SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 48
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES
VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007, PAGES 111–
120
Teaching Cause–Effect Text
Structure Through Social
Studies Content to At-Risk
Second Graders
Joanna P. Williams, Abigail M. Nubla-Kung, Simonne Pollini,
K. Brooke Stafford,
Amaya Garcia, and Anne E. Snyder
Abstract
This study evaluated the effectiveness of a comprehension
program integrated with social studies instruction designed for
at-risk second
graders. The program included instruction in cause–effect text
structure, emphasizing clue words, generic questions, graphic
organizers,
and the close analysis of specially constructed cause–effect
target paragraphs. This program was compared (a) to a content-
only program
that focused only on social studies and did not include text
structure instruction and (b) to a no-instruction control. Fifteen
classroom
teachers, randomly assigned to treatment, provided the
instruction. The program improved the comprehension of
instructional cause–
effect texts, and there were transfer effects on some
comprehension measures. The performance of the 2 instructed
groups did not differ
on any of the content measures, indicating that such integrated
instruction can be accomplished without a loss in the amount of
content
acquired. This study supports our previous findings on the
effectiveness of explicit instruction at the primary-grade level.
G
iven mounting pressures to en-
sure that students achieve high
levels of proficiency on stan-
dardized tests, many school districts
have begun to increase the amount of
time spent on reading and mathemat-
ics in the elementary school classroom.
Consequently, instruction in other sub-
ject areas has been significantly re-
duced (Vogler, 2003). In a survey of 299
nationally representative school dis-
tricts, for example, the Center on Edu-
cation Policy (2006) found that 71% of
these districts have reduced the amount
of time spent on teaching at least one
or more of the content areas.
Social studies is one content area
that has been frequently cut back, with
33% of school districts examined by the
Center on Education Policy (2006) re-
porting a significant reduction in the
portion of the school day spent on
social studies. This is unfortunate, as
there are many skills, such as critical
thinking, decision making, and prob-
lem solving, that can be acquired while
learning social studies content (Ferretti
& Okolo, 1996). Social studies instruc-
tion also encourages young learners to
consider the role that history plays in
shaping our communities (Brophy &
Alleman, 2002) and, perhaps most im-
portant, helps them to develop civic
competence (Ferretti & Okolo, 1996).
The extra time allotted to reading
in the elementary curriculum has been
greeted enthusiastically in most quar-
ters. However, even with the addi-
tional time allotments, there is a notable
deficit in today’s reading instruction:
There is a lack of focused and explicit
instruction in reading expository text
(Hoffman et al., 1994). In an observa-
tional study of 20 first-grade class-
rooms, Duke (2000) found that infor-
mational text was rarely used in
reading and writing activities. In the
classroom libraries she examined, fewer
than 10% of the books available to
students included informational text.
Similarly, a national survey by Camp-
bell, Kapinus, and Beatty (1995) of ele-
mentary classrooms indicated that few
if any of the books read aloud to stu-
dents include expository text.
Expository text is generally re-
garded as more difficult than narrative
text (Goldman & Rakestraw, 2000). Its
content is often unfamiliar and fre-
quently represents complex and ab-
stract concepts (Stein & Trabasso, 1981).
Moreover, expository text contains va-
rious text structures that can be diffi-
cult to recognize and interpret. These
structures include sequence, description,
compare–contrast, problem–solution,
and cause–effect (Meyer & Poon, 2001),
and they can be presented in isolation
or in conjunction with other structures.
Of the structures that can be found in
expository text, the cause–effect struc-
ture may be particularly difficult for
children (Ciardiello, 2002). Richgels,
McGee, Lomax, and Sheard (1987)
found that sixth graders were less able
to comprehend causation text structure
than other text structures. McCormick
(2003) has suggested that this structure
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES112
is difficult because of the complex
comprehension skills (i.e., making in-
ferences, judging sequence, and mak-
ing predictions) involved in the inter-
pretation of causes and effects.
By the time students reach fourth
grade, much of the content that they
encounter in the classroom is pre-
sented in the form of expository text
(Moss, 2005). Without an adequate un-
derstanding of the structures inherent
in expository text and the strategies
needed to comprehend the text that is
organized within these structures,
many students struggle with this
rather sudden shift to informational
texts (Duke, 2000). Evidence of this
struggle is highlighted by the well-
documented slump in fourth-grade
reading achievement (Chall, Jacobs, &
Baldwin, 1990).
In social studies instruction, as
much as 90% of the content that is
taught is derived from and structured
around informational text from text-
books (Myers & Savage, 2005). In fact,
many educators view textbooks as the
primary source of the social studies
curriculum (Brophy, 1992). Unfortu-
nately, these texts are often dense,
poorly structured, and riddled with ir-
relevant information that can distract
readers from more important concepts
(Beck, McKeown, & Gromoll, 1989).
Students are not given the tools with
which to read such text, and without
adequate skills, students—particularly
those with learning disabilities (LD)
and other students at risk for academic
failure—may be unable to access and
comprehend the information that is
presented to them (De La Paz & Mac-
Arthur, 2003).
There is evidence that through ex-
plicit instruction in text structure, low-
achieving students can develop and
refine their ability to comprehend ex-
pository text. Bakken, Mastropieri, and
Scruggs (1997) found that eighth-grade
students with LD who were explicitly
taught to apply specific strategies to-
ward the reading of expository text
were better able to recall important in-
formation from the text. Dickson (1999)
found that instruction in compare–
contrast structure was effective for
middle school students with a range of
learning needs. Only recently have
there been studies indicating that ele-
mentary school students, too, can ben-
efit from such instruction. Our own
work (Williams, Hall, & Lauer, 2004;
Williams et al., 2005) has demonstrated
the effectiveness of explicit instruction
in helping second graders at risk for
academic failure to improve their read-
ing comprehension of compare–contrast
text via the use of text structure. More-
over, these studies indicated that em-
bedding text structure instruction in
content lessons (on science content)
did not diminish the amount of content
acquired.
Present Study
We have developed a program that
provides instruction on a different text
structure, cause–effect, embedded in a
different subject matter domain, social
studies. Will our previous findings
hold? That is, will our instructional
program help second graders at risk
for academic failure to improve their
comprehension? Can this goal be ac-
complished without a decrease in the
amount of content knowledge that stu-
dents acquire from the program?
As in our previous work, we used
an explicit and structured instructional
model that included explanation and
modeling by the teacher and guided
and then independent practice (Wil-
liams, 1998), because low-achieving
students benefit from systematic and
intensive instruction (Gersten, Fuchs,
Williams, & Baker, 2001). Students
were taught to use clue words, generic
cause–effect questions, and graphic
organizers to help them identify and
analyze causes and effects in specially
constructed, well-structured target para-
graphs. As Goldman and Rakestraw
(2000) noted, readers are better able to
understand text when it has a clearly
defined structure.
Within each lesson of our pro-
gram, children were taught key vocab-
ulary words. Learning domain-specific
vocabulary allows students to become
familiar with the language of social
studies and to develop a level of ex-
pertise in the subject matter (Bryant,
Ugel, Thompson, & Hamff, 1999). The
social studies content taught in the
program was aligned with New York
state learning standards in social stud-
ies and English language arts. It cov-
ered three historical U.S. communities
(colonists, pioneers, and immigrants at
the turn of the 20th century). Each
community was presented with a focus
on the same three features: homes,
schools, and jobs. The program also
provided students with exposure to in-
formational trade books related to the
social studies content that was taught.
Informational trade books can provide
valuable sources of well-written and
authentic text that appeals to young
readers (Moss, 2005).
Most second-grade instruction in-
volves a great deal of speaking and lis-
tening, because many students at that
level are not yet fluent readers. We in-
cluded in our program a typical second-
grade mix of listening/speaking and
reading/writing tasks. The same cog-
nitive processes are basic to both lis-
tening and reading comprehension
(Perfetti, Marron, & Foltz, 1996), and
our goal was to improve comprehen-
sion as demonstrated in either oral or
written language.
This study evaluated the effec-
tiveness of our cause–effect instruc-
tional program for second graders at
risk for academic failure. We randomly
assigned intact classrooms (a) to our
text structure program, (b) to a com-
parison content-only program that used
the same materials as the text structure
program but that did not focus on the
cause–effect structure of the text, or
(c) to a no-instruction control.
Method
Design
We used a pretest–posttest design. Fif-
teen classroom teachers from three ele-
mentary schools in New York City vol-
VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 113
unteered to participate in the study.
The three schools were of similar de-
mographics; all three were categorized
as Title I schools. The total enrollment
across the schools included 76.5% His-
panic, 22% African American, 0.5% Eu-
ropean American, and 1% Asian or
other. Ninety-three percent of the stu-
dents received state aid in the form of
free or reduced lunch, and approxi-
mately 5% of the students were en-
rolled in either part-time or full-time
special education services.
The teachers were randomly as-
signed to one of three experimental
conditions: a text structure program
(n = 5), a content-only program (n = 5),
or a no-instruction control (n = 5). Con-
ditions were blocked by school. All sta-
tistical analyses were performed with
the classroom as the unit of analysis.
Participants
Three hundred thirty-seven students
were eligible for participation, and 260
of these students returned consent
forms; 243 students completed the
study (the other 17 students were reas-
signed to other classrooms or moved
out of the school district during the
study). All students in the 10 instruc-
tional classrooms (n = 160) received the
instructional program as taught by
their classroom teacher. We randomly
selected 12 students from each class-
room for the statistical analysis. In one
case, only 11 students completed the
posttests; we added as a 12th score the
mean score of these 11 students. Fifteen
of the 179 students had Individualized
Education Programs (IEPs), and 5 stu-
dents had been referred for IEPs. Table
1 presents demographic information
about the 179 students.
Materials
The content goal of the instructional
programs was to teach students about
three historical communities in the
United States—specifically, about
homes, schools, and jobs in these com-
munities. Both programs used biogra-
phies, other trade books, and specially
constructed cause–effect target para-
graphs. Selection of the books was
based on appropriateness of content
and quality of photographs. Ten short,
well-structured cause–effect paragraphs
were written specifically for the pro-
gram. In the first unit of the program
(Colonists), each target paragraph con-
sisted of several causation sentences;
each sentence included one cause, one
effect, and a clue word. In the second
unit (Pioneers), each paragraph con-
tained one cause and multiple effects.
The cause and each effect appeared in
separate sentences. The third unit (Im-
migrants) included both types of para-
graphs. Examples of the two types of
paragraphs are as follows:
From Lesson 5: One cause–one ef-
fect paragraph:
Colonists and Their Schools
The first school that colonial children
went to was called Dame School.
Children had to read books written
for grown-ups because there were no
storybooks for kids. In school, there
were no pencils; therefore, children
wrote with a piece of lead. Girls
needed to learn how to spin, cook,
and clean the house; thus, they stayed
home after they finished Dame
School. After Dame School, most boys
continued to go to school, since the
law said they had to go.
From Lesson 14: One cause–
multiple effects paragraph (this para-
graph also contained one sentence
with noncausal information.):
Pioneers and Their Jobs
When pioneers first moved to the
prairie, it was difficult to find a job.
Therefore, after the harvest, most
farmers had to travel away from their
families for a few months to work for
the railroad. Some men had construc-
tion jobs in nearby towns. Children
had to work by selling eggs. With the
money the families saved, they
bought leather, shoes, salt, and other
things they could not make them-
selves.
The readability level of the target
paragraphs was between third and
fourth grade (M = 3.40, SD = 0.84)
using the Dale-Chall Readability Scale
(Chall & Dale, 1995)—rather high, be-
cause some of the words in the text
(e.g., words relevant to the program
content and some of the clue words)
were unfamiliar words on the Dale-
Chall reading list. However, the stu-
dents in both the text structure and
TABLE 1
Demographic Characteristics and Reading Scores of Participants
by Condition
TS CO NI
Measure M SD M SD M SD
Gender
Male 27 29 22
Female 33 30 38
Age (years) 7.69 0.21 7.68 0.23 7.66 0.15
WRMT-R
Word Identification
Standard score 454.88 14.70 462.13 10.93 458.57 6.84
Grade equivalent 2.5 2.6 2.6
Passage Comprehension
Standard score 472.80 9.48 474.40 5.12 472.32 4.54
Grade equivalent 2.4 2.4 2.3
Total Reading Score
Standard score 463.84 12.01 468.23 8.03 465.44 5.06
Grade equivalent 2.4 2.5 2.4
Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only
condition; NI = no-instruction condition; WRMT-R =
Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (Woodcock, 1987).
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES114
content-only programs were quite fa-
miliar with these words because they
were taught as vocabulary words.
When we calculated the readability
level with our vocabulary words con-
sidered as familiar words, the mean
readability of the target paragraphs
was between second- and third-grade
level (M = 2.50, SD = 0.97).
Overview of the Two
Instructional Programs
Text Structure Program. The in-
structional program contained three
units, each of which focused on one
historical community. There were 22
lessons in all. First, an introductory les-
son introduced the concept of cause–
effect and also the program content
through a narrative book, On the Town:
A Community Adventure (Caseley, 2002),
which follows Charlie and his mother
as they explore their community. Then,
for each unit, there was a lesson that in-
troduced the community via a biogra-
phy of a young person living in that
community. This lesson included a
read-aloud and discussion and an in-
troduction to the relevant vocabulary.
There were six additional lessons in
each unit: two lessons each on homes,
schools, and jobs. Each pair of lessons
was organized around a target para-
graph and included further read-
alouds from a second trade book. The
first lesson on each feature included in-
struction on
1. Concept of cause and effect. The stu-
dents were introduced to the defi-
nition of cause and effect. Effect
was defined as a thing or event
that happens, and cause, as the per-
son, thing, or event that makes the
effect happen. Students developed
their understanding of these con-
cepts through picture cards,
matching, and cloze activities.
2. Cause–effect clue words. In Lesson 1,
students were introduced to four
cause–effect clue words: because,
since, therefore, and thus. As the pro-
gram continued, they memorized
the clue words and used them to
identify paragraphs as cause–effect
paragraphs.
3. Vocabulary. The vocabulary con-
