SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Running head: DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 1
To help or not to help? The role of disability stigma and demand
Elena R. Torry-Schrag
Macalester College
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 2
Abstract
This paper examines two factors hypothesized to influence willingness to help people with
disabilities: Attitude toward disability and perceived capacity of the helper. Stigma (presence or
absence of disability) and demand of help were manipulated in the context of a vignette.
Participants read the vignette and were asked to rate their likelihood of helping. They also
completed several measures of attitude toward disability. A significant main effect of demand
and a significant main effect of stigma were found, such that participants were more willing to
help in low demand scenarios, and also in high stigma scenarios. Additionally, there was a
significant difference of competence ratings between the groups “students at your college” and
“people with physical disabilities,” such that “people with physical disabilities” were rated as
significantly less competent than “students at your college.” Implications and future areas of
research are discussed.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 3
To help or not to help? The role of disability stigma and demand
Because people with disabilities have historically been excluded from mainstream society
through institutionalization and “special,” segregated schools (Linton, 1998), scholars and
activists within the domain of disability often focus on the social exclusion of people with
disabilities (Wendell, 1996). Lack of resources in education and an absence of job opportunities
continue to marginalize a large group of American citizens, even after the passage of legislation
like the Americans with Disabilities Act in 1990 (Neufeldt, 1995; Wendell, 1996). Cultural
views that disability should be a private matter furthers the isolation of people with disabilities,
forcing the experience of having a disability “underground” (Wendell, 1996, p. 60).
Other cultural values also influence attitudes toward disability. For instance, MacGregor
(2012) argues that the “independent individual” is a fundamental element of capitalist societies,
shaping not only the labor market but wider expectations for behavior (para 16). It sets
expectations for what constitutes “work,” and what type of people (read: bodies) are capable of
working. Restriction from the workplace based on able-bodied standards projects the idea that
people with disabilities are a burden on the larger society. This invalidates their identity as an
“active citizen,” removing all autonomy from the disabled person (para. 4).
American culture places a premium on independence, with “expectations of individual
productivity” in the workplace and everyday life (Wendell, 1996, p. 59). The cultural archetype
of the “self-made man” who achieves success through pulling himself up by his bootstraps is one
example of the value placed on autonomy and self reliance. This high value on independence
masks the reality that all people require some level of assistance (MacGregor, 2012; Wendell,
1996). However, by casting certain types of assistance as “entitlements” rather than “help,”
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 4
people who require additional help beyond the entitlement level are categorized outside the norm
of independence, creating an ​us/them dichotomy between nondisabled and disabled individuals
(Dreger, 2004; Wendell, 1996).
Indeed, research suggests that people with disabilities are widely characterized as needing
help. Rohmer and Louvet (2012) measured implicit stereotypes associated with people with
disabilities, and found they were judged to be less competent and less warm than nondisabled
people. Low competence ratings suggest that people with disabilities may be stereotyped as
relying on others and needing assistance. Furthermore, Nario-Redmond (2010) found that
disability stereotypes are consistent across type of disability. According to her research, people
with disabilities are most commonly characterized as “dependent,” “weak,” and “incompetent”
(p. 479), indicating that disabled people are widely and pervasively thought of in terms of a need
for assistance. The characterization of people with disabilities as weak and incompetent,
undesirable traits within the context of American individualism, contributed to their
marginalization by nondisabled people, including through such historically common acts as
institutionalization and forced sterilization (Crissey, 1975; Wehmeyer, 2003). While the
marginalization of disabled people may look different today, the ability of people with
disabilities to participate in all aspects of society is still highly limited, and should therefore be
an area of great concern. Research in this area may also prompt action on behalf of people with
disabilities to counteract the negative effects of social isolation and discrimination.
Social movements in the United States, including those on behalf of disabled people,
have historically relied on intergroup alliances, with non-marginalized individuals working to
end prejudice in personal and professional areas of the lives of marginalized groups (Brown &
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 5
Ostrove, 2013). Allies in the domain of disability are defined by Gil (2001) as people who “make
the effort to learn who their disabled associates are in their full glory and their full ordinariness,”
or “those people who just seem to “get it” (p. 368). Gil argues that allies can promote better
attitudes towards people with disabilities, which could be an important step in ameliorating the
social isolation of disabled people. However, little research has been done to illuminate barriers
that may prevent nondisabled people from becoming allies to people with physical disabilities.
This is an area worth exploring, because “interactions between majority and minority group
members are often fraught with uncertainty and misunderstanding,” (Wang, Silverman, Gwin, &
Dovidio, 2015, 491). Lack of contact between people with and without disabilities means that
nondisabled people may lack social scripts to guide their behavior, which could cause hesitation
and anxiety. Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) argue that the question of “how and when”
assistance should be provided or offered to people with disabilities is one with which
non-disabled people often struggle, “especially when interacting with strangers or acquaintances
who are disabled” (p. 3). It could be that nondisabled people perceive people with disabilities as
needing help, but are uncertain of their ability to help and the appropriateness of offering
assistance. This suggests that “help” as it relates to allyship in the context of disability is an
important area of inquiry.
As a result of the American premium on independence, there is a cultural norm to “let
others stand on their own two feet” (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 4). Deviation from that
norm can elicit strong and instantaneous negative reactions, such as “behavioral aversion”
(Dovidio, Pagotto, & Hebl, 2011, p. 159). If nondisabled individuals characterize disabled people
in terms of their need for help and additionally view needing help as a norm deviation, it is likely
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 6
that they would have negative reactions towards people with disabilities, including the desire to
avoid them. The context in which these evaluations occur are also influential. According to
Goffman (1963), social settings set expectations for “categories of persons likely to be
encountered there” (p. 256). Strangers in that setting are evaluated by their appearance, which
facilitates anticipation of their attributes and social identity. Because the built environment was
created by and for able-bodied people, people with disabilities may be more noticeable, and
seemingly out of place (Wendell, 1996). Their appearance as physically different from the
normative category of people in the environment may lead to evaluations of dependency, and the
need for help. According to Goffman, stigma symbols are “signs which are especially effective
in drawing attention to a debasing identity” (p. 44). It is possible, then, that the presence of a
wheelchair may activate stigmatized notions of people with disabilities. Goffman refers to
individuals whose stigmatized identity is evident “on the spot” as “the discredited” (p. 4). This
seems particularly apt for the domain of disability, where people with physical disabilities are
often seen as lacking the necessary credentials for autonomy and control. Goffman further states
that attitudes towards stigmatized individuals directly influence our actions toward them. He
offers the example of a nondisabled person desiring to avoid helping a blind person as just one
manifestation of stigma-influenced behavior.
Piliavin, Piliavin, and Rodin (1975) empirically investigated the effect of stigma on
willingness to help. They predicted that negative attitudes elicited by a physical stigma would
override the tendency to help someone in need. In their study, an actor with a “facial
disfigurement” (p. 432) rode on a subway car, and suddenly fell to the ground. Confederates
posing as subway riders tracked and coded helping behavior, and measured the length of time
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 7
before the actor was helped, either with physical touch or a verbal inquiry. Willingness to help
differed significantly by stigma: Unmarked victims received significantly more help than
“stigmatized victims” (p. 433). While this study did not specifically look at physical disability so
much as physical “disfigurement” (p. 432), the global attributions made toward people of across
disability (Nario-Redmond, 2010) suggests that stigma effects may also be generalizable to other
disability-related stigmas. This would suggest that the stigma of disability does cause
nondisabled individuals to avoid people with disabilities. It is therefore possible that nondisabled
people may be averse to forming alliances with people with disabilities because of unfavorable
attitudes related to helping stigmatized individuals.
Soble and Strickland (1974) also examined the effect of stigma on willingness to help.
They argued that interactions with stigmatized individuals causes unpleasant affective reactions
in non-stigmatized individuals. They further postulated that non-stigmatized individuals shape
their lives and routines to avoid contact with stigmatized individuals, to avoid feelings of
discomfort. These claims were tested in a study in which a confederate went door-to-door and
distributed a survey to participants and attempted to arrange a follow-up interview. In half of the
trials she appeared physically “normal,” and in the other half had an obvious physical disability.
Participants were significantly more likely to agree to an interview when her appearance was
“normal,” demonstrating that the presence physical disability triggers the desire to avoid
interactions with the stigmatized individual. Following the line of this study, nondisabled people
may be in opposition to the idea of forming an alliance with disabled individuals out of a desire
to avoid the feelings and attitudes elicited by the presence of disability.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 8
Conversely, there is another line of research that suggests that stigma towards people
with disabilities may work in a different manner, towards over-helping. The perception that
people with disabilities are low in capacity often leads to assumptions of helplessness (Rohmer
& Louvet, 2012). People who subscribe to these assumptions may be ​more inclined to help,
acting on the belief that the disabled person won’t be able to complete the task without
assistance. Supporting this conjecture, there is a robust body of research that indicates people
with disabilities “often receive more help than they would like or need” (Braithwaite & Eckstein,
2003, p. 3). Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) found this to be true even when the cost of helping
was high, such as the physically demanding task of pushing a wheelchair. In fact, some
participants who used wheelchairs reported nondisabled people taking control of their wheelchair
even when they explicitly refused the offer of assistance (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). The
pattern of nondisabled people assisting in the face of refusal suggests that the belief that people
with disabilities are incapable extends even to the disabled person’s perceptions of needing help.
Wang et al. (2015) believe these instances of unsolicited help arise from evaluations of disabled
people as low in competence, but high in warmth. Perceptions of warmth facilitate over-helping,
by framing the stigma of incompetence as something pitiable, rather than wholly aversive.
Doob and Ecker (1970) evaluated the effect of disability on helping behavior in a study
similar to that of Soble and Strickland (1974). They argued that feelings of sympathy for
disabled people would elicit greater compliance with requests for help. Confederates went
door-to-door asking whoever answered the door to complete a questionnaire. Half the
confederates wore an eye-patch, while the other half did not. Participants were significantly more
likely to accept the questionnaire when the confederate had an eyepatch, suggesting a stronger
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 9
desire to help disabled people than nondisabled people. Due to the global nature of disability
stereotypes, it is possible that this same pattern would emerge for people with more severe
physical disabilities (Nario-Redmond, 2010).
Slochower, Wein, Firstenberg, and DiGuilio (1980) tested just that. The authors criticized
previous studies for having a weak disability manipulation, and suggested that a more severe
disability would arouse high levels of anxiety and therefore decrease willingness to help.
Participants, students at an urban college, were approached by a confederate who either used a
wheelchair or did not. The confederates asked participants to donate to the March of Dimes, and
