SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 68
Week 8 Homework Exercise
CCMH/525 Version 3
1
University of Phoenix Material
Week 8 Homework Exercise
Answer the following questions in 25 to 50 words each,
covering material from Ch. 14 of Methods in Behavioral
Research:
1. What is replication? What role does it play in increasing the
external validity or generalizability of a study?
2. What is the difference between an exact replication and a
conceptual replication? Provide an example of each.
3. What is meta-analysis? How is it useful?
4. True or false: In many cases, meta-analysis involves
calculating an average effect size for a relationship between
variables. Explain your response.
5. Discuss the advantages and disadvantages of using college
students as research participants.
6. True or false: Many constructs, such as physical
attractiveness and self-esteem, appear constant across cultures;
thus, external validity is less of a concern when conducting
research on such constructs. Explain your response.
Copyright © 2017, 2015 by University of Phoenix. All rights
reserved.
Page 291
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
· Discuss the issues created by generalizing research results to
other populations, including potential problems using college
students as research participants.
· Discuss issues to consider regarding generalization of research
results to other cultures and ethnic groups.
· Describe the potential problem of generalizing to other
experimenters and suggest possible solutions.
· Discuss the importance of replications, distinguishing between
exact replications and conceptual replications.
· Distinguish between narrative literature reviews and meta-
analyses.
Page 292IN THIS CHAPTER, WE WILL CONSIDER THE
ISSUE OF GENERALIZATION OF RESEARCH
FINDINGS. When a single study is conducted with a particular
sample and procedure, can the results then be generalized to
other populations of research participants, or to other ways of
manipulating or measuring the variables? Recall from Chapter
4 that internal validity refers to the ability to infer that there is
a causal relationship between variables. External validity is the
extent to which findings may be generalized.
GENERALIZING TO OTHER POPULATIONS
Even though a researcher may randomly assign participants to
experimental conditions, rarely are participants randomly
selected from the general population. As we noted in Chapters
7 and 9, the individuals who participate in psychological
research are usually selected because they are available, and the
most available population consists of college students—or more
specifically, first- and second-year students enrolled in the
introductory psychology course to satisfy a general education
requirement. They may also be from a particular college or
university, may be volunteers, or may be mostly males or
mostly females. So, are our research findings limited to these
types of subjects, or can we generalize our findings to a more
general population? After considering these issues, we will
examine the larger issue of culture and how research findings
can be generalized to different cultural groups.
College Students
Smart (1966) found that college students were studied in over
70% of the articles published between 1962 and 1964 in
the Journal of Experimental Psychology and the Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology. Sears (1986) reported similar
percentages in 1980 and 1985 in a variety of social psychology
journals; Arnett (2008) found that 67% of the articles in the
2007 volume of the Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology used college student samples. The potential problem
is that such studies use a highly restricted population. Sears
points out that most of the students are first-year students and
sophomores taking the introductory psychology class. They
therefore tend to be young and to possess the characteristics of
emerging adults: a sense of self-identity that is still developing,
social and political attitudes that are in a state of flux, a high
need for peer approval, and unstable peer relationships. They
are intelligent with high cognitive abilities. Thus, what we
know about “people in general” may actually be limited to a
highly select and unusual group. Indeed, Peterson (2001) found
that students, as a group, are more homogenous than nonstudent
samples. That is, students are more similar to each other than
adults are similar to other adults in the general population.
Research by Henry (2008) illustrates how the use of college
students may affect the external validity of research on
prejudice. In his sample of articles Page 293from 1990 to 2005,
an increasing percentage of studies used college students as
participants. Further, in looking at the actual results of studies
on prejudice that compared college students with adults, he
reported a variety of differences among adults and college
students. For example, college students were less conservative
and rated women and ethnic minorities more favorably.
Volunteers
Researchers usually must ask people to volunteer to participate
in their research. At many colleges, introductory psychology
students are required either to volunteer for research or to
complete an alternative project. If you are studying populations
other than college students, you are even more dependent on
volunteers—for example, asking people at a homeowners'
association meeting to participate in a study of marital
interaction or conducting research on the Internet in which
people must go to your web page and then agree to participate
in the study, or conducting a telephone survey of county
residents to determine health care needs. In all these cases,
external validity of the findings may be limited because the data
from volunteers may be different from what would be obtained
with a more general sample. Some research indicates that
volunteers differ in various ways from nonvolunteers. In their
comprehensive study on the topic, Rosenthal and Rosnow
(1975) reported that volunteers tend to be more highly educated,
of a higher socioeconomic status, more in need of approval, and
more social.
Further, different kinds of people volunteer for different kinds
of experiments. In colleges, there may be a sign-up board with
the titles of many studies listed or a web page that manages
research participants and volunteer opportunities for the
university. Different types of people may be drawn to the study
titled “problem solving” than to the one titled “interaction in
small groups.” Available evidence indicates that the title does
influence who signs up (Hood & Back, 1971; Silverman &
Margulis, 1973).
Online Research
Another important consideration arises when asking participants
to volunteer for online surveys and experiments. Researchers
can find potential participants through online survey design
services. Psychologists are increasingly using Amazon
Mechanical Turk (https://www.mturk.com; Jacquet, 2011), a
website for recruiting people to work on many types of tasks
including participating in research for a specified payment. This
sort of sampling strategy has important implications for external
validity. While the online sample is more diverse than the
typical college student sample, there are still generalization
issues because Internet users represent a unique demographic.
The Pew Research Center's Internet and American Life Project
(Pew Internet, 2010) found that living in an urban/suburban
area, being college educated, being younger, and having a
higher income are all related to reporting more time online.
Thus, by asking Page 294for volunteers for an online survey,
researchers are sampling from a particular demographic that
may not generalize well to the population of interest.
Gender
Sometimes, researchers use only males or only females (or a
very disproportionate ratio of males to females) simply because
this is convenient or the procedures seem better suited to a
particular gender. Given the possible differences between males
and females, however, the results of such studies may not be
generalizable (Denmark, Russo, Frieze, & Sechzer, 1988).
Denmark et al. provide an example of studies on contraception
practices that use only females because of stereotypical
assumptions that only females are responsible for contraception.
They also point out several other ways that gender bias may
arise in psychological research, including confounding gender
with age or job status and selecting response measures that are
gender-stereotyped. The solution is to be aware of possible
gender differences and include both males and females in our
research investigations. Moreover, it is important to recognize
the ways that males and females might differentially interpret
independent variable manipulations or questions asked in a
questionnaire.
Locale
The location that participants are recruited from can also have
an impact on a study's external validity. Participants in one
locale may differ from participants in another locale. For
example, students at UCLA may differ from students at a nearby
state university, who in turn may differ from students at a
community college. People in Iowa may differ from people in
New York City. Thus, a finding obtained with the students in
one type of educational setting or in one geographic region may
not generalize to people in other settings or regions. In fact,
studies have explored how personality traits like extraversion
(the tendency to seek social stimulation) and openness to new
experiences vary across geographic areas. Rentfrow, Gosling,
and Potter (2008) looked at geographic differences in
personality traits among citizens of various U.S. states and
found extraversion to vary by state. People in midwestern states
tended to be more extraverted than people in northeastern
states, and people in western states tended to be more open to
new experiences. Thus, a study conducted in one location may
not generalize well to another, particularly if the variables in
question are related to location in some way.
Culture
Whether theories and research findings generalize across
cultures is a critically important issue. Some observers of
current psychological research have been very critical of the
types of samples employed in behavioral research. Based on
analyses of published research by Arnett (2008) and others,
Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010) contend that
psychology is built on the study of WEIRD Page 295(Western,
Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic) people. In many
cases, research samples consist primarily of college students
from the United States, other English-speaking countries, and
Europe. Ultimately, researchers wish to discover aspects of
human behavior that have universal applications but in fact
cannot generalize beyond their limited samples. This is, at its
heart, a critique of the external validity of behavioral research:
Does our human behavioral research generalize to all humans,
or is it really a study of the WEIRD?
Clearly, if psychologists want to understand human behavior,
they must understand human behavior across and among
cultures (Henrich et al., 2010; Miller, 1999). Miller described
research on self-concept by Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, and
Norasakkunkit (1997) to illustrate the benefits of incorporating
culture into psychological theory. Traditional theories of self-
concept are grounded in the culture of the United States and
Western Europe; the “self” is an individualistic concept where
people are independent from others and self-enhancement comes
from individual achievements. Kitayama and his colleagues take
a broader, cultural perspective: In contrast to the U.S. meaning
of self, in other cultures the “self” is a collective concept in
which self-esteem is derived from relationships with others.
Often, Japanese engage in self-criticism, which can be seen as
relationship-maintaining, whereas Americans work to maintain
and enhance self-esteem—thus, very different activities
contribute to a positive self-concept in the two cultures
(Kitayama et al., 1997). This is a very common theme in
research that incorporates culture in psychological processes:
“The significance of self-esteem, however, may be much more
specific to a culture than has typically been supposed in the
literature” (p. 1262).
Much of this cultural research centers on identifying similarities
and differences that may exist in personality and other
psychological characteristics, as well as ways that individuals
from different cultures respond to the same environments
(Matsumoto, 1994). Research by Kim, Sherman, and Taylor
(2008) provides another example of the limits of external
validity across cultural groups. This research focused on how
people from different cultures use social support to cope with
stress. In reviewing the research on the topic, they concluded
that Asians and Asian Americans might benefit from different
styles of social support as compared with European Americans.
For example, Asian Americans are more likely to benefit from
support that does not involve the sort of intense disclosure of
personal stressful events and feelings that is the hallmark of
support in many European American groups. Rather, they
suggest that Asians and Asian Americans may benefit more
from support that comes with the comforts of proximity (being
with close friends) rather than sharing.
These examples all focused on differences among cultures.
Many studies also find similarities across cultures. Evolutionary
psychologists, for instance, often conduct studies in different
cultural groups because they are looking for similarities across
cultures in order to see if a particular behavior or attitude can
be tied to our evolutionary past. For example, Singh, Dixson,
Jessop, Morgan, and Dixson (2010) wanted to see if a particular
aspect of beauty that is tied to greater reproductive success—
namely waist-to-hip ratio (e.g., the ratio for Page 296a 25-inch
waist and 35-inch hips is .71), which is related to sex hormones
and thus fertility—would be seen as attractive across cultures.
Diverse groups from Africa, Samoa, Indonesia, and New
Zealand evaluated photographs of females with small and large
waist-to-hip ratios. The researchers found that indeed, low
waist-to-hip ratio among females was seen as more attractive
across all these groups. In this example, the results obtained in
one culture do generalize to other cultures.
Nonhuman Animals
We noted in Chapter 3 that about 7% of psychological research
is conducted with nonhuman animals. Almost all of this
research is done with rats, mice, and birds. Most research with
other species is conducted to study the behavior of those
animals directly to gather information that may help with the
survival of endangered species and increase our understanding
of our bonds with nonhuman animals such as dogs, cats, and
horses (http://www.apa-hai.org/human-animal-interaction).
The basic research that psychologists conduct with nonhuman
animals is usually done with the expectation that the findings
can be generalized to humans. This research is important
because the research problems that are addressed require
procedures such as long-term observation that could not be done
with human samples. We do expect that we can generalize as
our underlying biological and behavioral patterns are shared. In
fact, the value of studying nonhuman animals has been
demonstrated by research that does apply to humans. These
applications include the biological bases of memory, food
preferences, sexual behavior, choice behavior, and drug
addictions. The American Psychological Association has
prepared a brochure on animal research:
(http://www.apa.org/research/responsible/research-animals.pdf).
In Defense of College Students
It is easy to criticize research on the basis of subject
characteristics, yet criticism by itself does not mean that results
cannot be generalized. Although we need to be concerned about
the potential problems of generalizing from unique populations
such as college students (cf. Sears, 1986), we should also keep
several things in mind when thinking about this issue. First,
criticisms of the use of any particular type of subject, such as
college students, in a study should be backed with good reasons
that a relationship would not be found with other types of
subjects. College students, after all, are human, and researchers
should not be blamed for not worrying about generalization to a
particular type of subject if there is no good reason to do so.
Moreover, college student bodies are increasingly diverse and
increasingly representative of the society as a whole (although
college students will always be characterized as having the
ability and motivation to pursue a college degree). Second,
replication of research studies provides a safeguard against the
limited external validity of a single study. Studies are replicated
at other colleges using different mixes of students, and Page
297many findings first established with college students are
later applied to other populations, such as children, aging
adults, and people in other countries. It is also worth noting that
Internet samples are increasingly used in many types of studies.
Although such studies raise their own issues of external
validity, they frequently complement studies based on college
student samples.
GENERALIZING ACROSS METHODS
The person who actually conducts the experiment is the source
of another external validity problem. In most research, only one
experimenter is used, and rarely is much attention paid to the
personal characteristics of the experimenter (McGuigan, 1963).
The main goal is to make sure that any influence the
experimenter has on subjects is constant throughout the
experiment. There is always the possibility, however, that the
results are generalizable only to certain types of experimenters.
Some of the important characteristics of experimenters have
been discussed by Kintz and his colleagues (Kintz, Delprato,
Mettee, Persons, & Schappe, 1965). These include the
experimenter's personality and gender and the amount of
practice in the role of experimenter. A warm, friendly
experimenter will almost certainly produce different results
from a cold, unfriendly experimenter. Participants also may
behave differently with male and female experimenters. It has
even been shown that rabbits learn faster when trained by
experienced experimenters (Brogden, 1962)! The influence of
the experimenter may depend as well on the characteristics of
the participants. For example, participants seem to perform
better when tested by an experimenter of the other sex
(Stevenson & Allen, 1964).
One solution to the problem of generalizing to other
experimenters is to use two or more experimenters. A fine
example of the use of multiple experimenters is a study by
Rubin (1975), who sent several male and female experimenters
to the Boston airport to investigate self-disclosure. The
experimenters revealed different kinds of information about
themselves to both male and female travelers and recorded the
passengers' self-disclosures in return. One interesting result was
that women tended to reveal more about themselves to male
experimenters, and men tended to reveal more about themselves
to female experimenters.
Pretests and Generalization
Researchers are often faced with the decision of whether to give
a pretest. Intuitively, pretesting seems to be a good idea. The
researcher can be sure that the groups are equivalent on the
pretest, and it is often more satisfying to see that individuals
changed their scores than it is to look only at group means on a
posttest. A pretest also enables the researcher to assess
mortality (attrition) effects when it is likely that some
participants will withdraw from an experiment. Page 298If you
give a pretest, you can determine whether the people who
withdrew are different from those who completed the study.
Pretesting, however, may limit the ability to generalize to
populations that did not receive a pretest. (cf. Lana, 1969).
Simply taking the pretest may cause subjects to behave
differently than they would without the pretest. Recall
from Chapter 8 that a Solomon four-group design (Solomon,
1949) can be used in situations in which a pretest is desirable
but there is concern over the possible impact of taking the
pretest. In the Solomon four-group design, half of the
participants are given the pretest; the other half receive the
posttest only. That is, the same experiment is conducted with
and without the pretest. Mortality effects can be assessed in the
pretest conditions. Also, the researcher can examine whether
there is an interaction between the independent variable and the
pretest: Are posttest scores on the dependent variable different
depending on whether the pretest was given? Sometimes,
researchers find that it is not feasible to conduct the study with
all four groups in a single experiment. In this case, the first
study can include the pretest; the study can be replicated later
without the pretest.
Generalizing from Laboratory Settings
Research conducted in a laboratory setting has the advantage of
allowing the experimenter to study the impact of independent
variables under highly controlled conditions. The internal
validity of the research is the primary consideration. The
question arises, however, as to whether the artificiality of the
laboratory setting limits the ability to generalize what is
observed in the laboratory to real-life settings.
Mook (1983) articulated one response to the artificiality issue:
Generalization to real-life settings is not relevant when the
purpose of the study was to investigate causal relationships
under carefully controlled conditions. Mook is concerned that a
“knee-jerk” criticism of laboratory research on the basis of
external validity is too common. Good research is what is most
important.
Another response to the laboratory artificiality criticism is to
examine the results of field experiments. Recall from Chapter
4 that in a field experiment, the researcher manipulates the
independent variable in a natural setting—a factory, a school, or
a street corner, for example.
Anderson, Lindsay, and Bushman (1999) asked whether
laboratory and field experiments that examine the same
variables do in fact produce the same results. To answer this
question, they found 38 pairs of studies for which a laboratory
investigation had a field experiment counterpart. The studies
were drawn from a variety of research areas including
aggression, helping, memory, leadership style, and depression.
Results of the laboratory and field experiments were in fact
very similar—the effect size of the independent variable on the
dependent variable was very similar in the two types of studies.
Thus, even though lab and field experiments are conducted in
different settings, the results Page 299are complementary rather
than contradictory. When findings are replicated using multiple
methods, our confidence in the external validity of the findings
increases.
SUPPORTING GOOD EXTERNAL VALIDITY
It may seem as if no research can possibly be generalizable! In
some ways, this is true. Furthermore, it can be very difficult to
understand the extent to which a study is generalizable; external
validity is an aspect of a study that we try to assess, but cannot
truly know. How, then, can we support good external validity?
There are a few ways that external validity can be supported.
The key way that external validity can be supported is related to
a study's methodology. Using a census, or a random sample will
always produce better external validity than using a nonrandom
sample. This, of course, is not always possible. Next, we will
explore a few other ways in which external validity can be
supported.
Generalization as a Statistical Interaction
The problem of generalization can be thought of as an
interaction in a factorial design (see Chapter 10). An interaction
occurs when a relationship between variables exists under one
condition but not another or when the nature of the relationship
is different in one condition than in another. Thus, if you
question the generalizability of a study that used only males,
you are suggesting that there is an interaction between gender
and the independent variable. Suppose, for example, that a
study examines the relationship between crowding and
aggression among males and reports that crowding is associated
with higher levels of aggression. You might then question
whether the results are generalizable to females.
Figure 14.1 shows four potential outcomes of a hypothetical
study on crowding and aggression that tested both males and
females. In each graph, the relationship between crowding and
aggression for males has been maintained. In Graph A, there is
no interaction—the behavior of males and females is virtually
identical. Thus, the results of the original all-male study could
be generalized to females. In Graph B, there is also no
interaction; the effect of crowding is identical for males and
