SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 61
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002David et al. •
Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
PRABU DAVID
GLENDA MORRISON
MELISSA A. JOHNSON
FELECIA ROSS
Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
Social Distance and Social Identification
in Third-Person Effects
The perceived effects of advertising on body-image factors were
tested in both
Black and White college-age women. After seeing magazine ads
that portrayed
either Black fashion models or White fashion models,
respondents rated per-
ceived effects of these ads on body-image factors. The effects
were rated on self,
on other Black women on campus, and other White women on
campus. When
projecting perceived effects on others—of the same race or a
different race—
both Blacks and Whites indicated that media effects would be
maximal when
the race of the model matched the race of the respondent.
However, when rating
perceived effects on self, whereas Blacks identified strongly
with Black models,
there was no significant difference in the way Whites identified
with fashion
models of either race. The results are examined within the
framework of social
distance and social identification.
Biases in social judgment about perceived negative media
effects on self and
others has drawn considerable attention from communication
scholars, par-
ticularly those who are interested in the connection between
such “third-
person perceptions” and the willingness to restrict media
content (e.g.,
Cohen, Mutz, Price, & Gunther, 1988; Davison, 1983; Duck,
Terry, & Hogg,
1995; Eveland, Nathanson, Detenber, & McLeod, 1999; Gunther
& Hwa,
1996; Perloff, 1989; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997).
For instance,
Shah, Faber, and Youn (1999) found a significant relationship
between
third-person perceptions and the willingness among respondents
to censor
advertising of controversial services, such as gambling. Other
researchers
have found support for controls on violent or misogynic rap
music (McLeod,
Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997), pornography (Gunther, 1995;
Rojas, Shah, &
270
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH, Vol. 29 No. 3, June 2002
270-294
© 2002 Sage Publications
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Faber, 1996), television violence (Rojas et al., 1996), and
political communica-
tion (Rucinski & Salmon, 1990; Salwen, 1998). Although the
link between
third- person perceptions and behavioral outcomes, such as
support for or
against policies, has not been clearly established, the underlying
theoretical
issues merit attention.
One of the interesting aspects of the third-person phenomenon
is the
social-distance corollary. As the social distance between a
respondent and a
comparison group increases, there is a widening of the third-
person gap. In
the typical demonstration, when college students are asked to
estimate the
perceived effect of media on self, followed by perceived effects
on other stu-
dents on campus (a group similar to the respondents), and
finally perceived
effects on other people in the state (a group that is socially
farther away and
less similar to the student participants), perceived effects on
other people in
the state has been observed to be greater than perceived effects
on students
on campus (e.g., Cohen et al., 1988; Gunther, 1991).
Perloff (1993) explained the social distance phenomenon on the
basis of
psychological distance. Psychological distance is generally
conceived as
degree of similarity or dissimilarity between the respondent and
the compar-
ison group on critical dimensions relevant to the media effect.
Some of the
dimensions of social and psychological distance have been
examined and
results are mixed with some interesting but predictable patterns
(Brosius &
Engel, 1996; Cohen et al., 1988; Eveland et al., 1999; McLeod
et al., 1997).
Brosius and Engel (1996) varied psychological distance by
using two
targets—friends and acquaintances as the close group and
people in general
as the distant group—and found that perceived effects were
greater for the
general group. Duck and Mullin (1995) varied comparison
groups on two
dimensions, namely vagueness and closeness. While closeness
was
operationalized as friends versus others, vagueness was varied
from the
“average person” to a specific person, such as a named
celebrity. Their find-
ings suggest that perceived effects were the greatest when the
comparison
group was a vague, distant other.
In a couple of studies, however, the social-distance effect has
been con-
founded by other more salient attributes such as likelihood of
exposure to the
message and severity of the message. In these studies (McLeod
et al., 1997;
Eveland et al., 1999) in which misogynist rap and death-metal
lyrics were
used as stimuli, students rated a specific intended target
audience as more
vulnerable than the more general group. For example, in the
earlier study
(McLeod et al., 1997), youth from New York or Los Angeles
were perceived to
be more vulnerable to “gangsta” rap lyrics than the average
person. Intu-
itively this is a powerful finding because it is logical to expect
youth in New
271
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
York and Los Angeles to be more vulnerable to gangster rap
because of the
gang-related reputations of these cities.
Eveland and colleagues conducted a similar two-part study
(Eveland
et al., 1999). In the first part, groups were “distanced” by age
and education.
Although there was partial support for the social-distance
corollary, again
respondents appeared to build into their answers common-sense
judgments
about who might be exposed to non-mainstream media such as
violent and
misogynist music. In several tests, betas for perceived media
exposure were
significantly larger than betas for perceived social distance,
overturning the
traditional social distance corollary. However, the present study
examines
fashion advertising, which is ubiquitous in U.S. mass media and
hence not
dependent on selective exposure.
In addition to perceived exposure considerations, the
aforementioned
study also dealt with media that feature violent or hateful
messages, ranging
from violent action films to death-metal music.
If the media stimuli are socially undesirable at some level, such
as porno-
graphic material or misogynistic lyrics, perceived exposure to
media is likely
to be highly relevant. Even before respondents focus on the
negative per-
ceived effect on others, it is likely that they engage in a subtler
form of third-
person bias by distancing themselves and people close to them
from exposure
to socially undesirable media content.
In short, it appears the effects of social distance are contingent
on general-
ity or specificity of the target group, similarity or dissimilarity
between the
respondent and the target group, the comparison group’s
perceived likelihood
of exposure to the target stimuli, and real-world heuristics.
Although a number of social-distance attributes have already
been exam-
ined, to our knowledge third-person differences based on race
or ethnicity
have not been studied. Moreover, as a variable, race offers a
promising avenue
to examine psychological mechanisms underlying in-group
versus out-group
differences based on social identification. Perloff (1993) noted
that social dis-
tance could be conceptualized in at least two ways: along a
continuum of “just
like me” to “not at all like me” or in terms of the homogeneity
and size of the
group. Race appears to fit both these continua. Moreover, as
media continue
to be more segmented and personalized, media researchers will
have to find
appropriate ways to study media perceptions among in-groups
and out-
groups. The third-person framework provides a viable platform
for testing
differences based on social identification.
Hence, in this article, we extend previous work on the social-
distance com-
ponent of third-person effects by introducing race as a marker
of social or psy-
chological distance. Using social identity as a theoretical
foundation, we focus
on how Black and White women perceive effects of advertising
on body-image
272
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
factors. The findings are discussed within the context of our
research on the
role of the media in shaping body-image ideals in young
women.
Media Portrayals and the Ideal Body Image
The ultra-thin model and the idealized images of beauty
presented as the
norm in advertising and other media content have been
criticized by scholars,
media critics, and popular-media writers. Researchers in public
health have
found that the mismatch between the ideal body image
portrayed in the
media and one’s actual body image leads to body-image
dissatisfaction
(Richins, 1991; Stice, Schupak-Neuberg, Shaw, & Stein, 1994;
Stormer &
Thompson, 1996). In extreme cases, body-image dissatisfaction
or physique
anxiety hurts an individual’s self-esteem or self-concept and
could even lead
to behavioral consequences such as eating disorders (e.g.,
Thompson, 1995).
Although the relationship between media use and body-image
percep-
tions is tenuous, there is an emerging body of research
documenting signifi-
cant relationships. For example, Harrison (2000) found a
significant relation-
ship between bulimia and exposure to television shows with
overweight
characters. In the same study, a significant relationship was
found between
thin-ideal magazine content and increased anorexia. Others have
found sig-
nificant relationships between selective media use and drive for
thinness
(Botta, 1999, 2000; Harrison & Cantor, 1997) and between
media exposure
and body-image elasticity (Myers & Biocca, 1992). Given these
findings, we
set out to examine self-reported perceptions of media effects on
body image.
Media Effects on Body-Image Factors
For the purposes of this study, media influences are presented
as a cluster of
effects with varying levels of undesirability, collectively
referred to as
body-image factors. If body-image dissatisfaction can be
expressed as a mis-
match between ideal and actual body-image perceptions, the
role of media
can be examined by assessing the extent to which they are
responsible for cre-
ating and triggering these body-image ideals. Body-image
dissatisfaction
itself is not inherently insidious as much as are the negative
outcomes that
could stem from the dissatisfaction. In some cases, body-image
dissatisfac-
tion could be channeled into a desirable outcome. For example,
the Nike com-
mercial of the “Just Do It” genre that exhorts viewers to
exercise is in some
ways equivalent to a public-service announcement that seeks a
positive out-
come. But the focus of this study is media portrayal of thin,
idealized models
in fashion magazines.
273
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
As just noted, one behavioral outcome measure of body-image
media
effects on women is an eating disorder such as anorexia or
bulimia. However,
in our framework, we explore a range of effects, including the
effect on percep-
tion of ideal body weight (a perceptual outcome) and effect on
perception of
one’s self-esteem (an attitudinal or psychological outcome). In
other words,
the dependent variables in our framework extend hierarchically
from the
less-desirable media effect that affects perceptions of ideal
body weight, to
the more undesirable effect on the respondent’s psychological
state, to the
most undesirable behavioral outcome that is manifested as an
eating disor-
der. This hierarchy of body-image media effects is referred to as
body-image
factors in this study. These factors have been examined in the
past and the
results suggest that as the degree of undesirability of the
outcome increases,
the third-person gap widens (David & Johnson, 1998).
Body-Image Dissatisfaction, Eating Disorders, and Race
Conventional wisdom and a growing body of empirical evidence
suggest that
Black women are less vulnerable to body-image dissatisfaction
and are more
proud of their bodies than Anglo-Americans and Hispanics
(Altabe, 1998;
Fitzgibbon et al., 1998; Story, French, Resnick, & Blum, 1995).
Studies show
that Blacks are less concerned about weight than are Whites
(White, Hudson,
& Campbell, 1985), exhibit fewer weight-reduction behaviors
such as dieting
and exercising (Kann & Ross, 1996), and have lower incidences
of eating dis-
orders than Whites (Gray, Ford, & Kelly, 1987). Ironically,
however, African
Americans exhibit these positive body-image attributes despite
having
higher body-mass index scores and higher fat composition than
Whites
(Akan & Grilo, 1995; Ellis, Abrams, & Wong, 1997; Field,
Colditz, & Peterson,
1997; French et al., 1997). In general, health researchers have
been more con-
cerned about obesity in African Americans than eating
disorders.
Harris (1995) observed that the varying degrees of emphasis on
thinness
among Blacks and Whites could be attributed to culturally
ingrained norms,
with thinness as a strongly internalized ideal among White
women, but less
of an ideal among Black women. Some argue that the
individualistic nature
of some White cultures emphasizes individual control—
including control of
body— rather than dependence on the family, which is more
common to some
Black cultures.
Despite the racial and ethnic differences on body image, a
number of vari-
ables associated with eating disorders are common among
women of all races.
For instance, regardless of race, eating disorders have some
common mark-
ers, such as perfectionist personality traits and dysfunctional
family environ-
ments. One study found that Black girls who were teased about
their weight
274
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
had a significantly higher drive for thinness than those who
were not teased
(Striegel-Moore, Schreiber, Pike, Wilfley, & Rodin, 1995),
suggesting that
Blacks are not immune to the social pressures of ideal body
image. Silber
(1986) argued that as Blacks make strides in the workplace,
there is growing
pressure to conform to the norms of the dominant culture,
namely the Euro-
centric ideals of beauty and attractiveness.
After taking into consideration the differing body-image values
and
norms between races, we expected some differences in third-
person percep-
tions about body image. At least two underlying processes are at
play when
projecting to others of a different race—one, the traditional
third-person
effect that widens with social distance; and two, the
confounding of social dis-
tance by race because of differences in perceived vulnerabilities
of the two
races, which are driven by cultural stereotypes and conventional
wisdom.
The findings by Eveland et al. (1999) would suggest that Whites
would be
more vulnerable to body-image media effects because it is
generally con-
strued that White women rather than Black women are the
intended targets
of ads that emphasize extreme thinness.
Social Distance and Social Identification
Dissimilarity based on race can be examined from the
standpoint of categori-
zation processes that are described in social-identity theory.
Social-identity
theory holds that our self-concept is based in part on the way
we perceive oth-
ers and how we interact with them (Hogg & Abrams, 1999;
Serino, 1998;
Tajfel, 1978, 1981; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner & Onorato,
1999). Self-
concept depends not only on personality traits of an individual
but also on
how an individual relates to others within a group. Because part
of a person’s
self-concept is based on group membership, social comparisons
between
groups produce pressures to earn collective self-esteem for the
in-group rela-
tive to out-groups. Self-categorization theory is a companion
theory that
specifies the social-cognition aspects and contrasts with others.
Using social identity and social comparison, some researchers
have
focused on racial and ethnic identity (Liebkind, 1992; Pelham &
Hetts, 1999)
because of its usefulness in studying stereotypes, prejudice, and
other behav-
iors in interracial settings. Prior research in this area suggests
that an indi-
vidual’s beliefs about personal and social identities play a key
role in group-
relevant judgments (Pelham & Hetts, 1999).
Studies in social-identity theory and social categorization
theory do not
specifically deal with the concept of social distance. However,
the intersection
between social distance and racial identity and how it affects
intergroup per-
ception has been explored. One study on race and social
distance found that
275
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Blacks rated Whites to be less acceptable than Whites rated
Blacks in the
three domains studied: classmate or coworker, next-door
neighbor, and inti-
mate contact (Hraba, Radloff, & Ray-Gray, 1999).
Social-identity scholars might explain this by looking at the
cultural
norms of the respondents, honing in on the individualistic
nature of some cul-
tural groups and the collectivist cultures of others. Collective
cultures put
more emphasis on the welfare of the group than the welfare of
the individual.
For instance, studies have found that there is a slight tendency
for individu-
alists to be less discriminating than collectivists in in-group
favoritism
(Morales, López-Sáez, & Vega, 1998).
Race, Social Distance, and Third-Person Effects
Our research objective was to formulate social distance using
race as a
marker and to examine how third-person media effects are
perceived for
in-groups and out-groups. If an ethnic or racial group is
perceived to be
socially more distant, the social-distance corollary of the third-
person effect
should hold up. The bias against out-groups or a bias in favor of
the in-group
can be interpreted as a self-serving motivational bias that
enhances the posi-
tions of in-group members. This bias in favor of the in-group
can be tied to
social identification.
In previous research on third-person effects, social distance has
been stud-
ied primarily as distance between the respondent and the
comparison group.
Another component of social distance is the distance between
the source of
the media message and the respondent. In essence, social
identity extends in
two directions: from the source of the message to the
respondent (Tyler &
Cook, 1984) and from the respondent to the comparison group.
Therefore, in
this article we attempt to extend previous research by
manipulating two com-
ponents of social distance that are critical to eliciting the third-
person effect:
similarity between the source and respondent, and similarity
between the
respondent and the comparison group. Similarity and
dissimilarity were
operationalized on the basis of race, which was an intentional
design artifact.
Hypotheses and Research Questions
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of how
body-image por-
trayals in fashion advertising are perceived by Black and White
college-age
women. In addition to perceived effects on self, both groups of
students esti-
mated perceived effects on other Black students and other White
students.
Both groups responded to two sets of magazine ads, one with
only White fash-
ion models and another set with only Black fashion models.
276
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
The first hypothesis captures the basic third-person perception
that oth-
ers are more vulnerable to media effects than self.
Hypothesis 1: Perceived effect on others will be greater than
perceived
effect on self.
The next hypothesis is derived from findings reported by David
and John-
son (1998) that the third-person gap widens as the media
outcome becomes
more undesirable. In other words, the third-person difference
for the eating-
disorder outcome would be greater than the third-person
difference for the
self-esteem outcome, which in turn will be greater than the
third-person gap
for the ideal-body-weight outcome.
Hypothesis 2: The difference between perceived effect on self
and others
will widen with an increase in social undesirability of the
outcome.
The next hypothesis is essentially a test of social identification,
which sug-
gests that respondents are more likely to identify with a similar
source who is
part of their in-group than someone who is dissimilar and part
of the out-
group.
Hypothesis 3: Perceived effect on self will be greater when the
race of the
respondent matches the race of the model in the ad than in the
mis-
matched condition.
In general, one would expect a widening of the third-person gap
with an
increase in social distance and dissimilarity between self and
the comparison
group. However, because of cultural stereotypes and the
different body-
image norms among Blacks and Whites, which were discussed
earlier, a dif-
ferential effect was predicted on the basis of race of the
respondent.
Hypothesis 4: For Black college women, perceived effect of
media on other
White women (dissimilar to self) will be greater than perceived
effect
on other Black women (similar to self).
Hypothesis 5: For White college women, perceived effect on
other White
women (similar to self) will be greater than perceived effect on
other
Black women (dissimilar to self).
In addition to these hypotheses, the critical research question
for the
study was to examine the interaction among race of the fashion
model, the
race of the respondent, and the race of the comparison group.
277
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Research Question 1: What are the interactions among the race
of the
respondent, the race of the comparison group, and the race of
the fash-
ion model on body-image factors?
Method
Scales
Two psychological constructs, self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965)
and social-
physique anxiety (Hart, Leary, & Rajeski, 1989), were measured
using 10-
item scales. Third-person perceptions were measured using a
11-point scale,
where 0 = no influence and 10 = considerable influence.
Stimulus
Ten ads were chosen from major fashion magazines, such as
Vogue, Cosmo-
politan, Essence, and Ebony. The authors chose the ads with
help from gradu-
ate students. Half of these ads featured thin Black models and
the other half
