SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 58
ACC: Financial Statement Analysis
Due on 7/27/17 – 5pm, CT.
Look at Starbucks company’s cash flow statement. Look at a
minimum of two years.
What are the major sources of cash in operating, investing, and
financing?
How about the major uses?
-
Please include reference. Thanks.
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for
this publication at:
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534
Examining the Validity Structure of
Qualitative Research.
Article · January 1997
CITATIONS
291
READS
13,076
1 author:
Some of the authors of this publication are
also working on theserelated projects:
Multiple and Mixed Paradigms View project
Toward a theory of causation in mixed
methods research. View project
R. Burke Johnson
University of South Alabama
63 PUBLICATIONS 6,256 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by R.
Burke Johnson on 23 January 2015.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded
file.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534_Examinin
g_the_Validity_Structure_of_Qualitative_Research?enrichId=rg
req-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534_Examinin
g_the_Validity_Structure_of_Qualitative_Research?enrichId=rg
req-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/project/Multiple-and-Mixed-
Paradigms?enrichId=rgreq-
43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/project/Toward-a-theory-of-
causation-in-mixed-methods-research?enrichId=rgreq-
43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre
q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre
q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_South_A
labama?enrichId=rgreq-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre
q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre
q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D
%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Examining the validity structure of qualitative research
R Burke Johnson
Education; Winter 1997; 118, 2; Research Library
pg. 282
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?el=1_x_100&
enrichId=rgreq-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox
ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw==
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
View publication statsView publication stats
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534
HHS.gov
The Belmont Report
Office of the Secretary
Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human
Subjects of
Research
The National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects
of Biomedical
and Behavioral Research
April 18, 1979
AGENCY: Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.
ACTION: Notice of Report for Public Comment.
SUMMARY: On July 12, 1974, the National Research Act (Pub.
L. 93-348) was signed into law, there-by
creating the National Commission for the Protection of Human
Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral
Research. One of the charges to the Commission was to identify
the basic ethical principles that should
underlie the conduct of biomedical and behavioral research
involving human subjects and to develop
guidelines which should be followed to assure that such
research is conducted in accordance with those
principles. In carrying out the above, the Commission was
directed to consider: (i) the boundaries
between biomedical and behavioral research and the accepted
and routine practice of medicine, (ii) the
role of assessment of risk-benefit criteria in the determination
of the appropriateness of research involving
human subjects, (iii) appropriate guidelines for the selection of
human subjects for participation in such
research and (iv) the nature and definition of informed consent
in various research settings.
The Belmont Report attempts to summarize the basic ethical
principles identified by the Commission in
the course of its deliberations. It is the outgrowth of an
intensive four-day period of discussions that were
held in February 1976 at the Smithsonian Institution's Belmont
Conference Center supplemented by the
monthly deliberations of the Commission that were held over a
period of nearly four years. It is a
statement of basic ethical principles and guidelines that should
assist in resolving the ethical problems
that surround the conduct of research with human subjects. By
publishing the Report in the Federal
Register, and providing reprints upon request, the Secretary
intends that it may be made readily available
to scientists, members of Institutional Review Boards, and
Federal employees. The two-volume Appendix,
containing the lengthy reports of experts and specialists who
assisted the Commission in fulfilling this part
of its charge, is available as DHEW Publication No. (OS) 78-
0013 and No. (OS) 78-0014, for sale by the
Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office,
Washington, D.C. 20402.
Office for Human Research Protections
U.S. Department of Health & Human Services
Unlike most other reports of the Commission, the Belmont
Report does not make specific
recommendations for administrative action by the Secretary of
Health, Education, and Welfare. Rather,
the Commission recommended that the Belmont Report be
adopted in its entirety, as a statement of the
Department's policy. The Department requests public comment
on this recommendation.
National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of
Biomedical and
Behavioral Research
Members of the Commission
• Kenneth John Ryan, M.D., Chairman, Chief of Staff, Boston
Hospital for Women.
