SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 162
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
1991, Vol. 59, No. 6,799-812
Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association.
Inc.
0022-006X/91/S3.00
Cultural Diversity and Treatment of Children
Roland G. Tharp
University of California, Santa Cruz
The increasing cultural diversity of child clients has produced a
cascade of new issues and concerns
for psychological practice, theory, and research. Available
evidence and pertinent theory are re-
viewed on such topics as the predictive utility and treatment
consequences of ethnic membership,
whether treatments should be generic or specific to cultural
groups, the degree of privilege that
should be accorded to same-culture therapists, and the relative
desirability of different modalities
of treatment for children of different cultural groups. The
concept of cultural compatibility of
treatment is explored and evaluated. A broad agenda of
hypotheses for research and development is
suggested, and some guidelines for clinical practice and policy
are proposed. It is concluded that
insofar as possible, treatment for all children should be
contextualized in their family's and commu-
nity's structure of meanings, relationships, and language.
The culturally diverse society that our nation has become
provides a challenge to our politics, our schools, our science,
and our profession. As reiterated by concerned commissions,
agencies, professional organizations, and review articles in-
cluding the current Guidelines for Providers of Psychological
Services to Ethnic andCulturatty Diverse Populations (American
Psychological Association, 1990), we are instructed that "Psy-
chologists should recognize ethnicity and culture as significant
parameters in understanding psychological processes" (p. 4)
before delivering services. What will be necessary to satisfy
this
new imperative? Self-examination? New specialized training?
Anthropological indoctrination, religious and spiritual relativ-
ism, multilingualism, "politically correct" thought? And most
to the point, what are the required practical clinical actions? In
discussing these questions, Pedersen and Marsella (1982) con-
clude that to know all and do all that a psychologist should is a
goal that accelerates beyond us.
The task is to bring some order out of this cascade of new
issues and concerns, in the light of available research evidence
and pertinent theory. I will suggest here that some broad out-
lines for strategy are emerging, with enough clarity at least to
pose an agenda of hypotheses for research and development,
and possibly even to provide an initial set of guidelines for
clinical practice and policy. These strategies can be stated
clearly enough to allow them to be tested by both formal
evalua-
tion and clinical experience.
Not to overpromise, it should be said that research on cul-
tural issues in clinical treatment is scant, particularly research
addressing such issues with children. We must rely on evidence
from other age groups and look to related literatures (family and
community) where child treatment is discussed. Cultural issues
in the education of children is a field somewhat more mature,
and if considered cautiously, evidence from that field can serve
by analogy. Other disciplines, notably anthropology and lin-
guistics, have a longer history with the study of ethnic and
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Ro-
land G. Tharp, Merrill College, University of California, Santa
Cruz,
California 95064.
cultural diversity, and psychology can anticipate from their ex-
perience. In attempting to discern the figure in this ground, we
can fit some of these diverse tesserae into the beginning of a
mosaic. Still, there are more spaces than tiles.
Basic Questions
Concerns arising from both theory and practice converge on
four basic questions about the implications of cultural member-
ship for the treatment of children. First, is there a useful level
of
developmental analysis, which I will call here ethnogenetic, that
accounts for important current features of a childls psychology
in terms of the historical forces operating on his or her ances-
tors in a time frame of hundreds to thousands of years? That is,
does ethnogenesis, which lies in the dimension of time between
phylogenesis and ontogenesis, provide critical elements for un-
derstanding and treating the child's present condition?
Second, are there present in cultures psychosocial features so
widely shared that membership is a useful guide for prescrip-
tion of clinical services? Or, conversely, does membership as a
guide result in stereotypical overgeneralization, and are more
conventional individual and family descriptors to be preferred?
Third, are there forms of treatment—either potential or ac-
tual—that are specifically or uniquely suited for the treatment
of children of different cultures? It is important to differentiate
specific from unique. Although one or another form of therapy
may be specifically suited for members of one culture, that does
not mean that the modality is exclusively suited to them; family
therapy, for example, might prove to be the treatment of choice
of members of several cultures, whereas the sweat lodge might
be uniquely suited to those of only one.
Fourth, are culture members privileged in the capacity to
treat or to investigate the treatment of children of their own
culture? Although the literature has not necessarily developed
in response to them, these questions may be borne in mind as
the available knowledge is reviewed.
Cultural Differences in Diagnosis and Treatment
Cultural Differences in Pathology
Some childhood disorders are of particular frequency in cer-
tain cultural groups, because of either characteristic social
799
800 ROLAND G. THARP
problems or psychosocial structures. Weisz, Suwanlert, Chaiya-
sit, and Walter (1987) consider psychosocial differences by con-
trasting Thailand, a Buddhist nation, which discourages chil-
dren's aggression and encourages inhibition, peacefulness,
politeness, and deference, with the United States, where inde-
pendence, competitiveness, and differentiation from the family
as the goal of socialization is widespread. In Thailand, children
and adolescents are referred for clinic treatment more often for
"overcontrolled syndrome" (fearfillness, sleep problems, soma-
ticizing), whereas in the United States referred children and
adolescents are reported more often with "undercontrolled
syndrome" (disobedience, fighting, arguing). The same general
pattern is discernible in an epidemiological study, although the
culturally consistent trends are more marked in parents' deci-
sions to refer for treatment than in their general ratings of their
children (Weisz, Suwanlert, Chaiyasit, Weiss, et at, 1987). An-
other "overcontrolled" society (Jamaican) has produced similar
results (Lambert, Weisz, & Knight, 1989).
Many describe minority status itself as a stressor, because it is
often associated with hostility and prejudice. According to
Moritsugu and Sue (1983) minority status also often is asso-
ciated with a lack of effective support during crises and the
danger of developing ineffective cognitive coping styles. The
status of all minorities, however, is not the same, and neither
are
their characteristic stressors nor responses to them. In the field
of minority education, the nature of stress and the responses to
it are often differentiated for the children of voluntary immi-
grant groups versus those of "involuntary" minority children.
Children of the latter (such as African Americans and Native
Americans) learn that education does not lead to economic
improvement, and they become withdrawn and hostile in
school. Children of recent minority groups, however, may feel
the stress of difference, but see that their people prosper
through education, and they may respond in school by striving.
Gibson and Ogbu (1991), with whose names this analysis is
associated, are now attempting to further differentiate among
minority groups as to the specific defenses and coping mecha-
nisms used.
As an example of disorders in response to culturally charac-
teristic stressors, Indochinese refugee children treated in Cali-
fornia were recovering from such severe stress that application
of categories from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (3rd ed., DSM-III, 1980, American Psychiat-
ric Association) was confounded (Krener & Sabin, 1985). The
pattern is found with Central American refugee children as well
(Arredondo, Orjuela, & Moore, 1989). "For every one of our
patients, Axis I DSM-III diagnoses could be reached on the
basis of the patient's fulfilling the criteria, but the diagnosis
was
felt to be incompletely descriptive or fundamentally wrong"
(Krener & Sabin, 1985, p. 457). The issue does not derive only
from specific stress; their Indochinese child patients revealed
cross-cultural child-rearing differences and their potential mis-
construing as psychopathology. For example, the American
concept of mourning includes a sequencing of stages and a time
limitation. Indochinese, who believe that the dead live after
bodily death, may be seen as manifesting pathological or in-
complete grief. "Particular DSM-III diagnostic categories. . .
break down when we attempt to apply them cross-culturally,
and . . . may have only qualified validity within our own cul-
ture" (Krener & Sabin, 1985, p. 457). These authors decry the
practice of applying the diagnostic scheme of one culture to
child patients from another.
Among indigenous minorities, there are also characteristic
child problems. Because of high rates of death and displace-
ment, Crow Indian children objectively experience repeated
and traumatic loss; they suffer a chronic condition of mourning
and depression (Long, 1983). The common plight of the acting-
out, depressed, poor, alienated Black male adolescent is an-
other example of a disorder so frequent in that group as to
require a psychocultural category. Paster (1985) provides a dis-
cussion of this syndrome, emphasizing the way that political,
historical, societal, economic, interpersonal, and intrapsychic
factors may be seen as intertwined and reciprocally reinforcing;
the article is an example of how ethnogenetic, ontogenetic, and
microgenetk levels of analysis integrate.
When such integrated-genetic analyses are performed, it is
difficult to discount cultural factors. The case can be made that
not only in minorities are cultural, ethnogenetic features pres-
ent in psychopathology.
The overwhelming evidence of cultural psychiatric literature. . .
is that cultural factors are a substantial part of every disorder
and
not a descriptive, picturesque component.. . . What is lacking in
the [DSM-III] system, from a Latin-American perspective, is the
explicit recognition of psychocultural categories or cultural
crite-
ria—or both—to aid in the identification of crucial categories
and in the description of clusters of symptoms. (Alarcon, 1983,
p.
104)
The even stronger case is made by Marsella and Higgenbotham
(1984), who argue that reserving the term culture-specific for
exotic disorders merely delays the awareness that all disorders
are culture-specific, and so is all treatment.
Cultural Differences in Treatment
Do clients of all cultures receive equal mental health services?
Meta-analyses of research conclude that the issue is not clear
(Atkinson, 1985; Sue & Zane, 1987) because of the usual re-
search problems of nonuniformity of treatment goals, inaccessi-
bility of records, inappropriate analog designs, and absence of
any process research. However, to illustrate the issue, we may
consider a set of data that appear to make the optimistic case,
that clients are offered about the same services according to
diagnosis and prognosis, without regard for ethnicity. Sue, Al-
len, and Conaway (1978) studied 13,450 clients and found no
evidence that Hispanic or Native-American clients in commu-
nity mental health facilities were offered inferior or discrimina-
tory services in either diagnosis, assigned personnel, or type of
treatment. However, Hispanic and Native American clients fre-
quently did not return after the initial treatment session. In
these facilities, the dropout rates were Hispanic 42%, Indian
55%, Black 52%, Asian-American 52%, and Anglo clients only
30%. Why are these rates so high for minority culture clients?
The authors indict not only the fit of the assigned type of ser-
vice, but the quality of the professional interaction. Thus the
same service is not necessarily responsive service, and in its
effects, identical is not necessarily equal.
Techniques that are of demonstrated utility in one culture
may not operate in the same way for others. In a study con-
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 801
ducted by Lieh-Mak, Lee, & Luk (1984), Hong Kong Chinese
parents were trained to be mediators of behavior therapy for
their own children, but the therapists reported severe culturally
based reluctance, arising from patterns of traditional family
structure and interaction. These parents found it difficult to
play with their children or respond contingently with praise or
ignoring, because this violated their accustomed mode of child
rearing, their beliefs about childhood problems, and their atti-
tudes about the proper role of doctors. Thus Lieh-Mak, Lee, &
Luk warn against the ethnocentric bias involved in importing
treatment modalities from one culture to another.
Cultural Differences in Knowledge
The core of the problem, according to King, Moody, Thomp-
son, & Bennett (1983), is that mental health intellectual and
institutional constructions
reflect the orientations of educators, researchers, and therapists
who too often make judgments about and define programs for
Black persons and communities with insufficient data on the na-
ture of these persons and communities, their beliefs, values or
their abilities and resources. We have almost developed an inca-
pacity to shift from our Anglo-American perceptions, to change
our attitudes and to foster real change. (King et al., 1983, p. 5)
This perception of establishment ignorance and nonrespon-
siveness is not limited to African-American psychologists.
Although non-Indian psychologists are becoming increasingly
in-
volved in mental health service delivery to American Indians,
they are, unfortunately, typically not prepared to work
effectively
with Indian clientele. The non-Indian psychologist may not be
aware of the cultural values, life-styles, family practices,
develop-
mental progressions, and needs of their American Indian clients.
This lack of cultural awareness typically results in conflicts and
frustrations for both the psychologist and his or her Indian
clients;
ultimately, American Indian children and families may not re-
ceive appropriate mental health services. (Everett, Proctor, &
Cartmell, 1983, p. 588)
Not only is the individual client harmed, but child workers who
devalue native culture may actually intensify social disorgani-
zation in Indian communities (Sullivan, 1983).
The single most important explanation for the problems in
service
delivery involves the inability of therapists to provide culturally
responsive forms of treatment.. . .Most therapists are not famil-
iar with the cultural backgrounds and life-styles of various
ethnic-
minority groups and have received training primarily developed
for Anglo, or mainstream, Americans. . . [and ] are often unable
to devise culturally appropriate forms of treatment, and ethnic-
minority clients frequently find mental health services strange,
foreign, or unhelpful. (Sue & Zane, 1987, p. 37)
Many mental health professionals find clients of some cul-
tures strange and unhelpful, or even harmful, for example in
the area of child abuse/neglect. Gray and Cosgrove (1985) inter-
viewed Mexican, Samoan, Vietnamese, Filipino, Japanese, and
Blackfoot as to their culturally normative child rearing values
and practices. The researchers then examined these practices
for the likelihood of their being misunderstood as "abuse" or
"neglect" by child-protective professionals. That potential for
misunderstanding is very high, particularly around two basic
topics: the degree of responsibility delegated to children and
the degree to which children should submit to adult domi-
nance. The authors suggest that ignorance of these differential
norms will lead to mistreatment of some parents by ethnocen-
tric social workers. It should be noted that the potential for
misunderstanding of one another's parenting conventions
across all ethnic groups is high, and not restricted to Euro-
Americans. However, the institutionalization of Euro-Ameri-
can norms into professional assumptions is a hazard to ad-
vances in psychocultural understanding and to the just percep-
tion of parents' behaviors within other cultures. A source book
on ethnic child socialization is Phinney and Rotheram (1987),
on cultural approaches to parenting is Bernstein (1991), and an
analysis of the use of such information in understanding child
abuse and neglect is Korbin (1980).
Much of the research and professional literature in culture
and mental health attempts to provide enough information to
sensitize psychologists to cultural issues in diagnosis and treat-
ment. Several journals have published special issues on culture
and mental health, and edited books on the topic are the most
typical medium of communication. The typical pattern of
these publications is to have several chapters on specific cul-
tures, book-ended by introductory and summary chapters that
attempt more general discussions.' This construction well com-
municates the state of analysis of the issues. The field suffers
from a shortage of empirical studies of program or variable
evaluation, and there are virtually no studies of process in
cross-cultural treatment. Nevertheless, this literature can sensi-
tize the therapist to some issues that may be present for cul-
turally unfamiliar clients.2
Differences Within Cultures
This general approach—to learn as much about cultures as
possible—is frustrated by significant differences among sub-
groups and individuals within cultures. Everett, Proctor, and
Cartmell (1983) point out the vast intertribal, interclan, urban-
traditional, and individual differences among American In-
dian clients. Isomura, Fine, and Lin (1987) discuss the differ-
ences in offering services to Japanese immigrant families of the
first, second, and third generations. Hispanic cultures differen-
tiate among themselves; Cuban-, Mexican-, and Puerto Rican-
Americans celebrate their distinctions as well as their common
causes. Although clients expect respect and understanding of
1 See, e.g, Atkinson, Morton, and Sue (1989); Pedersen,
Sartorius,
and Marsella (1984); Chunn, Dunston, and Ross-Sheriff (1983).
2 Notable journals include a special issue of Psychotherapy
titled
"Psychotherapy with Ethnic Minorities." There is a special issue
of the
American Journal of Social Psychiatry on psychiatric care of
minority
groups, ftmily therapy with immigrant families is treated in
Journal of
Strategic and Systemic Therapies, and Journal of Drug Issues
has a
special issue on "Alcohol Problems and Minority Youth." A
useful
compilation of orientations to family structure and function,
with im-
plications for family treatment, is discussed for 20 different
cultural
groups in McColdrick, Pearce, & Giordano (1982), and more
recently
by Ho (1987,1990), who presents useful tabular comparisons of
family-
therapy relevant dimensions by culture. For child treatment, the
most
useful volumes of this sort are Children of Color: Psychological
Interven-
tions With Minority Youth (Gibbs & Huang, 1989) and Ethnic
Issues in
Adolescent Mental Health (Stiffman & Davis, 1990).
802 ROLAND G. THARP
their culture and values, all will resent being seen merely as a
representative of a cultural central tendency.
Issues of culture and treatment exist today in a context of
tempestuous intercultural relations in the political and eco-
nomic arena, putting us in hazard of using cultural member-
ship as no more than stereotypical overgeneralization. A part
of the research agenda must therefore be to unpack the cultural
variable (Whiting, 1976) so that differentiating characteristics
within culture can be understood for clinical implications for
individuals. In this way culture can be analyzed for its variable
influence on individuals, in contrast to approaches that assign
an equal value to culture for all members of a group. I am
unaware of such work within mental health disciplines, but an
idea of the strategy is given by work in culture and education
(e.g., Weisner, Gallimore, & Jordan, 1988). Gallimore, Reese,
Balzano, Benson, and Goldenberg (1991), in investigating the
correlates of academic success for children of Mexican immi-
grants, found that the domestic variable with the strongest rela-
tionship to child school success is whether the father uses skills
of literacy/numeracy in his employment (not the level of
father's
education). This kind of finer grained analysis of cultural and
community life would allow us accurately to perceive the dy-
namics of culture in the daily life of the individual child in the
consulting chair.
Faced with these contradictions, confusions, similarities, and
differences, mental health professionals have advocated every
position on the continuum. Some writers bundle all "children
of color" together and believe their cause is common. Other
writers call for culturally derived diversity of treatments, and
others insist on a universality of the form of treatment, al-
though of course purified by a correction of racist attitudes.
Psychological science is now searching for a system of under-
standing, and some theoretical guidance that
goes beyond a study of exotic populations from different
cultures
and examines guidelines for excellence for mental health as a
whole. . .the case for emphasizing cultural context in the under-
standing, treatment, and prevention of mental disorders has
been
made extensively and persuasively. . .The problem we face,
how-
ever is going from general knowledge about culture traditions to
specific mental health practices. This is where the
disagreements,
debates, and arguments begin and where the pragmatists fre-
quently lose interest. (Sartorius, Pedersen, & Marsella, 1984,
p. 282)
The Cultural Compatibility Hypothesis
The hypothesis of cultural compatibility suggests that treat-
ment is more effective when compatible with client culture pat-
terns. The hypothesis has a more mature theoretical and re-
search base in child education (Tharp, 1989) than in child men-
tal health, but the issues may be argued in substantially the
same terms. Three forms of this hypothesis exist. The strong
form, or culturally specific version, suggests that the most
effec-
tive interventions for different cultures will be different and
specific (if not unique) to cultures. Proponents are associated
with the effort to derive culturally based modalities or varia-
tions of treatment.
A weaker form is the two-type hypothesis, which suggests
that there are two types of cultures, and therefore two types of
most-effective clinical interventions. The first type is the
major-
ity, or Euro-American, culture; the second type includes those
cultures whose students typically experience problems in
schools, who are by-and-large children of color, less industrial-
ized, urbanized, or western acculturated, and who thus share
crucial incompatibilities with standard mental health prac-
tices. In this position, effective treatment strategies for children
of color would not be critically different from one another. This
position is more salient in social work (e.g., Lum, 1986) than in
psychology.
The null form of the cultural compatibility hypothesis is the
universalistic argument that effective treatments will follow the
same course for all cultures. This is the default hypothesis of
clinical psychology, in that the unreflective proceed as though
there are no significant differences. However, many universal-
ists agree that there are some variant subroutines that make
minority children and their families more comfortable and are
willing to engage in the search for these accommodations.
The challenge faced by all is to balance universality and plu-
ralism, preserving an authentic responsiveness to individual
differences while avoiding an impractically large number of
