SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 95
Assignment 3: Annual Review and Create yourAssignment 3:
Annual Review and Create your
Dream Job (425 points)Dream Job (425 points) ››
Students, please view the "Submit a Clickable Rubric
Assignment" in the Course Info section.
Instructors, training on how to grade is within the Instructor
Center.
Assignment 3: Annual Review and Create Your Dream Job
Due Week 10 and worth 425 points
Imagine you work at a company and it is time for an employee
named Jim’s annual review. While he
was a model employee the first nine (9) months of the year,
recently Jim has been coming in late. It
has not been just a few minutes each day, either. It is starting to
cause problems in the production
line. In this assignment, write a one (1) page summary of your
conversation with Jim. How will you
address his recent performance issues while still praising him
for his previous nine (9) months of
good work? Your goal is to balance the negative and positive
feedback so that Jim will leave
motivated to do his best.
Write a one (1) page paper in which you:
1. Explain how you will address Jim’s recent performance
issues.
2. Suggest both constructive and positive feedback designed so
that Jim will leave motivated
to do his best.
3. Format your assignment according to the following
formatting requirements:
a. This course requires use of new Strayer Writing Standards
(SWS). The format is
different than other Strayer University courses. Please take a
moment to review the
SWS documentation for details.
b. Typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size
12), with one-inch margins
on all sides.
c. Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment,
your name, your
professor’s name, the course title, and the date. The cover page
is not included in the
required page length.
The specific course learning outcomes associated with this
assignment are:
Explain effective approaches to the broad spectrum of employee
relations, including career
development, fostering ethical behavior, discipline, labor
relations, and dismissals.
Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of
performance appraisal
programs.
Use technology and information resources to research issues in
human resource
management.
Write clearly and concisely about human resource management
using proper writing
mechanics.
In this assignment, you get the chance to create your dream job
and to build its compensation plan
and appraisal performance.
Write a six to eight (6-8) page paper in which you:
1. Create a job description and specifications for your dream
job.
2. Design a compensation and benefits package related to your
dream job.
https://blackboard.strayer.edu/webapps/assignment/uploadAssig
nment?content_id=_27010276_1&course_id=_239406_1&group
_id=&mode=view
https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/STANDA
RDIZED/StrayerWritingStandards/Strayer_Writing_Standards.p
df
3. Rationalize your compensation and benefits package. Be sure
to indicate the research and
considerations that went into the design of the compensation
and benefits package.
4. Imagine this is the only position of its kind in the
organization. From this perspective, design
a performance appraisal program to assess your job
performance.
5. Rationalize your performance appraisal program. Be sure to
indicate the research and
considerations that went into the design of the performance
appraisal program.
6. Use at least three (3) quality resources in this assignment.
Note: Wikipedia and similar
websites do not qualify as quality resources.
7. Format your assignment according to the following
formatting requirements:
a. This course requires use of new Strayer Writing Standards
(SWS). The format is
different than other Strayer University courses. Please take a
moment to review the
SWS documentation for details.
b. Typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size
12), with one-inch margins
on all sides.
c. Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment,
your name, your
professor’s name, the course title, and the date. The cover page
is not included in the
required page length.
d. Include a reference page. Citations and references must
follow SWS format. The
reference page is not included in the required page length.
The specific course learning outcomes associated with this
assignment are:
Discuss job analysis, job descriptions, and specifications.
Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of
employee compensation
programs.
Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of
performance appraisal
programs.
Use technology and information resources to research issues in
human resource
management.
Write clearly and concisely about human resource management
using proper writing
mechanics.
Click here to view the grading rubric for this assignment.
https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/STANDA
RDIZED/StrayerWritingStandards/Strayer_Writing_Standards.p
df
https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/BUS/310/
1192/Week%2010/Week%2010%20Assignment%203%20Rubric.
html
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC
ode=hedp20
Educational Psychologist
ISSN: 0046-1520 (Print) 1532-6985 (Online) Journal homepage:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hedp20
Acknowledging the Whiteness of Motivation
Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance
Ellen L. Usher
To cite this article: Ellen L. Usher (2018) Acknowledging the
Whiteness of Motivation
Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance, Educational
Psychologist, 53:2, 131-144, DOI:
10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220
To link to this article:
https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220
Published online: 10 Apr 2018.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 187
View Crossmark data
Citing articles: 1 View citing articles
Acknowledging the Whiteness of Motivation
Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance
Ellen L. Usher
Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, University of
Kentucky
This commentary revisits compelling arguments put forth by the
contributors to this special
issue on the role of race and ethnicity in academic motivation
research. First, an overview of
how race and motivation have been positioned in prominent
theories of motivation is
provided and juxtaposed with those offered in culturally
responsive pedagogical approaches
championed by multicultural educators. Special emphasis is
placed on how teachers’ and
learners’ sociohistorical and cultural contexts might influence
motivation. Second, the
critical role of identity and membership complexity is explored.
Third, suggestions for more
culturally attentive research methods are offered. The final
section includes research and
practice recommendations for supporting the academic
motivation of diverse learners.
Challenges to theoretical and methodological assumptions about
race, ethnicity, and culture
are offered throughout, including a call to address the role that
racism, power, and privilege
have played in perpetuating inequality in motivation research
conducted in the United States.
No one can be in the world, with the world, and with others
and maintain a posture of neutrality. I cannot be in the
world decontextualized, simply observing life. — Paulo
Freire (1998, p. 73)
“Line up at the back of the room by skin color,” came my
professor’s directive. It was the first day of a graduate
school seminar. My classmates looked around at each other
nervously, wondering if she was serious. “Go ahead,” the
professor repeated. “Stand up.” I was feeling uncomfort-
able. I enjoyed being an observer. But moments later, I was
baring my arms alongside fellow doctoral students as we
positioned ourselves against the back wall of the room on
the basis of our . . . melanin. “Okay, good. Notice how
you’re feeling and where you are in the line,” the professor
continued. (I was standing somewhere near the fairer half
of middle.) “Let’s talk about what this means.” Here, in one
of the rare moments in my life, I was forced to confront my
Whiteness. Thus began weeks of in-depth discussions about
the social construct of race and the unearned advantages
and disadvantages it has conferred to people in nearly all
spheres of life. The activity shook me out of a slumber—
one from which I am still awakening. What does Whiteness
mean to me personally and to the systems I am a part of?
In the United States and around the globe, the costs of
White supremacy are increasingly evident. Among children
and adolescents, these costs are often first felt in the places
where they spend most of their lives—school. Disparities in
educational achievement and opportunity between children
of different racial and ethnic groups have long been a fea-
ture of U.S. public schools (APA Task Force, 2012). Com-
pared to their White and Asian American counterparts,
African American, Native American, and Latinx students
score lower academically, are more likely to face disciplin-
ary sanctions, are assigned to lower academic tracks, and
drop out of school at higher rates. Differential educational
practices based on implicit and explicit stereotyping, preju-
dice, and discrimination perpetuate disparities. These
inequities come at high psychological, social, cultural, and
economic cost, not only to people of color but to all people
(Brown, 2017).
In this commentary, I argue that the contributions in this
special issue signify the ways in which Whiteness has
shaped research on academic motivation. I support this
argument by framing my remarks around three emergent
themes in this special issue. First, I address how race and
ethnicity have featured in prevailing theories of motivation
and emphasize the importance of contextualizing educa-
tional psychology research. Second, I consider the founda-
tional role of identity and membership in academic
motivation. Third, I describe how researchers might
sharpen their methodological approach (i.e., the who,
where, when, and how) in ways that reflect a more inclusive
Correspondence should be addressed to Ellen L. Usher,
Educational,
School, and Counseling Psychology, University of Kentucky,
249 Dickey
Hall, Lexington, KY 40506. E-mail: [email protected]
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 53(2), 131–144, 2018
Copyright � Division 15, American Psychological Association
ISSN: 0046-1520 print / 1532-6985 online
DOI: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220
understanding of motivational processes. I then discuss the
implications of these three themes for culturally responsive
education and research.
I fully recognize the elusive boundaries and vast scope
that the term “Whiteness” implies (see Andersen, 2003, for
a critique). I nevertheless have elected to use the term to
refer to the tendency of motivation researchers to prioritize
perspectives, participants, measures, and ways of knowing
that assume White as “normal” or default. I suggest that
acknowledging the Whiteness of our research is an impor-
tant step toward developing a more complete and just
understanding of human motivation.
THEORETICAL FRAMING
Searching for Universals in a Sea of Whiteness
The contributors to this special issue have all noted that the
bulk of pages published on human motivation and learning
has been written by White men (and more recently women)
who have based their theories of motivation on evidence
largely gathered from other White people of similar social
strata. Most motivation theories dominating educational
psychology journals today have been crafted and refined
under the assumption that human motivational processes
are either absolute (culturally invariant) or universal (grant-
ing that the degree to which certain motives are expressed
might differ according to environmental conditions).
Implicit in these approaches has been an assumption that
studying White people is an acceptable starting point for
the development of theories of motivation for use with
“other” populations.
This was evident at the 2017 American Educational
Research Association (AERA) meeting where a panel of
distinguished motivation theorists (all White; five men, one
woman) spoke to a packed audience about their theories’
contributions and prospects. After their overviews, Sandra
Graham (2017), the discussant and a woman of color,
offered the following observation:
In the very excellent summaries that all of these presenters
gave, never once was the word “race/ethnicity” mentioned
. . .. I’m not going to ask you whether you’ve been con-
cerned with diversity in your studies elaborating the theory,
but I do want to ask you how important you think this issue
is. Looking at your theory as a whole and the ethnic repre-
sentation of samples, do you think that most of the subjects
were White and middle class? And if the answer is “yes,”
“maybe,” or “I’m not sure,” how can you assure the young
scholars and the senior scholars in this audience . . . that
your theory has generality and relevance?
Her question was met with resounding applause. The
scholars’ responses largely reiterated their aim to discover
universal principles. Identifying universals, Bernie Weiner
(2017) explained, helps “still the waters to see a little
deeper and decrease the complexity.” He added emphati-
cally, “I’m a main effect person . . .. I don’t care who my
sample is, really, because I’m looking for similarities.”
Weiner’s views leaned toward the “top-down, theory
driven, etic-oriented approach” described by Zusho and
Clayton (2011) as “absolutist” (p. 248). From this perspec-
tive, motivational processes are viewed as culture free. The
theorists flanking Weiner were more universalistic in their
claims. Dale Schunk (2017) acknowledged that a social
cognitive theoretical perspective described general princi-
ples but its application in educational settings depends on
numerous sociocultural factors, such as ethnic/racial iden-
tity and immigration status. Ed Deci (2017) described
cross-cultural studies that demonstrated the universality of
fundamental needs.
Although the authors of this special issue suggest that a
universal theory of motivation that is culturally grounded
might one day be attainable, they point to the dearth of
research evidence reflecting the experiences and perspec-
tives of members of historically marginalized groups. They
applaud the use of situative perspectives, noticeably absent
from the AERA panel, in which motivation is viewed as
“arising through [individuals’] participation in social, cul-
tural, and historical contexts or systems” (Turner & Nolen,
2015, p. 168). Situative approaches recognize that the par-
ticular beliefs, values, and motives students develop are
necessarily “fraught with ambiguity [and] sensitive to the
occasion” (Bruner, 1996, p. 6). They acknowledge that the
prominence and weight given to any particular motive must
be understood in relation to systems of power, position, and
privilege (Nolen, Horn, & Ward, 2015).
The Whiteness of prominent theories of motivation has,
by default, framed motivational phenomena in terms of the
dominant group (i.e., White middle-class people composing
the global minority). No contributors make this point as
directly as Gray, Hope, and Matthews (this issue), who note
that publications in mainstream educational psychology
reflect White norms and rarely incorporate race-based theo-
retical or methodological approaches (and see DeCuir-
Gunby & Schutz, 2014, 2017). Several adverse side effects
of these theoretical and methodological oversights are
described in all four articles. First, scholars fail to consider
the role that being “othered” plays in the functioning of
oppressed and marginalized groups. Second, motivation is
divorced from the sociohistorical contexts in which learners
and teachers operate. Third, motivation theory and research
are assumed to be neutral and objective.
Framing the Problem: From Deficits and “Othering”
to Needs as Universal
Urdan and Bruchmann (this issue) provide an abbreviated
historical account of how researchers have framed a
132 USHER
cultural understanding of motivation. They note that some
scholars have taken a deficit approach, assuming that,
whether by nurture or nature, certain cultural groups have
different motives and may even lack an achievement
motive altogether. Some have explained ethnic minority
students’ motivation in terms of a defense against environ-
mental threats both within and outside of their ethnic group.
Even a framework like Fordham and Ogbu’s (1986) opposi-
tional stance defines the psychological experiences and
motives of the “underclass” in terms of a response to the
dominant group. Such an approach also reflects a deficit,
explaining the motivation of oppressed individuals as a
defense against those in power whose motives are assumed
to be purer. Frames like these discount the fact that a
healthy response sometimes involves defiance and that
“sickness might consist of not having symptoms when you
should” (Maslow, 1968, p. 7). From one perspective,
motives and behaviors are considered maladaptive; from
another, they can be viewed as a sign of health (e.g., the
rejection of an oppressive system).
Needs-based theoretical frames have suggested that
humans are motivated not only to overcome deficiencies
but also to become fully actualized. Needs are hierar-
chically structured such that lower order needs (e.g., safety,
belonging) must typically be fulfilled before higher order
(i.e., growth, self-actualization) needs are sought (Maslow,
1968). The self-determination framework posits that the
fulfillment of autonomy, relatedness, and competence needs
is central to autonomous motivation, which is based on a
universal set of identifiable “social and cultural nutrients”
(Ryan & Deci, 2017, p. 4). Dweck (2017) advanced a simi-
lar universal theory in which motivation is seen as a product
of basic and composite needs, which are subsumed by an
overarching need for self-coherence and integration. Needs
are the constituents of goal-driven behavior, shepherded by
beliefs, emotions, and tendencies that “can take very differ-
ent forms in different cultures” (p. 694).
I have provided only a snapshot of the theoretical frames
used to guide motivation research, focusing on those rele-
vant to the articles in this special issue. This brings me to
two critical observations. First, how a motivational problem
is framed will influence what researchers observe. Carol
Gilligan (1993), a pioneering psychologist who brought a
feminine perspective to human development research,
wrote that “theory can blind observation” (p. 25). In a field
historically dominated by White male American scholars, it
is unlikely that the theoretical mappings of human motiva-
tion fully reflect the experiences of all people.
Second, once they have a paradigmatic framework,
researchers are reluctant to modify it. To do so would
come at great cognitive and professional expense. As Kuhn
(1970) suggested, “to desert the paradigm is to cease prac-
ticing the science it defines” (p. 34). Researchers are predis-
posed to look to the familiar for an explanation of the new
(Dewey, 1933). They rarely abandon the frameworks in
which they were trained. Critically investigating our own
frames of reference is among our most challenging profes-
sional tasks. Kumar, Zusho, and Bondie (this issue) call on
the field to consider its own shortsightedness by exploring
other paradigms (e.g., critical race theory, culturally rele-
vant pedagogy) that have done a better job of including the
perspectives of marginalized groups.
Considering Contexts at Multiple Levels
The articles in this special issue note that, in theory and in
practice, motivation researchers have not sufficiently con-
sidered the political, social, and cultural landscapes in
which their studies have been conducted. Indeed, many
have attempted to position themselves as impartial observ-
ers of universal principles, assuming these to be best under-
stood in isolation from life’s noisiness. This does not hold
up to Freire’s assertion that no human is impartial or decon-
textualized. Researchers must more honestly account for
what a long line of educational psychologists have estab-
lished: Students’ cognition, emotions, motives, and social
relationships have taken root in particular sociohistorical
contexts.
Graham’s (this issue) corpus of rigorous research dem-
onstrates how context can be examined in a way that gener-
ates new theoretical insights and questions. Her work
considers how the local landscape—in particular, the
school’s ethnic makeup—might alter students’ perceptions
of themselves and others by affecting cross-ethnic friend-
ships, complex identity development, and perceived vulner-
ability. One key finding is that students’ cross-ethnic
friendships mirror lines of the U.S. racial power hierarchy
such that students with low power are less likely to be
befriended than are students of high power (i.e., White).
Graham’s research provides evidence that shifts in school
demographics can also be distressing to learners, particu-
larly when their ethnic group’s representation declines.
School context matters.
Nevertheless, much remains to be known about how
school diversity and ethnic composition are connected to
students’ social relationships and to academic motivation
and learning. The nature and strength of these relationships
depend on how ethnic composition and diversity are opera-
tionalized (see Brown, 2017; Rjosk, Richter, L€udtke, &
Eccles, 2017). Is there a tipping point at which representa-
tion of one’s group is motivationally advantageous? More-
over, is it motivationally consequential when a school’s
ethnic composition does not match the ethnic composition
of the particular classrooms within it? Graham and her col-
leagues have begun to investigate such effects. When stu-
dents attend “ethnically diverse” schools in which the
highest performing classes are full of students with the
lightest skin tones, a covert message of discrimination is
readily apparent. Such patterns reinforce some of the most
pervasive negative racial stereotypes—that students of
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 133
color are less academically capable. When the ethnic com-
position of their classes does not match that of their school,
all students rate the environment as more oppressive (Juvo-
nen, Kogachi, & Graham, 2017).
Graham’s (this issue) observation that, by 2040, White
students will be in the ethnic minority in U.S. public
schools is statistically correct but potentially misleading. In
different regions of the United States, students’ opportuni-
ties for contact with cultural outgroup members vary
widely. One of my undergraduate students who attended
public schools in the Appalachian region of eastern Ken-
tucky recently remarked that she had never been in class
with a non-White student until she came to college. The
demography of hometowns like hers is not rapidly chang-
ing. On the other hand, half of Kentucky’s African Ameri-
can students live in Louisville’s urban hub, where they
account for more than 80% of students enrolled at some
schools (Kentucky Department of Education, 2017). How
will students in these distinct settings develop racialized
identities, intergroup attitudes, and academic self-beliefs?
Geographic context matters too.
Gray et al. (this issue) highlight the role of the U.S. socio-
historical context in which Black learners have been consis-
tently denied membership and full participatory rights. They
refer to Carter G. Woodson’s (1933/1990) observation that
schools in the United States “fail to cultivate the agency,
strength, and skills of Black Americans, which may there-
fore be left underdeveloped” (p. 7). Schooling in the United
States is still “structurally White” in that it promotes and
sustains the cultural legacies, values, and behaviors of the
White owning class. In this regard, many schools and those
leading them often do fail to nourish students of color. This
threatens not only students’ sense of belonging but their
very sense of who they are. But Woodson’s claim also seems
to carry a problematic assumption that human agency,
strength, and intellect are at the mercy of the surrounding
context to cultivate. Stories of human triumph in adverse cir-
cumstances tell of other possibilities. As Holocaust survivor
Victor Frankl (1959) wrote, “Everything can be taken from
a [hu]man but one thing: the last of the human freedom—to
choose one’s attitude in any given set of circumstances, to
choose one’s own way” (p. 66). I am not promoting a
“bootstraps” approach or suggesting that oppressed individ-
uals are uniquely responsible for overcoming unjust cir-
cumstances. I simply note that context, although important,
is not destiny. A scientific study of motivation must account
for both the conditioned and agentic nature of human
experience.
A situative approach to the study of motivation acknowl-
edges that contextual features of the environment mean dif-
ferent things to different people (Nolen et al., 2015). As
young people come to understand themselves as part of a
larger social structure brought about by sociohistorical
position and status, their perception of choice likely makes
a motivational difference, as the contributing authors of
this special issue suggest. Consider the following contexts
in which choice may or may not be available: one’s ethnic
minority status (e.g., involuntary and voluntary; visible or
not), one’s source of income (e.g., parents’ employment
status, family wealth), one’s home (e.g., location, ameni-
ties, access to resources, safety), one’s schools (e.g., eco-
nomic/social capital to relocate for top schools), one’s
classes (e.g., advocating to be placed in an experienced
teacher’s class, in a gifted program, or withheld from lower
tracks), one’s learning (e.g., power and privilege to have
top teachers, hire private tutors), and even one’s very iden-
tity (e.g., assigned by others or self). Researchers often
compare groups of students using some or all of these cate-
gories but without a consideration of the underlying percep-
tions students have of their autonomous choice in such
matters (Have I/we been placed here or do I/we fully
endorse being here?).
Assessing perceptions of choice and status might be eas-
ier than assessing perceptions of discrimination, particu-
larly among younger children (Brown, 2017). What might
be the cumulative effect of not feeling autonomous or
agentic in one or more of these areas? I raise this question
because I endorse a view of human functioning that posi-
tions humans not as passive recipients of their environmen-
tal contexts but as agents who are capable of selecting and
creating environments for themselves (Bandura, 1986).
Even within the same imposed environment, two students
may select quite different social contexts for themselves.
This agentic capacity notwithstanding, young people often
come to view themselves as powerless in a larger social
structure that has robbed them and their families of posi-
tion, status, and more (Kumar et al., this issue).
By considering not only the contextual features of
learners’ environments but also learners’ perceptions of
them, researchers will be better positioned to advocate for
practices and policies that recognize the situated nature of
motivation and the sociohistorical barriers facing youth
who have been (and who still are) marginalized. One of the
hallmarks of a social cognitive theory of learning and moti-
vation is that our lives are both “partly fated and partly
free” (James, 1899/2001, p. 95). What learners attend to,
how they view the contexts in which they live, and whether
they see themselves as agents is therefore central to how
they develop and function.
Identity and Membership
Educational psychologists have accepted the centrality of
self and identity in the study of human motivation (see
Pajares & Schunk, 2006). Most studies on motivation fea-
ture some measure of self-perception as a precursor to
human action, whether involving one’s identity (Who am
I?), one’s perceived efficacy (Am I capable?), or one’s per-
sonal regard (Am I worthy?). The collective work of this
134 USHER
special issue points to the relatively less frequent consider-
ation of culture in self-systems as they relate to motivation.
From life’s beginning, humans are members of social
groups. Being a member of a group is an important part of
determining one’s place in the social world, developing a
sense of esteem in one’s self and in one’s group, and differ-
entiating one’s self from outgroup members (Bigler, Hayes,
& Patterson, 2017). How people come to see themselves
partly depends on the meaning they make of their social
memberships and how these memberships are viewed by
others in their cultural environment (Kitayama & Uskul,
2011). Cultural and social identities can be complex. Even
individuals who share phenotypic characteristics are poly-
cultural by virtue of their many cultural memberships, such
as their ancestry, socioeconomic class, linguistic heritage,
and family composition (Morris, Chiu, & Liu, 2015). Like-
wise, the manner in which one’s social identity activates
certain motivational sequences is contextually situated and
therefore subject to change (Hand & Gresalfi, 2015). A
young male high school student living in rural Pennsylvania
participates in glee club, plays football, is Latino, and nar-
rowly avoids academic probation. How might researchers
begin to operationalize the young man’s identity complex-
ity and its effect on his motivation? As Graham (this issue)
aptly observes, much depends on which social identity is
most salient to the student at a given moment, something
not well captured by universalistic approaches to under-
standing human motivation (or identity).
Psychologists have investigated the complexity of
identity from many angles: developmental processes
(e.g., crisis/resolution), status (e.g., achieved, foreclosed),
domain (e.g., sexual, academic, athletic, ethnic), and