• Joseph V. Brady, Ph.D., Professor of Behavioral Biology,
Johns Hopkins University.
• Robert E. Cooke, M.D., President, Medical College of
Pennsylvania.
• Dorothy I. Height, President, National Council of Negro
Women, Inc.
• Albert R. Jonsen, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Bioethics,
University of California at San Francisco.
• Patricia King, J.D., Associate Professor of Law, Georgetown
University Law Center.
• Karen Lebacqz, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Christian
Ethics, Pacific School of Religion.
• *** David W. Louisell, J.D., Professor of Law, University of
California at Berkeley.
• Donald W. Seldin, M.D., Professor and Chairman, Department
of Internal Medicine, University of Texas
at Dallas.
• ***Eliot Stellar, Ph.D., Provost of the University and
Professor of Physiological Psychology, University of
Pennsylvania.
• *** Robert H. Turtle, LL.B., Attorney, VomBaur, Coburn,
Simmons & Turtle, Washington, D.C.
• ***Deceased.
Table of Contents
• Ethical Principles and Guidelines for Research Involving
Human Subjects
• A. Boundaries Between Practice and Research
• B. Basic Ethical Principles
1. Respect for Persons
2. Beneficence
3. Justice
• C. Applications
1. Informed Consent
2. Assessment of Risk and Benefits
3. Selection of Subjects
Ethical Principles & Guidelines for Research Involving Human
Subjects
Scientific research has produced substantial social benefits. It
has also posed some troubling ethical
questions. Public attention was drawn to these questions by
reported abuses of human subjects in
biomedical experiments, especially during the Second World
War. During the Nuremberg War Crime
Trials, the Nuremberg code was drafted as a set of standards for
judging physicians and scientists who
had conducted biomedical experiments on concentration camp
prisoners. This code became the prototype
of many later codes[1] intended to assure that research
involving human subjects would be carried out in
an ethical manner.
The codes consist of rules, some general, others specific, that
guide the investigators or the reviewers of
research in their work. Such rules often are inadequate to cover
complex situations; at times they come
into conflict, and they are frequently difficult to interpret or
apply. Broader ethical principles will provide a
basis on which specific rules may be formulated, criticized and
interpreted.
Three principles, or general prescriptive judgments, that are
relevant to research involving human subjects
are identified in this statement. Other principles may also be
relevant. These three are comprehensive,
however, and are stated at a level of generalization that should
assist scientists, subjects, reviewers and
interested citizens to understand the ethical issues inherent in
research involving human subjects. These
principles cannot always be applied so as to resolve beyond
dispute particular ethical problems. The
objective is to provide an analytical framework that will guide
the resolution of ethical problems arising
from research involving human subjects.
This statement consists of a distinction between research and
practice, a discussion of the three basic
ethical principles, and remarks about the application of these
principles.
[RETURN TO TABLE OF CONTENTS]
Part A: Boundaries Between Practice & Research
A. Boundaries Between Practice and Research
It is important to distinguish between biomedical and behavioral
research, on the one hand, and the
practice of accepted therapy on the other, in order to know what
activities ought to undergo review for the
protection of human subjects of research. The distinction
between research and practice is blurred partly
because both often occur together (as in research designed to
evaluate a therapy) and partly because
notable departures from standard practice are often called
"experimental" when the terms "experimental"
and "research" are not carefully defined.
For the most part, the term "practice" refers to interventions
that are designed solely to enhance the well-
being of an individual patient or client and that have a
reasonable expectation of success. The purpose of
medical or behavioral practice is to provide diagnosis,
preventive treatment or therapy to particular
individuals [2]. By contrast, the term "research' designates an
activity designed to test an hypothesis,
permit conclusions to be drawn, and thereby to develop or
contribute to generalizable knowledge
(expressed, for example, in theories, principles, and statements
of relationships). Research is usually
described in a formal protocol that sets forth an objective and a
set of procedures designed to reach that
objective.