kinds of treatments (Huang & Gibbs, 1989). The obstacles are
several, but they include pervasive psychological theory that
lacks concepts crucial to the solution, and in which culture
itself
plays almost no role as a variable.
Proponents of all three positions appear to be moderately
satisfied with the existing monolith of treatment, as derived
from, practiced by, and practiced for Europeans and Euro-
Americans. Whether their hypotheses call for erecting many
other treatment structures, as in the culturally specific position;
or creating one other alternate for children of color, as in the
two-type hypothesis; or only for modifying and polishing the
universalist structure, there is no systematic understanding
that the serious examination of the issues of culture and treat-
ment may well call for a restructuring of the monolith itself,
even for children of its own.
Approaches to Culturally Compatible Treatment
Each of these hypotheses operates in the same arena and
takes its different positions on certain salient issues. These in-
clude modifying professional role behaviors, assigning same-
ethnicity professionals, and searching for the process details
that will create or modify compatible procedures.
Modified Professional Roles in Cross-Cultural Treatment
Jenkins (1985), in discussing Afro-American clients, sug-
gests that they should be approached in a down-to-earth and
egalitarian manner that reduces status differences. In this way a
positive affective response is made more likely, and early treat-
ment attrition is reduced. By contrast, it is not uncommon that
a more authoritative, formal, and concrete therapist role is rec-
ommended for Japanese clients (e.g, Isomura, Fine, & Lin,
1987). Hong (1988) recommends that a psychologist dealing
with an Asian-American clientele operate in a context of gen-
eral family practice as a primary care provider, similar to the
traditional family doctor who provides treatment for clients
and their families, thus assessing the strong and traditional role
of the family, minimizing client families' inhibition against
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 803
seeking mental health services, and taking advantage of the
Asian-American's respect and deference to doctors.
Juarez (198S) recommends a firm, instructing, professional
demeanor when working with the Hispanic client, as do most
who write from the Latino perspective. However, consider this
quotation from Montijo (1985):
Any therapeutic model that strives to serve poor Puerto Ricans
or
poor people in general cannot. . .enhance the control and hierar-
chical superiority of the therapist at the cost of the greater sense
of
self-esteem produced by the assumption of greater autonomy,
as-
sertiveness, and responsibility by patients, (p. 439)
This contradiction is consistent with differences that flow
from economic class, as well as differences in opinion as to
whether mental health treatment should be seen first as an
instrumentality that feeds and is fed by an existing social order.
Furthermore, as in all efforts to match treatment to culture,
there are wide individual and subgroup/acculturation differ-
ences in client preferences for therapist roles (Sue & Zane,
1987).
Even if there were agreement on recommendations for profes-
sional role stances toward members of different cultures, prob-
lems remain, including the difficulty for any therapist of mani-
festing flexible role behaviors and maintaining authenticity.
More fundamentally, many forms of treatment have theoretical
requirements for specific role characteristics of the therapist.
How much cultural variation in therapist behavior could be
allowed without wounding the core of client-centered therapy,
or psychoanalysis, or any theory driven treatment?
Ethnicity ofTherapists: The Question of Privileged Status
When there are pervasive social attitudes from one culture
toward another, are they likely to infiltrate the therapeutic rela-
tionship? Spurlock (1985) and Sykes (1987) both recommend an
early and frank discussion of the comfort of the client in Black/
White cross-cultural treatment, thus assisting both parties to
understand racially based attitudes. Greene (1985), following
Kupers (1981), articulates four general stances that are expres-
sions of racism, and for which White therapists are enjoined to
self-examination. They are (a) bigotry, "a conscious or uncon-
scious belief in White supremacy and as a consequence, the
feeling that the Black patient's problems are an outgrowth of
the patient's inferiority"; (b) color blindness, which "may repre-
sent the therapist's resistance to confronting the meaning of the
color difference'1, (c) paternalism, which "involves the attribu-
tion of all of the patient's problems to society and the effects of
racism. To do this will fail to help patients to understand any
role they may have in their dilemma"; and (d), "often a result of
the therapist's racial guilt, is the unquestioning compliance
with the rhetoric of Black power . . . [which] can result in a
failure or reluctance to set appropriate limits or interpret act-
ing-out." The Black patient may consciously or unconsciously
put the White therapist to a series of tests to determine the
acceptance as an individual. "It remains, however, the thera-
pist's responsibility to be familiar with the Black patient's cul-
ture to some extent, and with his or her own personal feelings
and motivations for and about working with Black patients"
(Greene, 1985; all quotations in this paragraph are from pp.
392-393). Is there any acceptable stance? Or are same-ethnicity
therapists privileged in knowledge and attitude and thus in
power of effectiveness?
College youths have clear preferences for counselors who are
like themselves—in many dimensions, not only ethnicity Sam-
ples give somewhat different results, depending on geographi-
cal location, but by and large students prefer counselors who
are well educated and of the same ethnicity and gender and who
share their attitudes and values; by and large students report
themselves more likely to use counseling services when their
preferences are met (Atkinson, Furlong, & Poston, 1986; Atkin-
son, Poston, Furlong, & Mercado, 1989; Haviland, Horswill,
(JConnell, & Dynneson, 1983; Ponterotto, Alexander, & Hink-
ston. 1988). Preference is not the same as effectiveness. In a
discussion of counseling with Mexican-American youth, De-
Blassie (1976) insists that a therapist need not be Hispanic to be
effective. Rather, common humanity is sufficient, and if solid
counseling skills and correct, generous counselor attitudes are
present, so too will be effectiveness. However, this author goes
on to report many areas of values and beliefs that are arguably
specific to Mexican-American youth, knowledge of which is
critical to empathic understanding (DeBlassie. 1976).
Sue (1988), who has labored long in the field of culture and
treatment, reviews the literature on ethnic matching of thera-
pist and client in psychotherapy and finds contradictory and
inconclusive evidence as to whether matching is superior. He
distinguishes between ethnic membership (which emphasizes
national or geographic origin of ancestors) and cultural mem-
bership (which emphasizes current identifications with the
group or groups, and their commonalities of values, attitudes,
motives, etc.). Although concluding that ethnic matching is
irrel-
evant, whereas cultural matching is likely to be an authentic
distal variable affecting outcome, Sue urges researchers to con-
sider more proximal variables, such as how cultural knowledge
is translated into particular therapeutic behaviors and deci-
sions. This position emphasizes the therapist's capacity for
correct understanding and for comfortable communication.
The issue is not whether patients are treated more effectively by
same-race, same-class, or same-sex therapists, but whether the
therapists' interpretation of the clients' cultural experience
creates the ambience that is necessary to establish rapport and
an
empathic bond which facilitates the therapeutic process.
(Juarez,
1985, p. 441)
This resolution is similar to that espoused for cultural re-
search by the Cuban-American anthropologist Dominguez
(1985; 1986), who has worked both as "member" and "outsider"
in cultural research. Her position is that "native" members' ac-
counts of their own situation may well be privileged, because of
their intimate, subjective, and empathic knowledge. This does
not excuse native anthropologists' accounts from the disci-
plines of their scholarship and profession and does not invali-
date the outsider anthropologist's account, over which in terms
of objectivity the native account is not ipso facto privileged. By
analogy, it appears that psychological treatment, relying so
heavily on both subjective and objective accuracy of perception,
must attempt to maximize that accuracy in a variety of ways.
Ethnic matching may contribute, but as a matter of practicality
it is not an available strategy, and were it to be adopted, it
would
804 ROLAND G. THARP
exclude minority-culture therapists from treating most clients.
Are there other ways?
Culturally Specific Treatment Modalities
Many writers (e.g^ da Silva, 1984) urge therapists to become
aware of ethnogenetically determined patterns of help-seeking.
For example, folk healers may be approached for folk diseases
and scientific healers sought for infectious diseases, wounds,
and refractory emotional disorders. These patterns for some
Hispanic Americans are discussed in Tharp and Meadow
(1973). Among urban Colorado Hispanic-American women,
20% had visited a curandero (folk healer) for treatment, and
12%
had taken one or more of their children for treatment (Rivera,
1988). These treatments are far from conventional psychologi-
cal treatments in form, and they may involve prayers, rest, rit-
ual, and the ingestion of herbs. Many progressive treatment
centers provide for cooperative work among scientific and folk
practitioners.
More than specific forms, there are presumptions and meta-
processes in the traditional treatment practices of many cul-
tures that are drastically different from values inherent in Euro-
pean and American mental health treatment. LaFromboise,
Trimble, and Mohatt (1990) provide an excellent description of
American Indian healing processes, in which "religion, physiol-
ogy, and psychology intertwine" (p. 632) and where traditional
healing encourages the client to transcend the ego by experienc-
ing self as embedded in and expressive of community. Tradi-
tional ceremonies reinforce adherence to cultural values and
remind participants of the importance of family and commu-
nity networks. Indeed, the family, kin, and friends coalesce into
an interlocking network to observe and understand and to inte-
grate the individual back into the social life of the group. Un-
like conventional psychological treatments, American Indian
healing ceremonies and procedures usually involve the client's
family and community members along with the healer and
client. This collective treatment of the entire network "heals"
not only the individual, but reaffirms the norms and solidarity
of the entire group (Kaplan & Johnson, 1964).
A third route toward cultural compatibility is to specifically
design treatment modalities on and for members of specific
cultures. An excellent example is cuento therapy, developed for
Puerto Rican children by Costantino, Malgady, and Rogler
(1986), but no doubt equally appropriate for other Hispanic
groups for whom cuentos (cautionary folktales) are familiar so-
cialization devices. In this technique, cuentos that were either
traditional or adapted to reflect current conflicts in American
life were read to high-risk kindergarten through third-grade
children by their (bilingual, bicultural) therapists and their
mothers, and a discussion was then fostered exploring the
meaning of the tale. Results indicated that cuento therapy sig-
nificantly reduced children's trait anxiety relative to traditional
therapy and to no intervention, and this trend was stable over 1
year. Cuento therapy also increased Wechsler Intelligence Test
for Children-revised (WISC-R) Comprehension subtest
scores. The authors discuss this technique as a modeling ther-
apy, which is attractive to the children because of the cultural
familiarity of the story characters, and as a cognitive develop-
ment task, useful because of the familiarity of the story-telling
modality itself. They also point out the relevance of the litera-
ture on cognitive and social development of disadvantaged mi-
nority children, who respond well to fairy-tale, story-telling
formats in classroom contexts. We should also note that the
involvement of the mothers is another feature highly recom-
mended by most students of Latin-American child treatment,
regardless of treatment modality.
Choice of Treatment Levels and Modalities:
Compatibility Through Context
However desirable specific-culture treatments are held to be,
it must be noted that very few have been designed and reported,
although the issue of cultural compatibility has been with us for
nearly 40 years. The history of the cultural compatibility move-
ment in education has a similar pattern. In spite of heroic ef-
forts, almost no culturally "invented" specific educational mo-
dalities have been designed to survive the practicalities of
schools. In the culture and education movement, most
compatibilities have been established through choosing estab-
lished modalities that per se allow for greater influence of the
child's culture, or at least do not demand incompatible child
behavior. The majority of mental health programs for minority
children in the literature appear to be using that same tactic: By
electing modalities that naturally include family and commu-
nity members and settings, some compatibility is assured by the
objective introduction of the cultural context. I will discuss
those modalities in an order that represents a rough progression
from lesser toward greater contextualization in the client's cul-
ture.
Psychotherapy
In the eyes of the public, individually conducted talking
treatment conducted by a professional is the paradigmatic
treatment form. Many clients of many cultures insist on receiv-
ing it because they have been convinced of its primacy and are
well aware of its elevated status. Indeed psychotherapy is not
per se ineffective for any culture (Sue, 1988); it can be made to
work for clients. For example, Paster (1985) makes a heroic
argument for the adaptations needed to psychodynamic psycho-
therapy so that it can be used with depressed, acting-out Black
male adolescents. Tyler, Sussewell, and Williams-McCoy
(1985)
address the issue of providing culturally sensitive services by
an
attempt to preserve the basic structure and concepts of psycho-
therapy while reconciling the universalist and particularist po-
sitions into a "transcendist" perspective of an "ethnic validity
model" that stresses that "persons from different ethnic/racial
backgrounds are psychosocially different but that under some
circumstances those differences can be transcended in ther-
apy" (p. 312).
With what reliability and regularity can therapists expect to
establish that transcendence? It appears to this writer that, be-
cause of two basic conditions, importing culture into individual
child psychotherapy is heavy freight indeed. First, individual
psychotherapy is a culturally specific form of treatment, just as
surely as is the sweat lodge or the herbalism of the curandero;
and second, of all the modalities at the command of the mental
health professions, individual psychotherapy most rigidly ex-
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 805
eludes family or community members who might in feet import
the cultural context. In any event, the preponderance of writers
propose forms of treatment for minority children that struc-
turally involve participants who are themselves culture carriers.
Group Therapy
Kahn, Lewis, and Galvez (1974) reported a counseling pro-
gram for miscreant Papago Indian youth. The authors quickly
moved from an individual to a group therapy format, although
they improvised a number of changes to the usual confessional-
and-problem-sharing approach:
The cultural truth that Papagos are very reluctant to reveal per-
sonal and intimate details to others, particularly in a setting
with
their peers, appeared to be a major hurdle in developing much
group [therapy] interaction. It appeared that the main thing sus-
taining the group in early sessions was the money being paid for
attendance . . . [then] the therapists selected what they judged
would be a relevant problem or topic for these boys. The first
part
of the sessions [was] given to formal presentations.. . . How to
meet girls, venereal disease, homosexuality, drinking and
alcohol-
ism, the role of the male in the family, etc.. . . The boys would
talk about subjects or ask questions in the abstract or in terms
of a
third person." (Kahn, Lewis, & Galvez, p. 239-240)
The boys also confronted the [non-Indian] therapists about
their motivations and real reasons for working with them on the
reservation. They did develop regular attendance and much
improved arrest and school truancy rates.
Recourse to group treatment of children of cultures unfamil-
iar to the therapist can have many advantages, including the
socialization of new clients into an unfamiliar process, as well
as providing a socialization of the therapist into the client cul-
ture. Spurlock (1985) presents an instructive case in which a
White resident treats Black adolescents in just such a situation.
Problem Solving and Social Skills Training
Many writers recommend for Native American clients a
structured, problem-solving, skill-training approach, which
teaches through modeling and rehearsing those everyday skills
that are useful to Indian adolescents in adaptive living (e.g,
Long, 1983; Kahn et al, 1974; LaFromboise & Low, 1989; La-
Fromboise & Rowe, 1983; LaFromboise et al, 1990). Such pro-
grams have been successfully applied with Indian adolescents
to reduce substance abuse (Bobo, Cvetkovich, Gilchrist, Trim-
ble, & Schinke, 1987; Schinke et al, 1988), and adolescent sui-
cide (LaFromboise & BigFoot, 1988).
Globetti (1988) has reviewed alcohol education programs for
minority youth and concludes that similar programs are suited
for African-American and Hispanic youth as well. He does not
avoid the question of whether such an approach can be effective
without corresponding changes in family and neighborhood
conditions or without correction of the feelings of anomie, feel-
ings of oppression, marginal status, and the unemployment of
minority poverty. However, among Black, Hispanic, and Indian
minority communities, there are resources available for com-
munity treatment, including a strong abstinence sentiment es-
pecially among women and youth. A psychosocial skills ap-
proach reaches youth before they enter the age where drinking
patterns and attitudes become rigid. This approach emphasizes
values awareness and enhances self-esteem and a sense of self-
power through increasing abilities for stress reduction, decision
making, and behavioral alternatives such as assertiveness, re-
fusal skills, and social competency. Even minority youths in
effective social training groups for alcoholism prevention are
concerned with problem drinking by their family members.
Edwards and Edwards (1988) urge that Indian families be in-
volved, including nondrinking members and the extended fam-
ily
Family Therapy
Family therapy is often recommended by therapists and theo-
rists who are concerned to establish more compatible and more
effective treatment. Several considerations enter into this
broad-based recommendation. For example, consider that tan-
gle of racial attitudes, transferences, countertransferences, and
guilt that Greene (1985) cautions against in White treatment of
Black adolescents. Sykes (1987) recommends structured family
therapy (short-term, goal-directed) as a way of avoiding those
tangles, because the presence of the family establishes a Black
context in which the White therapist is a facilitator and in
which immediate problem solving is the focus.
The emphasis on extended family and on the individual as
part of a family in Asian culture is discussed by Chin (1983).
The Western view of family is more restrictive and based on the
nuclear family with the ultimate goal of a separation from the
family. The emphasis on nuclear family boundaries and
individual
"privacy" in Western families, when imposed on Asian-
American
families, often fails to appreciate the supportive networks,
social
cohesiveness, and sense of affiliation of the extended family.
The
diagnostic process needs to examine the indusiveness and inter-
dependence of kinship relationships in terms of how they might
facilitate the therapeutic process." (p. 107)
Because family therapy brings kin into the process itself, they
are less likely to be overlooked in the diagnostic process. In the
same way, family therapy mobilizes the available family re-
sources for impact on the situation of the child. Kim (1985)
endorses structured family therapy as the presumptive treat-
ment for children of Asian Americans and provides a valuable
discussion of the issues likely to arise in Asian families; merely
to resort to family treatment is no final escape for the therapist
from the need for cultural knowledge.
Structured family therapy was recommended by Inclan
(1985) for Puerto Rican and other Hispanic clients. Structural
family therapy was found to protect the integrity of the family
at 1-year follow-up, more than did psychodynamic child ther-
apy, although the two treatments were apparently equal and
superior to control conditions for 69 six- to twelve-year-old
His-
panic boys (Szapocznik, Rio, Murray, & Cohen, 1989).
Family therapy is recommended for Latin-American school
children by Vazquez-Nuttal, Avila-Vivas, and Morales-Barreto
(1984) because of the strong Hispanic emphasis on the family.
These authors recommend that the therapist explore the im-
portance of ethnic values and traditions to the family, the strain
between traditional family values and contemporaneous Ameri-
can life, intergenerational strains that are likely loci of these
value conflicts, and the extent to which the family relies on
neighborhood and school institutions.
806 ROLAND G. THARP
As Falicov and Brudner-White (1983) have discussed, the ac-
ceptance of the extended family as the unit of analysis reveals
vulnerabilities and sources of growth enhancement that are not
dreamed of in nuclear-family-based treatment tactics and the-
ories. Black families, even those of the stereotypical father-ab-
sent, unemployed-mother pattern, often have strengths and re-
sources in extended family and social networks that are com-
pletely overlooked by the therapist with a narrow view of
"family" (Spurlock, 1985). Juarez (1985) also discusses the best
fit for Hispanic children and recommends family therapy, but
also a form of megafamily treatment, "network therapy," a col-
laborative involvement of all significant members of the child's
social world.
temic interaction with those of the network members. Network
therapy is held to combat the depersonalization of the accultur-
ating, urban environment; and because it mobilizes the
strength of the support network, it contextualizes treatment
directly in the cultural nexus of family and community
members. Network therapy has been discussed as a specific
culturally consistent treatment and prevention approach for
American Indian communities (LaFromboise et al, 1990). In
Native Hawaiian communities, the entire network of family
and support participants are involved in a traditional process of
healing and reconciliation called hob'ponopono; Mokuau (1990)
has reported on the practice and its implications and possibili-
ties for the treatment of children.
Home-Based Treatment
Home-based programs embed treatment in a cultural
surround and thus are far less insulated from the influences of
home, family, and community. "The most well-documented,
effective therapeutic treatment with lower socioeconomic class
Blacks has been home-visiting programs aimed at supporting
and counseling mothers of young children" (Thomas &
Dansby, 1985, p. 400), particularly those such as described in
Gray and Ruttle (1980), that have positive effects on child lan-
