SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 8
Download to read offline
CASE REPORT
A Binocular Approach to Treating Amblyopia:
Antisuppression Therapy
Robert F. Hess*, Behzad Mansouri*, and Benjamin Thompson*
ABSTRACT
Purpose. We developed a binocular treatment for amblyopia based on antisuppression therapy.
Methods. A novel procedure is outlined for measuring the extent to which the fixing eye suppresses the fellow amblyopic
eye. We hypothesize that suppression renders a structurally binocular system, functionally monocular.
Results. We demonstrate using three strabismic amblyopes that information can be combined normally between their
eyes under viewing conditions where suppression is reduced. Also, we show that prolonged periods of viewing (under
the artificial conditions of stimuli of different contrast in each eye) during which information from the two eyes is
combined leads to a strengthening of binocular vision in such cases and eventual combination of binocular information
under natural viewing conditions (stimuli of the same contrast in each eye). Concomitant improvement in monocular
acuity of the amblyopic eye occurs with this reduction in suppression and strengthening of binocular fusion. Furthermore,
in each of the three cases, stereoscopic function is established.
Conclusions. This provides the basis for a new treatment of amblyopia, one that is purely binocular and aimed at reducing
suppression as a first step.
(Optom Vis Sci 2010;87:697–704)
Key Words: amblyopia, global motion, contrast, binocular summation, dichoptic interaction, treatment of amblyopia
T
he most common treatment for improving monocular func-
tion involves patching the good eye to force the amblyopic
eye to improve. Although there is often improvement to
monocular function for amblyopic children younger than 12
years,1
this does not always result in binocular function.2
There is
a need for alternate approaches that might be more effective in
children, might be applicable to even adults who have been left
permanently visually disabled and whose treatment has been aban-
doned,1
might promote cooperation between the two eyes with the
eventual hope of establishing some rudimentary form of depth
vision, and will not have adverse psychosocial side effects.
Our understanding of the binocular deficit of amblyopes, par-
ticularly strabismics, has changed in recent years. We now know
that the loss of the binocular responsiveness of cortical cells in
strabismic animals is largely reversible3
by ionophoretic applica-
tions of bicuculline (selective blocker of GABAA receptors), sug-
gesting a functional suppression of the input from the strabismic
eye rather than a loss of cells driven by that eye’s input.4
Further-
more, there is reason to doubt the claim that humans with ambly-
opia do not possess binocular mechanisms, since Baker et al.5
showed normal binocular contrast summation in adult strabismic
amblyopes when the signal attenuation by the amblyopic eye is
accounted for (i.e., using signals whose contrast are normalized to
threshold), suggesting that the apparent lack of binocular combi-
nation found previously was simply because of an imbalance in the
monocular signals before the point of summation. All of these
results on amblyopic animals and humans point to the fact that
strabismic amblyopes do have intact, but suppressed, binocular
mechanisms. In support of this, it has been shown that the reason
why binocular combination does not normally occur for suprath-
reshold motion and orientation tasks in strabismic amblyopia is
because of interocular suppression.6
A reduction in suppression
leads to normal levels of binocular combination in strabismic am-
blyopia, revealing the presence of functioning binocular cortical
mechanisms. Finally, it has been shown that the monocular vision
of adult amblyopes can be improved after only 10-min application
of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the visual cortex,
suggesting that a significant part of the monocular loss may be
suppressive in nature.7
Thus, there is converging evidence for the
conjecture that strabismic amblyopes possess cortical cells with
binocular connections but that under binocular (and to a lesser
extent, monocular) viewing, suppressive mechanisms render their
*PhD
McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University,
Montreal, Quebec, Canada (RFH), and Division of Internal Medicine (BT),
Department of Optometry (BM), University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand.
1040-5488/10/8709-0697/0 VOL. 87, NO. 9, PP. 697–704
OPTOMETRY AND VISION SCIENCE
Copyright © 2010 American Academy of Optometry
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
cortex functionally monocular. The consequence is that amblyopia
is an intrinsically binocular problem and not the monocular prob-
lem on which current patching treatment is predicated. Thought
of in this way, the binocular problem involving suppression should
be tackled at the very outset if one is to achieve a good binocular
outcome as opposed to hoping binocular vision will be regained
simply as a consequence of acuity recovery in the amblyopic eye,
which is the current approach and which is often not found to be
the case.2
Recently, we developed a novel way of quantifying suppression.6
We showed, under a wide variety of conditions, when the signal to
the fellow fixing eye is reduced in strength (in this context this is
the contrast of the stimuli), strabismic amblyopes can combine
information between their two eyes, as normal patients do. Thus,
our current notion of suppression is that it reduced the efficiency
with which the signal seen by the amblyopic eye is transduced. The
extent to which the signal strength to the fellow fixing eye needs to
be reduced allows one to quantify the degree of suppression. Here,
we report the results of three case studies where continual and
intensive measurement of the degree of suppression leads, in itself,
to a reduction of the degree of suppression until it is eliminated. In
other words, providing artificial viewing conditions under which
binocular combination can take place results in a strengthening of
binocular vision, so that it can occur under a wider variety of
interocular viewing conditions. Eventually, binocular combina-
tion can occur under more natural viewing conditions when the
eyes view objects of the same physical contrast. This finding pro-
vides the basis for a new binocularly based treatment of amblyopia
in which the suppressive imbalance is measured and treated as a
first step. We found that in many cases, the reduction in suppres-
sion led not only to a reestablishment of stereoscopic function but
also to a reduction in the monocular acuity deficit, attesting to the
primal importance of suppression in the amblyopic syndrome.
METHODS
Observers
We discuss the case results of three adult strabismic amblyopes
who underwent antisuppression therapy. Refraction in all observ-
ers was undertaken, and vision was corrected to best visual acuity.
The “Declaration of Helsinki” was followed and informed consent
was obtained from all observers before data collection.
Dichoptic Global Motion
The Measurement of Suppression
To measure the ability of amblyopic observers to binocularly
combine motion information, we used random dot kinemato-
grams and a coherence motion discrimination task (see Ref. 6 for
details). These stimuli are constructed of two populations of mov-
ing dots. The “signal” population consists of dots that all move in
the same direction, termed the “coherent” direction. Conversely,
the “noise” population has no common motion direction because
all the dots move in random directions. The ratio of signal to noise
dots required to determine the coherent motion direction is called
the motion coherence threshold. Therefore, by using these stimuli
with signal and noise separated dichoptically, one can assess the
degree to which underlying mechanisms combine information
from two eyes.
We have previously used this approach to study binocular inter-
actions in normals8
and strabismic amblyopes,6
and the test has a
high test/retest reliability (r ϭ 0.89; p Ͻ 0.0001). Performance
(that is the signal magnitude required to reach the threshold crite-
rion) was quantified by changing the signal to noise ratio in the
random dot kinematogram. The extent to which information was
