SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 6
discuss the author’ s viewpoints, what studies were done, and be sure to
discuss your opinion of the article.
discuss the author’ s viewpoints, what studies were done, and be sure to discuss your
opinion of the article.While You Were SleepingBy DANIEL WILLIAMSIn the field of sleep
and dreams, these are promising times. But there’ s been no year more momentous than
1953. Until then, scientists had equated sleep with flicking off a desk lamp. For more than
two decades they’ d been able to record brain activity in sleep, but the feeling was, why
bother? Why waste reams of costly graph paper making electroencephalogram recordings
of what was thought to be a neurological desert? With no strong expectation of finding
otherwise, University of Chicago researchers Eugene Aserinski and Nathaniel Kleitman
decided it was worth doing, monitoring 10 subjects in a laboratory. Their findings turned
our understanding of the sleeping brain upside down.What they discovered was a sleep
state in which the brain is, in many ways, every bit as active as when it’ s awake; a state in
which, compared with other stages of sleep, the heart beats faster, breathing quickens,
blood pressure and blood flow to the brain (and sexual organs) rise, while the eyes move
rapidly beneath their lids. Brain waves are low-voltage and high-frequency—
the opposite to
the brain waves of deep sleep, more like what goes on when a person is awake, thinking and
talking. Awoken from this paradoxical state that Aserinski and Kleitman called Rapid Eye
Movement (REM) sleep, subjects could usually recall vivid dreams. In a single swoop, the
pair had not only uncovered what many regard as a third state of consciousness, but raised
expectations that the mysteries of how and why we dream might soon be solved.The quest
for answers has been hindered by doubt: is dreaming a mystery worth solving? Science has
long had an uneasy relationship with our nocturnal imaginings. While some brilliant
practitioners have worked—
and do work—
in the field, its links with mysticism and Freudian
theory have repelled others like a bad odor. Everybody dreams and most people talk about
theirs now and again. But once, as children, we learn to distinguish these delusions from
reality, dreams usually become no more than a sideshow, sometimes disturbing,
occasionally poignant, but mostly something to be forgotten, quickly and completely, if they
were remembered in the first place.It’ s easy, in other words, to lose sight of what a
remarkable phenomenon dreaming is. Every night, devoid of external sensory stimulation,
our brains screen internally-projected films concocted from pieces of our own thoughts.
Nearly always in the lead role, we flee from danger, triumph and flop in our areas of
endeavor and enjoy passionate encounters with people we yearn for or hardly know. We do
these things and countless others not in a state of detachment but rather, despite the
bizarre distortions typical of dreams, convinced the events are real and with our emotions
and senses engaged. That the movies we watch in theaters were so engrossing.The
discovery of REM sleep was supposed to be an opening through which science could throw
light on dreaming. And it was, up to a point. But it has also been an obstacle to progress.
What happened was that many scientists became convinced that REM sleep and dreaming
are more or less the same thing—
that they’ re underpinned by the same physiological
processes. REM became known as “ dreaming sleep,” but that is almost certainly an
oversimplification. “ Sadly, there’ s an authoritative school of thought that will not let go of
this idea,” says Mark Solms, professor of neuropsychology at the University of Cape Town,
South Africa. “ As a discipline we’ ve been lost because we’ ve been conflating the
two.” There are tensions in the field, but “ it’ s starting to break out of its malaise,” says
Robert Stickgold, associate professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School’ s Center for
Sleep and Cognition. Recently, scientists have abandoned stagnant theories of dreaming and
postulated new and intriguing ones, with experiments underway in various parts of the
world aimed at establishing the function of our nightly hallucinations. If recent work
suggests anything, it’ s that there is such a function, or more than one, and that dreams
aren’ t just neural waste. They may improve the quality of our sleep. They may prepare us
for danger. They may embed memories. There are more theories than answers. But after
years in the doldrums, dream research is moving forward again.for all the recent advances
in brain science, it shouldn’ t surprise that the riddle of dreaming hasn’ t been cracked.
“ We still don’ t know why we sleep, let alone why we dream,” says Dorothy Bruck,
professor of psychology at Melbourne’ s Victoria University. It seems commonsensical that
sleep is a restorative phase for brain and body, and there’ s some evidence that the effects
of sleep deprivation are the result of minor brain damage that would normally be repaired
when we’ re asleep. But despite their best efforts, scientists have been unable to pinpoint
what’ s going on in sleep that makes it essential.Still, sleep research is a breeze compared to
studying dreaming. In the former, you can at least be certain you’ re observing a sleeping
person. “ But we don’ t have a device that looks into a person’ s head and sees dreams
happening in real time,” says Russell Conduit, a lecturer in the school of psychology,
psychiatry and psychological medicine at Melbourne’ s Monash University. Instead, dream
researchers rely on what he calls the “ faulty methodology” of waking subjects and asking
them what was going on in their heads immediately before they were woken. But because
certain parts of the brain are switched off during sleep, it shouldn’ t be assumed that
subjects’ answers will be accurate. They may have been having a dream but simply
weren’ t paying attention, can’ t remember it, or both. “ If you took a lot of the dream
research to a physicist,” says Conduit, “ they’ d laugh at you.” Before delving into the latest
theories, then, it’ s worth reviewing what we know about dreaming and the sleep state in
which we seem to do most of it. REM follows four stages of sleep known collectively as non-
REM sleep, in which brain activity becomes progressively more subdued. In REM—
which
occurs four or five times a night and lasts about 30 minutes at a time—
our muscles become
paralyzed, which could be a mechanism for preventing the acting out of our dreams. About
half of a baby’ s sleep is REM compared to a quarter of an adult’ s. The body craves it: when
cut short in experiments REM returns more insistently in subsequent periods of sleep.
Neuroimaging has told us a lot about the dreaming—
or at least the REM—
brain. Those areas
that are active include the brain stem (responsible for basic functions like heartbeat
regulation), the limbic system (which mediates emotions, learning and memory) and parts
of the forebrain involved in processing sensory information. Shut down, meanwhile, are the
bits responsible for the most sophisticated mental processes, such as logical and ordered