donations were counted by two researchers who observed the interactions unobtrusively.
Donation results revealed that confederates in wheelchairs received significantly more donations
than non-wheelchair confederates. While donating money is not a physically demanding task, if
disability arouses the same attitudes of helplessness and pity, the same results may be seen in
higher demand scenarios.
Other research suggests that the potential helper’s beliefs about their personal capability
are more influential than attitudes when deciding whether to help someone. Many people with
physical disabilities have a need for instrumental help. This type of help is defined as “something
active or physical to help the seeker” (Barbee & Cunningham, 1994, cited in Braithwaite &
Eckstein, 2003, p. 3), and may be a barrier to forming alliances, depending on the perceived
capability of the individual helping. Ajzen and Madden (1986) investigated the effect of
perceived behavioral control on predicted future behavior. They found that the more participants
believed they had control over the situation, the more likely they were to say they would
complete a particular behavior. Perceived control may be a proxy for perceived capability, since
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 10
requisite knowledge, abilities, and skills were identified as resources needed for control. The
perception of lacking these factors with regard to helping a disabled person would decrease
perceived behavioral control, and subsequently, the likelihood of engaging in helping behavior.
Due to the social isolation of people with disabilities, it is likely that many nondisabled people
feel they lack the resources to succeed in a situation where they would be required to provide
help to a person with a disability. Feeling disempowered or unable to provide physical assistance
to disabled people would therefore decrease the likelihood of offering assistance and, more
broadly, pursuing an ally relationship with them.
Research concerning barriers to bystander intervention with regards to sexual violence
offers support for this position. Bennett, Banyard, and Garnhart (2014) surveyed participants
about barriers to providing help in situations where sexual assault or rape may occur. One barrier
identified was “failure to intervene due to skills deficit” (p. 486). This interpersonal factor was
described as a “lack of confidence in ability to help,” (p. 487) and was mentioned by the highest
percentage of participants. Overall, participants who perceived greater barriers were less likely to
report engaging in helping behavior. The domains of sexual violence and disability may at first
seem unrelated; however, the shared requirement of physical intervention/assistance makes the
results of Bennett et al.’s research applicable to the present study.
Existing studies have measured behavior, but not the underlying beliefs about personal
capability and attitudes that may motivate them. The present study seeks to examine which is
more influential when it comes to making the decision to help a person with a physical disability:
attitudes toward the need for help or the potential helper’s beliefs about their capacity to help a
disabled person. Although willingness to help is not exactly transferable to “ally” behavior, a
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 11
main characteristic that has been found to separate “allies” from low-prejudiced individuals is
willingness to offer support (Brown & Ostrove, 2013). Therefore, willingness to offer support,
sometimes literal and physical, could be a foundation to forming an alliance across ability.
Participants read one of four vignettes that vary by demand (high and low) and perceived
stigma (high and low) and then rated their willingness to help in that situation. If attitudes
towards people with disabilities are more influential when making the decision to help a person
with a disability, there are two divergent possibilities. If participants have a distancing attitude
toward disability, then they will be more willing to help in low stigma situations, regardless of
demand. An over-helping attitude, however, would cause participants to help more when stigma
is high, regardless of demand. Alternatively, if the potential helper’s perceived personal
capability is more dominant in this decision-making process, then participants will be more
willing to help in low demand situations than in high demand ones, regardless of stigma.
Method
Participants
One hundred sixty-five participants (94 females, 28 males, 2 otherwise-identifying) were
recruited through social media (Facebook), email, and word of mouth. Of those participants, 124
provided demographic data (94 females, 28 males, 2 otherwise-identifying, M age = 25.38, age
range: 18-63). A majority of participants (n = 104) identified as white. Other racial/ethnic
identities included biracial/multiracial (9), Latin@ (6), African American/Black (2), Asian/Asian
American (1), and Other (2). Participants were not given any form of compensation for their
participation.
Procedure
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 12
Participants followed a link either from a Facebook post or email to an online survey
created through the Qualtrics platform. After reading a consent form, participants clicked “I
agree” to indicate that they were 18 years or older and had read the consent form. On the next
page, they were asked to imagine they were riding a city bus. They then read one of four
vignettes and rated the likelihood (on a Likert scale of 1-6) that they would help in that scenario.
In the low demand scenario, an individual on the bus announces that they need one more
signature on their petition. In the high demand scenario, they observed someone on the bus
struggling with grocery bags, attempting to get money out to pay the bus fare. These scenarios
also varied by level of perceived stigma, with high perceived stigma relating to disability
(petition for more jobs for people with disabilities/person struggling with groceries uses a
wheelchair) and low perceived stigma not relating to disability (petition for student loan
forgiveness/person struggling with groceries is able-bodied) (See Appendix A). After reading the
vignette, they completed measures of attitude toward disability. Finally, they were asked to think
back to the scenario they had read and rate on a Likert scale of 1-6 how much they could have
helped, regardless of whether they wanted to. Participants were also given the option to explain
their rating in an open-ended response box. After clicking the next button, they were directed to a
page where they entered demographic information (age in years, gender identity, ethnicity, and
whether or not they identified as having a disability). Demographics were collected by text entry,
so participants were not limited to a set list of choices, except for disability identity, which was a
“yes/no” choice. They then clicked the next button again, which took them to the debriefing
form, outlining the true nature of the study.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 13
Randomization procedures were put in place such that an equal number of participants
read and responded to each vignette.
Design
A 2 x 2 factorial design was used in which stigma (high vs low) and demand (high vs
low) were both between-subjects variables. The dependent variable was likelihood to help, which
was measured using a Likert scale from 1 (Very Unlikely) to 6 (Very Likely).
Measures
Attitude toward disability was assessed using three measures. First, participants
completed an adapted version of the feeling thermometer (Converse & Presser, 1986).
Participants feelings towards different groups, including “people with physical disabilities,” were
measured via warmth ratings ranging from 0 (very cold, very unfavorable) to 100 (very warm,
very favorable). Examples of questions include: “How warm do you feel toward other
Macalester Students?” and “How warm do you feel toward members of the Democratic Party?”
This type of measure has been used in research regarding attitudes towards various social groups,
including racial outgroups (Williamson, Bishop, & Hood, 2014) and LGBT individuals (Haddok,
Zanna, & Esses, 1993), as well as research in the field of intergroup contact (Shook, Hopkins, &
Koech, 2016).
Participants also rated how competent they perceived the same list of groups as in the
feelings thermometer measure. Attitude towards out-groups is often assessed along the two
dimensions of warmth and competence (e.g. Rohmer & Louvet, 2012). Furthermore, it has been
shown that while warmth ratings can increase as a result of exposure to the perspective of
disabled people, competence ratings remain low, resulting in an overall negative evaluation of
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 14
the individual (Silverman, Gwinn, & Van Boven, 2015). Therefore, it is important not to conflate
a high rating of warmth with a positive attitude toward people with individuals, as it may not
coincide with a high rating of competence. Comparing the two ratings will give a holistic view of
participants’ attitudes.
Finally, participants completed an adapted measure of individualism (Triandis &
Gelfand, 1998) to assess attitudes toward independence/self-reliance. Participants rated the
extent to which they agreed with nine statements (three from the original measure and six created
for this study) regarding individualistic attitudes from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree).
Examples of statements include: “I’d rather depend on myself than others,” and “It is important
to me that I can do things on my own,” and “It’s better to do things on your own than ask for
help” (See Appendix B).​ ​According to Triandis and Gelfand (1998), the reliability of the original
scale was found to be good (Cronbach’s α = .67), but it has been substantially modified for the
current study. Internal consistency for the current measure was found to be good (Cronbach’s α =
.78).
Perceived capability was measured by asking participants to reflect on the situation they
had previously read. They were asked to rate how much they think they could have helped,
regardless of whether they wanted to, on a Likert scale from 1 (Unable) to 6 (Completely able).
This was followed by an open-ended “Explain your rating” response box.
Results
A two-way analysis of variance assessing the effect of demand and stigma on willingness
to help indicated a main effect of demand, ​F(1, 154) = 5.91, ​p < .05, such that willingness to help
was higher in low demand scenarios (​M = 4.32, ​SD = .173), than in high demand scenarios (​M =
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 15
3.75, ​SD = .158). The main effect of stigma was also significant, ​F(1,154) = 25.58, ​p < .05, such
that participants were more likely to help in disability-related scenarios (​M = 4.63, SD = .173)
than nondisabled targets (​M = 3.45, SD = .158). The interaction effect was nonsignificant, ​F(1,
154) = .967, ​p > .05 (see Figure 1).
Follow-up paired t-tests examined participants’ attitudes toward people with disabilities
by comparing their warmth and competence ratings of “people with physical disabilities”
compared to their in-group of “students at your college. There were no significant differences in
participants’ warmth ratings of “people with physical disabilities” (​M = 72.02) and “students at
your college,” (​M = 73.52, ​t[142] = -.872, n.s.) However, there was a significant difference in
participants’ competence ratings. “People with physical disabilities” (​M = 67.16) were rated
significantly less competent than “students at your college” (​M = 75.44, ​t[132] = -4.97, ​p < .05).
[see Figure 2]
Discussion
This study sought to evaluate the effects of attitudes toward the need for help and the
potential helper’s beliefs about their capacity on willingness to help. If participants had a
distancing attitude toward disability, then they would have been more willing to help in low
stigma situations, regardless of demand. An over-helping attitude, however, would have caused
participants to help more when stigma is high, regardless of demand. Alternatively, if the
potential helper’s perceived personal capability was more dominant in this decision-making
process, then participants would have been more willing to help in low demand situations than in
high demand ones, regardless of stigma.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 16
The results support the over-helping hypothesis. Participants were more likely to help in
high stigma situations, even when demand was high. Current research supports the phenomenon
of over-helping (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003; Wang et al., 2015). Researchers have
hypothesized that this trend exists because people with disabilities are seen as requiring outside
assistance. Supporting this, the low competence ratings for “People with physical disabilities”
compared with “Students at your college” suggests that people with disabilities were indeed
perceived as needing help. It is possible that the increased helping in high stigma scenarios was
caused by the perception that people with disabilities are incompetent and dependent on
able-bodied people intervening. People with disabilities often received unwanted assistance
because they are perceived as incompetent, even when they explicitly refuse the offer
(Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). The response of one participant offers anecdotal support for this
interpretation:
Possibly, this question was aimed mostly at people who don't use wheelchairs. I happen
to use a wheelchair myself. I might hold the groceries for the person or otherwise assist
them. However, if I did I would ask first. As a wheelchair user (I hate the expression
"wheelchair bound"), I am often offered help and sometimes given it without asking,
even for very simple tasks, where 90% of the time help is not needed or wanted. So I am
sensitive to that. The person might prefer to get their own stuff. By the way, many
wheelchair users are able to help (in a physical way) both other people with disabilities
and without. For example, I can hold the door for someone, pick something up, etc...but
wheelchair users are usually seen as not capable of much, physically and sometimes
