SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 88
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 1
Applying the Five Pillars to Literacy Instruction
With Students Who Have Moderate to Severe Disabilities:
Issues and Concerns
Lewis B. Jackson
University of Northern Colorado
Diane L. Ryndak and
Ann-Marie Orlando
University of Florida
Kara Halley
Metro State College of Denver
Karen McCaleb
Texas A&M University Corpus Christi
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 2
Abstract
The findings and recommendation of the National Reading
Panel (National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development, 2000) have influenced how
literacy skills are conceptualized
and taught in schools. Although the report’s findings and
recommendation were directed at
students without disabilities, they have the potential to impact
instruction and instructional
research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities.
To explore this, we used the
National Reading Panel’s five pillars framework (i.e., phonemic
awareness, phonics, fluency,
vocabulary, and reading comprehension) to raise issues and
concerns about literacy instruction
research and practices with students who have moderate to
severe disabilities. In our discussion,
we assume the point of view of teachers who wish to improve
their practices by delving into the
literacy research base, opening with a discussion of how
scientific evidence can serve as a guide
for improving literacy instruction. This is followed by a
discussion of specific issues and
concerns related to each of the five pillars, illustrated by
research studies in which the
participants have moderate to severe disabilities. We conclude
by summarizing our concerns; by
exploring additional concerns that go across the five pillars; and
by posing arguments that
present questions regarding the applicability of the National
Reading Panel’s (2000) findings for
students with moderate to severe disabilities. Lastly, the paper
considers the potential role of
literacy research and practice as contributing factors in an
unreconciled dichotomy between a
body of research demonstrating the power of evidence-based
instruction with these students, and
another body of research showing the continued denial of
literacy opportunities to them in
schools.
Keywords: Literacy; Reading; National Reading Panel;
moderate to severe disabilities;
evidence-based instruction
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 3
Applying the Five Pillars to Literacy Instruction
With Students Who Have Moderate to Severe Disabilities:
Issues and Concerns
In the broadest sense, literacy involves understanding, using,
and producing print for a
variety of purposes, where print may include text, symbols,
and/or images. While the ability to
engage in literacy activities is critical for participation in a
print-driven society, Lonigan and
Shanahan (2010) indicated that pinpointing what actually
constitutes “literacy” is not a
straightforward process. Part of the dilemma lies with
determining what skills best define,
characterize, and promote literacy competence, an issue with
practical implications for teachers.
To be effective, teachers must select for instruction those
literacy skills that are most essential for
their students to complete literacy tasks in today’s increasingly
complex and demanding society.
This issue also has implications for research designed to
enhance our understanding, and
improve our delivery, of literacy interventions. Teachers are
being asked to use practices based
on scientific evidence, requiring that they survey and critically
evaluate the extant research on
literacy interventions to identify effective instructional
practices for realizing particular literacy
outcomes. One could say that a litmus test for examining the
research on literacy interventions is
whether a study or review of studies discloses sufficient
information regarding the pedagogies
employed and the outcomes realized, such that teachers can use
the information to inform their
practices. Teachers, however, also must consider external
validity, or the generalizability of
research results (Campbell & Stanley, 1963); that is, whether a
particular study, a group of
studies, or a review of studies can contribute meaningfully to
their decisions about practice
because there are parallels between the participants, contexts,
and outcomes reported in the
research and those within their own instructional situations. As
noted by Campbell and Stanley,
“in the area of teaching, the doubts frequently expressed as to
the applicability in actual practice
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 4
of the results of highly artificial experiments are judgments
about external validity” (p. 17).
While the process of choosing essential literacy outcomes and
delineating effective
pedagogies for students with moderate to severe disabilities
might seem to be straightforward, it
is in fact fraught with difficulties. In the broader field of
literacy, this is evident in the protracted
“reading wars” that have occurred regarding what constitutes
the best approaches to reading
instruction (e.g., Flippo, 1999). “Reading wars” refers to
repeated shifts across the years in what
defines the essential outcomes and most effective instructional
methods in reading and writing
instruction, shifting between meaning- and code-based
approaches. To illustrate, when reviewing
this history Cassidy, Valadez, and Garrett (2010) described how
the whole language movement
(i.e., a meaning-based approach) emerged in the 1980s largely
in reaction to the phonics-based
movement (i.e., a code-based approach) of the previous era.
Partially as an attempt to end the debates fueled by the whole
language versus phonics
dichotomy, in 1997 the U.S. Congress authorized the formation
of a panel of experts to examine
the empirical research on literacy instruction with school-aged
readers who do not have
disabilities, and to make recommendations about effective
practices in terms of the scientific
evidence (Cassidy et al., 2010). The result was the formation of
the National Reading Panel,
which provided recommendations grouped into what are now
called the “five pillars” of reading
instruction: (1) phonemic awareness, (2) phonics, (3) fluency,
(4) vocabulary, and (5) text
comprehension (National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, 2000; henceforth
referred to as NRP, 2000). Although intended to attenuate the
pendulum swings between
meaning- and code-based approaches, the years following the
report’s publication were marked
by a shift to code-based thinking, possibly the result of the
report’s heavy emphasis on code-
based approaches (Cassidy et al.). Whether or not the panel
intended for the five pillars to
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 5
delineate “the most important areas for reading instruction”
(Cassidy et al., p. 644), these pillars
now influence how reading instruction is conceptualized and
implemented in many schools.
Although impacted by the debates between meaning- and code-
based approaches to
literacy instruction, teachers of students with moderate to
severe disabilities have faced an
additional dichotomy in their efforts to delineate evidence-
based approaches to, and maximize
student outcomes from, literacy instruction. This dichotomy is
between a body of evidence
showing that certain forms of instruction are effective when
teaching specific types of reading
skills to students with moderate to severe disabilities (Browder,
Wakeman, Spooner, Ahlgrim-
Delzell, & Algozzine, 2006; Whalon, Otaiba, & Delano, 2009),
and another body of evidence
suggesting that literacy opportunities are systematically denied
to these students (Kliewer &
Biklen, 2001; Ryndak, Morrison, & Sommerstein, 1999;
Zascavage & Keefe, 2004). These latter
studies describe attitudes of school personnel that presume
inherent limitations in these students’
abilities to become literate, as well as the failure of general and
special education programs to
provide them access to literacy activities and communities
evident throughout schools.
This dichotomy between evidence of success at literacy tasks
when provided instruction
and restricted literacy opportunities likely has complex roots,
involving both the policies and
procedures of educational systems and the attitudes and beliefs
of school personnel. It also is
possible, however, that part of the problem lies with the content
taught and the pedagogies used
when literacy skills are taught to students with moderate to
severe disabilities. Perhaps what is
taught and how it is taught to these students, when contrasted
with what is taught and how it is
taught to typical students in general education, is sufficiently
discrepant to discourage viewing
these efforts and their outcomes as relevant indices of potential
for success in the larger literacy
activities and communities of schools.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 6
Considering that teachers are being asked to base instructional
decisions on research
(Chhabra & McCardle, 2004; Reyna, 2004), and in light of the
foregoing issues, it might be
productive to examine literacy instruction research conducted
with students who have moderate
to severe disabilities, and consider what this research promotes
as useful when selecting literacy
outcomes and instructional practices for these students.
Moreover, given that schools must
provide all students with access to, and ensure their progress in,
the general curriculum
(Copeland & Cosbey, 2009; McLaughlin, 2010), it might be
productive to consider whether this
research offers teachers guidance on using literacy instruction
to promote these broader ends.
Although focused on the learning of typical school-aged
students, the five pillars
delineated by the National Reading Panel (NRP, 2000) might
offer a useful framework for
exploring the foregoing issues. Both Downing (2005) and
Copeland and Keefe (2007) noted that
definitions of literacy typically employed when defining and
describing instruction with students
who have moderate to severe disabilities often emphasize the
teaching of reading and writing
skills related to the literal interpretation of text, and this
perspective seems consistent with the
NRP (2000) report. In addition, there seems to be a substantial
body of quantitative research with
students who have moderate to severe disabilities that employs
direct instructional processes that
are intended to affect discrete literacy skills, an approach to
instruction that would appear to be
consistent with the NRP’s emphasis on direct and explicit
instruction. Finally, other
examinations of the reading instruction literature have
employed this framework to usefully
organize relevant research (e.g., Browder et al., 2006).
Accordingly, in this paper, we will use the NRP’s five pillars to
frame our exploration of
issues and concerns about literacy instruction research for
students with moderate to severe
disabilities. To provide a practical focus, we will also do this
through the lens of teachers who
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 7
might be examining the literature in search of ways to
incorporate in their instruction literacy
practices that are supported by research with students who have
moderate to severe disabilities.
The first part of this paper opens with a discussion on the nature
and relevance of scientific
evidence when used to guide reading instruction, followed by
sub-sections for each of the five
pillars. Each sub-section provides a discussion of specific
issues and concerns related to that
pillar, coupled with illustrations drawn from the extant research
on literacy instruction. The
second and final part of this paper summarizes concerns with
the research associated with each
of the five pillars; explores issues and concerns that go across
the pillars; poses questions about
the applicability of the National Reading Panel’s (2000)
findings for literacy instruction for
students with moderate to severe disabilities; and re-considers
the dichotomy described above
between evidence of success at literacy tasks and restricted
literacy opportunities. In this section,
we also acknowledge limitations associated with the critical
analysis offered in this paper.
Scientific Evidence, and Issues and Concerns Related to the
Five Pillars
Chhabra and McCardle (2004) asserted that the NRP (2000)
report established high
standards for evaluating scientific research when planning and
providing reading instruction.
These authors asserted that teachers should access research
reports and peer-reviewed journals
“to read the information first hand, to decide for themselves
how good the evidence is” (p. 9).
What constitutes scientific evidence lies at the core of any
discussion about how to use
research to make instructional decisions. In her discussion of
reading research and the nature of
evidence, Reyna (2004) argued that one way to define scientific
evidence is to distinguish it from
alternative forms of “evidence” that might be used in
instructional decision-making. She noted
that classroom instruction, when not based on scientific
evidence, can be based on appeals to
tradition, philosophy, intuition, superstition, and anecdote, and
she suggested that such appeals
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 8
have been “the only options available” to many teachers of
reading. Reyna further argued that
scientific evidence requires knowing about and using processing
models of learning, which take
into account factors such as attention, motivation, and the
storage and retrieval of information.
She asserted that all who are involved in instructional decision-
making need to be familiar with
basic processes of learning, and by extension with what the
research says about how to engage
those processes most effectively to teach reading and other
literacy skills.
We suggest that, while the foregoing views on how to
characterize scientific evidence are
important, they do not fully go to the heart of the nature of
“scientific evidence” when applied to
instructional decision-making, and that teachers must consider
at least three other factors when
evaluating instructional research. The first and most obvious
factor we suggest, and the one most
consistent with Reyna’s (2004) arguments, is whether a strategy
or instructional approach has
been demonstrated to have, or not have, an effect; for example,
whether skill acquisition
occurred, whether the occurrence of a performance change can
be linked to the described
intervention and not to other reported or non-reported events,
and whether acquisition occurred
more rapidly or efficiently using one method over another. The
second factor that we suggest
teachers must consider is the robustness of the findings; that is,
does a study provide evidence
that skills acquired by the students are retained across time or
are shown to generalize into
relevant and purposeful activities and settings outside of the
immediate instructional situation.
Third and finally, we suggest that teachers must consider the
utility of a study’s findings; that is,
does a study address or show how the skills that were acquired
by students promote or result in
the formation of other more advanced skills, provide access to
curriculum or other materials, or
contribute to successful functioning within a broader range of
literacy activities.
Addressing these three issues within a published report of
research could be difficult, and
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 9
might require researchers of literacy instruction to go beyond
the intended scopes and purposes
of their studies. It also could be argued that researchers need
not elaborate on how the discrete
skills learned by students during their intervention studies are
connected with other skills or
contribute to advanced activity participation, if such
relationships seem to have been established
elsewhere in the extant research. Nevertheless, we propose that
asking teachers to use scientific
evidence in planning instruction requires researchers to apply a
stringent standard of what
constitutes evidence of meaningful change. We propose that
such a standard extends the analysis
process associated with a study’s results from simply showing
how a discrete skill set was
acquired and toward showing whether a broader range of
variables associated with the literacy
lives of the students have been impacted. This is not unlike
asking researchers addressing
literacy practices in their studies to also address the social
validity of their findings (Wolf, 1978).
The immediate implications of our expanded view of the nature
of scientific evidence is
that it will color what we propose are the important issues and
concerns related to the five pillars
of reading delineated by the NRP (2000). As we discuss each
pillar, we will share our
perceptions of what a teacher needs to see in a study, or in a
body of studies, to gain confidence
in the evidence. We believe that by doing so, the concept of
scientific evidence can be fleshed
out to improve the utility of future research for teachers.
Phonemic Awareness
Phonemes are the smallest units of sound that “influence the
meaning of words” (Gillon,
2004, p. 7); hence, phonemic awareness is “the awareness that
the speech stream consists of a
sequence of sounds” (Yopp & Yopp, 2000, p. 130). Instruction
in phonemic awareness involves
assisting students to focus on and manipulate sounds in orally-
presented syllables and words
(NRP, 2000), and measurement of phonemic awareness involves
tasks that use spoken language
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 10
(Gillon). In its findings, the NRP reported that explicit training
in phonemic awareness resulted
in improvements in “phonemic awareness, reading, and
spelling,” and that “teaching children to
manipulate phonemes” was highly effective across a variety of
students (p. 7).
In their meta-analysis of reading intervention research with
students who have moderate
to severe disabilities published between 1975 and 2003,
Browder et al. (2006) reported only 5
studies that included phonemic awareness instruction, none of
which appear to have been
candidates for inclusion in their effect size analysis. However,
we think that teachers who comb
the research literature looking for studies that address the
effects of instruction on phonemic
awareness for students who have moderate to severe disabilities
will find a number of studies
suggesting that explicit, one-to-one instruction can lead to the
acquisition of targeted sound
recognition and production skills when taught in isolated or
special education settings
(Hoogeveen & Smeets,1988; Hoogeveen, Birkhoff, Smeets,
Lancioni, & Boelens, 1989).
We have, however, four concerns about the use of this research
to guide teachers’
decision-making for literacy instruction. First, we wonder how
the available research in this area
can inform practice when some number of these studies mix
phonemic awareness instruction
with instruction in other skills, such as skills associated with
printed material, phonics, and
vocabulary (Bracey, Maggs, & Morath, 1975; Browder,
Ahlgrim-Delzell, Courtade, Gibbs, &
Flowers, 2008; Conners, Rosenquist, Sligh, Atwell, & Kiser,
2006; Goetz et al., 2008).
Certainly, given the successes of these studies, it seems
reasonable to propose that instruction in
phonemic awareness skills should be routinely integrated with
instruction in other reading skills
(see also Gillon, 2004), and that instruction in phonemic
awareness can be supported by using
pictures and real words (see also Allor, Mathes, Champlin, &
Cheatham, 2009). Nevertheless,
because the phonemic awareness instruction is confounded with
instruction in other skills in
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 11
these studies, the reported changes in neither phonemic
awareness nor other skills can be linked
unequivocally to the phonemic awareness component of the
instruction. If the broader literacy
instruction research base in moderate to severe disabilities also
reflects this pattern, then the
contribution of phonemic awareness instruction toward literacy
skill building is unclear.
Second, we question whether phonemic awareness skills taught
to students with moderate
to severe disabilities in relative isolation can have the desired
cumulative effects sought by
teachers, in which skills taught early in the process facilitate
the acquisition of skills taught
subsequently. Hoogeveen and Smeets (1988), for instance,
implemented a reading program with
seven young Dutch students, beginning with word blending
tasks, followed by syllable and
phoneme blending in isolation, and ending with instruction in
reading one-syllable words. Some
“spill-over effects” were reported, in which students produced
correct responses in later steps of
the reading program without direct training; however, the
authors concluded that “the training
effects were considerably stronger than the spillover effects” (p.
51). That is, the authors
concluded that the progress that occurred as students advanced
step-by-step through the reading
program was much more likely to be a function of direct
instruction on each step, rather than the
generalization of skills from previous steps.
Third, we note that numerous trials often seem to be required to
achieve acquisition of
these very basic sound recognition skills, and we wonder about
the causal factors underlying this
pattern. It seems to us that there might be a belief that
instruction is necessarily intense and
prolonged because of the presence of the students’ disabilities.
However, there also is the
possibility that the amount of time for instruction and the
number of trials reported are intense
and prolonged not because of the student’s disability, but
because: (a) the student perceives the
instruction or content as tedious, uninteresting, or unrelated to
their personal interests; or (b)
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 12
there is an absence of other, more varied experiences with
literacy that could lend meaning and
purpose to the phonemic awareness skill acquisition (e.g.,
storybook reading).
Fourth and finally, if an ultimate goal of literacy instruction is
for the student to access
and comprehend content (Snow, 2002), then teachers must think
strategically about how they
portion their limited instructional time and resources in relation
to different literacy skill sets. We
remain uncertain as to how focused instruction in phonemic
awareness contributes in the long
run to the emergence of meaning-based skills (e.g., reading
comprehension), which can more
directly facilitate content access. None of the studies referred to
above adequately examined this
issue, and the NRP’s finding that explicit instruction in
phonemic awareness can benefit other
reading skills can be confirmed only with respect to instruction
for students with moderate to
severe disabilities through additional research. We contend that
there is a need for longitudinal
research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities
in various contexts in which a
broad range of phonemic awareness experiences (e.g., direct
instruction, embedded instruction,
incidental learning opportunities) are correlated with learning
other literacy skills, especially
learning to comprehend vocabulary and text. Moreover, we see
a need for this research to
conceptualize theoretical mechanisms that can: (a) account not
only for phonemic awareness
skill acquisition, but also for both failed and successful skill
retention and generalization; and (b)
explain how the development of other literacy skills is impeded
or facilitated, given either a
deficient or well-developed skill base in phonemic awareness.
Phonics
Phonics is a “way of teaching reading and spelling that stresses
symbol-sound
relationships” (Yopp & Yopp, 2000, p. 131). The NRP (2000)
emphasized phonics instruction as
an important vehicle for acquiring the ability to identify and use
printed letter-sound
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 13
relationships both in isolation and in a variety of print contexts.
An essential reading skill that is
facilitated by phonics instruction is decoding – the process of
sounding out words by using the
regularities associated with letters and groups of letters to
facilitate recognition of familiar words
and to assist in identifying unknown words (Gough & Tunmer,
1986). While noting that phonics
instruction is insufficient when used alone, the NRP strongly
recommended systematic explicit
phonics instruction for realizing the “foundational skills” of
reading in students (p. 9). The NRP
asserted that when systematic phonics instruction is employed,
“significant benefits” are accrued
in reading and spelling, and benefits are noted in reading
comprehension, for students of all ages
and for students who are struggling readers (pp. 9-10).
There is a dearth of studies examining instruction for students
with moderate to severe
disabilities in phonological structure and decoding processes
associated with printed words (see
Joseph & Seery, 2004); nevertheless, there are studies
suggesting that systematic phonics
instruction might hold promise for teaching these and related
skills. For example, Bracey et al.
(1975) used the “task-analyzed, structured” materials of the
Direct Instructional System for
Teaching and Remediation (DISTAR) Reading Level 1 program,
along with token reinforcement,
to teach six children, aged 7 to14 years, skills for producing and
blending sounds in response to
written letters and words. Fallon, Light, McNaughton, Drager,
and Hammer (2004) combined
systematic instruction in the phonological structures of words
with word recognition tasks to
promote reading specific single words, reading other words with
the same letters, and reading
both types of words in text for five students ranging in age from
5 to14 years. A unique feature
of their study was that the participants were nonverbal, thus
their study required substituting
picture-word association responses for spoken responses across
all tasks. All five students
reached criteria on single-word reading; three responded
correctly to some novel words
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 14
composed of the same letters; and four responded correctly to
some targeted and untaught words
presented in text. In a final illustration, Bradford, Shippen,
Alberto, Houchins, and Flores (2006)
examined the use of systematic instruction to teach three
students, aged 12 to 15 years, to sound
out and read words both in isolation and in text formats. They
used a modified version of Level
A of the Corrective Reading Program curriculum (Engelmann,
Meyer, Carnine, Becker, Eisele,
& Johnson, 1998), teaching the students to say and write letter
sounds, to say and write sounds in
words, and to sound out words while saying them fast. Over a
six-month period, all three
students showed evidence of identifying letter sound
correspondences, sounding out and
blending words, and applying these skills when reading
sentences and short passages. The
students also showed increased recognition of words from either
the Edmark Reading Program
or the Dolch Sight Word list.
The foregoing studies exemplify the promise of systematic
instruction, especially when
using programmed materials. However, we have four concerns
related to what teachers should
expect as a result of their instruction and one concern related to
how teachers address the context
in which instruction occurs. Our first concern is that the
foregoing and other studies (e.g.,
Gersten & Maggs, 1982) do not provide evidence of changes in
reading comprehension – a
desired outcome of phonics-based instruction (NRP, 2000).
Given that students with Autism
Spectrum Disorder can acquire “code-based skills” relatively
easily but then show persistent
problems with comprehension (Whalon et al., 2009), teachers
who are considering the use of
instructional time to teach students with moderate to severe
disabilities decoding and other
phonics-related skills need more scientific evidence showing
how phonics instruction can
concurrently build and support comprehension.
A second concern is that programmed instructional packages
available to teachers tend to
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 15
construct skills in an additive fashion, with skills overlearned in
isolation then becoming the
same expectations within programmed text passages. Teachers
need scientific evidence that
skills learned from reading programmed text transfer to other
authentic reading materials, and the
foregoing studies do not provide sufficient evidence of this. A
third concern of ours is that
teachers will wonder about the role of rote memorization, as
opposed to the “true” problem-
solving processes of word attack and decoding, when students
are taught phonics-related word
recognition strategies via systematic, discrete trial instruction.
Put differently, teachers need
more information than is presently provided about the
mechanisms of letter and word processing
that are employed by phonics-learning students with moderate
to severe disabilities when they
are engaged in tasks such as reading text passages. A fourth
concern relates to one of the primary