evaluative components (e.g., centrality, salience, private/
public regard). For example, in a review of research on
Black racial identity, DeCuir-Gunby (2009) described the
various ways in which the subjective experiences of
Black students have been measured and how they differ-
entially relate to academic outcomes and well-being.
Some measures have been designed to capture all
learners’ ethnic identity, regardless of their ethnic heri-
tage (e.g., Phinney, 1992). Deciding how to assess
students’ subjective identity in studies of academic moti-
vation is not a straightforward matter and must depend
on the particular research aims. Urdan and Bruchmann
(this issue) suggest that researchers be mindful of the
dynamic properties of identity, such as salience and
valence. Gray et al. (this issue) similarly suggest that
researchers consider one’s sense of belonging within
one’s own cultural heritage group(s), which may be
similar to a sense of private regard (Sellers, Rowley,
Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, 1997). Together, the articles
in this issue recommend that academic motivation
researchers include subjective measures of racial, ethnic,
and cultural identity, which have increased in number
and breadth in the past two decades (e.g., Cokley, 2007;
Hughes, Del Toro, & Way, 2017; Oyserman, 2008;
Uma~na-Taylor et al., 2014; Verkuyten, 2016).
Only one article in this special issue (Urdan & Bruch-
mann, this issue) discusses the role of intersecting identities
as important associates of achievement motivation, despite
the fact that all decry the tendency by motivation research-
ers to treat race and ethnicity as monolithic. To conduct
research that considers the complexity of identity content
will require contextualized approaches that “defy tidy
developmental stories” (Galliher, McLean, & Syed, 2017,
p. 2012). Such approaches address intersectionality—the
ways in which an individual’s multiple social identities
jointly affect experience and psychological functioning
(Crenshaw, 1991). Cole (2009) offered several recommen-
dations for how researchers might consider intersectionality
as it pertains to psychological processes such as motivation.
For example, researchers should acknowledge that variation
exists between individuals who identify with a particular
group (e.g., there is no such thing as “the Latino experi-
ence”). One is not only female, one might also be Asian
American, socioeconomically disadvantaged, a first-gener-
ation immigrant, and so on. Researchers must also under-
stand the unearned advantages and disadvantages
associated with one’s membership in a particular group
given the group’s social and historical location. This
involves seeking to understand the affordances of a
learner’s intersecting identities, not only as reflected in the
social hierarchies of race and ethnicity but also in other
social categories (e.g., immigration status, ableness, sexual-
ity). Learners whose identities differ in certain respects
might find points of commonality in other respects.
How is identity complexity ultimately related to
motivation and learning? Research is mixed. On the one
hand, identity complexity can serve as a safeguard from
social/environmental threats such as discrimination and
bias. An individual with a complex social identity may
be better positioned to flexibly activate an appropriate
response or to summon necessary resources in a given
situation. But this depends on contextual factors.
Graham’s (this issue) research takes a person-in-context
approach, making evident the ways in which one’s eth-
nic identity interacts within particular racially diverse
contexts to activate motivational processes. For exam-
ple, attributional patterns following peer victimization
differ according to students’ ethnic representation at
school. Less able to attribute their suffering to external
causes, such as lack of power or status, students in the
ethnic majority internalize the blame by overidentifying
with it, whereas ethnic minority students who are vic-
timized (particularly when also in the numeric minority
at school) have many possible external explanations for
their unfortunate situation.
These findings notwithstanding, plenty of evidence has
pointed to the stress of having to navigate multiple iden-
tities, especially when one or more have been viewed as
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 135
“less than” or “other” (Gray et al., this issue), or when
one’s identities are in conflict with one another (Kumar
et al., this issue). Compartmentalization of certain identi-
ties might allow young people to avoid cognitive or
social conflict, but often this comes at a high psychologi-
cal cost (Boykin, 1986; Galliher et al., 2017; McKown &
Strambler, 2009). Du Bois (1903/1994) forewarned that
the history of African Americans would be described in
terms of such a tension in the Self: “this longing to attain
self-conscious manhood, to merge his double self into a
better and truer self” (p. 2). Maslow (1968) similarly
remarked that the average human regularly fights a “civil
war within . . . between the forces of the inner depths and
the forces of defense and control” needed to manage exter-
nal pressures (p. 156). Being a member of a negatively ste-
reotyped group likely intensifies the battle. One might ask,
then, what are the “inner depths” that bring about the “truer
self”? How might they be assessed? What are their motiva-
tional implications?
The development of an integrated sense of self and of
one’s place in society is central to psychological well-
being (Dweck, 2017). Basic threats to one’s group mem-
berships include feeling discriminated against, perceiv-
ing bias from others, sensing the ignorance of others to
one’s lived experiences, receiving overt and covert mes-
sages of “otherness,” and being treated unfairly. These
factors often do occur explicitly. But the perception that
they might exist despite others’ overt action is equally
strong from a psychological perspective. Surrounded by
threats, both real and perceived, a person might natu-
rally employ self-protective strategies (e.g., withdrawal/
avoidance, self-aggrandizement) or might succumb in
less desirable ways (e.g., depression, anxiety). The con-
sequences of chronic uncertainty about whether one is
fully accepted by one’s social environment or not can
have a lifetime of repercussions—academic, social,
emotional, physical, and financial (Bigler et al., 2017;
Brown, 2017). And, as the articles in this issue boldly
note, institutional racism and White supremacy have
made it much more difficult for students of color to feel
like they belong in U.S. schools.
One potential solution is to help learners activate an
“individuated identity” whereby they describe their
affinities in their own terms (Urdan & Bruchmann, this
issue). Core values interventions, in which student
affirm their personal values through a brief writing exer-
cise, have been shown to reduce racial achievement dis-
parities (G. L. Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006).
Self-affirmations affect motivation by triggering cogni-
tive processes that mitigate environmental threats and
promote a sense of empowerment (G. L. Cohen, Garcia,
& Goyer, 2017). Exploring the intricacies of how indi-
viduals view themselves (e.g., integrational vs. disinte-
grational identity) and how structures in schools and in
the broader environment promote (and thwart) positive
self-development is a vital line of ongoing inquiry in
motivation research.
METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS
Nearly three decades ago, Bruner (1990) denounced a
tendency by psychologists to exalt “neat little studies”
over approaches that would answer more complex ques-
tions about the nature of the mind and how it is shaped
by culture. Too many psychologists, he argued, were
engaged in “methodolatry” while ignoring more interest-
ing psychological questions (p. xi). Not much has
changed. The cleanly executed, sophisticated models
that populate the pages of mainstream educational psy-
chology journals will not result in generalizable princi-
ples about motivation “because contexts, attitudes, and
meanings contribute systematic variance that must be
included to reveal lawful ecological contrasts” (Ceci,
Kahan, & Braman, 2010, p. 28). The methods that
researchers have selected to examine motivation there-
fore contribute to incomplete theoretical knowledge that
does not reflect the breadth of human experience (Urdan
& Bruchmann, this issue). In this section, I consider
how suggestions from the articles in this special issue
inform the ways in which motivation research has been
and can be conducted. Following a typical methodology,
I consider the implications of this body of work on the
who, where, when, and how of our research.
The Who
Graham’s (1992) historical analysis revealed that White
and middle-class subjects dominated the pages of psychol-
ogy journals in the 1970s and 1980s. Henrich, Heine, and
Norenzayan (2010) described a continuing trend, noting
that most research subjects in psychological science were
WEIRD (i.e., Western [and White], educated, industrial-
ized, rich, and democratic) despite the fact that most
humans do not fit this description. The educational psychol-
ogists whose words are published in this and similar jour-
nals are also predominantly WEIRD, so it seems
understandable that they would seek the convenience of
recruiting like-minded participants and the familiarity of
seeing research like theirs make it through the publication
gate.
In light of Freire’s (1998) reminder that no one can
observe the world and remain neutral, motivation research
must continue to expand its who, in terms of not only its
participants but also its investigators. When Graham (2017)
launched her pointed question to the AERA panel about
how particular motivation theories had addressed race, eth-
nicity, and culture, Urdan (2017) replied, “I would probably
ask different questions if I didn’t bring the frame that I
bring as a White man.” The field of educational
136 USHER
psychology, he added, should “diversify the people who are
asking the questions and interpreting the research.” Indeed.
What are the consequences of this limited who both in
terms of the observers and the observed? I offer are a few.
1. Motivation theories and constructs are normed on
Eurocentric samples and contexts.
2. Researchers therefore contribute to the marginaliza-
tion of others who do not follow predicted, “default”
patterns.
3. The implications for practice that researchers offer do
not generalize to most individuals and sociocultural
locations.
4. Researchers’ own biases and privileges guide what
they see and, in turn, the story they tell.
Welcoming diverse perspectives—indeed, seeking and
privileging them—is therefore paramount to inclusivity in
motivation research and theory.
The Where
As all contributions to this issue have emphasized, a
culturalized view of motivation requires close attention
to contextual factors that cue and activate different cog-
nitive and motivational sequences. The four articles in
this special issue take a U.S.-specific sociohistorical per-
spective, but motivation research has fortunately become
increasingly global and cross-national as vibrant hubs of
research have developed on several continents, casting
new light on motivational processes in non-U.S. cultural
contexts (see McInerney & Liem, 2018). Researchers
should be sure to describe the broader contexts in which
their research takes place. Ecological, semantic, geo-
graphic, and other contexts influence how motivational
processes are selected and engaged (Ceci et al., 2010).
In their day-to-day lives, many learners must navigate
diverse circumstances that present competing goals, val-
ues, and demands. As Kumar et al. (this issue) and
others note, dissonance between the cultural values of
home and school has been associated with lower aca-
demic motivation (e.g., Tyler, Burris, & Coleman,
2016). How is achievement motivation affected by dis-
cordant messages across other contexts such as those
encountered online, in extracurricular activities, and in
the community? Are dissonant wheres felt more acutely
by learners from particular racial or cultural identities?
Withstanding environmental dissonance is not always
easy or advantageous, but for some it might be accom-
panied by opportunities for growth. As Kumar et al.
advise, researchers should continue to target the causes
and effects of dissonance on motivation in complex
learning contexts and to examine the ways in which cul-
turally relevant and responsive pedagogies might allevi-
ate some of this dissonance.
The When
An ecologically valid approach to understanding motiva-
tion considers the historical, political, and sociocultural
contexts that shape individuals’ beliefs, positionality,
and interpretations in the world. Motivation is situated
and dynamic; researchers must therefore recognize the
when of their research (Nolen et al., 2015). This opposes
any goal of reaching timeless generalizations about what
motivates people. As Cronbach (1975) observed,
“generalizations decay” (p. 122). How can the field and
its methodological approaches be more sensitive to
changes over time? For example, with respect to stages
of human development, most motivational processes
have been studied in mid- to late adolescence. Consider-
ably less work has examined the motivational impacts
of culture, race, and ethnicity during childhood, when
identity development becomes more complex and aware-
ness of discrimination and inequity first emerges
(Brown, 2017). A sociopolitical consideration of time
might also be useful. In the United States, for instance,
investigators might ask how students from different eth-
nic groups have perceived their sense of belonging since
the presidential election of Donald Trump or in light of
certain immigration policy decisions. The emergence
of more effective and differentiated real-time measures
of motivation (e.g., biometric data, computer tracing)
will also enable new ways of investigating the relative
stability of motivation constructs in individualized ways
(Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue).
The How
Perhaps the most important point raised by Urdan and
Bruchmann (this issue) is that researchers’ own theories
guide how they have examined the relationships between
culture, race, ethnicity, and student motivation. Their meth-
ods—the how of research—in turn reinforce researchers’
assumptions about the motivation of diverse groups.
Sophisticated statistical models and large samples are
assumed to enable scholars to reach generalizations that
become guiding theoretical principles. However, the
“conceptual economy” afforded by oft-used quantitative
designs that consider a handful of White-normed constructs
is that “the flavor of the particular situation, individual,
event, or object is lost” (Eisner, 1998, p. 38). A more
nuanced understanding of motivation emerges from first-
person accounts of motivation, beliefs, or goals obtained by
subjective reports. Still, I wonder how much room is avail-
able for qualitative inquiry in motivation research. A glance
at our journals shows that few pages are reserved for small
total sample size, inductive models. Such studies are often
deemed nonscientific, less rigorous, and lacking in external
validity. I agree with Urdan and Bruchmann’s central mes-
sage: Diverse methodological approaches are needed, any
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 137
one of which must be viewed as offering a partial
explanation.
Regardless of their methodological approach, research-
ers must be thoughtful when selecting the tools they use to
carry out their work. Motivation constructs have compli-
cated measurement histories. Gray et al. (this issue) note
that learners’ sense of belonging has been assessed in at
least 18 ways, leading researchers to draw different conclu-
sions about how belonging and its various features (e.g.,
affect, fairness, safety, perceived importance) are related to
motivation. Like motives, identities are complex and multi-
dimensional. As researchers seek to include measures that
more sensitively assess race-, culture-, and ethnicity-based
constructs (e.g., identity, socialization, opportunity), they
will need to consider how to do so effectively in diverse
learning environments. Measures tailored to the particular
experiences of one cultural group (e.g., Black identity) are
not likely appropriate for other groups. Items that refer gen-
erally to “your culture” might be problematic given the
breadth of students’ diverse, and intersecting, cultural
memberships (A. B. Cohen, 2009).
One danger of using students’ group membership in
motivation research is that it treats each group (e.g., nation-
ality, ethnicity, race) as a static entity—an error-free indica-
tor of a set of values, beliefs, institutions, and behaviors that
can “distinguish one group from another” (Banks, 1997, p.
124). Kumar et al. (this issue) suggest that the empirical
landscape of academic motivation research would be
enhanced by studies that promote culture to a prominent
role and motives to a secondary one. Such an approach
acknowledges that a learner’s culture is not just “out there”
in the distal sense; it is also “in here” in the personal sense,
both by explicit adoption of sociocultural norms and by
internalization of beliefs, norms, and behaviors that have
been tacitly transmitted. Graham’s (this issue) work pro-
vides an excellent example of methods that assess this dis-
tinction. Rather than assuming that overt group
membership or participation in certain activities means that
a student identifies with them, Graham and her colleagues
asked adolescents to describe the group memberships that
are important to them as well as the extent to which partici-
pating in these groups is something that they think ethnic
ingroup and outgroup members would do. This provides a
more personalized starting place from which to investigate
other questions.
Just as group membership alone is insufficient for cap-
turing students’ subjective perceptions of their relationship
to their social context, use of the classroom as a unit of
analysis might overlook the meaning students make of their
learning environments. Students who get along well with
their teachers and perceive them to be fair likely reap a
motivation benefit, whereas those who feel marginalized or
discriminated against by their teachers or peers likely suffer
motivationally. One way that researchers can better assess
the effect of teachers and pedagogical practices on student
motivation is by considering students’ perceptions of their
teachers’ pedagogical approach (e.g., asset based, deficit
based). This could be supplemented by assessing learners’
perceptions of their teachers’ beliefs and expectations (e.g.,
“My teacher believes that every student in our class can
learn.” “Teachers at my school value students of different
cultural backgrounds.”). Examining the association
between what students and teachers believe to be the func-
tion of schooling and education (e.g., to overcome deficits,
to view one another as resources) might also reveal aspects
of the classroom climate that give rise to different motiva-
tional consequences.
Researchers might also consider group beliefs as the unit
of analysis. Gray et al. (this issue) note the historical impor-
tance of collective agency among African-heritage commu-
nity members, which includes the cultivation of cultural
knowledge. As students learn about their own cultural heri-
tage, how does their collective identity or sense of agency
change? How does this affect them individually? The 2008
Obama campaign slogan, “Yes We Can,” reflected a grow-
ing sense of social and political empowerment among Afri-
can Americans as well as members of other cultural groups.
How might participation in sociopolitical movements (e.g.,
Black Lives Matter) change Black students’ collective and
individual beliefs related to self, other, and academics (see
Hargons et al., 2017)? Researchers might investigate the
motivational function of culturalized collective beliefs.
The educational psychology community has lagged
behind its social psychology counterparts in using measures
that assess implicit thoughts and feelings, their antecedents
(e.g., social primes), and their motivating power. The ways
in which implicit or explicit forces jointly guide human
behavior remain unclear (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue).
For example, Devos, Huynh, and Banaji (2012) suggested
that discrepancies between one’s implicit and explicit self-
image can motivate actions that defend or protect the ego.
They showed that individuals with high explicit self-esteem
but low implicit self-esteem are more likely to become
defensive and to engage self-enhancing strategies. Other
research has demonstrated the ways in which one’s social
environment (real or imagined) can prime certain self-pre-
sentational styles, which in turn can influence internal self-
beliefs and motivation. “An audience does not even have to
be present to shape how people think about and present
themselves” (Schlenker, 2012, p. 554). Urdan and Bruch-
mann suggest that implicit measures of motivation might
be more predictive of behavior than explicit measures, but
this will require further evidence. Educational psycholo-
gists might extend and replicate social psychological
research by taking laboratory experiments to the more natu-
ralistic setting of the classroom.
Excellent suggestions for the how of culturally-sensitive
research that expand on many of the points raised in this
special issue are provided by Awad, Patall, Rackley, and
Reilly (2016). As they and these contributors have
138 USHER
suggested, measures of motivation must be meaningful and
valid for the populations of interest. Researchers must use
care when using measures validated with certain groups
(most likely American, White, middle-class, native-English
speakers) with other groups, and they must not assume that
all respondents make similar meaning of items normed on
WEIRD samples (Miller, Goyal, & Wice, 2017).
IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE
The articles in this special issue raise many implications for
research and practice. I have selected several of them to dis-
cuss in this section.
A Critical Approach: Confronting White Supremacy and
Colorblindness in Research
Critical race theory challenges the notion that existing theo-
ries of academic motivation were developed with all learn-
ers in mind. The articles in this issue also highlight the
ways in which school-based practices are not culturally
neutral. In many U.S. public schools, classroom practices,
rules, curricula, and ways of knowing are often laden with
the values of the privileged and powerful whose aims,
whether conscious or unconscious, are to maintain the cur-
rent power structure (hooks, 2010). Particularly in moments
of perceived or actual scarcity of resources, those holding
power reassert their status and privilege in overt and covert
ways, through words, gestures, practices, curricula, poli-
cies, and, yes, even theories (Kumar et al., this issue; Urdan
& Bruchmann, this issue). Schools and the broader society
provide routine reminders to certain students of their status
as the unwelcome “other.” These forms of discrimination
can lead students to experience unquantifiable race- and
class-based stress (Berliner, 2013). Gray et al. (this issue)
rightly assert that educational research that is not properly
situated sociohistorically can give way to color-blind rec-
ommendations that do not sufficiently address systemic
structural inequities.
Unfortunately, many motivation researchers take such a
color-blind approach. For example, popular motivation
constructs such as “growth mindset” and “grit” have been
decried as culturally blind antidotes to race-based achieve-
ment gaps that promote victim blaming and perpetuate the
myth of meritocracy (e.g., Gorski, 2016). Former Boston
high school principal Linda Nathan (2017) dispelled the
myth that “grit” is the beacon of hope for students from
marginalized identities. Through the compelling stories of
her own students, Nathan explained that to view student
motivation as the great equalizer is to “ignore the invisible
threads of inequity that are far less likely to tighten around
the necks of our dominant-culture students” (p. 105). Many
motivation scholars reading this will agree. But Nathan’s
need to explain this is evidence that we researchers have
failed to offer a sufficiently critical message that reflects
the complex micro- and macrocontexts in which children
live and learn (Kumar et al., this issue).
Gray et al. (this issue) offer several helpful recommen-
dations for researchers interested in questioning “the acul-
tural assumptions” of our guiding theoretical frameworks.
For instance, scholars can spend time in the settings that
they are investigating. Grounded observations might help
researchers more carefully consider how students from his-
torically marginalized groups experience the school day,
and how they are perceived by school personnel, fellow stu-
dents, and themselves. In turn, scholars can provide a more
thoughtful description of the context in which their studies
take place (Awad et al., 2016). Researchers can also con-
sider, and explicitly acknowledge, their own positionality
in their research. Editors might prioritize the publication of
studies that consider the cultural, racial, and economic cli-
mates in which motivation research has been conducted.
Culturally Responsive and Relevant Educational
Practice
The Culturally Responsive and Relevant Educational Prac-
tice (CRRE) framework described by Kumar et al. (this
issue) posits that meaningful learning occurs when (a) the
content to be learned reflects students’ cultural diversity,
(b) the process of learning aligns with individual and cul-
tural modes of learning, and (c) the school’s sociocultural
context is in harmony with the home’s sociocultural con-
text. From one perspective, CRRE seems to advance a set
of pedagogical recommendations that are based on the prin-
ciples of cognitive science and information-processing the-
ory (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). Connecting to one’s
culture in meaningful ways is a powerful way to enrich cog-
nitive processing, learning, attention, and underlying meta-
cognitive processes (e.g., attention, self-regulation, interest,
competence, engagement), which subsequently enhance
motivation (Mayer, 2012). This is likely why Ladson-Bill-
ings (1995), foreshadowing the sentiments expressed in this
special issue, noted that culturally responsive teaching is
“just good teaching!” (p. 159).
From another perspective, however, the strong claims
made by CRRE scholars are not fully supported by psycho-
logical research. For example, some claim that educational
practices in the United States fail to provide a meaningful
learning experience for students of color. Although this is
true in one respect, it bears noting that critical race theory
would not exist if students who were forced to sit through
years of “imperialist, White supremacist, patriarchal” mes-
saging were incapable of seeing meaning in and beyond it
(hooks, 2016). From a psychological perspective, freedom
confers the possibility of becoming conscious of how we
have been educated, thereby marking “the essential differ-
ence between conditioned and determined existence”
(Freire, 1998, p. 54). Insights from both cognitive science
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 139
and critical education acknowledge humans as meaning
makers, capable of detecting discrepancies in their environ-
ment. These distinctly human capacities enable learners to
exercise agency in their own lives and to challenge the sta-
tus quo (Bandura, 1986). This fact does not dismiss the
important realization brought to light by CRRE scholars
that the American educational system, on the whole, vali-
dates and reinforces Eurocentric viewpoints as “correct,”
thereby marginalizing the viewpoints of members of the
global majority (i.e., people of color). By juxtaposing
CRRE with motivation research, Kumar et al. (this issue)
effectively dispel the myth of cultural and racial neutrality
and show how schools and curricula perpetuate inequality
and undermine motivation.
What, then, can teachers and schools do to redirect (his-
torically Eurocentric and racist) educational policies so that
they are inclusive of all learners? Several contributors to
this special issue contend that, to be maximally motivating,
pedagogical approaches should be culturally reflective, cul-
turally aligned, and harmonious between home and school.
They describe a handful of research studies demonstrating
how authentic attempts by educators to legitimize those
who have been historically “othered” have improved
students’ achievement, engagement, and motivation. This
is certainly a first step. But the preceding recommendations
raise many questions. For instance, how and to what extent
shall the content to be learned reflect each student’s cultural
heritage? How culturally aligned must the learning pro-
cesses be (and with whose culture)? What indication will
teachers have that sufficient home-school harmony is
reached?
Several contributors describe “culturally valued”
instructional approaches (e.g., hip-hop) that teachers have
used to connect with their students. But, as Urdan and
Bruchmann (this issue) point out, well-intended approaches
for enhancing culturally relevance can, when poorly
applied, promote racial and ethnic stereotypes or come off
as insensitive or offensive. Such approaches might also be
dismissive of within-group differences. The ultimate effec-
tiveness of any approach in promoting learning and motiva-
tion depends on numerous factors, such as the school
context, each learner’s demeanor, the with-it-ness and art-