combined binocularly was quantified by only allowing one eye to
see the signal and the other eye to see the noise (Fig. 1). In a
binocularly normal individual, the noise seen by one eye makes the
detection of the motion direction of the signal elements seen by the
other eye more difficult. However, it does not matter which eye
sees the signal and which sees the noise. There is a “dichoptic
balance” in the threshold performance. In amblyopes with sup-
pression, it matters which eye sees the signal and which eye sees
the noise. In the most extreme case, if the fellow fixing eye sees the
signal and the amblyopic eye sees the noise, then owing to the
suppression of the amblyopic eye by the fellow fixing eye, perfor-
mance will be at ceiling. On the other hand, if the amblyopic eye
sees the signal and the fellow fixing eye sees the noise, then the
performance will be at chance. Thus, one would expect there to be
an imbalance in the dichoptic thresholds because of suppression.
By suitably imbalancing the strength of the signals seen by the
fellow fixing eye (be it signal or noise), we found that balanced
dichoptic performance could be obtained, reflecting the fact that
the information from the two eyes was being combined binocu-
larly. In other words, imbalancing the input to the amblyopic
binocular visual system can result in a balanced output, namely
normal binocular combination. The extent of the signal imbalance
needed to achieve this balanced performance provides a measure of
the degree of suppression.
The dichoptic stimuli were produced using a large, eight-mirror
stereoscope to allow signal and noise dots to be presented sepa-
rately to each eye, and thresholds were measured using a standard
up/down staircase procedures. The testing field was circular with a
diameter of 7°, having an outer peripheral fusion frame that was
used to ensure correct alignment of dichoptic images in the case
where strong suppression prevented the central nonius markers
from being used. This was the case for the first strabismic amblyope
(case 1).
Stimulus
Visual Acuity and Stereo Acuity
Visual acuity was measured with a Snellen letter chart at 6 m,
and stereo acuity using the preschool Randot test at 30-cm viewing
distance.
RESULTS
Case 1
Case 1, a 44-year-old man, who was an emmetrope, presented
with a constant 20° left esotropia and grossly reduced acuity (20/
400) in the left eye that could not be improved with a refractive
correction. He had a history of strabismic amblyopia from the age
of 4 years, when the strabismus was first detected but had not
698 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al.
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
undertaken any patching and had not had surgery. He did not
show fusion on the Worth 4 dot test (distance and near) and had
no measurable stereopsis on the preschool Randot test (near). Our
measurement of his suppression using our dichoptic global motion
stimulus is shown in Fig. 2A. Here, we are plotting the dichoptic
threshold ratio, which is the ratio of the performance when the
amblyopic eye sees the noise and the fellow eye the signal compared
with vice versa. In a binocularly normal observer, it does not matter
whether the right eye sees the signal and the left eye sees the noise
or vice versa because the information from the two eyes is com-
bined and the binocular signal to noise ratio is the same in these
two situations. In this case, we would expect the dichoptic thresh-
old ratio as expressed in Fig. 1A, B to be at unity. The extent to
which the dichoptic ratio is above unity signifies that there is an
imbalance in the combination of binocular information, and in the
case of strabismus, this is because of suppression. We quantify
the degree of suppression by seeing how much we have to offset the
contrast (i.e., reduce it in the fellow fixing eye) to establish equal
performance (i.e., a dichoptic threshold ratio of unity). The ab-
scissa is the contrast ratio of the stimulus (be it signal or noise) seen
by each eye. The contrast of his fellow fixing eye had to be reduced
by a factor of ϳ8 before there was evidence that information was
being combined between his two eyes (the balance point is indicated
by the interocular contrast ratio that corresponds to a dichoptic
threshold ratio of unity, i.e., x axis intercept). This indicates a strong
level of suppression exerted on the amblyopic by the fixing eye under
binocular viewing conditions.
This subject came in for 3 weeks, (4 days a week, average of 350
threshold measurements per week) repeating this measurement of
the balance point under the assumption that by providing condi-
tions over an extended time where the suppression by the fixing eye
is reduced (by reducing the contrast of the signal and noise seen by
the fixing eye) that this would lead to a strengthening of the bin-
ocular connections that underlay the combination of left and right
eye information. Summary measurements of the balance point
after the first (dashed line) and last (filled symbols) week of training
are shown in Fig. 2B. It is clear that after a substantial amount of
binocular training, the degree of suppression is reduced, as re-
flected in the fact that the contrast of stimuli shown to the fixing
eye now needed to be reduced to a much lesser extent. In this case,
the balance point (i.e., the contrast ratio at which the dichoptic
threshold ratio was unity) only changed a little (i.e., from a contrast
ratio of eight before treatment to one of five after treatment);
however, large changes occurred in the extent to which the fellow
fixing eye’s signal needed to be reduced in contrast for binocular
combination to take place (i.e., for stimuli of equal contrast, the
FIGURE 1.
Schematic presentation of the random dot kinematogram shown to each eye during dichoptic viewing. Black arrows show the signal dots schematically,
which were moving, in the same direction (up vs. down) within a trial. White arrows represent the noise dots schematically, which were moving in
random directions. In different trials, signal dots were shown to the fixing eye and noise dots to the fellow amblyopic eye and vice versa. The ratio of
the thresholds obtained for these two types of dichoptic presentations was computed as the dichoptic threshold ratio.
Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 699
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
FIGURE 2.
Results are shown for Case 1 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular
viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each
eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training.
700 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al.
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
dichoptic threshold ratio was 11 before treatment, and three after
treatment).The fact that it took 1 or 2 weeks before improvements
were seen was probably because of the limited treatment duration.
Another way of quantifying the extent to which suppression is
reduced, and as a consequence binocular combination is strength-
ened, is to compare results where each eye receives stimuli of the
same contrast (conditions where suppression is maximal). This is
relevant to everyday viewing where the physical contrast of stimuli
impinging on the retinae is of identical contrast. This corresponds
to the condition where dichoptic motion thresholds (e.g., Fig. 2A,
B) are most different (leftmost data in these figures, i.e., an intero-
cular contrast ratio of unity) owing to the strong suppression that
occurs from the fellow fixing eye to the amblyopic eye under these
conditions. In Fig. 2C, we plot the dichoptic motion thresholds of
each eye (from which the ratios were computed for Fig. 2A, B) as a
function of the weeks of training. Our binocular training regime
improves the dichoptic motion threshold of the amblyopic eye
(i.e., when the amblyopic eye sees the signal and the fixing eye the
noise) although having much less impact on the dichoptic motion
thresholds of the fixing eye (i.e., when the fixing eye views the
signal and the amblyopic eye the noise). Over time, the threshold
of the amblyopic eye approaches that of the fixing eye (i.e., the ratio
of the dichoptic thresholds is approaching unity in Fig. 2A, B).
What this means is that, over time, suppression is being reduced
and the two eyes of this strabismic amblyope are now successfully
combining information of comparable contrasts between the two