mentally.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 17
There was no difference in warmth ratings between “People with physical disabilities”
compared with “Students at your college,” however, which suggests participants’ attitude toward
disability and the need for help is characterized more by pity, rather than aversion. The “warm
glow” hypothesis of assistance, which suggests that people feel good about helping others, may
also be at play here (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 5). Participant responses offer partial
support for a “warm glow” effect. Many participants reported that helping others was important
to them, citing a “sense of duty.” One participant specifically mentioned people with disabilities
as a population they publically support. While on the face of it, a desire to help people with
disabilities may seem beneficial, the way in which help is given has serious implications. Help
can reinforce stereotypes of people with disabilities as fully dependent, and puts the helper in a
“one-up” position (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). Unwanted help can also lower the self-esteem
of disabled people, since it implies they are incapable of functioning on their own (Wang et al.,
2014).
A major limitation of this study was the setting of the vignette. In reviewing participants’
written responses about their capacity to help, it appeared the scenario itself had an unintended
effect on behavior. Many participants explained their willingness to help directly in the context
of being on a public bus. For example, one participant wrote “The reason why I choose that I
would unlikely help the person is because normally sometimes people feel very defensive while
riding the bus.” Other participants gave similar responses, stating that their seating position on
the bus (e.g. close to the front vs in the back) would have influenced their behavior. In a less
ambiguous scenario it is possible that helping behavior would have been different. However,
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 18
based on the responses, it seems as if helping behavior would only ​increase, which does not
invalidate the current findings.
Future research could approach the question of help from the perspective of people with
disabilities, a strategy that is decidedly lacking in the literature (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003).
Many scholars have written about the importance of including people with disabilities in the
research process, transforming their role from subject to researcher (Kitchin, 2000; Stefánsdóttir,
& Traustadóttir, 2015). ​Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) claim that a majority of the research
regarding social support has focused on the providers of support, rather than the recipients, and
argue for a change. Additionally, research that has been conducted from the point of view of
people with disabilities has focused on their ​reactions to help, as opposed to how people with
disabilities take an active role in obtaining needed help (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003; Wang et
al., 2014). Questions could include an inquiry into the types of help that are considered
appropriate, as well as ​how people with disabilities would prefer to be helped​. With this
information, nondisabled people could be better equipped to help people with disabilities in an
appropriate way, which could lay the foundations for an effective ally relationship.
Future research should also evaluate other “ally behaviors.” As this study demonstrates,
willingness to help is not equivalent to being an ally, if accompanied with negative attitudes
surrounding disability. Research investigating desired characteristics of white allies from the
perspectives of people of color identified qualities that fall into two areas, “informed action” and
“affirmation” (Brown & Ostrove, 2013, p. 2216). Respectively, these include qualities such as
“My friend takes action against bias among his or her own racial/ethnic group,” and “My friend
is respectful toward me” (Brown & Ostrove, 2013, p. 2216). People with disabilities may desire
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 19
these same qualities in able-bodied allies, have a completely different set of desired qualities, or
want a combination of the two. Willingness to help may still be a foundational aspect of forming
an alliance, but other factors may be equally or more important and should be identified to assist
nondisabled people in forming a holistic ally relationship with disabled people.
A third focus of future research should be on changing perceptions of incompetence of
disabled people. Researchers could evaluate different methods of information transfer such as
media portrayals of disabled people, intergroup contact, or a combination of various methods.
Intergroup contact is a common method used to change negative attitudes of outgroup members.
While it has been successful in decreasing anxiety and disconfirming stereotypes in some cases
(Plant, 2004), intergroup contact can also lead to the creation of “subtypes,” categories of
individuals within stereotyped groups who are seen as the exception to the norm (Dunn 2015).
Therefore, more research is needed to determine what factors cause intergroup contact to be
effective in challenging stereotypic beliefs. Changing perceptions of incompetence is of vital
importance if relationships between people with and without disabilities are to transform from
helper-helpee dynamics to that of an alliance. More broadly, viewing people with disabilities as
competent, capable individuals could lead to larger social changes, such as improving
accessibility of public spaces. Wendell (1996) argues that “the public presence of people with
disabilities has many potential benefits,” (p. 63), including increasing understanding of disabled
people, which in turn lessens negative attitudes toward disability.
To date, there is an absence of literature concerning alliances between people with and
without disabilities. The present study seeks to ameliorate that absence by exploring a central
component of interactions between disabled and nondisabled individuals: offers of help. The
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 20
results of this study suggest that people with disabilities do not suffer from a lack of help as a
result of stigma, but rather an abundance of help, perhaps unwanted. Additionally, this study
builds on previous literature in establishing that people with disabilities are viewed as
comparably warm as in-group members, but less competent. Views of low competence have
serious implications for the lives of disabled people, since views of the dominant group
(nondisabled people) determine the treatment of people with disabilities, including their access to
public spaces and resources. By exploring factors that influence desire to form alliances with
people with disabilities, we can work to combat prevailing attitudes that isolate disabled people
and keep them from reaching their full potential.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 21
References
Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and
perceived behavioral control. ​Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, ​22, 453-474.
doi:10.1016/0022-1031(86)90045-4
Bennett, S., Banyard, V. L., & Garnhart, L. (2014). To act or not to act, that is the question?
Barriers and facilitators of bystander intervention. ​Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
29(3), 476-496. doi:10.1177/0886260513505210
Braithwaite, D. O., & Eckstein, N. J. (2003). How people with disabilities communicatively
manage assistance: Helping as instrumental social support. ​Journal of Applied
Communication Research, ​31, 1-26. doi:10.1080/00909880305374
Brown, K. T., & Ostrove, J. M. (2013). What does it mean to be an ally?: The perception of
allies from the perspective of people of color. ​Journal of Applied Social Psychology, ​43,
2211-2222. doi:10.1111/jasp.12172
Converse, J. M., & Presser, S. (1986). S​urvey questions: Handcrafting the standardized
questionnaire. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Doob, A. N., & Ecker, B. P. (1970). Stigma and compliance. ​Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, ​14, 302-304. doi:10.1037/h0028996
Dovidio, J. F., Pagotto, L., & Hebl, M. R. (2011). Implicit attitudes and discrimination against
people with physical disabilities. In R. L. Wiener, S. L. Willborn, R. L. Wiener, S. L.
Willborn (Eds.), ​Disability and aging discrimination: Perspectives in law and psychology
(pp. 157-183). New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media.
doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-6293-5_9
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 22
Dunn, D. S. (2015). ​The social psychology of disability. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Gil, C. (2001). Divided understandings: The social experience of disability. In G.L. Albrecht,
K.D. Seelman, & M. Bury (Eds.), ​Handbook of disability studies (351-372). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P., &amp; Esses, V. M. (1993). Assessing the structure of prejudicial
attitudes: The case of attitudes toward homosexuals. ​Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 65, 1105-1118.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1105
Linton, S. (1998).​ Claiming disability: Knowledge and identity. NY: NYU Press.
MacGregor, M. (2012). Citizenship in name only: Constructing meaningful citizenship
through a recalibration of the values attached to waged labor. ​Disability Studies
Quarterly, ​32.
Nario-Redmond, M. R. (2010). Cultural stereotypes of disabled and non-disabled men and
women: Consensus for global category representations and diagnostic domains. ​British
Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 471-488.
Piliavin, I. M., Piliavin, J. A., & Rodin, J. (1975). Costs, diffusion, and the stigmatized victim.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ​32, 429-438. doi:10.1037/h0077092
Plant, E. A. (2004). Responses to interracial interactions over time. P​ersonality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1458-1471. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264244
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 23
Rohmer, O., & Louvet, E. (2012). Implicit measures of the stereotype content associated with
disability. ​British Journal of Social Psychology, ​51, 732-740.
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02087.x
Slochower, J., Wein, L., Firstenberg, S., & DiGuilio, J. (1980). Severe physical handicaps and
helping behavior. ​The Journal of Social Psychology, 112, 313-314.
doi:10.1080/00224545.1980.9924338
Soble, S. L., & Strickland, L. H. (1974). Physical stigma, interaction, and compliance. ​Bulletin of
the Psychonomic Society, ​4, 130-132.
Triandis, Harry C., & Gelfand, Michele J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and
vertical individualism and collectivism. ​Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74, 118-128. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118
Wang, K., Silverman, A., Gwinn, J. D., & Dovidio, J. F. (2015). Independent or ungrateful?
Consequences of confronting patronizing help for people with disabilities. ​Group
Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 489-503.
Wendell, S. (1996). ​The rejected body: Feminist philosophical reflections on disability. NY:
Routledge.
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 24
Figure 1. ​Main effect of stigma and demand, with participants more willing to help in low
demand scenarios, and high stigma scenarios
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 25
Figure 2.​ Warmth and competence ratings; there was a nonsignificant difference in
participants’ warmth ratings, but a significant difference in their competence ratings, of
“students at your college” and “people with physical disabilities”
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 26
Appendix A
Vignettes
Low demand, low stigma
Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At
the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They announce they are collecting signatures for a
petition asking for student loan forgiveness for local colleges, and that they only need one more
signature.
How likely is it that you would volunteer to sign the petition?
1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely)
Low demand, high stigma
Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At
the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They announce they are collecting signatures for a
petition asking to build a group home for people with physical disabilities in your neighborhood,
and that they only need one more signature.
How likely is it that you would volunteer to sign the petition?
1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely)
High demand, low stigma
Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At
the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They have several bags of groceries on their lap, and
are trying to hold onto the groceries while reaching for their purse.
How likely is it that you would offer to help hold their groceries while they pay the
driver?
1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely)
High demand, high stigma
Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At
the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They have several bags of groceries on their lap, and
are trying to hold onto the groceries while reaching for their purse, which is hanging from the
back of their wheelchair.
How likely is it that you would offer to help hold their groceries while they pay the
driver?
1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely)
DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 27
Appendix B
Measurement Scale for Individualism
Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements
1 (Strongly Disagree) 2 3 4 5 6 (Strongly Agree)
I’d rather depend on myself than others
It’s better to work with others than alone
I rely on myself most of the time
I prefer to do things with others, rather than on my own
It is important to me that I can do things on my own
I rely on others most of the time
It’s better to do things on your own than ask for help
I will ask for help if I need it
I like to act and complete tasks independently

