SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 49
Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users
and nearby peers
Faria Sana a, Tina Weston b,c, Nicholas J. Cepeda b,c,*
a McMaster University, Department of Psychology,
Neuroscience, & Behaviour, 1280 Main Street West, Hamilton,
ON L8S 4K1, Canada
b York University, Department of Psychology, 4700 Keele
Street, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
c York University, LaMarsh Centre for Child and Youth
Research, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, ON M3J 1P3, Canada
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 11 September 2012
Received in revised form
5 October 2012
Accepted 12 October 2012
Keywords:
Laptops
Multitasking
Attentional control
Pedagogy
a b s t r a c t
Laptops are commonplace in university classrooms. In light of
cognitive psychology theory on costs
associated with multitasking, we examined the effects of in-
class laptop use on student learning in
a simulated classroom. We found that participants who
multitasked on a laptop during a lecture scored
lower on a test compared to those who did not multitask, and
participants who were in direct view of
a multitasking peer scored lower on a test compared to those
who were not. The results demonstrate that
multitasking on a laptop poses a significant distraction to both
users and fellow students and can be
detrimental to comprehension of lecture content.
! 2012 Elsevier Ltd.
1. Introduction
Multitasking is ingrained in our daily lives. As you read this
article, you may also be attending to a text message, sipping
coffee, or writing
out a list of to-dos. Such a lifestyle is intended to increase
efficiency; however, there are limitations to how well multiple
tasks can be carried
out concurrently (Posner, 1982). Multitasking places
considerable demands on cognitive resources, which, in turn,
degrades overall
performance, as well as performance on each task in isolation
(Broadbent,1958). The issue of multitasking and its
consequences has become
a growing concern in education, as students are more commonly
found engaged with their laptops or smartphones during class
time. The
current study investigated the effect of laptop multitasking on
both users and nearby peers in a classroom setting.
There is a host of theoretical and experimental research on
divided attention and dual-task interference, terms that we
consider
homologous to multitasking and therefore relevant to the
current discussion. Research suggests that we have limited
resources available to
attend to, process, encode, and store information for later
retrieval (Posner, 1982). When focused on a single primary
task, our attentional
resources are well directed and uninterrupted, and information
is adequately processed, encoded, and stored (Naveh-Benjamin,
Craik,
Perretta, & Tonev, 2000). When we add a secondary task,
attention must be divided, and processing of incoming
information becomes
fragmented. As a result, encoding is disrupted, and this reduces
the quantity and quality of information that is stored (Pashler,
1994). When
we eventually retrieve information that was processed without
interruptions, as a primary task, we are likely to experience
minimal errors.
When we retrieve information that was processed via
multitasking or with significant interruptions from a secondary
task, we are more
likely to experience some form of performance decrement
(Wickens & Hollands, 2000).
Indeed, managing two or more tasks at one time requires a great
deal of attention. Attentional resources are not infinite (Konig,
Buhner, &
Murling, 2005; Pashler, 1994). When the level of available
attentional resources is less than what is required to complete
two simultaneous
tasks, performance decrements are experienced since both tasks
are competing for the same limited resources. This is especially
true if both
tasks are competing for resources within the same sensory
modality (Navon & Gopher, 1979; Wickens, 2002; Wickens &
Hollands, 2000).
Limits to attentional resources means the quality (accuracy) and
efficiency (reaction time) at which multiple tasks are processed
will be
compromised (Rubinstein, Meyer, & Evans, 2001). Numerous
experimental studies have shown performance decrements under
conditions
of multitasking or divided attention (e.g., Broadbent, 1958;
Tulving & Thomson, 1973).
* Corresponding author. York University, LaMarsh Centre for
Child and Youth Research, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, ON M3J
1P3, Canada. Tel.: þ1 416 736 2100x33266;
fax: 1 416 736 5814.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (F. Sana), [email protected]
(T. Weston), [email protected] (N.J. Cepeda).
Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect
Computers & Education
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/compedu
0360-1315 ! 2012 Elsevier Ltd.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003
Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–31
Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license.
Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license.
Delta:1_surname
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03601315
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/compedu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
emyil LU
笔记本电脑在大学教室里很常见。根据认知心理学关于多任务相关成本的理论,
我们在模拟教室中研究了课堂内使用笔记本电脑对学生学习的影响。我们发现,
在一次讲座中在笔记本电脑上进行多任务处理的参与者在一次测试中的得分低于
没有进行多任务处理的参与者,而直接面对多任务处理同伴的参与者在一次测试
中的得分低于没有进行多任务处理的参与者。研究结果表明,笔记本电脑上的多
任务处理对用户和同学来说都是一个很大的干扰,并且可能会对课堂内容的理解
造成损害。
一。介绍
多任务处理在我们的日常生活中根深蒂固。当你读到这篇文章的时候,你可能也
在处理一条短信,喝咖啡,或者写一份待办事项清单。这样的生活方式旨在提高
效率;然而,多个任务同时执行的程度是有限制的(波斯纳,1982)。多任务
处理对认知资源提出了相当大的要求,反过来,认知资源又会降低总体性能,同
时也会降低单独执行每个任务的性能(Broadbent,1958)。多任务处理问题
及其后果已经成为教育界日益关注的问题,因为学生在上课时间更常使用笔记本
电脑或智能手机。目前的研究调查了在教室环境中,笔记本电脑多任务处理对用
户和附近同龄人的影响。
关于注意力分散和双重任务干扰,有大量的理论和实验研究,我们认为这些术语
与多任务是同源的,因此与当前的讨论有关。研究表明,我们可以处理、处理、
编码和存储信息以供以后检索的资源是有限的(Posner,1982)。当我们专注
于一个单一的主要任务时,我们的注意力资源得到了很好的引导和不间断的处理
,信息得到了充分的处理、编码和存储(Naveh
Benjamin、Craik、Perretta和Tonev,2000)。当我们添加第二个任务
时,必须分散注意力,对传入信息的处理变得支离破碎。结果,编码被中断,这
降低了存储信息的数量和质量(Pashler,1994)。当我们最终检索在没有中
断的情况下处理过的信息时,作为主要任务,我们可能会遇到最小的错误。当我
们从次要任务中检索通过多任务处理或有重大中断的信息时,我们更可能会经历
某种形式的性能下降(Wickens&Hollands,2000)。
实际上,一次管理两个或多个任务需要大量的关注。注意资源不是无限的(Kon
ig,Buhner,&Murling,2005;Pashler,1994)。当可用注意力资源
的水平低于完成两个同时任务所需的水平时,由于两个任务都在争夺相同的有限
资源,因此会出现性能下降。如果两个任务都在同一感官模式下竞争资源,这一
点尤其正确(Navon&Gopher,1979;Wickens,2002;Wickens&H
ollands,2000)。注意资源的限制意味着处理多个任务的质量(准确性)和
效率(反应时间)将受到损害(Rubinstein,Meyer,&Evans,2001)。
大量的实验研究表明,在多任务或注意力分散的情况下,表现会下降(例如,Br
oadbent,1958;Tulving&Thomson,1973)。
Theoretical and empirical findings on multitasking are
especially significant when considered in the context of student
learning. In
classroom environments, students tend to switch back and forth
between academic and non-academic tasks (Fried, 2008). This
behavior
poses concerns for learning. The presumed primary tasks in
many university classes are to listen to a lecture, consolidate
information spoken
by the instructor and presented on information slides, take
notes, and ask or respond to questions. On their own, these
activities require
effort. If a secondary task is introduced, particularly one that is
irrelevant to the learning context, attention must shift back and
forth
between primary and secondary tasks, thereby taxing attentional
resources. This multitasking can result in weaker encoding of
primary
information into long-term memory (Bailey & Konstan, 2006;
Ophira, Nass, & Wagner, 2009).
The personal computer provides a compelling source of
classroom distraction and has become commonplace on
university campuses.
Survey data estimates that 99% of incoming freshmen own a
laptop (University of Virginia, 2009) and about 65% of students
bring their laptop
to class (Fried, 2008). Research on educational laptop use
addresses both the pros and cons of using this technology in the
classroom. On the
one hand, laptops have been shown to assist learning through
active approaches to teaching (Finn & Inman, 2004) and
promotion of academic
success (Lindorth & Bergquist, 2010; Weaver & Nilson, 2005).
When used for academic purposes such as taking notes and
using software
programs (Driver, 2002), accessing supplemental resources and
web-based activities (Debevec, Shih, & Kashyap, 2006), and
viewing Power-
Point slides (McVay, Snyder, & Graetz, 2005), in-class laptop
use can increase satisfaction, motivation, and engagement
among students (Fried,
2008; Hyden, 2005; Weaver & Nilson, 2005). On the other
hand, studies suggest that students who use laptops in class
report low satisfaction
with their education, are more likely to multitask in class, and
are more distracted (Wurst, Smarkola, & Gaffney, 2008).
Student self-reports and
classroom observations suggest that laptops are being used for
non-academic purposes, such as instant messaging and playing
games (Barak,
Lipson, & Lerman, 2006; Driver, 2002), checking email and
watching movies (Finn & Inman, 2004), and browsing the
Internet (Bugeja, 2007).
Access to online entertainment makes it increasingly difficult
for instructors to be “more interesting than YouTube”
(Associated Press, 2010,
p. 10), especially if students aren’t intrinsically motivated by
the subject materials. Moreover, time spent multitasking with
these activities is
significant; data from one study estimates that students
multitask for approximately 42% of class time (Kraushaar &