eyes. The improved binocular combination that results was re-
flected in the establishment of stereoscopic function. Stereoscopic
sensitivity was limited to 200 arc secs but was present for the first
time (Fig. 2E).
We were surprised to find that monocular acuity of the am-
blyopic eye improved as a result of our antisuppression therapy.
These results are shown in Fig. 2D where Snellen line letter
acuity is plotted against the period of training. A significant
improvement accompanies the reduction of suppression even in
this adult amblyope.
Case 2
Case 2 was a 45-year-old man who had a history of anisome-
tropic amblyopia [R, Ϫ1.75 diopter (D)/ϩ0.50 ϫ 90°; L, ϩ1.25
D] which had been first detected at the age of 11 years and treated
with a mixture of patching for 1 to 2 years and refractive correction
at the age of 11 years. No surgery had been undertaken. He pre-
sented with a 3-D anisometropia, a 6° constant esotropia (also
detected at the age of 11 years), a mild degree of amblyopia (20/
63), no fusion on the Worth 4 dot test, and no measurable stere-
opsis at near.
Using our global motion measurement of suppression, we found
a degree of suppression that could be nullified when the contrast of
the stimuli viewed by the fixing eye were reduced by a factor of 4
(Fig. 3A).
Over the antisuppression treatment period of 5 weeks (4 days a
week, average of 350 threshold measurements per week), there was
a steady change in the degree of suppression exerted by the fixing
eye. This is reflected in a change in the derived balance point (i.e.,
the interocular contrast ratio where the dichoptic threshold ratio
between the fixing and amblyopic eyes is unity, x axis intercept in
Fig. 3B) or by the progressive improvement in the dichoptic mo-
tion threshold for the amblyopic eye for stimuli of equal contrasts
in the two eyes (Fig. 3C). Monocular acuity improved in the am-
blyopic eye from 20/63 to 20/30 (Fig. 3D), and stereopsis was
established with an acuity of 20 arc secs (Fig. 3E).
Case 3
Case 3 was a 33-year-old woman with a history of strabismic
amblyopia (R, Ϫ1.00 D; L, Ϫ0.50 D) having been first detected at
the age of 5 years. Two years of constant patching was undertaken,
but there had been no surgical intervention (visual acuity at the end
of this patching was not available). She presented with a small but
bilaterally equivalent amount of myopia, a small angle (4°) con-
stant esotropia with intermittent fusion on the Worth 4 dot (dis-
tance and near) test but no measurable stereopsis (near). The acuity
in the deviating eye was 20/80. The measurement of suppression
using the balance point determination with the dichoptic motion
stimulus showed a mild suppression by the fixing eye that could
only be neutralized by reducing the contrast in the fellow fixing eye
by a factor of 3 (Fig. 4A).
During a period of 5-week training (3 days a week, average of
100 threshold measurements per week), using our antisuppression
therapy, the degree of suppression gradually disappeared (indi-
cated by a dichoptic ratio of 1 for equal interocular contrasts in Fig.
4B). Another reflection of this reduction in suppression is the
improvement that occurred in her dichoptic motion thresholds for
her amblyopic eye (signal to amblyopic eye, noise over the training
period). Stereopsis (near) was established with an acuity of 30 arc
secs (Fig. 4E), and monocular acuity in the amblyopic eye im-
proved from 20/80 to 20/25 (Fig. 4D).
DISCUSSION
We describe a new quantitative method for the clinical measure-
ment of suppression, something that is done in either a binary
fashion (i.e., worth 4 dot test) or using methods that are coarse (i.e.,
the Sbisa Bar) or uncalibrated (i.e., reducing illumination for one
eye on the synoptophore) in the clinic at present. The method is
based on a signal to noise approach but applied within the context
of dichoptic stimulation. This allowed us to demonstrate, for the
first time, that threshold and suprathreshold information can be
combined between the eyes of strabismic amblyopes under suit-
able, albeit artificial, viewing conditions.5–7,9
Suppression is a well-
known clinical entity, but it is rarely measured quantitatively and
rarely used to direct the treatment approach. We believe this is
unfortunate because the current animal3,4,10,11
and human5,6
research on amblyopia suggests that it is primarily a binocular
problem with suppression being the key feature. We strongly rec-
ommend that suppression is measured in a quantitative way along
the lines suggested here.
Furthermore, we show here, for three subjects, that intensive
training using this suppression measurement approach leads to a
progressive strengthening of binocular vision in strabismic am-
blyopes such that they can eventually operate under natural view-
ing conditions where the left and right image contrast is equal. We
found this to be the case in 8/10 amblyopes tested so far, and it
should be emphasized that all subjects were adult amblyopes well
Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 701
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
FIGURE 3.
Results are shown for case 2 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular
viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each
eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training.
702 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al.
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
FIGURE 4.
Results are shown for case 3 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular
viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each
eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training.
Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 703
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
beyond the accepted “critical period” for patching therapy.1
Con-
current with this improvement in the efficacy of binocular combi-
nation, we also found that stereopsis in all three cases presented
here, and in a majority of cases studied so far, was established and
the monocular acuity also improved. These improvements were
significant, stable, and in some cases large.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study is supported by a CIHR grant MOP 53346 (to RFH).
Received February 23, 2010; accepted April 29, 2010.
REFERENCES
1. Epelbaum M, Milleret C, Buisseret P, Dufier JL. The sensitive period
for strabismic amblyopia in humans. Ophthalmology 1993;100:
323–7.
2. Scheiman MM, Hertle RW, Beck RW, Edwards AR, Birch E, Cotter
SA, Crouch ER Jr, Cruz OA, Davitt BV, Donahue S, Holmes JM,
Lyon DW, Repka MX, Sala NA, Silbert DI, Suh DW, Tamkins SM.
Randomized trial of treatment of amblyopia in children aged 7 to 17
years. Arch Ophthalmol 2005;123:437–47.
3. Mower GD, Christen WG, Burchfiel JL, Duffy FH. Microionto-
phoretic bicuculline restores binocular responses to visual cortical
neurons in strabismic cats. Brain Res 1984;309:168–72.
4. Sengpiel F, Jirmann KU, Vorobyov V, Eysel UT. Strabismic suppres-
sion is mediated by inhibitory interactions in the primary visual cor-
tex. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:1750–8.
5. Baker DH, Meese TS, Mansouri B, Hess RF. Binocular summation
of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthal-
mol Vis Sci 2007;48:5332–8.
6. Mansouri B, Thompson B, Hess RF. Measurement of suprathreshold
binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res 2008;48:2775–84.
7. Thompson B, Mansouri B, Koski L, Hess RF. Brain plasticity in the
adult: modulation of function in amblyopia with rTMS. Curr Biol
2008;18:1067–71.
8. Hess RF, Hutchinson CV, Ledgeway T, Mansouri B. Binocular in-
fluences on global motion processing in the human visual system.
Vision Res 2007;47:1682–92.
9. Baker DH, Meese TS, Hess RF. Contrast masking in strabismic
amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular
summation. Vision Res 2008;48:1625–40.
10. Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and ambly-
opia in strabismus. Eye (Lond) 1996;10(Pt 2):250–8.
11. Sengpiel F, Blakemore C, Kind PC, Harrad R. Interocular suppres-
sion in the visual cortex of strabismic cats. J Neurosci 1994;14:
6855–71.
Robert F. Hess
687 Pine Avenue West (H4-14)
Montreal, Quebec, Canada PQ H3A 1A1
e-mail: robert.hess@mcgill.ca
704 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al.
Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010