More Related Content

What's hot

Deconstructing Microaggressions
Deconstructing MicroaggressionsDeconstructing Microaggressions
Deconstructing Microaggressions
jpiperato
 
Prejudice and stereotyping
Prejudice and stereotypingPrejudice and stereotyping
Prejudice and stereotyping
Tayyab Hussain
 
Social rejection
Social rejectionSocial rejection
Social rejection
Domina Petric
 
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycultDiversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
nand15
 
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayananStamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
Kesho Conference
 
SOC 3000 Disability Theory
SOC 3000 Disability TheorySOC 3000 Disability Theory
SOC 3000 Disability Theory
Holly Ferrin
 
Abnormality Among Normality
Abnormality Among NormalityAbnormality Among Normality
Abnormality Among Normality
Holly Ferrin
 
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They? My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
Steve Brown
 
11 Article excerpts
11 Article excerpts11 Article excerpts
11 Article excerpts
Michael Sayre
 
Disabilities
DisabilitiesDisabilities
Disabilities
CareInvalids
 
Resilience
ResilienceResilience
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
Lisa Mears
 
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared ResourceSociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
sociologyexchange.co.uk
 
Final paper sociology
Final paper sociologyFinal paper sociology
Final paper sociology
Mechelle Davidson
 
Having less, giving more
Having less, giving moreHaving less, giving more
Having less, giving more
Whinda Y. Sabri
 
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity
 
Senior Project Final Paper
Senior Project Final PaperSenior Project Final Paper
Senior Project Final Paper
guest1d9146
 
Digification (1)
Digification (1)Digification (1)
Digification (1)
KseniiaNazarova
 
Multiracial theory presentation
Multiracial theory presentationMultiracial theory presentation
Multiracial theory presentation
Judy Juarez
 

What's hot (19)

Deconstructing Microaggressions
Deconstructing MicroaggressionsDeconstructing Microaggressions
Deconstructing Microaggressions
 
Prejudice and stereotyping
Prejudice and stereotypingPrejudice and stereotyping
Prejudice and stereotyping
 
Social rejection
Social rejectionSocial rejection
Social rejection
 
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycultDiversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
Diversity presentationlearner’s namecapella universitycult
 
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayananStamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
Stamp out stigma in kenya by vijay narayanan
 
SOC 3000 Disability Theory
SOC 3000 Disability TheorySOC 3000 Disability Theory
SOC 3000 Disability Theory
 
Abnormality Among Normality
Abnormality Among NormalityAbnormality Among Normality
Abnormality Among Normality
 
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They? My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
My Friends Can Call me "A Crip"-Do They?
 
11 Article excerpts
11 Article excerpts11 Article excerpts
11 Article excerpts
 
Disabilities
DisabilitiesDisabilities
Disabilities
 
Resilience
ResilienceResilience
Resilience
 
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
Bias Presentation Extra (6 Jul 15)
 
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared ResourceSociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
SociologyExchange.co.uk Shared Resource
 
Final paper sociology
Final paper sociologyFinal paper sociology
Final paper sociology
 
Having less, giving more
Having less, giving moreHaving less, giving more
Having less, giving more
 
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
Equity and Access: Thinking Transformatively about Race, Opportunity, & Socia...
 