purposes for learning to read; that is, accessing subject matter
and acquiring new information.
We contend that teachers need more scientific evidence than
provided in the extant literature as
to how students with moderate to severe disabilities who are
receiving instruction on decoding
skills can use these skills to access and utilize authentic age-
appropriate text materials. Fifth and
finally, while there is evidence that systematic instruction
conducted in self-contained or isolated
contexts can lead to performance changes in phonics-related
skills for students with moderate to
severe disabilities, we contend that teachers need proof that the
same results could not have been
more readily achieved -- and with other academic and social
benefits -- via general education
class participation, with or without brief pull-aside
interventions.
There exists the possibility that our five concerns will prove to
be unfounded. However,
we raise these concerns to encourage researchers to address
them in future reviews of the
literature and in their future research, so that teachers will have
the scientific evidence that they
need to confidently apply research results in their classroom
practices.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 16
Fluency
Fluency is a composite concept consisting of rate, accuracy, and
prosody. More proficient
readers display greater ease and natural expression in their oral
production of continuous
narrative and appear to comprehend text more as they read it
(Hicks, 2009; NRP, 2000). The
NRP (2000) extended this finding by interpreting the research
as showing that, as students build
fluency via guided oral reading, they also develop
comprehension skills.
When considering the research on fluency for students with
moderate to severe
disabilities, there appear to be three ways that fluency
instruction is being conceptualized. First,
we interpret some of the research as conceptualizing instruction
on fluency as teaching
nonreaders to accurately read aloud sequences of words that
originally were taught in isolation
(see Brown et al., 1972; Brown & Perlmutter, 1971). For
example, Brown and Perlmutter (1971)
successfully taught seven students who were nonreaders to read
word-by-word 9 short sentences
(e.g., “The penny is on top of the box”), and then perform
simple motor response related to the
sentences (e.g., pointing to the penny on top of the box).
Second, we interpret some of the
research as conceptualizing instruction on fluency as teaching
students who already possess
some text reading skills to improve their oral reading accuracy.
For example, Singh and
colleagues (see Singh, 1990; Singh & Singh, 1985a; Singh &
Singh, 1984; Singh & Singh, 1986;
Singh, Singh, & Winton, 1984) focused on accurate reading and
the self-correction of reading
errors through the use of guided oral reading of short passages
and stories, and found a variety of
systematic interventions that had a positive impact on oral
reading accuracy. Third, we interpret
some of the research as conceptualizing instruction on fluency
as teaching multiple reading skills
across several or all of the five pillars, and aspects of fluency
serve as indices of changes in
reading proficiency. For example, over a one-year period in
self-contained classrooms Apffel,
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 17
Kelleher, Lilly, and Richardson (1975) used either the DISTAR
reading program that emphasizes
“a phonics approach to reading,” or the Rebus reading program
that emphasizes a whole word
approach (p. 230), to teach 60 students labeled “trainable
mentally retarded.” Based on progress
in oral reading skills (e.g., reading accuracy and errors) the
authors concluded that, regardless of
which program was used, “…nearly all of the 60 participating
TMR students demonstrated some
capacity to profit from reading instruction” (p. 229). More
recently a study by Osborn et al.
(2007) reflected this conceptualization of fluency when over a
six-month period they used peer
tutors to augment instruction regularly provided to elementary-
aged students, including 31
students with moderate to severe disabilities. The instruction
targeted skills related to all five of
the pillars and was provided during one-to-one tutoring sessions
lasting 30-minutes each, for
three or four days each week. They found that students in the
experimental group showed
significantly higher scores on the fluency subtest of the
Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early
Literacy Skills (DIBELS; Good & Kaminski, 1996).
The studies reflecting all three conceptualizations of fluency
instruction suggest that
specific forms of systematic instruction can be effective when
applied to aspects of fluency.
However, teachers reviewing these studies need to look beyond
evidence of acquisition and into
issues of the value and utility of the reported results.
We are concerned when fluency is conceptualized in the first
way (i.e., teaching skills for
accurately reading and responding to sequences of discrete
words previously taught in isolation)
whether teachers can anticipate generalization to other
curriculum-relevant reading materials
following significant investments of instruction and time.
Reading to acquire meaning relies on a
generative process in which word interchangeability is a
necessary quality. While the Brown and
Perlmutter (1971) study described above was groundbreaking in
showing how literacy goals
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 18
involving connected text were attainable by students with
moderate to severe disabilities, the
study reported no generalization data in relation to
combinations of other different words.
To address the foregoing concern, teachers might augment their
instruction with matrix
training, which has been reported to be successful in the
communication instruction literature
(e.g., Goldstein, 1983; Nigam, Schlosser, & Lloyd, 2006;
Striefel, Wetherby, & Karlan, 1978). In
this approach, symbols or words representing action words,
objects, and adjectives are
systematically varied in relation to each other during
instruction, and generalization to novel
word combinations within the matrix of targeted words is
examined as instruction proceeds. At
least with respect to oral language and augmentative
communication systems some positive
results have been reported; however, teachers still should look
for evidence that students can
move beyond words and word sequences contained within the
matrices, and such evidence is not
adequate in these studies.
When fluency is conceptualized in the second way (i.e.,
improving accuracy during oral
reading of connected text) we note the inadequate consideration
of two key components of
fluency, rate and prosody. There are, however, additional
concerns that relate directly to the
robustness of the successful skill changes that are reported. For
instance, in the foregoing studies
by Singh and colleagues: (a) improvements in reading accuracy
were documented mostly within
intervention contexts in which there was high teacher control, a
condition which might promote
students’ dependence on the instructing adults (McNaughton,
1981); (b) not all measures of
accuracy showed improvements from baseline to intervention;
(c) generalized use of skills was
seldom assessed and, when assessed, it did not always occur;
(d) maintenance of skills was
seldom assessed and, when it was assessed, data were collected
over short time intervals and
with the same text passages that were used during instruction;
and finally, (e) measures of
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 19
reading comprehension were used infrequently and, when used,
the results were equivocal.
When fluency is conceptualized in the third way (i.e., multiple
reading skills are taught
addressing some or all of the five pillars, and fluency indices
are reported) we have concerns
regarding the usefulness and the robustness of the research
evidence. For example, Apffel et al.
(1975) reported overall positive changes in oral reading
proficiency in relation to both reading
programs; however, the results of an analysis of patterns of
variation in skill production showed
that (a) individual students varied notably in their responses to
the two different programs, and
(b) oral reading “error rates” and “correct rates” varied
considerably even in students receiving
instruction in the same program. We also are concerned about
the usefulness of their findings
because they report, “Children who had not been able to
demonstrate adequate progress were
reassigned to TMR classes where reading instruction was less
strongly emphasized” (p. 232).
Few details were provided about the criteria used to re-assign
students to the other classes, and
no information was provided on the number of students
dropped. Attrition and lack of details
raise questions regarding the applicability of the study’s
evidence to the full population of
students. Similarly, while Osborn et al. (2007) reported
positive changes in fluency in relation to
their peer-tutoring program, there were other reported results
that raised questions about the
learning that occurred in their study participants. Specifically,
instruction occurred across all five
pillars; yet, there were no significant results on any of the other
reading measures, including a
composite Woodcock-Johnson index (Woodcock-Johnson
Proficiency Battery – Revised,
Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) that incorporated reading
comprehension. Finally, across
the studies considered here, evidence that changes in fluency
are accompanied by changes in
reading comprehension seem insufficient. For teachers who
want reading instruction to
ultimately contribute to comprehension, we believe this absence
of evidence should be troubling.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 20
When considering the need to employ instructional methods that
best equip students for
accessing and comprehending curriculum content, we contend
that more research is needed on
how fluency is impacted by instruction in phonological and
decoding skills, and how such
outcomes relate to reading comprehension. To illustrate,
consider that reading rate is a function
of both reading proficiency and text complexity (Hicks, 2009;
Kuhn, Schwanenflugel, &
Meisinger, 2010), and the mark of the proficient reader is not
rate per se but rather “appropriate
pacing” so that comprehension is facilitated (Kuhn et al.). In
their study that taught decoding
skills, Bradford et al. (2006) reported that fluency worsened as
students advanced in their reading
skills and were exposed to longer text passages. They stated that
“…as passages became longer,
[the students’] fluency seemed to be [adversely] affected” (p.
341). This raises questions about
the course and function of fluency change when documented
progress is occurring in reading
skills related to the other pillars, especially phonics. We wonder
whether what Bradford et al.
saw with the onset of fluency problems was changes in reading
pace that reflected emerging and
tentative efforts by the students to comprehend the more
complex material. Alternatively, we
wonder whether these breakdowns reflect difficulties in
decoding or word recall when the
quantity of information being memorized as a consequence of
rote instruction is substantial. If
the former, then further research is needed to determine whether
concurrent instruction in
comprehension could alleviate fluency problems when progress
is being made in rudimentary
skills, such as decoding and word recognition; if the latter, we
need to better understand how to
enhance learning, perhaps considering more closely the role of
both instructional timing and
affect in how reading skills are acquired. Regardless, evidence
that bears on specific learning
patterns such as reported above would assist teachers in
understanding how instructional
methods reported as “successful” should be configured and
delivered to ensure that students with
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 21
moderate to severe disabilities are being taught meaningful and
useful literacy skills.
Vocabulary
The NRP (2000) characterized vocabulary knowledge as skills
for recognizing and
understanding words presented both orally and in print. The
panel acknowledged the historical
importance of vocabulary instruction in the teaching of reading,
and it portrayed the acquisition
of vocabulary as a dynamic process, characterized by
“continuous growth in word knowledge,”
by the interplay of decoding skills with word knowledge, and by
the ongoing recall of previous
word knowledge when encountering new words (p. 13).
Emphasizing that the “essence” of
reading is comprehension, the NRP asserted that the magnitude
of a person’s vocabulary
knowledge is a critical factor in his or her ability at “making
sense of text” (p.13). The NRP
stressed the importance of repetition and repeated exposure to
words when teaching vocabulary,
and it called for direct and explicit instruction on vocabulary.
Finally, the NRP asserted that
vocabulary knowledge is not just the ability to identify words
on sight; rather, it is an integral,
progressive, and ever expanding aspect of reading
comprehension.
When considering research studies in which successful
vocabulary instruction has been
demonstrated for students with moderate to severe disabilities,
we have two concerns. First,
although there has been a longstanding emphasis on vocabulary
instruction with students who
have moderate to severe disabilities (Browder et al., 2006;
Browder & Xin, 1998; Singh &
Singh, 1985b), we are concerned that this vocabulary
instruction has sometimes focused on
teaching students to memorize word lists that do not reflect the
NRP (2000) emphasis on
vocabulary acquisition as a progressive, iterative, and ever-
expanding process. For instance,
there are studies focusing on the acquisition of lists of discrete
vocabulary words that are
selected (a) based on their conceptual relation to community
purposes, such as reading signs,
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 22
recognizing danger, or completing common forms (e.g., Ault,
Gast, & Wolery, 1988; Blyden,
1989; Collins & Stinson, 1994; Doyle, Gast, Wolery, Ault, &
Meyer, 1992; Karsh, Repp, &
Lenz, 1990; Miracle, Collins, Schuster, & Grisham-Brown,
2001; Singleton, Schuster, Morse, &
Collins, 1999); and/or (b) based on consideration of what very
young or beginning readers might
be expected to learn or easily perform (Barudin & Hourcade,
1990; Koury & Browder, 1986;
Van der Bijl, Alant, & Lloyd, 2006). We think that these
methods for selecting and teaching
vocabulary words can be limiting, because they are not
explicitly designed to provide students
with moderate to severe disabilities substantive links to
narrative text materials, to relevant
grade-level curriculum, or to interest-based materials.
Second, we are concerned that, when comprehension was
assessed in these vocabulary
instruction research studies, it was not always assessed in a
manner that is consistent with NRP
(2000) vocabulary instruction concepts. For example, there are
studies in which comprehension
was assessed through students’: (a) recognition of the correct
word when shown a picture (Basil
& Reyes, 2003; Doyle, Wolery, Gast, & Ault., 1990; Griffen,
Schuster, & Morse, 1998); (b)
verbalization of a simple definition when shown the target word
and given a verbal prompt
(Gast, Wolery, Morris, Doyle, & Meyer, 1990; Griffen et al.,
1998); (c) recall of a target word
when exposed to different or more natural contexts (e.g., Doyle
et al., 1990; Rohena, Jitendra, &
Browder, 2002; Singleton et al., 1999); or (d) performance of a
motor response consistent with
the meaning of the word (Collins & Griffen, 1996; Doyle et al.,
1990).
While there are probably a number of reasons why studies of
vocabulary instruction
involving students with moderate to severe disabilities have
chosen their particular dependent
measures, we suggest that one reason might lie with the
intentions and purposes behind these
studies. It seems likely that a number of these studies have
focused less on how to teach a full
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 23
complement of vocabulary reading skills and more on the
relative effectiveness of specific
universally applicable intervention strategies (e.g., prompting
processes, reinforcement
strategies). We suggest that, when research is concentrated on
what constitutes effective discrete
trial instruction, the investigators might have chosen literacy
outcomes less for their potential to
provide deep and abiding understandings of vocabulary, and
more because they are
parsimonious, precise, and reliable for assessing cause, effect,
and efficiency. We suggest that
single word identification, matching, and recognition measures,
with and without simple
measures of word understanding, meet the latter conditions.
While acknowledging that these studies might have achieved
their purpose of providing
scientific evidence that a particular strategy “works,” we are
nevertheless concerned that these
studies could be used by teachers to draw conclusions about
what to teach, instead of conclusions
solely about how to teach. Thus, teachers reviewing the
literature for reasonable and feasible
outcomes to go along with their evidence-based strategies for
literacy instruction could easily
misinterpret these particular studies as providing evidence of
useful outcomes as well. Moreover,
we are concerned that practices in schools might now reflect
this dilemma, with both
instructional strategies and literacy outcomes from this research
literature reflected in teacher
practices. For example, we contend that teachers are using
discrete trial, prompting and
reinforcement formats to teach information related to weather,
dates, holidays, and personal data
via repeated cycles of calendar and morning circle; and survival
and community word
recognition (e.g., stop, exit, and poison) via frequent,
intermittent flash card routines. Aside from
concerns about how students with moderate to severe
disabilities might experience at an
emotional level these instructional processes when they are
delivered repeatedly across their
grades and without reference to age-appropriateness or
meaningful and motivating contexts,
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 24
there is a pressing need for research to demonstrate whether
such instruction actually leads to
generalized use of the targeted vocabulary across relevant life
contexts (Browder & Xin, 1998),
to meaningful and lasting vocabulary comprehension, or to
“continuous growth of word
knowledge” (NRP, 2000, p. 13). Hence, we believe that
teachers’ pervasive use of these formats
of literacy instruction must be viewed critically. We are also
concerned that this type of
instruction might be self-perpetuating, promoting in teachers, as
well as parents and
administrators, a false sense that valued outcomes are resulting
from instruction, because the
target words appear to have face validity and students might
respond reliably during instructional
trials, as shown in the research.
We emphasize that we cannot infer cause and effect between
research of this type and
observed teacher choices about what vocabulary to teach in
schools; yet, there is plausibility to
our stance that some relationship exists, and that it could
become stronger as teachers are
increasingly pressured to justify their practices and outcomes
based on the scientific literature.
Hence, it behooves researchers interested in direct, discrete trial
instruction protocols to take
seriously this possibility, and begin considering different
choices in what is taught to
demonstrate effect, paying closer attention to the NRP (2000)
characterization of vocabulary
knowledge as a progressive and constructive process.
Comprehension
Reading comprehension is the sine qua non of literacy
proficiency, entailing text reading
together with understanding (Raphael & Au, 2005; Snow, 2002).
It is an “intentional problem
solving thinking” process in which the reader “derives meaning
from text” through active
interaction with the text (NRP, 2000, p. 14). The NRP found
evidence that comprehension “is
enhanced” when students relate their own “knowledge and
experience” to the ideas being
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 25
expressed in the material being read. It requires not only the
ability to access familiar and easy
text, but also the ability to access, learn, and reflect on
unfamiliar and difficult text (Snow,
2002). The NRP (2000) called for explicit instruction,
identifying seven research-based
approaches to explicit instruction: (a) comprehension
monitoring, (b) cooperative learning, (c)
graphic and semantic organizers, (d) question answering, (e)
question generation, (f) story
structure, and (g) summarization. Finally, the panel stressed
that the outcome of instruction
should be students’ acquisition and use of strategies for
independent text comprehension, such as
independently using knowledge of text structure, generating and
answering questions, and
retelling and summarizing (Mills, 2009).
Literacy instruction within and across the five NRP pillars
should ultimately assist
students with moderate to severe disabilities to derive meaning
through their own efforts to
understand connected text. As shown in the previous sections,
we have concerns that there is an
evidence gap between demonstrations of success at teaching
basic skills of reading and
demonstrations of the emergence of independence in reading
comprehension. Clearly, teachers
of students with moderate to severe disabilities could benefit
from evidence that particular
methods of phonological, fluency, and vocabulary skill
instruction concurrently support text
comprehension skill acquisition. Additionally, teachers of
students with moderate to severe
disabilities could benefit from research that explores multi-
component instruction in which
reading comprehension capabilities are systematically taught
and are primary outcomes achieved
alongside the basic skills of reading.
For example, Allor, Mathes, Roberts, Jones, and Champlin
(2010) conducted a multi-
component reading instruction study which reported measures
of comprehension for their
participants in first to fourth grade from 10 public schools, who
were randomly assigned to either
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 26
a treatment (n= 16) or contrast (n=12) group. Students in the
contrast group received instruction
that already was typically provided in their programs, while
students in the treatment group
received “daily, comprehensive reading instruction in small
groups . . . for approximately 40
minutes per session” (p. 3) for “one to one and a half years” (p.
16). Intervention materials
consisted of a modified and expanded version of a packaged
program (i.e., Early Interventions in
Reading). Instruction on reading comprehension focused on
“comprehension strategies” (p. 9)
using storybook read-alouds, stories with decodable text,
supporting pictures, and other
materials. While most of their dependent measures focused on
basic skills (e.g., phonemic
awareness), pre- and post-data on reading comprehension also
were collected using the passage
comprehension test of The Woodcock Language Proficiency
Battery – Revised (Woodcock,
1991). Small gains were reported for both groups, but the
treatment group showed greater gains,
and there was a moderate effect size reported (.69) that was
statistically significant.
While these results are promising, we have three concerns that
we believe compromise
this evidence. First, insufficient information is reported on the
educational programming
provided for students in the contrast group; that is, their
programs could have focused on any
content, from reading comprehension to self-care. Hence,
“significant results” might imply only
that intensive prolonged reading instruction is better than no
reading instruction at all (see
Slavin, 2008). Second, replication is made difficult by the
modifications made to the standard
programs, reportedly to match the baseline levels of students.
Insufficient information is
provided about the modifications made per student, and the
process used to match the
modifications to students’ needs. Third and finally, the
intervention had effects on reading
comprehension for some students, but it seemed to have less
impact on other students.
Specifically, it was reported that only 8 of the 16 participants
(50%) actually achieved program
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 27
levels at which they could be “working on basic comprehension
strategies, such as retelling
stories, sequencing main events, and story grammar” (p. 18).
In light of the NRP (2000) emphasis on reading comprehension
strategy instruction, we
believe that teachers need more research addressing this
instruction when it is grounded in grade-
level general education content and contexts. As an illustration,
consider what instruction might
look like when using the Mills (2009) sequence for teaching
comprehension strategies within and
across reading lessons for a heterogeneous class of students: (1)
activating prior knowledge, (2)
making inferences, (3) using knowledge of text structure, (4)
visualizing the story and related
events, (5) generating and answering questions, and (6) retelling
and summarizing. These
strategies should be modeled during speaking and listening
activities before, during, and after
reading and telling stories, a process consistent with the NRP
recommendation that teachers
repeatedly demonstrate strategy application during literacy
activities while encouraging students
to use them. The scientific evidence examined by teachers of
students with moderate to severe
disabilities should inform them about the supports necessary
and the learning that can occur
when reading comprehension involves constructing and
reconstructing knowledge in relation to
repeated experiences with text, and where the instruction
consists of overlapping and complexly
sequenced activities that involve interactions with typical peers.
Conclusions, Questions, and Limitations
We used the five-pillars framework advanced by the NRP
(2000) to explore issues and
concerns about literacy instruction with students who have
moderate to severe disabilities. Our
focus was on what teachers need to learn when reviewing
research to improve their instructional
practices and to achieve better literacy outcomes for these
students. We raised concerns about
what constitutes adequate scientific evidence for teachers; we
offered studies from the extant
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 28
research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities
to illustrate these concerns; and
we suggested ways to improve research to better serve the needs
of teachers.
The NRP (2000) report strongly recommended using direct,
explicit instruction across the
five pillars of reading instruction. When selecting examples to
illustrate our concerns, we chose
quantitative studies that defined literacy in relation to reading
and writing, and that focused on
direct instruction of discrete skills for students with moderate to
severe disabilities. We recognize
that the studies that we present here do not fully represent the
spectrum of literacy instruction
studies with students who have moderate to severe disabilities.
For instance, the extant literature
includes quantitative studies on the impact of embedding
literacy instruction in general education
activities (e.g., McDonnell, Johnson, Polychronis, & Riesen,
2002). However, our intent here
was not to provide a comprehensive review of the literature.
Rather, it was to provide research
illustrations of studies that emphasized instruction of skills
associated with each of the five
pillars, and that appeared to mirror what we believe are aspects
of the NRP report’s findings and
orientation to instruction that are in need of critical discussion
and more research.
We also acknowledge that we did not adequately address issues
of research design.
Instead, we used a scenario in which teachers working from a
five pillars orientation search for
evidence about approaches to instruction within studies that
likely vary in rigor with respect to
methodological criteria. By concentrating solely on possible
research review activities of
teachers who instruct students with moderate to severe
disabilities, we presented what we believe
are discrepancies between the evidence that they might see and
the evidence that they might
need. The most prominent concerns we raised by using this
process touch on issues of external
validity. On the one hand, teachers might find themselves
skeptical as to whether focused code-
based instruction has validity if the ultimate goal of their own
classroom instruction is to
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 29
facilitate independence in text understanding and access to the
general curriculum. On the other
hand, teachers might be misled to believe that certain forms of
discrete trial vocabulary
instruction are valid for their students as long as the lists of
vocabulary items are functional and
relate to things students need to know in their daily lives. We
