SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 36
marketing aspects of cultural distance
Carlos M. P. Sousa
There is a general consensus in the literature
that when firms decide to enter foreign
markets, they must adjust to a different cultural
environment and be prepared for challenges,
such as differences in language, lifestyles,
cultural standards, consumer preferences, and
purchasing power, among others (see GLOBAL
MARKETING STRATEGY; CROSS-CULTURAL
PSYCHOLOGY OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR;
STANDARDIZATION/ADAPTATION OF
INTERNATIONAL MARKETING STRA-
TEGY; BASE OF THE PYRAMID MARKETS:
CULTURE INSIGHTS AND MARKETING
IMPLICATIONS). To assess these differences
between countries, a new body of literature has
emerged around the concept of cultural distance,
which has been used to measure the scope and
scale of these differences. The assumption is
that cultural differences between the home and
the foreign markets create a distance, which, in
turn, influences the activity of the firm in the
international arena. As a result, few concepts
in the international-marketing literature
have gained broader attention than ‘‘cultural
distance.’’ This concept has been identified as a
key variable in explaining the behavior of firms
in a vast array of areas including entry-mode
choice, foreign-market selection, level of control
(see MARKET ENTRY AND EXPANSION),
international-marketing strategies (see GLOBAL
MARKETING STRATEGY: PERSPECTIVES
AND APPROACHES), and performance (see
EXPORT PERFORMANCE). Mixed empirical
results, however, have been found in the
literature regarding the importance of this
construct. Whereas some studies have found
cultural distance to significantly influence the
activity of the firm in the international arena
(Barkema and Vermeulen, 1997), other studies
have found no significant or clear relationship
(Mitra and Golder, 2002).
Various reasons have been proposed to explain
these inconclusive results. On the one hand,
some scholars have argued that the preferences
and tastes of consumers in different countries are
converging to a global norm (Levitt, 1983), and
hence the effect of cultural distance is likely to
dilute progressively. The notion that national
cultures are converging appears to be plau-
sible considering the emergence of the Internet,
greater ease of information flow, and more
frequent/easier international travel. However,
this argument is disputed by Barkema and
Vermeulen (1997), who use data spanning almost
three decades (1966–1994) in their study, and
find that the effect of cultural distance does not
decrease over time, that is, that cultural values
and distances remain stable. This may be due
to the fact that such changes concern conver-
gences in superficial appearances of culture (i.e.,
symbols, heroes, and rituals) and that they
do not necessarily signal a convergence in the
values embedded in national cultures (Hofstede,
2006).
The inconsistencies could also be attributed to
the difficulty that exists in the conceptualization
and operationalization of the cultural-distance
construct. For instance, as Sousa and Bradley
(2006, 2008) note, many authors have used
cultural distance and psychic distance (see
MARKETING ASPECTS OF PSYCHIC DIST-
ANCE) interchangeably with no clear distinction
between them, a contamination that has been
disputed by these authors and which may
undermine the validity of the research (see
RESEARCH RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY).
Cultural distance has been defined as the degree
to which cultural values in one country are
different from those in another country (Sousa
and Bradley, 2006). Accordingly, it should be
used to assess differences in national culture.
Several frameworks have been advanced and
used in the measurement of the extent to which
different cultures are similar or different, such
as that of Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck (1961)
and Ronen and Shenkar (1985). However,
the most widely used framework is that of
Hofstede’s (1980) cultural dimensions. To
arrive at a measure of cultural distance, Kogut
and Singh (1988) were the first to combine
Hofstede’s dimensions into one aggregate
measure of cultural distance among countries,
and many studies have subsequently used
this formula or an adapted version, as a
measure of cultural distance. However, while
a tangible and convenient tool, the Kogut
and Singh index has been liable to the same
criticism as that leveled against Hofstede’s value
Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing, edited by
Jagdish N. Sheth and Naresh K. Malhotra.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
ye
Line
2 marketing aspects of cultural distance
dimensions, for example, nonexhaustiveness,
outdated data, assumptions about the linearity,
additivity, and normal distributions of scores
(Shenkar, 2001). As a result, there has been an
increasing call in the literature to develop new
measures (see VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY)
to operationalize cultural distance as it may
be unrealistic to expect that a single measure
can fully discern the underlying differences
across countries (Tihanyi, Griffith, and Russell,
2005). Some recent studies may offer some
interesting alternatives to measure cultural
distance (Schwartz, 1994) and should, therefore,
be explored in future studies.
Bibliography
Barkema, H.G. and Vermeulen, F. (1997) What differ-
ences in the cultural backgrounds of partners are
detrimental for international joint ventures. Journal
of International Business Studies, 28 (4), 845–864.
Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture’s Consequences: International
Differences in Work-related Values, Sage Publications,
Beverly Hills.
Hofstede, G. (2006) What did GLOBE really measure?
Researchers’ minds versus respondents’ minds.
Journal of International Business Studies, 37 (6),
882–896.
Kluckhohn, F.R. and Strodtbeck, F.L. (1961) Variations
in Value Orientations, Row, Peterson, Evanston.
Kogut, B. and Singh, H. (1988) The effect of national
culture on the choice of entry mode. Journal of Inter-
national Business Studies, 19 (3), 411–432.
Levitt, T. (1983) The globalization of markets. Harvard
Business Review, 61 (3), 92–102.
Mitra, D. and Golder, P.N. (2002) Whose culture matters?
Near-market knowledge and its impact on foreign
market entry timing. Journal of Marketing Research,
39 (3), 350–365.
Ronen, S. and Shenkar, O. (1985) Clustering countries
on attitudinal dimensions: a review and synthesis.
Academy of Management Review, 10 (3), 435–454.
Schwartz S.H. (1994) in Individualism and Collectivism:
Theory, Method and Applications (eds U. Kim H.C.
Triandis C. Kagitçibasi et al.), Sage Publications,
Thousand Oaks, pp. 85–119.
Shenkar, O. (2001) Cultural distance revisited: towards a
more rigorous conceptualization and measurement of
cultural differences. Journal of International Business
Studies, 32 (3), 519–535.
Sousa, C.M.P. and Bradley, F. (2006) Cultural distance
and psychic distance: two peas in a pod? Journal of
International Marketing, 14 (1), 49–70.
Sousa, C.M.P. and Bradley, F. (2008) Cultural distance
and psychic distance: refinements in conceptualisation
and measurement. Journal of Marketing Management,
24 (5/6), 467–488.
Tihanyi, L., Griffith, D.A., and Russell, C.J. (2005)
The effect of cultural distance on entry mode choice,
international diversification, and MNE performance:
a meta-analysis. Journal of International Business
Studies, 36 (3), 270–283.
cross-cultural psychology of consumer
behavior
Minkyung Koo and Sharon Shavitt
INTRODUCTION
As new global markets emerge, and existing
markets become increasingly segmented along
ethnic or subcultural lines, the need to market
effectively to consumers who have different
cultural values has never been more important.
Thus, it is no surprise that in the last several
years, culture has rapidly emerged as a central
focus of research in consumer behavior. This
development followed on the heels of extensive
social psychological research on culture, which
provided a strong theoretical foundation for the
consumer-behavior studies that followed.
What is culture? Culture consists of shared
knowledge that provides the standards for
perceiving, believing, evaluating, feeling, comm-
unicating, and acting among those who share a
language, a historical period, and a geographic
location. As a psychological construct, culture
can be studied in multiple ways – across nations,
across ethnic groups within nations, across
individuals within nations (focusing on cultural
orientation), and across situations through the
priming of cultural values. The dimensions of
individualism versus collectivism, independence
versus interdependence, and analysis versus
holism have in recent years received significant
research attention. This attention has resulted
in a great number of studies revealing both
antecedents and consequences of the cultural
differences between East Asian and North
American cultures. As discussed subsequently,
regardless of how culture is studied, cultural
distinctions have important implications for
advertising content, persuasiveness of appeals,
consumer motivation, and consumer judgment
processes.
Article scope and overview This article reviews
major cultural constructs and theoretical impli-
cations of cultural differences in consumer infor-
mation processing, judgments, and choices. Our
review is necessarily selective, focusing on find-
ings specific to the consumer domain rather than
providing a general review of cultural differences
(for an excellent general review, see Wyer, Chiu,
and Hong, 2009). It should also be noted that
because of space limitations, this article does
not cover some major topics in cross-cultural
consumer behaviors such as self-regulation and
risk-taking, as well as methodological issues such
as response styles and biases (Shavitt, Lee, and
Torelli, 2009; Shavitt, Torelli, and Wong, 2009).
In this article, the cultural constructs of
individualism/collectivism and the indepen-
dent/interdependent self-construals associated
with them are given special attention because
extensive research has demonstrated the
implications of these distinctions for processes
and outcomes relevant to consumer behavior.
The most recent refinements to these constructs
are briefly reviewed in an attempt to identify
additional cultural variables likely to enhance
the understanding of cross-cultural consumer
behavior. We also review cultural differences
in thinking styles as a major emerging cultural
distinction and focus on their implications for
consumer-behavior research. Finally, we close
with a review of cross-cultural differences in
advertising content and the persuasiveness of
appeals.
KEY CONSTRUCTS AND DIMENSIONS OF
CULTURE
Individualism versus collectivism. The cons-
tructs of individualism and collectivism represent
the most broadly used dimensions of cultural
variability for cross-cultural comparison. In
individualistic cultures, people value inde-
pendence from others and subordinate the
goals of their in-groups to their own personal
goals. In collectivistic cultures, in contrast,
individuals value interdependent relationships
to others and subordinate their personal goals
to those of their in-groups (Hofstede, 1980;
Triandis, 1995). The key distinction involves
the extent to which one defines the self in
relation to others. In individualistic cultural
contexts, people tend to have an independent
self-construal whereby the self is defined
as autonomous and unique. In collectivistic
cultural contexts, by contrast, people tend to
have an interdependent self-construal whereby
Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing, edited by
Jagdish N. Sheth and Naresh K. Malhotra.
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
2 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior
the self is seen as inextricably and fundamentally
embedded within a larger social network of roles
and relationships (Markus and Kitayama, 1991).
National cultures that celebrate the values
of independence, such as the United States,
Canada, Germany, and Denmark, are typically
categorized as individualistic societies in which
an independent self-construal is common. In
contrast, cultures that nurture the values of
fulfilling one’s obligations over one’s own
personal wishes, including most East Asian and
Latin American countries, are categorized as
collectivistic societies in which an interdepen-
dent self-construal is common (Hofstede, 1980;
Triandis, 1995).
A large body of research in psychology
has demonstrated the many implications of
individualism/collectivism and independent/
interdependent self-construals for social perce-
ption and social behavior (Markus and Kitayama,
1991; Triandis, 1995). These findings indicate
consistently that individualists and people with
an independent self-construal are oriented
toward products and experiences that promote
achievement and autonomy, offer personal bene-
fits, and enable expression of one’s distinctive
qualities. On the other hand, collectivists and
people with an interdependent self-construal are
oriented toward products and experiences that
allow one to avoid negative outcomes, maintain
harmony and strong social connections with
others, and dutifully fulfill social roles.
Although a given self-construal can be more
chronically accessible in a particular culture,
cultures generally provide sufficient experiences
with independent and interdependent views of
the self to allow either type of self-construal to be
primed (Oyserman, Coon, and Kemmelmeier,
2002; Oyserman and Lee, 2007). Numerous
studies have established that these activated
self-views impact judgments in ways that
parallel cross-national differences (Shavitt, Lee,
and Torelli, 2009), for instance, by activating
distinct self goals (Lalwani and Shavitt, 2009).
People in general, and especially bicultural
people, can readily switch back and forth
between independent and interdependent
cultural frames in response to their contexts.
For instance, Lau-Gesk (2003) found that
independent (interdependent) self-construals
were temporarily activated when bicultural
consumers were exposed to individually focused
(interpersonally focused) appeals.
In sum, the distinctions between individual-
istic and collectivistic societies, and independent
and interdependent self-construals, are crucial
to the understanding of cross-cultural differ-
ences in consumer behavior. The studies to be
reviewed here offer extensive evidence that these
cultural classifications have fundamental impli-
cations for consumption-related outcomes.
Refined individualism versus collectivism. The
conceptualizations of individualism and collec-
tivism, and independence/interdependence,
have historically been broad and multidimen-
sional, summarizing a host of differences in
focus of attention, self-definitions, motiva-
tions, emotional connections to in-groups,
as well as belief systems and behavioral
patterns (Hofstede, 1980; Oyserman, Coon,
and Kemmelmeier, 2002). In addition, recent
studies have proposed useful refinements to
these broader cultural categories (Shavitt, Lee,
and Torelli, 2009). These studies suggest that
the nature and meaning of individualism and
collectivism varies across gender and ethnic
lines, as well as across family groupings and
institutions. Although the breadth of the
individualism-collectivism constructs lends
integrative strengths, research indicates that
further refinements of these categories can
enhance the prediction of consumer behavior.
The horizontal/vertical distinction. With
in the individualism-collectivism framework,
Triandis et al. (Triandis, 1995; Triandis and
Gelfand, 1998) have recently introduced a
further distinction between societies that are
horizontal (valuing equality) and those that are
vertical (emphasizing hierarchy), and a scale to
measure these orientations at the individual level.
The horizontal/vertical distinction emerges
from the observation that American or British
individualism differs from, say, Norwegian or
Danish individualism in much the same way that
