 out of 10 000.1 Those with molecu- larly defined causes make up roughly 20% of the cases, but the heritability has been estimated to be 90%, suggesting as yet undiscovered causes. However, there are new reports suggesting that the previous estimate of heritability was too high and may need to be adjusted downwards.2
  • 81. The diagnostic criteria have evolved with increasing clin- ical and molecular understanding of this umbrella term. This aspect makes the diagnosis more challenging as the clinical spectrum is highly variable and the etiological sub- grouping tends to change with the ever-growing molecular data now fed by high throughput techniques such as array comparative genomic hybridization (CGH), whole exome and whole genome sequencing. The judgment of the physician, critical in achieving accurate prognosis and genetic counseling, requires a systematic approach. By incorporating these techniques along with careful clinical and neuropsychological assessment, a more accurate diagnosis of an ASD disorder can be achieved. Distinguishing between essential autism and complex (syndromic) autism might be considered the starting point of this systematic approach.3 Of all individuals meeting cri- teria for autism, essential autism makes up approximately 75% of the cases. Although essentially a diagnosis of exclu- sion, the main characteristics are the lack of dysmorphic features, higher male to female ratio (6:1), higher sibling recurrence risk (up to 35%), and positive family history (up to 20%). Syndromic autism, on the other hand, is characterized by accompanying recognizable patterns of dysmorphology, a reduced male to female ratio (3.5:1), lower recurrence risk (4%–6%), and family history to a lesser extent (up to 9%).1 Clinical recognition of well-known phenotypes leading to a targeted molecular testing approach can strengthen the hand of the clinician in answering additional questions about the recurrence risk and prognosis according to the molecular basis identified by targeted testing. However, for most forms of essential autism, there is no familiar pheno- type that points to one particular genetic cause or another.
  • 82. In the absence of a specific genetic test option after clinical evaluation, array CGH testing is recommended as it has been reported to provide the highest diagnostic yield in cases with ASD – ranging from 10%4 to 18%.5 Once such a genetic alteration is identified in an affected individual, more specific information about diagnosis, prognosis, recurrence risks, and possible targeted therapy options can be conveyed to the family. ASD phenotypes caused by genetic alterations can be divided into three subgroups: cytogenetic alterations that can be detected by conventional karyotyping (not the primary focus of this paper). These comprise classic chro- mosomal syndromes such as Turner and Down syndrome, and large segmental cytogenetic alterations that have been associated with autism.6 Other subgroups are single gene disorders (detected in <5%) and copy number variants (detected in 10%–35% of the cases1). NON-SYNDROMIC AND POSSIBLE TREATABLE FORMS OF ASD: SINGLE GENE DISORDERS Single gene disorders are mainly composed of metabolic conditions in which accompanying ASD features are 12 DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12278 © 2013 Mac Keith Press reported. Among these, phenylketonuria is common enough to be included in newborn screening programs in most countries. Comorbidity of ASD and phenylketonuria was consistently described in the literature in up to 6% of patients.7 The fact that none of the 62 patients with phen- ylketonuria who were treated with the phenylketonuria diet early in life met ASD diagnostic criteria, whereas 5.7% (2 out of 35) of individuals with late diagnosis met diagnostic
  • 83. criteria, underlines the importance of early diagnosis in successful treatment in such metabolic conditions.8 Condi- tions such as mitochondrial disorders, adenylsuccinate lyase and creatine deficiencies may also phenocopy ASD. Accompanying autistic features in these disorders range from 0.4% to 80%.1,9,10 Although mitochondrial disorders can present with autistic features, atypical findings of hypo- tonia, fatigue with activity, failure to thrive, intermittent episodes of regression, especially those after fever and ele- vated plasma lactate concentrations, make diagnosis of the condition more straightforward. Non-specific features such as epilepsy and intellectual disability that accompany autism in the setting of mitochondrial disorders on the other hand, can make the diagnosis more challenging. A recent addition to this group of disorders was made by Novarino et al.11 who reported mutations in the BCKDK gene in the affected individuals from three consan- guineous families who had epilepsy, autistic features, and intellectual disability. The encoded protein is responsible for phosphorylation-mediated inactivation of the E1a sub- unit of branched-chain ketoacid dehydrogenase enzyme. Patients with BCKDK mutations displayed reductions in BCKDK messenger RNA and protein, E1a phosphoryla- tion, and plasma branched-chain amino acids. Bckdk knock- out mice showed abnormal brain amino acid profiles and neurobehavioral deficits that respond to dietary supple- mentation. By supplementing the diet of humans with branched-chain amino acids, the authors were able to nor- malize plasma branched-chain amino acids levels, but the degree to which neurocognitive changes are treatable or reversible remains to be determined. ASD ASSOCIATED WITH RECOGNIZABLE PATTERNS OF MALFORMATIONS CAUSED BY SINGLE GENE DISORDERS