When a clinician departs in a significant way from standard or
accepted practice, the innovation does not,
in and of itself, constitute research. The fact that a procedure is
"experimental," in the sense of new,
untested or different, does not automatically place it in the
category of research. Radically new procedures
of this description should, however, be made the object of
formal research at an early stage in order to
determine whether they are safe and effective. Thus, it is the
responsibility of medical practice
committees, for example, to insist that a major innovation be
incorporated into a formal research project
[3].
Research and practice may be carried on together when research
is designed to evaluate the safety and
efficacy of a therapy. This need not cause any confusion
regarding whether or not the activity requires
review; the general rule is that if there is any element of
research in an activity, that activity should
undergo review for the protection of human subjects.
Part B: Basic Ethical Principles
B. Basic Ethical Principles
The expression "basic ethical principles" refers to those general
judgments that serve as a basic
justification for the many particular ethical prescriptions and
evaluations of human actions. Three basic
principles, among those generally accepted in our cultural
tradition, are particularly relevant to the ethics
of research involving human subjects: the principles of respect
of persons, beneficence and justice.
1. Respect for Persons. -- Respect for persons incorporates at
least two ethical convictions: first, that
individuals should be treated as autonomous agents, and second,
that persons with diminished autonomy
are entitled to protection. The principle of respect for persons
thus divides into two separate moral
requirements: the requirement to acknowledge autonomy and the
requirement to protect those with
diminished autonomy.
An autonomous person is an individual capable of deliberation
about personal goals and of acting under
the direction of such deliberation. To respect autonomy is to
give weight to autonomous persons'
considered opinions and choices while refraining from
obstructing their actions unless they are clearly
detrimental to others. To show lack of respect for an
autonomous agent is to repudiate that person's
considered judgments, to deny an individual the freedom to act
on those considered judgments, or to
withhold information necessary to make a considered judgment,
when there are no compelling reasons to
do so.
However, not every human being is capable of self-
determination. The capacity for self-determination
matures during an individual's life, and some individuals lose
this capacity wholly or in part because of
illness, mental disability, or circumstances that severely restrict
liberty. Respect for the immature and the
incapacitated may require protecting them as they mature or
while they are incapacitated.
Some persons are in need of extensive protection, even to the
point of excluding them from activities
which may harm them; other persons require little protection
beyond making sure they undertake activities
freely and with awareness of possible adverse consequence. The
extent of protection afforded should
depend upon the risk of harm and the likelihood of benefit. The
judgment that any individual lacks
autonomy should be periodically reevaluated and will vary in
different situations.
In most cases of research involving human subjects, respect for
persons demands that subjects enter into
the research voluntarily and with adequate information. In some
situations, however, application of the
principle is not obvious. The involvement of prisoners as
subjects of research provides an instructive
example. On the one hand, it would seem that the principle of
respect for persons requires that prisoners
not be deprived of the opportunity to volunteer for research. On
the other hand, under prison conditions
they may be subtly coerced or unduly influenced to engage in
research activities for which they would not
otherwise volunteer. Respect for persons would then dictate that
prisoners be protected. Whether to allow
prisoners to "volunteer" or to "protect" them presents a
dilemma. Respecting persons, in most hard cases,
is often a matter of balancing competing claims urged by the
principle of respect itself.
2. Beneficence. -- Persons are treated in an ethical manner not
only by respecting their decisions and
protecting them from harm, but also by making efforts to secure
their well-being. Such treatment falls
under the principle of beneficence. The term "beneficence" is
often understood to cover acts of kindness
or charity that go beyond strict obligation. In this document,
beneficence is understood in a stronger
sense, as an obligation. Two general rules have been formulated
as complementary expressions of
beneficent actions in this sense: (1) do not harm and (2)
maximize possible benefits and minimize
possible harms.