Novak, 2010).
Importantly, distractions from in-class multitasking correlate
with decrements in learning. Students who multitask on laptops
during
class time have impaired comprehension of course material and
poorer overall course performance (Barak et al., 2006;
Hembrooke & Gay,
2003; Kraushaar & Novak, 2010). In a recent study, Wood et al.
(2012) measured the detriments of technology-based
multitasking in
a classroom setting. This study is one of few in the field that
employed an experimental design (much of the literature is self-
report). Students
were assigned either to a single multitasking condition (using
Facebook, MSN, email, or cell phone texting), a control group
(paper and pencil
notes-only or word processing notes-only), or a free-use-of-
technology condition (where participants could choose to
multitask or not
multitask on their laptop as much as they wished). Over the
course of three class lectures, participants’ comprehension of
the material was
assessed via quizzes. In general, paper and pencil control
participants outperformed multitasking participants on the quiz
assessments
(particularly MSN and Facebook users). However, as the
authors admit, there were limitations to the methodology of this
study. Most
noteworthy was that 43% of participants self-reported that they
did not adhere to their assigned instructions across all three
lectures. For
example, a participant assigned to the Facebook multitasking
condition may have multitasked on Facebook and on MSN (i.e.,
two forms of
multitasking when they were instructed only to use one form),
or chosen not to multitask on Facebook at all. Therefore, the
experimental
manipulation was not successful, calling into question the
validity of the quiz data. Although the authors corrected for this
limitation in post-
hoc analyses, the results should be interpreted with caution as
they are reliant on self-report and the sample size of each group
was
significantly reduced. Wood et al.’s findings are relevant but
restricted in terms of the pedagogical recommendations that can
be offered. One
goal of the present study was to replicate the findings of Wood
et al. using a more controlled design and more stringent fidelity
measures.
Disrupting one’s own learning is an individual choice; harming
the learning of other students in the class is disrespectful.
Laptop
distractions due to movement of images and laptop screen
lighting (Melerdiercks, 2005) and multitasking activities (Crook
& Barrowcliff,
2001) may cause involuntary shifts of attention among students
in close proximity to laptop users (Barak et al., 2006; Chun &
Wolfe,
2001; Finn & Inman, 2004). These studies suggest that students
are annoyed and distracted by laptop use. However, to our
knowledge,
no studies have directly measured the effects of distraction
caused by laptop users on surrounding peers’ learning.
Therefore, a second goal
of the present study was to examine the indirect effects of
laptop multitasking on student learning.
2. Experiment 1
In Experiment 1, we investigated whether multitasking on a
laptop would hinder learning as measured by performance on a
compre-
hension test. All participants were asked to attend to a
university-style lecture and take notes using their laptops as a
primary task. Half the
participants, by random assignment, received additional
instructions to complete a series of non-lecture-related online
tasks at any
convenient point during the lecture. These tasks were
considered secondary and were meant to mimic typical student
web browsing during
class in terms of both quality and quantity. We hypothesized
that participants who multitasked while attending to the lecture
would have
lower comprehension scores compared to participants who did
not multitask.
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
Forty-four undergraduate students from a large comprehensive
university in a large Canadian city participated in the study (25
females;
M age ¼ 18.9 years, SD ¼ 2.0). All participants were enrolled
in an Introductory Psychology course and received course credit
for partici-
pating in the experiment. Participants represented a variety of
undergraduate disciplines (i.e., not only psychology). They were
recruited
using an online portal designed for psychology research, which
explained that the study involved listening to a class lecture and
filling out
a few questionnaires. Only students who could bring a personal
laptop to the experiment were invited to participate. Forty
participants were
included in the final data analysis, which included two
experimental conditions: multitasking (n ¼ 20) and no
multitasking (n ¼ 20). Of the
four participants removed from the analysis, two had previous
knowledge of the lecture content (as measured by a screening
questionnaire),
one performed below chance on the comprehension test, and one
failed to follow instructions. The former two participants were
removed
from the no multitasking condition, and the latter two
participants were removed from the multitasking condition.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–31 25
emyil LU
从学生学习的角度来看,关于多任务的理论和经验发现尤其重要。在课堂环境中
,学生倾向于在学术和非学术任务之间来回切换(Fried,2008)。这种行为引
起了对学习的关注。在许多大学课堂上,假定的主要任务是听讲座,巩固讲师所
讲的信息,并在信息幻灯片上展示,做笔记,提出或回答问题。这些活动本身就
需要努力。如果引入次要任务,特别是与学习上下文无关的任务,则注意力必须
在主任务和次要任务之间来回移动,从而征税注意力资源。这种多任务处理会导
致初级信息在长期记忆中的编码减弱(Bailey&Konstan,2006;Ophira,
Nass,&Wagner,2009)。
个人电脑提供了一个令人信服的来源,教室分心,并已成为常见的大学校园。调
查数据估计99%的新生拥有笔记本电脑(弗吉尼亚大学,2009年),约65%的
学生带着笔记本电脑来上课(Fried,2008年)。关于教育用笔记本电脑的研究
既解决了在课堂上使用这项技术的利弊。一方面,笔记本电脑通过积极的教学方
法(Finn&Inman,2004)和促进学术成功来帮助学习(Lindorth&Berg
quist,2010;Weaver&Nilson,2005)。当用于学术目的时,例如记笔
记和使用软件程序(Driver,2002),访问补充资源和基于网络的活动(Deb
evec,Shih,&Kashyap,2006),以及观看Power-
Point幻灯片(McVay,Snyder,&Graetz,2005),课堂上使用笔记本
电脑可以提高满意度和动力,以及学生之间的参与(Fried,2008;Hyden,
2005;Weaver&Nilson,2005)。另一方面,研究表明,在课堂上使用笔
记本电脑的学生对自己的教育满意度较低,更有可能在课堂上进行多任务处理,
而且更容易分心(Wurst、Smarkola和Gaffney,2008)。学生自我报告和
课堂观察表明,笔记本电脑被用于非学术目的,例如即时信息和玩游戏(Barak
,Lipson,&Lerman,2006;Driver,2002)、查看电子邮件和观看电
影(Finn&Inman,2004)以及浏览互联网(Bugeja,2007)。网络娱乐
使得教师变得越来越难“比YouTube更有趣”(美联社,2010年,第10页),
尤其是如果学生不是天生受主题材料的激励。此外,使用这些活动的多任务处理
时间是显著的;来自一项研究的数据估计学生的任务约占课堂时间的42%(K劳
什哈尔和诺瓦克,2010)。
重要的是,课堂上多任务的分心与学习能力的下降有关。上课时间在笔记本电脑
上进行多任务处理的学生对课程材料的理解能力受损,整体课程表现较差(Bara
k等人,2006年;Hembrooke&Gay,2003年;Kraushaar&Novak,2
010年)。在最近的一项研究中,伍德等人。(2012)衡量了课堂环境中基于
技术的多任务处理的危害。这项研究是该领域少数采用实验设计的研究之一(大
部分文献是自报告的)。学生被分配到一个单独的多任务状态(使用Facebook
、MSN、电子邮件或手机短信)、一个控制组(仅纸条和铅笔笔记或文字处理
笔记)或一个免费的技术使用状态(参与者可以选择在笔记本电脑上执行多任务
或不执行多任务)。在三堂课的过程中,参与者对材料的理解通过测验来评估。
一般来说,纸笔控制的参与者在测验评估中的表现优于多任务参与者(尤其是M
SN和Facebook用户)。然而,正如作者所承认的,这项研究的方法有局限性
。最值得注意的是,43%的受试者自我报告说,他们在所有三次讲座中都没有遵
守指定的指导。例如,分配到Facebook多任务条件的参与者可能在Facebook
和MSN上有多任务(即,当他们被指示只使用一个表单时,有两种多任务形式
),或者选择根本不在Facebook上执行多任务。因此,实验操作并不成功,这
就对测验数据的有效性提出了质疑。尽管作者在事后分析中纠正了这一局限性,
但结果应谨慎解释,因为它们依赖于自我报告,并且每组的样本量显著减少。伍
德等人的发现是相关的,但就可以提供的教学建议而言,这一发现受到了限制。
本研究的一个目标是复制伍德等人的发现。采用更可控的设计和更严格的保真度
测量。
扰乱自己的学习是个人的选择;损害班上其他学生的学习是不尊重的。笔记本电
脑分散注意力,由于移动的图像和笔记本电脑屏幕照明(MeLedieCKS,200
5)和多任务活动(克鲁克和BARROKLIFY,2001)可能导致无意间转移
注意力的学生之间的接近笔记本电脑用户(Barak等人,2006;春和沃尔夫,2
001;芬恩和英曼,2004)。这些研究表明,学生对使用笔记本电脑感到恼火
和分心。然而,据我们所知,还没有研究直接测量笔记本电脑用户分心对周围同
伴学习的影响。因此,本研究的第二个目标是研究笔记本电脑多任务处理对学生
学习的间接影响。
2。实验1
在实验1中,我们研究了笔记本电脑上的多任务是否会阻碍学习,这是通过综合
测试的表现来衡量的。所有参与者都被要求参加一个大学式的讲座,并以笔记本
电脑为主要任务做笔记。一半的参与者,通过随机分配,在讲座期间的任何方便
的时间点,收到了完成一系列与讲座无关的在线任务的额外指示。这些任务被认
为是次要的,旨在从质量和数量上模拟课堂上典型的学生网络浏览。我们假设参
加讲座的多任务参与者与没有多任务的参与者相比,理解分数更低。
2.1条。方法
2.1.1条。参与者
来自加拿大大城市一所大型综合性大学的44名本科生(25名女性;M年龄1/4
18.9岁,SD 1/4
2.0)参加了该研究。所有参与者都参加了心理学入门课程,并获得了参与实验
的学分。参与者代表各种本科学科(即,不仅是心理学)。他们是使用一个为心
理学研究设计的在线门户网站招募的,该网站解释说,这项研究包括听一堂课堂
讲座和填写一些问卷。只有能带个人笔记本电脑参加实验的学生才被邀请参加。
40名参与者被纳入最终数据分析,其中包括两个实验条件:多任务(n
1/420)和无多任务(n1/420)。从分析中剔除的四名受试者中,有两名之前
对课程内容有所了解(通过筛选问卷进行测量),一名在理解测试中的表现低于
预期,还有一名未能按照说明进行。前两名参与者被从无多任务状态中移除,后
两名参与者被从多任务状态中移除。
2.1.2. Materials
A 45-min PowerPoint lecture on meteorology was created by
one of the experimenters (TW) in conjunction with her peer
colleagues and
a faculty member with many years of teaching experience. The
lecture was based on topics taken from an introductory
meteorology
textbook (Ahrens, 1999; e.g., discriminating cloud types,
pressure systems, thunderstorm development). A second faculty
member with
expertise in Geology and Earth Sciences reviewed the lecture
and approved the accuracy of its content, level of difficulty, and
consistency
with materials presented in related undergraduate classes (S.
Carey, personal communication, August 16, 2012). The same
experimenter
(TW), an upper-level graduate student with lecturing
experience, acted as the “professor” and presented the lecture
live to the class. At the
time of data collection, TW had practiced and given this lecture
over a dozen times for an independent study. She followed a
memorized
script.
For the multitasking condition, a set of 12 online tasks was
created. An example of a task was “What is on Channel 3
tonight at 10 pm?”
These online tasks were meant to mimic typical student
browsing during class in terms of both quality (i.e., visiting
websites of interest to
a young adult sample, such as Google, YouTube, and Facebook)
and quantity [w40% of class time, as suggested by Kraushaar
and Novak
(2010)]. A pilot study (n ¼ 5), confirmed that completion of the
tasks was not overwhelming; tasks could be completed in w15
min (or
33% of the lecture time). Although we do not know if the
participants of our study multitask more or less than 33% during
lectures in a real-