More Related Content

What's hot

Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids finSensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
MUBOSScz
 
anomalous retinal correspondence
anomalous retinal correspondenceanomalous retinal correspondence
anomalous retinal correspondence
Rajeshwori
 

What's hot (20)

Anomalies of accomodation ‫‬
Anomalies of accomodation ‫‬Anomalies of accomodation ‫‬
Anomalies of accomodation ‫‬
 
Convergence & its anomalies
Convergence  &  its anomaliesConvergence  &  its anomalies
Convergence & its anomalies
 
Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids finSensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
Sensory perceptionexaminationandaids fin
 
anomalous retinal correspondence
anomalous retinal correspondenceanomalous retinal correspondence
anomalous retinal correspondence
 
Sensory evaluation of squint
Sensory  evaluation of squintSensory  evaluation of squint
Sensory evaluation of squint
 
Binocular vision final
Binocular vision finalBinocular vision final
Binocular vision final
 
Accommodation
AccommodationAccommodation
Accommodation
 
Microtropia
MicrotropiaMicrotropia
Microtropia
 
Convergence insufficiency
Convergence insufficiencyConvergence insufficiency
Convergence insufficiency
 
Basics of binocular vision
Basics of binocular visionBasics of binocular vision
Basics of binocular vision
 
Accommodation: measurement and its anomalies
Accommodation:  measurement and its  anomaliesAccommodation:  measurement and its  anomalies
Accommodation: measurement and its anomalies
 
Fusion
FusionFusion
Fusion
 
binocular single vision
binocular single visionbinocular single vision
binocular single vision
 
Accomodation
AccomodationAccomodation
Accomodation
 
My low vision rehabilitation in multiple handicapped patients
My low vision rehabilitation in multiple handicapped patientsMy low vision rehabilitation in multiple handicapped patients
My low vision rehabilitation in multiple handicapped patients
 
Accommodation reflex opthamalogy mbbs pptx slides
Accommodation reflex opthamalogy mbbs pptx slidesAccommodation reflex opthamalogy mbbs pptx slides
Accommodation reflex opthamalogy mbbs pptx slides
 
accommodation,reflexes,defects of vision
accommodation,reflexes,defects of visionaccommodation,reflexes,defects of vision
accommodation,reflexes,defects of vision
 
Retinal correspondence
Retinal correspondenceRetinal correspondence
Retinal correspondence
 
Aspheric vs Spheric IOL
Aspheric vs Spheric IOLAspheric vs Spheric IOL
Aspheric vs Spheric IOL
 
Binocular vision
Binocular visionBinocular vision
Binocular vision
 

Viewers also liked

Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Yesenia Castillo Salinas
 
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
Yesenia Castillo Salinas
 

Viewers also liked (19)

Procrsmed00485 0178
Procrsmed00485 0178Procrsmed00485 0178
Procrsmed00485 0178
 
The clinical course_of_intermittent_exotropia.9
The clinical course_of_intermittent_exotropia.9The clinical course_of_intermittent_exotropia.9
The clinical course_of_intermittent_exotropia.9
 
Mop chapter06
Mop chapter06Mop chapter06
Mop chapter06
 
Monocular vernier acuity_in_normally_binocular,.7
Monocular vernier acuity_in_normally_binocular,.7Monocular vernier acuity_in_normally_binocular,.7
Monocular vernier acuity_in_normally_binocular,.7
 
April 5-2013-vrics
April 5-2013-vricsApril 5-2013-vrics
April 5-2013-vrics
 
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
 
Improvement of visual_function_in_an_adult.14
Improvement of visual_function_in_an_adult.14Improvement of visual_function_in_an_adult.14
Improvement of visual_function_in_an_adult.14
 
Effect of amblyopia_on_self_esteem_in_children.10
Effect of amblyopia_on_self_esteem_in_children.10Effect of amblyopia_on_self_esteem_in_children.10
Effect of amblyopia_on_self_esteem_in_children.10
 
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
Evaluating relative accommodations_in_general.10
 
Prevalence of amblyopia_and_refractive_errors_in.19
Prevalence of amblyopia_and_refractive_errors_in.19Prevalence of amblyopia_and_refractive_errors_in.19
Prevalence of amblyopia_and_refractive_errors_in.19
 
Reliability of binocular_vision_measurements_used.12
Reliability of binocular_vision_measurements_used.12Reliability of binocular_vision_measurements_used.12
Reliability of binocular_vision_measurements_used.12
 
Parallel testing infinity_balance__instrument_and.12
Parallel testing infinity_balance__instrument_and.12Parallel testing infinity_balance__instrument_and.12
Parallel testing infinity_balance__instrument_and.12
 
Mop chapter09
Mop chapter09Mop chapter09
Mop chapter09
 
Mop chapter05
Mop chapter05Mop chapter05
Mop chapter05
 
Mop chapter04
Mop chapter04Mop chapter04
Mop chapter04
 
Mop chapter08
Mop chapter08Mop chapter08
Mop chapter08
 
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
Accommodative training to_reduce_nearwork_induced.11
 
Retinoscopy
RetinoscopyRetinoscopy
Retinoscopy
 
Tests
TestsTests
Tests
 

Similar to A binocular approach_to_treating_amblyopia_.11

Sensory Adaptation to Strabismus
Sensory Adaptation to StrabismusSensory Adaptation to Strabismus
Sensory Adaptation to Strabismus
Hossein Mirzaie
 
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopyAnterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
Patricia Chirinos
 
ASOP-06-0634.pdf
ASOP-06-0634.pdfASOP-06-0634.pdf
ASOP-06-0634.pdf
BABLI SHARMA
 
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
Yasmine Abdulrahman
 
Amblyopia Management
Amblyopia ManagementAmblyopia Management
Amblyopia Management
siraj safi
 

Similar to A binocular approach_to_treating_amblyopia_.11 (20)

Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
Binocular balance in_normal_vision_and_its.9
 
Binocular interaction in amblyopia and its clinical feasibility
Binocular interaction in amblyopia  and its  clinical feasibilityBinocular interaction in amblyopia  and its  clinical feasibility
Binocular interaction in amblyopia and its clinical feasibility
 
Active Vision Therapy in Management of Amblyopia (healthkura.com)
Active Vision Therapy in Management of Amblyopia (healthkura.com)Active Vision Therapy in Management of Amblyopia (healthkura.com)
Active Vision Therapy in Management of Amblyopia (healthkura.com)
 
Spasm of the_near_reflex_triggered_by_disruption.9
Spasm of the_near_reflex_triggered_by_disruption.9Spasm of the_near_reflex_triggered_by_disruption.9
Spasm of the_near_reflex_triggered_by_disruption.9
 
Sensory Adaptation to Strabismus
Sensory Adaptation to StrabismusSensory Adaptation to Strabismus
Sensory Adaptation to Strabismus
 
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopyAnterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
Anterior segment imaging: ultrasound biomicroscopy
 
Amblyopia: Screening and Management
Amblyopia: Screening and ManagementAmblyopia: Screening and Management
Amblyopia: Screening and Management
 
3.ophthalmology workup.3
3.ophthalmology workup.33.ophthalmology workup.3
3.ophthalmology workup.3
 
ASOP-06-0634.pdf
ASOP-06-0634.pdfASOP-06-0634.pdf
ASOP-06-0634.pdf
 
Development and treatment_of_astigmatism_related.19
Development and treatment_of_astigmatism_related.19Development and treatment_of_astigmatism_related.19
Development and treatment_of_astigmatism_related.19
 
Strabismic ambylopia
Strabismic ambylopia Strabismic ambylopia
Strabismic ambylopia
 
Evaluation of visual function with opaque media
Evaluation of visual function with opaque media Evaluation of visual function with opaque media
Evaluation of visual function with opaque media
 
Binocular Single Vision Tests
Binocular Single Vision TestsBinocular Single Vision Tests
Binocular Single Vision Tests
 
Synoptophore.pptx
Synoptophore.pptxSynoptophore.pptx
Synoptophore.pptx
 
BINOCULAR VISION PowerPoint presentation.pptx
BINOCULAR VISION PowerPoint presentation.pptxBINOCULAR VISION PowerPoint presentation.pptx
BINOCULAR VISION PowerPoint presentation.pptx
 
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
Consideration of Binocular Parameters in Spectacle Correction of Anisometropi...
 
