SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 7
Download to read offline
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design




                   AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO
                     DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER AND OLDER DRIVERS

                            William J. Horrey, Mary F. Lesch, & Angela Garabet
                                         Center for Safety Research
                                Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety
                                            Hopkinton, MA USA
                                 E-mail: william.horrey@libertymutual.com

        Summary: Although many studies have documented the performance decrements
        associated with driver distractions, few have examined drivers’ awareness of
        these distraction effects. The current study purports to measure how well-
        calibrated drivers are with respect to their own performance when distracted.
        Forty drivers completed a series of tasks on a hand-held or hands-free cell phone
        while driving an instrumented vehicle around a closed test track. Subjective
        estimates of performance decrements were recorded and compared to actual
        decrements observed on multiple measures of driving performance. Although
        their driving performance suffered in dual-task conditions, drivers were generally
        not well-calibrated to the magnitude of the distraction effects (r = -.38 to .16). In
        some cases, estimates of distraction were opposite of the observed effects (i.e.,
        smaller estimates of distraction corresponded to larger performance deficits).
        There were some age and gender differences. We discuss the implications of these
        findings for potential mitigation strategies for distracted driving.

Over the past several years, numerous studies have documented the effects of engaging in cell
phone conversations or other distracting tasks while driving. In general, these studies show that
distracted drivers have slowed responses to critical traffic events or to discrete stimuli and are
more likely to miss external events such as a changing traffic light, among other effects (Alm &
Nilsson, 1994; Hancock, Lesch & Simmons, 2003; Horrey & Wickens, 2006; McKnight &
McKnight, 1993; Strayer & Johnston, 2001). Much of the focus in such studies has been on
detecting the presence and measuring the magnitude of performance decrements for different
types of in-vehicle tasks or device configurations. However, to date there has not been much
examination of drivers’ awareness of these distraction effects.

The perception or awareness of distraction effects may influence drivers’ decisions or their
willingness to engage in distracting activities while on the road. For example, drivers that are not
calibrated with respect to the magnitude of distraction effects may engage in activities because
they don’t realize their performance is compromised. In an earlier study, Lesch and Hancock
(2004) found that a priori ratings of confidence in dealing with distracting tasks were not related
to actual performance while distracted for female drivers, suggesting that some drivers may not
be aware of performance decrements while distracted. In their study, younger and older drivers
performed a memory and recall test while navigating an instrumented vehicle around a closed
test track (some data is presented by Hancock et al., 2003). On some trials, drivers were
presented with a visual prompt for the memory task that occurred in conjunction with a changing
traffic light. Measures of stopping performance were assessed and compared against confidence




                                                               54
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design



ratings. Overall, there was little association between the performance and confidence in female
drivers; however, the disparities were greatest for older drivers.

In the current study, we wished to establish how well-calibrated drivers are with respect to
distraction effects—that is, whether drivers’ estimates of the magnitude of distraction reflect
actual performance decrements on several driving tasks. We expand on the results from Lesch
and Hancock (2004) using a distraction task that did not require visual processing. Furthermore,
we were interested in whether subjective estimates of distraction and actual distraction varied as
a function of phone type (hand-held, hands-free). Drivers in this study drove an instrumented van
on a closed test track while performing a continuous mental arithmetic task administered over a
hand-held or hands-free cell phone. Subjective measures of distraction effects were recorded and
compared to actual performance on multiple measures of driving performance.

METHODS

Participants

Forty drivers, divided into two age groups, were recruited through advertisements in local
newspapers. The younger driver group consisted of 20 drivers between the ages of 18 and 34 (M
= 21.8 yrs, SD = 4.3). The older driver group consisted of 20 drivers between the ages of 55 and
82 (M = 64.1 yrs, SD = 7.7). Males and females were (approximately) balanced across the two
age groups. All had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Drivers were paid $20 for each
hour of participation.

Materials

The experiment was conducted on a 0.5-mile (0.8 kilometer) closed-loop test track. The two-lane
track was delineated to allow for continuous driving. A signalized intersection, located at the end
of a straightaway, was controlled through track-based infrared sensors and vehicle-based GPS
and speed information.

The instrumented vehicle was a 2002 Ford Windstar minivan, outfitted with several sensors and
computers. A PC computer rack, mounted directly behind the driver, controlled and coordinated
various aspects of the data acquisition and stimulus presentation. Auditory stimuli for the phone
tasks were presented via handheld phone (Avaya model 3626) or through speakers mounted
behind the driver (Sony model SRS-T100PC).

Five pace clocks were positioned at various points around the track. These 0.5-m (20-inch)
diameter clocks were mounted on 1.8 m (6 foot) towers and placed adjacent to the drivers’ lane.
The bottom half of the clock was green and the top half was red. The arrow indicator moved
around the clock at a constant rate and completed the full rotation every 12 seconds, on average
(varied across clocks).




                                                               55
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design




Procedure

At the start of the two-hour session, drivers completed an informed consent form, tests for visual
functioning and several questionnaires. Drivers were then introduced to the safety features of the
instrumented van and given several minutes of practice to familiarize themselves with the
handling of the vehicle, the driving tasks, and the track.

For all conditions, drivers were allowed to select their speed (to an upper limit of 48 kph [30
mph]). When approaching the pace clocks, drivers were told to adjust their speed, either by
accelerating or braking, in order to pass the clock when the arrow indicator was in the green
portion of the clock (and not the red). In doing so, they were told to avoid bringing the vehicle to
a full stop and exceeding the speed limit. On a random subset of trials (37.5%), the traffic light at
the signalized intersection changed from red to green. On these occasions, drivers were
instructed to bring the vehicle to a complete stop as quickly as they could and to try to stop
before they reached a stop line marked by two traffic cones.

