SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 141
Linguistic Acculturation and Context on Self-Esteem:
Hispanic Youth Between Cultures
Rose M. Perez
Published online: 16 February 2011
� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
Abstract Immigrant adolescents must negotiate two cultures: the
host culture and
their native culture. This study explored how self-esteem is
moderated by the effect
of linguistic acculturation and context. An ordinary least-
squares regression model,
controlling for fixed effects, produced results supporting the
hypothesis that
linguistic acculturation moderates the effect of context on self-
esteem. The self-
esteem of Hispanic adolescents who were less linguistically
acculturated was found
to be more favorable when with family than with friends and the
reverse was found
for the more linguistically acculturated participants.
Adolescents in the middle of
the linguistic acculturation process had the widest variance in
self-esteem between
times they were with their families and times in other contexts;
they experienced
more positive self-esteem with anyone but family. Findings
underscore the need to
better understand the complex process of linguistic
acculturation and its effects on
self-esteem. This research also demonstrates the practical utility
of a fixed-effects
model for reducing bias in cross-cultural research.
Keywords Linguistic acculturation � Hispanics � Fixed effects
� Self-esteem �
Experience Sampling Method (ESM)
Hispanic immigrants and their children are not only the fastest
growing population
in this country, but they are also among the poorest (Portes and
Rumbaut 2006).
The author welcomes communication at the Fordham University
Graduate School of Social Service.
She wishes to acknowledge the dissertation committee who
helped guide completion of a doctoral
dissertation on which this article is based.
R. M. Perez (&)
Fordham University Graduate School of Social Service, 113
West 60th Street,
New York, NY 10023, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
123
Child Adolesc Soc Work J (2011) 28:203–228
DOI 10.1007/s10560-011-0228-y
For Hispanic youth, the pathway to higher social mobility is
fraught with formidable
obstacles, like discrimination and suboptimal inner-city schools,
that render them
unprepared for the challenges of the labor market, and it
appears that, counter to
traditional patterns of immigrant incorporation, they are
experiencing downward
adjustment. This becomes a problem for Hispanics, and for U.S.
society as a whole,
given the increasing size of the Hispanic population. If Hispanic
educational and
economic indicators do not improve, as their proportions grow,
poverty in the
United States will grow correspondingly.
Redfield et al. (1936) defined acculturation as the ‘‘phenomena
which result when
groups of individuals having different cultures come into
continuous first-hand
contact, with subsequent changes in the original cultural
patterns of either or both
groups.’’ Yet, time in the United States is typically associated
with second-culture
acquisition (Rudmin 2010), part of a complex and stressful
process (Ward 1996).
For Hispanic youth, acculturation often occurs in a context
influenced by, often
hostile environments that are not easy to navigate. Arguably,
the multifaceted
process of acculturation can add to the already tumultuous
emotions experienced in
adolescence and may impact self-esteem. As a result, immigrant
youth acculturate
in different ways, influenced in large part by the contexts with
which they interact
(Piedra and Engstrom 2009). The contextual changes that young
immigrants face in
the normal course of their lives may vary widely and if these
contexts are culturally
distant from one another, the cultural norms between them can
be difficult to
reconcile. For instance, a young person who has little mastery
of the English
language may not have a positive sense of self when in the
company of more
acculturated peers or classmates. In those cases, they may feel
closer to their
families with whom they are likely to be more culturally
matched (e.g., how they
value the importance of family) and despite the generational
differences.
Poor self-esteem has been associated problem behaviors, like
dropping out of
school and poor social and behavioral adjustment (Weiner
2000). More broadly, a
recent trend, referred to as the ‘‘immigrant health paradox’’ is
based on, a consistent,
and growing body of evidence mostly from the field of
epidemiology suggests that
acculturated adolescents experience lower levels of well-being
than less accultur-
ated counterparts in the United States (Harker 2001; Mendoza et
al. 2007; Rumbaut
1997). Although findings of this paradox are too numerous to
cite and largely
focused on Mexicans (Alegrı́a et al. 2008), consider that in a
sample of Hispanic
adolescents, acculturation has been associated with higher
substance use (Greenman
and Xie 2008; Turner and Gil 2002), chronic illness (Wickrama
et al. 2007), and
lower risky sexual behaviors (Weiss and Tillman 2009). Vega
and Alegrı́a (2001)
found some support to suggest that Hispanics have better mental
health upon arrival
than after achieving higher levels of acculturation. Researchers
exploring the
possible reasons for negative association between acculturation
and well-being
often include loss of family resources such as conflicts within
families stemming
from acculturation gaps between parents and their children as
factors shown to
reduce family cohesion (Smokowski et al. 2007, 2008). As
Hispanics, defined as
people who trace their ancestry to countries that share a
common language, history,
and certain cultural traits, which may or may not carry across
the generations as the
peoples acculturate (Cafferty 1985), continue to increase in size
it is important to
204 R. M. Perez
123
understand their continuing problematic patterns of
acculturation, which run counter
to the popular notion that immigrants are better off
acculturating.
Today more is known about the important role that acculturation
plays as a
moderator of the well-being of Hispanic youth, and it is
acknowledged that full
assimilation into mainstream U.S. life does not take place in
just one generation.
The relationship between acculturation and self-esteem has been
found to vary in
myriad ways (Berry et al. 2006; Bornstein and Cote 2007; Lang
et al. 1982). Much
of the variation in findings may be attributed to methodological
differences in how
acculturation is conceptualized and measured (Rudmin 2009).
As an individual-
level proxy for acculturation, language acquisition occurs
quickly, especially in
young people. Furthermore, it captures a complex process that
includes affective,
cognitive, and behavioral components (Cuéllar et al. 1995).
Language fluency has
been shown to account for a large portion of the variance in
multiple-item measures
(Rogler et al. 1991) and it continues to function as the most
broadly used measure of
cultural practice. All acculturation scales, whether complex or
simple, are imperfect
in some ways. Escobar and Vega (2000), after reviewing the
literature, recommend
using individual measures (e.g., language and length of stay)
rather than
multidimensional ones with uncertain value, as is currently the
case in the field,
until a single acculturation measure is fully validated
scientifically.
Because little is known about a young Hispanic’s well-being as
they interact with
different and probably widely differing environments, this study
set out to
understand the inter-relationships between acculturation, the
contexts in which
youth interact, and their effect on self-esteem. Data collected
using the Experience
Sampling Method, a repetitive sampling technique that assessed
adolescents’ self-
esteem in many different contexts throughout the course of one
week, is analyzed in
this study to argue that acculturation and context moderate self-
esteem. Using
advanced statistical design that allowed background differences
to be held constant
to reduce bias contributes to the literature on better
understanding the role of
acculturation, context, and self-esteem across groups dissimilar
in many socio-
economic dimensions (e.g., parental income, personality, age,
and gender). This
study can assist policy makers and practitioners to better
position programs and
policies in ways that better reflect on the experiences and needs
of this population.
Person-Context Perspective
The effects on well-being from interactions between people and
the contexts in
which time is spent are important aspects of ecologically valid
assessment
(Bronfenbrenner 1986). Ecologically valid assessment views a
person’s well-being
holistically, part of a broader system related to the environment
of which he or she is
a part (Sameroff 1995). Ecological theorizing is a core social-
work perspective. The
perspective of person-context interaction congruence is
predicated on the idea that
people’s well-being will be better when they are living in
contexts in which they
feel there is a good fit (Emmons et al. 1985). For example, if
youth are happy when
in the context of family they are likely to benefit from
socialization better than if
they were in conflict with their parents on a regular basis. The
person-context
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 205
123
congruence perspective is a helpful theoretical lens through
which one can observe
the effect of acculturation and context on self-esteem. The
importance of studying
complex person-in-an-environment effects on well-being is
highlighted in the
literature on stress and coping (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). For
example,
adolescents who are better able to focus their attention on
challenging and
productive activities have been found to be more resilient than
those who cannot
(Garmezy et al. 1984). Therefore, if there is congruence
between spending time with
friends and acculturation level, an adolescent’s self-esteem
should be positive
relative to other contexts in which he or she spends time. For
instance, if adolescents
are interested in fitting into peer culture, and they feel accepted
when in the context
of peers, it is expected that they will report positive self-
esteem.
As Hispanic immigrant youth acculturate to U.S. life, they face
many linguistic
and cultural challenges along the way and sometimes these
adversely affect their
well-being—including self-esteem. Hispanic youth upon arrival
struggle to adapt to
the culture and language of the United States and during this
time they must
reconcile among the different contexts in which they spend
time. Usually by virtue
of being in school, younger individuals learn the language of
the host culture
relatively easily and adapt rapidly, although acculturation can
be moderated by the
surrounding cultures of the communities in which they live.
Parents are typically
slower to learn English. Cultural and linguistic retention are
likely to be of more
practical value in communities where the immigrant’s native
language is widely
spoken. Among immigrant adolescents in the United States,
unless they are recently
arrived the preferred language is typically English. For many,
learning a second
language means losing their first (Fillmore 1991). If their first
language was
Spanish, it is possible that their parents are bilingual or have
yet to learn English. As
a result, language use is an effective proxy measure. For
example, youth who speak
Spanish are likely to have arrived recently in the United States.
Among those whose
first language was Spanish, they are likely to be speaking
English after a few years
in the United States, but their parents are likely to retain
Spanish longer and, by
proxy, most likely their culture as well. Youth whose first
language was English
likely have highly acculturated parents with whom they are
probably culturally
matched.
Context and Self-Esteem
The important role that context—defined as time spent with
people such as family,
friends, at school, with others, or alone—plays in the lives of
immigrant youth
cannot be understated. Immigrant youth on any given day can
enter into many
different types of contexts ranging from situations that require
varying levels of
acculturation. For instance, with parents who are less
acculturated and who do not
speak English, they must accommodate to their acculturation
level. A reverse
situation more than likely occurs at school, where the language
of instruction is
typically English. As youth enter and exit these widely different
contexts they must
on their own reconcile their beliefs and ideas and make
decisions about which set of
206 R. M. Perez
123
norms and values they will adopt. This can be terribly confusing
during times where
the contexts differ significantly from one another.
Self-esteem is one of many variables that are often used to
assess well-being
among youth. However, self-esteem has been assessed as a
global measure or one
that is context-dependent. This study focuses on the latter
definition, where less is
known. A context-dependent measure of self-esteem means that
assessing it can
vary greatly depending on the circumstances that individuals are
experiencing when
they are being assessed. James (1890) first identified the
importance of context
regarding self-esteem when he described it as the ratio of
expectations to
achievement. In this definition, self-esteem fluctuates according
to the particular
conditions of the context, or state. An individual’s perceived
achievement of a goal
is related to the importance that the individual places on
achievement of that
particular goal. Hence, under this definition, self-esteem
depends on the frame of
reference, including the context, where the subjective judgments
that individuals use
to evaluate their accomplishments take place. In this two-part
process, the
individual’s perception of success or failure to reach goals
influences the outcome.
Thus, lowered expectations can increase self-esteem, and
expectations that are too
high can result in unfulfilled accomplishments and therefore
lower self-esteem.
Students with higher self-esteem are believed to be better
adjusted, be more
accomplished in school, and have close, trusting relationships
with their parents
(Gove et al. 1990).
The level of acculturation and contexts in which youth spend
time play an
important role in how youth feel about themselves.
Psychologists and sociologists
have used self-esteem measures to evaluate levels of well-being
between
immigrants and nonimmigrants and between immigrants of
different generations
(Berry et al. 2006; Leondari 2001). Some studies have explored
the relationship
between the level of acculturation and global self-esteem;
however, findings have
been inconclusive. The effects of contextual factors on self-
esteem warrant
consideration (Boden et al. 2008). For example, in the context
of the Hispanic
family, family cohesion has been observed to weaken with
acculturation to U.S. life
(Sabogal et al. 1987). A consequence of family conflict as a
young person adapts to
U.S. life contributes to lowered well-being, including self-
esteem (Portes and Zady
2002; Smokowski and Bacallao 2007).
In families, self-esteem can be used to detect the presence or
absence of
conflict, although conflict within a family is a difficult variable
to define and
measure (Pawlak and Klein 1997). Vega et al. (1995) reported
that both low- and
high-acculturation groups experienced conflict related to
language use. The less
acculturated reported experiencing stress both at home and at
school. The more
acculturated reported experiencing language conflicts,
perceiving discrimination,
and feeling that they were in a closed society. The authors
speculated that the
native-born/high-acculturation group might perceive
discrimination to be a
greater hindrance to their desire to succeed than the less
acculturated group.
Success is an integral part of the self-esteem equation as
defined by James
(1890).
The importance of family as a perceived source of support has
been found to be
robust across different Hispanic subgroups (e.g., Mexicans,
Cubans, and Central
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 207
123
Americans) (Sabogal et al. 1987) but has been found to manifest
differently between
Anglo and Mexican cultures (Luna et al. 1996). Familism,
which refers to the
within-family support systems is typically identified as
protective factors for young
Hispanics. Family cohesion has been found to be a protective
factor against
adolescent acting out (e.g., conduct problems, aggressive
behavior, and rule-
breaking) (Marsiglia et al. 2009). Hispanics who follow
familismo and retain aspects
of native culture are believed to enjoy closer family bonds in a
collective and
respectful orientation. Respeto, or respect for elders, is an
example of another
Hispanic/U.S. cultural difference. Acculturating Hispanic youth
may offend their
elders if they internalize the U.S. norm, which emphasizes the
individual’s rights
and autonomy.
A problematic aspect of acculturation is that often younger
family members
outpace older ones by learning the language of the host society
more rapidly,
something that facilitates their access to societal institutions.
Asymmetrical
acculturation such as this can introduce an acculturation gap
with the potential
for family discord (Kwak 2003; Portes and Rumbaut 2006;
Szapocznik et al. 1986)
and increased intergenerational conflict (Szapocznik et al.
1986). Acculturation gaps
in Hispanic families have been found to be inversely associated
with familial
obligation (Sabogal et al. 1987) and family cohesion
(Smokowski et al. 2008).
Parent-adolescent conflict, which has been found to increase
over subsequent
immigrant generations has been shown to moderate the negative
association
between acculturation and self-esteem (Dennis et al. 2010).
Indeed, when children
acculturate faster than their parents, as often happens, conflicts
in the family occur,
creating additional stressors for its members (Szapocznik et al.
1986).
Prior studies found that adolescents in the United States
allocate more of their
time to their friends than to their families. They also reported
that they enjoyed the
time they spent with their friends much more than the time they
spent with their
families. Although this is likely to be normative behavior (and
attitude) in
adolescence, it is of concern because it may get in the way of
receiving needed
sanctions from parents, which is a necessary part of the
socialization process
(Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984). For instance, some
research has found lack of
delinquency related to time spent with family on weekends
(Pabon 1998), and, more
generally, parental monitoring (Halgunseth et al. 2006). Of
course, it should be
noted that some research has found that peers can play a
positive role in the lives of
adolescents (Youniss and Smollar 1985), and this can be an
important source of self-
esteem. Thus, the existing literature has not focused much on
understanding how
Hispanic adolescents differ on the amount of time they spend
across different
contexts and this too will be explored in this study.
Other contexts besides family and friends have also been found
to affect the self-
esteem of young people (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984;
Csikszentmihalyi and
Schneider 2000). Some studies found students to be unhappy or
disengaged,
whereas others found that Hispanic students, more than any
other ethnic/racial
group, experience higher levels of enjoyment and affect
(measured by scores on a
scale measuring sociable, proud, happy, and relaxed) in school
(Shernoff et al.
2000). Time spent alone is not associated with positive
emotions (Hunter and
Csikszentmihalyi 2003; Larson and Csikszentmihalyi 1980), but
it can act as a stress
208 R. M. Perez
123
buffer (Larson and Lee 1996) and arguably is needed to engage
in productive tasks
(Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984). For introverted personality
types, spending
time alone has been found to be more problematic than for the
extroverted
(Brandstätter 1994).
Summary and Hypothesis
This study is motivated by the important and growing concern
of the downward
effects of acculturation (and its concomitant constructs, lowered
family cohesion
and conflict) on the well-being of Hispanic adolescents. Using
the lens of person-
context congruence to understand the dynamic changes in self-
esteem correspond-
ing with changes in the contexts where adolescents spend time,
this research set out