featured thin White models. These models were chosen on the
basis of thin-
ness, attractiveness, skin tone, hair type, and sexiness. The
Black models var-
ied from dark to lighter skin tones, with short natural hair to
straightened
hair. The skin tones of the White models varied from extremely
fair to well
tanned, with hair color ranging from brunette to light blonde.
We tried to match the ads featuring the Black and White models
on a num-
ber of dimensions, including style of dress, extent of skin
exposed, activity or
pose, and the mood conveyed by the ad. To minimize the role of
familiarity,
ads with well-known supermodels were excluded.
Participants and Study Design
Eighty female undergraduate students participated in the study
for extra
credit. One half of the participants were Black, and the other
half were White.
In the absence of a random between-subjects experimental
manipulation, it
would be accurate to characterize this study as a quasi-
experiment, with a 2
(Respondent’s race: Black, White) × 2 (Order: perceived effect
on self first,
perceived effect on self last) × 2 (Race of the fashion models:
Black, White) × 3
(Social distance: self, other Black women, other White women)
mixed design.
Respondent’s race was the only between-subjects factor. Race
of the fashion
models and social distance were both within-subject factors.
278
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Procedure
When students arrived, they were given a consent form and the
self-esteem
and physique-anxiety scales, which were stapled together as a
booklet. After
filling out the scales, students evaluated the ads. Each ad was
evaluated on
three 10-point scales, which tapped attractiveness of the model,
thinness of
the model, and the similarity between the model’s body type
and the respon-
dent’s body type.
The ads were evaluated in two sets, with five ads in each set.
One set of ads
featured only Black models, and the other set featured only
White models.
After evaluating all the ads in a set, respondents rated the
perceived effect of
these ads on one’s own body image, the perceived effect on
other Black (or
White) women on campus, and the perceived effect on other
White (or Black)
women on campus.
Perceived effect of advertising was obtained for three body-
image factors:
ideal body weight, self-esteem, and likelihood of developing an
eating disor-
der. Perceived effects of advertising on all the body-image
factors were mea-
sured using an 11-point scale (0 = no influence and 10 =
considerable influ-
ence). The question was phrased using the following format:
How much
influence do you think these ads have on your perception of
ideal body
weight? This question was varied by changing the comparison
group to “other
Black women on campus” and “other White women on campus,”
and repeated
for “effect on self-esteem” and the “likelihood that that these
ads could lead to
an eating disorder.”
One half of the students rated the ads with Black models first,
and the
other half rated the ads with White models first. Order of
evaluation of the
third-person perceptions was counterbalanced through the use of
four ver-
sions of the third-person questionnaire (two versions for the
Black students
and two versions for the White students). In one version, the
first assessment
was on self, which was followed by other Black/White women
(same race as
the respondent), which was followed by other White/Black
women (of a differ-
ent race). Essentially, in one version the questions began with
perceived
effect on self and extended to those who were most dissimilar.
In the other
version, the questions began with perceived effects on those
who are most dis-
similar and moved inward toward perceived effects on self. This
was a conser-
vative protocol, given that previous explorations of the impact
of question
order on third-person perceptions have found minimal
differences (David &
Johnson, 1998; Gunther, 1991, 1995; Price & Tewksbury, 1996;
Tiedge,
Silverblatt, Havice, & Rosenfeld, 1991).
279
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
At the end of the experiment, before debriefing, students
answered some
media-use questions and provided personal data including race,
height, and
weight.
Results
Manipulation Check
Black students found Black models more attractive (M = 6.0,
SD = 2.5) than
White models (M = 3.9, SD = 2.4). They rated their body types
to be more simi-
lar (M = 3.3, SD = 2.4) to the body types of the Black models
than to the body
types of the White models (M = 2.1, SD = 2.3). Also, Black
students rated the
White models as thinner (M = 7.7, SD = 2.0) than the Black
models (M = 6.7,
SD = 1.8). White students, on the other hand, found Black
models to be
slightly more attractive (M = 6.9, SD = 2.3) than White models
(M = 6.1, SD =
2.3). In line with the ratings provided by the Black students,
White students
also found the White models to be thinner (M = 8.0, SD = 1.4)
than the Black
models (M = 7.5, SD = 1.5). For similarity of body type, White
students did not
see any differences based on the race of the model. All
significant differences
were at p < .05 and were tested using paired t tests (see Table
1).
Although the significant differences in similarity and
attractiveness by
the race of the models for Black respondents is indicative of the
intended
social identification manipulation, there was a reversal among
White stu-
dents, who found Black models more attractive and saw no
difference in simi-
larity based on the race of the model. However, the finding that
both Blacks
and Whites found the White models to be thinner than Black
models does
raise the question of the validity of our manipulation. Despite
our finding of
null difference in the pretest, in which the set of ads with White
models and
280
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
Table 1
Mean Ratings of Attractiveness, Thinness, and
Similarity to Fashion Models by Race of the Students
Black Respondents White Respondents
Black White Black White
Models Models Models Models
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Attractiveness 6.0 2.5 3.9 2.4 6.9 2.3 6.1 2.3
Thinness 6.7 1.8 7.7 2.0 7.5 1.5 8.0 1.4
Similarity 3.3 2.4 2.1 2.3 4.0 2.3 3.7 2.3
Note. Responses were measured on a 10-point scale. Higher
scores mean higher attractiveness,
thinness, and similarity.
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
the set with Black models were administered between subjects,
some signifi-
cant differences for thinness were observed in this study.
However, because
the focus of our study is on relative differences between
assessments rather
than the absolute value of the perceived media effect, we
continued with the
analysis. Also, we have tried to control for this difference by
using the thin-
ness evaluation as a covariate in the analysis of variance.
Next, we compared the physique anxiety and self-esteem scores
by the
race of the respondent. Reliabilities were high for both scales,
with
Cronbach’s alpha = .93 for physique anxiety and Cronbach’s
alpha = .84 for
self-esteem. The physique-anxiety score and self-esteem score
for Black par-
ticipants were not significantly different from the corresponding
scores for
White participants. These results suggest that both groups were
comparable
in terms of the their physique anxiety and self-esteem scores.
Analysis of Variance
Given the complexity of the design, each of the body-image
factors, namely
ideal body weight, self-esteem, and eating disorders, were
analyzed sepa-
rately. We used a 2 (Black respondent, White respondent) × 2
(Order: per-
ceived effect on self first, perceived effect on self last) × 2
(Black fashion mod-
els, White fashion models) × 3 (perceived effect on self,
perceived effect on
others of a similar race, perceived effect on others of a different
race) mixed-
factor design. In addition, physique anxiety, self-esteem,
thinness rating of
Black models and thinness rating of White models were entered
as covariates.
In preliminary analysis, when self-esteem was entered as a
covariate, it was
not statistically significant. In subsequent analysis, self-esteem
was not
included as a covariate. A summary of the means is presented in
Table 2.
For the ideal-body-weight outcome, main effects for physique
anxiety, F(1,
73) = 9.98, MSE = 11.43, p < .005, ω2 = .06, and thinness
rating of Black mod-
els, F(1, 73) = 10.26, MSE = 11.43, p <.005, ω2 = .06, were
significant. In addi-
tion, the expected three-way interaction, Race of Model ×
Comparison
Group × Race of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 30.16, MSE = 3.01, p
< .001, ω2 = .09,
was significant.
For the self-esteem outcome, main effects for physique anxiety,
F(1, 73) =
7.33, MSE = 9.56, p < .01, ω2 = .03, and thinness rating of
Black models, F(1,
73) = 10.72, MSE = 9.56, p <.01, ω2 = .05, were significant.
The critical
three-way interaction captured by Race of Model × Comparison
Group × Race
of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 22.63, MSE = 3.41, p < .001, ω2 =
.08, was also
significant.
For the eating-disorder outcome, the main effect for physique
anxiety, F(1,
73) = 4.87, MSE = 13.59, p < .05, ω2 = .03, and the thinness
rating of Black
281
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
models, F(1, 73) = 4.11, MSE = 13.59, p < .05, ω2 = .03, were
significant. The
targeted three-way interaction among Race of Model ×
Comparison Group ×
Race of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 22.28, MSE = 2.25, p < .001,
ω2 = .05, was
significant.
In within-subjects designs, multiple observations are obtained
from the
same subject and the independence assumption is violated,
which in turn
inflates the F value. The Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon and the
Huynh-Feldt
epsilon are corrections to the F value to account for the
violations of com-
pound symmetry and sphericity. After applying these
corrections, the critical
three-way interaction was significant at p < .001.
In summary, physique anxiety, which was entered as a
covariate, had a sig-
nificant effect on the ratings provided by the respondents. Also,
the thinness
rating of the Black models was significant. The order in which
the third-
person ratings were obtained was not significant. The key
finding was the
expected three-way interaction—Race of the Model ×
Comparison Group ×
Race of Respondent—which was significant for each of the
three body-image
factors after controlling for the difference in perceived thinness
between
Black and White models. To explain this interaction, we
examined the rela-
tionships among these three factors through a series of mean
comparisons.
282
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
Table 2
Perceived Effects of Advertising on Self and Others as a
Function
of the Race of the Respondent and the Race of the Fashion
Model
Black Respondents White Respondents
Black White Black White
Models Models Models Models
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Body Weight
Self 5.5 3.1 3.6 3.0 5.8 3.1 5.9 2.4
Other Blacks 6.1 2.5 3.7 2.4 6.3 2.1 5.0 1.9
Other Whites 4.3 3.4 9.1 1.8 6.7 2.0 7.5 1.5
Self-Esteem
Self 3.9 2.7 2.5 2.5 4.5 2.7 4.5 2.5
Other Blacks 5.3 2.1 3.3 2.4 5.8 2.3 4.8 2.0
Other Whites 3.9 3.5 8.7 1.3 5.9 2.1 6.8 2.0
Eating Disorder
Self 2.1 3.0 1.6 2.7 2.2 2.6 2.5 2.7
Other Blacks 3.5 2.6 2.8 2.2 5.2 2.1 4.0 2.4
Other Whites 4.2 3.5 8.6 1.6 5.4 2.1 6.2 2.1
Note. Means are ratings on a 11-point scale from 0 (no
influence) to 10 (considerable influence).
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Third-Person Effects When Race of
Model Matched Race of the Respondent
First, perceived effect on others of the same race was
significantly greater
than perceived effect on self when there was a match between
the race of the
model and the race of the respondents. Among Whites, the
perceived effect on
self and on other White respondents was significantly different
for the body
image outcome (Self M = 5.9, Other Whites M = 7.5, Mean
difference = 1.6,
t(39) = 4.69, p < .001), the self-esteem outcome (Self M = 4.5,
Other Whites M =
6.8, Mean difference = 2.3, t(39) = 7.58, p < .001), and the
eating-disorder out-
come (Self M = 2.5, Other Whites M = 6.2, Mean difference =
3.7, t(39) = 8.82,
p < .001). Also, as the undesirability of the outcome increased,
the third-
person gap widened significantly among the White respondents
(see Figure 1).
Among Blacks, the third-person effect was significant for only
two of the
three outcomes, and there was no systematic widening of the
gap with
increasing undesirability. The perceived effect on self and other
Black stu-
dents was not significant for the body-weight outcome (Self M
= 5.5, Other
Blacks M = 6.1, Mean difference = 0.6), but was significant for
the self-esteem
outcome (Self M = 3.9, Other Blacks M = 5.3, Mean difference
= 1.4, t(39) =
2.90, p <. 005) and the eating-disorder outcome (Self M = 2.1,
Other Blacks
M = 3.5, Mean difference = 1.4, t(39) = 3.15, p < .005).
Baseline Effects on Self Among Blacks and Whites
When the race of the model matched the race of the respondent,
there were no
statistically significant differences in perceived effect on self
between Black
and White respondents for all three outcomes, namely perceived
effect on
ideal body weight, effect on self-esteem, and likelihood to lead
to an eating
283
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
Figure 1. Perceived Effect of Same-Race Fashion Models on
Self and Other Women of
the Same Race for Three Body-Image Factors
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
disorder. The absence of a significant difference between
Blacks and Whites
on perceived effect on self runs contrary to the conventional
wisdom that
Black women consider themselves less vulnerable to body-
image media
effects than White women. The implications of this finding will
be taken up in
the discussion section.
Race of the Model and Perceived Effect on Self
For the Black respondents, perceived effect on self was
significantly different
when the race of the model was different, suggesting strong
social identifica-
tion. When exposed to Black models, Blacks showed
significantly higher
effect on self (body weight M = 5.5, self-esteem M = 3.9, eating
disorder M =
2.1) than when exposed to White models (body weight M = 3.6,
self-esteem
M = 2.5, eating disorder M = 1.6). All paired t tests were
significantly different
at p < .05. In contrast, among White respondents there was no
significant
effect of the race of the model on the perceived effect on self.
Possible reasons
for the absence of social identification among the White
participants are
addressed in the discussion.
Race of the Model and Perceived Effect on Others of the Same
Race
The purpose of this line of analysis was to examine how
participants project
social identification onto others of their race. Whereas the
Black respondents
continued to indicate strong social identification for other Black
women
(their in-group), the White respondents also indicated strong
social identifi-
cation for their in-group, which was a reversal of the pattern
that Whites pro-
vided for themselves. Black respondents stated that other Black
women
would be affected more when the race of the model was Black
(body weight
M = 6.1, self-esteem M = 5.3, eating disorder M = 3.5) than
when the race of
the model was White (body weight M = 3.7, self-esteem M =
3.3, eating disor-
der M = 2.8). By the same token, Whites indicated that other
Whites would be
influenced more by White models (body weight M = 7.5, self-
esteem M = 6.8,
eating disorder M = 6.2) than they would by Black models
(body weight M =
6.7, self-esteem M = 5.9, eating disorder M = 5.4). All tests
were significant at
p < .05.
Race of the Model and Perceived
Effect on Others of a Different Race
Finally, we examined the perceived effect on other students of a
different race
(out-group) as a function of the race of the model. When
projecting to other
284
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
students of a different race, both Blacks and Whites rated the
out-groups as
showing strong social identification. However, there was a
marked asymme-
try in terms of how Blacks and Whites saw social identification
by the out-
group. Black participants’ estimation of social identification by
the White
students was significantly higher than the White participants’
estimation of
social identification by other Whites and Blacks.
Among Black participants, perceived effects on other White
students after
exposure to White models were notably higher (body weight M
= 9.1, self-
esteem M = 8.7, eating disorder M = 8.6) compared to perceived
effects after
exposure to Black models (body weight M = 4.3, self-esteem M
= 3.9, eating
disorder M = 4.2). Among White respondents, although a
similar pattern was
evident, the size of the projected effects of social identification
was smaller,
with perceived effects on other Black students after exposure to
Black models
(body weight M = 6.3, self-esteem M = 5.8, eating disorder M =
5.2) being
higher than effects estimated after exposure to White models
(body weight
M = 5.0, self-esteem M = 4.8, eating-disorder M = 4.0). All
relevant within
group mean differences were tested using paired t tests and
were significant
at p < .01 (see Figure 2).
In the discussion, the results are examined within the
framework of
social-identification theory and the cultural and racial
stereotypes associ-
ated with body-image factors.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine third-person
perceptions related to
body-image factors by manipulating two key variables on the
basis of race:
social distance between the respondent and the comparison
group, and social
distance between the respondent and the media source. In
addition, we tried
to replicate the results reported in a study by David and Johnson
(1998),
wherein the third-person effect widened with the undesirability
of the
body-image outcome. To study these issues, we examined five
hypotheses and
some research questions. The discussion is grouped into three
sections. We
will start with the findings related to the third-person effect.
Then we will
take up the results associated with social distance before
discussing the
interaction between social distance and race.
Third-Person Effects
To examine the basic third-person effect within this quasi-
experimental
design, the perceived effect on others was examined without any
confounding
in terms of the race of the model or the race of the comparison
group. In other
285
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
words, the third-person results discussed in this section are
based on per-
ceived effect of media on self and others of the same race
following exposure
to fashion models also of the same race as the respondent.
In support of Hypothesis 1, there was strong evidence of third-
person
effects across the board, which indicates that respondents
perceived media
effects on others to be stronger than effect on self. As predicted
in Hypo- the-
sis 2, the third-person gap widened with increasing
undesirability of the out-
come. This trend was particularly strong among White
respondents but was
not as pronounced among Black respondents. Among Whites,
the third- per-
son gap increased from 1.57 for body weight to 2.33 for self-
esteem to 3.67 for
eating disorders. Among Blacks, the gap increased from 0.68 for
body weight
to 1.35 for self-esteem to 1.43 for eating disorders. As
discussed later, the dif-
ference in magnitudes also suggests that Black and White
students’ beliefs
about the power of media differ.
The dynamics of the widening of the third-person gap with
outcome
undesirability was similar to the findings reported by David and
Johnson
(1998). As the undesirability of the outcome increased, the
White partici-
pants trimmed down the effect on self but did not make
comparable adjust-
ments for others. Another way to interpret the perceived
differential effects
between self and others is that while participants made
adjustments for the
likelihood of occurrence on self for the more serious outcomes,
which have a
lower base rate of occurrence, they failed to make similar
adjustments when
extrapolating to others.
One explanation for the lack of clear support for Hypothesis 2
among
Blacks could be due to the differences in the interpretation of
undesirability
of the outcomes. First, based on previous findings, there is
evidence that ideal
286
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
Figure 2. Perceived Effect on Self and Others as a Function of
the Race of the Respon-
dent, Race of the Fashion Models, and Race of the Comparison
Group for the
Self-Esteem Outcome
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
body weight is not treated with as much emphasis among Black
women as it
is among White women. Hence, the absence of a third-person
effect for body
weight among Blacks is not surprising. Furthermore, the
absence of the wid-
ening of the third-person gap with undesirability can be
attributed to the low
incidence of clinical eating disorders among Blacks compared to
Whites.
Social Distance and Third-Person Effect
When social distance was narrowly defined based on race, the
traditionally
robust effects of social distance were nullified or reversed. It
appears that the
participants’ perceptions of in-group and out-group norms took
precedence
over normative social distance defined along racial lines.
Race and the Perceptions of Body-Image Media Effects on Self
Conventional wisdom, cultural differences, and some findings
in the litera-
ture (e.g., Akan & Grilo, 1995; Altabe, 1998) suggest that Black
women are
less vulnerable to body-image effects compared to Whites.
Although this
study does not provide irrefutable evidence to the contrary, the
findings from
this study suggest that there were no statistically significant
differences
between Blacks and Whites in terms of perceived media effects
when the race
of the model matched the race of the respondent.
The absence of a significant difference between Blacks and
Whites on per-
ceived effects on self deserves attention. Social commentators
and media
observers have noted that as Black women make progress in
their careers
and achieve higher status, they feel the same pressure that