females. However, in this graph, males are more aggressive than
females. Although such a difference is interesting, it is not a
factor in generalization because the overall relationship between
crowding and aggression is present for both males and females.
Graphs C and D do show interactions. In both, the original
results with males cannot be generalized to females. In Graph
C, there is no relationship between crowding and aggression for
females. In Graph D, the interaction tells us that a positive
relationship between crowding and aggression exists for males
but that a negative relationship exists for females. As it turns
out, Graph D describes the results of several studies on this
topic (cf. Freedman, Levy, Buchanan, & Price, 1972).
Page 300
FIGURE 14.1
Outcomes of a hypothetical experiment on crowding and
aggression
Note: The presence of an interaction indicates that the results
for males cannot be generalized to females.
Researchers can address issues of external validity that stem
from the use of different populations by including subject type
as a variable in the study. By including variables such as
gender, age, or ethnic group in the design of the study, the
results may be analyzed to determine whether there are
interaction effects like the ones illustrated in Figure 14.1.
The Importance of Replications
Replication of research is a way of overcoming any problems of
generalization that occur in a single study. There are two types
of replications to consider: exact replications and conceptual
replications.
Exact replications An exact replication is an attempt to replicate
precisely the procedures of a study to see whether the same
results are obtained. A researcher who obtains an unexpected
finding will frequently attempt Page 301a replication to make
sure that the finding is reliable. If you are starting your own
work on a problem, you may try to replicate a crucial study to
make sure that you understand the procedures and can obtain the
same results. Often, exact replications occur when a researcher
builds on the findings of a prior study. For example, suppose
you are intrigued by Singh et al.'s (2010) research on waist-to-
hip ratio that was mentioned previously. Singh reports that
males rate females with a ratio of .70 as most attractive. In your
research, you might replicate the procedures used in the original
study and expand on the original research. For example, you
might study this phenomenon with males similar to those in the
original sample as well as males from different cultures or age
groups. When you replicate the original research findings using
very similar procedures, your confidence in the external validity
of the original findings is increased.
The “Mozart effect” provides us with an interesting example of
the importance of replications. In the original study by
Rauscher, Shaw, and Ky (1993), college students listened to 10
minutes of a Mozart sonata. These students then showed better
performance on a spatial-reasoning measure drawn from the
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale than students exposed to a
relaxation tape or simple silence. This finding received a great
deal of attention in the press as people quickly generalized it to
the possibility of increasing children's intelligence with Mozart
sonatas. In fact, one state governor began producing Mozart
CDs to distribute in maternity wards, and entrepreneurs began
selling Mozart kits to parents over the Internet. Over the next
few years, however, there were many failures to replicate the
Mozart effect (see Steele, Bass, & Crook, 1999). We noted
above that failures to replicate may occur because the exact
conditions for producing the effect were not used. In this case,
Rauscher and Shaw (1998) responded to the many replication
failures by precisely describing the conditions necessary to
produce the Mozart effect. However, Steele et al. (1999) and
McCutcheon (2000) were unable to obtain the effect even
though they followed the recommendations of Rauscher and
Shaw. Research on the Mozart effect continues. Some recent
findings suggest that the effect is limited to music that also
increases arousal; it is this arousal that can cause better
performance following exposure to the music (Thompson,
Schellenberg, & Husain, 2001). Bangerter and Heath (2004)
present a detailed analysis of the development of the research
on the Mozart effect.
A single failure to replicate does not reveal much, though; it is
unrealistic to assume, on the basis of a single failure to
replicate, that the previous research is necessarily invalid.
Failures to replicate share the same problems as nonsignificant
results, discussed in Chapter 13. A failure to replicate could
mean that the original results are invalid, but it could also mean
that the replication attempt was flawed. For example, if the
replication is based on the procedure as reported in a journal
article, it is possible that the article omitted an important aspect
of the procedure. For this reason, it is usually a good idea to
write to the researcher to obtain detailed information on all of
the materials that were used in the study.
Page 302Several scientific societies are encouraging systematic
replications of important scientific findings. The
journal Perspectives on Psychological Science (published by the
Association for Psychological Science) is sponsoring the
publication of Registered Research Replications
(http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/replication).
Multiple groups of researchers will undertake replications of
important studies using procedures that are made public before
initiating the research. When completed, all of the replications
will be described in a single report. In addition to the
Psychological Science initiative, the online journal PLOS
ONE (Public Library of Science) has developed the
Reproducibility Initiative to encourage independent replication
of research in the clinical sciences (Pattinson, 2012). Such
developments should lead to greater understanding of the
generalizability of research findings.
Conceptual replications A conceptual replication is the use of
different procedures to replicate a research finding. In a
conceptual replication, researchers attempt to understand the
relationships among abstract conceptual variables by using new,
or different, operational definitions of those variables.
Conceptual replications are even more important than exact
replications in furthering our understanding of behavior.
In most research, a key goal is to discover whether a
relationship between conceptual variables exists. In the original
Mozart effect study, researchers examined the effect
of exposure to classical music on spatial reasoning. These are
conceptual variables; in the actual study, specific operational
definitions of the variables were used. Exposure to classical
music was operationalized as 10 minutes of exposure to the
Mozart Sonata for Two Pianos in D Major. Spatial
reasoning was operationalized as performance on a particular
spatial reasoning measure.
In a conceptual replication, the same independent variable is
operationalized in a different way, and the dependent variable
may be measured in a different way, too. Conceptual
replications are extremely important in the social sciences
because the variables used are complex and can be
operationalized in different ways. Complete understanding of
any variable involves studying the variable using a variety of
operational definitions. A crucial generalization question is
whether the relationship holds when other ways of manipulating
or measuring the variables are studied. Sometimes the
conceptual replication may involve an alternative stimulus (e.g.,
a different Mozart sonata, a selection by a different composer)
or an alternative dependent measure (e.g., a different spatial-
reasoning task). Or as we previously noted, the same variables
are sometimes studied in both laboratory and field settings.
When conceptual replications produce similar results, our
confidence in the generalizability of relationships between
variables is greatly increased.
This discussion should also alert you to an important way of
thinking about research findings. The findings represent
relationships between conceptual variables but are grounded in
specific operations. You may read about the specific methods
employed in a study conducted 20 years ago and question Page
303whether the study could be replicated today. You might also
speculate that the methods used in a study are so unusual that
they could never generalize to other situations. These concerns
are not as serious when placed within the context of conceptual
replications because, although operational definitions can
change over time, the underlying conceptual variable often
remains more consistent. Admittedly, a specific method from a
study conducted at one time might not be effective today, given
changes in today's political and cultural climate. A conceptual
replication of the manipulation, however, would demonstrate
that the relationship between the conceptual theoretical
variables is still present. Similarly, the narrow focus of a
particular study is less problematic if the general finding is
replicated with different procedures.
Evaluating Generalizations via Literature Reviews and Meta-
analyses
Researchers have traditionally drawn conclusions about the
external validity of research findings by conducting literature
reviews. In a literature review, a reviewer reads a number of
studies that address a particular topic and then writes a paper
that summarizes and evaluates the literature. The Publication
Manual of the American Psychological Association provides the
following description: “Literature reviews, including research
syntheses and meta-analyses, are critical evaluations of material
that has already been published.… By organizing, integrating,
and evaluating previously published material, authors of
literature reviews consider the progress of research toward
clarifying a problem” (APA, 2010, p. 10). The literature review
provides information that (1) summarizes what has been found,
(2) tells the reader which findings are strongly supported and
which are only weakly supported in the literature, (3) points out
inconsistent findings and areas in which research is lacking, and
(4) discusses future directions for research.
Sometimes a review will be a narrative in which the author
provides descriptions of research findings and draws
conclusions about the literature. The conclusions in a narrative
literature review are based on the subjective impressions of the
reviewer. Another technique for comparing a large number of
studies in an area is meta-analysis (Borenstein, Hedges,
Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). In a meta-analysis, the researcher
combines the actual results of a number of studies. The analysis
consists of a set of statistical procedures that employ effect
sizes to compare a given finding across many different studies.
Instead of relying on judgments obtained in a narrative
literature review, you can draw statistical conclusions from this
material. The statistical procedures need not concern you. They
involve examining several features of the results of studies,
including the effect sizes and significance levels obtained. The
important point here is that meta-analysis is a method for
determining the reliability of a finding by examining the results
from many different studies.
Stewart and Chambless (2009) conducted a meta-analysis of
research on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy
(CBT) for anxiety disorders. Page 304Both a traditional
literature review and a meta-analysis begin with a body of
previous research on a topic; in this case, Stewart and
Chambless located 56 studies using CBT with adults diagnosed
with an anxiety disorder (including panic disorder, social
anxiety, post traumatic stress disorder, generalized anxiety
disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder). Studies that
included an additional medication treatment were excluded. The
researchers performed a statistical analysis of the results of
these studies and concluded that CBT was effective in treating
all of the types of anxiety disorders. In a traditional literature
review, it can be difficult to provide the type of general
conclusion that was reached with the meta-analysis because it is
necessary to integrate information from many studies with
different experimental designs, disorders, and measures of
anxiety.
One of the most important reasons a meta-analysis can lead to
clear conclusions is that meta-analysis studies focus on effect
size (recall that an effect size represents the extent to which two
variables are associated, see page 256). A typical table in a
meta-analysis will show the effect size obtained in a number of
studies along with a summary of the average effect size across
the studies. More important, the analysis allows comparisons of
the effect sizes in different types of studies to allow tests of
hypotheses. For example, Miller and Downey (1999) analyzed
the results of 71 studies that examined the relationship between
weight and self-esteem. Table 14.1 shows a few of the findings.
The effect size r averaged across all studies was −.18: Heavier
weight is associated with lower self-esteem. However, several
variables moderate the relationship between weight and self-
esteem. Thus, the effect size is larger when the weight variable
is a report of self-perceived rather than actual weight, and the
relationship between weight and self-esteem is somewhat larger
for females than for males. Finally, the effect is greater among
individuals with a high socioeconomic background.
TABLE 14.1 Some meta-analysis findings for weight and self-
esteem
Page 305Both narrative reviews and meta-analyses provide
valuable information and in fact are often complementary. A
meta-analysis allows statistical, quantitative conclusions
whereas a narrative review identifies trends in the literature and
directions for future study—a more qualitative approach. A
study by Bushman and Wells (2001) points to an interesting way
in which knowledge of meta-analysis can improve the way that
we interpret information for literature reviews.
The reviewers in their study were undergraduates who were
provided with both titles and information about the findings of
20 studies dealing with the effect of attitude similarity on
attraction. Sometimes the titles were salient with respect to the
findings (“Birds of a Feather Flock Together”) and others were
nonsalient (“Research Studies Who Likes Whom”). Salient titles
are obviously easier to remember. When asked to draw
conclusions about the findings, naive reviewers with no
knowledge of meta-analysis overestimated the size of the
similarity–attraction relationship when provided with salient
titles. Other reviewers were given brief training in meta-
analysis; these reviewers drew accurate conclusions about the
actual findings. That is, they were not influenced by the article
title. Thus, even without conducting a meta-analysis, a
background in meta-analysis can be beneficial when reviewing
research findings.
USING RESEARCH TO IMPROVE LIVES
In a presidential address to the American Psychological
Association, George Miller (1969) discussed “psychology as a
means of promoting human welfare” and spoke of “giving
psychology away.” Miller was addressing the broadest issue of
generalization, taking what we know about human behavior and
allowing it to be applied by many people in all areas of
everyday life. Zimbardo's (2004) presidential address to the
American Psychological Association described many ways in
which Miller's call to give psychology away is being honored.
The impact of psychological research can be seen in areas such
as health (programs to promote health-related behaviors related
to stress, heart disease, and sexually transmitted diseases), law
and criminal justice (providing data on the effects of 6- versus
12-person juries and showing how law enforcement personnel
can improve the accuracy of eyewitness identification),
education (providing methods for encouraging academic
performance or reducing conflict among different ethnic
groups), and work environments (providing workers with more
control and improving the ways that people interact with
computers and other machines in the workplace). In addition,
psychologists are using the Internet to provide the public with
information on parenting, education, mental health, and Page
306many other topics—for example, the websites of the
American Psychological Association and the Association for
Psychological Science
(http://www.apa.org; http://www.psychologicalscience.org),
national mental health resource websites
(http://www.mentalhealth.gov/ and http://www.samhsa.gov/),
and many individual psychologists who are sharing their
expertise with the public.
We have discussed only a few of the ways that basic research
has been applied to improve people's lives. Despite all the
potential problems of generalizing research findings that were
highlighted in this chapter, the evidence suggests that we can
generalize our findings to many aspects of our lives.
ILLUSTRATIVE ARTICLE: GENERALIZING RESULTS
Driving around in a 4,000-pound automobile is a dangerous
thing. Motor vehicle accidents are among the leading
preventable causes of death in the United States every year.
Distraction is one of the most common causes of automobile
accidents, and talking to another person is a very common
distraction.
In an effort to observe the impact of conversation on driving,
Drews, Pasupathi, and Strayer (2008) conducted a study using a
driving simulator that tracks errors committed by drivers. The
researchers varied the type of conversation. In one condition,
participants had a conversation with a passenger; in another
condition, participants talked on a cell phone. There was also a
no conversation, control condition. As you would expect,
having any conversation resulted in more driving errors.
However, the number of driving errors was highest in the cell
phone condition.
For this exercise, acquire and read the article:
Drews, F., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. (2008). Passenger and
cell phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 392–400.
doi:10.1037/a0013119
After reading the article, consider the following:
1. Describe how well you think the sample of participants in
this study generalizes to other groups of people. What about
age? What about sex?
2. In this study, participants were told to have a conversation
about a time when “their lives were threatened.” Do you think
that the results of this study would be different if the
conversation were about something else? How so? Why?
3. Do you think that the findings from this study would
generalize to other cultures? Do you think that a sample of
college students in Mexico, Italy, and Germany would generate
similar results? Why or why not?
4. How well do you think the driving simulator generalizes to
real-world driving? What would you change to improve the
generalizability of the simulator?Page 307
5. Evaluate the internal validity of this study. Explain your
answer.
6. Evaluate the external validity of this study. Explain your
answer.
Study Terms
Conceptual replication (p. 302)
Exact replication (p. 300)
External validity (p. 292)
Literature review (p. 303)
Meta-analysis (p. 303)
Replication (p. 300)
Solomon four-group design (p. 298)
Review Questions
1. What is external validity?
2. Why should a researcher be concerned about generalizing to
other populations?
3. How can the fact that most studies are conducted with
college students, volunteers, and individuals from a limited
location and culture potentially impact external validity?
4. How does the use of the Internet to recruit subjects and
collect data impact external validity?
5. What is the source of the problem of generalizing to other
experimenters? How can this problem be solved?
6. Why is it important to pretest a problem for generalization?
Discuss the reasons why including a pretest may affect the
ability to generalize results.
7. Distinguish between an exact replication and a conceptual
replication. What is the value of a conceptual replication?
8. What is a meta-analysis?
Activities
1. It is easy to collect data for experiments and surveys on the
Internet. Anyone in the world who is connected to the Internet
can participate in an online experiment or survey. Use a search
term such as “psychological research on the Internet” to find
some studies that are being conducted. Page 308What issues of
generalization might arise when interpreting the results of such
studies? Does the computer aspect of the research make this
research less generalizable than traditional research, or does the
fact that people throughout the world can participate make it
more generalizable? Could you empirically answer this
question?
2. Use PsycINFO to find abstracts of articles that included
race, ethnicity, gender, or nationality as a key variable.
Consider topics such as body image, rumination, academic
achievement, or identity development. What conclusions do the
authors of these studies draw about generalization?
3. Find a meta-analysis published in a journal; two good
sources are the Review of Educational
Research and Psychological Bulletin. What conclusions were
drawn from the meta-analysis? How were studies selected for
the analysis? How was the concept of effect size discussed in
the meta-analysis?
ChapterResearch Training: Joys and Challenges
It takes many years to become truly skilled at a complex set of
tasks, such as becoming a skilled potter. For example, in Japan,
it is common for an apprentice potter to make the same vase
form for many months just to acquire the specific skills to
consistently make 15–20 identical vases in a row. Expertise
develops over time within an environment that fosters
development.
In this chapter we discuss the process of becoming a skilled
researcher and the type of training environments that seem to be
most helpful for students to acquire the necessary skills and
attitudes. For many students in counseling, graduate school is
their first introduction to research and research design, and it
evokes many emotional reactions that come with any novel
experience, from joy and excitement to anxiety and
disenchantment. We seek to prepare students to approach
research with enthusiasm for the creativity involved and with a
willingness to learn the intricacies of the craft and the joy of
creating new knowledge for the profession. In addition, we want
to promote an awareness of the anxiety that may be created by
learning a technical skill that may not be central to one's
interests but is required to accomplish a goal (that is, obtaining
a graduate degree and becoming a professional in counseling).
We address four topics in this chapter. First, we provide a brief
overview of how science intersects with counseling and
psychology training through the scientist-practitioner model and
the topic of evidence-based practice. Second, we identify and
discuss some issues related to the developmental process of
acquiring research competencies, specifically the joy as well as
the challenges and fears. Third, we discuss ways in which
counselors and counseling psychologists can train others to
become competent, eager, and productive researchers. In this
part of the chapter, we discuss what is known about research
training, and how training environments can be structured to
create the opportunity for students both to learn about research,
and to consume and produce quality research products. Fourth,
we discuss ways to broaden the concept of scientific training to
include scientific thinking skills as well as research application
skills. Most important, throughout this chapter we emphasize
the developmental process of acquiring skills step by step to
become a skilled researcher and scientist.SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE
The Scientist-Practitioner Model
Bridging the gap between research and practice in counseling is
an important issue (Murray, 2009). As we mention in Chapter 1,
many graduate training programs in counseling espouse
the scientist-practitioner model of training. Basically, this
model consists of training in both scientific and practitioner
activities; the basic assumption is that students trained in both
science and practice will be better prepared for the multitude of
employment demands in the counseling profession. The
scientific activities include courses that focus on the philosophy
of science, qualitative and quantitative designs and methods,
statistics, evaluation, counseling research literature, and often
involvement in research projects (Larson & Besett-Alesch,
2000). The practice-oriented side includes courses such as
counseling methods, counseling theories, personality,
assessment, and involvement in a variety of practicum
experiences. When students enter a graduate training program in
counseling, they typically have a wide range of interests along