The Hippocratic maxim "do no harm" has long been a
fundamental principle of medical ethics. Claude
Bernard extended it to the realm of research, saying that one
should not injure one person regardless of
the benefits that might come to others. However, even avoiding
harm requires learning what is harmful;
and, in the process of obtaining this information, persons may
be exposed to risk of harm. Further, the
Hippocratic Oath requires physicians to benefit their patients
"according to their best judgment." Learning
what will in fact benefit may require exposing persons to risk.
The problem posed by these imperatives is
to decide when it is justifiable to seek certain benefits despite
the risks involved, and when the benefits
should be foregone because of the risks.
The obligations of beneficence affect both individual
investigators and society at large, because they
extend both to particular research projects and to the entire
enterprise of research. In the case of
particular projects, investigators and members of their
institutions are obliged to give forethought to the
maximization of benefits and the reduction of risk that might
occur from the research investigation. In the
case of scientific research in general, members of the larger
society are obliged to recognize the longer
term benefits and risks that may result from the improvement of
knowledge and from the development of
novel medical, psychotherapeutic, and social procedures.
The principle of beneficence often occupies a well-defined
justifying role in many areas of research
involving human subjects. An example is found in research
involving children. Effective ways of treating
childhood diseases and fostering healthy development are
benefits that serve to justify research involving
children -- even when individual research subjects are not direct
beneficiaries. Research also makes it
possible to avoid the harm that may result from the application
of previously accepted routine practices
that on closer investigation turn out to be dangerous. But the
role of the principle of beneficence is not
always so unambiguous. A difficult ethical problem remains, for
example, about research that presents
more than minimal risk without immediate prospect of direct
benefit to the children involved. Some have
argued that such research is inadmissible, while others have
pointed out that this limit would rule out
much research promising great benefit to children in the future.
Here again, as with all hard cases, the
different claims covered by the principle of beneficence may
come into conflict and force difficult choices.
3. Justice. -- Who ought to receive the benefits of research and
bear its burdens? This is a question of
justice, in the sense of "fairness in distribution" or "what is
deserved." An injustice occurs when some
benefit to which a person is entitled is denied without good
reason or when some burden is imposed
unduly. Another way of conceiving the principle of justice is
that equals ought to be treated equally.
However, this statement requires explication. Who is equal and
who is unequal? What considerations
justify departure from equal distribution? Almost all
commentators allow that distinctions based on
experience, age, deprivation, competence, merit and position do
sometimes constitute criteria justifying
differential treatment for certain purposes. It is necessary, then,
to explain in what respects people should
be treated equally. There are several widely accepted
formulations of just ways to distribute burdens and
benefits. Each formulation mentions some relevant property on
the basis of which burdens and benefits
should be distributed. These formulations are (1) to each person
an equal share, (2) to each person
according to individual need, (3) to each person according to
individual effort, (4) to each person
according to societal contribution, and (5) to each person
according to merit.
Questions of justice have long been associated with social
practices such as punishment, taxation and
political representation. Until recently these questions have not
generally been associated with scientific
research. However, they are foreshadowed even in the earliest
reflections on the ethics of research
involving human subjects. For example, during the 19th and
early 20th centuries the burdens of serving as
research subjects fell largely upon poor ward patients, while the
benefits of improved medical care flowed
primarily to private patients. Subsequently, the exploitation of
unwilling prisoners as research subjects in
Nazi concentration camps was condemned as a particularly
flagrant injustice. In this country, in the
1940's, the Tuskegee syphilis study used disadvantaged, rural
black men to study the untreated course of
a disease that is by no means confined to that population. These
subjects were deprived of demonstrably
effective treatment in order not to interrupt the project, long
after such treatment became generally
available.