guage development and a sustained improvement in mothers'
teaching styles. These criteria are well met by the carefully
evaluated home-visiting programs of Roberts and his asso-
ciates, working principally with Native Hawaiians (Roberts,
Wasik, Casto, & Ramey, 1991; Roberts & Magrab, 1991). These
authors rightly urge that culturally compatible programs must
be continually vitalized by a staff committed to the principle of
compatibility and influenced by community participation.
Even more deeply embedded in context is the Homebuilders
program, which trains, places, and supports professional
workers who become temporarily resident or quasi-resident in
the homes of their children clients, most of whom have already
been targeted for placement outside of the home (Kinney, Haa-
pala, & Booth, 1991). Although home-maintenance success
rates are impressive for all categories of children, children of
color had a significantly higher chance of remaining at home
during and after Homebuilders treatment than did their White,
non-Hispanic counterparts (Fraser, Pecora, & Haapala, 1991).
These data are consistent with the view that contextualization
of service is particularly critical for minority children.
Network Treatment
Network therapy (see Schoenfeld, Halevy-Martini, Hemley-
Van der Velden, & Ruhf, 198 5,1986, for descriptions and
evalua-
tions) involves a group of family, relatives, and friends who are
organized into a network. It is informal in operation. The thera-
pist's role is to catalyze and conduct the process, but the forces
of healing and correction are those of the support system of the
network itself. Ordinarily conducted in the home, it can include
50 people or more. Network therapy can be used for solving a
common or shared problem of the clan, family, or community,
or the network can be mobilized in response to the problem of a
single child or adolescent. Even in the latter case, the patient's
behaviors, experiences, and goals are understood in their sys-
Community Intervention
Contextuality may be seen as extending from family to ex-
tended family to community to the entire ecocultural niche.
Culturally oriented writers consistently call for an ecologically
oriented, contextualized analysis of psychological phenomena
and consequently a diagnosis and treatment scheme that lo-
cates the child in the sociocultural nexus—and indeed recog-
nizes that the intervention target for some child problems lies
in family, school, or community (Huang & Gibbs, 1989). Illus-
trating that critical difference, under new environmental con-
ditions of migration, displacement, or community change, the
family and the school themselves may become sources of stress
for the child and lose their value as social support networks
(Canino, Earley, & Rogler, 1980).
Ramirez (1980) discusses the complexity of family and com-
munity as problem/support units among urban Mexican Ameri-
cans. His study found that the propinquity and size of extended
family was positively related to mental health status among
Mexican Americans in Detroit. But nearness in time to Mexico
and markers of traditional culture were negatively related to
mental health status. Apparently an effective support structure
develops slowly, postmigration. It appears that as a family mi-
grates, it slowly develops a network, gains education, and im-
proves in mental health status. Furthermore, family support is a
structural phenomenon that operates in a mundane, solid, and
workaday manner: The presence of someone in the network
who offers "emotional understanding" and such "counselor-
like" qualities is unrelated to mental health status. Assisting a
referred child in a recent immigrant community may require
assisting a family, group of families, or entire community to
organize for mutual assistance.
An example of a developmentally oriented community psy-
chology program is that of OTJonnell and Tharp (1990), which
is based on the principles of neo-Vygotskian socio-historical
theory. In their analyses, the real client of the consultation is
the
social system that produces or maintains the problem. No per-
manent improvement in a child's condition can be expected
unless the sustaining context is also assisted to change. The
consultant must locate the levers of influence that can bring
about some reorganization. The ultimate goal of the consultant
is to empower and to increase the self-assistance of the commu-
nity and thus the growth or development of its members, partic-
ularly the referred child or children. The route to that goal is
through reorganization of activity settings. Through the pro-
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 807
cesses of interaction and assisted performance in activity set-
tings, the targeted community members gain greater compe-
tence, skill level, and solidarity. The basic task of intervention
is
to design new or changed activity settings in which that growth
and development can occur. The influence of the consultant is
used to generate or organize the resources necessary to produce
those activity settings. The consultant, working in the triadic
model (Tharp & Note, 1988; Tharp & Wetzel, 1969), exercises
the consulting effects through mediators who are themselves
members of the settings or of the community context.
A fine example of such a program is that for prevention of
drug abuse by American Indian youth by the Indian Drug Pre-
vention Program (IDPP) of Washington State (Bobo et al,
1987). The consultants established an all-Indian advisory
board, worked with members to establish goals and guidelines,
developed together a curriculum of skills training (modeling,
instructing, and practicing), contacted gatekeepers of services
in six different communities, located venues and activity set-
tings in which the program could be operated, trained and
cooperated with local schools and other agencies, and left the
communities competent to carry out the positively evaluated
program. Other examples of similar community programs are
available in Edwards and Edwards (1988).
The IDPP operates primarily in schools. The movement to-
ward integrated mental health services in schools is particularly
important for cultural minorities. Red Horse (1982) described a
short-lived but well-conceived program that operated for In-
dian public school adolescent girls in Minnesota. It included
credit-bearing social skills training and psychosocial develop-
mental materials, cultural enrichment materials provided by
Indian elders, pregnancy prevention, group counseling, and a
professional staif of counselors who assisted the girls to bridge
and build extended family relationships both in the city and on
the nearby reservation. The creation of new activity settings
and the mobilization of resourcesof the family and community
to assist the performance of the adolescent girls is a model of
community program design. Another exemplary program is a
school psychology program for Hispanics that involves "sys-
tems networking among the nuclear and extended family, com-
padres, church personnel, indigenous support systems, and
school [which] affords pooling of human, informational, and
mental health resources for more effective, efficient, and eth-
nospecific services for Hispanics" (Rosado, 1986, p. 196).
Status of the Evidence for the Cultural
Compatibility Hypotheses
As we have now seen, there is virtual unanimity among
writers that some degree of compatibility between culture and
treatment is necessary. That unanimity, however, does not con-
clude the case. A useful understanding of compatibility requires
evidence that will allow unpacking the concept into its potent
and inert components. The necessary breadth of that evidence
is not yet present in the child-treatment literature. The absence
can be felt especially in the paucity of process studies of child
treatment, where interactions could be examined by charting
flows and glitches against the culturally based repertoires of
participants.
Evidence of that sort is present to a greater degree in the
study of child education. Although these parallel data can be
taken only as suggestive, they can help to formulate hypotheses
for research in the clinical and consulting context. In education
research on children, there is a substantial body of evidence
demonstrating cultural diversities in at least four domains that
make a difference in educational process and outcome: in lin-
guistics and sociolinguistics, cognition, motivation (state and
trait), and social organizational proclivities (reviewed in Tharp,
1989). As an illustration of the consequences for schooling,
effective monocultural classrooms for Native Indian children
have been shown to be markedly different from those for Native
Hawaiian children, in social organization, in patterns of child
control, in management of gender interaction, and in the pat-
terns of the teaching conversation between and among children
and teacher. As one example of an "unpacked" cultural vari-
able, sociolinguistic studies have demonstrated crucial differ-
ences in the courtesies and conventions of conversation for dif-
ferent ethnic groups, differences that have strong emotional
effects on children and condition the child's reaction to instruc-
tion, from willing engagement to hostile withdrawal.
Although this might be taken as evidence for the "strong"
cultural compatibility solution of designing classrooms for spe-
cific cultures, specific-culture classrooms are largely impracti-
cal. The typical classroom of our nation today is mwft/cultural.3
Of course, clinical services are not, and a variety of specific
culturally compatible services might be feasible within existing
service delivery institutional structures.
Before leaping to recommendations, however, we must note
in that same review of culture-and-education research (Tharp,
1989) considerable evidence for the weak or "two-type" form of
the hypothesis—that the children of all those cultures who typi-
cally underachieve in schools share the requirement for a modi-
fied kind of education that is not the same as that provided by
the majority-oriented schools. The educational research evi-
dence speaks for two conditions shared by successful
classrooms for children of underachieving minorities: (a) con-
textualization of learning activities into settings and topics that
are meaningful in the children's daily life and (b) assisting chil-
dren in developing the language of instruction, the latter assur-
ing the child of adequate participation in the learning activities.
These two factors are consistent with the evidence for child
treatment reviewed in this article: Contextualization of treat-
ment in the meanings and processes of cultural life is the most
insistent urging of all who work in child culture and mental
health. And logic dictates that treatment cannot be effective
unless it is delivered in a language the child and the family can
understand and use.
However, the universalist position does not lack evidence
either. For example, just as in the clinical domain, specific edu-
cational "treatments" growing from the institutions of specific
cultures have not emerged in any great number and in general
3 Certainly there are monocultural classrooms present in
society,
and these have been of great interest to researchers in culture
and
education because they allow cultural processes in teaching and
learn-
ing to emerge clearly. Likewise, investigating both process and
out-
come studies of various treatment modalities within a single
culture is
a research strategy of promise.
808 ROLAND G. THARP
have not survived the realities and necessities of schooling.
Rather, the cultural-compatibility movement in education ap-
pears to have settled on the least-change principle (Tharp et al.,
1984), which calls not for inventing entire new pedagogies or
teaching modalities, but for the careful selection of modalities
of demonstrated effectiveness in real schools and by working
teachers. The selection of such modalities may be quite differ-
ent for children of different cultures, and it is certain that the
instantiation of the modalities will be modified by contextua-
lizing them in the experience and language of the children's
daily lives. This appears to be entirely consistent with the expe-
rience of the more youthful culture-and-treatment movement.
Few specific treatment modalities have been offered. Certain
modalities, however, are overwhelmingly preferred by thera-
pists knowledgeable of certain cultures; and each instantiation
is recommended to be conditioned by the culture. Thus, family
therapy is repeatedly recommended for Hispanic children, but
the recommendation is equally strong that the family must be
treated in ways that reflect that family's composition, values,
and language.
On the basis of this discussion, it appears that the most via-
ble hypothesis is a universalist one, but not the "unrversalist"
hypothesis as previously understood. That is, clinical service
for everyone must be contextualized in the values, processes,
and language of the clients' culture, and that is the universalist
principle. Existing services are already contextualized for ma-
jority, English-speaking culture members because treatment
procedures grew out of that culture and language. Were it not
so, prescription would have to see to it.
When the strong, the weak, and the null forms of a hypothe-
sis are all verified, we know the question needs rephrasing.
Evidence and inference suggest the following as the best next
direction for investigation. Cultural compatibility, rather than
an accommodation needed for certain subsets of people, should
be understood as an aspect of a universally required contextua-
lization of services. The appropriate question would then be-
come: How can therapists, program developers, and re-
searchers develop heuristics for assuring the conditions of con-
text uality and language accessibility? Some previous inquiry is
available to that question.
Linguistic and Sociolinguistic Accessibility of Services
The problem of assessing and providing treatment across a
language barrier is central in the provision of services to chil-
dren of diverse cultures. This issue is most familiar to psychol-
ogy as an issue in testing: English-language interaction, in bi-
lingual children, is not a clear window through which cognitive
functioning can be estimated. And the problems associated
with attempting treatment when the therapist is not competent
in the basic language of the clients are formidable indeed.
In an effort to increase the linguistic accessibility, bilingual
and bicultural staff may be incorporated into the staff of men-
tal health facilities, as advocated early by Scott and Delgado
(1979). Acosta and Cristo (1981) describe a clinic for Hispanics
that recruited community members to serve as translators for
the English-speaking professional staff. Training was provided
in the basic concepts and vocabulary of psychotherapy. These
translators also served as cultural informants, explaining to the
therapists the meaning of some of the client's topics in therapy.
There is a danger, however, as pointed out by Lappin (1983),
in having a "house ethnic," often a paraprofessional, who is
called in to translate. Many individuals and cultural groups
want a professional regardless of race. In fact, there is a danger
in using any translator, who becomes a switchboard, a distorter,
and an additional principal in an already complex interaction.
Therapists must be cautious in using the most bilingual family
member as translator for all, particularly when this is the child,
who, while most flexible in language use may also be most
psychologically vulnerable to the dynamic forces of family and
therapist interaction. Nevertheless, there is evidence that pro-
vision of Spanish language opportunities in clinic programs
does increase utilization rates in Hispanic communities
(Rogler, Malgady, Costantino, & Blumenthal, 1987).
However, it is not simply language code proficiency that
creates misunderstandings and frustrations in cross-cultural
service delivery Differing Sociolinguistic and paralinguistic
patterns are barriers to communication. For example, Michaels
(1984) has shown that children of different cultures tell their
stories in different ways, with startling audience effects. In her
study, White children were topic-centered in their narratives,
with thematic cohesion and a temporal reference. Black chil-
dren used a topic-associating style consisting of a series of im-
plicitly associated anecdotal segments with no explicit state-
ment of an overall theme or point. White adults criticized the
topic-associating style as incoherent, but Black adults found it
interesting with lots of detail and description. It is apparent that
this cultural difference in basic language structure can lead to
quite different judgments and predictions in the treatment
room.
Process studies of cross-cultural treatment of children are
lacking. Again we must look to culture-and-education, where
such research is growing. A recent review (Tharp, 1989) reveals
that in determining relationship, learning, and satisfaction in
cross-cultural settings, there is enormous weight in variables of
the courtesies and conventions ofcomersation, such as the
length
of pause between speakers (wait-time), rhythm of speech and
event, and participation structures (the patterns and conditions
for speaking and listening). When Sociolinguistic school/home
compatibilities are present, children are more comfortable, and
they participate and display their abilities appropriately As one
example, Black migrant children, whose schools view them as
below grade level and unresponsive, speak and behave with
complexity and competence in home settings. In addition, they
exhibit full competence and full participation at church. The
similarities of Sociolinguistic and behavioral conventions be-
tween church and home offer an example to schools of how
formal institutions can engage their young by compatibilities of
expectations with child repertoires (Lein, 1975).
Many strategies have been devised for achieving linguistic
compatibility for children in schools. Because schooling is so
heavily language dependent, a first-order goal for effective edu-
cation is to provide adequate language development. This goal
is very likely parallel to child treatment, which is also a
learning
modality heavily dependent on language exchanges. To the de-
gree that treatment does occur through language, we may ex-
pect that special attention will be required to ensure linguistic
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 809
and sociolinguistic compatibilities. This makes cross-cultural
psychotherapy particularly problematic for both therapist and
client. In the dimension of language, it seems clear that culture
members are indeed privileged, and same-language therapists
are advantageous to clients. However, when therapists do serve
clients of a different language, compensatory strategies are
available, the most powerful of which is to contextualize treat-
ment in the culture of the client. By importing other family and
community members into the treatment itself, familiar lan-
guage becomes a part of the treatment context.
Contextuality of Treatment
Sue and Zane (1987) observe that therapist/client cultural
matching is a distal variable and that efficacy lies in the proxi-
mal issues of how well the therapist is able to (a) meet the
client's conceptualization of the problem, (b) require acceptable
behaviors as means for problem resolution, and (c) share goals
for treatment. The literature considered here suggests that the
probability of those proximal conditions being achieved is lim-
ited by the degree to which social, linguistic, and other process
variables are congruent. There seems little doubt that all else
being equal, culturally matched psychologists and clients are
more likely to achieve that congruence and those conditions.
Most therapists can have little hope of sufficient detailed
knowl-
edge of many (if any) cultures other than their natal one and
almost no hope of mastering a variety of linguistic and socio-
linguistic codes. This leads us to consider the probability of
achieving process congruence and the Sue and Zane conditions
as a function of the modality of treatment.
Treatment modalities for children may be arrayed on a con-
tinuum of inclusiveness or social contextuality, from individual
psychotherapy to group therapy to family therapy to network
therapy to community intervention. Historically, these modali-
ties developed in roughly that order. Thus therapists of my own
(senior) cohort were trained, by and large, in the individual
model, with less intense training in groups, less still on fami-
lies, and little direct experience of network or community
methods, that is, with a decreasing emphasis on the expanding
continuum of social contextuality Although that pattern has
eased in many training programs during the past 30 years, there
is perhaps a residual tropism toward the individual end of the
continuum in the prescriptive inclinations of our field.
If the reading of the literature offered here is correct, a rever-
sal of that strategy is indicated. That is, for a therapist facing a
client across a cultural chasm, the treatment of first consider-
ation should be community intervention; that of second consid-
eration, network therapy; that of third, family treatment;
fourth, group treatment; and last of all, individual treatment.
The work of therapy is potent when the therapist and child
share so much that the therapist can presume a thorough knowl-
edge of the workings of the child's family and community and
can teach the youngster knowingly, confidently, and credibly
Because Euro-American culture has produced most therapists
and most clients, most clients and therapists have been
matched in culture and language. Insulated within a common
culture, we have not recognized cultural contextuality as a vari-
able because it has not varied. We have not noticed the basic
condition that because it is decontextualized, individual ther-
apy depends more than any other modality on shared values,
semiotics, habits, and expectations between therapist and
client. When these are not present, resources of commonality
have to be provided by expanding to family/network/commu-
nity interventions, even for majority culture members. It ap-
pears that the conclusions available from the culture and treat-
ment literature are not restricted to strategies for exotic culture
members. As we should have expected, an expanded range of
observations leads to a more comprehensive universalism. It is
paradoxical but welcome, that facing the problems of treating
others is teaching us how better to treat ourselves.
Implications for Research and Theoretical Programs
Humanity has a common base, provided by evolutionary
processes; but over long periods of historical time, ethnoge-
netic processes have put our common humanity through differ-
ent filters. To recover our common features, we must study
those filters and come to know how historical processes have
created differing psychological and behavioral realities for dif-
ferent peoples. These differences are not trivial, and they are
not surface. They include differences in motivation, cognition,
social organizational principles, communication patterns, val-
ues, and semiotic structures, as well as accustomed ways of
teaching and learning. Further research into ethnogenetic pro-
cesses is a necessity for advances in culture-and-treatment and
also holds promise for the discovery of a broader range of fun-
damental processes of psychology and sociology. This agendum
falls within the program of the sociohistorical theoretical move-
ment (e.g., Cole, 1985; Tharp & Gallimore, 1988; Vygotsky,
1929,1978; Wertsch, 1985).
On the other hand, just because we have been blind to "cul-
ture," we must not now be blinded by it. Each culture member
has also had a different life history and accumulation of learn-
ing. Thus ethnogenetic effects are filtered and conditioned by
ontogenetic and microgenetic processes; and in our rush to
solve problems of treatment arising from cultural differences,
we must not overlook the resulting wide range of variance
within cultures. Indeed the definitions and boundaries of eth-
nicities are under constant negotiation and redefinition (Do-
minguez, 1986). Psychology comes to the porch of cultural
stud-
ies just as the volatility of the concept of ethnicity is coming