During the experimental blocks, drivers performed a concurrent phone task—a variation of the
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977). Numbers between 1 and 9 were
randomly presented every 7 seconds, either through the hand-held phone or through the vehicle
speakers (hands-free). The task involved adding two consecutive numbers and responding
verbally. Drivers were given a small monetary bonus for each correct response (5 cents per
correct response, up to $20 over the whole experiment).

Drivers completed three blocks of 8 laps, each lasting approximately 15-minutes. In two blocks,
drivers performed the PASAT (once using the hand-held phone, once using the hands-free
system). In the third block, drivers performed the driving tasks alone, with no PASAT. Drivers
also completed a short, 3-minute baseline block for the PASAT, performed while the vehicle was
stationary. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across driver.

Measures of pace clock accuracy (number of correct clock events out of 45 per block), braking
response time (from the onset of the traffic light change) and stopping errors (failing to stop or
failing to stop before the stop line) were recorded. In between blocks, drivers completed a
modified NASA TLX (Hart & Staveland, 1988) that included subjective ratings of performance
for all of the relevant driving sub-tasks (pace clocks, stopping task) and for the PASAT.

RESULTS

Driving Performance

Prior to examining drivers’ calibration of distraction effects, we examined the dual-task costs in
driving performance associated with the cell phone tasks (i.e., a manipulation check). In general,
we used a mixed ANOVA with the variables of Task, Age and Gender (with the exception of the
stopping errors, which were not normally distributed; for these we employ non-parametric tests).
There were no significant effects of age and gender on any of the measures of driving
performance.




                                                               56
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design



Table 1 shows the mean performance scores across dependent measure and task condition. There
was a significant effect of task condition across all measures of performance: pace clock
accuracy (F(2,68) = 27.1, p < .01); braking response time (F(2,60) = 5.8, p < .01); and stopping
errors ( χ F (2) = 16.0, p < .001). The hand-held and hands-free PASAT conditions yielded poorer
           2


performance on all metrics than in the single-task driving baseline (all p’s < .01); however, in
general, there were no differences between hand-held and hands-free cell phones, replicating
previous results (e.g., Strayer & Johnston, 2001; Horrey & Wickens, 2006).

                 Table 1. Summary of driving performance data by task condition
                       Task Condition           Pace Clock          Brake Response           Stop Light
                                               Accuracy (%)            Time (s)              Errors (%)
                           Baseline               82.2 (1.2)           0.88 (0.02)             26 (6)
                          Hand-held               71.1 (1.6)           0.96 (0.03)             48 (6)
                         Hands-free               73.9 (1.2)           0.94 (0.03)             48 (5)
                        Note. Standard error appears in parentheses.


Calibration to Distraction Effects

In order to examine drivers’ calibration to the effects of in-vehicle distraction, we first calculated
estimated and actual distraction effects from the subjective performance ratings and from actual
performance along the various measures of driving performance. Subjective estimates of
distraction were expressed as a % difference between self-rated performance in the driving
baseline block and self-rated performance in the distraction block (the baseline condition was
used as a reference in order to control for any systematic biases in ratings). Actual distraction
effects were expressed as the % difference in performance from the baseline block to the dual-
task block. Thus, negative values denoted an actual or estimated loss in performance for the
distraction condition relative to baseline, whereas a positive value indicated a gain in
performance.

Next, we examined the relationship between subjective estimates of distraction effects and actual
distraction effects. If drivers are well-calibrated, we would expect a positive correlation between
the estimated and actual distraction effects. That is, larger estimations of distraction effects
would correspond to larger observed performance decrements. However, if drivers are not well-
calibrated to the magnitude of distraction effects, then there would be no such relationship or a
negative relationship may exist.

The results from a correlational analysis are shown in Table 2. Overall, there were no significant
relationships between estimates of distraction effects and actual performance decrements,
lending support to the notion that drivers were not well-calibrated to the distracting effects of a
concurrent in-vehicle task. In the hands-free condition, there was a significant relationship for
stopping errors (r = -.38); however, this relationship is in the negative direction. Here, drivers
that estimated the smallest performance decrements were actually exhibiting the largest ones!
The confidence intervals around the correlations in Table 2 suggest that, in general, the poor




                                                               57
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design



 calibration of drivers to the effects of distraction were consistent across phone type and
 dependent measure.

 An examination of the different driver groups revealed some interesting findings. For example,
 older male drivers were actually well-calibrated to the magnitude of distraction effects for the
 stopping task (stopping errors, r = .71; brake RT, r = .82). In contrast, younger males showed
 strong associations in the opposite direction (stopping errors, r = -.91; brake RT, r = -.65, the
 latter result was marginally significant). That is, young male drivers that thought they were doing
 better were actually doing worse than others. In general, female drivers did not exhibit any
 significant relationships between estimated and actual performance loss (Lesch & Hancock,
 2004).