to better understand whether acculturation and context together
moderate self-
esteem. There have been studies that explored the effects of
acculturation on self-
esteem and there are studies that support the idea that self-
esteem has state and trait
qualities. However, not much is known about how self-esteem
changes in response
to changes in context together with the level of acculturation. It
is also important to
consider the effect of background differences of participants
(e.g., personality) on
self-esteem because of their potential for biasing results. In
nonexperimental
research, it is difficult to compare groups, because people vary
in many ways.
Because self-esteem is thought to have both state (i.e.,
momentary or fluid qualities
varying according to the circumstances in which one finds
oneself) and trait (i.e.,
global personality) qualities, one way to isolate the effect of
contextual changes
while holding trait constant is to employ person-context
congruence models, which
help researchers to assess moment-by-moment changes in self-
esteem. It is equally
helpful to control for background differences by employing
statistical techniques
(e.g., fixed-effects models) that hold background differences
constant (Wooldridge
2006).
This study explores differences in language use, context, and
the associated self-
esteem changes while comparing cross-cultural groups. Drawing
on person-context
congruence theories, the focus is on how self-esteem changes
when adolescents
move from one context to another. This study explores how
youth at different levels
of linguistic acculturation feel in the various contexts where
they spend significant
amounts of time and that are important to their development.
Particular emphasis is
placed on how youth at different levels of linguistic
acculturation feel when with
family versus nonfamily. If, for example, poorly linguistically
acculturated youth
have better self-esteem with family than with friends, and the
reverse is true of more
linguistically acculturated youth, linguistic acculturation can be
said to moderate the
experience between context (e.g., family) and self-esteem.
Controlling for
unobserved background differences, and the type of activity
youth were engaged
in, we would predict that linguistic acculturation would
moderate the effect of
context on self-esteem. Specifically, given prior research, we
hypothesize that with
increased linguistic acculturation, self-esteem will be lower
with family than with
friends and others.
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 209
123
Methodology
Research Design
This study used data from a large, nationally representative
sample employing the
Experience Sampling Method (ESM). ESM, a technique popular
in positive
psychology, employs a repeated-measure assessment technique
over the course of
1 week to assist with in situ data collection, which provides
higher reliability than
methods based on one-time questionnaires that rely on recall
(Csikszentmihalyi and
Schneider 2000). In this study, ESM helps us understand how
linguistic
acculturation moderates the adolescent experience of emotional
well-being in
important contexts, especially between home and friends. Use of
a fixed-effects
model to analyze ESM data is advantageous because it can
control for background
differences, often a major problem in cross-cultural assessments
(Wooldridge 2006).
Although many ESM researchers aggregate the data to the
person level, this study
uses a fixed-effects statistical model, which makes it possible to
analyze how self-
esteem changes from moment to moment as participants enter
different contexts—
with family, friends, school, alone, or elsewhere. The model
also allows the
researcher to control for background differences (i.e., fixed
effects).
Sample
Participants for this study were drawn from the first wave of
data, gathered in
1992–1993, from the longitudinal Alfred P. Sloan Study of
Youth and Social
Development, which had three waves of ESM data extending to
1997. Sloan Center
researchers sought participants from 33 public middle and high
schools, in 12
locations across the country, with diverse levels of
urbanization, racial/ethnic
composition, and economic diversity, in grades 6, 8, 10, and 12.
A protocol for
collecting this data in a way that protected human subjects was
in place at the
University of Chicago, where a team of faculty and students
received IRB approval
for the study.
This study focuses principally on Hispanic adolescents. For this
analysis, 796
participants met selection criteria. Each of these participants
filled out up to 56
Experience Sampling Forms (ESFs), thereby contributing
22,335 response sets,
which were then analyzed. (This approach will be discussed in
the section on
procedures and data analysis, below.) Of the Hispanic
subsample for this study,
which totaled 105, about 66% were of Mexican descent, 10%
were Puerto Rican,
1% were Cuban, and the remainder were from various countries
in Latin America.
All Hispanic participants were included in order to attain
sufficient power, given the
large number of variables needed for analysis. Hispanics were
contrasted with three
comparison groups: 487 White non-Hispanic, English-dominant
participants who
met the inclusion criteria for comparison purposes; 158 non-
Hispanic others whose
first and current language was English (mostly African
Americans); and 46 who
reported speaking multiple languages as children and/or in the
present adolescent
period.
210 R. M. Perez
123
Procedures
To ensure that participants understood the instruments they
would be completing,
participants met in groups with a researcher before and after the
study. Student
participants were asked to fill out the same ESM instrument
multiple times during
the course of a week, to a maximum of 56 response sets per
participant. Participants
were instructed to fill out their questionnaires each time their
preprogrammed
wristwatches signaled them to do so. These signals were set to
go off at eight
random times per day for up to 7 days.
Measures and Variables
Instruments
A one-time questionnaire, NELS, based on the National
Education Longitudinal
Study 1988–1994, provided language data (Hafner et al. 1990).
Following the ESM,
an ESF was filled out each time a participant was signaled (up
to a possible total of
56 ESFs). Although not all participants responded to all
questions, multiple
responses were recorded for most participants. All instruments
were administered in
Spanish or English, according to participants’ language
preference.
Techniques such as ESM (Prescott et al. 1981) facilitate the
exploration
of person-context interactions (Csikszentmihalyi and
Csikszentmihalyi 1988;
Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984; Csikszentmihalyi and
Schneider 2000), and
because they obtain repeated measures for each individual,
score high in reliability
(Hektner et al. 2002). ESM gathers multiple scores per
respondent unobtrusively,
because no interviewer is present, and in situ, in the context in
which the participant
is engaged when surveyed (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1987;
Hormuth 1986;
Moneta and Csikszentmihalyi 1996). There is no reliance on
participants’ ability to
recall deep emotions, which are often difficult to remember,
much less express.
Outcome Variable: Self-Esteem
The self-esteem variable is a composite (average) score based
on select items from
the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; to reduce participant fatigue,
some scales were
shortened (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1987). Self-esteem
items included ‘‘living
up to the expectations of others,’’ ‘‘living up to your own
expectations,’’ ‘‘feeling
good about self,’’ ‘‘succeeding at present activity,’’ and
‘‘feeling in control’’ on a
scale from 0 to 9 (0 = ‘‘not at all’’; 9 = ‘‘very much’’). One
additional variable,
‘‘Were you succeeding at what you were doing?,’’ originally on
a 1–9 scale, was
transformed to a 0–9 scale.
Explanatory Set of Variables: Context
The mutually exclusive binary variables for context are defined
by time when the
respondent filled out the ESF: when engaged in schoolwork,
alone, with friends,
with families, or in other contexts.
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 211
123
Schoolwork. The first was time defined by occasions when the
respondent
specified being at a place categorized by a coder as schoolwork;
if these times
overlapped with other contexts, only time in this context
counted.
Alone. Time alone consists of time with no one else.
Family. The family variable includes times when the participant
reported being
with mother, father, both, a sibling, or another relative and in
no other context.
Friends. Time with friends consists of times when the
participant was with a
friend but in no other context.
Other contexts. Times in other contexts include occasions when
the respondent
was with relatives, siblings, strangers, or others, as well as in
multiple contexts,
but not at school or work. When a participant’s time overlapped
with other
contexts, the responses were placed into the ‘‘other context’’
variable.
The categories were forced to be mutually exclusive and
exhaustive and
the variable ‘‘family’’ was left out of the regression equations.
Main effects
for ‘‘context’’ may be included in the fixed-effects model
because context varies
for each individual (variation by response signal rather than just
between
individuals).
Explanatory Set of Variables: Acculturation Group
Based on two binary variables (race/ethnicity and language),
participants were
placed into six acculturation groups representing a continuum
between unaccultur-
ated and acculturated states.
Less-Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. For the first of
these, the less-
linguistically acculturated Hispanics group, Spanish was the
first language they
learned to speak and still spoke most often at the time of the
study.
Somewhat-Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. For the
second group, the
somewhat linguistically acculturated Hispanics, the first
language learned was
Spanish, but English was currently spoken most often.
Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. The third, the
linguistically acculturated
Hispanics group, consists of Hispanics whose first and current
language was
English.
White Non-Hispanic English-Dominant. The principal
benchmark group, against
which all of the Hispanic subgroups were compared, constituted
the fourth group,
White, non-Hispanic English-dominant, whose first and current
language was
English. Other benchmark groups were included to better
compare Hispanics.
Multiple Languages. The fifth group, a multiple-languages
group, consisted of
participants of any ethnic heritage who spoke multiple
languages, either from
birth or at the time of the interview.
Non-Hispanic Other. The sixth group, non-Hispanic other,
included all non-
Hispanic participants who were neither Hispanic nor White;
these were non-
Hispanic and non-White participants whose first and current
dominant language
was English.
212 R. M. Perez
123
Explanatory Set of Variables: Moderators of Linguistic
Acculturation
and Context
The self-esteem of Hispanic youth at various levels of linguistic
acculturation is
expected to vary with the context that respondents are in. These
mutually exclusive
and exhaustive binary variables consist of designations formed
by the interaction
between the binary linguistic acculturation group variable (less-
linguistically
acculturated Hispanics, somewhat linguistically acculturated
Hispanics, linguisti-
cally acculturated Hispanics, White, non-Hispanic English-
dominant, multiple-
language, non-Hispanic other) and the binary context variable
(with family, with
friends, in school or work, alone, or other contexts). The
linguistic acculturation
group variable does not vary for an individual, and cannot be
used in a fixed-effects
model; however, their inclusion is allowed by matching them
with context variables
that fluctuate from observation to observation.
Control Variables: Activities
Explicit control variables for the types of activities engaged in
were measured by
the ESM survey question, ‘‘What was the main thing you were
doing?’’ Responses
to this question were classified into five categories of binary
variables: leisure, work,
maintenance (e.g., brushing teeth), deviant, and distressed. The
criteria used to
categorize activities are roughly similar to those found by
Csikszentmihalyi and
Schneider (2000). Cases in which the respondent failed to
specify the activity being
performed at the time of the response signal were dropped from
the model. For
instance, activities involving personal care, sitting, relaxing, or
thinking were
classified as maintenance. Talking to someone, going to a
movie, playing sports, and
so on, were classified as leisure. A ‘‘distressed’’ activity
consisted of a situation in
which the person was crying or in emotional pain (e.g.,
attending a funeral). The
reference group in the equation is leisure; because these
variables represent all
leisure activities and the remaining variables are mutually
exclusive and exhaustive,
leisure was left out of the regression model.
Data Analysis Using a Fixed-Effects Model
To explore how the effect of linguistic acculturation on self-
esteem varies by
context, while controlling explicitly for the type of activity
participants were
involved in, this study used an analytic framework with a fixed-
effects, multiple-
regression model. Fixed-effects regression models allowed
participants’ self-esteem
scores in one context (e.g., when with parents) to be observed
relative to another
context (e.g., friends). Relative within-person contextually
based measures have,
among other advantages, the ability to avoid inexact
comparisons across people and
allow person-by-context factors to be assessed firsthand.
Exploration of relative
changes in how a person feels across different contexts, as
opposed to comparisons
between groups, helps to control for group heterogeneity.
Fixed-effects models are
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 213
123
useful in controlling for unobserved background differences
across participants and
in helping to reduce autocorrelation in the data.
Figure 1 shows a schematic of the proposed general fixed-
effects model. The
people with whom time is spent define context. Acculturation
groups are implicitly
controlled because demographic and other person-level
characteristics that do not
change within the person automatically drop out of a fixed-
effects model. The only
way in which the person-level variables do not drop out of the
equation is by
matching them with variables that do not drop out. So, this
study created interaction
terms between the person-level variables that do not change
from person to person
in the course of 1 week (e.g., linguistic acculturation) with each
of the context
variables (e.g., family, friends), which do change as participants
went from one
context to the next during the course of the study. The
interaction terms are change
scores showing how participants’ self-esteem was experienced.
Observations of
linguistic acculturation group by context are measurable and
were useful for
answering the research questions of interest. In addition, any
unobserved person-
level characteristics that do not change within the person, as
well as specified
(i.e., observed) activities in which adolescents engage, are
controlled.
The fixed-effects model demonstrates the relationship between
self-esteem and
other variables using ordinary least squares with fixed effects.
y ¼ bo þ X1b1 þ X2b2 þ X3b3 þ X4b4 þ ai þ e
where y stands for one of three well-being indicators, b0 is the
constant, X1 is a
vector of activity variables, X2 is a vector of context variables,
X3 is a vector of
interaction terms, X4 stands for the time varying intercept
(response signal dummies
1… 56), ai is the expected unobserved time invariant effects
where i indicates
person 1… N.
Controls Acculturation (Implicitly controlled)*
Activities (work, maintenance, leisure,
deviant, distressed)
Unobserved fixed effects
Less-acculturated Hispanics (Spanish)
Somewhat acculturated Hispanics
Acculturated Hispanics (English)
White non-Hispanics (English)
Multiple languages
Non-Hispanic Other (English)
Self-esteem
Context
Family
Friends
Alone
School/Work
Other
Fig. A fixed-effects model where linguistic acculturation group
moderates context. * A main effect of
linguistic acculturation cannot be explicitly estimated using
fixed effects because it does not vary within
an individual
214 R. M. Perez
123
Fixed-effects models correct for methodological problems, such
as omitted
variable bias and some autocorrelation, while allowing full use
of the dataset. Fixed-
effects models implicitly control for any unmeasured trait that
varies between, but
not within, individuals over time, and eliminate the dependence
found in
longitudinal data.
Results
Descriptive
Descriptive statistics are shown in Tables 1 and 2. Applying
chi-square statistics to
person-level data, Table 1 shows that participants’ demographic
characteristics
differ across the language use groups with respect to grade,
socioeconomic class of
community, and gender. Also, participants appear to spend the
bulk of their time on
leisure activities, followed by work and maintenance tasks (see
Table 2). The
context in which they spent the bulk of their time was on
schoolwork (ranging from
31% of the less linguistically acculturated Hispanics to 40.5%
for the most
linguistically acculturated Hispanics). Time with family
occupied the next highest
time allocation with White non-Hispanics spending the least
amount of time (20%)
with family, to less acculturated Hispanics spending 33% of
their time with family.
Across all groups, time spent with friends rather than
schoolwork was minor; less
acculturated Hispanics allocated 5% of their time with friends,
whereas the most
linguistically acculturated Hispanics spent 11% of their time
with friends.
Interestingly, less acculturated Hispanics spent the least amount
of time on
schoolwork and the most with family. Also, employing Wald
chi-square statistics on
disaggregated data, Table 2 shows that the groups differ
significantly in some but
not all of the types of activities in which they engaged and the
contexts in which
they spent time. Participants in the different language-use
groups differed
significantly only on time spent at leisure and maintenance.
Within each context,
there were significant differences between the groups on
schoolwork, family, and
friends. All in all, these results confirm the presence of
intergroup heterogeneity,
which was controlled by the fixed-effects model. Fixed-effects
models, by holding
all person-level differences constant, minimize measurement
bias.
Fixed Effects
Using STATA to analyze data, controlling for fixed effects, or
rather background
differences across participants, and also the type of activity
engaged in (e.g., work,
maintenance, deviant, or distressed), data support the
hypothesis that linguistic
acculturation moderates the effect of context on self-esteem.
For most of the
participant groups studied, results showed that linguistic
acculturation level did
indeed moderate the effect of context. In other words, the
combination of
participants’ level of linguistic acculturation and whom they
spend time with,
tempered participants’ ability to experience positive or negative
self-esteem relative
to being with family. The hypothesis was that with increased
linguistic
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 215
123
T
a
b
le
1
S
u
m
m
a
ry
st
a
ti
st
ic
s
fo
r
d
e
m
o
g
ra
p
h
ic
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
L
e
ss
a
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
S
o
m
e
w
h
a
t
a
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
A
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
W
h
it
e
n
o
n
-H
is
p
E
n
g
li
sh
M
u
lt
ip
le
la
n
g
u
a
g
e
s
N
o
n
-H
is
p
a
n
ic
o
th
e
r
M
is
si
n
g
T
o
ta
l
C
h
i-
sq
u
a
re
p
-v
a
lu
e
/d
f
N
o
.
o
f
re
sp
o
n
se
s
7
0
5
1
,4
0
7
9
8
7
1
4
,1
0
7
1
,2
0
1
3
,9
2
8
4
,4
6
3
2
6
,7
9
8
P
e
rc
e
n
t
o
f
to
ta
l
2
.6
0
%
5
.3
0
%
3
.7
0
%
5
2
.6
0
%
4
.5
0
%
1
4
.7
0
%
1
6
.7
0
%
1
0
0
.0
0
%
N
o
.
o
f
p
a
rt
ic
ip
a
n
ts
2
2
4
4
3
9
4
8
7
4
6
1
5
8
2
3
3
1
,0
2
9
P
e
rc
e
n
t
o
f
to
ta
l
2
.1
0
%
4
.3
0
%
3
.8
0
%
4
7
.3
0
%
4
.5
0
%
1
5
.4
0
%
2
2
.6
0
%
1
0
0
.0
0
%
G
ra
d
e
(6
th
–
1
2
th
)a
S
ix
th
2
7
.3
2
7
.3
2
8
.2
2
3
.8
3
9
.1
2
3
.4
3
9
.1
2
8
.3
4
4
.3
p