also raised questions that touch on
the validity of multi-component instructional packages,
suggesting that teachers need to know
more about how such packages lead to comprehension, how
generalization beyond the
immediate material of the packages can be realized, and whether
the hours of instruction
required when using such packages will eventually pay off, via
promoting access for their
students to grade-appropriate general curriculum. Since, in this
era of standards, districts
increasing might look to commercial packages as ways to both
standardize instruction and
provide alternative learning experiences to learners who do not
appear to fit the norm of the
“typical student,” we suggest that these questions are especially
disconcerting, requiring a closer
look at the literature base relevant to these issues.
Reflecting our concerns with external validity, we suggest that
teachers need scientific
evidence that is drawn more from studies that match conditions
of good classroom teaching, and
we recommend that studies in which instructional strategies
drawn from the five pillars are
examined should match more closely the ecologies and
activities of general education classes. As
an example, let us consider vocabulary instruction. To both
improve vocabulary instruction and
make it relevant to grade-level general curriculum we believe
teachers need scientific evidence
from studies that parallel their reality; that is, studies that
incorporate complex sets of
independent variables found in general education classes and
sets of dependent measures that
express richer forms of learning. For instance, a study might
first propose ways that vocabulary
can be selected for instruction so that it is correlated with
grade-appropriate textual content and
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 30
matches the learning processes of students with different ability
levels. Researchers then could
examine the results of a sequence of complex instructional
activities that continuously link
vocabulary acquisition back to relevant text, measuring both
lexicon acquisition and connected
text comprehension in different learners (see Stahl & Bravo,
2010). Furthermore, teachers would
benefit if such research also assessed patterns of semantic
learning. A useful tool for assessing
patterns of student learning in this regard might be a network or
semantic map, displaying
interconnections between the units of information or the
concepts underlying a lesson’s key
vocabulary. Finally, teachers also would benefit if such
research took into consideration the
previous knowledge and real interests of each student, and
processes of information exchange
each student demonstrates when learning is occurring, such as
in cooperative learning formats.
When effective teachers plan and implement meaningful literacy
instruction they utilize
decision-making processes such as the foregoing; that is,
processes that account for a multitude
of ecological, intra-student, and curriculum content variables.
Hence, we contend that
instructional research also should reflect, as much as possible,
these same sets of variables. An
implication of this approach to literacy instruction research is
that discrete trial techniques (e.g.,
time delay) might best be examined less as universally
applicable strategies independent of
content and context, and more as integral components of larger
instructional frameworks. The
research on embedded instruction by McDonnell and his
colleagues (e.g., McDonnell et al.,
2002) begins to address such issues. Research of this type does
not devalue discrete trial
techniques, but it does communicate their contributions to
learning in proportion to that of other
components of effective instruction, and it demonstrates their
effectiveness in a manner that
assists teachers to better incorporate them into general
education contexts and activities.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 31
There is another external validity concern that we have that
does not arise from studies
within our own field; instead it arises from the NRP (2000)
findings themselves, and this concern
raises questions about the generalizability of the scientific
evidence of the report with respect to
students with moderate to severe disabilities. To understand this
concern, it is necessary to
contrast students with moderate to severe disabilities with
another group of students for whom
the report might have clear merit -- students who do not have an
intellectual disability but who
are struggling with reading and might, therefore, receive
specialized, direct instruction, perhaps
as part of a response-to-intervention (RtI) process. Prior to
receiving an intervention, a particular
student within this latter group might be struggling, but he or
she still advances through the
grades and steadily accumulates a history of experiences and
opportunities to read, to try to
understand what he/she reads, and to acquire literacy skills
across general education contexts and
content areas. In contrast, a student with a moderate to severe
intellectual disability is much more
likely to be served for all or most of the day throughout his/her
school career in a self-contained
class (Smith, 2007); this means he or she does not experience at
any point in his or her school
career the range of literacy activities and communities that are
routinely part of life for the
student who is a struggling reader.
In the case of the struggling reader, specialized interventions
that are direct, isolated, and
remedial augment other literacy activities and experiences that
occur during their school day,
with the goal being to improve their reading for better access to
the general curriculum. In the
case of the student with a moderate to severe intellectual
disability, instruction that is direct,
isolated, and remedial frequently supplants the literacy
activities and experiences that occur in
other contexts during their school day, with the goal being to
improve discrete reading-related
skills. We acknowledge that there is an absence of research on
the effects and implications of
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 32
this disparity; however, based on what we know about the
impact of exposure-to-print on literacy
skills (Anderson, Wilson, & Fielding, 1988), and what we know
about the significance of context
as a variable impacting the results of instruction (Jackson,
Ryndak, & Wehmeyer, 2008-2009),
we propose that, on average, the results of direct instruction
will differ in these two cases, and
that a student receiving the restricted range of experiences
likely is at a disadvantage when
contrasted with the student who is exposed to a wider range of
experiences. This point of view is
further supported by what we know about generalization (Stokes
& Baer, 1977): For the student
with an intellectual disability in the self-contained setting, there
would be limited opportunities
and thus little support for skills to generalize from literacy
relevant contexts of the school into
the specialized intervention situation; inversely, there would be
limited opportunities and thus
little support for skills to generalize from the specialized
intervention situation into other relevant
contexts of the school or larger community.
The latter concern relates directly to NRP (2000) findings with
respect to direct, explicit
instruction. From an external validity standpoint, we suggest
that while the report’s evidence is
likely applicable to struggling readers receiving direct and
specialized instruction designed to
augment a range of experiences within and across general
education contexts, it might not be
applicable to students for whom the direct and specialized
instruction fully replaces general
education literacy experiences. In light of this, we think that
referencing the NRP evidence as a
justification for explicit direct instruction for students served in
self-contained settings might be
an improper extension of these findings.
We can go a step further by examining how the NRP’s (2000)
evidence for the value of
“explicit instruction” might apply in these two different
circumstances. It is likely that many
teachers would define explicitness as the clarity by which they
present directives and demand
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 33
responses from a student within an instructional routine. We
think, however, that explicitness
should refer not to whether a target outcome of instruction is
clear to educators, but rather to
whether it is clear to students; that is, whether students
experience cognitive clarity with respect
to instructional expectations (Cunningham & Cunningham,
2002; Downing, 1971). For the
struggling reader who experiences literacy opportunities in
multiple settings, the purpose and
applicability of the instruction might be obvious (i.e., explicit),
and learning is facilitated. For the
student with a moderate to severe disability who experiences
literacy opportunities mainly in one
segregated special education context, the purpose and
applicability of the specialized instruction
might not be obvious (i.e., less explicit), and learning might
then lack the necessary robustness
for acquisition, retention, generalization, and wider application.
Put differently, we suggest that
“explicitness” might not be present in the latter situation
despite “clear” instructions, and that
learning might be short-lived because skills being taught cannot
be linked cognitively by the
student to literacy activities that are of consequence in his or
her life at school.
Given the foregoing, we perhaps have a partial answer to the
dichotomy raised at the start
of this paper: students with moderate to severe disabilities are
being denied opportunities to be
part of larger literacy communities even though their potential
for learning and using literacy has
been demonstrated. The dilemma might lie in part with learning
differences that are largely a
consequence of being isolated from general education and
receiving, perhaps exclusively,
specialized instruction in segregated contexts. Potential for
progress, therefore, is less likely for
students with moderate to severe disabilities not due to their
disabilities, but due to their lack of
exposure to the many and varied literacy opportunities that
constitute what we call “general
education.” It is a vicious cycle, in which the direct instruction
processes produce results that
cannot be fully applied, refined, and generalized because of lack
of access to the larger arena of
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 34
literacy opportunities, and the continuation of apparent skill
deficiencies and instructional
problems (e.g., prompt dependence, lack of generalization, poor
retention) provide yet another
reason to maintain student confinement in segregated special
education contexts. We believe that
the perspective of teachers, researchers, and others in education
about the learning potential of
these students would change if the students were provided full
access to general education
content in general education contexts, while supplementing
their instruction in those contexts
with learning adaptations or, if needed, short-term specialized
instruction. We also believe that
the scientific evidence made available to teachers in future
literacy instruction research can help
promote this, if it more directly emulates the conditions and
variables associated with quality
instruction with this content and in these settings.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 35
References
Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., Champlin, T., & Cheatham, J. P.
(2009). Research-based techniques
for teaching early reading skills to students with intellectual
disabilities. Education and
Training in Developmental Disabilities, 44(3), 356-366.
Allor, J., Mathes, P., Roberts, K., Jones, F., & Champlin, T.
(2010). Teaching students with
moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An experimental
examination of a
comprehensive reading program. Education and Training in
Autism and Developmental
Disabilities, 45(1), 3-22.
Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988).
Growth in reading and how children
spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly,
23, 285-303.
Apffel, J. A., Kelleher, J., Lilly, M. S., & Richardson, R.
(1975). Developmental reading for
moderately retarded children. Education and Training of the
Mentally Retarded, 10, 229-
235.
Ault, M. M., Gast, D. L., & Wolery, M. (1988). Comparison of
progressive and constant time-
delay procedures in teaching community-sign word reading.
American Journal on Mental
Retardation, 93, 44-56.
Barudin, S. I., & Hourcade, J. J. (1990). Relative effectiveness
of three methods of reading
instruction in developing specific recall and transfer skills in
learners with moderate and
severe mental retardation. Education and Training in Mental
Retardation, 25, 286-291.
Basil, C., & Reyes, S. (2003). Acquisition of literacy skills by
children with severe disability.
Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 19.
Blyden, A. E. (1989). Survival word acquisition in mentally
retarded adolescents with
multihandicaps: Effects of color-reversed stimulus materials.
Journal of Special
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 36
Education, 22(4), 493-501.
Bracey, S., Maggs, A., & Morath, P. (1975). The effects of a
direct phonics approach in teaching
reading. The Australian Journal on the Education of Backward
Children, 22, 83-90.
Bradford, S., Shippen, M. E., Alberto, P., Houchins, D. E., &
Flores, M. (2006). Using
systematic instruction to teach decoding skills to middle school
students with moderate
intellectual disabilities. Education and Training in
Developmental Disabilities, 41(4),
333-343.
Browder, D. M., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., Courtade, G., Gibbs, S.
L., & Flowers, C. (2008).
Evaluation of the effectiveness of an early literacy program for
students with significant
developmental disabilities. Exceptional Children, 75, 33-52.
Browder, D. M., Wakeman, S. Y., Spooner, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell,
L., & Algozzine, B. (2006).
Research on reading instruction for individuals with significant
cognitive disabilities.
Exceptional Children, 72, 392-410.
Browder, D. M., & Xin, Y. P. (1998). A meta-analysis and
review of sight word research and its
implications for teaching functional reading to individuals with
moderate and severe
disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 32, 130-153.
Brown, L., Jones, S., Troccolo, E., Heiser, C., Bellamy, T., &
Sontag, E. (1972). Teaching
functional reading to young trainable students: Toward
longitudinal objectives. Journal
of Special Education, 6, 237-246.
Brown, L., & Perlmutter, L. (1971). Teaching functional reading
to trainable level retarded
students. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 6,
74-84.
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and
quasi-experimental designs for
research. Chicago: Rand McNally.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 37
Cassidy, J., Valadez, C. M., & Garrett, S. D. (2010). Literacy
trends and issues: A look at the
five pillars and the cement that supports them. The Reading
Teacher, 63(8), 644-655.
Chhabra, V., & McCardle, P. (2004). Contributions to evidence-
based research. In P. McCardle
& V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence in reading research
(pp. 3-11). Baltimore:
Brookes.
Collins, B. C., & Griffen, A. K. (1996). Teaching students with
moderate disabilities to make
safe responses to product warning labels. Education and
Treatment of Children, 19, 30-
45.
Collins, B. C., & Stinson, D. M. (1994). Teaching generalized
reading of product warning labels
to adolescents with mental disabilities through the use of key
words. Exceptionality, 5(3),
163-181.
Conners, F. A., Rosenquist, C. J., Sligh, A. C., Atwell, J. A., &
Kiser, T. (2006). Phonological
reading skills acquisition by children with mental retardation.
Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 27, 121-137.
Copeland, S. R., & Cosbey, J. (2009). Making progress in the
general curriculum: Rethinking
effective instructional practices. Research and Practice for
Persons With Severe
Disabilities, 33-34, 214-227.
Copeland, S. R., & Keefe, E. B. (2007). Effective literacy
instruction for students with
moderate or severe disabilities. Baltimore: Brookes.
Cunningham, P. M., & Cunnigham, J. W. (2002). What we know
about how to teach phonics. In
A. E. F. S. J. Samuels (Ed.), What research has to say about
reading instruction. Newark,
DE: International Reading Association.
Downing, J. (1971, July). The cognitive clarity theory of
learning to read. Paper
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 38
presented at the Annual Meeting of the United Kingdom
Reading Association.
Manchester, England.
Downing, J. E. (2005). Teaching literacy to students with
significant disabilities.
Thousand Oaks, California: Corwin Press.
Doyle, P. M., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Meyer, S.
(1992). Teaching discrete skills
to students with moderate mental retardation in small-group
instructional arrangements.
Exceptionality, 3(4), 233-253.
Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., & Ault, M. J. (1990).
Comparison of constant time delay
and the system of least prompts in teaching preschoolers with
developmental delays.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11, 1-22.
Edmark reading program: Word recognition (2nd ed.) (1992).
Cambridge, MA: Edmark
Corporation.
Eikeseth, S., & Jahr, E. (2001). The UCLA reading and writing
program: An evaluation of the
beginning stages. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22,
289-307.
Engelmann, S., Meyer, L., Carnine, L., Becker, W., Eisele, J., &
Johnson, G. (1998). Corrective
reading decoding strategies. Columbus, OH: SRA/McGraw-
Hill.
Fallon, K. A., Light, J., McNaughton, D., Drager, K., &
Hammer, C. (2004). The effects of direct
instruction on the single-word reading skills of children who
require augmentative and
alternative communication. Journal of Speech, Language &
Hearing Research, 47, 1424-
1439.
Flippo, R. F. (1999). Redefining the reading wars: the war
against reading researchers.
Educational Leadership, 57(2), 38-41.
Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Morris, L. L., Doyle, P. M., & Meyer,
S. (1990). Teaching sight word
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 39
reading in a group instructional arrangement using constant
time delay. Exceptionality,
1(2), 81-96.
Gersten, R. M., & Maggs, A. (1982). Teaching the general case
to moderately retarded children:
Evaluation of a five year project. Analysis and Intervention in
Developmental
Disabilities, 2(4), 329-343.
Gillon, G. T. (2004). Phonological awareness: From research to
practice. New York: Guilford
Press.
Goetz, K., Hulme, C., Brigstocke, S., Carroll, J. M., Nasir, L.,
& Snowling, M. (2008). Training
reading and phoneme awareness in children with Down
Syndrome. Reading and Writing:
An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 395-412.
Goldstein, H. (1983). Recombinative generalization:
Relationship between environmental
conditions and the linguistic repertoires of language learners.
Analysis and Intervention in
Developmental Disabilities, 3, 279-293.
Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (1996). Assessment for
instructional decisions: Toward a
proactive/prevention model of decision-making for early
literacy skills. School
Psychology Quarterly, 11, 326−336.
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and
reading disability. Remedial and
Special Education, 7(1), 6-10.
Griffen, A. K., Schuster, J. W., & Morse, T. E. (1998). The
acquisition of instructive feedback: A
comparison of continuous versus intermittent presentation
schedules. Education and
Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
33, 42-61.
Hicks, C. P. (2009). A lesson on reading fluency learned from
The Tortoise and the Hare. The
Reading Teacher, 63(4), 319-323.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 40
Hoogeveen, F. R., Birkhoff, A. E., Smeets, P. M., Lancioni, G.
E., & Boelens, H. H. (1989).
Establishing phonemic segmentation in moderately retarded
children. Remedial and
Special Education, 10(3), 47-53.
Hoogeveen, F. R., & Smeets, P. M. (1988). Establishing
phoneme blending in trainable mentally
retarded children. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2), 46-53.
Jackson, L. B., Ryndak, D. L., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2008-
2009). The dynamic relationship
between context, curriculum, and student learning: A case for
inclusive education as a
research-based practice. Research and Practice for Persons With
Severe Disabilities,
33(4)/ 34(1), 175-195.
Joseph, L. M., & Seery, M. E. (2004). Where is the phonics?
Remedial and Special Education,
25, 88-94.
Karsh, K. G., Repp, A. C., & Lenz, M. W. (1990). A comparison
of the task demonstration
model and the standard prompting hierarchy in teaching word
identification to persons
with moderate retardation. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 11, 395-410.
Kliewer, C., & Biklen, D. (2001). "School's not really a place
for reading": A research synthesis
of the literate lives of students with severe disabilities. Journal
of the Association for
Persons with Severe Handicaps, 26, 1-12.
Koury, M., & Browder, D. M. (1986). The use of delay to teach
sight words by peer tutors
classified as moderately mentally retarded. Education and
Training of the Mentally
Retarded, 21, 252-258.
Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010).
Aligning theory and assessment
of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of
fluency. Reading Research
Quarterly, 45(2), 230-251.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 41
Lonigan, C. J., & Shanahan, T. (2010). Developing early
literacy skills: Things we know
we know and things we know we don't know. Educational
Researcher, 39(4),
340-346.
McDonnell, J., Johnson, J. W., Polychronis, S., & Riesen, T.
(2002). Effects of embedded
instruction on students with moderate disabilities enrolled in
general education classes.
Education and Training in Mental Retardation and
Developmental Disabilities, 37, 363-
377.
McLaughlin, M. J. (2010). Evolving interpretations of
educational equity and students with
disabilities. Exceptional Children, 76(3), 265-278.
McNaughton, S. (1981). Low progress readers and teacher
instructional behavior during oral
reading: The risk of maintaining instructional dependence. The
Exceptional Child, 28,
167-176.
Mills, K. A. (2009). Floating on a sea of talk: Reading
comprehension through speaking and
listening. The Reading Teacher, 63(4), 325-329.
Miracle, S. A., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Grisham-
Brown, J. (2001). Peer-versus teacher-
delivered instruction: Effects on acquisition and maintenance.
Education and Training in
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 36, 373-
385.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(2000). Report of the National
Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific
research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction (NIH
Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Nigam, R., Schlosser, R. W., & Lloyd, L. L. (2006).
Concomitant use of the matrix strategy and
the mand-model procedure in teaching graphic symbol
combinations. Augmentative and
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 42
Alternative Communication, 22(3), 160-177.
Osborn, J., Freeman, A., Burley, M., Wilson, R., Jones, E., &
Rychener, S. (2007). Effect of
tutoring on reading achievement for students with cognitive
disabilities, specific learning
disabilities, and students receiving Title I services. Education
and Training in
Developmental Disabilities, 42, 467-474.
Raphael, T. E., & Au, K. H. (2005). QAR: Enhancing
comprehension and test taking across
grades and content areas. The Reading Teacher, 59, 206-221.
Reyna, V. F. (2004). Why scientific research? The importance
of evidence in changing
educational practices. In P. McCardle & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The
voice of evidence in
reading research (pp. 47-58). Baltimore: Brookes.
Rohena, E. I., Jitendra, A. K., & Browder, D. M. (2002).
Comparison of the effects of Spanish
and English constant time delay instruction on sight word
reading by Hispanic learners
with mental retardation. Journal of Special Education, 36, 169-
184.
Ryndak, D. L., Morrison, A. P., & Sommerstein, L. (1999).
Literacy before and after inclusion in
general education settings: A case study. Journal of the
Association for Persons with
Severe Handicaps, 24, 5-22.
Singh, J., & Singh, N. (1985a). Comparison of word-supply and
word-analysis error correction
procedures on oral reading by mentally retarded children.
American Journal on Mental
Deficiency, 90(1), 64-70.
Singh, N. N. (1990). Effects of two error-correction procedures
on oral reading errors. Behavior
Modification, 14(2), 188-199.
Singh, N. N., & Singh, J. (1984). Antecedent control of oral
reading errors and self-corrections
by mentally retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 17, 111-119.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 43
Singh, N. N., & Singh, J. (1985b). Reading acquisition and
remediation in the mentally retarded.
International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 14,
165-199.
Singh, N., & Singh, J. (1986). A behavioural remediation
program for oral reading: effects on
errors and comprehension. Educational Psychology, 6(2), 105-
114.
Singh, N. N., Singh, J., & Winton, A. S. W. (1984). Positive
practice overcorrection of oral
reading errors. Behavior Modification, 8(1), 23-37.
Singleton, D. K., Schuster, J. W., Morse, T. E., & Collins, B. C.
(1999). A comparison of
antecedent prompt and test and simultaneous prompting
procedures in teaching grocery
words to adolescents with mental retardation. Education and
Training in Mental
Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 34, 182-199.
Slavin, R. E. (2008). What works? Issues in synthesizing
educational program evaluations.
Educational Researcher, 37(1), 5-14.
Smith, P. (2007). Have we made any progress? Including
students with intellectual disabilities in
regular classrooms. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
45(5), 297-309.
Snow, C. E. (2002). Reading for understanding: toward a
research and development program in
reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.
Stahl, K. A., & Bravo, M. A. (2010). Contemporary classroom
vocabulary assessment for
content areas. The Reading Teacher, 63(7), 566-578.
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of
generalization. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349-367.
Striefel, S., Wetherby, B., & Karlan, G. (1978). Developing
generalized instruction-following
behavior in severely retarded people. In C. E. Meyers (Ed.),
Quality of life in severely
and profoundly mentally retarded people: Research foundations
for improvement.
LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 44
Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.
Van der Bijl, C., Alant, E., & Lloyd, L. (2006). A comparison
of two strategies of sight word
instruction in children with mental disability. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 27,
43-55.
Whalon, K. J., Otaiba, S. A., & Delano, M. E. (2009). Evidence-
based reading instruction for
individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Focus on Autism
and Other Developmental
Disorders, 24, 3-16.
Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective
measurement or how applied
behavior analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 11, 203-214.
Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock language proficiency
battery–Revised. Chicago: Riverside.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001).
Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of
Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Yopp, H. K., & Yopp, R. H. (2000). Supporting phonemic
awareness development in the
classroom. The Reading Teacher, 54(2), 130-143.
Zascavage, V. T., & Keefe, C. H. (2004). Students with severe
speech and physical impairments:
Opportunity barriers to literacy. Focus on Autism and Other
Developmental Disorders,
19, 223-234.
1.
A retailer hasACCOUNTS receivable of $70000, total current
liabilities of 440000, and $50000 in case. What is the quick
ratio?
1.4
1.5
2.0
3.0
5 points
QUESTION 2
Assume that the retailer in problem 1 has totalINVENTORY on
hand of $120,000. What is the current ratio?
0.65
3.00
4.60
6.00
5 points
QUESTION 3
A clothing store hasACCOUNTS receivable totaling
$50,000,ACCOUNTS payable totaling $45,000, and net sales of
$50,000. What is the collection period?
50
183
227
365
5 points
QUESTION 4
Assume that the retailer in problem 3 has accounts receivable of
$15,000 and net sales of $30,000. What is the new collection
period?
91
183
274
335
5 points
QUESTION 5
A firm has total assets of $500,000, total liabilities of $350,000,
and annual net sales of $1,000,000. What is the assets-to-net-
sales ratio?
5%
7%
30%
50%
5 points
Click Save and Submit to save and