Against this historical background, it can be seen how
conceptions of justice are relevant to research
involving human subjects. For example, the selection of
research subjects needs to be scrutinized in order
to determine whether some classes (e.g., welfare patients,
particular racial and ethnic minorities, or
persons confined to institutions) are being systematically
selected simply because of their easy
availability, their compromised position, or their
manipulability, rather than for reasons directly related to
the problem being studied. Finally, whenever research
supported by public funds leads to the
development of therapeutic devices and procedures, justice
demands both that these not provide
advantages only to those who can afford them and that such
research should not unduly involve persons
from groups unlikely to be among the beneficiaries of
subsequent applications of the research.
Part C: Applications
C. Applications
Applications of the general principles to the conduct of research
leads to consideration of the following
requirements: informed consent, risk/benefit assessment, and
the selection of subjects of research.
1. Informed Consent. -- Respect for persons requires that
subjects, to the degree that they are capable,
be given the opportunity to choose what shall or shall not
happen to them. This opportunity is provided
when adequate standards for informed consent are satisfied.
While the importance of informed consent is unquestioned,
controversy prevails over the nature and
possibility of an informed consent. Nonetheless, there is
widespread agreement that the consent process
can be analyzed as containing three elements: information,
comprehension and voluntariness.
Information. Most codes of research establish specific items for
disclosure intended to assure that
subjects are given sufficient information. These items generally
include: the research procedure, their
purposes, risks and anticipated benefits, alternative procedures
(where therapy is involved), and a
statement offering the subject the opportunity to ask questions
and to withdraw at any time from the
research. Additional items have been proposed, including how
subjects are selected, the person
responsible for the research, etc.
However, a simple listing of items does not answer the question
of what the standard should be for
judging how much and what sort of information should be
provided. One standard frequently invoked in
medical practice, namely the information commonly provided
by practitioners in the field or in the locale, is
inadequate since research takes place precisely when a common
understanding does not exist. Another
standard, currently popular in malpractice law, requires the
practitioner to reveal the information that
reasonable persons would wish to know in order to make a
decision regarding their care. This, too, seems
insufficient since the research subject, being in essence a
volunteer, may wish to know considerably more
about risks gratuitously undertaken than do patients who deliver
themselves into the hand of a clinician for
needed care. It may be that a standard of "the reasonable
volunteer" should be proposed: the extent and
nature of information should be such that persons, knowing that
the procedure is neither necessary for
their care nor perhaps fully understood, can decide whether they
wish to participate in the furthering of
knowledge. Even when some direct benefit to them is
anticipated, the subjects should understand clearly
the range of risk and the voluntary nature of participation.
A special problem of consent arises where informing subjects of
some pertinent aspect of the research is
likely to impair the validity of the research. In many cases, it is
sufficient to indicate to subjects that they
are being invited to participate in research of which some
features will not be revealed until the research is
concluded. In all cases of research involving incomplete
disclosure, such research is justified only if it is
clear that (1) incomplete disclosure is truly necessary to
accomplish the goals of the research, (2) there
are no undisclosed risks to subjects that are more than minimal,
and (3) there is an adequate plan for
debriefing subjects, when appropriate, and for dissemination of
research results to them. Information
about risks should never be withheld for the purpose of eliciting
the cooperation of subjects, and truthful
answers should always be given to direct questions about the
research. Care should be taken to
distinguish cases in which disclosure would destroy or
invalidate the research from cases in which
disclosure would simply inconvenience the investigator.
Comprehension. The manner and context in which information
is conveyed is as important as the
information itself. For example, presenting information in a
disorganized and rapid fashion, allowing too
little time for consideration or curtailing opportunities for
questioning, all may adversely affect a subject's
ability to make an informed choice.
Because the subject's ability to understand is a function of
intelligence, rationality, maturity and language,
it is necessary to adapt the presentation of the information to
the subject's capacities. Investigators are
responsible for ascertaining that the subject has comprehended
the information. While there is always an
obligation to ascertain that the information about risk to
subjects is complete and adequately
comprehended, when the risks are more serious, that obligation
increases. On occasion, it may be
suitable to give some oral or written tests of comprehension.