with the scientist-practitioner continuum. These interests often
change over time, not only during graduate training but also
throughout their career; thus, students need to prepare
themselves broadly to allow for career changes over time.
The scientist-practitioner model goes back to over 60 years. The
clinical psychologists developed what they dubbed the scientist-
practitioner model of training, which is also referred to as the
Boulder model (Raimy, 1950).
Meara et al. (1988) succinctly captured the scientist-practitioner
model:
Psychologists, whatever their work, are professionals and their
attitude toward their work is scientific. The scientist-
professional model is an integrated approach to knowledge that
recognizes the interdependence of theory, research, and
practice. The model emphasizes systematic and thoughtful
analyses of human experiences and judicious application of the
knowledge and attitudes gained from such analyses. An attitude
of scholarly inquiry is critical to all the activities of those
educated in the scientist-professional model. The model
encompasses a variety of research methods, assessment
techniques, and intervention strategies. The counseling
psychologist is engaged in the pursuit and application of
psychological knowledge to promote optimal development for
individuals, groups, and systems (including families), and to
provide remedies for the psychological difficulties that
encumber them. To implement these goals, the scientist-
professional psychologist adopts a scientific approach based on
observation of psychological phenomena. This approach
generates theoretical constructs and propositions, which are in
turn tested as hypotheses (Claiborn, 1987; Pepinsky &
Pepinsky, 1954). (p. 368)
The scientist-practitioner model has been the predominant
model of most counseling psychology programs with 73% of the
programs specifically referring to the scientist-practitioner
model in their program description (Horn et al., 2007).
However, over the years there have been some critiques,
especially in the earlier years about the utility of the scientist-
practitioner model, which students may also have concerns
about today. These included questions such as whether most
practitioners use research findings in their practice. Moreover,
it could also be that graduate training in practitioner activities
does not adequately incorporate scientific activities, and vice
versa (e.g., Stoltenberg et al., 2000). Some people might think
that perhaps the type of research being conducted is too distant
from the reality of the practitioner (see, e.g., Howard, 1984), or
perhaps our research methods reduce counseling phenomena to
meaningless numbers (see, e.g., Goldman, 1976). Yet another
factor could be that students admitted to graduate programs
have predominantly social or interpersonal interests (see, e.g.,
Magoon & Holland, 1984).
Therefore, other training models have emerged. These models
share the core goals of integrating science and practice, but with
different emphasis. For example, the practitioner-scholar
model places more emphasis on the training of practitioners and
was closely associated with the emergence of professional
schools of psychology and Psy.D. degrees. This model also
parallels the idea of local clinical scientists (Stricker &
Trierweiler, 1995) and practice-based evidence (Barkham &
Mellor-Clark, 2000) that emphasize using clinical work and
observations of clients to collect data and test clinical
hypotheses. On the other end of the science-practice training
spectrum, the clinical science model (Academy of Psychological
Clinical Science, 2014) is similar to the scientist-practitioner
model in theory, but places even more emphasis on science and
uses empirical approaches to advance clinical science.
The importance of science in the psychology field has not
diminished, but rather has grown over the past few decades.
There has been increased emphasis on utilizing scientific
findings in mental health practices. Thus, it is important for
students to have the scientific skills to critically evaluate
research findings rather than simply accepting knowledge from
research as authoritative prescriptions (Stoltenberg et al., 2000).
Integrating the scientist-practitioner model with current trends
in the mental health field is important, and there are
recommendations on how the scientist-practitioner model can be
better implemented (see Stoltenberg et al., 2000). Chwalisz
(2003) suggested that evidence-based practice is the framework
for training scientist-practitioners in the 21st century. The
evidence-based practice movement in psychology has become a
main focus over the past decade.
Evidence-Based Practice
The increased demands from health care systems and policies to
integrate science and psychology practice have made evidence-
based practice an important movement in the psychology and
mental health field (Baker, McFall, & Shoham, 2008).
Evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP) is “the
integration of the best available research with clinical expertise
in the context of patient characteristics, culture, and
preferences.” (APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based
Practice, 2006, p. 273). The field of medicine is an example of
how EBPP has shaped the improvement of patient outcomes in
utilizing research in clinical practice (Woolf & Atkins, 2001).
EBPP is a more comprehensive concept than a closely related
movement of establishing empirically supported
treatments (ESTs), which refers to the specific treatment that
works for certain disorders or problems. Whereas ESTs focus on
the treatment approach, EBPP starts with the client and with the
attempt to utilize research evidence to inform treatment to
achieve the best possible outcome for the client. Moreover,
EBPP includes a broader scope of clinical/counseling aspects,
such as assessment, case formulation, counseling relationships,
etc.
In APA's 2006 Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based
Practice, research evidence, clinical expertise, and patient
characteristics were all emphasized as critical components
leading to best practices. In terms of this research evidence, a
broad array of scientific results related to intervention
strategies, assessment, clinical problems, and client populations
are all included. Multiple types of research evidence such as
efficacy, effectiveness, cost effectiveness, cost benefit,
epidemiological, and treatment utilization are all recognized as
contributors to informing best psychological practices. EBPP
also requires mental health practitioners to recognize that
treatment method (Nathan & Gorman, 2007),
counselors/therapists (Wampold, 2001), therapy relationship
(Norcross, 2002), and the client (Bohart & Tallman, 1999) are
all critical components to achieve best outcomes in
psychological practices, and therefore research evidence on any
of these aspects should not be overlooked.
Clinical expertise is also a critical component of EBPP (APA
Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice, 2006).
Counselors and psychologists are recognized as essential
contributors in identifying the best research evidence based on
the client's characteristics, background, presenting issue, and
preferences. Thus, it is important that counselors and
psychologists are trained not only as practitioners but also as
scientists so that their practice is informed by scientific
literature. Client characteristics, culture, and preferences are
also important parts of EBPP. Counselors and psychologists
should take into account what works for whom when providing
guidelines for effective practice. EBPP requires attention to
client characteristics such as gender, sexual orientation, culture,
ethnicity, race, age, religion, nationality, disability status, etc.
In addition to client characteristics, it is also important to
consider the client's values, religious beliefs, worldviews,
goals, and preferences for treatment when determining and
utilizing the best available treatment for the client.
Although APA and many training programs have supported
EBPP, there has also been resistance toward EBPP. Similar to
the critiques of the scientist-practitioner model, there have been
several reasons that EBPP raises concerns. Lilienfeld and
colleagues (2013) examined the roots of resistance to EBPP by
clinical psychologists and summarized six sources:
(a) naïve realism, which can lead clinicians to conclude
erroneously that client change is due to an intervention itself
rather than to a host of competing explanations; (b) deep-seated
misconceptions regarding human nature (e.g., mistaken beliefs
regarding the causal primacy of early experiences) that can
hinder the adoption of evidence-based treatments; (c) statistical
misunderstandings regarding the application of group
probabilities to individuals; (d) erroneous apportioning of the
burden of proof on skeptics rather than proponents of untested
therapies; (e) widespread mischaracterizations of what EBP
entails; and (f) pragmatic, educational, and attitudinal obstacles,
such as the discomfort of many practitioners with evaluating the
increasingly technical psychotherapy outcome literature.
(Lilienfeld, Ritschel, Lynn, Cautin, & Latzman, 2013, p. 883)
In addition to highlighting the roots of resistance, Lilienfeld et
al. (2013) also offered suggestions on how to address these
resistances among students in training. They suggested exposing
students to a better awareness that they are not immune from
cognitive biases, such as confirmation bias, hindsight bias, and
illusory correlation. They also suggested graduate instructors to
not only convey accurate information, but also disabuse
students of inaccurate information, especially around
misunderstandings of the human nature. They also argue that
students should not just be taught the “hows” of implementing
therapy techniques, but more so the “whys” of how these
approaches have been established as effective. In sum,
Lilienfeld et al. stressed the importance of critical thinking and
scientific training of students in the mental health field. For
more detailed information on EBPP, as well as the resistance
toward EBPP, please see APA Presidential Task Force on
Evidence-Based Practice (2006) and Lilienfeld et al. (2013).
Science and Practice Training
The focus between research and practice has been an issue for
training programs that emphasize both. Sometimes the model
has been interpreted as a 50–50 split of performing science and
practice activities. The type of model in its ideal form (i.e.,
implicitly 50% practitioner and 50% scientist/researcher) may
be just that—an ideal that is rarely found in reality. Gelso
(1979) proposed that it may be more realistic to train students
in both domains (in varying degrees depending on their
interests) with the expectation that students will find a suitable
place for themselves in performing relevant activities on the
scientist-practitioner continuum. Thus, one student might prefer
a 20–80 split while another might choose a 75–25 split.
Sometimes there have been implicit values attached to either
science or practice; that is, some educators might value science
more than practice, and thus feel more satisfaction when a new
graduate obtains the “best” job, which to them is an academic
position complete with a myriad of scientific pursuits. We
strongly believe that science and practice are both highly valued
activities in the counseling profession, and that as a profession
we are stronger (and only can survive) when we train students
to be competent in both science and practice. In short, it is
important for the profession to equally value various points
along this performance continuum.
We prefer to conceptualize the core of the scientist-practitioner
model in terms of scientific or critical thinking. In short, we are
suggesting that the role of scientific or critical thinking is a
central outcome of a wide range of science and practice
activities. The choice of whether a graduate engages in science
or practice activities is not the most important outcome, but
rather whether the graduate can utilize scientific or critical
thinking in whatever professional activities she or he chooses.
For example, a counseling center staff member might be
engaged primarily in direct client service and, say, one program
evaluation project (see Chapter 22); this might be a 5%–95%
science-practice split of professional activities. A faculty
member (three-quarters time) with a quarter-time direct service
appointment in the counseling center might have a 75%–25%
split of professional activities. In addition to the quantitative
difference in how research and practice is split, the role of
research can also be qualitatively different. Gelso and Fretz
(2001) proposed a three-level model of how research functions
for counselors. First, the minimum level involves counselors
being able to consume and apply research in their work with
clients. The second level involves counselors being able to use
critical thinking to develop and test hypotheses during their
work with clients. The third level involves being able to
conduct empirical studies and to present the research findings.
Regardless of the type of professional activities a person selects
along the scientist-practitioner continuum, we maintain that
counselors utilize scientific thinking within their practice and
science activities.
Baker, McFall, and Shoham (2008) suggested that the lack of
adequate scientific training and utilization of scientific
knowledge has been a major problem in the development of the
clinical psychology field, which is also a reflection of the
mental health profession:
Clinical psychologists' failure to achieve a more significant
impact on clinical and public health may be traced to their deep
ambivalence about the role of science and their lack of adequate
science training, which leads them to value personal clinical
experience over research evidence, use assessment practices
that have dubious psychometric support, and not use the
interventions for which there is the strongest evidence of
efficacy. Clinical psychology resembles medicine at a point in
its history when practitioners were operating in a largely
prescientific manner. Prior to the scientific reform of medicine
in the early 1900s, physicians typically shared the attitudes of
many of today's clinical psychologists, such as valuing personal
experience over scientific research. (p. 67)
In the end, there is no doubt that research has enhanced the
practice of counseling. Even though a practitioner may not be
able to cite a specific reference, his or her graduate school
training was likely based on a tremendous amount of research
data, all the way from personality theory to intervention
strategies. The accumulation may be slow, but the data
eventually advance our working knowledge of the field. The
scientific method and research have advanced the field, but they
could be even more helpful. In addition, training in both science
and practice has become more sophisticated with not only more
integration but also more emphasis on critical thinking, which is
the core of all science and practice activities of the counseling
profession. For more information on a broad array of
recommendations for enhancing training in the scientist-
practitioner model and how EBPP is a framework for training
scientist-practitioners in the 21st century, see Chwalisz
(2003).JOYS AND CHALLENGES IN ACQUIRING
RESEARCH COMPETENCIES
Students will often experience a number of different reactions
to research training, and these emotional reactions can change
throughout the course of their graduate training and actually
many years beyond (see Table 2.1). Sometimes students will be
elated with the joy of discovering new and useful information
from their research. Other times students will become restless
and disgruntled with the minutiae associated with research. In
addition, some students will experience obstacles such as
negative self-talk, procrastination, perfectionist tendencies, and
a range of “shoulds” (e.g., I should be better at math; see
Heppner & Heppner, 2004).
TABLE 2.1Common Student Reactions toward Research and
Our Suggestions
Student Feelings
Our Suggestions
I don't know enough to conduct any meaningful research.
Volunteer to help on a research project; slowly work your way
up to learn the more advanced skills.
I am a fraud and really don't know how to do research.
Feelings of anxiety and doubt associated with research are
normal; they are often due to having limited experiences.
What if I make a mistake and report inaccurate findings?
It is a learning process; allow yourself time to learn and develop
more advanced skills.
I'm scared of running into debilitating writing blocks in my
research.
Acquiring research and thinking skills occurs over a long period
of time; sometimes it is a sign that you may need to acquire
more knowledge of the literature.
I am afraid that my results will be statistically nonsignificant.
Programmatic research often results in the accrual of knowledge
over time; nonsignificant results are also helpful information.
The whole thesis process seems overwhelming, tedious, long,
anxiety producing, confusing, and complicated.
The more you get involved in research, the more self-
efficacious you will feel.
I just want to practice; I am not interested in science/research.
Be open to learning more about the research process as well as
the research literature.
science = statistics
Science and research are broader than quantitative studies that
utilize statistics. Critical thinking is the core of science.
I don't believe that research in counseling can make a difference
in the profession.
Read literature review and meta-analysis articles to get a better
understanding of cumulative knowledge base.
Science is too intellectual, too cognitive, and unrelated to the
experiences of real people.
As you obtain more experience with research activities, your
research interests may increase and you might see a higher
value on the role of research.
The developmental process of acquiring research competencies
takes considerable time, years in fact, and often differs across
students. Throughout this chapter are reflections from
experienced and skillful researchers who provide some useful
glimpses into this developmental process of acquiring an array
of research competencies. A great deal of wisdom is
communicated by these prolific and creative scholars, and we
appreciate their candid perspectives on their own developmental
journey.
These reflections on their journeys throughout the chapter
depict various aspects of the developmental process of
acquiring research competencies. From our collective
experiences as authors of this text, we have observed that many
students question whether they can adequately learn the skills
associated with research and science: How can I, a mere
graduate student, come up with a good research idea? What if I
get bogged down in my research and never finish my degree?
Statistics scare me—what if I make a mistake in analyzing my
data? We contend that these and other fears are common and
developmentally normal. We believe it is essential to discuss
the negative affective and cognitive components associated with
research, and thus the first section focuses on such common
student reactions, namely (a) performance anxiety and efficacy;
(b) the false dichotomous conceptualization as research versus
practice; and (c) belief in oneself as a researcher, and science in
general.
Performance Anxiety and Efficacy From our experience working
with students, we have found that performance anxiety is a
central and crucial affective issue. For example, common
student disclosures include: “I don't know enough to conduct
any meaningful research on my own. How can I do a thesis?”
“Any study that I could do would be so basic and bland as to
make it worthless.” “I feel completely inadequate in even
conceiving a study that examines counseling from a unique
perspective.” “Although I made it through the statistics and
research design courses, once I start to do some research, I'll be
‘found out’; I am a fraud, and really don't know how to do
research.” “What if I make a mistake and report inaccurate
findings?” “I've had trouble writing before, and I'm scared to
death that I will run into debilitating writing blocks again in my
research.” “I am afraid that my results will be statistically
nonsignificant, and all of my research efforts and time will just
be one big waste.” “I've seen other students in the throes of a
dissertation. The whole process seems overwhelming, tedious,
long, anxiety producing, confusing, and complicated.”
It is important to put these and similar feelings into the context
of the typical entering graduate student. Most incoming
graduate students have had some experience (or even a lot) with
helping other people, and consequently the benefits of
counseling and other practitioner activities are abundantly clear.
Moreover, for most of us, helping people is a personally
rewarding experience, and students have an understanding of
those rewards. Conversely, it has been our experience that most
incoming students have had considerably less experience with
science. Typically, students do not imagine themselves making
contributions to the profession by publishing in our professional
journals. Since students have had little, if any, research
experience, they often have legitimate reasons to question and
doubt their skills and abilities—these feelings, concerns, and
fears are thus developmentally normal. In fact, it is not
uncommon even for faculty to experience performance anxiety
and question their efficacy (e.g., see Research Journey
2.1). This suggests to us that acquiring the necessary research
and thinking skills occurs over a long period of time; it does not
happen all at once. In fact, expert performance is found
associated with active engagement in deliberate practice, which
involves training on particular tasks, receiving immediate
feedback, having time for evaluation, problem solving, and the
opportunity to refine behaviors (Ericsson, 2008).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.1
Developing competencies in research is not an event. Instead, it
is a slow journey; as some markers are achieved, new
competency goals quickly surface, even as a senior faculty
member. I am committed to life-long learning! This applies to
life in general and also to my life as a researcher. I did not
always hear encouraging words from others about my ability to
carry out a project or the importance of my work. Early in my
career, I focused on the negative messages that I received. It
took a while for me to tune out the naysayers and find ways to
identify growth areas to strengthen my research so that it could
contribute to the literature in the ways that I found important. I
have had to learn how to believe in what I do and in my ideas. I
was able to do this by seeking feedback from those who
conducted similar research—from people who “got” my work
and thus were able to identify strengths and limitations of the
work in a constructive way. I now recognize that to be a good
researcher requires feedback and input from a number of
sources. To help develop a research project, it is absolutely
essential to share your work with others and get feedback at all
stages. Over the years, I have learned that it is okay to have a
project be underdeveloped in its initial conceptualization—
others do not judge me on where I start, but rather on where I
end up. That is, how I have incorporated the ideas and
suggestions from others and how much the project has
developed over time. Knowing this has provided me the space to
make mistakes, learn, and grow.
—Helen Neville, Ph.D.
As students acquire and begin to apply their knowledge about
research design, we have been struck by many students' surprise
and actual delight about research as they get past their initial
fears. We often have heard comments like, “I'm
actually enjoying this research course”; “I never thought I
would be a researcher; now I'm beginning to reconsider my
career goals”; “This research stuff is contagious; I'm starting to
think about research studies a lot and I want to collect data
now!” (e.g., see Research Journey 2.2). In sum, the major point
is that beginning graduate students often have considerably less
information about research and science than about practitioner
activities. Moreover, there is evidence that as students obtain
more experience with research activities (especially if it is early
in their training), their research interests increase and they
place a higher value on the role of research (e.g., Gelso,
Baumann, Chui, & Savela, 2013).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.2
When I started graduate school, my dream was to become a
director of a counseling center. Research was not part of my
career dreams, and I had no sense that I could ever publish in a
professional journal. However, subsequent research experiences
