SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional Readings
Beck, H. P., Levinson, S. & Irons, G. (2009). Finding Little
Albert: A journey to John B. Watson’s infant laboratory.
American Psychologist, 64, 605-614.
Beck, H. P., Levinson, S., & Irons, G. (2010). The evidence
supports Douglas Merritte as Little Albert. American
Psychologist, 65, 297-303.
Fridlund, A. J., Beck, H. P., Goldie, W. D., & Irons, G. (2012).
Little Albert: A neurologically impaired child. History of
Psychology, 15, 302-327.
Harris, B. (1979). What ever happened to Little Albert?
American Psychologist, 34, 151-160.
Powell, R. A. (2011). Little Albert, lost or found: Further
difficulties with the Douglas Merritte hypothesis. Sources,
Research Notes, and News, 106.
Powell, R. A., Digdon, N. Harris, B., & Smithson, C. (2014).
Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner, and Little Albert.
Albert Barger as “Psychology’s Lost Boy.” American
Psychologist, 69, 600-611.
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication
at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645
Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand
Up?
Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
CITATIONS
4
READS
1,369
1 author:
Richard Griggs
University of Florida
121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs
on 20 July 2017.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2
?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
Topical Article
Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little
Albert Please Stand Up?
Richard A. Griggs1
Abstract
This article is concerned with the recent debate about the
identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant
subject in Watson
and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For
decades, psychologists and psychology students have been
intrigued by the
mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions
to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence
of cov-
erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to
provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance
organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search
and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A
summative
comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is
likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not.
Keywords
Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology
According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con-
veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the-
ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the
Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi-
ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned
with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s
(1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con-
cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the
infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost
boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo-
gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys-
tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were
lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences.
Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to
questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones,
such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray,
1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif-
ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research
notes and papers, which may have included information about
Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two
competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden-
tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al.,
2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie,
& Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, &
Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig-
don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi-
cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of
this identity debate is not available in current introductory text-
books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications,
Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc-
tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro-
posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility
that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the
experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for
introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years
before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to
update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate.
It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis-
cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by
psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom
presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert
identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the
two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For
the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence,
this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading
the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates
will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi-
dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion
of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas
Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were
published first.
Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these
discussions with some general background material important
to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the
foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and
Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi-
ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants
1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville,
FL, USA
Corresponding Author:
Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville,
FL 32225, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Teaching of Psychology
2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18
ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
top.sagepub.com
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://top.sagepub.com
http://top.sagepub.com/
which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment
(Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro-
vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g.,
Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the
search. However, it is important to note that there were
inconsis-
tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further
complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson,
1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to
as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo-
nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the
fact
that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home
for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins,
where
the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert
and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con-
ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home
(Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall
Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity
of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer-
ritte was Little Albert.
Douglas Merritte
Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall
Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al.,
2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the
discovery
of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the
school’s
president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first
part of the experiment was likely done in late November or
early
December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this
part
of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck
deter-
mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in
1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother.
A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman
Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three
women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel
Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the
title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck
and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking
various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage
records, to determine whether any of these three women had
given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel
Carter was eliminated because she was an African American
and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who
was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very
plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no
evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis-
covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March
9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the
Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed
that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in
Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that
his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had
given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla
would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as
a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment.
A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name
that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert
B. The American Psychological Association did not have an
ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there
was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for
experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant
subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation
with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck
and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why
the infant in the study might have been named Albert B.
According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat-
son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal
grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John
Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert
Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have
playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus.
The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery
of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck
et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas
when
an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and
some
enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s
movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images
did
not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more
thor-
ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent
bio-
metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the
photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the
visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive
identifica-
tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic
data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes
shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living
with
his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment
and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial
base-
line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis
that
Douglas was Albert.
If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that
tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from
hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could
not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated
that
he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons
(2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if
Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden
life
and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke.
Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination
of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert
appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained
medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert
was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2
Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the
film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha-
vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of
Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this
hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con-
genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s
experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s
medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further
argued that there were ample sources of information available
Griggs 15
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of
Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were
Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con-
tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was
‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that
Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know
about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri-
ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable
Little Albert experiment.
Albert Barger
Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other
researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte
hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was
far from closed and thus warranted further investigation
(Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin-
der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty
with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base-
line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported
age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the
baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case.
Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have
been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al.,
making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point
of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that
Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained
with his mother and had not been adopted.
Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy
and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s
(2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments
and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell
Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their
own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid
in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo-
gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further
investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck
et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided
at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a
genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the
Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, &
Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married
Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom
was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name
Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and
Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar-
riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles
Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the
oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed
son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth
and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns
Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A.
Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi-
cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as
Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the
experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that
although his given name was William Albert, he was typically
called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014).
Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou-
glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have
been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In
addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the
age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final
experimental session took place and when his mother removed
him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file,
however,
indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age,
about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little
Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell
and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight
at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that
reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21
pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer-
ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time.
Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat-
son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and
well-developed child.
Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little
Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had
numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent
with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus.
In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of
these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al.
claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen-
cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when
confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon
et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears
to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition,
Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film
clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert
seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little
awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the
clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions
to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near
him,
so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig-
don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging
only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen-
tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques-
tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological
impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in
their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological
deficits
from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly
speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga-
tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise
an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig-
don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014).
If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about
Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at
the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals
and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first
learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but
not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general.
The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike
16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par-
tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely
ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con-
ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse
effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before
anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the
famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as
his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and
did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence,
we will never know what his reaction would have been when
learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert.
However, when asked what her uncle would have thought
about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been
thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10).
Epilogue
As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor
to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more
likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert
Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas
Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key
attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed.
" His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the
infant in Watson and Rayner’s study.
" His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase
of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body
weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film
clips of the experiment.
" His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as
Little Albert’s age on that day.
" His general state of health as an infant matches that
described by Watson and Rayner (1920).
Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these
key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However,
according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all
Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little
Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014),
although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is
very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary
to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly
after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it
appears
that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he
was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the
hospital
and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and
Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson
(1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was
just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such
an
adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little
Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had
a
credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does.
Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson
was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know-
ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research.
This is very important because the story alleging such behavior
has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge-
lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been
exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very
unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with
the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory
psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little
Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and
textbooks becomes of critical importance.
As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol-
ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for
a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing
their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help
to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in
the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be
misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that
students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton,
2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com-
munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks,
so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook
authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about
our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this
goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author received no financial support for the research,
authorship,
and/or publication of this article.
Notes
1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher
Education
(2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less
for-
mal style without references. I highly recommend that
introductory
psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article
because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture
and
textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version
at
http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747
includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and
several
historical photos related to the search for Little Albert.
2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by
Fridlund,
a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist,
were
made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical
records,
but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood
psycho-
pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical
records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014).
All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as
Waterman’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior
assessments
and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b).
3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al.
(2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat-
son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg-
ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they
Griggs 17
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The
reason for these differences is presently unknown.
4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of
Albert’s neu-
rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of
Watson’s
film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal
baby that
Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out
by
Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would
also
apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is
neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication,
July
8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was
neurologically
impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment.
To
illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy
suggests the
following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the
Lit-
tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily
available on
the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief
handout
to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired,
another
third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and
the final
third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate
Albert’s
developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of
Albert
should prompt a more general class discussion about the
subjectivity
of observations and why scientific approaches require …
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication
at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645
Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand
Up?
Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
CITATIONS
4
READS
1,369
1 author:
Richard Griggs
University of Florida
121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs
on 20 July 2017.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2
?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
Topical Article
Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little
Albert Please Stand Up?
Richard A. Griggs1
Abstract
This article is concerned with the recent debate about the
identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant
subject in Watson
and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For
decades, psychologists and psychology students have been
intrigued by the
mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions
to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence
of cov-
erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to
provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance
organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search
and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A
summative
comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is
likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not.
Keywords
Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology
According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con-
veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the-
ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the
Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi-
ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned
with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s
(1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con-
cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the
infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost
boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo-
gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys-
tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were
lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences.
Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to
questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones,
such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray,
1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif-
ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research
notes and papers, which may have included information about
Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two
competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden-
tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al.,
2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie,
& Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, &
Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig-
don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi-
cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of
this identity debate is not available in current introductory text-
books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications,
Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc-
tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro-
posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility
that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the
experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for
introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years
before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to
update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate.
It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis-
cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by
psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom
presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert
identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the
two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For
the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence,
this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading
the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates
will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi-
dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion
of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas
Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were
published first.
Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these
discussions with some general background material important
to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the
foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and
Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi-
ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants
1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville,
FL, USA
Corresponding Author:
Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville,
FL 32225, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Teaching of Psychology
2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18
ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
top.sagepub.com
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://top.sagepub.com
http://top.sagepub.com/
which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment
(Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro-
vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g.,
Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the
search. However, it is important to note that there were
inconsis-
tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further
complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson,
1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to
as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo-
nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the
fact
that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home
for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins,
where
the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert
and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con-
ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home
(Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall
Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity
of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer-
ritte was Little Albert.
Douglas Merritte
Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall
Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al.,
2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the
discovery
of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the
school’s
president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first
part of the experiment was likely done in late November or
early
December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this
part
of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck
deter-
mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in
1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother.
A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman
Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three
women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel
Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the
title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck
and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking
various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage
records, to determine whether any of these three women had
given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel
Carter was eliminated because she was an African American
and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who
was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very
plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no
evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis-
covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March
9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the
Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed
that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in
Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that
his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had
given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla
would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as
a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment.
A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name
that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert
B. The American Psychological Association did not have an
ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there
was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for
experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant
subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation
with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck
and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why
the infant in the study might have been named Albert B.
According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat-
son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal
grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John
Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert
Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have
playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus.
The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery
of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck
et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas
when
an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and
some
enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s
movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images
did
not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more
thor-
ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent
bio-
metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the
photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the
visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive
identifica-
tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic
data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes
shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living
with
his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment
and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial
base-
line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis
that
Douglas was Albert.
If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that
tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from
hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could
not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated
that
he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons
(2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if
Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden
life
and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke.
Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination
of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert
appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained
medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert
was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2
Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the
film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha-
vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of
Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this
hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con-
genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s
experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s
medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further
argued that there were ample sources of information available
Griggs 15
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of
Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were
Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con-
tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was
‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that
Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know
about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri-
ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable
Little Albert experiment.
Albert Barger
Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other
researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte
hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was
far from closed and thus warranted further investigation
(Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin-
der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty
with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base-
line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported
age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the
baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case.
Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have
been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al.,
making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point
of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that
Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained
with his mother and had not been adopted.
Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy
and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s
(2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments
and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell
Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their
own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid
in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo-
gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further
investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck
et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided
at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a
genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the
Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, &
Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married
Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom
was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name
Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and
Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar-
riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles
Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the
oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed
son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth
and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns
Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A.
Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi-
cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as
Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the
experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that
although his given name was William Albert, he was typically
called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014).
Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou-
glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have
been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In
addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the
age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final
experimental session took place and when his mother removed
him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file,
however,
indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age,
about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little
Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell
and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight
at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that
reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21
pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer-
ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time.
Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat-
son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and
well-developed child.
Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little
Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had
numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent
with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus.
In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of
these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al.
claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen-
cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when
confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon
et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears
to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition,
Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film
clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert
seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little
awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the
clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions
to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near
him,
so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig-
don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging
only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen-
tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques-
tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological
impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in
their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological
deficits
from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly
speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga-
tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise
an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig-
don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014).
If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about
Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at
the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals
and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first
learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but
not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general.
The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike
16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par-
tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely
ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con-
ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse
effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before
anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the
famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as
his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and
did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence,
we will never know what his reaction would have been when
learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert.
However, when asked what her uncle would have thought
about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been
thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10).
Epilogue
As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor
to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more
likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert
Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas
Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key
attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed.
" His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the
infant in Watson and Rayner’s study.
" His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase
of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body
weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film
clips of the experiment.
" His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as
Little Albert’s age on that day.
" His general state of health as an infant matches that
described by Watson and Rayner (1920).
Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these
key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However,
according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all
Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little
Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014),
although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is
very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary
to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly
after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it
appears
that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he
was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the
hospital
and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and
Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson
(1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was
just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such
an
adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little
Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had
a
credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does.
Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson
was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know-
ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research.
This is very important because the story alleging such behavior
has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge-
lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been
exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very
unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with
the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory
psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little
Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and
textbooks becomes of critical importance.
As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol-
ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for
a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing
their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help
to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in
the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be
misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that
students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton,
2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com-
munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks,
so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook
authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about
our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this
goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author received no financial support for the research,
authorship,
and/or publication of this article.
Notes
1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher
Education
(2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less
for-
mal style without references. I highly recommend that
introductory
psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article
because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture
and
textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version
at
http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747
includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and
several
historical photos related to the search for Little Albert.
2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by
Fridlund,
a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist,
were
made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical
records,
but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood
psycho-
pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical
records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014).
All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as
Waterman’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior
assessments
and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b).
3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al.
(2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat-
son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg-
ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they
Griggs 17
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The
reason for these differences is presently unknown.
4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of
Albert’s neu-
rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of
Watson’s
film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal
baby that
Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out
by
Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would
also
apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is
neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication,
July
8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was
neurologically
impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment.
To
illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy
suggests the
following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the
Lit-
tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily
available on
the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief
handout
to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired,
another
third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and
the final
third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate
Albert’s
developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of
Albert
should prompt a more general class discussion about the
subjectivity
of observations and why scientific approaches require …
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication
at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645
Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand
Up?
Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
CITATIONS
4
READS
1,369
1 author:
Richard Griggs
University of Florida
121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs
on 20 July 2017.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U
p?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2
?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r
greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
Topical Article
Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little
Albert Please Stand Up?
Richard A. Griggs1
Abstract
This article is concerned with the recent debate about the
identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant
subject in Watson
and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For
decades, psychologists and psychology students have been
intrigued by the
mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions
to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence
of cov-
erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to
provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance
organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search
and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A
summative
comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is
likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not.
Keywords
Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology
According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con-
veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the-
ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the
Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi-
ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned
with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s
(1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con-
cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the
infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost
boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo-
gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys-
tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were
lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences.
Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to
questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones,
such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray,
1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif-
ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research
notes and papers, which may have included information about
Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two
competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden-
tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al.,
2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie,
& Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, &
Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig-
don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi-
cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of
this identity debate is not available in current introductory text-
books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications,
Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc-
tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro-
posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility
that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the
experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for
introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years
before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to
update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate.
It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis-
cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by
psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom
presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert
identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the
two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For
the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence,
this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading
the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates
will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi-
dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion
of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas
Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were
published first.
Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these
discussions with some general background material important
to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the
foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and
Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi-
ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants
1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville,
FL, USA
Corresponding Author:
Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville,
FL 32225, USA.
Email: [email protected]
Teaching of Psychology
2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18
ª The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668
top.sagepub.com
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://top.sagepub.com
http://top.sagepub.com/
which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment
(Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro-
vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g.,
Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the
search. However, it is important to note that there were
inconsis-
tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further
complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson,
1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to
as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo-
nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the
fact
that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home
for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins,
where
the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert
and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con-
ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home
(Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall
Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity
of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer-
ritte was Little Albert.
Douglas Merritte
Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall
Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al.,
2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the
discovery
of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the
school’s
president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first
part of the experiment was likely done in late November or
early
December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this
part
of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck
deter-
mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in
1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother.
A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman
Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three
women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel
Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the
title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck
and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking
various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage
records, to determine whether any of these three women had
given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel
Carter was eliminated because she was an African American
and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who
was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very
plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no
evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis-
covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March
9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the
Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed
that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in
Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that
his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had
given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla
would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as
a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment.
A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name
that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert
B. The American Psychological Association did not have an
ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there
was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for
experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant
subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation
with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck
and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why
the infant in the study might have been named Albert B.
According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat-
son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal
grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John
Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert
Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have
playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus.
The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery
of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck
et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas
when
an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and
some
enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s
movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images
did
not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more
thor-
ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent
bio-
metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the
photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the
visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive
identifica-
tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic
data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes
shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living
with
his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the
experiment
and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial
base-
line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis
that
Douglas was Albert.
If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that
tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from
hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could
not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated
that
he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons
(2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if
Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden
life
and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke.
Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination
of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert
appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained
medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert
was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2
Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the
film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha-
vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of
Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this
hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con-
genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s
experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s
medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further
argued that there were ample sources of information available
Griggs 15
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of
Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were
Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con-
tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was
‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that
Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know
about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri-
ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable
Little Albert experiment.
Albert Barger
Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other
researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte
hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was
far from closed and thus warranted further investigation
(Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin-
der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty
with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base-
line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported
age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the
baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case.
Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have
been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al.,
making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point
of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that
Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained
with his mother and had not been adopted.
Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy
and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s
(2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments
and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell
Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their
own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid
in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo-
gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further
investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck
et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided
at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a
genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the
Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, &
Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married
Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom
was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name
Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and
Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar-
riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles
Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the
oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed
son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth
and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns
Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A.
Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi-
cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as
Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the
experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that
although his given name was William Albert, he was typically
called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014).
Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou-
glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have
been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In
addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the
age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final
experimental session took place and when his mother removed
him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file,
however,
indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age,
about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little
Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell
and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight
at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that
reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21
pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer-
ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time.
Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat-
son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and
well-developed child.
Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little
Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had
numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent
with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus.
In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of
these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al.
claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen-
cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when
confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon
et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears
to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition,
Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film
clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert
seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little
awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the
clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions
to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near
him,
so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig-
don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging
only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen-
tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques-
tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological
impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in
their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological
deficits
from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly
speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga-
tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise
an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig-
don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014).
If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about
Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at
the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals
and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first
learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but
not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general.
The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike
16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1)
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par-
tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely
ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con-
ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse
effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before
anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the
famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as
his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and
did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence,
we will never know what his reaction would have been when
learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert.
However, when asked what her uncle would have thought
about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been
thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10).
Epilogue
As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor
to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more
likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert
Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas
Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key
attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed.
" His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the
infant in Watson and Rayner’s study.
" His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase
of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body
weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film
clips of the experiment.
" His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as
Little Albert’s age on that day.
" His general state of health as an infant matches that
described by Watson and Rayner (1920).
Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these
key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However,
according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all
Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little
Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014),
although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is
very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary
to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly
after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it
appears
that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he
was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the
hospital
and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and
Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson
(1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was
just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such
an
adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little
Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had
a
credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does.
Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson
was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know-
ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research.
This is very important because the story alleging such behavior
has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge-
lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been
exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very
unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with
the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory
psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little
Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and
textbooks becomes of critical importance.
As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol-
ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for
a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing
their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help
to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in
the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be
misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that
students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton,
2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com-
munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks,
so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook
authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about
our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this
goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with
respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author received no financial support for the research,
authorship,
and/or publication of this article.
Notes
1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher
Education
(2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less
for-
mal style without references. I highly recommend that
introductory
psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article
because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture
and
textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version
at
http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747
includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and
several
historical photos related to the search for Little Albert.
2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by
Fridlund,
a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist,
were
made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical
records,
but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood
psycho-
pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical
records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014).
All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as
Waterman’s
assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior
assessments
and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b).
3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al.
(2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat-
son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg-
ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they
Griggs 17
by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://top.sagepub.com/
analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The
reason for these differences is presently unknown.
4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of
Albert’s neu-
rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of
Watson’s
film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal
baby that
Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out
by
Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would
also
apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is
neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication,
July
8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was
neurologically
impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment.
To
illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy
suggests the
following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the
Lit-
tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily
available on
the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief
handout
to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired,
another
third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and
the final
third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate
Albert’s
developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of
Albert
should prompt a more general class discussion about the
subjectivity
of observations and why scientific approaches require …
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication
at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368
Whatever Happened to Little Albert?
Article in American Psychologist · February 1979
DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.2.151
CITATIONS
160
READS
2,841
1 author:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on
these related projects:
History of Psychiatry View project
History of Psychology View project
Ben Harris
University of New Hampshire
48 PUBLICATIONS 377 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Ben Harris on
06 December 2016.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368_Whatever
_Happened_to_Little_Albert?enrichId=rgreq-
e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368_Whatever
_Happened_to_Little_Albert?enrichId=rgreq-
e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/project/History-of-Psychiatry-
5?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/project/History-of-Psychology-
7?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg
req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg
req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_New_Ha
mpshire?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg
req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg
req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz
o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
Whatever Happened to Little Albert?
BEN HARRIS Vassar College
ABSTRACT: John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner's
1920 conditioning of the infant Albert B. is a well-
known piece of social science folklore. Using pub-
lished sources, this article reviews the study's actual
procedures and its relationship to Watson's career and
work. The article also presents a history of psycholo-
gists' accounts of the Albert study, focusing on the
study's distortion by Watson himself, general textbook
authors, behavior therapists, and most recently, a
prominent learning theorist. The author proposes pos-
sible causes for these distortions and analyzes the
Albert study as an example of myth making in the
history of psychology.
Almost 60 years after it was first reported, Watson
and Rayner's (1920) attempted conditioning of
the infant Albert B. is one of the most widely
cited experiments in textbook psychology. Under-
graduate textbooks of general, developmental, and
abnormal psychology use Albert's conditioning to
illustrate the applicability of classical conditioning
to the development and modification of human
emotional behavior. More specialized books focus-
ing on psychopathology and behavior therapy (e.g.,
Eysenck, 1960) cite Albert's conditioning as an ex-
perimental model of psychopathology (i.e., a rat
phobia) and often use Albert to introduce a dis-
cussion of systematic desensitization as a treat-
ment of phobic anxiety.
Unfortunately, most accounts of Watson and
Rayner's research with Albert feature as much
fabrication and distortion as they do fact. From
information about Albert himself to 'the basic ex-
perimental methods and results, no detail of the
original study has escaped misrepresentation in the
telling and retelling of this bit of social science
folklore.
There has recently been a revival of interest
in Watson's conditioning research and theorizing
(e.g., MacKenzie, 1972; Seligman, 1971; Weimer
& Palermo, 1973; Samelson, Note 1), and in the
mythology of little Albert (Cornwell & Hobbs,
1976; Larson, 1978; Prytula, Oster, & Davis,
1977). However, there has yet to be a complete
examination of the methodology and results of the
Vol. 34, No. 2, 151-160
Albert study and of the process by which the
study's details have been altered over the years.
In the spirit of other investigations of classic
studies in psychology (e.g., Ellenberger, 1972;
Parsons, 1974) it is time to examine Albert's con-
ditioning in light of current theories of learning.
It is also time to examine how the Albert study
has been portrayed over the years, in the hope of
discovering how changes in psychological theory
have affected what generations of psychologists
have told each other about Albert.
The Experiment
As described by Watson and Rayner (1920), an
experimental study was undertaken to answer three
questions: (1) Can an infant be conditioned to
fear an animal that appears simultaneously with
a loud, fear-arousing sound? ( 2 ) Would such fear
transfer to other animals or to inanimate objects?
(3) How long would such fears persist? In at-
tempting to answer these questions, Watson and
Rayner selected an infant named Albert B., whom
they described as "healthy," and "stolid and un-
emotional" (p. 1). At approximately 9 months
of age, Albert was tested and was judged to show
no fear when successively observing a number of
live animals (e.g., a rat, a rabbit, a dog, and a
monkey), and various inanimate objects (e.g.,
cotton, human masks, a burning newspaper). He
was, however, judged to show fear whenever a
long steel bar was unexpectedly struck with a claw
hammer just behind his back.
Two months after testing Albert's apparently
unconditioned reactions to various stimuli, Watson
and Rayner attempted to condition him to fear a
Preparation of this article was aided by the textbook
and literature searches of Nancy Kinsey, the helpful com-
ments of Mike Wessels, and the bibliographic assistance
of Cedric Larson. The author also thanks Bill Wood-
ward and Ernest Hilgard for their comments on earlier
versions of this work.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Ben Harris,
Box 368, Vassar College, Poughkeepsie, New York 12601.
AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • FEBRUARY 1979 • 151
Copyright 1979 by the American Psychological Association,
Inc.
0003-066X/79/3402-01Sl$00.75
white rat. This was done by presenting a white
rat to Albert, followed by a loud clanging sound
(of the hammer and steel bar) whenever Albert
touched the animal. After seven pairings of the
rat and noise (in two sessions, one week apart),
Albert reacted with crying and avoidance when
the rat was presented without the loud noise.
In order to test the generalization of Albert's
fear response, 5 days later he was presented with
the rat, a set of familiar wooden blocks, a rabbit,
a short-haired dog, a sealskin coat, a package of
white cotton, the heads of Watson and two assist-
ants (inverted so that Albert could touch their
hair), and a bearded Santa Glaus mask. Albert
seemed to show a strong fear response to the rat,
the rabbit, the dog, and the sealskin coat; a "nega-
tive" response to the mask and Watson's hair; and
a mild response to the cotton. Also, Albert played
freely with the wooden blocks and the hair of
Watson's assistants.
After an additional 5 days, Watson recondi-
tioned Albert to the rat (one trial, rat paired with
noise) and also attempted to condition Albert di-
rectly to fear the previously presented rabbit (one
trial) and dog (one trial). When the effects of
this procedure were tested in a different, larger
room, it was found that Albert showed only a
slight reaction to the rat, the dog, and the rabbit.
Consequently, Watson attempted "to freshen the
reaction to the rat" (p. 9) by presenting it with
the loud noise. Soon after this, the dog began to
bark loudly at Albert, scaring him and the experi-
menters and further confounding the experiment.
To answer their third question concerning the
permanence of conditioned responses over time,
Watson and Rayner conducted a final series of
tests on Albert after 31 days of neither condition-
ing nor extinction trials. In these tests, Albert
showed fear when touching the Santa Claus mask,
the sealskin coat, the rat, the rabbit, and the dog.
At the same time, however, he initiated contact
with the coat and the rabbit, showing "strife be-
tween withdrawal and the tendency to manipulate"
(Watson & Rayner, 1920, p. 10). Following these
final tests, Albert's mother removed him from the
hospital where the experiment had been conducted.
(According to their own account, Watson and
Rayner knew a month in advance the day that
Albert would no longer be available to them.)
The Context of Watson and
Rayner's Study
What was the relationship of the Albert experi-
ment to the rest of Watson's work? On a per-
sonal level, this work was the final published
project of Watson's academic career, although he
supervised a subsequent, related study of the de-
conditioning of young children's fears '(M. C.
Jones, 1924a, 1924b). From a theoretical per-
spective, the Albert study provided an empirical
test of a theory of behavior and emotional de-
velopment that Watson had constructed over a
number of years.
Although Watson had publicly declared himself
a "behaviorist" in early 1913, he apparently did
not become interested in the conditioning of motor
and autonomic responses until late 1914, when he
read a French edition of Bekhterev's Objective
Psychology (see Hilgard & Marquis, 1940). By
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx
Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx

More Related Content

More from jesssueann

Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docxMajor Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
jesssueann
 
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docxMajor Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
jesssueann
 
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docxMaintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
jesssueann
 
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docxMain content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
jesssueann
 
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docxMacro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
jesssueann
 
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docxM.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
jesssueann
 
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docxM4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
jesssueann
 
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency .docx
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency   .docxmake a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency   .docx
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency .docx
jesssueann
 
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docxLove Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
jesssueann
 
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docxMajor Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
jesssueann
 
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docxMajor Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
jesssueann
 
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docxMajor Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
jesssueann
 
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docxLooking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
jesssueann
 
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docxMajor Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
jesssueann
 
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docxMacroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
jesssueann
 
M A T T D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
M A T T  D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docxM A T T  D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
M A T T D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
jesssueann
 
M A R C H 2 0 1 5F O R W A R D ❚ E N G A G E D ❚ .docx
M A R C H  2 0 1 5F O R W A R D   ❚   E N G A G E D   ❚   .docxM A R C H  2 0 1 5F O R W A R D   ❚   E N G A G E D   ❚   .docx
M A R C H 2 0 1 5F O R W A R D ❚ E N G A G E D ❚ .docx
jesssueann
 
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docxLymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
jesssueann
 
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docxLukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
jesssueann
 
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docxLove in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
jesssueann
 

More from jesssueann (20)

Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docxMajor Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
Major Assessment 2 The Educated Person” For educators to be ef.docx
 
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docxMajor Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
Major Assessment 4 Cultural Bias Investigation Most educators agree.docx
 
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docxMaintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
Maintaining privacy and confidentiality always is also vital. Nurses.docx
 
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docxMain content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
Main content15-2aHow Identity Theft OccursPerpetrators of iden.docx
 
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docxMacro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
Macro Presentation – Australia Table of ContentOver.docx
 
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docxM.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
M.S Aviation Pty Ltd TA Australian School of Commerce RTO N.docx
 
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docxM4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
M4.3 Case StudyCase Study ExampleJennifer S. is an Army veter.docx
 
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency .docx
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency   .docxmake a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency   .docx
make a histogram out of this information Earthquake Frequency .docx
 
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docxLove Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
Love Language Project FINAL PAPERLove Language Project Part .docx
 
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docxMajor Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
Major Computer Science What are the core skills and knowledge y.docx
 
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docxMajor Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
Major Crime in Your CommunityUse the Internet to search for .docx
 
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docxMajor Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
Major Assignment - Learning NarrativeWrite a learning narr.docx
 
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docxLooking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
Looking to have this work done AGAIN. It was submitted several times.docx
 
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docxMajor Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
Major Assessment 1 Develop a Platform of Beliefs The following .docx
 
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docxMacroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
Macroeconomics PaperThere are currently three major political ap.docx
 
M A T T D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
M A T T  D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docxM A T T  D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
M A T T D O N O V A NThings in the Form o f a Prayer in.docx
 
M A R C H 2 0 1 5F O R W A R D ❚ E N G A G E D ❚ .docx
M A R C H  2 0 1 5F O R W A R D   ❚   E N G A G E D   ❚   .docxM A R C H  2 0 1 5F O R W A R D   ❚   E N G A G E D   ❚   .docx
M A R C H 2 0 1 5F O R W A R D ❚ E N G A G E D ❚ .docx
 
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docxLymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
Lymphedema following breast cancer The importance of surgic.docx
 
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docxLukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
Lukas Nelson and his wife Anne and their three daughters had been li.docx
 
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docxLove in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
Love in the Time of Cholera, as the title indicates, interweaves e.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
PsychoTech Services
 
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
nitinpv4ai
 
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation resultsTemple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
Krassimira Luka
 
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
Payaamvohra1
 
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptxRESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
zuzanka
 
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsxData Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
Prof. Dr. K. Adisesha
 
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptxCapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
CapitolTechU
 
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
nitinpv4ai
 
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptxContiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
Kalna College
 
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGHKHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
shreyassri1208
 
Information and Communication Technology in Education
Information and Communication Technology in EducationInformation and Communication Technology in Education
Information and Communication Technology in Education
MJDuyan
 
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end examBPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
sonukumargpnirsadhan
 
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptxA Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
OH TEIK BIN
 
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT Internet resources for social science
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT  Internet resources for social science220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT  Internet resources for social science
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT Internet resources for social science
Kalna College
 
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in useHow to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
Celine George
 
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
Kalna College
 
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdfCIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
blueshagoo1
 
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
ShwetaGawande8
 
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
Mohammad Al-Dhahabi
 
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
EduSkills OECD
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
Gender and Mental Health - Counselling and Family Therapy Applications and In...
 
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
Oliver Asks for More by Charles Dickens (9)
 
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation resultsTemple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
Temple of Asclepius in Thrace. Excavation results
 
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
NIPER 2024 MEMORY BASED QUESTIONS.ANSWERS TO NIPER 2024 QUESTIONS.NIPER JEE 2...
 
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptxRESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
RESULTS OF THE EVALUATION QUESTIONNAIRE.pptx
 
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsxData Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
Data Structure using C by Dr. K Adisesha .ppsx
 
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptxCapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
CapTechTalks Webinar Slides June 2024 Donovan Wright.pptx
 
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
Haunted Houses by H W Longfellow for class 10
 
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptxContiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
Contiguity Of Various Message Forms - Rupam Chandra.pptx
 
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGHKHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
KHUSWANT SINGH.pptx ALL YOU NEED TO KNOW ABOUT KHUSHWANT SINGH
 
Information and Communication Technology in Education
Information and Communication Technology in EducationInformation and Communication Technology in Education
Information and Communication Technology in Education
 
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end examBPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
BPSC-105 important questions for june term end exam
 
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptxA Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
A Free 200-Page eBook ~ Brain and Mind Exercise.pptx
 
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT Internet resources for social science
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT  Internet resources for social science220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT  Internet resources for social science
220711130083 SUBHASHREE RAKSHIT Internet resources for social science
 
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in useHow to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
How to Fix [Errno 98] address already in use
 
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
220711130088 Sumi Basak Virtual University EPC 3.pptx
 
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdfCIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
CIS 4200-02 Group 1 Final Project Report (1).pdf
 
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
INTRODUCTION TO HOSPITALS & AND ITS ORGANIZATION
 
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
skeleton System.pdf (skeleton system wow)
 
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
Andreas Schleicher presents PISA 2022 Volume III - Creative Thinking - 18 Jun...
 

Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional ReadingsBe.docx

  • 1. Little Albert Historical Detective Paper Additional Readings Beck, H. P., Levinson, S. & Irons, G. (2009). Finding Little Albert: A journey to John B. Watson’s infant laboratory. American Psychologist, 64, 605-614. Beck, H. P., Levinson, S., & Irons, G. (2010). The evidence supports Douglas Merritte as Little Albert. American Psychologist, 65, 297-303. Fridlund, A. J., Beck, H. P., Goldie, W. D., & Irons, G. (2012). Little Albert: A neurologically impaired child. History of Psychology, 15, 302-327. Harris, B. (1979). What ever happened to Little Albert? American Psychologist, 34, 151-160. Powell, R. A. (2011). Little Albert, lost or found: Further difficulties with the Douglas Merritte hypothesis. Sources, Research Notes, and News, 106. Powell, R. A., Digdon, N. Harris, B., & Smithson, C. (2014). Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner, and Little Albert. Albert Barger as “Psychology’s Lost Boy.” American Psychologist, 69, 600-611. See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645 Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014 DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 CITATIONS
  • 2. 4 READS 1,369 1 author: Richard Griggs University of Florida 121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs on 20 July 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- 8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
  • 3. o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2 ?enrichId=rgreq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-8f239edd57592bdf8d2e36d42dfb05cf- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf Topical Article Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Richard A. Griggs1
  • 4. Abstract This article is concerned with the recent debate about the identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant subject in Watson and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For decades, psychologists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence of cov- erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A summative comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not. Keywords Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con- veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the- ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi- ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s (1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con- cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo- gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys- tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences. Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to
  • 5. questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones, such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray, 1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif- ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research notes and papers, which may have included information about Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden- tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al., 2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, & Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, & Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig- don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi- cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of this identity debate is not available in current introductory text- books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications, Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc- tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro- posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate. It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis- cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence, this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi- dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion
  • 6. of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were published first. Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these discussions with some general background material important to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi- ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants 1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA Corresponding Author: Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville, FL 32225, USA. Email: [email protected] Teaching of Psychology 2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18 ª The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 top.sagepub.com by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://top.sagepub.com http://top.sagepub.com/ which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment (Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro- vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g.,
  • 7. Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the search. However, it is important to note that there were inconsis- tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson, 1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo- nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the fact that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins, where the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con- ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home (Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer- ritte was Little Albert. Douglas Merritte Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al., 2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the discovery of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the school’s president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first part of the experiment was likely done in late November or early December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this part of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck deter-
  • 8. mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in 1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother. A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage records, to determine whether any of these three women had given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel Carter was eliminated because she was an African American and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis- covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March 9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment. A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert B. The American Psychological Association did not have an ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why
  • 9. the infant in the study might have been named Albert B. According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat- son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus. The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas when an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and some enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images did not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more thor- ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent bio- metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive identifica- tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living with his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial base- line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis that Douglas was Albert.
  • 10. If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated that he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons (2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden life and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke. Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2 Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha- vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con- genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further argued that there were ample sources of information available Griggs 15 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of
  • 11. Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con- tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was ‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri- ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable Little Albert experiment. Albert Barger Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was far from closed and thus warranted further investigation (Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin- der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base- line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case. Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al., making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained with his mother and had not been adopted. Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo- gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further
  • 12. investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar- riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A. Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi- cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that although his given name was William Albert, he was typically called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014). Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou- glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final experimental session took place and when his mother removed him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file, however, indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age, about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight
  • 13. at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21 pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer- ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time. Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat- son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and well-developed child. Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus. In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al. claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen- cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition, Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near him, so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig- don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen- tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques- tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological deficits from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga-
  • 14. tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig- don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014). If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general. The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike 16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1) by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par- tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con- ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence, we will never know what his reaction would have been when learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert. However, when asked what her uncle would have thought about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10). Epilogue
  • 15. As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed. " His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the infant in Watson and Rayner’s study. " His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film clips of the experiment. " His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as Little Albert’s age on that day. " His general state of health as an infant matches that described by Watson and Rayner (1920). Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However, according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it appears that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the hospital and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson
  • 16. (1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such an adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had a credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does. Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know- ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research. This is very important because the story alleging such behavior has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge- lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and textbooks becomes of critical importance. As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol- ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton, 2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com- munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks, so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga.
  • 17. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Notes 1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher Education (2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less for- mal style without references. I highly recommend that introductory psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture and textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version at http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747 includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and several
  • 18. historical photos related to the search for Little Albert. 2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by Fridlund, a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist, were made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical records, but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood psycho- pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014). All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as Waterman’s assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior assessments and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b). 3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat- son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg- ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they
  • 19. Griggs 17 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The reason for these differences is presently unknown. 4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of Albert’s neu- rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of Watson’s film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal baby that Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out by Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would also apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication, July 8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was neurologically impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment.
  • 20. To illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy suggests the following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the Lit- tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily available on the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief handout to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired, another third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and the final third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate Albert’s developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of Albert should prompt a more general class discussion about the subjectivity of observations and why scientific approaches require … See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645
  • 21. Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014 DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 CITATIONS 4 READS 1,369 1 author: Richard Griggs University of Florida 121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs on 20 July 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog
  • 22. y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- 2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2 ?enrichId=rgreq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-2f02b29066d8d0d9493376c66cccd15a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