into focus, and although belated, our arrival may well be wel-
comed by the house of social science. Individuals define them-
selves in and out of ethnic identities, and that psychological
identification is of more moment in treatment issues than is
that of race. Psychologists have the means to understand those
processes and how they operate in and through psychological
treatment.
Another major research need lies within the field of clinical
and consulting psychology itself. The paucity of process studies
in the treatment of different culture members, particularly in a
variety of modalities, is the greatest single need for evidence.
Critical data and its analysis will require methods of investiga-
tion less familiar to psychology than they might be, methods
such as ethnography, microethnography, and discourse analy-
sis. Expanding our range of research operations need not await
major retooling of psychologists' methodology. Research part-
nerships made with scientists of other appropriate disciplines,
810 ROLAND G. THARP
such as sociolinguistics, anthropology, or community develop-
ment, could allow an immediate infusion of data that reveal
cultural processes in their immediacy and at their points of
impact—in the situations and communications of treatment
itself.4
Are there immediate, imperative, specific hypotheses for in-
vestigation? Certainly all conclusions offered above have the
status of hypothesis. As much as on direct evidence, this analy-
sis perforce relied on reasoning and analogy. My strategy was to
suggest the figure in the ground by putting those tiles in the
mosaic as placeholders, pending the arrival of more direct evi-
dence drawn from the treatment of children. About the figure
that will eventually be clear, there is one thing that now appears
certain. These cultures meeting in the consulting room will
clarify not only the processes of the cultures themselves, but
will bring to our notice things about treatment that we do not
yet suspect.
4 As an example of how this strategy was used in the culture-
and-
educational domain, see Tharp et al. (1984).
References
Acosta, F. X, & Cristo, M. H. (1981). Development of a
bilingual
interpreter program: An alternative model for Spanish-speaking
ser-
vices. Professional Psychology, 12,474-482.
Alcohol problems and minority youth. [Special issue]. (1988).
Journal
of Drug Issues, 18(1).
Alarcon, R. D. (1983). A Latin American perspective on DSM-
III.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 140,102-104.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and
statistical
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association. (1990). Guidelines for
providers
of psychological services to ethnic and culturally diverse
populations.
Washington, DC: Author.
Arredondo, P., Orjuela, E, & Moore, L. (1989). Family therapy
with
Central American war refugee families. Journal of Strategic and
Sys-
temic Therapies, 8, 28-35.
Atkinson, D. R. (1985). Research on cross-cultural counseling
and psy-
chotherapy: A review and update of reviews. In R Pedeisen
(Ed.),
Handbook of cross-cultural counseling and therapy (pp. 191-
197).
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Atkinson, D. R., Furlong, M. J, & Poston, W C. (1986). Afro-
American
preferences for counselor characteristics. Journal ofCounseling
Psy-
chology, 33, 326-330.
Atkinson, D. R., Morton, G., & Sue, D. W (Eds.). (1989).
Counseling
American minorities: A cross-cultural perspective (3rd ed.).
Dubuque,
IA: William C. Brown.
Atkinson, D. R., Poston, W C, Furlong, M. J., & Mercado, P.
(1989).
Ethnic group preferences for counselor characteristics. Journal
of
Counseling Psychology, 36, 68-72.
Bobo, J. K., Cvetkovich, G, Gilchrist, L. D, Trimble, J. E.,
&Schinke,
S. (1987). Cross-cultural service delivery to minority
communities.
Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 263-272.
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (1991). Cultural approaches to
parenting. New
York: Erlbaum.
Canino, I. A., Earley, B. F., & Rogler, L. H. (1980). The Puerto
Rican
child in New York City: Stress and mental health. Bronx, NY:
Ford-
ham University Hispanic Research Center.
Chin, J. L. (1983). Diagnostic considerations in working with
Asian-
Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53,100-109.
Chunn, J. C., II, Dunston, P. J, &Ross-Sherifl; F. (Eds.). (1983).
Mental
health and people of color: Curriculum development and
change.
Washington, DC: Howani University Press.
Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: Where
culture
and cognition create each other. In J. V Wertsch (Ed.), Culture,
com-
munication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 146-
161).
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Costantino, G., Malgady, R. G., & Rogler, L. H. (1986). Cuento
therapy:
A culturally sensitive modality for Puerto Rican children.
Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 639-645.
da Silva, G. C. (1984). Awareness of Hispanic cultural issues in
the
health care setting. Children's Health Care, 13, 4-10.
DeBlassie, R. R. (1976). Counseling with Mexican American
youth: Pre-
conceptions and processes. Austin, TX: Learning Concepts, Inc.
Dominguez, V (1985). Intended and unintended messages: The
schol-
arly defense of onels own "people." Nieuwe West-lndischeGids,
209-
222.
Dominguez, V (1986). White by definition. New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
Edwards, E. D, & Edwards, M. E. (1988). Alcoholism
prevention/treat-
ment and Native American youth: A community approach.
Journal
of Drug Issues, 18(1), 103-114.
Everett, E, Proctor, N., & Cartmell, B. (1983). Providing
psychological
services to American Indian children and families. Professional
Psy-
chology: Research and Practice, 14, 588-603.
Falicos C. J., &. Brudner-White, L. (1983). The shifting family
triangle:
The issue of cultural and contextual relativity. In J. C. Hansen
& C.}.
Falicov (Eds.), Cultural perspectives in family therapy (pp. 51-
67).
Rockville, MD: Aspen.
[Family therapy with immigrant families], (1989). Journal of
Strategic
and Systemic Therapies, ^(Summer).
Fraser, M. W, Pecora, P. J., & Haapala, D. A. (1991).
Familiesin crisis:
The impact of intensive family preservation services.
Hawthorne, NY:
Aldine de Gruyter.
Gallimore, R., Reese, L. J., Balzano, S., Benson, C, &
Goldenberg, C.
(1991, April). Ecocultural sources of early literacy experiences:
Job-re-
auired literacy, home literacy environments, and school reading.
Paper
read at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational
Research
Association, Chicago.
Gibbs, J. T, Huang, L. N.,& Associates (Eds.). (1989). Children
of color:
Psychological intervention with minority youth. San Francisco:
Jos-
sey-Bass.
Gibson, M., &Ogbu, J. (Eds.). (1991). Minority status and
schooling: A
comparative study of immigrant and involuntary minorities.
New
York: Garland Press.
Globetti, G. (1988). Alcohol education programs and minority
youth.
Journal of Drug Issues, 18,115-129.
Gray, E., & Cosgrove, J. (1985). Ethnocentric perception of
child-rear-
ing practices in protective services. Child Abuse and Neglect,
9,389-
396.
Gray, S. W, & Ruttle, K. (1980). The family-oriented home
visiting
program: A longitudinal study Genetic Psychology Monographs,
102, 299-316.
Greene, B. A. (1985). Considerations in the treatment of black
patients
by white therapists. Psychotherapy, 22, 389-393.
Haviland, M. G., Horswill, R. K., O'Connell, J. J., & Dynneson,
V Y
(1983). Native American college students' preference for
counselor
race and sex and the likelihood of their use of a counseling
center.
Journal ofCounseling Psychology, 30, 267-270.
Ho, M. K. (1987). Family therapy with ethnic minorities.
Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Ho, M. K. (1990). Intermarried couples in therapy. Springfield,
IL:
Charles C Thomas.
Hong, G. K. (1988). A general family practitioner approach for
Asian-
American mental health services. Professional Psychology:
Research
and Practice, 19, 600-605.
SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 811
Huang, L. N, & Gibbs, J. T. (1989). Rrture directions:
Implications for
research, training, and practice. In J. T. Gibbs, L. N. Huang, &
Asso-
ciates (Eds.), Children of color: Psychological intervention with
minor-
ity youth (pp. 375-403). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Inclan, J. (1985). Variations in value orientation in mental
health work
with Puerto Ricans. Psychotherapy, 22, 324-334.
Isomura, T., Fine, S, & Lin, T. (1987). Two Japanese families:
A cul-
tural perspective. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 282-286.
Jenkins, A. H. (1985). Attending to self-activity in the Afro-
American
client. Psychotherapy, 22, 335-341.
Juarez, R. (1985). Core issues in psychotherapy with the
Hispanic
child. Psychotherapy, 22, 441-448.
Kahn, M. W, Lewis, J, & Galvez, E. (1974). An evaluation
study of
group therapy procedure with reservation adolescent Indians.
Psy-
chotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 11, 239-242.
Kaplan, B., & Johnson, D (1964). The social meaningofNavajo
psycho-
pathology and psychotherapy. In A. Kiev (Ed.), Magic, faith,
and
healing (pp. 203-299). New York: Free Press.
Kim, S. C. (1985). Family therapy for Asian Americans: A
strategic
structural framework. Psychotherapy, 22, 342-348.
King, L. M., Moody, S., Thompson, EX, & Bennett, M. (1983).
Black
psychology reconsidered: Notes toward curriculum
development.
In J. C. Chunn II, P. J. Dunston, & F. Ross-Sheriff (Eds.),
Mental
health and people of color (pp. 3-22). Washington, DC: Howard
Uni-
versity Press.
Kinney, J., Haapala, D, & Booth, C. (1991). Keeping families
together:
The homebuilders model. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Korbin, J. E. (1980). The cultural context of child abuse and
neglect.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 4, 3-13.
Krener, P G., & Sabin, C (1985). Indochinese immigrant
children:
Problems in psychiatric diagnosis. Journal of the American
Academy
of Child Psychiatry, 24.453-458.
Kupers, T. (1981). Public therapy The practice of psychotherapy
in the
public mental health clinic. New York: Macmillan.
LaFromboise, T. Q, & BigFoot, D. (1988). Cultural and
cognitive con-
siderations in the prevention of American Indian adolescent
suicide.
Journal of Adolescence, II, 139-153.
LaFromboise, T. D, & Low, K. G. (1989). American Indian
children
and adolescents. In J. T. Gibbs, L. N. Huang, & Associates
(Eds.),
Children of color: Psychological intervention with minority
youth, pp.
114-147. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
LaFromboise, T. D., & Rowe, W (1983). Skills training for
bicultural
competence: Rationale and application. Journal of Counseling
Psy-
chology, 30,589-595.
LaFromboise, T. D., Trimble, J. E., & Mohatt, G. V (1990).
Counseling
intervention and American Indian tradition: An integrative ap-
proach. Counseling Psychologist, 18, 628-654.
Lambert, M. C., Weisz, J. R., & Knight, F. (1989). Over- and
undercon-
trolled clinic referral problems of Jamaican and American
children
and adolescents: The cultural general and the culture specific.
Jour-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 467-472.
Lappin, J. (1983). On becoming a culturally conscious family
thera-
pist. In J. C. Hansen (EdJ, Cultural perspectives in family
therapy,
(pp. 122-136). Rockville, MD: Aspen.
Lein, L. (1975). "M>u were talkin' though, oh yes, you was."
Black
American migrant children: Their speech at home and school.
Council on Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 6(4), 1 -11.
Lieh-Mak, F, Lee, P W H., & Luk, S. L. (1984). Problems
encountered
in teaching Chinese parents to be behavior therapists.
Psychologia,
27, 56-64.
Long, K. (1983). The experience of repeated and traumatic loss
among
Crow Indian children: Response patterns and intervention
strate-
gies. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52,116-126.
Lum, D. (1986). Social work practice and people of color: A
process-
stage approach. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Marsella, A. J., & Higgenbotham, H. N. (1984). Traditional
Asian med-
icine: Applications to psychiatric services in developing
nations. In
P. Pedersen, N. Sartorius, & A. Marsella (Eds.), Mental health
ser-
vices: The cross cultural context (pp. 175-197). Beverly Hills,
CA:
Sage.
McGoldrick, M, Pearce, J., & Giordano, J. (Eds.). (1982).
Ethnicity and
family therapy. New York: Guilford Press.
Michaels, S. (1984). Listening and responding: Hearing the
logic in
children's classroom narratives. Theory Into Practice, 23, 218-
244.
Mokuau, N. (1990). A family centered approach in Native
Hawaiian
culture. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary
Human
Services, 71, 607-613.
Montijo, J. A. (1985). Therapeutic relationships with the poor:
A
Puerto Rican perspective. Psychotherapy, 22, 436-440.
Moritsugu, J., & Sue, S. (1983). Minority status as a stressor. In
R. D.
Felner, L. A. Jason, J. N. Moritsugu, & S. S. Farber (Eds.),
Preventive
psychology: Theory, research and practice (pp. 162-174).
Elmsford,
NY Pergamon Press.
OTJonnell, C. R., & Tharp, R. G. (1990). Community
intervention
guided by theoretical development. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen,
&
A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of behavior
modification
and therapy (2nd ed., pp. 251-266). New fork: Plenum Press.
Paster, V S. (1985). Adapting psychotherapy for the depressed,
unac-
culturated, acting-out, black male adolescent. Psychotherapy,
22,
408-417.
Pedersen, P E., & Marsella, A. J. (1982). The ethical crisis for
cross-cul-
tural counseling and therapy Professional Psychology, 13, 492-
500.
Pedersen, P. E., Sartorius, N, & Marsella, A. (Eds). (1984).
Mental
health services: The cross cultural context. Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage.
Phinney, J. S, &Rotheram, M. J. (Eds.). (1987). Children^
ethnic sociali-
zation: Pluralism and development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Ponterotto, J. G, Alexander, C, & Hinkston, J. (1988). Afro-
American
preferences for counselor characteristics: A replication and
exten-
sion. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35,175-182.
[Psychiatric care of minority groups, Special issue]. (1983).
American
Journal of Social Psychiatry, 3(2).
Psychotherapy with ethnic minorities [Special issue). (1985).
Psycho-
therapy, 22(2).
Ramirez, O. (1980). Extended family phenomena and mental
health
among urban Mexican Americans. Unpublished doctoral
disserta-
tion, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan.
Red Horse, Y. (1982). A cultural network model: Perspectives
for ado-
lescent services and paraprofessional training. In S. M. Manson
(Ed.), New directions in prevention among American Indian and
Alaska native communities (pp. 173-184). Portland, OR: Oregon
Health Sciences University.
Rivera, G. (1988). Hispanic folk medicine utilization in urban
Colo-
rado. Sociology and Social Research, 72, 237-241.
Roberts, R. N., & Magrab, P. R. (1991). The psychologist's role
in a
family-centered approach to practice, training, and research
with
young children. American Psychologist, 46,144-148.
Roberts, R. N, Wasik, B. H, Casto, G., & Ramey, C. T. (1991).
Family
support in the home: Programs, policy and social change.
American
Psychologist, 46,131-137.
Rogler, L., Malgady, R., Costantino, G, & Bhimenthal, R.
(1987). What
do culturally sensitive mental health services mean? The case of
Hispanics. American Psychologist, 42. 565-570.
Rosado, J. W, Jr. (1986). Toward an interfacing of Hispanic
cultural
variables with school psychology service delivery systems.
Profes-
sional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17,191-199.
Sartorius, N., Pedersen, P. B, & Marsella, A. J. (1984). Mental
health
services across cultures: Some concluding thoughts. In P.
Pedersen,
812 ROLAND G. THARP
N. Sartorius, & A. Marsella (Eds.), Mental health services: The
cross
cultural context, {pp. 271-279). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Schinke, S. P., Orlandi, M. A., Botvin, G. J., Gilchrist, L, D.,
Trimble,
J. E., & Locklear, V S. (1988). Preventing substance abuse
among
American Indian adolescents: A bicultural competence skills ap-
proach. Journal of Counseling Psychology 35, 87-90.
Schoenfeld, P., Halevy, J., Hemley-Van der Velden, E., & Ruhf,
L. (1986).
Long-term outcome of network therapy. Hospital and
Community
Psychiatry, 37, 373-376.
Schoenfeld, P., Halevy-Martini, J., Hemley-Van der Velden, E.,
&Ruhf,
L. (1985). Network therapy: An outcome study of! 2 social
networks.
Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 281-287.
Scott, J. E, & Delgado, M. (1979), Planning mental health
programs for
Hispanic communities. Social Casework, 60,451-456.
Spurlock, J. (1985). Assessment and therapeutic intervention of
black
children in America. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Psy-
chiatry, 24,168-174.
Stiffinan, A. R., & Davis, L. E. (Eds). (1990). Ethnicissues in
adolescent
mental health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Sue, S. (1988). Psychotherapeutic services for ethnic minorities.
Ameri-
can Psychologist, 43, 301-308.
Sue, S., Allen, D. B., & Conaway, L. (1978). The
responsiveness and
equality of mental health care to Chicanes and Native
Americans.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 6,137-146.
Sue, S., & Zane, N. (1987). The role of culture and cultural
techniques in
psychotherapy: A critique and reformulation. American
Psycholo-
gist, 42, 37-45.
Sullivan, T. (1983). Native children in treatment: Clinical,
social and
cultural issues. Journal of Child Care, 1, 75-94.
Sykes, D. K., Jr. (1987). An approach to working with black
youth in
cross cultural therapy. Clinical Social Work Journal, 15, 260-
270.
Szapocznik, J., Rio, A., Murray, E., & Cohen, R. (1989).
Structural
family versus psychodynamic child therapy for problematic His-
panic boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57,
571-
578.
Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocuhural variables and constants:
Effects on
teaching and learning in schools. American Psychologist, 44,
349-
359.
Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life:
Teaching
and learning in social context. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Tharp, R. G, Jordan, C., Speidel, G. E, Au, K. H., Klein, T. W,
Cal-
kins, R. P., Sloat, K. C. M., & Gallimore, R. (1984). Product
and
process in applied developmental research: Education and the
chil-
dren of a minority. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B.
Rogoff(Eds.),
Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 91-141).
Hills-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tharp, R. G., & Meadow, A. (1973). Differential change in folk
disease
concepts. Kevista Interamericana de Psicologia, 7, 55-63.
Tharp, R. G, & Note, M. (1988). The triadic model of
consultation:
New developments. In F. West (Ed.), School consultation:
Interdisci-
plinary perspectives on theory, research, training, and practice
(pp.
35-51). Austin, TX: Research and Training Project on School
Con-
sultation, University of Texas at Austin, & Association of
Educa-
tional and Psychological Consultants.
Tharp, R. G., & Wetzel, R. J. (1969). Behavior modification in
the natu-
ral environment. New York: Academic Press.
Thomas, M. B., &Dansby, P. G. (1985). Black clients: Family
structure,
therapeutic issues, and strengths. Psychotherapy, 22, 398-407.
Tyler, F. B, Sussewell, D. R., & Williams-McCoy, J. (1985).
Ethnic valid-
ity in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 22, 311-320.
Vazquez-Nuttal, E, Avila-Vivas, Z., & Morales-Barreto, G.
(1984).
Working with Latin American families. Family Therapy
Collections,
9, 75-90.
Vygotsky, L. (1929). The problem of the cultural development
of the
child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 36, 415-432,
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of
higher psycho-
logical processes (M. Cole, V John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E.
Souber-
man, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1988).
Unpackaging cul-
tural effects on classroom learning: Hawaiian peer assistance
and
child-generated activity. Anthropology and Education
Quarterly, 19,
327-353.
Weisz, J. R., Suwanlert, S., Chaiyasit, W, & Walter, B. R.
(1987). Over-
and under-controlled referral problems among children and
adoles-
cents from Thailand and the United States: The vat and wai of
cul-
tural differences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 55,
719-726.
Weisz,!. R, Suwanlert, S, Chaiyasit, W, Weiss, B., Achenbach,
T. M, &
Walter, B. R. (1987). Epidemiology of behavioral and emotional
problems among Thai and American children: Parent reports for
ages 6 to 11. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adoles-
cent Psychiatry, 26, 890-897.
Wertsch, J. Y (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of
mind. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Whiting, B. B. (1976). The problem of the packaged variable. In
K.
Riegel& J. A. Meacham (Eds.). The developing individual in a
chang-
ing world: Historical and cultural issues (Vol. 1, pp. 303-309).
Chi-
cago: Aldine.
Received January 9,1991
Revision received May 3,1991
Accepted May 15,1991 •
CHAPTER FOURTEEN: Juvenile Corrections: Probation,
Community Treatment, and Institutionalization
CHAPTER OUTLINE
JUVENILE PROBATION
· Historical Development
Expanding Community Treatment
Contemporary Juvenile Probation
Duties of Juvenile Probation Officers
PROBATION INNOVATIONS
· Intensive Supervision
Electronic Monitoring
Restorative Justice
Balanced Probation
Restitution
Residential Community Treatment
SECURE CORRECTIONS
· History of Juvenile Institutions
What Does This Mean to Me?
JUVENILE INSTITUTIONS TODAY: PUBLIC AND PRIVATE
· Population Trends
Physical Conditions
THE INSTITUTIONALIZED JUVENILE
· Male Inmates
FOCUS ON DELINQUENCY: Mental Health Needs of Juvenile
Inmates on the Rise
Female Inmates
CORRECTIONAL TREATMENT FOR JUVENILES
· Individual Treatment Techniques: Past and Present
Group Treatment Techniques
Educational, Vocational, and Recreational Programs
Wilderness Programs
Professional Spotlight: Kristi Swanson
Juvenile Boot Camps
THE LEGAL RIGHT TO TREATMENT
· The Struggle for Basic Civil Rights
JUVENILE AFTERCARE AND REENTRY
· Supervision
JUVENILE DELINQUENCY: Treatment: Using the Intensive
Aftercare Program (IAP) Model
Aftercare Revocation Procedures
FUTURE OF JUVENILE CORRECTIONS
LEARNING OBJECTIVES
After reading this chapter you should:
· 1. Be familiar with juvenile probation.
· 2. Know about new approaches for providing probation
services to juvenile offenders.
· 3. Understand past and current trends in the use of juvenile
institutions and key issues facing the institutionalized juvenile
offender.
· 4. Be able to identify current juvenile correctional treatment
approaches and comment on their effectiveness in reducing
recidivism.
· 5. Know about aftercare and reentry for juvenile offenders.
REAL CASES/REAL PEOPLE: Karen’s Story
Karen Gilligan, age 16, was the oldest of four children living
with their parents in a small rural community. Her mother
worked two jobs, her father was unemployed, and both parents
drank heavily. Karen’s high school attendance was sporadic.
She started to experiment with alcohol and vandalized local
businesses. After being arrested in a stolen car on several
occasions, Karen was referred to juvenile court and was put on
community supervision and probation. An initial assessment
was provided by her probation officer, and formal dispositional
recommendations were made to the court. She would remain at
home on house arrest for 60 days, attend school regularly and
maintain at least a C average, follow an alcohol and drug
assessment program, and participate in weekly family therapy
with her parents. Karen was also ordered to cooperate with the
juvenile restitution program, pay her restitution in full within
six months, and participate in the Community Adolescent
Intensive Supervision Program, as arranged by her probation
officer.
Not used to being accountable to anyone, Karen struggled
initially with all the new rules and expectations. She missed
some of her initial appointments and skipped some classes at
school. Karen’s probation officer began making unannounced
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx

More Related Content

Similar to Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx

AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxAUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxrock73
 
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docxsleeperharwell
 
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docxrobert345678
 
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docxKorean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx4934bk
 
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docx
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docxDo We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docx
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docxpetehbailey729071
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxsimonithomas47935
 
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docx
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docxMini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docx
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docxannandleola
 
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015sadtono
 
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...National Aboriginal Health Organization
 
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1Michelle Singh
 
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docxLynellBull52
 
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Andrzej Pankalla
 
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Andrzej Pankalla
 
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docx
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docxStudent PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docx
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docxdeanmtaylor1545
 
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing blazelaj2
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docx
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docxHandedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docx
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docxTangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docxperryk1
 

Similar to Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx (20)

AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docxAUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
AUTHORGerald V. Mohatt Joseph Trimble Ryan A. DicksonTITLE.docx
 
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx
8Some Implications for Research and Practice[C]ultural meanings,.docx
 
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx
22CHAPTER 2 Cultural CompetencyAchieving cultural .docx
 
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docxKorean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx
Korean American Female Perspectives on Disability Article Summary.docx
 
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docx
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docxDo We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docx
Do We Overemphasize the Role of Culture in the Behavior ofRa.docx
 
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docxData Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
Data Collection and the Topic of Your InterestData collection pr.docx
 
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docx
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docxMini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docx
Mini ResearchHow parents deal with the education.pdf 1.docx
 
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015Acculturation 12 oct 2015
Acculturation 12 oct 2015
 
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...
Self-Esteem as a Culturally-Biased Construct and Using of Narrative Story-tel...
 