in my coursework, especially with my doctoral advisor, David
N. Dixon, shifted my career aspirations toward academia.
Several years later after having a modicum of success in
publishing, I realized that the most important goal was the
accumulation of knowledge from the systematic study of a
particular topic, and subsequent theory development. Several
more years later, I more fully realized the importance of the
context surrounding my research topic, and in particular, the
many variables (age, SES, race) within participants' cultural
context that greatly affected our research findings. I also
realized our research efforts alone were insufficient to create
the knowledge bases needed for tomorrow, and that the
worldviews and power of journal editors, editorial boards, and
others in leadership positions in our profession played a critical
role in the creation of our future knowledge bases. This
realization led me to engage in a number of professional roles
(e.g., editorial boards, assuming editorships, involvement and
leadership positions in professional organizations), all aimed at
improving knowledge bases and promoting social justice in
counseling psychology. In the end, one of the things I greatly
value in our profession is the many ways that we can join hands
with diverse partners to create knowledge bases and theories to
promote the development of a broad array of people.
—Puncky Heppner, Ph.D.
Thus, our advice to students regarding performance anxiety is
first of all to recognize that feelings of anxiety and doubt
associated with research are normal, and often reflect where one
is in the developmental process of acquiring new competencies,
in this case research competencies. Second, we strongly
recommend that students get involved with research projects to
slowly develop various research competencies. We believe the
best way to create a sense of research efficacy is to learn and
perform those skills. For example, students might offer to
volunteer to help on a research project to learn the basic
research skills, and slowly work their way up to learn the more
advanced skills (e.g., see Research Journey 2.3). In essence,
both our experience and our research have suggested that the
more students get involved in research, the more self-efficacy
they tend to feel; the more efficacious students feel about
conducting research, the more positive they are about the
training environment and interest in research (Gelso et al.,
2013; Kahn, 2001).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.3
I had the good fortune of becoming Professor Donald Atkinson's
doctoral advisee in counseling psychology at the University of
California, Santa Barbara, back in 1996. I learned firsthand via
Don's work the power of research—that is, good research work
can lead to significant changes in our profession and society
(e.g., increased attention to multiculturalism). It was also
during that time that my research training became more
solidified through several courses on research methods and
statistics (by the way, I used the first edition of this book in my
main research methods course and it couldn't have offered me a
better start!). Most importantly, however, it was the day-to-day
work on several research studies with Don and other students
that taught me the intricacies of research, starting from problem
conceptualization and moving into research design, data
collection, data analyses, and manuscript publication. So in
hindsight, I mostly “learned by doing.” And I've come to
understand that there is no magic formula to becoming a good
researcher but to have good people around you who also are
enthusiastic about research. At this point in my career, as a full
professor with tenure and the editor of Asian American Journal
of Psychology, I continue to believe that research success is a
function of inquisitive and informed researchers working
together for a common goal. So my recommendation continues
to be: “Get involved, join a research team, study the topic of
your passion, and don't forget to enjoy the process! The
outcome will then take care of itself.”
—Bryan S. K. Kim, Ph.D.
Threats, Unrewarding Training, and Disenchantment For any of
a number of reasons, students' research training is often less
than rewarding, and actually often rather threatening, all of
which can result in student disenchantment. It is not uncommon
for students to enter graduate school with ambivalence and
anxiety about the academic and research requirements that they
have to meet over the next several years. Many students are
intimidated by their instructors and professors; in particular
those that teach courses in which the students feel less
competent in (such as statistics and research methodology,
which are less directly associated with counseling practice).
Moreover, the idea of coming up with an innovative research
topic for their thesis is quite daunting. It is hard enough to
generate adequate research questions for a study, let alone
finding measures, collecting data, statistical analysis, and
writing all these up into a lengthy thesis! With the anxiety
experienced around research, the program and training can feel
quite threatening. Unfortunately, when students have little
experience with research and science, one bad experience can
create tremendous attitudinal and motivational damage and
move a student a long way from future scientific activities.
Some training programs may actually decrease students' interest
in research.
The profession has come a long way in enhancing research
training over the last two decades. Consideration of students'
affective reactions, values, and individual differences, and of
the training environment will most likely continue to enhance
the development of students' research efficacy. In addition,
consideration of cultural values and social connections are
especially important issues for some racial/ethnic minority and
international students whose cultural values make it more
difficult to seek and participate in the research groups without
others' invitation or help.
The False Dichotomous Conceptualization of Science versus
Practice Another developmental issue is how students
conceptualize science and research, and their relationship to
practice. A common statement is, “I just want to practice; I am
not interested in science/research.” Sometimes the scientific
enterprise is erroneously conceptualized as totally separated
from applied work in counseling. Thus, a false dichotomy is
established, science versus practice. Actually there is interplay
between science and practice. Our belief is that those students
who develop skills in both arenas will be better equipped to do
the work of the counseling profession (for example,
see Research Journey 2.4).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.4
When I started my undergraduate studies, I was sure that I
wanted to be a police detective. Although I realized that path
was not in the cards for me, the drive to find solutions to
mysteries has never left. I quickly realized when it comes to
research that the world is far more complex than I originally
thought. Additionally, with complex questions come complex
answers that frequently result in multiple equally valid
solutions. This process is what draws me into doing research
and at the same time the multitude of possibilities and enormous
amounts of information can be paralyzing—a common theme I
hear from my students. Yet I have found two sources of
inspiration: (a) working with others (counselors, researchers,
and clients) to be sure I am expanding the way I am thinking
about any given topic and to stay connected to the lived
experience of those who matter the most, and (b) tackling
specific questions so that each study is manageable. In this way,
each study is akin to adding to an ongoing conversation, and
developing a research agenda or doing multiple studies in the
same area allow for the conversation to continue. I find this
process very gratifying and rewarding!
—Jesse Owen, Ph.D..
Moreover, one of the hallmarks of graduate work in counseling
is to acquire critical thinking skills, the ability to identify and
process information with fewer biases, stereotypes, and
assumptions; formulate hypotheses; gather data; and make
informed decisions. These critical thinking skills are essential
in both the practice and science arenas. Higher critical thinking
ability has been found as a key characteristic of master
therapists (identified as having exceptional mastery of therapy
by their peers; Jennings & Skovholt, 2015; Ronnestad &
Skovholt, 2013; Skovholt, 2012). A qualitative meta-analysis of
six studies that examined characteristics of master therapists in
different countries found that master therapists have
developed more sophisticated and complex conceptualization of
clients' presenting issues (Jennings & Skovholt, 2015). One of
the key themes identified across master therapists in all the
studies was the capacity for cognitive complexity and intricate
conceptualization. This capacity was developed through
ongoing learning through reflection, feedback, and teaching
others.
Our advice to students is to avoid a narrow conceptualization of
research and science and to not prematurely conclude that
research and science have little or nothing to offer the
practitioner (Skovholt, 2012). Rather, we encourage practice-
oriented students to be open to learning more about the research
process as well as the research literature, and how these
experiences can inform practice and improve their counseling
competence. The scientific method is simply a set of tools to
establish a controlled mode of inquiry about all of our
professional activities, research and practice.
Disbelief in Oneself as a Researcher and Science in General A
critically important belief in the process of becoming a
competent researcher is believing in one's ability to conduct
meaningful research as well as belief in the utility of research,
that research does contribute to the profession's knowledge
bases, and that our research can and does make a difference in
clients' lives (e.g., see Research Journey 2.5).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.5
Being involved in research that has the potential to make a
difference is a meaningful aspect of my work and motivates me
to persist when encountering inherent obstacles in the research
process. After graduating from college with degrees in Biology
and Sociology (and a minor in Women's Studies), I worked in an
inner city battered women's shelter. At the shelter, I became
interested in what differentiated women who returned to the
abuser from women who struggled to create nonviolent lives for
themselves and their children. In part, this desire to understand
critical social problems motivated me to purse a master's
degree, where I focused my thesis on shelter workers'
perceptions of battered women. I loved how my work in the
shelter informed and was informed by my research. I also began
to see that while helping people individually was rewarding to
me, I might be able to make a difference through sharing my
research findings with others. My doctoral program solidified
my passion for research as I was given the opportunity to study
topics of interest that had real-world applications, including
helping women to achieve economic self-sufficiency. I
sometimes struggle with a component of research (e.g.,
collecting data from difficult to obtain samples), but as soon as
this task becomes very painful, the next step in research brings
new and exciting challenges (e.g., analyzing the data!). Also,
working with students and colleagues on research teams makes
the research process a wonderful, fun, and memorable
adventure.
—Karen O'Brien, Ph.D.
It is common for students to not initially feel efficacious as a
researcher nor to believe that our research in counseling can
make a difference in the profession. Most beginning students do
not have much of an understanding of the role of science in our
profession, and since they do not have a long history in the
profession they are unable to identify how previous research has
contributed to the profession. Similarly, students do not have a
clear understanding of the utility of the scientific method as a
way of thinking about either research or clients. Finally, the
student typically has had relatively little experience of
expanding his or her understanding about clients and applied
problems by engaging in research, reading the professional
literature, and contemplating the implication of empirical
findings. One unfortunate outcome is that students sometimes
fill this void with misinformation, like “science = statistics.” Or
students believe that “science is too intellectual, too cognitive,
and unrelated to the experiences of real people.” Or “the
scientific method is so mechanical and reductionistic that it
cannot be used adequately to study the complexities of real
human behavior.” Conversely, seasoned researchers understand
both the challenges of conducting meaningful research and the
gradual but steady accumulation of knowledge from our
research. Even though it is rare that any one study has a major
impact on the profession, programmatic research often does
result in the accrual of knowledge over time, and often achieves
significant conclusions. For example, many of the chapters in
the APA Handbook of Counseling Psychology (Fouad, Carter, &
Subich, 2012) are based on extensive literature reviews and
nicely reflect the utility of our research in creating knowledge
as well as enhancing the sophistication of our conceptualization
of relevant constructs in counseling.
Clara Hill, a creative and prolific counseling researcher, was
invited to write a personal account of her evolution as a
researcher. Her account illustrates how she made decisions, how
complex the process of research can be, and how she coped with
the ups and downs along the way and, most important, her
“crisis of faith in research”:
After this series of studies I underwent a crisis in terms of
research. I had achieved tenure, had my first baby, and turned
30. I started questioning everything. Because I could see so
many imperfections in my research, I was not very proud of my
work. It had taken an incredible amount of time for little “hard
information.” I could not even remember the results of many of
my studies. Further, I seemed woefully far from even describing
what happens in the counseling process let alone understanding
what counselor behaviors were useful in effecting change. I
despaired that counseling was far too complex to be studied. I
had previously had lots of doubts about my research, but it was
mostly related to self-confidence in my research abilities. This
crisis seemed to have more to do with whether I felt that
research was a viable means to answer questions about the
counseling process. (Hill, 1984, p. 105)
Hill's behind-the-scenes disclosures are illuminating as well as
often relieving for many students and faculty. We strongly
encourage students to read this article, perhaps during their first
course on research methods. We have found that Hill's
frustrations, jubilations, feelings of inadequacy, and doubts
about the role of research resonate strongly with students. The
evolution of Hill's confidence in the utility of conducting
research in her own way not only sends a useful message, but
also illuminates the developmental processes of students and
faculty in becoming skilled and competent researchers.
From our experience, it is critical not only that researchers feel
efficacious in conducting relevant and important research, but
also that they begin to perceive that their research is important,
that it contributes useful information to our professional
knowledge bases, and that it can make a difference in the
profession as well as the lives of our clients. Such beliefs often
are the result of identifying implications of your research
findings to practice, being cited in the professional literature in
meaningful ways, or seeing either that others are using your
research findings in applied settings or that your research is
helping other researchers subsequently ask other important
questions.
Belief in the utility of research can also result from reading the
professional literature to learn the impact of others' research.
Our advice is for students to not only read and reflect on the
literature, but also to read literature reviews in particular (see
Major Contributions in The Counseling Psychologist or review
papers in Psychological Bulletin). In addition, we advise
students to get involved in conducting research on topics that
are really important to them, either topics that they are very
interested in or ones that reflect their personal values in some
way (e.g., see Research Journey 2.6).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.6
I firmly believe that the best researchers usually have a
connection and passion about their research. I developed my
research program, in part, to help me understand and make
sense of my own experiences as an African American man in a
predominantly White college environment struggling to do well
academically, while at the same time trying to learn more about
my history and culture. This desire to better understand myself
and the experiences of other people like me led to a
dissertation, my first publication, several other articles, and a
book. Everyone has experienced challenges in writing and
conducting research at some point, and everybody has
experienced rejection when trying to publish something. I have
learned that it is not always the “smartest” people who can
successfully carry out a research project and publish it, but
often it is those individuals who are the most resilient.
—Kevin Cokley, Ph.D.A MODEL FOR RESEARCH TRAINING
The purpose of this section is to discuss a number of factors
that are important in enhancing research training. Our goal is
not only to enhance students' awareness of what appears to be
some of the major constructs in research training, but also to
promote additional attention and research on this important
developmental process. Based on the culminated research on
effective research training, Kahn (2001) proposed a model that
incorporated various factors, which is presented in Figure 2.1.
We will discuss this model and the empirical evidence that
supports parts of it, as well as add a few other important aspects
of research training not included in the model. We begin by
discussing each of the major constructs in the model: (a)
investigative interests, (b) research training environment, (c)
relationship with mentor, (d) research outcome expectations, (e)
research self-efficacy, (f) research interest, (g) scholarly
activity, and (h) year in program.
FIGURE 2.1Parameter Estimates for the Modified Structural
Model of Kahn (2001)
Constructs of the Model
Investigative Interests Most students in counseling and
counseling psychology can be categorized into one of Holland's
(1992) three personality types: social, artistic, or investigative.
Research has shown that the personality type of counseling
students is related to their level of interest in research and in
research courses, choice of graduate program types with varying
levels of research focus, and to research productivity
(Mallinckrodt et al. 1990; Tinsley, Tinsley, Boone, & Shim-Li,
1993). Specifically, it appears that students who are
investigative types have more interest in research activities and
ultimately produce more research (e.g., see Research Journey
2.7). Accordingly, Kahn (2001) found that a student's
investigative interest would be directly related to expectations
of research outcome and research interest, which in turn would
be related to research productivity. In particular, investigative
types are hypothesized to be more interested in research, and
social types less interested.
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.7
My passion for science originated at a very early age, when I
found myself setting up mini “experiments” to test various
questions. I was curious about the world and about human
interactions in the world. At this young age I also was acutely
aware of individuals whose experiences in the world were
different from my own. I viewed humans as equal with unequal
opportunities. After multiple undergraduate transfers, I was
fortunate to have ended up in a college that provided me access
to mentoring and advising that enabled my interests, skills, and
efficacy in science to flourish. Were it not for these individuals
and my experiences, I would have never realized that a doctoral
degree or a career as a scientist-practitioner was a viable
option. Throughout my graduate training and career, my passion
to share my excitement for the scientific process in a way that is
meaningful for others (e.g., via knowledge in research design
and analysis, consultation with organizations, developing and
testing hypotheses with clients in psychotherapy) has continued
to blossom. As I reflect upon my research journey, I appreciate
the combination of factors that have contributed. These personal
characteristics (I am deeply curious about the world), beliefs (I
believe that science has the potential to impact practices and
prompt positive change in the world), and values (opportunities,
and access to them, matter) continue to shape the work in which
I choose to engage and the questions that I choose to ask.
Mindi Thompson, Ph.D.
Research Training Environment Gelso, Baumann, Chui, and
Savela (2013) discussed 10 ingredients of graduate training
programs that foster positive attitudes toward research. These
components have been identified as important features of the
research training environment: (a) faculty modeling of scientific
behavior; (b) the positive reinforcement of scientific activity,
both formally and informally; (c) early and minimally
threatening involvement in research; (d) emphasizing science as
a partly social-interpersonal experience; (e) emphasizing that all
studies are limited and flawed; (f) teaching and valuing varied
approaches to research; (g) teaching students to look inward for
research ideas when they are developmentally ready to do so;
(h) the wedding of science and practice; (i) relevant statistics
and the logic of design; and (j) teaching how research can be
done in practice settings.
An illustration of a positive training environment that included
the 10 components would be having an environment that
involved students in research as early as possible and in ways
that are interesting, involving, and consistent with their skill
level. Many counseling students are socially oriented,
increasing the social aspects of research through the forms of
advising, and research teams will increase the attractiveness of
the endeavor for them. In research groups, students of various
levels can participate to the extent that they feel comfortable
and have something to contribute. Research advising and
modeling is beyond transferring skills and involves having
research mentors exhibit healthy attitudes toward research
including both the excitement and disappointment in response to
its trials and tribulations. Honest expressions of reinforcement
to students all along the way in the research process are an
important part of building interest. Providing a nonintimidating
atmosphere for learning is important. Students should not feel
pressured to design the perfect study; rather, they should feel
motivated to create a study that can address their research
questions. Also, many counseling students have little fondness
for statistics and are turned off to research because they equate
doing research with studying for statistics examinations. Some
students will be attracted to quantitative methods and others to
qualitative methods, and these preferences should be respected
and honored. Not only do students benefit by using a method
that they enjoy, but the field benefits from this methodological
pluralism (Gelso et al., 1988). A true integration of science and
practice will make research more attractive, especially to the
majority of students whose primary interest is in practice.
Looking inward to rely on clinical experience is an example of
wedding science and practice. Students who have a deep interest
in their topic finish their dissertations faster than those who are
motivated to finish their programs but who pick a convenient
topic of study. Not all research environments are ideal, but it is
important to make the most out of them (e.g., see Research
Journey 2.8).
RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.8
I applied to graduate school with the goal of conducting
community-engaged research on Asian immigrant families and
disseminating this research through community organizations
and churches. However, I ended up at a graduate program that
did not have such research opportunities at the time. Rather than
get disheartened, I had to make the most of what was available
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx
Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx

Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01Kishor Ade
 
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docx
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docxSocw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docx
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docxsdfghj21
 
Survey research ch17-1
Survey research ch17-1Survey research ch17-1
Survey research ch17-1kholodOlemat
 
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating ads
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating adsTeaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating ads
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating adssharon methvin
 
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docxRunning Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledAbigail Burmeister
 
Types of qualitative research design.pdf
Types of qualitative research design.pdfTypes of qualitative research design.pdf
Types of qualitative research design.pdfMaryRoseAlegria
 
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016Katie Carroll
 
Negative effects of neo segregation
Negative effects of neo segregationNegative effects of neo segregation
Negative effects of neo segregationHilary Pate
 

Similar to Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx (12)

Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01Research Methodology Module-01
Research Methodology Module-01
 
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docx
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docxSocw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docx
Socw 6310 week 7 SOCW 6301 Social Work Practice Research.docx
 
Survey research ch17-1
Survey research ch17-1Survey research ch17-1
Survey research ch17-1
 
Presentation1
Presentation1Presentation1
Presentation1
 
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating ads
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating adsTeaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating ads
Teaching undergradutae statisitcs using dating ads
 
First monday
First mondayFirst monday
First monday
 
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docxRunning Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
 
First monday
First mondayFirst monday
First monday
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
 
Types of qualitative research design.pdf
Types of qualitative research design.pdfTypes of qualitative research design.pdf
Types of qualitative research design.pdf
 
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016
Qualitative Psychology - Feb 2016
 
Negative effects of neo segregation
Negative effects of neo segregationNegative effects of neo segregation
Negative effects of neo segregation
 

More from helzerpatrina

Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docxMost patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docxhelzerpatrina
 
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docx
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docxMotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docx
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docx
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docxMost public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docx
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docx
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docxMr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docx
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docxhelzerpatrina
 
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docxMrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxMuch has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxhelzerpatrina
 
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docx
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docxMSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docx
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docxhelzerpatrina
 
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docx
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docxMSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docx
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docx
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docxMuch of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docx
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Mt. Baker Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docx
Mt. Baker   Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docxMt. Baker   Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docx
Mt. Baker Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docx
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docxMotivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docx
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docx
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docxMotivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docx
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docx
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docxMotivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docx
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docxMrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docxhelzerpatrina
 
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docx
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docxMOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docx
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docxhelzerpatrina
 
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docx
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docxmple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docx
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docxhelzerpatrina
 
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docx
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docxMore and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docx
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docx
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docxModule Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docx
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docx
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docxMonica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docx
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docxhelzerpatrina
 
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docx
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docxModule 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docx
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docxhelzerpatrina
 

More from helzerpatrina (20)

Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docxMost patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docx
Most patients with mental health disorders are not aggressive. H.docx
 
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docx
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docxMotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docx
MotivationExplain your motivation for applying to this prog.docx
 
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docx
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docxMost public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docx
Most public policy is made from within government agencies. Select a.docx
 
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docx
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docxMr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docx
Mr. Smith brings his 4-year-old son to your primary care office. He .docx
 
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docxMrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after rep.docx
 
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxMuch has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
 
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docx
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docxMSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docx
MSN 5550 Health Promotion Prevention of Disease Case Study Module 2.docx
 
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docx
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docxMSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docx
MSEL Strategy Mid-term Instructions Miguel Rivera-SantosFormat.docx
 
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docx
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docxMuch of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docx
Much of the focus in network security centers upon measures in preve.docx
 
Mt. Baker Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docx
Mt. Baker   Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docxMt. Baker   Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docx
Mt. Baker Hazards Hazard Rating Score High silic.docx
 
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docx
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docxMotivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docx
Motivation and Cognitive FactorsQuestion AAlfred Hit.docx
 
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docx
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docxMotivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docx
Motivation in OrganizationsMotivation i.docx
 
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docx
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docxMotivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docx
Motivations to Support Charity-Linked Events After Exposure to.docx
 
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docxMrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docx
Mrs. Walsh, a woman in her 70s, was in critical condition after.docx
 
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docx
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docxMOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docx
MOVIE TITLE IS LIAR LIAR starring JIM CARREYProvide the name o.docx
 
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docx
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docxmple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docx
mple selection, and assignment to groups (as applicable). Describe.docx
 
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docx
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docxMore and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docx
More and more businesses have integrated social media into every asp.docx
 
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docx
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docxModule Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docx
Module Five Directions for the ComparisonContrast EssayWrite a.docx
 
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docx
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docxMonica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docx
Monica asked that we meet to see if I could help to reduce the d.docx
 
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docx
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docxModule 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docx
Module 6 AssignmentPlease list and describe four types of Cy.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 