thought. It’ s a profile that fits neatly with the subjective experience of dreaming: vivid
images, strong emotion and snippets of memory, but a shortage of coherence. Rather like a
David Lynch film.All mammals experience REM, but some have more than others. It appears
the less mature a species is at birth, the more REM it has. Assuming for a moment that REM
equals dreaming, the opossum and armadillo are among the most prolific dreamers, while
dolphins do very little of it; humans are in the middle. Research suggests our dreams
become more complex as our mental abilities develop. The dreams of very small children
don’ t just seem prosaic because tikes lack the eloquence to bring them to life in the
retelling—
they really are prosaic. Two-thirds of dreams are almost exclusively visual, a
quarter feature sound and a smaller fraction smell and taste. Nine out of 10 contain
emotion, most commonly mild anxiety or frustration. Our dreams tend not to be
reproductions of past events but rather, according to research led by Tore Nielsen, director
of the Dream & Nightmare Laboratory at Sacr?-Coeur Hospital in Montreal,
reinterpretations of events that happened at two distinct time periods: yesterday and about
a week ago. We also dream about upcoming events and conversations we’ d like to have.
Other times the content is so weird who knows where it comes from?Recent developments
in dream research won’ t make sense without first touching on the academic thunderbolt of
1977, when a paper by two Harvard neurophysiologists, Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley,
ran in the American Journal of Psychiatry. At the time, Sigmund Freud’ s theory of dreams
(which holds, in part, that dreams preserve sleep by distracting the brain with reflections of
the unconscious) was a pillar of psychiatry. In The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An
Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process, the Harvard pair challenged
Freudian theory on virtually every point. They argued that dreams are nonsense created
when the forebrain makes “ the best of a bad job in producing even partially coherent
dream imagery from the relatively noisy signals” sent up to it from the brain stem at the
onset of REM. Their paper served to yank dreaming from the realms of the psychological
and plonk it in a dreary, physiological bucket.Later, the English molecular biologist Francis
Crick, a co-discoverer in the 1950s of the structure of the DNA molecule, drained a little
more romance from dreaming. His and theoretical biologist Graeme Mitchison’ s “ reverse
learning” theory held that dreams rid the brain of superfluous notions, and that without
this regular flushing brain overload would manifest as hallucinations and obsessions. There
are echoes of this idea in the perspective of Drew Dawson, director of the University of
South Australia’ s Centre for Sleep Research: “ I tend to think of dreaming as a bit like
backwashing the swimming pool filter.” While hugely influential, Hobson and McCarley’ s
Activation-Synthesis model attracted hordes of critics, who protested that many dreams
aren’ t merely cognitive fragments nor a succession of chaotic images, but so story-like,
sequential and dramatic that the thinking brain must surely have played a more substantial
role in their production than the last-minute editing of a pile of neural bloopers. And
there’ s the matter of lucid dreaming, in which people become aware in the course of a
dream that they are, in fact, dreaming, and are able to control the course of events—
a
phenomenon that strengthens the case for higher-brain involvement in dream construction.
The lucid dreamer can apparently apply certain techniques to prolong the dream and take it
in delightful directions. “ The experiences are so convincing,” says Victoria University’ s
Bruck, “ it seems as if another level of reality exists.” But the wrecking job on the notion that
dreams are a random by-product of REM sleep was carried out by the South African
neuroscientist and psychoanalyst Solms, who was working at the Royal London Hospital in
the 1990s when he made his career-defining discoveries. Solms wasn’ t alone at the time in
realizing that dreaming occurred outside periods of REM, that it was also common at sleep
onset and shortly before waking in the morning. But he found an even weaker spot in the
Hobson-McCarley hypothesis. If their theory was right, then people with damage to a part of
the brain stem called the pons—the on-off switch for REM sleep—
shouldn’ t be having
dreams. Solms, however, had five patients with lesions in precisely that region, and while
they weren’ t having REM, they were nonetheless reporting dreams.Even more interesting
to Solms were 53 Royal London Hospital patients with healthy brain stems who said they’ d
stopped dreaming. Most of them had damage to the part of the brain that generates spatial
imagery. That made sense: if you can’ t create pictures in your mind, how are you going to
dream? It was the circumstances of the remaining nine patients that fascinated Solms. They
had damage to the white matter of the ventromesial quadrant of the frontal lobes, an area
linked to the transmission of the chemical dopamine and crucially involved in motivation,
urges and cravings. These patients still experienced REM sleep but reported having lost
both the capacity to dream and all sense of spontaneity, drive and love of life. They did what
they were told and that was about it.It seemed to Solms that dreams must themselves be
associated with driving urges—
a very Freudian take—
but he needed more evidence in the
form of more people with lesions in this particular spot. Nowadays, damage to that part of
the brain is rare, normally a result of strokes or tumors. But it was a lot more common in
the ’ 50s and ’ 60s when some mental illnesses were treated by removing it in an operation
called a prefrontal leukotomy. Solms waded through the literature and found hundreds of
case studies in which the effects of this procedure were described. To his amazement,
reported loss of dreaming was one of them. “ So I thought I’ d discovered something new,”
says Solms, “ but it turned out to be something we’ d documented long ago but had
forgotten.” In the field of dreams, however, his findings were no less significant for that:
Solms had shown REM and dreaming to be dissociable states and produced a compelling
case that the higher brain has the central role in dream creation.Solms, who believes science
is getting closer to answering the key questions about dreaming, is leading two studies at
the University of Cape Town with that goal in mind. One involves using functional magnetic
resonance imaging to try to disentangle the REM brain from the dreaming brain. He wants
to obtain images of the dreaming/non-dreaming brain at sleep onset and note the
differences between the two. “ If we can image what’ s going on at that point, then we’ ll get
a clear handle on what mechanisms are important for dreaming as opposed to REM sleep,”
says Solms, who predicts a key role for the motivational part of the brain.His other study
involves comparing the sleeping ability of subjects who dream with that of subjects who