istry of the teacher, the quality of the student–teacher rela-
tionship, and the intersecting identities of all involved
(DiAngelo & Sensoy, 2010). Each of these can be interest-
ing points of inquiry in culturally relevant teaching and its
impact on student motivation.
Another way that school environments can be culturally
responsive is by embracing difference while creating inclu-
sivity. Such learning environments communicate a sense
that “you are at home here and free to express yourself as
unique.” Gray et al. (this issue) refer to this as creating
opportunities for cultural distinctiveness. In this way,
belonging is nurtured by schools and teachers who help
learners feel connected to and affirmed in their cultural
heritage and yet distinct as individuals. Education philoso-
pher Maxine Greene (1993) observed that “the more contin-
uous and authentic personal encounters can be, the less
likely it will be for categorizing and distancing to take
place” (p. 13). Gray et al. (this issue) and Kumar et al. (this
issue) propose several ways in which this can happen,
including cultural representation in curricula and cultural
diversity among school personnel. They suggest that stu-
dents feel more empowered when they perceive their teach-
ers as critical, reflective thinkers who are willing to invite
alternative viewpoints in their classrooms. These opportuni-
ties foster legitimacy, validation, esteem, and compatibility,
all of which can increase achievement motivation.
Another way in which schools can legitimize students as
citizens is by providing opportunities for authentic engage-
ment within the broader community (Gray et al., this issue).
The link between students’ participatory civic engagement
and academic motivation suggests that authentic engage-
ment can be viewed as an “emancipatory” practice by
enhancing students’ sense of efficacy, social connectedness,
and altruism. Investigating the mechanisms by which
students’ motivation is changed by civic engagement (and
vice versa) provides an interesting path forward.
Clarifying the Role of Cultural Competence
Kumar et al. (this issue) contend that cultivating teachers’
and students’ cultural competence can enhance academic
motivation and competence. They suggest that culturally
competent teachers will be familiar with the history, cus-
toms, and values of the cultural groups represented in their
school and show awareness of how culture informs their
own and others’ thoughts, emotions, and behaviors. The
antidote to cultural incompetence, they argue, is critical
self-reflection, which involves questioning one’s own val-
ues, assumptions, and ways of understanding the world.
These are laudable pursuits that raise further questions.
First, how do learners, educators, researchers, and policy-
makers engage in honest, critical self-reflection? Can they
do so alone? Second, who decides when self-reflection has
been sufficiently critical? What are its consequences? For
instance, critical self-reflection might engender a level of
paralyzing shame or guilt. It can also provoke fear and
anger that deepen racial divides. Is it enough, for example,
for a White woman to understand her power and privilege
as she enters her classroom? This seems to fulfill the
requirements of cultural competency that the authors put
forth, but it may still be insufficient. As Sue (2017) argued,
“The transformation to developing cultural competence and
becoming a White ally is more than a cognitive or intellec-
tual exercise. Lived experience and lived reality are crucial
elements” (p. 712).
Some researchers have begun to investigate the types of
lived experiences that foster cultural awareness and under-
standing. Teacher education programs can provide
140 USHER
opportunities (e.g., field experiences) for teachers to inter-
act with individuals’ different from them and to reflect on
their own cultural frames of reference. Indeed, researchers
have begun to investigate the relationship between
teachers’ experiences with culturally diverse students,
exposure to multicultural education curricula, instructional
practices, and motivational approaches (Kumar & Lauer-
mann, 2018).
As the collective voices in this special issue well note,
neither culture nor cultural competence is static. As one or
both change, new frictions, joys, challenges, and opportuni-
ties emerge. One must search again to rediscover self and
other in new contexts. This means that cultural competence
can neither be “achieved” nor “maintained” as some have
claimed. Because culture is inherently social, the adequacy
of one’s cultural knowledge and practice depends on the
particulars of one’s social environment.
The Motivational Impact of Being Seen
Learners who are members of historically marginalized
groups often describe the experience of being invisible,
silenced, and even erased. The teacher–student relation-
ship is a daily occasion for the affirmation (or denial) of
one’s personhood. The harmful consequences of being
overlooked or outcast have been well documented by
educational and developmental psychologists (Brown,
2017). What, then, might be the corollaries of being
seen? The Zulu greeting sawubona, literally meaning,
“I/We see you,” implies much more than these three
words suggest. It offers an acknowledgment from one
ancestral viewpoint to another, establishing the observer
as witness not only to the other’s presence but simulta-
neously to one’s own (Bishop, n.d.). This serves as an
invitation and agreement to reciprocal participation in
the life of another person. The response, ngikhona,
acknowledges, “I am here,” or more precisely, “Until
you saw me, I didn’t exist.” Being fully seen by another
serves as a reflection of one’s own inner capacities and
as a recognition of one’s connection to another, even
another whose plight is unknown or different. It recog-
nizes that one’s well-being is connected to another’s. In
this way, to see others fully is the cornerstone of a
socially just pedagogy.
This idea offers a ready connection between one of the
foundational assumptions of CRRE (e.g., teacher as warm
demander) and need-based motivation frameworks (e.g.,
need for relatedness). Noddings’s (2013) notion of critical
care describes just this type of interconnection.
I do not need to establish a deep, lasting, time-consuming
personal relationship with every student. What I must do is
to be totally and nonselectively present to the student—to
each student—as he addresses me. The time interval may
be brief but the encounter is total. (p. 180)
Being seen involves a brief but total encounter.
Warmth is conveyed by the teacher’s willingness to see
the student entirely and nonjudgmentally and to attribute
to the student the best intent. CRRE has emphasized the
role of teacher care in helping all learners succeed
(Kumar et al., this issue), but I am not convinced that
commonly used measures of care adequately capture
this deeper sense of respectful connection. I urge
researchers to consider ways to assess students’ percep-
tions of being seen in this fuller sense and then to
examine its connection to motivation and well-being.
Coming Full Circle
In his treatise on culturalizing educational psychology,
Pajares (2007) described the tension many social scientists
experience when describing particulars at the expense of
advancing universals, and vice versa. Savvy educational
psychologists have learned to pad their theoretical frame-
works and interpreted findings with contextual caveats. But
as writer Wendell Berry (2000) lamented, even this recon-
ciliation is incomplete:
However generous the acknowledgment of context, the
results of the research still cannot be applied both generally
and sensitively. Finally it is “brought home” to a specific
community of persons and creatures in a specific place. If it
is then applied in its abstract or generalized or marketable
form, it will obscure the uniqueness of the subject persons
or creatures or places, or of their community, and this sort
of application is almost invariably destructive. (p. 147)
As we must situate and contextualize our research
endeavors, we must also admit that we ourselves are
socially, historically, and contextually situated. Keeping
this in mind as we write for others, we can show proper dis-
cernment in the application of theoretical knowledge to
human lives and systems (Jung, 1945/1998).
In one of his least cited but most insightful talks, “On a
Certain Blindness in Human Beings,” William James
(1899/2001) observed that “neither the whole of truth nor
the whole of good is revealed to any single observer,
although each observer gains a partial superiority of insight
from the peculiar position in which he stands” (p. 129). All
too often, and even within the pages of this special issue,
we assume that our findings or our experiences reflect real-
ity, neglecting to acknowledge the limits of our particular
vantage point (Oyserman, 2017). We have gathered the
data, invested our time, controlled for certain factors, and
dutifully responded to reviewers. At each tick of the clock,
it is easier to become entrenched in our belief that our grasp
on reality is complete. However, we would do better to
acknowledge our ignorance and to ask, “How [can we] act
well—sensitively, compassionately, without irreparable
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 141
damage—on the basis of partial knowledge?” (Berry, 2000,
p. 147).
As a field, we could stand to be humbler and more
nuanced. For White researchers, this means recognizing the
unearned privileges that allow us to remain blind to the
experiences of non-White individuals with little personal or
professional consequence. Acknowledging the Whiteness
of our approaches need not lead to paralysis, however.
Even “incremental steps taken in confrontation of White-
ness will help uncover deeper aspects of the problem and
suggest further appropriate responses” (powell, 2012,
p. 84). Thus is our challenge as educational psychologists.
CODA
During my years in graduate school, I frequently walked
by the office of Professor Jackie Irvine, the same profes-
sor who encouraged me to confront my Whiteness on
the first day of her graduate seminar. On her door was
plastered a lone purple sticker bearing the large-font
words of Audre Lorde (1984): “Your silence will not
protect you” (p. 41). Lorde’s words and Irvine’s instruc-
tion serve as reminders that to engage in our work as if
blind to race and color is to prolong racial injustice.
Instead, the call is to engage in antiracism by striving
“to change the norms and practices that allow racism to
exist” in schools and in our research (Bonilla-Silva,
2018, p. 243). I would like to commend the authors of
this special issue for their willingness to address the
ways in which racism and White supremacy have
affected research on academic motivation, particularly
in the United States. Such courage, accompanied by a
“sense of moral outrage and urgency on behalf of all
children” (Irvine, 1999, p. 252), will be necessary for
researchers and practitioners as they, in their unique
ways, seek to help all learners thrive.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would not have been able to write this commentary
without the education I received from faculty and col-
leagues from the Division of Educational Studies at
Emory University who broadened and challenged my
perspectives on sociohistorical and psychological sys-
tems of power and privilege. I am grateful to Professors
Jackie Irvine, Vanessa Siddle Walker, Carole Hahn, and
Frank Pajares. I thank my current colleagues, Kenneth
Tyler and Christia Brown, for their valuable insights on
this work. I am fortunate to work with a stellar team of
undergraduate and graduate students who, on a daily
basis, help me think about how to conduct motivation
research in ways more relevant to learners from histori-
cally marginalized groups. Special thanks to Isaac
Hayes, Abigail Love, John Eric Lingat, and Laura Page,
for their comments. Finally, to my mom, Dr. Jody
Usher, for her unwavering commitment to helping me
(and many others) confront fear and use my voice to
promote justice.
ORCID
Ellen L. Usher http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9301-8987
REFERENCES
American Psychological Association, Presidential Task Force
on Educa-
tional Disparities. (2012). Ethnic and racial disparities in
education:
Psychology’s contributions to understanding and reducing
disparities.
Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ed/resources/racial-
disparities.aspx
Andersen, M. L. (2003). Whitewashing race: A critical
perspective on
whiteness. In A. W. Doane & E. Bonilla-Silva (Eds.), White
out: The
continuing significance of racism (pp. 21–35). New York, NY:
Routledge.
Awad, G. H., Patall, E. A., Rackley, K. R., & Reilly, E. D.
(2016). Recom-
mendations for culturally sensitive research methods. Journal of
Educa-
tional and Psychological Consultation, 26, 283–303.
doi:10.1080/
10474412.2015.1046600
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Banks, J. A. (1997). Educating citizens in a multicultural
society. New
York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Berliner, D. C. (2013). Inequality, poverty, and the socialization
of
America’s youth for the responsibilities of citizenship. Theory
Into
Practice, 52, 203–209. doi:10.1080/00405841.2013.804314
Berry, W. (2000). Life is a miracle: An essay against modern
superstition.
Washington, DC: Counterpoint.
Bigler, R. S., Hayes, A. R., & Patterson, M. M. (2017). Social
striving:
Social group membership and children’s motivations and
competencies.
In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook
of compe-
tence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd ed., pp. 547–
565).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bishop, O. (n.d.). Global Oneness Project: Sawubona [Video
file]. Retrieved
from
https://www.globalonenessproject.org/library/interviews/sawubo
na
Bonilla-Silva, E. (2018). Racism without racists: Colorblind
racism and
the persistence of racial inequality in the United States (5th
ed.). Lan-
ham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Boykin, W. (1986). The triple quandary and the schooling of
Afro-Ameri-
can children. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of
minority
children: New perspectives (pp. 56–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brown, C. S. (2017). Discrimination in childhood and
adolescence: A
developmental intergroup approach. New York, NY: Routledge.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University
Press.
Bruner, J. (1996). Culture of education. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard Univer-
sity Press.
Ceci, S. J., Kahan, D. M., & Braman, D. (2010). The WEIRD
are even
weirder than you think: Diversifying contexts is as important as
diversi-
fying samples. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 87–88.
doi:10.1017/
S0140525X10000063
Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American
Psychologist, 64,
194–204. doi:10.1037/a0015308
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006).
Reducing the
racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention.
Science,
313, 1307–1310. doi:10.1126/science.1128317
142 USHER
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., & Goyer, J. P. (2017). Turning point:
Targeted,
tailored, and timely psychological intervention. In A. J. Elliot,
C. S.
Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and
motiva-
tion: Theory and application (2nd ed., pp. 657–686). New York,
NY:
Guilford Press.
Cokley, K. (2007). Critical issues in the measurement of ethnic
and racial
identity: A referendum on the state of the field. Journal of
Counseling
Psychology, 54, 224–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.3.224
Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology.
American
Psychologist, 64, 170–180. doi:10.1037/a0014564
Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality,
identity poli-
tics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law
Review, 43,
1241–1299. doi:10.2307/1229039
Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific
psychol-
ogy. American Psychologist, 30, 116–127.
doi:10.1037/h0076829
Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory. In A. C. Koenka
& A. L.
Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and
tomorrow:
Reflections of founders and descendants (Symposium). San
Antonio,
TX: American Educational Research Association.
DeCuir-Gunby, J. T. (2009). A review of the racial identity
develop-
ment of African American adolescents: The role of education.
Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 103–124. doi:10.3102/
0034654308325897
DeCuir-Gunby, J. T., & Schutz, P. A. (2014). Researching race
within edu-
cational psychology contexts. Educational Psychologist, 49,
244–260.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2014.957828
DeCuir-Gunby, J. T., & Schutz, P. A. (Eds.). (2017). Race and
ethnicity in
the study of motivation in education. New York, NY:
Routledge.
Devos, T., Huynh, Q. -L., & Banaji, M. R. (2012). Implicit self
and iden-
tity. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self
and iden-
tity (2nd ed., pp. 155–179). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. Boston, MA: D.C. Heath.
DiAngelo, R., & Sensory, €O. (2010). OK, I get it! Now tell me
how to do
it!”: Why we can’t just tell you how to do critical multicultural
educa-
tion. Multicultural Perspectives, 12, 97–102. doi:10.1080/
15210960.2010.481199
Du Bois, W. E. B. (1994). Souls of Black folk. Chicago, IL:
Dover. (Origi-
nal work published 1903)
Dweck, C. S. (2017). From needs to goals and representations:
Foundations
for a unified theory of motivation, personality, and
development. Psy-
chological Review, 124, 689–719. doi:10.1037/rev0000082
Eisner, E. W. (1998). The englightened eye: Qualitative inquiry
and the
enhancement of educational practice. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice
Hall.
Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. (1986). Black students’ school success:
Coping
with the “burden of ‘acting white.’” Urban Review, 18, 176–
206.
doi:10.1007/BF01112192
Frankl, V. (1959). Man’s search for meaning. Boston, MA:
Beacon Press.
Freire, P. (1998). Pedagogy of freedom: Ethics, democracy, and
civic cour-
age. Oxford, UK: Rowman & Littlefield.
Galliher, R. V., McLean, K. C., & Syed, M. (2017). An
integrated develop-
mental model for studying identity content in context.
Developmental
Psychology, 53, 2011–2022. doi:10.1037/dev0000299
Gilligan, C. (1993). In a different voice: Psychological theory
and wom-
en’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gorski, P. C. (2016). Poverty and the ideological imperative: A
call to
unhook from deficit and grit ideology and to strive for
structural ideol-
ogy in teacher education. Journal of Education for Teaching, 42,
378–
386. doi:10.1080/02607476.2016.1215546
Graham, S. (1992). “Most of the subjects were White and
middle class”:
Trends in published research on African Americans in selected
APA
journals, 1970–1989. American Psychologist, 47, 629–639.
doi:10.1037/
0003-066X.47.5.629
Graham, S. (2017). Discussant. In A. C. Koenka & A. L.
Wigfield (Chairs),
Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections
of
founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX:
American
Educational Research Association.
Graham, S. (this issue). Race/ethnicity and social adjustment of
adoles-
cents: How (not if) school diversity matters. Educational
Psychologist,
53. doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1428805
Gray, D. L., Hope, E. C., & Matthews, J. S. (this issue). Black
and belong-
ing at school: A case for interpersonal, instructional, and
institutional
opportunity structures. Educational Psychologist, 53.
doi:10.1080/
00461520.2017.1421466
Greene, M. (1993). The passions of pluralism: Multiculturalism
and the
expanding community. Educational Researcher, 22(1), 13–18.
Hand, V., & Gresalfi, M. (2015). The joint accomplishment of
identity.
Educational Psychologist, 50, 190–203.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.
1075401
Hargons, C., Mosley, D., Falconer, J., Faloughi, R., Singh, A.,
Cokley, K.,
& Stevens-Watkins, D. (2017). Black lives matter: A call to
action for
counseling psychology leaders. The Counseling Psychologist,
45, 873–
901. doi:10.1177/0011000017733048
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond
WEIRD:
Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behavioral and
Brain Scien-
ces, 33, 111–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725
hooks, b. (2010). Teaching critical thinking: Practical wisdom.
New York,
NY: Routledge.
hooks, b. (2016 April). A conversation on race and education in
the 21st
century. Berea, KY: Opening plenary address at the Dialogue on
Race
and Education in the 21st Century, Berea College.
Hughes, D. L., Del Toro, J., & Way, N. (2017). Interrelations
among
dimensions of ethnic-racial identity during adolescence.
Developmental
Psychology, 53, 2139–2153. doi:10.1037/dev0000401
Irvine, J. J. (1999). The education of children whose nightmares
come both
day and night. Journal of Negro Education, 68, 244–253.
doi:10.2307/
2668099
James, W. (2001). Talks to teachers on psychology and to
students on some
of life’s ideals. Mineola, NY: Dover. (Original work published
1899)
Jung, C. G. (1998). Man in his relation to others. In J. Jacobi
(Ed.),
Psychological reflections: An anthology of Jung’s writings,
1905–1961
(pp. 81–182). London, UK: Routledge. (Original work published
1945)
Juvonen, J., Kogachi, K., & Graham, S. (2017). When and how
do students
benefit from ethnic diversity in middle school? Child
Development.
doi:10.1111/cdev.12834
Kentucky Department of Education. (2017). School report card.
Retrieved
from http://applications.education.ky.gov/src/Default.aspx
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the
brain: Current
evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology,
62, 419–
449. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolution (2nd
ed.). Chi-
cago: University of Chicago Press.
Kumar, R., & Lauermann, F. (2018). Cultural beliefs and
instructional
intentions: Do experiences in teacher education institutions
matter?
American Educational Research Journal. Advance online
publica-
tion. doi:10.3102/0002831217738508
Kumar, R., Zusho, A., & Bondie, R. (this issue). Weaving
cultural rele-
vance and achievement motivation into inclusive classroom
cultures.
Educational Psychologist, 53.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1432361
Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The
case for cul-
turally relevant pedagogy. Theory Into Practice, 34, 159–165.
doi:10.1080/00405849509543675
Lorde, A. (1984). Sister outsider: Essays and speeches.
Trumansburg, NY:
Crossing Press.
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York,
NY: Van
Nostrand Reinhold.
Mayer, R. E. (2012). Information processing. In K. R. Harris, S.
Graham,
& T. C. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook,
Vol. 1:
Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 85–99).
Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 143
McInerney, D. M., & Liem, G. A. D. (Eds.). (2018). Big
theories revisited
2: A volume of research on sociocultural influences on
motivation and
learning. Charlotte, NC: Information Age.
McKown, C., & Strambler, M. J. (2009). Developmental
antecedents and
social and academic consequences of stereotype-consciousness
in mid-
dle childhood. Child Development, 80, 1643–1659.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2009.01359.x
Miller, J. G., Goyal, N., & Wice, M. (2017). A cultural
psychology of agency:
Morality, motivation, and reciprocity. Perspectives on
Psychological Sci-
ence, 12, 867–875. doi:10.1177/1745691617706099
Morris, M. W., Chiu, C-Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural
psychology.
Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631–659.
doi:10.1146/annurev-
psych-010814-015001
Nathan, L. F. (2017). When grit isn’t enough: A high school
principal
examines how poverty and inequality thwart the college-for-all
promise.
Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Noddings, N. (2013). Caring: A relational approach to ethics
and moral
education (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Nolen, S. B., Horn, I. S., & Ward, C. J. (2015). Situating
motivation. Educa-
tional Psychologist, 50, 234–247.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.1075399
Oyserman, D. (2008). Racial-ethnic self-schemas:
Multidimensional iden-
tity-based motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,
1186–
1198. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.003
Oyserman, D. (2017). Culture three ways: Culture and
subcultures within
countries. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 435–463.
doi:10.1146/
annurev-psych-122414-033617
Pajares, F. (2007). Culturalizing educational psychology. In F.
Salili & R.
Hoosain (Eds.), Culture, motivation, and learning (pp. 19–42).
Char-
lotte, NC: Information Age.
Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2006). The self and academic
motivation:
Theory and research after the cognitive revolution. In J. M.
Royer (Ed.),
The impact of the cognitive revolution on educational
psychology
(pp. 165–198). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Phinney, J. (1992). The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure: A
new scale
for use with adolescents and young adults from diverse groups.
Journal
of Adolescent Research, 7, 156–176.
doi:10.1177/074355489272003
powell, j. a. (2012). Racing to justice: Transforming our
conceptions of self
and other to build an inclusive society. Bloomington: Indiana
University
Press.
Rjosk, C., Richter, D., L€udtke, O., & Eccles, J. S. (2017).
Ethnic composi-
tion and heterogeneity in the classroom: Their measurement and
rela-
tionship with student outcomes. Journal of Educational
Psychology,
109, 1188–1204. doi:10.1037/edu0000185
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory:
Basic psy-
chological needs in motivation, development, and wellness.
New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Schlenker, B. R. (2012). Self-presentation. In M. R. Leary & J.
P. Tangney
(Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed., pp. 542–570).
New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Schunk, D. H. (2017). Motivation and social cognitive theory.
In A. C.
Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday,
today,
and tomorrow: Reflections of founders and descendants
(Symposium).
San Antonio, TX: American Educational Research Association.
Sellers, R. M., Rowley, S. A. J., Chavous, T. M., Shelton, J. N.,
& Smith,
M. A. (1997). Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity: A
prelimi-
nary investigation of reliability and construct validity. Journal
of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 73, 805–815. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.73.4.805
Sue, D. W. (2017). The challenges of becoming a White ally.
The Counsel-
ing Psychologist, 45, 706–716. doi:10.1177/0011000017719323
Turner, J. C., & Nolen, S. B. (2015). Introduction: The
relevance of the sit-
uative perspective in educational psychology. Educational
Psychologist,
50, 167–172. doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.1075404
Tyler, K. M., Burris, J. L., & Coleman, S. T. (2016).
Investigating the asso-
ciation between home-school dissonance and disruptive
classroom
behaviors for urban middle school students. The Journal of
Early Ado-
lescence. Advance online publication.
doi:10.1177/0272431616678987
Uma~na-Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W.
E.,
Rivas-Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., . . . Seaton, E. (2014). Ethnic
and racial
identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An
integrated
conceptualization. Child Development, 85(1), 21–39.
doi:10.1111/
cdev.12196
Urdan, T. C. (2017). Achievement goal theory: Where it came
from and
where it is going. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs),
Motivation
theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders
and
descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX: American
Educational
Research Association.
Urdan, T., & Bruchmann, K. (this issue). Examining the
academic motiva-
tion of a diverse student population: A consideration of
methodology.
Educational Psychologist, 53.
doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1440234
Verkuyten, M. (2016). Further conceptualizing ethnic and racial
identity
research: The social identity approach and its dynamic model.
Child
Development, 87, 1796–1812. doi:10.1111/cdev.12555
Weiner, B. (2017). Attribution theory. In A. C. Koenka & A. L.
Wigfield
(Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow:
Reflections
of founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX:
American
Educational Research Association.
Woodson, C. G. (1990). The mis-education of the negro.
Trenton, NJ: First
Africa World Press. (Original work published 1933)
Zusho, A., & Clayton, K. (2011). Culturalizing achievement
goal theory
and research. Educational Psychologist, 46, 239–260.
doi:10.1080/
00461520.2011.614526
144 USHER
HR Mgmt. Week 9 Discussion
Decisions: Motivation vs. Ability (20 points)
Discuss whether ability or motivation is more important in
selecting the right person for the right job. Explain your
rationale.
HR Mgmt. Week 10 Discussion
Overcoming the Challenges Associated with Employee
Feedback (10 points)
Assess the challenges of managers providing accurate, timely,
and effective feedback to employees. Recommend how
managers can overcome any two (2) of the challenges you
identified.
Making a Big Positive Impact on an Organization (10 points)
Of the concepts covered in the textbook, determine which, if
implemented well, would have the greatest impact on an
organization. Provide a rationale with your response.