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1
Ap Psychology Case Study Chapter 1
 
Oregon University September 2008 Relational Intercultural With Latino Families
Oregon University September 2008 Relational Intercultural With Latino FamiliesOregon University September 2008 Relational Intercultural With Latino Families
Oregon University September 2008 Relational Intercultural With Latino Families
 
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx
· In the methodology you should not say that you are going to look.docx
 
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
Identidad étnica como predictor del bienestar: Estudio exploratorio transcult...
 
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
Ethnic Identity as predictor for the well-being: An exploratory transcultural...
 
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docx
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docxStudent PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docx
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing .docx
 
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing
Student PaperCultural Competency in Baccalaureate Nursing
 
2002 learning from latino families afta research conference
2002 learning from latino families afta research conference2002 learning from latino families afta research conference
2002 learning from latino families afta research conference
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docx
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docxHandedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docx
Handedness and the Diverse Gender-Related Personality Traits i.docx
 
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docxTangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
 

More from croysierkathey

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docxcroysierkathey
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docxcroysierkathey
 

More from croysierkathey (20)

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology1991, Vol. 59, .docx

  • 1. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1991, Vol. 59, No. 6,799-812 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0022-006X/91/S3.00 Cultural Diversity and Treatment of Children Roland G. Tharp University of California, Santa Cruz The increasing cultural diversity of child clients has produced a cascade of new issues and concerns for psychological practice, theory, and research. Available evidence and pertinent theory are re- viewed on such topics as the predictive utility and treatment consequences of ethnic membership, whether treatments should be generic or specific to cultural groups, the degree of privilege that should be accorded to same-culture therapists, and the relative desirability of different modalities of treatment for children of different cultural groups. The concept of cultural compatibility of treatment is explored and evaluated. A broad agenda of hypotheses for research and development is
  • 2. suggested, and some guidelines for clinical practice and policy are proposed. It is concluded that insofar as possible, treatment for all children should be contextualized in their family's and commu- nity's structure of meanings, relationships, and language. The culturally diverse society that our nation has become provides a challenge to our politics, our schools, our science, and our profession. As reiterated by concerned commissions, agencies, professional organizations, and review articles in- cluding the current Guidelines for Providers of Psychological Services to Ethnic andCulturatty Diverse Populations (American Psychological Association, 1990), we are instructed that "Psy- chologists should recognize ethnicity and culture as significant parameters in understanding psychological processes" (p. 4) before delivering services. What will be necessary to satisfy this new imperative? Self-examination? New specialized training? Anthropological indoctrination, religious and spiritual relativ- ism, multilingualism, "politically correct" thought? And most to the point, what are the required practical clinical actions? In discussing these questions, Pedersen and Marsella (1982) con- clude that to know all and do all that a psychologist should is a goal that accelerates beyond us. The task is to bring some order out of this cascade of new issues and concerns, in the light of available research evidence and pertinent theory. I will suggest here that some broad out- lines for strategy are emerging, with enough clarity at least to pose an agenda of hypotheses for research and development, and possibly even to provide an initial set of guidelines for clinical practice and policy. These strategies can be stated clearly enough to allow them to be tested by both formal evalua- tion and clinical experience.
  • 3. Not to overpromise, it should be said that research on cul- tural issues in clinical treatment is scant, particularly research addressing such issues with children. We must rely on evidence from other age groups and look to related literatures (family and community) where child treatment is discussed. Cultural issues in the education of children is a field somewhat more mature, and if considered cautiously, evidence from that field can serve by analogy. Other disciplines, notably anthropology and lin- guistics, have a longer history with the study of ethnic and Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ro- land G. Tharp, Merrill College, University of California, Santa Cruz, California 95064. cultural diversity, and psychology can anticipate from their ex- perience. In attempting to discern the figure in this ground, we can fit some of these diverse tesserae into the beginning of a mosaic. Still, there are more spaces than tiles. Basic Questions Concerns arising from both theory and practice converge on four basic questions about the implications of cultural member- ship for the treatment of children. First, is there a useful level of developmental analysis, which I will call here ethnogenetic, that accounts for important current features of a childls psychology in terms of the historical forces operating on his or her ances- tors in a time frame of hundreds to thousands of years? That is, does ethnogenesis, which lies in the dimension of time between phylogenesis and ontogenesis, provide critical elements for un-
  • 4. derstanding and treating the child's present condition? Second, are there present in cultures psychosocial features so widely shared that membership is a useful guide for prescrip- tion of clinical services? Or, conversely, does membership as a guide result in stereotypical overgeneralization, and are more conventional individual and family descriptors to be preferred? Third, are there forms of treatment—either potential or ac- tual—that are specifically or uniquely suited for the treatment of children of different cultures? It is important to differentiate specific from unique. Although one or another form of therapy may be specifically suited for members of one culture, that does not mean that the modality is exclusively suited to them; family therapy, for example, might prove to be the treatment of choice of members of several cultures, whereas the sweat lodge might be uniquely suited to those of only one. Fourth, are culture members privileged in the capacity to treat or to investigate the treatment of children of their own culture? Although the literature has not necessarily developed in response to them, these questions may be borne in mind as the available knowledge is reviewed. Cultural Differences in Diagnosis and Treatment Cultural Differences in Pathology Some childhood disorders are of particular frequency in cer- tain cultural groups, because of either characteristic social 799 800 ROLAND G. THARP
  • 5. problems or psychosocial structures. Weisz, Suwanlert, Chaiya- sit, and Walter (1987) consider psychosocial differences by con- trasting Thailand, a Buddhist nation, which discourages chil- dren's aggression and encourages inhibition, peacefulness, politeness, and deference, with the United States, where inde- pendence, competitiveness, and differentiation from the family as the goal of socialization is widespread. In Thailand, children and adolescents are referred for clinic treatment more often for "overcontrolled syndrome" (fearfillness, sleep problems, soma- ticizing), whereas in the United States referred children and adolescents are reported more often with "undercontrolled syndrome" (disobedience, fighting, arguing). The same general pattern is discernible in an epidemiological study, although the culturally consistent trends are more marked in parents' deci- sions to refer for treatment than in their general ratings of their children (Weisz, Suwanlert, Chaiyasit, Weiss, et at, 1987). An- other "overcontrolled" society (Jamaican) has produced similar results (Lambert, Weisz, & Knight, 1989).
  • 6. Many describe minority status itself as a stressor, because it is often associated with hostility and prejudice. According to Moritsugu and Sue (1983) minority status also often is asso- ciated with a lack of effective support during crises and the danger of developing ineffective cognitive coping styles. The status of all minorities, however, is not the same, and neither are their characteristic stressors nor responses to them. In the field of minority education, the nature of stress and the responses to it are often differentiated for the children of voluntary immi- grant groups versus those of "involuntary" minority children. Children of the latter (such as African Americans and Native Americans) learn that education does not lead to economic improvement, and they become withdrawn and hostile in school. Children of recent minority groups, however, may feel the stress of difference, but see that their people prosper through education, and they may respond in school by striving. Gibson and Ogbu (1991), with whose names this analysis is associated, are now attempting to further differentiate among
  • 7. minority groups as to the specific defenses and coping mecha- nisms used. As an example of disorders in response to culturally charac- teristic stressors, Indochinese refugee children treated in Cali- fornia were recovering from such severe stress that application of categories from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., DSM-III, 1980, American Psychiat- ric Association) was confounded (Krener & Sabin, 1985). The pattern is found with Central American refugee children as well (Arredondo, Orjuela, & Moore, 1989). "For every one of our patients, Axis I DSM-III diagnoses could be reached on the basis of the patient's fulfilling the criteria, but the diagnosis was felt to be incompletely descriptive or fundamentally wrong" (Krener & Sabin, 1985, p. 457). The issue does not derive only from specific stress; their Indochinese child patients revealed cross-cultural child-rearing differences and their potential mis- construing as psychopathology. For example, the American concept of mourning includes a sequencing of stages and a time
  • 8. limitation. Indochinese, who believe that the dead live after bodily death, may be seen as manifesting pathological or in- complete grief. "Particular DSM-III diagnostic categories. . . break down when we attempt to apply them cross-culturally, and . . . may have only qualified validity within our own cul- ture" (Krener & Sabin, 1985, p. 457). These authors decry the practice of applying the diagnostic scheme of one culture to child patients from another. Among indigenous minorities, there are also characteristic child problems. Because of high rates of death and displace- ment, Crow Indian children objectively experience repeated and traumatic loss; they suffer a chronic condition of mourning and depression (Long, 1983). The common plight of the acting- out, depressed, poor, alienated Black male adolescent is an- other example of a disorder so frequent in that group as to require a psychocultural category. Paster (1985) provides a dis- cussion of this syndrome, emphasizing the way that political, historical, societal, economic, interpersonal, and intrapsychic
  • 9. factors may be seen as intertwined and reciprocally reinforcing; the article is an example of how ethnogenetic, ontogenetic, and microgenetk levels of analysis integrate. When such integrated-genetic analyses are performed, it is difficult to discount cultural factors. The case can be made that not only in minorities are cultural, ethnogenetic features pres- ent in psychopathology. The overwhelming evidence of cultural psychiatric literature. . . is that cultural factors are a substantial part of every disorder and not a descriptive, picturesque component.. . . What is lacking in the [DSM-III] system, from a Latin-American perspective, is the explicit recognition of psychocultural categories or cultural crite- ria—or both—to aid in the identification of crucial categories and in the description of clusters of symptoms. (Alarcon, 1983, p. 104) The even stronger case is made by Marsella and Higgenbotham (1984), who argue that reserving the term culture-specific for exotic disorders merely delays the awareness that all disorders are culture-specific, and so is all treatment. Cultural Differences in Treatment
  • 10. Do clients of all cultures receive equal mental health services? Meta-analyses of research conclude that the issue is not clear (Atkinson, 1985; Sue & Zane, 1987) because of the usual re- search problems of nonuniformity of treatment goals, inaccessi- bility of records, inappropriate analog designs, and absence of any process research. However, to illustrate the issue, we may consider a set of data that appear to make the optimistic case, that clients are offered about the same services according to diagnosis and prognosis, without regard for ethnicity. Sue, Al- len, and Conaway (1978) studied 13,450 clients and found no evidence that Hispanic or Native-American clients in commu- nity mental health facilities were offered inferior or discrimina- tory services in either diagnosis, assigned personnel, or type of treatment. However, Hispanic and Native American clients fre- quently did not return after the initial treatment session. In these facilities, the dropout rates were Hispanic 42%, Indian 55%, Black 52%, Asian-American 52%, and Anglo clients only 30%. Why are these rates so high for minority culture clients? The authors indict not only the fit of the assigned type of ser-
  • 11. vice, but the quality of the professional interaction. Thus the same service is not necessarily responsive service, and in its effects, identical is not necessarily equal. Techniques that are of demonstrated utility in one culture may not operate in the same way for others. In a study con- SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 801 ducted by Lieh-Mak, Lee, & Luk (1984), Hong Kong Chinese parents were trained to be mediators of behavior therapy for their own children, but the therapists reported severe culturally based reluctance, arising from patterns of traditional family structure and interaction. These parents found it difficult to play with their children or respond contingently with praise or ignoring, because this violated their accustomed mode of child rearing, their beliefs about childhood problems, and their atti- tudes about the proper role of doctors. Thus Lieh-Mak, Lee, & Luk warn against the ethnocentric bias involved in importing treatment modalities from one culture to another. Cultural Differences in Knowledge The core of the problem, according to King, Moody, Thomp- son, & Bennett (1983), is that mental health intellectual and institutional constructions reflect the orientations of educators, researchers, and therapists who too often make judgments about and define programs for Black persons and communities with insufficient data on the na-
  • 12. ture of these persons and communities, their beliefs, values or their abilities and resources. We have almost developed an inca- pacity to shift from our Anglo-American perceptions, to change our attitudes and to foster real change. (King et al., 1983, p. 5) This perception of establishment ignorance and nonrespon- siveness is not limited to African-American psychologists. Although non-Indian psychologists are becoming increasingly in- volved in mental health service delivery to American Indians, they are, unfortunately, typically not prepared to work effectively with Indian clientele. The non-Indian psychologist may not be aware of the cultural values, life-styles, family practices, develop- mental progressions, and needs of their American Indian clients. This lack of cultural awareness typically results in conflicts and frustrations for both the psychologist and his or her Indian clients; ultimately, American Indian children and families may not re- ceive appropriate mental health services. (Everett, Proctor, & Cartmell, 1983, p. 588) Not only is the individual client harmed, but child workers who devalue native culture may actually intensify social disorgani- zation in Indian communities (Sullivan, 1983). The single most important explanation for the problems in service delivery involves the inability of therapists to provide culturally responsive forms of treatment.. . .Most therapists are not famil- iar with the cultural backgrounds and life-styles of various ethnic- minority groups and have received training primarily developed for Anglo, or mainstream, Americans. . . [and ] are often unable
  • 13. to devise culturally appropriate forms of treatment, and ethnic- minority clients frequently find mental health services strange, foreign, or unhelpful. (Sue & Zane, 1987, p. 37) Many mental health professionals find clients of some cul- tures strange and unhelpful, or even harmful, for example in the area of child abuse/neglect. Gray and Cosgrove (1985) inter- viewed Mexican, Samoan, Vietnamese, Filipino, Japanese, and Blackfoot as to their culturally normative child rearing values and practices. The researchers then examined these practices for the likelihood of their being misunderstood as "abuse" or "neglect" by child-protective professionals. That potential for misunderstanding is very high, particularly around two basic topics: the degree of responsibility delegated to children and the degree to which children should submit to adult domi- nance. The authors suggest that ignorance of these differential norms will lead to mistreatment of some parents by ethnocen- tric social workers. It should be noted that the potential for misunderstanding of one another's parenting conventions across all ethnic groups is high, and not restricted to Euro- Americans. However, the institutionalization of Euro-Ameri- can norms into professional assumptions is a hazard to ad- vances in psychocultural understanding and to the just percep- tion of parents' behaviors within other cultures. A source book on ethnic child socialization is Phinney and Rotheram (1987), on cultural approaches to parenting is Bernstein (1991), and an analysis of the use of such information in understanding child abuse and neglect is Korbin (1980). Much of the research and professional literature in culture and mental health attempts to provide enough information to sensitize psychologists to cultural issues in diagnosis and treat- ment. Several journals have published special issues on culture and mental health, and edited books on the topic are the most typical medium of communication. The typical pattern of
  • 14. these publications is to have several chapters on specific cul- tures, book-ended by introductory and summary chapters that attempt more general discussions.' This construction well com- municates the state of analysis of the issues. The field suffers from a shortage of empirical studies of program or variable evaluation, and there are virtually no studies of process in cross-cultural treatment. Nevertheless, this literature can sensi- tize the therapist to some issues that may be present for cul- turally unfamiliar clients.2 Differences Within Cultures This general approach—to learn as much about cultures as possible—is frustrated by significant differences among sub- groups and individuals within cultures. Everett, Proctor, and Cartmell (1983) point out the vast intertribal, interclan, urban- traditional, and individual differences among American In- dian clients. Isomura, Fine, and Lin (1987) discuss the differ- ences in offering services to Japanese immigrant families of the first, second, and third generations. Hispanic cultures differen- tiate among themselves; Cuban-, Mexican-, and Puerto Rican- Americans celebrate their distinctions as well as their common causes. Although clients expect respect and understanding of 1 See, e.g, Atkinson, Morton, and Sue (1989); Pedersen, Sartorius, and Marsella (1984); Chunn, Dunston, and Ross-Sheriff (1983). 2 Notable journals include a special issue of Psychotherapy titled "Psychotherapy with Ethnic Minorities." There is a special issue of the American Journal of Social Psychiatry on psychiatric care of minority
  • 15. groups, ftmily therapy with immigrant families is treated in Journal of Strategic and Systemic Therapies, and Journal of Drug Issues has a special issue on "Alcohol Problems and Minority Youth." A useful compilation of orientations to family structure and function, with im- plications for family treatment, is discussed for 20 different cultural groups in McColdrick, Pearce, & Giordano (1982), and more recently by Ho (1987,1990), who presents useful tabular comparisons of family- therapy relevant dimensions by culture. For child treatment, the most useful volumes of this sort are Children of Color: Psychological Interven- tions With Minority Youth (Gibbs & Huang, 1989) and Ethnic Issues in Adolescent Mental Health (Stiffman & Davis, 1990). 802 ROLAND G. THARP
  • 16. their culture and values, all will resent being seen merely as a representative of a cultural central tendency. Issues of culture and treatment exist today in a context of tempestuous intercultural relations in the political and eco- nomic arena, putting us in hazard of using cultural member- ship as no more than stereotypical overgeneralization. A part of the research agenda must therefore be to unpack the cultural variable (Whiting, 1976) so that differentiating characteristics within culture can be understood for clinical implications for individuals. In this way culture can be analyzed for its variable influence on individuals, in contrast to approaches that assign an equal value to culture for all members of a group. I am unaware of such work within mental health disciplines, but an idea of the strategy is given by work in culture and education (e.g., Weisner, Gallimore, & Jordan, 1988). Gallimore, Reese, Balzano, Benson, and Goldenberg (1991), in investigating the correlates of academic success for children of Mexican immi- grants, found that the domestic variable with the strongest rela- tionship to child school success is whether the father uses skills of literacy/numeracy in his employment (not the level of father's education). This kind of finer grained analysis of cultural and community life would allow us accurately to perceive the dy- namics of culture in the daily life of the individual child in the consulting chair. Faced with these contradictions, confusions, similarities, and differences, mental health professionals have advocated every position on the continuum. Some writers bundle all "children of color" together and believe their cause is common. Other writers call for culturally derived diversity of treatments, and others insist on a universality of the form of treatment, al- though of course purified by a correction of racist attitudes. Psychological science is now searching for a system of under- standing, and some theoretical guidance that
  • 17. goes beyond a study of exotic populations from different cultures and examines guidelines for excellence for mental health as a whole. . .the case for emphasizing cultural context in the under- standing, treatment, and prevention of mental disorders has been made extensively and persuasively. . .The problem we face, how- ever is going from general knowledge about culture traditions to specific mental health practices. This is where the disagreements, debates, and arguments begin and where the pragmatists fre- quently lose interest. (Sartorius, Pedersen, & Marsella, 1984, p. 282) The Cultural Compatibility Hypothesis The hypothesis of cultural compatibility suggests that treat- ment is more effective when compatible with client culture pat- terns. The hypothesis has a more mature theoretical and re- search base in child education (Tharp, 1989) than in child men- tal health, but the issues may be argued in substantially the same terms. Three forms of this hypothesis exist. The strong form, or culturally specific version, suggests that the most effec- tive interventions for different cultures will be different and specific (if not unique) to cultures. Proponents are associated with the effort to derive culturally based modalities or varia- tions of treatment. A weaker form is the two-type hypothesis, which suggests that there are two types of cultures, and therefore two types of most-effective clinical interventions. The first type is the major-
  • 18. ity, or Euro-American, culture; the second type includes those cultures whose students typically experience problems in schools, who are by-and-large children of color, less industrial- ized, urbanized, or western acculturated, and who thus share crucial incompatibilities with standard mental health prac- tices. In this position, effective treatment strategies for children of color would not be critically different from one another. This position is more salient in social work (e.g., Lum, 1986) than in psychology. The null form of the cultural compatibility hypothesis is the universalistic argument that effective treatments will follow the same course for all cultures. This is the default hypothesis of clinical psychology, in that the unreflective proceed as though there are no significant differences. However, many universal- ists agree that there are some variant subroutines that make minority children and their families more comfortable and are willing to engage in the search for these accommodations. The challenge faced by all is to balance universality and plu- ralism, preserving an authentic responsiveness to individual differences while avoiding an impractically large number of kinds of treatments (Huang & Gibbs, 1989). The obstacles are several, but they include pervasive psychological theory that lacks concepts crucial to the solution, and in which culture itself plays almost no role as a variable. Proponents of all three positions appear to be moderately satisfied with the existing monolith of treatment, as derived from, practiced by, and practiced for Europeans and Euro- Americans. Whether their hypotheses call for erecting many other treatment structures, as in the culturally specific position; or creating one other alternate for children of color, as in the two-type hypothesis; or only for modifying and polishing the universalist structure, there is no systematic understanding
  • 19. that the serious examination of the issues of culture and treat- ment may well call for a restructuring of the monolith itself, even for children of its own. Approaches to Culturally Compatible Treatment Each of these hypotheses operates in the same arena and takes its different positions on certain salient issues. These in- clude modifying professional role behaviors, assigning same- ethnicity professionals, and searching for the process details that will create or modify compatible procedures. Modified Professional Roles in Cross-Cultural Treatment Jenkins (1985), in discussing Afro-American clients, sug- gests that they should be approached in a down-to-earth and egalitarian manner that reduces status differences. In this way a positive affective response is made more likely, and early treat- ment attrition is reduced. By contrast, it is not uncommon that a more authoritative, formal, and concrete therapist role is rec- ommended for Japanese clients (e.g, Isomura, Fine, & Lin, 1987). Hong (1988) recommends that a psychologist dealing with an Asian-American clientele operate in a context of gen- eral family practice as a primary care provider, similar to the traditional family doctor who provides treatment for clients and their families, thus assessing the strong and traditional role of the family, minimizing client families' inhibition against SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 803 seeking mental health services, and taking advantage of the Asian-American's respect and deference to doctors. Juarez (198S) recommends a firm, instructing, professional
  • 20. demeanor when working with the Hispanic client, as do most who write from the Latino perspective. However, consider this quotation from Montijo (1985): Any therapeutic model that strives to serve poor Puerto Ricans or poor people in general cannot. . .enhance the control and hierar- chical superiority of the therapist at the cost of the greater sense of self-esteem produced by the assumption of greater autonomy, as- sertiveness, and responsibility by patients, (p. 439) This contradiction is consistent with differences that flow from economic class, as well as differences in opinion as to whether mental health treatment should be seen first as an instrumentality that feeds and is fed by an existing social order. Furthermore, as in all efforts to match treatment to culture, there are wide individual and subgroup/acculturation differ- ences in client preferences for therapist roles (Sue & Zane, 1987). Even if there were agreement on recommendations for profes- sional role stances toward members of different cultures, prob- lems remain, including the difficulty for any therapist of mani- festing flexible role behaviors and maintaining authenticity. More fundamentally, many forms of treatment have theoretical requirements for specific role characteristics of the therapist. How much cultural variation in therapist behavior could be allowed without wounding the core of client-centered therapy, or psychoanalysis, or any theory driven treatment? Ethnicity ofTherapists: The Question of Privileged Status When there are pervasive social attitudes from one culture toward another, are they likely to infiltrate the therapeutic rela-
  • 21. tionship? Spurlock (1985) and Sykes (1987) both recommend an early and frank discussion of the comfort of the client in Black/ White cross-cultural treatment, thus assisting both parties to understand racially based attitudes. Greene (1985), following Kupers (1981), articulates four general stances that are expres- sions of racism, and for which White therapists are enjoined to self-examination. They are (a) bigotry, "a conscious or uncon- scious belief in White supremacy and as a consequence, the feeling that the Black patient's problems are an outgrowth of the patient's inferiority"; (b) color blindness, which "may repre- sent the therapist's resistance to confronting the meaning of the color difference'1, (c) paternalism, which "involves the attribu- tion of all of the patient's problems to society and the effects of racism. To do this will fail to help patients to understand any role they may have in their dilemma"; and (d), "often a result of the therapist's racial guilt, is the unquestioning compliance with the rhetoric of Black power . . . [which] can result in a failure or reluctance to set appropriate limits or interpret act- ing-out." The Black patient may consciously or unconsciously put the White therapist to a series of tests to determine the acceptance as an individual. "It remains, however, the thera- pist's responsibility to be familiar with the Black patient's cul- ture to some extent, and with his or her own personal feelings and motivations for and about working with Black patients" (Greene, 1985; all quotations in this paragraph are from pp. 392-393). Is there any acceptable stance? Or are same-ethnicity therapists privileged in knowledge and attitude and thus in power of effectiveness? College youths have clear preferences for counselors who are like themselves—in many dimensions, not only ethnicity Sam- ples give somewhat different results, depending on geographi- cal location, but by and large students prefer counselors who are well educated and of the same ethnicity and gender and who share their attitudes and values; by and large students report