can’ t because of brain lesions. Solms argues that because our motivational drive is fully
active while we’ re asleep, our brain’ s way of keeping us asleep and undertaking the
necessary repairs is by tricking us, through dreams, into thinking we’ re up and about and
pursuing our desires. It’ s the neurological equivalent of putting on a DVD for the kids so
the main players in the house can get some shut-eye. “ Dreams replace the real actions that
are instigated by our motivational impulses while we’ re awake,” Solms says. His hunch is
that the non-dreamers in his study will wake up more often during the night than the
dreamers, especially during REM sleep: “ The dream,” he says, “ is what keeps you
asleep.” Others approach dreams from a different angle. An argument that resonates with
many is that whatever the explanation for dreaming, humans must do it for the same reason
that all mammals have done it for more than 100 million years; any theory must make as
much sense when applied to a rabbit as to a person. In the same Darwinian vein, sleeping
and dreaming must serve important functions because they’ re vulnerable states and
natural selection would have eliminated them if they didn’ t provide compensating benefits.
In the ancestral environment, human life was short and perilous; ever-lurking predators
threatened survival and reproductive success. The biological function of dreaming, argues
AnttiRevonsuo, professor of psychology at the University of Turku, Finland, is to simulate
threatening events so to prepare the dreamer for recognizing and avoiding danger.The
threat-simulation theory, first presented in 2000, “ is built on the actual empirical evidence
we have concerning the content of dreams,” Revonsuo says. “ It’ s surprising how many
theories of dreaming there are that are not based on any systematic review of the
evidence.” He cites studies showing that, typically, dreams are too seldom sweet, and that
negative feelings, dangerous scenarios and aggression are over-represented. Based on
ongoing work with PhD student KatjaValli, Revonsuo estimates that the average “ non-
traumatized” young adult has, conservatively, 300 threat-simulation dreams a year. In the
dreams of both men and women, male strangers and wild animals are most often the
enemy, and the dreamer’ s typical responses are running and hiding, often in a state of
terror.If dreams are biased toward simulating ancestral threats, the traces of these biases
would be strongest early in life, before the brain has adjusted to the realities of the
contemporary environment. Sure enough, Revonsuo says, research shows that animals
make up about 30% of all characters in the dreams of children aged 2-6 compared to 5% of
adults’ . True, the animals of children’ s dreams are often fluffy and harmless, but almost
half the time they’ re frightening creatures—
snakes, bears, lions, gorillas—
that children
would seldom, if ever, have encountered in waking life.The reason we don’ t dream about
reading and writing, Revonsuo speculates in his original paper, is not because these
activities don’ t engage our emotions but because they’ re “ cultural latecomers that have
[yet] to be effortlessly hammered into our evolved cognitive architecture.” Revonsuo
knows that his theory pleases neither Freudians nor neuroscientists. “ If, for the supporters
of the psychological theories, it grants too little meaning to dreams,” he says, “ for the
supporters of neurophysiological random-noise theories, it grants dreaming far too
much.” Harvard’ s Stickgold believes dreams have a different function entirely. “ I think it’ s
pretty clear now that sleep and dreaming serve to process memories from the last day and
all the way back,” he says. “ Sleep can strengthen memories…and help extract the meaning
of events by building associative networks with other memories. Dreaming is probably a
high-level version of this processing.” Clearly, he adds, you don’ t have to remember your
dreams for these processes to work. “ The brain is tuning your memory circuits as you
sleep, and remembering the imagery created during this process may be fun, may be
instructive, but is almost undoubtedly a freebie.” Stickgold’ s evidence includes an
experiment he led in 2000 when Harvard researchers were able to elicit the same dream in
a bunch of people as they drifted off to sleep. They did this by exposing 27 subjects to an
intensive three-day course in the computer game Tetris, which involves assembling
geometric shapes. By the second night of training, 17 subjects had reported having the same
dream image—
f alling Tetris pieces—
indicating to Stickgold that the need to learn prods the
brain to dream. More of these kinds of studies are needed, he says, “ because as we learn to
manipulate dream content, we can start to figure out what the rules are that the brain uses
in selecting material for our dreams.” Though not sold on the memory-consolidation
theory, the Dream & Nightmare Laboratory’ s Nielsen sees merit in it. Of course, if dreaming
does embed memories it’ s doing it in ways we don’ t understand, he says. “ Perhaps
memory needs to be sliced and diced and then reassembled in odd ways in order for
consolidation to be maximized.” Psychotherapists tend to regard a lot of the research into
dreaming as missing the point. Scientists, they say, can theorize all they like about
dreaming’ s function and physiological underpinnings, but why dreams matter is their
effect on the dreamer. The man contemplating an extramarital affair dreams of the dire
consequences of having one. He awakens to feel not only exquisite relief that he was
dreaming but determined to walk the line. If, as Solms believes, dreams spring from the
motivational part of our brain at a time when other parts that inhibit us are off-line, “ it
follows that there’ s value in interpreting dreams,” he says. They provide a “ privileged,
unfiltered access” to what’ s on a person’ s mind. Mark Blagrove, a lecturer in psychology
at the University of Wales, where he runs a sleep laboratory, thinks it’ s possible the search
for a biological function of dreaming could be futile. “ It could just be,” he says, “ that our
elaborate dreams are a side effect of the fact that we have a highly evolved imagination.” All
the competing theories on why we dream may be wrong. One or more of them could be
right. “ I have no doubt that dreams can be enjoyable, informative, even revelatory to the
dreamer,” says Harvard’ s Stickgold. “ But dream analysis is a more tricky question. The
more dogmatic and doctrinaire the beliefs of the analyst, the less useful and potentially
more destructive the analysis process becomes.” People should understand, he adds, that
dreams aren’ t constructed with the goal of delivering a message; they don’ t have an
inherent meaning. “ But when you look at your dreams after you wake up…you can often
feel the associative networks that were activated during dream construction, and trace
them back a ways, and maybe discover a new way of looking at events in your life, of
looking at yourself, at others, or at the world at large.” Maybe that’ s worth a third of our
lives asleep, perchance dreaming.