More Related Content

Similar to LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 1 .docx

A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher Education
A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher EducationA Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher Education
A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher EducationSarah Adams
 
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business l
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business lUnit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business l
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business lojas18
 
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnReciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnannes86
 
Data Driven Decision Making
Data Driven Decision MakingData Driven Decision Making
Data Driven Decision MakingMelissa Roth
 
Rti For Cec
Rti For CecRti For Cec
Rti For CecJ C
 
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...juraikha
 
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...juraikha
 
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  ClassroomsAssessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based ClassroomsWendy Belieu
 
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIDiagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIrathx039
 
Diagnosis the missing ingredient in RTI assessment
Diagnosis  the missing ingredient in RTI assessmentDiagnosis  the missing ingredient in RTI assessment
Diagnosis the missing ingredient in RTI assessmentherma273
 
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclassesrealhope
 
Howard walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingHoward walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingWilliam Kritsonis
 
Howard walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingHoward walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingWilliam Kritsonis
 
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientation
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientationA wee evaluative study of the academic orientation
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientationAlexander Decker
 

Similar to LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 1 .docx (20)

A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher Education
A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher EducationA Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher Education
A Critical Analysis Of Research On Reading Teacher Education
 
084 119e
084 119e084 119e
084 119e
 
084 119e
084 119e084 119e
084 119e
 
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business l
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business lUnit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business l
Unit 5 assignment contract terms template law204 – business l
 
Serafini implement-rw
Serafini implement-rwSerafini implement-rw
Serafini implement-rw
 
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnReciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
 
Data Driven Decision Making
Data Driven Decision MakingData Driven Decision Making
Data Driven Decision Making
 
Rti For Cec
Rti For CecRti For Cec
Rti For Cec
 
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
 
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
Reading Comprehension Strategies: An International Comparison of Teacher Pref...
 