  • 22. themselves more likely to use counseling services when their preferences are met (Atkinson, Furlong, & Poston, 1986; Atkin- son, Poston, Furlong, & Mercado, 1989; Haviland, Horswill, (JConnell, & Dynneson, 1983; Ponterotto, Alexander, & Hink- ston. 1988). Preference is not the same as effectiveness. In a discussion of counseling with Mexican-American youth, De- Blassie (1976) insists that a therapist need not be Hispanic to be effective. Rather, common humanity is sufficient, and if solid counseling skills and correct, generous counselor attitudes are present, so too will be effectiveness. However, this author goes on to report many areas of values and beliefs that are arguably specific to Mexican-American youth, knowledge of which is critical to empathic understanding (DeBlassie. 1976). Sue (1988), who has labored long in the field of culture and treatment, reviews the literature on ethnic matching of thera- pist and client in psychotherapy and finds contradictory and inconclusive evidence as to whether matching is superior. He distinguishes between ethnic membership (which emphasizes national or geographic origin of ancestors) and cultural mem- bership (which emphasizes current identifications with the group or groups, and their commonalities of values, attitudes, motives, etc.). Although concluding that ethnic matching is irrel- evant, whereas cultural matching is likely to be an authentic distal variable affecting outcome, Sue urges researchers to con- sider more proximal variables, such as how cultural knowledge is translated into particular therapeutic behaviors and deci- sions. This position emphasizes the therapist's capacity for correct understanding and for comfortable communication. The issue is not whether patients are treated more effectively by same-race, same-class, or same-sex therapists, but whether the therapists' interpretation of the clients' cultural experience creates the ambience that is necessary to establish rapport and an
  • 23. empathic bond which facilitates the therapeutic process. (Juarez, 1985, p. 441) This resolution is similar to that espoused for cultural re- search by the Cuban-American anthropologist Dominguez (1985; 1986), who has worked both as "member" and "outsider" in cultural research. Her position is that "native" members' ac- counts of their own situation may well be privileged, because of their intimate, subjective, and empathic knowledge. This does not excuse native anthropologists' accounts from the disci- plines of their scholarship and profession and does not invali- date the outsider anthropologist's account, over which in terms of objectivity the native account is not ipso facto privileged. By analogy, it appears that psychological treatment, relying so heavily on both subjective and objective accuracy of perception, must attempt to maximize that accuracy in a variety of ways. Ethnic matching may contribute, but as a matter of practicality it is not an available strategy, and were it to be adopted, it would 804 ROLAND G. THARP exclude minority-culture therapists from treating most clients. Are there other ways? Culturally Specific Treatment Modalities Many writers (e.g^ da Silva, 1984) urge therapists to become aware of ethnogenetically determined patterns of help-seeking. For example, folk healers may be approached for folk diseases and scientific healers sought for infectious diseases, wounds, and refractory emotional disorders. These patterns for some Hispanic Americans are discussed in Tharp and Meadow
  • 24. (1973). Among urban Colorado Hispanic-American women, 20% had visited a curandero (folk healer) for treatment, and 12% had taken one or more of their children for treatment (Rivera, 1988). These treatments are far from conventional psychologi- cal treatments in form, and they may involve prayers, rest, rit- ual, and the ingestion of herbs. Many progressive treatment centers provide for cooperative work among scientific and folk practitioners. More than specific forms, there are presumptions and meta- processes in the traditional treatment practices of many cul- tures that are drastically different from values inherent in Euro- pean and American mental health treatment. LaFromboise, Trimble, and Mohatt (1990) provide an excellent description of American Indian healing processes, in which "religion, physiol- ogy, and psychology intertwine" (p. 632) and where traditional healing encourages the client to transcend the ego by experienc- ing self as embedded in and expressive of community. Tradi- tional ceremonies reinforce adherence to cultural values and remind participants of the importance of family and commu- nity networks. Indeed, the family, kin, and friends coalesce into an interlocking network to observe and understand and to inte- grate the individual back into the social life of the group. Un- like conventional psychological treatments, American Indian healing ceremonies and procedures usually involve the client's family and community members along with the healer and client. This collective treatment of the entire network "heals" not only the individual, but reaffirms the norms and solidarity of the entire group (Kaplan & Johnson, 1964). A third route toward cultural compatibility is to specifically design treatment modalities on and for members of specific cultures. An excellent example is cuento therapy, developed for Puerto Rican children by Costantino, Malgady, and Rogler (1986), but no doubt equally appropriate for other Hispanic
  • 25. groups for whom cuentos (cautionary folktales) are familiar so- cialization devices. In this technique, cuentos that were either traditional or adapted to reflect current conflicts in American life were read to high-risk kindergarten through third-grade children by their (bilingual, bicultural) therapists and their mothers, and a discussion was then fostered exploring the meaning of the tale. Results indicated that cuento therapy sig- nificantly reduced children's trait anxiety relative to traditional therapy and to no intervention, and this trend was stable over 1 year. Cuento therapy also increased Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children-revised (WISC-R) Comprehension subtest scores. The authors discuss this technique as a modeling ther- apy, which is attractive to the children because of the cultural familiarity of the story characters, and as a cognitive develop- ment task, useful because of the familiarity of the story-telling modality itself. They also point out the relevance of the litera- ture on cognitive and social development of disadvantaged mi- nority children, who respond well to fairy-tale, story-telling formats in classroom contexts. We should also note that the involvement of the mothers is another feature highly recom- mended by most students of Latin-American child treatment, regardless of treatment modality. Choice of Treatment Levels and Modalities: Compatibility Through Context However desirable specific-culture treatments are held to be, it must be noted that very few have been designed and reported, although the issue of cultural compatibility has been with us for nearly 40 years. The history of the cultural compatibility move- ment in education has a similar pattern. In spite of heroic ef- forts, almost no culturally "invented" specific educational mo- dalities have been designed to survive the practicalities of schools. In the culture and education movement, most compatibilities have been established through choosing estab-
  • 26. lished modalities that per se allow for greater influence of the child's culture, or at least do not demand incompatible child behavior. The majority of mental health programs for minority children in the literature appear to be using that same tactic: By electing modalities that naturally include family and commu- nity members and settings, some compatibility is assured by the objective introduction of the cultural context. I will discuss those modalities in an order that represents a rough progression from lesser toward greater contextualization in the client's cul- ture. Psychotherapy In the eyes of the public, individually conducted talking treatment conducted by a professional is the paradigmatic treatment form. Many clients of many cultures insist on receiv- ing it because they have been convinced of its primacy and are well aware of its elevated status. Indeed psychotherapy is not per se ineffective for any culture (Sue, 1988); it can be made to work for clients. For example, Paster (1985) makes a heroic argument for the adaptations needed to psychodynamic psycho- therapy so that it can be used with depressed, acting-out Black male adolescents. Tyler, Sussewell, and Williams-McCoy (1985) address the issue of providing culturally sensitive services by an attempt to preserve the basic structure and concepts of psycho- therapy while reconciling the universalist and particularist po- sitions into a "transcendist" perspective of an "ethnic validity model" that stresses that "persons from different ethnic/racial backgrounds are psychosocially different but that under some circumstances those differences can be transcended in ther- apy" (p. 312). With what reliability and regularity can therapists expect to establish that transcendence? It appears to this writer that, be-
  • 27. cause of two basic conditions, importing culture into individual child psychotherapy is heavy freight indeed. First, individual psychotherapy is a culturally specific form of treatment, just as surely as is the sweat lodge or the herbalism of the curandero; and second, of all the modalities at the command of the mental health professions, individual psychotherapy most rigidly ex- SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 805 eludes family or community members who might in feet import the cultural context. In any event, the preponderance of writers propose forms of treatment for minority children that struc- turally involve participants who are themselves culture carriers. Group Therapy Kahn, Lewis, and Galvez (1974) reported a counseling pro- gram for miscreant Papago Indian youth. The authors quickly moved from an individual to a group therapy format, although they improvised a number of changes to the usual confessional- and-problem-sharing approach: The cultural truth that Papagos are very reluctant to reveal per- sonal and intimate details to others, particularly in a setting with their peers, appeared to be a major hurdle in developing much group [therapy] interaction. It appeared that the main thing sus- taining the group in early sessions was the money being paid for attendance . . . [then] the therapists selected what they judged would be a relevant problem or topic for these boys. The first part of the sessions [was] given to formal presentations.. . . How to meet girls, venereal disease, homosexuality, drinking and alcohol-
  • 28. ism, the role of the male in the family, etc.. . . The boys would talk about subjects or ask questions in the abstract or in terms of a third person." (Kahn, Lewis, & Galvez, p. 239-240) The boys also confronted the [non-Indian] therapists about their motivations and real reasons for working with them on the reservation. They did develop regular attendance and much improved arrest and school truancy rates. Recourse to group treatment of children of cultures unfamil- iar to the therapist can have many advantages, including the socialization of new clients into an unfamiliar process, as well as providing a socialization of the therapist into the client cul- ture. Spurlock (1985) presents an instructive case in which a White resident treats Black adolescents in just such a situation. Problem Solving and Social Skills Training Many writers recommend for Native American clients a structured, problem-solving, skill-training approach, which teaches through modeling and rehearsing those everyday skills that are useful to Indian adolescents in adaptive living (e.g, Long, 1983; Kahn et al, 1974; LaFromboise & Low, 1989; La- Fromboise & Rowe, 1983; LaFromboise et al, 1990). Such pro- grams have been successfully applied with Indian adolescents to reduce substance abuse (Bobo, Cvetkovich, Gilchrist, Trim- ble, & Schinke, 1987; Schinke et al, 1988), and adolescent sui- cide (LaFromboise & BigFoot, 1988). Globetti (1988) has reviewed alcohol education programs for minority youth and concludes that similar programs are suited for African-American and Hispanic youth as well. He does not avoid the question of whether such an approach can be effective without corresponding changes in family and neighborhood conditions or without correction of the feelings of anomie, feel-
  • 29. ings of oppression, marginal status, and the unemployment of minority poverty. However, among Black, Hispanic, and Indian minority communities, there are resources available for com- munity treatment, including a strong abstinence sentiment es- pecially among women and youth. A psychosocial skills ap- proach reaches youth before they enter the age where drinking patterns and attitudes become rigid. This approach emphasizes values awareness and enhances self-esteem and a sense of self- power through increasing abilities for stress reduction, decision making, and behavioral alternatives such as assertiveness, re- fusal skills, and social competency. Even minority youths in effective social training groups for alcoholism prevention are concerned with problem drinking by their family members. Edwards and Edwards (1988) urge that Indian families be in- volved, including nondrinking members and the extended fam- ily Family Therapy Family therapy is often recommended by therapists and theo- rists who are concerned to establish more compatible and more effective treatment. Several considerations enter into this broad-based recommendation. For example, consider that tan- gle of racial attitudes, transferences, countertransferences, and guilt that Greene (1985) cautions against in White treatment of Black adolescents. Sykes (1987) recommends structured family therapy (short-term, goal-directed) as a way of avoiding those tangles, because the presence of the family establishes a Black context in which the White therapist is a facilitator and in which immediate problem solving is the focus. The emphasis on extended family and on the individual as part of a family in Asian culture is discussed by Chin (1983).
  • 30. The Western view of family is more restrictive and based on the nuclear family with the ultimate goal of a separation from the family. The emphasis on nuclear family boundaries and individual "privacy" in Western families, when imposed on Asian- American families, often fails to appreciate the supportive networks, social cohesiveness, and sense of affiliation of the extended family. The diagnostic process needs to examine the indusiveness and inter- dependence of kinship relationships in terms of how they might facilitate the therapeutic process." (p. 107) Because family therapy brings kin into the process itself, they are less likely to be overlooked in the diagnostic process. In the same way, family therapy mobilizes the available family re- sources for impact on the situation of the child. Kim (1985) endorses structured family therapy as the presumptive treat- ment for children of Asian Americans and provides a valuable discussion of the issues likely to arise in Asian families; merely to resort to family treatment is no final escape for the therapist from the need for cultural knowledge. Structured family therapy was recommended by Inclan (1985) for Puerto Rican and other Hispanic clients. Structural family therapy was found to protect the integrity of the family at 1-year follow-up, more than did psychodynamic child ther- apy, although the two treatments were apparently equal and superior to control conditions for 69 six- to twelve-year-old His- panic boys (Szapocznik, Rio, Murray, & Cohen, 1989). Family therapy is recommended for Latin-American school children by Vazquez-Nuttal, Avila-Vivas, and Morales-Barreto (1984) because of the strong Hispanic emphasis on the family.
  • 31. These authors recommend that the therapist explore the im- portance of ethnic values and traditions to the family, the strain between traditional family values and contemporaneous Ameri- can life, intergenerational strains that are likely loci of these value conflicts, and the extent to which the family relies on neighborhood and school institutions. 806 ROLAND G. THARP As Falicov and Brudner-White (1983) have discussed, the ac- ceptance of the extended family as the unit of analysis reveals vulnerabilities and sources of growth enhancement that are not dreamed of in nuclear-family-based treatment tactics and the- ories. Black families, even those of the stereotypical father-ab- sent, unemployed-mother pattern, often have strengths and re- sources in extended family and social networks that are com- pletely overlooked by the therapist with a narrow view of "family" (Spurlock, 1985). Juarez (1985) also discusses the best fit for Hispanic children and recommends family therapy, but also a form of megafamily treatment, "network therapy," a col- laborative involvement of all significant members of the child's social world. temic interaction with those of the network members. Network therapy is held to combat the depersonalization of the accultur- ating, urban environment; and because it mobilizes the strength of the support network, it contextualizes treatment directly in the cultural nexus of family and community members. Network therapy has been discussed as a specific culturally consistent treatment and prevention approach for American Indian communities (LaFromboise et al, 1990). In Native Hawaiian communities, the entire network of family and support participants are involved in a traditional process of healing and reconciliation called hob'ponopono; Mokuau (1990)
  • 32. has reported on the practice and its implications and possibili- ties for the treatment of children. Home-Based Treatment Home-based programs embed treatment in a cultural surround and thus are far less insulated from the influences of home, family, and community. "The most well-documented, effective therapeutic treatment with lower socioeconomic class Blacks has been home-visiting programs aimed at supporting and counseling mothers of young children" (Thomas & Dansby, 1985, p. 400), particularly those such as described in Gray and Ruttle (1980), that have positive effects on child lan- guage development and a sustained improvement in mothers' teaching styles. These criteria are well met by the carefully evaluated home-visiting programs of Roberts and his asso- ciates, working principally with Native Hawaiians (Roberts, Wasik, Casto, & Ramey, 1991; Roberts & Magrab, 1991). These authors rightly urge that culturally compatible programs must be continually vitalized by a staff committed to the principle of compatibility and influenced by community participation. Even more deeply embedded in context is the Homebuilders program, which trains, places, and supports professional workers who become temporarily resident or quasi-resident in the homes of their children clients, most of whom have already been targeted for placement outside of the home (Kinney, Haa- pala, & Booth, 1991). Although home-maintenance success rates are impressive for all categories of children, children of color had a significantly higher chance of remaining at home during and after Homebuilders treatment than did their White, non-Hispanic counterparts (Fraser, Pecora, & Haapala, 1991). These data are consistent with the view that contextualization of service is particularly critical for minority children. Network Treatment
  • 33. Network therapy (see Schoenfeld, Halevy-Martini, Hemley- Van der Velden, & Ruhf, 198 5,1986, for descriptions and evalua- tions) involves a group of family, relatives, and friends who are organized into a network. It is informal in operation. The thera- pist's role is to catalyze and conduct the process, but the forces of healing and correction are those of the support system of the network itself. Ordinarily conducted in the home, it can include 50 people or more. Network therapy can be used for solving a common or shared problem of the clan, family, or community, or the network can be mobilized in response to the problem of a single child or adolescent. Even in the latter case, the patient's behaviors, experiences, and goals are understood in their sys- Community Intervention Contextuality may be seen as extending from family to ex- tended family to community to the entire ecocultural niche. Culturally oriented writers consistently call for an ecologically oriented, contextualized analysis of psychological phenomena and consequently a diagnosis and treatment scheme that lo- cates the child in the sociocultural nexus—and indeed recog- nizes that the intervention target for some child problems lies in family, school, or community (Huang & Gibbs, 1989). Illus- trating that critical difference, under new environmental con- ditions of migration, displacement, or community change, the family and the school themselves may become sources of stress for the child and lose their value as social support networks (Canino, Earley, & Rogler, 1980). Ramirez (1980) discusses the complexity of family and com- munity as problem/support units among urban Mexican Ameri- cans. His study found that the propinquity and size of extended family was positively related to mental health status among Mexican Americans in Detroit. But nearness in time to Mexico
  • 34. and markers of traditional culture were negatively related to mental health status. Apparently an effective support structure develops slowly, postmigration. It appears that as a family mi- grates, it slowly develops a network, gains education, and im- proves in mental health status. Furthermore, family support is a structural phenomenon that operates in a mundane, solid, and workaday manner: The presence of someone in the network who offers "emotional understanding" and such "counselor- like" qualities is unrelated to mental health status. Assisting a referred child in a recent immigrant community may require assisting a family, group of families, or entire community to organize for mutual assistance. An example of a developmentally oriented community psy- chology program is that of OTJonnell and Tharp (1990), which is based on the principles of neo-Vygotskian socio-historical theory. In their analyses, the real client of the consultation is the social system that produces or maintains the problem. No per- manent improvement in a child's condition can be expected unless the sustaining context is also assisted to change. The consultant must locate the levers of influence that can bring about some reorganization. The ultimate goal of the consultant is to empower and to increase the self-assistance of the commu- nity and thus the growth or development of its members, partic- ularly the referred child or children. The route to that goal is through reorganization of activity settings. Through the pro- SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 807 cesses of interaction and assisted performance in activity set- tings, the targeted community members gain greater compe- tence, skill level, and solidarity. The basic task of intervention is
  • 35. to design new or changed activity settings in which that growth and development can occur. The influence of the consultant is used to generate or organize the resources necessary to produce those activity settings. The consultant, working in the triadic model (Tharp & Note, 1988; Tharp & Wetzel, 1969), exercises the consulting effects through mediators who are themselves members of the settings or of the community context. A fine example of such a program is that for prevention of drug abuse by American Indian youth by the Indian Drug Pre- vention Program (IDPP) of Washington State (Bobo et al, 1987). The consultants established an all-Indian advisory board, worked with members to establish goals and guidelines, developed together a curriculum of skills training (modeling, instructing, and practicing), contacted gatekeepers of services in six different communities, located venues and activity set- tings in which the program could be operated, trained and cooperated with local schools and other agencies, and left the communities competent to carry out the positively evaluated program. Other examples of similar community programs are available in Edwards and Edwards (1988). The IDPP operates primarily in schools. The movement to- ward integrated mental health services in schools is particularly important for cultural minorities. Red Horse (1982) described a short-lived but well-conceived program that operated for In- dian public school adolescent girls in Minnesota. It included credit-bearing social skills training and psychosocial develop- mental materials, cultural enrichment materials provided by Indian elders, pregnancy prevention, group counseling, and a professional staif of counselors who assisted the girls to bridge and build extended family relationships both in the city and on the nearby reservation. The creation of new activity settings and the mobilization of resourcesof the family and community to assist the performance of the adolescent girls is a model of community program design. Another exemplary program is a
  • 36. school psychology program for Hispanics that involves "sys- tems networking among the nuclear and extended family, com- padres, church personnel, indigenous support systems, and school [which] affords pooling of human, informational, and mental health resources for more effective, efficient, and eth- nospecific services for Hispanics" (Rosado, 1986, p. 196). Status of the Evidence for the Cultural Compatibility Hypotheses As we have now seen, there is virtual unanimity among writers that some degree of compatibility between culture and treatment is necessary. That unanimity, however, does not con- clude the case. A useful understanding of compatibility requires evidence that will allow unpacking the concept into its potent and inert components. The necessary breadth of that evidence is not yet present in the child-treatment literature. The absence can be felt especially in the paucity of process studies of child treatment, where interactions could be examined by charting flows and glitches against the culturally based repertoires of participants. Evidence of that sort is present to a greater degree in the study of child education. Although these parallel data can be taken only as suggestive, they can help to formulate hypotheses for research in the clinical and consulting context. In education research on children, there is a substantial body of evidence demonstrating cultural diversities in at least four domains that make a difference in educational process and outcome: in lin- guistics and sociolinguistics, cognition, motivation (state and trait), and social organizational proclivities (reviewed in Tharp, 1989). As an illustration of the consequences for schooling, effective monocultural classrooms for Native Indian children have been shown to be markedly different from those for Native
  • 37. Hawaiian children, in social organization, in patterns of child control, in management of gender interaction, and in the pat- terns of the teaching conversation between and among children and teacher. As one example of an "unpacked" cultural vari- able, sociolinguistic studies have demonstrated crucial differ- ences in the courtesies and conventions of conversation for dif- ferent ethnic groups, differences that have strong emotional effects on children and condition the child's reaction to instruc- tion, from willing engagement to hostile withdrawal. Although this might be taken as evidence for the "strong" cultural compatibility solution of designing classrooms for spe- cific cultures, specific-culture classrooms are largely impracti- cal. The typical classroom of our nation today is mwft/cultural.3 Of course, clinical services are not, and a variety of specific culturally compatible services might be feasible within existing service delivery institutional structures. Before leaping to recommendations, however, we must note in that same review of culture-and-education research (Tharp, 1989) considerable evidence for the weak or "two-type" form of the hypothesis—that the children of all those cultures who typi- cally underachieve in schools share the requirement for a modi- fied kind of education that is not the same as that provided by the majority-oriented schools. The educational research evi- dence speaks for two conditions shared by successful classrooms for children of underachieving minorities: (a) con- textualization of learning activities into settings and topics that are meaningful in the children's daily life and (b) assisting chil- dren in developing the language of instruction, the latter assur- ing the child of adequate participation in the learning activities. These two factors are consistent with the evidence for child treatment reviewed in this article: Contextualization of treat- ment in the meanings and processes of cultural life is the most insistent urging of all who work in child culture and mental
  • 38. health. And logic dictates that treatment cannot be effective unless it is delivered in a language the child and the family can understand and use. However, the universalist position does not lack evidence either. For example, just as in the clinical domain, specific edu- cational "treatments" growing from the institutions of specific cultures have not emerged in any great number and in general 3 Certainly there are monocultural classrooms present in society, and these have been of great interest to researchers in culture and education because they allow cultural processes in teaching and learn- ing to emerge clearly. Likewise, investigating both process and out- come studies of various treatment modalities within a single culture is a research strategy of promise. 808 ROLAND G. THARP have not survived the realities and necessities of schooling. Rather, the cultural-compatibility movement in education ap- pears to have settled on the least-change principle (Tharp et al., 1984), which calls not for inventing entire new pedagogies or teaching modalities, but for the careful selection of modalities of demonstrated effectiveness in real schools and by working teachers. The selection of such modalities may be quite differ- ent for children of different cultures, and it is certain that the instantiation of the modalities will be modified by contextua- lizing them in the experience and language of the children's daily lives. This appears to be entirely consistent with the expe-
  • 39. rience of the more youthful culture-and-treatment movement. Few specific treatment modalities have been offered. Certain modalities, however, are overwhelmingly preferred by thera- pists knowledgeable of certain cultures; and each instantiation is recommended to be conditioned by the culture. Thus, family therapy is repeatedly recommended for Hispanic children, but the recommendation is equally strong that the family must be treated in ways that reflect that family's composition, values, and language. On the basis of this discussion, it appears that the most via- ble hypothesis is a universalist one, but not the "unrversalist" hypothesis as previously understood. That is, clinical service for everyone must be contextualized in the values, processes, and language of the clients' culture, and that is the universalist principle. Existing services are already contextualized for ma- jority, English-speaking culture members because treatment procedures grew out of that culture and language. Were it not so, prescription would have to see to it. When the strong, the weak, and the null forms of a hypothe- sis are all verified, we know the question needs rephrasing. Evidence and inference suggest the following as the best next direction for investigation. Cultural compatibility, rather than an accommodation needed for certain subsets of people, should be understood as an aspect of a universally required contextua- lization of services. The appropriate question would then be- come: How can therapists, program developers, and re- searchers develop heuristics for assuring the conditions of con- text uality and language accessibility? Some previous inquiry is available to that question. Linguistic and Sociolinguistic Accessibility of Services The problem of assessing and providing treatment across a language barrier is central in the provision of services to chil-
  • 40. dren of diverse cultures. This issue is most familiar to psychol- ogy as an issue in testing: English-language interaction, in bi- lingual children, is not a clear window through which cognitive functioning can be estimated. And the problems associated with attempting treatment when the therapist is not competent in the basic language of the clients are formidable indeed. In an effort to increase the linguistic accessibility, bilingual and bicultural staff may be incorporated into the staff of men- tal health facilities, as advocated early by Scott and Delgado (1979). Acosta and Cristo (1981) describe a clinic for Hispanics that recruited community members to serve as translators for the English-speaking professional staff. Training was provided in the basic concepts and vocabulary of psychotherapy. These translators also served as cultural informants, explaining to the therapists the meaning of some of the client's topics in therapy. There is a danger, however, as pointed out by Lappin (1983), in having a "house ethnic," often a paraprofessional, who is called in to translate. Many individuals and cultural groups want a professional regardless of race. In fact, there is a danger in using any translator, who becomes a switchboard, a distorter, and an additional principal in an already complex interaction. Therapists must be cautious in using the most bilingual family member as translator for all, particularly when this is the child, who, while most flexible in language use may also be most psychologically vulnerable to the dynamic forces of family and therapist interaction. Nevertheless, there is evidence that pro- vision of Spanish language opportunities in clinic programs does increase utilization rates in Hispanic communities (Rogler, Malgady, Costantino, & Blumenthal, 1987). However, it is not simply language code proficiency that creates misunderstandings and frustrations in cross-cultural service delivery Differing Sociolinguistic and paralinguistic