More Related Content

Similar to discuss the what studies were and be sure to discuss.docx

Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07
Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07
Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07SaraAton
 
Believe or not
Believe or notBelieve or not
Believe or notabahnasy
 
Chapter i dream have a meaning
Chapter i dream have a meaningChapter i dream have a meaning
Chapter i dream have a meaningdhanesh gabrial
 
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docx
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docxBRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docx
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docxjackiewalcutt
 

Similar to discuss the what studies were and be sure to discuss.docx (11)

Ispectrum magazine #04
Ispectrum magazine #04Ispectrum magazine #04
Ispectrum magazine #04
 
Myelination Essay
Myelination EssayMyelination Essay
Myelination Essay
 
Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07
Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07
Sleep And Neural Function 7.6.07
 
Believe or not
Believe or notBelieve or not
Believe or not
 
Dreams- Dr. Armaan Singh
Dreams- Dr. Armaan SinghDreams- Dr. Armaan Singh
Dreams- Dr. Armaan Singh
 
Edges of perception
Edges of perceptionEdges of perception
Edges of perception
 
Dreams
DreamsDreams
Dreams
 
Q3L12 - Dreams
Q3L12 - DreamsQ3L12 - Dreams
Q3L12 - Dreams
 
Chapter i dream have a meaning
Chapter i dream have a meaningChapter i dream have a meaning
Chapter i dream have a meaning
 
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docx
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docxBRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docx
BRAINS, MINDS, AND CONSCIOuSNESSNo area of the debate betwee.docx
 
CONSCIOUSNESS & TRANSCENDENCE
CONSCIOUSNESS & TRANSCENDENCECONSCIOUSNESS & TRANSCENDENCE
CONSCIOUSNESS & TRANSCENDENCE
 

More from bkbk37

Range of.docx
Range of.docxRange of.docx
Range of.docxbkbk37
 
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docx
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docxRalph Waldo Emerson.docx
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docxbkbk37
 
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docx
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docxRaising Minimum An explanation of the its.docx
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docxbkbk37
 
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docx
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docxRail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docx
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docxbkbk37
 
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docx
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docxRacism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docx
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docxbkbk37
 
Race and.docx
Race and.docxRace and.docx
Race and.docxbkbk37
 
R2P and Syria.docx
R2P and Syria.docxR2P and Syria.docx
R2P and Syria.docxbkbk37
 
Racial Disparities.docx
Racial Disparities.docxRacial Disparities.docx
Racial Disparities.docxbkbk37
 
Race and Technology.docx
Race and Technology.docxRace and Technology.docx
Race and Technology.docxbkbk37
 
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docx
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docxQuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docx
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docxbkbk37
 
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docx
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docxQuestions What are the purposes of Just.docx
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docxbkbk37
 