Binocular Single Vision Ophthalmology .pptx
Binocular Single Vision Ophthalmology .pptxBinocular Single Vision Ophthalmology .pptx
Binocular Single Vision Ophthalmology .pptx
 
Introduction to binocular single vision (BSV)
Introduction to binocular single vision (BSV)Introduction to binocular single vision (BSV)
Introduction to binocular single vision (BSV)
 
Cutting the encircling band
Cutting the encircling bandCutting the encircling band
Cutting the encircling band
 
Amblyopia Management
Amblyopia ManagementAmblyopia Management
Amblyopia Management
 

More from Yesenia Castillo Salinas

More from Yesenia Castillo Salinas (20)

Tfm final final_2011
Tfm final final_2011Tfm final final_2011
Tfm final final_2011
 
Tfm lucia morchón
Tfm lucia morchónTfm lucia morchón
Tfm lucia morchón
 
Terapia visual-y-comportamental-frente-al-aprendizaje
Terapia visual-y-comportamental-frente-al-aprendizajeTerapia visual-y-comportamental-frente-al-aprendizaje
Terapia visual-y-comportamental-frente-al-aprendizaje
 
Terapia visual-ii
Terapia visual-iiTerapia visual-ii
Terapia visual-ii
 
Terapia de__accion__visual
Terapia  de__accion__visualTerapia  de__accion__visual
Terapia de__accion__visual
 
Terapia visual
Terapia visualTerapia visual
Terapia visual
 
Terapia visual en la escuela
Terapia visual en la escuelaTerapia visual en la escuela
Terapia visual en la escuela
 
Terapia visual 1
Terapia visual 1Terapia visual 1
Terapia visual 1
 
Tema 2-format-paloma-sobrado
Tema 2-format-paloma-sobradoTema 2-format-paloma-sobrado
Tema 2-format-paloma-sobrado
 
Tema 1 ocw
Tema 1 ocwTema 1 ocw
Tema 1 ocw
 
Revital visioninyourpractice
Revital visioninyourpracticeRevital visioninyourpractice
Revital visioninyourpractice
 
Puell óptica fisiológica
Puell óptica fisiológicaPuell óptica fisiológica
Puell óptica fisiológica
 
Prom coi vision 2
Prom coi vision 2Prom coi vision 2
Prom coi vision 2
 
Prescribing spectacles in_children__a_pediatric.9
Prescribing spectacles in_children__a_pediatric.9Prescribing spectacles in_children__a_pediatric.9
Prescribing spectacles in_children__a_pediatric.9
 
Op00306 c
Op00306 cOp00306 c
Op00306 c
 
Leccion 17 texto
Leccion 17 textoLeccion 17 texto
Leccion 17 texto
 
Evolucion del ojo
Evolucion del ojoEvolucion del ojo
Evolucion del ojo
 
Eoft m01 t03
Eoft m01 t03Eoft m01 t03
Eoft m01 t03
 
Entrenamiento visual
Entrenamiento visualEntrenamiento visual
Entrenamiento visual
 
Congenital optic nerve_hypoplasia.5
Congenital optic nerve_hypoplasia.5Congenital optic nerve_hypoplasia.5
Congenital optic nerve_hypoplasia.5
 

Recently uploaded

Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
Sheetaleventcompany
 
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
Genuine Call Girls
 
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
Sheetaleventcompany
 
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
Sheetaleventcompany
 
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
Sheetaleventcompany
 
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
Sheetaleventcompany
 
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
Sheetaleventcompany
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
Goa Call Girl Service 📞9xx000xx09📞Just Call Divya📲 Call Girl In Goa No💰Advanc...
 
Kolkata Call Girls Shobhabazar 💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃 Top Class Call Gir...
Kolkata Call Girls Shobhabazar  💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃  Top Class Call Gir...Kolkata Call Girls Shobhabazar  💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃  Top Class Call Gir...
Kolkata Call Girls Shobhabazar 💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃 Top Class Call Gir...
 
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
Ahmedabad Call Girls Book Now 8980367676 Top Class Ahmedabad Escort Service A...
 
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
💚Chandigarh Call Girls Service 💯Piya 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girls In Chandigarh No...
 
7 steps How to prevent Thalassemia : Dr Sharda Jain & Vandana Gupta
7 steps How to prevent Thalassemia : Dr Sharda Jain & Vandana Gupta7 steps How to prevent Thalassemia : Dr Sharda Jain & Vandana Gupta
7 steps How to prevent Thalassemia : Dr Sharda Jain & Vandana Gupta
 
tongue disease lecture Dr Assadawy legacy
tongue disease lecture Dr Assadawy legacytongue disease lecture Dr Assadawy legacy
tongue disease lecture Dr Assadawy legacy
 
Kolkata Call Girls Naktala 💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃 Top Class Call Girl Se...
Kolkata Call Girls Naktala  💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃  Top Class Call Girl Se...Kolkata Call Girls Naktala  💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃  Top Class Call Girl Se...
Kolkata Call Girls Naktala 💯Call Us 🔝 8005736733 🔝 💃 Top Class Call Girl Se...
 
Circulatory Shock, types and stages, compensatory mechanisms
Circulatory Shock, types and stages, compensatory mechanismsCirculatory Shock, types and stages, compensatory mechanisms
Circulatory Shock, types and stages, compensatory mechanisms
 
Exclusive Call Girls Bangalore {7304373326} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Bangal...
Exclusive Call Girls Bangalore {7304373326} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Bangal...Exclusive Call Girls Bangalore {7304373326} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Bangal...
Exclusive Call Girls Bangalore {7304373326} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Bangal...
 
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
💚Call Girls In Amritsar 💯Anvi 📲🔝8725944379🔝Amritsar Call Girl No💰Advance Cash...
 
Call Girls Rishikesh Just Call 9667172968 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Rishikesh Just Call 9667172968 Top Class Call Girl Service AvailableCall Girls Rishikesh Just Call 9667172968 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
Call Girls Rishikesh Just Call 9667172968 Top Class Call Girl Service Available
 
(RIYA)🎄Airhostess Call Girl Jaipur Call Now 8445551418 Premium Collection Of ...
(RIYA)🎄Airhostess Call Girl Jaipur Call Now 8445551418 Premium Collection Of ...(RIYA)🎄Airhostess Call Girl Jaipur Call Now 8445551418 Premium Collection Of ...
(RIYA)🎄Airhostess Call Girl Jaipur Call Now 8445551418 Premium Collection Of ...
 
Low Cost Call Girls Bangalore {9179660964} ❤️VVIP NISHA Call Girls in Bangalo...
Low Cost Call Girls Bangalore {9179660964} ❤️VVIP NISHA Call Girls in Bangalo...Low Cost Call Girls Bangalore {9179660964} ❤️VVIP NISHA Call Girls in Bangalo...
Low Cost Call Girls Bangalore {9179660964} ❤️VVIP NISHA Call Girls in Bangalo...
 
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
Premium Call Girls Nagpur {9xx000xx09} ❤️VVIP POOJA Call Girls in Nagpur Maha...
 
💰Call Girl In Bangalore☎️63788-78445💰 Call Girl service in Bangalore☎️Bangalo...
💰Call Girl In Bangalore☎️63788-78445💰 Call Girl service in Bangalore☎️Bangalo...💰Call Girl In Bangalore☎️63788-78445💰 Call Girl service in Bangalore☎️Bangalo...
💰Call Girl In Bangalore☎️63788-78445💰 Call Girl service in Bangalore☎️Bangalo...
 