Japanese or Korean collectivism differs from the
collectivism of the Israeli kibbutz. Specifically,
in vertical individualist (VI) societies (e.g.,
United States and Great Britain), people strive
to become distinguished and acquire status
via competition (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong,
cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 3
2009); whereas in horizontal individualist (HI)
cultural contexts (e.g., Sweden and Norway),
people value uniqueness but are not especially
interested in becoming distinguished and
achieving high status (Nelson and Shavitt,
2002). In contrast, in vertical collectivistic
(VC) societies (e.g., Korea and Japan), people
emphasize the subordination of their goals to
those of their in-groups, submit to the will of
authority, and support competitions between
their in-groups and out-groups. Finally, in hori-
zontal collectivist (HC) cultural contexts (e.g.,
exemplified historically by the Israeli Kibbutz),
people see themselves as similar to others, and
emphasize shared goals and sociability, but
instead of submitting to authority, their view
of power focuses on benevolence and helping
others (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009).
However, the modal comparisons in consumer
research are between the United States (VI)
and any of a number of Pacific Rim coun-
tries (VC). This means that much of what
is known about consumer behavior in indi-
vidualistic and collectivistic societies reflects
vertical forms of these syndromes and may not
generalize, for example, comparisons between
Sweden (HI) and Israel (HC) or other sets of
horizontal cultures. As an example, conformity
in product choice, as examined by Kim and
Markus (1999), may be a tendency specific to VC
cultures, in which deference to authority and to
in-group wishes is stressed. Much lower levels
of conformity may be observed in HC cultures,
which emphasize sociability but not deference
(Triandis and Gelfand, 1998). Thus, differences
in consumers’ conformity between Korea (VC)
and the United States (VI) may not charac-
terize broad individualism-collectivism differ-
ences, because levels of product conformity in
HC contexts might not exceed those in HI
contexts.
Indeed, several recent studies of this
horizontal/vertical cultural distinction have
provided evidence for its value as a predictor
of new consumer psychology phenomena and as
a basis for refining the understanding of known
phenomena (Shavitt et al., 2006). For instance,
Lalwani, Shavitt, and Johnson (2006) showed
that differences in the self-presentational
responses observed for individualists and
collectivists are mediated at the individual level
by the horizontal but not the vertical versions
of these cultural orientations. This suggests
that culturally linked self-presentational
efforts reflect distinct goals of being seen as
self-reliant and capable (valued in HI contexts)
versus sociable and benevolent (valued in HC
contexts).
Further evidence for the value of the
horizontal-vertical distinction comes from a
study of country-of-origin effects. Gürhan-Canli
and Maheswaran (2000) demonstrated that
the tendency to favor products from one’s
own country over foreign products emerged
more strongly in Japan (a VC culture) than in
the United States (a VI culture). Mediation
analyses using individual consumers’ self-rated
cultural values further indicated that only the
vertical aspect of individualism and collectivism
accounted for the country-of-origin effects
in Japan. In other words, the collectivistic
tendency to favor one’s own country’s products
appeared to be driven by cultural values that
stress hierarchy, competition, and deference
to in-group wishes, not by values that stress
interdependence more generally.
In line with this, as noted earlier, research
suggests that mental representations of power in
terms of status and competition versus benevo-
lence differ reliably between vertical and
horizontal cultural backgrounds and orien-
tations. These differences impact consumer
information processing and the interpretation of
power-related stimuli (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli,
2009). Finally, content analyses of magazine
advertisements in several countries suggested
that status-oriented themes of hierarchy, luxury,
prestige, and distinction were generally more
prevalent in societies presumed to have vertical
cultural profiles (e.g., Korea, Russia) than a
horizontal cultural profile (Denmark) (Shavitt
et al., 2006).
Culture and thinking styles. East Asian and
North American cultural differences have
been well documented in social psychological
research, especially in terms of the differ-
ences in individualistic-collectivistic values
and independent-interdependent self-systems
(Markus and Kitayama, 1991). Many of these
cross-cultural studies of consumer behavior
have provided evidence that advertising (such
4 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior
as magazine ads, Internet advertising, and
TV commercials) from Western cultures is in
general more individualistic and less collec-
tivistic than advertising from Asian cultures
(Morling and Lamoreaux, 2008), and that
consumers from Western cultures are more
likely to be persuaded by individualistic ads
and those from East Asian cultures are more
likely to be persuaded by collectivistic ads
(Han and Shavitt, 1994). However, relatively
little research has been done on cross-cultural
differences in consumers’ thinking orientations.
The following section provides a general review
of cultural differences in thinking styles in
addition to the findings of relevant studies of
consumer behavior and advertising effects.
Analytic versus holistic thinking. Broadly
speaking, Westerners tend to adopt an analytic
thinking style that emphasizes the indep-
endence of individual objects, whereas East
Asians tend to adopt a holistic view emphasizing
that the world is composed of interrelated
elements (Nisbett et al., 2001). The analytic
style of Westerners and the holistic style of
East Asians have been demonstrated in various
cognitive domains such as attention, causal
reasoning, perception of change, tolerance of
contradiction, and categorization.
The analytic style of attention is field indepen-
dent (mainly oriented toward an object itself),
whereas holistic attention is field dependent
(focused on the relationship between objects
and/or the field in which they are embedded)
(Nisbett et al., 2001). This difference in the
orientation of attention is also seen in the way
East Asians and Westerners perceive and explain
social events. East Asians tend to assume that
each element in the world is somehow inter-
twined, and thus an event or object can be
understood only in the context of the whole
set of relevant factors. By contrast, Westerners
tend to explain a certain event in terms of direct
causal links, thereby considering fewer reasons
than East Asians, who tend to consider a broader
set of reasons, regardless of their relevance to the
event (Choi et al., 2003).
Furthermore, in explaining causality of a
social event, analytic thinkers tend to focus on
the internal dispositions of an actor, whereas
holistic thinkers tend to consider a broader set of
reasons (including both dispositional and contex-
tual information) and are therefore less likely to
attribute an outcome to an actor’s internal char-
acteristics (Nisbett et al., 2001). This has impli-
cations for brand judgments, as well. Monga
and John (2007) found that negative publicity
influences analytic (vs. holistic) thinkers more
heavily, and thus changes their beliefs about
a brand to a greater degree because analytic
thinkers are less likely to consider contextual
information, and thus are more likely to attribute
negative product information to the brand.
From the analytic perspective, objects exist
independently, and thus the essence of the
objects is stable over time. This assumption
promotes a linear perception of change in which
no drastic deviation is expected in the pattern
of stability or change of a phenomenon (Nisbett,
2003). By contrast, the holistic view of the
world assumes that objects are interrelated, and
therefore it is less likely that a phenomenon
will remain stable over time. This perspective
results in a cyclic perception in which people
tend to predict fluctuating trends for an event.
For example, in predicting future stock-market
trends and making investment decisions, Cana-
dians are more likely to make judgments based
on recent trends than are Chinese people; thus,
when compared to the Chinese, Canadians are
more willing to buy stocks when they are in an
increasing trend and less willing to buy when
stock prices are decreasing (Ji, Zhang, and Guo,
2008).
The cyclic perception of change and expecta-
tion of instability prevalent among East Asians
renders a Yin-Yang belief that a characteristic
of an object can potentially transform into its
opposite. Consequently, East Asians tend to
hold a dialectical perception in which apparently
opposing concepts can simultaneously be true
and can peacefully coexist (Nisbett et al., 2001).
When confronted with opposing propositions,
East Asians tend to resolve contradictions by
choosing a middle ground, whereas Westerners
to rely on formal logic in resolving contradictions
by choosing one of the opposing propositions.
For example, US consumers tend to resolve
incongruities with an attenuation strategy in
which one piece of information is favored over
another inconsistent piece of information. In
contrast, Hong Kong Chinese consumers tend
cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 5
to follow an additive strategy in which both
pieces of information are combined to influence
judgments (Aaker and Sengupta, 2000).
East Asians and Westerners also perceive
conflicting emotions in different ways. For
example, Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi (1999) showed
that Chinese tend to hold a dialectical perception
that pleasant and unpleasant emotions can be
experienced at the same time. Thus, their
frequency judgment for pleasant emotions is
positively correlated with their frequency judg-
ment for unpleasant emotions. By contrast, this
study found that for Americans the perceived
frequency of pleasant emotions is inversely
correlated with the perceived frequency of
unpleasant emotions. Schimmack, Oishi, and
Diener (2002) analyzed 38 nationalities and
demonstrated that this cultural difference
results from dialectical thinking, not from a
difference in individualistic-collectivistic values.
Moreover, Williams and Aaker (2002) demon-
strated that opposing emotions (e.g., both
happiness and sadness) in persuasion appeals
elicit more positive attitudes among Asian
Americans than among European Americans.
Westerners pay more attention to individual
objects and attribute causality to them, whereas
East Asians focus more on the field. Westerners
are more accustomed to formulating rules that
govern internal properties of objects and tend
to categorize things by applying those rules. In
contrast, East Asians organize objects on the
basis of their relationship to other objects or
to the field (Nisbett, 2003), and therefore they
tend to categorize objects according to their
overall similarities. Thus, when presented with
pictures of a panda, a monkey, and a banana, East
Asians tend to categorize the monkey and banana
together based on the relationship between the
two, whereas Westerners tend to categorize the
panda and monkey into one group based on
the traits that characterize them (Ji, Zhang, and
Nisbett, 2004).
Cultural differences in the way people cate-
gorize objects (rule/trait-based vs. similarity/
relationship-based) also appear in the way
they organize and store brand information.
For example, Ng and Houston (2006) showed
that Americans are less likely to retrieve
brand exemplars (i.e., specific products or
subcategories) than brand beliefs (i.e., general
descriptive or evaluative thoughts), whereas the
reverse was the case for Singaporeans. These
results emerged from an analytic tendency to
focus on ‘‘global beliefs’’ abstracted from prior
product experiences and a holistic tendency
to focus on contextual and incidental details
about the product. Similarly, Monga and John
(2008) found that, compared to Americans,
Indians tend to perceive a higher degree of fit
between a parent brand (e.g., Kodak) and its
brand extension (e.g., Kodak filing cabinet,
Kodak greeting cards), and to evaluate the
brand extension more positively. This result
reflects Indians’ holistic tendency to base their
judgments more heavily on the relationships
between brand extensions and parent brands
than do their American counterparts.
A variety of methods and techniques have
been developed to measure cultural differences
in thinking styles (Choi, Koo, and Choi, 2007;
Ji, Zhang, and Nisbett, 2004; Monga and
John, 2007), including responses to cognitive
tasks, scenarios and questions, physiological
measures, a scale, and analyses of various
cultural products. Furthermore, priming an
independent versus interdependent view of self
has also been found to promote analytic and
holistic modes of thinking, respectively. For
example, people primed with an independent
self-view were more likely to focus on a focal
object and thus were better at finding an
embedded figure by separating the figure from
its background than were those primed with an
interdependent self-view (see Oyserman and
Lee, 2007, for a review).
Additional dimensions. Numerous other
cultural distinctions deserve further attention in
consumer research. A focus upon these relatively
under-researched constructs as antecedents
may allow for broadening the range of cultural
differences beyond those currently investigated.
For instance, Schwartz’s (1992) circumplex
structure of values, which is highly robust
cross-nationally, parallels the HI, VI, HC, VC
typology and offers a particularly detailed and
comprehensive basis for classification. In his
large-scale studies of work values, Hofstede
(1980) derived three other dimensions of
cultural variation in addition to individualism:
power distance (acceptance of power inequality in
6 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior
organizations, a construct conceptually relevant
to the vertical/horizontal distinction), uncer-
tainty avoidance (the degree of tolerance for
ambiguity or uncertainty about the future), and
masculinity/femininity (preference for achieve-
ment and assertiveness versus modesty and
nurturing relationships). Indeed, individualism
was the second dimension identified by Hofstede
(1980), whereas power distance emerged as
the first dimension. A few marketing-oriented
studies have employed Hofstede’s nation-level
classifications (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009),
but more potential remains for identifying
consequences for consumer judgments and
behaviors. For instance, uncertainty avoidance
has been conceptualized as a syndrome related to
anxiety, rule orientation, need for security, and
deference to experts (Hofstede, 1980). As such,
one might speculate that the level of uncertainty
avoidance in a culture will predict the tendency
for advertisements to use fear appeals or appeals
to safety and security, and the tendency for
advertisements to employ expert spokespersons.
Differences along this cultural dimension may
also predict patterns in the diffusion of product
innovations, particularly innovations whose
purchase entails a degree of risk.
CULTURE AND PERSUASIVE APPEALS
Most research on cultural influences on judg-
ment and persuasion has examined the
implications of individualism/collectivism or
independent/interdependent self-construals. In
general, the findings suggest that the prevalence
or the persuasiveness of a given type of appeal
matches the cultural value orientation of the
society (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). For
instance, appeals to individuality, personal
benefits, and achievement are usually more
prevalent and persuasive in individualistic
compared to collectivistic cultures, whereas