  • 23. Topical Article Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Richard A. Griggs1 Abstract This article is concerned with the recent debate about the identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant subject in Watson and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For decades, psychologists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence of cov- erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A summative comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not. Keywords Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con- veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the- ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi- ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s (1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con-
  • 24. cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo- gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys- tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences. Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones, such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray, 1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif- ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research notes and papers, which may have included information about Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden- tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al., 2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, & Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, & Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig- don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi- cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of this identity debate is not available in current introductory text- books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications, Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc- tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro- posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate. It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis- cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom
  • 25. presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence, this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi- dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were published first. Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these discussions with some general background material important to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi- ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants 1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA Corresponding Author: Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville, FL 32225, USA. Email: [email protected] Teaching of Psychology 2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18 ª The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 top.sagepub.com by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 26. http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://top.sagepub.com http://top.sagepub.com/ which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment (Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro- vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g., Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the search. However, it is important to note that there were inconsis- tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson, 1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo- nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the fact that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins, where the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con- ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home (Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer- ritte was Little Albert. Douglas Merritte Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al., 2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the discovery of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the
  • 27. school’s president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first part of the experiment was likely done in late November or early December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this part of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck deter- mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in 1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother. A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage records, to determine whether any of these three women had given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel Carter was eliminated because she was an African American and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis- covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March 9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment. A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name
  • 28. that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert B. The American Psychological Association did not have an ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why the infant in the study might have been named Albert B. According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat- son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus. The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas when an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and some enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images did not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more thor- ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent bio- metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive identifica- tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living
  • 29. with his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial base- line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis that Douglas was Albert. If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated that he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons (2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden life and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke. Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2 Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha- vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con- genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further argued that there were ample sources of information available
  • 30. Griggs 15 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con- tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was ‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri- ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable Little Albert experiment. Albert Barger Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was far from closed and thus warranted further investigation (Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin- der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base- line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case. Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al., making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained with his mother and had not been adopted.
  • 31. Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo- gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar- riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A. Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi- cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that although his given name was William Albert, he was typically called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014). Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou- glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the
  • 32. age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final experimental session took place and when his mother removed him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file, however, indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age, about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21 pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer- ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time. Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat- son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and well-developed child. Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus. In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al. claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen- cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition, Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near him, so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig- don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging
  • 33. only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen- tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques- tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological deficits from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga- tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig- don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014). If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general. The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike 16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1) by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par- tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con- ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence,
  • 34. we will never know what his reaction would have been when learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert. However, when asked what her uncle would have thought about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10). Epilogue As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed. " His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the infant in Watson and Rayner’s study. " His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film clips of the experiment. " His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as Little Albert’s age on that day. " His general state of health as an infant matches that described by Watson and Rayner (1920). Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However, according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary
  • 35. to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it appears that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the hospital and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson (1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such an adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had a credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does. Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know- ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research. This is very important because the story alleging such behavior has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge- lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and textbooks becomes of critical importance. As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol- ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be
  • 36. misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton, 2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com- munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks, so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Notes 1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher Education (2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less for- mal style without references. I highly recommend that introductory psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture and
  • 37. textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version at http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747 includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and several historical photos related to the search for Little Albert. 2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by Fridlund, a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist, were made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical records, but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood psycho- pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014). All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as Waterman’s assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior assessments and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b).
  • 38. 3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat- son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg- ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they Griggs 17 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The reason for these differences is presently unknown. 4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of Albert’s neu- rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of Watson’s film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal baby that Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out by Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would also apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is
  • 39. neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication, July 8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was neurologically impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment. To illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy suggests the following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the Lit- tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily available on the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief handout to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired, another third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and the final third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate Albert’s developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of Albert should prompt a more general class discussion about the subjectivity
  • 40. of observations and why scientific approaches require … See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645 Psychology's Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Article in Teaching of Psychology · January 2014 DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 CITATIONS 4 READS 1,369 1 author: Richard Griggs University of Florida 121 PUBLICATIONS 2,392 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Griggs on 20 July 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
  • 41. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/273339645_Psycholog y%27s_Lost_Boy_Will_the_Real_Little_Albert_Please_Stand_U p?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- 440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Florida2 ?enrichId=rgreq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz
  • 42. o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Richard_Griggs?enrichId=r greq-440de3d87ca4ddb19d83701a280d3db3- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3MzMzOTY0NTtBUz o1MTgyOTExODU5NzkzOTJAMTUwMDU4MTY1NjI5Nw%3D %3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf Topical Article Psychology’s Lost Boy: Will the Real Little Albert Please Stand Up? Richard A. Griggs1 Abstract This article is concerned with the recent debate about the identity of psychology’s lost boy—Little Albert, the infant subject in Watson and Rayner’s classic experiment on fear conditioning. For decades, psychologists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mystery of Albert’s fate. Now two evidentiary-based solutions to this mystery have been proposed. Given the present absence of cov- erage in introductory textbooks, the purpose of this article is to provide a cornerstone resource for teachers to use as an advance organizer to the literature on this debate. Synopses of the search and resulting evidence for each candidate are provided. A summative comparison of the evidence indicates that Albert Barger is likely Little Albert and that Douglas Merritte is not. Keywords Little Albert, introductory psychology, history of psychology