cepts were related to the content
of the program: community, home,
school, job, pioneer, colonist, immi-
grant, sod house, keeping room, tene-
ment, schoolhouse, Dame School, pub-
lic school, farmer, blacksmith, and
sweatshop. The vocabulary words
were embedded in the target para-
graphs; they were also presented
as a list of words that were ex-
plained and illustrated through
examples.
4. Trade book read-aloud and discussion.
Specific passages from a trade
book were read aloud. Students
were encouraged to ask questions
and share comments.
5. Community chart. There was a com-
munity chart for each unit, which
was used for further review of the
content vocabulary. During the sec-
ond and third units, words from
previous charts were reviewed.
6. Cause–effect questions. Beginning in
Unit 2, with the introduction of the
second type of paragraph (one
cause–multiple effects), the stu-
dents learned two generic ques-
tions. These questions were de-
signed to help them focus on the
cause–effect information in the text
rather than on irrelevant details or
prior knowledge. The two ques-
tions were (a) What is the cause?
and (b) What is the effect?
7. Read-aloud and analysis of the target
paragraph. Students read the target
paragraph first silently and then
aloud. The class analyzed the
cause–effect structure of the first
type of paragraph (one cause–one
effect) by circling the clue word
and underlining the part of the
sentence that followed the clue
word (the cause or the effect).
Causes and cause clue words were
marked with a blue crayon, and
effects and effect clue words with a
green crayon. For the second type
of paragraph (one cause–multiple
effects), the students circled the
clue word and underlined what
followed it. Then they used the
cause–effect questions to guide the
analysis of the rest of the para-
graph. Sentences that included
noncausal information were
introduced in Lesson 12 and were
identified as irrelevant to the
causal analysis.
The second lesson on each feature
included instruction on
1. Graphic organizer. The students
completed a graphic organizer for
the target paragraph. There were
two organizers, one for each
paragraph type (see Chambliss &
Calfee, 1998, for examples). This
helped the students visually
organize the three elements (cause,
effect, and clue word) in a cause–
effect sentence.
2. Comprehension questions. Students
answered three types of questions
about the target paragraph. The
first was noncausal (e.g., How
many teachers worked in a school-
house?). The second type asked
about causes (e.g., Why did a
schoolhouse only have one
teacher?), and the third asked
about effects (e.g., What are the
effects of having very little money
for schools?). We showed the
students that they could find the
answers to the comprehension
questions in the completed graphic
organizer as well as in the target
paragraph. The students were
encouraged to answer in complete
sentences.
3. Lesson review. Each lesson ended
with a review of the strategies
(clue words, cause–effect ques-
tions, and graphic organizer) as
well as the content covered.
Content-Only Program. The con-
tent-only program taught the same so-
cial studies content and used the same
materials (target paragraphs, trade
books, and charts) as the text structure
program, but it did not focus on cause–
effect structure. As in the text structure
program, there were 22 lessons: one in-
VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 115
troductory lesson; three lessons intro-
ducing each historical community via
the biography; and two lessons on each
of the three features within each unit.
The introductory lesson focused
on the general content of the program
via On the Town: A Community Adven-
ture (Caseley, 2002). In Lesson 2, the
Unit 1 biography was presented; the
lesson included a read-aloud and dis-
cussion and an introduction to the vo-
cabulary. The first lesson on each fea-
ture included instruction on
1. KWL chart. Comparison lessons
began with a discussion of the
students’ background knowledge
about the community (Colonist,
Pioneer, or Immigrant) and the
feature (home, school, or job) that
were the foci of the lesson. This
was done through a KWL pro-
cedure (what I Know, what I Want
to know, and what I Learned; Ogle,
1986). The teacher recorded re-
sponses to what I Know about a
focus feature on a chart. Then the
students, with the help of the
teacher, generated questions about
what they wanted to know, and
the teacher recorded these. The
what I Learned portion of the chart
was completed after the trade book
read-aloud, the vocabulary, and the
community chart activity. At this
point, the students attempted to
answer the questions they had
generated at the beginning of the
lesson and added other details that
were discussed during the course
of the lesson.
2. Vocabulary. A review of the vocabu-
lary words was conducted as in the
text structure program.
3 Trade book read-aloud and discussion.
This was the same activity as in the
text structure program, and also
included further discussion
questions.
4. Community Chart. This was the
same activity as in the text struc-
ture program.
5. Graphic organizer (content). Students
filled in an information web. In the
center of the web was a labeled
depiction of a home, school, or
job, surrounded by several blank
circles connected by a line to the
center picture. Students filled in
the circles with information that
they learned during the lesson.
The second lesson on each feature
included instruction on
1. Read-aloud of the target paragraph.
Students read, first silently and
then aloud, the same target
cause–effect paragraphs that were
presented in the text structure
program; however, they did not
examine the text structure and
identify it as a cause–effect
paragraph. Rather, the paragraph
was presented to the students as
another opportunity to read and
learn about the community and
the feature that was targeted in
the lesson.
2. Comprehension questions. The stu-
dents in the content-only program
answered the same noncausal
questions that the students in the
text structure program answered.
As there were no causation ques-
tions, the students in the content-
only program also answered addi-
tional questions pertaining to the
information in the target para-
graph. The students were en-
couraged to answer in complete
sentences.
3. Journal entry. Students drew pic-
tures of something related to the
lesson’s topic (e.g., immigrant jobs)
and wrote a paragraph about their
drawing.
4. Lesson review. Each lesson con-
cluded with a review of the con-
tent learned in that lesson, includ-
ing the features learned about the
communities (home, school, and
job) and the vocabulary.
Procedure
Introducing the Program to Teach-
ers. Individual training sessions (30–
45 min) were held to familiarize teach-
ers with the program that they were to
teach. We discussed the program’s
overall goals and reviewed each sec-
tion of the lessons.
Pretest. The pretest, conducted
in two 30–45-min sessions, consisted of
audiotaped individual interviews. In
the first session, students completed
the Word Identification and Passage
Comprehension subtests of the Wood-
cock Reading Mastery Test–Revised
(WRMT-R), Form H (Woodcock, 1987).
The second pretest session contained
three strategy and two outcome mea-
sures. The strategy measures included
locating clue words in a paragraph, lo-
cating cause–effect clauses, and recall-
ing the cause–effect questions. The out-
come measures assessed knowledge of
vocabulary concepts and ability to pro-
vide well-structured cause–effect state-
ments (i.e., sentences using accurate in-
formation from the text, including a
clue word) in response to comprehen-
sion questions based on a one cause–
one effect paragraph.
Lesson Attendance and Feedback.
Teachers were asked to take atten-
dance and to complete feedback sheets
at the end of each lesson.
Classroom Observations. Obser-
vations were conducted in the 10 in-
structional program classrooms to as-
sess fidelity to treatment. Each of the 10
classrooms was observed twice over
the course of the instruction. Observers
recorded whether each lesson section
was completed. Three observers made
independent observations of the same
five lessons. Interrater reliability for
each of the observer pairs was 100%.
Thereafter, each observation was made
by a single observer.
Posttest. The posttest consisted
of individual, audiotaped interviews
and was conducted in two sessions of
30–45 min each. During one session,
the students completed Form G of the
Word Identification and Passage Com-
prehension subtests of the WRMT-R
(Woodcock, 1987). The other session
included an extensive array of strategy
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES116
and outcome measures. The strategy
measures included Locating Clue
Words; Underlining Clauses; Complet-
ing the Graphic Organizer for a one
cause–one effect paragraph; and Re-
calling the Cause–Effect Questions.
The content outcome measures as-
sessed Feature Questions (knowledge
of explicit information about the fea-
tures of the three historical communi-
ties); Non-Feature Questions (knowl-
edge of other content presented in the
target paragraphs); and Vocabulary
Definitions.
The comprehension outcome mea-
sures required students to answer
three types of questions (noncausal,
cause, and effect questions) concerning
a series of paragraphs that for the most
part involved social studies content.
Several of these measures required oral
responses:
• to a one cause–one effect paragraph
that had appeared during instruc-
tion (assessing the effects of Explicit
Teaching);
• to two novel one cause–multiple
effects paragraphs, one of which
contained information about an
untaught feature (games) of an
instructed community (Near
Transfer), whereas the other con-
tained information unrelated to the
program content (Far Transfer); and
• to two paragraphs, one of each
paragraph type, taken from less
well-structured trade books (Trans-
fer to Authentic Text).
Scoring. Scoring guidelines were
developed for each measure on the
basis of a small sample (n = 18) of ran-
domly selected protocols, 6 from each
of the three experimental conditions.
Criteria for judging whether a re-
sponse was correct, and how many
points to give each response, were de-
termined. Two scorers completed inde-
pendent blind scoring of 25% of the
protocols. Interrater reliability for scor-
ing (number of agreements divided by
the number of items) ranged from 95%
to 100% across measures.
Teacher Debriefing. After the
posttest, meetings were held with each
teacher who taught an instructional
program to explain the purpose of the
study more fully and to get feedback.
Teachers in the no-instruction control
condition were also debriefed and
were given the program materials for
use with their classes.
Results
Characteristics of the
Participants
No significant differences were found
among the three experimental condi-
tions (text structure, content-only, and
no instruction) on mean age, F(2, 12) =
0.03; mean standard score on the
WRMT-R Word Identification subtest,
F(2, 12) = 0.52, and WRMT-R Passage
Comprehension subtest, F(2, 12) = 0.13;
and mean standard Total Reading
score from the WRMT-R, F(2, 12) = 0.32
(see Table 1).
The proportion of lessons (out of
22) that each participant attended and
the mean proportion for each of the 10
classrooms that received instruction
(text structure and content-only) were
calculated. The mean proportion of
lessons attended was .93 (SD = .02) for
the text structure group and .92 (SD =
.05) for the content-only group, with no
significant difference between the
groups, t(8) = .41. The lowest mean
proportion of lessons attended by any
one of the 10 classrooms was .81.
Pretest
No significant differences were found
as a function of instructional condition
on any of the pretest measures.
Posttest
Mean proportions and standard devia-
tions of the posttest measures are pre-
sented in Tables 2 and 3.
Four Strategy Measures. On the
first measure, Locating Clue Words,
1 point was given for each clue word
located in the paragraph; possible
TABLE 2
Posttest Performance by Condition: Proportion of Correct
Responses on Strategy Measures
TSa COa NIa
Measure M SD M SD M SD F (2, 12) Specific comparisons ES
Locating Clue Words .86 .05 .21 .09 .16 .04 197.39*** TS >
CO*** 9.34
TS > NI*** 15.19
Underlining Clauses .70 .07 .04 .08 .02 .03 100.71*** TS >
CO*** 6.14
TS > NI*** 9.22
Completing Graphic Organizer .11 .11 .02 .02 .01 .01 3.71
Recalling Cause–Effect Questions .04 .05 .00 .00 .00 .00 2.67
Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only
condition; NI = no-instruction condition.
an = 5.
***p < .001.
VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 117
scores ranged from 0 to 4. There was an
overall effect of treatment, F(2, 12) =
197.39, p = .000. Specific comparisons
indicated that the text structure group
scored significantly higher than either
the content-only ( p = .000) or the no-
instruction group ( p = .000). There was
no difference between the content-only
and the no-instruction groups (see Ta-
ble 2).
On the next measure, Underlining
Clauses, 1 point was given for each
cause and each effect clause correctly
underlined; possible scores ranged
from 0 to 4. There was a main effect of
treatment, F(2, 12) = 100.71, p = .000.
The text structure group scored signif-
icantly higher than either the content-
only group ( p = .000) or the no-
instruction group ( p = .000). No
difference between the content-only
and the no-instruction groups was
found (see Table 2).
On the Completing the Graphic
Organizer measure, 1 point was given
for each portion of the organizer that
was filled in correctly. Possible scores
ranged from 0 to 4. The effect of treat-
ment was not significant, F(2, 12) =
3.71, ns.
On the final strategy measure, Re-
calling Cause–Effect Questions, possi-
ble scores ranged from 0 to 2. The effect
of treatment was not significant, F(2,
12) = 2.67, ns (see Table 2). It should be
noted that the pattern of results on the
latter two measures, though not signif-
icant, was in line with the expectation
that the text structure group would
outperform the other two groups.
Three Content Outcome Mea-
sures. These measures assessed the
content taught in both instructional
programs. Similar results were found
for all three content outcome measures
(see Table 3).
The first measure was Feature
Questions, tapping information pre-
sented in the target paragraphs about
the three features (home, school, job) of
the communities. There were six fea-
ture questions; possible scores ranged
from 0 to 6. There was a main effect of
treatment, F(2, 12) = 8.57, p = .005. Spe-
cific comparisons indicated that the
text structure group scored signifi-
cantly higher than the no-instruction
group ( p = .006), and the content-only
group also scored significantly higher
than the no-instruction group ( p =
.003). There was no difference between
the text structure and the content-only
groups.
The next measure was Non-
Feature Questions, tapping other in-
formation presented in the target para-
graphs. There were four such ques-
tions; possible scores ranged from 0 to
4. There was an overall effect of treat-
ment, F(2, 12) = 9.03, p = .004. The text
structure group scored significantly
higher than the no-instruction group
( p = .005), and the content-only group
also scored significantly higher than
the no-instruction group ( p = .002). No
difference was observed between the
text structure and the content-only
groups.
The final content outcome mea-
sure was Vocabulary Definitions; there
were eight items, and possible scores
ranged from 0 to 8. There was an over-
all effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 23.24,
p = .000. Specific comparisons indi-
cated that the text structure group
scored significantly higher than the no-
instruction group ( p = .000), and the
content-only group also scored signifi-
cantly higher than the no-instruction
group ( p = .000). There was no differ-
ence between the text structure and the
content-only groups.
Five Comprehension Outcome
Measures. These measures involved,
for the most part, paragraphs with so-
cial studies content, and they required
students to answer three types of ques-
tions: noncausal questions, cause ques-
tions, and effect questions. Two kinds
of paragraphs were used: one cause–one
effect paragraphs and one cause–multiple
effects paragraphs. With regard to the
one cause–one effect paragraph, 1 point
was given for each correct answer to
the noncausal question, the cause
question, and the effect question, re-
spectively. With regard to the one
cause–multiple effects paragraph (en-
tailing three to four effects), 1 point
was given for each correct answer to
the noncausal question and to the
cause question, respectively. For the ef-
fect question, 1 point was given for
each correct effect given.
Explicit Teaching. The first com-
prehension outcome measure assessed