appeals to group benefits, harmony, and confor-
mity are usually more prevalent and persuasive
in collectivistic compared to individualistic
cultures. Such evidence for ‘‘cultural matching’’
in the nature of appeals has been followed by
studies examining the distinct psychological
processes driving persuasion across cultures.
These studies suggest that culture can affect how
people process and interpret product-related
information. It can determine the type of
information that is weighed more heavily for
making judgments (e.g., product attributes vs
other consumers’ opinions). However, brand
and product characteristics can constrain
the role of cultural variables in information
processing and persuasion, with some brands
and products serving as stronger carriers of
cultural values (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong,
2009).
Cultural differences in the content of message
appeals. Cross-cultural content analyses of
advertisements can yield valuable evidence
about distinctions in cultural values. For
instance, American advertisers and consumer
researchers often assume that consumer learning
about the brand precedes other marketing
effects, such as liking and buying the brand.
Thus, advertisements that attempt to teach
the consumer about the brand are typical in
the United States, although other types of
advertisements are also used.
In contrast, as Miracle (1987) suggested, the
typical goal of advertisements in Japan appears
very different. There, advertisements tend to
focus on ‘‘making friends’’ with the audience
and showing that the company understands their
feelings. The assumption is that consumers will
buy once they feel familiar with and have a
sense of trust in the company. Because Japan,
Korea, and other Pacific Rim countries are
collectivist cultures that tend toward implicit
and indirect communication practices (Triandis,
1995), Miracle suggested that the mood and
tone of commercials in these countries will
be particularly important in establishing good
feelings about the advertiser. Several studies
have supported these notions, showing that
advertisements in Japan and Korea, compared
to those in the United States, rely more on
symbolism, mood, and aesthetics and less on
direct approaches such as brand comparisons
(Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). The ads may
be equally informative about the brand across
cultures. It is the type of appeal that will vary.
For instance, a content analysis of magazine
advertisements revealed that in Korea, compared
to the United States, advertisements are more
focused on family well-being, interdependence,
group goals, and harmony, whereas they are
cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 7
less focused on self-improvement, ambition,
personal goals, independence, and individuality
(Han and Shavitt, 1994). However, as one might
expect, the nature of the advertised product
moderated these effects. Cultural differences
emerged strongly only for products that tend
to be purchased and used along with other
persons (e.g., groceries, cars). Products that
do not tend to be shared (e.g., health and
beauty aids, clothing) are promoted more in
terms of personal, individualistic benefits in both
countries.
Paralleling the overall cross-national differ-
ences, a content analysis by Kim and Markus
(1999) indicated that Korean advertisements,
compared to US advertisements, were character-
ized by more conformity themes (e.g., respect for
collective values and beliefs) and fewer unique-
ness themes (e.g., rebelling against collective
values and beliefs). Website content in Eastern
and Western countries also appears to differ
in the emphasis on individual versus collective
activities (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009).
Finally, it is important to note that, in
countries experiencing rapid economic growth,
advertising content does not necessarily reflect
existing cultural values, instead promoting new,
aspirational values such as individuality and
modernity. For instance, in China, in recent
years, westernized ad appeals are increasingly
common. Appeals to youth/modernity, indi-
viduality/independence, and technology are
especially salient in Chinese advertisements
that target the younger generation (Zhang and
Shavitt, 2003). Similarly, during a period of
rapid transition in South Korea’s economy
(1968–1998), content analysis of advertisements
revealed substantial shifts toward individu-
alistic, modernity-oriented appeals (Han and
Shavitt, 2005).
Cultural differences in judgment and persuasion.
Research suggests that the persuasiveness of
appeals may mirror the cultural differences in
their prevalence. An experiment by Han and
Shavitt (1994) showed that appeals to individu-
alistic values (e.g., ‘‘Solo cleans with a softness
that you will love’’) were more persuasive in the
United States and appeals to collectivistic values
(e.g., ‘‘Solo cleans with a softness that your
family will love’’) were more persuasive in Korea.
Again, however, this effect was much more
evident for products that are shared (laundry
detergent, clothes iron) than for those that are
not (chewing gum, running shoes).
Zhang and Gelb (1996) found a similar
pattern in the persuasiveness of individualistic
versus collectivistic appeals in an experiment
conducted in the United States and China.
Moreover, this effect appeared to be moder-
ated by whether the advertised product is
socially visible (camera) versus privately used
(toothbrush). Finally, Wang and Mowen
(1997) showed in a US sample that individual
differences in separateness/connectedness self-
schema (i.e., the degree to which one views
the self as independent of or interconnected
with important others) predicts attitudes toward
individualistic versus collectivistic ad appeals
for a credit card. Thus, cultural orientation
and national culture have implications for the
effectiveness of appeals. However, such cultural
differences would only be anticipated for those
products or uses that are relevant to both
personal and group goals.
Cultural differences in persuasion are also
revealed in the diagnosticity of certain types
of information. For instance, Aaker and
Maheswaran (1997) showed that consensus
information regarding other consumers’
opinions is not treated as a heuristic cue by
Hong Kong Chinese (as it is in the United
States) but is instead perceived and processed
as diagnostic information. Thus, collectivists
resolve incongruity in favor of consensus
information, not brand attributes. This would
be expected in a culture that stresses conformity
and responsiveness to others’ views. On the
other hand, cues whose (low) diagnosticity is not
expected to vary cross-culturally (e.g., number
of attributes presented) elicit similar heuristic
processing in the United States and Hong Kong.
Finally, because cognitive associations with
power vary with horizontal and vertical
cultural orientations and with ethnicity, as
noted earlier, Torelli et al., found differences
in the interpretive processes and mindsets
triggered when power is salient. Specifically,
people whose cultural orientation predisposes a
status-oriented view of power activate cognitive
processes that facilitate defending their power,
such as reasserting control by confirming
8 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior
prior stereotypes about a brand. In contrast,
people whose cultural orientation predisposes
a benevolence-oriented view of power activate
cognitive processes that facilitate helping others,
such as by forming accurate, careful impressions
of brands (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong, 2009).
CONCLUSIONS
As marketing efforts are increasingly global-
ized, understanding cross-cultural consumer
behavior has become a key focus of consumer
research. In recent years, research in consumer
behavior has addressed a broadening set of
cross-cultural issues and dimensions. Research
has provided an enhanced understanding of the
relations between culture and self-construal,
motivation, thinking style, and consumer
persuasion. Research has also begun to address
the psychological mechanisms underlying cross-
cultural differences in consumer judgments,
and the products and contexts for which these
differences are most likely to be observed.
Understanding cultural differences has become
crucial for effective marketing and advertising.
In future research, it will be important to
further distinguish cultural similarities and
differences in consumer judgments, identify
within-culture or subgroup differences that
parallel between-culture differences, and
explore their rich implications in consumer
behavior.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Preparation of this article was supported
by Grant #1R01HD053636-01A1from the
National Institutes of Health, Grant #0648539
from the National Science Foundation, and
Grant #63842 from the Robert Wood Johnson
Foundation to Sharon Shavitt.
Bibliography
Aaker, J.L. and Maheswaran, D. (1997) The effect
of cultural orientation on persuasion. Journal of
Consumer Research, 24 (3), 315–328.
Aaker, J.L. and Sengupta, J. (2000) Addivity versus
attenuation: the role of culture in the resolution
of information incongruity. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 9 (2), 67–82.
Bagozzi, R.P., Wong, N., and Yi, Y. (1999) The role
of culture and gender in the relationship between
positive and negative affect. Cognition and Emotion,
13 (6), 641–672.
Choi, I., Dalal, R., Kim-Prieto, C., and Park, H. (2003)
Culture and judgement of causal relevance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (1), 46–59.
Choi, I., Koo, M., and Choi, J. (2007) Measuring the
analytic vs. the holistic thinking style. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 33 (5), 691–705.
Gürhan-Canli, Z. and Maheswaran, D. (2000) Cultural
variations in country of origin effects. Journal of
Marketing Research, 37 (3), 309–317.
Han, S.-P. and Shavitt, S. (1994) Persuasion and culture:
advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic
societies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30
(4), 326.
Han, S. and Shavitt, S. (2005) Westernization of cultural
values in Korean advertising: a longitudinal content
analysis of magazine ads from 1968-1998, in Advances
in consumer research, Vol. 32 (eds G. Menon and A.R.
Rao), Association for Consumer Research, Provo,
UT, pp. 249–250.
Hofstede, G.H. (1980) Culture’s Consequences: Inter-
national Differences in Work-Related Values, Sage,
Newbury, Park.
Ji, L.J., Zhang, Z., and Guo, T. (2008) To buy or to sell:
cultural differences in stock market decisions based
on stock price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision
Making, 21 (4), 399–413.
Ji, L.J., Zhang, Z., and Nisbett, R.E. (2004) Is it Culture,
or is it language? Examination of language effects in
cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 87 (1), 57–65.
Kim, H.S. and Markus, H.R. (1999) Deviance or unique-
ness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (4),
785–800.
Lalwani, A.K. and Shavitt, S. (2009) The ‘‘me’’ I claim to
be: cultural self-construal elicits self-presentational
goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 97 (1), 88–102.
Lalwani, A., Shavitt, S., and Johnson, T.P. (2006) What is
the relation between cultural orientation and socially
desirable responding? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 90 (1), 165–178.
Lau-Gesk, L.G. (2003) Activating culture through
persuasion appeals: an examination of the bicultural
consumer. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3),
301–315.
Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (1991) Culture and the
self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motiva-
tion. Psychological Review, 98 (2), 224–253.
cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 9
Miracle, G.E. (1987) Feel-Do-Learn: an alternative
sequence underlying Japanese consumer response to
television commercials, in Proceedings of the L.A.
Conference of the American Academy of Advertising (ed.
F.G. Feasley), The University of South Carolina,
Columbia.
Monga, A.B. and John, D.R. (2007) Cultural differences
in brand extension evaluation: the influence of analytic
versus holistic thinking. Journal of Consumer Research,
33 (4), 529–536.
Monga, A.B. and John, D.R. (2008) When does negative
brand publicity hurt? The moderating influence of
analytic versus holistic thinking. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 18 (4), 320–332.
Morling, B. and Lamoreaux, M. (2008) Measuring culture
outside the head: a meta-analysis of cultural prod-
ucts. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12 (3),
199–221.
Nelson, M.R. and Shavitt, S. (2002) Horizontal and
vertical individualism and achievement values: a
multi-method examination of Denmark and the U.S.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (5), 439–458.
Ng, S. and Houston, M.J. (2006) Exemplars or beliefs?
The impact of self-view on the nature and relative
influence of brand associations. Journal of Consumer
Research, 32 (4), 519–529.
Nisbett, R.E. (2003) The Geography of Thought: How
Asians and Westerners think Differently..., and Why,
Free Press, New York.
Nisbett, R.E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan,
A. (2001) Culture and systems of thought: holistic
versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108
(2), 291–310.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H.M., and Kemmelmeier,
M. (2002) Rethinking individualism and collec-
tivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and
meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128 (1), 3–72.
Oyserman, D. and Lee, S.W.-S. (2007) Priming ‘culture’:
culture as situated cognition, in Handbook of Cultural
Psychology (eds S. Kitayama and D. Cohen), Guilford
Press, New York, pp. 255–282.
Schwartz, S.H. (1992) Universals in the content and
structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical
tests in 20 countries, in Advances in experimental social
psychology, Vol. 25 (ed. M.P. Zanna), Academic Press,
San Diego, pp. 1–65.
Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A.K., Zhang, J., and Torelli,
C.J. (2006) The horizontal/vertical distinction in
cross-cultural consumer research. Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 16 (4), 325–356.
Shavitt, S., Lee, A., and Torelli, C. (2009) New directions
in cross-cultural consumer psychology, in The Social
Psychology of Consumer Behavior, a volume in the
series, Frontiers of Social Psychology (eds M. Wänke,
A.W. Kruglanski, and J.P. Forgas) Series Editors,
Psychology Press, New York, pp. 227–250.
Shavitt, S., Torelli, C., and Wong, J. (2009), Identity-
based motivation in a consumer context. Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 19 (3), pp. 261–266.
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., and Diener, E. (2002) Cultural
influences on the relation between pleasant emotions
and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies
or individualism-collectivism? Cognition and Emotion,
16 (6), 705–719.
Triandis, H.C. (1995) Individualism and collectivism,
Westview Press, Boulder.
Triandis, H.C. and Gelfand, M.J. (1998) Converging
measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism
and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74 (1), 118–128.
Wang, C.L. and Mowen, J.C. (1997) The separate-
ness-connectedness self-schema: scale development
and application to message construction. Psychology
and Marketing, 14 (2), 185–207.
Williams, P. and Aaker, J. (2002) Can mixed emotions
peacefully co-exist? Journal of Consumer Research,
28 (4), 636–649.
Wyer, R.S., Chiu, C.-Y., and Hong, Y.-Y. (eds) (2009).
Understanding Culture: Theory, Research and Applica-
tion, Psychology Press, New York.
Zhang, Y. and Gelb, B.D. (1996) Matching advertising
appeals to culture: the influence of products’ use
conditions. Journal of Advertising, 25 (3), 29–46.
Zhang, J. and Shavitt, S. (2003) Cultural values in adver-
tisements to the Chinese X-generation: promoting
modernity and individualism. Journal of Advertising,
32 (1), 23–33.