Senior Project Final Paper
Senior Project Final PaperSenior Project Final Paper
Senior Project Final Paper
 
Digification (1)
Digification (1)Digification (1)
Digification (1)
 
Multiracial theory presentation
Multiracial theory presentationMultiracial theory presentation
Multiracial theory presentation
 

Viewers also liked

Andy ResumeUSE
Andy ResumeUSEAndy ResumeUSE
Andy ResumeUSE
Andrew Kinder
 
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition PicturesNorth Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
Imsuk Ha
 
Derecho minero 9
Derecho minero 9Derecho minero 9
Derecho minero 9
Hernán Araya Sepúlveda
 
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitrajeComercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
Hernán Araya Sepúlveda
 
Music magazines
Music magazinesMusic magazines
Music magazines
Danwalkermediastudies
 
Derecho minero 1
Derecho minero 1Derecho minero 1
Derecho minero 1
Hernán Araya Sepúlveda
 
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
DATA4U DATA4U
 
Keshav Metals_1
Keshav Metals_1Keshav Metals_1
Keshav Metals_1
Raman Ajgaonkar
 
roldan siao cv
roldan siao cvroldan siao cv
roldan siao cv
Roldan Siao
 
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHAPPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
Richa Verma
 
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of TourismMedia Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
Sean Arthur
 
Introduction to BP debate
Introduction to BP debateIntroduction to BP debate
Introduction to BP debate
makassar muhammadiyah university
 

Viewers also liked (12)

Andy ResumeUSE
Andy ResumeUSEAndy ResumeUSE
Andy ResumeUSE
 
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition PicturesNorth Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
North Korea Political Prison Camps Exhibition Pictures
 
Derecho minero 9
Derecho minero 9Derecho minero 9
Derecho minero 9
 
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitrajeComercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
Comercio internacional la institucion juridica del arbitraje
 
Music magazines
Music magazinesMusic magazines
Music magazines
 
Derecho minero 1
Derecho minero 1Derecho minero 1
Derecho minero 1
 
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
đề Thi thử thptqg năm 2017 có đáp án 15
 
Keshav Metals_1
Keshav Metals_1Keshav Metals_1
Keshav Metals_1
 
roldan siao cv
roldan siao cvroldan siao cv
roldan siao cv
 
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHAPPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
PPT SALES TRAINING RICHA
 
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of TourismMedia Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
Media Deck - New Mexico Department of Tourism
 
Introduction to BP debate
Introduction to BP debateIntroduction to BP debate
Introduction to BP debate
 

Similar to TorrySchrag_DirectedResearchFinalManuscript

Working with Disability
Working with DisabilityWorking with Disability
Working with Disability
Ruth Barkan
 
Learning Disabilities
Learning DisabilitiesLearning Disabilities
Learning Disabilities
Tanya Williams
 
Geographies of Disability
Geographies of DisabilityGeographies of Disability
Geographies of Disability
James Hiscocks
 
Disabled in America
Disabled in AmericaDisabled in America
Disabled in America
Rob Crawford
 
The language of respect
The language of respectThe language of respect
The language of respect
Laura Chapman
 
Capitalism and Impairments
Capitalism and ImpairmentsCapitalism and Impairments
Capitalism and Impairments
CollectiveResistance
 
Disability Policy Paper
Disability Policy PaperDisability Policy Paper
Disability Policy Paper
CassidyLong1
 
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
UsamaShabir11
 
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
Hayley Gibson
 
Qualitative Research PDF
Qualitative Research PDFQualitative Research PDF
Qualitative Research PDF
santiago_jasmin
 
Susan Wendell The rejected body
Susan Wendell The rejected bodySusan Wendell The rejected body
Susan Wendell The rejected body
Ammar farooq
 
Susan Wendell
Susan Wendell Susan Wendell
Susan Wendell
Ammar farooq
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Abigail Burmeister
 
2Nd Ed Pp.docx
2Nd Ed Pp.docx2Nd Ed Pp.docx
2Nd Ed Pp.docx
write4
 
Disability and Capitalism clip
Disability and Capitalism clipDisability and Capitalism clip
Disability and Capitalism clip
Jared Wood
 
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdfPeople With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
sdfghj21
 
Questioning Normality Through Disability Studies
Questioning Normality Through Disability StudiesQuestioning Normality Through Disability Studies
Questioning Normality Through Disability Studies
Melissa Cardenas-Dow
 
Disability Representations in the Media
 Disability Representations in the Media Disability Representations in the Media
Disability Representations in the Media
Danianese Woods
 
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docxVoices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
stirlingvwriters
 
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
William Kritsonis
 

Similar to TorrySchrag_DirectedResearchFinalManuscript (20)

Working with Disability
Working with DisabilityWorking with Disability
Working with Disability
 
Learning Disabilities
Learning DisabilitiesLearning Disabilities
Learning Disabilities
 
Geographies of Disability
Geographies of DisabilityGeographies of Disability
Geographies of Disability
 
Disabled in America
Disabled in AmericaDisabled in America
Disabled in America
 
The language of respect
The language of respectThe language of respect
The language of respect
 
Capitalism and Impairments
Capitalism and ImpairmentsCapitalism and Impairments
Capitalism and Impairments
 
Disability Policy Paper
Disability Policy PaperDisability Policy Paper
Disability Policy Paper
 
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
Understanding Gender: A Journey Beyond Stereotypes*
 
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
 
Qualitative Research PDF
Qualitative Research PDFQualitative Research PDF
Qualitative Research PDF
 
Susan Wendell The rejected body
Susan Wendell The rejected bodySusan Wendell The rejected body
Susan Wendell The rejected body
 
Susan Wendell
Susan Wendell Susan Wendell
Susan Wendell
 
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of DisabledPeer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
Peer Acceptance and the Emotional Well-Being of Disabled
 
2Nd Ed Pp.docx
2Nd Ed Pp.docx2Nd Ed Pp.docx
2Nd Ed Pp.docx
 
Disability and Capitalism clip
Disability and Capitalism clipDisability and Capitalism clip
Disability and Capitalism clip
 
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdfPeople With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
People With A Mental Disability Or Handicap Are.pdf
 
Questioning Normality Through Disability Studies
Questioning Normality Through Disability StudiesQuestioning Normality Through Disability Studies
Questioning Normality Through Disability Studies
 
Disability Representations in the Media
 Disability Representations in the Media Disability Representations in the Media
Disability Representations in the Media
 
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docxVoices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
Voices of Ability and Disability At various points.docx
 
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
Woodbury, christine results of a disabilty awareness assignment nfsej v2...
 

