SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2: 209-217
Forage quality and patch choice by wapiti
{Cervus elaphus)
John F. Wilmshurst,1
John M. Fryxell," and Robert J. Hudsonb
'Department of Zoology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario NIG 2W1, Canada, and
b
Department of Animal Science, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta T6G 2P5, Canada
Recent models suggest that herbivores might optimize energy gain by selecting patches of intermediate vegetation biomass. We
tested this hypothesis in wapiti (Cervus elaphus) by estimating daily rates of energy gain in relation to grass biomass and by
measuring patch choice in experimental pastures in which grass biomass was manipulated by mowing. The digestible energy
content of grasses declined with increasing biomass due to maturational changes in fiber and lignin content. Daily rates of dry
matter intake by wapiti increased with grass biomass at a decelerating rate, implying a Type II functional response. Linking
these values to published ad libitum energy intake and energy expenditure parameters, Fryxell's (1991) model predicted that
the daily rate of energy gain should be highest when wapiti feed in grasslands with 1000-1100 kg/ha. In trials in which grass
biomass within a mosaic of patches was manipulated experimentally between 800-2900 kg/ha, wapiti preferred patches of 1200
kg/ha, close to the value predicted by the energy gain model. Our results suggest that the rate of energy gain by wapiti is
constrained by both grass biomass and grass fiber content, the latter of which varies inversely with grass biomass. Behavioral
preference for grass patches of intermediate biomass and fiber content could help explain patterns of aggregation and seasonal
migration reported previously for wapiti. Key words: Cervidae, Cervus elaphus, digestible energy, forage maturation, functional
response, grasses, herbivory, patch choice, wapiti. [Behav Ecot 6:209-217 (1995)]
Optimal foraging theory assumes that foraging decisions
by herbivores should be strongly influenced by physi-
ological and environmental constraints on rates of nutrient
uptake. Two such constraints commonly invoked for verte-
brate grazers are the effect of plant density on the short-term
rate of food intake (availability constraint) and the effect of
digestive capacity on the long-term rate of energy assimilation
(processing constraint) (Belovsky, 1978, 1986; Fryxell, 1991;
Owen-Smith and Novellie, 1982).
The short-term rate of food intake (i.e., the functional re-
sponse) of herbivores should be related positively to plant
size, bite size, and plant density (Gross et al., 199Sa,b; Shipley
et al., 1994; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992; Spalinger and
Hobbs, 1992). Accordingly, several studies have shown that
herbivore functional responses are often positive but decel-
erating functions of plant density or biomass (Fryxell et al.,
1994; Fryxell and Doucet, 1993; Gross et al., 1993a,b; Lund-
berg, 1988; Short, 1985; Wickstrom et al., 1984).
The digestive capacity of herbivores is primarily governed
by the interaction of energy and fiber in their diet (Broom
and Arnold, 1986). The digestibility of energy in grasses for
grazing ruminants is inversely related to cell wall (fiber) con-
tent of the forage (Mould and Robbins, 1982; Osbourn, 1989;
Van Soest, 1982). As grasses mature the proportion of cell wall
in their tissues increases, reducing digestibility (Laycock and
Price, 1970; Oelberg, 1956; Osbourn, 1989). The processing
time (digestion and passage) in the gut often increases as
plants mature (Blaxter et al., 1961; White, 1983). This suggests
that both digestibility and the rate of turnover of ingesta
should be negatively related to plant biomass, if biomass is
positively associated with plant maturation stage.
This inverse correlation between availability and processing
constraints creates a potential trade-off for grazing herbivores
(Demment and Van Soest, 1985; Fryxell, 1991; Hobbs and
Swift, 1988; McNaughton, 1984; Owen-Smith and Novellie,
Received 23 December 1993; revised 16 June 1994; accepted 24
June 1994.
1045-2249/95/S5.00 © 1995 International Society for Behavioral Ecology
1982). At low biomass, the processing rate is high but the
short-term rate of intake is low, whereas at high pasture bio-
mass, the processing rate is low but the short-term rate of
intake is high. The net rate of energy intake for grazing her-
bivores should be maximized accordingly on patches of inter-
mediate plant biomass (the forage maturation hypothesis).
Moreover, natural selection should favor grazers that choose
vegetation patches of intermediate biomass, yielding the high-
est rate of daily energy gain.
We tested the forage maturation hypothesis using wapiti
(Cervus elaphus L.) feeding in a mosaic of grass patches that
were manipulated experimentally. Wapiti are large, gregarious
ruminants of western North America. Although wapiti are
known to feed on a wide variety of plants, they are primarily
grazers (Baker and Hobbs, 1987; Boyce, 1989; Hobbs et al.,
1981; Nelson and Leege, 1982). To predict the processing
constraint, we measured changes in fiber content in relation
to grass biomass. To predict the availability constraint, we mea-
sured the functional response by wapiti to changes in grass
biomass. We used Fryxell's (1991) model and the data on en-
ergy constraints of foraging wapiti to predict daily rates of
energy gain in relation to grass biomass. We then tested
whether wapiti preferred patches of intermediate biomass, as
predicted in general by the forage maturation hypothesis.
METHODS
The model
In this section, we summarize the model used to predict the
net rate of energy intake by wapiti and report parameter val-
ues obtained from the literature. Model structure is described
in greater detail in Fryxell (1991). We defined the availability
constraint (/,, measured in kj/day) as the maximum amount
of digestible energy that an herbivore could consume given
an unlimited gut capacity. We calculated this constraint as the
product of the digestible energy content of forage (Q, in kj/
kg), the maximum time available for foraging per day (T, in
h/day) and the short-term rate of dry matter intake (C, in
kg/h).
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
210 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2
Recent theoretical work (Spalinger and Hobbs, 1992) sup-
ported by a growing body of experimental studies (Gross et
al., 199Sa,b; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992; Spalinger et al.,
1988) suggests that functional responses of herbivores should
vary according to plant spacing, plant apparency, and the size
and spatial distribution of bite-sized portions among individ-
ual stems. Consequently, a wide variety of functional response
forms are theoretically plausible, depending on the ecological
circumstances. Wapiti usually feed on a dense sward of closely
spaced grass stems, with each bite representing a substantial
fraction of an individual rameL Under these conditions, the
Michaelis-Menton formula should well approximate the func-
tional response (Spalinger and Hobbs, 1992), and this has
been corroborated in a previous field study (Wickstrom et al.,
1984). We therefore calculated the availability constraint as
QTaV
1
b + V
(1)
where a is the maximum rate of forage consumption per her-
bivore (in kg/h), Vis forage biomass (in kg/ha), and b is the
biomass at which the intake rate is Vi maximum (in kg/ha).
The maximum daily foraging time for wapiti has been re-
ported by both Gates and Hudson (1983) and Wairimu and
Hudson (1993) as 13 h, which we used to estimate 7! In our
results, we show how Q varies with gravs biomass.
We defined the digestive constraint (/,, in kj/day) as the
maximum amount of energy that a herbivore could digest per
day given ad libitum consumption. Previous research suggests
that ad libitum consumption by wapiti is the following positive
function of digestible energy content of the forage (Mould
and Robbins, 1982):
I? = 20.3Q - 45.5. (2)
We estimated energetic expenditure (X) as the sum of rest-
ing metabolic expenditure plus the cost of eating and loco-
motion while foraging
X = 24w + F(x + yW) (3)
where w is the resting metabolic expenditure (in kj/h), Fix
the total time spent foraging per day (in h/day), xis the cost
of standing plus eating (in kj/h), and jrV* is the cost of loco-
motion (in kj/h).
The resting metabolic expenditure for a 170-kg wapiti (the
mass of our experimental animals) was reported by Gates and
Hudson (1978) as 733 kj/h. Gates and Hudson (1978) also
give a value of 925 kj/h for the cost of resting plus standing,
which we added to the cost of eating reported by Wickstrom
et al. (1984) of 241 kj/h, yielding a total value of x = 1166
kj/h. Values of y and z for wapiti were determined from Wick-
strom et al. (1984) as 6766 kj/h and -0.6 respectively.
Net daily energy intake was estimated as / - X, where / is
the minimum of /, and Z, (note that our definition of net
energy differs from definitions found elsewhere in the physi-
ological literature; e.g., Robbins, 1983).
Study area and animals
We conducted research at the Ministik Reid Station, 45 km
southeast of Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, from May to August
1991. The experiments took place on a 2-ha, mixed-species
pasture composed of Kentucky bluegrass (Pon pralmsis L.),
smooth brome (Bromus inXrrmus Leysxer), clover (Trifotium
sp.), dandelion (Taraxacum ojficinale Weber), and Canada
thistle (drsiiim arvrnsel^).
In May, the pasture was stocked with four yearling females.
Animals had access to ad libitum water and a mineral lick, but
were not supplementalh/ fed during the study. Each animal
W.LS fitted with a numbered ear tag and colored collar to en-
able identification from a distance. Daily human activity on
the pasture ensured that the wapiti were acclimated to the
presence of an observer. We used five additional yearlings in
bite mass trials. The animals were weighed monthly on an
electronic balance (±1 kg).
Forage intake
Short-term rates of grazing intake can be calculated as the
product of bile size and cropping rate (Hudson and Watkins,
1986; Wickstrom et al., 1984). To determine bite size, individ-
ual wapiti were each presented with a pre-weighed sod mea-
suring 25 cm X 76 cm. These sods were simply samples of the
typical grass pasture cut into rectangular blocks. We cut each
sod in half and trimmed it until it weighed less than 4 kg (the
capacity of the balance), and dien placed each sod half in a
plastic container and weighed them on an electronic balance
(±0.01 g). One container (experimental) was staked to the
ground in a 15 m X 8 m feeding pen devoid of vegetation,
whereas the control container was placed outside the pen.
The number of bites taken from the experimental sod by a
captive yearling wapiti was recorded for a maximum of 15 min
(to limit desiccation) or maximum of 15 bites (to limit deple-
tion), whichever occurred first- The experimental and control
sods were then re-weighed. Bite mass (j, measured in g/bite)
was calculated as
(4)
where p, is the pre-grazing mass of the experimental sod (in
g), fa is the post-grazing mass of the experimental sod (in g),
r is the proportion of evaporative loss determined from the
control sod, g is the number of bites taken, and h is the pro-
portion of dry matter in the fresh vegetation.
Cropping rates of wapiti were measured during a foraging
trial conducted with patches of widely varying grass biomass
(described in detail below). From an elevated platform, we
continuously observed a focal animal foraging on a mosaic of
8 X 10 m patches of known biomass. Each bite, defined as a
single grasping and tearing of the grass, was entered into an
event recorder that also recorded time during each cropping
sequence. We defined a cropping sequence as beginning
when the focal animal put its head down to graze and ending
when it lifted its head. Uninterrupted cropping rate was cal-
culated as the number of bites taken during the cropping
sequence divided by time. As wapiti typically spent a small
portion of foraging bouts engaged in activities other than
cropping plants, we corrected for this bias by multiplying by
the proportion of time that the wapiti actually spent in crop-
ping sequences during foraging bouts.
The short-term rate of dry matter intake (kg/h) was cal-
culated as the product of bite mass (g/bite) multiplied by the
cropping rate (bites/min) multiplied by 60 min/h and 0.001
kg/g, to keep the measurement units consistent. Parameters
a and b from the Michaelis-Menton formula (Equation 2)
were estimated by fitting to intake data using a standard least-
squares algorithm (Wickstrom et al., 1984).
Forage growth and quality
To obtain undisturbed forage samples for the estimation of
digestible energy content in relation to grass biomass, we
erected 10 wire mesh grazing exclosures (basal area 1.5 m1
,
2-m high) at random locations on the pasture. Each exrlosure
was sampled biweekly from 10 May to 16 August 1991.
Samples were taken by securing a 0.125 m5
wire hoop to
the ground beneath the exclosure and clipping all vegetation
rooted within the hoop at ground level. A previously unsuut-
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 211
pled pan of the exclosurc was sampled each time. Vegetation
samples were stored in a cool room at approximately 2°C and
frozen within 3 days of collection. They were dried at 60°C
for 48 h for analysis and weighed (±0.01 g). Samples were
then ground through a 1-mm diameter screen in a Wiley mill
and stored at room temperature. A subsample from each sam-
ple was analyzed for dry matter content (AOAC, 1980).
Hence, biomass values are reported as kg dry matter/ha (kg-
DM/ha).
For ruminants, digestible energy content of forage depends
on fiber content. The greater die concentration of fiber, par-
ticularly lignin, the less digestible the forage (Van Soest,
1982). We analyzed forage samples for neutral detergent fiber
(NDF), acid detergent fiber (ADF), permanganate lignin con-
tent (Van Soest and Robertson, 1980; Van Soest and Wine,
1967), and Kjeldahl nitrogen. Digestibility was calculated us-
ing Mould and Robbins' (1982) regression equations for wap-
iti. The percent digestible dry matter (DDM) of each sample
is the sum of the digestible percentage of the neutral deter-
gent soluble (NDS) fraction and the digestible percentage of
the NDF fraction. Following Mould and Robbins (1982), we
calculated DDM as the sum of
1.00
and
Digestible NDF(%) = %NDF(176.92 - 40.5(ln x)) (6)
where x is the percent of the ADF diat is lignin and cutin.
Percent digestible energy (DE) was subsequently calculated
from Mould and Robbins (1982) according to
DE = -0.61 + 0.98(DDM). (7)
The relationship between DE and sample biomass was dien
determined using least squares regression. The gross energy
content of each sample was determined by bomb calorimetry
(Parr, 1977).
Patch use experiment
To test whedier wapiti selectively exploit patches of interme-
diate biomass, we conducted feeding trials on two areas of the
2-ha pasture in which a mosaic of patches at different growth
stages had been created by mowing. Animals were allowed to
move freely among the patches, and preferences were assessed
by recording the amount of time spent on each patch (Roy-
ama, 1970; Simons and Alcock, 1971; Smith and Dawkins,
1971).
On the two most level areas of die pasture, two 30 m x 40
m paddocks were enclosed with 2-m-high deer fence. Within
each paddock, 15 patches of 8 X 10 m were surveyed and
marked with a stake at each corner. We arranged patches in
three blocks of five along a slight elevational gradient in die
pasture to control statistically (Dutilleul, 1993) for possible
confounding effects due to spatial variation in soil moisture
or shading. Al 2-week intervals, one patch in each block was
randomly selected and mowed 50 mm above the ground widi
a commercial mower. When all patches had been mowed
once, the paddock was left to regrow for 2 weeks before ani-
mals were admitted. This mowing regime produced a mosaic
of grass patches widi 2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 weeks regrowth.
The patch use experiment was initiated by removing die
30-m fence from die south end of the paddock and allowing