More Related Content

Similar to Assignment 3 Annual Review and Create yourAssignment 3 Annua.docx

New teacher Induction Program Session 3A
New teacher Induction Program Session 3ANew teacher Induction Program Session 3A
New teacher Induction Program Session 3ASharon Seslija
 
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxWomens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxlefrancoishazlett
 
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxWomens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxhelzerpatrina
 
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docxgerardkortney
 
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxAt UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxjaggernaoma
 
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxAt UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxjasoninnes20
 
Transition Plan Lesson Plans
Transition Plan Lesson PlansTransition Plan Lesson Plans
Transition Plan Lesson Plansdgoldie09
 
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docx
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docxAssignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docx
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docxwrite31
 
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newAsh edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newchrishjennies
 
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newAsh edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newvindaniel123
 
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptx
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptxPresentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptx
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptxKerrolHeslop
 
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docx
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docxIntroduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docx
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docxvrickens
 
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docx
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docxAppendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docx
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docxjesuslightbody
 
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find infor
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find inforDiscussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find infor
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find inforVinaOconner450
 
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docx
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docxRunning head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docx
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docxtoltonkendal
 
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.ppt
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.pptMeda5700 masteringpossibilities2.ppt
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.pptMichelle Childress
 
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docx
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docxAssignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docx
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docxbraycarissa250
 
Human Growth & Development Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 co
Human Growth & Development  Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 coHuman Growth & Development  Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 co
Human Growth & Development Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 coNarcisaBrandenburg70
 
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021Victoria Arthur
 

Similar to Assignment 3 Annual Review and Create yourAssignment 3 Annua.docx (20)

New teacher Induction Program Session 3A
New teacher Induction Program Session 3ANew teacher Induction Program Session 3A
New teacher Induction Program Session 3A
 