TorrySchrag_DirectedResearchFinalManuscript

  • 1. Running head: DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 1 To help or not to help? The role of disability stigma and demand Elena R. Torry-Schrag Macalester College
  • 2. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 2 Abstract This paper examines two factors hypothesized to influence willingness to help people with disabilities: Attitude toward disability and perceived capacity of the helper. Stigma (presence or absence of disability) and demand of help were manipulated in the context of a vignette. Participants read the vignette and were asked to rate their likelihood of helping. They also completed several measures of attitude toward disability. A significant main effect of demand and a significant main effect of stigma were found, such that participants were more willing to help in low demand scenarios, and also in high stigma scenarios. Additionally, there was a significant difference of competence ratings between the groups “students at your college” and “people with physical disabilities,” such that “people with physical disabilities” were rated as significantly less competent than “students at your college.” Implications and future areas of research are discussed.
  • 3. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 3 To help or not to help? The role of disability stigma and demand Because people with disabilities have historically been excluded from mainstream society through institutionalization and “special,” segregated schools (Linton, 1998), scholars and activists within the domain of disability often focus on the social exclusion of people with disabilities (Wendell, 1996). Lack of resources in education and an absence of job opportunities continue to marginalize a large group of American citizens, even after the passage of legislation like the Americans with Disabilities Act in 1990 (Neufeldt, 1995; Wendell, 1996). Cultural views that disability should be a private matter furthers the isolation of people with disabilities, forcing the experience of having a disability “underground” (Wendell, 1996, p. 60). Other cultural values also influence attitudes toward disability. For instance, MacGregor (2012) argues that the “independent individual” is a fundamental element of capitalist societies, shaping not only the labor market but wider expectations for behavior (para 16). It sets expectations for what constitutes “work,” and what type of people (read: bodies) are capable of working. Restriction from the workplace based on able-bodied standards projects the idea that people with disabilities are a burden on the larger society. This invalidates their identity as an “active citizen,” removing all autonomy from the disabled person (para. 4). American culture places a premium on independence, with “expectations of individual productivity” in the workplace and everyday life (Wendell, 1996, p. 59). The cultural archetype of the “self-made man” who achieves success through pulling himself up by his bootstraps is one example of the value placed on autonomy and self reliance. This high value on independence masks the reality that all people require some level of assistance (MacGregor, 2012; Wendell, 1996). However, by casting certain types of assistance as “entitlements” rather than “help,”
  • 4. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 4 people who require additional help beyond the entitlement level are categorized outside the norm of independence, creating an ​us/them dichotomy between nondisabled and disabled individuals (Dreger, 2004; Wendell, 1996). Indeed, research suggests that people with disabilities are widely characterized as needing help. Rohmer and Louvet (2012) measured implicit stereotypes associated with people with disabilities, and found they were judged to be less competent and less warm than nondisabled people. Low competence ratings suggest that people with disabilities may be stereotyped as relying on others and needing assistance. Furthermore, Nario-Redmond (2010) found that disability stereotypes are consistent across type of disability. According to her research, people with disabilities are most commonly characterized as “dependent,” “weak,” and “incompetent” (p. 479), indicating that disabled people are widely and pervasively thought of in terms of a need for assistance. The characterization of people with disabilities as weak and incompetent, undesirable traits within the context of American individualism, contributed to their marginalization by nondisabled people, including through such historically common acts as institutionalization and forced sterilization (Crissey, 1975; Wehmeyer, 2003). While the marginalization of disabled people may look different today, the ability of people with disabilities to participate in all aspects of society is still highly limited, and should therefore be an area of great concern. Research in this area may also prompt action on behalf of people with disabilities to counteract the negative effects of social isolation and discrimination. Social movements in the United States, including those on behalf of disabled people, have historically relied on intergroup alliances, with non-marginalized individuals working to end prejudice in personal and professional areas of the lives of marginalized groups (Brown &
  • 5. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 5 Ostrove, 2013). Allies in the domain of disability are defined by Gil (2001) as people who “make the effort to learn who their disabled associates are in their full glory and their full ordinariness,” or “those people who just seem to “get it” (p. 368). Gil argues that allies can promote better attitudes towards people with disabilities, which could be an important step in ameliorating the social isolation of disabled people. However, little research has been done to illuminate barriers that may prevent nondisabled people from becoming allies to people with physical disabilities. This is an area worth exploring, because “interactions between majority and minority group members are often fraught with uncertainty and misunderstanding,” (Wang, Silverman, Gwin, & Dovidio, 2015, 491). Lack of contact between people with and without disabilities means that nondisabled people may lack social scripts to guide their behavior, which could cause hesitation and anxiety. Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) argue that the question of “how and when” assistance should be provided or offered to people with disabilities is one with which non-disabled people often struggle, “especially when interacting with strangers or acquaintances who are disabled” (p. 3). It could be that nondisabled people perceive people with disabilities as needing help, but are uncertain of their ability to help and the appropriateness of offering assistance. This suggests that “help” as it relates to allyship in the context of disability is an important area of inquiry. As a result of the American premium on independence, there is a cultural norm to “let others stand on their own two feet” (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 4). Deviation from that norm can elicit strong and instantaneous negative reactions, such as “behavioral aversion” (Dovidio, Pagotto, & Hebl, 2011, p. 159). If nondisabled individuals characterize disabled people in terms of their need for help and additionally view needing help as a norm deviation, it is likely
  • 6. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 6 that they would have negative reactions towards people with disabilities, including the desire to avoid them. The context in which these evaluations occur are also influential. According to Goffman (1963), social settings set expectations for “categories of persons likely to be encountered there” (p. 256). Strangers in that setting are evaluated by their appearance, which facilitates anticipation of their attributes and social identity. Because the built environment was created by and for able-bodied people, people with disabilities may be more noticeable, and seemingly out of place (Wendell, 1996). Their appearance as physically different from the normative category of people in the environment may lead to evaluations of dependency, and the need for help. According to Goffman, stigma symbols are “signs which are especially effective in drawing attention to a debasing identity” (p. 44). It is possible, then, that the presence of a wheelchair may activate stigmatized notions of people with disabilities. Goffman refers to individuals whose stigmatized identity is evident “on the spot” as “the discredited” (p. 4). This seems particularly apt for the domain of disability, where people with physical disabilities are often seen as lacking the necessary credentials for autonomy and control. Goffman further states that attitudes towards stigmatized individuals directly influence our actions toward them. He offers the example of a nondisabled person desiring to avoid helping a blind person as just one manifestation of stigma-influenced behavior. Piliavin, Piliavin, and Rodin (1975) empirically investigated the effect of stigma on willingness to help. They predicted that negative attitudes elicited by a physical stigma would override the tendency to help someone in need. In their study, an actor with a “facial disfigurement” (p. 432) rode on a subway car, and suddenly fell to the ground. Confederates posing as subway riders tracked and coded helping behavior, and measured the length of time
  • 7. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 7 before the actor was helped, either with physical touch or a verbal inquiry. Willingness to help differed significantly by stigma: Unmarked victims received significantly more help than “stigmatized victims” (p. 433). While this study did not specifically look at physical disability so much as physical “disfigurement” (p. 432), the global attributions made toward people of across disability (Nario-Redmond, 2010) suggests that stigma effects may also be generalizable to other disability-related stigmas. This would suggest that the stigma of disability does cause nondisabled individuals to avoid people with disabilities. It is therefore possible that nondisabled people may be averse to forming alliances with people with disabilities because of unfavorable attitudes related to helping stigmatized individuals. Soble and Strickland (1974) also examined the effect of stigma on willingness to help. They argued that interactions with stigmatized individuals causes unpleasant affective reactions in non-stigmatized individuals. They further postulated that non-stigmatized individuals shape their lives and routines to avoid contact with stigmatized individuals, to avoid feelings of discomfort. These claims were tested in a study in which a confederate went door-to-door and distributed a survey to participants and attempted to arrange a follow-up interview. In half of the trials she appeared physically “normal,” and in the other half had an obvious physical disability. Participants were significantly more likely to agree to an interview when her appearance was “normal,” demonstrating that the presence physical disability triggers the desire to avoid interactions with the stigmatized individual. Following the line of this study, nondisabled people may be in opposition to the idea of forming an alliance with disabled individuals out of a desire to avoid the feelings and attitudes elicited by the presence of disability.
  • 8. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 8 Conversely, there is another line of research that suggests that stigma towards people with disabilities may work in a different manner, towards over-helping. The perception that people with disabilities are low in capacity often leads to assumptions of helplessness (Rohmer & Louvet, 2012). People who subscribe to these assumptions may be ​more inclined to help, acting on the belief that the disabled person won’t be able to complete the task without assistance. Supporting this conjecture, there is a robust body of research that indicates people with disabilities “often receive more help than they would like or need” (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 3). Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) found this to be true even when the cost of helping was high, such as the physically demanding task of pushing a wheelchair. In fact, some participants who used wheelchairs reported nondisabled people taking control of their wheelchair even when they explicitly refused the offer of assistance (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). The pattern of nondisabled people assisting in the face of refusal suggests that the belief that people with disabilities are incapable extends even to the disabled person’s perceptions of needing help. Wang et al. (2015) believe these instances of unsolicited help arise from evaluations of disabled people as low in competence, but high in warmth. Perceptions of warmth facilitate over-helping, by framing the stigma of incompetence as something pitiable, rather than wholly aversive. Doob and Ecker (1970) evaluated the effect of disability on helping behavior in a study similar to that of Soble and Strickland (1974). They argued that feelings of sympathy for disabled people would elicit greater compliance with requests for help. Confederates went door-to-door asking whoever answered the door to complete a questionnaire. Half the confederates wore an eye-patch, while the other half did not. Participants were significantly more likely to accept the questionnaire when the confederate had an eyepatch, suggesting a stronger