the effect of explicit teaching. It in-
volved a one cause–one effect para-
graph and required a written response.
There was a main effect of treatment
for the noncausal question, F(2, 12) =
5.82, p = .017. Both the text structure
and the content-only groups scored
significantly higher than the no-
instruction group (both ps = .012). No
difference was found between the text
structure and the content-only groups.
For the cause question, there was no ef-
fect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 1.39, ns.
However, for the effect question, there
was an effect of treatment, F(2, 12) =
4.89, p = .028; the text structure group
scored significantly higher than either
the content-only ( p = .029) or the no-
instruction group ( p = .014). No differ-
ence was found between the content-
only and the no-instruction groups
(see Table 3).
Transfer. These four measures re-
quired oral responses to noncausal,
cause, and effect questions. Two of the
paragraphs, both one cause–multiple
effects, were written in the same for-
mat as the target paragraphs (one near
and one far transfer). Two were taken
from trade books (i.e., they were au-
thentic texts); one of them entailed one
cause–one effect, and the other en-
tailed one cause–multiple effects.
The three one cause–multiple ef-
fects passages (Near Transfer, Far
Transfer, and Authentic) yielded simi-
lar results. There were no effects of
treatment for the noncausal question,
F(2, 12) = 1.59, 3.62, and 0.07, respec-
tively; nor for the cause question, F(2,
12) = 0.51, 0.48, and 0.33, respectively.
However, there was an effect of treat-
ment for the effect question, F(2, 12) =
9.76, p = .003; F(2, 12) = 11.78, p = .001;
and F(2, 12) =9.81, p = .003, respec-
tively. Specific comparisons indicated
that the text structure group scored sig-
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES118
nificantly higher than either the con-
tent-only ( p = .002, .002, and .005, re-
spectively) or the no-instruction group
( p = .003, 001, and .001, respectively).
There was no difference between the
content-only and the no-instruction
groups (see Table 3).
On the fourth transfer measure,
the one cause–one effect authentic text,
there was no effect of treatment for the
noncausal question, the cause ques-
tion, or the effect question, F(2, 12) =
0.48, 0.83, and 0.48, respectively.
Effect Sizes
Effect sizes ranged from 6.14 to 15.19
on the strategy measures, from 2.55 to
5.66 on the content outcome measures,
and from 1.57 to 3.24 on the compre-
hension outcome measures, all of
which are considered large effects
(Cohen, 1988).
Fidelity to Treatment
Because of scheduling difficulties,
there were 19 classroom observations
instead of 20. Teachers taught every
section of every lesson, except for the
TABLE 3
Posttest Performance by Condition: Proportion of Correct
Responses on Content and Comprehension Outcome Measures
TSa COa NIa
Measure M SD M SD M SD F (2, 12) Specific comparisons ES
Content Outcome Measures
Feature Questions .41 .20 .45 .16 .08 .05 8.57** TS > NI** 2.55
CO > NI** 3.43
Non-Feature Questions .41 .19 .47 .22 .05 .01 9.03** TS > NI**
3.51
CO > NI** 3.63
Vocabulary Definitions .48 .10 .56 .15 .13 .02 23.24*** TS >
NI*** 5.66
CO > NI*** 4.96
Comprehension Outcome Measures
Explicit Teaching, Written, One cause–
one effect
Noncausal Question .77 .18 .76 .17 .42 .20 5.82* TS > NI* 1.84
CO > NI* 1.84
Cause Question .61 .25 .46 .23 .38 .19 1.39
Effect Question .30 .22 .08 .06 .05 .07 4.89* TS > CO* 1.57
TS > NI* 1.72
Transfer, Oral, One cause–multiple
effects
Near Transfer
Noncausal Question .60 .18 .61 .19 .45 .10 1.59
Cause Question .27 .22 .17 .11 .20 .10 0.51
Effect Question .33 .16 .07 .06 .08 .06 9.76** TS > CO** 2.34
TS > NI** 2.27
Far Transfer
Noncausal Question .57 .22 .68 .10 .38 .18 3.62
Cause Question .07 .11 .10 .07 .05 .07 0.48
Effect Question .28 .10 .10 .05 .08 .05 11.78*** TS > CO**
2.47
TS > NI*** 2.59
Authentic Text
Noncausal Question .65 .12 .66 .15 .63 .10 0.07
Cause Question .63 .24 .69 .05 .62 .13 0.33
Effect Question .42 .10 .22 .12 .17 .05 9.81** TS > CO** 1.88
TS > NI*** 3.24
Transfer, Oral, One cause–one effect
Authentic Text
Noncausal Question .70 .14 .76 .08 .75 .08 0.48
Cause Question .67 .17 .56 .12 .58 .13 0.83
Effect Question .43 .23 .49 .10 .38 .17 0.48
Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only
condition; NI = no-instruction condition.
an = 5.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 119
lesson review. The review section was
taught during 3 of the 10 observations
of text structure lessons and during 5
of the 9 observations of the content-
only lessons.
Discussion
This study extends the findings of
Williams et al. (2005) to the content
area of social studies and strengthens
the conclusion that explicit compre-
hension instruction can be effective at
the primary-grade level. On the Ex-
plicit Teaching paragraph, the text
structure group scored higher than the
other groups on the effect question.
This group did not score higher on the
cause question; perhaps the students
were sufficiently familiar with the
cause concept before the instruction
began. The explicit instruction pro-
vided by the text structure program,
however, was necessary for the more
difficult effect concept. We did not ex-
pect a difference between the text
structure and the content-only groups
on the noncausal question; our pro-
gram was focused specifically on
cause–effect.
The performance of the two in-
structed groups did not differ on any of
the three content outcome measures;
thus, text structure instruction can be
accomplished within the framework of
content area instruction without a loss
in the amount of content acquired.
The results of the present study,
however, did not replicate the strong
transfer effects found in the Williams et
al. (2005) study, although the data sug-
gest that some transfer occurred. That
is, on three of the four comprehension
outcome measures that assessed trans-
fer, students who had been taught via
the text structure program performed
significantly better on questions that
involved effects than did students who
received the content-only program.
However, the text structure group stu-
dents were no better than the other stu-
dents when answering questions about
causes. The results on the noncausal
question of the near and far transfer
paragraphs showed the same pattern
as was seen in the explicit teaching
paragraph (i.e., the two instructed
groups scored higher than the no-
instruction group did), although the
effect did not reach significance.
One of the four transfer para-
graphs did not show the positive ef-
fects of the text structure training on
the effect question. This was possibly
due to the fact that its content was
rather abstract (i.e., salt and its uses),
and it was the only paragraph for
which it was difficult to develop reli-
able scoring.
One possible reason why transfer
effects were not as clear in this study is
that the cause–effect structure is more
difficult than the compare–contrast
structure used in our earlier work (Ciar-
diello, 2002; McCormick, 2003; Rich-
gels et al., 1987). If this is the case, then
providing a greater amount of instruc-
tion on cause–effect than we did in the
present study might lead to more
transfer. Having two text types may
also have proved too challenging for
second graders. Futhermore, the cause–
effect text structure was presented
from the beginning in a paragraph for-
mat. For young novice readers, a text
of this length might be too distracting.
Because this study was designed
to evaluate the effectiveness of an in-
tact instructional program, it cannot be
used to determine the relative impor-
tance of any of the individual program
components. Other experimental de-
signs are required for that purpose.
However, on the basis of a detailed ex-
amination of our results, of the infor-
mal data gathered during classroom
observations, and of the teacher feed-
back, we have identified several areas
where we believe modifications in the
program are warranted. We have re-
vised the program and have under-
taken a replication of the study. We are
devoting more attention to the concept
of cause–effect using familiar content.
We do this at the beginning of the pro-
gram, although we do not completely
eliminate activities involving social
studies content early in the program.
We also focus more heavily on text
analysis of independent sentences (i.e.,
sentences that are not embedded in
paragraphs). We are also teaching only
the one cause–one effect pattern, rather
than introducing one cause–multiple
effects paragraphs. Clue words are
added to the lessons gradually, and the
graphic organizer and generic ques-
tions are simplified.
Many students at the second-grade
level have not yet mastered word
recognition. However, they should not
be deprived of comprehension instruc-
tion before they become fluent readers.
Our program represents the type of lis-
tening and reading instruction that can
work at this grade level for all stu-
dents, including those at risk for aca-
demic failure. We believe that this type
of instruction has the potential for im-
proving comprehension on both oral
and written tasks, at the same time that
it gives students the opportunity to ac-
quire basic knowledge of social studies.
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Joanna P. Williams, PhD, is a professor of
psychology and education at Teachers College,
Columbia University. Her current research in-
terests are reading comprehension and the de-
velopment of instruction for primary grade stu-
dents, including those with learning disabilities
and others at risk for academic failure. Abigail
M. Nubla-Kung, MA, Simonne Pollini, MA,
Amaya Garcia, BA, and Anne G. Snyder,
MAT, are currently graduate research assis-
tants at Teachers College, Columbia University,
specializing in reading comprehension and edu-
cational psychology. K. Brooke Stafford, PhD,
currently works as a researcher in the Division
of Teaching and Learning at the New York City
Department of Education, supporting the de-
velopment and implementation of curriculum
and professional development in the New York
City public schools. Address: Joanna Williams,
Box 238, Teachers College, Columbia Univer-
sity, 525 West 120th St., New York, NY 10027.
AUTHORS’ NOTES
1. This research was funded by the Institute of
Educational Sciences, U.S. Department of
Education.
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES120
2. We thank Amy Endo, Chi-Shun Lien, Anne-
Marie Tan, and Hsiao-Ling Weng for their
help with data collection and processing.
REFERENCES
Bakken, J. P., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs,
T. E. (1997). Reading comprehension of
expository science material and students
with learning disabilities: A comparison
of strategies. The Journal of Special Educa-
tion, 31, 300–324.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Gromoll,
E. W. (1989). Learning from social studies
texts. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 99–158.
Brophy, J. (1992). The de facto national cur-
riculum in U.S. elementary social studies:
Critique of a representative example. The
Journal of Curriculum Studies, 24, 401–447.
Brophy, J., & Alleman, J. (2002). Learning
and teaching about cultural universals in
primary-grade social studies. The Elemen-
tary School Journal, 103, 99–114.
Bryant, D. P., Ugel, N., Thompson, S., &
Hamff, A. (1999). Instructional strategies
for content-area reading instruction. In-
tervention in School and Clinic, 34, 293–302.
Campbell, J. R., Kapinus, B., & Beatty, A. S.
(1995). Interviewing children about their lit-
eracy experiences: Data from NAEP’s in-
tegrated reading performance at Grade 4.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Ed-
ucation.
Caseley, J. (2002). On the town: A community
adventure. New York: Greenwillow.
Center on Education Policy. (2006). From the
capitol to the classroom: Year 4 of the No
Child Left Behind Act. Washington, DC:
Author.
Chall, J. S., & Dale, E. (1995). Readability re-
visited: The new Dale-Chall readability for-
mula. Cambridge, MA: Brookline.
Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E.
(1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children
fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
Chambliss, M. J., & Calfee, R. C. (1998).
Textbooks for learning. Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
Ciardiello, A. V. (2002). Helping adolescents
understand cause/effect text structure in
social studies. Social Studies, 93, 31–36.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for
the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
De La Paz, S., & MacArthur, C. A. (2003).
Knowing the how and why of history:
Expectations for secondary students with
and without learning disabilities. Learn-
ing Disability Quarterly, 26, 142–154.
Dickson, S. (1999). Integrating reading and
writing to teach compare–contrast text
structure: A research-based methodology.
Reading and Writing Quarterly, 14, 49–79.
Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The
scarcity of informational texts in first
grade. Reading Research Quarterly, 35,
202–224.
Ferretti, R. P., & Okolo, C. M. (1996). Au-
thenticity in learning: Multimedia design
projects in the social studies for students
with disabilities. Journal of Learning Dis-
abilities, 29, 45–60.
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., &
Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading com-
prehension strategies to students with
learning disabilities: A review of re-
search. Review of Educational Research, 71,
279–320.
Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. (2000).
Structural aspects of constructing mean-
ing from text. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mo-
senthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.),
Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp.
311–335). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hoffman, J. V., McCarthy, S. J., Abbott, J.,
Christian, C., Corman, L., Curry, C., et al.
(1994). So what’s new in the new basals?
A focus on first grade. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 26, 47–73.
McCormick, S. (2003). Instructing students
who have literacy problems (4th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall.
Meyer, B. J. F., & Poon, L. W. (2001). Effects
of structure training and signaling on re-
call of text. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 93, 141–159.
Moss, B. (2005). Making a case and a place
for effective content area literacy instruc-
tion in the elementary grades. The Read-
ing Teacher, 59, 46–55.
Myers, M. P., & Savage, T. (2005). Enhanc-
ing student comprehension of social
studies material. Social Studies, 96, 18–24.
Ogle, D. (1986). K-W-L: A teaching model
that develops active reading of exposi-
tory text. The Reading Teacher, 39, 564–570.
Perfetti, C. A., Marron, M. A., & Foltz, P. W.
(1996). Sources of comprehension failure:
Theoretical perspectives and case stud-
ies. In C. Cornoldi & J. V. Oakhill (Eds.),
Reading comprehension difficulties: Pro-
cesses and remediation (pp. 137–165). Mah-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Richgels, D. J., McGee, L. M., Lomax, R. G.,
& Sheard, C. (1987). Awareness of four
text structures: Effects on recall of expos-
itory text. Reading Research Quarterly, 22,
177–196.
Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1981). What’s in
a story: An approach to comprehension
and instruction. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Ad-
vances in instructional psychology (Vol. 2,
pp. 213–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vogler, K. (2003). Where does social studies
fit in a high-stakes testing environment?
Social Studies, 94, 207–211.
Williams, J. P. (1998). Improving the com-
prehension of disabled readers. Annals of
Dyslexia, 48, 213–238.
Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., & Lauer, K.
(2004). Teaching expository text structure
to young at-risk learners: Building the ba-
sics of comprehension instruction. Excep-
tionality, 12, 129–144.
Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., Lauer, K. D.,
Stafford, K. B., DeSisto, L. A., & deCani,
J. S. (2005). Expository text comprehen-
sion in the primary grade classroom.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 235–
248.
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading
mastery tests—Revised. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Service.
Title
Authors & Date
Research Question(s)
Key Terms
Methods
Results
Major Conclusion(s)
Gaps & Limitations
Key Concept 1, pg #
(Written in your own words! No copy/paste!)
Key Concept 2, pg #
(Written in your own words! No copy/paste!)
Key Concept 3, pg #
(Written in your own words! No copy/paste!)
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIESVOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCHA.docx