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxWomens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
 
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docxWomens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
Womens Reproductive RightsWomens reproductive rights are contr.docx
 
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx
· Assignment 3 Creating a Compelling VisionLeaders today must be .docx
 
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxAt UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
 
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docxAt UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
At UC, it is a priority that students are provided with strong edu.docx
 
Transition Plan Lesson Plans
Transition Plan Lesson PlansTransition Plan Lesson Plans
Transition Plan Lesson Plans
 
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docx
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docxAssignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docx
Assignment Using Leadership to Improve Ethical Performance.docx
 
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newAsh edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
 
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models newAsh edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
Ash edu 650 week 3 assignment different school models new
 
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptx
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptxPresentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptx
Presentation-Experiencing The Impact of Gender Issues.pptx
 
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docx
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docxIntroduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docx
Introduction to Business 75-100 Integrative Project Phase OneCo.docx
 
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docx
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docxAppendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docx
Appendix AEducational Leadership Goals and Learning Outcomes.docx
 
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find infor
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find inforDiscussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find infor
Discussion 1 Use references and citations Research to find infor
 
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docx
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docxRunning head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docx
Running head WOODSREDU5000-1WOODSREDU5000-42A.docx
 
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.ppt
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.pptMeda5700 masteringpossibilities2.ppt
Meda5700 masteringpossibilities2.ppt
 
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docx
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docxAssignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docx
Assignment 2 Fink Step 3Due Week 7 and worth 200 pointsFor .docx
 
232269
232269232269
232269
 
Human Growth & Development Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 co
Human Growth & Development  Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 coHuman Growth & Development  Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 co
Human Growth & Development Advocacy ProjectAssignmentEDPS 210 co
 
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021
Eng 309 Syllabus Spring 2021
 

More from ursabrooks36447

Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docx
Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docxAssignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docx
Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docx
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docxAssignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docx
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docxAssignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docx
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docxAssignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docx
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docx
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docxAssignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docx
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docxAssignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docx
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docxAssignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docx
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docx
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docxAssignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docx
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docx
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docxAssignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docx
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docx
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docxAssignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docx
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docx
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docxAssignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docx
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docx
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docxAssignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docx
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docxAssignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docxAssignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docx
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docxAssignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docx
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docx
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docxAssignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docx
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docx
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docxAssignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docx
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docx
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docxAssignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docx
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docx
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docxAssignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docx
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docxursabrooks36447
 
Assignment 3 TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3 .docx
Assignment 3  TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3  .docxAssignment 3  TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3  .docx
Assignment 3 TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3 .docxursabrooks36447
 

More from ursabrooks36447 (20)

Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docx
Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docxAssignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docx
Assignment 3 Email Based on Listening Skills ScenarioMonica.docx
 
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docx
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docxAssignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docx
Assignment 3 Elastic and Inelastic TrafficWrite a three to four.docx
 
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docxAssignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social Justice Social justice has been .docx
 
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docx
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docxAssignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docx
Assignment 3 Discussion QuestionsYour facilitator will guide yo.docx
 
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docx
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docxAssignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docx
Assignment 3 Direct ManipulationDue Week 8 and worth 80 points.docx
 
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docxAssignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docx
Assignment 3 Defining Social JusticeSocial justice has been defin.docx
 
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docx
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docxAssignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docx
Assignment 3 Data SourcesFor this assignment, you will explore th.docx
 
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docx
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docxAssignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docx
Assignment 3 Cultural Activity ReportDue Week 10 and worth 100 po.docx
 
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docx
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docxAssignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docx
Assignment 3 Colin Powell’s Leadership Lessons PaperBy Frid.docx
 
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docx
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docxAssignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docx
Assignment 3 Corrections Case StudyDue Week 10 and worth .docx
 
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docx
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docxAssignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docx
Assignment 3 Chapter 6 The Communication ProcessAssignment #3.docx
 
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docx
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docxAssignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docx
Assignment 3 Areas of SpecializationThere are many wonderful oppo.docx
 
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docxAssignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of RightsWhen the Constitution was completed in.docx
 
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docxAssignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docx
Assignment 3 Bill of Rights When the Constitution was completed i.docx
 
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docx
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docxAssignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docx
Assignment 3 Assault, Battery, and Crimes against PersonsRe.docx
 
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docx
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docxAssignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docx
Assignment 3 America as a Superpower For History 105 Dr..docx
 
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docx
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docxAssignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docx
Assignment 3 (Hypotheses, Methods, and Measurements) Name ______.docx
 
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docx
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docxAssignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docx
Assignment 3 10 Cs for Writing Effectively Review the ema.docx
 
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docx
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docxAssignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docx
Assignment 3 - Information Systems Concepts  2 pages of disc.docx
 
Assignment 3 TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3 .docx
Assignment 3  TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3  .docxAssignment 3  TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3  .docx
Assignment 3 TCSS 143 Programming Assignment 3 .docx
 

Recently uploaded

SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonScience lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonJericReyAuditor
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptx
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptxENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptx
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptxAnaBeatriceAblay2
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 

Recently uploaded (20)

SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonScience lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptx
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptxENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptx
ENGLISH5 QUARTER4 MODULE1 WEEK1-3 How Visual and Multimedia Elements.pptx
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 