  • 9. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 9 desire to help disabled people than nondisabled people. Due to the global nature of disability stereotypes, it is possible that this same pattern would emerge for people with more severe physical disabilities (Nario-Redmond, 2010). Slochower, Wein, Firstenberg, and DiGuilio (1980) tested just that. The authors criticized previous studies for having a weak disability manipulation, and suggested that a more severe disability would arouse high levels of anxiety and therefore decrease willingness to help. Participants, students at an urban college, were approached by a confederate who either used a wheelchair or did not. The confederates asked participants to donate to the March of Dimes, and donations were counted by two researchers who observed the interactions unobtrusively. Donation results revealed that confederates in wheelchairs received significantly more donations than non-wheelchair confederates. While donating money is not a physically demanding task, if disability arouses the same attitudes of helplessness and pity, the same results may be seen in higher demand scenarios. Other research suggests that the potential helper’s beliefs about their personal capability are more influential than attitudes when deciding whether to help someone. Many people with physical disabilities have a need for instrumental help. This type of help is defined as “something active or physical to help the seeker” (Barbee & Cunningham, 1994, cited in Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 3), and may be a barrier to forming alliances, depending on the perceived capability of the individual helping. Ajzen and Madden (1986) investigated the effect of perceived behavioral control on predicted future behavior. They found that the more participants believed they had control over the situation, the more likely they were to say they would complete a particular behavior. Perceived control may be a proxy for perceived capability, since
  • 10. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 10 requisite knowledge, abilities, and skills were identified as resources needed for control. The perception of lacking these factors with regard to helping a disabled person would decrease perceived behavioral control, and subsequently, the likelihood of engaging in helping behavior. Due to the social isolation of people with disabilities, it is likely that many nondisabled people feel they lack the resources to succeed in a situation where they would be required to provide help to a person with a disability. Feeling disempowered or unable to provide physical assistance to disabled people would therefore decrease the likelihood of offering assistance and, more broadly, pursuing an ally relationship with them. Research concerning barriers to bystander intervention with regards to sexual violence offers support for this position. Bennett, Banyard, and Garnhart (2014) surveyed participants about barriers to providing help in situations where sexual assault or rape may occur. One barrier identified was “failure to intervene due to skills deficit” (p. 486). This interpersonal factor was described as a “lack of confidence in ability to help,” (p. 487) and was mentioned by the highest percentage of participants. Overall, participants who perceived greater barriers were less likely to report engaging in helping behavior. The domains of sexual violence and disability may at first seem unrelated; however, the shared requirement of physical intervention/assistance makes the results of Bennett et al.’s research applicable to the present study. Existing studies have measured behavior, but not the underlying beliefs about personal capability and attitudes that may motivate them. The present study seeks to examine which is more influential when it comes to making the decision to help a person with a physical disability: attitudes toward the need for help or the potential helper’s beliefs about their capacity to help a disabled person. Although willingness to help is not exactly transferable to “ally” behavior, a
  • 11. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 11 main characteristic that has been found to separate “allies” from low-prejudiced individuals is willingness to offer support (Brown & Ostrove, 2013). Therefore, willingness to offer support, sometimes literal and physical, could be a foundation to forming an alliance across ability. Participants read one of four vignettes that vary by demand (high and low) and perceived stigma (high and low) and then rated their willingness to help in that situation. If attitudes towards people with disabilities are more influential when making the decision to help a person with a disability, there are two divergent possibilities. If participants have a distancing attitude toward disability, then they will be more willing to help in low stigma situations, regardless of demand. An over-helping attitude, however, would cause participants to help more when stigma is high, regardless of demand. Alternatively, if the potential helper’s perceived personal capability is more dominant in this decision-making process, then participants will be more willing to help in low demand situations than in high demand ones, regardless of stigma. Method Participants One hundred sixty-five participants (94 females, 28 males, 2 otherwise-identifying) were recruited through social media (Facebook), email, and word of mouth. Of those participants, 124 provided demographic data (94 females, 28 males, 2 otherwise-identifying, M age = 25.38, age range: 18-63). A majority of participants (n = 104) identified as white. Other racial/ethnic identities included biracial/multiracial (9), Latin@ (6), African American/Black (2), Asian/Asian American (1), and Other (2). Participants were not given any form of compensation for their participation. Procedure
  • 12. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 12 Participants followed a link either from a Facebook post or email to an online survey created through the Qualtrics platform. After reading a consent form, participants clicked “I agree” to indicate that they were 18 years or older and had read the consent form. On the next page, they were asked to imagine they were riding a city bus. They then read one of four vignettes and rated the likelihood (on a Likert scale of 1-6) that they would help in that scenario. In the low demand scenario, an individual on the bus announces that they need one more signature on their petition. In the high demand scenario, they observed someone on the bus struggling with grocery bags, attempting to get money out to pay the bus fare. These scenarios also varied by level of perceived stigma, with high perceived stigma relating to disability (petition for more jobs for people with disabilities/person struggling with groceries uses a wheelchair) and low perceived stigma not relating to disability (petition for student loan forgiveness/person struggling with groceries is able-bodied) (See Appendix A). After reading the vignette, they completed measures of attitude toward disability. Finally, they were asked to think back to the scenario they had read and rate on a Likert scale of 1-6 how much they could have helped, regardless of whether they wanted to. Participants were also given the option to explain their rating in an open-ended response box. After clicking the next button, they were directed to a page where they entered demographic information (age in years, gender identity, ethnicity, and whether or not they identified as having a disability). Demographics were collected by text entry, so participants were not limited to a set list of choices, except for disability identity, which was a “yes/no” choice. They then clicked the next button again, which took them to the debriefing form, outlining the true nature of the study.
  • 13. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 13 Randomization procedures were put in place such that an equal number of participants read and responded to each vignette. Design A 2 x 2 factorial design was used in which stigma (high vs low) and demand (high vs low) were both between-subjects variables. The dependent variable was likelihood to help, which was measured using a Likert scale from 1 (Very Unlikely) to 6 (Very Likely). Measures Attitude toward disability was assessed using three measures. First, participants completed an adapted version of the feeling thermometer (Converse & Presser, 1986). Participants feelings towards different groups, including “people with physical disabilities,” were measured via warmth ratings ranging from 0 (very cold, very unfavorable) to 100 (very warm, very favorable). Examples of questions include: “How warm do you feel toward other Macalester Students?” and “How warm do you feel toward members of the Democratic Party?” This type of measure has been used in research regarding attitudes towards various social groups, including racial outgroups (Williamson, Bishop, & Hood, 2014) and LGBT individuals (Haddok, Zanna, & Esses, 1993), as well as research in the field of intergroup contact (Shook, Hopkins, & Koech, 2016). Participants also rated how competent they perceived the same list of groups as in the feelings thermometer measure. Attitude towards out-groups is often assessed along the two dimensions of warmth and competence (e.g. Rohmer & Louvet, 2012). Furthermore, it has been shown that while warmth ratings can increase as a result of exposure to the perspective of disabled people, competence ratings remain low, resulting in an overall negative evaluation of
  • 14. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 14 the individual (Silverman, Gwinn, & Van Boven, 2015). Therefore, it is important not to conflate a high rating of warmth with a positive attitude toward people with individuals, as it may not coincide with a high rating of competence. Comparing the two ratings will give a holistic view of participants’ attitudes. Finally, participants completed an adapted measure of individualism (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998) to assess attitudes toward independence/self-reliance. Participants rated the extent to which they agreed with nine statements (three from the original measure and six created for this study) regarding individualistic attitudes from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Examples of statements include: “I’d rather depend on myself than others,” and “It is important to me that I can do things on my own,” and “It’s better to do things on your own than ask for help” (See Appendix B).​ ​According to Triandis and Gelfand (1998), the reliability of the original scale was found to be good (Cronbach’s α = .67), but it has been substantially modified for the current study. Internal consistency for the current measure was found to be good (Cronbach’s α = .78). Perceived capability was measured by asking participants to reflect on the situation they had previously read. They were asked to rate how much they think they could have helped, regardless of whether they wanted to, on a Likert scale from 1 (Unable) to 6 (Completely able). This was followed by an open-ended “Explain your rating” response box. Results A two-way analysis of variance assessing the effect of demand and stigma on willingness to help indicated a main effect of demand, ​F(1, 154) = 5.91, ​p < .05, such that willingness to help was higher in low demand scenarios (​M = 4.32, ​SD = .173), than in high demand scenarios (​M =
  • 15. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 15 3.75, ​SD = .158). The main effect of stigma was also significant, ​F(1,154) = 25.58, ​p < .05, such that participants were more likely to help in disability-related scenarios (​M = 4.63, SD = .173) than nondisabled targets (​M = 3.45, SD = .158). The interaction effect was nonsignificant, ​F(1, 154) = .967, ​p > .05 (see Figure 1). Follow-up paired t-tests examined participants’ attitudes toward people with disabilities by comparing their warmth and competence ratings of “people with physical disabilities” compared to their in-group of “students at your college. There were no significant differences in participants’ warmth ratings of “people with physical disabilities” (​M = 72.02) and “students at your college,” (​M = 73.52, ​t[142] = -.872, n.s.) However, there was a significant difference in participants’ competence ratings. “People with physical disabilities” (​M = 67.16) were rated significantly less competent than “students at your college” (​M = 75.44, ​t[132] = -4.97, ​p < .05). [see Figure 2] Discussion This study sought to evaluate the effects of attitudes toward the need for help and the potential helper’s beliefs about their capacity on willingness to help. If participants had a distancing attitude toward disability, then they would have been more willing to help in low stigma situations, regardless of demand. An over-helping attitude, however, would have caused participants to help more when stigma is high, regardless of demand. Alternatively, if the potential helper’s perceived personal capability was more dominant in this decision-making process, then participants would have been more willing to help in low demand situations than in high demand ones, regardless of stigma.
  • 16. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 16 The results support the over-helping hypothesis. Participants were more likely to help in high stigma situations, even when demand was high. Current research supports the phenomenon of over-helping (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003; Wang et al., 2015). Researchers have hypothesized that this trend exists because people with disabilities are seen as requiring outside assistance. Supporting this, the low competence ratings for “People with physical disabilities” compared with “Students at your college” suggests that people with disabilities were indeed perceived as needing help. It is possible that the increased helping in high stigma scenarios was caused by the perception that people with disabilities are incompetent and dependent on able-bodied people intervening. People with disabilities often received unwanted assistance because they are perceived as incompetent, even when they explicitly refuse the offer (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). The response of one participant offers anecdotal support for this interpretation: Possibly, this question was aimed mostly at people who don't use wheelchairs. I happen to use a wheelchair myself. I might hold the groceries for the person or otherwise assist them. However, if I did I would ask first. As a wheelchair user (I hate the expression "wheelchair bound"), I am often offered help and sometimes given it without asking, even for very simple tasks, where 90% of the time help is not needed or wanted. So I am sensitive to that. The person might prefer to get their own stuff. By the way, many wheelchair users are able to help (in a physical way) both other people with disabilities and without. For example, I can hold the door for someone, pick something up, etc...but wheelchair users are usually seen as not capable of much, physically and sometimes mentally.