.0
0
1
E
ig
h
th
4
5
.5
3
1
.8
2
8
.2
2
8
.5
4
3
.5
2
9
.1
2
2
.3
2
8
.4
d
f
=
1
8
T
e
n
th
1
8
.2
2
2
.7
1
8
2
7
.5
1
3
2
4
.7
1
7
.2
2
3
.3
T
w
e
lf
th
9
.1
1
8
.2
2
5
.6
2
0
.1
4
.4
2
2
.8
2
1
.5
2
0
S
o
c
io
e
c
o
n
o
m
ic
st
a
tu
s
o
f
c
o
m
m
u
n
it
y
(S
C
C
)a
P
o
o
r
8
1
.8
5
4
.6
1
5
.4
1
.6
1
9
.6
1
7
.7
3
1
.3
1
6
.1
3
4
7
.5
p

.0
0
1
0
9
.1
2
5
.6
2
3
.2
4
.4
5
.1
1
2
.5
1
6
.1
d
f
=
2
4
M
id
d
le
c
la
ss
1
8
.2
2
9
.6
5
3
.9
3
2
.9
5
8
.7
5
5
.1
3
8
.6
3
9
.1
0
4
.6
5
.1
2
4
.4
1
5
.2
1
9
.6
1
4
.2
1
8
.9
U
p
p
e
r
c
la
ss
0
2
.3
0
1
7
.9
2
.2
2
.5
3
.4
9
.8
P
e
rc
e
n
t
fe
m
a
le
5
9
.1
5
9
.1
6
1
.5
5
4
.6
5
6
.5
7
2
.8
4
3
.8
5
5
.6
3
3
.2
p

.0
0
1
d
f
=
6
a
S
u
m
s
to
1
0
0
%
w
it
h
in
e
a
c
h
g
ro
u
p
216 R. M. Perez
123
T
a
b
le
2
S
u
m
m
a
ry
st
a
ti
st
ic
s
fo
r
a
c
ti
v
it
y
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
L
e
ss
a
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
S
o
m
e
w
h
a
t
a
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
A
c
c
u
lt
u
ra
te
d
H
is
p
a
n
ic
s
W
h
it
e
n
o
n
-H
is
p
a
n
ic
M
u
lt
ip
le
la
n
g
u
a
g
e
s
N
o
n
-H
is
p
a
n
ic
O
th
e
r
T
o
ta
l
W
a
ld
c
h
i-
sq
u
a
re
p
-v
a
lu
e
A
c
ti
v
it
y
a
L
e
is
u
re
4
7
.6
9
4
0
.5
4
4
2
.4
9
4
4
.3
5
3
9
.0
1
4
2
.5
6
4
3
.5
4
1
1
.7
1
p

.0
5
W
o
rk
2
7
.6
7
2
7
.7
2
2
8
.8
0
3
0
.3
4
3
0
.8
3
2
8
.3
8
2
9
.7
1
5
.2
1
n
.s
.
M
a
in
te
n
a
n
c
e
2
4
.2
1
3
1
.3
8
2
8
.4
0
2
4
.8
8
2
9
.9
0
2
8
.8
0
2
6
.3
7
2
8
.0
5
p

.0
0
1
D
e
v
ia
n
t
0
.1
4
0
.1
4
0
.3
1
0
.3
1
0
.2
6
0
.1
8
0
.2
7
2
.0
8
n
.s
.
D
is
tr
e
ss
e
d
0
.2
9
0
.2
1
0
.0
0
0
.1
2
0
.0
0
0
.0
8
0
.1
1
2
.9
6
n
.s
.
C
o
n
te
x
ta
S
c
h
o
o
lw
o
rk
3
0
.9
1
3
4
.7
2
4
0
.4
9
4
1
.3
1
3
9
.8
8
3
8
.6
3
4
0
.0
0
3
2
.2
5
p

.0
0
1
A
lo
n
e
1
9
.5
9
1
9
.8
7
1
7
.2
8
2
1
.4
8
1
8
.6
3
2
1
.2
5
2
0
.9
4
4
.1
8
n
.s
.
F
a
m
il
y
3
2
.9
5
2
7
.5
8
2
4
.1
3
1
9
.8
5
2
8
.5
4
2
4
.9
3
2
2
.2
8
3
1
.4
6
p
.0
0
1
F
ri
e
n
d
s
5
.3
7
8
.6
6
1
0
.7
4
8
.7
7
6
.6
0
6
.6
3
8
.2
5
1
1
.4
3
p