Karl r.wirth learning to learn
Karl r.wirth   learning to learnKarl r.wirth   learning to learn
Karl r.wirth learning to learn
 
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  ClassroomsAssessment Of Teaching And Learning In  Literature-Based  Classrooms
Assessment Of Teaching And Learning In Literature-Based Classrooms
 
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTIDiagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
Diagnosis: The Missing Ingredient from RTI
 
Diagnosis the missing ingredient in RTI assessment
Diagnosis  the missing ingredient in RTI assessmentDiagnosis  the missing ingredient in RTI assessment
Diagnosis the missing ingredient in RTI assessment
 
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses
7.effectiveteachingmethodsforlargerclasses
 
1/1
1/11/1
1/1
 
D1b1
D1b1D1b1
D1b1
 
Howard walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingHoward walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mapping
 
Howard walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mappingHoward walters concept mapping
Howard walters concept mapping
 
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientation
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientationA wee evaluative study of the academic orientation
A wee evaluative study of the academic orientation
 

More from SHIVA101531

Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxAnswer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxAnswer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxAnswer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxAnswer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxAnswer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxAnswer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxAnswer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxAnswer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxAnswer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxAnswer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxAnswer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxAnswer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxAnswer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxAnswer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxAnswer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxAnswer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxAnswer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxAnswer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxSHIVA101531
 
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxAnswer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxSHIVA101531
 
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxSHIVA101531
 

More from SHIVA101531 (20)

Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docxAnswer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
Answer the following questions in a minimum of 1-2 paragraphs ea.docx
 
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docxAnswer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
Answer the following questions using scholarly sources as references.docx
 
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docxAnswer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
Answer the following questions about this case studyClient .docx
 
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docxAnswer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
Answer the following questions using art vocabulary and ideas from L.docx
 
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docxAnswer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
Answer the following questions in a total of 3 pages (900 words). My.docx
 
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docxAnswer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
Answer the following questions No single word responses (at lea.docx
 
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docxAnswer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
Answer the following questions based on the ethnography Dancing Skel.docx
 
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docxAnswer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
Answer the following questions to the best of your ability1) De.docx
 
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docxAnswer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
Answer the following questionDo you think it is necessary to .docx
 
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docxAnswer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
Answer the following question. Use facts and examples to support.docx
 
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docxAnswer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
Answer the bottom questions  in apa format and decent answer no shor.docx
 
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docxAnswer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
Answer the following below using the EXCEL attachment. chapter 5.docx
 
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docxAnswer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
Answer the following prompts about A Germanic People Create a Code .docx
 
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docxAnswer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
Answer the following discussion board question below minumun 25.docx
 
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docxAnswer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
Answer the following questions about IT Project Management. What.docx
 
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docxAnswer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
Answer the following in at least 100 words minimum each1.Of.docx
 
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docxAnswer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
Answer the following questions(at least 200 words) and responses 2 p.docx
 
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docxAnswer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
Answer the following questions in a Word document and upload it by M.docx
 
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docxAnswer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
Answer the following questions in complete sentences. Each answer sh.docx
 
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docxANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
ANSWER THE DISCUSSION QUESTION 250 WORDS MINDiscussion Q.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........LeaCamillePacle
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfSpandanaRallapalli
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.arsicmarija21
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfAMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfphamnguyenenglishnb
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayQuarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayMakMakNepo
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptxRaw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
Raw materials used in Herbal Cosmetics.pptx
 
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdfAMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
AMERICAN LANGUAGE HUB_Level2_Student'sBook_Answerkey.pdf
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayQuarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
 

LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 1 .docx

  • 1. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 1 Applying the Five Pillars to Literacy Instruction With Students Who Have Moderate to Severe Disabilities: Issues and Concerns Lewis B. Jackson University of Northern Colorado Diane L. Ryndak and Ann-Marie Orlando University of Florida Kara Halley Metro State College of Denver Karen McCaleb Texas A&M University Corpus Christi
  • 2. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 2 Abstract The findings and recommendation of the National Reading Panel (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000) have influenced how literacy skills are conceptualized and taught in schools. Although the report’s findings and recommendation were directed at students without disabilities, they have the potential to impact instruction and instructional research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities. To explore this, we used the National Reading Panel’s five pillars framework (i.e., phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, and reading comprehension) to raise issues and concerns about literacy instruction research and practices with students who have moderate to severe disabilities. In our discussion,
  • 3. we assume the point of view of teachers who wish to improve their practices by delving into the literacy research base, opening with a discussion of how scientific evidence can serve as a guide for improving literacy instruction. This is followed by a discussion of specific issues and concerns related to each of the five pillars, illustrated by research studies in which the participants have moderate to severe disabilities. We conclude by summarizing our concerns; by exploring additional concerns that go across the five pillars; and by posing arguments that present questions regarding the applicability of the National Reading Panel’s (2000) findings for students with moderate to severe disabilities. Lastly, the paper considers the potential role of literacy research and practice as contributing factors in an unreconciled dichotomy between a body of research demonstrating the power of evidence-based instruction with these students, and another body of research showing the continued denial of literacy opportunities to them in schools. Keywords: Literacy; Reading; National Reading Panel;
  • 4. moderate to severe disabilities; evidence-based instruction LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 3 Applying the Five Pillars to Literacy Instruction With Students Who Have Moderate to Severe Disabilities: Issues and Concerns In the broadest sense, literacy involves understanding, using, and producing print for a variety of purposes, where print may include text, symbols, and/or images. While the ability to engage in literacy activities is critical for participation in a print-driven society, Lonigan and Shanahan (2010) indicated that pinpointing what actually constitutes “literacy” is not a straightforward process. Part of the dilemma lies with determining what skills best define, characterize, and promote literacy competence, an issue with practical implications for teachers. To be effective, teachers must select for instruction those literacy skills that are most essential for their students to complete literacy tasks in today’s increasingly
  • 5. complex and demanding society. This issue also has implications for research designed to enhance our understanding, and improve our delivery, of literacy interventions. Teachers are being asked to use practices based on scientific evidence, requiring that they survey and critically evaluate the extant research on literacy interventions to identify effective instructional practices for realizing particular literacy outcomes. One could say that a litmus test for examining the research on literacy interventions is whether a study or review of studies discloses sufficient information regarding the pedagogies employed and the outcomes realized, such that teachers can use the information to inform their practices. Teachers, however, also must consider external validity, or the generalizability of research results (Campbell & Stanley, 1963); that is, whether a particular study, a group of studies, or a review of studies can contribute meaningfully to their decisions about practice because there are parallels between the participants, contexts, and outcomes reported in the research and those within their own instructional situations. As
  • 6. noted by Campbell and Stanley, “in the area of teaching, the doubts frequently expressed as to the applicability in actual practice LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 4 of the results of highly artificial experiments are judgments about external validity” (p. 17). While the process of choosing essential literacy outcomes and delineating effective pedagogies for students with moderate to severe disabilities might seem to be straightforward, it is in fact fraught with difficulties. In the broader field of literacy, this is evident in the protracted “reading wars” that have occurred regarding what constitutes the best approaches to reading instruction (e.g., Flippo, 1999). “Reading wars” refers to repeated shifts across the years in what defines the essential outcomes and most effective instructional methods in reading and writing instruction, shifting between meaning- and code-based approaches. To illustrate, when reviewing this history Cassidy, Valadez, and Garrett (2010) described how the whole language movement
  • 7. (i.e., a meaning-based approach) emerged in the 1980s largely in reaction to the phonics-based movement (i.e., a code-based approach) of the previous era. Partially as an attempt to end the debates fueled by the whole language versus phonics dichotomy, in 1997 the U.S. Congress authorized the formation of a panel of experts to examine the empirical research on literacy instruction with school-aged readers who do not have disabilities, and to make recommendations about effective practices in terms of the scientific evidence (Cassidy et al., 2010). The result was the formation of the National Reading Panel, which provided recommendations grouped into what are now called the “five pillars” of reading instruction: (1) phonemic awareness, (2) phonics, (3) fluency, (4) vocabulary, and (5) text comprehension (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; henceforth referred to as NRP, 2000). Although intended to attenuate the pendulum swings between meaning- and code-based approaches, the years following the report’s publication were marked
  • 8. by a shift to code-based thinking, possibly the result of the report’s heavy emphasis on code- based approaches (Cassidy et al.). Whether or not the panel intended for the five pillars to LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 5 delineate “the most important areas for reading instruction” (Cassidy et al., p. 644), these pillars now influence how reading instruction is conceptualized and implemented in many schools. Although impacted by the debates between meaning- and code- based approaches to literacy instruction, teachers of students with moderate to severe disabilities have faced an additional dichotomy in their efforts to delineate evidence- based approaches to, and maximize student outcomes from, literacy instruction. This dichotomy is between a body of evidence showing that certain forms of instruction are effective when teaching specific types of reading skills to students with moderate to severe disabilities (Browder, Wakeman, Spooner, Ahlgrim- Delzell, & Algozzine, 2006; Whalon, Otaiba, & Delano, 2009),
  • 9. and another body of evidence suggesting that literacy opportunities are systematically denied to these students (Kliewer & Biklen, 2001; Ryndak, Morrison, & Sommerstein, 1999; Zascavage & Keefe, 2004). These latter studies describe attitudes of school personnel that presume inherent limitations in these students’ abilities to become literate, as well as the failure of general and special education programs to provide them access to literacy activities and communities evident throughout schools. This dichotomy between evidence of success at literacy tasks when provided instruction and restricted literacy opportunities likely has complex roots, involving both the policies and procedures of educational systems and the attitudes and beliefs of school personnel. It also is possible, however, that part of the problem lies with the content taught and the pedagogies used when literacy skills are taught to students with moderate to severe disabilities. Perhaps what is taught and how it is taught to these students, when contrasted with what is taught and how it is taught to typical students in general education, is sufficiently
  • 10. discrepant to discourage viewing these efforts and their outcomes as relevant indices of potential for success in the larger literacy activities and communities of schools. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 6 Considering that teachers are being asked to base instructional decisions on research (Chhabra & McCardle, 2004; Reyna, 2004), and in light of the foregoing issues, it might be productive to examine literacy instruction research conducted with students who have moderate to severe disabilities, and consider what this research promotes as useful when selecting literacy outcomes and instructional practices for these students. Moreover, given that schools must provide all students with access to, and ensure their progress in, the general curriculum (Copeland & Cosbey, 2009; McLaughlin, 2010), it might be productive to consider whether this research offers teachers guidance on using literacy instruction to promote these broader ends.
  • 11. Although focused on the learning of typical school-aged students, the five pillars delineated by the National Reading Panel (NRP, 2000) might offer a useful framework for exploring the foregoing issues. Both Downing (2005) and Copeland and Keefe (2007) noted that definitions of literacy typically employed when defining and describing instruction with students who have moderate to severe disabilities often emphasize the teaching of reading and writing skills related to the literal interpretation of text, and this perspective seems consistent with the NRP (2000) report. In addition, there seems to be a substantial body of quantitative research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities that employs direct instructional processes that are intended to affect discrete literacy skills, an approach to instruction that would appear to be consistent with the NRP’s emphasis on direct and explicit instruction. Finally, other examinations of the reading instruction literature have employed this framework to usefully organize relevant research (e.g., Browder et al., 2006). Accordingly, in this paper, we will use the NRP’s five pillars to
  • 12. frame our exploration of issues and concerns about literacy instruction research for students with moderate to severe disabilities. To provide a practical focus, we will also do this through the lens of teachers who LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 7 might be examining the literature in search of ways to incorporate in their instruction literacy practices that are supported by research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities. The first part of this paper opens with a discussion on the nature and relevance of scientific evidence when used to guide reading instruction, followed by sub-sections for each of the five pillars. Each sub-section provides a discussion of specific issues and concerns related to that pillar, coupled with illustrations drawn from the extant research on literacy instruction. The second and final part of this paper summarizes concerns with the research associated with each of the five pillars; explores issues and concerns that go across the pillars; poses questions about
  • 13. the applicability of the National Reading Panel’s (2000) findings for literacy instruction for students with moderate to severe disabilities; and re-considers the dichotomy described above between evidence of success at literacy tasks and restricted literacy opportunities. In this section, we also acknowledge limitations associated with the critical analysis offered in this paper. Scientific Evidence, and Issues and Concerns Related to the Five Pillars Chhabra and McCardle (2004) asserted that the NRP (2000) report established high standards for evaluating scientific research when planning and providing reading instruction. These authors asserted that teachers should access research reports and peer-reviewed journals “to read the information first hand, to decide for themselves how good the evidence is” (p. 9). What constitutes scientific evidence lies at the core of any discussion about how to use research to make instructional decisions. In her discussion of reading research and the nature of evidence, Reyna (2004) argued that one way to define scientific evidence is to distinguish it from
  • 14. alternative forms of “evidence” that might be used in instructional decision-making. She noted that classroom instruction, when not based on scientific evidence, can be based on appeals to tradition, philosophy, intuition, superstition, and anecdote, and she suggested that such appeals LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 8 have been “the only options available” to many teachers of reading. Reyna further argued that scientific evidence requires knowing about and using processing models of learning, which take into account factors such as attention, motivation, and the storage and retrieval of information. She asserted that all who are involved in instructional decision- making need to be familiar with basic processes of learning, and by extension with what the research says about how to engage those processes most effectively to teach reading and other literacy skills. We suggest that, while the foregoing views on how to characterize scientific evidence are
  • 15. important, they do not fully go to the heart of the nature of “scientific evidence” when applied to instructional decision-making, and that teachers must consider at least three other factors when evaluating instructional research. The first and most obvious factor we suggest, and the one most consistent with Reyna’s (2004) arguments, is whether a strategy or instructional approach has been demonstrated to have, or not have, an effect; for example, whether skill acquisition occurred, whether the occurrence of a performance change can be linked to the described intervention and not to other reported or non-reported events, and whether acquisition occurred more rapidly or efficiently using one method over another. The second factor that we suggest teachers must consider is the robustness of the findings; that is, does a study provide evidence that skills acquired by the students are retained across time or are shown to generalize into relevant and purposeful activities and settings outside of the immediate instructional situation. Third and finally, we suggest that teachers must consider the utility of a study’s findings; that is,
  • 16. does a study address or show how the skills that were acquired by students promote or result in the formation of other more advanced skills, provide access to curriculum or other materials, or contribute to successful functioning within a broader range of literacy activities. Addressing these three issues within a published report of research could be difficult, and LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 9 might require researchers of literacy instruction to go beyond the intended scopes and purposes of their studies. It also could be argued that researchers need not elaborate on how the discrete skills learned by students during their intervention studies are connected with other skills or contribute to advanced activity participation, if such relationships seem to have been established elsewhere in the extant research. Nevertheless, we propose that asking teachers to use scientific evidence in planning instruction requires researchers to apply a stringent standard of what constitutes evidence of meaningful change. We propose that
  • 17. such a standard extends the analysis process associated with a study’s results from simply showing how a discrete skill set was acquired and toward showing whether a broader range of variables associated with the literacy lives of the students have been impacted. This is not unlike asking researchers addressing literacy practices in their studies to also address the social validity of their findings (Wolf, 1978). The immediate implications of our expanded view of the nature of scientific evidence is that it will color what we propose are the important issues and concerns related to the five pillars of reading delineated by the NRP (2000). As we discuss each pillar, we will share our perceptions of what a teacher needs to see in a study, or in a body of studies, to gain confidence in the evidence. We believe that by doing so, the concept of scientific evidence can be fleshed out to improve the utility of future research for teachers. Phonemic Awareness Phonemes are the smallest units of sound that “influence the meaning of words” (Gillon,
  • 18. 2004, p. 7); hence, phonemic awareness is “the awareness that the speech stream consists of a sequence of sounds” (Yopp & Yopp, 2000, p. 130). Instruction in phonemic awareness involves assisting students to focus on and manipulate sounds in orally- presented syllables and words (NRP, 2000), and measurement of phonemic awareness involves tasks that use spoken language LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 10 (Gillon). In its findings, the NRP reported that explicit training in phonemic awareness resulted in improvements in “phonemic awareness, reading, and spelling,” and that “teaching children to manipulate phonemes” was highly effective across a variety of students (p. 7). In their meta-analysis of reading intervention research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities published between 1975 and 2003, Browder et al. (2006) reported only 5 studies that included phonemic awareness instruction, none of which appear to have been candidates for inclusion in their effect size analysis. However,
  • 19. we think that teachers who comb the research literature looking for studies that address the effects of instruction on phonemic awareness for students who have moderate to severe disabilities will find a number of studies suggesting that explicit, one-to-one instruction can lead to the acquisition of targeted sound recognition and production skills when taught in isolated or special education settings (Hoogeveen & Smeets,1988; Hoogeveen, Birkhoff, Smeets, Lancioni, & Boelens, 1989). We have, however, four concerns about the use of this research to guide teachers’ decision-making for literacy instruction. First, we wonder how the available research in this area can inform practice when some number of these studies mix phonemic awareness instruction with instruction in other skills, such as skills associated with printed material, phonics, and vocabulary (Bracey, Maggs, & Morath, 1975; Browder, Ahlgrim-Delzell, Courtade, Gibbs, & Flowers, 2008; Conners, Rosenquist, Sligh, Atwell, & Kiser, 2006; Goetz et al., 2008). Certainly, given the successes of these studies, it seems
  • 20. reasonable to propose that instruction in phonemic awareness skills should be routinely integrated with instruction in other reading skills (see also Gillon, 2004), and that instruction in phonemic awareness can be supported by using pictures and real words (see also Allor, Mathes, Champlin, & Cheatham, 2009). Nevertheless, because the phonemic awareness instruction is confounded with instruction in other skills in LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 11 these studies, the reported changes in neither phonemic awareness nor other skills can be linked unequivocally to the phonemic awareness component of the instruction. If the broader literacy instruction research base in moderate to severe disabilities also reflects this pattern, then the contribution of phonemic awareness instruction toward literacy skill building is unclear. Second, we question whether phonemic awareness skills taught to students with moderate to severe disabilities in relative isolation can have the desired cumulative effects sought by
  • 21. teachers, in which skills taught early in the process facilitate the acquisition of skills taught subsequently. Hoogeveen and Smeets (1988), for instance, implemented a reading program with seven young Dutch students, beginning with word blending tasks, followed by syllable and phoneme blending in isolation, and ending with instruction in reading one-syllable words. Some “spill-over effects” were reported, in which students produced correct responses in later steps of the reading program without direct training; however, the authors concluded that “the training effects were considerably stronger than the spillover effects” (p. 51). That is, the authors concluded that the progress that occurred as students advanced step-by-step through the reading program was much more likely to be a function of direct instruction on each step, rather than the generalization of skills from previous steps. Third, we note that numerous trials often seem to be required to achieve acquisition of these very basic sound recognition skills, and we wonder about the causal factors underlying this
  • 22. pattern. It seems to us that there might be a belief that instruction is necessarily intense and prolonged because of the presence of the students’ disabilities. However, there also is the possibility that the amount of time for instruction and the number of trials reported are intense and prolonged not because of the student’s disability, but because: (a) the student perceives the instruction or content as tedious, uninteresting, or unrelated to their personal interests; or (b) LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 12 there is an absence of other, more varied experiences with literacy that could lend meaning and purpose to the phonemic awareness skill acquisition (e.g., storybook reading). Fourth and finally, if an ultimate goal of literacy instruction is for the student to access and comprehend content (Snow, 2002), then teachers must think strategically about how they portion their limited instructional time and resources in relation to different literacy skill sets. We remain uncertain as to how focused instruction in phonemic
  • 23. awareness contributes in the long run to the emergence of meaning-based skills (e.g., reading comprehension), which can more directly facilitate content access. None of the studies referred to above adequately examined this issue, and the NRP’s finding that explicit instruction in phonemic awareness can benefit other reading skills can be confirmed only with respect to instruction for students with moderate to severe disabilities through additional research. We contend that there is a need for longitudinal research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities in various contexts in which a broad range of phonemic awareness experiences (e.g., direct instruction, embedded instruction, incidental learning opportunities) are correlated with learning other literacy skills, especially learning to comprehend vocabulary and text. Moreover, we see a need for this research to conceptualize theoretical mechanisms that can: (a) account not only for phonemic awareness skill acquisition, but also for both failed and successful skill retention and generalization; and (b) explain how the development of other literacy skills is impeded
  • 24. or facilitated, given either a deficient or well-developed skill base in phonemic awareness. Phonics Phonics is a “way of teaching reading and spelling that stresses symbol-sound relationships” (Yopp & Yopp, 2000, p. 131). The NRP (2000) emphasized phonics instruction as an important vehicle for acquiring the ability to identify and use printed letter-sound LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 13 relationships both in isolation and in a variety of print contexts. An essential reading skill that is facilitated by phonics instruction is decoding – the process of sounding out words by using the regularities associated with letters and groups of letters to facilitate recognition of familiar words and to assist in identifying unknown words (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). While noting that phonics instruction is insufficient when used alone, the NRP strongly recommended systematic explicit phonics instruction for realizing the “foundational skills” of
  • 25. reading in students (p. 9). The NRP asserted that when systematic phonics instruction is employed, “significant benefits” are accrued in reading and spelling, and benefits are noted in reading comprehension, for students of all ages and for students who are struggling readers (pp. 9-10). There is a dearth of studies examining instruction for students with moderate to severe disabilities in phonological structure and decoding processes associated with printed words (see Joseph & Seery, 2004); nevertheless, there are studies suggesting that systematic phonics instruction might hold promise for teaching these and related skills. For example, Bracey et al. (1975) used the “task-analyzed, structured” materials of the Direct Instructional System for Teaching and Remediation (DISTAR) Reading Level 1 program, along with token reinforcement, to teach six children, aged 7 to14 years, skills for producing and blending sounds in response to written letters and words. Fallon, Light, McNaughton, Drager, and Hammer (2004) combined systematic instruction in the phonological structures of words with word recognition tasks to
  • 26. promote reading specific single words, reading other words with the same letters, and reading both types of words in text for five students ranging in age from 5 to14 years. A unique feature of their study was that the participants were nonverbal, thus their study required substituting picture-word association responses for spoken responses across all tasks. All five students reached criteria on single-word reading; three responded correctly to some novel words LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 14 composed of the same letters; and four responded correctly to some targeted and untaught words presented in text. In a final illustration, Bradford, Shippen, Alberto, Houchins, and Flores (2006) examined the use of systematic instruction to teach three students, aged 12 to 15 years, to sound out and read words both in isolation and in text formats. They used a modified version of Level A of the Corrective Reading Program curriculum (Engelmann, Meyer, Carnine, Becker, Eisele,
  • 27. & Johnson, 1998), teaching the students to say and write letter sounds, to say and write sounds in words, and to sound out words while saying them fast. Over a six-month period, all three students showed evidence of identifying letter sound correspondences, sounding out and blending words, and applying these skills when reading sentences and short passages. The students also showed increased recognition of words from either the Edmark Reading Program or the Dolch Sight Word list. The foregoing studies exemplify the promise of systematic instruction, especially when using programmed materials. However, we have four concerns related to what teachers should expect as a result of their instruction and one concern related to how teachers address the context in which instruction occurs. Our first concern is that the foregoing and other studies (e.g., Gersten & Maggs, 1982) do not provide evidence of changes in reading comprehension – a desired outcome of phonics-based instruction (NRP, 2000). Given that students with Autism Spectrum Disorder can acquire “code-based skills” relatively
  • 28. easily but then show persistent problems with comprehension (Whalon et al., 2009), teachers who are considering the use of instructional time to teach students with moderate to severe disabilities decoding and other phonics-related skills need more scientific evidence showing how phonics instruction can concurrently build and support comprehension. A second concern is that programmed instructional packages available to teachers tend to LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 15 construct skills in an additive fashion, with skills overlearned in isolation then becoming the same expectations within programmed text passages. Teachers need scientific evidence that skills learned from reading programmed text transfer to other authentic reading materials, and the foregoing studies do not provide sufficient evidence of this. A third concern of ours is that teachers will wonder about the role of rote memorization, as opposed to the “true” problem-