Week 8 Homework ExerciseCCMH525 Version 31University of P.docx

  • 1. Week 8 Homework Exercise CCMH/525 Version 3 1 University of Phoenix Material Week 8 Homework Exercise Answer the following questions in 25 to 50 words each, covering material from Ch. 14 of Methods in Behavioral Research: 1. What is replication? What role does it play in increasing the external validity or generalizability of a study? 2. What is the difference between an exact replication and a conceptual replication? Provide an example of each. 3. What is meta-analysis? How is it useful? 4. True or false: In many cases, meta-analysis involves calculating an average effect size for a relationship between variables. Explain your response. 5. Discuss the advantages and disadvantages of using college students as research participants. 6. True or false: Many constructs, such as physical attractiveness and self-esteem, appear constant across cultures; thus, external validity is less of a concern when conducting research on such constructs. Explain your response. Copyright © 2017, 2015 by University of Phoenix. All rights reserved.
  • 2. Page 291 LEARNING OBJECTIVES · Discuss the issues created by generalizing research results to other populations, including potential problems using college students as research participants. · Discuss issues to consider regarding generalization of research results to other cultures and ethnic groups. · Describe the potential problem of generalizing to other experimenters and suggest possible solutions. · Discuss the importance of replications, distinguishing between exact replications and conceptual replications. · Distinguish between narrative literature reviews and meta- analyses. Page 292IN THIS CHAPTER, WE WILL CONSIDER THE ISSUE OF GENERALIZATION OF RESEARCH FINDINGS. When a single study is conducted with a particular sample and procedure, can the results then be generalized to other populations of research participants, or to other ways of manipulating or measuring the variables? Recall from Chapter 4 that internal validity refers to the ability to infer that there is a causal relationship between variables. External validity is the extent to which findings may be generalized. GENERALIZING TO OTHER POPULATIONS Even though a researcher may randomly assign participants to experimental conditions, rarely are participants randomly selected from the general population. As we noted in Chapters 7 and 9, the individuals who participate in psychological research are usually selected because they are available, and the most available population consists of college students—or more specifically, first- and second-year students enrolled in the introductory psychology course to satisfy a general education requirement. They may also be from a particular college or university, may be volunteers, or may be mostly males or mostly females. So, are our research findings limited to these
  • 3. types of subjects, or can we generalize our findings to a more general population? After considering these issues, we will examine the larger issue of culture and how research findings can be generalized to different cultural groups. College Students Smart (1966) found that college students were studied in over 70% of the articles published between 1962 and 1964 in the Journal of Experimental Psychology and the Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. Sears (1986) reported similar percentages in 1980 and 1985 in a variety of social psychology journals; Arnett (2008) found that 67% of the articles in the 2007 volume of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology used college student samples. The potential problem is that such studies use a highly restricted population. Sears points out that most of the students are first-year students and sophomores taking the introductory psychology class. They therefore tend to be young and to possess the characteristics of emerging adults: a sense of self-identity that is still developing, social and political attitudes that are in a state of flux, a high need for peer approval, and unstable peer relationships. They are intelligent with high cognitive abilities. Thus, what we know about “people in general” may actually be limited to a highly select and unusual group. Indeed, Peterson (2001) found that students, as a group, are more homogenous than nonstudent samples. That is, students are more similar to each other than adults are similar to other adults in the general population. Research by Henry (2008) illustrates how the use of college students may affect the external validity of research on prejudice. In his sample of articles Page 293from 1990 to 2005, an increasing percentage of studies used college students as participants. Further, in looking at the actual results of studies on prejudice that compared college students with adults, he reported a variety of differences among adults and college students. For example, college students were less conservative and rated women and ethnic minorities more favorably. Volunteers
  • 4. Researchers usually must ask people to volunteer to participate in their research. At many colleges, introductory psychology students are required either to volunteer for research or to complete an alternative project. If you are studying populations other than college students, you are even more dependent on volunteers—for example, asking people at a homeowners' association meeting to participate in a study of marital interaction or conducting research on the Internet in which people must go to your web page and then agree to participate in the study, or conducting a telephone survey of county residents to determine health care needs. In all these cases, external validity of the findings may be limited because the data from volunteers may be different from what would be obtained with a more general sample. Some research indicates that volunteers differ in various ways from nonvolunteers. In their comprehensive study on the topic, Rosenthal and Rosnow (1975) reported that volunteers tend to be more highly educated, of a higher socioeconomic status, more in need of approval, and more social. Further, different kinds of people volunteer for different kinds of experiments. In colleges, there may be a sign-up board with the titles of many studies listed or a web page that manages research participants and volunteer opportunities for the university. Different types of people may be drawn to the study titled “problem solving” than to the one titled “interaction in small groups.” Available evidence indicates that the title does influence who signs up (Hood & Back, 1971; Silverman & Margulis, 1973). Online Research Another important consideration arises when asking participants to volunteer for online surveys and experiments. Researchers can find potential participants through online survey design services. Psychologists are increasingly using Amazon Mechanical Turk (https://www.mturk.com; Jacquet, 2011), a website for recruiting people to work on many types of tasks including participating in research for a specified payment. This
  • 5. sort of sampling strategy has important implications for external validity. While the online sample is more diverse than the typical college student sample, there are still generalization issues because Internet users represent a unique demographic. The Pew Research Center's Internet and American Life Project (Pew Internet, 2010) found that living in an urban/suburban area, being college educated, being younger, and having a higher income are all related to reporting more time online. Thus, by asking Page 294for volunteers for an online survey, researchers are sampling from a particular demographic that may not generalize well to the population of interest. Gender Sometimes, researchers use only males or only females (or a very disproportionate ratio of males to females) simply because this is convenient or the procedures seem better suited to a particular gender. Given the possible differences between males and females, however, the results of such studies may not be generalizable (Denmark, Russo, Frieze, & Sechzer, 1988). Denmark et al. provide an example of studies on contraception practices that use only females because of stereotypical assumptions that only females are responsible for contraception. They also point out several other ways that gender bias may arise in psychological research, including confounding gender with age or job status and selecting response measures that are gender-stereotyped. The solution is to be aware of possible gender differences and include both males and females in our research investigations. Moreover, it is important to recognize the ways that males and females might differentially interpret independent variable manipulations or questions asked in a questionnaire. Locale The location that participants are recruited from can also have an impact on a study's external validity. Participants in one locale may differ from participants in another locale. For example, students at UCLA may differ from students at a nearby state university, who in turn may differ from students at a
  • 6. community college. People in Iowa may differ from people in New York City. Thus, a finding obtained with the students in one type of educational setting or in one geographic region may not generalize to people in other settings or regions. In fact, studies have explored how personality traits like extraversion (the tendency to seek social stimulation) and openness to new experiences vary across geographic areas. Rentfrow, Gosling, and Potter (2008) looked at geographic differences in personality traits among citizens of various U.S. states and found extraversion to vary by state. People in midwestern states tended to be more extraverted than people in northeastern states, and people in western states tended to be more open to new experiences. Thus, a study conducted in one location may not generalize well to another, particularly if the variables in question are related to location in some way. Culture Whether theories and research findings generalize across cultures is a critically important issue. Some observers of current psychological research have been very critical of the types of samples employed in behavioral research. Based on analyses of published research by Arnett (2008) and others, Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010) contend that psychology is built on the study of WEIRD Page 295(Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic) people. In many cases, research samples consist primarily of college students from the United States, other English-speaking countries, and Europe. Ultimately, researchers wish to discover aspects of human behavior that have universal applications but in fact cannot generalize beyond their limited samples. This is, at its heart, a critique of the external validity of behavioral research: Does our human behavioral research generalize to all humans, or is it really a study of the WEIRD? Clearly, if psychologists want to understand human behavior, they must understand human behavior across and among cultures (Henrich et al., 2010; Miller, 1999). Miller described research on self-concept by Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, and
  • 7. Norasakkunkit (1997) to illustrate the benefits of incorporating culture into psychological theory. Traditional theories of self- concept are grounded in the culture of the United States and Western Europe; the “self” is an individualistic concept where people are independent from others and self-enhancement comes from individual achievements. Kitayama and his colleagues take a broader, cultural perspective: In contrast to the U.S. meaning of self, in other cultures the “self” is a collective concept in which self-esteem is derived from relationships with others. Often, Japanese engage in self-criticism, which can be seen as relationship-maintaining, whereas Americans work to maintain and enhance self-esteem—thus, very different activities contribute to a positive self-concept in the two cultures (Kitayama et al., 1997). This is a very common theme in research that incorporates culture in psychological processes: “The significance of self-esteem, however, may be much more specific to a culture than has typically been supposed in the literature” (p. 1262). Much of this cultural research centers on identifying similarities and differences that may exist in personality and other psychological characteristics, as well as ways that individuals from different cultures respond to the same environments (Matsumoto, 1994). Research by Kim, Sherman, and Taylor (2008) provides another example of the limits of external validity across cultural groups. This research focused on how people from different cultures use social support to cope with stress. In reviewing the research on the topic, they concluded that Asians and Asian Americans might benefit from different styles of social support as compared with European Americans. For example, Asian Americans are more likely to benefit from support that does not involve the sort of intense disclosure of personal stressful events and feelings that is the hallmark of support in many European American groups. Rather, they suggest that Asians and Asian Americans may benefit more from support that comes with the comforts of proximity (being with close friends) rather than sharing.
  • 8. These examples all focused on differences among cultures. Many studies also find similarities across cultures. Evolutionary psychologists, for instance, often conduct studies in different cultural groups because they are looking for similarities across cultures in order to see if a particular behavior or attitude can be tied to our evolutionary past. For example, Singh, Dixson, Jessop, Morgan, and Dixson (2010) wanted to see if a particular aspect of beauty that is tied to greater reproductive success— namely waist-to-hip ratio (e.g., the ratio for Page 296a 25-inch waist and 35-inch hips is .71), which is related to sex hormones and thus fertility—would be seen as attractive across cultures. Diverse groups from Africa, Samoa, Indonesia, and New Zealand evaluated photographs of females with small and large waist-to-hip ratios. The researchers found that indeed, low waist-to-hip ratio among females was seen as more attractive across all these groups. In this example, the results obtained in one culture do generalize to other cultures. Nonhuman Animals We noted in Chapter 3 that about 7% of psychological research is conducted with nonhuman animals. Almost all of this research is done with rats, mice, and birds. Most research with other species is conducted to study the behavior of those animals directly to gather information that may help with the survival of endangered species and increase our understanding of our bonds with nonhuman animals such as dogs, cats, and horses (http://www.apa-hai.org/human-animal-interaction). The basic research that psychologists conduct with nonhuman animals is usually done with the expectation that the findings can be generalized to humans. This research is important because the research problems that are addressed require procedures such as long-term observation that could not be done with human samples. We do expect that we can generalize as our underlying biological and behavioral patterns are shared. In fact, the value of studying nonhuman animals has been demonstrated by research that does apply to humans. These applications include the biological bases of memory, food
  • 9. preferences, sexual behavior, choice behavior, and drug addictions. The American Psychological Association has prepared a brochure on animal research: (http://www.apa.org/research/responsible/research-animals.pdf). In Defense of College Students It is easy to criticize research on the basis of subject characteristics, yet criticism by itself does not mean that results cannot be generalized. Although we need to be concerned about the potential problems of generalizing from unique populations such as college students (cf. Sears, 1986), we should also keep several things in mind when thinking about this issue. First, criticisms of the use of any particular type of subject, such as college students, in a study should be backed with good reasons that a relationship would not be found with other types of subjects. College students, after all, are human, and researchers should not be blamed for not worrying about generalization to a particular type of subject if there is no good reason to do so. Moreover, college student bodies are increasingly diverse and increasingly representative of the society as a whole (although college students will always be characterized as having the ability and motivation to pursue a college degree). Second, replication of research studies provides a safeguard against the limited external validity of a single study. Studies are replicated at other colleges using different mixes of students, and Page 297many findings first established with college students are later applied to other populations, such as children, aging adults, and people in other countries. It is also worth noting that Internet samples are increasingly used in many types of studies. Although such studies raise their own issues of external validity, they frequently complement studies based on college student samples. GENERALIZING ACROSS METHODS The person who actually conducts the experiment is the source of another external validity problem. In most research, only one experimenter is used, and rarely is much attention paid to the personal characteristics of the experimenter (McGuigan, 1963).
  • 10. The main goal is to make sure that any influence the experimenter has on subjects is constant throughout the experiment. There is always the possibility, however, that the results are generalizable only to certain types of experimenters. Some of the important characteristics of experimenters have been discussed by Kintz and his colleagues (Kintz, Delprato, Mettee, Persons, & Schappe, 1965). These include the experimenter's personality and gender and the amount of practice in the role of experimenter. A warm, friendly experimenter will almost certainly produce different results from a cold, unfriendly experimenter. Participants also may behave differently with male and female experimenters. It has even been shown that rabbits learn faster when trained by experienced experimenters (Brogden, 1962)! The influence of the experimenter may depend as well on the characteristics of the participants. For example, participants seem to perform better when tested by an experimenter of the other sex (Stevenson & Allen, 1964). One solution to the problem of generalizing to other experimenters is to use two or more experimenters. A fine example of the use of multiple experimenters is a study by Rubin (1975), who sent several male and female experimenters to the Boston airport to investigate self-disclosure. The experimenters revealed different kinds of information about themselves to both male and female travelers and recorded the passengers' self-disclosures in return. One interesting result was that women tended to reveal more about themselves to male experimenters, and men tended to reveal more about themselves to female experimenters. Pretests and Generalization Researchers are often faced with the decision of whether to give a pretest. Intuitively, pretesting seems to be a good idea. The researcher can be sure that the groups are equivalent on the pretest, and it is often more satisfying to see that individuals changed their scores than it is to look only at group means on a posttest. A pretest also enables the researcher to assess
  • 11. mortality (attrition) effects when it is likely that some participants will withdraw from an experiment. Page 298If you give a pretest, you can determine whether the people who withdrew are different from those who completed the study. Pretesting, however, may limit the ability to generalize to populations that did not receive a pretest. (cf. Lana, 1969). Simply taking the pretest may cause subjects to behave differently than they would without the pretest. Recall from Chapter 8 that a Solomon four-group design (Solomon, 1949) can be used in situations in which a pretest is desirable but there is concern over the possible impact of taking the pretest. In the Solomon four-group design, half of the participants are given the pretest; the other half receive the posttest only. That is, the same experiment is conducted with and without the pretest. Mortality effects can be assessed in the pretest conditions. Also, the researcher can examine whether there is an interaction between the independent variable and the pretest: Are posttest scores on the dependent variable different depending on whether the pretest was given? Sometimes, researchers find that it is not feasible to conduct the study with all four groups in a single experiment. In this case, the first study can include the pretest; the study can be replicated later without the pretest. Generalizing from Laboratory Settings Research conducted in a laboratory setting has the advantage of allowing the experimenter to study the impact of independent variables under highly controlled conditions. The internal validity of the research is the primary consideration. The question arises, however, as to whether the artificiality of the laboratory setting limits the ability to generalize what is observed in the laboratory to real-life settings. Mook (1983) articulated one response to the artificiality issue: Generalization to real-life settings is not relevant when the purpose of the study was to investigate causal relationships under carefully controlled conditions. Mook is concerned that a “knee-jerk” criticism of laboratory research on the basis of
  • 12. external validity is too common. Good research is what is most important. Another response to the laboratory artificiality criticism is to examine the results of field experiments. Recall from Chapter 4 that in a field experiment, the researcher manipulates the independent variable in a natural setting—a factory, a school, or a street corner, for example. Anderson, Lindsay, and Bushman (1999) asked whether laboratory and field experiments that examine the same variables do in fact produce the same results. To answer this question, they found 38 pairs of studies for which a laboratory investigation had a field experiment counterpart. The studies were drawn from a variety of research areas including aggression, helping, memory, leadership style, and depression. Results of the laboratory and field experiments were in fact very similar—the effect size of the independent variable on the dependent variable was very similar in the two types of studies. Thus, even though lab and field experiments are conducted in different settings, the results Page 299are complementary rather than contradictory. When findings are replicated using multiple methods, our confidence in the external validity of the findings increases. SUPPORTING GOOD EXTERNAL VALIDITY It may seem as if no research can possibly be generalizable! In some ways, this is true. Furthermore, it can be very difficult to understand the extent to which a study is generalizable; external validity is an aspect of a study that we try to assess, but cannot truly know. How, then, can we support good external validity? There are a few ways that external validity can be supported. The key way that external validity can be supported is related to a study's methodology. Using a census, or a random sample will always produce better external validity than using a nonrandom sample. This, of course, is not always possible. Next, we will explore a few other ways in which external validity can be supported. Generalization as a Statistical Interaction
  • 13. The problem of generalization can be thought of as an interaction in a factorial design (see Chapter 10). An interaction occurs when a relationship between variables exists under one condition but not another or when the nature of the relationship is different in one condition than in another. Thus, if you question the generalizability of a study that used only males, you are suggesting that there is an interaction between gender and the independent variable. Suppose, for example, that a study examines the relationship between crowding and aggression among males and reports that crowding is associated with higher levels of aggression. You might then question whether the results are generalizable to females. Figure 14.1 shows four potential outcomes of a hypothetical study on crowding and aggression that tested both males and females. In each graph, the relationship between crowding and aggression for males has been maintained. In Graph A, there is no interaction—the behavior of males and females is virtually identical. Thus, the results of the original all-male study could be generalized to females. In Graph B, there is also no interaction; the effect of crowding is identical for males and females. However, in this graph, males are more aggressive than females. Although such a difference is interesting, it is not a factor in generalization because the overall relationship between crowding and aggression is present for both males and females. Graphs C and D do show interactions. In both, the original results with males cannot be generalized to females. In Graph C, there is no relationship between crowding and aggression for females. In Graph D, the interaction tells us that a positive relationship between crowding and aggression exists for males but that a negative relationship exists for females. As it turns out, Graph D describes the results of several studies on this topic (cf. Freedman, Levy, Buchanan, & Price, 1972). Page 300 FIGURE 14.1 Outcomes of a hypothetical experiment on crowding and
  • 14. aggression Note: The presence of an interaction indicates that the results for males cannot be generalized to females. Researchers can address issues of external validity that stem from the use of different populations by including subject type as a variable in the study. By including variables such as gender, age, or ethnic group in the design of the study, the results may be analyzed to determine whether there are interaction effects like the ones illustrated in Figure 14.1. The Importance of Replications Replication of research is a way of overcoming any problems of generalization that occur in a single study. There are two types of replications to consider: exact replications and conceptual replications. Exact replications An exact replication is an attempt to replicate precisely the procedures of a study to see whether the same results are obtained. A researcher who obtains an unexpected finding will frequently attempt Page 301a replication to make sure that the finding is reliable. If you are starting your own work on a problem, you may try to replicate a crucial study to make sure that you understand the procedures and can obtain the same results. Often, exact replications occur when a researcher builds on the findings of a prior study. For example, suppose you are intrigued by Singh et al.'s (2010) research on waist-to- hip ratio that was mentioned previously. Singh reports that males rate females with a ratio of .70 as most attractive. In your research, you might replicate the procedures used in the original study and expand on the original research. For example, you might study this phenomenon with males similar to those in the original sample as well as males from different cultures or age groups. When you replicate the original research findings using very similar procedures, your confidence in the external validity of the original findings is increased. The “Mozart effect” provides us with an interesting example of the importance of replications. In the original study by Rauscher, Shaw, and Ky (1993), college students listened to 10
  • 15. minutes of a Mozart sonata. These students then showed better performance on a spatial-reasoning measure drawn from the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale than students exposed to a relaxation tape or simple silence. This finding received a great deal of attention in the press as people quickly generalized it to the possibility of increasing children's intelligence with Mozart sonatas. In fact, one state governor began producing Mozart CDs to distribute in maternity wards, and entrepreneurs began selling Mozart kits to parents over the Internet. Over the next few years, however, there were many failures to replicate the Mozart effect (see Steele, Bass, & Crook, 1999). We noted above that failures to replicate may occur because the exact conditions for producing the effect were not used. In this case, Rauscher and Shaw (1998) responded to the many replication failures by precisely describing the conditions necessary to produce the Mozart effect. However, Steele et al. (1999) and McCutcheon (2000) were unable to obtain the effect even though they followed the recommendations of Rauscher and Shaw. Research on the Mozart effect continues. Some recent findings suggest that the effect is limited to music that also increases arousal; it is this arousal that can cause better performance following exposure to the music (Thompson, Schellenberg, & Husain, 2001). Bangerter and Heath (2004) present a detailed analysis of the development of the research on the Mozart effect. A single failure to replicate does not reveal much, though; it is unrealistic to assume, on the basis of a single failure to replicate, that the previous research is necessarily invalid. Failures to replicate share the same problems as nonsignificant results, discussed in Chapter 13. A failure to replicate could mean that the original results are invalid, but it could also mean that the replication attempt was flawed. For example, if the replication is based on the procedure as reported in a journal article, it is possible that the article omitted an important aspect of the procedure. For this reason, it is usually a good idea to write to the researcher to obtain detailed information on all of
  • 16. the materials that were used in the study. Page 302Several scientific societies are encouraging systematic replications of important scientific findings. The journal Perspectives on Psychological Science (published by the Association for Psychological Science) is sponsoring the publication of Registered Research Replications (http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/replication). Multiple groups of researchers will undertake replications of important studies using procedures that are made public before initiating the research. When completed, all of the replications will be described in a single report. In addition to the Psychological Science initiative, the online journal PLOS ONE (Public Library of Science) has developed the Reproducibility Initiative to encourage independent replication of research in the clinical sciences (Pattinson, 2012). Such developments should lead to greater understanding of the generalizability of research findings. Conceptual replications A conceptual replication is the use of different procedures to replicate a research finding. In a conceptual replication, researchers attempt to understand the relationships among abstract conceptual variables by using new, or different, operational definitions of those variables. Conceptual replications are even more important than exact replications in furthering our understanding of behavior. In most research, a key goal is to discover whether a relationship between conceptual variables exists. In the original Mozart effect study, researchers examined the effect of exposure to classical music on spatial reasoning. These are conceptual variables; in the actual study, specific operational definitions of the variables were used. Exposure to classical music was operationalized as 10 minutes of exposure to the Mozart Sonata for Two Pianos in D Major. Spatial reasoning was operationalized as performance on a particular spatial reasoning measure. In a conceptual replication, the same independent variable is operationalized in a different way, and the dependent variable
  • 17. may be measured in a different way, too. Conceptual replications are extremely important in the social sciences because the variables used are complex and can be operationalized in different ways. Complete understanding of any variable involves studying the variable using a variety of operational definitions. A crucial generalization question is whether the relationship holds when other ways of manipulating or measuring the variables are studied. Sometimes the conceptual replication may involve an alternative stimulus (e.g., a different Mozart sonata, a selection by a different composer) or an alternative dependent measure (e.g., a different spatial- reasoning task). Or as we previously noted, the same variables are sometimes studied in both laboratory and field settings. When conceptual replications produce similar results, our confidence in the generalizability of relationships between variables is greatly increased. This discussion should also alert you to an important way of thinking about research findings. The findings represent relationships between conceptual variables but are grounded in specific operations. You may read about the specific methods employed in a study conducted 20 years ago and question Page 303whether the study could be replicated today. You might also speculate that the methods used in a study are so unusual that they could never generalize to other situations. These concerns are not as serious when placed within the context of conceptual replications because, although operational definitions can change over time, the underlying conceptual variable often remains more consistent. Admittedly, a specific method from a study conducted at one time might not be effective today, given changes in today's political and cultural climate. A conceptual replication of the manipulation, however, would demonstrate that the relationship between the conceptual theoretical variables is still present. Similarly, the narrow focus of a particular study is less problematic if the general finding is replicated with different procedures. Evaluating Generalizations via Literature Reviews and Meta-