More Related Content

Similar to JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIESVOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCHA.docx

A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners Writing Skill
A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners   Writing SkillA Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners   Writing Skill
A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners Writing SkillMartha Brown
 
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy  Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy  Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...April Knyff
 
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docx
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docxRunning head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docx
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docxtoltonkendal
 
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVERPhil Sparrow
 
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisDr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisWilliam Kritsonis
 
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011Sumberslidehare
 
Employing Social Media in Text-Based Instruction
Employing Social Media in Text-Based InstructionEmploying Social Media in Text-Based Instruction
Employing Social Media in Text-Based InstructionAJHSSR Journal
 
Language and education
Language and educationLanguage and education
Language and educationEdgar Eslit
 
Case study about IEC reading classes
Case study about IEC reading classes Case study about IEC reading classes
Case study about IEC reading classes Ehsan Ataei
 
Teachers' Questioning in Reading Lessons
Teachers' Questioning in Reading LessonsTeachers' Questioning in Reading Lessons
Teachers' Questioning in Reading Lessonsjuraikha
 
Writing Listening Speaking in the California Framwork
Writing Listening Speaking in the California FramworkWriting Listening Speaking in the California Framwork
Writing Listening Speaking in the California Framworkteamteach
 
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...paperpublications3
 
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...Alexander Decker
 
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  ClassroomsAssessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based ClassroomsWendy Belieu
 

Similar to JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIESVOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCHA.docx (20)

A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners Writing Skill
A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners   Writing SkillA Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners   Writing Skill
A Study On Strategy Instruction And EFL Learners Writing Skill
 
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy  Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy  Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...
Apprenticeship In Academic Literacy Three K-12 Literacy Strategies To Suppor...
 