    Table 2. Correlations between the magnitude of estimated and actual distraction effects
                 (the difference between baseline and distraction conditions)
 Driver Group         Δ Brake Response Time                   Δ Pace Clock Accuracy                     Δ Stop Light Errors
                                           Free                                    Free
                        Held                                    Held                                    Held              Free
                         .06                -.02                 .16               .10                   .11              -.38*
    Overall
                     (-.28, .39)        (-.33, .31)          (-.18, .46)       (-.22, .40)           (-.23, .42)       (-.62, -.07)

                      -.65             -.33              -.07              .30                            .05             -.91**
 Young Males
                  (-.93, .10)      (-.79, .38)       (-.70, .62)      (-.41, .78)                    (-.64, .69)       (-.98, -.66)
                     .82**              .45               .40             -.22                            .30              .71*
 Older Males
                   (.34, .96)      (-.26, .84)       (-.36, .84)      (-.74, .48)                    (-.46, .80)        (.15, .93)
                      .07              .13               .42              .51                            .34                -.61
Young Females
                  (-.62, .70)      (-.58, .73)       (-.34, .85)      (-.18, .86)                    (-.37, .80)       (-.91, .09)
                      .38              -.04              -.23             -.29                           -.29               -.56
Older Females
                  (-.38, .83)      (-.65, .60)       (-.77, .52)      (-.78, .41)                    (-.80, .46)       (-.88, .11)
 Note. 95% confidence intervals are shown in parentheses. * p < .05, ** p < .01.


 DISCUSSION

 In the current study, younger and older drivers were asked to complete a phone task on a hand-
 held or hands-free cell phone while navigating a closed test-track in an instrumented vehicle.
 Compared to baseline driving, we observed dual-task decrements on all measures of driving
 performance, replicating previous results (Alm & Nilsson, 1994; Hancock et al., 2003; McKnight
 & McKnight, 1993; Strayer & Johnston, 2001).

 To examine drivers’ calibration to distraction effects, we compared drivers’ subjective estimates
 of distraction with actual performance decrements, based on a number of measures of driving
 performance. The results suggest that, for the most part, drivers are not well-calibrated to the
 distracting effects of a hand-held or hands-free cell phone conversation. Across all measures of
 performance, subjective estimates of distraction effects were not related to the actual magnitude
 of the distraction. In some cases, driver’s estimates of distraction were opposite of what was
 observed in actual performance. That is, drivers that estimated the smallest (or no) distraction
 effects exhibited the largest ones. In general, a disconnection between performance and
 awareness was observed across driving measure and phone type.




                                                                 58
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design



Analysis of the driver groups, though limited by a small sample size, showed some differences.
For female drivers, there were no significant relationships across all measures and phone types
(also as shown by Lesch & Hancock, 2004); however, there were some mixed results for males.
For example, on some measures young and older males revealed a nearly equal but opposite
relationship between estimated and actual distraction effects. Younger males, in particular, were
poorly calibrated, suggesting that this may be an important group for targeted remediation.

The willingness to engage in distracting activities may be a function of drivers’ perceptions of
performance decrements. As such, drivers may engage in distracting activities simply because
they do not realize that their performance is degraded or they may be overconfident in their skills
and their ability to deal with distractions while behind the wheel (Wogalter & Mayhorn, 2005).

Strategies for improving drivers’ calibration to distraction effects include both driver- and
technology-based approaches. For the former, training to recognize or attend more closely to
their driving activities may help drivers’ determine when their performance is below “baseline.”
One question that remains is whether drivers would continue to perform in-vehicle tasks if they
were perfectly calibrated to the distraction effects. In other words, the issue could become one of
risk tolerance and the degree to which drivers are willing to take on the additional risk of the in-
vehicle task.

Some technological innovations are intended to monitor driver performance and behaviors in real
time and provide alerts when the system infers that a distracting activity is inappropriate (e.g.,
Donmez, Boyle, & Lee, in press). Thus, these systems have some authority in deciding when the
distraction effects of in-vehicle activities are too severe. However, discrepancies between the
driver and the system’s estimated level of distraction could create some conflicts. For example, a
driver’s mental model of the system may be violated if warnings are issued for events which the
driver does not believe to be problematic. The consequences of such a mismatch could include
system disuse deriving from reduced trust in the system (Lee & Moray, 1994; Parasuraman &
Riley, 1997) or worse (Sarter & Woods, 1995). Providing the raw data underlying the system
functions may be a key consideration.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors are grateful to Peter Teare, Eric Jones, Richard Holihan, and Ed Correa for their
upkeep and maintenance of the instrumented van and related equipment. We also thank Yueng-
hsiang Huang and Michael Blair for their helpful comments regarding an earlier version of this
paper.

REFERENCES

Alm, H. & Nilsson, L. (1994). Changes in driver behaviour as a function of handsfree mobile
   phones—A simulator study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26, 441-451.
Ashley, S. (2001). Driving the info highway. Scientific American, 285, 52-58.
Brookhuis, K.A., de Vries, G., & de Waard, D. (1991). The effects of mobile telephoning on
   driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 23, 309-316.




                                                               59
PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design



Donmez, B., Boyle, L.N., & Lee, J.D. (In press). Safety implications of providing real-time
   feedback to distracted drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention.
Gronwall, D.M.A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from
   concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367-373.
Hancock, P.A., Lesch, M., & Simmons, L. (2003). The distraction effects of phone use during a
   crucial driving maneuver. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35(4), 501-514.
Hart, S.G., & Staveland, L.E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results
   of empirical and theoretical research. In P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human Mental
   Workload. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 139-183.
Horrey, W.J. & Wickens, C.D. (2006) Examining the impact of cell phone conversations on
   driving using meta-analytic techniques. Human Factors, 48(1), 196-205.
Lee, J.D. & Moray, N. (1994). Trust, self-confidence, and operators’ adaptation to automation.
   International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 40, 153-184.
Lesch, M.F. & Hancock, P.A. (2004). Driving performance during concurrent cell-phone use:
   Are drivers aware of their performance decrements? Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36,
   471-480.
McKnight, A.J. & McKnight, A.S. (1993). The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention.
  Accident Analysis and Prevention, 25, 259-265.
Parasuraman, R. & Riley, V. (1997). Humans and automation: Use, misuse, disuse, abuse.
   Human Factors, 39(2), 230-253.
Sarter, N.B. & Woods, D.D. (1995). How in the world did we ever get into that mode? Mode
    error and awareness in supervisory control. Human Factors, 37(1), 5-19.
Strayer, D.L. & Johnston, W.A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated
    driving and conversing on a cellular phone. Psychological Science, 12(6), 462-466.
Wogalter, M.S. & Mayhorn, C.B. (2005). Perceptions of driver distraction by cellular phone
  users and nonusers. Human Factors, 47(2), 455-467.