world setting, the online tasks were within the critical range of
previously reported time spent on multitasking in real-world
classrooms
(w40%), and therefore unlikely to artificially increase the costs
of multitasking.
The primary measure of learning was a four-option multiple-
choice comprehension test with 20 questions evaluating simple
knowledge
(i.e., basic retention of facts from the lecture) and 20 questions
evaluating application of knowledge (i.e., applying a concept
from the lecture
to solve a novel problem). Question type was included as
variable to examine whether multitasking outcomes might differ
depending on the
difficulty level of the material being tested. There is some
evidence to suggest that multitasking may be particularly
detrimental to complex
knowledge (e.g., Foerde, Knowlton, & Poldrack, 2006). The
ordering of the questions on the test (simple vs. complex) was
intermixed.
Participants also completed a brief questionnaire that collected
demographic data (e.g., age, gender, and fluency in English)
and screened
participants for prior familiarity with the lecture content and
general interest in the lecture presentation. Additionally, there
were two
questions, both listed on a 7-point Likert scale, directed toward
participants in the multitasking condition: (1) To what extent do
you think
the act of multitasking hindered your learning of the lecture
material? (1 ¼ did not hinder my learning; 4 ¼ somewhat
hindered my
learning; 7 ¼ definitely hindered my learning) and (2) To what
extent do you think your multitasking hindered the learning
experience of
other students? (1 ¼ did not hinder others’ learning; 4 ¼
somewhat hindered others’ learning; 7 ¼ definitely hindered
others’ learning).
Responses to these questions allowed us to measure subjective
student views on multitasking outcomes.
2.1.3. Design and procedure
All participants were asked to bring their personal laptops to the
experiment. They received an instruction sheet and a consent
form. The
instruction sheet asked participants to attend to the lecture and
use their personal laptop to take notes on the information being
presented,
just as they might normally do in class. In addition to taking
lecture notes, half of the participants (randomly selected) were
instructed to
complete the 12 online tasks at some point during the lecture.
The experiment was conducted in a classroom with four rows of
tables, each with five chairs. Therefore, since there was a
maximum of 20
seats, we repeated the experiment three times to obtain a total
sample of 44 participants [each repeat included roughly the
same number of
total participants (range: 14–15), and an equal divide of
participants within the two experimental conditions].
Participants faced a projector
screen at the front of the classroom. Instruction sheets were
randomly placed at each seat. Thus, seat location of participants
in each
condition was fully random. Participants were randomly
presented with a seat number as they entered the classroom and
were asked to
settle in and read the instruction sheet and consent form at their
assigned seat. While all participants were instructed to take
notes on their
laptops during the lecture, some were also required to complete
the series of online tasks. An experimenter (FS) remained at the
back of the
classroom during the lecture presentation and used a seating
map to track participants’ seat location, monitor participants’
screen activities,
and ensure that all instructions were being followed. At the end
of the lecture, participants were asked to email their notes and
(if
applicable) their responses to the online tasks to the
experimenters and, finally, to put away their laptops. The
comprehension test
immediately followed and a 30 min time limit was enforced (as
time limits are realistic of typical university examinations). All
participants
completed the test within the time limit. Once the experimenters
collected all the tests, participants responded to the
questionnaire, were
debriefed and dismissed.
2.1.4. Fidelity measures
FS closely monitored participants’ activities throughout the
lecture presentation, observing each participant’s activities at
least once
every 3–4 min interval. This was done to ensure that all
participants adhered to their assigned instructions. If a
participant was not on task
(e.g., a non-multitasker engaging in multitasking activities, a
multitasker browsing on a website irrelevant to the online tasks,
or a multi-
tasker completely ignoring the online tasks), they were probed
once and reminded of their specific instructions. If a participant
was probed
more than 2 times, their data were discarded from the final
analysis (n ¼ 1).
Participants’ notes and online task answers were analyzed for
completion and quality. In terms of the online tasks, all
multitaskers attempted
to answer at least some of the tasks. On average, multitaskers
successfully completed 9 out of the 12 tasks (M ¼ 0.75; SD ¼
0.25). In terms of
participants’ notes, all participants took some form of notes on
the lecture content. Notes were scored for quality (1–5) by the
experimenter
most familiar with the material (TW). She was blind to
participants’ condition while scoring. A score of 1 meant the
participant attempted to
copy the lecture slides verbatim, but the notes were
disorganized and/or missing information. A score of 3 meant the
participant copied the
lecture slides verbatim, but did not include additional
information presented verbally by the lecturer. A score of 5
meant the participant copied
all slide information and included all information presented
verbally by the lecturer. Analysis of the quality scores revealed
that multitaskers’
notes (M ¼ 2.7, SD ¼ 1.2) were of a poorer quality than non-
multitaskers’ notes (M ¼ 4.1, SD ¼ 1.0), t(34) ¼ 3.6, p ¼ .001,
u2 ¼ .23.
Therefore, our fidelity measures (i.e., participant monitoring,
discarded data, analysis of notes and online tasks files) clearly
show that
participants stayed on task throughout the experiment. It is
evident that multitasking played a role in impairing
participants’ note-taking
ability.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–3126
emyil LU
2.1.2条。材料
其中一位实验者(TW)和她的同事以及一位有多年教学经验的教职员工共同制
作了一个45分钟的气象学PowerPoint讲座。讲座的主题取自气象学入门教材
(Ahrens,1999;例如,区分云类型、压力系统、雷暴发展)。第二位具有地
质学和地球科学专业知识的教师对讲座进行了回顾,并对其内容的准确性、难度
水平以及与相关本科课程中所提供材料的一致性进行了认可(S.Carey,《个人
沟通》,2012年8月16日)。同一名实验者(TW)是一名有授课经验的高级
研究生,他担任“教授”,并将讲座现场呈现给全班同学。在收集资料的时候,T
W已经为一项独立的研究做了十多次的练习和演讲。她遵循一个熟记的剧本。
对于多任务情况,创建了一组12个联机任务。一个任务的例子是“今晚10点3频
道有什么节目?“这些在线任务旨在模拟典型的学生在课堂上浏览的质量(即访问
年轻人感兴趣的网站,如谷歌、YouTube和Facebook)和数量(Kraushaa
r和Novak(2010年)建议的课堂时间的40%)。一项试点研究(n 1/4
5)证实,任务的完成不是压倒性的;任务可以在15分钟内完成(或33%的授课
时间)。尽管我们不知道在真实世界的课堂上,我们研究的参与者多任务处理的
比例是否在33%以上,但在线任务在之前报道的真实课堂多任务处理时间(w4
0%)的临界范围内,因此不太可能人为地增加多任务处理的成本。
学习的主要衡量标准是一个四选项的多项选择理解测试,其中20个问题评估简单
知识(即基本保留讲座中的事实)和20个问题评估知识的应用(即应用讲座中的
概念解决新问题)。问题类型作为变量被包括进来,以检验多任务处理的结果是
否会因被测材料的难度水平而不同。有证据表明,多任务可能对复杂知识特别有
害(例如,Foerde、Knowlton和Poldrack,2006)。测试中问题的顺序
(简单与复杂)是混合的。
参与者还完成了一份简短的问卷调查,收集了人口统计数据(例如,年龄、性别
和英语流利程度),并筛选了参与者之前对演讲内容的熟悉程度和对演讲的总体
兴趣。此外,还有两个问题,都列在7分利克特量表上,针对多任务情况下的参
与者:(1)你认为多任务行为在多大程度上阻碍了你对讲稿的学习?(1
1/4没有妨碍我的学习;4 1/4在一定程度上妨碍了我的学习;7
1/4肯定妨碍了我的学习)和(2)你认为你的多任务处理在多大程度上妨碍了其
他学生的学习体验?(1 1/4不妨碍他人学习;4
1/4在一定程度上妨碍他人学习;7
1/4肯定妨碍他人学习)。对这些问题的回答使我们能够衡量学生对多任务结果
的主观看法。
2.1.3条。设计和程序
所有参与者都被要求带上他们的个人笔记本电脑参加实验。他们收到了一份说明
书和一份同意书。教学表要求参与者参加讲座,并使用他们的个人笔记本电脑记
录所呈现的信息,就像他们通常在课堂上做的那样。除了做课堂笔记,一半的参
与者(随机选择)被要求在课堂上的某个时间点完成12项在线任务。
实验是在一间教室里进行的,教室里有四排桌子,每排有五把椅子。因此,由于
有最多20个座位,我们重复实验三次,以获得44名参与者的总样本[每个重复包
括大致相同数量的总参与者(范围:14至15),以及在两个实验条件下均等的
参与者的分界线]。参与者面对着教室前面的投影仪屏幕。在每个座位上随机放置
说明书。因此,在每种情况下,参与者的座位位置都是完全随机的。参与者在进
入教室时随机获得一个座位号,要求他们在指定的座位上安顿下来并阅读教学表
和同意书。虽然所有的参与者都被要求在讲座中用笔记本电脑做笔记,但也有一
些人被要求完成一系列的在线任务。一名实验者(FS)在演讲期间留在教室后面
,并使用座位图跟踪参与者的座位位置,监控参与者的屏幕活动,并确保所有指
示都得到遵守。在讲座结束时,参与者被要求将他们的笔记和(如果适用的话)
他们对在线任务的回应发送给实验者,最后,把他们的笔记本电脑收起来。紧接
着进行了理解测试,并强制执行了30分钟的时间限制(因为时间限制对于典型的
大学考试来说是现实的)。所有参与者都在规定的时间内完成了测试。一旦实验
者收集了所有的测试,参与者回答了问卷,听取了汇报,并被解雇。
2.1.4条。忠实度
四季酒店在演讲过程中密切监控参与者的活动,每3-
4分钟至少观察一次参与者的活动。这样做是为了确保所有参与者都遵守他们指
定的指示。如果参与者没有执行任务(例如,一个非多任务者从事多任务活动,
一个多任务者浏览与在线任务无关的网站,或者一个多任务者完全忽略在线任务
),他们会被探测一次并被提醒他们的特定指令。如果参与者被调查超过2次,
则他们的数据将从最终分析中丢弃(n 1/41)。
分析参与者的笔记和在线任务答案的完成情况和质量。就在线任务而言,所有多
任务处理者都试图回答至少一些任务。平均而言,多任务者成功完成了12项任务
中的9项(M
1/40.75;sd1/40.25)。就参与者的笔记而言,所有参与者都对讲座内容做了
某种形式的笔记。笔记的质量(1-
5)由最熟悉材料的实验者(TW)评分。她在得分时对参与者的情况视而不见
。分数为1表示参与者试图逐字复制演讲幻灯片,但笔记杂乱无章和/或缺少信息
。分数为3意味着参与者逐字复制了演讲幻灯片,但不包括讲师口头提供的附加
信息。5分意味着参与者复制了所有幻灯片信息,并包含了讲师口头陈述的所有
信息。对质量分数的分析显示,多任务者的笔记(M 1/42.7,SD
1/41.2)质量比非多任务者的笔记(M 1/44.1,SD
1/41.0),t(34)1/43.6,p 1/4.001,U21/4.23)差。
因此,我们的忠诚度测量(即参与者监控、丢弃的数据、笔记分析和在线任务文
件)清楚地表明,参与者在整个实验过程中都在执行任务。很明显,多任务处理
在削弱参与者记笔记的能力方面起到了一定的作用。
2.1.5. Results and discussion
There were no demographic differences between participants of
the two conditions in terms of age, gender, fluency in English,
or high
school GPA. To examine potential differences between
conditions on the comprehension test, a 2 (condition:
multitasking, no multi-
tasking) # 2 (question type: simple, complex) mixed factorial
ANOVA was conducted with condition as a between-subjects
factor and
question type as a within-subjects factor. The main effect of
condition was significant, F(1,38) ¼ 10.2, p ¼ .003, u2 ¼ .20.
Participants who
multitasked during the lecture (M ¼ 0.55, SD ¼ 0.11, n ¼ 20)
scored significantly lower than participants who did not
multitask (M ¼ 0.66,
SD ¼ 0.12, n ¼ 20). The main effect of question type was also