four wapiti to enter. From an elevated platform behind the
north end of die paddock, an observer recorded time allo-
cation and cropping rates widiin patches. Observations were
made from first to last light for diree consecutive days or until
all four animals spent one bout foraging outside die paddock.
H
O.00
Digestible NDS(%) = -21.88 + 1.11 %NDS (5) £
0
UJ
IQ.
B
° 2000 4000
GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha)
Figure 1
Wapiti bite size (a) and cropping rate (b) in relation to grass
biomasj. See text for regression equations.
At night, die animals were removed, and the paddock was
closed.
Three forage samples were taken from each patch before
and after the trial. Samples were taken, as before, by securing
a 0.125 m' wire hoop to die ground and clipping die vege-
tation rooted widiin. Samples were dried and weighed as de-
scribed earlier and analyzed to estimate forage quality in each
patch.
Where appropriate to meet the criteria for statistical anal-
yses, we have log- or arcsine-transformed variables.
RESULTS
Model parameter estimates
We estimated bite mass using sods ranging in biomass between
551-3128 kg/ha. Bite mass (in g/bite) increased linearly widi
grass biomass (Figure la; y = 0.087 + 0.0014* t = 2.379, p
= .03, r1
= .303, n = 15). Cropping rates were determined
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
212 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2
2000 4000
GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha)
Figure 2
The daily rate of dry matier intake by wapiti in relation to grass
biomass. See text for regression equation.
on forage patches ranging from 653-3557 kg/ha. Wapiti spent
10% (±1.0) of each foraging bout in activities other than
cropping. Therefore, bite rates were multiplied by 0.90 to
eliminate this bias. Cropping rates were a negative curvilinear
function of grass biomass (Figure lb; y = 438,357/(x + 7706),
r1
= .315, n = 18), consistent with previous grazing studies
(Gross et al., 1993a; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992). Dry matter
intake values, calculated from the product of bite mass and
cropping rates, suggested a Type II functional response, as
postulated (Figure 2). Parameters a and b for Equation 1 were
estimated as 1.87 kg/h and 1604 kg/ha, respectively.
Grass biomass (in kg/ha) in the ungrazed exclosures in-
creased over time (in days since 1 January 1991) at a decel-
erating rate (y = 15.467*/(196.0 + x), r1
= .636, n = 70). To
distinguish between possible effects of grass biomass (matu-
ration) and time on forage quality, we conducted multiple
regression analyses for each of percentage NDF, ADF, lignin,
and protein using sampled biomass and date as independent
variables, in each case with 70 forage samples. Percentage
NDF increased with both grass biomass (Figure 3a; y = 1.64
+ 7.75 ln(x), r* = .40, t = 6.65, p < .001) and date (y =
-123.8 + 35.2 ln(z), r3
= .34, I = 5.88, p < .001), but in a
multiple regression analysis only the coefficient for grass bio-
mass was significant (4, = 2.91, p = .05, 4 = 1.42, p = .16).
Percentage ADF increased with both grass biomass (Figure 3b;
y = -14.3 + 6.7 ln(x), r1
= .59, t = 10.0, p < .001) and date
(Figure 4a; y = -132.3 + 32.3 ln(z), r* = .56, t = 9.3, p <
.001), with both coefficients significant in the multiple re-
gression analysis (y = -77.7 + 4.14 ln(x) + 15.9 ln(z), T2
=
.64, L, = 3.9, p < .001, t, = 3.04, p < .001). Percentage lignin
also increased with both grass biomass (y = -3.75 + 1.25
ln(x), r1
= .26, t = 4.92, p < .001) and date (Figure 4b; y =
-39.0 + 8.55 n(z), r1
= .65, 4 = 10.9, p < .001), but in a
multiple regression analysis only the coefficient for date was
significant (t, = 0.8, p = .43, 4 = 5.8, p < .001). Hence, based
on variables with the strongest explanatory power, we used
univariate estimates of percentage NDF and lignin and the
multivariate relationship of percentage ADF to estimate per-
centage DE by the equation y = 210.9 - 0.67 ln(x) - 27.9
ln(z). As our patch use trials were conducted over the same
100 1000 10000
GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha)
Figure 3
Percentage neutral detergent fiber (a) and acid detergent fiber (b)
from grazing exclosure samples in relation to grass biomass. See
text for regression equations.
week, the time effects are simply constants in the model of
energy gain, but they do improve the precision of our digest-
ible energy estimates.
Percentage protein in the undisturbed samples decreased
with both grass biomass (y = 36.3 — 6.3 ln(x), r1
= .50, t =
6.30, p < .001) and date (y = 101.7 - 17.3 ln(z), r1
= .59, /
= 7.52, p < .001). Multiple regression indicated that only the
effect of date was significant when date and grass biomass
were considered together (t, = 1.47, p = .15, 4 = 3.30, p =
.002).
Linking of the foraging parameters and maturational vari-
ation in DE content with estimates of energy expenditure in
the model, we calculated daily rates of net energy intake in
relation to grass biomass. Our model predicted that daily en-
ergy gains should rise steeply at low forage biomass, peak at
roughly 1000-1100 kg/ha, then slope downward with further
increases in grass biomass (Figure 5). At the optimal grass
biomass, the maximum daily intake of DE should be 53 kj/
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
WUmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 21S
10.0
in ••
100 150 200 250
JULIAN DATE
Figure 4
Percentage acid detergent fiber (a) and lignin (b) from grazing
exdosure samples in relation to Julian date. See text for regression
equation*.
kg body mass, or 9010 kj/day for the 170 kg animals used in
our study.
Patch use experiment
In the foraging experiment, grass biomass within patches
spanned the predicted optimum, ranging between 796 kg/ha
in the patches with 2 weeks growth and 2867 kg/ha in the
patches with 10 weeks growth (Figure 6). The patches with 4
weeks growth (mean = 1212 kg/ha) were closest to optimal,
according to the model.
There was no significant difference in DE content between
the experimental patches and the ungrazed exclosures (AN-
COVA: /",l n = 1.13, p = .29), indicating that model param-
eters based on the exclosures should be appropriate to the
experimental patches. The total time spent by the animals on
each paddock did not differ (ANOVA: FtM = 2.553, p = .132),
so the data for each paddock were pooled for subsequent
analysis.
2000 4000
GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha)
Figure 5
Net energy intake predicted for wapiti in relation to gran biomass.
The optimal value is marked by an asterisk.
We used two types of statistical procedures to test whether
wapiti exploited grass patches of intermediate biomass: (1)
polynomial regression and (2) ANOVA with Tukey post-hoc
comparisons of daily foraging time and percentage foraging
time per day in relation to the number of weeks of uninter-
rupted grass growth (2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 weeks) within patches.
In die case of polynomial regression, the critical test is wheth-
er the coefficient of a second order term is significantly less
than 0, implying that animals use patches of intermediate
growth more heavily than other patches. More specifically, die
patches widi 4 weeks growth would be predicted to yield the
4000
2 A 6 8 10 12
PATCH GROWTH (weeks)
Figure 6
Grass biomass within patches in relation to the number of weeks of
grass growth within patches. Vertical bars are 1 SE-
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
214 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2
highest rate of energy gain and therefore be preferred relative
to all other patches. We took a conservative approach in an-
alyzing patch use data to reduce the possibility of spurious
inference due to pseudoreplication (Aebischer et al., 1994).
Accordingly, we lumped data for replicates of the same patch
growth treatment. This left us with 59 degrees of freedom,
corresponding to 4 animals, 3 days of observation, and 5 cat-
egories of patches. We used conventional parametric tests be-
cause there was Iitde evidence of non-normality in die source
data or in the residuals. There was Iitde reason to suspect that
use of one patch type precluded use of other patches (Ae-
bischer et al., 1994; Johnson, 1980) because none of the study
animals foraged for more than 4.2 hours out of die maximum
12 hours possible during each observation period and diree
replicate patches of similar biomass were always available to
study animals.
Polynomial regression indicated that daily foraging time (in
s) was highest in patches of intermediate growth (in weeks)
(Figure 7a;^= -2176 + 1581x - 119**, r* = .377, ^ = 5.601,
p < .001, L^ = -5.155, p < .001). Inclusion of trial day im-
proved slightly die degree of fit (r* = .439, / = -2.496, p =
.02). Similarly, significant variation occurred among individ-
uals with respect to die quadratic relationship between daily
foraging time and patch growth stage (r1
— 0.473, I = 1.886,
p = .005).
We normalized daily foraging time data by dividing time per
patch by the total time summed over all patches. We dien
arcsine-transformed these normalized patch use data and re-
gressed die transformed values against weeks of patch growdi.
As before, percentage foraging time per patch was better pre-
dicted by a quadratic dian a linear model (Figure 7b; y = 14.4
+ 14.2JC- 1.05^,7" = .559,1^ = 7.885, p<. 001, <*, = -7.118,
p < .001), implying diat patches of intermediate growdi were
used most heavily.
Results of die regression tests were corroborated by ANO-
VA_ Daily foraging time varied significandy amongst die five
patch categories (FAM = 13.254, p < .001). When multifactor
ANOVA models were examined, dairy foraging time was also
affected by trial day (F = 5.385, p < .01) and animal (F =
8.658, p < .001). The arcsine-transformed values for percent-
age foraging time also varied amongst patches (F^M = 42.871,
p < .001). In die latter case, no direct effect of eidier trial
day or animal (p > .10) was detectable statistically, but an
interaction effect did imply significant variation among ani-
mals widi respect to die relationship between patch growth
stage and percentage patch use by wapiti (F = 7.352, p <
.001).
Wapiti spent 1.3% of dieir foraging time in patches widi 2
weeks growdi, well below die value of 20% expected from a
random model. Wapiti spent over 30% of dieir time in patch-
es widi 4 weeks growdi and lesser amounts of time in patches
widi longer growdi, as predicted by die optimal energy gain
model. Tukey's test for multiple post-hoc comparisons (a <
0.05) confirmed diat patches widi 2 weeks of growth were
used significandy less dian die others, patches widi 4 weeks
of growdi were used significandy more dian die odiers, and
patches with 6, 8, or 10 weeks growdi were indistinguishable
statistically.
DISCUSSION
Three conditions must be met for die forage maturation hy-
podiesis to apply to patch selection by grazing herbivores: (1)
short-term rates of grazing intake must increase widi plant
biomass, (2) ad libitum limits on intake must be linked to
indices of forage quality such as digestible energy or protein
content, and (3) digestible energy or protein content must
decline widi increasing plant biomass widiin patches. We have
4000
2 4 6 8 10 12
PATCH GROWTH (weeks)
Figure 7
Daily foraging time (a) and percentage foraging time per day (b) in
relation to the number of weeks of grass growth within patches.
Vertical bars ire 1 SE. Expected values under the null hypothesis
are indicated by the dotted line.
shown diat diese conditions exist for wapiti and have dem-
onstrated experimentally diat wapiti preferentially selected
patches of intermediate abundance.
Patch use patterns during die trial indicated diat wapiti pre-
ferred grass patches widi 1200 kg/ha, close to die model pre-
diction. This result is also consistent widi results recendy re-
ported by Langvatn and Hanley (1993) for red deer (Cervus
daphus). They conclude, however diat patch selectivity by red
deer optimized die rate of protein intake radier than die rate
of energy gain, as we suggest. Indeed, protein levels in our
pastures did decrease over die growing season at die same
time that digestible energy was declining. Hence, digestible
energy and protein content covaried strongly in both our
study and diat of Langvatn and Hanley (1993). We show else-
where (Wilmshurst JF and Fryxell JM, unpublished) diat die
difference between our conclusions and diose of Langvatn
and Hanley Ls largely a matter of die time period of foraging
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 215
under consideration. By incorporating a digestive constraint
in our model, we consider the implications of gut fill over an
entire foraging bout on net energy intake. Langvatn and Han-
ley (1993) predicted intake using only the availability con-
straint and model instantaneous intake. Hence, patterns of
forage selection that maximize long-term rates of energy in-
take also maximize short-term rates of protein intake, at least
in the two systems that have been studied thus far. To resolve
this issue, studies are needed from systems with weak covari-
ation, if this is indeed possible, between protein content and
energy digestibility.
Variation in forage quality is associated with a number of
factors. First and foremost, physiological processes cause the
accumulation of fiber as individual ramets grow and mature
(Kamstra, 1973; Wake, 1963). Other factors, such as temper-
ature and humidity (Wilson et al., 1976), soil moisture (Cook
et al., 1961), and light intensity (VanDyne and Heady, 1965),
may result in temporal variation in forage quality that are not
associated with increasing biomass per se (Van Soest et al.,
1978). These factors played a relatively large role in forage
quality variation measured here, particularly with respect to
lignin, which continued to accumulate even after growth had
slowed late in the growing season. Like maturational variation
in forage quality, these additional sources represent poten-
tially important variation in forage quality for herbivores in
their natural ranges.
Strictly speaking, our optimality model suggests that wapiti
should have spent all of their time in the most profitable
patches, whereas they spent considerable amounts of time for-
aging in other patches. A common feature of most foraging
experiments, such partial preferences could be due to (1) in-
dividual differences in optima among animals, (2) discrimi-
nation errors in selecting optimal patches, or (3) sampling to
update information (Krebs and McQeery, 1984). Any or all
of these factors could contribute to an animal spending time
in less profitable patches.
We would caution against the indiscriminant use of the op-
timality model for ungulates of sizes differing from that of the
yearling wapiti examined here. Both bite size and cropping
rate parameters vary allometrically among grazers (Gross et
al., 1993b; Shipley et al., 1994; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992),
which could change the constraint curves markedly. More-
over, energetic value and processing time can vary consider-
ably among various food items available to herbivores (Baker
and Hobbs, 1987; Belovsky, 1978, 1986; Fryxell et al., 1994;
Spalinger et al., 1986), which could influence patterns of
patch use strongly.
The pattern of patch use exhibited by wapiti could help
explain seasonal movement patterns of wapiti. In montane
habitat, wapiti migrate along an altitudinal gradient biannu-
ally, moving to higher altitudes in the spring and to lower
altitudes in the fall (Boyce, 1989). As has been suggested for
bighorn sheep (Ouis canadensis) (Festa-Bianchet, 1988; He-
bert, 1973) and red deer (Qrruus daphus L.) (Albon and La-
ngvatn, 1992), wapiti may time their migration to exploit high
quality emergent forage as the snow cover melts in the spring.
The forage selection patterns we observed suggest that if mi-
gration is timed to maximize net energy intake, then wapiti
should lag several weeks (depending on vegetation growth
rates) behind the melt Several studies report that wapiti move
to higher elevations a short time after snow melt in the spring
(Brazda, 1953; Murie, 1951; Ward et al., 1973). Brazda (1953:
Table 3) suggested that wapiti do not occupy the summer
range until the forage has reached a "succulent . .. develop-
ing" stage. However, Brazda did not measure seasonal
changes in plant nutritional quality.
What evidence is there that the three conditions necessary
for the application of the forage maturation hypothesis found
to hold here may also exist in other systems? Gross et al.
(1993b) have recendy demonstrated that functional responses
of 12 species of herbivores ranging in body size from 0.05 to
548 kg increase monotonically. Previous studies have shown
similar trends with other taxa (Fryxell et al., 1994; Fryxell and
Doucet, 1993; Lundberg, 1988; Renecker and Hudson, 1986;
Short, 1985), suggesting that Type II functional responses are
common among grazing herbivores. Variation in DE intake
with respect to forage DE content has not been widely studied.
However, data for other ungulates have shown this relation-
ship to be positive and decelerating (Ammann et al., 1973;
Clancy et al., 1976). The mechanism behind this relationship
should apply to most ruminants and possibly even hind-gut
fermenters (Duncan et al., 1990). Finally, maturational de-
clines in forage quality have been extensively documented,
varying in degree among forage species (Armstrong et al.,
1986; Fonnesbeck et al., 1981; Lewis et al., 1975) and among
climatic zones (Barton et al., 1976; Kamstra, 1973). Hence,
conditions favoring selection for patches of intermediate
plant abundance could apply to many large grazing herbi-
vores.
The trade-off between forage quality and abundance has
interesting implications for the application of optimal patch
use models to herbivores (Charnov, 1976; Stephens and
Krebs, 1986). One might predict from such models diat vari-
ation in resource abundance among patches should decline
over time as patches of high resource abundance become de-
pleted, provided that rates of resource renewal are minor rel-
ative to rates of consumption. Unlike classic patch choice
models, our work suggests that herbivores should concentrate
in patches of intermediate resource abundance. Accordingly,
patches of high resource abundance should tend to be ig-
nored and accumulate biomass, if by chance they have es-
caped attack at earlier stages of growth, whereas patches of
lower abundance should experience periodic depletion and
regrowth. Hence, maturational effects for herbivores should
lead to increased spatial variation in resource abundance over
time, contrary to classic patch use models.
A number of factors are thought to contribute to aggregat-
ed spatial distributions of large herbivores, including predator
avoidance (Estes, 1974;Jarman, 1974) and exploitation of spa-
tially concentrated resources (Kreulen, 1975; McNaughton,
1988, 1990; Sinclair, 1977; Western, 1975). Hence, habitat se-
lection for patches of intermediate resource abundance could
lead to aggregation provided that resources are patchy in
space or time (Fryxell and Sinclair, 1988; Gordon, 1988; Mad-
dock, 1979). Indeed, energy-maximizing decisions by herbi-
vores themselves could lead to aggregations that maintain
patches at a preferred biomass level (Fryxell, 1991; Hobbs and
Swift, 1988; McNaughton, 1984). Hence, understanding the
adaptive basis of patch selection (Gordon and Illius, 1989),
such as that demonstrated in our study, may lead to improved
understanding of migratory and grouping behavior by wapiti
or other generalist grazers.
Understanding patterns of forage selection by herbivores in
relation to energetic constraints is an important step to pre-
dicting habitat use and the resulting long-term dynamics of
plant-herbivore communities (Hobbs and Swift, 1985; Illius et
al., 1992). Determination of these constraints should better
enable us to model herbivore population dynamics effectively
and also gain insight into the processes that ultimately limit
and regulate population growth.
We would like to thank C Olsen, S. Poluch, and C Doucet for tech-
nical assistance. We also thank S. Albon, J. Atkinson, N. T. Hobbs, D.
Kramer, E. Izm D. Lavigne, R. Walton, and an anonymous reviewer
for comments on an earlier version of this article. Research was sup-
ported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
216 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2
(NSERC) operating grant to J.M.F., Agricultural Research Council of
Alberta grant to R.J.H., and a Sigma Xi research grant and Ontario
Graduate and NSERC scholarships to J.F.W.
REFERENCES
Aebischer NJ, Robertson PA, Kenward RE, 1994. Compositional anal-
ysis of habitat use from animal radio-tracking data. Ecology 74:
1315-1323.
Albon SD, Langvatn R, 1992. Plant phenology and the benefits of
migration in a temperate ungulate. Oikos 65302-513.
Ammann AP, Cowan RL, Mothershead CL, Baumgardt BR, 1973. Dry
matter and energy intake in relation to digestibility in white-tailed
deer.J Wildl Manage 37:195-201.
Armstrong RH, Common TC, Smith HK, 1986. The voluntary intake
and in vivo digestibility of herbage harvested from indigenous hill
plant communities. Grass Forage Sci 41:53-60.
Association of Official Analytical Chemists (AOAC), 1980. Official
methods of analysis of the Association of Official Analytical Chem-
isti (Horwitz W, ed). Washington, D.C: Association of Official An-
alytical Chemists.
Baker DL, Hobba NT, 1987. Strategies of digestion: digestive efficiency
and retention time of forage diets in montane ungulates. Can J
Zool 65:1978-1984.
Barton FE, Amos HE, Burdkk D, Wilson RL, 1976. Relationship of
chemical analysis to in vitro digestibility for selected tropical and
temperate grasses. J Anim Sci 43:505-513.
Belovsky GE, 1978. Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: the
moose. Theor Popul Biol 14:103-134.
Belovsky GE, 1986. Optimal foraging and community structure: im-
plications for a guild of generalist grassland herbivores. Oecologia
70:35-52.
Blaxter KL, Wainma FW, Wilson RS, 1961. The regulation of food
intake by sheep. Anim Prod 3:51-61.
Boyce MS, 1989. The Jackson elk herd: intensive wildlife management
in North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Brazda AR, 1953. Elk migration patterns, and some of the factors
affecting movements in the Gallatin River drainage, Montana. J
Wildl Manage 17:9-23.
Broom DM, Arnold CW, 1986. Selection by grazing sheep of pasture
plants at low herbage availability and responses of the plants to
grazing. AustJ Agric Res 37327-538.
Charnov EI-, 1976. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem.
Theor Popul Biol 9:129-136.
Clancy M, Bull LS, Wangness PJ, Baumgardt BR, 1976. Digestible en-
ergy intake of complete diets by wethers and lactating ewes. J Anim
Sci 42:961-969.
Cook CW, Mattox JE. Harris LE, 1961. Comparative daily consump-
tion and digestibility of summer range forage by wet and dry ewes.
J Anim Sci 20:866-870.
Demment MW, Van Soest PJ, 1985. A nutritional explanation for body-
size patterns of ruminant and nonruminant herbivores. Am Nat
125:641-672.
Duncan P, Foose TJ, Gordon 1J, Gakahu CG, LJoyde M, 1990. Com-
parative nutrient extraction from forages by grazing bovids and
equids: a test of the nutritional model of equid/bovid competition
and coexistence. Oecologia 84:411-418.
Dutilleul P, 1993. Spatial heterogeneity and the design of ecological
field experiments. Ecology 74:1646-1658.
Estes RD, 1974. Social organization of the African Bovidae. In: The
behaviour of ungulates and its relation to management (Geist V,
Walthcr F. eds). Morges, Switzerland: International Union for Con-
servation of Nature and Natural Resources; 166-205.
Festa-Bianchet M, 1988. Seasonal range selection in bighorn sheep:
conflicts between forage quality, forage quantity and predator
avoidance. Oecologia 75:580-586.
Fonnesbeck PV, Christiansen JL. Harris LE, 1981. Factors affecting
digestibility of nutrients by sheep. J Anim Sci 52363-376.
Fryxell JM, 1991. Forage quality and aggregation by large herbivores.
Am Nat 138:478-498.
FryxellJM, Doucei CM, 1993. Diet choice and the functional response
of beavers. Ecology 74:1297-1306.
Fryxell JM, Sinclair ARE, 1988. Seasonal migration by white-eared
knob in relation to resources. Afr J Ecol 26:17-31.
Fryxell JM, Vamosi SM, Walton RA, Doucet CM, 1994. Retention time
and the functional response of beavers. Oikos 71:207-214..
Gates C, Hudson RJ, 1978. Energy costs of locomouon in wapiti. Acta
Theriol 23:365-370.
Gates CC, Hudson RJ, 1983. Foraging behaviour of wapiti in a boreal
forest enclosure. Nat Can 110:197-206.
Gordon IJ, 1988. Facilitation of red deer grazing by cattle and its
impact on red deer performance. J Appl Ecol 25:1-10.
Gordon IJ, Illius AW, 1989. Resource partitioning by ungulates on the
Isle of Rhum. Oecologia 79:383-389.
Gross JE, Hobbs NT, Wunder BA, 1993a. Independent variables for
predicting intake rate of mammalian herbivores: biomass density,
plant density, or bite size? Oikos 68:75-81.
Cross JE, Shipley LA, Hobbs NT, Spalinger DE, Wunder BA, 1993b.
Foraging by herbivores in food-concentrated patches: tests of a
mechanistic model of functional response. Ecology 74:778-791.
Hebert DM, 1973. AJtitudinal migration as a factor in the nutrition
of bighorn sheep (PhD dissertation). Vancouver, British Columbia:
University of British Columbia.
Hobbs NT, Baker DL, EllisJE, Swift DM, 1981. Composition and qual-
ity of elk winter diets in Colorado. J Wildl Manage 45:156-171.
Hobbs NT, Swift DM, 1985. Estimates of habitat carrying capacity in-
corporating explicit nutritional constraints. J Wildl Manage 49:814-
822.
Hobbs NT, Swift DM, 1988. Crazing in herds: when are nutritional
benefits realized? Am Nat 131:760-764.
Hudson RJ, Watkins WG, 1986. Foraging rates of wapiti on green and
cured pastures. Can J Zool 64:1705-1708.
Illius AW, Clark DA, Hodgson J, 1992. Discrimination and patch
choice by sheep grazing grass-clover swards. J Anim Ecol 61:183-
194.
Jarman PJ, 1974. The social organisation of antelope in relation to
their ecology. Behaviour 48:215-266.
Johnson DH, 1980. The comparison of usage and availability mea-
surements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology 61:65-71.
Kamstra LD, 1973. Seasonal changes in quality of some important
range grasses. J Range Manage 26:289-291.
KrebsJR, McQeery RH, 1984. Individual optimization in behavioural
ecology. In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach (Kret»
JR. Davies NB, eds). Oxford: Blackwell; 91-121.
Kreulen D, 1975. Wildebeest habitat selection in the Serengeti plains,
Tanzania, in relation to calcium and lactation: a preliminary report.
East Afr Wildl J 13:297-304.
Langvatn R, Hanley TA, 1993. Feeding-patch choice by red deer in
relation to foraging efficiency: an experiment Oecologia 95:164—
170.
Laycock WA, Price DA, 1970. Factors influencing forage quality. In:
Range and wildlife habitat evaluation—a research symposium. U S
Dep Agric Misc Publ 1147:37-47.
Lewis CE, Lowrey RS, Monso WG, Knox FE, 1975. Seasonal trends in
nutrients and cattle digestibility of forage on pine-wiregrass range.
J Anim Sci 41:208-212.
Lundberg P, 1988. Functional response of a small mammalian herbi-
vore: the dijc equation revisited. J Anim Ecol 57:999—1006.
Maddock L, 1979. The "migration" and grazing succession. In: Ser-
engeti: dynamics of an ecosystem (Sinclair ARE, Norton-Griffiths
M, eds). Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 104—129.
McNaughton SJ, 1984. Grazing lawns: animals in herds, plant form.
and convolution. Am Nat 124:863-886.
McNaughton SJ, 1988. Mineral nutrition and spatial concentrations
of African ungulates. Nature 334:343-345.
McNaughton SJ, 1990. Mineral nutrition and seasonal movements of
African ungulates. Nature 345:613-615.
Mould ED, Robbins CT, 1982. Digestive capabilities in elk compared
to white-tailed deer. J Wild! Manage 46:22-29.
Murie OJ, 1951. The elk of North America. Harrisburg, Pennsylvania:
Stack,pole.
NelsonJR, Leege TA, 1982. Nutritional requirements and food habits.
In: Elk of North America: ecology and management (Thomas JW,
Toweill DE, eds). Harrisburg, Pennsylvania: Stackpole; 323-367.
Oelberg K, 1956. Factors affecting the nutritive value of range forage.
J Range Manage 9:220-225.
Osbourn DF, 198*). The feeding value of grass and grass products. In:
Grass: its production and utilization (Holmes W. ed). Oxford.
Blackwell; 89-124.
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 217
Owen-Smith N, Novellie P, 1982. What should a clever ungulate eat?
Am Nat 119:151-178.
Parr Instruments, 1977. ASTM standards for bomb calorimetry. Phil-
adelphia: American Society for Technical Material!.
Renecker LA, Hudson RJ, 1986. Seasonal foraging rate of moose in
aspen boreal habitats. J Wild] Manage 50:143-147.
Robbins CT, 1983. Wildlife feeding and nutrition. Orlando: Academic
Press.
Royama T, 1970. Factors governing the hunting behaviour and selec-
tion of food by the great tit (Parus major l_). J Anim Ecol 39:619—
668.
Shipley LA, Gross JE, Spalinger DE, Hobbs NT, Wunder BA, 1994.
The scaling of intake rate in mammalian herbivores. Am Nat 143:
1055-1082.
Shipley LA, Spalinger DE, 1992. Mechanics of browsing in dense food
patches; effects of plant and animal morphology on intake rate.
CanJ Zool 70:1743-1752.
Short J, 1985. The functional response of kangaroos, sheep and rab-
biu in an arid grazing system. J Appl Ecol 22:435-447.
Simons S, Alcock J, 1971. Learning and the foraging persistence of
white-crowned sparrows ZonotriMa leucophrjs. Ibis 113:477-482.
Sinclair ARE, 1977. The African buffalo. Chicago: University of Chi-
cago Press.
Smith JNM, Dawkins R, 1971. The hunting behaviour of individual
great tits in relation to spatial variation in their food density. Anim
Behav 19:695-706.
Spalinger DE, Hanley TA, Robbins CT, 1988. Analysts of the function-
al response in foraging in the Sitka black-tailed deer. Ecology 69:
1166-1175.
Spalinger DE, Hobbs NT, 1992. Mechanisms of foraging in mamma-
lian herbivores; new models of functional response. Am Nat 140:
325-348.
Spalinger DE, Robbins CT, Hanley TA, 1986. The assessment of han-
dling time in ruminants: the effect of plant chemical and physical
structure on the rate of breakdown of plant particles in the rumen
of mule deer and elk. CanJ Zool 64:312-321.
Stephens DW, KrebsJR, 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton. NewJersey:
Princeton University Press.
VanDyne CM, Heady HF, 1965. Dietary chemical composition of cattle
and sheep grazing in common on a dry annual range. J Range
Manage 18:78-86.
Van Soest PJ, 1982. Nutritional ecology of the ruminant. Corvallis,
Oregon: O and B Books.
Van Soest PJ, Mertens DR, Deinum B, 1978. Preharvest factors influ-
encing quality of conserved forage. J Anim Sci 47:712-720.
Van Soest PJ, Robertson JB, 1980. Systems of analysis for evaluating
fibrous feeds. In: Standardization of analytical methodology for
feeds, Ottawa, Canada, 12-14 March 1979 (Pigden WJ, Balch CC,
Craham M, eds). Ottawa; IDRC-134e; 49-60.
Van Soest PJ, Wine RH, 1967. Use of detergents in the analysis of
fibrous feeds. IV. Determination of plant cell wall constituents. J
Assoc Off Anal Chem 5CK50-55.
Wairimu S, Hudson RJ, 199S. Compensatory foraging behaviour of
young wapiti stags. Appl Anim Behav Sci 36:65-79.
Waite R, 1963. Botanical and chemical changes in maturing grass and
their effect on its digestibility. Agric Prog 38:50-56.
Ward AL, Cupal JJ, Lea AL, Oakley CA, Weeks RW, 1973. Elk behav-
iour in relation to rattle grazing, forest recreation and traffic. In:
Thirty-eighth North American wildlife and natural resources con-
ference. Washington. D.C, 18-21 March 1973 (Trefethen JB, ed).
Washington, D.C: Wildlife Management Institute; 327-337.
Western D, 1975. Water availability and its influence on the structure
and dynamics of a savannah large mammal community. East Afr
WildlJ 13:265-286.
White RC, 1983. Foraging patterns and their multiplier effects on
productivity of northern ungulates. Oikos 40:377-384.
Wickstrom ML, Robbins CT, Hanley TA, Spalinge DE, Parish SM,
1984. Food intake and foraging energetics of elk and mule deer. J
Wild] Manage 48:1285-1301.
Wilson JR, Taylor AO, Dolby CR, 1976. Temperature and atmospheric
humidity effects on cell wall content and associated changes in dry
matter digestibility of some tropical and temperate grasses. N Z J
Agric Res 19:41-46.
atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom

More Related Content

What's hot

Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoors
Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoorsBehavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoors
Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoorsAmanda D'avila Verardi
 
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...IOSR Journals
 
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...Alexander Decker
 
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...ILRI
 
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...TERN Australia
 
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculture
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic AgricultureVijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculture
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculturevijay bhosekar
 
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...paperpublications3
 
Non-steady-state modeling
Non-steady-state modelingNon-steady-state modeling
Non-steady-state modelingReza Imamidoost
 
Providence_MSc defense University of Twente
Providence_MSc defense University of TwenteProvidence_MSc defense University of Twente
Providence_MSc defense University of TwenteProvidence AKAYEZU
 
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)Carlos Alberto Monteiro
 
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)Carlos Alberto Monteiro
 
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...Avjinder (Avi) Kaler
 
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...World Agroforestry (ICRAF)
 
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reef
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral ReefPatch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reef
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reefstoyleg
 
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...Agriculture Journal IJOEAR
 
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...Journal of Research in Biology
 
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...Avjinder (Avi) Kaler
 

What's hot (20)

Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoors
Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoorsBehavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoors
Behavior and performance of lactating sows and pigletsreared indoors or outdoors
 
Energy requirements
Energy requirementsEnergy requirements
Energy requirements
 
Gjesm150171451593800
Gjesm150171451593800Gjesm150171451593800
Gjesm150171451593800
 
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...
Effect of Replacement of Sweet Orange (Citrus Sinensis) Peel Meal with Maize ...
 
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...
Evaluation of heat treated small ruminants dropping as an alternative feedstu...
 
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...
Feed intake, apparent total tract digestibility, and nitrogen balance in Bora...
 
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...
David Lindenmayer_Transforming long-term plot-based research in Australia: LT...
 
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculture
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic AgricultureVijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculture
Vijay Bhosekar_ Published Article_ Organic Agriculture
 
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...
Evaluation of Different Herbaceous Legume Hays As Protein Supplements for Gro...
 
Non-steady-state modeling
Non-steady-state modelingNon-steady-state modeling
Non-steady-state modeling
 
Providence_MSc defense University of Twente
Providence_MSc defense University of TwenteProvidence_MSc defense University of Twente
Providence_MSc defense University of Twente
 
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)
Seminário 4 egerton-warburton_et_al-2000-ecological_applications_mycorrhiza (2)
 
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)
Seminário 1 collins et all 2002_plant community (2)
 
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...
Association mapping identifies loci for canopy coverage in diverse soybean ge...
 