Race Course Road } Book Call Girls in Bangalore | Whatsapp No 6378878445 VIP ...
Race Course Road } Book Call Girls in Bangalore | Whatsapp No 6378878445 VIP ...Race Course Road } Book Call Girls in Bangalore | Whatsapp No 6378878445 VIP ...
Race Course Road } Book Call Girls in Bangalore | Whatsapp No 6378878445 VIP ...
 
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
Premium Call Girls Dehradun {8854095900} ❤️VVIP ANJU Call Girls in Dehradun U...
 
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
❤️Amritsar Escorts Service☎️9815674956☎️ Call Girl service in Amritsar☎️ Amri...
 
💚Reliable Call Girls Chandigarh 💯Niamh 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girl In Chandigarh N...
💚Reliable Call Girls Chandigarh 💯Niamh 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girl In Chandigarh N...💚Reliable Call Girls Chandigarh 💯Niamh 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girl In Chandigarh N...
💚Reliable Call Girls Chandigarh 💯Niamh 📲🔝8868886958🔝Call Girl In Chandigarh N...
 
Bhawanipatna Call Girls 📞9332606886 Call Girls in Bhawanipatna Escorts servic...
Bhawanipatna Call Girls 📞9332606886 Call Girls in Bhawanipatna Escorts servic...Bhawanipatna Call Girls 📞9332606886 Call Girls in Bhawanipatna Escorts servic...
Bhawanipatna Call Girls 📞9332606886 Call Girls in Bhawanipatna Escorts servic...
 