significant, F(1,38) ¼ 17.7, p < .001, u2 ¼ .30. Participants
scored higher on simple
factual questions (M ¼ 0.60, SD ¼ 0.13, n ¼ 20) than on
complex apply-your-knowledge questions (M ¼ 0.56, SD ¼
0.13, n ¼ 20). This main
effect simply reflects the difficulty of the questions created.
The interaction was not significant, F(1,38) ¼ 0.79, p ¼ .380.
These findings
demonstrate a strong, detrimental effect of multitasking on
comprehension scores. Overall, participants who multitasked
scored 11% lower
on a post-lecture comprehension test (Fig. 1).
3. Experiment 2
In Experiment 2, we investigated whether being in direct view
of a multitasking peer would negatively influence learning as
measured
by performance on a comprehension test. A new group of
participants was asked to take notes using paper and pencil
while attending to the
lecture. Some participants were strategically seated throughout
the classroom so that they were in view of multitasking
confederates on
laptops, while others had a distraction-free view of the lecture.
Confederates mimicked multitaskers from Experiment 1 by
typing notes on
the lecture and performing other concurrent, irrelevant online
tasks. We hypothesized that participants who were seated in
view of
multitasking peers would have lower comprehension scores
compared to participants who had minimal or no visual
distraction from
multitasking peers.
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
Thirty-nine undergraduate students from the same university
participated in the study (26 females; M age ¼ 20.3 years, SD ¼
4.2). None
had participated in Experiment 1. Recruitment procedures and
participant incentives were the same as in Experiment 1. Thirty-
eight
participants were included in the final data analysis, which
included two experimental conditions: in view of multitasking
peers (n ¼ 19)
and not in view of multitasking peers (n ¼ 19). The one
participant excluded from the analysis was removed because of
prior familiar with the
lecture content (as measured by a screening questionnaire).
Thirty-six undergraduate students were recruited to be
confederates.
3.1.2. Materials
The same materials were used as in Experiment 1 with the
exception of two questions on the questionnaire. Instead of
asking about
whether or not multitasking hindered self and peer learning, the
two questions in this experiment were directed toward
participants in
view of technology: (1) To what extent were you distracted by
other students’ laptop use around you? (1 ¼ not distracted at
all;
4 ¼ somewhat distracted; 7 ¼ very distracted) and (2) To what
extent do you think being in view of other students’ laptop use
hindered your
learning of the lecture material? (1 ¼ did not hinder my
learning; 4 ¼ somewhat hindered my learning; 7 ¼ definitely
hindered my
learning). Responses to these questions provided us with
subjective student measures on whether or not their
multitasking peers were
a distraction, and whether they perceived this distraction to be a
barrier to learning.
3.1.3. Design and procedure
As in Experiment 1, participants were asked to bring their
personal laptops to the experiment; however, only those
assigned as
confederates actually used their laptops (n ¼ 36). The
experimental participants (n ¼ 38) were instructed to keep their
laptops in their
knapsacks.
Fig. 1. Proportion correct on the comprehension test as a
function of condition (multitasking vs. no multitasking).
Multitasking lowered test performance by 11%, p < .01. Error
bars
represent standard error of the mean.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–31 27
emyil LU
2.1.5条。结果和讨论
两种情况的参与者在年龄、性别、英语流利程度或高中平均成绩方面没有人口统
计学差异。为了检验理解测试条件之间的潜在差异,采用2(条件:多任务,无
多任务)2(问题类型:简单,复杂)混合因素方差分析,条件为被试间因素,
问题类型为被试内因素。条件的主要影响是显著的,F(1,38)1/4 10.2,p
1/4.003,u2 1/4.20。在讲座中进行多任务处理的参与者(M 1/4
0.55,SD 1/4 0.11,n 1/4
20)的得分显著低于未进行多任务处理的参与者(M 1/4 0.66,SD 1/4
0.12,n 1/4
20)。问题类型的主要影响也很显著,F(1,38)1/417.7,p<0.001,u2
1/4.30。参与者在简单事实问题(M 1/4 0.60,SD 1/4 0.13,n 1/4
20)上的得分高于复杂应用知识问题(M 1/4 0.56,SD 1/4 0.13,n 1/4
20)。这一主要效果只是反映了问题产生的难度。相互作用不显著,F(1,38)
1/40.79,p
1/4.380。这些发现表明,多任务处理对理解成绩有很强的不利影响。总的来
说,多任务的参与者在课后理解测试中的得分要低11%(图1)。
三。实验二
在实验2中,我们研究了在理解测试中,直接面对多任务同伴是否会对学习产生
负面影响。一组新的参与者被要求在听课时用纸和铅笔做笔记。一些参与者策略
性地坐在教室里,这样他们就可以在笔记本电脑上看到多任务联盟,而另一些人
则可以自由地观看讲座。同盟者模仿实验1中的多任务者,在讲座上输入笔记,
并执行其他并发的、不相关的在线任务。我们假设坐在多任务同伴面前的受试者
的理解得分低于那些对多任务同伴的视觉干扰最小或没有的受试者。
3.1条。方法
3.1.1条。参与者
来自同一所大学的39名本科生(26名女性;M年龄1/4 20.3岁,SD 1/4
4.2)参加了研究。无
参加了实验1。招聘程序和参与者激励措施与实验1相同。38名参与者被纳入最
终数据分析,其中包括两个实验条件:针对多任务同伴(n
1/419)和不针对多任务同伴(n1/419)。被排除在分析之外的一名受试者因
事先熟悉讲座内容(通过筛选问卷测量)而被剔除。36名大学生被招募为同盟军
。
3.1.2条。材料
除问卷上的两个问题外,实验1使用的材料与实验1相同。这个实验中的两个问题
不是问多任务是否妨碍了自我学习和同伴学习,而是从技术的角度对参与者提出
的:(1)你在多大程度上被周围其他学生使用笔记本电脑分散了注意力?(1
1/4完全没有分心;4 1/4有点分心;7
1/4非常分心)和(2)您认为考虑到其他学生使用笔记本电脑的情况,在多大程
度上妨碍了您学习讲义?(1 1/4没有妨碍我的学习;4
1/4在一定程度上妨碍了我的学习;7
1/4肯定妨碍了我的学习)。对这些问题的回答为我们提供了一个主观的学生衡
量标准,即他们的多任务同伴是否是一种干扰,以及他们是否认为这种干扰是学
习的障碍。
3.1.3条。设计和程序
与实验1一样,参与者被要求携带他们的个人笔记本电脑参加实验;然而,只有
那些被指定为同盟者的人才实际使用他们的笔记本电脑(n
1/436)。实验参与者(n 1/438)被要求将笔记本电脑放在背包中。
所有参与者都收到了一份指导表和一份同意书。同盟会的说明书上解释说,他们
是同盟会成员,他们被要求在浏览互联网(如电子邮件、脸谱网)和在演讲时假
装对演讲内容做笔记之间进行切换。事实上,他们被告知不需要注意讲座。参与
者的教学用纸要求他们保存笔记本电脑,并使用实验者提供的纸和铅笔对授课内
容进行书面记录,就像他们在课堂上通常做的那样。
房间布置与实验1相同。再次,由于最多有20个座位,我们重复实验四次,以获
得39名实验参与者和36名联盟成员的总样本[每个重复包括大致相同数量的总参
与者(范围:18~20),在课堂中与参与者相比(在2:1比)大致更多。在每个
座位上都战略性地放置了说明书和同意书。参与者在进入教室时随机获得座位号
。一些参与者坐在两个多任务联合会的后面(即,他们在左视野中看到一个笔记
本电脑用户,在右视野中看到另一个笔记本电脑用户;图2)。这些参与者是从
多任务同伴的角度考虑的。其他的参与者坐在和他们一样,被要求在讲座上做书
面记录的参与者后面。这些参与者被认为没有考虑到多任务对等(图2)。
讲座如实验1所示。演讲时,一名实验者留在教室后面,用座位图跟踪参与者和
同盟者的座位位置,监控笔记本电脑屏幕活动和记录,并确保所有指示都得到遵
守。讲座结束后,从参与者那里收集书面笔记,要求同盟者完成他们的工作并存
储他们的笔记本电脑。这时,盟军被要求离开这个房间。他们被其中一个实验者
听取了汇报,然后被解雇了。其余的参与者接受了理解测试,并执行了30分钟的
时间限制,所有参与者都在该时间限制内成功完成了测试。为了保持动机水平,
参与者被告知,那些离开教室的学生将在一天后返回,届时他们将完成延迟的理
解测试。一旦实验者收集了所有的测试,参与者回答了问卷,听取了汇报,并被
解雇。
All participants received an instruction sheet and a consent
form. The confederates’ instruction sheet explained that they
were
confederates, and they were required to use their laptops to flip
between browsing the Internet (e.g., email, Facebook) and
pretending to
take notes on the lecture content as the lecture was presented. In
fact, they were told they were not required to pay attention to
the lecture.
The participants’ instruction sheet asked them to keep their
laptops stored, and to use the paper and pencil provided by the
experimenters
to take written notes on the lecture content, just as they might
normally do in class.
The room set-up was the same as in Experiment 1. Again, since
there was a maximum of 20 seats, we repeated the experiment
four times
to obtain a total sample of 39 experimental participants and 36
confederates [each repeat included roughly the same number of
total
participants (range: 18–20), with more confederates in the
classroom than participants (roughly a 2:1 ratio)]. Instruction
sheets and consent
forms were strategically placed at each seat. Participants were
randomly presented with a seat number as they entered the
classroom. Some
participants were seated so that they were behind two
multitasking confederates (i.e., they were in view of one laptop
user in their left
visual field and another laptop user in their right visual field;
Fig. 2). These participants were considered in view of
multitasking peers. Other
participants were seated behind participants who, like
themselves, were asked to take written notes on the lecture.
These participants were
considered not in view of multitasking peers (Fig. 2).
The lecture was presented as in Experiment 1. While the lecture
was being presented, an experimenter (FS) remained at the back
of the
classroom and used a seating map to track participants’ and
confederates’ seat locations, monitor laptop screen activities
and note-taking,
and ensure that all instructions were being followed. When the
lecture ended, written notes were collected from the
participants, and
confederates were asked to finish up their work and store their
laptops. At this point, confederates were asked to leave the
room. They were
debriefed by one of the experimenters and dismissed. The
remaining participants were given the comprehension test and a
30 min time
limit was enforced, with all participants successfully
completing the test within the time limit. To maintain
motivation levels, participants
were told a cover story that those students who had left the
classroom were going to return one day later, at which time they
would
complete a delayed comprehension test. Once the experimenters
collected all the tests, participants responded to the
questionnaire, were
debriefed and dismissed.
3.1.4. Fidelity measures
FS closely monitored participants’ activities throughout the
lecture presentation, as in Experiment 1. This was done to
ensure that all
participants adhered to their assigned instructions. If a
participant or confederate was not on task (e.g., a confederate
not using their laptop,
Fig. 2. Visual representation of participants who were and were
not in view of a multitasking peer. In view participants were
strategically seated behind two confederates, with one
confederate’s laptop screen w45$ to the participant’s right and
the other’s w45$ to the participant’s left. Not in view
participants were seated similarly behind two experimental
subjects who took handwritten notes.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–3128
emyil LU
3.1.4条。忠实度
在整个演讲过程中,如实验1,FS密切监控参与者的活动。这样做是为了确保所
有参与者都遵守他们指定的指示。如果参与者或联盟成员没有执行任务(例如,
联盟成员没有使用他们的笔记本电脑,或者参与者没有做任何笔记),则会对他