.0
5
O
th
e
r
c
o
n
te
x
t
1
1
.1
8
9
.1
7
7
.3
6
8
.5
9
6
.3
5
8
.5
6
8
.5
3
6
.0
9
n
.s
.
A
ll
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
a
re
m
e
a
n
s
w
h
ic
h
c
a
n
b
e
in
te
rp
re
te
d
a
s
p
e
rc
e
n
ts
A
c
ti
v
it
y
&
c
o
n
te
x
t
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
a
re
c
lu
st
e
re
d
to
th
e
p
e
rs
o
n
le
v
e
l.
D
e
m
o
g
ra
p
h
ic
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
d
o
n
o
t
c
h
a
n
g
e
w
it
h
in
th
e
p
e
rs
o
n
a
S
u
m
s
to
1
0
0
%
w
it
h
in
e
a
c
h
g
ro
u
p
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 217
123
acculturation, self-esteem was predicted to be lower with family
than with friends;
prior research showed that familism declines with acculturation
and that gaps grow
as children outpace their parents in the linguistic acculturation
process. However,
results are more nuanced than anticipated. Instead of seeing a
progression toward
higher self-esteem with friends than with family as linguistic
acculturation
increased, it was the group in the middle, the somewhat
linguistically acculturated,
rather than those at the high end of the linguistic acculturation
process, who had
higher self-esteem with friends than with family.
Table 3 Fixed-effects results (n = 796 participants contributing
22,335 response signals)
Self-esteem
B SE Sig.
Deviant -0.36 .26
Work 0.21 .03 ***
Maintenance -0.18 .03 ***
Distressed -3.07 .59 ***
Friends 0.25 .05 ***
Friends * multiple languages -0.41 .22 *
Friends * less linguistically acculturated -0.27 .32
Friends * linguistically acculturated Hispanics -0.10 .17
Friends * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.57 .17 ***
Friends * non-Hispanic others -0.06 .14
Schoolwork -0.08 .04 **
Schoolwork * multiple languages 0.28 .14 **
Schoolwork * low-acculturated Hispanics 0.20 .15
Schoolwork * linguistically acculturated Hispanics 0.39 .14 ***
Schoolwork * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.49 .11 ***
Schoolwork * non-Hispanic others 0.11 .08
Alone 0.09 .04 **
Alone * multiple languages -0.37 .17 **
Alone * less linguistically acculturated -0.25 .21
Alone * linguistically acculturated Hispanics -0.21 .18
Alone * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.15 .14
Alone * non-Hispanic others -0.24 .09 **
Other 0.16 .05 **
Other * multiple languages -0.01 .24
Other * less linguistically acculturated 0.35 .22
Other * linguistically acculturated Hispanics 0.22 .23
Other * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.31 .17 *
Other * non-Hispanic others -0.23 .12 *
Because fixed-effects models are used, main effects for
language use group cannot be estimated by the
model. Only interaction effects for language-use group are
presented. The referent group for the activities
(deviant, work, maintenance, distressed) is leisure; for the
contexts it is White non-Hispanic, English-
dominant with family. Significance values are designated as
follows: *** p  .01, ** p  .05, * p  .10
218 R. M. Perez
123
As can be seen in Table 3, coefficients are change scores as
participants’ self-
esteem changed from one context to the next. These are based
on changes to each
person’s base response; the first signal responded to acts as the
control to which all
subsequent responses are compared. Because all the
acculturation groups are
mutually exclusive and exhaustive, this table shows the main
effect, which is
essentially the change score for the referent group, the White
non-Hispanic English-
dominant group when with family. Because the fixed-effects
model drops all scores
that do not change within the person, all acculturation groups
were by design,
combined with the contexts, which did change from response
signal to response
signal to form interaction terms, such as low linguistically
acculturated Hispanics
with friends for example. The coefficients shown in Table 3 are
the resulting change
scores from these interactions.
In Table 3, the first coefficient within each context (friends,
schoolwork, alone,
and other), which is listed in bold letters at the beginning of
each context section,
corresponds to the referent group (i.e., White, non-Hispanic,
English-dominant
group, because the categories are mutually exclusive and
exhaustive). This is
followed by the interaction terms between each of the other
acculturation groups
and context. For example, a self-esteem coefficient of .25 (p 
.001) for the
‘‘friends’’’ effect means that the White, non-Hispanic, English-
dominant group
experiences significantly more positive self-esteem when with
friends than when
with family. The interaction terms that follow within each
context represent the
moderation effect on self-esteem for each group within each
context. So, for
example, the coefficient within the friends context
corresponding to Spanish-
dominant Hispanics showed that self-esteem was moderated by -
.27 (not
significant [n.s.]). This coefficient, when added to the friend’s
effect of .25, implies
that being Spanish-dominant translates to -.03 effect on self-
esteem, meaning that
the less linguistically acculturated Hispanics experience self-
esteem no differently
with friends than with family. Likewise, going down the column
to the schoolwork
context main effect (also in bold), when engaged in schoolwork,
the White, non-
Hispanic, English-dominant group, relative to when with family
(the other referent
group), experience lower self-esteem when engaged in
schoolwork, -.08 (p  .05).
The more interesting coefficients are the interaction terms
within each context and
these will be discussed next.
Assuming that the referent group, by virtue of its much larger
numerical
representation in this data, is a good proxy for host culture (to
which immigrants are
assumed to aspire to acculturate), the main-effect score in this
analysis can stand as
a benchmark to which the different groups may be compared. It
was expected that
within the friends’ context, the two more linguistically
acculturated groups would
show a higher self-esteem with friends than with family by
virtue of associated loss
of familism with acculturation described in the literature.
However, the most
linguistically acculturated Hispanics in this study, like the less
linguistically
acculturated, show no significant differences between being
with friends and being
with family; self-esteem, -.10 (n.s.). They therefore still appear
to have more
positive self-esteem with friends (as do most adolescents), but
this difference is not
significant.
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 219
123
For the less linguistically acculturated, the Spanish-dominant
group for all the
contexts except schoolwork, the effect of self-esteem was
moderated downward,
-.27 (n.s.); in other words, their negative interaction effect
scores suggest that the
group is more inclined to higher self-esteem when with family
than in any other
context but schoolwork. These results imply that the self-esteem
of the less
linguistically acculturated Hispanics is not significantly
different between times
spent with family versus friends. This outcome is noteworthy,
considering that the
effect of familism is likely to be stronger for the less
linguistically acculturated and,
conceivably, that speaking Spanish most often may distance
them from their friends
and classmates if English is the dominant language in these
contexts, as would be
expected in most U.S. cities.
By contrast, the opposite pattern is observed for the somewhat
linguistically
acculturated—those who learned Spanish first, but as
adolescents speak English
most often. It is interesting that the somewhat linguistically
acculturated have higher
self-esteem when with friends than when with family; it is
equally interesting that
the gap in self-esteem between each of the other contexts
(schoolwork, alone, or
with others) and the family context is also large for this group,
and in the same
upward (positive) direction. Data showed an upwardly
moderated effect on self-
esteem (toward the positive) for the somewhat linguistically
acculturated when with
friends, creating a self-esteem gap between friends and family
that is wider than that
of any other linguistic acculturation group. In three of the four
contexts, the
somewhat linguistically acculturated Hispanics exhibit the
widest gaps in self-
esteem relative to when with family than any other group. In
almost all cases, these
effects are statistically significant at p  .05 or better.
It was expected that multiple language participants, having both
ability to speak
English and retention of native language, and by proxy culture,
would afford
multiple language speakers more versatility, enhancing their
self-esteem across
different contexts. However, the data reveal a surprising
pattern. These data showed
speakers of multiple languages to have lower self-esteem with
the friends than with
family context, -.41 (p  .10), in a pattern that is similar but
more pronounced than
that of the least linguistically acculturated Hispanic group.
In sum, this study showed that being with family was associated
with higher self-
esteem for participants at low or at high linguistic acculturation
levels, but not for
those in the middle of the linguistic acculturation process.
Therefore, the idea that
the strength of family weakens with linguistic acculturation
(Sabogal et al. 1987) is
not fully explained or reflected here. The findings of this study
demonstrate that
acculturation and context did moderate self-esteem, but this did
not occur in the
expected direction. Self-esteem, defined by changes in
momentary affective states
as participants move from one context to another, is moderated
by the effect of
linguistic acculturation and context. However, where it had
been expected that self-
esteem would decline with increased linguistic acculturation, it
was found that
Hispanic adolescents at low or high linguistic acculturation
levels experience
juxtaposed patterns of self-esteem with those in the middle of
the acculturation
process. For the least and the most linguistically acculturated,
there is little
difference in self-esteem when with family versus when with
others. Being Hispanic
Spanish-language monolingual and speaking more than one
language moderated the
220 R. M. Perez
123
effect of context on self-esteem in a direction favoring positive
self-esteem with
family. In other words, these groups are more likely to be happy
with both family
and friends. By contrast, those in the middle of the linguistic
acculturation path,
termed the ‘‘somewhat linguistically acculturated,’’ appear to
have higher self-
esteem with anyone other than family.
At the low and high ends of the linguistic acculturation
spectrum—the high and
the low acculturated more than the bilinguals, Hispanic
adolescents are more likely
to experience more person-context cultural congruence when
with their parents/
family. At the low end, those whose first language and current
language preference
was Spanish, are more than likely youth in monolingual families
where (at least
from a linguistic perspective) there is more congruence.
Participants at the high end
of the linguistic acculturation process (i.e., individuals who
reported learning
English first and but currently speak English most of the time)
probably come from
English monolingual families; again, there is language
congruence, because for the
participants to have learned English first, the parents had to
have known how to
speak English. In these instances, the low and high linguistic
acculturation groups’
results showed a clear pattern of linguistic acculturation
moderating the experience
in favor of family rather than friends. For the middle group, the
somewhat
linguistically acculturated, who first learned Spanish but
reported that currently
English is spoken most, results show a marked preference for
time with friends
rather than with family, supporting the idea that cultural
incongruence may be a
factor in the family context.
Discussion
These findings contribute to the literature on the effect of
linguistic acculturation
and context on self-esteem. This study, by showing that
linguistic acculturation and
context moderated self-esteem, suggests that the complex
relationships between the
different contexts in which youth spend time, combined with
their level of linguistic
acculturation, affect their emotional well-being. However,
although it had been
hypothesized that the emotional distance between parents and
children would have
been most evident as acculturation increased, this was not the
case. The patterns
observed in this research were not as predicted by the loss of
family cohesion,
suspected to be at root of many findings in the immigrant health
paradox research—
where declines in well-being have been observed as
acculturation increases. In a
study such as this, where adolescent’s self-esteem was
evaluated in different
contexts relative to when with parents, it had been expected that
youth at higher
levels of linguistic acculturation would have lower self-esteem
with their families
than with their friends by virtue of expected declines in
familism over time, as
orientation to dominant culture and away from the culture of
parents coincides with
acculturation. However, the evidence supports a different idea—
that it is the middle
of the process that more clearly showed a pattern of low self-
esteem with family
than in any other context.
Specific concern for the middle of the process was evident in
these findings. As
compared to low or high linguistic acculturation, participants in
the middle of the
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 221
123
acculturation process experienced significantly higher self-
esteem in any context but
with their families. More research is needed to unpack this
interesting finding. Time
with family is important for socialization. Also, familism,
especially for Hispanic
families (Gonzales et al. 2004) has been identified as a
protective factor for
immigrant youth against depression (Gil-Rivas et al. 2003). The
results of this study
indicate that the parent-youth dynamics of Hispanics in the
middle of the
acculturation process needs to be better understood. The results
support various
explanations. For instance, as posited long ago by Park and
Stonequist, it is possible
that home life is very difficult for Hispanic youth mid-process.
Alternatively,
acculturating Hispanics in the middle of the process may be
adapting to the aspect
of U.S. culture in which familism gives way to individualism.
Or, there may be
tension at home due to the clash of cultures, assuming the
parents are less
acculturated. Unfortunately, this study could not address every
aspect of the
question. Clearly, a major concern is the finding that self-
esteem was consistently
higher in any context but with family. This may be associated
with a parental
inability to socialize children to the culture the parents are most
familiar with and,
lacking that ability, their children may be adopting peer-culture.
Of course, there are many possible alternative explanations to
these findings and
these are related to the study’s limitations. First, it may be that
loss of family
cohesion is but one of many factors that acculturating youth
face. At school, the less
acculturated who prefer to speak Spanish most often may not be
much happier with
family, but rather may feel lower self-esteem with classmates
and friends by virtue
of their lack of mastery of the English language. Likewise, their
more linguistically
acculturated Hispanic peers who, by virtue of speaking English,
attempt to gain
entry into social institutions of the dominant culture, yet may
feel the ill-effects
associated discrimination, alienation, or marginalization that
affects many minority
groups in the United States (Smokowski et al. 2007; Vega and
Aguilar 1995).
Although this study employed rigorous analytical methods and
drew on data with
a large random sample containing repetitive data collection,
considered to have high
reliability, many assumptions were stated regarding the
challenges involved in
assessing the relationship between the variables. In practice,
many other complex-
ities are involved in the study of acculturation and language is
but one variable.
Furthermore, the quality of the interactions as participants go
from one context to
another is not known in its entirety. It may be that the presence
of linguistic
acculturation gaps are exacerbated by mixed immigration status
within the family
and gender/power issues among members, and minimized by
such things as
collegial communication and empathy among members.
Furthermore, the language
of the parents is not known, casting some doubt on the true
effect of person-context
congruence. Finally, before generalizing, one should remember
that the context in
which people live is always changing and the impact of macro-
level factors can
have an impact on the micro-level ones (Perez 2011).
Acculturation research has existed for nearly 100 years, yet
there is still no solid
cross-cultural measure for how to assess this process (Rudmin
2009). At the same
time, there is a relatively recent proliferation of acculturation
research (Rudmin
2009), and some recommend its inclusion in all well-being
research (Lara et al.
2005). In truth, much of the prior acculturation research on
well-being has been
222 R. M. Perez
123
based on language, and despite a growing trend toward
bicultural and multidimen-
sional measures, other researchers continue to advocate for
simple individual-level
proxy measures for acculturation (Escobar and Vega 2000).
Language acquisition
captures a complex process that includes affective, cognitive,
and behavioral
components (Cuéllar et al. 1995), and has been shown to be
influential in more
complex acculturation measures (Rogler et al. 1991). Accurately
capturing a
complex and dynamic process such as acculturation with a
single measure is likely
to remain difficult considering the large number of
multidirectional factors affecting
the process. Although the findings of this research help to point
out the important
effect of context together with acculturation, it is clear that
more research is needed
to disentangle these important effects among the large and
growing sector of
immigrant children the United States.
Implications
These findings underscore the need to better understand the
effects of linguistic
acculturation on self-esteem and the practical utility of a fixed-
effects model.
Evidence from this study showed that context and language-use
moderated self-
esteem. Essentially, the participant in the middle had better
self-esteem in any
context but with family. Although the times have changed from
the days when Park
and Stonequist wrote about the marginal man, it is interesting
that it was adolescents
in the middle of the process—those whose first language was
Spanish and whose
second language was English—who were different from the
other two acculturation
levels with respect to their families. Clearly, more research is
needed before we can
determine the benefits—or drawbacks—of the relationship
between their families of
Hispanic youth and their self-esteem relative to other contexts
in which they spend
time.
Research suggests that Hispanics will continue to present
varying levels of
language use for years to come—probably retaining their native
language and
culture longer than other immigrant group. Thus, the
implications of this study for
policy, practice, and research are critical given the size and
growth rate of this
population, in addition to their continual population
replenishment from Latin
America (Jiménez 2008), and their transnational (Levitt 2003),
circular (Massey
1987), and undocumented patterns of migration.
Research
This study also has implications for future research. The
combination of a fixed-
effects model with a unique data-collection method, ESM,
afforded an unusual and
beneficial research design that allowed for robust cross-cultural
comparisons. Cross-
cultural research is often challenged by the difficulty of
comparing dissimilar
people. Fixed-effects models have the advantage of being able
to hold background
effects constant so that cross-cultural comparisons are less
prone to selection bias.
A second implication for research, regarding the effects of
linguistic acculturation
on development, is that more understanding of the dynamic
between immigrant
youth at the different levels of linguistic acculturation and their
families, is
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 223
123
warranted. Another possibly fruitful path for future research is
better understanding
the effects of language on the brain or, more generally, on
healthy development. The
multiple-languages group was expected to be relatively
indifferent about where they
spent time. It was predicted that participants who spoke
multiple languages would
experience few or no self-esteem differences across the contexts
in which they
spend time, by virtue of having learned enough and having
enough resources to
cope in a variety of contexts. The literature on
multicompetence, in particular,
shows that the structures of the brain develop differently when
individuals learn
more than one language at the same time (Kim et al. 1997).
These differences are
hypothesized to lead to higher levels of cognition, in that
multilingual people have
access to additional linguistic knowledge not otherwise possible
(Han 2004;
Pavlenko 2003).
Finally, it is hoped that cross-cultural researchers who work
with panel data
containing multiple measures per person see the advantage of
the fixed-effects
model. The fixed-effects model as applied to the panel data
(i.e., repetitive sampling
technique) used in this study allowed us to control for
background differences. This
was advantageous to reduce bias. This model controls selection
bias by way of the
demeaning process, which assesses each person’s changes from
his or her first
recorded response so that each person serves as his or her own
control, and only
variables that change from response to response are included.
Because each person
serves as his or her own base of comparison, the variables that
do not change over
time are simply held constant and thus do not bias the results.
In this way,
observable variables that do not vary for a person (e.g., gender,
grade, socioeco-
nomic status) fall out of the model because nothing changes
from response signal to
response signal; in this way they are implicitly controlled.
Unobservable variables
are also held constant. This is useful for controlling such things
as aspects of
personality, which can predispose people to prefer some
contexts or activities and to
experience particular affect. For example, an extroverted person
may have a low
self-esteem when alone, whereas an introverted person may be
much happier alone.
Practice
For practitioners working with young people learning a second
language, it is
critical to understand that important associated cultural
differences to their linguistic
differences in how they relate with family and friends and also
how they affect self-
esteem. Human services staff working with Hispanic youth may,
first and foremost,
want to assess the degree to which the family is experiencing
linguistic
acculturation gaps, and also to what extent these are merely a
nuisance rather
than causes of problematic behaviors. Linguistic acculturation
in this study was
treated as a moderator of self-esteem; in other words, there are
more than likely
additional familial and cultural issues that underlie problem
behaviors. Language
use, and more specifically, linguistic retention has been found
to be protective
factors bridging gaps between parents and youth and
contributing to global self-
worth (Birman 1998). For among more acculturated adolescents
more understand-
ing is needed to regarding the more problematic areas
negatively affecting
224 R. M. Perez
123
well-being—whether self-esteem, family conflict, or something
else that is not fully
understood as of yet.
Social workers may use the information found in this study and
others like it to
help bridge linguistic acculturation gaps between immigrant
parents and accultu-
rating offspring as has been found helpful by some practitioners
working with
troubled Hispanic adolescents (Szapocznik et al. 1986).
Exploring bridges between
youth and parental culture during adolescence can help
facilitate parental
socialization. Knowing that most immigrant youth progress
from low to high
linguistic acculturation, it may be worthwhile to explore how
benefits of cultural
retention (e.g., familismo, respeto) could be better preserved, so
that those in the
middle of the process do not feel estranged from their parental
culture.
Alternatively, children who do not feel positive self-esteem
with parents may not
be able to receive important enculturation and if they seek their
friends out they may
not benefit from the quality understanding that their parents are
better able to
transmit.
Policy
Regarding policy, there is currently no concerted federal effort
to incorporate
newcomers, linguistically or otherwise, into U.S. society
(Jiménez 2007). Further-
more, the problematic adaptation of more acculturated Hispanic
youth, as posited by
evidence from the immigrant health paradox, suggest that
macro-level interventions
led by the U.S. government would be justified (Perez 2011).
Addressing the needs of
Hispanic youth at the age most crucial to the transition to
adulthood can be seen as
an investment.
References
Alegrı́a, M., Canino, G., Shrout, P. E., Woo, M., Duan, N.,
Vila, D., et al. (2008). Prevalence of mental
illness in immigrant and non-immigrant U.S. Latino groups.
American Journal of Psychiatry,
165(3), 359–369.
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Kwak, K., & Sam, D. L. (2006).
Introduction: Goals and research framework
for studying immigrant youth. In J. W. Berry, J. S. Phinney, D.
L. Sam, & P. Vedder (Eds.),
Immigrant youth in cultural transition: Acculturation, identity,
and adaptation across national
contexts (pp. 1–14). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Birman, D. (1998). Biculturalism and perceived competence of
Latino immigrant adolescents. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 26(3), 335–354.
Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2008). Does
adolescent self-esteem predict later life
outcomes? A test of the causal role of self-esteem. Development
& Psychopathology, 20(1),
319–339.
Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2007). Knowledge of child
development and family interactions among
immigrants to America: Perspectives from developmental
science. In J. E. Lansford, K. Deater-
Deckard, & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Immigrant families in
contemporary society (pp. 121–136).
New York: Guilford Press.
Brandstätter, H. (1994). Well-being and motivational person-
environment fit: A time-sampling study of
emotions. European Journal of Personality, 8(2), 75–93.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context
for human development: Research
perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742.
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 225
123
Cafferty, P. S. J. (1985). Language and social assimilation. In
P. S. J. Cafferty & W. McGready (Eds.),
Hispanics in the United States: A new social agenda. New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988).
Optimal experience: Psychological
studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent:
Conflict and growth in the teenage years.
New York: Basic Books.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and
reliability of the experience-sampling method.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 525–536.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schneider, B. (Eds.). (2000).
Becoming adult: How teenagers prepare for the
world of work. New York: Basic Books.
Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation
rating scale for Mexican Americans-II:
A revision of the original ARSMA Scale. Hispanic Journal of
Behavioral Sciences, 17(3), 275–304.
Dennis, J., Basañez, T., & Farahmand, A. (2010).
Intergenerational conflicts among Latinos in early
adulthood: Separating values conflicts with parents from
acculturation conflicts. Hispanic Journal of
Behavioral Sciences, 32(1), 118.
Emmons, R. A., Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Choice of
situations and congruence models of
interactionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 6(6),
693–702.
Escobar, J. I., & Vega, W. A. (2000). Mental health and
immigration’s AAAs: Where are we and where
do we go from here? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
188(11), 736–740.
Fillmore, L. W. (1991). When learning a second language means
losing the first. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 6(3), 323–346.
Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study
of stress and competence in children:
A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child
Development, 55, 97–111.
Gil-Rivas, V., Greenberger, E., Chen, C., & Montero y Lopez-
Lena, M. (2003). Understanding depressed
mood in the context of a family-oriented culture. Adolescence,
38(149), 93.
Gonzales, N. A., Knight, G. P., Birman, D., & Sirolli, A. A.
(2004). Acculturation and enculturation
among Latino youth. In K. I. Maton, C. J. Schellenbach, B. J.
Leadbeater, & A. L. Solarz (Eds.),
Investing in children, youth, families, and communities:
Strengths-based research and policy
(pp. 285–302). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Gove, W. R., Style, C. B., & Hughes, M. (1990). The effect of
marriage on the well-being of adults:
A theoretical analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 11(1), 4–35.
Greenman, E., & Xie, Y. (2008). Is assimilation theory dead?
The effect of assimilation on adolescent
well-being. Social Science Research, 37(1), 109–137.
Hafner, A., Ingels, S., Schneider, B., & Stevenson, D. (1990). A
profile of American eighth-grade
mathematics and science instruction: National Education
Longitudinal Study of 1988. Washington,
DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Halgunseth, L., Ispa, J., & Rudy, D. (2006). Parental control in
Latino families: An integrated review of
the literature. Child Development, 77(5), 1282–1297.
Han, Z. (2004). Review of effects of the second language on the
first. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 26(3), 488–489.
Harker, K. (2001). Immigrant generation, assimilation, and
adolescent psychological well being. Social
Forces, 79, 970–1004.
Hektner, J. M., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Bechtel, R. B., &
Churchman, A. (2002). The experience sampling
method: Measuring the context and the content of lives
Handbook of environmental psychology (pp.
233–243). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experiences in situ.
Journal of Personality, 54(1), 262–293.
Hunter, J. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). The positive
psychology of interested adolescents. Journal
of Youth & Adolescence, 32(1), 27–35.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New
York: Henry Holt.
Jiménez, T. (2007). From newcomers to Americans: An
integration policy for a nation of immigrants.
Immigration Policy in Focus, 5(11). Retrieved from
www.immigrationpolicy.org.
Jiménez, T. (2008). Mexican immigrant replenishment and the
continuing significance of ethnicity and
race. American Journal of Sociology, 113(6), 1527–1567.
Kim, K. H. S., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K.-M., & Hirsch, J. (1997).
Distinct cortical areas associated with
native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171–174.
Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: A review of
intergenerational family relations for
immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development,
46(2–3), 15–136.
226 R. M. Perez
123
http://www.immigrationpolicy.org
Lang, J. G., Muñoz, R. F., Bernal, G., & Sorensen, J. L. (1982).
Quality of life and psychological well-
being in a bicultural Latino community. Hispanic Journal of
Behavioral Sciences, 4(4), 433–450.
Lara, M., Gamboa, C., Kahramanian, M. I., Morales, L. S., &
Bautista, D. E. H. (2005). Acculturation and
Latino health in the United States: A review of the literature
and its sociopolitical context. Annual
Review of Public Health, 26, 367–397.
Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980). The significance of
time alone in adolescent development.
Journal of Current Adolescent Medicine, 2, 33–40.
Larson, R., & Lee, M. (1996). The capacity to be alone as a
stress buffer. Journal of Social Psychology,
136, 5–16.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and
coping. New York: Springer.
Leondari, A. (2001). The impact of acculturation on immigrant
children’s self-perceptions, feelings of
loneliness and social status. Educational & Child Psychology,
18(4), 35–46.
Levitt, P. (2003). Transnational villagers. In J. Stone & D.
Rutledge (Eds.), Race and ethnicity:
Comparative and theoretical approaches (p. 424). Malden, MA:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Luna, I., de Ardon, E. T., Lim, Y. M., Cromwell, S. L., Phillips,
L. R., & Russell, C. K. (1996).
The relevance of familism in cross-cultural studies of family
caregiving. Western Journal of Nursing
Research, 18(3), 267–283.
Marsiglia, F., Parsai, M., & Kulis, S. (2009). Effects of
familism and family cohesion on problem
behaviors among adolescents in Mexican immigrant families in
the southwest United States. Journal
of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work, 18(3), 18.
Massey, D. (1987). Understanding Mexican migration to the
United States. American Journal of
Sociology, 92(6), 1372–1403.
Mendoza, F. S., Javier, J. R., & Burgos, A. E. (2007). Health of
children in immigrant families.
In J. E. Lansford, K. Deater-Deckard, & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.),
Immigrant families in
contemporary society (pp. 30–50). New York: Guilford Press.
Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of
perceived challenges and skills on the
quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64(2),
276–310.
Pabon, E. (1998). Hispanic adolescent delinquency and the
family: A discussion of sociocultural
influences. Adolescence, 33(132), 941–955.
Pavlenko, A. (2003). ‘‘I never knew I was a bilingual’’:
Reimagining teacher identities in TESOL. Journal
of Language, Identity, & Education, 2(4), 251–268.
Pawlak, J. L., & Klein, H. A. (1997). Parental conflict and self-
esteem: The rest of the story. Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 158(3), 303–313.
Perez, R. M. (2011). Latino mental health: Acculturation
challenges in service provision. In L. P. Buki &
L. M. Piedra (Eds.), Haciendo camino al andar: Creating
infrastructures for Latino mental health.
New York: Springer.
Piedra, L. M., & Engstrom, D. W. (2009). Segmented
assimilation theory and the Life Model: An
integrated approach to understanding immigrants and their
children. Social Work, 54(3), 270–277.
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A
portrait (3rd ed.). Berkeley & Los Angeles:
University of California Press.
Portes, P. R., & Zady, M. F. (2002). Self-esteem in the
adaptation of Spanish-speaking adolescents:
The role of immigration, family conflict and depression.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences,
24(3), 296–318.
Prescott, S., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1981).
Environmental effects on cognitive and affective
states: The experiential time sampling approach. Social
Behavior & Personality, 9(1), 23–32.
Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovits, M. J. (1936).
Memorandum on the study of acculturation.
American Anthropologist, 38, 149–152.
Rogler, L. H., Cortes, D. E., & Malgady, R. G. (1991).
Acculturation and mental health status among
Hispanics: Convergence and new directions for research.
American Psychologist, 46, 585–597.
Rudmin, F. W. (2009). Constructs, measurements and models of
acculturation and acculturative stress.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33(2), 106–123.
Rudmin, F. W. (2010). Acculturation measures. In E. Mpofu &
T. Oakland (Eds.), Rehabilitation and
health assessment: Applying ICF guidelines (pp. 353–379). New
York: Springer.
Rumbaut, R. G. (1997). Assimilation and its discontents:
Between rhetoric and reality. International
Migration Review, 31, 923–960.
Sabogal, F., Marı́n, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., & Marı́n, B. V.
(1987). Hispanic familism and acculturation:
What changes and what doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
Sciences, 9(4), 397–412.
Language & Context on Self-Esteem 227
123
Sameroff, A. (1995). General system theories and
developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti &
D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmenal psychopathology. Vol. 1:
Theory and methods. New York: Wiley.
Shernoff, D. J., Knauth, S., & Makris, E. (2000). The quality of
classroom experiences. In
M. Csikszentmihalyi & B. Schneider (Eds.), Becoming adult
(pp. 141–164). New York: Basic
Books.
Smokowski, P. R., & Bacallao, M. L. (2007). Acculturation,
internalizing mental health symptoms, and
self-esteem: Cultural experiences of Latino adolescents in North
Carolina. Child Psychiatry and
Human Development, 37(3), 273–292.
Smokowski, P. R., Chapman, M. V., & Bacallao, M. L. (2007).
Acculturation risk and protective factors
and mental health symptoms in immigrant Latino adolescents.
Journal of Human Behavior in the
Social Environment, 16(3), 33–55.
Smokowski, P. R., Rose, R., & Bacallao, M. L. (2008).
Acculturation and Latino family processes: How
cultural involvement, biculturalism, and acculturation gaps
influence family dynamics. Family
Relations, 57(3), 295–308.
Szapocznik, J., Rio, A., Perez-Vidal, A., Kurtines, W., Hervis,
O., & Santisteban, D. (1986). Bicultural
Effectiveness Training (BET): An experimental test of an
intervention modality for families
experiencing intergenerational/intercultural conflict. Hispanic
Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 8(4),
303–330.
Turner, R., & Gil, A. (2002). Psychiatric and substance use
disorders in South Florida: Racial/ethnic and
gender contrasts in a young adult cohort. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 59(1), 43.
Vega, L., & Aguilar, B. (1995). Differential effects of
discrimination on the ethnic identity of Mexican
Americans: The role of the person and the situation. PhD,
University of California, Santa Cruz, CA.
Vega, W. A., & Alegrı́a, M. (2001). Latino mental health and
treatment in the United States. Health
Issues in the Latino Community (pp. 179–208). San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
Vega, W. A., Khoury, E. L., Zimmerman, R. S., & Gil, A. G.
(1995). Cultural conflicts and problem
behaviors of Latino adolescents in home and school
environments. Journal of Community
Psychology, 23(2), 167–179.
Ward, C. (1996). Acculturation. In D. Landis & R. Bhagat
(Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (2nd
ed., pp. 124–147). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Weiner, B. (2000). Intrapersonal and interpersonal theories of
motivation from an attributional
perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 12(1), 1–14.
Weiss, U. K., & Tillman, K. H. (2009). Risky sexual behaviors
among hispanic young adults in South
Florida: Nativity, age at immigration and gender differences.
Perspectives on Sexual and
Reproductive Health, 41(4), 202–209.
Wickrama, K., Elder, G., & Abraham, W. (2007). Rurality and
ethnicity in adolescent physical illness:
Are children of the growing rural Latino population at excess
health risk? Journal of Rural Health,
23(3), 228–237.
Wooldridge, J. M. (2006). Introductory econometrics: A modern
approach (3rd ed.). Mason, OH:
Thompson South-Western.
Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescent relations with
mothers, fathers, and friends. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
228 R. M. Perez
123
Copyright of Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal is the
property of Springer Science & Business Media
B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple
sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