Whites do to con-
form to the ideal body image that dominates the overall culture.
It is also pos-
sible that the effect of media images on self was heightened for
the Black par-
ticipants in this study because we matched the race of the media
model with
the race of the respondent. It is plausible that even if Blacks can
generally
ignore the ideals of body image presented in the mainstream
media predomi-
nantly through White models, they could be vulnerable to body-
image pres-
sures presented in the Black media in the form of Black fashion
models. This
highlights the need for social responsibility among ethnic media
as well as
general-market media in body-image portrayals.
Social Identification Hypothesis
We had predicted in Hypothesis 3 that the perceived effect on
self would be
greater when the race of the model matched the race of the
respondent. This
was true for the Black participants but not for Whites. Across
all three
287
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
outcomes, among White participants there were no statistically
significant
differences on perceived effect on self on the basis of the race
of the model.
There are at least three explanations for the absence of a main
effect for
race of the model on White respondents. One is the social
desirability bias not
to appear racist. This supports the motivational explanation of
third-person
effect, because not being racist is an esteem-preserver or
enhancer. The other
interpretation is that the fashion industry selects models that fit
common
standards of thinness and attractiveness and the White students
did not see
a significant difference based on the race of the model. Studies
show, for
instance, that Black models in advertisements tend to be light-
skinned
(Entman & Book, 2000). Nonetheless, White students perceived
the race of
the model to have a significant effect on other White women
and other Black
women. In short, although White respondents’ own ratings
suggest that they
saw themselves as color blind, they did not project the same
pattern to other
in-group (other Whites) and out-group (other Blacks) women.
A third possibility is that Blacks in the entertainment industry—
includ-
ing fashion—may be more accepted than in other social roles.
Even if White
students perceived Black models as different from them, the
fashion industry
is a safe milieu in which to be different. White college students
may have per-
ceived racial difference in fashion models as exotic—an ideal in
the fashion
industry. Merelman (1995) would argue that this is an example
of the success
of Black cultural projection among Whites, who are accustomed
to seeing
Blacks in glamorous roles in the entertainment industry. Our
finding that
White students found Black models to be more attractive is one
indicator of
this.
Unlike White respondents, Black respondents showed strong in-
group
social identification, which is expected among ethnic or
minority groups in a
pluralistic culture. The differences in social identification
between Blacks
and Whites led to other interesting effects when respondents
were asked to
project the perceived effect of race of the model to other Whites
and Blacks.
Social Identification and Projecting to Others
Using the race of the fashion model as a cue for social
identification, we tested
the validity of hypotheses presented in Hypothesis 4 and
Hypothesis 5. The
findings related to Hypothesis 4 and Hypothesis 5 varied
considerably based
on the race of the model and the race of the respondent,
indicating strong
interactions with social distance and social identification.
Among Blacks, perceived effects on other White women (out-
group) was
greater than perceived effects on other Black women (in-group)
only after
exposure to the White fashion models. After exposure to Black
fashion
288
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
models, Blacks rated perceived effects on other Black women
(in-group)
higher than perceived effects on other White women (out-
group). This offers
only partial support for Hypothesis 4 because the social distance
corollary
was held only for Black fashion models.
Among Whites, perceived negative effect of media on other
White women
(in-group) was greater than perceived effect on other Black
women (out-
group) when the race of model was White. Essentially, this was
a reversal of
the social-distance corollary, which was predicted in Hypothesis
5. But when
the race of the model was Black, there were no statistically
significant differ-
ences in terms of how the White respondents perceived effects
on in-group
and out-group.
In summary, when projecting to others, both Blacks and Whites
perceived
significant social identification by other women. This effect was
significant
for others of the same race and others of a different race. When
projecting
media effect on others, the key difference between Black and
White respon-
dents was the magnitude of the effect. This mismatch is best
captured by the
ratings provided by Blacks for the Whites (out-group), which
ranged from 8.6
to 9 on a 10-point scale, compared to ratings of 6.2 to 7.5
provided by Whites
for their in-group.
In this article we have focused primarily on social identification
as the
mechanism to explain the perceived differences associated with
the race of
the model. An alternate explanation for the perceived immunity
that African
Americans expressed toward White fashion models might be
rooted in their
suspicions of the general-market media in the United States,
where histori-
cally they are accustomed to negative portrayals (e.g.,
Greenberg & Brand,
1994; Wilson & Gutiérrez, 1995). Finally, one cannot ignore the
role of racial
and cultural differences when interpreting these results. The
conventional
belief that Blacks are less vulnerable than Whites to body image
factors is a
powerful heuristic, which certainly could have played an
important role in
these judgments about others.
Directions for Future Research
Black students’ manifestation of a stronger social identity may
be explained
by the more collectivist leanings of minority cultures. In
addition, because
social comparisons between groups produce pressures to
achieve collective
self-esteem (e.g., Hogg & Mullin, 1999), African-American
respondents may
have exhibited a greater desire to preserve Black group self-
esteem. This is a
topic that deserves further scrutiny. One of the other areas of
social-identity
theory not previously discussed is the natural tension between
an individ-
ual’s desire to assimilate and to differentiate (e.g., Brewer &
Pickett, 1999).
289
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
At one level, the basic third-person perception is an example of
differentia-
tion, whereas the strong in-group versus out-group pattern
exhibited by
Blacks could be an indicator of assimilation. In the future, it
would be benefi-
cial to examine the differences within and between groups in
terms of how
they relate to differentiation and affiliation.
The results point to a need to explore the interplay between
social identity
and social-desirability aspects of the third-person. Social
psychologists Tajfel
and Turner (1979) claimed that definition of the “other” stems
from the need
to maintain a positive social identity. How Black and White
college students
conceive their own identities and introduce “otherness” into
their vision of
media effects on themselves and others is an area to pursue. In
addition, how
cultural projections via mass media might mediate these
identities and ver-
sions of otherness is a compelling area of study.
One of the limitations of our study is the thinness manipulation
because
there was a significant difference in the perceived thinness of
the Black and
White models. Although we tried to address this by introducing
thinness rat-
ings of the models as covariates in the analysis, it is possible
that this differ-
ence could have skewed the results. Clearly this study should be
replicated
with stimuli that are more rigorously balanced on all the critical
attributes.
Finally, this study is a quasi-experiment with only 80
participants, which has
the typical limitations of any experiment. To improve ecological
validity, it
would be useful to replicate the study with a larger survey
sample.
Conclusions
The findings from this study confirm some of the popular
stereotypes about
perceived effects of glamour advertising on body-image factors
in Black and
White college-age women. There were a number of significant
findings. Both
Blacks and Whites exhibited strong third-person effects when
there was no
confounding by race. Also, the third-person gap widened with
increasing
undesirability of the outcome, particularly among Whites. When
projecting
media effects on others, both Blacks and Whites indicated that
others would
identify more closely with same race or in-group models than
with different
race or out-group models.
When estimating perceived effects on self, however, Blacks and
Whites dif-
fered slightly. Whereas Whites reported that the race of the
model does not
make a significant difference on perceived effect on self, Blacks
identified
closely with Black models. Moreover, contrary to the widely
held perception
that Blacks are less vulnerable to body-image effects compared
to Whites,
there were no significant differences between the two groups on
perceived
effect on self when the race of the models matched the race of
the
290
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
respondents. In the face of corroborating evidence (e.g., Botta,
2000), it seems
plausible that the gap between Blacks and Whites on body-
image vulnerabil-
ities might be closing.
We hope the findings from this study add to the discussion on
the role of
advertising in creating norms of ideal beauty. Obviously, more
systematic
research programs are needed to accurately assess negative
body-image con-
sequences that could result from the idealized beauty presented
in the media.
References
Akan, G. E., & Grilo, C. M. (1995). Sociocultural influences on
eating attitudes
and behaviors, body image, and psychological functioning: A
comparison
of African American, Asian American, and Caucasian college
women.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(2), 181-187.
Altabe, M. (1998). Ethnicity and body image: Quantitative and
qualitative
analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23(2), 153-
159.
Botta, R. A. (1999). Television images and adolescent girls’
body image distur-
bance. Journal of Communication, 49(2), 22-41.
Botta, R. A. (2000). The mirror of television: A comparison of
black and white
adolescents’ body image. Journal of Communication, 5(3), 144-
159.
Brewer, M. B., & Pickett, C. L. (1999). Distinctiveness motives
as a source of
the social self. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. John
(Eds.), The psychol-
ogy of the social self (pp. 71-87). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brosius, H., & Engel, D. (1996). The causes of third-person
effects: Unrealistic
optimism, impersonal impact, or generalized negative attitudes
toward
media influence? International Journal of Public Opinion
Research, 8(2),
142-162.
Cohen, J., Mutz, D., Price, V., & Gunther, A. (1988). Perceived
impact of defa-
mation: An experiment on third-person effects. Public Opinion
Quarterly,
52(2), 161-173.
David, P., & Johnson, M.A. (1998). The role of self in third-
person effects about
body image. Journal of Communication, 48(4), 37-58.
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in
communication. Public Opin-
ion Quarterly, 47(1), 1-15.
Duck, J. M., & Mullin, B. (1995). The perceived impact of the
mass media:
Reconsidering the third person effect. European Journal of
Social Psy-
chology, 25, 77-93.
Duck, J. M., Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1995). The perceived
influence of AIDS
advertising: Third-person effects in the context of positive
media content.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(3), 305-325.
Entman, R. M., & Book, C. (2000). Light makes right: Skin
color and racial
hierarchy in television advertising. In R. Andersen & L. Strate
(Eds.), Crit-
ical studies in media commercialism (p. 214-224). New York:
Oxford.
Ellis, K. J., Abrams, S. A., & Wong, W. W. (1997). Body
composition of a young,
multiethnic female population. American Journal of Clinical
Nutrition,
65(3), 724-731.
291
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Eveland, W. P., Jr., Nathanson, A. I., Detenber, B. H., &
McLeod, D. M. (1999).
Rethinking the social distance corollary: Perceived likelihood of
exposure
and the third-person perception. Communication Research,
26(3), 275-302.
Field, A. E., Colditz, G. A., & Peterson, K. E. (1997).
Racial/ethnic and gender
differences in concern with weight and in bulimic behaviors
among adoles-
cents. Obesity Research, 5(5), 447-454.
Fitzgibbon, M. L., Spring, B., Avellone, M.E., Blackman, L.R.,
Pingitore, R.,
Stolley, M.R. (1998). Correlates of binge eating in Hispanic,
Black, and
White women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24(1),
43-52.
French, S. A., Story, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Downes, B.,
Resnick, M., &
Blum, R. (1997). Ethnic differences in psychosocial and health
behavior
correlates of dieting, purging, and binge eating in a population-
based sam-
ple of adolescent females. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 22(3),
315-322.
Gray, J. J., Ford, K., & Kelly, L. M. (1987). The prevalence of
bulimia in a Black
college population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6,
733-740.
Greenberg, B. S., & Brand, J. E. (1994). Minorities and the
mass media: 1970s
to 1990s. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media effects:
Advances in the-
ory and research (pp. 273-314). Norwood, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gunther, A. C. (1991). What we think others think: Cause and
consequences
in the third-person effect. Communication Research, 18(3), 355-
372.
Gunther, A. C. (1995). Overrating the X-rating: The third-
person perception
and support for censorship of pornography. Journal of
Communication,
45(1), 27-38.
Gunther, A. C., & Hwa, A. P. (1996). Public perceptions of
television influence
and opinions about censorship in Singapore. International
Journal of Pub-
lic Opinion Research, 8(3), 248-265.
Harris, S. M. (1995). Family, self, and sociocultural
contributions to
body-image attitudes of African American women. Psychology
of Women
Quarterly, 19, 129-145.
Harrison, K. (2000). The body electric: Thin-ideal media and
eating disorders
in adolescents. Journal of Communication, 50(3) 119-143.
Harrison, K., & Cantor, J. (1997). The relationship between
media consump-
tion and eating disorders. Journal of Communication, 47(1), 40-
67.
Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rajeski, W. J. (1989). The
measurement of social
physique anxiety. Journal of Sport Psychology, 11, 94-104.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1999). Social identity and social
cognition: Histori-
cal background and current trends. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg
(Eds.),
Social identity and social cognition (pp. 1-25). Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Hogg, M. A., & Mullin, B. A. (1999). Joining groups to reduce
uncertainty:
Subjective uncertainty reduction and group identification. In D.
Abrams
& M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social cognition (pp.
248-279).
Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Hraba, J., Radloff, T., & Ray-Gray, P. (1999). A comparison of
black and white
social distance. Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 536-539.
Kann, L., & Ross, J. G. (1996). Youth risk behavior
surveillance—United
States, 1995. Centers for Disease Control. Retrieved from
http://www.cdc.
gov/epo/mmwr/preview/mmwwrnum/00043812.num
292
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
Liebkind, K. (1992). Ethnic identity—Challenging the
boundaries of social
psychology. In G. M. Breakwell (Ed.), Social psychology of
identity and the
self concept (pp. 147-185). London: Surrey University Press.
McLeod, D. M., Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Nathanson, A. I. (1997).
Support for cen-
sorship of violent and misogynic rap lyrics: An analysis of the
third-person
effect. Communication Research, 24(2), 153-174.
Merelman, R. M. (1995). Representing Black culture: Racial
conflict and cul-
tural politics in the United States. New York: Routledge.
Morales, J. F., López-Sáez, M., & Vega, L. (1998).
Discrimination and beliefs
on discrimination in individualists and collectivists. In S.
Worchel, J. F.
Morales, D. Páez, & J. Deschamps (Eds.), Social identity:
International per-
spectives (pp. 199-210). London: Sage.
Myers, P. N., & Biocca, F. (1992). The elastic body image: The
effect of televi-
sion advertising and programming on body image distortions in
young
women. Journal of Communication, 42(3), 108-133.
Pelham, B. W., & Hetts, J. J. (1999). Implicit and explicit
personal and social
identity: Toward a more complete understanding of the social
self. In T. R.
Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. John (Eds.), The psychology of
the social self
(pp. 115-143). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Perloff, R. M. (1989). Ego involvement and the third person
effect of televised
news coverage. Communication Research, 16(2), 236-262.
Perloff, R. M. (1993). Third-person effect research 1983-1992:
A review and
synthesis. International Journal of Public Opinion Research,
5(2),
167-184.
Price, V., & Tewksbury, D. (1996). Measuring the third-person
effect of news:
The impact of question order, contrast and knowledge.
International Jour-
nal of Public Opinion Research, 8(2), 120-141.
Richins, M. L. (1991). Social comparison and the idealized
images of advertis-
ing. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(1), 71-83.
Rojas, H., Shah, D. V., & Faber, R. J. (1996). For the good of
others: Censorship
and the third-person effect. International Journal of Public
Opinion
Research, 8(2), 163-186.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Rucinski, D., & Salmon, C. T. (1990). The “other” as the
vulnerable voter: A
study of the third-person effect in the 1988 U.S. presidential
campaign.
International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 2(4), 345-
368.
Salwen, M. B. (1998). Perceptions of media influence and
support for censor-
ship: Third-person effect in the 1996 presidential election.
Communica-
tion Research, 25(3), 259-285.
Salwen, M. B., & Driscoll, P. D. (1997). Consequences of third-
person percep-
tion in support of press restrictions in the O. J. Simpson trial.
Journal of
Communication, 47(2), 60-78.
Shah, D. V., Faber, R. J., & Youn, S. (1999). Susceptibility and
severity: Percep-
tual dimensions underlying the third-person effect.
Communication
Research, 26(2), 240-267.
Serino, C. (1998). The personal-social interplay: Social-
cognitive prospects on
identity and self-others comparison. In S. Worchel, J. F.
Morales, D. Páez, &
293
David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/
J. Deschamps, Social identity: International perspectives (pp.
24-43). Lon-
don: Sage.
Silber, T. (1986). Anorexia nervosa in Blacks and Hispanics.
International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 121-128.
Stice, E., Schupak-Neuberg, E., Shaw, H. E., & Stein, R. I.
(1994). Relation of
media exposure to eating disorder symptomatology: An
examination of
mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
103(4), 836-840.
Stormer, S. M., & Thompson, J. K. (1996). Explanations of
body image distur-
bance: A test of maturational status, negative verbal
commentary, social
comparison, and sociocultural hypotheses. International Journal
of
Eating Disorders, 19(2), 193-202.
Story, M., French, S. A., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W.
(1995). Ethnic/racial
and socioeconomic differences in dieting behaviors and body
image per-
ceptions in adolescents. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 18(2),
173-179.
Striegel-Moore, R. H., Schreiber, G. B., Pike, K. M., Wilfley,
D. E., & Rodin, J.
(1995). Drive for thinness in Black and White preadolescent
girls. Interna-
tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(1), 59-69.
Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups:
Studies in the social
psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies
in social psy-
chology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of
intergroup conflict.
In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of
intergroup
relations (pp. 33-47). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of
intergroup
behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of
intergroup
relations (2nd ed., pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson Hall.
Thompson, J. K. (1995). Assessment of body image. In D. B.
Allison (Ed.),
Handbook of assessment methods for eating behaviors and
weight-related
problems: Measures, theory, and research (pp. 119-144).
Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Tiedge, J. T., Silverblatt, A., Havice, M. J., & Rosenfeld, R.
(1991). Discrepancy
between perceived first-person and perceived third-person mass
media
effects. Journalism Quarterly, 68(1/2), 141-154.
Turner, J. C., & Onorato, R. S. (1999). Social identity,
personality, and the
self-concept: A self-categorization perspective. In T. R. Tyler,
R. M. Kramer,
& O. P. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 11-
46). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Tyler, T. R., & Cook, F. L. (1984). The mass media and
judgments of risk: Dis-
tinguishing impact on personal and societal level judgments.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 47(4), 693-708.
White, W. C., Hudson, L., & Campbell, S. (1985). Bulimarexia
and Black
women: A brief report. Psychotherapy, 22, 449-450.
Wilson, C. C., II, & Gutiérrez, F. (1995). Race,
multiculturalism and the media:
From mass to class communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
294
COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://crx.sagepub.com/