们进行调查并提醒他们具体的指示。所有同盟国和参与者都遵守了他们的指示。
使用实验1中报告的相同量表对参与者的笔记质量进行评分。所有参与者都对讲
座内容做了某种形式的笔记。不考虑多任务同伴的参与者笔记(M 1/4
3.6,SD 1/4 1.3)的质量与考虑多任务同伴的参与者笔记(M 1/4 3.7,SD
1/4 1.2)的质量相似,t<1。
因此,我们的忠实度测量(即参与者监控、笔记分析)清楚地表明,参与者在整
个实验过程中坚持任务,并做了全面的笔记。考虑到多任务的同龄人并没有降低
笔记质量。
3.1.5条。结果和讨论
两种情况的参与者在年龄、性别、英语流利程度或高中平均成绩方面没有人口统
计学差异。为了检验理解测试中不同条件之间的潜在差异,采用2(条件:考虑
多任务,而不是考虑多任务)2(问题类型:简单,复杂)混合因子方差分析,
条件为被试间因素,问题类型为被试内因素。病情的主要影响显著,F(1,36)
1/4 21.5,p<0.001,u2
1/4.36。多任务同伴的参与者在测试中的得分(M 1/4 0.56,SD 1/4
0.12,n 1/4 19)明显低于没有多任务同伴的参与者(M 1/4 0.73,SD
1/4 0.12,n 1/4
19)。问题类型的主要影响也很显著,F(1,36)1/411.3,p
1/4.002,u21/4.21。参与者在简单问题(M 1/4 0.69,SD 1/4 0.14,n
1/4 20)上的得分高于复杂问题(M 1/4 0.60,SD 1/4 0.15,n 1/4
20)。相互作用不显著,F(1,36)1/40.91,p
1/4.347。这些发现表明,同龄人的多任务处理分散了那些试图只关注演讲的
参与者的注意力。那些考虑到多任务的同龄人在课后理解测试中的得分要低17%
(图3)。
四。一般性讨论
我们的实验复制了一个重要的发现并引入了一个新的发现。首先,当参与者在学
习过程中执行多个任务时,他们的理解能力会受到损害,一个是听讲稿和做笔记
的主要任务,另一个是完成不相关的在线任务的次要任务。这一结果并不令人惊
讶,并且与其他研究(例如,Barak等人,2006;Hembrooke&Gay,200
3;Kraushaar&Novak,2010;Wood等人,2012)报告的结果一致,但
我们使用了一种更为可控的程序来确认。第二,坐在多任务环境中的参与者的理
解能力受损。这一发现表明,尽管这些参与者积极地尝试学习材料(如综合笔记
所证明的那样,其质量与那些对演讲有清晰看法的人相似),但他们的同伴的选
择使他们处于不利地位。
我们的实验应用于自然界,结果,并没有对多任务或注意力理论做出重大贡献。
然而,研究结果与理论一致,即分配给任务的注意程度直接关系到所处理信息的
质量和数量。虽然我们没有直接测量注意力,但所有参与者都在积极地倾听和记
录所提供的信息。我们有很强的保真度指标,这使得我们能够更确定(与之前的
研究相比)与我们的操作无关的因素不会给数据增加其他方差来源。因此,我们
可以推测,注意力是由于我们的操作而受损的,无论是以自我多任务的形式还是
以多任务同伴的视角。
在实验1中,参与者听讲座,做笔记,完成在线任务。这种同时执行多个任务的
做法似乎阻碍了测试时的信息检索,可能是由于学习过程中编码不好(多任务者
笔记质量较差)和分配有限注意力资源的效率低下所致。在实验2中,受试者在
他们的周边视觉中有分散注意力的活动时,倾听所呈现的信息并做笔记。邦联的
笔记本电脑屏幕可能分散了参与者的注意力,使他们无法将全部注意力集中在演
讲上。参与者仍然能够在讲座上做笔记;但是,缺乏完全的注意力集中可能会影
响对所写信息的详细阐述和处理,从而降低理解测试期间成功的检索尝试。未来
的实验应旨在弥合记忆和注意的认知原则与应用教学研究之间的鸿沟,以明确和
直接地测试这些围绕多任务和注意的理论主张。更严格的方法可以提供进一步的
实验证据来检验这些假设。例如,可以使用眼睛跟踪方法来确定学生注意力转移
的时间、地点和持续时间,以及从讲课时间向外看是否与特定于学生错过的信息
的考试成绩相关。
or a participant not taking any notes), they were probed and
reminded of their specific instructions. All confederates and
participants
complied with their instructions.
Participants’ notes were scored for quality using the same scale
reported in Experiment 1. All participants took some form of
notes on the
lecture content. The notes of participants not in view of
multitasking peers (M ¼ 3.6, SD ¼ 1.3) were similar in quality
to the notes of
participants in view of multitasking peers (M ¼ 3.7, SD ¼ 1.2),
t < 1.
Therefore, our fidelity measures (i.e., participant monitoring,
analysis of notes) clearly show that participants stayed on-task
throughout
the experiment and took comprehensive notes. Being in view of
multitasking peers did not reduce note quality.
3.1.5. Results and discussion
There were no demographic differences between participants of
the two conditions in terms of age, gender, fluency in English,
or high
school GPA. To examine potential differences between
conditions on the comprehension test, a 2 (condition: in view of
multitasking, not in
view of multitasking) # 2 (question type: simple, complex)
mixed factorial ANOVA was conducted with condition as a
between-subjects
factor and question type as a within-subjects factor. The main
effect of condition was significant, F(1,36) ¼ 21.5, p < .001, u2
¼ .36.
Participants in view of multitasking peers scored significantly
lower on the test (M ¼ 0.56, SD ¼ 0.12, n ¼ 19) than
participants not in view of
multitasking peers (M ¼ 0.73, SD ¼ 0.12, n ¼ 19). The main
effect of question type was also significant, F(1,36) ¼ 11.3, p ¼
.002, u2 ¼ .21.
Participants scored higher on simple questions (M ¼ 0.69, SD ¼
0.14, n ¼ 20) than on complex questions (M ¼ 0.60, SD ¼ 0.15,
n ¼ 20). The
interaction was not significant, F(1,36) ¼ 0.91, p ¼ .347. These
findings suggest that peer multitasking distracted participants
who were
attempting to pay sole attention to the lecture. Those in view of
a multitasking peer scored 17% lower on a post-lecture
comprehension test
(Fig. 3).
4. General discussion
Our experiments replicate one important finding and introduce a
new finding. First, participants’ comprehension was impaired
when
they performed multiple tasks during learning, one being the
primary task of attending to the lecture material and taking
notes, and the
other being the secondary task of completing unrelated online
tasks. This result is not surprising and is consistent with those
reported by
other studies (e.g., Barak et al., 2006; Hembrooke & Gay, 2003;
Kraushaar & Novak, 2010; Wood et al., 2012), but we confirm
it using a more
controlled procedure. Second, comprehension was impaired for
participants who were seated in view of peers engaged in
multitasking. This
finding suggests that despite actively trying to learn the
material (as evidenced by comprehensive notes, similar in
quality to those with
a clear view of the lecture), these participants were placed at a
disadvantage by the choices of their peers.
Our experiments were applied in nature and, as a result, do not
make major contributions to multitasking or attention theory.
However,
the results are consistent with theory, namely that the degree of
attention that is allotted to a task is directly related to the
quality and
quantity of information processed. Although we did not directly
measure attention, all participants were actively listening to and
taking
notes on the information presented. We had strong fidelity
measures that allowed us to be more certain (compared to
previous studies) that
factors unrelated to our manipulation were not adding other
sources of variance to the data. Thus, we can speculate that
attention was
impaired due to our manipulation, either in the form of self-
multitasking or being in view of a multitasking peer.
In Experiment 1, participants were listening to the lecture,
taking notes, and completing the online tasks. This exercise of
carrying out
multiple tasks at the same time seemed to have impeded
retrieval of information at the test, likely as a result of poor
encoding during
learning (as evidenced by multitaskers’ poorer quality of notes)
and inefficiency at allocating limited attentional resources. In
Experiment 2,
participants were listening to the information being presented
and taking notes while in the presence of distracting activity in
their
peripheral vision. Confederates’ laptop screens may have
distracted participants from directing their full attention to the
lecture. Partici-
pants were still able to take notes on the lecture; however, a
lack of complete attentional focus may have compromised the
elaboration and
processing of the information being written, thereby lowering
successful retrieval attempts during the comprehension test.
Future
experiments should aim to bridge the gap between cognitive
principles of memory and attention and applied pedagogical
research to
definitively and directly test these theoretical claims
surrounding multitasking and attention. More stringent methods
could provide further
experimental evidence to test these hypotheses. For example,
one could use eye-tracking methodologies to determine when,
where, and for
Fig. 3. Proportion correct on the comprehension test as a
function of condition (view to multitasking vs. no view to
multitasking). Being in view of multitasking peers lowered test
performance by 17%, p < .001. Error bars represent standard
error of the mean.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–31 29
emyil LU
问卷调查的结果增加了对技术对学习影响的讨论。实验1的回答表明,处于多任
务状态的参与者意识到,在讲座期间进行多任务处理会“在一定程度上阻碍”他们
的学习(M 1/4 5.5,SD 1/4
2.0)。然而,他们估计同龄人的学习“几乎不会受到阻碍”(M 1/4 3.3,SD
1/4
1.9)。相比之下,同伴分心(实验2)观察到的效果大小几乎是自我分心(实验
1)观察到的效果大小的两倍。多任务者似乎能够以某种程度上减少分心的方式
来安排多任务活动的时间。那些考虑到多任务的人似乎被引诱去观看其他学生的
笔记本电脑屏幕,即使是在课堂上不合适的时刻,因此对那些考虑到多任务的人
来说,与实际的多任务人相比,造成了更糟糕的学习。这些结论应谨慎解释,并
应在今后的研究中得到贯彻。我们的结论仅仅基于效应大小;由于方法上的差异
,我们不能直接比较不同实验的测试性能。
实验2的问卷调查结果表明,受试者被附近的同盟者“有点分心”(M 1/4
3.3,SD 1/4
2.1),并且考虑到多任务同伴,“几乎”阻碍了他们自己的学习(M 1/4
2.7;SD 1/4
1.6)。因此,总的来说,调查问卷评分表明学生没有接触到同龄人行为的间接
后果。
尽管有文献表明多任务可能对复杂知识的学习特别有害(例如,Foerde等人,2
006年),但我们的结果表明,多任务对简单事实学习和复杂应用学习的损害程
度相同。因此,即使学习了一个新的事实(例如,“哪种云类型在大气中发现得最
高?”?)可能会被自我多任务干扰,或者被同龄人的多任务干扰。
相关的,多任务可能有不同的整体效果取决于任务的难度被耍。一些研究表明,
如果一项主要任务更困难或更新颖,那么它必然需要更大程度的注意力资源才能
在令人满意的水平上完成任务(Kahneman,1973;Posner&Boies,197
1;Styles,2006)。因此,只有在没有其他任务必须同时完成,或者任何次
要任务相对简单或自动(即,如果次要任务不需要很多注意力资源;Kahnema
n&Treisman,1984年)的情况下,才能很好地完成主要任务。后一种情况是
我们实验1的场景。参与者被要求在一项主要任务(需要很多注意力资源)中学
习一些新奇的东西,同时参加一项简单的次要任务(尽管程度不同,但仍然需要
注意力资源)。我们设计了初级和中级任务的难度级别,以模拟已报告为典型课
堂行为的内容(即,学生在参加课堂讲座和查看电子邮件、Facebook和与朋友
聊天之间来回切换)。我们的研究结果表明,尽管第二项任务对大学生来说是相
当不理智的(即随意的上网浏览),但它仍然会对第一项任务的表现产生影响,
正如多任务者的测试分数降低所证明的那样。未来的研究可以通过操纵主要和/或
次要任务的难度水平来进一步研究课堂上多任务的影响,而不是当前设计的操纵
。根据双重任务理论(如Pashler,1994),随着初级或中级任务难度的增加
(如参加物理讲座的学生,但选择把大部分时间花在为下一次历史考试而学习上
。
根据这项研究中报告的证据,我们可以向教育者推荐什么方法来管理课堂上的笔
记本电脑使用?禁止使用笔记本电脑是极端的,也是没有根据的。不可忽视的是
,笔记本电脑在适当使用时会促进积极的学习成果(例如,基于网络的研究、流
行测验、在线案例研究和讨论线索;例如,Finn&Inman,2004年)。当笔记
本电脑被严格用于记笔记时,打字笔记对学习的积极影响与书面笔记相似(Qua
de,1996)。我们的结果通过初步的交叉实验比较证实了这一发现,也就是说
,我们发现,在实验1的无多任务状态(输入笔记的参与者)和实验2的无技术状
态(分别为M 1/4 4.1和M 1/4
3.6)以及随后的理解测试方面,实验1的参与者(输入笔记的参与者)和实验2
的参与者(写笔记的参与者)之间没有显著差异分数(分别为M 1/4 0.66和M
1/4
0.73,记住实验1中的非多任务者有时会考虑多任务者,这可以解释他们的理解
测试分数较低的原因)。因此,出于各种原因,笔记本电脑应该仍然是现代教室
的一种工具,也许有一些合理的限制。
一个建议是教师在课程开始时与学生讨论使用笔记本电脑的后果(Gasser&Pal
frey,2009)。教师可以告知学生滥用笔记本电脑的负面教育结果,并将他们
的观点与学生的观点进行比较和对比。在本次讨论中,全班同学可以集体提出一
些在整个学期课堂上实施的技术礼仪规则(例如,如果你打算同时处理多项任务
,坐在教室后面,这样至少其他学生不会感到困扰;McCreary,2009)。这
样一来,技术和分心的问题就凸显出来了,学生可以做出明智的选择,而不是假
设他们(和他们的同龄人)对多任务缺陷免疫。
另一个建议是明确禁止在学习不需要技术的课程中使用笔记本电脑。有人可能会
争辩说,在课本和讲稿幻灯片上通常提供信息的课程,不需要笔记本电脑,其程
度与动手学习是课程的一个综合组成部分的课程相同,可能是以专门的计算机软
件的形式。这个建议是谨慎的,因为有些学生可能不会从没有笔记本电脑的课程
中受益。例如,残疾学生通常依靠计算机技术来帮助学习(Fichten等人,200
1年)。因此,也许可以允许笔记本电脑在所有课程中使用,但仅限于基于课程