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docx
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docxRunning head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docx
Running head THE EFFECTIVENESS OF NARRATIVE INPUT CHARTS 1 .docx
 
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
2ND ASSIGNMENT WITH COVER
 
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. KritsonisDr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
Dr. Butler & Dr. W.A. Kritsonis
 
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011
The reading teacher volume 65 issue 3 2011
 
Employing Social Media in Text-Based Instruction
Employing Social Media in Text-Based InstructionEmploying Social Media in Text-Based Instruction
Employing Social Media in Text-Based Instruction
 
Reading Next
Reading NextReading Next
Reading Next
 
Reading Next
Reading NextReading Next
Reading Next
 
Language and education
Language and educationLanguage and education
Language and education
 
Sla
SlaSla
Sla
 
Literacy by design
Literacy by designLiteracy by design
Literacy by design
 
Case study about IEC reading classes
Case study about IEC reading classes Case study about IEC reading classes
Case study about IEC reading classes
 
Teachers' Questioning in Reading Lessons
Teachers' Questioning in Reading LessonsTeachers' Questioning in Reading Lessons
Teachers' Questioning in Reading Lessons
 
Writing Listening Speaking in the California Framwork
Writing Listening Speaking in the California FramworkWriting Listening Speaking in the California Framwork
Writing Listening Speaking in the California Framwork
 
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...
The Effectiveness of Thematic Teaching Approach on Developing Reading Skills ...
 
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...
Curriculum integration in social studies as predictor of academic performance...
 
Morgan
MorganMorgan
Morgan
 
Research paper dena rogers
Research paper dena rogersResearch paper dena rogers
Research paper dena rogers
 
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  ClassroomsAssessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
 

More from tawnyataylor528

•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docxtawnyataylor528
 
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docxtawnyataylor528
 
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docxtawnyataylor528
 
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docxtawnyataylor528
 
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docxtawnyataylor528
 
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docxtawnyataylor528
 
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docxtawnyataylor528
 
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docxtawnyataylor528
 
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docxtawnyataylor528
 
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docxtawnyataylor528
 
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docxtawnyataylor528
 

More from tawnyataylor528 (20)

•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
 
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
 
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
 
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
 
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
 
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
 
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
 
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
 
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
 
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
 
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
 
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
 
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
 
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
 
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
 
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
 
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
 
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
 
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
 
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
 

Recently uploaded

The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 

Recently uploaded (20)

The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 

JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIESVOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCHA.docx