Assignment 3 Annual Review and Create yourAssignment 3 Annua.docx

  • 1. Assignment 3: Annual Review and Create yourAssignment 3: Annual Review and Create your Dream Job (425 points)Dream Job (425 points) ›› Students, please view the "Submit a Clickable Rubric Assignment" in the Course Info section. Instructors, training on how to grade is within the Instructor Center. Assignment 3: Annual Review and Create Your Dream Job Due Week 10 and worth 425 points Imagine you work at a company and it is time for an employee named Jim’s annual review. While he was a model employee the first nine (9) months of the year, recently Jim has been coming in late. It has not been just a few minutes each day, either. It is starting to cause problems in the production line. In this assignment, write a one (1) page summary of your conversation with Jim. How will you address his recent performance issues while still praising him for his previous nine (9) months of good work? Your goal is to balance the negative and positive feedback so that Jim will leave motivated to do his best. Write a one (1) page paper in which you: 1. Explain how you will address Jim’s recent performance issues. 2. Suggest both constructive and positive feedback designed so
  • 2. that Jim will leave motivated to do his best. 3. Format your assignment according to the following formatting requirements: a. This course requires use of new Strayer Writing Standards (SWS). The format is different than other Strayer University courses. Please take a moment to review the SWS documentation for details. b. Typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size 12), with one-inch margins on all sides. c. Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment, your name, your professor’s name, the course title, and the date. The cover page is not included in the required page length. The specific course learning outcomes associated with this assignment are: Explain effective approaches to the broad spectrum of employee relations, including career development, fostering ethical behavior, discipline, labor relations, and dismissals. Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of performance appraisal programs. Use technology and information resources to research issues in human resource management. Write clearly and concisely about human resource management
  • 3. using proper writing mechanics. In this assignment, you get the chance to create your dream job and to build its compensation plan and appraisal performance. Write a six to eight (6-8) page paper in which you: 1. Create a job description and specifications for your dream job. 2. Design a compensation and benefits package related to your dream job. https://blackboard.strayer.edu/webapps/assignment/uploadAssig nment?content_id=_27010276_1&course_id=_239406_1&group _id=&mode=view https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/STANDA RDIZED/StrayerWritingStandards/Strayer_Writing_Standards.p df 3. Rationalize your compensation and benefits package. Be sure to indicate the research and considerations that went into the design of the compensation and benefits package. 4. Imagine this is the only position of its kind in the organization. From this perspective, design a performance appraisal program to assess your job performance. 5. Rationalize your performance appraisal program. Be sure to indicate the research and considerations that went into the design of the performance appraisal program.
  • 4. 6. Use at least three (3) quality resources in this assignment. Note: Wikipedia and similar websites do not qualify as quality resources. 7. Format your assignment according to the following formatting requirements: a. This course requires use of new Strayer Writing Standards (SWS). The format is different than other Strayer University courses. Please take a moment to review the SWS documentation for details. b. Typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size 12), with one-inch margins on all sides. c. Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment, your name, your professor’s name, the course title, and the date. The cover page is not included in the required page length. d. Include a reference page. Citations and references must follow SWS format. The reference page is not included in the required page length. The specific course learning outcomes associated with this assignment are: Discuss job analysis, job descriptions, and specifications. Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of employee compensation programs. Analyze various techniques, considerations, and designs of
  • 5. performance appraisal programs. Use technology and information resources to research issues in human resource management. Write clearly and concisely about human resource management using proper writing mechanics. Click here to view the grading rubric for this assignment. https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/STANDA RDIZED/StrayerWritingStandards/Strayer_Writing_Standards.p df https://blackboard.strayer.edu/bbcswebdav/institution/BUS/310/ 1192/Week%2010/Week%2010%20Assignment%203%20Rubric. html Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalC ode=hedp20 Educational Psychologist ISSN: 0046-1520 (Print) 1532-6985 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hedp20 Acknowledging the Whiteness of Motivation Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance Ellen L. Usher To cite this article: Ellen L. Usher (2018) Acknowledging the Whiteness of Motivation
  • 6. Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance, Educational Psychologist, 53:2, 131-144, DOI: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220 Published online: 10 Apr 2018. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 187 View Crossmark data Citing articles: 1 View citing articles Acknowledging the Whiteness of Motivation Research: Seeking Cultural Relevance Ellen L. Usher Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, University of Kentucky This commentary revisits compelling arguments put forth by the contributors to this special issue on the role of race and ethnicity in academic motivation research. First, an overview of how race and motivation have been positioned in prominent theories of motivation is
  • 7. provided and juxtaposed with those offered in culturally responsive pedagogical approaches championed by multicultural educators. Special emphasis is placed on how teachers’ and learners’ sociohistorical and cultural contexts might influence motivation. Second, the critical role of identity and membership complexity is explored. Third, suggestions for more culturally attentive research methods are offered. The final section includes research and practice recommendations for supporting the academic motivation of diverse learners. Challenges to theoretical and methodological assumptions about race, ethnicity, and culture are offered throughout, including a call to address the role that racism, power, and privilege have played in perpetuating inequality in motivation research conducted in the United States. No one can be in the world, with the world, and with others and maintain a posture of neutrality. I cannot be in the world decontextualized, simply observing life. — Paulo Freire (1998, p. 73) “Line up at the back of the room by skin color,” came my
  • 8. professor’s directive. It was the first day of a graduate school seminar. My classmates looked around at each other nervously, wondering if she was serious. “Go ahead,” the professor repeated. “Stand up.” I was feeling uncomfort- able. I enjoyed being an observer. But moments later, I was baring my arms alongside fellow doctoral students as we positioned ourselves against the back wall of the room on the basis of our . . . melanin. “Okay, good. Notice how you’re feeling and where you are in the line,” the professor continued. (I was standing somewhere near the fairer half of middle.) “Let’s talk about what this means.” Here, in one of the rare moments in my life, I was forced to confront my Whiteness. Thus began weeks of in-depth discussions about the social construct of race and the unearned advantages and disadvantages it has conferred to people in nearly all spheres of life. The activity shook me out of a slumber— one from which I am still awakening. What does Whiteness mean to me personally and to the systems I am a part of?
  • 9. In the United States and around the globe, the costs of White supremacy are increasingly evident. Among children and adolescents, these costs are often first felt in the places where they spend most of their lives—school. Disparities in educational achievement and opportunity between children of different racial and ethnic groups have long been a fea- ture of U.S. public schools (APA Task Force, 2012). Com- pared to their White and Asian American counterparts, African American, Native American, and Latinx students score lower academically, are more likely to face disciplin- ary sanctions, are assigned to lower academic tracks, and drop out of school at higher rates. Differential educational practices based on implicit and explicit stereotyping, preju- dice, and discrimination perpetuate disparities. These inequities come at high psychological, social, cultural, and economic cost, not only to people of color but to all people (Brown, 2017). In this commentary, I argue that the contributions in this
  • 10. special issue signify the ways in which Whiteness has shaped research on academic motivation. I support this argument by framing my remarks around three emergent themes in this special issue. First, I address how race and ethnicity have featured in prevailing theories of motivation and emphasize the importance of contextualizing educa- tional psychology research. Second, I consider the founda- tional role of identity and membership in academic motivation. Third, I describe how researchers might sharpen their methodological approach (i.e., the who, where, when, and how) in ways that reflect a more inclusive Correspondence should be addressed to Ellen L. Usher, Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, University of Kentucky, 249 Dickey Hall, Lexington, KY 40506. E-mail: [email protected] EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 53(2), 131–144, 2018 Copyright � Division 15, American Psychological Association ISSN: 0046-1520 print / 1532-6985 online DOI: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1442220
  • 11. understanding of motivational processes. I then discuss the implications of these three themes for culturally responsive education and research. I fully recognize the elusive boundaries and vast scope that the term “Whiteness” implies (see Andersen, 2003, for a critique). I nevertheless have elected to use the term to refer to the tendency of motivation researchers to prioritize perspectives, participants, measures, and ways of knowing that assume White as “normal” or default. I suggest that acknowledging the Whiteness of our research is an impor- tant step toward developing a more complete and just understanding of human motivation. THEORETICAL FRAMING Searching for Universals in a Sea of Whiteness The contributors to this special issue have all noted that the bulk of pages published on human motivation and learning has been written by White men (and more recently women)
  • 12. who have based their theories of motivation on evidence largely gathered from other White people of similar social strata. Most motivation theories dominating educational psychology journals today have been crafted and refined under the assumption that human motivational processes are either absolute (culturally invariant) or universal (grant- ing that the degree to which certain motives are expressed might differ according to environmental conditions). Implicit in these approaches has been an assumption that studying White people is an acceptable starting point for the development of theories of motivation for use with “other” populations. This was evident at the 2017 American Educational Research Association (AERA) meeting where a panel of distinguished motivation theorists (all White; five men, one woman) spoke to a packed audience about their theories’ contributions and prospects. After their overviews, Sandra Graham (2017), the discussant and a woman of color,
  • 13. offered the following observation: In the very excellent summaries that all of these presenters gave, never once was the word “race/ethnicity” mentioned . . .. I’m not going to ask you whether you’ve been con- cerned with diversity in your studies elaborating the theory, but I do want to ask you how important you think this issue is. Looking at your theory as a whole and the ethnic repre- sentation of samples, do you think that most of the subjects were White and middle class? And if the answer is “yes,” “maybe,” or “I’m not sure,” how can you assure the young scholars and the senior scholars in this audience . . . that your theory has generality and relevance? Her question was met with resounding applause. The scholars’ responses largely reiterated their aim to discover universal principles. Identifying universals, Bernie Weiner (2017) explained, helps “still the waters to see a little deeper and decrease the complexity.” He added emphati- cally, “I’m a main effect person . . .. I don’t care who my sample is, really, because I’m looking for similarities.” Weiner’s views leaned toward the “top-down, theory
  • 14. driven, etic-oriented approach” described by Zusho and Clayton (2011) as “absolutist” (p. 248). From this perspec- tive, motivational processes are viewed as culture free. The theorists flanking Weiner were more universalistic in their claims. Dale Schunk (2017) acknowledged that a social cognitive theoretical perspective described general princi- ples but its application in educational settings depends on numerous sociocultural factors, such as ethnic/racial iden- tity and immigration status. Ed Deci (2017) described cross-cultural studies that demonstrated the universality of fundamental needs. Although the authors of this special issue suggest that a universal theory of motivation that is culturally grounded might one day be attainable, they point to the dearth of research evidence reflecting the experiences and perspec- tives of members of historically marginalized groups. They applaud the use of situative perspectives, noticeably absent from the AERA panel, in which motivation is viewed as
  • 15. “arising through [individuals’] participation in social, cul- tural, and historical contexts or systems” (Turner & Nolen, 2015, p. 168). Situative approaches recognize that the par- ticular beliefs, values, and motives students develop are necessarily “fraught with ambiguity [and] sensitive to the occasion” (Bruner, 1996, p. 6). They acknowledge that the prominence and weight given to any particular motive must be understood in relation to systems of power, position, and privilege (Nolen, Horn, & Ward, 2015). The Whiteness of prominent theories of motivation has, by default, framed motivational phenomena in terms of the dominant group (i.e., White middle-class people composing the global minority). No contributors make this point as directly as Gray, Hope, and Matthews (this issue), who note that publications in mainstream educational psychology reflect White norms and rarely incorporate race-based theo- retical or methodological approaches (and see DeCuir- Gunby & Schutz, 2014, 2017). Several adverse side effects
  • 16. of these theoretical and methodological oversights are described in all four articles. First, scholars fail to consider the role that being “othered” plays in the functioning of oppressed and marginalized groups. Second, motivation is divorced from the sociohistorical contexts in which learners and teachers operate. Third, motivation theory and research are assumed to be neutral and objective. Framing the Problem: From Deficits and “Othering” to Needs as Universal Urdan and Bruchmann (this issue) provide an abbreviated historical account of how researchers have framed a 132 USHER cultural understanding of motivation. They note that some scholars have taken a deficit approach, assuming that, whether by nurture or nature, certain cultural groups have different motives and may even lack an achievement motive altogether. Some have explained ethnic minority
  • 17. students’ motivation in terms of a defense against environ- mental threats both within and outside of their ethnic group. Even a framework like Fordham and Ogbu’s (1986) opposi- tional stance defines the psychological experiences and motives of the “underclass” in terms of a response to the dominant group. Such an approach also reflects a deficit, explaining the motivation of oppressed individuals as a defense against those in power whose motives are assumed to be purer. Frames like these discount the fact that a healthy response sometimes involves defiance and that “sickness might consist of not having symptoms when you should” (Maslow, 1968, p. 7). From one perspective, motives and behaviors are considered maladaptive; from another, they can be viewed as a sign of health (e.g., the rejection of an oppressive system). Needs-based theoretical frames have suggested that humans are motivated not only to overcome deficiencies but also to become fully actualized. Needs are hierar-
  • 18. chically structured such that lower order needs (e.g., safety, belonging) must typically be fulfilled before higher order (i.e., growth, self-actualization) needs are sought (Maslow, 1968). The self-determination framework posits that the fulfillment of autonomy, relatedness, and competence needs is central to autonomous motivation, which is based on a universal set of identifiable “social and cultural nutrients” (Ryan & Deci, 2017, p. 4). Dweck (2017) advanced a simi- lar universal theory in which motivation is seen as a product of basic and composite needs, which are subsumed by an overarching need for self-coherence and integration. Needs are the constituents of goal-driven behavior, shepherded by beliefs, emotions, and tendencies that “can take very differ- ent forms in different cultures” (p. 694). I have provided only a snapshot of the theoretical frames used to guide motivation research, focusing on those rele- vant to the articles in this special issue. This brings me to two critical observations. First, how a motivational problem
  • 19. is framed will influence what researchers observe. Carol Gilligan (1993), a pioneering psychologist who brought a feminine perspective to human development research, wrote that “theory can blind observation” (p. 25). In a field historically dominated by White male American scholars, it is unlikely that the theoretical mappings of human motiva- tion fully reflect the experiences of all people. Second, once they have a paradigmatic framework, researchers are reluctant to modify it. To do so would come at great cognitive and professional expense. As Kuhn (1970) suggested, “to desert the paradigm is to cease prac- ticing the science it defines” (p. 34). Researchers are predis- posed to look to the familiar for an explanation of the new (Dewey, 1933). They rarely abandon the frameworks in which they were trained. Critically investigating our own frames of reference is among our most challenging profes- sional tasks. Kumar, Zusho, and Bondie (this issue) call on the field to consider its own shortsightedness by exploring
  • 20. other paradigms (e.g., critical race theory, culturally rele- vant pedagogy) that have done a better job of including the perspectives of marginalized groups. Considering Contexts at Multiple Levels The articles in this special issue note that, in theory and in practice, motivation researchers have not sufficiently con- sidered the political, social, and cultural landscapes in which their studies have been conducted. Indeed, many have attempted to position themselves as impartial observ- ers of universal principles, assuming these to be best under- stood in isolation from life’s noisiness. This does not hold up to Freire’s assertion that no human is impartial or decon- textualized. Researchers must more honestly account for what a long line of educational psychologists have estab- lished: Students’ cognition, emotions, motives, and social relationships have taken root in particular sociohistorical contexts. Graham’s (this issue) corpus of rigorous research dem-
  • 21. onstrates how context can be examined in a way that gener- ates new theoretical insights and questions. Her work considers how the local landscape—in particular, the school’s ethnic makeup—might alter students’ perceptions of themselves and others by affecting cross-ethnic friend- ships, complex identity development, and perceived vulner- ability. One key finding is that students’ cross-ethnic friendships mirror lines of the U.S. racial power hierarchy such that students with low power are less likely to be befriended than are students of high power (i.e., White). Graham’s research provides evidence that shifts in school demographics can also be distressing to learners, particu- larly when their ethnic group’s representation declines. School context matters. Nevertheless, much remains to be known about how school diversity and ethnic composition are connected to students’ social relationships and to academic motivation and learning. The nature and strength of these relationships
  • 22. depend on how ethnic composition and diversity are opera- tionalized (see Brown, 2017; Rjosk, Richter, L€udtke, & Eccles, 2017). Is there a tipping point at which representa- tion of one’s group is motivationally advantageous? More- over, is it motivationally consequential when a school’s ethnic composition does not match the ethnic composition of the particular classrooms within it? Graham and her col- leagues have begun to investigate such effects. When stu- dents attend “ethnically diverse” schools in which the highest performing classes are full of students with the lightest skin tones, a covert message of discrimination is readily apparent. Such patterns reinforce some of the most pervasive negative racial stereotypes—that students of SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 133 color are less academically capable. When the ethnic com- position of their classes does not match that of their school, all students rate the environment as more oppressive (Juvo- nen, Kogachi, & Graham, 2017).
  • 23. Graham’s (this issue) observation that, by 2040, White students will be in the ethnic minority in U.S. public schools is statistically correct but potentially misleading. In different regions of the United States, students’ opportuni- ties for contact with cultural outgroup members vary widely. One of my undergraduate students who attended public schools in the Appalachian region of eastern Ken- tucky recently remarked that she had never been in class with a non-White student until she came to college. The demography of hometowns like hers is not rapidly chang- ing. On the other hand, half of Kentucky’s African Ameri- can students live in Louisville’s urban hub, where they account for more than 80% of students enrolled at some schools (Kentucky Department of Education, 2017). How will students in these distinct settings develop racialized identities, intergroup attitudes, and academic self-beliefs? Geographic context matters too. Gray et al. (this issue) highlight the role of the U.S. socio-
  • 24. historical context in which Black learners have been consis- tently denied membership and full participatory rights. They refer to Carter G. Woodson’s (1933/1990) observation that schools in the United States “fail to cultivate the agency, strength, and skills of Black Americans, which may there- fore be left underdeveloped” (p. 7). Schooling in the United States is still “structurally White” in that it promotes and sustains the cultural legacies, values, and behaviors of the White owning class. In this regard, many schools and those leading them often do fail to nourish students of color. This threatens not only students’ sense of belonging but their very sense of who they are. But Woodson’s claim also seems to carry a problematic assumption that human agency, strength, and intellect are at the mercy of the surrounding context to cultivate. Stories of human triumph in adverse cir- cumstances tell of other possibilities. As Holocaust survivor Victor Frankl (1959) wrote, “Everything can be taken from a [hu]man but one thing: the last of the human freedom—to
  • 25. choose one’s attitude in any given set of circumstances, to choose one’s own way” (p. 66). I am not promoting a “bootstraps” approach or suggesting that oppressed individ- uals are uniquely responsible for overcoming unjust cir- cumstances. I simply note that context, although important, is not destiny. A scientific study of motivation must account for both the conditioned and agentic nature of human experience. A situative approach to the study of motivation acknowl- edges that contextual features of the environment mean dif- ferent things to different people (Nolen et al., 2015). As young people come to understand themselves as part of a larger social structure brought about by sociohistorical position and status, their perception of choice likely makes a motivational difference, as the contributing authors of this special issue suggest. Consider the following contexts in which choice may or may not be available: one’s ethnic minority status (e.g., involuntary and voluntary; visible or
  • 26. not), one’s source of income (e.g., parents’ employment status, family wealth), one’s home (e.g., location, ameni- ties, access to resources, safety), one’s schools (e.g., eco- nomic/social capital to relocate for top schools), one’s classes (e.g., advocating to be placed in an experienced teacher’s class, in a gifted program, or withheld from lower tracks), one’s learning (e.g., power and privilege to have top teachers, hire private tutors), and even one’s very iden- tity (e.g., assigned by others or self). Researchers often compare groups of students using some or all of these cate- gories but without a consideration of the underlying percep- tions students have of their autonomous choice in such matters (Have I/we been placed here or do I/we fully endorse being here?). Assessing perceptions of choice and status might be eas- ier than assessing perceptions of discrimination, particu- larly among younger children (Brown, 2017). What might be the cumulative effect of not feeling autonomous or
  • 27. agentic in one or more of these areas? I raise this question because I endorse a view of human functioning that posi- tions humans not as passive recipients of their environmen- tal contexts but as agents who are capable of selecting and creating environments for themselves (Bandura, 1986). Even within the same imposed environment, two students may select quite different social contexts for themselves. This agentic capacity notwithstanding, young people often come to view themselves as powerless in a larger social structure that has robbed them and their families of posi- tion, status, and more (Kumar et al., this issue). By considering not only the contextual features of learners’ environments but also learners’ perceptions of them, researchers will be better positioned to advocate for practices and policies that recognize the situated nature of motivation and the sociohistorical barriers facing youth who have been (and who still are) marginalized. One of the hallmarks of a social cognitive theory of learning and moti-
  • 28. vation is that our lives are both “partly fated and partly free” (James, 1899/2001, p. 95). What learners attend to, how they view the contexts in which they live, and whether they see themselves as agents is therefore central to how they develop and function. Identity and Membership Educational psychologists have accepted the centrality of self and identity in the study of human motivation (see Pajares & Schunk, 2006). Most studies on motivation fea- ture some measure of self-perception as a precursor to human action, whether involving one’s identity (Who am I?), one’s perceived efficacy (Am I capable?), or one’s per- sonal regard (Am I worthy?). The collective work of this 134 USHER special issue points to the relatively less frequent consider- ation of culture in self-systems as they relate to motivation. From life’s beginning, humans are members of social
  • 29. groups. Being a member of a group is an important part of determining one’s place in the social world, developing a sense of esteem in one’s self and in one’s group, and differ- entiating one’s self from outgroup members (Bigler, Hayes, & Patterson, 2017). How people come to see themselves partly depends on the meaning they make of their social memberships and how these memberships are viewed by others in their cultural environment (Kitayama & Uskul, 2011). Cultural and social identities can be complex. Even individuals who share phenotypic characteristics are poly- cultural by virtue of their many cultural memberships, such as their ancestry, socioeconomic class, linguistic heritage, and family composition (Morris, Chiu, & Liu, 2015). Like- wise, the manner in which one’s social identity activates certain motivational sequences is contextually situated and therefore subject to change (Hand & Gresalfi, 2015). A young male high school student living in rural Pennsylvania participates in glee club, plays football, is Latino, and nar-