的网站(如果可能的话)。
最终,学生必须为自己的学习负起责任;然而,热情的教师可以影响学生在课堂
上选择如何引导他们的注意力。第三个建议是为教育工作者提供资源,帮助他们
创建丰富的、信息丰富的、交互式的课程,与非课程网站的吸引力相竞争,这样
学生就不会在第一时间滥用笔记本电脑。这可能包括将笔记本电脑融入实时课堂
练习。例如,教师可以要求学生在互联网上搜索缺失的讲座信息,或者找到一个
有趣的在线视频与全班分享。此外,教师可以使用一个共享的网站,让学生能够
对演讲概念的难度等级进行排名,从而允许教师在课堂上评估学生的理解水平。
然后,教师可以复习这些概念,并在课程结束前向学生提供反馈。事实上,有创
造力的教师可以塑造学生在课堂上选择使用笔记本电脑的方式,从而使笔记本电
脑的使用具有建设性。
how long a student’s attention is diverted, and whether looking-
away-from-lecture time correlates with test performance
specific to the
information missed by the student.
Results from the questionnaires add to the discussion of
technology’s impact on learning. Responses from Experiment 1
show that
participants in the multitasking condition were aware that
multitasking during the lecture would “somewhat hinder” their
learning
(M ¼ 5.5, SD ¼ 2.0). However, they estimated peers’ learning
would be “barely hindered” (M ¼ 3.3, SD ¼ 1.9). By contrast,
the observed effect
size from peer distraction (Experiment 2) was nearly twice as
large as the observed self-distraction effect size (Experiment 1).
Multitaskers
appear to have been able to time their multitasking activities in
a manner that reduced distraction to some degree. Those in view
of
multitasking appear to have been lured into watching other
students’ laptop screens even during inopportune moments of
the lecture, thus
creating worse learning for those in view of a multitasker
compared to the actual person who was multitasking. These
conclusions should be
interpreted with caution and deserve follow up in future studies.
Our conclusions are based solely on effect size; we cannot
directly compare
test performance across experiments due to methodological
variation.
Questionnaire responses from Experiment 2 suggest that
participants reported being “somewhat distracted” by nearby
confederates
(M ¼ 3.3, SD ¼ 2.1), and that being in view of a multitasking
peer “barely” hindered their own learning (M ¼ 2.7; SD ¼ 1.6).
Thus, overall, the
questionnaire ratings suggest students are not in touch with the
indirect consequences of their peers’ actions.
Despite literature suggesting that multitasking may be
particularly detrimental to the learning of complex knowledge
(e.g., Foerde et al.,
2006), our results show that multitasking impaired both simple
factual learning and complex application learning to the same
degree.
Therefore, even the learning of a new fact (e.g., “Which cloud
type is found highest in the atmosphere?”) can be interrupted by
self-
multitasking or distraction from peers who are multitasking.
Relatedly, multitasking may have different overall effects
depending on the difficulty of the tasks being juggled. Some
studies suggest
that if a primary task is more difficult or novel, it will
inherently require a greater degree of attentional resources to
perform the task at
a satisfactory level (Kahneman,1973; Posner & Boies,1971;
Styles, 2006). Therefore, the primary task may only be
performed well if no other
tasks must be completed at the same time, or if any secondary
task is relatively simple or automatic (i.e., if the secondary task
does not
require many attentional resources; Kahneman & Treisman,
1984). This latter case was the scenario of our Experiment 1.
Participants were
asked to learn something novel in a primary task (where many
attentional resources were required), while simultaneously
attending to
a simple secondary task (where attentional resources were still
required, albeit not to the same extent). We designed the
difficulty level of
the primary and secondary tasks to mimic what has been
reported as typical classroom behaviors (i.e., students who
switch back and forth
between attending to a classroom lecture and checking e-mail,
Facebook, and IMing with friends). Our results suggest that
even though the
secondary task was rather mindless for an undergraduate student
(i.e., casual Internet browsing) it still had an impact on the
performance of
the primary task, as evidenced by multitaskers’ lowered test
scores. Future studies could further examine the impact of
multitasking in the
classroom by manipulating the level of difficulty of the primary
and/or secondary tasks beyond the manipulations of the current
design.
According to dual task theories (e.g., Pashler,1994), one would
expect to see greater deficits in learning performance as the
difficulty level of
either primary or secondary tasks increases (e.g., a student who
attends their physics lecture, but chooses to spend most of the
class time
studying for a history exam taking place during the next
period).
In light of the evidence reported in this study, what might we
recommend to educators as a means of managing laptop use in
the
classroom? A ban on laptops is extreme and unwarranted. It
cannot be overlooked that laptops foster positive learning
outcomes when used
appropriately (e.g., web-based research, pop quizzes, online
case studies, and discussion threads; e.g., Finn & Inman, 2004).
When laptops
are used strictly for note-taking purposes, typed notes have been
shown to have similar positive influences on learning compared
to written
notes (Quade, 1996). Our results confirm this finding through a
rudimentary cross-experiment comparison; that is, we saw no
striking
differences between participants in the no multitasking
condition of Experiment 1 (participants who typed notes) and
participants in the no
view of technology condition of Experiment 2 (participants who
wrote notes) in terms of quality of notes (M ¼ 4.1 and M ¼ 3.6,
respectively)
as well as subsequent comprehension test scores (M ¼ 0.66 and
M ¼ 0.73, respectively, keeping in mind that non-multitaskers
in Exper-
iment 1 sometimes were in view of multitaskers, which could
explain their qualitatively lower comprehension test score).
Thus, for a variety
of reasons, laptops should remain a tool of the modern
classroom, perhaps with some sensible constraints.
One suggestion is for teachers to discuss the consequences of
laptop use with their students at the outset of a course (Gasser
&
Palfrey, 2009). Teachers are in a position to inform students
about negative educational outcomes of laptop misuse, as well
as to
compare and contrast their views with the views of their
students. In this discussion, the class could collectively come
up with a few
rules of technology etiquette that are enforced in the classroom
throughout the semester (e.g., sit at the back of the classroom if
you
plan to multitask, so at least other students are not bothered;
McCreary, 2009). In this way, the issue of technology and
distraction is
highlighted and students can make informed choices, rather than
assuming they (and their peers) are immune to multitasking
deficits.
Another suggestion is to explicitly discourage laptop use in
courses where technology is not necessary for learning. One
could argue that
courses where information is generally presented in textbooks
and on lecture slides do not require a laptop to the same extent
as courses
where hands-on learning is an integrated component of the
course, likely in the form of specialized computer software.
This recommen-
dation is made with caution as some students might not benefit
from a course without laptops. For example, students with
disabilities often
rely on computer technology to assist in learning (Fichten et al.,
2001). Therefore, perhaps one could allow laptops in all courses
but restrict
the use of the Internet to course-based websites only (if
possible).
Ultimately students must take accountability for their own
learning; however, enthusiastic instructors can influence how
students
choose to direct their attention during class time. A third
suggestion is to provide educators with resources to help them
create enriching,
informative, and interactive classes that can compete with the
allure of non-course websites, so that students are deterred from
misusing
their laptop in the first place. This could include incorporating
the laptop into real-time classroom exercises. For example,
instructors could
ask their students to search the Internet for missing lecture
information, or to find an interesting online video to share with
the class.
Furthermore, instructors could use a shared website where
students are able to rank the difficulty level of lecture concepts,
thereby allowing
the instructor to gauge student comprehension levels in class.
The instructor could then review these concepts and provide
feedback to
students prior to the end of the class. Indeed, inventive
instructors can shape how students choose to use their laptops
during class time, so
that laptop use is constructive.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–3130
emyil LU
为了有效地将技术融入课堂,我们必须继续研究技术使用对学习的积极和消极影
响。虽然本研究只从基础讲座(即即时学习)基础学习,未来的研究可以检查多
任务对长期保留的影响,并可以调查主题材料的差异。分散注意力和双重任务执
行的认知理论可以帮助我们理解学习的本质以及什么会分散我们的注意力。应用
研究,使用随机化实验设计,将允许我们研究在学习过程中任务活动可以最大化
和分心最小化的方式。我们必须扪心自问:在什么情况下使用笔记本电脑的好处
大于坏处?归根结底,有吸引力的教师和专注的学习者需要努力工作并保持专注
,以使课堂学习保持在最佳水平。
致谢
这项研究部分得到了约克大学健康学院的资助。我们感谢伊琳娜·卡普勒帮助制作
讲座和理解测试材料。
In order to effectively integrate technology into classrooms, we
must continue to examine the consequencesdboth positive and
neg-
ativedof technology use on learning. While the present research
examined only foundational learning from a lecture (i.e.,
immediate
learning), future research could examine the effects of
multitasking on longer-term retention, and could investigate
subject material