More Related Content

Similar to marketing aspects of cultural distanceCarlos M. P. Sousa.docx

CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT ON GLOBAL MARKE...
CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT  ON GLOBAL MARKE...CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT  ON GLOBAL MARKE...
CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT ON GLOBAL MARKE...Maxie Tran
 
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docx
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docxProblems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docx
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docxbriancrawford30935
 
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteering
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteeringMertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteering
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteeringMeaghan Mertens
 
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...LeapFrog Strategy
 
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docxransayo
 
Business relationships cross cultural analysis
Business relationships  cross cultural analysisBusiness relationships  cross cultural analysis
Business relationships cross cultural analysisshibrah76
 
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...//disruptiVesemiOtics//
 
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docx
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docxUnderstanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docx
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docxmarilucorr
 
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the internationalisation of SMEs_G ...
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the    internationalisation of SMEs_G ...The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the    internationalisation of SMEs_G ...
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the internationalisation of SMEs_G ...Gonzalo Shoobridge
 
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behavior
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behaviorArticle an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behavior
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behaviorgohar Iqbal
 
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and society
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and societyMini paper 1-tynes-culture and society
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and societyDanika Tynes, Ph.D.
 
Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx
 Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx
Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docxaryan532920
 
60-69
60-6960-69
60-69Jen W
 
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docx
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docxSubcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docx
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docxpicklesvalery
 
Essay On Population.pdf
Essay On Population.pdfEssay On Population.pdf
Essay On Population.pdfChristine Love
 
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and cultureThe production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culturedivyaparmar44
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxjesssueann
 
English assingment 2 semster
English assingment 2 semsterEnglish assingment 2 semster
English assingment 2 semsterkitty MaNo
 
Culture, Coronavirus and Globalisation
Culture, Coronavirus and GlobalisationCulture, Coronavirus and Globalisation
Culture, Coronavirus and GlobalisationLorna Donaldson
 

Similar to marketing aspects of cultural distanceCarlos M. P. Sousa.docx (20)

CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT ON GLOBAL MARKE...
CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT  ON GLOBAL MARKE...CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT  ON GLOBAL MARKE...
CULTURAL DISTANCE: HOW IS IT MEASURED AND HOW DOES IT IMPACT ON GLOBAL MARKE...
 
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docx
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docxProblems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docx
Problems and Perspectives in Management, Volume 12, Issue 1, 2.docx
 
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteering
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteeringMertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteering
MertensCultureandtheBusinessofGivingandVolunteering
 
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...
Insights cultural diversity and revolutionary change semiotics in emerging ma...
 
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
@charset utf-8;@font-face {font-family Champagne;.docx
 
Business relationships cross cultural analysis
Business relationships  cross cultural analysisBusiness relationships  cross cultural analysis
Business relationships cross cultural analysis
 
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...
George rossolatos seminar on branding, brand equity, brand semiotic models an...
 