  • 1. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007, PAGES 111– 120 Teaching Cause–Effect Text Structure Through Social Studies Content to At-Risk Second Graders Joanna P. Williams, Abigail M. Nubla-Kung, Simonne Pollini, K. Brooke Stafford, Amaya Garcia, and Anne E. Snyder Abstract This study evaluated the effectiveness of a comprehension program integrated with social studies instruction designed for at-risk second graders. The program included instruction in cause–effect text structure, emphasizing clue words, generic questions, graphic organizers, and the close analysis of specially constructed cause–effect target paragraphs. This program was compared (a) to a content- only program that focused only on social studies and did not include text structure instruction and (b) to a no-instruction control. Fifteen classroom teachers, randomly assigned to treatment, provided the instruction. The program improved the comprehension of instructional cause– effect texts, and there were transfer effects on some comprehension measures. The performance of the 2 instructed
  • 2. groups did not differ on any of the content measures, indicating that such integrated instruction can be accomplished without a loss in the amount of content acquired. This study supports our previous findings on the effectiveness of explicit instruction at the primary-grade level. G iven mounting pressures to en- sure that students achieve high levels of proficiency on stan- dardized tests, many school districts have begun to increase the amount of time spent on reading and mathemat- ics in the elementary school classroom. Consequently, instruction in other sub- ject areas has been significantly re- duced (Vogler, 2003). In a survey of 299 nationally representative school dis- tricts, for example, the Center on Edu- cation Policy (2006) found that 71% of these districts have reduced the amount of time spent on teaching at least one or more of the content areas. Social studies is one content area that has been frequently cut back, with 33% of school districts examined by the Center on Education Policy (2006) re- porting a significant reduction in the portion of the school day spent on social studies. This is unfortunate, as there are many skills, such as critical thinking, decision making, and prob- lem solving, that can be acquired while
  • 3. learning social studies content (Ferretti & Okolo, 1996). Social studies instruc- tion also encourages young learners to consider the role that history plays in shaping our communities (Brophy & Alleman, 2002) and, perhaps most im- portant, helps them to develop civic competence (Ferretti & Okolo, 1996). The extra time allotted to reading in the elementary curriculum has been greeted enthusiastically in most quar- ters. However, even with the addi- tional time allotments, there is a notable deficit in today’s reading instruction: There is a lack of focused and explicit instruction in reading expository text (Hoffman et al., 1994). In an observa- tional study of 20 first-grade class- rooms, Duke (2000) found that infor- mational text was rarely used in reading and writing activities. In the classroom libraries she examined, fewer than 10% of the books available to students included informational text. Similarly, a national survey by Camp- bell, Kapinus, and Beatty (1995) of ele- mentary classrooms indicated that few if any of the books read aloud to stu- dents include expository text. Expository text is generally re- garded as more difficult than narrative text (Goldman & Rakestraw, 2000). Its
  • 4. content is often unfamiliar and fre- quently represents complex and ab- stract concepts (Stein & Trabasso, 1981). Moreover, expository text contains va- rious text structures that can be diffi- cult to recognize and interpret. These structures include sequence, description, compare–contrast, problem–solution, and cause–effect (Meyer & Poon, 2001), and they can be presented in isolation or in conjunction with other structures. Of the structures that can be found in expository text, the cause–effect struc- ture may be particularly difficult for children (Ciardiello, 2002). Richgels, McGee, Lomax, and Sheard (1987) found that sixth graders were less able to comprehend causation text structure than other text structures. McCormick (2003) has suggested that this structure JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES112 is difficult because of the complex comprehension skills (i.e., making in- ferences, judging sequence, and mak- ing predictions) involved in the inter- pretation of causes and effects. By the time students reach fourth grade, much of the content that they encounter in the classroom is pre- sented in the form of expository text (Moss, 2005). Without an adequate un-
  • 5. derstanding of the structures inherent in expository text and the strategies needed to comprehend the text that is organized within these structures, many students struggle with this rather sudden shift to informational texts (Duke, 2000). Evidence of this struggle is highlighted by the well- documented slump in fourth-grade reading achievement (Chall, Jacobs, & Baldwin, 1990). In social studies instruction, as much as 90% of the content that is taught is derived from and structured around informational text from text- books (Myers & Savage, 2005). In fact, many educators view textbooks as the primary source of the social studies curriculum (Brophy, 1992). Unfortu- nately, these texts are often dense, poorly structured, and riddled with ir- relevant information that can distract readers from more important concepts (Beck, McKeown, & Gromoll, 1989). Students are not given the tools with which to read such text, and without adequate skills, students—particularly those with learning disabilities (LD) and other students at risk for academic failure—may be unable to access and comprehend the information that is presented to them (De La Paz & Mac- Arthur, 2003). There is evidence that through ex-
  • 6. plicit instruction in text structure, low- achieving students can develop and refine their ability to comprehend ex- pository text. Bakken, Mastropieri, and Scruggs (1997) found that eighth-grade students with LD who were explicitly taught to apply specific strategies to- ward the reading of expository text were better able to recall important in- formation from the text. Dickson (1999) found that instruction in compare– contrast structure was effective for middle school students with a range of learning needs. Only recently have there been studies indicating that ele- mentary school students, too, can ben- efit from such instruction. Our own work (Williams, Hall, & Lauer, 2004; Williams et al., 2005) has demonstrated the effectiveness of explicit instruction in helping second graders at risk for academic failure to improve their read- ing comprehension of compare–contrast text via the use of text structure. More- over, these studies indicated that em- bedding text structure instruction in content lessons (on science content) did not diminish the amount of content acquired. Present Study We have developed a program that provides instruction on a different text structure, cause–effect, embedded in a
  • 7. different subject matter domain, social studies. Will our previous findings hold? That is, will our instructional program help second graders at risk for academic failure to improve their comprehension? Can this goal be ac- complished without a decrease in the amount of content knowledge that stu- dents acquire from the program? As in our previous work, we used an explicit and structured instructional model that included explanation and modeling by the teacher and guided and then independent practice (Wil- liams, 1998), because low-achieving students benefit from systematic and intensive instruction (Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker, 2001). Students were taught to use clue words, generic cause–effect questions, and graphic organizers to help them identify and analyze causes and effects in specially constructed, well-structured target para- graphs. As Goldman and Rakestraw (2000) noted, readers are better able to understand text when it has a clearly defined structure. Within each lesson of our pro- gram, children were taught key vocab- ulary words. Learning domain-specific vocabulary allows students to become familiar with the language of social studies and to develop a level of ex-
  • 8. pertise in the subject matter (Bryant, Ugel, Thompson, & Hamff, 1999). The social studies content taught in the program was aligned with New York state learning standards in social stud- ies and English language arts. It cov- ered three historical U.S. communities (colonists, pioneers, and immigrants at the turn of the 20th century). Each community was presented with a focus on the same three features: homes, schools, and jobs. The program also provided students with exposure to in- formational trade books related to the social studies content that was taught. Informational trade books can provide valuable sources of well-written and authentic text that appeals to young readers (Moss, 2005). Most second-grade instruction in- volves a great deal of speaking and lis- tening, because many students at that level are not yet fluent readers. We in- cluded in our program a typical second- grade mix of listening/speaking and reading/writing tasks. The same cog- nitive processes are basic to both lis- tening and reading comprehension (Perfetti, Marron, & Foltz, 1996), and our goal was to improve comprehen- sion as demonstrated in either oral or written language. This study evaluated the effec- tiveness of our cause–effect instruc-
  • 9. tional program for second graders at risk for academic failure. We randomly assigned intact classrooms (a) to our text structure program, (b) to a com- parison content-only program that used the same materials as the text structure program but that did not focus on the cause–effect structure of the text, or (c) to a no-instruction control. Method Design We used a pretest–posttest design. Fif- teen classroom teachers from three ele- mentary schools in New York City vol- VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 113 unteered to participate in the study. The three schools were of similar de- mographics; all three were categorized as Title I schools. The total enrollment across the schools included 76.5% His- panic, 22% African American, 0.5% Eu- ropean American, and 1% Asian or other. Ninety-three percent of the stu- dents received state aid in the form of free or reduced lunch, and approxi- mately 5% of the students were en- rolled in either part-time or full-time special education services.
  • 10. The teachers were randomly as- signed to one of three experimental conditions: a text structure program (n = 5), a content-only program (n = 5), or a no-instruction control (n = 5). Con- ditions were blocked by school. All sta- tistical analyses were performed with the classroom as the unit of analysis. Participants Three hundred thirty-seven students were eligible for participation, and 260 of these students returned consent forms; 243 students completed the study (the other 17 students were reas- signed to other classrooms or moved out of the school district during the study). All students in the 10 instruc- tional classrooms (n = 160) received the instructional program as taught by their classroom teacher. We randomly selected 12 students from each class- room for the statistical analysis. In one case, only 11 students completed the posttests; we added as a 12th score the mean score of these 11 students. Fifteen of the 179 students had Individualized Education Programs (IEPs), and 5 stu- dents had been referred for IEPs. Table 1 presents demographic information about the 179 students. Materials The content goal of the instructional
  • 11. programs was to teach students about three historical communities in the United States—specifically, about homes, schools, and jobs in these com- munities. Both programs used biogra- phies, other trade books, and specially constructed cause–effect target para- graphs. Selection of the books was based on appropriateness of content and quality of photographs. Ten short, well-structured cause–effect paragraphs were written specifically for the pro- gram. In the first unit of the program (Colonists), each target paragraph con- sisted of several causation sentences; each sentence included one cause, one effect, and a clue word. In the second unit (Pioneers), each paragraph con- tained one cause and multiple effects. The cause and each effect appeared in separate sentences. The third unit (Im- migrants) included both types of para- graphs. Examples of the two types of paragraphs are as follows: From Lesson 5: One cause–one ef- fect paragraph: Colonists and Their Schools The first school that colonial children went to was called Dame School. Children had to read books written for grown-ups because there were no storybooks for kids. In school, there were no pencils; therefore, children
  • 12. wrote with a piece of lead. Girls needed to learn how to spin, cook, and clean the house; thus, they stayed home after they finished Dame School. After Dame School, most boys continued to go to school, since the law said they had to go. From Lesson 14: One cause– multiple effects paragraph (this para- graph also contained one sentence with noncausal information.): Pioneers and Their Jobs When pioneers first moved to the prairie, it was difficult to find a job. Therefore, after the harvest, most farmers had to travel away from their families for a few months to work for the railroad. Some men had construc- tion jobs in nearby towns. Children had to work by selling eggs. With the money the families saved, they bought leather, shoes, salt, and other things they could not make them- selves. The readability level of the target paragraphs was between third and fourth grade (M = 3.40, SD = 0.84) using the Dale-Chall Readability Scale (Chall & Dale, 1995)—rather high, be- cause some of the words in the text (e.g., words relevant to the program content and some of the clue words)
  • 13. were unfamiliar words on the Dale- Chall reading list. However, the stu- dents in both the text structure and TABLE 1 Demographic Characteristics and Reading Scores of Participants by Condition TS CO NI Measure M SD M SD M SD Gender Male 27 29 22 Female 33 30 38 Age (years) 7.69 0.21 7.68 0.23 7.66 0.15 WRMT-R Word Identification Standard score 454.88 14.70 462.13 10.93 458.57 6.84 Grade equivalent 2.5 2.6 2.6 Passage Comprehension Standard score 472.80 9.48 474.40 5.12 472.32 4.54 Grade equivalent 2.4 2.4 2.3 Total Reading Score Standard score 463.84 12.01 468.23 8.03 465.44 5.06 Grade equivalent 2.4 2.5 2.4 Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only condition; NI = no-instruction condition; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (Woodcock, 1987).
  • 14. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES114 content-only programs were quite fa- miliar with these words because they were taught as vocabulary words. When we calculated the readability level with our vocabulary words con- sidered as familiar words, the mean readability of the target paragraphs was between second- and third-grade level (M = 2.50, SD = 0.97). Overview of the Two Instructional Programs Text Structure Program. The in- structional program contained three units, each of which focused on one historical community. There were 22 lessons in all. First, an introductory les- son introduced the concept of cause– effect and also the program content through a narrative book, On the Town: A Community Adventure (Caseley, 2002), which follows Charlie and his mother as they explore their community. Then, for each unit, there was a lesson that in- troduced the community via a biogra- phy of a young person living in that community. This lesson included a read-aloud and discussion and an in- troduction to the relevant vocabulary. There were six additional lessons in each unit: two lessons each on homes,
  • 15. schools, and jobs. Each pair of lessons was organized around a target para- graph and included further read- alouds from a second trade book. The first lesson on each feature included in- struction on 1. Concept of cause and effect. The stu- dents were introduced to the defi- nition of cause and effect. Effect was defined as a thing or event that happens, and cause, as the per- son, thing, or event that makes the effect happen. Students developed their understanding of these con- cepts through picture cards, matching, and cloze activities. 2. Cause–effect clue words. In Lesson 1, students were introduced to four cause–effect clue words: because, since, therefore, and thus. As the pro- gram continued, they memorized the clue words and used them to identify paragraphs as cause–effect paragraphs. 3. Vocabulary. The vocabulary con- cepts were related to the content of the program: community, home, school, job, pioneer, colonist, immi- grant, sod house, keeping room, tene- ment, schoolhouse, Dame School, pub- lic school, farmer, blacksmith, and sweatshop. The vocabulary words