differences. Cognitive theories of divided attention and dual-
task performance can help us understand the nature of how we
learn and what
distracts us. Applied research, using randomized experimental
designs, will allow us to examine ways in which on-task
activities during
learning can be maximized and distraction minimized. We must
ask ourselves: Under what conditions do the benefits of laptop
use
outweigh the detriments? Ultimately, engaging instructors and
dedicated learners will need to work hard and stay focused to
keep
classroom learning at an optimal level.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by a grant from the York
University Faculty of Health. We thank Irina Kapler for helping
to create the
lecture and comprehension test materials.
References
Ahrens, D. C. (1999). Meteorology today: An introduction to
weather, climate and the environment. California: Thompson
Higher Education.
Associated Press. (2010). At universities, is better learning a
click away? Education Week, 29, 10.
Bailey, B. A., & Konstan, J. A. (2006). On the need for
attention-aware systems: measuring effects of interruption on
task performance, error rate, and affective state. Computers
in Human Behavior, 22, 685–708.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2005.12.009.
Barak, M., Lipson, A., & Lerman, S. (2006). Wireless laptops as
means for promoting active learning in large lecture halls.
Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 38,
245–263.
Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and communication. Oxford:
Pergamon.
Bugeja, M. J. (2007). Distractions in the wireless classroom.
The Chronicle of Higher Education, 53, C1–C5, Retrieved from.
http://www.chronicle.com.
Chun, M. M., & Wolfe, J. (2001). Visual attention. In E. B.
Goldstein (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of perception (pp. 272–
310). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.
Crook, C., & Barrowcliff, D. (2001). Ubiquitous computing on
campus: patterns of engagement by university students.
International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 13,
245–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327590IJHC1302_9.
Debevec, K., Shih, M., & Kashyap, V. (2006). Learning
strategies and performance in a technology-integrated
classroom. Journal of Research on Technology in Education,
38, 293–307.
Driver, M. (2002). Exploring student perceptions of group
interactions and class satisfaction in the web-enhanced
classroom. The Internet & Higher Education, 5, 35–45. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1096-7516(01)00076-8.
Fichten, C. S., Asuncion, J., Barile, M., Généreux, C., Fossey,
M., Judd, D., et al. (2001). Technology integration for students
with disabilities: empirically based recommendations
for faculty. Educational Research and Evaluation, 7, 185–221.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/edre.7.2.185.3869.
Finn, S., & Inman, J. G. (2004). Digital unity and digital divide:
surveying alumni to study effects of a campus laptop initiative.
Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 36,
297–317.
Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006).
Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 11778–
11783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0602659103.
Fried, C. B. (2008). In-class laptop use and its effects on
student learning. Computers & Education, 50, 906–914.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2006.09.006.
Gasser, U., & Palfrey, J. (2009). Mastering multitasking.
Educational Leadership, 66, 14–19.
Hembrooke, H., & Gay, G. (2003). The laptop and the lecture:
the effects of multitasking in learning environments. Journal of
Computing in Higher Education, 15, 46–64. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02940852.
Hyden, P. (2005). Teaching statistics by taking advantage of the
laptop’s ubiquity. New Directions for Teaching and Learning,
101, 37–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/tl.184.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall.
Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of
attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, D. R. Davies, &
J. Beatty (Eds.), Variants of attention (pp. 29–61). New
York: Academic Press.
Konig, C. J., Buhner, M., & Murling, F. (2005). Working
memory, fluid intelligence, and attention are predictors of
multitasking performance, but polychronicity and extra-
version are not. Human Performance, 18, 243–266.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327043hup1803_3.
Kraushaar, J. M., & Novak, D. C. (2010). Examining the effects
of student multitasking with laptops during the lecture. Journal
of Information Systems Education, 21, 241–251.
Lindorth, T., & Bergquist, M. (2010). Laptopers in an
educational practice: promoting the personal learning situation.
Computers & Education, 54, 311–320. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.compedu.2009.07.014.
McCreary, J. R. (2009). The laptop-free zone. Valparaiso
University Law Review, 43, 1–87, Retrieved from.
http://ssrn.com/abstract¼1280929.
McVay, G. J., Snyder, K. D., & Graetz, K. A. (2005). Evolution
of a laptop university: a case study. British Journal of
Educational Technology, 36, 513–524.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
j.1467-8535.2005.00487.x.
Melerdiercks, K. (2005). The dark side of the laptop university.
Journal of Information Ethics, 14, 9–11.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3172/JIE.14.1.9.
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Perretta, J. G., & Tonev,
S. T. (2000). The effects of divided attention on encoding and
retrieval processes: the resiliency of retrieval
processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A,
609–625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755914.
Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human
processing systems. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.86.3.214.
Ophira, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, D. (2009). Cognitive control in
media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 106, 15583–15587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/
pnas.0903620106.
Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data
and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220.
Posner, M. (1982). Cumulative development of attentional
theory. American Psychologist, 37, 168–179.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.168.
Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention.
Psychological Review, 78, 391–408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0031333.
Quade, A. M. (1996). An assessment of retention and depth of
processing associated with notetaking using traditional paper
and pencil and on-line notepad during computer-
delivered instruction. In Proceedings of the annual national
convention of the association for educational communications
and technology.
Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001).
Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 27, 763–797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-
1523.27.4.763.
Styles, E. A. (2006). The psychology of attention (2nd ed.).
England: Psychology Press.
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity
and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological
Review, 50, 352–373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0020071.
University of Virginia. (2009). UVa first year student computer
inventory. Retrieved from.
http://itc.virginia.edu/students/inventory/2009/.
Weaver, B. E., & Nilson, L. B. (2005). Laptops in class: what
are they good for? What can you do with them? New Directions
for Teaching and Learning, 101, 3–13. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/tl.181.
Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance
prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, 3(2), 159–
177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14639220210123806.
Wickens, C. D., & Hollands, J. G. (2000). Engineering
psychology and human performance (3rd ed.). New Jersey:
Prentice Hall.
Wood, E., Zivcakova, L., Gentile, P., Archer, K., De Pasquale,
D., & Nosko, A. (2012). Examining the impact of off-task
multi-tasking with technology on real-time classroom
learning. Computers & Education, 58, 365–374.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.08.029.
Wurst, C., Smarkola, C., & Gaffney, M. A. (2008). Ubiquitous
laptop usage in higher education: effects on student
achievement, student satisfaction, and constructivist
measures in honors and traditional classrooms. Computers &
Education, 51, 1766–1783.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2008.05.006.
F. Sana et al. / Computers & Education 62 (2013) 24–31 31
http://www.chronicle.com
http://ssrn.com/abstract%3d1280929
http://ssrn.com/abstract%3d1280929
http://itc.virginia.edu/students/inventory/2009/Laptop
multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users and
nearby peers1. Introduction2. Experiment 12.1. Method2.1.1.
Participants2.1.2. Materials2.1.3. Design and procedure2.1.4.
Fidelity measures2.1.5. Results and discussion3. Experiment
23.1. Method3.1.1. Participants3.1.2. Materials3.1.3. Design
and procedure3.1.4. Fidelity measures3.1.5. Results and
discussion4. General discussionAcknowledgmentsReferences
Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users .docx