More Related Content

Similar to Linguistic Acculturation and Context on Self-EsteemHispanic.docx

Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...
Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...
Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...Driessen Research
 
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docx
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docxMarquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docx
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docxinfantsuk
 
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof Affluence
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof AffluenceLuthar(2003)The Cultureof Affluence
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof AffluenceJudy Gressel
 
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation Proposal
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation ProposalA Sample Qualitative Dissertation Proposal
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation ProposalClaire Webber
 
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docx
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docxJournal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docx
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docxtawnyataylor528
 
VU RESEARCH PROPOSAL
VU RESEARCH PROPOSALVU RESEARCH PROPOSAL
VU RESEARCH PROPOSALLinh Vu
 
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015sadtono
 
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docxJournal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docxcroysierkathey
 
Gender Socialisation Gender
Gender Socialisation GenderGender Socialisation Gender
Gender Socialisation GenderMarisela Stone
 
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special IssueAdvances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special IssueKatie Naple
 
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docx
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docxJournal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docx
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docxcroysierkathey
 
Article - National FORUM Journals
Article - National FORUM JournalsArticle - National FORUM Journals
Article - National FORUM JournalsWilliam Kritsonis
 
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docx
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docxRunning Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docx
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docxcharisellington63520
 
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUr
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUrExamining the role of parental factors on depression amongUr
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUrBetseyCalderon89
 
Social factors and second language acquistion
Social factors and second language acquistionSocial factors and second language acquistion
Social factors and second language acquistionBibi Halima
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docx
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docxAssessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docx
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docxdavezstarr61655
 
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learningUniversitas Kanjuruhan Malang
 
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxAttitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxrock73
 
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxAttitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxcelenarouzie
 

Similar to Linguistic Acculturation and Context on Self-EsteemHispanic.docx (20)

Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...
Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...
Geert Driessen (2002) SEE School composition and achievement in primary educa...
 
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docx
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docxMarquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docx
Marquette University[email protected]Psychology Faculty Re.docx
 
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof Affluence
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof AffluenceLuthar(2003)The Cultureof Affluence
Luthar(2003)The Cultureof Affluence
 
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation Proposal
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation ProposalA Sample Qualitative Dissertation Proposal
A Sample Qualitative Dissertation Proposal
 
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docx
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docxJournal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docx
Journal of Family Issues2017, Vol. 38(7) 904 –920© The A.docx
 
VU RESEARCH PROPOSAL
VU RESEARCH PROPOSALVU RESEARCH PROPOSAL
VU RESEARCH PROPOSAL
 
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
 
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docxJournal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
 
Gender Socialisation Gender
Gender Socialisation GenderGender Socialisation Gender
Gender Socialisation Gender
 
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special IssueAdvances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
 
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docx
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docxJournal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docx
Journal of Youth and Adolescence (2019) 48864–875httpsdo.docx
 
Article - National FORUM Journals
Article - National FORUM JournalsArticle - National FORUM Journals
Article - National FORUM Journals
 
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docx
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docxRunning Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docx
Running Head Racial DiscriminationHouston 6Racial Discrim.docx
 
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUr
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUrExamining the role of parental factors on depression amongUr
Examining the role of parental factors on depression amongUr
 
Social factors and second language acquistion
Social factors and second language acquistionSocial factors and second language acquistion
Social factors and second language acquistion
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docx
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docxAssessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docx
Assessing low-income African-American pre-schoolers’behaviou.docx
 
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
1. the state of the art of secondary esl teaching and learning
 
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxAttitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
 
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docxAttitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
Attitude Similarity in Three-Generation Families Socializatio.docx
 

More from SHIVA101531

Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxAnswer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxAnswer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxAnswer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxAnswer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxAnswer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxAnswer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxAnswer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxAnswer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxAnswer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxAnswer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxAnswer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxAnswer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxAnswer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxAnswer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxAnswer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxAnswer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxAnswer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxAnswer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxAnswer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxSHIVA101531
 
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxSHIVA101531
 

More from SHIVA101531 (20)

Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxAnswer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
 
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxAnswer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
 
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxAnswer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
 
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxAnswer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
 
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxAnswer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
 
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxAnswer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
 
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxAnswer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
 
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxAnswer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
 
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxAnswer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
 
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxAnswer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
 
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxAnswer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
 
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxAnswer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
 
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxAnswer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
 
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxAnswer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
 
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxAnswer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
 
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxAnswer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
 
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxAnswer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
 
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxAnswer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
 
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxAnswer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
 