More Related Content

Similar to COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002David et al. • Body Image, R.docx

SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPryde
SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPrydeSocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPryde
SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPrydeEmma Roderick
 
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docx
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docxJournalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docx
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docxtawnyataylor528
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch E
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch EAu Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch E
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch EEileen Miskovitch
 
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)Dylan Russell
 
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docxvickeryr87
 
Theory of Mass Media and Socialization
Theory of Mass Media and SocializationTheory of Mass Media and Socialization
Theory of Mass Media and SocializationM.A Haque
 
Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222AdiOnita
 
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptx
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptxpreseentation naveed 2-1.pptx
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptxIrajArham
 
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docx
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docxRunning head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docx
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docxcharisellington63520
 
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdf
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdfCognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdf
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdfssuser1867b7
 
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdf
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdfAn Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdf
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdfSara Parker
 
Mass media and its impact on society
Mass media and its impact on societyMass media and its impact on society
Mass media and its impact on societySanjana027
 
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdf
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdfWhat Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdf
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdfSajid Amit
 
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docx
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docxRunning head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docx
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docxtodd521
 
Wk 7 – SG1006 – The active audience
Wk 7 – SG1006 –  The active audienceWk 7 – SG1006 –  The active audience
Wk 7 – SG1006 – The active audienceCarolina Matos
 
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of Islamophobia
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of IslamophobiaMedia Agenda Setting and the rise of Islamophobia
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of IslamophobiaAda Siddique
 
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptx
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptxPsychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptx
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptxatayevalachyn06
 
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communication
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communicationBjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communication
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communicationRai University
 

Similar to COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002David et al. • Body Image, R.docx (20)

SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPryde
SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPrydeSocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPryde
SocialMediaandPoliticalExpressionRoderickandPryde
 
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docx
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docxJournalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docx
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly2015, Vol. 92(2) 42.docx
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch E
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch EAu Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch E
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Miskovitch E
 
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)
Research Paper (Stereotype Activation and Application)
 
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx
21st Century Communication A Reference Handbook Media U.docx
 