  • 30. rowly avoids academic probation. How might researchers begin to operationalize the young man’s identity complex- ity and its effect on his motivation? As Graham (this issue) aptly observes, much depends on which social identity is most salient to the student at a given moment, something not well captured by universalistic approaches to under- standing human motivation (or identity). Psychologists have investigated the complexity of identity from many angles: developmental processes (e.g., crisis/resolution), status (e.g., achieved, foreclosed), domain (e.g., sexual, academic, athletic, ethnic), and evaluative components (e.g., centrality, salience, private/ public regard). For example, in a review of research on Black racial identity, DeCuir-Gunby (2009) described the various ways in which the subjective experiences of Black students have been measured and how they differ- entially relate to academic outcomes and well-being. Some measures have been designed to capture all
  • 31. learners’ ethnic identity, regardless of their ethnic heri- tage (e.g., Phinney, 1992). Deciding how to assess students’ subjective identity in studies of academic moti- vation is not a straightforward matter and must depend on the particular research aims. Urdan and Bruchmann (this issue) suggest that researchers be mindful of the dynamic properties of identity, such as salience and valence. Gray et al. (this issue) similarly suggest that researchers consider one’s sense of belonging within one’s own cultural heritage group(s), which may be similar to a sense of private regard (Sellers, Rowley, Chavous, Shelton, & Smith, 1997). Together, the articles in this issue recommend that academic motivation researchers include subjective measures of racial, ethnic, and cultural identity, which have increased in number and breadth in the past two decades (e.g., Cokley, 2007; Hughes, Del Toro, & Way, 2017; Oyserman, 2008; Uma~na-Taylor et al., 2014; Verkuyten, 2016).
  • 32. Only one article in this special issue (Urdan & Bruch- mann, this issue) discusses the role of intersecting identities as important associates of achievement motivation, despite the fact that all decry the tendency by motivation research- ers to treat race and ethnicity as monolithic. To conduct research that considers the complexity of identity content will require contextualized approaches that “defy tidy developmental stories” (Galliher, McLean, & Syed, 2017, p. 2012). Such approaches address intersectionality—the ways in which an individual’s multiple social identities jointly affect experience and psychological functioning (Crenshaw, 1991). Cole (2009) offered several recommen- dations for how researchers might consider intersectionality as it pertains to psychological processes such as motivation. For example, researchers should acknowledge that variation exists between individuals who identify with a particular group (e.g., there is no such thing as “the Latino experi- ence”). One is not only female, one might also be Asian
  • 33. American, socioeconomically disadvantaged, a first-gener- ation immigrant, and so on. Researchers must also under- stand the unearned advantages and disadvantages associated with one’s membership in a particular group given the group’s social and historical location. This involves seeking to understand the affordances of a learner’s intersecting identities, not only as reflected in the social hierarchies of race and ethnicity but also in other social categories (e.g., immigration status, ableness, sexual- ity). Learners whose identities differ in certain respects might find points of commonality in other respects. How is identity complexity ultimately related to motivation and learning? Research is mixed. On the one hand, identity complexity can serve as a safeguard from social/environmental threats such as discrimination and bias. An individual with a complex social identity may be better positioned to flexibly activate an appropriate response or to summon necessary resources in a given
  • 34. situation. But this depends on contextual factors. Graham’s (this issue) research takes a person-in-context approach, making evident the ways in which one’s eth- nic identity interacts within particular racially diverse contexts to activate motivational processes. For exam- ple, attributional patterns following peer victimization differ according to students’ ethnic representation at school. Less able to attribute their suffering to external causes, such as lack of power or status, students in the ethnic majority internalize the blame by overidentifying with it, whereas ethnic minority students who are vic- timized (particularly when also in the numeric minority at school) have many possible external explanations for their unfortunate situation. These findings notwithstanding, plenty of evidence has pointed to the stress of having to navigate multiple iden- tities, especially when one or more have been viewed as SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 135
  • 35. “less than” or “other” (Gray et al., this issue), or when one’s identities are in conflict with one another (Kumar et al., this issue). Compartmentalization of certain identi- ties might allow young people to avoid cognitive or social conflict, but often this comes at a high psychologi- cal cost (Boykin, 1986; Galliher et al., 2017; McKown & Strambler, 2009). Du Bois (1903/1994) forewarned that the history of African Americans would be described in terms of such a tension in the Self: “this longing to attain self-conscious manhood, to merge his double self into a better and truer self” (p. 2). Maslow (1968) similarly remarked that the average human regularly fights a “civil war within . . . between the forces of the inner depths and the forces of defense and control” needed to manage exter- nal pressures (p. 156). Being a member of a negatively ste- reotyped group likely intensifies the battle. One might ask, then, what are the “inner depths” that bring about the “truer self”? How might they be assessed? What are their motiva-
  • 36. tional implications? The development of an integrated sense of self and of one’s place in society is central to psychological well- being (Dweck, 2017). Basic threats to one’s group mem- berships include feeling discriminated against, perceiv- ing bias from others, sensing the ignorance of others to one’s lived experiences, receiving overt and covert mes- sages of “otherness,” and being treated unfairly. These factors often do occur explicitly. But the perception that they might exist despite others’ overt action is equally strong from a psychological perspective. Surrounded by threats, both real and perceived, a person might natu- rally employ self-protective strategies (e.g., withdrawal/ avoidance, self-aggrandizement) or might succumb in less desirable ways (e.g., depression, anxiety). The con- sequences of chronic uncertainty about whether one is fully accepted by one’s social environment or not can have a lifetime of repercussions—academic, social,
  • 37. emotional, physical, and financial (Bigler et al., 2017; Brown, 2017). And, as the articles in this issue boldly note, institutional racism and White supremacy have made it much more difficult for students of color to feel like they belong in U.S. schools. One potential solution is to help learners activate an “individuated identity” whereby they describe their affinities in their own terms (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). Core values interventions, in which student affirm their personal values through a brief writing exer- cise, have been shown to reduce racial achievement dis- parities (G. L. Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006). Self-affirmations affect motivation by triggering cogni- tive processes that mitigate environmental threats and promote a sense of empowerment (G. L. Cohen, Garcia, & Goyer, 2017). Exploring the intricacies of how indi- viduals view themselves (e.g., integrational vs. disinte- grational identity) and how structures in schools and in
  • 38. the broader environment promote (and thwart) positive self-development is a vital line of ongoing inquiry in motivation research. METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS Nearly three decades ago, Bruner (1990) denounced a tendency by psychologists to exalt “neat little studies” over approaches that would answer more complex ques- tions about the nature of the mind and how it is shaped by culture. Too many psychologists, he argued, were engaged in “methodolatry” while ignoring more interest- ing psychological questions (p. xi). Not much has changed. The cleanly executed, sophisticated models that populate the pages of mainstream educational psy- chology journals will not result in generalizable princi- ples about motivation “because contexts, attitudes, and meanings contribute systematic variance that must be included to reveal lawful ecological contrasts” (Ceci, Kahan, & Braman, 2010, p. 28). The methods that
  • 39. researchers have selected to examine motivation there- fore contribute to incomplete theoretical knowledge that does not reflect the breadth of human experience (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). In this section, I consider how suggestions from the articles in this special issue inform the ways in which motivation research has been and can be conducted. Following a typical methodology, I consider the implications of this body of work on the who, where, when, and how of our research. The Who Graham’s (1992) historical analysis revealed that White and middle-class subjects dominated the pages of psychol- ogy journals in the 1970s and 1980s. Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan (2010) described a continuing trend, noting that most research subjects in psychological science were WEIRD (i.e., Western [and White], educated, industrial- ized, rich, and democratic) despite the fact that most humans do not fit this description. The educational psychol-
  • 40. ogists whose words are published in this and similar jour- nals are also predominantly WEIRD, so it seems understandable that they would seek the convenience of recruiting like-minded participants and the familiarity of seeing research like theirs make it through the publication gate. In light of Freire’s (1998) reminder that no one can observe the world and remain neutral, motivation research must continue to expand its who, in terms of not only its participants but also its investigators. When Graham (2017) launched her pointed question to the AERA panel about how particular motivation theories had addressed race, eth- nicity, and culture, Urdan (2017) replied, “I would probably ask different questions if I didn’t bring the frame that I bring as a White man.” The field of educational 136 USHER psychology, he added, should “diversify the people who are
  • 41. asking the questions and interpreting the research.” Indeed. What are the consequences of this limited who both in terms of the observers and the observed? I offer are a few. 1. Motivation theories and constructs are normed on Eurocentric samples and contexts. 2. Researchers therefore contribute to the marginaliza- tion of others who do not follow predicted, “default” patterns. 3. The implications for practice that researchers offer do not generalize to most individuals and sociocultural locations. 4. Researchers’ own biases and privileges guide what they see and, in turn, the story they tell. Welcoming diverse perspectives—indeed, seeking and privileging them—is therefore paramount to inclusivity in motivation research and theory. The Where As all contributions to this issue have emphasized, a
  • 42. culturalized view of motivation requires close attention to contextual factors that cue and activate different cog- nitive and motivational sequences. The four articles in this special issue take a U.S.-specific sociohistorical per- spective, but motivation research has fortunately become increasingly global and cross-national as vibrant hubs of research have developed on several continents, casting new light on motivational processes in non-U.S. cultural contexts (see McInerney & Liem, 2018). Researchers should be sure to describe the broader contexts in which their research takes place. Ecological, semantic, geo- graphic, and other contexts influence how motivational processes are selected and engaged (Ceci et al., 2010). In their day-to-day lives, many learners must navigate diverse circumstances that present competing goals, val- ues, and demands. As Kumar et al. (this issue) and others note, dissonance between the cultural values of home and school has been associated with lower aca-
  • 43. demic motivation (e.g., Tyler, Burris, & Coleman, 2016). How is achievement motivation affected by dis- cordant messages across other contexts such as those encountered online, in extracurricular activities, and in the community? Are dissonant wheres felt more acutely by learners from particular racial or cultural identities? Withstanding environmental dissonance is not always easy or advantageous, but for some it might be accom- panied by opportunities for growth. As Kumar et al. advise, researchers should continue to target the causes and effects of dissonance on motivation in complex learning contexts and to examine the ways in which cul- turally relevant and responsive pedagogies might allevi- ate some of this dissonance. The When An ecologically valid approach to understanding motiva- tion considers the historical, political, and sociocultural contexts that shape individuals’ beliefs, positionality,
  • 44. and interpretations in the world. Motivation is situated and dynamic; researchers must therefore recognize the when of their research (Nolen et al., 2015). This opposes any goal of reaching timeless generalizations about what motivates people. As Cronbach (1975) observed, “generalizations decay” (p. 122). How can the field and its methodological approaches be more sensitive to changes over time? For example, with respect to stages of human development, most motivational processes have been studied in mid- to late adolescence. Consider- ably less work has examined the motivational impacts of culture, race, and ethnicity during childhood, when identity development becomes more complex and aware- ness of discrimination and inequity first emerges (Brown, 2017). A sociopolitical consideration of time might also be useful. In the United States, for instance, investigators might ask how students from different eth- nic groups have perceived their sense of belonging since
  • 45. the presidential election of Donald Trump or in light of certain immigration policy decisions. The emergence of more effective and differentiated real-time measures of motivation (e.g., biometric data, computer tracing) will also enable new ways of investigating the relative stability of motivation constructs in individualized ways (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). The How Perhaps the most important point raised by Urdan and Bruchmann (this issue) is that researchers’ own theories guide how they have examined the relationships between culture, race, ethnicity, and student motivation. Their meth- ods—the how of research—in turn reinforce researchers’ assumptions about the motivation of diverse groups. Sophisticated statistical models and large samples are assumed to enable scholars to reach generalizations that become guiding theoretical principles. However, the “conceptual economy” afforded by oft-used quantitative
  • 46. designs that consider a handful of White-normed constructs is that “the flavor of the particular situation, individual, event, or object is lost” (Eisner, 1998, p. 38). A more nuanced understanding of motivation emerges from first- person accounts of motivation, beliefs, or goals obtained by subjective reports. Still, I wonder how much room is avail- able for qualitative inquiry in motivation research. A glance at our journals shows that few pages are reserved for small total sample size, inductive models. Such studies are often deemed nonscientific, less rigorous, and lacking in external validity. I agree with Urdan and Bruchmann’s central mes- sage: Diverse methodological approaches are needed, any SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 137 one of which must be viewed as offering a partial explanation. Regardless of their methodological approach, research- ers must be thoughtful when selecting the tools they use to
  • 47. carry out their work. Motivation constructs have compli- cated measurement histories. Gray et al. (this issue) note that learners’ sense of belonging has been assessed in at least 18 ways, leading researchers to draw different conclu- sions about how belonging and its various features (e.g., affect, fairness, safety, perceived importance) are related to motivation. Like motives, identities are complex and multi- dimensional. As researchers seek to include measures that more sensitively assess race-, culture-, and ethnicity-based constructs (e.g., identity, socialization, opportunity), they will need to consider how to do so effectively in diverse learning environments. Measures tailored to the particular experiences of one cultural group (e.g., Black identity) are not likely appropriate for other groups. Items that refer gen- erally to “your culture” might be problematic given the breadth of students’ diverse, and intersecting, cultural memberships (A. B. Cohen, 2009). One danger of using students’ group membership in
  • 48. motivation research is that it treats each group (e.g., nation- ality, ethnicity, race) as a static entity—an error-free indica- tor of a set of values, beliefs, institutions, and behaviors that can “distinguish one group from another” (Banks, 1997, p. 124). Kumar et al. (this issue) suggest that the empirical landscape of academic motivation research would be enhanced by studies that promote culture to a prominent role and motives to a secondary one. Such an approach acknowledges that a learner’s culture is not just “out there” in the distal sense; it is also “in here” in the personal sense, both by explicit adoption of sociocultural norms and by internalization of beliefs, norms, and behaviors that have been tacitly transmitted. Graham’s (this issue) work pro- vides an excellent example of methods that assess this dis- tinction. Rather than assuming that overt group membership or participation in certain activities means that a student identifies with them, Graham and her colleagues asked adolescents to describe the group memberships that
  • 49. are important to them as well as the extent to which partici- pating in these groups is something that they think ethnic ingroup and outgroup members would do. This provides a more personalized starting place from which to investigate other questions. Just as group membership alone is insufficient for cap- turing students’ subjective perceptions of their relationship to their social context, use of the classroom as a unit of analysis might overlook the meaning students make of their learning environments. Students who get along well with their teachers and perceive them to be fair likely reap a motivation benefit, whereas those who feel marginalized or discriminated against by their teachers or peers likely suffer motivationally. One way that researchers can better assess the effect of teachers and pedagogical practices on student motivation is by considering students’ perceptions of their teachers’ pedagogical approach (e.g., asset based, deficit based). This could be supplemented by assessing learners’
  • 50. perceptions of their teachers’ beliefs and expectations (e.g., “My teacher believes that every student in our class can learn.” “Teachers at my school value students of different cultural backgrounds.”). Examining the association between what students and teachers believe to be the func- tion of schooling and education (e.g., to overcome deficits, to view one another as resources) might also reveal aspects of the classroom climate that give rise to different motiva- tional consequences. Researchers might also consider group beliefs as the unit of analysis. Gray et al. (this issue) note the historical impor- tance of collective agency among African-heritage commu- nity members, which includes the cultivation of cultural knowledge. As students learn about their own cultural heri- tage, how does their collective identity or sense of agency change? How does this affect them individually? The 2008 Obama campaign slogan, “Yes We Can,” reflected a grow- ing sense of social and political empowerment among Afri-
  • 51. can Americans as well as members of other cultural groups. How might participation in sociopolitical movements (e.g., Black Lives Matter) change Black students’ collective and individual beliefs related to self, other, and academics (see Hargons et al., 2017)? Researchers might investigate the motivational function of culturalized collective beliefs. The educational psychology community has lagged behind its social psychology counterparts in using measures that assess implicit thoughts and feelings, their antecedents (e.g., social primes), and their motivating power. The ways in which implicit or explicit forces jointly guide human behavior remain unclear (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). For example, Devos, Huynh, and Banaji (2012) suggested that discrepancies between one’s implicit and explicit self- image can motivate actions that defend or protect the ego. They showed that individuals with high explicit self-esteem but low implicit self-esteem are more likely to become defensive and to engage self-enhancing strategies. Other
  • 52. research has demonstrated the ways in which one’s social environment (real or imagined) can prime certain self-pre- sentational styles, which in turn can influence internal self- beliefs and motivation. “An audience does not even have to be present to shape how people think about and present themselves” (Schlenker, 2012, p. 554). Urdan and Bruch- mann suggest that implicit measures of motivation might be more predictive of behavior than explicit measures, but this will require further evidence. Educational psycholo- gists might extend and replicate social psychological research by taking laboratory experiments to the more natu- ralistic setting of the classroom. Excellent suggestions for the how of culturally-sensitive research that expand on many of the points raised in this special issue are provided by Awad, Patall, Rackley, and Reilly (2016). As they and these contributors have 138 USHER
  • 53. suggested, measures of motivation must be meaningful and valid for the populations of interest. Researchers must use care when using measures validated with certain groups (most likely American, White, middle-class, native-English speakers) with other groups, and they must not assume that all respondents make similar meaning of items normed on WEIRD samples (Miller, Goyal, & Wice, 2017). IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE The articles in this special issue raise many implications for research and practice. I have selected several of them to dis- cuss in this section. A Critical Approach: Confronting White Supremacy and Colorblindness in Research Critical race theory challenges the notion that existing theo- ries of academic motivation were developed with all learn- ers in mind. The articles in this issue also highlight the ways in which school-based practices are not culturally neutral. In many U.S. public schools, classroom practices,
  • 54. rules, curricula, and ways of knowing are often laden with the values of the privileged and powerful whose aims, whether conscious or unconscious, are to maintain the cur- rent power structure (hooks, 2010). Particularly in moments of perceived or actual scarcity of resources, those holding power reassert their status and privilege in overt and covert ways, through words, gestures, practices, curricula, poli- cies, and, yes, even theories (Kumar et al., this issue; Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). Schools and the broader society provide routine reminders to certain students of their status as the unwelcome “other.” These forms of discrimination can lead students to experience unquantifiable race- and class-based stress (Berliner, 2013). Gray et al. (this issue) rightly assert that educational research that is not properly situated sociohistorically can give way to color-blind rec- ommendations that do not sufficiently address systemic structural inequities. Unfortunately, many motivation researchers take such a
  • 55. color-blind approach. For example, popular motivation constructs such as “growth mindset” and “grit” have been decried as culturally blind antidotes to race-based achieve- ment gaps that promote victim blaming and perpetuate the myth of meritocracy (e.g., Gorski, 2016). Former Boston high school principal Linda Nathan (2017) dispelled the myth that “grit” is the beacon of hope for students from marginalized identities. Through the compelling stories of her own students, Nathan explained that to view student motivation as the great equalizer is to “ignore the invisible threads of inequity that are far less likely to tighten around the necks of our dominant-culture students” (p. 105). Many motivation scholars reading this will agree. But Nathan’s need to explain this is evidence that we researchers have failed to offer a sufficiently critical message that reflects the complex micro- and macrocontexts in which children live and learn (Kumar et al., this issue). Gray et al. (this issue) offer several helpful recommen-
  • 56. dations for researchers interested in questioning “the acul- tural assumptions” of our guiding theoretical frameworks. For instance, scholars can spend time in the settings that they are investigating. Grounded observations might help researchers more carefully consider how students from his- torically marginalized groups experience the school day, and how they are perceived by school personnel, fellow stu- dents, and themselves. In turn, scholars can provide a more thoughtful description of the context in which their studies take place (Awad et al., 2016). Researchers can also con- sider, and explicitly acknowledge, their own positionality in their research. Editors might prioritize the publication of studies that consider the cultural, racial, and economic cli- mates in which motivation research has been conducted. Culturally Responsive and Relevant Educational Practice The Culturally Responsive and Relevant Educational Prac- tice (CRRE) framework described by Kumar et al. (this issue) posits that meaningful learning occurs when (a) the
  • 57. content to be learned reflects students’ cultural diversity, (b) the process of learning aligns with individual and cul- tural modes of learning, and (c) the school’s sociocultural context is in harmony with the home’s sociocultural con- text. From one perspective, CRRE seems to advance a set of pedagogical recommendations that are based on the prin- ciples of cognitive science and information-processing the- ory (Urdan & Bruchmann, this issue). Connecting to one’s culture in meaningful ways is a powerful way to enrich cog- nitive processing, learning, attention, and underlying meta- cognitive processes (e.g., attention, self-regulation, interest, competence, engagement), which subsequently enhance motivation (Mayer, 2012). This is likely why Ladson-Bill- ings (1995), foreshadowing the sentiments expressed in this special issue, noted that culturally responsive teaching is “just good teaching!” (p. 159). From another perspective, however, the strong claims made by CRRE scholars are not fully supported by psycho-
  • 58. logical research. For example, some claim that educational practices in the United States fail to provide a meaningful learning experience for students of color. Although this is true in one respect, it bears noting that critical race theory would not exist if students who were forced to sit through years of “imperialist, White supremacist, patriarchal” mes- saging were incapable of seeing meaning in and beyond it (hooks, 2016). From a psychological perspective, freedom confers the possibility of becoming conscious of how we have been educated, thereby marking “the essential differ- ence between conditioned and determined existence” (Freire, 1998, p. 54). Insights from both cognitive science SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 139 and critical education acknowledge humans as meaning makers, capable of detecting discrepancies in their environ- ment. These distinctly human capacities enable learners to exercise agency in their own lives and to challenge the sta-
  • 59. tus quo (Bandura, 1986). This fact does not dismiss the important realization brought to light by CRRE scholars that the American educational system, on the whole, vali- dates and reinforces Eurocentric viewpoints as “correct,” thereby marginalizing the viewpoints of members of the global majority (i.e., people of color). By juxtaposing CRRE with motivation research, Kumar et al. (this issue) effectively dispel the myth of cultural and racial neutrality and show how schools and curricula perpetuate inequality and undermine motivation. What, then, can teachers and schools do to redirect (his- torically Eurocentric and racist) educational policies so that they are inclusive of all learners? Several contributors to this special issue contend that, to be maximally motivating, pedagogical approaches should be culturally reflective, cul- turally aligned, and harmonious between home and school. They describe a handful of research studies demonstrating how authentic attempts by educators to legitimize those