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 

Linguistic Acculturation and Context on Self-EsteemHispanic.docx

  • 1. Linguistic Acculturation and Context on Self-Esteem: Hispanic Youth Between Cultures Rose M. Perez Published online: 16 February 2011 � Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract Immigrant adolescents must negotiate two cultures: the host culture and their native culture. This study explored how self-esteem is moderated by the effect of linguistic acculturation and context. An ordinary least- squares regression model, controlling for fixed effects, produced results supporting the hypothesis that linguistic acculturation moderates the effect of context on self- esteem. The self- esteem of Hispanic adolescents who were less linguistically acculturated was found to be more favorable when with family than with friends and the reverse was found for the more linguistically acculturated participants. Adolescents in the middle of
  • 2. the linguistic acculturation process had the widest variance in self-esteem between times they were with their families and times in other contexts; they experienced more positive self-esteem with anyone but family. Findings underscore the need to better understand the complex process of linguistic acculturation and its effects on self-esteem. This research also demonstrates the practical utility of a fixed-effects model for reducing bias in cross-cultural research. Keywords Linguistic acculturation � Hispanics � Fixed effects � Self-esteem � Experience Sampling Method (ESM) Hispanic immigrants and their children are not only the fastest growing population in this country, but they are also among the poorest (Portes and Rumbaut 2006). The author welcomes communication at the Fordham University Graduate School of Social Service. She wishes to acknowledge the dissertation committee who helped guide completion of a doctoral dissertation on which this article is based. R. M. Perez (&)
  • 3. Fordham University Graduate School of Social Service, 113 West 60th Street, New York, NY 10023, USA e-mail: [email protected] 123 Child Adolesc Soc Work J (2011) 28:203–228 DOI 10.1007/s10560-011-0228-y For Hispanic youth, the pathway to higher social mobility is fraught with formidable obstacles, like discrimination and suboptimal inner-city schools, that render them unprepared for the challenges of the labor market, and it appears that, counter to traditional patterns of immigrant incorporation, they are experiencing downward adjustment. This becomes a problem for Hispanics, and for U.S. society as a whole, given the increasing size of the Hispanic population. If Hispanic educational and economic indicators do not improve, as their proportions grow, poverty in the United States will grow correspondingly.
  • 4. Redfield et al. (1936) defined acculturation as the ‘‘phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with subsequent changes in the original cultural patterns of either or both groups.’’ Yet, time in the United States is typically associated with second-culture acquisition (Rudmin 2010), part of a complex and stressful process (Ward 1996). For Hispanic youth, acculturation often occurs in a context influenced by, often hostile environments that are not easy to navigate. Arguably, the multifaceted process of acculturation can add to the already tumultuous emotions experienced in adolescence and may impact self-esteem. As a result, immigrant youth acculturate in different ways, influenced in large part by the contexts with which they interact (Piedra and Engstrom 2009). The contextual changes that young immigrants face in the normal course of their lives may vary widely and if these contexts are culturally
  • 5. distant from one another, the cultural norms between them can be difficult to reconcile. For instance, a young person who has little mastery of the English language may not have a positive sense of self when in the company of more acculturated peers or classmates. In those cases, they may feel closer to their families with whom they are likely to be more culturally matched (e.g., how they value the importance of family) and despite the generational differences. Poor self-esteem has been associated problem behaviors, like dropping out of school and poor social and behavioral adjustment (Weiner 2000). More broadly, a recent trend, referred to as the ‘‘immigrant health paradox’’ is based on, a consistent, and growing body of evidence mostly from the field of epidemiology suggests that acculturated adolescents experience lower levels of well-being than less accultur- ated counterparts in the United States (Harker 2001; Mendoza et al. 2007; Rumbaut
  • 6. 1997). Although findings of this paradox are too numerous to cite and largely focused on Mexicans (Alegrı́a et al. 2008), consider that in a sample of Hispanic adolescents, acculturation has been associated with higher substance use (Greenman and Xie 2008; Turner and Gil 2002), chronic illness (Wickrama et al. 2007), and lower risky sexual behaviors (Weiss and Tillman 2009). Vega and Alegrı́a (2001) found some support to suggest that Hispanics have better mental health upon arrival than after achieving higher levels of acculturation. Researchers exploring the possible reasons for negative association between acculturation and well-being often include loss of family resources such as conflicts within families stemming from acculturation gaps between parents and their children as factors shown to reduce family cohesion (Smokowski et al. 2007, 2008). As Hispanics, defined as people who trace their ancestry to countries that share a common language, history,
  • 7. and certain cultural traits, which may or may not carry across the generations as the peoples acculturate (Cafferty 1985), continue to increase in size it is important to 204 R. M. Perez 123 understand their continuing problematic patterns of acculturation, which run counter to the popular notion that immigrants are better off acculturating. Today more is known about the important role that acculturation plays as a moderator of the well-being of Hispanic youth, and it is acknowledged that full assimilation into mainstream U.S. life does not take place in just one generation. The relationship between acculturation and self-esteem has been found to vary in myriad ways (Berry et al. 2006; Bornstein and Cote 2007; Lang et al. 1982). Much of the variation in findings may be attributed to methodological differences in how
  • 8. acculturation is conceptualized and measured (Rudmin 2009). As an individual- level proxy for acculturation, language acquisition occurs quickly, especially in young people. Furthermore, it captures a complex process that includes affective, cognitive, and behavioral components (Cuéllar et al. 1995). Language fluency has been shown to account for a large portion of the variance in multiple-item measures (Rogler et al. 1991) and it continues to function as the most broadly used measure of cultural practice. All acculturation scales, whether complex or simple, are imperfect in some ways. Escobar and Vega (2000), after reviewing the literature, recommend using individual measures (e.g., language and length of stay) rather than multidimensional ones with uncertain value, as is currently the case in the field, until a single acculturation measure is fully validated scientifically. Because little is known about a young Hispanic’s well-being as they interact with
  • 9. different and probably widely differing environments, this study set out to understand the inter-relationships between acculturation, the contexts in which youth interact, and their effect on self-esteem. Data collected using the Experience Sampling Method, a repetitive sampling technique that assessed adolescents’ self- esteem in many different contexts throughout the course of one week, is analyzed in this study to argue that acculturation and context moderate self- esteem. Using advanced statistical design that allowed background differences to be held constant to reduce bias contributes to the literature on better understanding the role of acculturation, context, and self-esteem across groups dissimilar in many socio- economic dimensions (e.g., parental income, personality, age, and gender). This study can assist policy makers and practitioners to better position programs and policies in ways that better reflect on the experiences and needs of this population.
  • 10. Person-Context Perspective The effects on well-being from interactions between people and the contexts in which time is spent are important aspects of ecologically valid assessment (Bronfenbrenner 1986). Ecologically valid assessment views a person’s well-being holistically, part of a broader system related to the environment of which he or she is a part (Sameroff 1995). Ecological theorizing is a core social- work perspective. The perspective of person-context interaction congruence is predicated on the idea that people’s well-being will be better when they are living in contexts in which they feel there is a good fit (Emmons et al. 1985). For example, if youth are happy when in the context of family they are likely to benefit from socialization better than if they were in conflict with their parents on a regular basis. The person-context Language & Context on Self-Esteem 205 123
  • 11. congruence perspective is a helpful theoretical lens through which one can observe the effect of acculturation and context on self-esteem. The importance of studying complex person-in-an-environment effects on well-being is highlighted in the literature on stress and coping (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). For example, adolescents who are better able to focus their attention on challenging and productive activities have been found to be more resilient than those who cannot (Garmezy et al. 1984). Therefore, if there is congruence between spending time with friends and acculturation level, an adolescent’s self-esteem should be positive relative to other contexts in which he or she spends time. For instance, if adolescents are interested in fitting into peer culture, and they feel accepted when in the context of peers, it is expected that they will report positive self- esteem.
  • 12. As Hispanic immigrant youth acculturate to U.S. life, they face many linguistic and cultural challenges along the way and sometimes these adversely affect their well-being—including self-esteem. Hispanic youth upon arrival struggle to adapt to the culture and language of the United States and during this time they must reconcile among the different contexts in which they spend time. Usually by virtue of being in school, younger individuals learn the language of the host culture relatively easily and adapt rapidly, although acculturation can be moderated by the surrounding cultures of the communities in which they live. Parents are typically slower to learn English. Cultural and linguistic retention are likely to be of more practical value in communities where the immigrant’s native language is widely spoken. Among immigrant adolescents in the United States, unless they are recently arrived the preferred language is typically English. For many, learning a second
  • 13. language means losing their first (Fillmore 1991). If their first language was Spanish, it is possible that their parents are bilingual or have yet to learn English. As a result, language use is an effective proxy measure. For example, youth who speak Spanish are likely to have arrived recently in the United States. Among those whose first language was Spanish, they are likely to be speaking English after a few years in the United States, but their parents are likely to retain Spanish longer and, by proxy, most likely their culture as well. Youth whose first language was English likely have highly acculturated parents with whom they are probably culturally matched. Context and Self-Esteem The important role that context—defined as time spent with people such as family, friends, at school, with others, or alone—plays in the lives of immigrant youth cannot be understated. Immigrant youth on any given day can enter into many
  • 14. different types of contexts ranging from situations that require varying levels of acculturation. For instance, with parents who are less acculturated and who do not speak English, they must accommodate to their acculturation level. A reverse situation more than likely occurs at school, where the language of instruction is typically English. As youth enter and exit these widely different contexts they must on their own reconcile their beliefs and ideas and make decisions about which set of 206 R. M. Perez 123 norms and values they will adopt. This can be terribly confusing during times where the contexts differ significantly from one another. Self-esteem is one of many variables that are often used to assess well-being among youth. However, self-esteem has been assessed as a global measure or one
  • 15. that is context-dependent. This study focuses on the latter definition, where less is known. A context-dependent measure of self-esteem means that assessing it can vary greatly depending on the circumstances that individuals are experiencing when they are being assessed. James (1890) first identified the importance of context regarding self-esteem when he described it as the ratio of expectations to achievement. In this definition, self-esteem fluctuates according to the particular conditions of the context, or state. An individual’s perceived achievement of a goal is related to the importance that the individual places on achievement of that particular goal. Hence, under this definition, self-esteem depends on the frame of reference, including the context, where the subjective judgments that individuals use to evaluate their accomplishments take place. In this two-part process, the individual’s perception of success or failure to reach goals influences the outcome. Thus, lowered expectations can increase self-esteem, and
  • 16. expectations that are too high can result in unfulfilled accomplishments and therefore lower self-esteem. Students with higher self-esteem are believed to be better adjusted, be more accomplished in school, and have close, trusting relationships with their parents (Gove et al. 1990). The level of acculturation and contexts in which youth spend time play an important role in how youth feel about themselves. Psychologists and sociologists have used self-esteem measures to evaluate levels of well-being between immigrants and nonimmigrants and between immigrants of different generations (Berry et al. 2006; Leondari 2001). Some studies have explored the relationship between the level of acculturation and global self-esteem; however, findings have been inconclusive. The effects of contextual factors on self- esteem warrant consideration (Boden et al. 2008). For example, in the context of the Hispanic
  • 17. family, family cohesion has been observed to weaken with acculturation to U.S. life (Sabogal et al. 1987). A consequence of family conflict as a young person adapts to U.S. life contributes to lowered well-being, including self- esteem (Portes and Zady 2002; Smokowski and Bacallao 2007). In families, self-esteem can be used to detect the presence or absence of conflict, although conflict within a family is a difficult variable to define and measure (Pawlak and Klein 1997). Vega et al. (1995) reported that both low- and high-acculturation groups experienced conflict related to language use. The less acculturated reported experiencing stress both at home and at school. The more acculturated reported experiencing language conflicts, perceiving discrimination, and feeling that they were in a closed society. The authors speculated that the native-born/high-acculturation group might perceive discrimination to be a
  • 18. greater hindrance to their desire to succeed than the less acculturated group. Success is an integral part of the self-esteem equation as defined by James (1890). The importance of family as a perceived source of support has been found to be robust across different Hispanic subgroups (e.g., Mexicans, Cubans, and Central Language & Context on Self-Esteem 207 123 Americans) (Sabogal et al. 1987) but has been found to manifest differently between Anglo and Mexican cultures (Luna et al. 1996). Familism, which refers to the within-family support systems is typically identified as protective factors for young Hispanics. Family cohesion has been found to be a protective factor against adolescent acting out (e.g., conduct problems, aggressive behavior, and rule- breaking) (Marsiglia et al. 2009). Hispanics who follow
  • 19. familismo and retain aspects of native culture are believed to enjoy closer family bonds in a collective and respectful orientation. Respeto, or respect for elders, is an example of another Hispanic/U.S. cultural difference. Acculturating Hispanic youth may offend their elders if they internalize the U.S. norm, which emphasizes the individual’s rights and autonomy. A problematic aspect of acculturation is that often younger family members outpace older ones by learning the language of the host society more rapidly, something that facilitates their access to societal institutions. Asymmetrical acculturation such as this can introduce an acculturation gap with the potential for family discord (Kwak 2003; Portes and Rumbaut 2006; Szapocznik et al. 1986) and increased intergenerational conflict (Szapocznik et al. 1986). Acculturation gaps in Hispanic families have been found to be inversely associated with familial obligation (Sabogal et al. 1987) and family cohesion
  • 20. (Smokowski et al. 2008). Parent-adolescent conflict, which has been found to increase over subsequent immigrant generations has been shown to moderate the negative association between acculturation and self-esteem (Dennis et al. 2010). Indeed, when children acculturate faster than their parents, as often happens, conflicts in the family occur, creating additional stressors for its members (Szapocznik et al. 1986). Prior studies found that adolescents in the United States allocate more of their time to their friends than to their families. They also reported that they enjoyed the time they spent with their friends much more than the time they spent with their families. Although this is likely to be normative behavior (and attitude) in adolescence, it is of concern because it may get in the way of receiving needed sanctions from parents, which is a necessary part of the socialization process (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984). For instance, some
  • 21. research has found lack of delinquency related to time spent with family on weekends (Pabon 1998), and, more generally, parental monitoring (Halgunseth et al. 2006). Of course, it should be noted that some research has found that peers can play a positive role in the lives of adolescents (Youniss and Smollar 1985), and this can be an important source of self- esteem. Thus, the existing literature has not focused much on understanding how Hispanic adolescents differ on the amount of time they spend across different contexts and this too will be explored in this study. Other contexts besides family and friends have also been found to affect the self- esteem of young people (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984; Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider 2000). Some studies found students to be unhappy or disengaged, whereas others found that Hispanic students, more than any other ethnic/racial group, experience higher levels of enjoyment and affect (measured by scores on a
  • 22. scale measuring sociable, proud, happy, and relaxed) in school (Shernoff et al. 2000). Time spent alone is not associated with positive emotions (Hunter and Csikszentmihalyi 2003; Larson and Csikszentmihalyi 1980), but it can act as a stress 208 R. M. Perez 123 buffer (Larson and Lee 1996) and arguably is needed to engage in productive tasks (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984). For introverted personality types, spending time alone has been found to be more problematic than for the extroverted (Brandstätter 1994). Summary and Hypothesis This study is motivated by the important and growing concern of the downward effects of acculturation (and its concomitant constructs, lowered family cohesion and conflict) on the well-being of Hispanic adolescents. Using
  • 23. the lens of person- context congruence to understand the dynamic changes in self- esteem correspond- ing with changes in the contexts where adolescents spend time, this research set out to better understand whether acculturation and context together moderate self- esteem. There have been studies that explored the effects of acculturation on self- esteem and there are studies that support the idea that self- esteem has state and trait qualities. However, not much is known about how self-esteem changes in response to changes in context together with the level of acculturation. It is also important to consider the effect of background differences of participants (e.g., personality) on self-esteem because of their potential for biasing results. In nonexperimental research, it is difficult to compare groups, because people vary in many ways. Because self-esteem is thought to have both state (i.e., momentary or fluid qualities varying according to the circumstances in which one finds
  • 24. oneself) and trait (i.e., global personality) qualities, one way to isolate the effect of contextual changes while holding trait constant is to employ person-context congruence models, which help researchers to assess moment-by-moment changes in self- esteem. It is equally helpful to control for background differences by employing statistical techniques (e.g., fixed-effects models) that hold background differences constant (Wooldridge 2006). This study explores differences in language use, context, and the associated self- esteem changes while comparing cross-cultural groups. Drawing on person-context congruence theories, the focus is on how self-esteem changes when adolescents move from one context to another. This study explores how youth at different levels of linguistic acculturation feel in the various contexts where they spend significant amounts of time and that are important to their development. Particular emphasis is
  • 25. placed on how youth at different levels of linguistic acculturation feel when with family versus nonfamily. If, for example, poorly linguistically acculturated youth have better self-esteem with family than with friends, and the reverse is true of more linguistically acculturated youth, linguistic acculturation can be said to moderate the experience between context (e.g., family) and self-esteem. Controlling for unobserved background differences, and the type of activity youth were engaged in, we would predict that linguistic acculturation would moderate the effect of context on self-esteem. Specifically, given prior research, we hypothesize that with increased linguistic acculturation, self-esteem will be lower with family than with friends and others. Language & Context on Self-Esteem 209 123
  • 26. Methodology Research Design This study used data from a large, nationally representative sample employing the Experience Sampling Method (ESM). ESM, a technique popular in positive psychology, employs a repeated-measure assessment technique over the course of 1 week to assist with in situ data collection, which provides higher reliability than methods based on one-time questionnaires that rely on recall (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider 2000). In this study, ESM helps us understand how linguistic acculturation moderates the adolescent experience of emotional well-being in important contexts, especially between home and friends. Use of a fixed-effects model to analyze ESM data is advantageous because it can control for background differences, often a major problem in cross-cultural assessments (Wooldridge 2006). Although many ESM researchers aggregate the data to the person level, this study
  • 27. uses a fixed-effects statistical model, which makes it possible to analyze how self- esteem changes from moment to moment as participants enter different contexts— with family, friends, school, alone, or elsewhere. The model also allows the researcher to control for background differences (i.e., fixed effects). Sample Participants for this study were drawn from the first wave of data, gathered in 1992–1993, from the longitudinal Alfred P. Sloan Study of Youth and Social Development, which had three waves of ESM data extending to 1997. Sloan Center researchers sought participants from 33 public middle and high schools, in 12 locations across the country, with diverse levels of urbanization, racial/ethnic composition, and economic diversity, in grades 6, 8, 10, and 12. A protocol for collecting this data in a way that protected human subjects was in place at the
  • 28. University of Chicago, where a team of faculty and students received IRB approval for the study. This study focuses principally on Hispanic adolescents. For this analysis, 796 participants met selection criteria. Each of these participants filled out up to 56 Experience Sampling Forms (ESFs), thereby contributing 22,335 response sets, which were then analyzed. (This approach will be discussed in the section on procedures and data analysis, below.) Of the Hispanic subsample for this study, which totaled 105, about 66% were of Mexican descent, 10% were Puerto Rican, 1% were Cuban, and the remainder were from various countries in Latin America. All Hispanic participants were included in order to attain sufficient power, given the large number of variables needed for analysis. Hispanics were contrasted with three comparison groups: 487 White non-Hispanic, English-dominant participants who met the inclusion criteria for comparison purposes; 158 non-
  • 29. Hispanic others whose first and current language was English (mostly African Americans); and 46 who reported speaking multiple languages as children and/or in the present adolescent period. 210 R. M. Perez 123 Procedures To ensure that participants understood the instruments they would be completing, participants met in groups with a researcher before and after the study. Student participants were asked to fill out the same ESM instrument multiple times during the course of a week, to a maximum of 56 response sets per participant. Participants were instructed to fill out their questionnaires each time their preprogrammed wristwatches signaled them to do so. These signals were set to go off at eight