  • 17. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 17 There was no difference in warmth ratings between “People with physical disabilities” compared with “Students at your college,” however, which suggests participants’ attitude toward disability and the need for help is characterized more by pity, rather than aversion. The “warm glow” hypothesis of assistance, which suggests that people feel good about helping others, may also be at play here (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003, p. 5). Participant responses offer partial support for a “warm glow” effect. Many participants reported that helping others was important to them, citing a “sense of duty.” One participant specifically mentioned people with disabilities as a population they publically support. While on the face of it, a desire to help people with disabilities may seem beneficial, the way in which help is given has serious implications. Help can reinforce stereotypes of people with disabilities as fully dependent, and puts the helper in a “one-up” position (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). Unwanted help can also lower the self-esteem of disabled people, since it implies they are incapable of functioning on their own (Wang et al., 2014). A major limitation of this study was the setting of the vignette. In reviewing participants’ written responses about their capacity to help, it appeared the scenario itself had an unintended effect on behavior. Many participants explained their willingness to help directly in the context of being on a public bus. For example, one participant wrote “The reason why I choose that I would unlikely help the person is because normally sometimes people feel very defensive while riding the bus.” Other participants gave similar responses, stating that their seating position on the bus (e.g. close to the front vs in the back) would have influenced their behavior. In a less ambiguous scenario it is possible that helping behavior would have been different. However,
  • 18. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 18 based on the responses, it seems as if helping behavior would only ​increase, which does not invalidate the current findings. Future research could approach the question of help from the perspective of people with disabilities, a strategy that is decidedly lacking in the literature (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003). Many scholars have written about the importance of including people with disabilities in the research process, transforming their role from subject to researcher (Kitchin, 2000; Stefánsdóttir, & Traustadóttir, 2015). ​Braithwaite and Eckstein (2003) claim that a majority of the research regarding social support has focused on the providers of support, rather than the recipients, and argue for a change. Additionally, research that has been conducted from the point of view of people with disabilities has focused on their ​reactions to help, as opposed to how people with disabilities take an active role in obtaining needed help (Braithwaite & Eckstein, 2003; Wang et al., 2014). Questions could include an inquiry into the types of help that are considered appropriate, as well as ​how people with disabilities would prefer to be helped​. With this information, nondisabled people could be better equipped to help people with disabilities in an appropriate way, which could lay the foundations for an effective ally relationship. Future research should also evaluate other “ally behaviors.” As this study demonstrates, willingness to help is not equivalent to being an ally, if accompanied with negative attitudes surrounding disability. Research investigating desired characteristics of white allies from the perspectives of people of color identified qualities that fall into two areas, “informed action” and “affirmation” (Brown & Ostrove, 2013, p. 2216). Respectively, these include qualities such as “My friend takes action against bias among his or her own racial/ethnic group,” and “My friend is respectful toward me” (Brown & Ostrove, 2013, p. 2216). People with disabilities may desire
  • 19. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 19 these same qualities in able-bodied allies, have a completely different set of desired qualities, or want a combination of the two. Willingness to help may still be a foundational aspect of forming an alliance, but other factors may be equally or more important and should be identified to assist nondisabled people in forming a holistic ally relationship with disabled people. A third focus of future research should be on changing perceptions of incompetence of disabled people. Researchers could evaluate different methods of information transfer such as media portrayals of disabled people, intergroup contact, or a combination of various methods. Intergroup contact is a common method used to change negative attitudes of outgroup members. While it has been successful in decreasing anxiety and disconfirming stereotypes in some cases (Plant, 2004), intergroup contact can also lead to the creation of “subtypes,” categories of individuals within stereotyped groups who are seen as the exception to the norm (Dunn 2015). Therefore, more research is needed to determine what factors cause intergroup contact to be effective in challenging stereotypic beliefs. Changing perceptions of incompetence is of vital importance if relationships between people with and without disabilities are to transform from helper-helpee dynamics to that of an alliance. More broadly, viewing people with disabilities as competent, capable individuals could lead to larger social changes, such as improving accessibility of public spaces. Wendell (1996) argues that “the public presence of people with disabilities has many potential benefits,” (p. 63), including increasing understanding of disabled people, which in turn lessens negative attitudes toward disability. To date, there is an absence of literature concerning alliances between people with and without disabilities. The present study seeks to ameliorate that absence by exploring a central component of interactions between disabled and nondisabled individuals: offers of help. The
  • 20. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 20 results of this study suggest that people with disabilities do not suffer from a lack of help as a result of stigma, but rather an abundance of help, perhaps unwanted. Additionally, this study builds on previous literature in establishing that people with disabilities are viewed as comparably warm as in-group members, but less competent. Views of low competence have serious implications for the lives of disabled people, since views of the dominant group (nondisabled people) determine the treatment of people with disabilities, including their access to public spaces and resources. By exploring factors that influence desire to form alliances with people with disabilities, we can work to combat prevailing attitudes that isolate disabled people and keep them from reaching their full potential.
  • 21. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 21 References Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. ​Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, ​22, 453-474. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(86)90045-4 Bennett, S., Banyard, V. L., & Garnhart, L. (2014). To act or not to act, that is the question? Barriers and facilitators of bystander intervention. ​Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29(3), 476-496. doi:10.1177/0886260513505210 Braithwaite, D. O., & Eckstein, N. J. (2003). How people with disabilities communicatively manage assistance: Helping as instrumental social support. ​Journal of Applied Communication Research, ​31, 1-26. doi:10.1080/00909880305374 Brown, K. T., & Ostrove, J. M. (2013). What does it mean to be an ally?: The perception of allies from the perspective of people of color. ​Journal of Applied Social Psychology, ​43, 2211-2222. doi:10.1111/jasp.12172 Converse, J. M., & Presser, S. (1986). S​urvey questions: Handcrafting the standardized questionnaire. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Doob, A. N., & Ecker, B. P. (1970). Stigma and compliance. ​Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ​14, 302-304. doi:10.1037/h0028996 Dovidio, J. F., Pagotto, L., & Hebl, M. R. (2011). Implicit attitudes and discrimination against people with physical disabilities. In R. L. Wiener, S. L. Willborn, R. L. Wiener, S. L. Willborn (Eds.), ​Disability and aging discrimination: Perspectives in law and psychology (pp. 157-183). New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-6293-5_9
  • 22. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 22 Dunn, D. S. (2015). ​The social psychology of disability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Gil, C. (2001). Divided understandings: The social experience of disability. In G.L. Albrecht, K.D. Seelman, & M. Bury (Eds.), ​Handbook of disability studies (351-372). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P., &amp; Esses, V. M. (1993). Assessing the structure of prejudicial attitudes: The case of attitudes toward homosexuals. ​Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1105-1118.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1105 Linton, S. (1998).​ Claiming disability: Knowledge and identity. NY: NYU Press. MacGregor, M. (2012). Citizenship in name only: Constructing meaningful citizenship through a recalibration of the values attached to waged labor. ​Disability Studies Quarterly, ​32. Nario-Redmond, M. R. (2010). Cultural stereotypes of disabled and non-disabled men and women: Consensus for global category representations and diagnostic domains. ​British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 471-488. Piliavin, I. M., Piliavin, J. A., & Rodin, J. (1975). Costs, diffusion, and the stigmatized victim. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ​32, 429-438. doi:10.1037/h0077092 Plant, E. A. (2004). Responses to interracial interactions over time. P​ersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1458-1471. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264244
  • 23. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 23 Rohmer, O., & Louvet, E. (2012). Implicit measures of the stereotype content associated with disability. ​British Journal of Social Psychology, ​51, 732-740. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02087.x Slochower, J., Wein, L., Firstenberg, S., & DiGuilio, J. (1980). Severe physical handicaps and helping behavior. ​The Journal of Social Psychology, 112, 313-314. doi:10.1080/00224545.1980.9924338 Soble, S. L., & Strickland, L. H. (1974). Physical stigma, interaction, and compliance. ​Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, ​4, 130-132. Triandis, Harry C., & Gelfand, Michele J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. ​Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118-128. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118 Wang, K., Silverman, A., Gwinn, J. D., & Dovidio, J. F. (2015). Independent or ungrateful? Consequences of confronting patronizing help for people with disabilities. ​Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 489-503. Wendell, S. (1996). ​The rejected body: Feminist philosophical reflections on disability. NY: Routledge.
  • 24. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 24 Figure 1. ​Main effect of stigma and demand, with participants more willing to help in low demand scenarios, and high stigma scenarios
  • 25. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 25 Figure 2.​ Warmth and competence ratings; there was a nonsignificant difference in participants’ warmth ratings, but a significant difference in their competence ratings, of “students at your college” and “people with physical disabilities”
  • 26. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 26 Appendix A Vignettes Low demand, low stigma Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They announce they are collecting signatures for a petition asking for student loan forgiveness for local colleges, and that they only need one more signature. How likely is it that you would volunteer to sign the petition? 1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely) Low demand, high stigma Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They announce they are collecting signatures for a petition asking to build a group home for people with physical disabilities in your neighborhood, and that they only need one more signature. How likely is it that you would volunteer to sign the petition? 1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely) High demand, low stigma Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They have several bags of groceries on their lap, and are trying to hold onto the groceries while reaching for their purse. How likely is it that you would offer to help hold their groceries while they pay the driver? 1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely) High demand, high stigma Imagine that you are standing at the bus stop outside your house. You board the bus. At the next stop, an individual boards the bus. They have several bags of groceries on their lap, and are trying to hold onto the groceries while reaching for their purse, which is hanging from the back of their wheelchair. How likely is it that you would offer to help hold their groceries while they pay the driver? 1 (Very Unlikely) 2 3 4 5 6 (Very Likely)
  • 27. DISABILITY STIGMA AND DEMAND 27 Appendix B Measurement Scale for Individualism Please rate the extent to which you agree with the following statements 1 (Strongly Disagree) 2 3 4 5 6 (Strongly Agree) I’d rather depend on myself than others It’s better to work with others than alone I rely on myself most of the time I prefer to do things with others, rather than on my own It is important to me that I can do things on my own I rely on others most of the time It’s better to do things on your own than ask for help I will ask for help if I need it I like to act and complete tasks independently