                                                               60

More Related Content

Similar to How to keep your finances on track

Hollandand Rathod
Hollandand RathodHollandand Rathod
Hollandand RathodMSullman
 
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...Erik Roelofs
 
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...Julie J. Kang, Ph.D.
 
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13MSullman
 
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviour
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviourDoes accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviour
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviourErik Sommarström
 
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3Ben Lester
 
Ieeepro techno solutions 2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoring
Ieeepro techno solutions   2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoringIeeepro techno solutions   2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoring
Ieeepro techno solutions 2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoringsrinivasanece7
 
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on Drivers
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on DriversStudy on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on Drivers
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on DriversSoumyajitDas47
 
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05guest756a14
 
Skippon reed & robbins 2013
Skippon reed & robbins 2013 Skippon reed & robbins 2013
Skippon reed & robbins 2013 MSullman
 
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1Ben Lester
 
7. Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies
7.	Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies7.	Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies
7. Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologiesOren_Musicant
 
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...inventionjournals
 

Similar to How to keep your finances on track (20)

Hollandand Rathod
Hollandand RathodHollandand Rathod
Hollandand Rathod
 
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...
Validity of an on-road driver performance assessment within an initial driver...
 
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...
Schwarz et al._2016_The Detection of Visual Distraction using Vehicle and Dri...
 
HF2006pp805-821
HF2006pp805-821HF2006pp805-821
HF2006pp805-821
 
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13
Rosenbloom i safe final 20.8.13
 
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviour
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviourDoes accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviour
Does accurate self-awareness increase traffic safety behaviour
 
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3
DA poster_2015_FCW_1.3
 
Ieeepro techno solutions 2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoring
Ieeepro techno solutions   2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoringIeeepro techno solutions   2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoring
Ieeepro techno solutions 2013 ieee embedded project driving safety monitoring
 
Sucha
SuchaSucha
Sucha
 
5100termproj422
5100termproj4225100termproj422
5100termproj422
 
Session 50 Andereas Tapani
Session 50 Andereas TapaniSession 50 Andereas Tapani
Session 50 Andereas Tapani
 
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on Drivers
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on DriversStudy on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on Drivers
Study on Postural Analysis and Ergonomic Interference on Drivers
 
Session 19 Jan Andersson
Session 19 Jan AnderssonSession 19 Jan Andersson
Session 19 Jan Andersson
 
Teen Driving final
Teen Driving finalTeen Driving final
Teen Driving final
 
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05
Isa Driver Acceptance Revised Article Ejtir Nov05
 
Skippon reed & robbins 2013
Skippon reed & robbins 2013 Skippon reed & robbins 2013
Skippon reed & robbins 2013
 
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1
DA poster_2015_serialization_2.1
 
Mental models of eco driving
Mental models of eco drivingMental models of eco driving
Mental models of eco driving
 
7. Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies
7.	Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies7.	Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies
7. Drivers’ safety behavior research using in-vehicle technologies
 
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...
To Find out the Relationship between Errors, Lapses, Violations and Traffic A...
 

More from pleasure16

Park & Pay Stations
Park & Pay StationsPark & Pay Stations
Park & Pay Stationspleasure16
 
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in Maine
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in MaineRecycling of Cellular Telephones in Maine
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in Mainepleasure16
 
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M S
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M SC O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M S
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M Spleasure16
 
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defense
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-DefenseJob-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defense
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defensepleasure16
 
AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...
 AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ... AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...
AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...pleasure16
 
Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...
 Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ... Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...
Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...pleasure16
 
Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking
 Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking
Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Trackingpleasure16
 
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...pleasure16
 
ftc.gov/phonefraud
 ftc.gov/phonefraud ftc.gov/phonefraud
ftc.gov/phonefraudpleasure16
 
Cell Phone Plans
Cell Phone PlansCell Phone Plans
Cell Phone Planspleasure16
 
phone records could track rapist, expert says
 phone records could track rapist, expert says phone records could track rapist, expert says
phone records could track rapist, expert sayspleasure16
 
Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down
 Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down
Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it downpleasure16
 
Cell Phone Report
Cell Phone ReportCell Phone Report
Cell Phone Reportpleasure16
 
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...pleasure16
 
CELL PHONE SAVVY:
 CELL PHONE SAVVY: CELL PHONE SAVVY:
CELL PHONE SAVVY:pleasure16
 
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animals
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of AnimalsCity of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animals
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animalspleasure16
 
NEW FAAST FALL 04.indd
NEW FAAST FALL 04.inddNEW FAAST FALL 04.indd
NEW FAAST FALL 04.inddpleasure16
 
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
 Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Reportpleasure16
 

More from pleasure16 (20)

Park & Pay Stations
Park & Pay StationsPark & Pay Stations
Park & Pay Stations
 
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in Maine
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in MaineRecycling of Cellular Telephones in Maine
Recycling of Cellular Telephones in Maine
 
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M S
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M SC O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M S
C O N T RO L L I N G E L E C T RO N I C S V I A S M S
 
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defense
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-DefenseJob-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defense
Job-Hunt FREE 15-Minute Guide to Layoff Self-Defense
 
AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...
 AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ... AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...
AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER ...
 
eCopy, Inc.
 eCopy, Inc. eCopy, Inc.
eCopy, Inc.
 
Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...
 Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ... Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...
Address Book IntegrAtIon wIth Jd edwArds enterprIseone And Jd ...
 
Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking
 Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking
Cell Phone–Based System Could Improve HIV/AIDS Drug Tracking
 
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...
2008 DEER HUNTING INFORMATION AND ON-LINE/TOLL FREE TELEPHONE ...
 
ftc.gov/phonefraud
 ftc.gov/phonefraud ftc.gov/phonefraud
ftc.gov/phonefraud
 
Cell Phone Plans
Cell Phone PlansCell Phone Plans
Cell Phone Plans
 
phone records could track rapist, expert says
 phone records could track rapist, expert says phone records could track rapist, expert says
phone records could track rapist, expert says
 
Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down
 Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down
Bus left you waiting in the cold? Use your cell phone to track it down
 
Cell Phone Report
Cell Phone ReportCell Phone Report
Cell Phone Report
 
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...
PRLog.Org - Free Web Service Lets You Locate, Track, Sync, Protect ...
 
CELL PHONE SAVVY:
 CELL PHONE SAVVY: CELL PHONE SAVVY:
CELL PHONE SAVVY:
 
JUST HATCHED
 JUST HATCHED JUST HATCHED
JUST HATCHED
 
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animals
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of AnimalsCity of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animals
City of San Antonio Code: Regulating the Care and Control of Animals
 
NEW FAAST FALL 04.indd
NEW FAAST FALL 04.inddNEW FAAST FALL 04.indd
NEW FAAST FALL 04.indd
 
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
 Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report
 

Recently uploaded

SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024
SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024
SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024Scott Keck-Warren
 
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks..."LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...Fwdays
 
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii SoldatenkoFwdays
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfAlex Barbosa Coqueiro
 
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning era
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning eraArtificial intelligence in the post-deep learning era
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning eraDeakin University
 
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsScanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsRizwan Syed
 
AI as an Interface for Commercial Buildings
AI as an Interface for Commercial BuildingsAI as an Interface for Commercial Buildings
AI as an Interface for Commercial BuildingsMemoori
 
APIForce Zurich 5 April Automation LPDG
APIForce Zurich 5 April  Automation LPDGAPIForce Zurich 5 April  Automation LPDG
APIForce Zurich 5 April Automation LPDGMarianaLemus7
 
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024Neo4j
 
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024Enterprise Knowledge
 
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...shyamraj55
 
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other Frameworks
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other FrameworksBenefits Of Flutter Compared To Other Frameworks
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other FrameworksSoftradix Technologies
 
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr BaganFwdays
 
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubUnleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubKalema Edgar
 
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry Innovation
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry InnovationBeyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry Innovation
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry InnovationSafe Software
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024BookNet Canada
 
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdf
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdfBluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdf
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdfngoud9212
 
Understanding the Laravel MVC Architecture
Understanding the Laravel MVC ArchitectureUnderstanding the Laravel MVC Architecture
Understanding the Laravel MVC ArchitecturePixlogix Infotech
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Hot Sexy call girls in Panjabi Bagh 🔝 9953056974 🔝 Delhi escort Service
Hot Sexy call girls in Panjabi Bagh 🔝 9953056974 🔝 Delhi escort ServiceHot Sexy call girls in Panjabi Bagh 🔝 9953056974 🔝 Delhi escort Service
Hot Sexy call girls in Panjabi Bagh 🔝 9953056974 🔝 Delhi escort Service
 
SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024
SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024
SQL Database Design For Developers at php[tek] 2024
 
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks..."LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...
"LLMs for Python Engineers: Advanced Data Analysis and Semantic Kernel",Oleks...
 
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko
"Debugging python applications inside k8s environment", Andrii Soldatenko
 
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdfUnraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
Unraveling Multimodality with Large Language Models.pdf
 
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning era
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning eraArtificial intelligence in the post-deep learning era
Artificial intelligence in the post-deep learning era
 
E-Vehicle_Hacking_by_Parul Sharma_null_owasp.pptx
E-Vehicle_Hacking_by_Parul Sharma_null_owasp.pptxE-Vehicle_Hacking_by_Parul Sharma_null_owasp.pptx
E-Vehicle_Hacking_by_Parul Sharma_null_owasp.pptx
 
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL CertsScanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
Scanning the Internet for External Cloud Exposures via SSL Certs
 
AI as an Interface for Commercial Buildings
AI as an Interface for Commercial BuildingsAI as an Interface for Commercial Buildings
AI as an Interface for Commercial Buildings
 
APIForce Zurich 5 April Automation LPDG
APIForce Zurich 5 April  Automation LPDGAPIForce Zurich 5 April  Automation LPDG
APIForce Zurich 5 April Automation LPDG
 
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024
Build your next Gen AI Breakthrough - April 2024
 
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024
Designing IA for AI - Information Architecture Conference 2024
 
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...
Automating Business Process via MuleSoft Composer | Bangalore MuleSoft Meetup...
 