Questions to Each group you read about is.docx
Questions to Each group you read about is.docxQuestions to Each group you read about is.docx
Questions to Each group you read about is.docxbkbk37
 
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docx
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docxQuestions that must be answered in your plus other.docx
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docxbkbk37
 
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docx
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docxQuestions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docx
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docxbkbk37
 
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docx
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docxQuestion Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docx
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docxbkbk37
 
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docx
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docxQuestion Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docx
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docxbkbk37
 
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docx
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docxQuestion Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docx
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docxbkbk37
 
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docx
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docxQuestion share your perspective on personal data as a.docx
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docxbkbk37
 
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docx
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docxQEP Assignment Death Penalty.docx
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docxbkbk37
 
Question In your what are the main workforce.docx
Question In your what are the main workforce.docxQuestion In your what are the main workforce.docx
Question In your what are the main workforce.docxbkbk37
 

More from bkbk37 (20)

Range of.docx
Range of.docxRange of.docx
Range of.docx
 
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docx
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docxRalph Waldo Emerson.docx
Ralph Waldo Emerson.docx
 
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docx
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docxRaising Minimum An explanation of the its.docx
Raising Minimum An explanation of the its.docx
 
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docx
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docxRail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docx
Rail Project A goal of the Obama administration.docx
 
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docx
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docxRacism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docx
Racism toward Indigenous peoples in Canada.docx
 
Race and.docx
Race and.docxRace and.docx
Race and.docx
 
R2P and Syria.docx
R2P and Syria.docxR2P and Syria.docx
R2P and Syria.docx
 
Racial Disparities.docx
Racial Disparities.docxRacial Disparities.docx
Racial Disparities.docx
 
Race and Technology.docx
Race and Technology.docxRace and Technology.docx
Race and Technology.docx
 
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docx
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docxQuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docx
QuickBooks uses windows API to follow orders to get updates.docx
 
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docx
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docxQuestions What are the purposes of Just.docx
Questions What are the purposes of Just.docx
 
Questions to Each group you read about is.docx
Questions to Each group you read about is.docxQuestions to Each group you read about is.docx
Questions to Each group you read about is.docx
 
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docx
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docxQuestions that must be answered in your plus other.docx
Questions that must be answered in your plus other.docx
 
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docx
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docxQuestions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docx
Questions for Brief Explicit Spiritual.docx
 
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docx
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docxQuestion Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docx
Question Libya recently announced that it is claiming a.docx
 
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docx
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docxQuestion Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docx
Question Use the Internet or the IGlobal Resource.docx
 
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docx
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docxQuestion Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docx
Question Please define motivation and discuss why it is.docx
 
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docx
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docxQuestion share your perspective on personal data as a.docx
Question share your perspective on personal data as a.docx
 
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docx
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docxQEP Assignment Death Penalty.docx
QEP Assignment Death Penalty.docx
 
Question In your what are the main workforce.docx
Question In your what are the main workforce.docxQuestion In your what are the main workforce.docx
Question In your what are the main workforce.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfChris Hunter
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Shubhangi Sonawane
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701bronxfugly43
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxRamakrishna Reddy Bijjam
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxnegromaestrong
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...Poonam Aher Patil
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
Mehran University Newsletter Vol-X, Issue-I, 2024
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 