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docx
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docxUnderstanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docx
Understanding the ConsumerCulture and Cultural Change1.docx
 
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the internationalisation of SMEs_G ...
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the    internationalisation of SMEs_G ...The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the    internationalisation of SMEs_G ...
The role of multi-ethnic workforces in the internationalisation of SMEs_G ...
 
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behavior
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behaviorArticle an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behavior
Article an integrative framework for cross cultural consumer behavior
 
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and society
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and societyMini paper 1-tynes-culture and society
Mini paper 1-tynes-culture and society
 
Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx
 Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx
Atravesando FronterasBorder Crossings A Critical Ethnog.docx
 
60-69
60-6960-69
60-69
 
Final report on Ethnocentrism
Final report on EthnocentrismFinal report on Ethnocentrism
Final report on Ethnocentrism
 
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docx
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docxSubcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docx
Subcultures of Consumption AnEthnography of the New Bikers.docx
 
Essay On Population.pdf
Essay On Population.pdfEssay On Population.pdf
Essay On Population.pdf
 
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and cultureThe production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
 
English assingment 2 semster
English assingment 2 semsterEnglish assingment 2 semster
English assingment 2 semster
 
Culture, Coronavirus and Globalisation
Culture, Coronavirus and GlobalisationCulture, Coronavirus and Globalisation
Culture, Coronavirus and Globalisation
 

More from alfredacavx97

Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docx
Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docxContinually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docx
Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docxalfredacavx97
 
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docx
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docxContext There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docx
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docxalfredacavx97
 
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docx
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docxContinue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docx
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docxalfredacavx97
 
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docx
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docxContinue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docx
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docxalfredacavx97
 
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docx
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docxCONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docx
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docxalfredacavx97
 
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docx
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docxMedia and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docx
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docxalfredacavx97
 
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docx
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docxCoping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docx
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docxalfredacavx97
 
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docx
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docxMEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docx
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docxalfredacavx97
 
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al. .docx
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al.              .docxMedeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al.              .docx
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al. .docxalfredacavx97
 
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docx
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docxMeasuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docx
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docxalfredacavx97
 
Meaningfulness vs. S.docx
Meaningfulness vs. S.docxMeaningfulness vs. S.docx
Meaningfulness vs. S.docxalfredacavx97
 
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score 20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docx
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score  20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docxContributing to the Team’s Work Score  20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docx
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score 20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docxalfredacavx97
 
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docx
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docxMeasuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docx
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docxalfredacavx97
 
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docx
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docxControversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docx
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docxalfredacavx97
 
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docx
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docxControl measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docx
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docxalfredacavx97
 
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docx
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docxContrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docx
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docxalfredacavx97
 
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docx
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docxMeasure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docx
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docxalfredacavx97
 
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docx
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docxMeasures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docx
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docxalfredacavx97
 
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docx
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docxMDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docx
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docxalfredacavx97
 
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English Table Of Contents.docx
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English  Table Of Contents.docxMDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English  Table Of Contents.docx
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English Table Of Contents.docxalfredacavx97
 

More from alfredacavx97 (20)

Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docx
Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docxContinually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docx
Continually in our changing society we are learning how to interact .docx
 
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docx
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docxContext There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docx
Context There are four main categories of computer crimeComput.docx
 
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docx
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docxContinue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docx
Continue to use the case study  (A&D High Tech) and Risk Management .docx
 
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docx
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docxContinue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docx
Continue to use the case study, evaluate, and assess risk. Use quali.docx
 
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docx
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docxCONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docx
CONTEXT ASSIGNMENT # 6For this assignment, we are going to take .docx
 
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docx
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docxMedia and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docx
Media and SocietyMedia HistoryJOHN DEWEY – 185.docx
 
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docx
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docxCoping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docx
Coping with Terrorism  Is the United States making progress in re.docx
 
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docx
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docxMEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docx
MEDIA AND DIVERSITY IN CULTURECOM-530 MEDIA AND DIVE.docx
 
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al. .docx
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al.              .docxMedeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al.              .docx
Medeiros LNB de, Silva DR da, Guedes CDFS et al. .docx
 
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docx
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docxMeasuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docx
Measuring to Improve Medication Reconciliationin a Large Sub.docx
 
Meaningfulness vs. S.docx
Meaningfulness vs. S.docxMeaningfulness vs. S.docx
Meaningfulness vs. S.docx
 
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score 20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docx
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score  20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docxContributing to the Team’s Work Score  20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docx
Contributing to the Team’s Work Score 20 pts.20 - 25 pts..docx
 
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docx
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docxMeasuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docx
Measuring Performance at Intuit A Value-Added Component in ERM Pr.docx
 
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docx
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docxControversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docx
Controversial Issue in Microbiology Assignment Use of antibacte.docx
 
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docx
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docxControl measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docx
Control measures for noncommunicable disease may start with basic sc.docx
 
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docx
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docxContrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docx
Contrasting Africa and Europes economic development.Why did Europ.docx
 
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docx
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docxMeasure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docx
Measure the dependence of the resistance in the spinel Lu2V2O7 on .docx
 
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docx
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docxMeasures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docx
Measures of Similaritv and Dissimilaritv 65the comparison .docx
 
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docx
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docxMDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docx
MDS 4100 Communication Law Case Study Privacy CASE .docx
 
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English Table Of Contents.docx
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English  Table Of Contents.docxMDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English  Table Of Contents.docx
MDDtoDOC - GSS2018 Ballot 1 - English Table Of Contents.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxUnboundStockton
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 