  • 30. random times per day for up to 7 days. Measures and Variables Instruments A one-time questionnaire, NELS, based on the National Education Longitudinal Study 1988–1994, provided language data (Hafner et al. 1990). Following the ESM, an ESF was filled out each time a participant was signaled (up to a possible total of 56 ESFs). Although not all participants responded to all questions, multiple responses were recorded for most participants. All instruments were administered in Spanish or English, according to participants’ language preference. Techniques such as ESM (Prescott et al. 1981) facilitate the exploration of person-context interactions (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi 1988; Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1984; Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider 2000), and because they obtain repeated measures for each individual, score high in reliability
  • 31. (Hektner et al. 2002). ESM gathers multiple scores per respondent unobtrusively, because no interviewer is present, and in situ, in the context in which the participant is engaged when surveyed (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1987; Hormuth 1986; Moneta and Csikszentmihalyi 1996). There is no reliance on participants’ ability to recall deep emotions, which are often difficult to remember, much less express. Outcome Variable: Self-Esteem The self-esteem variable is a composite (average) score based on select items from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; to reduce participant fatigue, some scales were shortened (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson 1987). Self-esteem items included ‘‘living up to the expectations of others,’’ ‘‘living up to your own expectations,’’ ‘‘feeling good about self,’’ ‘‘succeeding at present activity,’’ and ‘‘feeling in control’’ on a scale from 0 to 9 (0 = ‘‘not at all’’; 9 = ‘‘very much’’). One additional variable, ‘‘Were you succeeding at what you were doing?,’’ originally on
  • 32. a 1–9 scale, was transformed to a 0–9 scale. Explanatory Set of Variables: Context The mutually exclusive binary variables for context are defined by time when the respondent filled out the ESF: when engaged in schoolwork, alone, with friends, with families, or in other contexts. Language & Context on Self-Esteem 211 123 Schoolwork. The first was time defined by occasions when the respondent specified being at a place categorized by a coder as schoolwork; if these times overlapped with other contexts, only time in this context counted. Alone. Time alone consists of time with no one else. Family. The family variable includes times when the participant reported being with mother, father, both, a sibling, or another relative and in no other context. Friends. Time with friends consists of times when the participant was with a
  • 33. friend but in no other context. Other contexts. Times in other contexts include occasions when the respondent was with relatives, siblings, strangers, or others, as well as in multiple contexts, but not at school or work. When a participant’s time overlapped with other contexts, the responses were placed into the ‘‘other context’’ variable. The categories were forced to be mutually exclusive and exhaustive and the variable ‘‘family’’ was left out of the regression equations. Main effects for ‘‘context’’ may be included in the fixed-effects model because context varies for each individual (variation by response signal rather than just between individuals). Explanatory Set of Variables: Acculturation Group Based on two binary variables (race/ethnicity and language), participants were placed into six acculturation groups representing a continuum between unaccultur- ated and acculturated states.
  • 34. Less-Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. For the first of these, the less- linguistically acculturated Hispanics group, Spanish was the first language they learned to speak and still spoke most often at the time of the study. Somewhat-Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. For the second group, the somewhat linguistically acculturated Hispanics, the first language learned was Spanish, but English was currently spoken most often. Linguistically Acculturated Hispanics. The third, the linguistically acculturated Hispanics group, consists of Hispanics whose first and current language was English. White Non-Hispanic English-Dominant. The principal benchmark group, against which all of the Hispanic subgroups were compared, constituted the fourth group, White, non-Hispanic English-dominant, whose first and current language was English. Other benchmark groups were included to better compare Hispanics. Multiple Languages. The fifth group, a multiple-languages group, consisted of
  • 35. participants of any ethnic heritage who spoke multiple languages, either from birth or at the time of the interview. Non-Hispanic Other. The sixth group, non-Hispanic other, included all non- Hispanic participants who were neither Hispanic nor White; these were non- Hispanic and non-White participants whose first and current dominant language was English. 212 R. M. Perez 123 Explanatory Set of Variables: Moderators of Linguistic Acculturation and Context The self-esteem of Hispanic youth at various levels of linguistic acculturation is expected to vary with the context that respondents are in. These mutually exclusive and exhaustive binary variables consist of designations formed by the interaction between the binary linguistic acculturation group variable (less-
  • 36. linguistically acculturated Hispanics, somewhat linguistically acculturated Hispanics, linguisti- cally acculturated Hispanics, White, non-Hispanic English- dominant, multiple- language, non-Hispanic other) and the binary context variable (with family, with friends, in school or work, alone, or other contexts). The linguistic acculturation group variable does not vary for an individual, and cannot be used in a fixed-effects model; however, their inclusion is allowed by matching them with context variables that fluctuate from observation to observation. Control Variables: Activities Explicit control variables for the types of activities engaged in were measured by the ESM survey question, ‘‘What was the main thing you were doing?’’ Responses to this question were classified into five categories of binary variables: leisure, work, maintenance (e.g., brushing teeth), deviant, and distressed. The criteria used to categorize activities are roughly similar to those found by Csikszentmihalyi and
  • 37. Schneider (2000). Cases in which the respondent failed to specify the activity being performed at the time of the response signal were dropped from the model. For instance, activities involving personal care, sitting, relaxing, or thinking were classified as maintenance. Talking to someone, going to a movie, playing sports, and so on, were classified as leisure. A ‘‘distressed’’ activity consisted of a situation in which the person was crying or in emotional pain (e.g., attending a funeral). The reference group in the equation is leisure; because these variables represent all leisure activities and the remaining variables are mutually exclusive and exhaustive, leisure was left out of the regression model. Data Analysis Using a Fixed-Effects Model To explore how the effect of linguistic acculturation on self- esteem varies by context, while controlling explicitly for the type of activity participants were involved in, this study used an analytic framework with a fixed-
  • 38. effects, multiple- regression model. Fixed-effects regression models allowed participants’ self-esteem scores in one context (e.g., when with parents) to be observed relative to another context (e.g., friends). Relative within-person contextually based measures have, among other advantages, the ability to avoid inexact comparisons across people and allow person-by-context factors to be assessed firsthand. Exploration of relative changes in how a person feels across different contexts, as opposed to comparisons between groups, helps to control for group heterogeneity. Fixed-effects models are Language & Context on Self-Esteem 213 123 useful in controlling for unobserved background differences across participants and in helping to reduce autocorrelation in the data. Figure 1 shows a schematic of the proposed general fixed- effects model. The
  • 39. people with whom time is spent define context. Acculturation groups are implicitly controlled because demographic and other person-level characteristics that do not change within the person automatically drop out of a fixed- effects model. The only way in which the person-level variables do not drop out of the equation is by matching them with variables that do not drop out. So, this study created interaction terms between the person-level variables that do not change from person to person in the course of 1 week (e.g., linguistic acculturation) with each of the context variables (e.g., family, friends), which do change as participants went from one context to the next during the course of the study. The interaction terms are change scores showing how participants’ self-esteem was experienced. Observations of linguistic acculturation group by context are measurable and were useful for answering the research questions of interest. In addition, any unobserved person-
  • 40. level characteristics that do not change within the person, as well as specified (i.e., observed) activities in which adolescents engage, are controlled. The fixed-effects model demonstrates the relationship between self-esteem and other variables using ordinary least squares with fixed effects. y ¼ bo þ X1b1 þ X2b2 þ X3b3 þ X4b4 þ ai þ e where y stands for one of three well-being indicators, b0 is the constant, X1 is a vector of activity variables, X2 is a vector of context variables, X3 is a vector of interaction terms, X4 stands for the time varying intercept (response signal dummies 1… 56), ai is the expected unobserved time invariant effects where i indicates person 1… N. Controls Acculturation (Implicitly controlled)* Activities (work, maintenance, leisure, deviant, distressed) Unobserved fixed effects Less-acculturated Hispanics (Spanish) Somewhat acculturated Hispanics Acculturated Hispanics (English) White non-Hispanics (English) Multiple languages Non-Hispanic Other (English)
  • 41. Self-esteem Context Family Friends Alone School/Work Other Fig. A fixed-effects model where linguistic acculturation group moderates context. * A main effect of linguistic acculturation cannot be explicitly estimated using fixed effects because it does not vary within an individual 214 R. M. Perez 123 Fixed-effects models correct for methodological problems, such as omitted variable bias and some autocorrelation, while allowing full use of the dataset. Fixed- effects models implicitly control for any unmeasured trait that varies between, but not within, individuals over time, and eliminate the dependence found in longitudinal data. Results
  • 42. Descriptive Descriptive statistics are shown in Tables 1 and 2. Applying chi-square statistics to person-level data, Table 1 shows that participants’ demographic characteristics differ across the language use groups with respect to grade, socioeconomic class of community, and gender. Also, participants appear to spend the bulk of their time on leisure activities, followed by work and maintenance tasks (see Table 2). The context in which they spent the bulk of their time was on schoolwork (ranging from 31% of the less linguistically acculturated Hispanics to 40.5% for the most linguistically acculturated Hispanics). Time with family occupied the next highest time allocation with White non-Hispanics spending the least amount of time (20%) with family, to less acculturated Hispanics spending 33% of their time with family. Across all groups, time spent with friends rather than schoolwork was minor; less
  • 43. acculturated Hispanics allocated 5% of their time with friends, whereas the most linguistically acculturated Hispanics spent 11% of their time with friends. Interestingly, less acculturated Hispanics spent the least amount of time on schoolwork and the most with family. Also, employing Wald chi-square statistics on disaggregated data, Table 2 shows that the groups differ significantly in some but not all of the types of activities in which they engaged and the contexts in which they spent time. Participants in the different language-use groups differed significantly only on time spent at leisure and maintenance. Within each context, there were significant differences between the groups on schoolwork, family, and friends. All in all, these results confirm the presence of intergroup heterogeneity, which was controlled by the fixed-effects model. Fixed-effects models, by holding all person-level differences constant, minimize measurement bias.
  • 44. Fixed Effects Using STATA to analyze data, controlling for fixed effects, or rather background differences across participants, and also the type of activity engaged in (e.g., work, maintenance, deviant, or distressed), data support the hypothesis that linguistic acculturation moderates the effect of context on self-esteem. For most of the participant groups studied, results showed that linguistic acculturation level did indeed moderate the effect of context. In other words, the combination of participants’ level of linguistic acculturation and whom they spend time with, tempered participants’ ability to experience positive or negative self-esteem relative to being with family. The hypothesis was that with increased linguistic Language & Context on Self-Esteem 215 123 T
  • 101. u m s to 1 0 0 % w it h in e a c h g ro u p Language & Context on Self-Esteem 217 123 acculturation, self-esteem was predicted to be lower with family
  • 102. than with friends; prior research showed that familism declines with acculturation and that gaps grow as children outpace their parents in the linguistic acculturation process. However, results are more nuanced than anticipated. Instead of seeing a progression toward higher self-esteem with friends than with family as linguistic acculturation increased, it was the group in the middle, the somewhat linguistically acculturated, rather than those at the high end of the linguistic acculturation process, who had higher self-esteem with friends than with family. Table 3 Fixed-effects results (n = 796 participants contributing 22,335 response signals) Self-esteem B SE Sig. Deviant -0.36 .26 Work 0.21 .03 *** Maintenance -0.18 .03 *** Distressed -3.07 .59 ***
  • 103. Friends 0.25 .05 *** Friends * multiple languages -0.41 .22 * Friends * less linguistically acculturated -0.27 .32 Friends * linguistically acculturated Hispanics -0.10 .17 Friends * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.57 .17 *** Friends * non-Hispanic others -0.06 .14 Schoolwork -0.08 .04 ** Schoolwork * multiple languages 0.28 .14 ** Schoolwork * low-acculturated Hispanics 0.20 .15 Schoolwork * linguistically acculturated Hispanics 0.39 .14 *** Schoolwork * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.49 .11 *** Schoolwork * non-Hispanic others 0.11 .08 Alone 0.09 .04 ** Alone * multiple languages -0.37 .17 ** Alone * less linguistically acculturated -0.25 .21 Alone * linguistically acculturated Hispanics -0.21 .18 Alone * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.15 .14 Alone * non-Hispanic others -0.24 .09 **
  • 104. Other 0.16 .05 ** Other * multiple languages -0.01 .24 Other * less linguistically acculturated 0.35 .22 Other * linguistically acculturated Hispanics 0.22 .23 Other * somewhat acculturated Hispanics 0.31 .17 * Other * non-Hispanic others -0.23 .12 * Because fixed-effects models are used, main effects for language use group cannot be estimated by the model. Only interaction effects for language-use group are presented. The referent group for the activities (deviant, work, maintenance, distressed) is leisure; for the contexts it is White non-Hispanic, English- dominant with family. Significance values are designated as follows: *** p .01, ** p .05, * p .10 218 R. M. Perez 123 As can be seen in Table 3, coefficients are change scores as participants’ self- esteem changed from one context to the next. These are based on changes to each
  • 105. person’s base response; the first signal responded to acts as the control to which all subsequent responses are compared. Because all the acculturation groups are mutually exclusive and exhaustive, this table shows the main effect, which is essentially the change score for the referent group, the White non-Hispanic English- dominant group when with family. Because the fixed-effects model drops all scores that do not change within the person, all acculturation groups were by design, combined with the contexts, which did change from response signal to response signal to form interaction terms, such as low linguistically acculturated Hispanics with friends for example. The coefficients shown in Table 3 are the resulting change scores from these interactions. In Table 3, the first coefficient within each context (friends, schoolwork, alone, and other), which is listed in bold letters at the beginning of each context section,
  • 106. corresponds to the referent group (i.e., White, non-Hispanic, English-dominant group, because the categories are mutually exclusive and exhaustive). This is followed by the interaction terms between each of the other acculturation groups and context. For example, a self-esteem coefficient of .25 (p .001) for the ‘‘friends’’’ effect means that the White, non-Hispanic, English- dominant group experiences significantly more positive self-esteem when with friends than when with family. The interaction terms that follow within each context represent the moderation effect on self-esteem for each group within each context. So, for example, the coefficient within the friends context corresponding to Spanish- dominant Hispanics showed that self-esteem was moderated by - .27 (not significant [n.s.]). This coefficient, when added to the friend’s effect of .25, implies that being Spanish-dominant translates to -.03 effect on self- esteem, meaning that the less linguistically acculturated Hispanics experience self-
  • 107. esteem no differently with friends than with family. Likewise, going down the column to the schoolwork context main effect (also in bold), when engaged in schoolwork, the White, non- Hispanic, English-dominant group, relative to when with family (the other referent group), experience lower self-esteem when engaged in schoolwork, -.08 (p .05). The more interesting coefficients are the interaction terms within each context and these will be discussed next. Assuming that the referent group, by virtue of its much larger numerical representation in this data, is a good proxy for host culture (to which immigrants are assumed to aspire to acculturate), the main-effect score in this analysis can stand as a benchmark to which the different groups may be compared. It was expected that within the friends’ context, the two more linguistically acculturated groups would show a higher self-esteem with friends than with family by virtue of associated loss
  • 108. of familism with acculturation described in the literature. However, the most linguistically acculturated Hispanics in this study, like the less linguistically acculturated, show no significant differences between being with friends and being with family; self-esteem, -.10 (n.s.). They therefore still appear to have more positive self-esteem with friends (as do most adolescents), but this difference is not significant. Language & Context on Self-Esteem 219 123 For the less linguistically acculturated, the Spanish-dominant group for all the contexts except schoolwork, the effect of self-esteem was moderated downward, -.27 (n.s.); in other words, their negative interaction effect scores suggest that the group is more inclined to higher self-esteem when with family than in any other context but schoolwork. These results imply that the self-esteem
  • 109. of the less linguistically acculturated Hispanics is not significantly different between times spent with family versus friends. This outcome is noteworthy, considering that the effect of familism is likely to be stronger for the less linguistically acculturated and, conceivably, that speaking Spanish most often may distance them from their friends and classmates if English is the dominant language in these contexts, as would be expected in most U.S. cities. By contrast, the opposite pattern is observed for the somewhat linguistically acculturated—those who learned Spanish first, but as adolescents speak English most often. It is interesting that the somewhat linguistically acculturated have higher self-esteem when with friends than when with family; it is equally interesting that the gap in self-esteem between each of the other contexts (schoolwork, alone, or with others) and the family context is also large for this group, and in the same
  • 110. upward (positive) direction. Data showed an upwardly moderated effect on self- esteem (toward the positive) for the somewhat linguistically acculturated when with friends, creating a self-esteem gap between friends and family that is wider than that of any other linguistic acculturation group. In three of the four contexts, the somewhat linguistically acculturated Hispanics exhibit the widest gaps in self- esteem relative to when with family than any other group. In almost all cases, these effects are statistically significant at p .05 or better. It was expected that multiple language participants, having both ability to speak English and retention of native language, and by proxy culture, would afford multiple language speakers more versatility, enhancing their self-esteem across different contexts. However, the data reveal a surprising pattern. These data showed speakers of multiple languages to have lower self-esteem with the friends than with family context, -.41 (p .10), in a pattern that is similar but
  • 111. more pronounced than that of the least linguistically acculturated Hispanic group. In sum, this study showed that being with family was associated with higher self- esteem for participants at low or at high linguistic acculturation levels, but not for those in the middle of the linguistic acculturation process. Therefore, the idea that the strength of family weakens with linguistic acculturation (Sabogal et al. 1987) is not fully explained or reflected here. The findings of this study demonstrate that acculturation and context did moderate self-esteem, but this did not occur in the expected direction. Self-esteem, defined by changes in momentary affective states as participants move from one context to another, is moderated by the effect of linguistic acculturation and context. However, where it had been expected that self- esteem would decline with increased linguistic acculturation, it was found that Hispanic adolescents at low or high linguistic acculturation levels experience
  • 112. juxtaposed patterns of self-esteem with those in the middle of the acculturation process. For the least and the most linguistically acculturated, there is little difference in self-esteem when with family versus when with others. Being Hispanic Spanish-language monolingual and speaking more than one language moderated the 220 R. M. Perez 123 effect of context on self-esteem in a direction favoring positive self-esteem with family. In other words, these groups are more likely to be happy with both family and friends. By contrast, those in the middle of the linguistic acculturation path, termed the ‘‘somewhat linguistically acculturated,’’ appear to have higher self- esteem with anyone other than family. At the low and high ends of the linguistic acculturation spectrum—the high and the low acculturated more than the bilinguals, Hispanic
  • 113. adolescents are more likely to experience more person-context cultural congruence when with their parents/ family. At the low end, those whose first language and current language preference was Spanish, are more than likely youth in monolingual families where (at least from a linguistic perspective) there is more congruence. Participants at the high end of the linguistic acculturation process (i.e., individuals who reported learning English first and but currently speak English most of the time) probably come from English monolingual families; again, there is language congruence, because for the participants to have learned English first, the parents had to have known how to speak English. In these instances, the low and high linguistic acculturation groups’ results showed a clear pattern of linguistic acculturation moderating the experience in favor of family rather than friends. For the middle group, the somewhat linguistically acculturated, who first learned Spanish but
  • 114. reported that currently English is spoken most, results show a marked preference for time with friends rather than with family, supporting the idea that cultural incongruence may be a factor in the family context. Discussion These findings contribute to the literature on the effect of linguistic acculturation and context on self-esteem. This study, by showing that linguistic acculturation and context moderated self-esteem, suggests that the complex relationships between the different contexts in which youth spend time, combined with their level of linguistic acculturation, affect their emotional well-being. However, although it had been hypothesized that the emotional distance between parents and children would have been most evident as acculturation increased, this was not the case. The patterns observed in this research were not as predicted by the loss of family cohesion,