Theory of Mass Media and Socialization
Theory of Mass Media and SocializationTheory of Mass Media and Socialization
Theory of Mass Media and Socialization
 
Audience Analysis
Audience AnalysisAudience Analysis
Audience Analysis
 
Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222
 
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptx
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptxpreseentation naveed 2-1.pptx
preseentation naveed 2-1.pptx
 
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docx
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docxRunning head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docx
Running head RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE1RESEARCH PAPER TEMPLATE.docx
 
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdf
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdfCognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdf
Cognitive Biases and Communication Strength in Social Networks.pdf
 
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdf
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdfAn Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdf
An Overview of Agenda Setting Theory in Mass Communications.pdf
 
Mass media and its impact on society
Mass media and its impact on societyMass media and its impact on society
Mass media and its impact on society
 
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdf
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdfWhat Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdf
What Drives Violent Extremism and How to Counter It.pdf
 
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docx
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docxRunning head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docx
Running head SEXUALITY, MEDIA, AND ATTRACTION 1 SEXUALITY,.docx
 
Wk 7 – SG1006 – The active audience
Wk 7 – SG1006 –  The active audienceWk 7 – SG1006 –  The active audience
Wk 7 – SG1006 – The active audience
 
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of Islamophobia
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of IslamophobiaMedia Agenda Setting and the rise of Islamophobia
Media Agenda Setting and the rise of Islamophobia
 
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptx
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptxPsychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptx
Psychological Effects and Influences of Media-1.pptx
 
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communication
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communicationBjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communication
Bjmc i, cp, unit-iii,theories of mass communication
 
Weinberger07
Weinberger07Weinberger07
Weinberger07
 

More from clarebernice

Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docx
Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docxConsider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docx
Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docx
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docxConsider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docx
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docxclarebernice
 
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docx
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docxconsider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docx
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docx
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docxConsider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docx
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docx
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docxConsider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docx
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docxclarebernice
 
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docx
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docxConsider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docx
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docx
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docxConsider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docx
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docx
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docxConsider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docx
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docx
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docxConsider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docx
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docxclarebernice
 
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docx
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docxConsider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docx
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docx
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docxConsider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docx
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docx
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docxConsider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docx
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docx
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docxConsider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docx
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docx
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docxConsider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docx
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docx
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docxConsider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docx
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docx
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docxConsider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docx
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docx
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docxConsider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docx
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docx
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docxConsider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docx
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docx
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docxConsider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docx
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docxclarebernice
 
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docx
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docxConsider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docx
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docxclarebernice
 

More from clarebernice (20)

Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docx
Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docxConsider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docx
Consider the vision for a successful Southwest Transit marketing tea.docx
 
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docx
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docxConsider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docx
Consider the various ways to create effective communication in teams.docx
 
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docx
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docxconsider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docx
consider the unique and varied forms of slaveryenslavement in Afric.docx
 
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docx
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docxConsider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docx
Consider the types of digital technology advances that exist and how.docx
 
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docx
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docxConsider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docx
Consider the two following statements Photosynthesis and cellular .docx
 
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docx
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docxConsider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docx
Consider the study on Ethnography you described last week, Remind us.docx
 
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docx
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docxConsider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docx
Consider the role of HR in a rapidly-changing world. What cha.docx
 
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docx
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docxConsider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docx
Consider the scenarios involving the unwilling moral agents of J.docx
 
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docx
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docxConsider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docx
Consider the scenario below.A toxic waste dump company wants to .docx
 
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docx
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docxConsider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docx
Consider the role of interest groups in the policy-making process, w.docx
 
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docx
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docxConsider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docx
Consider the role of stakeholders in addressing a health problem a.docx
 
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docx
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docxConsider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docx
Consider the quote by Adam Fuss in this module in which he describes.docx
 
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docx
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docxConsider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docx
Consider the obstacles that Phoenix Jackson had to overcome on h.docx
 
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docx
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docxConsider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docx
Consider the nurse leader’s role in achieving the IHI Quadruple Ai.docx
 
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docx
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docxConsider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docx
Consider the music business as a supply network. How has music d.docx
 
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docx
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docxConsider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docx
Consider the mean of a cluster of objects from a binary transact.docx
 
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docx
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docxConsider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docx
Consider the importance of using a variety of assessments in the.docx
 
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docx
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docxConsider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docx
Consider the importance of visuals in connecting with an audienc.docx
 
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docx
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docxConsider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docx
Consider the imagery you created in your mind as you interacted with.docx
 
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docx
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docxConsider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docx
Consider the followingContrast Soviet and post-Soviet migration.docx
 

Recently uploaded

How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17Celine George
 
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answers
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answerslatest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answers
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answersdalebeck957
 
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptxJoelynRubio1
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...ZurliaSoop
 
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationBasic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationNeilDeclaro1
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxExploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and ModificationsMJDuyan
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...Poonam Aher Patil
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfDr Vijay Vishwakarma
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSCeline George
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxmarlenawright1
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxJisc
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...pradhanghanshyam7136
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxAreebaZafar22
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfNirmal Dwivedi
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structuredhanjurrannsibayan2
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the ClassroomPooky Knightsmith
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 

Recently uploaded (20)

How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
How to Create and Manage Wizard in Odoo 17
 
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answers
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answerslatest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answers
latest AZ-104 Exam Questions and Answers
 
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
 
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationBasic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxExploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptxTowards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
Towards a code of practice for AI in AT.pptx
 
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...Kodo Millet  PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
Kodo Millet PPT made by Ghanshyam bairwa college of Agriculture kumher bhara...
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 

COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002David et al. • Body Image, R.docx