  • 60. who have been historically “othered” have improved students’ achievement, engagement, and motivation. This is certainly a first step. But the preceding recommendations raise many questions. For instance, how and to what extent shall the content to be learned reflect each student’s cultural heritage? How culturally aligned must the learning pro- cesses be (and with whose culture)? What indication will teachers have that sufficient home-school harmony is reached? Several contributors describe “culturally valued” instructional approaches (e.g., hip-hop) that teachers have used to connect with their students. But, as Urdan and Bruchmann (this issue) point out, well-intended approaches for enhancing culturally relevance can, when poorly applied, promote racial and ethnic stereotypes or come off as insensitive or offensive. Such approaches might also be dismissive of within-group differences. The ultimate effec- tiveness of any approach in promoting learning and motiva-
  • 61. tion depends on numerous factors, such as the school context, each learner’s demeanor, the with-it-ness and art- istry of the teacher, the quality of the student–teacher rela- tionship, and the intersecting identities of all involved (DiAngelo & Sensoy, 2010). Each of these can be interest- ing points of inquiry in culturally relevant teaching and its impact on student motivation. Another way that school environments can be culturally responsive is by embracing difference while creating inclu- sivity. Such learning environments communicate a sense that “you are at home here and free to express yourself as unique.” Gray et al. (this issue) refer to this as creating opportunities for cultural distinctiveness. In this way, belonging is nurtured by schools and teachers who help learners feel connected to and affirmed in their cultural heritage and yet distinct as individuals. Education philoso- pher Maxine Greene (1993) observed that “the more contin- uous and authentic personal encounters can be, the less
  • 62. likely it will be for categorizing and distancing to take place” (p. 13). Gray et al. (this issue) and Kumar et al. (this issue) propose several ways in which this can happen, including cultural representation in curricula and cultural diversity among school personnel. They suggest that stu- dents feel more empowered when they perceive their teach- ers as critical, reflective thinkers who are willing to invite alternative viewpoints in their classrooms. These opportuni- ties foster legitimacy, validation, esteem, and compatibility, all of which can increase achievement motivation. Another way in which schools can legitimize students as citizens is by providing opportunities for authentic engage- ment within the broader community (Gray et al., this issue). The link between students’ participatory civic engagement and academic motivation suggests that authentic engage- ment can be viewed as an “emancipatory” practice by enhancing students’ sense of efficacy, social connectedness, and altruism. Investigating the mechanisms by which
  • 63. students’ motivation is changed by civic engagement (and vice versa) provides an interesting path forward. Clarifying the Role of Cultural Competence Kumar et al. (this issue) contend that cultivating teachers’ and students’ cultural competence can enhance academic motivation and competence. They suggest that culturally competent teachers will be familiar with the history, cus- toms, and values of the cultural groups represented in their school and show awareness of how culture informs their own and others’ thoughts, emotions, and behaviors. The antidote to cultural incompetence, they argue, is critical self-reflection, which involves questioning one’s own val- ues, assumptions, and ways of understanding the world. These are laudable pursuits that raise further questions. First, how do learners, educators, researchers, and policy- makers engage in honest, critical self-reflection? Can they do so alone? Second, who decides when self-reflection has been sufficiently critical? What are its consequences? For
  • 64. instance, critical self-reflection might engender a level of paralyzing shame or guilt. It can also provoke fear and anger that deepen racial divides. Is it enough, for example, for a White woman to understand her power and privilege as she enters her classroom? This seems to fulfill the requirements of cultural competency that the authors put forth, but it may still be insufficient. As Sue (2017) argued, “The transformation to developing cultural competence and becoming a White ally is more than a cognitive or intellec- tual exercise. Lived experience and lived reality are crucial elements” (p. 712). Some researchers have begun to investigate the types of lived experiences that foster cultural awareness and under- standing. Teacher education programs can provide 140 USHER opportunities (e.g., field experiences) for teachers to inter- act with individuals’ different from them and to reflect on
  • 65. their own cultural frames of reference. Indeed, researchers have begun to investigate the relationship between teachers’ experiences with culturally diverse students, exposure to multicultural education curricula, instructional practices, and motivational approaches (Kumar & Lauer- mann, 2018). As the collective voices in this special issue well note, neither culture nor cultural competence is static. As one or both change, new frictions, joys, challenges, and opportuni- ties emerge. One must search again to rediscover self and other in new contexts. This means that cultural competence can neither be “achieved” nor “maintained” as some have claimed. Because culture is inherently social, the adequacy of one’s cultural knowledge and practice depends on the particulars of one’s social environment. The Motivational Impact of Being Seen Learners who are members of historically marginalized groups often describe the experience of being invisible,
  • 66. silenced, and even erased. The teacher–student relation- ship is a daily occasion for the affirmation (or denial) of one’s personhood. The harmful consequences of being overlooked or outcast have been well documented by educational and developmental psychologists (Brown, 2017). What, then, might be the corollaries of being seen? The Zulu greeting sawubona, literally meaning, “I/We see you,” implies much more than these three words suggest. It offers an acknowledgment from one ancestral viewpoint to another, establishing the observer as witness not only to the other’s presence but simulta- neously to one’s own (Bishop, n.d.). This serves as an invitation and agreement to reciprocal participation in the life of another person. The response, ngikhona, acknowledges, “I am here,” or more precisely, “Until you saw me, I didn’t exist.” Being fully seen by another serves as a reflection of one’s own inner capacities and as a recognition of one’s connection to another, even
  • 67. another whose plight is unknown or different. It recog- nizes that one’s well-being is connected to another’s. In this way, to see others fully is the cornerstone of a socially just pedagogy. This idea offers a ready connection between one of the foundational assumptions of CRRE (e.g., teacher as warm demander) and need-based motivation frameworks (e.g., need for relatedness). Noddings’s (2013) notion of critical care describes just this type of interconnection. I do not need to establish a deep, lasting, time-consuming personal relationship with every student. What I must do is to be totally and nonselectively present to the student—to each student—as he addresses me. The time interval may be brief but the encounter is total. (p. 180) Being seen involves a brief but total encounter. Warmth is conveyed by the teacher’s willingness to see the student entirely and nonjudgmentally and to attribute to the student the best intent. CRRE has emphasized the
  • 68. role of teacher care in helping all learners succeed (Kumar et al., this issue), but I am not convinced that commonly used measures of care adequately capture this deeper sense of respectful connection. I urge researchers to consider ways to assess students’ percep- tions of being seen in this fuller sense and then to examine its connection to motivation and well-being. Coming Full Circle In his treatise on culturalizing educational psychology, Pajares (2007) described the tension many social scientists experience when describing particulars at the expense of advancing universals, and vice versa. Savvy educational psychologists have learned to pad their theoretical frame- works and interpreted findings with contextual caveats. But as writer Wendell Berry (2000) lamented, even this recon- ciliation is incomplete: However generous the acknowledgment of context, the results of the research still cannot be applied both generally
  • 69. and sensitively. Finally it is “brought home” to a specific community of persons and creatures in a specific place. If it is then applied in its abstract or generalized or marketable form, it will obscure the uniqueness of the subject persons or creatures or places, or of their community, and this sort of application is almost invariably destructive. (p. 147) As we must situate and contextualize our research endeavors, we must also admit that we ourselves are socially, historically, and contextually situated. Keeping this in mind as we write for others, we can show proper dis- cernment in the application of theoretical knowledge to human lives and systems (Jung, 1945/1998). In one of his least cited but most insightful talks, “On a Certain Blindness in Human Beings,” William James (1899/2001) observed that “neither the whole of truth nor the whole of good is revealed to any single observer, although each observer gains a partial superiority of insight from the peculiar position in which he stands” (p. 129). All
  • 70. too often, and even within the pages of this special issue, we assume that our findings or our experiences reflect real- ity, neglecting to acknowledge the limits of our particular vantage point (Oyserman, 2017). We have gathered the data, invested our time, controlled for certain factors, and dutifully responded to reviewers. At each tick of the clock, it is easier to become entrenched in our belief that our grasp on reality is complete. However, we would do better to acknowledge our ignorance and to ask, “How [can we] act well—sensitively, compassionately, without irreparable SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 141 damage—on the basis of partial knowledge?” (Berry, 2000, p. 147). As a field, we could stand to be humbler and more nuanced. For White researchers, this means recognizing the unearned privileges that allow us to remain blind to the experiences of non-White individuals with little personal or
  • 71. professional consequence. Acknowledging the Whiteness of our approaches need not lead to paralysis, however. Even “incremental steps taken in confrontation of White- ness will help uncover deeper aspects of the problem and suggest further appropriate responses” (powell, 2012, p. 84). Thus is our challenge as educational psychologists. CODA During my years in graduate school, I frequently walked by the office of Professor Jackie Irvine, the same profes- sor who encouraged me to confront my Whiteness on the first day of her graduate seminar. On her door was plastered a lone purple sticker bearing the large-font words of Audre Lorde (1984): “Your silence will not protect you” (p. 41). Lorde’s words and Irvine’s instruc- tion serve as reminders that to engage in our work as if blind to race and color is to prolong racial injustice. Instead, the call is to engage in antiracism by striving “to change the norms and practices that allow racism to
  • 72. exist” in schools and in our research (Bonilla-Silva, 2018, p. 243). I would like to commend the authors of this special issue for their willingness to address the ways in which racism and White supremacy have affected research on academic motivation, particularly in the United States. Such courage, accompanied by a “sense of moral outrage and urgency on behalf of all children” (Irvine, 1999, p. 252), will be necessary for researchers and practitioners as they, in their unique ways, seek to help all learners thrive. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would not have been able to write this commentary without the education I received from faculty and col- leagues from the Division of Educational Studies at Emory University who broadened and challenged my perspectives on sociohistorical and psychological sys- tems of power and privilege. I am grateful to Professors Jackie Irvine, Vanessa Siddle Walker, Carole Hahn, and
  • 73. Frank Pajares. I thank my current colleagues, Kenneth Tyler and Christia Brown, for their valuable insights on this work. I am fortunate to work with a stellar team of undergraduate and graduate students who, on a daily basis, help me think about how to conduct motivation research in ways more relevant to learners from histori- cally marginalized groups. Special thanks to Isaac Hayes, Abigail Love, John Eric Lingat, and Laura Page, for their comments. Finally, to my mom, Dr. Jody Usher, for her unwavering commitment to helping me (and many others) confront fear and use my voice to promote justice. ORCID Ellen L. Usher http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9301-8987 REFERENCES American Psychological Association, Presidential Task Force on Educa- tional Disparities. (2012). Ethnic and racial disparities in education:
  • 74. Psychology’s contributions to understanding and reducing disparities. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ed/resources/racial- disparities.aspx Andersen, M. L. (2003). Whitewashing race: A critical perspective on whiteness. In A. W. Doane & E. Bonilla-Silva (Eds.), White out: The continuing significance of racism (pp. 21–35). New York, NY: Routledge. Awad, G. H., Patall, E. A., Rackley, K. R., & Reilly, E. D. (2016). Recom- mendations for culturally sensitive research methods. Journal of Educa- tional and Psychological Consultation, 26, 283–303. doi:10.1080/ 10474412.2015.1046600 Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Banks, J. A. (1997). Educating citizens in a multicultural society. New
  • 75. York, NY: Teachers College Press. Berliner, D. C. (2013). Inequality, poverty, and the socialization of America’s youth for the responsibilities of citizenship. Theory Into Practice, 52, 203–209. doi:10.1080/00405841.2013.804314 Berry, W. (2000). Life is a miracle: An essay against modern superstition. Washington, DC: Counterpoint. Bigler, R. S., Hayes, A. R., & Patterson, M. M. (2017). Social striving: Social group membership and children’s motivations and competencies. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of compe- tence and motivation: Theory and application (2nd ed., pp. 547– 565). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Bishop, O. (n.d.). Global Oneness Project: Sawubona [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.globalonenessproject.org/library/interviews/sawubo na
  • 76. Bonilla-Silva, E. (2018). Racism without racists: Colorblind racism and the persistence of racial inequality in the United States (5th ed.). Lan- ham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. Boykin, W. (1986). The triple quandary and the schooling of Afro-Ameri- can children. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority children: New perspectives (pp. 56–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brown, C. S. (2017). Discrimination in childhood and adolescence: A developmental intergroup approach. New York, NY: Routledge. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bruner, J. (1996). Culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- sity Press. Ceci, S. J., Kahan, D. M., & Braman, D. (2010). The WEIRD are even weirder than you think: Diversifying contexts is as important as diversi-
  • 77. fying samples. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 87–88. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X10000063 Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64, 194–204. doi:10.1037/a0015308 Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310. doi:10.1126/science.1128317 142 USHER Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., & Goyer, J. P. (2017). Turning point: Targeted, tailored, and timely psychological intervention. In A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, & D. S. Yeager (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva- tion: Theory and application (2nd ed., pp. 657–686). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • 78. Cokley, K. (2007). Critical issues in the measurement of ethnic and racial identity: A referendum on the state of the field. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 224–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.3.224 Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. American Psychologist, 64, 170–180. doi:10.1037/a0014564 Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity poli- tics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review, 43, 1241–1299. doi:10.2307/1229039 Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychol- ogy. American Psychologist, 30, 116–127. doi:10.1037/h0076829 Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio,
  • 79. TX: American Educational Research Association. DeCuir-Gunby, J. T. (2009). A review of the racial identity develop- ment of African American adolescents: The role of education. Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 103–124. doi:10.3102/ 0034654308325897 DeCuir-Gunby, J. T., & Schutz, P. A. (2014). Researching race within edu- cational psychology contexts. Educational Psychologist, 49, 244–260. doi:10.1080/00461520.2014.957828 DeCuir-Gunby, J. T., & Schutz, P. A. (Eds.). (2017). Race and ethnicity in the study of motivation in education. New York, NY: Routledge. Devos, T., Huynh, Q. -L., & Banaji, M. R. (2012). Implicit self and iden- tity. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and iden- tity (2nd ed., pp. 155–179). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. Boston, MA: D.C. Heath.
  • 80. DiAngelo, R., & Sensory, €O. (2010). OK, I get it! Now tell me how to do it!”: Why we can’t just tell you how to do critical multicultural educa- tion. Multicultural Perspectives, 12, 97–102. doi:10.1080/ 15210960.2010.481199 Du Bois, W. E. B. (1994). Souls of Black folk. Chicago, IL: Dover. (Origi- nal work published 1903) Dweck, C. S. (2017). From needs to goals and representations: Foundations for a unified theory of motivation, personality, and development. Psy- chological Review, 124, 689–719. doi:10.1037/rev0000082 Eisner, E. W. (1998). The englightened eye: Qualitative inquiry and the enhancement of educational practice. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. (1986). Black students’ school success: Coping with the “burden of ‘acting white.’” Urban Review, 18, 176– 206.
  • 81. doi:10.1007/BF01112192 Frankl, V. (1959). Man’s search for meaning. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Freire, P. (1998). Pedagogy of freedom: Ethics, democracy, and civic cour- age. Oxford, UK: Rowman & Littlefield. Galliher, R. V., McLean, K. C., & Syed, M. (2017). An integrated develop- mental model for studying identity content in context. Developmental Psychology, 53, 2011–2022. doi:10.1037/dev0000299 Gilligan, C. (1993). In a different voice: Psychological theory and wom- en’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gorski, P. C. (2016). Poverty and the ideological imperative: A call to unhook from deficit and grit ideology and to strive for structural ideol- ogy in teacher education. Journal of Education for Teaching, 42, 378– 386. doi:10.1080/02607476.2016.1215546 Graham, S. (1992). “Most of the subjects were White and
  • 82. middle class”: Trends in published research on African Americans in selected APA journals, 1970–1989. American Psychologist, 47, 629–639. doi:10.1037/ 0003-066X.47.5.629 Graham, S. (2017). Discussant. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX: American Educational Research Association. Graham, S. (this issue). Race/ethnicity and social adjustment of adoles- cents: How (not if) school diversity matters. Educational Psychologist, 53. doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1428805 Gray, D. L., Hope, E. C., & Matthews, J. S. (this issue). Black and belong- ing at school: A case for interpersonal, instructional, and institutional opportunity structures. Educational Psychologist, 53.
  • 83. doi:10.1080/ 00461520.2017.1421466 Greene, M. (1993). The passions of pluralism: Multiculturalism and the expanding community. Educational Researcher, 22(1), 13–18. Hand, V., & Gresalfi, M. (2015). The joint accomplishment of identity. Educational Psychologist, 50, 190–203. doi:10.1080/00461520.2015. 1075401 Hargons, C., Mosley, D., Falconer, J., Faloughi, R., Singh, A., Cokley, K., & Stevens-Watkins, D. (2017). Black lives matter: A call to action for counseling psychology leaders. The Counseling Psychologist, 45, 873– 901. doi:10.1177/0011000017733048 Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Scien- ces, 33, 111–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725
  • 84. hooks, b. (2010). Teaching critical thinking: Practical wisdom. New York, NY: Routledge. hooks, b. (2016 April). A conversation on race and education in the 21st century. Berea, KY: Opening plenary address at the Dialogue on Race and Education in the 21st Century, Berea College. Hughes, D. L., Del Toro, J., & Way, N. (2017). Interrelations among dimensions of ethnic-racial identity during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 53, 2139–2153. doi:10.1037/dev0000401 Irvine, J. J. (1999). The education of children whose nightmares come both day and night. Journal of Negro Education, 68, 244–253. doi:10.2307/ 2668099 James, W. (2001). Talks to teachers on psychology and to students on some of life’s ideals. Mineola, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1899) Jung, C. G. (1998). Man in his relation to others. In J. Jacobi
  • 85. (Ed.), Psychological reflections: An anthology of Jung’s writings, 1905–1961 (pp. 81–182). London, UK: Routledge. (Original work published 1945) Juvonen, J., Kogachi, K., & Graham, S. (2017). When and how do students benefit from ethnic diversity in middle school? Child Development. doi:10.1111/cdev.12834 Kentucky Department of Education. (2017). School report card. Retrieved from http://applications.education.ky.gov/src/Default.aspx Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419– 449. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357 Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolution (2nd ed.). Chi- cago: University of Chicago Press. Kumar, R., & Lauermann, F. (2018). Cultural beliefs and instructional
  • 86. intentions: Do experiences in teacher education institutions matter? American Educational Research Journal. Advance online publica- tion. doi:10.3102/0002831217738508 Kumar, R., Zusho, A., & Bondie, R. (this issue). Weaving cultural rele- vance and achievement motivation into inclusive classroom cultures. Educational Psychologist, 53. doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1432361 Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The case for cul- turally relevant pedagogy. Theory Into Practice, 34, 159–165. doi:10.1080/00405849509543675 Lorde, A. (1984). Sister outsider: Essays and speeches. Trumansburg, NY: Crossing Press. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Mayer, R. E. (2012). Information processing. In K. R. Harris, S.
  • 87. Graham, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook, Vol. 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 85–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. SEEKING CULTURAL RELEVANCE 143 McInerney, D. M., & Liem, G. A. D. (Eds.). (2018). Big theories revisited 2: A volume of research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning. Charlotte, NC: Information Age. McKown, C., & Strambler, M. J. (2009). Developmental antecedents and social and academic consequences of stereotype-consciousness in mid- dle childhood. Child Development, 80, 1643–1659. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2009.01359.x Miller, J. G., Goyal, N., & Wice, M. (2017). A cultural psychology of agency:
  • 88. Morality, motivation, and reciprocity. Perspectives on Psychological Sci- ence, 12, 867–875. doi:10.1177/1745691617706099 Morris, M. W., Chiu, C-Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631–659. doi:10.1146/annurev- psych-010814-015001 Nathan, L. F. (2017). When grit isn’t enough: A high school principal examines how poverty and inequality thwart the college-for-all promise. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Noddings, N. (2013). Caring: A relational approach to ethics and moral education (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Nolen, S. B., Horn, I. S., & Ward, C. J. (2015). Situating motivation. Educa- tional Psychologist, 50, 234–247. doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.1075399 Oyserman, D. (2008). Racial-ethnic self-schemas: Multidimensional iden- tity-based motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,
  • 89. 1186– 1198. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.003 Oyserman, D. (2017). Culture three ways: Culture and subcultures within countries. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 435–463. doi:10.1146/ annurev-psych-122414-033617 Pajares, F. (2007). Culturalizing educational psychology. In F. Salili & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Culture, motivation, and learning (pp. 19–42). Char- lotte, NC: Information Age. Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2006). The self and academic motivation: Theory and research after the cognitive revolution. In J. M. Royer (Ed.), The impact of the cognitive revolution on educational psychology (pp. 165–198). Greenwich, CT: Information Age. Phinney, J. (1992). The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure: A new scale for use with adolescents and young adults from diverse groups. Journal
  • 90. of Adolescent Research, 7, 156–176. doi:10.1177/074355489272003 powell, j. a. (2012). Racing to justice: Transforming our conceptions of self and other to build an inclusive society. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Rjosk, C., Richter, D., L€udtke, O., & Eccles, J. S. (2017). Ethnic composi- tion and heterogeneity in the classroom: Their measurement and rela- tionship with student outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 1188–1204. doi:10.1037/edu0000185 Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psy- chological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Schlenker, B. R. (2012). Self-presentation. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed., pp. 542–570). New York,
  • 91. NY: Guilford Press. Schunk, D. H. (2017). Motivation and social cognitive theory. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX: American Educational Research Association. Sellers, R. M., Rowley, S. A. J., Chavous, T. M., Shelton, J. N., & Smith, M. A. (1997). Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity: A prelimi- nary investigation of reliability and construct validity. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 73, 805–815. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.73.4.805 Sue, D. W. (2017). The challenges of becoming a White ally. The Counsel- ing Psychologist, 45, 706–716. doi:10.1177/0011000017719323 Turner, J. C., & Nolen, S. B. (2015). Introduction: The relevance of the sit- uative perspective in educational psychology. Educational
  • 92. Psychologist, 50, 167–172. doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.1075404 Tyler, K. M., Burris, J. L., & Coleman, S. T. (2016). Investigating the asso- ciation between home-school dissonance and disruptive classroom behaviors for urban middle school students. The Journal of Early Ado- lescence. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0272431616678987 Uma~na-Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W. E., Rivas-Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., . . . Seaton, E. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization. Child Development, 85(1), 21–39. doi:10.1111/ cdev.12196 Urdan, T. C. (2017). Achievement goal theory: Where it came from and where it is going. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders
  • 93. and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX: American Educational Research Association. Urdan, T., & Bruchmann, K. (this issue). Examining the academic motiva- tion of a diverse student population: A consideration of methodology. Educational Psychologist, 53. doi:10.1080/00461520.2018.1440234 Verkuyten, M. (2016). Further conceptualizing ethnic and racial identity research: The social identity approach and its dynamic model. Child Development, 87, 1796–1812. doi:10.1111/cdev.12555 Weiner, B. (2017). Attribution theory. In A. C. Koenka & A. L. Wigfield (Chairs), Motivation theory yesterday, today, and tomorrow: Reflections of founders and descendants (Symposium). San Antonio, TX: American Educational Research Association. Woodson, C. G. (1990). The mis-education of the negro.
  • 94. Trenton, NJ: First Africa World Press. (Original work published 1933) Zusho, A., & Clayton, K. (2011). Culturalizing achievement goal theory and research. Educational Psychologist, 46, 239–260. doi:10.1080/ 00461520.2011.614526 144 USHER HR Mgmt. Week 9 Discussion Decisions: Motivation vs. Ability (20 points) Discuss whether ability or motivation is more important in selecting the right person for the right job. Explain your rationale. HR Mgmt. Week 10 Discussion Overcoming the Challenges Associated with Employee Feedback (10 points) Assess the challenges of managers providing accurate, timely, and effective feedback to employees. Recommend how managers can overcome any two (2) of the challenges you identified. Making a Big Positive Impact on an Organization (10 points) Of the concepts covered in the textbook, determine which, if implemented well, would have the greatest impact on an
  • 95. organization. Provide a rationale with your response.