  • 43. According to Jarrett (2008), psychology’s foundation as con- veyed in its introductory textbooks is arguably not built of the- ory but with the rock of classic experiments, such as the Stanford Prison Experiment and Milgram’s obedience experi- ments (see also Smyth, 2001a, 2001b). This article is concerned with one of these classic experiments, Watson and Rayner’s (1920) Little Albert experiment. More specifically, it is con- cerned with the recent controversy about the true identity of the infant subject of that study—Little Albert, ‘‘psychology’s lost boy’’ (Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009). For decades, psycholo- gists and psychology students have been intrigued by the mys- tery of Albert’s identity, his fate, and whether there were lasting effects of his fear-conditioning experiences. Until recently, there were no evidentiary-based answers to questions about Albert’s fate but rather only facetious ones, such as ‘‘Albert is probably a successful furrier’’ (Murray, 1973, p. 5). This search for answers was made even more dif- ficult because Watson, late in his life, burned all of his research notes and papers, which may have included information about Little Albert (Buckley, 1989). Now, however, there are two competing evidentiary-based answers as to Albert’s iden- tity—Douglas Merritte (Beck & Irons, 2011; Beck et al., 2009; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2010; Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, & Irons, 2012a, 2012b) and Albert Barger (Digdon, Powell, & Harris, 2014; Digdon, Powell, & Smithson, 2014; Powell, Dig- don, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). Because the supporting publi- cations for each proposed Albert are so new, a discussion of this identity debate is not available in current introductory text- books. In fact, given the recency of the relevant publications, Griggs (2014) found that less than 40% of the current introduc- tory textbooks in his text sample even mentioned the first pro- posed candidate, and only one text mentioned the possibility that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the
  • 44. experiment. In addition, given the 3-year revision cycle for introductory textbooks (Griggs, 2006), it will be a few years before the current set of introductory textbooks are able to update their coverage of the Little Albert identity debate. It is the purpose of this article to provide an up-to-date dis- cussion of the debate to be used as a cornerstone resource by psychology teachers (and textbook authors) for their classroom presentations on (or textbook coverage of) the Little Albert identity saga.1 My synopses of the searches that identified the two candidates only provide the highlights of each search. For the full details, the cited references should be consulted. Hence, this article should be used as an advance organizer for reading the articles relevant to this debate. The two Albert candidates will be discussed separately, but some comparison of the evi- dence for the two candidates will be provided in the discussion of the second candidate, Albert Barger. I will discuss Douglas Merritte first because the articles advancing his candidacy were published first. Before discussing either candidate, I need to preface these discussions with some general background material important to solving this almost 100-year-old cold case. It concerns the foundation from which the search for Albert began. Watson and Rayner’s (1920) published account of the Little Albert experi- ment, a movie that Watson made of his research with infants 1 Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA Corresponding Author: Richard A. Griggs, 4515 Breakwater Row West, Jacksonville, FL 32225, USA. Email: [email protected] Teaching of Psychology 2015, Vol. 42(1) 14-18
  • 45. ª The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0098628314562668 top.sagepub.com by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://top.sagepub.com http://top.sagepub.com/ which included some footage of the Little Albert experiment (Watson, 1923), and the personal information that Watson pro- vided about Albert in other accounts of the experiment (e.g., Watson & Watson, 1921) comprised the foundation for the search. However, it is important to note that there were inconsis- tencies in Watson’s various accounts of the experiment, further complicating the search for Albert (Harris, 1979; Samelson, 1980). It is also important to note that Albert was referred to as ‘‘Albert B.’’ in the experiment. Whether this was a pseudo- nym or Albert’s actual name was not known. In addition, the fact that Albert’s mother was a wet nurse at the Harriet Lane Home for Invalid Children, a pediatric facility at Johns Hopkins, where the experiment was conducted, is critical to the search. Albert and his mother lived at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment. The Phipps Clinic where Watson and Rayner con- ducted their research was located next to the Harriet Lane Home (Fridlund et al., 2012a). This then is the starting point for Hall Beck and his fellow researchers in their search for the identity of Albert that culminated with their proposal that Douglas Mer- ritte was Little Albert.
  • 46. Douglas Merritte Goaded by student questions about the fate of Little Albert, Hall Beck became determined to try to find answers (Beck et al., 2009). A search of the Johns Hopkins archives led to the discovery of a series of memos exchanged between Watson and the school’s president. These memos allowed Beck to determine that the first part of the experiment was likely done in late November or early December in 1919. Using the age of Albert at the time of this part of the experiment (given in Watson & Rayner, 1920), Beck deter- mined that Albert was born between March 2 and March 16 in 1919. The next step was to try to identify Albert’s mother. A check of the Johns Hopkins census of 1920 by Sharman Levinson, one of Beck’s coinvestigators, revealed that three women were listed as ‘‘foster mothers’’—Pearl Barger, Ethel Carter, and Arvilla Merritte—and it seemed plausible that the title of foster mother would encompass being a wet nurse. Beck and his research team then spent hundreds of hours checking various types of records, such as birth, death, and marriage records, to determine whether any of these three women had given birth to a boy in March 1919 (Beck et al., 2009). Ethel Carter was eliminated because she was an African American and Albert appears to have been Caucasian. Pearl Barger (who was of particular interest because the B. in Albert B. could very plausibly have stood for Barger) was eliminated because no evidence that she had a child was found. However, it was dis- covered that Arvilla Merritte had given birth to a boy on March 9, 1919, and that both mother and son had lived together on the Johns Hopkins campus. Next, a genealogical search revealed
  • 47. that two of Arvilla’s grandchildren were currently living in Maryland. Gary Irons, one of the grandchildren, confirmed that his grandmother had worked at the Harriet Lane Home and had given birth to a son named Douglas Merritte. Thus, Arvilla would probably still have been lactating and able to serve as a wet nurse at the time of the Little Albert experiment. A problematic aspect of these findings concerned the name that Watson and Rayner assigned to their infant subject, Albert B. The American Psychological Association did not have an ethics code at the time of the Little Albert experiment so there was no need for confidentiality and the use of pseudonyms for experimental subjects. Watson and Rayner named their infant subject Albert B. and not Douglas M. However, a conversation with Charles Brewer, an expert on John Watson, provided Beck and his colleagues with at least a tenable explanation of why the infant in the study might have been named Albert B. According to Brewer, it could have been an instance of Wat- son’s playful use of names. Watson’s mother and maternal grandmother were very religious, and Watson was named John Broadus in honor of a prominent Baptist minister, John Albert Broadus (Beck et al., 2009). Hence, Watson possibly may have playfully derived Albert B. from John Albert Broadus. The next phase of the search began with a fortuitous discovery of an old trunk with contents from Arvilla Merritte’s life (Beck et al., 2009). Among the contents was a portrait of Douglas when an infant. A comparison of a photograph of this portrait and some enlarged stills that Beck made of Little Albert from Watson’s movie of the experiment followed. This comparison of images did not reveal anything substantive, making it clear that a more thor- ough, expert biometric analysis was warranted. A subsequent
  • 48. bio- metric analysis, however, only led to the conclusion that the photograph and stills could be of the same person. Although the visual and biometric comparisons ruled out a definitive identifica- tion of Albert, Beck et al. (2009) argued that these photographic data in conjunction with their other findings of 10 attributes shared by Little Albert and Douglas Merritte, such as living with his mother at the Harriet Lane Home at the time of the experiment and that Douglas was the same age as Albert when the initial base- line data were collected, strongly supported their hypothesis that Douglas was Albert. If Douglas Merritte were Little Albert, then what would that tell us about Albert’s fate? Sadly, Douglas Merritte died from hydrocephalus in 1925 at the age of 6. How he acquired it could not be determined by Beck et al. (2009), but they speculated that he had contracted meningitis. Fridlund, Beck, Goldie, and Irons (2012a) later reported that Douglas’s nephew was not sure if Douglas was ever able to walk during his short, illness-laden life and that it is unclear as to whether he ever spoke. Fridlund et al. (2012a) also argued that a closer examination of the clips from Watson’s (1923) film in which Little Albert appeared and the subsequent review of some newly obtained medical records of Douglas Merritte revealed that Albert was neurologically impaired at the time of the experiment.2 Fridlund et al.’s detailed analyses of Albert’s behavior in the film clips suggested to them that Albert had substantial beha-
  • 49. vioral and neurological deficits. A subsequent examination of Douglas Merritte’s medical records was consistent with this hypothesis in that they showed that Douglas suffered from con- genital hydrocephalus. The records also indicated that Albert’s experimental sessions occurred during periods when Douglas’s medical condition was relatively stable. Fridlund et al. further argued that there were ample sources of information available Griggs 15 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ to Watson that would have almost certainly made him aware of Douglas’s medical condition. Thus, if Douglas Merritte were Albert, then these new findings by Fridlund et al. not only con- tradict Watson and Rayner’s assertion that Little Albert was ‘‘normal’’ and ‘‘healthy’’ but also lead to the conclusion that Watson and Rayner would have almost certainly had to know about Little Albert’s medical condition, raising even more seri- ous ethical questions about the already ethically questionable Little Albert experiment. Albert Barger Soon after the publication of Beck et al. (2009), some other researchers outlined difficulties with the Douglas Merritte hypothesis and argued that the Little Albert identity case was far from closed and thus warranted further investigation (Powell, 2010, 2011; Reese, 2010; but see Beck et al.’s rejoin- der, 2010). For example, Powell (2011) pointed out a difficulty with Beck et al.’s estimated timeline for when the initial base- line session likely took place. The congruence of the reported age of Albert and Douglas Merritte’s age at the time of the
  • 50. baseline session was a critical component of Beck et al.’s case. Powell found evidence that the baseline session could have been delayed well beyond the time proposed by Beck et al., making Douglas older than Albert at that time. Another point of contention involved a comment by Watson (1925) that Albert was later adopted, but Douglas Merritte had remained with his mother and had not been adopted. Given such difficulties with the Douglas Merritte candidacy and concerns about the weak evidence for Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) claim that Little Albert had neurological impairments and the profound ethical implications of this claim, Russell Powell, Nancy Digdon, and Ben Harris decided to conduct their own search for an alternative candidate for Little Albert. To aid in the search, they enlisted the help of a professional genealo- gist, Christopher Smithson. They began their search by further investigating Pearl Barger, the foster mother for whom Beck et al. (2009) found no evidence of a baby while she resided at the hospital. Their first break came when they found a genealogical document on the Internet on the history of the Martinek family in Baltimore (Powell, Digdon, Harris, & Smithson, 2014). It revealed that Charles Martinek married Pearl Barger in 1921, that they had three children, one of whom was named Albert, and that Charles preferred to use the name Martin, which led to the discovery that Pearl Barger and Charles Martin had a baby in 1919, 2 years before their mar- riage. A search of U.S. census records revealed that Charles Martin was living in Baltimore in 1940 with three children, the oldest being William A., who was the same age as the unnamed son born to the Martins in 1919. Then a search of more birth and death certificates and the medical archives at Johns Hopkins, which included the medical records of William A. Barger and Douglas Merritte, led to more discoveries. Signifi- cantly, William A.’s name was recorded in his medical file as Albert Barger, thereby matching Little Albert’s name in the experiment—Albert B. This agrees with his niece’s report that
  • 51. although his given name was William Albert, he was typically called Albert throughout his life (Digdon et al., 2014). Powell et al. (2014) also found that Albert Barger, like Dou- glas Merritte, was the correct age (8 months 26 days) to have been Little Albert at the time of the initial baseline session. In addition, Albert Barger was discharged from the hospital at the age of 12 months 21 days, Little Albert’s age when the final experimental session took place and when his mother removed him from the hospital. Douglas Merritte’s medical file, however, indicated that he was discharged at 12 months 15 days of age, about a week earlier than Albert Barger and younger than Little Albert when he left the hospital. Of most significance, Powell and his colleagues further learned that Albert Barger’s weight at the time of the initial baseline session was very close to that reported for Little Albert by Watson and Rayner (1920), 21 pounds 15 ounces versus 21 pounds, respectively. Douglas Mer- ritte, however, only weighed 14 pounds 14 ounces at this time. Douglas’s extremely low body weight also conflicts with Wat- son and Rayner’s description of Little Albert as a healthy and well-developed child. Fridlund et al.’s (2012a) analysis of the clips with Little Albert from Watson’s film led them to believe that Albert had numerous behavioral and social deficits that were consistent with neurological impairment resulting from hydrocephalus. In contrast, Digdon, Powell, and Harris’s (2014) analysis of these clips suggested otherwise. For example, Fridlund et al. claimed that Little Albert showed no signs of social referen- cing, the tendency of infants to look toward caretakers when confronted with novelty. However, according to Digdon et al., there do appear to be some instances of what appears to be mutual gaze between Albert and Watson. In addition, Powell et al. (2014) contend that the selective nature of the film
  • 52. clips may account for Fridlund et al.’s observation that Albert seemed focused only on what was in front of him, with little awareness of the people around him. As they pointed out, the clips in Watson’s film were selected to show Albert’s reactions to the stimuli presented to him rather than to the people near him, so off-task behaviors were likely not included in the film. Dig- don et al. further pointed out that these 34 brief clips, averaging only 9 s (SD ¼ 6 s) in length, cannot be considered a represen- tative sample of Albert’s behavior and that it is certainly ques- tionable that anyone could validly diagnose neurological impairment from such a limited sample of behavior.3 Thus, in their opinion, any appraisal of behavioral or neurological deficits from these film clips of Little Albert would, ‘‘at best, be highly speculative.’’4 For more detail on Powell and his coinvestiga- tors’ analysis of these film clips and why the clips comprise an inadequate measure of Albert’s neurological status, see Dig- don, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014). If Albert Barger were Little Albert, what could be said about Albert’s fate? Albert Barger lived a long life, dying in 2007 at the age of 87.5 Did he grow up to have a fear of furry animals and objects? Powell et al. (2014) were surprised when they first learned from Barger’s niece that her uncle had an aversion (but not a particularly strong one) to dogs and animals in general. The aversion, however, appears to have been more of a dislike 16 Teaching of Psychology 42(1) by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ of animals than a phobia, but that his aversion was at least par-