  • 1. COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models PRABU DAVID GLENDA MORRISON MELISSA A. JOHNSON FELECIA ROSS Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models Social Distance and Social Identification in Third-Person Effects The perceived effects of advertising on body-image factors were tested in both Black and White college-age women. After seeing magazine ads that portrayed either Black fashion models or White fashion models, respondents rated per- ceived effects of these ads on body-image factors. The effects were rated on self, on other Black women on campus, and other White women on campus. When projecting perceived effects on others—of the same race or a different race— both Blacks and Whites indicated that media effects would be maximal when the race of the model matched the race of the respondent. However, when rating perceived effects on self, whereas Blacks identified strongly with Black models, there was no significant difference in the way Whites identified with fashion models of either race. The results are examined within the
  • 2. framework of social distance and social identification. Biases in social judgment about perceived negative media effects on self and others has drawn considerable attention from communication scholars, par- ticularly those who are interested in the connection between such “third- person perceptions” and the willingness to restrict media content (e.g., Cohen, Mutz, Price, & Gunther, 1988; Davison, 1983; Duck, Terry, & Hogg, 1995; Eveland, Nathanson, Detenber, & McLeod, 1999; Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Perloff, 1989; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997). For instance, Shah, Faber, and Youn (1999) found a significant relationship between third-person perceptions and the willingness among respondents to censor advertising of controversial services, such as gambling. Other researchers have found support for controls on violent or misogynic rap music (McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997), pornography (Gunther, 1995; Rojas, Shah, & 270 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH, Vol. 29 No. 3, June 2002 270-294 © 2002 Sage Publications at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 3. http://crx.sagepub.com/ Faber, 1996), television violence (Rojas et al., 1996), and political communica- tion (Rucinski & Salmon, 1990; Salwen, 1998). Although the link between third- person perceptions and behavioral outcomes, such as support for or against policies, has not been clearly established, the underlying theoretical issues merit attention. One of the interesting aspects of the third-person phenomenon is the social-distance corollary. As the social distance between a respondent and a comparison group increases, there is a widening of the third- person gap. In the typical demonstration, when college students are asked to estimate the perceived effect of media on self, followed by perceived effects on other stu- dents on campus (a group similar to the respondents), and finally perceived effects on other people in the state (a group that is socially farther away and less similar to the student participants), perceived effects on other people in the state has been observed to be greater than perceived effects on students on campus (e.g., Cohen et al., 1988; Gunther, 1991). Perloff (1993) explained the social distance phenomenon on the basis of
  • 4. psychological distance. Psychological distance is generally conceived as degree of similarity or dissimilarity between the respondent and the compar- ison group on critical dimensions relevant to the media effect. Some of the dimensions of social and psychological distance have been examined and results are mixed with some interesting but predictable patterns (Brosius & Engel, 1996; Cohen et al., 1988; Eveland et al., 1999; McLeod et al., 1997). Brosius and Engel (1996) varied psychological distance by using two targets—friends and acquaintances as the close group and people in general as the distant group—and found that perceived effects were greater for the general group. Duck and Mullin (1995) varied comparison groups on two dimensions, namely vagueness and closeness. While closeness was operationalized as friends versus others, vagueness was varied from the “average person” to a specific person, such as a named celebrity. Their find- ings suggest that perceived effects were the greatest when the comparison group was a vague, distant other. In a couple of studies, however, the social-distance effect has been con- founded by other more salient attributes such as likelihood of exposure to the message and severity of the message. In these studies (McLeod
  • 5. et al., 1997; Eveland et al., 1999) in which misogynist rap and death-metal lyrics were used as stimuli, students rated a specific intended target audience as more vulnerable than the more general group. For example, in the earlier study (McLeod et al., 1997), youth from New York or Los Angeles were perceived to be more vulnerable to “gangsta” rap lyrics than the average person. Intu- itively this is a powerful finding because it is logical to expect youth in New 271 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ York and Los Angeles to be more vulnerable to gangster rap because of the gang-related reputations of these cities. Eveland and colleagues conducted a similar two-part study (Eveland et al., 1999). In the first part, groups were “distanced” by age and education. Although there was partial support for the social-distance corollary, again respondents appeared to build into their answers common-sense judgments
  • 6. about who might be exposed to non-mainstream media such as violent and misogynist music. In several tests, betas for perceived media exposure were significantly larger than betas for perceived social distance, overturning the traditional social distance corollary. However, the present study examines fashion advertising, which is ubiquitous in U.S. mass media and hence not dependent on selective exposure. In addition to perceived exposure considerations, the aforementioned study also dealt with media that feature violent or hateful messages, ranging from violent action films to death-metal music. If the media stimuli are socially undesirable at some level, such as porno- graphic material or misogynistic lyrics, perceived exposure to media is likely to be highly relevant. Even before respondents focus on the negative per- ceived effect on others, it is likely that they engage in a subtler form of third- person bias by distancing themselves and people close to them from exposure to socially undesirable media content. In short, it appears the effects of social distance are contingent on general- ity or specificity of the target group, similarity or dissimilarity between the respondent and the target group, the comparison group’s perceived likelihood
  • 7. of exposure to the target stimuli, and real-world heuristics. Although a number of social-distance attributes have already been exam- ined, to our knowledge third-person differences based on race or ethnicity have not been studied. Moreover, as a variable, race offers a promising avenue to examine psychological mechanisms underlying in-group versus out-group differences based on social identification. Perloff (1993) noted that social dis- tance could be conceptualized in at least two ways: along a continuum of “just like me” to “not at all like me” or in terms of the homogeneity and size of the group. Race appears to fit both these continua. Moreover, as media continue to be more segmented and personalized, media researchers will have to find appropriate ways to study media perceptions among in-groups and out- groups. The third-person framework provides a viable platform for testing differences based on social identification. Hence, in this article, we extend previous work on the social- distance com- ponent of third-person effects by introducing race as a marker of social or psy- chological distance. Using social identity as a theoretical foundation, we focus on how Black and White women perceive effects of advertising on body-image 272
  • 8. COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ factors. The findings are discussed within the context of our research on the role of the media in shaping body-image ideals in young women. Media Portrayals and the Ideal Body Image The ultra-thin model and the idealized images of beauty presented as the norm in advertising and other media content have been criticized by scholars, media critics, and popular-media writers. Researchers in public health have found that the mismatch between the ideal body image portrayed in the media and one’s actual body image leads to body-image dissatisfaction (Richins, 1991; Stice, Schupak-Neuberg, Shaw, & Stein, 1994; Stormer & Thompson, 1996). In extreme cases, body-image dissatisfaction or physique anxiety hurts an individual’s self-esteem or self-concept and could even lead to behavioral consequences such as eating disorders (e.g., Thompson, 1995). Although the relationship between media use and body-image
  • 9. percep- tions is tenuous, there is an emerging body of research documenting signifi- cant relationships. For example, Harrison (2000) found a significant relation- ship between bulimia and exposure to television shows with overweight characters. In the same study, a significant relationship was found between thin-ideal magazine content and increased anorexia. Others have found sig- nificant relationships between selective media use and drive for thinness (Botta, 1999, 2000; Harrison & Cantor, 1997) and between media exposure and body-image elasticity (Myers & Biocca, 1992). Given these findings, we set out to examine self-reported perceptions of media effects on body image. Media Effects on Body-Image Factors For the purposes of this study, media influences are presented as a cluster of effects with varying levels of undesirability, collectively referred to as body-image factors. If body-image dissatisfaction can be expressed as a mis- match between ideal and actual body-image perceptions, the role of media can be examined by assessing the extent to which they are responsible for cre- ating and triggering these body-image ideals. Body-image dissatisfaction itself is not inherently insidious as much as are the negative outcomes that
  • 10. could stem from the dissatisfaction. In some cases, body-image dissatisfac- tion could be channeled into a desirable outcome. For example, the Nike com- mercial of the “Just Do It” genre that exhorts viewers to exercise is in some ways equivalent to a public-service announcement that seeks a positive out- come. But the focus of this study is media portrayal of thin, idealized models in fashion magazines. 273 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ As just noted, one behavioral outcome measure of body-image media effects on women is an eating disorder such as anorexia or bulimia. However, in our framework, we explore a range of effects, including the effect on percep- tion of ideal body weight (a perceptual outcome) and effect on perception of one’s self-esteem (an attitudinal or psychological outcome). In other words, the dependent variables in our framework extend hierarchically from the less-desirable media effect that affects perceptions of ideal body weight, to
  • 11. the more undesirable effect on the respondent’s psychological state, to the most undesirable behavioral outcome that is manifested as an eating disor- der. This hierarchy of body-image media effects is referred to as body-image factors in this study. These factors have been examined in the past and the results suggest that as the degree of undesirability of the outcome increases, the third-person gap widens (David & Johnson, 1998). Body-Image Dissatisfaction, Eating Disorders, and Race Conventional wisdom and a growing body of empirical evidence suggest that Black women are less vulnerable to body-image dissatisfaction and are more proud of their bodies than Anglo-Americans and Hispanics (Altabe, 1998; Fitzgibbon et al., 1998; Story, French, Resnick, & Blum, 1995). Studies show that Blacks are less concerned about weight than are Whites (White, Hudson, & Campbell, 1985), exhibit fewer weight-reduction behaviors such as dieting and exercising (Kann & Ross, 1996), and have lower incidences of eating dis- orders than Whites (Gray, Ford, & Kelly, 1987). Ironically, however, African Americans exhibit these positive body-image attributes despite having higher body-mass index scores and higher fat composition than Whites (Akan & Grilo, 1995; Ellis, Abrams, & Wong, 1997; Field, Colditz, & Peterson,
  • 12. 1997; French et al., 1997). In general, health researchers have been more con- cerned about obesity in African Americans than eating disorders. Harris (1995) observed that the varying degrees of emphasis on thinness among Blacks and Whites could be attributed to culturally ingrained norms, with thinness as a strongly internalized ideal among White women, but less of an ideal among Black women. Some argue that the individualistic nature of some White cultures emphasizes individual control— including control of body— rather than dependence on the family, which is more common to some Black cultures. Despite the racial and ethnic differences on body image, a number of vari- ables associated with eating disorders are common among women of all races. For instance, regardless of race, eating disorders have some common mark- ers, such as perfectionist personality traits and dysfunctional family environ- ments. One study found that Black girls who were teased about their weight 274 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 13. http://crx.sagepub.com/ had a significantly higher drive for thinness than those who were not teased (Striegel-Moore, Schreiber, Pike, Wilfley, & Rodin, 1995), suggesting that Blacks are not immune to the social pressures of ideal body image. Silber (1986) argued that as Blacks make strides in the workplace, there is growing pressure to conform to the norms of the dominant culture, namely the Euro- centric ideals of beauty and attractiveness. After taking into consideration the differing body-image values and norms between races, we expected some differences in third- person percep- tions about body image. At least two underlying processes are at play when projecting to others of a different race—one, the traditional third-person effect that widens with social distance; and two, the confounding of social dis- tance by race because of differences in perceived vulnerabilities of the two races, which are driven by cultural stereotypes and conventional wisdom. The findings by Eveland et al. (1999) would suggest that Whites would be more vulnerable to body-image media effects because it is generally con- strued that White women rather than Black women are the intended targets
  • 14. of ads that emphasize extreme thinness. Social Distance and Social Identification Dissimilarity based on race can be examined from the standpoint of categori- zation processes that are described in social-identity theory. Social-identity theory holds that our self-concept is based in part on the way we perceive oth- ers and how we interact with them (Hogg & Abrams, 1999; Serino, 1998; Tajfel, 1978, 1981; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner & Onorato, 1999). Self- concept depends not only on personality traits of an individual but also on how an individual relates to others within a group. Because part of a person’s self-concept is based on group membership, social comparisons between groups produce pressures to earn collective self-esteem for the in-group rela- tive to out-groups. Self-categorization theory is a companion theory that specifies the social-cognition aspects and contrasts with others. Using social identity and social comparison, some researchers have focused on racial and ethnic identity (Liebkind, 1992; Pelham & Hetts, 1999) because of its usefulness in studying stereotypes, prejudice, and other behav- iors in interracial settings. Prior research in this area suggests that an indi- vidual’s beliefs about personal and social identities play a key role in group-
  • 15. relevant judgments (Pelham & Hetts, 1999). Studies in social-identity theory and social categorization theory do not specifically deal with the concept of social distance. However, the intersection between social distance and racial identity and how it affects intergroup per- ception has been explored. One study on race and social distance found that 275 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ Blacks rated Whites to be less acceptable than Whites rated Blacks in the three domains studied: classmate or coworker, next-door neighbor, and inti- mate contact (Hraba, Radloff, & Ray-Gray, 1999). Social-identity scholars might explain this by looking at the cultural norms of the respondents, honing in on the individualistic nature of some cul- tural groups and the collectivist cultures of others. Collective cultures put more emphasis on the welfare of the group than the welfare of the individual. For instance, studies have found that there is a slight tendency
  • 16. for individu- alists to be less discriminating than collectivists in in-group favoritism (Morales, López-Sáez, & Vega, 1998). Race, Social Distance, and Third-Person Effects Our research objective was to formulate social distance using race as a marker and to examine how third-person media effects are perceived for in-groups and out-groups. If an ethnic or racial group is perceived to be socially more distant, the social-distance corollary of the third- person effect should hold up. The bias against out-groups or a bias in favor of the in-group can be interpreted as a self-serving motivational bias that enhances the posi- tions of in-group members. This bias in favor of the in-group can be tied to social identification. In previous research on third-person effects, social distance has been stud- ied primarily as distance between the respondent and the comparison group. Another component of social distance is the distance between the source of the media message and the respondent. In essence, social identity extends in two directions: from the source of the message to the respondent (Tyler & Cook, 1984) and from the respondent to the comparison group. Therefore, in this article we attempt to extend previous research by
  • 17. manipulating two com- ponents of social distance that are critical to eliciting the third- person effect: similarity between the source and respondent, and similarity between the respondent and the comparison group. Similarity and dissimilarity were operationalized on the basis of race, which was an intentional design artifact. Hypotheses and Research Questions The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of how body-image por- trayals in fashion advertising are perceived by Black and White college-age women. In addition to perceived effects on self, both groups of students esti- mated perceived effects on other Black students and other White students. Both groups responded to two sets of magazine ads, one with only White fash- ion models and another set with only Black fashion models. 276 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ The first hypothesis captures the basic third-person perception that oth-
  • 18. ers are more vulnerable to media effects than self. Hypothesis 1: Perceived effect on others will be greater than perceived effect on self. The next hypothesis is derived from findings reported by David and John- son (1998) that the third-person gap widens as the media outcome becomes more undesirable. In other words, the third-person difference for the eating- disorder outcome would be greater than the third-person difference for the self-esteem outcome, which in turn will be greater than the third-person gap for the ideal-body-weight outcome. Hypothesis 2: The difference between perceived effect on self and others will widen with an increase in social undesirability of the outcome. The next hypothesis is essentially a test of social identification, which sug- gests that respondents are more likely to identify with a similar source who is part of their in-group than someone who is dissimilar and part of the out- group. Hypothesis 3: Perceived effect on self will be greater when the race of the respondent matches the race of the model in the ad than in the mis- matched condition.
  • 19. In general, one would expect a widening of the third-person gap with an increase in social distance and dissimilarity between self and the comparison group. However, because of cultural stereotypes and the different body- image norms among Blacks and Whites, which were discussed earlier, a dif- ferential effect was predicted on the basis of race of the respondent. Hypothesis 4: For Black college women, perceived effect of media on other White women (dissimilar to self) will be greater than perceived effect on other Black women (similar to self). Hypothesis 5: For White college women, perceived effect on other White women (similar to self) will be greater than perceived effect on other Black women (dissimilar to self). In addition to these hypotheses, the critical research question for the study was to examine the interaction among race of the fashion model, the race of the respondent, and the race of the comparison group. 277 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 20. http://crx.sagepub.com/ Research Question 1: What are the interactions among the race of the respondent, the race of the comparison group, and the race of the fash- ion model on body-image factors? Method Scales Two psychological constructs, self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965) and social- physique anxiety (Hart, Leary, & Rajeski, 1989), were measured using 10- item scales. Third-person perceptions were measured using a 11-point scale, where 0 = no influence and 10 = considerable influence. Stimulus Ten ads were chosen from major fashion magazines, such as Vogue, Cosmo- politan, Essence, and Ebony. The authors chose the ads with help from gradu- ate students. Half of these ads featured thin Black models and the other half featured thin White models. These models were chosen on the basis of thin- ness, attractiveness, skin tone, hair type, and sexiness. The Black models var- ied from dark to lighter skin tones, with short natural hair to straightened