  • 29. solving processes of word attack and decoding, when students are taught phonics-related word recognition strategies via systematic, discrete trial instruction. Put differently, teachers need more information than is presently provided about the mechanisms of letter and word processing that are employed by phonics-learning students with moderate to severe disabilities when they are engaged in tasks such as reading text passages. A fourth concern relates to one of the primary purposes for learning to read; that is, accessing subject matter and acquiring new information. We contend that teachers need more scientific evidence than provided in the extant literature as to how students with moderate to severe disabilities who are receiving instruction on decoding skills can use these skills to access and utilize authentic age- appropriate text materials. Fifth and finally, while there is evidence that systematic instruction conducted in self-contained or isolated contexts can lead to performance changes in phonics-related skills for students with moderate to severe disabilities, we contend that teachers need proof that the same results could not have been
  • 30. more readily achieved -- and with other academic and social benefits -- via general education class participation, with or without brief pull-aside interventions. There exists the possibility that our five concerns will prove to be unfounded. However, we raise these concerns to encourage researchers to address them in future reviews of the literature and in their future research, so that teachers will have the scientific evidence that they need to confidently apply research results in their classroom practices. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 16 Fluency Fluency is a composite concept consisting of rate, accuracy, and prosody. More proficient readers display greater ease and natural expression in their oral production of continuous narrative and appear to comprehend text more as they read it (Hicks, 2009; NRP, 2000). The NRP (2000) extended this finding by interpreting the research as showing that, as students build
  • 31. fluency via guided oral reading, they also develop comprehension skills. When considering the research on fluency for students with moderate to severe disabilities, there appear to be three ways that fluency instruction is being conceptualized. First, we interpret some of the research as conceptualizing instruction on fluency as teaching nonreaders to accurately read aloud sequences of words that originally were taught in isolation (see Brown et al., 1972; Brown & Perlmutter, 1971). For example, Brown and Perlmutter (1971) successfully taught seven students who were nonreaders to read word-by-word 9 short sentences (e.g., “The penny is on top of the box”), and then perform simple motor response related to the sentences (e.g., pointing to the penny on top of the box). Second, we interpret some of the research as conceptualizing instruction on fluency as teaching students who already possess some text reading skills to improve their oral reading accuracy. For example, Singh and colleagues (see Singh, 1990; Singh & Singh, 1985a; Singh & Singh, 1984; Singh & Singh, 1986;
  • 32. Singh, Singh, & Winton, 1984) focused on accurate reading and the self-correction of reading errors through the use of guided oral reading of short passages and stories, and found a variety of systematic interventions that had a positive impact on oral reading accuracy. Third, we interpret some of the research as conceptualizing instruction on fluency as teaching multiple reading skills across several or all of the five pillars, and aspects of fluency serve as indices of changes in reading proficiency. For example, over a one-year period in self-contained classrooms Apffel, LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 17 Kelleher, Lilly, and Richardson (1975) used either the DISTAR reading program that emphasizes “a phonics approach to reading,” or the Rebus reading program that emphasizes a whole word approach (p. 230), to teach 60 students labeled “trainable mentally retarded.” Based on progress in oral reading skills (e.g., reading accuracy and errors) the authors concluded that, regardless of
  • 33. which program was used, “…nearly all of the 60 participating TMR students demonstrated some capacity to profit from reading instruction” (p. 229). More recently a study by Osborn et al. (2007) reflected this conceptualization of fluency when over a six-month period they used peer tutors to augment instruction regularly provided to elementary- aged students, including 31 students with moderate to severe disabilities. The instruction targeted skills related to all five of the pillars and was provided during one-to-one tutoring sessions lasting 30-minutes each, for three or four days each week. They found that students in the experimental group showed significantly higher scores on the fluency subtest of the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS; Good & Kaminski, 1996). The studies reflecting all three conceptualizations of fluency instruction suggest that specific forms of systematic instruction can be effective when applied to aspects of fluency. However, teachers reviewing these studies need to look beyond evidence of acquisition and into issues of the value and utility of the reported results.
  • 34. We are concerned when fluency is conceptualized in the first way (i.e., teaching skills for accurately reading and responding to sequences of discrete words previously taught in isolation) whether teachers can anticipate generalization to other curriculum-relevant reading materials following significant investments of instruction and time. Reading to acquire meaning relies on a generative process in which word interchangeability is a necessary quality. While the Brown and Perlmutter (1971) study described above was groundbreaking in showing how literacy goals LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 18 involving connected text were attainable by students with moderate to severe disabilities, the study reported no generalization data in relation to combinations of other different words. To address the foregoing concern, teachers might augment their instruction with matrix training, which has been reported to be successful in the communication instruction literature
  • 35. (e.g., Goldstein, 1983; Nigam, Schlosser, & Lloyd, 2006; Striefel, Wetherby, & Karlan, 1978). In this approach, symbols or words representing action words, objects, and adjectives are systematically varied in relation to each other during instruction, and generalization to novel word combinations within the matrix of targeted words is examined as instruction proceeds. At least with respect to oral language and augmentative communication systems some positive results have been reported; however, teachers still should look for evidence that students can move beyond words and word sequences contained within the matrices, and such evidence is not adequate in these studies. When fluency is conceptualized in the second way (i.e., improving accuracy during oral reading of connected text) we note the inadequate consideration of two key components of fluency, rate and prosody. There are, however, additional concerns that relate directly to the robustness of the successful skill changes that are reported. For instance, in the foregoing studies by Singh and colleagues: (a) improvements in reading accuracy
  • 36. were documented mostly within intervention contexts in which there was high teacher control, a condition which might promote students’ dependence on the instructing adults (McNaughton, 1981); (b) not all measures of accuracy showed improvements from baseline to intervention; (c) generalized use of skills was seldom assessed and, when assessed, it did not always occur; (d) maintenance of skills was seldom assessed and, when it was assessed, data were collected over short time intervals and with the same text passages that were used during instruction; and finally, (e) measures of LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 19 reading comprehension were used infrequently and, when used, the results were equivocal. When fluency is conceptualized in the third way (i.e., multiple reading skills are taught addressing some or all of the five pillars, and fluency indices are reported) we have concerns regarding the usefulness and the robustness of the research evidence. For example, Apffel et al.
  • 37. (1975) reported overall positive changes in oral reading proficiency in relation to both reading programs; however, the results of an analysis of patterns of variation in skill production showed that (a) individual students varied notably in their responses to the two different programs, and (b) oral reading “error rates” and “correct rates” varied considerably even in students receiving instruction in the same program. We also are concerned about the usefulness of their findings because they report, “Children who had not been able to demonstrate adequate progress were reassigned to TMR classes where reading instruction was less strongly emphasized” (p. 232). Few details were provided about the criteria used to re-assign students to the other classes, and no information was provided on the number of students dropped. Attrition and lack of details raise questions regarding the applicability of the study’s evidence to the full population of students. Similarly, while Osborn et al. (2007) reported positive changes in fluency in relation to their peer-tutoring program, there were other reported results that raised questions about the
  • 38. learning that occurred in their study participants. Specifically, instruction occurred across all five pillars; yet, there were no significant results on any of the other reading measures, including a composite Woodcock-Johnson index (Woodcock-Johnson Proficiency Battery – Revised, Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) that incorporated reading comprehension. Finally, across the studies considered here, evidence that changes in fluency are accompanied by changes in reading comprehension seem insufficient. For teachers who want reading instruction to ultimately contribute to comprehension, we believe this absence of evidence should be troubling. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 20 When considering the need to employ instructional methods that best equip students for accessing and comprehending curriculum content, we contend that more research is needed on how fluency is impacted by instruction in phonological and decoding skills, and how such
  • 39. outcomes relate to reading comprehension. To illustrate, consider that reading rate is a function of both reading proficiency and text complexity (Hicks, 2009; Kuhn, Schwanenflugel, & Meisinger, 2010), and the mark of the proficient reader is not rate per se but rather “appropriate pacing” so that comprehension is facilitated (Kuhn et al.). In their study that taught decoding skills, Bradford et al. (2006) reported that fluency worsened as students advanced in their reading skills and were exposed to longer text passages. They stated that “…as passages became longer, [the students’] fluency seemed to be [adversely] affected” (p. 341). This raises questions about the course and function of fluency change when documented progress is occurring in reading skills related to the other pillars, especially phonics. We wonder whether what Bradford et al. saw with the onset of fluency problems was changes in reading pace that reflected emerging and tentative efforts by the students to comprehend the more complex material. Alternatively, we wonder whether these breakdowns reflect difficulties in decoding or word recall when the
  • 40. quantity of information being memorized as a consequence of rote instruction is substantial. If the former, then further research is needed to determine whether concurrent instruction in comprehension could alleviate fluency problems when progress is being made in rudimentary skills, such as decoding and word recognition; if the latter, we need to better understand how to enhance learning, perhaps considering more closely the role of both instructional timing and affect in how reading skills are acquired. Regardless, evidence that bears on specific learning patterns such as reported above would assist teachers in understanding how instructional methods reported as “successful” should be configured and delivered to ensure that students with LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 21 moderate to severe disabilities are being taught meaningful and useful literacy skills. Vocabulary The NRP (2000) characterized vocabulary knowledge as skills for recognizing and
  • 41. understanding words presented both orally and in print. The panel acknowledged the historical importance of vocabulary instruction in the teaching of reading, and it portrayed the acquisition of vocabulary as a dynamic process, characterized by “continuous growth in word knowledge,” by the interplay of decoding skills with word knowledge, and by the ongoing recall of previous word knowledge when encountering new words (p. 13). Emphasizing that the “essence” of reading is comprehension, the NRP asserted that the magnitude of a person’s vocabulary knowledge is a critical factor in his or her ability at “making sense of text” (p.13). The NRP stressed the importance of repetition and repeated exposure to words when teaching vocabulary, and it called for direct and explicit instruction on vocabulary. Finally, the NRP asserted that vocabulary knowledge is not just the ability to identify words on sight; rather, it is an integral, progressive, and ever expanding aspect of reading comprehension. When considering research studies in which successful vocabulary instruction has been
  • 42. demonstrated for students with moderate to severe disabilities, we have two concerns. First, although there has been a longstanding emphasis on vocabulary instruction with students who have moderate to severe disabilities (Browder et al., 2006; Browder & Xin, 1998; Singh & Singh, 1985b), we are concerned that this vocabulary instruction has sometimes focused on teaching students to memorize word lists that do not reflect the NRP (2000) emphasis on vocabulary acquisition as a progressive, iterative, and ever- expanding process. For instance, there are studies focusing on the acquisition of lists of discrete vocabulary words that are selected (a) based on their conceptual relation to community purposes, such as reading signs, LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 22 recognizing danger, or completing common forms (e.g., Ault, Gast, & Wolery, 1988; Blyden, 1989; Collins & Stinson, 1994; Doyle, Gast, Wolery, Ault, & Meyer, 1992; Karsh, Repp, &
  • 43. Lenz, 1990; Miracle, Collins, Schuster, & Grisham-Brown, 2001; Singleton, Schuster, Morse, & Collins, 1999); and/or (b) based on consideration of what very young or beginning readers might be expected to learn or easily perform (Barudin & Hourcade, 1990; Koury & Browder, 1986; Van der Bijl, Alant, & Lloyd, 2006). We think that these methods for selecting and teaching vocabulary words can be limiting, because they are not explicitly designed to provide students with moderate to severe disabilities substantive links to narrative text materials, to relevant grade-level curriculum, or to interest-based materials. Second, we are concerned that, when comprehension was assessed in these vocabulary instruction research studies, it was not always assessed in a manner that is consistent with NRP (2000) vocabulary instruction concepts. For example, there are studies in which comprehension was assessed through students’: (a) recognition of the correct word when shown a picture (Basil & Reyes, 2003; Doyle, Wolery, Gast, & Ault., 1990; Griffen, Schuster, & Morse, 1998); (b) verbalization of a simple definition when shown the target word
  • 44. and given a verbal prompt (Gast, Wolery, Morris, Doyle, & Meyer, 1990; Griffen et al., 1998); (c) recall of a target word when exposed to different or more natural contexts (e.g., Doyle et al., 1990; Rohena, Jitendra, & Browder, 2002; Singleton et al., 1999); or (d) performance of a motor response consistent with the meaning of the word (Collins & Griffen, 1996; Doyle et al., 1990). While there are probably a number of reasons why studies of vocabulary instruction involving students with moderate to severe disabilities have chosen their particular dependent measures, we suggest that one reason might lie with the intentions and purposes behind these studies. It seems likely that a number of these studies have focused less on how to teach a full LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 23 complement of vocabulary reading skills and more on the relative effectiveness of specific universally applicable intervention strategies (e.g., prompting processes, reinforcement
  • 45. strategies). We suggest that, when research is concentrated on what constitutes effective discrete trial instruction, the investigators might have chosen literacy outcomes less for their potential to provide deep and abiding understandings of vocabulary, and more because they are parsimonious, precise, and reliable for assessing cause, effect, and efficiency. We suggest that single word identification, matching, and recognition measures, with and without simple measures of word understanding, meet the latter conditions. While acknowledging that these studies might have achieved their purpose of providing scientific evidence that a particular strategy “works,” we are nevertheless concerned that these studies could be used by teachers to draw conclusions about what to teach, instead of conclusions solely about how to teach. Thus, teachers reviewing the literature for reasonable and feasible outcomes to go along with their evidence-based strategies for literacy instruction could easily misinterpret these particular studies as providing evidence of useful outcomes as well. Moreover,
  • 46. we are concerned that practices in schools might now reflect this dilemma, with both instructional strategies and literacy outcomes from this research literature reflected in teacher practices. For example, we contend that teachers are using discrete trial, prompting and reinforcement formats to teach information related to weather, dates, holidays, and personal data via repeated cycles of calendar and morning circle; and survival and community word recognition (e.g., stop, exit, and poison) via frequent, intermittent flash card routines. Aside from concerns about how students with moderate to severe disabilities might experience at an emotional level these instructional processes when they are delivered repeatedly across their grades and without reference to age-appropriateness or meaningful and motivating contexts, LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 24 there is a pressing need for research to demonstrate whether such instruction actually leads to generalized use of the targeted vocabulary across relevant life
  • 47. contexts (Browder & Xin, 1998), to meaningful and lasting vocabulary comprehension, or to “continuous growth of word knowledge” (NRP, 2000, p. 13). Hence, we believe that teachers’ pervasive use of these formats of literacy instruction must be viewed critically. We are also concerned that this type of instruction might be self-perpetuating, promoting in teachers, as well as parents and administrators, a false sense that valued outcomes are resulting from instruction, because the target words appear to have face validity and students might respond reliably during instructional trials, as shown in the research. We emphasize that we cannot infer cause and effect between research of this type and observed teacher choices about what vocabulary to teach in schools; yet, there is plausibility to our stance that some relationship exists, and that it could become stronger as teachers are increasingly pressured to justify their practices and outcomes based on the scientific literature. Hence, it behooves researchers interested in direct, discrete trial instruction protocols to take
  • 48. seriously this possibility, and begin considering different choices in what is taught to demonstrate effect, paying closer attention to the NRP (2000) characterization of vocabulary knowledge as a progressive and constructive process. Comprehension Reading comprehension is the sine qua non of literacy proficiency, entailing text reading together with understanding (Raphael & Au, 2005; Snow, 2002). It is an “intentional problem solving thinking” process in which the reader “derives meaning from text” through active interaction with the text (NRP, 2000, p. 14). The NRP found evidence that comprehension “is enhanced” when students relate their own “knowledge and experience” to the ideas being LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 25 expressed in the material being read. It requires not only the ability to access familiar and easy text, but also the ability to access, learn, and reflect on unfamiliar and difficult text (Snow,
  • 49. 2002). The NRP (2000) called for explicit instruction, identifying seven research-based approaches to explicit instruction: (a) comprehension monitoring, (b) cooperative learning, (c) graphic and semantic organizers, (d) question answering, (e) question generation, (f) story structure, and (g) summarization. Finally, the panel stressed that the outcome of instruction should be students’ acquisition and use of strategies for independent text comprehension, such as independently using knowledge of text structure, generating and answering questions, and retelling and summarizing (Mills, 2009). Literacy instruction within and across the five NRP pillars should ultimately assist students with moderate to severe disabilities to derive meaning through their own efforts to understand connected text. As shown in the previous sections, we have concerns that there is an evidence gap between demonstrations of success at teaching basic skills of reading and demonstrations of the emergence of independence in reading comprehension. Clearly, teachers
  • 50. of students with moderate to severe disabilities could benefit from evidence that particular methods of phonological, fluency, and vocabulary skill instruction concurrently support text comprehension skill acquisition. Additionally, teachers of students with moderate to severe disabilities could benefit from research that explores multi- component instruction in which reading comprehension capabilities are systematically taught and are primary outcomes achieved alongside the basic skills of reading. For example, Allor, Mathes, Roberts, Jones, and Champlin (2010) conducted a multi- component reading instruction study which reported measures of comprehension for their participants in first to fourth grade from 10 public schools, who were randomly assigned to either LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 26 a treatment (n= 16) or contrast (n=12) group. Students in the contrast group received instruction that already was typically provided in their programs, while students in the treatment group
  • 51. received “daily, comprehensive reading instruction in small groups . . . for approximately 40 minutes per session” (p. 3) for “one to one and a half years” (p. 16). Intervention materials consisted of a modified and expanded version of a packaged program (i.e., Early Interventions in Reading). Instruction on reading comprehension focused on “comprehension strategies” (p. 9) using storybook read-alouds, stories with decodable text, supporting pictures, and other materials. While most of their dependent measures focused on basic skills (e.g., phonemic awareness), pre- and post-data on reading comprehension also were collected using the passage comprehension test of The Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery – Revised (Woodcock, 1991). Small gains were reported for both groups, but the treatment group showed greater gains, and there was a moderate effect size reported (.69) that was statistically significant. While these results are promising, we have three concerns that we believe compromise this evidence. First, insufficient information is reported on the educational programming
  • 52. provided for students in the contrast group; that is, their programs could have focused on any content, from reading comprehension to self-care. Hence, “significant results” might imply only that intensive prolonged reading instruction is better than no reading instruction at all (see Slavin, 2008). Second, replication is made difficult by the modifications made to the standard programs, reportedly to match the baseline levels of students. Insufficient information is provided about the modifications made per student, and the process used to match the modifications to students’ needs. Third and finally, the intervention had effects on reading comprehension for some students, but it seemed to have less impact on other students. Specifically, it was reported that only 8 of the 16 participants (50%) actually achieved program LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 27 levels at which they could be “working on basic comprehension strategies, such as retelling
  • 53. stories, sequencing main events, and story grammar” (p. 18). In light of the NRP (2000) emphasis on reading comprehension strategy instruction, we believe that teachers need more research addressing this instruction when it is grounded in grade- level general education content and contexts. As an illustration, consider what instruction might look like when using the Mills (2009) sequence for teaching comprehension strategies within and across reading lessons for a heterogeneous class of students: (1) activating prior knowledge, (2) making inferences, (3) using knowledge of text structure, (4) visualizing the story and related events, (5) generating and answering questions, and (6) retelling and summarizing. These strategies should be modeled during speaking and listening activities before, during, and after reading and telling stories, a process consistent with the NRP recommendation that teachers repeatedly demonstrate strategy application during literacy activities while encouraging students to use them. The scientific evidence examined by teachers of students with moderate to severe disabilities should inform them about the supports necessary
  • 54. and the learning that can occur when reading comprehension involves constructing and reconstructing knowledge in relation to repeated experiences with text, and where the instruction consists of overlapping and complexly sequenced activities that involve interactions with typical peers. Conclusions, Questions, and Limitations We used the five-pillars framework advanced by the NRP (2000) to explore issues and concerns about literacy instruction with students who have moderate to severe disabilities. Our focus was on what teachers need to learn when reviewing research to improve their instructional practices and to achieve better literacy outcomes for these students. We raised concerns about what constitutes adequate scientific evidence for teachers; we offered studies from the extant LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 28 research with students who have moderate to severe disabilities to illustrate these concerns; and we suggested ways to improve research to better serve the needs
  • 55. of teachers. The NRP (2000) report strongly recommended using direct, explicit instruction across the five pillars of reading instruction. When selecting examples to illustrate our concerns, we chose quantitative studies that defined literacy in relation to reading and writing, and that focused on direct instruction of discrete skills for students with moderate to severe disabilities. We recognize that the studies that we present here do not fully represent the spectrum of literacy instruction studies with students who have moderate to severe disabilities. For instance, the extant literature includes quantitative studies on the impact of embedding literacy instruction in general education activities (e.g., McDonnell, Johnson, Polychronis, & Riesen, 2002). However, our intent here was not to provide a comprehensive review of the literature. Rather, it was to provide research illustrations of studies that emphasized instruction of skills associated with each of the five pillars, and that appeared to mirror what we believe are aspects of the NRP report’s findings and orientation to instruction that are in need of critical discussion
  • 56. and more research. We also acknowledge that we did not adequately address issues of research design. Instead, we used a scenario in which teachers working from a five pillars orientation search for evidence about approaches to instruction within studies that likely vary in rigor with respect to methodological criteria. By concentrating solely on possible research review activities of teachers who instruct students with moderate to severe disabilities, we presented what we believe are discrepancies between the evidence that they might see and the evidence that they might need. The most prominent concerns we raised by using this process touch on issues of external validity. On the one hand, teachers might find themselves skeptical as to whether focused code- based instruction has validity if the ultimate goal of their own classroom instruction is to LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 29 facilitate independence in text understanding and access to the general curriculum. On the other