A binocular approach_to_treating_amblyopia_.11

  • 1. CASE REPORT A Binocular Approach to Treating Amblyopia: Antisuppression Therapy Robert F. Hess*, Behzad Mansouri*, and Benjamin Thompson* ABSTRACT Purpose. We developed a binocular treatment for amblyopia based on antisuppression therapy. Methods. A novel procedure is outlined for measuring the extent to which the fixing eye suppresses the fellow amblyopic eye. We hypothesize that suppression renders a structurally binocular system, functionally monocular. Results. We demonstrate using three strabismic amblyopes that information can be combined normally between their eyes under viewing conditions where suppression is reduced. Also, we show that prolonged periods of viewing (under the artificial conditions of stimuli of different contrast in each eye) during which information from the two eyes is combined leads to a strengthening of binocular vision in such cases and eventual combination of binocular information under natural viewing conditions (stimuli of the same contrast in each eye). Concomitant improvement in monocular acuity of the amblyopic eye occurs with this reduction in suppression and strengthening of binocular fusion. Furthermore, in each of the three cases, stereoscopic function is established. Conclusions. This provides the basis for a new treatment of amblyopia, one that is purely binocular and aimed at reducing suppression as a first step. (Optom Vis Sci 2010;87:697–704) Key Words: amblyopia, global motion, contrast, binocular summation, dichoptic interaction, treatment of amblyopia T he most common treatment for improving monocular func- tion involves patching the good eye to force the amblyopic eye to improve. Although there is often improvement to monocular function for amblyopic children younger than 12 years,1 this does not always result in binocular function.2 There is a need for alternate approaches that might be more effective in children, might be applicable to even adults who have been left permanently visually disabled and whose treatment has been aban- doned,1 might promote cooperation between the two eyes with the eventual hope of establishing some rudimentary form of depth vision, and will not have adverse psychosocial side effects. Our understanding of the binocular deficit of amblyopes, par- ticularly strabismics, has changed in recent years. We now know that the loss of the binocular responsiveness of cortical cells in strabismic animals is largely reversible3 by ionophoretic applica- tions of bicuculline (selective blocker of GABAA receptors), sug- gesting a functional suppression of the input from the strabismic eye rather than a loss of cells driven by that eye’s input.4 Further- more, there is reason to doubt the claim that humans with ambly- opia do not possess binocular mechanisms, since Baker et al.5 showed normal binocular contrast summation in adult strabismic amblyopes when the signal attenuation by the amblyopic eye is accounted for (i.e., using signals whose contrast are normalized to threshold), suggesting that the apparent lack of binocular combi- nation found previously was simply because of an imbalance in the monocular signals before the point of summation. All of these results on amblyopic animals and humans point to the fact that strabismic amblyopes do have intact, but suppressed, binocular mechanisms. In support of this, it has been shown that the reason why binocular combination does not normally occur for suprath- reshold motion and orientation tasks in strabismic amblyopia is because of interocular suppression.6 A reduction in suppression leads to normal levels of binocular combination in strabismic am- blyopia, revealing the presence of functioning binocular cortical mechanisms. Finally, it has been shown that the monocular vision of adult amblyopes can be improved after only 10-min application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the visual cortex, suggesting that a significant part of the monocular loss may be suppressive in nature.7 Thus, there is converging evidence for the conjecture that strabismic amblyopes possess cortical cells with binocular connections but that under binocular (and to a lesser extent, monocular) viewing, suppressive mechanisms render their *PhD McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada (RFH), and Division of Internal Medicine (BT), Department of Optometry (BM), University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. 1040-5488/10/8709-0697/0 VOL. 87, NO. 9, PP. 697–704 OPTOMETRY AND VISION SCIENCE Copyright © 2010 American Academy of Optometry Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 2. cortex functionally monocular. The consequence is that amblyopia is an intrinsically binocular problem and not the monocular prob- lem on which current patching treatment is predicated. Thought of in this way, the binocular problem involving suppression should be tackled at the very outset if one is to achieve a good binocular outcome as opposed to hoping binocular vision will be regained simply as a consequence of acuity recovery in the amblyopic eye, which is the current approach and which is often not found to be the case.2 Recently, we developed a novel way of quantifying suppression.6 We showed, under a wide variety of conditions, when the signal to the fellow fixing eye is reduced in strength (in this context this is the contrast of the stimuli), strabismic amblyopes can combine information between their two eyes, as normal patients do. Thus, our current notion of suppression is that it reduced the efficiency with which the signal seen by the amblyopic eye is transduced. The extent to which the signal strength to the fellow fixing eye needs to be reduced allows one to quantify the degree of suppression. Here, we report the results of three case studies where continual and intensive measurement of the degree of suppression leads, in itself, to a reduction of the degree of suppression until it is eliminated. In other words, providing artificial viewing conditions under which binocular combination can take place results in a strengthening of binocular vision, so that it can occur under a wider variety of interocular viewing conditions. Eventually, binocular combina- tion can occur under more natural viewing conditions when the eyes view objects of the same physical contrast. This finding pro- vides the basis for a new binocularly based treatment of amblyopia in which the suppressive imbalance is measured and treated as a first step. We found that in many cases, the reduction in suppres- sion led not only to a reestablishment of stereoscopic function but also to a reduction in the monocular acuity deficit, attesting to the primal importance of suppression in the amblyopic syndrome. METHODS Observers We discuss the case results of three adult strabismic amblyopes who underwent antisuppression therapy. Refraction in all observ- ers was undertaken, and vision was corrected to best visual acuity. The “Declaration of Helsinki” was followed and informed consent was obtained from all observers before data collection. Dichoptic Global Motion The Measurement of Suppression To measure the ability of amblyopic observers to binocularly combine motion information, we used random dot kinemato- grams and a coherence motion discrimination task (see Ref. 6 for details). These stimuli are constructed of two populations of mov- ing dots. The “signal” population consists of dots that all move in the same direction, termed the “coherent” direction. Conversely, the “noise” population has no common motion direction because all the dots move in random directions. The ratio of signal to noise dots required to determine the coherent motion direction is called the motion coherence threshold. Therefore, by using these stimuli with signal and noise separated dichoptically, one can assess the degree to which underlying mechanisms combine information from two eyes. We have previously used this approach to study binocular inter- actions in normals8 and strabismic amblyopes,6 and the test has a high test/retest reliability (r ϭ 0.89; p Ͻ 0.0001). Performance (that is the signal magnitude required to reach the threshold crite- rion) was quantified by changing the signal to noise ratio in the random dot kinematogram. The extent to which information was combined binocularly was quantified by only allowing one eye to see the signal and the other eye to see the noise (Fig. 1). In a binocularly normal individual, the noise seen by one eye makes the detection of the motion direction of the signal elements seen by the other eye more difficult. However, it does not matter which eye sees the signal and which sees the noise. There is a “dichoptic balance” in the threshold performance. In amblyopes with sup- pression, it matters which eye sees the signal and which eye sees the noise. In the most extreme case, if the fellow fixing eye sees the signal and the amblyopic eye sees the noise, then owing to the suppression of the amblyopic eye by the fellow fixing eye, perfor- mance will be at ceiling. On the other hand, if the amblyopic eye sees the signal and the fellow fixing eye sees the noise, then the performance will be at chance. Thus, one would expect there to be an imbalance in the dichoptic thresholds because of suppression. By suitably imbalancing the strength of the signals seen by the fellow fixing eye (be it signal or noise), we found that balanced dichoptic performance could be obtained, reflecting the fact that the information from the two eyes was being combined binocu- larly. In other words, imbalancing the input to the amblyopic binocular visual system can result in a balanced output, namely normal binocular combination. The extent of the signal imbalance needed to achieve this balanced performance provides a measure of the degree of suppression. The dichoptic stimuli were produced using a large, eight-mirror stereoscope to allow signal and noise dots to be presented sepa- rately to each eye, and thresholds were measured using a standard up/down staircase procedures. The testing field was circular with a diameter of 7°, having an outer peripheral fusion frame that was used to ensure correct alignment of dichoptic images in the case where strong suppression prevented the central nonius markers from being used. This was the case for the first strabismic amblyope (case 1). Stimulus Visual Acuity and Stereo Acuity Visual acuity was measured with a Snellen letter chart at 6 m, and stereo acuity using the preschool Randot test at 30-cm viewing distance. RESULTS Case 1 Case 1, a 44-year-old man, who was an emmetrope, presented with a constant 20° left esotropia and grossly reduced acuity (20/ 400) in the left eye that could not be improved with a refractive correction. He had a history of strabismic amblyopia from the age of 4 years, when the strabismus was first detected but had not 698 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 3. undertaken any patching and had not had surgery. He did not show fusion on the Worth 4 dot test (distance and near) and had no measurable stereopsis on the preschool Randot test (near). Our measurement of his suppression using our dichoptic global motion stimulus is shown in Fig. 2A. Here, we are plotting the dichoptic threshold ratio, which is the ratio of the performance when the amblyopic eye sees the noise and the fellow eye the signal compared with vice versa. In a binocularly normal observer, it does not matter whether the right eye sees the signal and the left eye sees the noise or vice versa because the information from the two eyes is com- bined and the binocular signal to noise ratio is the same in these two situations. In this case, we would expect the dichoptic thresh- old ratio as expressed in Fig. 1A, B to be at unity. The extent to which the dichoptic ratio is above unity signifies that there is an imbalance in the combination of binocular information, and in the case of strabismus, this is because of suppression. We quantify the degree of suppression by seeing how much we have to offset the contrast (i.e., reduce it in the fellow fixing eye) to establish equal performance (i.e., a dichoptic threshold ratio of unity). The ab- scissa is the contrast ratio of the stimulus (be it signal or noise) seen by each eye. The contrast of his fellow fixing eye had to be reduced by a factor of ϳ8 before there was evidence that information was being combined between his two eyes (the