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...
Mapping trees species in geographical and environmental space: a comparison o...
 
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reef
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral ReefPatch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reef
Patch Size Dependent Grazing on a Caribbean Coral Reef
 
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...
Ecophysiological Yield Components In Wheat Cultivars Under Variable Phosphoru...
 
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...
Evacuation of Fish meal using Soy protein and Carbohydrate sources in diets o...
 
FrankeO final poster
FrankeO final posterFrankeO final poster
FrankeO final poster
 
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...
Genome-Wide Association Mapping of Carbon Isotope and Oxygen Isotope Ratios i...
 

Viewers also liked

Viewers also liked (18)

Design tools
Design toolsDesign tools
Design tools
 
Wilmshurst et al 2006
Wilmshurst et al 2006Wilmshurst et al 2006
Wilmshurst et al 2006
 
Willlms et al 2002
Willlms et al 2002Willlms et al 2002
Willlms et al 2002
 
судалгаа1
судалгаа1судалгаа1
судалгаа1
 
Hish1
Hish1Hish1
Hish1
 
10
1010
10
 
3
33
3
 
хэр1
хэр1хэр1
хэр1
 
6
66
6
 
хэр1
хэр1хэр1
хэр1
 
Hish1
Hish1Hish1
Hish1
 
хэр3
хэр3хэр3
хэр3
 
11
1111
11
 
5
55
5
 
хэр3
хэр3хэр3
хэр3
 
1
11
1
 
IP정보분석사 교육 교안-지식재산권(제도)의 이해
IP정보분석사 교육 교안-지식재산권(제도)의 이해IP정보분석사 교육 교안-지식재산권(제도)의 이해
IP정보분석사 교육 교안-지식재산권(제도)의 이해
 
задание №2
задание №2задание №2
задание №2
 

Similar to Wapiti Foraging Linked to Energy Gain

Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...
Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...
Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...Open Access Research Paper
 
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Alexander Decker
 
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Alexander Decker
 
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTS
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTSEVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTS
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTSMr. Suresh R. Jambagi
 
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...Quarry Life Award by HeidelbergCement
 
Feed Sorting
Feed SortingFeed Sorting
Feed Sortingmiloshaa
 
Presentación Algas
Presentación AlgasPresentación Algas
Presentación AlgasH2RAmbiental
 
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docxRunning Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docxtoltonkendal
 
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docxRunning Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docxcharisellington63520
 
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)iosrphr_editor
 
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantation
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantationEffects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantation
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantationNia Nia
 
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...Innspub Net
 
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score board
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score boardJonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score board
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score boardAndy Fernando Siahaan
 
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...IJEAB
 

Similar to Wapiti Foraging Linked to Energy Gain (20)

PERFOR~2-23-11
PERFOR~2-23-11PERFOR~2-23-11
PERFOR~2-23-11
 
P ii
P iiP ii
P ii
 
Final Report
Final ReportFinal Report
Final Report
 
Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...
Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...
Interaction on the diet and substrate on the growth of Archachatina marginata...
 
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
 
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
Performance of yankasa rams fed andropogon gayanus (gamba grass) hay suppleme...
 
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTS
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTSEVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTS
EVOLUTION OF DIET BREADTH IN HERBIVORUS INSECTS
 
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...
Investigation into the insect biodiversity of grasslands surrounding the reed...
 
Feed Sorting
Feed SortingFeed Sorting
Feed Sorting
 
Presentación Algas
Presentación AlgasPresentación Algas
Presentación Algas
 
Rjl08
Rjl08Rjl08
Rjl08
 
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docxRunning Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
 
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docxRunning Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT  1  Sa.docx
Running Head SAMPLE FINAL LAB REPORT 1 Sa.docx
 
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
 
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantation
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantationEffects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantation
Effects of restricted feeding, low energy diet, and implantation
 
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...
Feeding rate requirements for Schilbe intermedius (Rüppel, 1832) fingerlings ...
 
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score board
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score boardJonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score board
Jonathan D.majer, Ants pass the bioindicator score board
 
organic nitrogen systems
organic nitrogen systemsorganic nitrogen systems
organic nitrogen systems
 
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...
Annual Biomass Production, Chemical Composition and In- sacco Degradability o...
 
Caos em uma comunidade
Caos em uma comunidadeCaos em uma comunidade
Caos em uma comunidade
 