marketing aspects of cultural distanceCarlos M. P. Sousa.docx

  • 1. marketing aspects of cultural distance Carlos M. P. Sousa There is a general consensus in the literature that when firms decide to enter foreign markets, they must adjust to a different cultural environment and be prepared for challenges, such as differences in language, lifestyles, cultural standards, consumer preferences, and purchasing power, among others (see GLOBAL MARKETING STRATEGY; CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR; STANDARDIZATION/ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL MARKETING STRA- TEGY; BASE OF THE PYRAMID MARKETS: CULTURE INSIGHTS AND MARKETING IMPLICATIONS). To assess these differences between countries, a new body of literature has emerged around the concept of cultural distance, which has been used to measure the scope and scale of these differences. The assumption is that cultural differences between the home and the foreign markets create a distance, which, in turn, influences the activity of the firm in the international arena. As a result, few concepts in the international-marketing literature have gained broader attention than ‘‘cultural distance.’’ This concept has been identified as a key variable in explaining the behavior of firms in a vast array of areas including entry-mode choice, foreign-market selection, level of control
  • 2. (see MARKET ENTRY AND EXPANSION), international-marketing strategies (see GLOBAL MARKETING STRATEGY: PERSPECTIVES AND APPROACHES), and performance (see EXPORT PERFORMANCE). Mixed empirical results, however, have been found in the literature regarding the importance of this construct. Whereas some studies have found cultural distance to significantly influence the activity of the firm in the international arena (Barkema and Vermeulen, 1997), other studies have found no significant or clear relationship (Mitra and Golder, 2002). Various reasons have been proposed to explain these inconclusive results. On the one hand, some scholars have argued that the preferences and tastes of consumers in different countries are converging to a global norm (Levitt, 1983), and hence the effect of cultural distance is likely to dilute progressively. The notion that national cultures are converging appears to be plau- sible considering the emergence of the Internet, greater ease of information flow, and more frequent/easier international travel. However, this argument is disputed by Barkema and Vermeulen (1997), who use data spanning almost three decades (1966–1994) in their study, and find that the effect of cultural distance does not decrease over time, that is, that cultural values and distances remain stable. This may be due to the fact that such changes concern conver- gences in superficial appearances of culture (i.e., symbols, heroes, and rituals) and that they do not necessarily signal a convergence in the
  • 3. values embedded in national cultures (Hofstede, 2006). The inconsistencies could also be attributed to the difficulty that exists in the conceptualization and operationalization of the cultural-distance construct. For instance, as Sousa and Bradley (2006, 2008) note, many authors have used cultural distance and psychic distance (see MARKETING ASPECTS OF PSYCHIC DIST- ANCE) interchangeably with no clear distinction between them, a contamination that has been disputed by these authors and which may undermine the validity of the research (see RESEARCH RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY). Cultural distance has been defined as the degree to which cultural values in one country are different from those in another country (Sousa and Bradley, 2006). Accordingly, it should be used to assess differences in national culture. Several frameworks have been advanced and used in the measurement of the extent to which different cultures are similar or different, such as that of Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck (1961) and Ronen and Shenkar (1985). However, the most widely used framework is that of Hofstede’s (1980) cultural dimensions. To arrive at a measure of cultural distance, Kogut and Singh (1988) were the first to combine Hofstede’s dimensions into one aggregate measure of cultural distance among countries, and many studies have subsequently used this formula or an adapted version, as a measure of cultural distance. However, while a tangible and convenient tool, the Kogut and Singh index has been liable to the same
  • 4. criticism as that leveled against Hofstede’s value Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing, edited by Jagdish N. Sheth and Naresh K. Malhotra. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons Ltd ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line
  • 5. ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line ye Line 2 marketing aspects of cultural distance dimensions, for example, nonexhaustiveness, outdated data, assumptions about the linearity, additivity, and normal distributions of scores (Shenkar, 2001). As a result, there has been an increasing call in the literature to develop new measures (see VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY) to operationalize cultural distance as it may be unrealistic to expect that a single measure
  • 6. can fully discern the underlying differences across countries (Tihanyi, Griffith, and Russell, 2005). Some recent studies may offer some interesting alternatives to measure cultural distance (Schwartz, 1994) and should, therefore, be explored in future studies. Bibliography Barkema, H.G. and Vermeulen, F. (1997) What differ- ences in the cultural backgrounds of partners are detrimental for international joint ventures. Journal of International Business Studies, 28 (4), 845–864. Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-related Values, Sage Publications, Beverly Hills. Hofstede, G. (2006) What did GLOBE really measure? Researchers’ minds versus respondents’ minds. Journal of International Business Studies, 37 (6), 882–896. Kluckhohn, F.R. and Strodtbeck, F.L. (1961) Variations in Value Orientations, Row, Peterson, Evanston. Kogut, B. and Singh, H. (1988) The effect of national culture on the choice of entry mode. Journal of Inter- national Business Studies, 19 (3), 411–432. Levitt, T. (1983) The globalization of markets. Harvard Business Review, 61 (3), 92–102. Mitra, D. and Golder, P.N. (2002) Whose culture matters? Near-market knowledge and its impact on foreign market entry timing. Journal of Marketing Research,
  • 7. 39 (3), 350–365. Ronen, S. and Shenkar, O. (1985) Clustering countries on attitudinal dimensions: a review and synthesis. Academy of Management Review, 10 (3), 435–454. Schwartz S.H. (1994) in Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method and Applications (eds U. Kim H.C. Triandis C. Kagitçibasi et al.), Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, pp. 85–119. Shenkar, O. (2001) Cultural distance revisited: towards a more rigorous conceptualization and measurement of cultural differences. Journal of International Business Studies, 32 (3), 519–535. Sousa, C.M.P. and Bradley, F. (2006) Cultural distance and psychic distance: two peas in a pod? Journal of International Marketing, 14 (1), 49–70. Sousa, C.M.P. and Bradley, F. (2008) Cultural distance and psychic distance: refinements in conceptualisation and measurement. Journal of Marketing Management, 24 (5/6), 467–488. Tihanyi, L., Griffith, D.A., and Russell, C.J. (2005) The effect of cultural distance on entry mode choice, international diversification, and MNE performance: a meta-analysis. Journal of International Business Studies, 36 (3), 270–283. cross-cultural psychology of consumer
  • 8. behavior Minkyung Koo and Sharon Shavitt INTRODUCTION As new global markets emerge, and existing markets become increasingly segmented along ethnic or subcultural lines, the need to market effectively to consumers who have different cultural values has never been more important. Thus, it is no surprise that in the last several years, culture has rapidly emerged as a central focus of research in consumer behavior. This development followed on the heels of extensive social psychological research on culture, which provided a strong theoretical foundation for the consumer-behavior studies that followed. What is culture? Culture consists of shared knowledge that provides the standards for perceiving, believing, evaluating, feeling, comm- unicating, and acting among those who share a language, a historical period, and a geographic location. As a psychological construct, culture can be studied in multiple ways – across nations, across ethnic groups within nations, across individuals within nations (focusing on cultural orientation), and across situations through the priming of cultural values. The dimensions of individualism versus collectivism, independence versus interdependence, and analysis versus holism have in recent years received significant research attention. This attention has resulted in a great number of studies revealing both antecedents and consequences of the cultural
  • 9. differences between East Asian and North American cultures. As discussed subsequently, regardless of how culture is studied, cultural distinctions have important implications for advertising content, persuasiveness of appeals, consumer motivation, and consumer judgment processes. Article scope and overview This article reviews major cultural constructs and theoretical impli- cations of cultural differences in consumer infor- mation processing, judgments, and choices. Our review is necessarily selective, focusing on find- ings specific to the consumer domain rather than providing a general review of cultural differences (for an excellent general review, see Wyer, Chiu, and Hong, 2009). It should also be noted that because of space limitations, this article does not cover some major topics in cross-cultural consumer behaviors such as self-regulation and risk-taking, as well as methodological issues such as response styles and biases (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009; Shavitt, Torelli, and Wong, 2009). In this article, the cultural constructs of individualism/collectivism and the indepen- dent/interdependent self-construals associated with them are given special attention because extensive research has demonstrated the implications of these distinctions for processes and outcomes relevant to consumer behavior. The most recent refinements to these constructs are briefly reviewed in an attempt to identify additional cultural variables likely to enhance the understanding of cross-cultural consumer
  • 10. behavior. We also review cultural differences in thinking styles as a major emerging cultural distinction and focus on their implications for consumer-behavior research. Finally, we close with a review of cross-cultural differences in advertising content and the persuasiveness of appeals. KEY CONSTRUCTS AND DIMENSIONS OF CULTURE Individualism versus collectivism. The cons- tructs of individualism and collectivism represent the most broadly used dimensions of cultural variability for cross-cultural comparison. In individualistic cultures, people value inde- pendence from others and subordinate the goals of their in-groups to their own personal goals. In collectivistic cultures, in contrast, individuals value interdependent relationships to others and subordinate their personal goals to those of their in-groups (Hofstede, 1980; Triandis, 1995). The key distinction involves the extent to which one defines the self in relation to others. In individualistic cultural contexts, people tend to have an independent self-construal whereby the self is defined as autonomous and unique. In collectivistic cultural contexts, by contrast, people tend to have an interdependent self-construal whereby Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing, edited by Jagdish N. Sheth and Naresh K. Malhotra. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
  • 11. 2 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior the self is seen as inextricably and fundamentally embedded within a larger social network of roles and relationships (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). National cultures that celebrate the values of independence, such as the United States, Canada, Germany, and Denmark, are typically categorized as individualistic societies in which an independent self-construal is common. In contrast, cultures that nurture the values of fulfilling one’s obligations over one’s own personal wishes, including most East Asian and Latin American countries, are categorized as collectivistic societies in which an interdepen- dent self-construal is common (Hofstede, 1980; Triandis, 1995). A large body of research in psychology has demonstrated the many implications of individualism/collectivism and independent/ interdependent self-construals for social perce- ption and social behavior (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995). These findings indicate consistently that individualists and people with an independent self-construal are oriented toward products and experiences that promote achievement and autonomy, offer personal bene- fits, and enable expression of one’s distinctive qualities. On the other hand, collectivists and people with an interdependent self-construal are oriented toward products and experiences that allow one to avoid negative outcomes, maintain harmony and strong social connections with
  • 12. others, and dutifully fulfill social roles. Although a given self-construal can be more chronically accessible in a particular culture, cultures generally provide sufficient experiences with independent and interdependent views of the self to allow either type of self-construal to be primed (Oyserman, Coon, and Kemmelmeier, 2002; Oyserman and Lee, 2007). Numerous studies have established that these activated self-views impact judgments in ways that parallel cross-national differences (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009), for instance, by activating distinct self goals (Lalwani and Shavitt, 2009). People in general, and especially bicultural people, can readily switch back and forth between independent and interdependent cultural frames in response to their contexts. For instance, Lau-Gesk (2003) found that independent (interdependent) self-construals were temporarily activated when bicultural consumers were exposed to individually focused (interpersonally focused) appeals. In sum, the distinctions between individual- istic and collectivistic societies, and independent and interdependent self-construals, are crucial to the understanding of cross-cultural differ- ences in consumer behavior. The studies to be reviewed here offer extensive evidence that these cultural classifications have fundamental impli- cations for consumption-related outcomes. Refined individualism versus collectivism. The conceptualizations of individualism and collec-
  • 13. tivism, and independence/interdependence, have historically been broad and multidimen- sional, summarizing a host of differences in focus of attention, self-definitions, motiva- tions, emotional connections to in-groups, as well as belief systems and behavioral patterns (Hofstede, 1980; Oyserman, Coon, and Kemmelmeier, 2002). In addition, recent studies have proposed useful refinements to these broader cultural categories (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). These studies suggest that the nature and meaning of individualism and collectivism varies across gender and ethnic lines, as well as across family groupings and institutions. Although the breadth of the individualism-collectivism constructs lends integrative strengths, research indicates that further refinements of these categories can enhance the prediction of consumer behavior. The horizontal/vertical distinction. With in the individualism-collectivism framework, Triandis et al. (Triandis, 1995; Triandis and Gelfand, 1998) have recently introduced a further distinction between societies that are horizontal (valuing equality) and those that are vertical (emphasizing hierarchy), and a scale to measure these orientations at the individual level. The horizontal/vertical distinction emerges from the observation that American or British individualism differs from, say, Norwegian or Danish individualism in much the same way that Japanese or Korean collectivism differs from the collectivism of the Israeli kibbutz. Specifically, in vertical individualist (VI) societies (e.g., United States and Great Britain), people strive