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other Frameworks
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other FrameworksBenefits Of Flutter Compared To Other Frameworks
Benefits Of Flutter Compared To Other Frameworks
 
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
"ML in Production",Oleksandr Bagan
 
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding ClubUnleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
Unleash Your Potential - Namagunga Girls Coding Club
 
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry Innovation
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry InnovationBeyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry Innovation
Beyond Boundaries: Leveraging No-Code Solutions for Industry Innovation
 
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024
New from BookNet Canada for 2024: BNC BiblioShare - Tech Forum 2024
 
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdf
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdfBluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdf
Bluetooth Controlled Car with Arduino.pdf
 
Understanding the Laravel MVC Architecture
Understanding the Laravel MVC ArchitectureUnderstanding the Laravel MVC Architecture
Understanding the Laravel MVC Architecture
 

How to keep your finances on track

  • 1. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design AWARENESS OF PERFORMANCE DECREMENTS DUE TO DISTRACTION IN YOUNGER AND OLDER DRIVERS William J. Horrey, Mary F. Lesch, & Angela Garabet Center for Safety Research Liberty Mutual Research Institute for Safety Hopkinton, MA USA E-mail: william.horrey@libertymutual.com Summary: Although many studies have documented the performance decrements associated with driver distractions, few have examined drivers’ awareness of these distraction effects. The current study purports to measure how well- calibrated drivers are with respect to their own performance when distracted. Forty drivers completed a series of tasks on a hand-held or hands-free cell phone while driving an instrumented vehicle around a closed test track. Subjective estimates of performance decrements were recorded and compared to actual decrements observed on multiple measures of driving performance. Although their driving performance suffered in dual-task conditions, drivers were generally not well-calibrated to the magnitude of the distraction effects (r = -.38 to .16). In some cases, estimates of distraction were opposite of the observed effects (i.e., smaller estimates of distraction corresponded to larger performance deficits). There were some age and gender differences. We discuss the implications of these findings for potential mitigation strategies for distracted driving. Over the past several years, numerous studies have documented the effects of engaging in cell phone conversations or other distracting tasks while driving. In general, these studies show that distracted drivers have slowed responses to critical traffic events or to discrete stimuli and are more likely to miss external events such as a changing traffic light, among other effects (Alm & Nilsson, 1994; Hancock, Lesch & Simmons, 2003; Horrey & Wickens, 2006; McKnight & McKnight, 1993; Strayer & Johnston, 2001). Much of the focus in such studies has been on detecting the presence and measuring the magnitude of performance decrements for different types of in-vehicle tasks or device configurations. However, to date there has not been much examination of drivers’ awareness of these distraction effects. The perception or awareness of distraction effects may influence drivers’ decisions or their willingness to engage in distracting activities while on the road. For example, drivers that are not calibrated with respect to the magnitude of distraction effects may engage in activities because they don’t realize their performance is compromised. In an earlier study, Lesch and Hancock (2004) found that a priori ratings of confidence in dealing with distracting tasks were not related to actual performance while distracted for female drivers, suggesting that some drivers may not be aware of performance decrements while distracted. In their study, younger and older drivers performed a memory and recall test while navigating an instrumented vehicle around a closed test track (some data is presented by Hancock et al., 2003). On some trials, drivers were presented with a visual prompt for the memory task that occurred in conjunction with a changing traffic light. Measures of stopping performance were assessed and compared against confidence 54
  • 2. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design ratings. Overall, there was little association between the performance and confidence in female drivers; however, the disparities were greatest for older drivers. In the current study, we wished to establish how well-calibrated drivers are with respect to distraction effects—that is, whether drivers’ estimates of the magnitude of distraction reflect actual performance decrements on several driving tasks. We expand on the results from Lesch and Hancock (2004) using a distraction task that did not require visual processing. Furthermore, we were interested in whether subjective estimates of distraction and actual distraction varied as a function of phone type (hand-held, hands-free). Drivers in this study drove an instrumented van on a closed test track while performing a continuous mental arithmetic task administered over a hand-held or hands-free cell phone. Subjective measures of distraction effects were recorded and compared to actual performance on multiple measures of driving performance. METHODS Participants Forty drivers, divided into two age groups, were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers. The younger driver group consisted of 20 drivers between the ages of 18 and 34 (M = 21.8 yrs, SD = 4.3). The older driver group consisted of 20 drivers between the ages of 55 and 82 (M = 64.1 yrs, SD = 7.7). Males and females were (approximately) balanced across the two age groups. All had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Drivers were paid $20 for each hour of participation. Materials The experiment was conducted on a 0.5-mile (0.8 kilometer) closed-loop test track. The two-lane track was delineated to allow for continuous driving. A signalized intersection, located at the end of a straightaway, was controlled through track-based infrared sensors and vehicle-based GPS and speed information. The instrumented vehicle was a 2002 Ford Windstar minivan, outfitted with several sensors and computers. A PC computer rack, mounted directly behind the driver, controlled and coordinated various aspects of the data acquisition and stimulus presentation. Auditory stimuli for the phone tasks were presented via handheld phone (Avaya model 3626) or through speakers mounted behind the driver (Sony model SRS-T100PC). Five pace clocks were positioned at various points around the track. These 0.5-m (20-inch) diameter clocks were mounted on 1.8 m (6 foot) towers and placed adjacent to the drivers’ lane. The bottom half of the clock was green and the top half was red. The arrow indicator moved around the clock at a constant rate and completed the full rotation every 12 seconds, on average (varied across clocks). 55