  • 18. analyses Researchers have traditionally drawn conclusions about the external validity of research findings by conducting literature reviews. In a literature review, a reviewer reads a number of studies that address a particular topic and then writes a paper that summarizes and evaluates the literature. The Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association provides the following description: “Literature reviews, including research syntheses and meta-analyses, are critical evaluations of material that has already been published.… By organizing, integrating, and evaluating previously published material, authors of literature reviews consider the progress of research toward clarifying a problem” (APA, 2010, p. 10). The literature review provides information that (1) summarizes what has been found, (2) tells the reader which findings are strongly supported and which are only weakly supported in the literature, (3) points out inconsistent findings and areas in which research is lacking, and (4) discusses future directions for research. Sometimes a review will be a narrative in which the author provides descriptions of research findings and draws conclusions about the literature. The conclusions in a narrative literature review are based on the subjective impressions of the reviewer. Another technique for comparing a large number of studies in an area is meta-analysis (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). In a meta-analysis, the researcher combines the actual results of a number of studies. The analysis consists of a set of statistical procedures that employ effect sizes to compare a given finding across many different studies. Instead of relying on judgments obtained in a narrative literature review, you can draw statistical conclusions from this material. The statistical procedures need not concern you. They involve examining several features of the results of studies, including the effect sizes and significance levels obtained. The important point here is that meta-analysis is a method for determining the reliability of a finding by examining the results from many different studies.
  • 19. Stewart and Chambless (2009) conducted a meta-analysis of research on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for anxiety disorders. Page 304Both a traditional literature review and a meta-analysis begin with a body of previous research on a topic; in this case, Stewart and Chambless located 56 studies using CBT with adults diagnosed with an anxiety disorder (including panic disorder, social anxiety, post traumatic stress disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder). Studies that included an additional medication treatment were excluded. The researchers performed a statistical analysis of the results of these studies and concluded that CBT was effective in treating all of the types of anxiety disorders. In a traditional literature review, it can be difficult to provide the type of general conclusion that was reached with the meta-analysis because it is necessary to integrate information from many studies with different experimental designs, disorders, and measures of anxiety. One of the most important reasons a meta-analysis can lead to clear conclusions is that meta-analysis studies focus on effect size (recall that an effect size represents the extent to which two variables are associated, see page 256). A typical table in a meta-analysis will show the effect size obtained in a number of studies along with a summary of the average effect size across the studies. More important, the analysis allows comparisons of the effect sizes in different types of studies to allow tests of hypotheses. For example, Miller and Downey (1999) analyzed the results of 71 studies that examined the relationship between weight and self-esteem. Table 14.1 shows a few of the findings. The effect size r averaged across all studies was −.18: Heavier weight is associated with lower self-esteem. However, several variables moderate the relationship between weight and self- esteem. Thus, the effect size is larger when the weight variable is a report of self-perceived rather than actual weight, and the relationship between weight and self-esteem is somewhat larger for females than for males. Finally, the effect is greater among
  • 20. individuals with a high socioeconomic background. TABLE 14.1 Some meta-analysis findings for weight and self- esteem Page 305Both narrative reviews and meta-analyses provide valuable information and in fact are often complementary. A meta-analysis allows statistical, quantitative conclusions whereas a narrative review identifies trends in the literature and directions for future study—a more qualitative approach. A study by Bushman and Wells (2001) points to an interesting way in which knowledge of meta-analysis can improve the way that we interpret information for literature reviews. The reviewers in their study were undergraduates who were provided with both titles and information about the findings of 20 studies dealing with the effect of attitude similarity on attraction. Sometimes the titles were salient with respect to the findings (“Birds of a Feather Flock Together”) and others were nonsalient (“Research Studies Who Likes Whom”). Salient titles are obviously easier to remember. When asked to draw conclusions about the findings, naive reviewers with no knowledge of meta-analysis overestimated the size of the similarity–attraction relationship when provided with salient titles. Other reviewers were given brief training in meta- analysis; these reviewers drew accurate conclusions about the actual findings. That is, they were not influenced by the article title. Thus, even without conducting a meta-analysis, a background in meta-analysis can be beneficial when reviewing research findings. USING RESEARCH TO IMPROVE LIVES In a presidential address to the American Psychological Association, George Miller (1969) discussed “psychology as a means of promoting human welfare” and spoke of “giving psychology away.” Miller was addressing the broadest issue of generalization, taking what we know about human behavior and allowing it to be applied by many people in all areas of everyday life. Zimbardo's (2004) presidential address to the
  • 21. American Psychological Association described many ways in which Miller's call to give psychology away is being honored. The impact of psychological research can be seen in areas such as health (programs to promote health-related behaviors related to stress, heart disease, and sexually transmitted diseases), law and criminal justice (providing data on the effects of 6- versus 12-person juries and showing how law enforcement personnel can improve the accuracy of eyewitness identification), education (providing methods for encouraging academic performance or reducing conflict among different ethnic groups), and work environments (providing workers with more control and improving the ways that people interact with computers and other machines in the workplace). In addition, psychologists are using the Internet to provide the public with information on parenting, education, mental health, and Page 306many other topics—for example, the websites of the American Psychological Association and the Association for Psychological Science (http://www.apa.org; http://www.psychologicalscience.org), national mental health resource websites (http://www.mentalhealth.gov/ and http://www.samhsa.gov/), and many individual psychologists who are sharing their expertise with the public. We have discussed only a few of the ways that basic research has been applied to improve people's lives. Despite all the potential problems of generalizing research findings that were highlighted in this chapter, the evidence suggests that we can generalize our findings to many aspects of our lives. ILLUSTRATIVE ARTICLE: GENERALIZING RESULTS Driving around in a 4,000-pound automobile is a dangerous thing. Motor vehicle accidents are among the leading preventable causes of death in the United States every year. Distraction is one of the most common causes of automobile accidents, and talking to another person is a very common distraction. In an effort to observe the impact of conversation on driving,
  • 22. Drews, Pasupathi, and Strayer (2008) conducted a study using a driving simulator that tracks errors committed by drivers. The researchers varied the type of conversation. In one condition, participants had a conversation with a passenger; in another condition, participants talked on a cell phone. There was also a no conversation, control condition. As you would expect, having any conversation resulted in more driving errors. However, the number of driving errors was highest in the cell phone condition. For this exercise, acquire and read the article: Drews, F., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. (2008). Passenger and cell phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 392–400. doi:10.1037/a0013119 After reading the article, consider the following: 1. Describe how well you think the sample of participants in this study generalizes to other groups of people. What about age? What about sex? 2. In this study, participants were told to have a conversation about a time when “their lives were threatened.” Do you think that the results of this study would be different if the conversation were about something else? How so? Why? 3. Do you think that the findings from this study would generalize to other cultures? Do you think that a sample of college students in Mexico, Italy, and Germany would generate similar results? Why or why not? 4. How well do you think the driving simulator generalizes to real-world driving? What would you change to improve the generalizability of the simulator?Page 307 5. Evaluate the internal validity of this study. Explain your answer. 6. Evaluate the external validity of this study. Explain your answer. Study Terms Conceptual replication (p. 302) Exact replication (p. 300)
  • 23. External validity (p. 292) Literature review (p. 303) Meta-analysis (p. 303) Replication (p. 300) Solomon four-group design (p. 298) Review Questions 1. What is external validity? 2. Why should a researcher be concerned about generalizing to other populations? 3. How can the fact that most studies are conducted with college students, volunteers, and individuals from a limited location and culture potentially impact external validity? 4. How does the use of the Internet to recruit subjects and collect data impact external validity? 5. What is the source of the problem of generalizing to other experimenters? How can this problem be solved? 6. Why is it important to pretest a problem for generalization? Discuss the reasons why including a pretest may affect the ability to generalize results. 7. Distinguish between an exact replication and a conceptual replication. What is the value of a conceptual replication? 8. What is a meta-analysis? Activities 1. It is easy to collect data for experiments and surveys on the Internet. Anyone in the world who is connected to the Internet can participate in an online experiment or survey. Use a search term such as “psychological research on the Internet” to find some studies that are being conducted. Page 308What issues of generalization might arise when interpreting the results of such studies? Does the computer aspect of the research make this research less generalizable than traditional research, or does the fact that people throughout the world can participate make it more generalizable? Could you empirically answer this question? 2. Use PsycINFO to find abstracts of articles that included race, ethnicity, gender, or nationality as a key variable.
  • 24. Consider topics such as body image, rumination, academic achievement, or identity development. What conclusions do the authors of these studies draw about generalization? 3. Find a meta-analysis published in a journal; two good sources are the Review of Educational Research and Psychological Bulletin. What conclusions were drawn from the meta-analysis? How were studies selected for the analysis? How was the concept of effect size discussed in the meta-analysis? ChapterResearch Training: Joys and Challenges It takes many years to become truly skilled at a complex set of tasks, such as becoming a skilled potter. For example, in Japan, it is common for an apprentice potter to make the same vase form for many months just to acquire the specific skills to consistently make 15–20 identical vases in a row. Expertise develops over time within an environment that fosters development. In this chapter we discuss the process of becoming a skilled researcher and the type of training environments that seem to be most helpful for students to acquire the necessary skills and attitudes. For many students in counseling, graduate school is their first introduction to research and research design, and it evokes many emotional reactions that come with any novel experience, from joy and excitement to anxiety and disenchantment. We seek to prepare students to approach research with enthusiasm for the creativity involved and with a willingness to learn the intricacies of the craft and the joy of creating new knowledge for the profession. In addition, we want to promote an awareness of the anxiety that may be created by learning a technical skill that may not be central to one's interests but is required to accomplish a goal (that is, obtaining a graduate degree and becoming a professional in counseling). We address four topics in this chapter. First, we provide a brief overview of how science intersects with counseling and
  • 25. psychology training through the scientist-practitioner model and the topic of evidence-based practice. Second, we identify and discuss some issues related to the developmental process of acquiring research competencies, specifically the joy as well as the challenges and fears. Third, we discuss ways in which counselors and counseling psychologists can train others to become competent, eager, and productive researchers. In this part of the chapter, we discuss what is known about research training, and how training environments can be structured to create the opportunity for students both to learn about research, and to consume and produce quality research products. Fourth, we discuss ways to broaden the concept of scientific training to include scientific thinking skills as well as research application skills. Most important, throughout this chapter we emphasize the developmental process of acquiring skills step by step to become a skilled researcher and scientist.SCIENCE AND PRACTICE The Scientist-Practitioner Model Bridging the gap between research and practice in counseling is an important issue (Murray, 2009). As we mention in Chapter 1, many graduate training programs in counseling espouse the scientist-practitioner model of training. Basically, this model consists of training in both scientific and practitioner activities; the basic assumption is that students trained in both science and practice will be better prepared for the multitude of employment demands in the counseling profession. The scientific activities include courses that focus on the philosophy of science, qualitative and quantitative designs and methods, statistics, evaluation, counseling research literature, and often involvement in research projects (Larson & Besett-Alesch, 2000). The practice-oriented side includes courses such as counseling methods, counseling theories, personality, assessment, and involvement in a variety of practicum experiences. When students enter a graduate training program in counseling, they typically have a wide range of interests along
  • 26. with the scientist-practitioner continuum. These interests often change over time, not only during graduate training but also throughout their career; thus, students need to prepare themselves broadly to allow for career changes over time. The scientist-practitioner model goes back to over 60 years. The clinical psychologists developed what they dubbed the scientist- practitioner model of training, which is also referred to as the Boulder model (Raimy, 1950). Meara et al. (1988) succinctly captured the scientist-practitioner model: Psychologists, whatever their work, are professionals and their attitude toward their work is scientific. The scientist- professional model is an integrated approach to knowledge that recognizes the interdependence of theory, research, and practice. The model emphasizes systematic and thoughtful analyses of human experiences and judicious application of the knowledge and attitudes gained from such analyses. An attitude of scholarly inquiry is critical to all the activities of those educated in the scientist-professional model. The model encompasses a variety of research methods, assessment techniques, and intervention strategies. The counseling psychologist is engaged in the pursuit and application of psychological knowledge to promote optimal development for individuals, groups, and systems (including families), and to provide remedies for the psychological difficulties that encumber them. To implement these goals, the scientist- professional psychologist adopts a scientific approach based on observation of psychological phenomena. This approach generates theoretical constructs and propositions, which are in turn tested as hypotheses (Claiborn, 1987; Pepinsky & Pepinsky, 1954). (p. 368) The scientist-practitioner model has been the predominant model of most counseling psychology programs with 73% of the programs specifically referring to the scientist-practitioner model in their program description (Horn et al., 2007). However, over the years there have been some critiques,
  • 27. especially in the earlier years about the utility of the scientist- practitioner model, which students may also have concerns about today. These included questions such as whether most practitioners use research findings in their practice. Moreover, it could also be that graduate training in practitioner activities does not adequately incorporate scientific activities, and vice versa (e.g., Stoltenberg et al., 2000). Some people might think that perhaps the type of research being conducted is too distant from the reality of the practitioner (see, e.g., Howard, 1984), or perhaps our research methods reduce counseling phenomena to meaningless numbers (see, e.g., Goldman, 1976). Yet another factor could be that students admitted to graduate programs have predominantly social or interpersonal interests (see, e.g., Magoon & Holland, 1984). Therefore, other training models have emerged. These models share the core goals of integrating science and practice, but with different emphasis. For example, the practitioner-scholar model places more emphasis on the training of practitioners and was closely associated with the emergence of professional schools of psychology and Psy.D. degrees. This model also parallels the idea of local clinical scientists (Stricker & Trierweiler, 1995) and practice-based evidence (Barkham & Mellor-Clark, 2000) that emphasize using clinical work and observations of clients to collect data and test clinical hypotheses. On the other end of the science-practice training spectrum, the clinical science model (Academy of Psychological Clinical Science, 2014) is similar to the scientist-practitioner model in theory, but places even more emphasis on science and uses empirical approaches to advance clinical science. The importance of science in the psychology field has not diminished, but rather has grown over the past few decades. There has been increased emphasis on utilizing scientific findings in mental health practices. Thus, it is important for students to have the scientific skills to critically evaluate research findings rather than simply accepting knowledge from research as authoritative prescriptions (Stoltenberg et al., 2000).
  • 28. Integrating the scientist-practitioner model with current trends in the mental health field is important, and there are recommendations on how the scientist-practitioner model can be better implemented (see Stoltenberg et al., 2000). Chwalisz (2003) suggested that evidence-based practice is the framework for training scientist-practitioners in the 21st century. The evidence-based practice movement in psychology has become a main focus over the past decade. Evidence-Based Practice The increased demands from health care systems and policies to integrate science and psychology practice have made evidence- based practice an important movement in the psychology and mental health field (Baker, McFall, & Shoham, 2008). Evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP) is “the integration of the best available research with clinical expertise in the context of patient characteristics, culture, and preferences.” (APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice, 2006, p. 273). The field of medicine is an example of how EBPP has shaped the improvement of patient outcomes in utilizing research in clinical practice (Woolf & Atkins, 2001). EBPP is a more comprehensive concept than a closely related movement of establishing empirically supported treatments (ESTs), which refers to the specific treatment that works for certain disorders or problems. Whereas ESTs focus on the treatment approach, EBPP starts with the client and with the attempt to utilize research evidence to inform treatment to achieve the best possible outcome for the client. Moreover, EBPP includes a broader scope of clinical/counseling aspects, such as assessment, case formulation, counseling relationships, etc. In APA's 2006 Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice, research evidence, clinical expertise, and patient characteristics were all emphasized as critical components leading to best practices. In terms of this research evidence, a broad array of scientific results related to intervention
  • 29. strategies, assessment, clinical problems, and client populations are all included. Multiple types of research evidence such as efficacy, effectiveness, cost effectiveness, cost benefit, epidemiological, and treatment utilization are all recognized as contributors to informing best psychological practices. EBPP also requires mental health practitioners to recognize that treatment method (Nathan & Gorman, 2007), counselors/therapists (Wampold, 2001), therapy relationship (Norcross, 2002), and the client (Bohart & Tallman, 1999) are all critical components to achieve best outcomes in psychological practices, and therefore research evidence on any of these aspects should not be overlooked. Clinical expertise is also a critical component of EBPP (APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice, 2006). Counselors and psychologists are recognized as essential contributors in identifying the best research evidence based on the client's characteristics, background, presenting issue, and preferences. Thus, it is important that counselors and psychologists are trained not only as practitioners but also as scientists so that their practice is informed by scientific literature. Client characteristics, culture, and preferences are also important parts of EBPP. Counselors and psychologists should take into account what works for whom when providing guidelines for effective practice. EBPP requires attention to client characteristics such as gender, sexual orientation, culture, ethnicity, race, age, religion, nationality, disability status, etc. In addition to client characteristics, it is also important to consider the client's values, religious beliefs, worldviews, goals, and preferences for treatment when determining and utilizing the best available treatment for the client. Although APA and many training programs have supported EBPP, there has also been resistance toward EBPP. Similar to the critiques of the scientist-practitioner model, there have been several reasons that EBPP raises concerns. Lilienfeld and colleagues (2013) examined the roots of resistance to EBPP by clinical psychologists and summarized six sources:
  • 30. (a) naïve realism, which can lead clinicians to conclude erroneously that client change is due to an intervention itself rather than to a host of competing explanations; (b) deep-seated misconceptions regarding human nature (e.g., mistaken beliefs regarding the causal primacy of early experiences) that can hinder the adoption of evidence-based treatments; (c) statistical misunderstandings regarding the application of group probabilities to individuals; (d) erroneous apportioning of the burden of proof on skeptics rather than proponents of untested therapies; (e) widespread mischaracterizations of what EBP entails; and (f) pragmatic, educational, and attitudinal obstacles, such as the discomfort of many practitioners with evaluating the increasingly technical psychotherapy outcome literature. (Lilienfeld, Ritschel, Lynn, Cautin, & Latzman, 2013, p. 883) In addition to highlighting the roots of resistance, Lilienfeld et al. (2013) also offered suggestions on how to address these resistances among students in training. They suggested exposing students to a better awareness that they are not immune from cognitive biases, such as confirmation bias, hindsight bias, and illusory correlation. They also suggested graduate instructors to not only convey accurate information, but also disabuse students of inaccurate information, especially around misunderstandings of the human nature. They also argue that students should not just be taught the “hows” of implementing therapy techniques, but more so the “whys” of how these approaches have been established as effective. In sum, Lilienfeld et al. stressed the importance of critical thinking and scientific training of students in the mental health field. For more detailed information on EBPP, as well as the resistance toward EBPP, please see APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice (2006) and Lilienfeld et al. (2013). Science and Practice Training The focus between research and practice has been an issue for training programs that emphasize both. Sometimes the model has been interpreted as a 50–50 split of performing science and
  • 31. practice activities. The type of model in its ideal form (i.e., implicitly 50% practitioner and 50% scientist/researcher) may be just that—an ideal that is rarely found in reality. Gelso (1979) proposed that it may be more realistic to train students in both domains (in varying degrees depending on their interests) with the expectation that students will find a suitable place for themselves in performing relevant activities on the scientist-practitioner continuum. Thus, one student might prefer a 20–80 split while another might choose a 75–25 split. Sometimes there have been implicit values attached to either science or practice; that is, some educators might value science more than practice, and thus feel more satisfaction when a new graduate obtains the “best” job, which to them is an academic position complete with a myriad of scientific pursuits. We strongly believe that science and practice are both highly valued activities in the counseling profession, and that as a profession we are stronger (and only can survive) when we train students to be competent in both science and practice. In short, it is important for the profession to equally value various points along this performance continuum. We prefer to conceptualize the core of the scientist-practitioner model in terms of scientific or critical thinking. In short, we are suggesting that the role of scientific or critical thinking is a central outcome of a wide range of science and practice activities. The choice of whether a graduate engages in science or practice activities is not the most important outcome, but rather whether the graduate can utilize scientific or critical thinking in whatever professional activities she or he chooses. For example, a counseling center staff member might be engaged primarily in direct client service and, say, one program evaluation project (see Chapter 22); this might be a 5%–95% science-practice split of professional activities. A faculty member (three-quarters time) with a quarter-time direct service appointment in the counseling center might have a 75%–25% split of professional activities. In addition to the quantitative difference in how research and practice is split, the role of
  • 32. research can also be qualitatively different. Gelso and Fretz (2001) proposed a three-level model of how research functions for counselors. First, the minimum level involves counselors being able to consume and apply research in their work with clients. The second level involves counselors being able to use critical thinking to develop and test hypotheses during their work with clients. The third level involves being able to conduct empirical studies and to present the research findings. Regardless of the type of professional activities a person selects along the scientist-practitioner continuum, we maintain that counselors utilize scientific thinking within their practice and science activities. Baker, McFall, and Shoham (2008) suggested that the lack of adequate scientific training and utilization of scientific knowledge has been a major problem in the development of the clinical psychology field, which is also a reflection of the mental health profession: Clinical psychologists' failure to achieve a more significant impact on clinical and public health may be traced to their deep ambivalence about the role of science and their lack of adequate science training, which leads them to value personal clinical experience over research evidence, use assessment practices that have dubious psychometric support, and not use the interventions for which there is the strongest evidence of efficacy. Clinical psychology resembles medicine at a point in its history when practitioners were operating in a largely prescientific manner. Prior to the scientific reform of medicine in the early 1900s, physicians typically shared the attitudes of many of today's clinical psychologists, such as valuing personal experience over scientific research. (p. 67) In the end, there is no doubt that research has enhanced the practice of counseling. Even though a practitioner may not be able to cite a specific reference, his or her graduate school training was likely based on a tremendous amount of research data, all the way from personality theory to intervention strategies. The accumulation may be slow, but the data
  • 33. eventually advance our working knowledge of the field. The scientific method and research have advanced the field, but they could be even more helpful. In addition, training in both science and practice has become more sophisticated with not only more integration but also more emphasis on critical thinking, which is the core of all science and practice activities of the counseling profession. For more information on a broad array of recommendations for enhancing training in the scientist- practitioner model and how EBPP is a framework for training scientist-practitioners in the 21st century, see Chwalisz (2003).JOYS AND CHALLENGES IN ACQUIRING RESEARCH COMPETENCIES Students will often experience a number of different reactions to research training, and these emotional reactions can change throughout the course of their graduate training and actually many years beyond (see Table 2.1). Sometimes students will be elated with the joy of discovering new and useful information from their research. Other times students will become restless and disgruntled with the minutiae associated with research. In addition, some students will experience obstacles such as negative self-talk, procrastination, perfectionist tendencies, and a range of “shoulds” (e.g., I should be better at math; see Heppner & Heppner, 2004). TABLE 2.1Common Student Reactions toward Research and Our Suggestions Student Feelings Our Suggestions I don't know enough to conduct any meaningful research. Volunteer to help on a research project; slowly work your way up to learn the more advanced skills. I am a fraud and really don't know how to do research. Feelings of anxiety and doubt associated with research are normal; they are often due to having limited experiences. What if I make a mistake and report inaccurate findings? It is a learning process; allow yourself time to learn and develop more advanced skills.