  • 57. hand, teachers might be misled to believe that certain forms of discrete trial vocabulary instruction are valid for their students as long as the lists of vocabulary items are functional and relate to things students need to know in their daily lives. We also raised questions that touch on the validity of multi-component instructional packages, suggesting that teachers need to know more about how such packages lead to comprehension, how generalization beyond the immediate material of the packages can be realized, and whether the hours of instruction required when using such packages will eventually pay off, via promoting access for their students to grade-appropriate general curriculum. Since, in this era of standards, districts increasing might look to commercial packages as ways to both standardize instruction and provide alternative learning experiences to learners who do not appear to fit the norm of the “typical student,” we suggest that these questions are especially disconcerting, requiring a closer look at the literature base relevant to these issues.
  • 58. Reflecting our concerns with external validity, we suggest that teachers need scientific evidence that is drawn more from studies that match conditions of good classroom teaching, and we recommend that studies in which instructional strategies drawn from the five pillars are examined should match more closely the ecologies and activities of general education classes. As an example, let us consider vocabulary instruction. To both improve vocabulary instruction and make it relevant to grade-level general curriculum we believe teachers need scientific evidence from studies that parallel their reality; that is, studies that incorporate complex sets of independent variables found in general education classes and sets of dependent measures that express richer forms of learning. For instance, a study might first propose ways that vocabulary can be selected for instruction so that it is correlated with grade-appropriate textual content and LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 30 matches the learning processes of students with different ability
  • 59. levels. Researchers then could examine the results of a sequence of complex instructional activities that continuously link vocabulary acquisition back to relevant text, measuring both lexicon acquisition and connected text comprehension in different learners (see Stahl & Bravo, 2010). Furthermore, teachers would benefit if such research also assessed patterns of semantic learning. A useful tool for assessing patterns of student learning in this regard might be a network or semantic map, displaying interconnections between the units of information or the concepts underlying a lesson’s key vocabulary. Finally, teachers also would benefit if such research took into consideration the previous knowledge and real interests of each student, and processes of information exchange each student demonstrates when learning is occurring, such as in cooperative learning formats. When effective teachers plan and implement meaningful literacy instruction they utilize decision-making processes such as the foregoing; that is, processes that account for a multitude of ecological, intra-student, and curriculum content variables.
  • 60. Hence, we contend that instructional research also should reflect, as much as possible, these same sets of variables. An implication of this approach to literacy instruction research is that discrete trial techniques (e.g., time delay) might best be examined less as universally applicable strategies independent of content and context, and more as integral components of larger instructional frameworks. The research on embedded instruction by McDonnell and his colleagues (e.g., McDonnell et al., 2002) begins to address such issues. Research of this type does not devalue discrete trial techniques, but it does communicate their contributions to learning in proportion to that of other components of effective instruction, and it demonstrates their effectiveness in a manner that assists teachers to better incorporate them into general education contexts and activities. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 31 There is another external validity concern that we have that does not arise from studies
  • 61. within our own field; instead it arises from the NRP (2000) findings themselves, and this concern raises questions about the generalizability of the scientific evidence of the report with respect to students with moderate to severe disabilities. To understand this concern, it is necessary to contrast students with moderate to severe disabilities with another group of students for whom the report might have clear merit -- students who do not have an intellectual disability but who are struggling with reading and might, therefore, receive specialized, direct instruction, perhaps as part of a response-to-intervention (RtI) process. Prior to receiving an intervention, a particular student within this latter group might be struggling, but he or she still advances through the grades and steadily accumulates a history of experiences and opportunities to read, to try to understand what he/she reads, and to acquire literacy skills across general education contexts and content areas. In contrast, a student with a moderate to severe intellectual disability is much more likely to be served for all or most of the day throughout his/her school career in a self-contained
  • 62. class (Smith, 2007); this means he or she does not experience at any point in his or her school career the range of literacy activities and communities that are routinely part of life for the student who is a struggling reader. In the case of the struggling reader, specialized interventions that are direct, isolated, and remedial augment other literacy activities and experiences that occur during their school day, with the goal being to improve their reading for better access to the general curriculum. In the case of the student with a moderate to severe intellectual disability, instruction that is direct, isolated, and remedial frequently supplants the literacy activities and experiences that occur in other contexts during their school day, with the goal being to improve discrete reading-related skills. We acknowledge that there is an absence of research on the effects and implications of LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 32 this disparity; however, based on what we know about the
  • 63. impact of exposure-to-print on literacy skills (Anderson, Wilson, & Fielding, 1988), and what we know about the significance of context as a variable impacting the results of instruction (Jackson, Ryndak, & Wehmeyer, 2008-2009), we propose that, on average, the results of direct instruction will differ in these two cases, and that a student receiving the restricted range of experiences likely is at a disadvantage when contrasted with the student who is exposed to a wider range of experiences. This point of view is further supported by what we know about generalization (Stokes & Baer, 1977): For the student with an intellectual disability in the self-contained setting, there would be limited opportunities and thus little support for skills to generalize from literacy relevant contexts of the school into the specialized intervention situation; inversely, there would be limited opportunities and thus little support for skills to generalize from the specialized intervention situation into other relevant contexts of the school or larger community. The latter concern relates directly to NRP (2000) findings with respect to direct, explicit
  • 64. instruction. From an external validity standpoint, we suggest that while the report’s evidence is likely applicable to struggling readers receiving direct and specialized instruction designed to augment a range of experiences within and across general education contexts, it might not be applicable to students for whom the direct and specialized instruction fully replaces general education literacy experiences. In light of this, we think that referencing the NRP evidence as a justification for explicit direct instruction for students served in self-contained settings might be an improper extension of these findings. We can go a step further by examining how the NRP’s (2000) evidence for the value of “explicit instruction” might apply in these two different circumstances. It is likely that many teachers would define explicitness as the clarity by which they present directives and demand LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 33 responses from a student within an instructional routine. We
  • 65. think, however, that explicitness should refer not to whether a target outcome of instruction is clear to educators, but rather to whether it is clear to students; that is, whether students experience cognitive clarity with respect to instructional expectations (Cunningham & Cunningham, 2002; Downing, 1971). For the struggling reader who experiences literacy opportunities in multiple settings, the purpose and applicability of the instruction might be obvious (i.e., explicit), and learning is facilitated. For the student with a moderate to severe disability who experiences literacy opportunities mainly in one segregated special education context, the purpose and applicability of the specialized instruction might not be obvious (i.e., less explicit), and learning might then lack the necessary robustness for acquisition, retention, generalization, and wider application. Put differently, we suggest that “explicitness” might not be present in the latter situation despite “clear” instructions, and that learning might be short-lived because skills being taught cannot be linked cognitively by the student to literacy activities that are of consequence in his or
  • 66. her life at school. Given the foregoing, we perhaps have a partial answer to the dichotomy raised at the start of this paper: students with moderate to severe disabilities are being denied opportunities to be part of larger literacy communities even though their potential for learning and using literacy has been demonstrated. The dilemma might lie in part with learning differences that are largely a consequence of being isolated from general education and receiving, perhaps exclusively, specialized instruction in segregated contexts. Potential for progress, therefore, is less likely for students with moderate to severe disabilities not due to their disabilities, but due to their lack of exposure to the many and varied literacy opportunities that constitute what we call “general education.” It is a vicious cycle, in which the direct instruction processes produce results that cannot be fully applied, refined, and generalized because of lack of access to the larger arena of LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 34
  • 67. literacy opportunities, and the continuation of apparent skill deficiencies and instructional problems (e.g., prompt dependence, lack of generalization, poor retention) provide yet another reason to maintain student confinement in segregated special education contexts. We believe that the perspective of teachers, researchers, and others in education about the learning potential of these students would change if the students were provided full access to general education content in general education contexts, while supplementing their instruction in those contexts with learning adaptations or, if needed, short-term specialized instruction. We also believe that the scientific evidence made available to teachers in future literacy instruction research can help promote this, if it more directly emulates the conditions and variables associated with quality instruction with this content and in these settings. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 35
  • 68. References Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., Champlin, T., & Cheatham, J. P. (2009). Research-based techniques for teaching early reading skills to students with intellectual disabilities. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 44(3), 356-366. Allor, J., Mathes, P., Roberts, K., Jones, F., & Champlin, T. (2010). Teaching students with moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An experimental examination of a comprehensive reading program. Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 45(1), 3-22. Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 285-303. Apffel, J. A., Kelleher, J., Lilly, M. S., & Richardson, R. (1975). Developmental reading for moderately retarded children. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 10, 229- 235.
  • 69. Ault, M. M., Gast, D. L., & Wolery, M. (1988). Comparison of progressive and constant time- delay procedures in teaching community-sign word reading. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 93, 44-56. Barudin, S. I., & Hourcade, J. J. (1990). Relative effectiveness of three methods of reading instruction in developing specific recall and transfer skills in learners with moderate and severe mental retardation. Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 25, 286-291. Basil, C., & Reyes, S. (2003). Acquisition of literacy skills by children with severe disability. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 19. Blyden, A. E. (1989). Survival word acquisition in mentally retarded adolescents with multihandicaps: Effects of color-reversed stimulus materials. Journal of Special LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 36 Education, 22(4), 493-501. Bracey, S., Maggs, A., & Morath, P. (1975). The effects of a
  • 70. direct phonics approach in teaching reading. The Australian Journal on the Education of Backward Children, 22, 83-90. Bradford, S., Shippen, M. E., Alberto, P., Houchins, D. E., & Flores, M. (2006). Using systematic instruction to teach decoding skills to middle school students with moderate intellectual disabilities. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 41(4), 333-343. Browder, D. M., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., Courtade, G., Gibbs, S. L., & Flowers, C. (2008). Evaluation of the effectiveness of an early literacy program for students with significant developmental disabilities. Exceptional Children, 75, 33-52. Browder, D. M., Wakeman, S. Y., Spooner, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., & Algozzine, B. (2006). Research on reading instruction for individuals with significant cognitive disabilities. Exceptional Children, 72, 392-410. Browder, D. M., & Xin, Y. P. (1998). A meta-analysis and review of sight word research and its implications for teaching functional reading to individuals with
  • 71. moderate and severe disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 32, 130-153. Brown, L., Jones, S., Troccolo, E., Heiser, C., Bellamy, T., & Sontag, E. (1972). Teaching functional reading to young trainable students: Toward longitudinal objectives. Journal of Special Education, 6, 237-246. Brown, L., & Perlmutter, L. (1971). Teaching functional reading to trainable level retarded students. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 6, 74-84. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 37 Cassidy, J., Valadez, C. M., & Garrett, S. D. (2010). Literacy trends and issues: A look at the five pillars and the cement that supports them. The Reading Teacher, 63(8), 644-655. Chhabra, V., & McCardle, P. (2004). Contributions to evidence- based research. In P. McCardle
  • 72. & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence in reading research (pp. 3-11). Baltimore: Brookes. Collins, B. C., & Griffen, A. K. (1996). Teaching students with moderate disabilities to make safe responses to product warning labels. Education and Treatment of Children, 19, 30- 45. Collins, B. C., & Stinson, D. M. (1994). Teaching generalized reading of product warning labels to adolescents with mental disabilities through the use of key words. Exceptionality, 5(3), 163-181. Conners, F. A., Rosenquist, C. J., Sligh, A. C., Atwell, J. A., & Kiser, T. (2006). Phonological reading skills acquisition by children with mental retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 121-137. Copeland, S. R., & Cosbey, J. (2009). Making progress in the general curriculum: Rethinking effective instructional practices. Research and Practice for Persons With Severe
  • 73. Disabilities, 33-34, 214-227. Copeland, S. R., & Keefe, E. B. (2007). Effective literacy instruction for students with moderate or severe disabilities. Baltimore: Brookes. Cunningham, P. M., & Cunnigham, J. W. (2002). What we know about how to teach phonics. In A. E. F. S. J. Samuels (Ed.), What research has to say about reading instruction. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Downing, J. (1971, July). The cognitive clarity theory of learning to read. Paper LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 38 presented at the Annual Meeting of the United Kingdom Reading Association. Manchester, England. Downing, J. E. (2005). Teaching literacy to students with significant disabilities. Thousand Oaks, California: Corwin Press. Doyle, P. M., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Meyer, S. (1992). Teaching discrete skills
  • 74. to students with moderate mental retardation in small-group instructional arrangements. Exceptionality, 3(4), 233-253. Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., & Ault, M. J. (1990). Comparison of constant time delay and the system of least prompts in teaching preschoolers with developmental delays. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11, 1-22. Edmark reading program: Word recognition (2nd ed.) (1992). Cambridge, MA: Edmark Corporation. Eikeseth, S., & Jahr, E. (2001). The UCLA reading and writing program: An evaluation of the beginning stages. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22, 289-307. Engelmann, S., Meyer, L., Carnine, L., Becker, W., Eisele, J., & Johnson, G. (1998). Corrective reading decoding strategies. Columbus, OH: SRA/McGraw- Hill. Fallon, K. A., Light, J., McNaughton, D., Drager, K., & Hammer, C. (2004). The effects of direct instruction on the single-word reading skills of children who require augmentative and
  • 75. alternative communication. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 47, 1424- 1439. Flippo, R. F. (1999). Redefining the reading wars: the war against reading researchers. Educational Leadership, 57(2), 38-41. Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Morris, L. L., Doyle, P. M., & Meyer, S. (1990). Teaching sight word LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 39 reading in a group instructional arrangement using constant time delay. Exceptionality, 1(2), 81-96. Gersten, R. M., & Maggs, A. (1982). Teaching the general case to moderately retarded children: Evaluation of a five year project. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2(4), 329-343. Gillon, G. T. (2004). Phonological awareness: From research to practice. New York: Guilford Press.
  • 76. Goetz, K., Hulme, C., Brigstocke, S., Carroll, J. M., Nasir, L., & Snowling, M. (2008). Training reading and phoneme awareness in children with Down Syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 395-412. Goldstein, H. (1983). Recombinative generalization: Relationship between environmental conditions and the linguistic repertoires of language learners. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 3, 279-293. Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (1996). Assessment for instructional decisions: Toward a proactive/prevention model of decision-making for early literacy skills. School Psychology Quarterly, 11, 326−336. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6-10. Griffen, A. K., Schuster, J. W., & Morse, T. E. (1998). The acquisition of instructive feedback: A comparison of continuous versus intermittent presentation schedules. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
  • 77. 33, 42-61. Hicks, C. P. (2009). A lesson on reading fluency learned from The Tortoise and the Hare. The Reading Teacher, 63(4), 319-323. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 40 Hoogeveen, F. R., Birkhoff, A. E., Smeets, P. M., Lancioni, G. E., & Boelens, H. H. (1989). Establishing phonemic segmentation in moderately retarded children. Remedial and Special Education, 10(3), 47-53. Hoogeveen, F. R., & Smeets, P. M. (1988). Establishing phoneme blending in trainable mentally retarded children. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2), 46-53. Jackson, L. B., Ryndak, D. L., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2008- 2009). The dynamic relationship between context, curriculum, and student learning: A case for inclusive education as a research-based practice. Research and Practice for Persons With Severe Disabilities, 33(4)/ 34(1), 175-195.
  • 78. Joseph, L. M., & Seery, M. E. (2004). Where is the phonics? Remedial and Special Education, 25, 88-94. Karsh, K. G., Repp, A. C., & Lenz, M. W. (1990). A comparison of the task demonstration model and the standard prompting hierarchy in teaching word identification to persons with moderate retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11, 395-410. Kliewer, C., & Biklen, D. (2001). "School's not really a place for reading": A research synthesis of the literate lives of students with severe disabilities. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 26, 1-12. Koury, M., & Browder, D. M. (1986). The use of delay to teach sight words by peer tutors classified as moderately mentally retarded. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 21, 252-258. Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research
  • 79. Quarterly, 45(2), 230-251. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 41 Lonigan, C. J., & Shanahan, T. (2010). Developing early literacy skills: Things we know we know and things we know we don't know. Educational Researcher, 39(4), 340-346. McDonnell, J., Johnson, J. W., Polychronis, S., & Riesen, T. (2002). Effects of embedded instruction on students with moderate disabilities enrolled in general education classes. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 37, 363- 377. McLaughlin, M. J. (2010). Evolving interpretations of educational equity and students with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 76(3), 265-278. McNaughton, S. (1981). Low progress readers and teacher instructional behavior during oral reading: The risk of maintaining instructional dependence. The Exceptional Child, 28,
  • 80. 167-176. Mills, K. A. (2009). Floating on a sea of talk: Reading comprehension through speaking and listening. The Reading Teacher, 63(4), 325-329. Miracle, S. A., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Grisham- Brown, J. (2001). Peer-versus teacher- delivered instruction: Effects on acquisition and maintenance. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 36, 373- 385. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Nigam, R., Schlosser, R. W., & Lloyd, L. L. (2006). Concomitant use of the matrix strategy and the mand-model procedure in teaching graphic symbol combinations. Augmentative and
  • 81. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 42 Alternative Communication, 22(3), 160-177. Osborn, J., Freeman, A., Burley, M., Wilson, R., Jones, E., & Rychener, S. (2007). Effect of tutoring on reading achievement for students with cognitive disabilities, specific learning disabilities, and students receiving Title I services. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 42, 467-474. Raphael, T. E., & Au, K. H. (2005). QAR: Enhancing comprehension and test taking across grades and content areas. The Reading Teacher, 59, 206-221. Reyna, V. F. (2004). Why scientific research? The importance of evidence in changing educational practices. In P. McCardle & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence in reading research (pp. 47-58). Baltimore: Brookes. Rohena, E. I., Jitendra, A. K., & Browder, D. M. (2002). Comparison of the effects of Spanish and English constant time delay instruction on sight word reading by Hispanic learners
  • 82. with mental retardation. Journal of Special Education, 36, 169- 184. Ryndak, D. L., Morrison, A. P., & Sommerstein, L. (1999). Literacy before and after inclusion in general education settings: A case study. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 5-22. Singh, J., & Singh, N. (1985a). Comparison of word-supply and word-analysis error correction procedures on oral reading by mentally retarded children. American Journal on Mental Deficiency, 90(1), 64-70. Singh, N. N. (1990). Effects of two error-correction procedures on oral reading errors. Behavior Modification, 14(2), 188-199. Singh, N. N., & Singh, J. (1984). Antecedent control of oral reading errors and self-corrections by mentally retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 111-119. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 43 Singh, N. N., & Singh, J. (1985b). Reading acquisition and
  • 83. remediation in the mentally retarded. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 14, 165-199. Singh, N., & Singh, J. (1986). A behavioural remediation program for oral reading: effects on errors and comprehension. Educational Psychology, 6(2), 105- 114. Singh, N. N., Singh, J., & Winton, A. S. W. (1984). Positive practice overcorrection of oral reading errors. Behavior Modification, 8(1), 23-37. Singleton, D. K., Schuster, J. W., Morse, T. E., & Collins, B. C. (1999). A comparison of antecedent prompt and test and simultaneous prompting procedures in teaching grocery words to adolescents with mental retardation. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 34, 182-199. Slavin, R. E. (2008). What works? Issues in synthesizing educational program evaluations. Educational Researcher, 37(1), 5-14. Smith, P. (2007). Have we made any progress? Including students with intellectual disabilities in regular classrooms. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
  • 84. 45(5), 297-309. Snow, C. E. (2002). Reading for understanding: toward a research and development program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Stahl, K. A., & Bravo, M. A. (2010). Contemporary classroom vocabulary assessment for content areas. The Reading Teacher, 63(7), 566-578. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349-367. Striefel, S., Wetherby, B., & Karlan, G. (1978). Developing generalized instruction-following behavior in severely retarded people. In C. E. Meyers (Ed.), Quality of life in severely and profoundly mentally retarded people: Research foundations for improvement. LITERACY INSTRUCTION ISSUES AND CONCERNS 44 Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation. Van der Bijl, C., Alant, E., & Lloyd, L. (2006). A comparison of two strategies of sight word
  • 85. instruction in children with mental disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 43-55. Whalon, K. J., Otaiba, S. A., & Delano, M. E. (2009). Evidence- based reading instruction for individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disorders, 24, 3-16. Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjective measurement or how applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 203-214. Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock language proficiency battery–Revised. Chicago: Riverside. Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. Yopp, H. K., & Yopp, R. H. (2000). Supporting phonemic awareness development in the classroom. The Reading Teacher, 54(2), 130-143. Zascavage, V. T., & Keefe, C. H. (2004). Students with severe speech and physical impairments: Opportunity barriers to literacy. Focus on Autism and Other
  • 86. Developmental Disorders, 19, 223-234. 1. A retailer hasACCOUNTS receivable of $70000, total current liabilities of 440000, and $50000 in case. What is the quick ratio? 1.4 1.5 2.0 3.0 5 points QUESTION 2 Assume that the retailer in problem 1 has totalINVENTORY on hand of $120,000. What is the current ratio? 0.65 3.00
  • 87. 4.60 6.00 5 points QUESTION 3 A clothing store hasACCOUNTS receivable totaling $50,000,ACCOUNTS payable totaling $45,000, and net sales of $50,000. What is the collection period? 50 183 227 365 5 points QUESTION 4 Assume that the retailer in problem 3 has accounts receivable of $15,000 and net sales of $30,000. What is the new collection period? 91 183
  • 88. 274 335 5 points QUESTION 5 A firm has total assets of $500,000, total liabilities of $350,000, and annual net sales of $1,000,000. What is the assets-to-net- sales ratio? 5% 7% 30% 50% 5 points Click Save and Submit to save and