  • 3. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design Procedure At the start of the two-hour session, drivers completed an informed consent form, tests for visual functioning and several questionnaires. Drivers were then introduced to the safety features of the instrumented van and given several minutes of practice to familiarize themselves with the handling of the vehicle, the driving tasks, and the track. For all conditions, drivers were allowed to select their speed (to an upper limit of 48 kph [30 mph]). When approaching the pace clocks, drivers were told to adjust their speed, either by accelerating or braking, in order to pass the clock when the arrow indicator was in the green portion of the clock (and not the red). In doing so, they were told to avoid bringing the vehicle to a full stop and exceeding the speed limit. On a random subset of trials (37.5%), the traffic light at the signalized intersection changed from red to green. On these occasions, drivers were instructed to bring the vehicle to a complete stop as quickly as they could and to try to stop before they reached a stop line marked by two traffic cones. During the experimental blocks, drivers performed a concurrent phone task—a variation of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977). Numbers between 1 and 9 were randomly presented every 7 seconds, either through the hand-held phone or through the vehicle speakers (hands-free). The task involved adding two consecutive numbers and responding verbally. Drivers were given a small monetary bonus for each correct response (5 cents per correct response, up to $20 over the whole experiment). Drivers completed three blocks of 8 laps, each lasting approximately 15-minutes. In two blocks, drivers performed the PASAT (once using the hand-held phone, once using the hands-free system). In the third block, drivers performed the driving tasks alone, with no PASAT. Drivers also completed a short, 3-minute baseline block for the PASAT, performed while the vehicle was stationary. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across driver. Measures of pace clock accuracy (number of correct clock events out of 45 per block), braking response time (from the onset of the traffic light change) and stopping errors (failing to stop or failing to stop before the stop line) were recorded. In between blocks, drivers completed a modified NASA TLX (Hart & Staveland, 1988) that included subjective ratings of performance for all of the relevant driving sub-tasks (pace clocks, stopping task) and for the PASAT. RESULTS Driving Performance Prior to examining drivers’ calibration of distraction effects, we examined the dual-task costs in driving performance associated with the cell phone tasks (i.e., a manipulation check). In general, we used a mixed ANOVA with the variables of Task, Age and Gender (with the exception of the stopping errors, which were not normally distributed; for these we employ non-parametric tests). There were no significant effects of age and gender on any of the measures of driving performance. 56
  • 4. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design Table 1 shows the mean performance scores across dependent measure and task condition. There was a significant effect of task condition across all measures of performance: pace clock accuracy (F(2,68) = 27.1, p < .01); braking response time (F(2,60) = 5.8, p < .01); and stopping errors ( χ F (2) = 16.0, p < .001). The hand-held and hands-free PASAT conditions yielded poorer 2 performance on all metrics than in the single-task driving baseline (all p’s < .01); however, in general, there were no differences between hand-held and hands-free cell phones, replicating previous results (e.g., Strayer & Johnston, 2001; Horrey & Wickens, 2006). Table 1. Summary of driving performance data by task condition Task Condition Pace Clock Brake Response Stop Light Accuracy (%) Time (s) Errors (%) Baseline 82.2 (1.2) 0.88 (0.02) 26 (6) Hand-held 71.1 (1.6) 0.96 (0.03) 48 (6) Hands-free 73.9 (1.2) 0.94 (0.03) 48 (5) Note. Standard error appears in parentheses. Calibration to Distraction Effects In order to examine drivers’ calibration to the effects of in-vehicle distraction, we first calculated estimated and actual distraction effects from the subjective performance ratings and from actual performance along the various measures of driving performance. Subjective estimates of distraction were expressed as a % difference between self-rated performance in the driving baseline block and self-rated performance in the distraction block (the baseline condition was used as a reference in order to control for any systematic biases in ratings). Actual distraction effects were expressed as the % difference in performance from the baseline block to the dual- task block. Thus, negative values denoted an actual or estimated loss in performance for the distraction condition relative to baseline, whereas a positive value indicated a gain in performance. Next, we examined the relationship between subjective estimates of distraction effects and actual distraction effects. If drivers are well-calibrated, we would expect a positive correlation between the estimated and actual distraction effects. That is, larger estimations of distraction effects would correspond to larger observed performance decrements. However, if drivers are not well- calibrated to the magnitude of distraction effects, then there would be no such relationship or a negative relationship may exist. The results from a correlational analysis are shown in Table 2. Overall, there were no significant relationships between estimates of distraction effects and actual performance decrements, lending support to the notion that drivers were not well-calibrated to the distracting effects of a concurrent in-vehicle task. In the hands-free condition, there was a significant relationship for stopping errors (r = -.38); however, this relationship is in the negative direction. Here, drivers that estimated the smallest performance decrements were actually exhibiting the largest ones! The confidence intervals around the correlations in Table 2 suggest that, in general, the poor 57