  • 21. hair. The skin tones of the White models varied from extremely fair to well tanned, with hair color ranging from brunette to light blonde. We tried to match the ads featuring the Black and White models on a num- ber of dimensions, including style of dress, extent of skin exposed, activity or pose, and the mood conveyed by the ad. To minimize the role of familiarity, ads with well-known supermodels were excluded. Participants and Study Design Eighty female undergraduate students participated in the study for extra credit. One half of the participants were Black, and the other half were White. In the absence of a random between-subjects experimental manipulation, it would be accurate to characterize this study as a quasi- experiment, with a 2 (Respondent’s race: Black, White) × 2 (Order: perceived effect on self first, perceived effect on self last) × 2 (Race of the fashion models: Black, White) × 3 (Social distance: self, other Black women, other White women) mixed design. Respondent’s race was the only between-subjects factor. Race of the fashion models and social distance were both within-subject factors. 278 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002
  • 22. at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ Procedure When students arrived, they were given a consent form and the self-esteem and physique-anxiety scales, which were stapled together as a booklet. After filling out the scales, students evaluated the ads. Each ad was evaluated on three 10-point scales, which tapped attractiveness of the model, thinness of the model, and the similarity between the model’s body type and the respon- dent’s body type. The ads were evaluated in two sets, with five ads in each set. One set of ads featured only Black models, and the other set featured only White models. After evaluating all the ads in a set, respondents rated the perceived effect of these ads on one’s own body image, the perceived effect on other Black (or White) women on campus, and the perceived effect on other White (or Black) women on campus. Perceived effect of advertising was obtained for three body- image factors: ideal body weight, self-esteem, and likelihood of developing an eating disor-
  • 23. der. Perceived effects of advertising on all the body-image factors were mea- sured using an 11-point scale (0 = no influence and 10 = considerable influ- ence). The question was phrased using the following format: How much influence do you think these ads have on your perception of ideal body weight? This question was varied by changing the comparison group to “other Black women on campus” and “other White women on campus,” and repeated for “effect on self-esteem” and the “likelihood that that these ads could lead to an eating disorder.” One half of the students rated the ads with Black models first, and the other half rated the ads with White models first. Order of evaluation of the third-person perceptions was counterbalanced through the use of four ver- sions of the third-person questionnaire (two versions for the Black students and two versions for the White students). In one version, the first assessment was on self, which was followed by other Black/White women (same race as the respondent), which was followed by other White/Black women (of a differ- ent race). Essentially, in one version the questions began with perceived effect on self and extended to those who were most dissimilar. In the other version, the questions began with perceived effects on those who are most dis-
  • 24. similar and moved inward toward perceived effects on self. This was a conser- vative protocol, given that previous explorations of the impact of question order on third-person perceptions have found minimal differences (David & Johnson, 1998; Gunther, 1991, 1995; Price & Tewksbury, 1996; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, & Rosenfeld, 1991). 279 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ At the end of the experiment, before debriefing, students answered some media-use questions and provided personal data including race, height, and weight. Results Manipulation Check Black students found Black models more attractive (M = 6.0, SD = 2.5) than White models (M = 3.9, SD = 2.4). They rated their body types to be more simi- lar (M = 3.3, SD = 2.4) to the body types of the Black models than to the body
  • 25. types of the White models (M = 2.1, SD = 2.3). Also, Black students rated the White models as thinner (M = 7.7, SD = 2.0) than the Black models (M = 6.7, SD = 1.8). White students, on the other hand, found Black models to be slightly more attractive (M = 6.9, SD = 2.3) than White models (M = 6.1, SD = 2.3). In line with the ratings provided by the Black students, White students also found the White models to be thinner (M = 8.0, SD = 1.4) than the Black models (M = 7.5, SD = 1.5). For similarity of body type, White students did not see any differences based on the race of the model. All significant differences were at p < .05 and were tested using paired t tests (see Table 1). Although the significant differences in similarity and attractiveness by the race of the models for Black respondents is indicative of the intended social identification manipulation, there was a reversal among White stu- dents, who found Black models more attractive and saw no difference in simi- larity based on the race of the model. However, the finding that both Blacks and Whites found the White models to be thinner than Black models does raise the question of the validity of our manipulation. Despite our finding of null difference in the pretest, in which the set of ads with White models and
  • 26. 280 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 Table 1 Mean Ratings of Attractiveness, Thinness, and Similarity to Fashion Models by Race of the Students Black Respondents White Respondents Black White Black White Models Models Models Models M SD M SD M SD M SD Attractiveness 6.0 2.5 3.9 2.4 6.9 2.3 6.1 2.3 Thinness 6.7 1.8 7.7 2.0 7.5 1.5 8.0 1.4 Similarity 3.3 2.4 2.1 2.3 4.0 2.3 3.7 2.3 Note. Responses were measured on a 10-point scale. Higher scores mean higher attractiveness, thinness, and similarity. at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ the set with Black models were administered between subjects, some signifi- cant differences for thinness were observed in this study. However, because the focus of our study is on relative differences between assessments rather than the absolute value of the perceived media effect, we
  • 27. continued with the analysis. Also, we have tried to control for this difference by using the thin- ness evaluation as a covariate in the analysis of variance. Next, we compared the physique anxiety and self-esteem scores by the race of the respondent. Reliabilities were high for both scales, with Cronbach’s alpha = .93 for physique anxiety and Cronbach’s alpha = .84 for self-esteem. The physique-anxiety score and self-esteem score for Black par- ticipants were not significantly different from the corresponding scores for White participants. These results suggest that both groups were comparable in terms of the their physique anxiety and self-esteem scores. Analysis of Variance Given the complexity of the design, each of the body-image factors, namely ideal body weight, self-esteem, and eating disorders, were analyzed sepa- rately. We used a 2 (Black respondent, White respondent) × 2 (Order: per- ceived effect on self first, perceived effect on self last) × 2 (Black fashion mod- els, White fashion models) × 3 (perceived effect on self, perceived effect on others of a similar race, perceived effect on others of a different race) mixed- factor design. In addition, physique anxiety, self-esteem, thinness rating of Black models and thinness rating of White models were entered
  • 28. as covariates. In preliminary analysis, when self-esteem was entered as a covariate, it was not statistically significant. In subsequent analysis, self-esteem was not included as a covariate. A summary of the means is presented in Table 2. For the ideal-body-weight outcome, main effects for physique anxiety, F(1, 73) = 9.98, MSE = 11.43, p < .005, ω2 = .06, and thinness rating of Black mod- els, F(1, 73) = 10.26, MSE = 11.43, p <.005, ω2 = .06, were significant. In addi- tion, the expected three-way interaction, Race of Model × Comparison Group × Race of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 30.16, MSE = 3.01, p < .001, ω2 = .09, was significant. For the self-esteem outcome, main effects for physique anxiety, F(1, 73) = 7.33, MSE = 9.56, p < .01, ω2 = .03, and thinness rating of Black models, F(1, 73) = 10.72, MSE = 9.56, p <.01, ω2 = .05, were significant. The critical three-way interaction captured by Race of Model × Comparison Group × Race of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 22.63, MSE = 3.41, p < .001, ω2 = .08, was also significant. For the eating-disorder outcome, the main effect for physique anxiety, F(1, 73) = 4.87, MSE = 13.59, p < .05, ω2 = .03, and the thinness rating of Black
  • 29. 281 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ models, F(1, 73) = 4.11, MSE = 13.59, p < .05, ω2 = .03, were significant. The targeted three-way interaction among Race of Model × Comparison Group × Race of Respondent, F(2, 146) = 22.28, MSE = 2.25, p < .001, ω2 = .05, was significant. In within-subjects designs, multiple observations are obtained from the same subject and the independence assumption is violated, which in turn inflates the F value. The Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon and the Huynh-Feldt epsilon are corrections to the F value to account for the violations of com- pound symmetry and sphericity. After applying these corrections, the critical three-way interaction was significant at p < .001. In summary, physique anxiety, which was entered as a covariate, had a sig- nificant effect on the ratings provided by the respondents. Also, the thinness rating of the Black models was significant. The order in which
  • 30. the third- person ratings were obtained was not significant. The key finding was the expected three-way interaction—Race of the Model × Comparison Group × Race of Respondent—which was significant for each of the three body-image factors after controlling for the difference in perceived thinness between Black and White models. To explain this interaction, we examined the rela- tionships among these three factors through a series of mean comparisons. 282 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 Table 2 Perceived Effects of Advertising on Self and Others as a Function of the Race of the Respondent and the Race of the Fashion Model Black Respondents White Respondents Black White Black White Models Models Models Models M SD M SD M SD M SD Body Weight Self 5.5 3.1 3.6 3.0 5.8 3.1 5.9 2.4 Other Blacks 6.1 2.5 3.7 2.4 6.3 2.1 5.0 1.9 Other Whites 4.3 3.4 9.1 1.8 6.7 2.0 7.5 1.5
  • 31. Self-Esteem Self 3.9 2.7 2.5 2.5 4.5 2.7 4.5 2.5 Other Blacks 5.3 2.1 3.3 2.4 5.8 2.3 4.8 2.0 Other Whites 3.9 3.5 8.7 1.3 5.9 2.1 6.8 2.0 Eating Disorder Self 2.1 3.0 1.6 2.7 2.2 2.6 2.5 2.7 Other Blacks 3.5 2.6 2.8 2.2 5.2 2.1 4.0 2.4 Other Whites 4.2 3.5 8.6 1.6 5.4 2.1 6.2 2.1 Note. Means are ratings on a 11-point scale from 0 (no influence) to 10 (considerable influence). at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ Third-Person Effects When Race of Model Matched Race of the Respondent First, perceived effect on others of the same race was significantly greater than perceived effect on self when there was a match between the race of the model and the race of the respondents. Among Whites, the perceived effect on self and on other White respondents was significantly different for the body image outcome (Self M = 5.9, Other Whites M = 7.5, Mean difference = 1.6, t(39) = 4.69, p < .001), the self-esteem outcome (Self M = 4.5, Other Whites M = 6.8, Mean difference = 2.3, t(39) = 7.58, p < .001), and the eating-disorder out-
  • 32. come (Self M = 2.5, Other Whites M = 6.2, Mean difference = 3.7, t(39) = 8.82, p < .001). Also, as the undesirability of the outcome increased, the third- person gap widened significantly among the White respondents (see Figure 1). Among Blacks, the third-person effect was significant for only two of the three outcomes, and there was no systematic widening of the gap with increasing undesirability. The perceived effect on self and other Black stu- dents was not significant for the body-weight outcome (Self M = 5.5, Other Blacks M = 6.1, Mean difference = 0.6), but was significant for the self-esteem outcome (Self M = 3.9, Other Blacks M = 5.3, Mean difference = 1.4, t(39) = 2.90, p <. 005) and the eating-disorder outcome (Self M = 2.1, Other Blacks M = 3.5, Mean difference = 1.4, t(39) = 3.15, p < .005). Baseline Effects on Self Among Blacks and Whites When the race of the model matched the race of the respondent, there were no statistically significant differences in perceived effect on self between Black and White respondents for all three outcomes, namely perceived effect on ideal body weight, effect on self-esteem, and likelihood to lead to an eating 283
  • 33. David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models Figure 1. Perceived Effect of Same-Race Fashion Models on Self and Other Women of the Same Race for Three Body-Image Factors at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ disorder. The absence of a significant difference between Blacks and Whites on perceived effect on self runs contrary to the conventional wisdom that Black women consider themselves less vulnerable to body- image media effects than White women. The implications of this finding will be taken up in the discussion section. Race of the Model and Perceived Effect on Self For the Black respondents, perceived effect on self was significantly different when the race of the model was different, suggesting strong social identifica- tion. When exposed to Black models, Blacks showed significantly higher effect on self (body weight M = 5.5, self-esteem M = 3.9, eating disorder M = 2.1) than when exposed to White models (body weight M = 3.6, self-esteem M = 2.5, eating disorder M = 1.6). All paired t tests were significantly different
  • 34. at p < .05. In contrast, among White respondents there was no significant effect of the race of the model on the perceived effect on self. Possible reasons for the absence of social identification among the White participants are addressed in the discussion. Race of the Model and Perceived Effect on Others of the Same Race The purpose of this line of analysis was to examine how participants project social identification onto others of their race. Whereas the Black respondents continued to indicate strong social identification for other Black women (their in-group), the White respondents also indicated strong social identifi- cation for their in-group, which was a reversal of the pattern that Whites pro- vided for themselves. Black respondents stated that other Black women would be affected more when the race of the model was Black (body weight M = 6.1, self-esteem M = 5.3, eating disorder M = 3.5) than when the race of the model was White (body weight M = 3.7, self-esteem M = 3.3, eating disor- der M = 2.8). By the same token, Whites indicated that other Whites would be influenced more by White models (body weight M = 7.5, self- esteem M = 6.8, eating disorder M = 6.2) than they would by Black models (body weight M = 6.7, self-esteem M = 5.9, eating disorder M = 5.4). All tests
  • 35. were significant at p < .05. Race of the Model and Perceived Effect on Others of a Different Race Finally, we examined the perceived effect on other students of a different race (out-group) as a function of the race of the model. When projecting to other 284 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ students of a different race, both Blacks and Whites rated the out-groups as showing strong social identification. However, there was a marked asymme- try in terms of how Blacks and Whites saw social identification by the out- group. Black participants’ estimation of social identification by the White students was significantly higher than the White participants’ estimation of social identification by other Whites and Blacks. Among Black participants, perceived effects on other White students after exposure to White models were notably higher (body weight M
  • 36. = 9.1, self- esteem M = 8.7, eating disorder M = 8.6) compared to perceived effects after exposure to Black models (body weight M = 4.3, self-esteem M = 3.9, eating disorder M = 4.2). Among White respondents, although a similar pattern was evident, the size of the projected effects of social identification was smaller, with perceived effects on other Black students after exposure to Black models (body weight M = 6.3, self-esteem M = 5.8, eating disorder M = 5.2) being higher than effects estimated after exposure to White models (body weight M = 5.0, self-esteem M = 4.8, eating-disorder M = 4.0). All relevant within group mean differences were tested using paired t tests and were significant at p < .01 (see Figure 2). In the discussion, the results are examined within the framework of social-identification theory and the cultural and racial stereotypes associ- ated with body-image factors. Discussion The purpose of this study was to examine third-person perceptions related to body-image factors by manipulating two key variables on the basis of race: social distance between the respondent and the comparison group, and social distance between the respondent and the media source. In
  • 37. addition, we tried to replicate the results reported in a study by David and Johnson (1998), wherein the third-person effect widened with the undesirability of the body-image outcome. To study these issues, we examined five hypotheses and some research questions. The discussion is grouped into three sections. We will start with the findings related to the third-person effect. Then we will take up the results associated with social distance before discussing the interaction between social distance and race. Third-Person Effects To examine the basic third-person effect within this quasi- experimental design, the perceived effect on others was examined without any confounding in terms of the race of the model or the race of the comparison group. In other 285 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ words, the third-person results discussed in this section are based on per-
  • 38. ceived effect of media on self and others of the same race following exposure to fashion models also of the same race as the respondent. In support of Hypothesis 1, there was strong evidence of third- person effects across the board, which indicates that respondents perceived media effects on others to be stronger than effect on self. As predicted in Hypo- the- sis 2, the third-person gap widened with increasing undesirability of the out- come. This trend was particularly strong among White respondents but was not as pronounced among Black respondents. Among Whites, the third- per- son gap increased from 1.57 for body weight to 2.33 for self- esteem to 3.67 for eating disorders. Among Blacks, the gap increased from 0.68 for body weight to 1.35 for self-esteem to 1.43 for eating disorders. As discussed later, the dif- ference in magnitudes also suggests that Black and White students’ beliefs about the power of media differ. The dynamics of the widening of the third-person gap with outcome undesirability was similar to the findings reported by David and Johnson (1998). As the undesirability of the outcome increased, the White partici- pants trimmed down the effect on self but did not make comparable adjust- ments for others. Another way to interpret the perceived differential effects
  • 39. between self and others is that while participants made adjustments for the likelihood of occurrence on self for the more serious outcomes, which have a lower base rate of occurrence, they failed to make similar adjustments when extrapolating to others. One explanation for the lack of clear support for Hypothesis 2 among Blacks could be due to the differences in the interpretation of undesirability of the outcomes. First, based on previous findings, there is evidence that ideal 286 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 Figure 2. Perceived Effect on Self and Others as a Function of the Race of the Respon- dent, Race of the Fashion Models, and Race of the Comparison Group for the Self-Esteem Outcome at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ body weight is not treated with as much emphasis among Black women as it is among White women. Hence, the absence of a third-person effect for body weight among Blacks is not surprising. Furthermore, the
  • 40. absence of the wid- ening of the third-person gap with undesirability can be attributed to the low incidence of clinical eating disorders among Blacks compared to Whites. Social Distance and Third-Person Effect When social distance was narrowly defined based on race, the traditionally robust effects of social distance were nullified or reversed. It appears that the participants’ perceptions of in-group and out-group norms took precedence over normative social distance defined along racial lines. Race and the Perceptions of Body-Image Media Effects on Self Conventional wisdom, cultural differences, and some findings in the litera- ture (e.g., Akan & Grilo, 1995; Altabe, 1998) suggest that Black women are less vulnerable to body-image effects compared to Whites. Although this study does not provide irrefutable evidence to the contrary, the findings from this study suggest that there were no statistically significant differences between Blacks and Whites in terms of perceived media effects when the race of the model matched the race of the respondent. The absence of a significant difference between Blacks and Whites on per- ceived effects on self deserves attention. Social commentators and media