balance point is indicated by the interocular contrast ratio that corresponds to a dichoptic threshold ratio of unity, i.e., x axis intercept). This indicates a strong level of suppression exerted on the amblyopic by the fixing eye under binocular viewing conditions. This subject came in for 3 weeks, (4 days a week, average of 350 threshold measurements per week) repeating this measurement of the balance point under the assumption that by providing condi- tions over an extended time where the suppression by the fixing eye is reduced (by reducing the contrast of the signal and noise seen by the fixing eye) that this would lead to a strengthening of the bin- ocular connections that underlay the combination of left and right eye information. Summary measurements of the balance point after the first (dashed line) and last (filled symbols) week of training are shown in Fig. 2B. It is clear that after a substantial amount of binocular training, the degree of suppression is reduced, as re- flected in the fact that the contrast of stimuli shown to the fixing eye now needed to be reduced to a much lesser extent. In this case, the balance point (i.e., the contrast ratio at which the dichoptic threshold ratio was unity) only changed a little (i.e., from a contrast ratio of eight before treatment to one of five after treatment); however, large changes occurred in the extent to which the fellow fixing eye’s signal needed to be reduced in contrast for binocular combination to take place (i.e., for stimuli of equal contrast, the FIGURE 1. Schematic presentation of the random dot kinematogram shown to each eye during dichoptic viewing. Black arrows show the signal dots schematically, which were moving, in the same direction (up vs. down) within a trial. White arrows represent the noise dots schematically, which were moving in random directions. In different trials, signal dots were shown to the fixing eye and noise dots to the fellow amblyopic eye and vice versa. The ratio of the thresholds obtained for these two types of dichoptic presentations was computed as the dichoptic threshold ratio. Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 699 Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 4. FIGURE 2. Results are shown for Case 1 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training. 700 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 5. dichoptic threshold ratio was 11 before treatment, and three after treatment).The fact that it took 1 or 2 weeks before improvements were seen was probably because of the limited treatment duration. Another way of quantifying the extent to which suppression is reduced, and as a consequence binocular combination is strength- ened, is to compare results where each eye receives stimuli of the same contrast (conditions where suppression is maximal). This is relevant to everyday viewing where the physical contrast of stimuli impinging on the retinae is of identical contrast. This corresponds to the condition where dichoptic motion thresholds (e.g., Fig. 2A, B) are most different (leftmost data in these figures, i.e., an intero- cular contrast ratio of unity) owing to the strong suppression that occurs from the fellow fixing eye to the amblyopic eye under these conditions. In Fig. 2C, we plot the dichoptic motion thresholds of each eye (from which the ratios were computed for Fig. 2A, B) as a function of the weeks of training. Our binocular training regime improves the dichoptic motion threshold of the amblyopic eye (i.e., when the amblyopic eye sees the signal and the fixing eye the noise) although having much less impact on the dichoptic motion thresholds of the fixing eye (i.e., when the fixing eye views the signal and the amblyopic eye the noise). Over time, the threshold of the amblyopic eye approaches that of the fixing eye (i.e., the ratio of the dichoptic thresholds is approaching unity in Fig. 2A, B). What this means is that, over time, suppression is being reduced and the two eyes of this strabismic amblyope are now successfully combining information of comparable contrasts between the two eyes. The improved binocular combination that results was re- flected in the establishment of stereoscopic function. Stereoscopic sensitivity was limited to 200 arc secs but was present for the first time (Fig. 2E). We were surprised to find that monocular acuity of the am- blyopic eye improved as a result of our antisuppression therapy. These results are shown in Fig. 2D where Snellen line letter acuity is plotted against the period of training. A significant improvement accompanies the reduction of suppression even in this adult amblyope. Case 2 Case 2 was a 45-year-old man who had a history of anisome- tropic amblyopia [R, Ϫ1.75 diopter (D)/ϩ0.50 ϫ 90°; L, ϩ1.25 D] which had been first detected at the age of 11 years and treated with a mixture of patching for 1 to 2 years and refractive correction at the age of 11 years. No surgery had been undertaken. He pre- sented with a 3-D anisometropia, a 6° constant esotropia (also detected at the age of 11 years), a mild degree of amblyopia (20/ 63), no fusion on the Worth 4 dot test, and no measurable stere- opsis at near. Using our global motion measurement of suppression, we found a degree of suppression that could be nullified when the contrast of the stimuli viewed by the fixing eye were reduced by a factor of 4 (Fig. 3A). Over the antisuppression treatment period of 5 weeks (4 days a week, average of 350 threshold measurements per week), there was a steady change in the degree of suppression exerted by the fixing eye. This is reflected in a change in the derived balance point (i.e., the interocular contrast ratio where the dichoptic threshold ratio between the fixing and amblyopic eyes is unity, x axis intercept in Fig. 3B) or by the progressive improvement in the dichoptic mo- tion threshold for the amblyopic eye for stimuli of equal contrasts in the two eyes (Fig. 3C). Monocular acuity improved in the am- blyopic eye from 20/63 to 20/30 (Fig. 3D), and stereopsis was established with an acuity of 20 arc secs (Fig. 3E). Case 3 Case 3 was a 33-year-old woman with a history of strabismic amblyopia (R, Ϫ1.00 D; L, Ϫ0.50 D) having been first detected at the age of 5 years. Two years of constant patching was undertaken, but there had been no surgical intervention (visual acuity at the end of this patching was not available). She presented with a small but bilaterally equivalent amount of myopia, a small angle (4°) con- stant esotropia with intermittent fusion on the Worth 4 dot (dis- tance and near) test but no measurable stereopsis (near). The acuity in the deviating eye was 20/80. The measurement of suppression using the balance point determination with the dichoptic motion stimulus showed a mild suppression by the fixing eye that could only be neutralized by reducing the contrast in the fellow fixing eye by a factor of 3 (Fig. 4A). During a period of 5-week training (3 days a week, average of 100 threshold measurements per week), using our antisuppression therapy, the degree of suppression gradually disappeared (indi- cated by a dichoptic ratio of 1 for equal interocular contrasts in Fig. 4B). Another reflection of this reduction in suppression is the improvement that occurred in her dichoptic motion thresholds for her amblyopic eye (signal to amblyopic eye, noise over the training period). Stereopsis (near) was established with an acuity of 30 arc secs (Fig. 4E), and monocular acuity in the amblyopic eye im- proved from 20/80 to 20/25 (Fig. 4D). DISCUSSION We describe a new quantitative method for the clinical measure- ment of suppression, something that is done in either a binary fashion (i.e., worth 4 dot test) or using methods that are coarse (i.e., the Sbisa Bar) or uncalibrated (i.e., reducing illumination for one eye on the synoptophore) in the clinic at present. The method is based on a signal to noise approach but applied within the context of dichoptic stimulation. This allowed us to demonstrate, for the first time, that threshold and suprathreshold information can be combined between the eyes of strabismic amblyopes under suit- able, albeit artificial, viewing conditions.5–7,9 Suppression is a well- known clinical entity, but it is rarely measured quantitatively and rarely used to direct the treatment approach. We believe this is unfortunate because the current animal3,4,10,11 and human5,6 research on amblyopia suggests that it is primarily a binocular problem with suppression being the key feature. We strongly rec- ommend that suppression is measured in a quantitative way along the lines suggested here. Furthermore, we show here, for three subjects, that intensive training using this suppression measurement approach leads to a progressive strengthening of binocular vision in strabismic am- blyopes such that they can eventually operate under natural view- ing conditions where the left and right image contrast is equal. We found this to be the case in 8/10 amblyopes tested so far, and it should be emphasized that all subjects were adult amblyopes well Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 701 Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 6. FIGURE 3. Results are shown for case 2 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training. 702 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 7. FIGURE 4. Results are shown for case 3 in terms of (A) the initial measurement of suppression (see text), (B) how this changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (C) how the dichoptic threshold for both eyes changed with prolonged artificial binocular viewing, (D) how visual acuity changed for each eye as a function of the training, and (E) the reinstating of stereopsis after training. Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. 703 Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010
  • 8. beyond the accepted “critical period” for patching therapy.1 Con- current with this improvement in the efficacy of binocular combi- nation, we also found that stereopsis in all three cases presented here, and in a majority of cases studied so far, was established and the monocular acuity also improved. These improvements were significant, stable, and in some cases large. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study is supported by a CIHR grant MOP 53346 (to RFH). Received February 23, 2010; accepted April 29, 2010. REFERENCES 1. Epelbaum M, Milleret C, Buisseret P, Dufier JL. The sensitive period for strabismic amblyopia in humans. Ophthalmology 1993;100: 323–7. 2. Scheiman MM, Hertle RW, Beck RW, Edwards AR, Birch E, Cotter SA, Crouch ER Jr, Cruz OA, Davitt BV, Donahue S, Holmes JM, Lyon DW, Repka MX, Sala NA, Silbert DI, Suh DW, Tamkins SM. Randomized trial of treatment of amblyopia in children aged 7 to 17 years. Arch Ophthalmol 2005;123:437–47. 3. Mower GD, Christen WG, Burchfiel JL, Duffy FH. Microionto- phoretic bicuculline restores binocular responses to visual cortical neurons in strabismic cats. Brain Res 1984;309:168–72. 4. Sengpiel F, Jirmann KU, Vorobyov V, Eysel UT. Strabismic suppres- sion is mediated by inhibitory interactions in the primary visual cor- tex. Cereb Cortex 2006;16:1750–8. 5. Baker DH, Meese TS, Mansouri B, Hess RF. Binocular summation of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthal- mol Vis Sci 2007;48:5332–8. 6. Mansouri B, Thompson B, Hess RF. Measurement of suprathreshold binocular interactions in amblyopia. Vision Res 2008;48:2775–84. 7. Thompson B, Mansouri B, Koski L, Hess RF. Brain plasticity in the adult: modulation of function in amblyopia with rTMS. Curr Biol 2008;18:1067–71. 8. Hess RF, Hutchinson CV, Ledgeway T, Mansouri B. Binocular in- fluences on global motion processing in the human visual system. Vision Res 2007;47:1682–92. 9. Baker DH, Meese TS, Hess RF. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vision Res 2008;48:1625–40. 10. Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. The neural basis of suppression and ambly- opia in strabismus. Eye (Lond) 1996;10(Pt 2):250–8. 11. Sengpiel F, Blakemore C, Kind PC, Harrad R. Interocular suppres- sion in the visual cortex of strabismic cats. J Neurosci 1994;14: 6855–71. Robert F. Hess 687 Pine Avenue West (H4-14) Montreal, Quebec, Canada PQ H3A 1A1 e-mail: robert.hess@mcgill.ca 704 Binocular Interactions in Amblyopia—Hess et al. Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 87, No. 9, September 2010