  • 5. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design calibration of drivers to the effects of distraction were consistent across phone type and dependent measure. An examination of the different driver groups revealed some interesting findings. For example, older male drivers were actually well-calibrated to the magnitude of distraction effects for the stopping task (stopping errors, r = .71; brake RT, r = .82). In contrast, younger males showed strong associations in the opposite direction (stopping errors, r = -.91; brake RT, r = -.65, the latter result was marginally significant). That is, young male drivers that thought they were doing better were actually doing worse than others. In general, female drivers did not exhibit any significant relationships between estimated and actual performance loss (Lesch & Hancock, 2004). Table 2. Correlations between the magnitude of estimated and actual distraction effects (the difference between baseline and distraction conditions) Driver Group Δ Brake Response Time Δ Pace Clock Accuracy Δ Stop Light Errors Free Free Held Held Held Free .06 -.02 .16 .10 .11 -.38* Overall (-.28, .39) (-.33, .31) (-.18, .46) (-.22, .40) (-.23, .42) (-.62, -.07) -.65 -.33 -.07 .30 .05 -.91** Young Males (-.93, .10) (-.79, .38) (-.70, .62) (-.41, .78) (-.64, .69) (-.98, -.66) .82** .45 .40 -.22 .30 .71* Older Males (.34, .96) (-.26, .84) (-.36, .84) (-.74, .48) (-.46, .80) (.15, .93) .07 .13 .42 .51 .34 -.61 Young Females (-.62, .70) (-.58, .73) (-.34, .85) (-.18, .86) (-.37, .80) (-.91, .09) .38 -.04 -.23 -.29 -.29 -.56 Older Females (-.38, .83) (-.65, .60) (-.77, .52) (-.78, .41) (-.80, .46) (-.88, .11) Note. 95% confidence intervals are shown in parentheses. * p < .05, ** p < .01. DISCUSSION In the current study, younger and older drivers were asked to complete a phone task on a hand- held or hands-free cell phone while navigating a closed test-track in an instrumented vehicle. Compared to baseline driving, we observed dual-task decrements on all measures of driving performance, replicating previous results (Alm & Nilsson, 1994; Hancock et al., 2003; McKnight & McKnight, 1993; Strayer & Johnston, 2001). To examine drivers’ calibration to distraction effects, we compared drivers’ subjective estimates of distraction with actual performance decrements, based on a number of measures of driving performance. The results suggest that, for the most part, drivers are not well-calibrated to the distracting effects of a hand-held or hands-free cell phone conversation. Across all measures of performance, subjective estimates of distraction effects were not related to the actual magnitude of the distraction. In some cases, driver’s estimates of distraction were opposite of what was observed in actual performance. That is, drivers that estimated the smallest (or no) distraction effects exhibited the largest ones. In general, a disconnection between performance and awareness was observed across driving measure and phone type. 58
  • 6. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design Analysis of the driver groups, though limited by a small sample size, showed some differences. For female drivers, there were no significant relationships across all measures and phone types (also as shown by Lesch & Hancock, 2004); however, there were some mixed results for males. For example, on some measures young and older males revealed a nearly equal but opposite relationship between estimated and actual distraction effects. Younger males, in particular, were poorly calibrated, suggesting that this may be an important group for targeted remediation. The willingness to engage in distracting activities may be a function of drivers’ perceptions of performance decrements. As such, drivers may engage in distracting activities simply because they do not realize that their performance is degraded or they may be overconfident in their skills and their ability to deal with distractions while behind the wheel (Wogalter & Mayhorn, 2005). Strategies for improving drivers’ calibration to distraction effects include both driver- and technology-based approaches. For the former, training to recognize or attend more closely to their driving activities may help drivers’ determine when their performance is below “baseline.” One question that remains is whether drivers would continue to perform in-vehicle tasks if they were perfectly calibrated to the distraction effects. In other words, the issue could become one of risk tolerance and the degree to which drivers are willing to take on the additional risk of the in- vehicle task. Some technological innovations are intended to monitor driver performance and behaviors in real time and provide alerts when the system infers that a distracting activity is inappropriate (e.g., Donmez, Boyle, & Lee, in press). Thus, these systems have some authority in deciding when the distraction effects of in-vehicle activities are too severe. However, discrepancies between the driver and the system’s estimated level of distraction could create some conflicts. For example, a driver’s mental model of the system may be violated if warnings are issued for events which the driver does not believe to be problematic. The consequences of such a mismatch could include system disuse deriving from reduced trust in the system (Lee & Moray, 1994; Parasuraman & Riley, 1997) or worse (Sarter & Woods, 1995). Providing the raw data underlying the system functions may be a key consideration. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors are grateful to Peter Teare, Eric Jones, Richard Holihan, and Ed Correa for their upkeep and maintenance of the instrumented van and related equipment. We also thank Yueng- hsiang Huang and Michael Blair for their helpful comments regarding an earlier version of this paper. REFERENCES Alm, H. & Nilsson, L. (1994). Changes in driver behaviour as a function of handsfree mobile phones—A simulator study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26, 441-451. Ashley, S. (2001). Driving the info highway. Scientific American, 285, 52-58. Brookhuis, K.A., de Vries, G., & de Waard, D. (1991). The effects of mobile telephoning on driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 23, 309-316. 59
  • 7. PROCEEDINGS of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design Donmez, B., Boyle, L.N., & Lee, J.D. (In press). Safety implications of providing real-time feedback to distracted drivers. Accident Analysis and Prevention. Gronwall, D.M.A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367-373. Hancock, P.A., Lesch, M., & Simmons, L. (2003). The distraction effects of phone use during a crucial driving maneuver. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35(4), 501-514. Hart, S.G., & Staveland, L.E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human Mental Workload. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 139-183. Horrey, W.J. & Wickens, C.D. (2006) Examining the impact of cell phone conversations on driving using meta-analytic techniques. Human Factors, 48(1), 196-205. Lee, J.D. & Moray, N. (1994). Trust, self-confidence, and operators’ adaptation to automation. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 40, 153-184. Lesch, M.F. & Hancock, P.A. (2004). Driving performance during concurrent cell-phone use: Are drivers aware of their performance decrements? Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36, 471-480. McKnight, A.J. & McKnight, A.S. (1993). The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 25, 259-265. Parasuraman, R. & Riley, V. (1997). Humans and automation: Use, misuse, disuse, abuse. Human Factors, 39(2), 230-253. Sarter, N.B. & Woods, D.D. (1995). How in the world did we ever get into that mode? Mode error and awareness in supervisory control. Human Factors, 37(1), 5-19. Strayer, D.L. & Johnston, W.A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular phone. Psychological Science, 12(6), 462-466. Wogalter, M.S. & Mayhorn, C.B. (2005). Perceptions of driver distraction by cellular phone users and nonusers. Human Factors, 47(2), 455-467. 60