  • 41. patterns are barriers to communication. For example, Michaels (1984) has shown that children of different cultures tell their stories in different ways, with startling audience effects. In her study, White children were topic-centered in their narratives, with thematic cohesion and a temporal reference. Black chil- dren used a topic-associating style consisting of a series of im- plicitly associated anecdotal segments with no explicit state- ment of an overall theme or point. White adults criticized the topic-associating style as incoherent, but Black adults found it interesting with lots of detail and description. It is apparent that this cultural difference in basic language structure can lead to quite different judgments and predictions in the treatment room. Process studies of cross-cultural treatment of children are lacking. Again we must look to culture-and-education, where such research is growing. A recent review (Tharp, 1989) reveals that in determining relationship, learning, and satisfaction in cross-cultural settings, there is enormous weight in variables of the courtesies and conventions ofcomersation, such as the length of pause between speakers (wait-time), rhythm of speech and event, and participation structures (the patterns and conditions for speaking and listening). When Sociolinguistic school/home compatibilities are present, children are more comfortable, and they participate and display their abilities appropriately As one example, Black migrant children, whose schools view them as below grade level and unresponsive, speak and behave with complexity and competence in home settings. In addition, they exhibit full competence and full participation at church. The similarities of Sociolinguistic and behavioral conventions be- tween church and home offer an example to schools of how formal institutions can engage their young by compatibilities of expectations with child repertoires (Lein, 1975). Many strategies have been devised for achieving linguistic
  • 42. compatibility for children in schools. Because schooling is so heavily language dependent, a first-order goal for effective edu- cation is to provide adequate language development. This goal is very likely parallel to child treatment, which is also a learning modality heavily dependent on language exchanges. To the de- gree that treatment does occur through language, we may ex- pect that special attention will be required to ensure linguistic SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 809 and sociolinguistic compatibilities. This makes cross-cultural psychotherapy particularly problematic for both therapist and client. In the dimension of language, it seems clear that culture members are indeed privileged, and same-language therapists are advantageous to clients. However, when therapists do serve clients of a different language, compensatory strategies are available, the most powerful of which is to contextualize treat- ment in the culture of the client. By importing other family and community members into the treatment itself, familiar lan- guage becomes a part of the treatment context. Contextuality of Treatment Sue and Zane (1987) observe that therapist/client cultural matching is a distal variable and that efficacy lies in the proxi- mal issues of how well the therapist is able to (a) meet the client's conceptualization of the problem, (b) require acceptable behaviors as means for problem resolution, and (c) share goals for treatment. The literature considered here suggests that the probability of those proximal conditions being achieved is lim- ited by the degree to which social, linguistic, and other process variables are congruent. There seems little doubt that all else being equal, culturally matched psychologists and clients are
  • 43. more likely to achieve that congruence and those conditions. Most therapists can have little hope of sufficient detailed knowl- edge of many (if any) cultures other than their natal one and almost no hope of mastering a variety of linguistic and socio- linguistic codes. This leads us to consider the probability of achieving process congruence and the Sue and Zane conditions as a function of the modality of treatment. Treatment modalities for children may be arrayed on a con- tinuum of inclusiveness or social contextuality, from individual psychotherapy to group therapy to family therapy to network therapy to community intervention. Historically, these modali- ties developed in roughly that order. Thus therapists of my own (senior) cohort were trained, by and large, in the individual model, with less intense training in groups, less still on fami- lies, and little direct experience of network or community methods, that is, with a decreasing emphasis on the expanding continuum of social contextuality Although that pattern has eased in many training programs during the past 30 years, there is perhaps a residual tropism toward the individual end of the continuum in the prescriptive inclinations of our field. If the reading of the literature offered here is correct, a rever- sal of that strategy is indicated. That is, for a therapist facing a client across a cultural chasm, the treatment of first consider- ation should be community intervention; that of second consid- eration, network therapy; that of third, family treatment; fourth, group treatment; and last of all, individual treatment. The work of therapy is potent when the therapist and child share so much that the therapist can presume a thorough knowl- edge of the workings of the child's family and community and can teach the youngster knowingly, confidently, and credibly Because Euro-American culture has produced most therapists and most clients, most clients and therapists have been
  • 44. matched in culture and language. Insulated within a common culture, we have not recognized cultural contextuality as a vari- able because it has not varied. We have not noticed the basic condition that because it is decontextualized, individual ther- apy depends more than any other modality on shared values, semiotics, habits, and expectations between therapist and client. When these are not present, resources of commonality have to be provided by expanding to family/network/commu- nity interventions, even for majority culture members. It ap- pears that the conclusions available from the culture and treat- ment literature are not restricted to strategies for exotic culture members. As we should have expected, an expanded range of observations leads to a more comprehensive universalism. It is paradoxical but welcome, that facing the problems of treating others is teaching us how better to treat ourselves. Implications for Research and Theoretical Programs Humanity has a common base, provided by evolutionary processes; but over long periods of historical time, ethnoge- netic processes have put our common humanity through differ- ent filters. To recover our common features, we must study those filters and come to know how historical processes have created differing psychological and behavioral realities for dif- ferent peoples. These differences are not trivial, and they are not surface. They include differences in motivation, cognition, social organizational principles, communication patterns, val- ues, and semiotic structures, as well as accustomed ways of teaching and learning. Further research into ethnogenetic pro- cesses is a necessity for advances in culture-and-treatment and also holds promise for the discovery of a broader range of fun- damental processes of psychology and sociology. This agendum falls within the program of the sociohistorical theoretical move- ment (e.g., Cole, 1985; Tharp & Gallimore, 1988; Vygotsky, 1929,1978; Wertsch, 1985).
  • 45. On the other hand, just because we have been blind to "cul- ture," we must not now be blinded by it. Each culture member has also had a different life history and accumulation of learn- ing. Thus ethnogenetic effects are filtered and conditioned by ontogenetic and microgenetic processes; and in our rush to solve problems of treatment arising from cultural differences, we must not overlook the resulting wide range of variance within cultures. Indeed the definitions and boundaries of eth- nicities are under constant negotiation and redefinition (Do- minguez, 1986). Psychology comes to the porch of cultural stud- ies just as the volatility of the concept of ethnicity is coming into focus, and although belated, our arrival may well be wel- comed by the house of social science. Individuals define them- selves in and out of ethnic identities, and that psychological identification is of more moment in treatment issues than is that of race. Psychologists have the means to understand those processes and how they operate in and through psychological treatment. Another major research need lies within the field of clinical and consulting psychology itself. The paucity of process studies in the treatment of different culture members, particularly in a variety of modalities, is the greatest single need for evidence. Critical data and its analysis will require methods of investiga- tion less familiar to psychology than they might be, methods such as ethnography, microethnography, and discourse analy- sis. Expanding our range of research operations need not await major retooling of psychologists' methodology. Research part- nerships made with scientists of other appropriate disciplines, 810 ROLAND G. THARP
  • 46. such as sociolinguistics, anthropology, or community develop- ment, could allow an immediate infusion of data that reveal cultural processes in their immediacy and at their points of impact—in the situations and communications of treatment itself.4 Are there immediate, imperative, specific hypotheses for in- vestigation? Certainly all conclusions offered above have the status of hypothesis. As much as on direct evidence, this analy- sis perforce relied on reasoning and analogy. My strategy was to suggest the figure in the ground by putting those tiles in the mosaic as placeholders, pending the arrival of more direct evi- dence drawn from the treatment of children. About the figure that will eventually be clear, there is one thing that now appears certain. These cultures meeting in the consulting room will clarify not only the processes of the cultures themselves, but will bring to our notice things about treatment that we do not yet suspect. 4 As an example of how this strategy was used in the culture- and-
  • 47. educational domain, see Tharp et al. (1984). References Acosta, F. X, & Cristo, M. H. (1981). Development of a bilingual interpreter program: An alternative model for Spanish-speaking ser- vices. Professional Psychology, 12,474-482. Alcohol problems and minority youth. [Special issue]. (1988). Journal of Drug Issues, 18(1). Alarcon, R. D. (1983). A Latin American perspective on DSM- III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140,102-104. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychological Association. (1990). Guidelines for providers of psychological services to ethnic and culturally diverse populations. Washington, DC: Author. Arredondo, P., Orjuela, E, & Moore, L. (1989). Family therapy with Central American war refugee families. Journal of Strategic and Sys- temic Therapies, 8, 28-35.
  • 48. Atkinson, D. R. (1985). Research on cross-cultural counseling and psy- chotherapy: A review and update of reviews. In R Pedeisen (Ed.), Handbook of cross-cultural counseling and therapy (pp. 191- 197). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Atkinson, D. R., Furlong, M. J, & Poston, W C. (1986). Afro- American preferences for counselor characteristics. Journal ofCounseling Psy- chology, 33, 326-330. Atkinson, D. R., Morton, G., & Sue, D. W (Eds.). (1989). Counseling American minorities: A cross-cultural perspective (3rd ed.). Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Atkinson, D. R., Poston, W C, Furlong, M. J., & Mercado, P. (1989). Ethnic group preferences for counselor characteristics. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 68-72. Bobo, J. K., Cvetkovich, G, Gilchrist, L. D, Trimble, J. E., &Schinke, S. (1987). Cross-cultural service delivery to minority communities. Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 263-272. Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (1991). Cultural approaches to parenting. New
  • 49. York: Erlbaum. Canino, I. A., Earley, B. F., & Rogler, L. H. (1980). The Puerto Rican child in New York City: Stress and mental health. Bronx, NY: Ford- ham University Hispanic Research Center. Chin, J. L. (1983). Diagnostic considerations in working with Asian- Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53,100-109. Chunn, J. C., II, Dunston, P. J, &Ross-Sherifl; F. (Eds.). (1983). Mental health and people of color: Curriculum development and change. Washington, DC: Howani University Press. Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: Where culture and cognition create each other. In J. V Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, com- munication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 146- 161). New York: Cambridge University Press. Costantino, G., Malgady, R. G., & Rogler, L. H. (1986). Cuento therapy: A culturally sensitive modality for Puerto Rican children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 639-645.
  • 50. da Silva, G. C. (1984). Awareness of Hispanic cultural issues in the health care setting. Children's Health Care, 13, 4-10. DeBlassie, R. R. (1976). Counseling with Mexican American youth: Pre- conceptions and processes. Austin, TX: Learning Concepts, Inc. Dominguez, V (1985). Intended and unintended messages: The schol- arly defense of onels own "people." Nieuwe West-lndischeGids, 209- 222. Dominguez, V (1986). White by definition. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Edwards, E. D, & Edwards, M. E. (1988). Alcoholism prevention/treat- ment and Native American youth: A community approach. Journal of Drug Issues, 18(1), 103-114. Everett, E, Proctor, N., & Cartmell, B. (1983). Providing psychological services to American Indian children and families. Professional Psy- chology: Research and Practice, 14, 588-603. Falicos C. J., &. Brudner-White, L. (1983). The shifting family triangle: The issue of cultural and contextual relativity. In J. C. Hansen & C.}.
  • 51. Falicov (Eds.), Cultural perspectives in family therapy (pp. 51- 67). Rockville, MD: Aspen. [Family therapy with immigrant families], (1989). Journal of Strategic and Systemic Therapies, ^(Summer). Fraser, M. W, Pecora, P. J., & Haapala, D. A. (1991). Familiesin crisis: The impact of intensive family preservation services. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Gallimore, R., Reese, L. J., Balzano, S., Benson, C, & Goldenberg, C. (1991, April). Ecocultural sources of early literacy experiences: Job-re- auired literacy, home literacy environments, and school reading. Paper read at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago. Gibbs, J. T, Huang, L. N.,& Associates (Eds.). (1989). Children of color: Psychological intervention with minority youth. San Francisco: Jos- sey-Bass. Gibson, M., &Ogbu, J. (Eds.). (1991). Minority status and schooling: A comparative study of immigrant and involuntary minorities. New
  • 52. York: Garland Press. Globetti, G. (1988). Alcohol education programs and minority youth. Journal of Drug Issues, 18,115-129. Gray, E., & Cosgrove, J. (1985). Ethnocentric perception of child-rear- ing practices in protective services. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9,389- 396. Gray, S. W, & Ruttle, K. (1980). The family-oriented home visiting program: A longitudinal study Genetic Psychology Monographs, 102, 299-316. Greene, B. A. (1985). Considerations in the treatment of black patients by white therapists. Psychotherapy, 22, 389-393. Haviland, M. G., Horswill, R. K., O'Connell, J. J., & Dynneson, V Y (1983). Native American college students' preference for counselor race and sex and the likelihood of their use of a counseling center. Journal ofCounseling Psychology, 30, 267-270. Ho, M. K. (1987). Family therapy with ethnic minorities. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ho, M. K. (1990). Intermarried couples in therapy. Springfield, IL:
  • 53. Charles C Thomas. Hong, G. K. (1988). A general family practitioner approach for Asian- American mental health services. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 600-605. SPECIAL SECTION: CULTURAL DIVERSITY 811 Huang, L. N, & Gibbs, J. T. (1989). Rrture directions: Implications for research, training, and practice. In J. T. Gibbs, L. N. Huang, & Asso- ciates (Eds.), Children of color: Psychological intervention with minor- ity youth (pp. 375-403). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Inclan, J. (1985). Variations in value orientation in mental health work with Puerto Ricans. Psychotherapy, 22, 324-334. Isomura, T., Fine, S, & Lin, T. (1987). Two Japanese families: A cul- tural perspective. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 282-286. Jenkins, A. H. (1985). Attending to self-activity in the Afro- American
  • 54. client. Psychotherapy, 22, 335-341. Juarez, R. (1985). Core issues in psychotherapy with the Hispanic child. Psychotherapy, 22, 441-448. Kahn, M. W, Lewis, J, & Galvez, E. (1974). An evaluation study of group therapy procedure with reservation adolescent Indians. Psy- chotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 11, 239-242. Kaplan, B., & Johnson, D (1964). The social meaningofNavajo psycho- pathology and psychotherapy. In A. Kiev (Ed.), Magic, faith, and healing (pp. 203-299). New York: Free Press. Kim, S. C. (1985). Family therapy for Asian Americans: A strategic structural framework. Psychotherapy, 22, 342-348. King, L. M., Moody, S., Thompson, EX, & Bennett, M. (1983). Black psychology reconsidered: Notes toward curriculum development. In J. C. Chunn II, P. J. Dunston, & F. Ross-Sheriff (Eds.), Mental health and people of color (pp. 3-22). Washington, DC: Howard
  • 55. Uni- versity Press. Kinney, J., Haapala, D, & Booth, C. (1991). Keeping families together: The homebuilders model. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Korbin, J. E. (1980). The cultural context of child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 4, 3-13. Krener, P G., & Sabin, C (1985). Indochinese immigrant children: Problems in psychiatric diagnosis. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24.453-458. Kupers, T. (1981). Public therapy The practice of psychotherapy in the public mental health clinic. New York: Macmillan. LaFromboise, T. Q, & BigFoot, D. (1988). Cultural and cognitive con- siderations in the prevention of American Indian adolescent suicide. Journal of Adolescence, II, 139-153. LaFromboise, T. D, & Low, K. G. (1989). American Indian children and adolescents. In J. T. Gibbs, L. N. Huang, & Associates
  • 56. (Eds.), Children of color: Psychological intervention with minority youth, pp. 114-147. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. LaFromboise, T. D., & Rowe, W (1983). Skills training for bicultural competence: Rationale and application. Journal of Counseling Psy- chology, 30,589-595. LaFromboise, T. D., Trimble, J. E., & Mohatt, G. V (1990). Counseling intervention and American Indian tradition: An integrative ap- proach. Counseling Psychologist, 18, 628-654. Lambert, M. C., Weisz, J. R., & Knight, F. (1989). Over- and undercon- trolled clinic referral problems of Jamaican and American children and adolescents: The cultural general and the culture specific. Jour- nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 467-472. Lappin, J. (1983). On becoming a culturally conscious family thera- pist. In J. C. Hansen (EdJ, Cultural perspectives in family
  • 57. therapy, (pp. 122-136). Rockville, MD: Aspen. Lein, L. (1975). "M>u were talkin' though, oh yes, you was." Black American migrant children: Their speech at home and school. Council on Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 6(4), 1 -11. Lieh-Mak, F, Lee, P W H., & Luk, S. L. (1984). Problems encountered in teaching Chinese parents to be behavior therapists. Psychologia, 27, 56-64. Long, K. (1983). The experience of repeated and traumatic loss among Crow Indian children: Response patterns and intervention strate- gies. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52,116-126. Lum, D. (1986). Social work practice and people of color: A process- stage approach. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Marsella, A. J., & Higgenbotham, H. N. (1984). Traditional Asian med- icine: Applications to psychiatric services in developing nations. In P. Pedersen, N. Sartorius, & A. Marsella (Eds.), Mental health
  • 58. ser- vices: The cross cultural context (pp. 175-197). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. McGoldrick, M, Pearce, J., & Giordano, J. (Eds.). (1982). Ethnicity and family therapy. New York: Guilford Press. Michaels, S. (1984). Listening and responding: Hearing the logic in children's classroom narratives. Theory Into Practice, 23, 218- 244. Mokuau, N. (1990). A family centered approach in Native Hawaiian culture. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 71, 607-613. Montijo, J. A. (1985). Therapeutic relationships with the poor: A Puerto Rican perspective. Psychotherapy, 22, 436-440. Moritsugu, J., & Sue, S. (1983). Minority status as a stressor. In R. D. Felner, L. A. Jason, J. N. Moritsugu, & S. S. Farber (Eds.), Preventive
  • 59. psychology: Theory, research and practice (pp. 162-174). Elmsford, NY Pergamon Press. OTJonnell, C. R., & Tharp, R. G. (1990). Community intervention guided by theoretical development. In A. S. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), International handbook of behavior modification and therapy (2nd ed., pp. 251-266). New fork: Plenum Press. Paster, V S. (1985). Adapting psychotherapy for the depressed, unac- culturated, acting-out, black male adolescent. Psychotherapy, 22, 408-417. Pedersen, P E., & Marsella, A. J. (1982). The ethical crisis for cross-cul- tural counseling and therapy Professional Psychology, 13, 492- 500. Pedersen, P. E., Sartorius, N, & Marsella, A. (Eds). (1984). Mental health services: The cross cultural context. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
  • 60. Phinney, J. S, &Rotheram, M. J. (Eds.). (1987). Children^ ethnic sociali- zation: Pluralism and development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ponterotto, J. G, Alexander, C, & Hinkston, J. (1988). Afro- American preferences for counselor characteristics: A replication and exten- sion. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35,175-182. [Psychiatric care of minority groups, Special issue]. (1983). American Journal of Social Psychiatry, 3(2). Psychotherapy with ethnic minorities [Special issue). (1985). Psycho- therapy, 22(2). Ramirez, O. (1980). Extended family phenomena and mental health among urban Mexican Americans. Unpublished doctoral disserta- tion, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan. Red Horse, Y. (1982). A cultural network model: Perspectives for ado- lescent services and paraprofessional training. In S. M. Manson (Ed.), New directions in prevention among American Indian and
  • 61. Alaska native communities (pp. 173-184). Portland, OR: Oregon Health Sciences University. Rivera, G. (1988). Hispanic folk medicine utilization in urban Colo- rado. Sociology and Social Research, 72, 237-241. Roberts, R. N., & Magrab, P. R. (1991). The psychologist's role in a family-centered approach to practice, training, and research with young children. American Psychologist, 46,144-148. Roberts, R. N, Wasik, B. H, Casto, G., & Ramey, C. T. (1991). Family support in the home: Programs, policy and social change. American Psychologist, 46,131-137. Rogler, L., Malgady, R., Costantino, G, & Bhimenthal, R. (1987). What do culturally sensitive mental health services mean? The case of Hispanics. American Psychologist, 42. 565-570. Rosado, J. W, Jr. (1986). Toward an interfacing of Hispanic cultural variables with school psychology service delivery systems. Profes-
  • 62. sional Psychology: Research and Practice, 17,191-199. Sartorius, N., Pedersen, P. B, & Marsella, A. J. (1984). Mental health services across cultures: Some concluding thoughts. In P. Pedersen, 812 ROLAND G. THARP N. Sartorius, & A. Marsella (Eds.), Mental health services: The cross cultural context, {pp. 271-279). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Schinke, S. P., Orlandi, M. A., Botvin, G. J., Gilchrist, L, D., Trimble, J. E., & Locklear, V S. (1988). Preventing substance abuse among American Indian adolescents: A bicultural competence skills ap- proach. Journal of Counseling Psychology 35, 87-90. Schoenfeld, P., Halevy, J., Hemley-Van der Velden, E., & Ruhf, L. (1986). Long-term outcome of network therapy. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 37, 373-376. Schoenfeld, P., Halevy-Martini, J., Hemley-Van der Velden, E., &Ruhf, L. (1985). Network therapy: An outcome study of! 2 social
  • 63. networks. Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 281-287. Scott, J. E, & Delgado, M. (1979), Planning mental health programs for Hispanic communities. Social Casework, 60,451-456. Spurlock, J. (1985). Assessment and therapeutic intervention of black children in America. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psy- chiatry, 24,168-174. Stiffinan, A. R., & Davis, L. E. (Eds). (1990). Ethnicissues in adolescent mental health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Sue, S. (1988). Psychotherapeutic services for ethnic minorities. Ameri- can Psychologist, 43, 301-308. Sue, S., Allen, D. B., & Conaway, L. (1978). The responsiveness and equality of mental health care to Chicanes and Native Americans. American Journal of Community Psychology, 6,137-146. Sue, S., & Zane, N. (1987). The role of culture and cultural techniques in psychotherapy: A critique and reformulation. American Psycholo- gist, 42, 37-45. Sullivan, T. (1983). Native children in treatment: Clinical,
  • 64. social and cultural issues. Journal of Child Care, 1, 75-94. Sykes, D. K., Jr. (1987). An approach to working with black youth in cross cultural therapy. Clinical Social Work Journal, 15, 260- 270. Szapocznik, J., Rio, A., Murray, E., & Cohen, R. (1989). Structural family versus psychodynamic child therapy for problematic His- panic boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 571- 578. Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocuhural variables and constants: Effects on teaching and learning in schools. American Psychologist, 44, 349- 359. Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life: Teaching and learning in social context. New York: Cambridge University Press. Tharp, R. G, Jordan, C., Speidel, G. E, Au, K. H., Klein, T. W, Cal- kins, R. P., Sloat, K. C. M., & Gallimore, R. (1984). Product and process in applied developmental research: Education and the chil-
  • 65. dren of a minority. In M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff(Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 91-141). Hills- dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tharp, R. G., & Meadow, A. (1973). Differential change in folk disease concepts. Kevista Interamericana de Psicologia, 7, 55-63. Tharp, R. G, & Note, M. (1988). The triadic model of consultation: New developments. In F. West (Ed.), School consultation: Interdisci- plinary perspectives on theory, research, training, and practice (pp. 35-51). Austin, TX: Research and Training Project on School Con- sultation, University of Texas at Austin, & Association of Educa- tional and Psychological Consultants. Tharp, R. G., & Wetzel, R. J. (1969). Behavior modification in the natu- ral environment. New York: Academic Press. Thomas, M. B., &Dansby, P. G. (1985). Black clients: Family structure, therapeutic issues, and strengths. Psychotherapy, 22, 398-407. Tyler, F. B, Sussewell, D. R., & Williams-McCoy, J. (1985). Ethnic valid- ity in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 22, 311-320.
  • 66. Vazquez-Nuttal, E, Avila-Vivas, Z., & Morales-Barreto, G. (1984). Working with Latin American families. Family Therapy Collections, 9, 75-90. Vygotsky, L. (1929). The problem of the cultural development of the child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 36, 415-432, Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psycho- logical processes (M. Cole, V John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souber- man, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1988). Unpackaging cul- tural effects on classroom learning: Hawaiian peer assistance and child-generated activity. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 19, 327-353. Weisz, J. R., Suwanlert, S., Chaiyasit, W, & Walter, B. R. (1987). Over- and under-controlled referral problems among children and adoles- cents from Thailand and the United States: The vat and wai of cul- tural differences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 719-726.
  • 67. Weisz,!. R, Suwanlert, S, Chaiyasit, W, Weiss, B., Achenbach, T. M, & Walter, B. R. (1987). Epidemiology of behavioral and emotional problems among Thai and American children: Parent reports for ages 6 to 11. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- cent Psychiatry, 26, 890-897. Wertsch, J. Y (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Whiting, B. B. (1976). The problem of the packaged variable. In K. Riegel& J. A. Meacham (Eds.). The developing individual in a chang- ing world: Historical and cultural issues (Vol. 1, pp. 303-309). Chi- cago: Aldine. Received January 9,1991 Revision received May 3,1991 Accepted May 15,1991 • CHAPTER FOURTEEN: Juvenile Corrections: Probation, Community Treatment, and Institutionalization CHAPTER OUTLINE JUVENILE PROBATION
  • 68. · Historical Development Expanding Community Treatment Contemporary Juvenile Probation Duties of Juvenile Probation Officers PROBATION INNOVATIONS · Intensive Supervision Electronic Monitoring Restorative Justice Balanced Probation Restitution Residential Community Treatment SECURE CORRECTIONS · History of Juvenile Institutions What Does This Mean to Me? JUVENILE INSTITUTIONS TODAY: PUBLIC AND PRIVATE · Population Trends Physical Conditions THE INSTITUTIONALIZED JUVENILE · Male Inmates FOCUS ON DELINQUENCY: Mental Health Needs of Juvenile Inmates on the Rise Female Inmates CORRECTIONAL TREATMENT FOR JUVENILES · Individual Treatment Techniques: Past and Present Group Treatment Techniques Educational, Vocational, and Recreational Programs Wilderness Programs Professional Spotlight: Kristi Swanson Juvenile Boot Camps THE LEGAL RIGHT TO TREATMENT · The Struggle for Basic Civil Rights JUVENILE AFTERCARE AND REENTRY · Supervision JUVENILE DELINQUENCY: Treatment: Using the Intensive Aftercare Program (IAP) Model Aftercare Revocation Procedures
  • 69. FUTURE OF JUVENILE CORRECTIONS LEARNING OBJECTIVES After reading this chapter you should: · 1. Be familiar with juvenile probation. · 2. Know about new approaches for providing probation services to juvenile offenders. · 3. Understand past and current trends in the use of juvenile institutions and key issues facing the institutionalized juvenile offender. · 4. Be able to identify current juvenile correctional treatment approaches and comment on their effectiveness in reducing recidivism. · 5. Know about aftercare and reentry for juvenile offenders. REAL CASES/REAL PEOPLE: Karen’s Story Karen Gilligan, age 16, was the oldest of four children living with their parents in a small rural community. Her mother worked two jobs, her father was unemployed, and both parents drank heavily. Karen’s high school attendance was sporadic. She started to experiment with alcohol and vandalized local businesses. After being arrested in a stolen car on several occasions, Karen was referred to juvenile court and was put on community supervision and probation. An initial assessment was provided by her probation officer, and formal dispositional recommendations were made to the court. She would remain at home on house arrest for 60 days, attend school regularly and maintain at least a C average, follow an alcohol and drug assessment program, and participate in weekly family therapy with her parents. Karen was also ordered to cooperate with the juvenile restitution program, pay her restitution in full within six months, and participate in the Community Adolescent Intensive Supervision Program, as arranged by her probation officer. Not used to being accountable to anyone, Karen struggled initially with all the new rules and expectations. She missed some of her initial appointments and skipped some classes at school. Karen’s probation officer began making unannounced