  • 16. were embedded in the target para- graphs; they were also presented as a list of words that were ex- plained and illustrated through examples. 4. Trade book read-aloud and discussion. Specific passages from a trade book were read aloud. Students were encouraged to ask questions and share comments. 5. Community chart. There was a com- munity chart for each unit, which was used for further review of the content vocabulary. During the sec- ond and third units, words from previous charts were reviewed. 6. Cause–effect questions. Beginning in Unit 2, with the introduction of the second type of paragraph (one cause–multiple effects), the stu- dents learned two generic ques- tions. These questions were de- signed to help them focus on the cause–effect information in the text rather than on irrelevant details or prior knowledge. The two ques- tions were (a) What is the cause? and (b) What is the effect? 7. Read-aloud and analysis of the target paragraph. Students read the target paragraph first silently and then aloud. The class analyzed the
  • 17. cause–effect structure of the first type of paragraph (one cause–one effect) by circling the clue word and underlining the part of the sentence that followed the clue word (the cause or the effect). Causes and cause clue words were marked with a blue crayon, and effects and effect clue words with a green crayon. For the second type of paragraph (one cause–multiple effects), the students circled the clue word and underlined what followed it. Then they used the cause–effect questions to guide the analysis of the rest of the para- graph. Sentences that included noncausal information were introduced in Lesson 12 and were identified as irrelevant to the causal analysis. The second lesson on each feature included instruction on 1. Graphic organizer. The students completed a graphic organizer for the target paragraph. There were two organizers, one for each paragraph type (see Chambliss & Calfee, 1998, for examples). This helped the students visually organize the three elements (cause, effect, and clue word) in a cause– effect sentence.
  • 18. 2. Comprehension questions. Students answered three types of questions about the target paragraph. The first was noncausal (e.g., How many teachers worked in a school- house?). The second type asked about causes (e.g., Why did a schoolhouse only have one teacher?), and the third asked about effects (e.g., What are the effects of having very little money for schools?). We showed the students that they could find the answers to the comprehension questions in the completed graphic organizer as well as in the target paragraph. The students were encouraged to answer in complete sentences. 3. Lesson review. Each lesson ended with a review of the strategies (clue words, cause–effect ques- tions, and graphic organizer) as well as the content covered. Content-Only Program. The con- tent-only program taught the same so- cial studies content and used the same materials (target paragraphs, trade books, and charts) as the text structure program, but it did not focus on cause– effect structure. As in the text structure program, there were 22 lessons: one in-
  • 19. VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 115 troductory lesson; three lessons intro- ducing each historical community via the biography; and two lessons on each of the three features within each unit. The introductory lesson focused on the general content of the program via On the Town: A Community Adven- ture (Caseley, 2002). In Lesson 2, the Unit 1 biography was presented; the lesson included a read-aloud and dis- cussion and an introduction to the vo- cabulary. The first lesson on each fea- ture included instruction on 1. KWL chart. Comparison lessons began with a discussion of the students’ background knowledge about the community (Colonist, Pioneer, or Immigrant) and the feature (home, school, or job) that were the foci of the lesson. This was done through a KWL pro- cedure (what I Know, what I Want to know, and what I Learned; Ogle, 1986). The teacher recorded re- sponses to what I Know about a focus feature on a chart. Then the students, with the help of the teacher, generated questions about what they wanted to know, and the teacher recorded these. The
  • 20. what I Learned portion of the chart was completed after the trade book read-aloud, the vocabulary, and the community chart activity. At this point, the students attempted to answer the questions they had generated at the beginning of the lesson and added other details that were discussed during the course of the lesson. 2. Vocabulary. A review of the vocabu- lary words was conducted as in the text structure program. 3 Trade book read-aloud and discussion. This was the same activity as in the text structure program, and also included further discussion questions. 4. Community Chart. This was the same activity as in the text struc- ture program. 5. Graphic organizer (content). Students filled in an information web. In the center of the web was a labeled depiction of a home, school, or job, surrounded by several blank circles connected by a line to the center picture. Students filled in the circles with information that they learned during the lesson.
  • 21. The second lesson on each feature included instruction on 1. Read-aloud of the target paragraph. Students read, first silently and then aloud, the same target cause–effect paragraphs that were presented in the text structure program; however, they did not examine the text structure and identify it as a cause–effect paragraph. Rather, the paragraph was presented to the students as another opportunity to read and learn about the community and the feature that was targeted in the lesson. 2. Comprehension questions. The stu- dents in the content-only program answered the same noncausal questions that the students in the text structure program answered. As there were no causation ques- tions, the students in the content- only program also answered addi- tional questions pertaining to the information in the target para- graph. The students were en- couraged to answer in complete sentences. 3. Journal entry. Students drew pic- tures of something related to the lesson’s topic (e.g., immigrant jobs) and wrote a paragraph about their
  • 22. drawing. 4. Lesson review. Each lesson con- cluded with a review of the con- tent learned in that lesson, includ- ing the features learned about the communities (home, school, and job) and the vocabulary. Procedure Introducing the Program to Teach- ers. Individual training sessions (30– 45 min) were held to familiarize teach- ers with the program that they were to teach. We discussed the program’s overall goals and reviewed each sec- tion of the lessons. Pretest. The pretest, conducted in two 30–45-min sessions, consisted of audiotaped individual interviews. In the first session, students completed the Word Identification and Passage Comprehension subtests of the Wood- cock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (WRMT-R), Form H (Woodcock, 1987). The second pretest session contained three strategy and two outcome mea- sures. The strategy measures included locating clue words in a paragraph, lo- cating cause–effect clauses, and recall- ing the cause–effect questions. The out- come measures assessed knowledge of vocabulary concepts and ability to pro-
  • 23. vide well-structured cause–effect state- ments (i.e., sentences using accurate in- formation from the text, including a clue word) in response to comprehen- sion questions based on a one cause– one effect paragraph. Lesson Attendance and Feedback. Teachers were asked to take atten- dance and to complete feedback sheets at the end of each lesson. Classroom Observations. Obser- vations were conducted in the 10 in- structional program classrooms to as- sess fidelity to treatment. Each of the 10 classrooms was observed twice over the course of the instruction. Observers recorded whether each lesson section was completed. Three observers made independent observations of the same five lessons. Interrater reliability for each of the observer pairs was 100%. Thereafter, each observation was made by a single observer. Posttest. The posttest consisted of individual, audiotaped interviews and was conducted in two sessions of 30–45 min each. During one session, the students completed Form G of the Word Identification and Passage Com- prehension subtests of the WRMT-R (Woodcock, 1987). The other session included an extensive array of strategy
  • 24. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES116 and outcome measures. The strategy measures included Locating Clue Words; Underlining Clauses; Complet- ing the Graphic Organizer for a one cause–one effect paragraph; and Re- calling the Cause–Effect Questions. The content outcome measures as- sessed Feature Questions (knowledge of explicit information about the fea- tures of the three historical communi- ties); Non-Feature Questions (knowl- edge of other content presented in the target paragraphs); and Vocabulary Definitions. The comprehension outcome mea- sures required students to answer three types of questions (noncausal, cause, and effect questions) concerning a series of paragraphs that for the most part involved social studies content. Several of these measures required oral responses: • to a one cause–one effect paragraph that had appeared during instruc- tion (assessing the effects of Explicit Teaching); • to two novel one cause–multiple effects paragraphs, one of which
  • 25. contained information about an untaught feature (games) of an instructed community (Near Transfer), whereas the other con- tained information unrelated to the program content (Far Transfer); and • to two paragraphs, one of each paragraph type, taken from less well-structured trade books (Trans- fer to Authentic Text). Scoring. Scoring guidelines were developed for each measure on the basis of a small sample (n = 18) of ran- domly selected protocols, 6 from each of the three experimental conditions. Criteria for judging whether a re- sponse was correct, and how many points to give each response, were de- termined. Two scorers completed inde- pendent blind scoring of 25% of the protocols. Interrater reliability for scor- ing (number of agreements divided by the number of items) ranged from 95% to 100% across measures. Teacher Debriefing. After the posttest, meetings were held with each teacher who taught an instructional program to explain the purpose of the study more fully and to get feedback. Teachers in the no-instruction control condition were also debriefed and were given the program materials for
  • 26. use with their classes. Results Characteristics of the Participants No significant differences were found among the three experimental condi- tions (text structure, content-only, and no instruction) on mean age, F(2, 12) = 0.03; mean standard score on the WRMT-R Word Identification subtest, F(2, 12) = 0.52, and WRMT-R Passage Comprehension subtest, F(2, 12) = 0.13; and mean standard Total Reading score from the WRMT-R, F(2, 12) = 0.32 (see Table 1). The proportion of lessons (out of 22) that each participant attended and the mean proportion for each of the 10 classrooms that received instruction (text structure and content-only) were calculated. The mean proportion of lessons attended was .93 (SD = .02) for the text structure group and .92 (SD = .05) for the content-only group, with no significant difference between the groups, t(8) = .41. The lowest mean proportion of lessons attended by any one of the 10 classrooms was .81. Pretest
  • 27. No significant differences were found as a function of instructional condition on any of the pretest measures. Posttest Mean proportions and standard devia- tions of the posttest measures are pre- sented in Tables 2 and 3. Four Strategy Measures. On the first measure, Locating Clue Words, 1 point was given for each clue word located in the paragraph; possible TABLE 2 Posttest Performance by Condition: Proportion of Correct Responses on Strategy Measures TSa COa NIa Measure M SD M SD M SD F (2, 12) Specific comparisons ES Locating Clue Words .86 .05 .21 .09 .16 .04 197.39*** TS > CO*** 9.34 TS > NI*** 15.19 Underlining Clauses .70 .07 .04 .08 .02 .03 100.71*** TS > CO*** 6.14 TS > NI*** 9.22 Completing Graphic Organizer .11 .11 .02 .02 .01 .01 3.71 Recalling Cause–Effect Questions .04 .05 .00 .00 .00 .00 2.67 Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only
  • 28. condition; NI = no-instruction condition. an = 5. ***p < .001. VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 117 scores ranged from 0 to 4. There was an overall effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 197.39, p = .000. Specific comparisons indicated that the text structure group scored significantly higher than either the content-only ( p = .000) or the no- instruction group ( p = .000). There was no difference between the content-only and the no-instruction groups (see Ta- ble 2). On the next measure, Underlining Clauses, 1 point was given for each cause and each effect clause correctly underlined; possible scores ranged from 0 to 4. There was a main effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 100.71, p = .000. The text structure group scored signif- icantly higher than either the content- only group ( p = .000) or the no- instruction group ( p = .000). No difference between the content-only and the no-instruction groups was found (see Table 2). On the Completing the Graphic Organizer measure, 1 point was given for each portion of the organizer that
  • 29. was filled in correctly. Possible scores ranged from 0 to 4. The effect of treat- ment was not significant, F(2, 12) = 3.71, ns. On the final strategy measure, Re- calling Cause–Effect Questions, possi- ble scores ranged from 0 to 2. The effect of treatment was not significant, F(2, 12) = 2.67, ns (see Table 2). It should be noted that the pattern of results on the latter two measures, though not signif- icant, was in line with the expectation that the text structure group would outperform the other two groups. Three Content Outcome Mea- sures. These measures assessed the content taught in both instructional programs. Similar results were found for all three content outcome measures (see Table 3). The first measure was Feature Questions, tapping information pre- sented in the target paragraphs about the three features (home, school, job) of the communities. There were six fea- ture questions; possible scores ranged from 0 to 6. There was a main effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 8.57, p = .005. Spe- cific comparisons indicated that the text structure group scored signifi- cantly higher than the no-instruction group ( p = .006), and the content-only
  • 30. group also scored significantly higher than the no-instruction group ( p = .003). There was no difference between the text structure and the content-only groups. The next measure was Non- Feature Questions, tapping other in- formation presented in the target para- graphs. There were four such ques- tions; possible scores ranged from 0 to 4. There was an overall effect of treat- ment, F(2, 12) = 9.03, p = .004. The text structure group scored significantly higher than the no-instruction group ( p = .005), and the content-only group also scored significantly higher than the no-instruction group ( p = .002). No difference was observed between the text structure and the content-only groups. The final content outcome mea- sure was Vocabulary Definitions; there were eight items, and possible scores ranged from 0 to 8. There was an over- all effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 23.24, p = .000. Specific comparisons indi- cated that the text structure group scored significantly higher than the no- instruction group ( p = .000), and the content-only group also scored signifi- cantly higher than the no-instruction group ( p = .000). There was no differ- ence between the text structure and the content-only groups.