  • 14. to become distinguished and acquire status via competition (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong, cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 3 2009); whereas in horizontal individualist (HI) cultural contexts (e.g., Sweden and Norway), people value uniqueness but are not especially interested in becoming distinguished and achieving high status (Nelson and Shavitt, 2002). In contrast, in vertical collectivistic (VC) societies (e.g., Korea and Japan), people emphasize the subordination of their goals to those of their in-groups, submit to the will of authority, and support competitions between their in-groups and out-groups. Finally, in hori- zontal collectivist (HC) cultural contexts (e.g., exemplified historically by the Israeli Kibbutz), people see themselves as similar to others, and emphasize shared goals and sociability, but instead of submitting to authority, their view of power focuses on benevolence and helping others (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). However, the modal comparisons in consumer research are between the United States (VI) and any of a number of Pacific Rim coun- tries (VC). This means that much of what is known about consumer behavior in indi- vidualistic and collectivistic societies reflects vertical forms of these syndromes and may not generalize, for example, comparisons between Sweden (HI) and Israel (HC) or other sets of horizontal cultures. As an example, conformity
  • 15. in product choice, as examined by Kim and Markus (1999), may be a tendency specific to VC cultures, in which deference to authority and to in-group wishes is stressed. Much lower levels of conformity may be observed in HC cultures, which emphasize sociability but not deference (Triandis and Gelfand, 1998). Thus, differences in consumers’ conformity between Korea (VC) and the United States (VI) may not charac- terize broad individualism-collectivism differ- ences, because levels of product conformity in HC contexts might not exceed those in HI contexts. Indeed, several recent studies of this horizontal/vertical cultural distinction have provided evidence for its value as a predictor of new consumer psychology phenomena and as a basis for refining the understanding of known phenomena (Shavitt et al., 2006). For instance, Lalwani, Shavitt, and Johnson (2006) showed that differences in the self-presentational responses observed for individualists and collectivists are mediated at the individual level by the horizontal but not the vertical versions of these cultural orientations. This suggests that culturally linked self-presentational efforts reflect distinct goals of being seen as self-reliant and capable (valued in HI contexts) versus sociable and benevolent (valued in HC contexts). Further evidence for the value of the horizontal-vertical distinction comes from a study of country-of-origin effects. Gürhan-Canli
  • 16. and Maheswaran (2000) demonstrated that the tendency to favor products from one’s own country over foreign products emerged more strongly in Japan (a VC culture) than in the United States (a VI culture). Mediation analyses using individual consumers’ self-rated cultural values further indicated that only the vertical aspect of individualism and collectivism accounted for the country-of-origin effects in Japan. In other words, the collectivistic tendency to favor one’s own country’s products appeared to be driven by cultural values that stress hierarchy, competition, and deference to in-group wishes, not by values that stress interdependence more generally. In line with this, as noted earlier, research suggests that mental representations of power in terms of status and competition versus benevo- lence differ reliably between vertical and horizontal cultural backgrounds and orien- tations. These differences impact consumer information processing and the interpretation of power-related stimuli (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). Finally, content analyses of magazine advertisements in several countries suggested that status-oriented themes of hierarchy, luxury, prestige, and distinction were generally more prevalent in societies presumed to have vertical cultural profiles (e.g., Korea, Russia) than a horizontal cultural profile (Denmark) (Shavitt et al., 2006). Culture and thinking styles. East Asian and North American cultural differences have been well documented in social psychological
  • 17. research, especially in terms of the differ- ences in individualistic-collectivistic values and independent-interdependent self-systems (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). Many of these cross-cultural studies of consumer behavior have provided evidence that advertising (such 4 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior as magazine ads, Internet advertising, and TV commercials) from Western cultures is in general more individualistic and less collec- tivistic than advertising from Asian cultures (Morling and Lamoreaux, 2008), and that consumers from Western cultures are more likely to be persuaded by individualistic ads and those from East Asian cultures are more likely to be persuaded by collectivistic ads (Han and Shavitt, 1994). However, relatively little research has been done on cross-cultural differences in consumers’ thinking orientations. The following section provides a general review of cultural differences in thinking styles in addition to the findings of relevant studies of consumer behavior and advertising effects. Analytic versus holistic thinking. Broadly speaking, Westerners tend to adopt an analytic thinking style that emphasizes the indep- endence of individual objects, whereas East Asians tend to adopt a holistic view emphasizing that the world is composed of interrelated elements (Nisbett et al., 2001). The analytic style of Westerners and the holistic style of
  • 18. East Asians have been demonstrated in various cognitive domains such as attention, causal reasoning, perception of change, tolerance of contradiction, and categorization. The analytic style of attention is field indepen- dent (mainly oriented toward an object itself), whereas holistic attention is field dependent (focused on the relationship between objects and/or the field in which they are embedded) (Nisbett et al., 2001). This difference in the orientation of attention is also seen in the way East Asians and Westerners perceive and explain social events. East Asians tend to assume that each element in the world is somehow inter- twined, and thus an event or object can be understood only in the context of the whole set of relevant factors. By contrast, Westerners tend to explain a certain event in terms of direct causal links, thereby considering fewer reasons than East Asians, who tend to consider a broader set of reasons, regardless of their relevance to the event (Choi et al., 2003). Furthermore, in explaining causality of a social event, analytic thinkers tend to focus on the internal dispositions of an actor, whereas holistic thinkers tend to consider a broader set of reasons (including both dispositional and contex- tual information) and are therefore less likely to attribute an outcome to an actor’s internal char- acteristics (Nisbett et al., 2001). This has impli- cations for brand judgments, as well. Monga and John (2007) found that negative publicity influences analytic (vs. holistic) thinkers more
  • 19. heavily, and thus changes their beliefs about a brand to a greater degree because analytic thinkers are less likely to consider contextual information, and thus are more likely to attribute negative product information to the brand. From the analytic perspective, objects exist independently, and thus the essence of the objects is stable over time. This assumption promotes a linear perception of change in which no drastic deviation is expected in the pattern of stability or change of a phenomenon (Nisbett, 2003). By contrast, the holistic view of the world assumes that objects are interrelated, and therefore it is less likely that a phenomenon will remain stable over time. This perspective results in a cyclic perception in which people tend to predict fluctuating trends for an event. For example, in predicting future stock-market trends and making investment decisions, Cana- dians are more likely to make judgments based on recent trends than are Chinese people; thus, when compared to the Chinese, Canadians are more willing to buy stocks when they are in an increasing trend and less willing to buy when stock prices are decreasing (Ji, Zhang, and Guo, 2008). The cyclic perception of change and expecta- tion of instability prevalent among East Asians renders a Yin-Yang belief that a characteristic of an object can potentially transform into its opposite. Consequently, East Asians tend to hold a dialectical perception in which apparently opposing concepts can simultaneously be true and can peacefully coexist (Nisbett et al., 2001).
  • 20. When confronted with opposing propositions, East Asians tend to resolve contradictions by choosing a middle ground, whereas Westerners to rely on formal logic in resolving contradictions by choosing one of the opposing propositions. For example, US consumers tend to resolve incongruities with an attenuation strategy in which one piece of information is favored over another inconsistent piece of information. In contrast, Hong Kong Chinese consumers tend cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 5 to follow an additive strategy in which both pieces of information are combined to influence judgments (Aaker and Sengupta, 2000). East Asians and Westerners also perceive conflicting emotions in different ways. For example, Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi (1999) showed that Chinese tend to hold a dialectical perception that pleasant and unpleasant emotions can be experienced at the same time. Thus, their frequency judgment for pleasant emotions is positively correlated with their frequency judg- ment for unpleasant emotions. By contrast, this study found that for Americans the perceived frequency of pleasant emotions is inversely correlated with the perceived frequency of unpleasant emotions. Schimmack, Oishi, and Diener (2002) analyzed 38 nationalities and demonstrated that this cultural difference results from dialectical thinking, not from a difference in individualistic-collectivistic values.
  • 21. Moreover, Williams and Aaker (2002) demon- strated that opposing emotions (e.g., both happiness and sadness) in persuasion appeals elicit more positive attitudes among Asian Americans than among European Americans. Westerners pay more attention to individual objects and attribute causality to them, whereas East Asians focus more on the field. Westerners are more accustomed to formulating rules that govern internal properties of objects and tend to categorize things by applying those rules. In contrast, East Asians organize objects on the basis of their relationship to other objects or to the field (Nisbett, 2003), and therefore they tend to categorize objects according to their overall similarities. Thus, when presented with pictures of a panda, a monkey, and a banana, East Asians tend to categorize the monkey and banana together based on the relationship between the two, whereas Westerners tend to categorize the panda and monkey into one group based on the traits that characterize them (Ji, Zhang, and Nisbett, 2004). Cultural differences in the way people cate- gorize objects (rule/trait-based vs. similarity/ relationship-based) also appear in the way they organize and store brand information. For example, Ng and Houston (2006) showed that Americans are less likely to retrieve brand exemplars (i.e., specific products or subcategories) than brand beliefs (i.e., general descriptive or evaluative thoughts), whereas the reverse was the case for Singaporeans. These
  • 22. results emerged from an analytic tendency to focus on ‘‘global beliefs’’ abstracted from prior product experiences and a holistic tendency to focus on contextual and incidental details about the product. Similarly, Monga and John (2008) found that, compared to Americans, Indians tend to perceive a higher degree of fit between a parent brand (e.g., Kodak) and its brand extension (e.g., Kodak filing cabinet, Kodak greeting cards), and to evaluate the brand extension more positively. This result reflects Indians’ holistic tendency to base their judgments more heavily on the relationships between brand extensions and parent brands than do their American counterparts. A variety of methods and techniques have been developed to measure cultural differences in thinking styles (Choi, Koo, and Choi, 2007; Ji, Zhang, and Nisbett, 2004; Monga and John, 2007), including responses to cognitive tasks, scenarios and questions, physiological measures, a scale, and analyses of various cultural products. Furthermore, priming an independent versus interdependent view of self has also been found to promote analytic and holistic modes of thinking, respectively. For example, people primed with an independent self-view were more likely to focus on a focal object and thus were better at finding an embedded figure by separating the figure from its background than were those primed with an interdependent self-view (see Oyserman and Lee, 2007, for a review). Additional dimensions. Numerous other
  • 23. cultural distinctions deserve further attention in consumer research. A focus upon these relatively under-researched constructs as antecedents may allow for broadening the range of cultural differences beyond those currently investigated. For instance, Schwartz’s (1992) circumplex structure of values, which is highly robust cross-nationally, parallels the HI, VI, HC, VC typology and offers a particularly detailed and comprehensive basis for classification. In his large-scale studies of work values, Hofstede (1980) derived three other dimensions of cultural variation in addition to individualism: power distance (acceptance of power inequality in 6 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior organizations, a construct conceptually relevant to the vertical/horizontal distinction), uncer- tainty avoidance (the degree of tolerance for ambiguity or uncertainty about the future), and masculinity/femininity (preference for achieve- ment and assertiveness versus modesty and nurturing relationships). Indeed, individualism was the second dimension identified by Hofstede (1980), whereas power distance emerged as the first dimension. A few marketing-oriented studies have employed Hofstede’s nation-level classifications (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009), but more potential remains for identifying consequences for consumer judgments and behaviors. For instance, uncertainty avoidance has been conceptualized as a syndrome related to anxiety, rule orientation, need for security, and