  • 53. tially due to his conditioning experiences cannot be entirely ruled out. In addition, according to Powell et al, Albert’s con- ditioning experiences did not appear to have had any adverse effects on his personality. Sadly, Albert Barger died before anyone could tell him that it was highly likely that he was the famous Little Albert in the psychological literature. As far as his niece knows, her uncle was unaware of the experiment and did not even know that his mother was once a wet nurse. Hence, we will never know what his reaction would have been when learning about the strong possibility that he was Little Albert. However, when asked what her uncle would have thought about all of this, his niece said that ‘‘he would have been thrilled’’ (Bartlett, 2014, p. B10). Epilogue As pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), applying Occam’s razor to this situation would indicate that Albert Barger is far more likely to have been Little Albert. The evidence for Albert Barger’s candidacy is more parsimonious than that for Douglas Merritte. Albert Barger matches Little Albert on all of the key attributes, so fewer assumptions are needed. " His name matches the Albert B. name assigned to the infant in Watson and Rayner’s study. " His body weight at the time of the initial baseline phase of the experiment matches Little Albert’s reported body weight and his chubby appearance in Watson’s film clips of the experiment. " His age on the day he left the hospital was the same as Little Albert’s age on that day. " His general state of health as an infant matches that described by Watson and Rayner (1920).
  • 54. Douglas Merritte does not match Little Albert on any of these key attributes and thus was likely not Little Albert. However, according to Bartlett (2014), Beck, Fridlund, and Goldie, all Merritte proponents, still believe Douglas Merritte was Little Albert.6 In addition, as pointed out by Powell et al. (2014), although the evidence that Albert Barger was Little Albert is very strong, it is not entirely conclusive. For example, contrary to Watson’s statement that Little Albert was adopted shortly after he left by an out-of-town family (Watson, 1925), it appears that Albert Barger was not adopted. It is possible though that he was informally adopted for a short time after leaving the hospital and then later reunited with his mother, perhaps after her and Charles Martin married. In addition, it is possible that Watson (1925) was wrong, and the Little Albert purported adoption was just a myth because there is no corroborative evidence of such an adoption (Beck et al., 2010). Regardless, even though the Little Albert saga has always had characters and plot, it has never had a credible conclusion. Perhaps now it does. Finally, if Albert Barger were Little Albert, then Watson was not guilty of the unethical, fraudulent behavior of know- ingly using a neurologically impaired infant in his research. This is very important because the story alleging such behavior has already become widespread on the Internet (e.g., DeAnge- lis, 2012). Hence, many psychology students have likely been exposed to this story, which has now been shown to be very unlikely. Given that there are already inaccuracy problems with the coverage of the Little Albert experiment in introductory psychology textbooks (see Griggs, 2014), getting the Little Albert identity saga correctly described in our classrooms and
  • 55. textbooks becomes of critical importance. As a beginning point in doing so, I recommend that psychol- ogy teachers and textbook authors use this article as a guide for a careful examination of the articles cited here in preparing their coverage of the search for Little Albert. This should help to insure its accuracy. The obvious downside of inaccuracies in the coverage of the Little Albert story is that students will be misled into accepting the story as fact, and sadly, it seems that students seldom question the stories that they are told (Burton, 2011). Thus, it is important to the psychological teaching com- munity to identify inaccuracies in our lectures and textbooks, so that they can be corrected and we as teachers and textbook authors do not continue to ‘‘give away’’ false information about our discipline. Hopefully, this article will help in achieving this goal, at least with respect to the Little Albert saga. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Notes 1. An article by Tom Bartlett in the Chronicle of Higher Education (2014) covers content similar to that in this article but in a less for-
  • 56. mal style without references. I highly recommend that introductory psychology teachers and textbook authors read Bartlett’s article because it will serve as an excellent resource for their lecture and textbook coverage of the Little Albert story. The online version at http://chronicle.com/article/The-Search-for-Psychologys/146747 includes video footage of the Little Albert experiment and several historical photos related to the search for Little Albert. 2. The assessments of Little Albert’s behavior in the film by Fridlund, a clinical psychologist, and Goldie, a pediatric neurologist, were made before the discovery of Douglas Merritte’s medical records, but the evaluation by Waterman, a specialist in childhood psycho- pathology, was made after the discovery of Merritte’s medical records (H. P. Beck, personal communication, August 12, 2014). All three assessments were made independently, and Goldie’s
  • 57. assessment was blind to Fridlund’s assessment just as Waterman’s assessment was blind to Fridlund and Goldie’s prior assessments and tentative hypotheses (Fridlund et al., 2012b). 3. Although Digdon, Powell, and Harris (2014) and Powell et al. (2014) described the film clips of Little Albert edited from Wat- son’s (1923) film as 5 min in length and divided into three seg- ments, Fridlund et al. (2012a) described the clips that they Griggs 17 by guest on January 14, 2015top.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://top.sagepub.com/ analyzed as 4 min in length and divided into four segments. The reason for these differences is presently unknown. 4. Fridlund et al. (2012a, pp. 21–22) claimed that signs of Albert’s neu- rological impairment may have eluded other viewers of Watson’s film because they were expecting to see the healthy, normal baby that
  • 58. Watson described repeatedly in his writings. But, as pointed out by Nancy Digdon, these expectancy effects on perception would also apply to the observations of viewers who believe that Albert is neurologically impaired (N. Digdon, personal communication, July 8, 2014). If viewers were led to think that Albert was neurologically impaired, then they would likely see signs of such impairment. To illustrate the power of such ‘‘expectancy effects,’’ Nancy suggests the following classroom demonstration, which involves showing the Lit- tle Albert film in class (free clips of this film are readily available on the Internet). Before presenting the film, give students a brief handout to prime one third of the class to expect Albert to be impaired, another third to expect Albert to be exceptionally well developed, and the final
  • 59. third with no prime. After the film, have students evaluate Albert’s developmental status. Differences in students’ appraisals of Albert should prompt a more general class discussion about the subjectivity of observations and why scientific approaches require … See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368 Whatever Happened to Little Albert? Article in American Psychologist · February 1979 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.2.151 CITATIONS 160 READS 2,841 1 author: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: History of Psychiatry View project
  • 60. History of Psychology View project Ben Harris University of New Hampshire 48 PUBLICATIONS 377 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Ben Harris on 06 December 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368_Whatever _Happened_to_Little_Albert?enrichId=rgreq- e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232506368_Whatever _Happened_to_Little_Albert?enrichId=rgreq- e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/History-of-Psychiatry- 5?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/History-of-Psychology- 7?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3
  • 61. D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_New_Ha mpshire?enrichId=rgreq-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Ben_Harris17?enrichId=rg req-e96cf4d19f7a54677e5ae4332b1982a6- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjUwNjM2ODtBUz o0MzY0MTUzNDAyNTcyODFAMTQ4MTA2MDkzMzk4OQ%3 D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf Whatever Happened to Little Albert? BEN HARRIS Vassar College
  • 62. ABSTRACT: John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner's 1920 conditioning of the infant Albert B. is a well- known piece of social science folklore. Using pub- lished sources, this article reviews the study's actual procedures and its relationship to Watson's career and work. The article also presents a history of psycholo- gists' accounts of the Albert study, focusing on the study's distortion by Watson himself, general textbook authors, behavior therapists, and most recently, a prominent learning theorist. The author proposes pos- sible causes for these distortions and analyzes the Albert study as an example of myth making in the history of psychology. Almost 60 years after it was first reported, Watson and Rayner's (1920) attempted conditioning of the infant Albert B. is one of the most widely cited experiments in textbook psychology. Under- graduate textbooks of general, developmental, and abnormal psychology use Albert's conditioning to illustrate the applicability of classical conditioning to the development and modification of human emotional behavior. More specialized books focus- ing on psychopathology and behavior therapy (e.g., Eysenck, 1960) cite Albert's conditioning as an ex- perimental model of psychopathology (i.e., a rat phobia) and often use Albert to introduce a dis- cussion of systematic desensitization as a treat- ment of phobic anxiety. Unfortunately, most accounts of Watson and Rayner's research with Albert feature as much fabrication and distortion as they do fact. From information about Albert himself to 'the basic ex- perimental methods and results, no detail of the
  • 63. original study has escaped misrepresentation in the telling and retelling of this bit of social science folklore. There has recently been a revival of interest in Watson's conditioning research and theorizing (e.g., MacKenzie, 1972; Seligman, 1971; Weimer & Palermo, 1973; Samelson, Note 1), and in the mythology of little Albert (Cornwell & Hobbs, 1976; Larson, 1978; Prytula, Oster, & Davis, 1977). However, there has yet to be a complete examination of the methodology and results of the Vol. 34, No. 2, 151-160 Albert study and of the process by which the study's details have been altered over the years. In the spirit of other investigations of classic studies in psychology (e.g., Ellenberger, 1972; Parsons, 1974) it is time to examine Albert's con- ditioning in light of current theories of learning. It is also time to examine how the Albert study has been portrayed over the years, in the hope of discovering how changes in psychological theory have affected what generations of psychologists have told each other about Albert. The Experiment As described by Watson and Rayner (1920), an experimental study was undertaken to answer three questions: (1) Can an infant be conditioned to fear an animal that appears simultaneously with a loud, fear-arousing sound? ( 2 ) Would such fear transfer to other animals or to inanimate objects? (3) How long would such fears persist? In at- tempting to answer these questions, Watson and
  • 64. Rayner selected an infant named Albert B., whom they described as "healthy," and "stolid and un- emotional" (p. 1). At approximately 9 months of age, Albert was tested and was judged to show no fear when successively observing a number of live animals (e.g., a rat, a rabbit, a dog, and a monkey), and various inanimate objects (e.g., cotton, human masks, a burning newspaper). He was, however, judged to show fear whenever a long steel bar was unexpectedly struck with a claw hammer just behind his back. Two months after testing Albert's apparently unconditioned reactions to various stimuli, Watson and Rayner attempted to condition him to fear a Preparation of this article was aided by the textbook and literature searches of Nancy Kinsey, the helpful com- ments of Mike Wessels, and the bibliographic assistance of Cedric Larson. The author also thanks Bill Wood- ward and Ernest Hilgard for their comments on earlier versions of this work. Requests for reprints should be sent to Ben Harris, Box 368, Vassar College, Poughkeepsie, New York 12601. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • FEBRUARY 1979 • 151 Copyright 1979 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0003-066X/79/3402-01Sl$00.75 white rat. This was done by presenting a white rat to Albert, followed by a loud clanging sound
  • 65. (of the hammer and steel bar) whenever Albert touched the animal. After seven pairings of the rat and noise (in two sessions, one week apart), Albert reacted with crying and avoidance when the rat was presented without the loud noise. In order to test the generalization of Albert's fear response, 5 days later he was presented with the rat, a set of familiar wooden blocks, a rabbit, a short-haired dog, a sealskin coat, a package of white cotton, the heads of Watson and two assist- ants (inverted so that Albert could touch their hair), and a bearded Santa Glaus mask. Albert seemed to show a strong fear response to the rat, the rabbit, the dog, and the sealskin coat; a "nega- tive" response to the mask and Watson's hair; and a mild response to the cotton. Also, Albert played freely with the wooden blocks and the hair of Watson's assistants. After an additional 5 days, Watson recondi- tioned Albert to the rat (one trial, rat paired with noise) and also attempted to condition Albert di- rectly to fear the previously presented rabbit (one trial) and dog (one trial). When the effects of this procedure were tested in a different, larger room, it was found that Albert showed only a slight reaction to the rat, the dog, and the rabbit. Consequently, Watson attempted "to freshen the reaction to the rat" (p. 9) by presenting it with the loud noise. Soon after this, the dog began to bark loudly at Albert, scaring him and the experi- menters and further confounding the experiment. To answer their third question concerning the permanence of conditioned responses over time,
  • 66. Watson and Rayner conducted a final series of tests on Albert after 31 days of neither condition- ing nor extinction trials. In these tests, Albert showed fear when touching the Santa Claus mask, the sealskin coat, the rat, the rabbit, and the dog. At the same time, however, he initiated contact with the coat and the rabbit, showing "strife be- tween withdrawal and the tendency to manipulate" (Watson & Rayner, 1920, p. 10). Following these final tests, Albert's mother removed him from the hospital where the experiment had been conducted. (According to their own account, Watson and Rayner knew a month in advance the day that Albert would no longer be available to them.) The Context of Watson and Rayner's Study What was the relationship of the Albert experi- ment to the rest of Watson's work? On a per- sonal level, this work was the final published project of Watson's academic career, although he supervised a subsequent, related study of the de- conditioning of young children's fears '(M. C. Jones, 1924a, 1924b). From a theoretical per- spective, the Albert study provided an empirical test of a theory of behavior and emotional de- velopment that Watson had constructed over a number of years. Although Watson had publicly declared himself a "behaviorist" in early 1913, he apparently did not become interested in the conditioning of motor and autonomic responses until late 1914, when he read a French edition of Bekhterev's Objective Psychology (see Hilgard & Marquis, 1940). By