Wapiti Foraging Linked to Energy Gain

  • 1. Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2: 209-217 Forage quality and patch choice by wapiti {Cervus elaphus) John F. Wilmshurst,1 John M. Fryxell," and Robert J. Hudsonb 'Department of Zoology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario NIG 2W1, Canada, and b Department of Animal Science, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta T6G 2P5, Canada Recent models suggest that herbivores might optimize energy gain by selecting patches of intermediate vegetation biomass. We tested this hypothesis in wapiti (Cervus elaphus) by estimating daily rates of energy gain in relation to grass biomass and by measuring patch choice in experimental pastures in which grass biomass was manipulated by mowing. The digestible energy content of grasses declined with increasing biomass due to maturational changes in fiber and lignin content. Daily rates of dry matter intake by wapiti increased with grass biomass at a decelerating rate, implying a Type II functional response. Linking these values to published ad libitum energy intake and energy expenditure parameters, Fryxell's (1991) model predicted that the daily rate of energy gain should be highest when wapiti feed in grasslands with 1000-1100 kg/ha. In trials in which grass biomass within a mosaic of patches was manipulated experimentally between 800-2900 kg/ha, wapiti preferred patches of 1200 kg/ha, close to the value predicted by the energy gain model. Our results suggest that the rate of energy gain by wapiti is constrained by both grass biomass and grass fiber content, the latter of which varies inversely with grass biomass. Behavioral preference for grass patches of intermediate biomass and fiber content could help explain patterns of aggregation and seasonal migration reported previously for wapiti. Key words: Cervidae, Cervus elaphus, digestible energy, forage maturation, functional response, grasses, herbivory, patch choice, wapiti. [Behav Ecot 6:209-217 (1995)] Optimal foraging theory assumes that foraging decisions by herbivores should be strongly influenced by physi- ological and environmental constraints on rates of nutrient uptake. Two such constraints commonly invoked for verte- brate grazers are the effect of plant density on the short-term rate of food intake (availability constraint) and the effect of digestive capacity on the long-term rate of energy assimilation (processing constraint) (Belovsky, 1978, 1986; Fryxell, 1991; Owen-Smith and Novellie, 1982). The short-term rate of food intake (i.e., the functional re- sponse) of herbivores should be related positively to plant size, bite size, and plant density (Gross et al., 199Sa,b; Shipley et al., 1994; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992; Spalinger and Hobbs, 1992). Accordingly, several studies have shown that herbivore functional responses are often positive but decel- erating functions of plant density or biomass (Fryxell et al., 1994; Fryxell and Doucet, 1993; Gross et al., 1993a,b; Lund- berg, 1988; Short, 1985; Wickstrom et al., 1984). The digestive capacity of herbivores is primarily governed by the interaction of energy and fiber in their diet (Broom and Arnold, 1986). The digestibility of energy in grasses for grazing ruminants is inversely related to cell wall (fiber) con- tent of the forage (Mould and Robbins, 1982; Osbourn, 1989; Van Soest, 1982). As grasses mature the proportion of cell wall in their tissues increases, reducing digestibility (Laycock and Price, 1970; Oelberg, 1956; Osbourn, 1989). The processing time (digestion and passage) in the gut often increases as plants mature (Blaxter et al., 1961; White, 1983). This suggests that both digestibility and the rate of turnover of ingesta should be negatively related to plant biomass, if biomass is positively associated with plant maturation stage. This inverse correlation between availability and processing constraints creates a potential trade-off for grazing herbivores (Demment and Van Soest, 1985; Fryxell, 1991; Hobbs and Swift, 1988; McNaughton, 1984; Owen-Smith and Novellie, Received 23 December 1993; revised 16 June 1994; accepted 24 June 1994. 1045-2249/95/S5.00 © 1995 International Society for Behavioral Ecology 1982). At low biomass, the processing rate is high but the short-term rate of intake is low, whereas at high pasture bio- mass, the processing rate is low but the short-term rate of intake is high. The net rate of energy intake for grazing her- bivores should be maximized accordingly on patches of inter- mediate plant biomass (the forage maturation hypothesis). Moreover, natural selection should favor grazers that choose vegetation patches of intermediate biomass, yielding the high- est rate of daily energy gain. We tested the forage maturation hypothesis using wapiti (Cervus elaphus L.) feeding in a mosaic of grass patches that were manipulated experimentally. Wapiti are large, gregarious ruminants of western North America. Although wapiti are known to feed on a wide variety of plants, they are primarily grazers (Baker and Hobbs, 1987; Boyce, 1989; Hobbs et al., 1981; Nelson and Leege, 1982). To predict the processing constraint, we measured changes in fiber content in relation to grass biomass. To predict the availability constraint, we mea- sured the functional response by wapiti to changes in grass biomass. We used Fryxell's (1991) model and the data on en- ergy constraints of foraging wapiti to predict daily rates of energy gain in relation to grass biomass. We then tested whether wapiti preferred patches of intermediate biomass, as predicted in general by the forage maturation hypothesis. METHODS The model In this section, we summarize the model used to predict the net rate of energy intake by wapiti and report parameter val- ues obtained from the literature. Model structure is described in greater detail in Fryxell (1991). We defined the availability constraint (/,, measured in kj/day) as the maximum amount of digestible energy that an herbivore could consume given an unlimited gut capacity. We calculated this constraint as the product of the digestible energy content of forage (Q, in kj/ kg), the maximum time available for foraging per day (T, in h/day) and the short-term rate of dry matter intake (C, in kg/h). atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 2. 210 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2 Recent theoretical work (Spalinger and Hobbs, 1992) sup- ported by a growing body of experimental studies (Gross et al., 199Sa,b; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992; Spalinger et al., 1988) suggests that functional responses of herbivores should vary according to plant spacing, plant apparency, and the size and spatial distribution of bite-sized portions among individ- ual stems. Consequently, a wide variety of functional response forms are theoretically plausible, depending on the ecological circumstances. Wapiti usually feed on a dense sward of closely spaced grass stems, with each bite representing a substantial fraction of an individual rameL Under these conditions, the Michaelis-Menton formula should well approximate the func- tional response (Spalinger and Hobbs, 1992), and this has been corroborated in a previous field study (Wickstrom et al., 1984). We therefore calculated the availability constraint as QTaV 1 b + V (1) where a is the maximum rate of forage consumption per her- bivore (in kg/h), Vis forage biomass (in kg/ha), and b is the biomass at which the intake rate is Vi maximum (in kg/ha). The maximum daily foraging time for wapiti has been re- ported by both Gates and Hudson (1983) and Wairimu and Hudson (1993) as 13 h, which we used to estimate 7! In our results, we show how Q varies with gravs biomass. We defined the digestive constraint (/,, in kj/day) as the maximum amount of energy that a herbivore could digest per day given ad libitum consumption. Previous research suggests that ad libitum consumption by wapiti is the following positive function of digestible energy content of the forage (Mould and Robbins, 1982): I? = 20.3Q - 45.5. (2) We estimated energetic expenditure (X) as the sum of rest- ing metabolic expenditure plus the cost of eating and loco- motion while foraging X = 24w + F(x + yW) (3) where w is the resting metabolic expenditure (in kj/h), Fix the total time spent foraging per day (in h/day), xis the cost of standing plus eating (in kj/h), and jrV* is the cost of loco- motion (in kj/h). The resting metabolic expenditure for a 170-kg wapiti (the mass of our experimental animals) was reported by Gates and Hudson (1978) as 733 kj/h. Gates and Hudson (1978) also give a value of 925 kj/h for the cost of resting plus standing, which we added to the cost of eating reported by Wickstrom et al. (1984) of 241 kj/h, yielding a total value of x = 1166 kj/h. Values of y and z for wapiti were determined from Wick- strom et al. (1984) as 6766 kj/h and -0.6 respectively. Net daily energy intake was estimated as / - X, where / is the minimum of /, and Z, (note that our definition of net energy differs from definitions found elsewhere in the physi- ological literature; e.g., Robbins, 1983). Study area and animals We conducted research at the Ministik Reid Station, 45 km southeast of Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, from May to August 1991. The experiments took place on a 2-ha, mixed-species pasture composed of Kentucky bluegrass (Pon pralmsis L.), smooth brome (Bromus inXrrmus Leysxer), clover (Trifotium sp.), dandelion (Taraxacum ojficinale Weber), and Canada thistle (drsiiim arvrnsel^). In May, the pasture was stocked with four yearling females. Animals had access to ad libitum water and a mineral lick, but were not supplementalh/ fed during the study. Each animal W.LS fitted with a numbered ear tag and colored collar to en- able identification from a distance. Daily human activity on the pasture ensured that the wapiti were acclimated to the presence of an observer. We used five additional yearlings in bite mass trials. The animals were weighed monthly on an electronic balance (±1 kg). Forage intake Short-term rates of grazing intake can be calculated as the product of bile size and cropping rate (Hudson and Watkins, 1986; Wickstrom et al., 1984). To determine bite size, individ- ual wapiti were each presented with a pre-weighed sod mea- suring 25 cm X 76 cm. These sods were simply samples of the typical grass pasture cut into rectangular blocks. We cut each sod in half and trimmed it until it weighed less than 4 kg (the capacity of the balance), and dien placed each sod half in a plastic container and weighed them on an electronic balance (±0.01 g). One container (experimental) was staked to the ground in a 15 m X 8 m feeding pen devoid of vegetation, whereas the control container was placed outside the pen. The number of bites taken from the experimental sod by a captive yearling wapiti was recorded for a maximum of 15 min (to limit desiccation) or maximum of 15 bites (to limit deple- tion), whichever occurred first- The experimental and control sods were then re-weighed. Bite mass (j, measured in g/bite) was calculated as (4) where p, is the pre-grazing mass of the experimental sod (in g), fa is the post-grazing mass of the experimental sod (in g), r is the proportion of evaporative loss determined from the control sod, g is the number of bites taken, and h is the pro- portion of dry matter in the fresh vegetation. Cropping rates of wapiti were measured during a foraging trial conducted with patches of widely varying grass biomass (described in detail below). From an elevated platform, we continuously observed a focal animal foraging on a mosaic of 8 X 10 m patches of known biomass. Each bite, defined as a single grasping and tearing of the grass, was entered into an event recorder that also recorded time during each cropping sequence. We defined a cropping sequence as beginning when the focal animal put its head down to graze and ending when it lifted its head. Uninterrupted cropping rate was cal- culated as the number of bites taken during the cropping sequence divided by time. As wapiti typically spent a small portion of foraging bouts engaged in activities other than cropping plants, we corrected for this bias by multiplying by the proportion of time that the wapiti actually spent in crop- ping sequences during foraging bouts. The short-term rate of dry matter intake (kg/h) was cal- culated as the product of bite mass (g/bite) multiplied by the cropping rate (bites/min) multiplied by 60 min/h and 0.001 kg/g, to keep the measurement units consistent. Parameters a and b from the Michaelis-Menton formula (Equation 2) were estimated by fitting to intake data using a standard least- squares algorithm (Wickstrom et al., 1984). Forage growth and quality To obtain undisturbed forage samples for the estimation of digestible energy content in relation to grass biomass, we erected 10 wire mesh grazing exclosures (basal area 1.5 m1 , 2-m high) at random locations on the pasture. Each exrlosure was sampled biweekly from 10 May to 16 August 1991. Samples were taken by securing a 0.125 m5 wire hoop to the ground beneath the exclosure and clipping all vegetation rooted within the hoop at ground level. A previously unsuut- atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 3. Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 211 pled pan of the exclosurc was sampled each time. Vegetation samples were stored in a cool room at approximately 2°C and frozen within 3 days of collection. They were dried at 60°C for 48 h for analysis and weighed (±0.01 g). Samples were then ground through a 1-mm diameter screen in a Wiley mill and stored at room temperature. A subsample from each sam- ple was analyzed for dry matter content (AOAC, 1980). Hence, biomass values are reported as kg dry matter/ha (kg- DM/ha). For ruminants, digestible energy content of forage depends on fiber content. The greater die concentration of fiber, par- ticularly lignin, the less digestible the forage (Van Soest, 1982). We analyzed forage samples for neutral detergent fiber (NDF), acid detergent fiber (ADF), permanganate lignin con- tent (Van Soest and Robertson, 1980; Van Soest and Wine, 1967), and Kjeldahl nitrogen. Digestibility was calculated us- ing Mould and Robbins' (1982) regression equations for wap- iti. The percent digestible dry matter (DDM) of each sample is the sum of the digestible percentage of the neutral deter- gent soluble (NDS) fraction and the digestible percentage of the NDF fraction. Following Mould and Robbins (1982), we calculated DDM as the sum of 1.00 and Digestible NDF(%) = %NDF(176.92 - 40.5(ln x)) (6) where x is the percent of the ADF diat is lignin and cutin. Percent digestible energy (DE) was subsequently calculated from Mould and Robbins (1982) according to DE = -0.61 + 0.98(DDM). (7) The relationship between DE and sample biomass was dien determined using least squares regression. The gross energy content of each sample was determined by bomb calorimetry (Parr, 1977). Patch use experiment To test whedier wapiti selectively exploit patches of interme- diate biomass, we conducted feeding trials on two areas of the 2-ha pasture in which a mosaic of patches at different growth stages had been created by mowing. Animals were allowed to move freely among the patches, and preferences were assessed by recording the amount of time spent on each patch (Roy- ama, 1970; Simons and Alcock, 1971; Smith and Dawkins, 1971). On the two most level areas of die pasture, two 30 m x 40 m paddocks were enclosed with 2-m-high deer fence. Within each paddock, 15 patches of 8 X 10 m were surveyed and marked with a stake at each corner. We arranged patches in three blocks of five along a slight elevational gradient in die pasture to control statistically (Dutilleul, 1993) for possible confounding effects due to spatial variation in soil moisture or shading. Al 2-week intervals, one patch in each block was randomly selected and mowed 50 mm above the ground widi a commercial mower. When all patches had been mowed once, the paddock was left to regrow for 2 weeks before ani- mals were admitted. This mowing regime produced a mosaic of grass patches widi 2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 weeks regrowth. The patch use experiment was initiated by removing die 30-m fence from die south end of the paddock and allowing four wapiti to enter. From an elevated platform behind the north end of die paddock, an observer recorded time allo- cation and cropping rates widiin patches. Observations were made from first to last light for diree consecutive days or until all four animals spent one bout foraging outside die paddock. H O.00 Digestible NDS(%) = -21.88 + 1.11 %NDS (5) £ 0 UJ IQ. B ° 2000 4000 GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha) Figure 1 Wapiti bite size (a) and cropping rate (b) in relation to grass biomasj. See text for regression equations. At night, die animals were removed, and the paddock was closed. Three forage samples were taken from each patch before and after the trial. Samples were taken, as before, by securing a 0.125 m' wire hoop to die ground and clipping die vege- tation rooted widiin. Samples were dried and weighed as de- scribed earlier and analyzed to estimate forage quality in each patch. Where appropriate to meet the criteria for statistical anal- yses, we have log- or arcsine-transformed variables. RESULTS Model parameter estimates We estimated bite mass using sods ranging in biomass between 551-3128 kg/ha. Bite mass (in g/bite) increased linearly widi grass biomass (Figure la; y = 0.087 + 0.0014* t = 2.379, p = .03, r1 = .303, n = 15). Cropping rates were determined atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 4. 