  • 24. deference to experts (Hofstede, 1980). As such, one might speculate that the level of uncertainty avoidance in a culture will predict the tendency for advertisements to use fear appeals or appeals to safety and security, and the tendency for advertisements to employ expert spokespersons. Differences along this cultural dimension may also predict patterns in the diffusion of product innovations, particularly innovations whose purchase entails a degree of risk. CULTURE AND PERSUASIVE APPEALS Most research on cultural influences on judg- ment and persuasion has examined the implications of individualism/collectivism or independent/interdependent self-construals. In general, the findings suggest that the prevalence or the persuasiveness of a given type of appeal matches the cultural value orientation of the society (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). For instance, appeals to individuality, personal benefits, and achievement are usually more prevalent and persuasive in individualistic compared to collectivistic cultures, whereas appeals to group benefits, harmony, and confor- mity are usually more prevalent and persuasive in collectivistic compared to individualistic cultures. Such evidence for ‘‘cultural matching’’ in the nature of appeals has been followed by studies examining the distinct psychological processes driving persuasion across cultures. These studies suggest that culture can affect how people process and interpret product-related information. It can determine the type of
  • 25. information that is weighed more heavily for making judgments (e.g., product attributes vs other consumers’ opinions). However, brand and product characteristics can constrain the role of cultural variables in information processing and persuasion, with some brands and products serving as stronger carriers of cultural values (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong, 2009). Cultural differences in the content of message appeals. Cross-cultural content analyses of advertisements can yield valuable evidence about distinctions in cultural values. For instance, American advertisers and consumer researchers often assume that consumer learning about the brand precedes other marketing effects, such as liking and buying the brand. Thus, advertisements that attempt to teach the consumer about the brand are typical in the United States, although other types of advertisements are also used. In contrast, as Miracle (1987) suggested, the typical goal of advertisements in Japan appears very different. There, advertisements tend to focus on ‘‘making friends’’ with the audience and showing that the company understands their feelings. The assumption is that consumers will buy once they feel familiar with and have a sense of trust in the company. Because Japan, Korea, and other Pacific Rim countries are collectivist cultures that tend toward implicit and indirect communication practices (Triandis, 1995), Miracle suggested that the mood and tone of commercials in these countries will
  • 26. be particularly important in establishing good feelings about the advertiser. Several studies have supported these notions, showing that advertisements in Japan and Korea, compared to those in the United States, rely more on symbolism, mood, and aesthetics and less on direct approaches such as brand comparisons (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). The ads may be equally informative about the brand across cultures. It is the type of appeal that will vary. For instance, a content analysis of magazine advertisements revealed that in Korea, compared to the United States, advertisements are more focused on family well-being, interdependence, group goals, and harmony, whereas they are cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 7 less focused on self-improvement, ambition, personal goals, independence, and individuality (Han and Shavitt, 1994). However, as one might expect, the nature of the advertised product moderated these effects. Cultural differences emerged strongly only for products that tend to be purchased and used along with other persons (e.g., groceries, cars). Products that do not tend to be shared (e.g., health and beauty aids, clothing) are promoted more in terms of personal, individualistic benefits in both countries. Paralleling the overall cross-national differ- ences, a content analysis by Kim and Markus
  • 27. (1999) indicated that Korean advertisements, compared to US advertisements, were character- ized by more conformity themes (e.g., respect for collective values and beliefs) and fewer unique- ness themes (e.g., rebelling against collective values and beliefs). Website content in Eastern and Western countries also appears to differ in the emphasis on individual versus collective activities (Shavitt, Lee, and Torelli, 2009). Finally, it is important to note that, in countries experiencing rapid economic growth, advertising content does not necessarily reflect existing cultural values, instead promoting new, aspirational values such as individuality and modernity. For instance, in China, in recent years, westernized ad appeals are increasingly common. Appeals to youth/modernity, indi- viduality/independence, and technology are especially salient in Chinese advertisements that target the younger generation (Zhang and Shavitt, 2003). Similarly, during a period of rapid transition in South Korea’s economy (1968–1998), content analysis of advertisements revealed substantial shifts toward individu- alistic, modernity-oriented appeals (Han and Shavitt, 2005). Cultural differences in judgment and persuasion. Research suggests that the persuasiveness of appeals may mirror the cultural differences in their prevalence. An experiment by Han and Shavitt (1994) showed that appeals to individu- alistic values (e.g., ‘‘Solo cleans with a softness that you will love’’) were more persuasive in the United States and appeals to collectivistic values
  • 28. (e.g., ‘‘Solo cleans with a softness that your family will love’’) were more persuasive in Korea. Again, however, this effect was much more evident for products that are shared (laundry detergent, clothes iron) than for those that are not (chewing gum, running shoes). Zhang and Gelb (1996) found a similar pattern in the persuasiveness of individualistic versus collectivistic appeals in an experiment conducted in the United States and China. Moreover, this effect appeared to be moder- ated by whether the advertised product is socially visible (camera) versus privately used (toothbrush). Finally, Wang and Mowen (1997) showed in a US sample that individual differences in separateness/connectedness self- schema (i.e., the degree to which one views the self as independent of or interconnected with important others) predicts attitudes toward individualistic versus collectivistic ad appeals for a credit card. Thus, cultural orientation and national culture have implications for the effectiveness of appeals. However, such cultural differences would only be anticipated for those products or uses that are relevant to both personal and group goals. Cultural differences in persuasion are also revealed in the diagnosticity of certain types of information. For instance, Aaker and Maheswaran (1997) showed that consensus information regarding other consumers’ opinions is not treated as a heuristic cue by Hong Kong Chinese (as it is in the United
  • 29. States) but is instead perceived and processed as diagnostic information. Thus, collectivists resolve incongruity in favor of consensus information, not brand attributes. This would be expected in a culture that stresses conformity and responsiveness to others’ views. On the other hand, cues whose (low) diagnosticity is not expected to vary cross-culturally (e.g., number of attributes presented) elicit similar heuristic processing in the United States and Hong Kong. Finally, because cognitive associations with power vary with horizontal and vertical cultural orientations and with ethnicity, as noted earlier, Torelli et al., found differences in the interpretive processes and mindsets triggered when power is salient. Specifically, people whose cultural orientation predisposes a status-oriented view of power activate cognitive processes that facilitate defending their power, such as reasserting control by confirming 8 cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior prior stereotypes about a brand. In contrast, people whose cultural orientation predisposes a benevolence-oriented view of power activate cognitive processes that facilitate helping others, such as by forming accurate, careful impressions of brands (Shavitt, Torelli and Wong, 2009). CONCLUSIONS As marketing efforts are increasingly global-
  • 30. ized, understanding cross-cultural consumer behavior has become a key focus of consumer research. In recent years, research in consumer behavior has addressed a broadening set of cross-cultural issues and dimensions. Research has provided an enhanced understanding of the relations between culture and self-construal, motivation, thinking style, and consumer persuasion. Research has also begun to address the psychological mechanisms underlying cross- cultural differences in consumer judgments, and the products and contexts for which these differences are most likely to be observed. Understanding cultural differences has become crucial for effective marketing and advertising. In future research, it will be important to further distinguish cultural similarities and differences in consumer judgments, identify within-culture or subgroup differences that parallel between-culture differences, and explore their rich implications in consumer behavior. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Preparation of this article was supported by Grant #1R01HD053636-01A1from the National Institutes of Health, Grant #0648539 from the National Science Foundation, and Grant #63842 from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation to Sharon Shavitt. Bibliography Aaker, J.L. and Maheswaran, D. (1997) The effect of cultural orientation on persuasion. Journal of
  • 31. Consumer Research, 24 (3), 315–328. Aaker, J.L. and Sengupta, J. (2000) Addivity versus attenuation: the role of culture in the resolution of information incongruity. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9 (2), 67–82. Bagozzi, R.P., Wong, N., and Yi, Y. (1999) The role of culture and gender in the relationship between positive and negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 13 (6), 641–672. Choi, I., Dalal, R., Kim-Prieto, C., and Park, H. (2003) Culture and judgement of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (1), 46–59. Choi, I., Koo, M., and Choi, J. (2007) Measuring the analytic vs. the holistic thinking style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33 (5), 691–705. Gürhan-Canli, Z. and Maheswaran, D. (2000) Cultural variations in country of origin effects. Journal of Marketing Research, 37 (3), 309–317. Han, S.-P. and Shavitt, S. (1994) Persuasion and culture: advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic societies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30 (4), 326. Han, S. and Shavitt, S. (2005) Westernization of cultural values in Korean advertising: a longitudinal content analysis of magazine ads from 1968-1998, in Advances in consumer research, Vol. 32 (eds G. Menon and A.R. Rao), Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 249–250.
  • 32. Hofstede, G.H. (1980) Culture’s Consequences: Inter- national Differences in Work-Related Values, Sage, Newbury, Park. Ji, L.J., Zhang, Z., and Guo, T. (2008) To buy or to sell: cultural differences in stock market decisions based on stock price trends. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21 (4), 399–413. Ji, L.J., Zhang, Z., and Nisbett, R.E. (2004) Is it Culture, or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87 (1), 57–65. Kim, H.S. and Markus, H.R. (1999) Deviance or unique- ness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (4), 785–800. Lalwani, A.K. and Shavitt, S. (2009) The ‘‘me’’ I claim to be: cultural self-construal elicits self-presentational goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (1), 88–102. Lalwani, A., Shavitt, S., and Johnson, T.P. (2006) What is the relation between cultural orientation and socially desirable responding? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (1), 165–178. Lau-Gesk, L.G. (2003) Activating culture through persuasion appeals: an examination of the bicultural consumer. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 301–315. Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (1991) Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motiva-
  • 33. tion. Psychological Review, 98 (2), 224–253. cross-cultural psychology of consumer behavior 9 Miracle, G.E. (1987) Feel-Do-Learn: an alternative sequence underlying Japanese consumer response to television commercials, in Proceedings of the L.A. Conference of the American Academy of Advertising (ed. F.G. Feasley), The University of South Carolina, Columbia. Monga, A.B. and John, D.R. (2007) Cultural differences in brand extension evaluation: the influence of analytic versus holistic thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (4), 529–536. Monga, A.B. and John, D.R. (2008) When does negative brand publicity hurt? The moderating influence of analytic versus holistic thinking. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18 (4), 320–332. Morling, B. and Lamoreaux, M. (2008) Measuring culture outside the head: a meta-analysis of cultural prod- ucts. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12 (3), 199–221. Nelson, M.R. and Shavitt, S. (2002) Horizontal and vertical individualism and achievement values: a multi-method examination of Denmark and the U.S. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (5), 439–458. Ng, S. and Houston, M.J. (2006) Exemplars or beliefs? The impact of self-view on the nature and relative influence of brand associations. Journal of Consumer
  • 34. Research, 32 (4), 519–529. Nisbett, R.E. (2003) The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners think Differently..., and Why, Free Press, New York. Nisbett, R.E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001) Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108 (2), 291–310. Oyserman, D., Coon, H.M., and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002) Rethinking individualism and collec- tivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128 (1), 3–72. Oyserman, D. and Lee, S.W.-S. (2007) Priming ‘culture’: culture as situated cognition, in Handbook of Cultural Psychology (eds S. Kitayama and D. Cohen), Guilford Press, New York, pp. 255–282. Schwartz, S.H. (1992) Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries, in Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 25 (ed. M.P. Zanna), Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 1–65. Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A.K., Zhang, J., and Torelli, C.J. (2006) The horizontal/vertical distinction in cross-cultural consumer research. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16 (4), 325–356. Shavitt, S., Lee, A., and Torelli, C. (2009) New directions in cross-cultural consumer psychology, in The Social Psychology of Consumer Behavior, a volume in the
  • 35. series, Frontiers of Social Psychology (eds M. Wänke, A.W. Kruglanski, and J.P. Forgas) Series Editors, Psychology Press, New York, pp. 227–250. Shavitt, S., Torelli, C., and Wong, J. (2009), Identity- based motivation in a consumer context. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19 (3), pp. 261–266. Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., and Diener, E. (2002) Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism-collectivism? Cognition and Emotion, 16 (6), 705–719. Triandis, H.C. (1995) Individualism and collectivism, Westview Press, Boulder. Triandis, H.C. and Gelfand, M.J. (1998) Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (1), 118–128. Wang, C.L. and Mowen, J.C. (1997) The separate- ness-connectedness self-schema: scale development and application to message construction. Psychology and Marketing, 14 (2), 185–207. Williams, P. and Aaker, J. (2002) Can mixed emotions peacefully co-exist? Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (4), 636–649. Wyer, R.S., Chiu, C.-Y., and Hong, Y.-Y. (eds) (2009). Understanding Culture: Theory, Research and Applica- tion, Psychology Press, New York. Zhang, Y. and Gelb, B.D. (1996) Matching advertising
  • 36. appeals to culture: the influence of products’ use conditions. Journal of Advertising, 25 (3), 29–46. Zhang, J. and Shavitt, S. (2003) Cultural values in adver- tisements to the Chinese X-generation: promoting modernity and individualism. Journal of Advertising, 32 (1), 23–33.