discuss the what studies were and be sure to discuss.docx

  • 1. discuss the author’ s viewpoints, what studies were done, and be sure to discuss your opinion of the article. discuss the author’ s viewpoints, what studies were done, and be sure to discuss your opinion of the article.While You Were SleepingBy DANIEL WILLIAMSIn the field of sleep and dreams, these are promising times. But there’ s been no year more momentous than 1953. Until then, scientists had equated sleep with flicking off a desk lamp. For more than two decades they’ d been able to record brain activity in sleep, but the feeling was, why bother? Why waste reams of costly graph paper making electroencephalogram recordings of what was thought to be a neurological desert? With no strong expectation of finding otherwise, University of Chicago researchers Eugene Aserinski and Nathaniel Kleitman decided it was worth doing, monitoring 10 subjects in a laboratory. Their findings turned our understanding of the sleeping brain upside down.What they discovered was a sleep state in which the brain is, in many ways, every bit as active as when it’ s awake; a state in which, compared with other stages of sleep, the heart beats faster, breathing quickens, blood pressure and blood flow to the brain (and sexual organs) rise, while the eyes move rapidly beneath their lids. Brain waves are low-voltage and high-frequency— the opposite to the brain waves of deep sleep, more like what goes on when a person is awake, thinking and talking. Awoken from this paradoxical state that Aserinski and Kleitman called Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep, subjects could usually recall vivid dreams. In a single swoop, the pair had not only uncovered what many regard as a third state of consciousness, but raised expectations that the mysteries of how and why we dream might soon be solved.The quest for answers has been hindered by doubt: is dreaming a mystery worth solving? Science has long had an uneasy relationship with our nocturnal imaginings. While some brilliant practitioners have worked— and do work— in the field, its links with mysticism and Freudian theory have repelled others like a bad odor. Everybody dreams and most people talk about theirs now and again. But once, as children, we learn to distinguish these delusions from reality, dreams usually become no more than a sideshow, sometimes disturbing, occasionally poignant, but mostly something to be forgotten, quickly and completely, if they were remembered in the first place.It’ s easy, in other words, to lose sight of what a remarkable phenomenon dreaming is. Every night, devoid of external sensory stimulation, our brains screen internally-projected films concocted from pieces of our own thoughts. Nearly always in the lead role, we flee from danger, triumph and flop in our areas of endeavor and enjoy passionate encounters with people we yearn for or hardly know. We do
  • 2. these things and countless others not in a state of detachment but rather, despite the bizarre distortions typical of dreams, convinced the events are real and with our emotions and senses engaged. That the movies we watch in theaters were so engrossing.The discovery of REM sleep was supposed to be an opening through which science could throw light on dreaming. And it was, up to a point. But it has also been an obstacle to progress. What happened was that many scientists became convinced that REM sleep and dreaming are more or less the same thing— that they’ re underpinned by the same physiological processes. REM became known as “ dreaming sleep,” but that is almost certainly an oversimplification. “ Sadly, there’ s an authoritative school of thought that will not let go of this idea,” says Mark Solms, professor of neuropsychology at the University of Cape Town, South Africa. “ As a discipline we’ ve been lost because we’ ve been conflating the two.” There are tensions in the field, but “ it’ s starting to break out of its malaise,” says Robert Stickgold, associate professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School’ s Center for Sleep and Cognition. Recently, scientists have abandoned stagnant theories of dreaming and postulated new and intriguing ones, with experiments underway in various parts of the world aimed at establishing the function of our nightly hallucinations. If recent work suggests anything, it’ s that there is such a function, or more than one, and that dreams aren’ t just neural waste. They may improve the quality of our sleep. They may prepare us for danger. They may embed memories. There are more theories than answers. But after years in the doldrums, dream research is moving forward again.for all the recent advances in brain science, it shouldn’ t surprise that the riddle of dreaming hasn’ t been cracked. “ We still don’ t know why we sleep, let alone why we dream,” says Dorothy Bruck, professor of psychology at Melbourne’ s Victoria University. It seems commonsensical that sleep is a restorative phase for brain and body, and there’ s some evidence that the effects of sleep deprivation are the result of minor brain damage that would normally be repaired when we’ re asleep. But despite their best efforts, scientists have been unable to pinpoint what’ s going on in sleep that makes it essential.Still, sleep research is a breeze compared to studying dreaming. In the former, you can at least be certain you’ re observing a sleeping person. “ But we don’ t have a device that looks into a person’ s head and sees dreams happening in real time,” says Russell Conduit, a lecturer in the school of psychology, psychiatry and psychological medicine at Melbourne’ s Monash University. Instead, dream researchers rely on what he calls the “ faulty methodology” of waking subjects and asking them what was going on in their heads immediately before they were woken. But because certain parts of the brain are switched off during sleep, it shouldn’ t be assumed that subjects’ answers will be accurate. They may have been having a dream but simply weren’ t paying attention, can’ t remember it, or both. “ If you took a lot of the dream research to a physicist,” says Conduit, “ they’ d laugh at you.” Before delving into the latest theories, then, it’ s worth reviewing what we know about dreaming and the sleep state in which we seem to do most of it. REM follows four stages of sleep known collectively as non- REM sleep, in which brain activity becomes progressively more subdued. In REM— which occurs four or five times a night and lasts about 30 minutes at a time— our muscles become paralyzed, which could be a mechanism for preventing the acting out of our dreams. About half of a baby’ s sleep is REM compared to a quarter of an adult’ s. The body craves it: when