212 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2 2000 4000 GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha) Figure 2 The daily rate of dry matier intake by wapiti in relation to grass biomass. See text for regression equation. on forage patches ranging from 653-3557 kg/ha. Wapiti spent 10% (±1.0) of each foraging bout in activities other than cropping. Therefore, bite rates were multiplied by 0.90 to eliminate this bias. Cropping rates were a negative curvilinear function of grass biomass (Figure lb; y = 438,357/(x + 7706), r1 = .315, n = 18), consistent with previous grazing studies (Gross et al., 1993a; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992). Dry matter intake values, calculated from the product of bite mass and cropping rates, suggested a Type II functional response, as postulated (Figure 2). Parameters a and b for Equation 1 were estimated as 1.87 kg/h and 1604 kg/ha, respectively. Grass biomass (in kg/ha) in the ungrazed exclosures in- creased over time (in days since 1 January 1991) at a decel- erating rate (y = 15.467*/(196.0 + x), r1 = .636, n = 70). To distinguish between possible effects of grass biomass (matu- ration) and time on forage quality, we conducted multiple regression analyses for each of percentage NDF, ADF, lignin, and protein using sampled biomass and date as independent variables, in each case with 70 forage samples. Percentage NDF increased with both grass biomass (Figure 3a; y = 1.64 + 7.75 ln(x), r* = .40, t = 6.65, p < .001) and date (y = -123.8 + 35.2 ln(z), r3 = .34, I = 5.88, p < .001), but in a multiple regression analysis only the coefficient for grass bio- mass was significant (4, = 2.91, p = .05, 4 = 1.42, p = .16). Percentage ADF increased with both grass biomass (Figure 3b; y = -14.3 + 6.7 ln(x), r1 = .59, t = 10.0, p < .001) and date (Figure 4a; y = -132.3 + 32.3 ln(z), r* = .56, t = 9.3, p < .001), with both coefficients significant in the multiple re- gression analysis (y = -77.7 + 4.14 ln(x) + 15.9 ln(z), T2 = .64, L, = 3.9, p < .001, t, = 3.04, p < .001). Percentage lignin also increased with both grass biomass (y = -3.75 + 1.25 ln(x), r1 = .26, t = 4.92, p < .001) and date (Figure 4b; y = -39.0 + 8.55 n(z), r1 = .65, 4 = 10.9, p < .001), but in a multiple regression analysis only the coefficient for date was significant (t, = 0.8, p = .43, 4 = 5.8, p < .001). Hence, based on variables with the strongest explanatory power, we used univariate estimates of percentage NDF and lignin and the multivariate relationship of percentage ADF to estimate per- centage DE by the equation y = 210.9 - 0.67 ln(x) - 27.9 ln(z). As our patch use trials were conducted over the same 100 1000 10000 GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha) Figure 3 Percentage neutral detergent fiber (a) and acid detergent fiber (b) from grazing exclosure samples in relation to grass biomass. See text for regression equations. week, the time effects are simply constants in the model of energy gain, but they do improve the precision of our digest- ible energy estimates. Percentage protein in the undisturbed samples decreased with both grass biomass (y = 36.3 — 6.3 ln(x), r1 = .50, t = 6.30, p < .001) and date (y = 101.7 - 17.3 ln(z), r1 = .59, / = 7.52, p < .001). Multiple regression indicated that only the effect of date was significant when date and grass biomass were considered together (t, = 1.47, p = .15, 4 = 3.30, p = .002). Linking of the foraging parameters and maturational vari- ation in DE content with estimates of energy expenditure in the model, we calculated daily rates of net energy intake in relation to grass biomass. Our model predicted that daily en- ergy gains should rise steeply at low forage biomass, peak at roughly 1000-1100 kg/ha, then slope downward with further increases in grass biomass (Figure 5). At the optimal grass biomass, the maximum daily intake of DE should be 53 kj/ atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 5. WUmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 21S 10.0 in •• 100 150 200 250 JULIAN DATE Figure 4 Percentage acid detergent fiber (a) and lignin (b) from grazing exdosure samples in relation to Julian date. See text for regression equation*. kg body mass, or 9010 kj/day for the 170 kg animals used in our study. Patch use experiment In the foraging experiment, grass biomass within patches spanned the predicted optimum, ranging between 796 kg/ha in the patches with 2 weeks growth and 2867 kg/ha in the patches with 10 weeks growth (Figure 6). The patches with 4 weeks growth (mean = 1212 kg/ha) were closest to optimal, according to the model. There was no significant difference in DE content between the experimental patches and the ungrazed exclosures (AN- COVA: /",l n = 1.13, p = .29), indicating that model param- eters based on the exclosures should be appropriate to the experimental patches. The total time spent by the animals on each paddock did not differ (ANOVA: FtM = 2.553, p = .132), so the data for each paddock were pooled for subsequent analysis. 2000 4000 GRASS BIOMASS (kg/ha) Figure 5 Net energy intake predicted for wapiti in relation to gran biomass. The optimal value is marked by an asterisk. We used two types of statistical procedures to test whether wapiti exploited grass patches of intermediate biomass: (1) polynomial regression and (2) ANOVA with Tukey post-hoc comparisons of daily foraging time and percentage foraging time per day in relation to the number of weeks of uninter- rupted grass growth (2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 weeks) within patches. In die case of polynomial regression, the critical test is wheth- er the coefficient of a second order term is significantly less than 0, implying that animals use patches of intermediate growth more heavily than other patches. More specifically, die patches widi 4 weeks growth would be predicted to yield the 4000 2 A 6 8 10 12 PATCH GROWTH (weeks) Figure 6 Grass biomass within patches in relation to the number of weeks of grass growth within patches. Vertical bars are 1 SE- atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 6. 214 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2 highest rate of energy gain and therefore be preferred relative to all other patches. We took a conservative approach in an- alyzing patch use data to reduce the possibility of spurious inference due to pseudoreplication (Aebischer et al., 1994). Accordingly, we lumped data for replicates of the same patch growth treatment. This left us with 59 degrees of freedom, corresponding to 4 animals, 3 days of observation, and 5 cat- egories of patches. We used conventional parametric tests be- cause there was Iitde evidence of non-normality in die source data or in the residuals. There was Iitde reason to suspect that use of one patch type precluded use of other patches (Ae- bischer et al., 1994; Johnson, 1980) because none of the study animals foraged for more than 4.2 hours out of die maximum 12 hours possible during each observation period and diree replicate patches of similar biomass were always available to study animals. Polynomial regression indicated that daily foraging time (in s) was highest in patches of intermediate growth (in weeks) (Figure 7a;^= -2176 + 1581x - 119**, r* = .377, ^ = 5.601, p < .001, L^ = -5.155, p < .001). Inclusion of trial day im- proved slightly die degree of fit (r* = .439, / = -2.496, p = .02). Similarly, significant variation occurred among individ- uals with respect to die quadratic relationship between daily foraging time and patch growth stage (r1 — 0.473, I = 1.886, p = .005). We normalized daily foraging time data by dividing time per patch by the total time summed over all patches. We dien arcsine-transformed these normalized patch use data and re- gressed die transformed values against weeks of patch growdi. As before, percentage foraging time per patch was better pre- dicted by a quadratic dian a linear model (Figure 7b; y = 14.4 + 14.2JC- 1.05^,7" = .559,1^ = 7.885, p<. 001, <*, = -7.118, p < .001), implying diat patches of intermediate growdi were used most heavily. Results of die regression tests were corroborated by ANO- VA_ Daily foraging time varied significandy amongst die five patch categories (FAM = 13.254, p < .001). When multifactor ANOVA models were examined, dairy foraging time was also affected by trial day (F = 5.385, p < .01) and animal (F = 8.658, p < .001). The arcsine-transformed values for percent- age foraging time also varied amongst patches (F^M = 42.871, p < .001). In die latter case, no direct effect of eidier trial day or animal (p > .10) was detectable statistically, but an interaction effect did imply significant variation among ani- mals widi respect to die relationship between patch growth stage and percentage patch use by wapiti (F = 7.352, p < .001). Wapiti spent 1.3% of dieir foraging time in patches widi 2 weeks growdi, well below die value of 20% expected from a random model. Wapiti spent over 30% of dieir time in patch- es widi 4 weeks growdi and lesser amounts of time in patches widi longer growdi, as predicted by die optimal energy gain model. Tukey's test for multiple post-hoc comparisons (a < 0.05) confirmed diat patches widi 2 weeks of growth were used significandy less dian die others, patches widi 4 weeks of growdi were used significandy more dian die odiers, and patches with 6, 8, or 10 weeks growdi were indistinguishable statistically. DISCUSSION Three conditions must be met for die forage maturation hy- podiesis to apply to patch selection by grazing herbivores: (1) short-term rates of grazing intake must increase widi plant biomass, (2) ad libitum limits on intake must be linked to indices of forage quality such as digestible energy or protein content, and (3) digestible energy or protein content must decline widi increasing plant biomass widiin patches. We have 4000 2 4 6 8 10 12 PATCH GROWTH (weeks) Figure 7 Daily foraging time (a) and percentage foraging time per day (b) in relation to the number of weeks of grass growth within patches. Vertical bars ire 1 SE. Expected values under the null hypothesis are indicated by the dotted line. shown diat diese conditions exist for wapiti and have dem- onstrated experimentally diat wapiti preferentially selected patches of intermediate abundance. Patch use patterns during die trial indicated diat wapiti pre- ferred grass patches widi 1200 kg/ha, close to die model pre- diction. This result is also consistent widi results recendy re- ported by Langvatn and Hanley (1993) for red deer (Cervus daphus). They conclude, however diat patch selectivity by red deer optimized die rate of protein intake radier than die rate of energy gain, as we suggest. Indeed, protein levels in our pastures did decrease over die growing season at die same time that digestible energy was declining. Hence, digestible energy and protein content covaried strongly in both our study and diat of Langvatn and Hanley (1993). We show else- where (Wilmshurst JF and Fryxell JM, unpublished) diat die difference between our conclusions and diose of Langvatn and Hanley Ls largely a matter of die time period of foraging atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 7. Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 215 under consideration. By incorporating a digestive constraint in our model, we consider the implications of gut fill over an entire foraging bout on net energy intake. Langvatn and Han- ley (1993) predicted intake using only the availability con- straint and model instantaneous intake. Hence, patterns of forage selection that maximize long-term rates of energy in- take also maximize short-term rates of protein intake, at least in the two systems that have been studied thus far. To resolve this issue, studies are needed from systems with weak covari- ation, if this is indeed possible, between protein content and energy digestibility. Variation in forage quality is associated with a number of factors. First and foremost, physiological processes cause the accumulation of fiber as individual ramets grow and mature (Kamstra, 1973; Wake, 1963). Other factors, such as temper- ature and humidity (Wilson et al., 1976), soil moisture (Cook et al., 1961), and light intensity (VanDyne and Heady, 1965), may result in temporal variation in forage quality that are not associated with increasing biomass per se (Van Soest et al., 1978). These factors played a relatively large role in forage quality variation measured here, particularly with respect to lignin, which continued to accumulate even after growth had slowed late in the growing season. Like maturational variation in forage quality, these additional sources represent poten- tially important variation in forage quality for herbivores in their natural ranges. Strictly speaking, our optimality model suggests that wapiti should have spent all of their time in the most profitable patches, whereas they spent considerable amounts of time for- aging in other patches. A common feature of most foraging experiments, such partial preferences could be due to (1) in- dividual differences in optima among animals, (2) discrimi- nation errors in selecting optimal patches, or (3) sampling to update information (Krebs and McQeery, 1984). Any or all of these factors could contribute to an animal spending time in less profitable patches. We would caution against the indiscriminant use of the op- timality model for ungulates of sizes differing from that of the yearling wapiti examined here. Both bite size and cropping rate parameters vary allometrically among grazers (Gross et al., 1993b; Shipley et al., 1994; Shipley and Spalinger, 1992), which could change the constraint curves markedly. More- over, energetic value and processing time can vary consider- ably among various food items available to herbivores (Baker and Hobbs, 1987; Belovsky, 1978, 1986; Fryxell et al., 1994; Spalinger et al., 1986), which could influence patterns of patch use strongly. The pattern of patch use exhibited by wapiti could help explain seasonal movement patterns of wapiti. In montane habitat, wapiti migrate along an altitudinal gradient biannu- ally, moving to higher altitudes in the spring and to lower altitudes in the fall (Boyce, 1989). As has been suggested for bighorn sheep (Ouis canadensis) (Festa-Bianchet, 1988; He- bert, 1973) and red deer (Qrruus daphus L.) (Albon and La- ngvatn, 1992), wapiti may time their migration to exploit high quality emergent forage as the snow cover melts in the spring. The forage selection patterns we observed suggest that if mi- gration is timed to maximize net energy intake, then wapiti should lag several weeks (depending on vegetation growth rates) behind the melt Several studies report that wapiti move to higher elevations a short time after snow melt in the spring (Brazda, 1953; Murie, 1951; Ward et al., 1973). Brazda (1953: Table 3) suggested