  • 31. Five Comprehension Outcome Measures. These measures involved, for the most part, paragraphs with so- cial studies content, and they required students to answer three types of ques- tions: noncausal questions, cause ques- tions, and effect questions. Two kinds of paragraphs were used: one cause–one effect paragraphs and one cause–multiple effects paragraphs. With regard to the one cause–one effect paragraph, 1 point was given for each correct answer to the noncausal question, the cause question, and the effect question, re- spectively. With regard to the one cause–multiple effects paragraph (en- tailing three to four effects), 1 point was given for each correct answer to the noncausal question and to the cause question, respectively. For the ef- fect question, 1 point was given for each correct effect given. Explicit Teaching. The first com- prehension outcome measure assessed the effect of explicit teaching. It in- volved a one cause–one effect para- graph and required a written response. There was a main effect of treatment for the noncausal question, F(2, 12) = 5.82, p = .017. Both the text structure and the content-only groups scored significantly higher than the no- instruction group (both ps = .012). No
  • 32. difference was found between the text structure and the content-only groups. For the cause question, there was no ef- fect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 1.39, ns. However, for the effect question, there was an effect of treatment, F(2, 12) = 4.89, p = .028; the text structure group scored significantly higher than either the content-only ( p = .029) or the no- instruction group ( p = .014). No differ- ence was found between the content- only and the no-instruction groups (see Table 3). Transfer. These four measures re- quired oral responses to noncausal, cause, and effect questions. Two of the paragraphs, both one cause–multiple effects, were written in the same for- mat as the target paragraphs (one near and one far transfer). Two were taken from trade books (i.e., they were au- thentic texts); one of them entailed one cause–one effect, and the other en- tailed one cause–multiple effects. The three one cause–multiple ef- fects passages (Near Transfer, Far Transfer, and Authentic) yielded simi- lar results. There were no effects of treatment for the noncausal question, F(2, 12) = 1.59, 3.62, and 0.07, respec- tively; nor for the cause question, F(2, 12) = 0.51, 0.48, and 0.33, respectively. However, there was an effect of treat- ment for the effect question, F(2, 12) =
  • 33. 9.76, p = .003; F(2, 12) = 11.78, p = .001; and F(2, 12) =9.81, p = .003, respec- tively. Specific comparisons indicated that the text structure group scored sig- JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES118 nificantly higher than either the con- tent-only ( p = .002, .002, and .005, re- spectively) or the no-instruction group ( p = .003, 001, and .001, respectively). There was no difference between the content-only and the no-instruction groups (see Table 3). On the fourth transfer measure, the one cause–one effect authentic text, there was no effect of treatment for the noncausal question, the cause ques- tion, or the effect question, F(2, 12) = 0.48, 0.83, and 0.48, respectively. Effect Sizes Effect sizes ranged from 6.14 to 15.19 on the strategy measures, from 2.55 to 5.66 on the content outcome measures, and from 1.57 to 3.24 on the compre- hension outcome measures, all of which are considered large effects (Cohen, 1988).
  • 34. Fidelity to Treatment Because of scheduling difficulties, there were 19 classroom observations instead of 20. Teachers taught every section of every lesson, except for the TABLE 3 Posttest Performance by Condition: Proportion of Correct Responses on Content and Comprehension Outcome Measures TSa COa NIa Measure M SD M SD M SD F (2, 12) Specific comparisons ES Content Outcome Measures Feature Questions .41 .20 .45 .16 .08 .05 8.57** TS > NI** 2.55 CO > NI** 3.43 Non-Feature Questions .41 .19 .47 .22 .05 .01 9.03** TS > NI** 3.51 CO > NI** 3.63 Vocabulary Definitions .48 .10 .56 .15 .13 .02 23.24*** TS > NI*** 5.66 CO > NI*** 4.96 Comprehension Outcome Measures Explicit Teaching, Written, One cause– one effect Noncausal Question .77 .18 .76 .17 .42 .20 5.82* TS > NI* 1.84 CO > NI* 1.84
  • 35. Cause Question .61 .25 .46 .23 .38 .19 1.39 Effect Question .30 .22 .08 .06 .05 .07 4.89* TS > CO* 1.57 TS > NI* 1.72 Transfer, Oral, One cause–multiple effects Near Transfer Noncausal Question .60 .18 .61 .19 .45 .10 1.59 Cause Question .27 .22 .17 .11 .20 .10 0.51 Effect Question .33 .16 .07 .06 .08 .06 9.76** TS > CO** 2.34 TS > NI** 2.27 Far Transfer Noncausal Question .57 .22 .68 .10 .38 .18 3.62 Cause Question .07 .11 .10 .07 .05 .07 0.48 Effect Question .28 .10 .10 .05 .08 .05 11.78*** TS > CO** 2.47 TS > NI*** 2.59 Authentic Text Noncausal Question .65 .12 .66 .15 .63 .10 0.07 Cause Question .63 .24 .69 .05 .62 .13 0.33 Effect Question .42 .10 .22 .12 .17 .05 9.81** TS > CO** 1.88 TS > NI*** 3.24 Transfer, Oral, One cause–one effect Authentic Text Noncausal Question .70 .14 .76 .08 .75 .08 0.48 Cause Question .67 .17 .56 .12 .58 .13 0.83 Effect Question .43 .23 .49 .10 .38 .17 0.48
  • 36. Note. TS = text structure condition; CO = content-only condition; NI = no-instruction condition. an = 5. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. VOLUME 40, NUMBER 2, MARCH/APRIL 2007 119 lesson review. The review section was taught during 3 of the 10 observations of text structure lessons and during 5 of the 9 observations of the content- only lessons. Discussion This study extends the findings of Williams et al. (2005) to the content area of social studies and strengthens the conclusion that explicit compre- hension instruction can be effective at the primary-grade level. On the Ex- plicit Teaching paragraph, the text structure group scored higher than the other groups on the effect question. This group did not score higher on the cause question; perhaps the students were sufficiently familiar with the cause concept before the instruction began. The explicit instruction pro- vided by the text structure program, however, was necessary for the more difficult effect concept. We did not ex- pect a difference between the text
  • 37. structure and the content-only groups on the noncausal question; our pro- gram was focused specifically on cause–effect. The performance of the two in- structed groups did not differ on any of the three content outcome measures; thus, text structure instruction can be accomplished within the framework of content area instruction without a loss in the amount of content acquired. The results of the present study, however, did not replicate the strong transfer effects found in the Williams et al. (2005) study, although the data sug- gest that some transfer occurred. That is, on three of the four comprehension outcome measures that assessed trans- fer, students who had been taught via the text structure program performed significantly better on questions that involved effects than did students who received the content-only program. However, the text structure group stu- dents were no better than the other stu- dents when answering questions about causes. The results on the noncausal question of the near and far transfer paragraphs showed the same pattern as was seen in the explicit teaching paragraph (i.e., the two instructed groups scored higher than the no- instruction group did), although the
  • 38. effect did not reach significance. One of the four transfer para- graphs did not show the positive ef- fects of the text structure training on the effect question. This was possibly due to the fact that its content was rather abstract (i.e., salt and its uses), and it was the only paragraph for which it was difficult to develop reli- able scoring. One possible reason why transfer effects were not as clear in this study is that the cause–effect structure is more difficult than the compare–contrast structure used in our earlier work (Ciar- diello, 2002; McCormick, 2003; Rich- gels et al., 1987). If this is the case, then providing a greater amount of instruc- tion on cause–effect than we did in the present study might lead to more transfer. Having two text types may also have proved too challenging for second graders. Futhermore, the cause– effect text structure was presented from the beginning in a paragraph for- mat. For young novice readers, a text of this length might be too distracting. Because this study was designed to evaluate the effectiveness of an in- tact instructional program, it cannot be used to determine the relative impor- tance of any of the individual program components. Other experimental de-
  • 39. signs are required for that purpose. However, on the basis of a detailed ex- amination of our results, of the infor- mal data gathered during classroom observations, and of the teacher feed- back, we have identified several areas where we believe modifications in the program are warranted. We have re- vised the program and have under- taken a replication of the study. We are devoting more attention to the concept of cause–effect using familiar content. We do this at the beginning of the pro- gram, although we do not completely eliminate activities involving social studies content early in the program. We also focus more heavily on text analysis of independent sentences (i.e., sentences that are not embedded in paragraphs). We are also teaching only the one cause–one effect pattern, rather than introducing one cause–multiple effects paragraphs. Clue words are added to the lessons gradually, and the graphic organizer and generic ques- tions are simplified. Many students at the second-grade level have not yet mastered word recognition. However, they should not be deprived of comprehension instruc- tion before they become fluent readers. Our program represents the type of lis- tening and reading instruction that can work at this grade level for all stu-
  • 40. dents, including those at risk for aca- demic failure. We believe that this type of instruction has the potential for im- proving comprehension on both oral and written tasks, at the same time that it gives students the opportunity to ac- quire basic knowledge of social studies. ABOUT THE AUTHORS Joanna P. Williams, PhD, is a professor of psychology and education at Teachers College, Columbia University. Her current research in- terests are reading comprehension and the de- velopment of instruction for primary grade stu- dents, including those with learning disabilities and others at risk for academic failure. Abigail M. Nubla-Kung, MA, Simonne Pollini, MA, Amaya Garcia, BA, and Anne G. Snyder, MAT, are currently graduate research assis- tants at Teachers College, Columbia University, specializing in reading comprehension and edu- cational psychology. K. Brooke Stafford, PhD, currently works as a researcher in the Division of Teaching and Learning at the New York City Department of Education, supporting the de- velopment and implementation of curriculum and professional development in the New York City public schools. Address: Joanna Williams, Box 238, Teachers College, Columbia Univer- sity, 525 West 120th St., New York, NY 10027. AUTHORS’ NOTES 1. This research was funded by the Institute of Educational Sciences, U.S. Department of
  • 41. Education. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES120 2. We thank Amy Endo, Chi-Shun Lien, Anne- Marie Tan, and Hsiao-Ling Weng for their help with data collection and processing. REFERENCES Bakken, J. P., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1997). Reading comprehension of expository science material and students with learning disabilities: A comparison of strategies. The Journal of Special Educa- tion, 31, 300–324. Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Gromoll, E. W. (1989). Learning from social studies texts. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 99–158. Brophy, J. (1992). The de facto national cur- riculum in U.S. elementary social studies: Critique of a representative example. The Journal of Curriculum Studies, 24, 401–447. Brophy, J., & Alleman, J. (2002). Learning and teaching about cultural universals in primary-grade social studies. The Elemen- tary School Journal, 103, 99–114. Bryant, D. P., Ugel, N., Thompson, S., & Hamff, A. (1999). Instructional strategies for content-area reading instruction. In-
  • 42. tervention in School and Clinic, 34, 293–302. Campbell, J. R., Kapinus, B., & Beatty, A. S. (1995). Interviewing children about their lit- eracy experiences: Data from NAEP’s in- tegrated reading performance at Grade 4. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Ed- ucation. Caseley, J. (2002). On the town: A community adventure. New York: Greenwillow. Center on Education Policy. (2006). From the capitol to the classroom: Year 4 of the No Child Left Behind Act. Washington, DC: Author. Chall, J. S., & Dale, E. (1995). Readability re- visited: The new Dale-Chall readability for- mula. Cambridge, MA: Brookline. Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- versity Press. Chambliss, M. J., & Calfee, R. C. (1998). Textbooks for learning. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Ciardiello, A. V. (2002). Helping adolescents understand cause/effect text structure in social studies. Social Studies, 93, 31–36. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
  • 43. NJ: Erlbaum. De La Paz, S., & MacArthur, C. A. (2003). Knowing the how and why of history: Expectations for secondary students with and without learning disabilities. Learn- ing Disability Quarterly, 26, 142–154. Dickson, S. (1999). Integrating reading and writing to teach compare–contrast text structure: A research-based methodology. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 14, 49–79. Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in first grade. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 202–224. Ferretti, R. P., & Okolo, C. M. (1996). Au- thenticity in learning: Multimedia design projects in the social studies for students with disabilities. Journal of Learning Dis- abilities, 29, 45–60. Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading com- prehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of re- search. Review of Educational Research, 71, 279–320. Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. (2000). Structural aspects of constructing mean- ing from text. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mo- senthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp.
  • 44. 311–335). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hoffman, J. V., McCarthy, S. J., Abbott, J., Christian, C., Corman, L., Curry, C., et al. (1994). So what’s new in the new basals? A focus on first grade. Journal of Reading Behavior, 26, 47–73. McCormick, S. (2003). Instructing students who have literacy problems (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall. Meyer, B. J. F., & Poon, L. W. (2001). Effects of structure training and signaling on re- call of text. Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 93, 141–159. Moss, B. (2005). Making a case and a place for effective content area literacy instruc- tion in the elementary grades. The Read- ing Teacher, 59, 46–55. Myers, M. P., & Savage, T. (2005). Enhanc- ing student comprehension of social studies material. Social Studies, 96, 18–24. Ogle, D. (1986). K-W-L: A teaching model that develops active reading of exposi- tory text. The Reading Teacher, 39, 564–570. Perfetti, C. A., Marron, M. A., & Foltz, P. W. (1996). Sources of comprehension failure: Theoretical perspectives and case stud- ies. In C. Cornoldi & J. V. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Pro- cesses and remediation (pp. 137–165). Mah-
  • 45. wah, NJ: Erlbaum. Richgels, D. J., McGee, L. M., Lomax, R. G., & Sheard, C. (1987). Awareness of four text structures: Effects on recall of expos- itory text. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 177–196. Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1981). What’s in a story: An approach to comprehension and instruction. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Ad- vances in instructional psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 213–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Vogler, K. (2003). Where does social studies fit in a high-stakes testing environment? Social Studies, 94, 207–211. Williams, J. P. (1998). Improving the com- prehension of disabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 213–238. Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., & Lauer, K. (2004). Teaching expository text structure to young at-risk learners: Building the ba- sics of comprehension instruction. Excep- tionality, 12, 129–144. Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., Lauer, K. D., Stafford, K. B., DeSisto, L. A., & deCani, J. S. (2005). Expository text comprehen- sion in the primary grade classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 235– 248.
  • 46. Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Title Authors & Date Research Question(s) Key Terms Methods Results
  • 47. Major Conclusion(s) Gaps & Limitations Key Concept 1, pg # (Written in your own words! No copy/paste!) Key Concept 2, pg # (Written in your own words! No copy/paste!) Key Concept 3, pg # (Written in your own words! No copy/paste!)