  • 41. observers have noted that as Black women make progress in their careers and achieve higher status, they feel the same pressure that Whites do to con- form to the ideal body image that dominates the overall culture. It is also pos- sible that the effect of media images on self was heightened for the Black par- ticipants in this study because we matched the race of the media model with the race of the respondent. It is plausible that even if Blacks can generally ignore the ideals of body image presented in the mainstream media predomi- nantly through White models, they could be vulnerable to body- image pres- sures presented in the Black media in the form of Black fashion models. This highlights the need for social responsibility among ethnic media as well as general-market media in body-image portrayals. Social Identification Hypothesis We had predicted in Hypothesis 3 that the perceived effect on self would be greater when the race of the model matched the race of the respondent. This was true for the Black participants but not for Whites. Across all three 287 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
  • 42. 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ outcomes, among White participants there were no statistically significant differences on perceived effect on self on the basis of the race of the model. There are at least three explanations for the absence of a main effect for race of the model on White respondents. One is the social desirability bias not to appear racist. This supports the motivational explanation of third-person effect, because not being racist is an esteem-preserver or enhancer. The other interpretation is that the fashion industry selects models that fit common standards of thinness and attractiveness and the White students did not see a significant difference based on the race of the model. Studies show, for instance, that Black models in advertisements tend to be light- skinned (Entman & Book, 2000). Nonetheless, White students perceived the race of the model to have a significant effect on other White women and other Black women. In short, although White respondents’ own ratings suggest that they saw themselves as color blind, they did not project the same pattern to other in-group (other Whites) and out-group (other Blacks) women.
  • 43. A third possibility is that Blacks in the entertainment industry— includ- ing fashion—may be more accepted than in other social roles. Even if White students perceived Black models as different from them, the fashion industry is a safe milieu in which to be different. White college students may have per- ceived racial difference in fashion models as exotic—an ideal in the fashion industry. Merelman (1995) would argue that this is an example of the success of Black cultural projection among Whites, who are accustomed to seeing Blacks in glamorous roles in the entertainment industry. Our finding that White students found Black models to be more attractive is one indicator of this. Unlike White respondents, Black respondents showed strong in- group social identification, which is expected among ethnic or minority groups in a pluralistic culture. The differences in social identification between Blacks and Whites led to other interesting effects when respondents were asked to project the perceived effect of race of the model to other Whites and Blacks. Social Identification and Projecting to Others Using the race of the fashion model as a cue for social identification, we tested the validity of hypotheses presented in Hypothesis 4 and
  • 44. Hypothesis 5. The findings related to Hypothesis 4 and Hypothesis 5 varied considerably based on the race of the model and the race of the respondent, indicating strong interactions with social distance and social identification. Among Blacks, perceived effects on other White women (out- group) was greater than perceived effects on other Black women (in-group) only after exposure to the White fashion models. After exposure to Black fashion 288 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ models, Blacks rated perceived effects on other Black women (in-group) higher than perceived effects on other White women (out- group). This offers only partial support for Hypothesis 4 because the social distance corollary was held only for Black fashion models. Among Whites, perceived negative effect of media on other White women (in-group) was greater than perceived effect on other Black women (out-
  • 45. group) when the race of model was White. Essentially, this was a reversal of the social-distance corollary, which was predicted in Hypothesis 5. But when the race of the model was Black, there were no statistically significant differ- ences in terms of how the White respondents perceived effects on in-group and out-group. In summary, when projecting to others, both Blacks and Whites perceived significant social identification by other women. This effect was significant for others of the same race and others of a different race. When projecting media effect on others, the key difference between Black and White respon- dents was the magnitude of the effect. This mismatch is best captured by the ratings provided by Blacks for the Whites (out-group), which ranged from 8.6 to 9 on a 10-point scale, compared to ratings of 6.2 to 7.5 provided by Whites for their in-group. In this article we have focused primarily on social identification as the mechanism to explain the perceived differences associated with the race of the model. An alternate explanation for the perceived immunity that African Americans expressed toward White fashion models might be rooted in their suspicions of the general-market media in the United States, where histori-
  • 46. cally they are accustomed to negative portrayals (e.g., Greenberg & Brand, 1994; Wilson & Gutiérrez, 1995). Finally, one cannot ignore the role of racial and cultural differences when interpreting these results. The conventional belief that Blacks are less vulnerable than Whites to body image factors is a powerful heuristic, which certainly could have played an important role in these judgments about others. Directions for Future Research Black students’ manifestation of a stronger social identity may be explained by the more collectivist leanings of minority cultures. In addition, because social comparisons between groups produce pressures to achieve collective self-esteem (e.g., Hogg & Mullin, 1999), African-American respondents may have exhibited a greater desire to preserve Black group self- esteem. This is a topic that deserves further scrutiny. One of the other areas of social-identity theory not previously discussed is the natural tension between an individ- ual’s desire to assimilate and to differentiate (e.g., Brewer & Pickett, 1999). 289 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19,
  • 47. 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ At one level, the basic third-person perception is an example of differentia- tion, whereas the strong in-group versus out-group pattern exhibited by Blacks could be an indicator of assimilation. In the future, it would be benefi- cial to examine the differences within and between groups in terms of how they relate to differentiation and affiliation. The results point to a need to explore the interplay between social identity and social-desirability aspects of the third-person. Social psychologists Tajfel and Turner (1979) claimed that definition of the “other” stems from the need to maintain a positive social identity. How Black and White college students conceive their own identities and introduce “otherness” into their vision of media effects on themselves and others is an area to pursue. In addition, how cultural projections via mass media might mediate these identities and ver- sions of otherness is a compelling area of study. One of the limitations of our study is the thinness manipulation because there was a significant difference in the perceived thinness of the Black and White models. Although we tried to address this by introducing
  • 48. thinness rat- ings of the models as covariates in the analysis, it is possible that this differ- ence could have skewed the results. Clearly this study should be replicated with stimuli that are more rigorously balanced on all the critical attributes. Finally, this study is a quasi-experiment with only 80 participants, which has the typical limitations of any experiment. To improve ecological validity, it would be useful to replicate the study with a larger survey sample. Conclusions The findings from this study confirm some of the popular stereotypes about perceived effects of glamour advertising on body-image factors in Black and White college-age women. There were a number of significant findings. Both Blacks and Whites exhibited strong third-person effects when there was no confounding by race. Also, the third-person gap widened with increasing undesirability of the outcome, particularly among Whites. When projecting media effects on others, both Blacks and Whites indicated that others would identify more closely with same race or in-group models than with different race or out-group models. When estimating perceived effects on self, however, Blacks and Whites dif-
  • 49. fered slightly. Whereas Whites reported that the race of the model does not make a significant difference on perceived effect on self, Blacks identified closely with Black models. Moreover, contrary to the widely held perception that Blacks are less vulnerable to body-image effects compared to Whites, there were no significant differences between the two groups on perceived effect on self when the race of the models matched the race of the 290 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ respondents. In the face of corroborating evidence (e.g., Botta, 2000), it seems plausible that the gap between Blacks and Whites on body- image vulnerabil- ities might be closing. We hope the findings from this study add to the discussion on the role of advertising in creating norms of ideal beauty. Obviously, more systematic research programs are needed to accurately assess negative body-image con- sequences that could result from the idealized beauty presented
  • 50. in the media. References Akan, G. E., & Grilo, C. M. (1995). Sociocultural influences on eating attitudes and behaviors, body image, and psychological functioning: A comparison of African American, Asian American, and Caucasian college women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(2), 181-187. Altabe, M. (1998). Ethnicity and body image: Quantitative and qualitative analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23(2), 153- 159. Botta, R. A. (1999). Television images and adolescent girls’ body image distur- bance. Journal of Communication, 49(2), 22-41. Botta, R. A. (2000). The mirror of television: A comparison of black and white adolescents’ body image. Journal of Communication, 5(3), 144- 159. Brewer, M. B., & Pickett, C. L. (1999). Distinctiveness motives as a source of the social self. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. John (Eds.), The psychol- ogy of the social self (pp. 71-87). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Brosius, H., & Engel, D. (1996). The causes of third-person effects: Unrealistic optimism, impersonal impact, or generalized negative attitudes toward
  • 51. media influence? International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 8(2), 142-162. Cohen, J., Mutz, D., Price, V., & Gunther, A. (1988). Perceived impact of defa- mation: An experiment on third-person effects. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52(2), 161-173. David, P., & Johnson, M.A. (1998). The role of self in third- person effects about body image. Journal of Communication, 48(4), 37-58. Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opin- ion Quarterly, 47(1), 1-15. Duck, J. M., & Mullin, B. (1995). The perceived impact of the mass media: Reconsidering the third person effect. European Journal of Social Psy- chology, 25, 77-93. Duck, J. M., Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1995). The perceived influence of AIDS advertising: Third-person effects in the context of positive media content. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17(3), 305-325. Entman, R. M., & Book, C. (2000). Light makes right: Skin color and racial hierarchy in television advertising. In R. Andersen & L. Strate (Eds.), Crit- ical studies in media commercialism (p. 214-224). New York: Oxford.
  • 52. Ellis, K. J., Abrams, S. A., & Wong, W. W. (1997). Body composition of a young, multiethnic female population. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 65(3), 724-731. 291 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ Eveland, W. P., Jr., Nathanson, A. I., Detenber, B. H., & McLeod, D. M. (1999). Rethinking the social distance corollary: Perceived likelihood of exposure and the third-person perception. Communication Research, 26(3), 275-302. Field, A. E., Colditz, G. A., & Peterson, K. E. (1997). Racial/ethnic and gender differences in concern with weight and in bulimic behaviors among adoles- cents. Obesity Research, 5(5), 447-454. Fitzgibbon, M. L., Spring, B., Avellone, M.E., Blackman, L.R., Pingitore, R., Stolley, M.R. (1998). Correlates of binge eating in Hispanic, Black, and White women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24(1), 43-52.
  • 53. French, S. A., Story, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Downes, B., Resnick, M., & Blum, R. (1997). Ethnic differences in psychosocial and health behavior correlates of dieting, purging, and binge eating in a population- based sam- ple of adolescent females. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22(3), 315-322. Gray, J. J., Ford, K., & Kelly, L. M. (1987). The prevalence of bulimia in a Black college population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 733-740. Greenberg, B. S., & Brand, J. E. (1994). Minorities and the mass media: 1970s to 1990s. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in the- ory and research (pp. 273-314). Norwood, NJ: Erlbaum. Gunther, A. C. (1991). What we think others think: Cause and consequences in the third-person effect. Communication Research, 18(3), 355- 372. Gunther, A. C. (1995). Overrating the X-rating: The third- person perception and support for censorship of pornography. Journal of Communication, 45(1), 27-38. Gunther, A. C., & Hwa, A. P. (1996). Public perceptions of television influence and opinions about censorship in Singapore. International
  • 54. Journal of Pub- lic Opinion Research, 8(3), 248-265. Harris, S. M. (1995). Family, self, and sociocultural contributions to body-image attitudes of African American women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 129-145. Harrison, K. (2000). The body electric: Thin-ideal media and eating disorders in adolescents. Journal of Communication, 50(3) 119-143. Harrison, K., & Cantor, J. (1997). The relationship between media consump- tion and eating disorders. Journal of Communication, 47(1), 40- 67. Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rajeski, W. J. (1989). The measurement of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport Psychology, 11, 94-104. Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1999). Social identity and social cognition: Histori- cal background and current trends. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social cognition (pp. 1-25). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Hogg, M. A., & Mullin, B. A. (1999). Joining groups to reduce uncertainty: Subjective uncertainty reduction and group identification. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social cognition (pp. 248-279). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
  • 55. Hraba, J., Radloff, T., & Ray-Gray, P. (1999). A comparison of black and white social distance. Journal of Social Psychology, 139(4), 536-539. Kann, L., & Ross, J. G. (1996). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 1995. Centers for Disease Control. Retrieved from http://www.cdc. gov/epo/mmwr/preview/mmwwrnum/00043812.num 292 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ Liebkind, K. (1992). Ethnic identity—Challenging the boundaries of social psychology. In G. M. Breakwell (Ed.), Social psychology of identity and the self concept (pp. 147-185). London: Surrey University Press. McLeod, D. M., Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Nathanson, A. I. (1997). Support for cen- sorship of violent and misogynic rap lyrics: An analysis of the third-person effect. Communication Research, 24(2), 153-174. Merelman, R. M. (1995). Representing Black culture: Racial conflict and cul- tural politics in the United States. New York: Routledge.
  • 56. Morales, J. F., López-Sáez, M., & Vega, L. (1998). Discrimination and beliefs on discrimination in individualists and collectivists. In S. Worchel, J. F. Morales, D. Páez, & J. Deschamps (Eds.), Social identity: International per- spectives (pp. 199-210). London: Sage. Myers, P. N., & Biocca, F. (1992). The elastic body image: The effect of televi- sion advertising and programming on body image distortions in young women. Journal of Communication, 42(3), 108-133. Pelham, B. W., & Hetts, J. J. (1999). Implicit and explicit personal and social identity: Toward a more complete understanding of the social self. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 115-143). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Perloff, R. M. (1989). Ego involvement and the third person effect of televised news coverage. Communication Research, 16(2), 236-262. Perloff, R. M. (1993). Third-person effect research 1983-1992: A review and synthesis. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 5(2), 167-184. Price, V., & Tewksbury, D. (1996). Measuring the third-person effect of news: The impact of question order, contrast and knowledge.
  • 57. International Jour- nal of Public Opinion Research, 8(2), 120-141. Richins, M. L. (1991). Social comparison and the idealized images of advertis- ing. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(1), 71-83. Rojas, H., Shah, D. V., & Faber, R. J. (1996). For the good of others: Censorship and the third-person effect. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 8(2), 163-186. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rucinski, D., & Salmon, C. T. (1990). The “other” as the vulnerable voter: A study of the third-person effect in the 1988 U.S. presidential campaign. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 2(4), 345- 368. Salwen, M. B. (1998). Perceptions of media influence and support for censor- ship: Third-person effect in the 1996 presidential election. Communica- tion Research, 25(3), 259-285. Salwen, M. B., & Driscoll, P. D. (1997). Consequences of third- person percep- tion in support of press restrictions in the O. J. Simpson trial. Journal of Communication, 47(2), 60-78.
  • 58. Shah, D. V., Faber, R. J., & Youn, S. (1999). Susceptibility and severity: Percep- tual dimensions underlying the third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(2), 240-267. Serino, C. (1998). The personal-social interplay: Social- cognitive prospects on identity and self-others comparison. In S. Worchel, J. F. Morales, D. Páez, & 293 David et al. • Body Image, Race, and Fashion Models at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/ J. Deschamps, Social identity: International perspectives (pp. 24-43). Lon- don: Sage. Silber, T. (1986). Anorexia nervosa in Blacks and Hispanics. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 121-128. Stice, E., Schupak-Neuberg, E., Shaw, H. E., & Stein, R. I. (1994). Relation of media exposure to eating disorder symptomatology: An examination of mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(4), 836-840.
  • 59. Stormer, S. M., & Thompson, J. K. (1996). Explanations of body image distur- bance: A test of maturational status, negative verbal commentary, social comparison, and sociocultural hypotheses. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19(2), 193-202. Story, M., French, S. A., Resnick, M. D., & Blum, R. W. (1995). Ethnic/racial and socioeconomic differences in dieting behaviors and body image per- ceptions in adolescents. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(2), 173-179. Striegel-Moore, R. H., Schreiber, G. B., Pike, K. M., Wilfley, D. E., & Rodin, J. (1995). Drive for thinness in Black and White preadolescent girls. Interna- tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 18(1), 59-69. Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psy- chology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-47). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
  • 60. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (2nd ed., pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson Hall. Thompson, J. K. (1995). Assessment of body image. In D. B. Allison (Ed.), Handbook of assessment methods for eating behaviors and weight-related problems: Measures, theory, and research (pp. 119-144). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tiedge, J. T., Silverblatt, A., Havice, M. J., & Rosenfeld, R. (1991). Discrepancy between perceived first-person and perceived third-person mass media effects. Journalism Quarterly, 68(1/2), 141-154. Turner, J. C., & Onorato, R. S. (1999). Social identity, personality, and the self-concept: A self-categorization perspective. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 11- 46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Tyler, T. R., & Cook, F. L. (1984). The mass media and judgments of risk: Dis- tinguishing impact on personal and societal level judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(4), 693-708. White, W. C., Hudson, L., & Campbell, S. (1985). Bulimarexia and Black
  • 61. women: A brief report. Psychotherapy, 22, 449-450. Wilson, C. C., II, & Gutiérrez, F. (1995). Race, multiculturalism and the media: From mass to class communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 294 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • June 2002 at UNIV OF NC PEMBROKE on September 19, 2016crx.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://crx.sagepub.com/