Special provision may need to be made when comprehension is
severely limited -- for example, by
conditions of immaturity or mental disability. Each class of
subjects that one might consider as
incompetent (e.g., infants and young children, mentally disable
patients, the terminally ill and the
comatose) should be considered on its own terms. Even for
these persons, however, respect requires
giving them the opportunity to choose to the extent they are
able, whether or not to participate in research.
The objections of these subjects to involvement should be
honored, unless the research entails providing
them a therapy unavailable elsewhere. Respect for persons also
requires seeking the permission of other
parties in order to protect the subjects from harm. Such persons
are thus respected both by
acknowledging their own wishes and by the use of third parties
to protect them from harm.
The third parties chosen should be those who are most likely to
understand the incompetent subject's
situation and to act in that person's best interest. The person
authorized to act on behalf of the subject
should be given an opportunity to observe the research as it
proceeds in order to be able to withdraw the
subject from the research, if such action appears in the subject's
best interest.
Voluntariness. An agreement to participate in research
constitutes a valid consent only if voluntarily
given. This element of informed consent requires conditions
free of coercion and undue influence.
Coercion occurs when an overt threat of harm is intentionally
presented by one person to another in order
to obtain compliance. Undue influence, by contrast, occurs
through an offer of an excessive, unwarranted,
inappropriate or improper reward or other overture in order to
obtain compliance. Also, inducements that
would ordinarily be acceptable may become undue influences if
the subject is especially vulnerable.
Unjustifiable pressures usually occur when persons in positions
of authority or commanding influence --
especially where possible sanctions are involved -- urge a
course of action for a subject. A continuum of
such influencing factors exists, however, and it is impossible to
state precisely where justifiable
persuasion ends and undue influence begins. But undue
influence would include actions such as
manipulating a person's choice through the controlling influence
of a close relative and threatening to
withdraw health services to which an individual would
otherwise be entitled.
2. Assessment of Risks and Benefits. -- The assessment of risks
and benefits requires a careful arrayal
of relevant data, including, in some cases, alternative ways of
obtaining the benefits sought in the
research. Thus, the assessment presents both an opportunity and
a responsibility to gather systematic
and comprehensive information about proposed research. For
the investigator, it is a means to examine
whether the proposed research is properly designed. For a
review committee, it is a method for
determining whether the risks that will be presented to subjects
are justified. For prospective subjects, the
assessment will assist the determination whether or not to
participate.
The Nature and Scope of Risks and Benefits. The requirement
that research be justified on the basis of
a favorable risk/benefit assessment bears a close relation to the
principle of beneficence, just as the moral
requirement that informed consent be obtained is derived
primarily from the principle of respect for
persons. The term "risk" refers to a possibility that harm may
occur. However, when expressions such as
"small risk" or "high risk" are used, they usually refer (often
ambiguously) both to the chance (probability)
of experiencing a harm and the severity (magnitude) of the
envisioned harm.
The term "benefit" is used in the research context to refer to
something of positive value related to health
or welfare. Unlike, "risk," "benefit" is not a term that expresses
probabilities. Risk is properly contrasted to
probability of benefits, and benefits are properly contrasted
with harms rather than risks of harm.
Accordingly, so-called risk/benefit assessments are concerned
with the probabilities and magnitudes of
possible harm and anticipated benefits. Many kinds of possible
harms and benefits need to be taken into
account. There are, for example, risks of psychological harm,
physical harm, legal harm, social harm and
economic harm and the corresponding benefits. While the most
likely types of harms to research subjects
are those of psychological or physical pain or injury, other
possible kinds should not be overlooked.
Risks and benefits of research may affect the individual
subjects, the families of the individual subjects,
and society at large (or special groups of subjects in society).