More Related Content

Similar to Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users .docx

Thesis Final Draft SCG
Thesis Final Draft SCGThesis Final Draft SCG
Thesis Final Draft SCG
Stephen Garner
 
Palithra2
Palithra2Palithra2
Palithra2
MEL SIG
 
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environmentStudent perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
gadislekio12345
 
Coloma 109
Coloma 109Coloma 109
Coloma 109
Mae
 
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
Alexander Decker
 
Saba synthesis paper education and technology
Saba synthesis paper education and technologySaba synthesis paper education and technology
Saba synthesis paper education and technology
nubiecitarussi
 

Similar to Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users .docx (20)

ICT in Education
ICT in EducationICT in Education
ICT in Education
 
Designing for learning
Designing for learningDesigning for learning
Designing for learning
 
Pages 8 35
Pages 8 35Pages 8 35
Pages 8 35
 
Thesis Final Draft SCG
Thesis Final Draft SCGThesis Final Draft SCG
Thesis Final Draft SCG
 
If i upload it, will they come using lazy user theory to explain student use ...
If i upload it, will they come using lazy user theory to explain student use ...If i upload it, will they come using lazy user theory to explain student use ...
If i upload it, will they come using lazy user theory to explain student use ...
 
Palithra2
Palithra2Palithra2
Palithra2
 
First monday
First mondayFirst monday
First monday
 
Using Gartner’s Hype Curve as a basis to analyze research on the educational ...
Using Gartner’s Hype Curve as a basis to analyze research on the educational ...Using Gartner’s Hype Curve as a basis to analyze research on the educational ...
Using Gartner’s Hype Curve as a basis to analyze research on the educational ...
 
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environmentStudent perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
Student perceptions on language learning in a technological environment
 
Synthesis paper
Synthesis paperSynthesis paper
Synthesis paper
 
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
 
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
 
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
Ziyanak, sebahattin the effectiveness of survey instruments nfaerj v29 n3 2016
 
Finalpaper!
Finalpaper!Finalpaper!
Finalpaper!
 
Coloma 109
Coloma 109Coloma 109
Coloma 109
 
First monday
First mondayFirst monday
First monday
 
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
Secondary students’ perspectives on the use of the interactive whiteboard for...
 
Saba synthesis paper education and technology
Saba synthesis paper education and technologySaba synthesis paper education and technology
Saba synthesis paper education and technology
 
A Hybrid Approach To Promoting Students Web-Based Problem-Solving Competence...
A Hybrid Approach To Promoting Students  Web-Based Problem-Solving Competence...A Hybrid Approach To Promoting Students  Web-Based Problem-Solving Competence...
A Hybrid Approach To Promoting Students Web-Based Problem-Solving Competence...
 
Thread: Discussion - Week 9 – DPSY-6111-1/DPSY-5111-1
Thread: Discussion - Week 9 – DPSY-6111-1/DPSY-5111-1Thread: Discussion - Week 9 – DPSY-6111-1/DPSY-5111-1
Thread: Discussion - Week 9 – DPSY-6111-1/DPSY-5111-1
 

More from croysierkathey

1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
croysierkathey
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
croysierkathey
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
croysierkathey
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
croysierkathey
 

More from croysierkathey (20)

1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
1.  Discuss the organization and the family role in every one of the.docx
 
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
1.  Compare and contrast DEmilios Capitalism and Gay Identity .docx
 
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
1.Purpose the purpose of this essay is to spread awareness .docx
 
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
1.  Tell us why it is your favorite film.2.  Talk about the .docx
 
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
1.What are the main issues facing Fargo and Town Manager Susan.docx
 
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
1.Writing Practice in Reading a PhotographAttached Files.docx
 
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
1.Some say that analytics in general dehumanize managerial activitie.docx
 
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
1.What is the psychological term for the symptoms James experiences .docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
1.Write at least 500 words discussing the benefits of using R with H.docx
 
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
1.What is Starbucks’ ROA for 2012, 2011, and 2010 Why might focusin.docx
 
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
1.  Discuss the cultural development of the Japanese and the Jewis.docx
 
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
1.  Discuss at least 2  contextual factors(family, peers,  school,.docx
 
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
1.Write at least 500 words in APA format discussing how to use senti.docx
 
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
1.The following clause was added to the Food and Drug Actthe S.docx
 
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
1.What are social determinants of health  Explain how social determ.docx
 
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
1.This week, we’ve been introduced to the humanities and have ta.docx
 
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
1.What are barriers to listening2.Communicators identif.docx
 
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
1.Timeline description and details There are multiple way.docx
 
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
1.The PresidentArticle II of the Constitution establishe.docx
 
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
1.What other potential root causes might influence patient fal.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
ciinovamais
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
QucHHunhnh
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
ZurliaSoop
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
KarakKing
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
Jual Obat Aborsi Hongkong ( Asli No.1 ) 085657271886 Obat Penggugur Kandungan...
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POSHow to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
How to Manage Global Discount in Odoo 17 POS
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
Making communications land - Are they received and understood as intended? we...
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student briefSpatium Project Simulation student brief
Spatium Project Simulation student brief
 
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functionsSalient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
Salient Features of India constitution especially power and functions
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 

Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users .docx