  • 3. cut short in experiments REM returns more insistently in subsequent periods of sleep. Neuroimaging has told us a lot about the dreaming— or at least the REM— brain. Those areas that are active include the brain stem (responsible for basic functions like heartbeat regulation), the limbic system (which mediates emotions, learning and memory) and parts of the forebrain involved in processing sensory information. Shut down, meanwhile, are the bits responsible for the most sophisticated mental processes, such as logical and ordered thought. It’ s a profile that fits neatly with the subjective experience of dreaming: vivid images, strong emotion and snippets of memory, but a shortage of coherence. Rather like a David Lynch film.All mammals experience REM, but some have more than others. It appears the less mature a species is at birth, the more REM it has. Assuming for a moment that REM equals dreaming, the opossum and armadillo are among the most prolific dreamers, while dolphins do very little of it; humans are in the middle. Research suggests our dreams become more complex as our mental abilities develop. The dreams of very small children don’ t just seem prosaic because tikes lack the eloquence to bring them to life in the retelling— they really are prosaic. Two-thirds of dreams are almost exclusively visual, a quarter feature sound and a smaller fraction smell and taste. Nine out of 10 contain emotion, most commonly mild anxiety or frustration. Our dreams tend not to be reproductions of past events but rather, according to research led by Tore Nielsen, director of the Dream & Nightmare Laboratory at Sacr?-Coeur Hospital in Montreal, reinterpretations of events that happened at two distinct time periods: yesterday and about a week ago. We also dream about upcoming events and conversations we’ d like to have. Other times the content is so weird who knows where it comes from?Recent developments in dream research won’ t make sense without first touching on the academic thunderbolt of 1977, when a paper by two Harvard neurophysiologists, Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley, ran in the American Journal of Psychiatry. At the time, Sigmund Freud’ s theory of dreams (which holds, in part, that dreams preserve sleep by distracting the brain with reflections of the unconscious) was a pillar of psychiatry. In The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process, the Harvard pair challenged Freudian theory on virtually every point. They argued that dreams are nonsense created when the forebrain makes “ the best of a bad job in producing even partially coherent dream imagery from the relatively noisy signals” sent up to it from the brain stem at the onset of REM. Their paper served to yank dreaming from the realms of the psychological and plonk it in a dreary, physiological bucket.Later, the English molecular biologist Francis Crick, a co-discoverer in the 1950s of the structure of the DNA molecule, drained a little more romance from dreaming. His and theoretical biologist Graeme Mitchison’ s “ reverse learning” theory held that dreams rid the brain of superfluous notions, and that without this regular flushing brain overload would manifest as hallucinations and obsessions. There are echoes of this idea in the perspective of Drew Dawson, director of the University of South Australia’ s Centre for Sleep Research: “ I tend to think of dreaming as a bit like backwashing the swimming pool filter.” While hugely influential, Hobson and McCarley’ s Activation-Synthesis model attracted hordes of critics, who protested that many dreams aren’ t merely cognitive fragments nor a succession of chaotic images, but so story-like, sequential and dramatic that the thinking brain must surely have played a more substantial
  • 4. role in their production than the last-minute editing of a pile of neural bloopers. And there’ s the matter of lucid dreaming, in which people become aware in the course of a dream that they are, in fact, dreaming, and are able to control the course of events— a phenomenon that strengthens the case for higher-brain involvement in dream construction. The lucid dreamer can apparently apply certain techniques to prolong the dream and take it in delightful directions. “ The experiences are so convincing,” says Victoria University’ s Bruck, “ it seems as if another level of reality exists.” But the wrecking job on the notion that dreams are a random by-product of REM sleep was carried out by the South African neuroscientist and psychoanalyst Solms, who was working at the Royal London Hospital in the 1990s when he made his career-defining discoveries. Solms wasn’ t alone at the time in realizing that dreaming occurred outside periods of REM, that it was also common at sleep onset and shortly before waking in the morning. But he found an even weaker spot in the Hobson-McCarley hypothesis. If their theory was right, then people with damage to a part of the brain stem called the pons—the on-off switch for REM sleep— shouldn’ t be having dreams. Solms, however, had five patients with lesions in precisely that region, and while they weren’ t having REM, they were nonetheless reporting dreams.Even more interesting to Solms were 53 Royal London Hospital patients with healthy brain stems who said they’ d stopped dreaming. Most of them had damage to the part of the brain that generates spatial imagery. That made sense: if you can’ t create pictures in your mind, how are you going to dream? It was the circumstances of the remaining nine patients that fascinated Solms. They had damage to the white matter of the ventromesial quadrant of the frontal lobes, an area linked to the transmission of the chemical dopamine and crucially involved in motivation, urges and cravings. These patients still experienced REM sleep but reported having lost both the capacity to dream and all sense of spontaneity, drive and love of life. They did what they were told and that was about it.It seemed to Solms that dreams must themselves be associated with driving urges— a very Freudian take— but he needed more evidence in the form of more people with lesions in this particular spot. Nowadays, damage to that part of the brain is rare, normally a result of strokes or tumors. But it was a lot more common in the ’ 50s and ’ 60s when some mental illnesses were treated by removing it in an operation called a prefrontal leukotomy. Solms waded through the literature and found hundreds of case studies in which the effects of this procedure were described. To his amazement, reported loss of dreaming was one of them. “ So I thought I’ d discovered something new,” says Solms, “ but it turned out to be something we’ d documented long ago but had forgotten.” In the field of dreams, however, his findings were no less significant for that: Solms had shown REM and dreaming to be dissociable states and produced a compelling case that the higher brain has the central role in dream creation.Solms, who believes science is getting closer to answering the key questions about dreaming, is leading two studies at the University of Cape Town with that goal in mind. One involves using functional magnetic resonance imaging to try to disentangle the REM brain from the dreaming brain. He wants to obtain images of the dreaming/non-dreaming brain at sleep onset and note the differences between the two. “ If we can image what’ s going on at that point, then we’ ll get a clear handle on what mechanisms are important for dreaming as opposed to REM sleep,” says Solms, who predicts a key role for the motivational part of the brain.His other study