  • 115. suspected to be at root of many findings in the immigrant health paradox research— where declines in well-being have been observed as acculturation increases. In a study such as this, where adolescent’s self-esteem was evaluated in different contexts relative to when with parents, it had been expected that youth at higher levels of linguistic acculturation would have lower self-esteem with their families than with their friends by virtue of expected declines in familism over time, as orientation to dominant culture and away from the culture of parents coincides with acculturation. However, the evidence supports a different idea— that it is the middle of the process that more clearly showed a pattern of low self- esteem with family than in any other context. Specific concern for the middle of the process was evident in these findings. As compared to low or high linguistic acculturation, participants in the middle of the Language & Context on Self-Esteem 221
  • 116. 123 acculturation process experienced significantly higher self- esteem in any context but with their families. More research is needed to unpack this interesting finding. Time with family is important for socialization. Also, familism, especially for Hispanic families (Gonzales et al. 2004) has been identified as a protective factor for immigrant youth against depression (Gil-Rivas et al. 2003). The results of this study indicate that the parent-youth dynamics of Hispanics in the middle of the acculturation process needs to be better understood. The results support various explanations. For instance, as posited long ago by Park and Stonequist, it is possible that home life is very difficult for Hispanic youth mid-process. Alternatively, acculturating Hispanics in the middle of the process may be adapting to the aspect of U.S. culture in which familism gives way to individualism.
  • 117. Or, there may be tension at home due to the clash of cultures, assuming the parents are less acculturated. Unfortunately, this study could not address every aspect of the question. Clearly, a major concern is the finding that self- esteem was consistently higher in any context but with family. This may be associated with a parental inability to socialize children to the culture the parents are most familiar with and, lacking that ability, their children may be adopting peer-culture. Of course, there are many possible alternative explanations to these findings and these are related to the study’s limitations. First, it may be that loss of family cohesion is but one of many factors that acculturating youth face. At school, the less acculturated who prefer to speak Spanish most often may not be much happier with family, but rather may feel lower self-esteem with classmates and friends by virtue of their lack of mastery of the English language. Likewise, their more linguistically
  • 118. acculturated Hispanic peers who, by virtue of speaking English, attempt to gain entry into social institutions of the dominant culture, yet may feel the ill-effects associated discrimination, alienation, or marginalization that affects many minority groups in the United States (Smokowski et al. 2007; Vega and Aguilar 1995). Although this study employed rigorous analytical methods and drew on data with a large random sample containing repetitive data collection, considered to have high reliability, many assumptions were stated regarding the challenges involved in assessing the relationship between the variables. In practice, many other complex- ities are involved in the study of acculturation and language is but one variable. Furthermore, the quality of the interactions as participants go from one context to another is not known in its entirety. It may be that the presence of linguistic acculturation gaps are exacerbated by mixed immigration status within the family
  • 119. and gender/power issues among members, and minimized by such things as collegial communication and empathy among members. Furthermore, the language of the parents is not known, casting some doubt on the true effect of person-context congruence. Finally, before generalizing, one should remember that the context in which people live is always changing and the impact of macro- level factors can have an impact on the micro-level ones (Perez 2011). Acculturation research has existed for nearly 100 years, yet there is still no solid cross-cultural measure for how to assess this process (Rudmin 2009). At the same time, there is a relatively recent proliferation of acculturation research (Rudmin 2009), and some recommend its inclusion in all well-being research (Lara et al. 2005). In truth, much of the prior acculturation research on well-being has been 222 R. M. Perez 123
  • 120. based on language, and despite a growing trend toward bicultural and multidimen- sional measures, other researchers continue to advocate for simple individual-level proxy measures for acculturation (Escobar and Vega 2000). Language acquisition captures a complex process that includes affective, cognitive, and behavioral components (Cuéllar et al. 1995), and has been shown to be influential in more complex acculturation measures (Rogler et al. 1991). Accurately capturing a complex and dynamic process such as acculturation with a single measure is likely to remain difficult considering the large number of multidirectional factors affecting the process. Although the findings of this research help to point out the important effect of context together with acculturation, it is clear that more research is needed to disentangle these important effects among the large and growing sector of
  • 121. immigrant children the United States. Implications These findings underscore the need to better understand the effects of linguistic acculturation on self-esteem and the practical utility of a fixed- effects model. Evidence from this study showed that context and language-use moderated self- esteem. Essentially, the participant in the middle had better self-esteem in any context but with family. Although the times have changed from the days when Park and Stonequist wrote about the marginal man, it is interesting that it was adolescents in the middle of the process—those whose first language was Spanish and whose second language was English—who were different from the other two acculturation levels with respect to their families. Clearly, more research is needed before we can determine the benefits—or drawbacks—of the relationship between their families of Hispanic youth and their self-esteem relative to other contexts in which they spend
  • 122. time. Research suggests that Hispanics will continue to present varying levels of language use for years to come—probably retaining their native language and culture longer than other immigrant group. Thus, the implications of this study for policy, practice, and research are critical given the size and growth rate of this population, in addition to their continual population replenishment from Latin America (Jiménez 2008), and their transnational (Levitt 2003), circular (Massey 1987), and undocumented patterns of migration. Research This study also has implications for future research. The combination of a fixed- effects model with a unique data-collection method, ESM, afforded an unusual and beneficial research design that allowed for robust cross-cultural comparisons. Cross- cultural research is often challenged by the difficulty of comparing dissimilar
  • 123. people. Fixed-effects models have the advantage of being able to hold background effects constant so that cross-cultural comparisons are less prone to selection bias. A second implication for research, regarding the effects of linguistic acculturation on development, is that more understanding of the dynamic between immigrant youth at the different levels of linguistic acculturation and their families, is Language & Context on Self-Esteem 223 123 warranted. Another possibly fruitful path for future research is better understanding the effects of language on the brain or, more generally, on healthy development. The multiple-languages group was expected to be relatively indifferent about where they spent time. It was predicted that participants who spoke multiple languages would experience few or no self-esteem differences across the contexts in which they
  • 124. spend time, by virtue of having learned enough and having enough resources to cope in a variety of contexts. The literature on multicompetence, in particular, shows that the structures of the brain develop differently when individuals learn more than one language at the same time (Kim et al. 1997). These differences are hypothesized to lead to higher levels of cognition, in that multilingual people have access to additional linguistic knowledge not otherwise possible (Han 2004; Pavlenko 2003). Finally, it is hoped that cross-cultural researchers who work with panel data containing multiple measures per person see the advantage of the fixed-effects model. The fixed-effects model as applied to the panel data (i.e., repetitive sampling technique) used in this study allowed us to control for background differences. This was advantageous to reduce bias. This model controls selection bias by way of the
  • 125. demeaning process, which assesses each person’s changes from his or her first recorded response so that each person serves as his or her own control, and only variables that change from response to response are included. Because each person serves as his or her own base of comparison, the variables that do not change over time are simply held constant and thus do not bias the results. In this way, observable variables that do not vary for a person (e.g., gender, grade, socioeco- nomic status) fall out of the model because nothing changes from response signal to response signal; in this way they are implicitly controlled. Unobservable variables are also held constant. This is useful for controlling such things as aspects of personality, which can predispose people to prefer some contexts or activities and to experience particular affect. For example, an extroverted person may have a low self-esteem when alone, whereas an introverted person may be much happier alone.
  • 126. Practice For practitioners working with young people learning a second language, it is critical to understand that important associated cultural differences to their linguistic differences in how they relate with family and friends and also how they affect self- esteem. Human services staff working with Hispanic youth may, first and foremost, want to assess the degree to which the family is experiencing linguistic acculturation gaps, and also to what extent these are merely a nuisance rather than causes of problematic behaviors. Linguistic acculturation in this study was treated as a moderator of self-esteem; in other words, there are more than likely additional familial and cultural issues that underlie problem behaviors. Language use, and more specifically, linguistic retention has been found to be protective factors bridging gaps between parents and youth and contributing to global self- worth (Birman 1998). For among more acculturated adolescents
  • 127. more understand- ing is needed to regarding the more problematic areas negatively affecting 224 R. M. Perez 123 well-being—whether self-esteem, family conflict, or something else that is not fully understood as of yet. Social workers may use the information found in this study and others like it to help bridge linguistic acculturation gaps between immigrant parents and accultu- rating offspring as has been found helpful by some practitioners working with troubled Hispanic adolescents (Szapocznik et al. 1986). Exploring bridges between youth and parental culture during adolescence can help facilitate parental socialization. Knowing that most immigrant youth progress from low to high linguistic acculturation, it may be worthwhile to explore how benefits of cultural
  • 128. retention (e.g., familismo, respeto) could be better preserved, so that those in the middle of the process do not feel estranged from their parental culture. Alternatively, children who do not feel positive self-esteem with parents may not be able to receive important enculturation and if they seek their friends out they may not benefit from the quality understanding that their parents are better able to transmit. Policy Regarding policy, there is currently no concerted federal effort to incorporate newcomers, linguistically or otherwise, into U.S. society (Jiménez 2007). Further- more, the problematic adaptation of more acculturated Hispanic youth, as posited by evidence from the immigrant health paradox, suggest that macro-level interventions led by the U.S. government would be justified (Perez 2011). Addressing the needs of Hispanic youth at the age most crucial to the transition to adulthood can be seen as
  • 129. an investment. References Alegrı́a, M., Canino, G., Shrout, P. E., Woo, M., Duan, N., Vila, D., et al. (2008). Prevalence of mental illness in immigrant and non-immigrant U.S. Latino groups. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(3), 359–369. Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Kwak, K., & Sam, D. L. (2006). Introduction: Goals and research framework for studying immigrant youth. In J. W. Berry, J. S. Phinney, D. L. Sam, & P. Vedder (Eds.), Immigrant youth in cultural transition: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation across national contexts (pp. 1–14). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Birman, D. (1998). Biculturalism and perceived competence of Latino immigrant adolescents. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26(3), 335–354. Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2008). Does adolescent self-esteem predict later life outcomes? A test of the causal role of self-esteem. Development & Psychopathology, 20(1), 319–339. Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2007). Knowledge of child development and family interactions among
  • 130. immigrants to America: Perspectives from developmental science. In J. E. Lansford, K. Deater- Deckard, & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Immigrant families in contemporary society (pp. 121–136). New York: Guilford Press. Brandstätter, H. (1994). Well-being and motivational person- environment fit: A time-sampling study of emotions. European Journal of Personality, 8(2), 75–93. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742. Language & Context on Self-Esteem 225 123 Cafferty, P. S. J. (1985). Language and social assimilation. In P. S. J. Cafferty & W. McGready (Eds.), Hispanics in the United States: A new social agenda. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and growth in the teenage years.
  • 131. New York: Basic Books. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 525–536. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Schneider, B. (Eds.). (2000). Becoming adult: How teenagers prepare for the world of work. New York: Basic Books. Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation rating scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA Scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17(3), 275–304. Dennis, J., Basañez, T., & Farahmand, A. (2010). Intergenerational conflicts among Latinos in early adulthood: Separating values conflicts with parents from acculturation conflicts. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 32(1), 118. Emmons, R. A., Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Choice of situations and congruence models of interactionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 6(6), 693–702. Escobar, J. I., & Vega, W. A. (2000). Mental health and immigration’s AAAs: Where are we and where do we go from here? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188(11), 736–740. Fillmore, L. W. (1991). When learning a second language means losing the first. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6(3), 323–346.
  • 132. Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 97–111. Gil-Rivas, V., Greenberger, E., Chen, C., & Montero y Lopez- Lena, M. (2003). Understanding depressed mood in the context of a family-oriented culture. Adolescence, 38(149), 93. Gonzales, N. A., Knight, G. P., Birman, D., & Sirolli, A. A. (2004). Acculturation and enculturation among Latino youth. In K. I. Maton, C. J. Schellenbach, B. J. Leadbeater, & A. L. Solarz (Eds.), Investing in children, youth, families, and communities: Strengths-based research and policy (pp. 285–302). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Gove, W. R., Style, C. B., & Hughes, M. (1990). The effect of marriage on the well-being of adults: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 11(1), 4–35. Greenman, E., & Xie, Y. (2008). Is assimilation theory dead? The effect of assimilation on adolescent well-being. Social Science Research, 37(1), 109–137. Hafner, A., Ingels, S., Schneider, B., & Stevenson, D. (1990). A profile of American eighth-grade mathematics and science instruction: National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
  • 133. Halgunseth, L., Ispa, J., & Rudy, D. (2006). Parental control in Latino families: An integrated review of the literature. Child Development, 77(5), 1282–1297. Han, Z. (2004). Review of effects of the second language on the first. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(3), 488–489. Harker, K. (2001). Immigrant generation, assimilation, and adolescent psychological well being. Social Forces, 79, 970–1004. Hektner, J. M., Csikszentmihalyi, M., Bechtel, R. B., & Churchman, A. (2002). The experience sampling method: Measuring the context and the content of lives Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 233–243). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experiences in situ. Journal of Personality, 54(1), 262–293. Hunter, J. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). The positive psychology of interested adolescents. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 32(1), 27–35. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt. Jiménez, T. (2007). From newcomers to Americans: An integration policy for a nation of immigrants. Immigration Policy in Focus, 5(11). Retrieved from www.immigrationpolicy.org. Jiménez, T. (2008). Mexican immigrant replenishment and the continuing significance of ethnicity and race. American Journal of Sociology, 113(6), 1527–1567.
  • 134. Kim, K. H. S., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K.-M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171–174. Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations for immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development, 46(2–3), 15–136. 226 R. M. Perez 123 http://www.immigrationpolicy.org Lang, J. G., Muñoz, R. F., Bernal, G., & Sorensen, J. L. (1982). Quality of life and psychological well- being in a bicultural Latino community. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 4(4), 433–450. Lara, M., Gamboa, C., Kahramanian, M. I., Morales, L. S., & Bautista, D. E. H. (2005). Acculturation and Latino health in the United States: A review of the literature and its sociopolitical context. Annual Review of Public Health, 26, 367–397. Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980). The significance of time alone in adolescent development. Journal of Current Adolescent Medicine, 2, 33–40. Larson, R., & Lee, M. (1996). The capacity to be alone as a stress buffer. Journal of Social Psychology,
  • 135. 136, 5–16. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Leondari, A. (2001). The impact of acculturation on immigrant children’s self-perceptions, feelings of loneliness and social status. Educational & Child Psychology, 18(4), 35–46. Levitt, P. (2003). Transnational villagers. In J. Stone & D. Rutledge (Eds.), Race and ethnicity: Comparative and theoretical approaches (p. 424). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Luna, I., de Ardon, E. T., Lim, Y. M., Cromwell, S. L., Phillips, L. R., & Russell, C. K. (1996). The relevance of familism in cross-cultural studies of family caregiving. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 18(3), 267–283. Marsiglia, F., Parsai, M., & Kulis, S. (2009). Effects of familism and family cohesion on problem behaviors among adolescents in Mexican immigrant families in the southwest United States. Journal of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work, 18(3), 18. Massey, D. (1987). Understanding Mexican migration to the United States. American Journal of Sociology, 92(6), 1372–1403. Mendoza, F. S., Javier, J. R., & Burgos, A. E. (2007). Health of children in immigrant families. In J. E. Lansford, K. Deater-Deckard, & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Immigrant families in
  • 136. contemporary society (pp. 30–50). New York: Guilford Press. Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective experience. Journal of Personality, 64(2), 276–310. Pabon, E. (1998). Hispanic adolescent delinquency and the family: A discussion of sociocultural influences. Adolescence, 33(132), 941–955. Pavlenko, A. (2003). ‘‘I never knew I was a bilingual’’: Reimagining teacher identities in TESOL. Journal of Language, Identity, & Education, 2(4), 251–268. Pawlak, J. L., & Klein, H. A. (1997). Parental conflict and self- esteem: The rest of the story. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158(3), 303–313. Perez, R. M. (2011). Latino mental health: Acculturation challenges in service provision. In L. P. Buki & L. M. Piedra (Eds.), Haciendo camino al andar: Creating infrastructures for Latino mental health. New York: Springer. Piedra, L. M., & Engstrom, D. W. (2009). Segmented assimilation theory and the Life Model: An integrated approach to understanding immigrants and their children. Social Work, 54(3), 270–277. Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2006). Immigrant America: A portrait (3rd ed.). Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press.
  • 137. Portes, P. R., & Zady, M. F. (2002). Self-esteem in the adaptation of Spanish-speaking adolescents: The role of immigration, family conflict and depression. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 24(3), 296–318. Prescott, S., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1981). Environmental effects on cognitive and affective states: The experiential time sampling approach. Social Behavior & Personality, 9(1), 23–32. Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovits, M. J. (1936). Memorandum on the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist, 38, 149–152. Rogler, L. H., Cortes, D. E., & Malgady, R. G. (1991). Acculturation and mental health status among Hispanics: Convergence and new directions for research. American Psychologist, 46, 585–597. Rudmin, F. W. (2009). Constructs, measurements and models of acculturation and acculturative stress. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33(2), 106–123. Rudmin, F. W. (2010). Acculturation measures. In E. Mpofu & T. Oakland (Eds.), Rehabilitation and health assessment: Applying ICF guidelines (pp. 353–379). New York: Springer. Rumbaut, R. G. (1997). Assimilation and its discontents: Between rhetoric and reality. International Migration Review, 31, 923–960. Sabogal, F., Marı́n, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., & Marı́n, B. V. (1987). Hispanic familism and acculturation:
  • 138. What changes and what doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9(4), 397–412. Language & Context on Self-Esteem 227 123 Sameroff, A. (1995). General system theories and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmenal psychopathology. Vol. 1: Theory and methods. New York: Wiley. Shernoff, D. J., Knauth, S., & Makris, E. (2000). The quality of classroom experiences. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & B. Schneider (Eds.), Becoming adult (pp. 141–164). New York: Basic Books. Smokowski, P. R., & Bacallao, M. L. (2007). Acculturation, internalizing mental health symptoms, and self-esteem: Cultural experiences of Latino adolescents in North Carolina. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 37(3), 273–292. Smokowski, P. R., Chapman, M. V., & Bacallao, M. L. (2007). Acculturation risk and protective factors and mental health symptoms in immigrant Latino adolescents. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 16(3), 33–55.
  • 139. Smokowski, P. R., Rose, R., & Bacallao, M. L. (2008). Acculturation and Latino family processes: How cultural involvement, biculturalism, and acculturation gaps influence family dynamics. Family Relations, 57(3), 295–308. Szapocznik, J., Rio, A., Perez-Vidal, A., Kurtines, W., Hervis, O., & Santisteban, D. (1986). Bicultural Effectiveness Training (BET): An experimental test of an intervention modality for families experiencing intergenerational/intercultural conflict. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 303–330. Turner, R., & Gil, A. (2002). Psychiatric and substance use disorders in South Florida: Racial/ethnic and gender contrasts in a young adult cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(1), 43. Vega, L., & Aguilar, B. (1995). Differential effects of discrimination on the ethnic identity of Mexican Americans: The role of the person and the situation. PhD, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA. Vega, W. A., & Alegrı́a, M. (2001). Latino mental health and treatment in the United States. Health Issues in the Latino Community (pp. 179–208). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Vega, W. A., Khoury, E. L., Zimmerman, R. S., & Gil, A. G. (1995). Cultural conflicts and problem behaviors of Latino adolescents in home and school
  • 140. environments. Journal of Community Psychology, 23(2), 167–179. Ward, C. (1996). Acculturation. In D. Landis & R. Bhagat (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (2nd ed., pp. 124–147). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Weiner, B. (2000). Intrapersonal and interpersonal theories of motivation from an attributional perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 12(1), 1–14. Weiss, U. K., & Tillman, K. H. (2009). Risky sexual behaviors among hispanic young adults in South Florida: Nativity, age at immigration and gender differences. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 41(4), 202–209. Wickrama, K., Elder, G., & Abraham, W. (2007). Rurality and ethnicity in adolescent physical illness: Are children of the growing rural Latino population at excess health risk? Journal of Rural Health, 23(3), 228–237. Wooldridge, J. M. (2006). Introductory econometrics: A modern approach (3rd ed.). Mason, OH: Thompson South-Western. Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescent relations with mothers, fathers, and friends. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 228 R. M. Perez 123
  • 141. Copyright of Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.