that wapiti do not occupy the summer range until the forage has reached a "succulent . .. develop- ing" stage. However, Brazda did not measure seasonal changes in plant nutritional quality. What evidence is there that the three conditions necessary for the application of the forage maturation hypothesis found to hold here may also exist in other systems? Gross et al. (1993b) have recendy demonstrated that functional responses of 12 species of herbivores ranging in body size from 0.05 to 548 kg increase monotonically. Previous studies have shown similar trends with other taxa (Fryxell et al., 1994; Fryxell and Doucet, 1993; Lundberg, 1988; Renecker and Hudson, 1986; Short, 1985), suggesting that Type II functional responses are common among grazing herbivores. Variation in DE intake with respect to forage DE content has not been widely studied. However, data for other ungulates have shown this relation- ship to be positive and decelerating (Ammann et al., 1973; Clancy et al., 1976). The mechanism behind this relationship should apply to most ruminants and possibly even hind-gut fermenters (Duncan et al., 1990). Finally, maturational de- clines in forage quality have been extensively documented, varying in degree among forage species (Armstrong et al., 1986; Fonnesbeck et al., 1981; Lewis et al., 1975) and among climatic zones (Barton et al., 1976; Kamstra, 1973). Hence, conditions favoring selection for patches of intermediate plant abundance could apply to many large grazing herbi- vores. The trade-off between forage quality and abundance has interesting implications for the application of optimal patch use models to herbivores (Charnov, 1976; Stephens and Krebs, 1986). One might predict from such models diat vari- ation in resource abundance among patches should decline over time as patches of high resource abundance become de- pleted, provided that rates of resource renewal are minor rel- ative to rates of consumption. Unlike classic patch choice models, our work suggests that herbivores should concentrate in patches of intermediate resource abundance. Accordingly, patches of high resource abundance should tend to be ig- nored and accumulate biomass, if by chance they have es- caped attack at earlier stages of growth, whereas patches of lower abundance should experience periodic depletion and regrowth. Hence, maturational effects for herbivores should lead to increased spatial variation in resource abundance over time, contrary to classic patch use models. A number of factors are thought to contribute to aggregat- ed spatial distributions of large herbivores, including predator avoidance (Estes, 1974;Jarman, 1974) and exploitation of spa- tially concentrated resources (Kreulen, 1975; McNaughton, 1988, 1990; Sinclair, 1977; Western, 1975). Hence, habitat se- lection for patches of intermediate resource abundance could lead to aggregation provided that resources are patchy in space or time (Fryxell and Sinclair, 1988; Gordon, 1988; Mad- dock, 1979). Indeed, energy-maximizing decisions by herbi- vores themselves could lead to aggregations that maintain patches at a preferred biomass level (Fryxell, 1991; Hobbs and Swift, 1988; McNaughton, 1984). Hence, understanding the adaptive basis of patch selection (Gordon and Illius, 1989), such as that demonstrated in our study, may lead to improved understanding of migratory and grouping behavior by wapiti or other generalist grazers. Understanding patterns of forage selection by herbivores in relation to energetic constraints is an important step to pre- dicting habitat use and the resulting long-term dynamics of plant-herbivore communities (Hobbs and Swift, 1985; Illius et al., 1992). Determination of these constraints should better enable us to model herbivore population dynamics effectively and also gain insight into the processes that ultimately limit and regulate population growth. We would like to thank C Olsen, S. Poluch, and C Doucet for tech- nical assistance. We also thank S. Albon, J. Atkinson, N. T. Hobbs, D. Kramer, E. Izm D. Lavigne, R. Walton, and an anonymous reviewer for comments on an earlier version of this article. Research was sup- ported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 8. 216 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 2 (NSERC) operating grant to J.M.F., Agricultural Research Council of Alberta grant to R.J.H., and a Sigma Xi research grant and Ontario Graduate and NSERC scholarships to J.F.W. REFERENCES Aebischer NJ, Robertson PA, Kenward RE, 1994. Compositional anal- ysis of habitat use from animal radio-tracking data. Ecology 74: 1315-1323. Albon SD, Langvatn R, 1992. Plant phenology and the benefits of migration in a temperate ungulate. Oikos 65302-513. Ammann AP, Cowan RL, Mothershead CL, Baumgardt BR, 1973. Dry matter and energy intake in relation to digestibility in white-tailed deer.J Wildl Manage 37:195-201. Armstrong RH, Common TC, Smith HK, 1986. The voluntary intake and in vivo digestibility of herbage harvested from indigenous hill plant communities. Grass Forage Sci 41:53-60. Association of Official Analytical Chemists (AOAC), 1980. Official methods of analysis of the Association of Official Analytical Chem- isti (Horwitz W, ed). Washington, D.C: Association of Official An- alytical Chemists. Baker DL, Hobba NT, 1987. Strategies of digestion: digestive efficiency and retention time of forage diets in montane ungulates. Can J Zool 65:1978-1984. Barton FE, Amos HE, Burdkk D, Wilson RL, 1976. Relationship of chemical analysis to in vitro digestibility for selected tropical and temperate grasses. J Anim Sci 43:505-513. Belovsky GE, 1978. Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: the moose. Theor Popul Biol 14:103-134. Belovsky GE, 1986. Optimal foraging and community structure: im- plications for a guild of generalist grassland herbivores. Oecologia 70:35-52. Blaxter KL, Wainma FW, Wilson RS, 1961. The regulation of food intake by sheep. Anim Prod 3:51-61. Boyce MS, 1989. The Jackson elk herd: intensive wildlife management in North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Brazda AR, 1953. Elk migration patterns, and some of the factors affecting movements in the Gallatin River drainage, Montana. J Wildl Manage 17:9-23. Broom DM, Arnold CW, 1986. Selection by grazing sheep of pasture plants at low herbage availability and responses of the plants to grazing. AustJ Agric Res 37327-538. Charnov EI-, 1976. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol 9:129-136. Clancy M, Bull LS, Wangness PJ, Baumgardt BR, 1976. Digestible en- ergy intake of complete diets by wethers and lactating ewes. J Anim Sci 42:961-969. Cook CW, Mattox JE. Harris LE, 1961. Comparative daily consump- tion and digestibility of summer range forage by wet and dry ewes. J Anim Sci 20:866-870. Demment MW, Van Soest PJ, 1985. A nutritional explanation for body- size patterns of ruminant and nonruminant herbivores. Am Nat 125:641-672. Duncan P, Foose TJ, Gordon 1J, Gakahu CG, LJoyde M, 1990. Com- parative nutrient extraction from forages by grazing bovids and equids: a test of the nutritional model of equid/bovid competition and coexistence. Oecologia 84:411-418. Dutilleul P, 1993. Spatial heterogeneity and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecology 74:1646-1658. Estes RD, 1974. Social organization of the African Bovidae. In: The behaviour of ungulates and its relation to management (Geist V, Walthcr F. eds). Morges, Switzerland: International Union for Con- servation of Nature and Natural Resources; 166-205. Festa-Bianchet M, 1988. Seasonal range selection in bighorn sheep: conflicts between forage quality, forage quantity and predator avoidance. Oecologia 75:580-586. Fonnesbeck PV, Christiansen JL. Harris LE, 1981. Factors affecting digestibility of nutrients by sheep. J Anim Sci 52363-376. Fryxell JM, 1991. Forage quality and aggregation by large herbivores. Am Nat 138:478-498. FryxellJM, Doucei CM, 1993. Diet choice and the functional response of beavers. Ecology 74:1297-1306. Fryxell JM, Sinclair ARE, 1988. Seasonal migration by white-eared knob in relation to resources. Afr J Ecol 26:17-31. Fryxell JM, Vamosi SM, Walton RA, Doucet CM, 1994. Retention time and the functional response of beavers. Oikos 71:207-214.. Gates C, Hudson RJ, 1978. Energy costs of locomouon in wapiti. Acta Theriol 23:365-370. Gates CC, Hudson RJ, 1983. Foraging behaviour of wapiti in a boreal forest enclosure. Nat Can 110:197-206. Gordon IJ, 1988. Facilitation of red deer grazing by cattle and its impact on red deer performance. J Appl Ecol 25:1-10. Gordon IJ, Illius AW, 1989. Resource partitioning by ungulates on the Isle of Rhum. Oecologia 79:383-389. Gross JE, Hobbs NT, Wunder BA, 1993a. Independent variables for predicting intake rate of mammalian herbivores: biomass density, plant density, or bite size? Oikos 68:75-81. Cross JE, Shipley LA, Hobbs NT, Spalinger DE, Wunder BA, 1993b. Foraging by herbivores in food-concentrated patches: tests of a mechanistic model of functional response. Ecology 74:778-791. Hebert DM, 1973. AJtitudinal migration as a factor in the nutrition of bighorn sheep (PhD dissertation). Vancouver, British Columbia: University of British Columbia. Hobbs NT, Baker DL, EllisJE, Swift DM, 1981. Composition and qual- ity of elk winter diets in Colorado. J Wildl Manage 45:156-171. Hobbs NT, Swift DM, 1985. Estimates of habitat carrying capacity in- corporating explicit nutritional constraints. J Wildl Manage 49:814- 822. Hobbs NT, Swift DM, 1988. Crazing in herds: when are nutritional benefits realized? Am Nat 131:760-764. Hudson RJ, Watkins WG, 1986. Foraging rates of wapiti on green and cured pastures. Can J Zool 64:1705-1708. Illius AW, Clark DA, Hodgson J, 1992. Discrimination and patch choice by sheep grazing grass-clover swards. J Anim Ecol 61:183- 194. Jarman PJ, 1974. The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour 48:215-266. Johnson DH, 1980. The comparison of usage and availability mea- surements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology 61:65-71. Kamstra LD, 1973. Seasonal changes in quality of some important range grasses. J Range Manage 26:289-291. KrebsJR, McQeery RH, 1984. Individual optimization in behavioural ecology. In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach (Kret» JR. Davies NB, eds). Oxford: Blackwell; 91-121. Kreulen D, 1975. Wildebeest habitat selection in the Serengeti plains, Tanzania, in relation to calcium and lactation: a preliminary report. East Afr Wildl J 13:297-304. Langvatn R, Hanley TA, 1993. Feeding-patch choice by red deer in relation to foraging efficiency: an experiment Oecologia 95:164— 170. Laycock WA, Price DA, 1970. Factors influencing forage quality. In: Range and wildlife habitat evaluation—a research symposium. U S Dep Agric Misc Publ 1147:37-47. Lewis CE, Lowrey RS, Monso WG, Knox FE, 1975. Seasonal trends in nutrients and cattle digestibility of forage on pine-wiregrass range. J Anim Sci 41:208-212. Lundberg P, 1988. Functional response of a small mammalian herbi- vore: the dijc equation revisited. J Anim Ecol 57:999—1006. Maddock L, 1979. The "migration" and grazing succession. In: Ser- engeti: dynamics of an ecosystem (Sinclair ARE, Norton-Griffiths M, eds). Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 104—129. McNaughton SJ, 1984. Grazing lawns: animals in herds, plant form. and convolution. Am Nat 124:863-886. McNaughton SJ, 1988. Mineral nutrition and spatial concentrations of African ungulates. Nature 334:343-345. McNaughton SJ, 1990. Mineral nutrition and seasonal movements of African ungulates. Nature 345:613-615. Mould ED, Robbins CT, 1982. Digestive capabilities in elk compared to white-tailed deer. J Wild! Manage 46:22-29. Murie OJ, 1951. The elk of North America. Harrisburg, Pennsylvania: Stack,pole. NelsonJR, Leege TA, 1982. Nutritional requirements and food habits. In: Elk of North America: ecology and management (Thomas JW, Toweill DE, eds). Harrisburg, Pennsylvania: Stackpole; 323-367. Oelberg K, 1956. Factors affecting the nutritive value of range forage. J Range Manage 9:220-225. Osbourn DF, 198*). The feeding value of grass and grass products. In: Grass: its production and utilization (Holmes W. ed). Oxford. Blackwell; 89-124. atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom
  • 9. Wilmshurst et al. • Forage selection by wapiti 217 Owen-Smith N, Novellie P, 1982. What should a clever ungulate eat? Am Nat 119:151-178. Parr Instruments, 1977. ASTM standards for bomb calorimetry. Phil- adelphia: American Society for Technical Material!. Renecker LA, Hudson RJ, 1986. Seasonal foraging rate of moose in aspen boreal habitats. J Wild] Manage 50:143-147. Robbins CT, 1983. Wildlife feeding and nutrition. Orlando: Academic Press. Royama T, 1970. Factors governing the hunting behaviour and selec- tion of food by the great tit (Parus major l_). J Anim Ecol 39:619— 668. Shipley LA, Gross JE, Spalinger DE, Hobbs NT, Wunder BA, 1994. The scaling of intake rate in mammalian herbivores. Am Nat 143: 1055-1082. Shipley LA, Spalinger DE, 1992. Mechanics of browsing in dense food patches; effects of plant and animal morphology on intake rate. CanJ Zool 70:1743-1752. Short J, 1985. The functional response of kangaroos, sheep and rab- biu in an arid grazing system. J Appl Ecol 22:435-447. Simons S, Alcock J, 1971. Learning and the foraging persistence of white-crowned sparrows ZonotriMa leucophrjs. Ibis 113:477-482. Sinclair ARE, 1977. The African buffalo. Chicago: University of Chi- cago Press. Smith JNM, Dawkins R, 1971. The hunting behaviour of individual great tits in relation to spatial variation in their food density. Anim Behav 19:695-706. Spalinger DE, Hanley TA, Robbins CT, 1988. Analysts of the function- al response in foraging in the Sitka black-tailed deer. Ecology 69: 1166-1175. Spalinger DE, Hobbs NT, 1992. Mechanisms of foraging in mamma- lian herbivores; new models of functional response. Am Nat 140: 325-348. Spalinger DE, Robbins CT, Hanley TA, 1986. The assessment of han- dling time in ruminants: the effect of plant chemical and physical structure on the rate of breakdown of plant particles in the rumen of mule deer and elk. CanJ Zool 64:312-321. Stephens DW, KrebsJR, 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton. NewJersey: Princeton University Press. VanDyne CM, Heady HF, 1965. Dietary chemical composition of cattle and sheep grazing in common on a dry annual range. J Range Manage 18:78-86. Van Soest PJ, 1982. Nutritional ecology of the ruminant. Corvallis, Oregon: O and B Books. Van Soest PJ, Mertens DR, Deinum B, 1978. Preharvest factors influ- encing quality of conserved forage. J Anim Sci 47:712-720. Van Soest PJ, Robertson JB, 1980. Systems of analysis for evaluating fibrous feeds. In: Standardization of analytical methodology for feeds, Ottawa, Canada, 12-14 March 1979 (Pigden WJ, Balch CC, Craham M, eds). Ottawa; IDRC-134e; 49-60. Van Soest PJ, Wine RH, 1967. Use of detergents in the analysis of fibrous feeds. IV. Determination of plant cell wall constituents. J Assoc Off Anal Chem 5CK50-55. Wairimu S, Hudson RJ, 199S. Compensatory foraging behaviour of young wapiti stags. Appl Anim Behav Sci 36:65-79. Waite R, 1963. Botanical and chemical changes in maturing grass and their effect on its digestibility. Agric Prog 38:50-56. Ward AL, Cupal JJ, Lea AL, Oakley CA, Weeks RW, 1973. Elk behav- iour in relation to rattle grazing, forest recreation and traffic. In: Thirty-eighth North American wildlife and natural resources con- ference. Washington. D.C, 18-21 March 1973 (Trefethen JB, ed). Washington, D.C: Wildlife Management Institute; 327-337. Western D, 1975. Water availability and its influence on the structure and dynamics of a savannah large mammal community. East Afr WildlJ 13:265-286. White RC, 1983. Foraging patterns and their multiplier effects on productivity of northern ungulates. Oikos 40:377-384. Wickstrom ML, Robbins CT, Hanley TA, Spalinge DE, Parish SM, 1984. Food intake and foraging energetics of elk and mule deer. J Wild] Manage 48:1285-1301. Wilson JR, Taylor AO, Dolby CR, 1976. Temperature and atmospheric humidity effects on cell wall content and associated changes in dry matter digestibility of some tropical and temperate grasses. N Z J Agric Res 19:41-46. atUniversityofManitobaonJanuary11,2011beheco.oxfordjournals.orgDownloadedfrom