  • 5. involves comparing the sleeping ability of subjects who dream with that of subjects who can’ t because of brain lesions. Solms argues that because our motivational drive is fully active while we’ re asleep, our brain’ s way of keeping us asleep and undertaking the necessary repairs is by tricking us, through dreams, into thinking we’ re up and about and pursuing our desires. It’ s the neurological equivalent of putting on a DVD for the kids so the main players in the house can get some shut-eye. “ Dreams replace the real actions that are instigated by our motivational impulses while we’ re awake,” Solms says. His hunch is that the non-dreamers in his study will wake up more often during the night than the dreamers, especially during REM sleep: “ The dream,” he says, “ is what keeps you asleep.” Others approach dreams from a different angle. An argument that resonates with many is that whatever the explanation for dreaming, humans must do it for the same reason that all mammals have done it for more than 100 million years; any theory must make as much sense when applied to a rabbit as to a person. In the same Darwinian vein, sleeping and dreaming must serve important functions because they’ re vulnerable states and natural selection would have eliminated them if they didn’ t provide compensating benefits. In the ancestral environment, human life was short and perilous; ever-lurking predators threatened survival and reproductive success. The biological function of dreaming, argues AnttiRevonsuo, professor of psychology at the University of Turku, Finland, is to simulate threatening events so to prepare the dreamer for recognizing and avoiding danger.The threat-simulation theory, first presented in 2000, “ is built on the actual empirical evidence we have concerning the content of dreams,” Revonsuo says. “ It’ s surprising how many theories of dreaming there are that are not based on any systematic review of the evidence.” He cites studies showing that, typically, dreams are too seldom sweet, and that negative feelings, dangerous scenarios and aggression are over-represented. Based on ongoing work with PhD student KatjaValli, Revonsuo estimates that the average “ non- traumatized” young adult has, conservatively, 300 threat-simulation dreams a year. In the dreams of both men and women, male strangers and wild animals are most often the enemy, and the dreamer’ s typical responses are running and hiding, often in a state of terror.If dreams are biased toward simulating ancestral threats, the traces of these biases would be strongest early in life, before the brain has adjusted to the realities of the contemporary environment. Sure enough, Revonsuo says, research shows that animals make up about 30% of all characters in the dreams of children aged 2-6 compared to 5% of adults’ . True, the animals of children’ s dreams are often fluffy and harmless, but almost half the time they’ re frightening creatures— snakes, bears, lions, gorillas— that children would seldom, if ever, have encountered in waking life.The reason we don’ t dream about reading and writing, Revonsuo speculates in his original paper, is not because these activities don’ t engage our emotions but because they’ re “ cultural latecomers that have [yet] to be effortlessly hammered into our evolved cognitive architecture.” Revonsuo knows that his theory pleases neither Freudians nor neuroscientists. “ If, for the supporters of the psychological theories, it grants too little meaning to dreams,” he says, “ for the supporters of neurophysiological random-noise theories, it grants dreaming far too much.” Harvard’ s Stickgold believes dreams have a different function entirely. “ I think it’ s pretty clear now that sleep and dreaming serve to process memories from the last day and
  • 6. all the way back,” he says. “ Sleep can strengthen memories…and help extract the meaning of events by building associative networks with other memories. Dreaming is probably a high-level version of this processing.” Clearly, he adds, you don’ t have to remember your dreams for these processes to work. “ The brain is tuning your memory circuits as you sleep, and remembering the imagery created during this process may be fun, may be instructive, but is almost undoubtedly a freebie.” Stickgold’ s evidence includes an experiment he led in 2000 when Harvard researchers were able to elicit the same dream in a bunch of people as they drifted off to sleep. They did this by exposing 27 subjects to an intensive three-day course in the computer game Tetris, which involves assembling geometric shapes. By the second night of training, 17 subjects had reported having the same dream image— f alling Tetris pieces— indicating to Stickgold that the need to learn prods the brain to dream. More of these kinds of studies are needed, he says, “ because as we learn to manipulate dream content, we can start to figure out what the rules are that the brain uses in selecting material for our dreams.” Though not sold on the memory-consolidation theory, the Dream & Nightmare Laboratory’ s Nielsen sees merit in it. Of course, if dreaming does embed memories it’ s doing it in ways we don’ t understand, he says. “ Perhaps memory needs to be sliced and diced and then reassembled in odd ways in order for consolidation to be maximized.” Psychotherapists tend to regard a lot of the research into dreaming as missing the point. Scientists, they say, can theorize all they like about dreaming’ s function and physiological underpinnings, but why dreams matter is their effect on the dreamer. The man contemplating an extramarital affair dreams of the dire consequences of having one. He awakens to feel not only exquisite relief that he was dreaming but determined to walk the line. If, as Solms believes, dreams spring from the motivational part of our brain at a time when other parts that inhibit us are off-line, “ it follows that there’ s value in interpreting dreams,” he says. They provide a “ privileged, unfiltered access” to what’ s on a person’ s mind. Mark Blagrove, a lecturer in psychology at the University of Wales, where he runs a sleep laboratory, thinks it’ s possible the search for a biological function of dreaming could be futile. “ It could just be,” he says, “ that our elaborate dreams are a side effect of the fact that we have a highly evolved imagination.” All the competing theories on why we dream may be wrong. One or more of them could be right. “ I have no doubt that dreams can be enjoyable, informative, even revelatory to the dreamer,” says Harvard’ s Stickgold. “ But dream analysis is a more tricky question. The more dogmatic and doctrinaire the beliefs of the analyst, the less useful and potentially more destructive the analysis process becomes.” People should understand, he adds, that dreams aren’ t constructed with the goal of delivering a message; they don’ t have an inherent meaning. “ But when you look at your dreams after you wake up…you can often feel the associative networks that were activated during dream construction, and trace them back a ways, and maybe discover a new way of looking at events in your life, of looking at yourself, at others, or at the world at large.” Maybe that’ s worth a third of our lives asleep, perchance dreaming.