  • 34. I'm scared of running into debilitating writing blocks in my research. Acquiring research and thinking skills occurs over a long period of time; sometimes it is a sign that you may need to acquire more knowledge of the literature. I am afraid that my results will be statistically nonsignificant. Programmatic research often results in the accrual of knowledge over time; nonsignificant results are also helpful information. The whole thesis process seems overwhelming, tedious, long, anxiety producing, confusing, and complicated. The more you get involved in research, the more self- efficacious you will feel. I just want to practice; I am not interested in science/research. Be open to learning more about the research process as well as the research literature. science = statistics Science and research are broader than quantitative studies that utilize statistics. Critical thinking is the core of science. I don't believe that research in counseling can make a difference in the profession. Read literature review and meta-analysis articles to get a better understanding of cumulative knowledge base. Science is too intellectual, too cognitive, and unrelated to the experiences of real people. As you obtain more experience with research activities, your research interests may increase and you might see a higher value on the role of research. The developmental process of acquiring research competencies takes considerable time, years in fact, and often differs across students. Throughout this chapter are reflections from experienced and skillful researchers who provide some useful glimpses into this developmental process of acquiring an array of research competencies. A great deal of wisdom is communicated by these prolific and creative scholars, and we appreciate their candid perspectives on their own developmental journey.
  • 35. These reflections on their journeys throughout the chapter depict various aspects of the developmental process of acquiring research competencies. From our collective experiences as authors of this text, we have observed that many students question whether they can adequately learn the skills associated with research and science: How can I, a mere graduate student, come up with a good research idea? What if I get bogged down in my research and never finish my degree? Statistics scare me—what if I make a mistake in analyzing my data? We contend that these and other fears are common and developmentally normal. We believe it is essential to discuss the negative affective and cognitive components associated with research, and thus the first section focuses on such common student reactions, namely (a) performance anxiety and efficacy; (b) the false dichotomous conceptualization as research versus practice; and (c) belief in oneself as a researcher, and science in general. Performance Anxiety and Efficacy From our experience working with students, we have found that performance anxiety is a central and crucial affective issue. For example, common student disclosures include: “I don't know enough to conduct any meaningful research on my own. How can I do a thesis?” “Any study that I could do would be so basic and bland as to make it worthless.” “I feel completely inadequate in even conceiving a study that examines counseling from a unique perspective.” “Although I made it through the statistics and research design courses, once I start to do some research, I'll be ‘found out’; I am a fraud, and really don't know how to do research.” “What if I make a mistake and report inaccurate findings?” “I've had trouble writing before, and I'm scared to death that I will run into debilitating writing blocks again in my research.” “I am afraid that my results will be statistically nonsignificant, and all of my research efforts and time will just be one big waste.” “I've seen other students in the throes of a dissertation. The whole process seems overwhelming, tedious, long, anxiety producing, confusing, and complicated.”
  • 36. It is important to put these and similar feelings into the context of the typical entering graduate student. Most incoming graduate students have had some experience (or even a lot) with helping other people, and consequently the benefits of counseling and other practitioner activities are abundantly clear. Moreover, for most of us, helping people is a personally rewarding experience, and students have an understanding of those rewards. Conversely, it has been our experience that most incoming students have had considerably less experience with science. Typically, students do not imagine themselves making contributions to the profession by publishing in our professional journals. Since students have had little, if any, research experience, they often have legitimate reasons to question and doubt their skills and abilities—these feelings, concerns, and fears are thus developmentally normal. In fact, it is not uncommon even for faculty to experience performance anxiety and question their efficacy (e.g., see Research Journey 2.1). This suggests to us that acquiring the necessary research and thinking skills occurs over a long period of time; it does not happen all at once. In fact, expert performance is found associated with active engagement in deliberate practice, which involves training on particular tasks, receiving immediate feedback, having time for evaluation, problem solving, and the opportunity to refine behaviors (Ericsson, 2008). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.1 Developing competencies in research is not an event. Instead, it is a slow journey; as some markers are achieved, new competency goals quickly surface, even as a senior faculty member. I am committed to life-long learning! This applies to life in general and also to my life as a researcher. I did not always hear encouraging words from others about my ability to carry out a project or the importance of my work. Early in my career, I focused on the negative messages that I received. It took a while for me to tune out the naysayers and find ways to identify growth areas to strengthen my research so that it could
  • 37. contribute to the literature in the ways that I found important. I have had to learn how to believe in what I do and in my ideas. I was able to do this by seeking feedback from those who conducted similar research—from people who “got” my work and thus were able to identify strengths and limitations of the work in a constructive way. I now recognize that to be a good researcher requires feedback and input from a number of sources. To help develop a research project, it is absolutely essential to share your work with others and get feedback at all stages. Over the years, I have learned that it is okay to have a project be underdeveloped in its initial conceptualization— others do not judge me on where I start, but rather on where I end up. That is, how I have incorporated the ideas and suggestions from others and how much the project has developed over time. Knowing this has provided me the space to make mistakes, learn, and grow. —Helen Neville, Ph.D. As students acquire and begin to apply their knowledge about research design, we have been struck by many students' surprise and actual delight about research as they get past their initial fears. We often have heard comments like, “I'm actually enjoying this research course”; “I never thought I would be a researcher; now I'm beginning to reconsider my career goals”; “This research stuff is contagious; I'm starting to think about research studies a lot and I want to collect data now!” (e.g., see Research Journey 2.2). In sum, the major point is that beginning graduate students often have considerably less information about research and science than about practitioner activities. Moreover, there is evidence that as students obtain more experience with research activities (especially if it is early in their training), their research interests increase and they place a higher value on the role of research (e.g., Gelso, Baumann, Chui, & Savela, 2013). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.2 When I started graduate school, my dream was to become a
  • 38. director of a counseling center. Research was not part of my career dreams, and I had no sense that I could ever publish in a professional journal. However, subsequent research experiences in my coursework, especially with my doctoral advisor, David N. Dixon, shifted my career aspirations toward academia. Several years later after having a modicum of success in publishing, I realized that the most important goal was the accumulation of knowledge from the systematic study of a particular topic, and subsequent theory development. Several more years later, I more fully realized the importance of the context surrounding my research topic, and in particular, the many variables (age, SES, race) within participants' cultural context that greatly affected our research findings. I also realized our research efforts alone were insufficient to create the knowledge bases needed for tomorrow, and that the worldviews and power of journal editors, editorial boards, and others in leadership positions in our profession played a critical role in the creation of our future knowledge bases. This realization led me to engage in a number of professional roles (e.g., editorial boards, assuming editorships, involvement and leadership positions in professional organizations), all aimed at improving knowledge bases and promoting social justice in counseling psychology. In the end, one of the things I greatly value in our profession is the many ways that we can join hands with diverse partners to create knowledge bases and theories to promote the development of a broad array of people. —Puncky Heppner, Ph.D. Thus, our advice to students regarding performance anxiety is first of all to recognize that feelings of anxiety and doubt associated with research are normal, and often reflect where one is in the developmental process of acquiring new competencies, in this case research competencies. Second, we strongly recommend that students get involved with research projects to slowly develop various research competencies. We believe the best way to create a sense of research efficacy is to learn and perform those skills. For example, students might offer to
  • 39. volunteer to help on a research project to learn the basic research skills, and slowly work their way up to learn the more advanced skills (e.g., see Research Journey 2.3). In essence, both our experience and our research have suggested that the more students get involved in research, the more self-efficacy they tend to feel; the more efficacious students feel about conducting research, the more positive they are about the training environment and interest in research (Gelso et al., 2013; Kahn, 2001). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.3 I had the good fortune of becoming Professor Donald Atkinson's doctoral advisee in counseling psychology at the University of California, Santa Barbara, back in 1996. I learned firsthand via Don's work the power of research—that is, good research work can lead to significant changes in our profession and society (e.g., increased attention to multiculturalism). It was also during that time that my research training became more solidified through several courses on research methods and statistics (by the way, I used the first edition of this book in my main research methods course and it couldn't have offered me a better start!). Most importantly, however, it was the day-to-day work on several research studies with Don and other students that taught me the intricacies of research, starting from problem conceptualization and moving into research design, data collection, data analyses, and manuscript publication. So in hindsight, I mostly “learned by doing.” And I've come to understand that there is no magic formula to becoming a good researcher but to have good people around you who also are enthusiastic about research. At this point in my career, as a full professor with tenure and the editor of Asian American Journal of Psychology, I continue to believe that research success is a function of inquisitive and informed researchers working together for a common goal. So my recommendation continues to be: “Get involved, join a research team, study the topic of your passion, and don't forget to enjoy the process! The
  • 40. outcome will then take care of itself.” —Bryan S. K. Kim, Ph.D. Threats, Unrewarding Training, and Disenchantment For any of a number of reasons, students' research training is often less than rewarding, and actually often rather threatening, all of which can result in student disenchantment. It is not uncommon for students to enter graduate school with ambivalence and anxiety about the academic and research requirements that they have to meet over the next several years. Many students are intimidated by their instructors and professors; in particular those that teach courses in which the students feel less competent in (such as statistics and research methodology, which are less directly associated with counseling practice). Moreover, the idea of coming up with an innovative research topic for their thesis is quite daunting. It is hard enough to generate adequate research questions for a study, let alone finding measures, collecting data, statistical analysis, and writing all these up into a lengthy thesis! With the anxiety experienced around research, the program and training can feel quite threatening. Unfortunately, when students have little experience with research and science, one bad experience can create tremendous attitudinal and motivational damage and move a student a long way from future scientific activities. Some training programs may actually decrease students' interest in research. The profession has come a long way in enhancing research training over the last two decades. Consideration of students' affective reactions, values, and individual differences, and of the training environment will most likely continue to enhance the development of students' research efficacy. In addition, consideration of cultural values and social connections are especially important issues for some racial/ethnic minority and international students whose cultural values make it more difficult to seek and participate in the research groups without others' invitation or help. The False Dichotomous Conceptualization of Science versus
  • 41. Practice Another developmental issue is how students conceptualize science and research, and their relationship to practice. A common statement is, “I just want to practice; I am not interested in science/research.” Sometimes the scientific enterprise is erroneously conceptualized as totally separated from applied work in counseling. Thus, a false dichotomy is established, science versus practice. Actually there is interplay between science and practice. Our belief is that those students who develop skills in both arenas will be better equipped to do the work of the counseling profession (for example, see Research Journey 2.4). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.4 When I started my undergraduate studies, I was sure that I wanted to be a police detective. Although I realized that path was not in the cards for me, the drive to find solutions to mysteries has never left. I quickly realized when it comes to research that the world is far more complex than I originally thought. Additionally, with complex questions come complex answers that frequently result in multiple equally valid solutions. This process is what draws me into doing research and at the same time the multitude of possibilities and enormous amounts of information can be paralyzing—a common theme I hear from my students. Yet I have found two sources of inspiration: (a) working with others (counselors, researchers, and clients) to be sure I am expanding the way I am thinking about any given topic and to stay connected to the lived experience of those who matter the most, and (b) tackling specific questions so that each study is manageable. In this way, each study is akin to adding to an ongoing conversation, and developing a research agenda or doing multiple studies in the same area allow for the conversation to continue. I find this process very gratifying and rewarding! —Jesse Owen, Ph.D.. Moreover, one of the hallmarks of graduate work in counseling is to acquire critical thinking skills, the ability to identify and
  • 42. process information with fewer biases, stereotypes, and assumptions; formulate hypotheses; gather data; and make informed decisions. These critical thinking skills are essential in both the practice and science arenas. Higher critical thinking ability has been found as a key characteristic of master therapists (identified as having exceptional mastery of therapy by their peers; Jennings & Skovholt, 2015; Ronnestad & Skovholt, 2013; Skovholt, 2012). A qualitative meta-analysis of six studies that examined characteristics of master therapists in different countries found that master therapists have developed more sophisticated and complex conceptualization of clients' presenting issues (Jennings & Skovholt, 2015). One of the key themes identified across master therapists in all the studies was the capacity for cognitive complexity and intricate conceptualization. This capacity was developed through ongoing learning through reflection, feedback, and teaching others. Our advice to students is to avoid a narrow conceptualization of research and science and to not prematurely conclude that research and science have little or nothing to offer the practitioner (Skovholt, 2012). Rather, we encourage practice- oriented students to be open to learning more about the research process as well as the research literature, and how these experiences can inform practice and improve their counseling competence. The scientific method is simply a set of tools to establish a controlled mode of inquiry about all of our professional activities, research and practice. Disbelief in Oneself as a Researcher and Science in General A critically important belief in the process of becoming a competent researcher is believing in one's ability to conduct meaningful research as well as belief in the utility of research, that research does contribute to the profession's knowledge bases, and that our research can and does make a difference in clients' lives (e.g., see Research Journey 2.5). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.5
  • 43. Being involved in research that has the potential to make a difference is a meaningful aspect of my work and motivates me to persist when encountering inherent obstacles in the research process. After graduating from college with degrees in Biology and Sociology (and a minor in Women's Studies), I worked in an inner city battered women's shelter. At the shelter, I became interested in what differentiated women who returned to the abuser from women who struggled to create nonviolent lives for themselves and their children. In part, this desire to understand critical social problems motivated me to purse a master's degree, where I focused my thesis on shelter workers' perceptions of battered women. I loved how my work in the shelter informed and was informed by my research. I also began to see that while helping people individually was rewarding to me, I might be able to make a difference through sharing my research findings with others. My doctoral program solidified my passion for research as I was given the opportunity to study topics of interest that had real-world applications, including helping women to achieve economic self-sufficiency. I sometimes struggle with a component of research (e.g., collecting data from difficult to obtain samples), but as soon as this task becomes very painful, the next step in research brings new and exciting challenges (e.g., analyzing the data!). Also, working with students and colleagues on research teams makes the research process a wonderful, fun, and memorable adventure. —Karen O'Brien, Ph.D. It is common for students to not initially feel efficacious as a researcher nor to believe that our research in counseling can make a difference in the profession. Most beginning students do not have much of an understanding of the role of science in our profession, and since they do not have a long history in the profession they are unable to identify how previous research has contributed to the profession. Similarly, students do not have a clear understanding of the utility of the scientific method as a way of thinking about either research or clients. Finally, the
  • 44. student typically has had relatively little experience of expanding his or her understanding about clients and applied problems by engaging in research, reading the professional literature, and contemplating the implication of empirical findings. One unfortunate outcome is that students sometimes fill this void with misinformation, like “science = statistics.” Or students believe that “science is too intellectual, too cognitive, and unrelated to the experiences of real people.” Or “the scientific method is so mechanical and reductionistic that it cannot be used adequately to study the complexities of real human behavior.” Conversely, seasoned researchers understand both the challenges of conducting meaningful research and the gradual but steady accumulation of knowledge from our research. Even though it is rare that any one study has a major impact on the profession, programmatic research often does result in the accrual of knowledge over time, and often achieves significant conclusions. For example, many of the chapters in the APA Handbook of Counseling Psychology (Fouad, Carter, & Subich, 2012) are based on extensive literature reviews and nicely reflect the utility of our research in creating knowledge as well as enhancing the sophistication of our conceptualization of relevant constructs in counseling. Clara Hill, a creative and prolific counseling researcher, was invited to write a personal account of her evolution as a researcher. Her account illustrates how she made decisions, how complex the process of research can be, and how she coped with the ups and downs along the way and, most important, her “crisis of faith in research”: After this series of studies I underwent a crisis in terms of research. I had achieved tenure, had my first baby, and turned 30. I started questioning everything. Because I could see so many imperfections in my research, I was not very proud of my work. It had taken an incredible amount of time for little “hard information.” I could not even remember the results of many of my studies. Further, I seemed woefully far from even describing what happens in the counseling process let alone understanding
  • 45. what counselor behaviors were useful in effecting change. I despaired that counseling was far too complex to be studied. I had previously had lots of doubts about my research, but it was mostly related to self-confidence in my research abilities. This crisis seemed to have more to do with whether I felt that research was a viable means to answer questions about the counseling process. (Hill, 1984, p. 105) Hill's behind-the-scenes disclosures are illuminating as well as often relieving for many students and faculty. We strongly encourage students to read this article, perhaps during their first course on research methods. We have found that Hill's frustrations, jubilations, feelings of inadequacy, and doubts about the role of research resonate strongly with students. The evolution of Hill's confidence in the utility of conducting research in her own way not only sends a useful message, but also illuminates the developmental processes of students and faculty in becoming skilled and competent researchers. From our experience, it is critical not only that researchers feel efficacious in conducting relevant and important research, but also that they begin to perceive that their research is important, that it contributes useful information to our professional knowledge bases, and that it can make a difference in the profession as well as the lives of our clients. Such beliefs often are the result of identifying implications of your research findings to practice, being cited in the professional literature in meaningful ways, or seeing either that others are using your research findings in applied settings or that your research is helping other researchers subsequently ask other important questions. Belief in the utility of research can also result from reading the professional literature to learn the impact of others' research. Our advice is for students to not only read and reflect on the literature, but also to read literature reviews in particular (see Major Contributions in The Counseling Psychologist or review papers in Psychological Bulletin). In addition, we advise students to get involved in conducting research on topics that
  • 46. are really important to them, either topics that they are very interested in or ones that reflect their personal values in some way (e.g., see Research Journey 2.6). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.6 I firmly believe that the best researchers usually have a connection and passion about their research. I developed my research program, in part, to help me understand and make sense of my own experiences as an African American man in a predominantly White college environment struggling to do well academically, while at the same time trying to learn more about my history and culture. This desire to better understand myself and the experiences of other people like me led to a dissertation, my first publication, several other articles, and a book. Everyone has experienced challenges in writing and conducting research at some point, and everybody has experienced rejection when trying to publish something. I have learned that it is not always the “smartest” people who can successfully carry out a research project and publish it, but often it is those individuals who are the most resilient. —Kevin Cokley, Ph.D.A MODEL FOR RESEARCH TRAINING The purpose of this section is to discuss a number of factors that are important in enhancing research training. Our goal is not only to enhance students' awareness of what appears to be some of the major constructs in research training, but also to promote additional attention and research on this important developmental process. Based on the culminated research on effective research training, Kahn (2001) proposed a model that incorporated various factors, which is presented in Figure 2.1. We will discuss this model and the empirical evidence that supports parts of it, as well as add a few other important aspects of research training not included in the model. We begin by discussing each of the major constructs in the model: (a) investigative interests, (b) research training environment, (c) relationship with mentor, (d) research outcome expectations, (e) research self-efficacy, (f) research interest, (g) scholarly
  • 47. activity, and (h) year in program. FIGURE 2.1Parameter Estimates for the Modified Structural Model of Kahn (2001) Constructs of the Model Investigative Interests Most students in counseling and counseling psychology can be categorized into one of Holland's (1992) three personality types: social, artistic, or investigative. Research has shown that the personality type of counseling students is related to their level of interest in research and in research courses, choice of graduate program types with varying levels of research focus, and to research productivity (Mallinckrodt et al. 1990; Tinsley, Tinsley, Boone, & Shim-Li, 1993). Specifically, it appears that students who are investigative types have more interest in research activities and ultimately produce more research (e.g., see Research Journey 2.7). Accordingly, Kahn (2001) found that a student's investigative interest would be directly related to expectations of research outcome and research interest, which in turn would be related to research productivity. In particular, investigative types are hypothesized to be more interested in research, and social types less interested. RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.7 My passion for science originated at a very early age, when I found myself setting up mini “experiments” to test various questions. I was curious about the world and about human interactions in the world. At this young age I also was acutely aware of individuals whose experiences in the world were different from my own. I viewed humans as equal with unequal opportunities. After multiple undergraduate transfers, I was fortunate to have ended up in a college that provided me access to mentoring and advising that enabled my interests, skills, and efficacy in science to flourish. Were it not for these individuals and my experiences, I would have never realized that a doctoral degree or a career as a scientist-practitioner was a viable
  • 48. option. Throughout my graduate training and career, my passion to share my excitement for the scientific process in a way that is meaningful for others (e.g., via knowledge in research design and analysis, consultation with organizations, developing and testing hypotheses with clients in psychotherapy) has continued to blossom. As I reflect upon my research journey, I appreciate the combination of factors that have contributed. These personal characteristics (I am deeply curious about the world), beliefs (I believe that science has the potential to impact practices and prompt positive change in the world), and values (opportunities, and access to them, matter) continue to shape the work in which I choose to engage and the questions that I choose to ask. Mindi Thompson, Ph.D. Research Training Environment Gelso, Baumann, Chui, and Savela (2013) discussed 10 ingredients of graduate training programs that foster positive attitudes toward research. These components have been identified as important features of the research training environment: (a) faculty modeling of scientific behavior; (b) the positive reinforcement of scientific activity, both formally and informally; (c) early and minimally threatening involvement in research; (d) emphasizing science as a partly social-interpersonal experience; (e) emphasizing that all studies are limited and flawed; (f) teaching and valuing varied approaches to research; (g) teaching students to look inward for research ideas when they are developmentally ready to do so; (h) the wedding of science and practice; (i) relevant statistics and the logic of design; and (j) teaching how research can be done in practice settings. An illustration of a positive training environment that included the 10 components would be having an environment that involved students in research as early as possible and in ways that are interesting, involving, and consistent with their skill level. Many counseling students are socially oriented, increasing the social aspects of research through the forms of advising, and research teams will increase the attractiveness of the endeavor for them. In research groups, students of various
  • 49. levels can participate to the extent that they feel comfortable and have something to contribute. Research advising and modeling is beyond transferring skills and involves having research mentors exhibit healthy attitudes toward research including both the excitement and disappointment in response to its trials and tribulations. Honest expressions of reinforcement to students all along the way in the research process are an important part of building interest. Providing a nonintimidating atmosphere for learning is important. Students should not feel pressured to design the perfect study; rather, they should feel motivated to create a study that can address their research questions. Also, many counseling students have little fondness for statistics and are turned off to research because they equate doing research with studying for statistics examinations. Some students will be attracted to quantitative methods and others to qualitative methods, and these preferences should be respected and honored. Not only do students benefit by using a method that they enjoy, but the field benefits from this methodological pluralism (Gelso et al., 1988). A true integration of science and practice will make research more attractive, especially to the majority of students whose primary interest is in practice. Looking inward to rely on clinical experience is an example of wedding science and practice. Students who have a deep interest in their topic finish their dissertations faster than those who are motivated to finish their programs but who pick a convenient topic of study. Not all research environments are ideal, but it is important to make the most out of them (e.g., see Research Journey 2.8). RESEARCH JOURNEY 2.8 I applied to graduate school with the goal of conducting community-engaged research on Asian immigrant families and disseminating this research through community organizations and churches. However, I ended up at a graduate program that did not have such research opportunities at the time. Rather than get disheartened, I had to make the most of what was available