Previous codes and Federal regulations
have required that risks to subjects be outweighed by the sum of
both the anticipated benefit to the
subject, if any, and the anticipated benefit to society in the form
of knowledge to be gained from the
research. In balancing these different elements, the risks and
benefits affecting the immediate research
subject will normally carry special weight. On the other hand,
interests other than those of the subject may
on some occasions be sufficient by themselves to justify the
risks involved in the research, so long as the
subjects' rights have been protected. Beneficence thus requires
that we protect against risk of harm to
subjects and also that we be concerned about the loss of the
substantial benefits that might be gained
from research.
The Systematic Assessment of Risks and Benefits. It is
commonly said that benefits and risks must be
"balanced" and shown to be "in a favorable ratio." The
metaphorical character of these terms draws
attention to the difficulty of making precise judgments. Only on
rare occasions will quantitative techniques
be available for the scrutiny of research protocols. However, the
idea of systematic, nonarbitrary analysis
of risks and benefits should be emulated insofar as possible.
This ideal requires those making decisions
about the justifiability of research to be thorough in the
accumulation and assessment of information about
all aspects of the research, and to consider alternatives
systematically. This procedure renders the
assessment of research more rigorous and precise, while making
communication between review board
members and investigators less subject to misinterpretation,
misinformation and conflicting judgments.
Thus, there should first be a determination of the validity of the
presuppositions of the research; then the
nature, probability and magnitude of risk should be
distinguished with as much clarity as possible. The
method of ascertaining risks should be explicit, especially
where there is no alternative to the use of such
vague categories as small or slight risk. It should also be
determined whether an investigator's estimates
of the probability of harm or benefits are reasonable, as judged
by known facts or other available studies.
Finally, assessment of the justifiability of research should
reflect at least the following considerations: (i)
Brutal or inhumane treatment of human subjects is never
morally justified. (ii) Risks should be reduced to
those necessary to achieve the research objective. It should be
determined whether it is in fact necessary
to use human subjects at all. Risk can perhaps never be entirely
eliminated, but it can often be reduced by
careful attention to alternative procedures. (iii) When research
involves significant risk of serious
impairment, review committees should be extraordinarily
insistent on the justification of the risk (looking
usually to the likelihood of benefit to the subject -- or, in some
rare cases, to the manifest voluntariness of
the participation). (iv) When vulnerable populations are
involved in research, the appropriateness of
involving them should itself be demonstrated. A number of
variables go into such judgments, including the
nature and degree of risk, the condition of the particular
population involved, and the nature and level of
the anticipated benefits. (v) Relevant risks and benefits must be
thoroughly arrayed in documents and
procedures used in the informed consent process.
3. Selection of Subjects. -- Just as the principle of respect for
persons finds expression in the
requirements for consent, and the principle of beneficence in
risk/benefit assessment, the principle of
justice gives rise to moral requirements that there be fair
procedures and outcomes in the selection of
research subjects.
Justice is relevant to the selection of subjects of research at two
levels: the social and the individual.
Individual justice in the selection of subjects would require that
researchers exhibit fairness: thus, they
should not offer potentially beneficial research only to some
patients who are in their favor or select only
"undesirable" persons for risky research. Social justice requires
that distinction be drawn between classes
of subjects that ought, and ought not, to participate in any
particular kind of research, based on the ability
of members of that class to bear burdens and on the
appropriateness of placing further burdens on
already burdened persons. Thus, it can be considered a matter of
social justice that there is an order of
preference in the selection of classes of subjects (e.g., adults
before children) and that some classes of
potential subjects (e.g., the institutionalized mentally infirm or
prisoners) may be involved as research
subjects, if at all, only on certain conditions.
Injustice may appear in the selection of subjects, even if
individual subjects are selected fairly by
investigators and treated fairly in the course of research. Thus
injustice arises from social, racial, sexual
and cultural biases institutionalized in society. Thus, even if
individual researchers are treating their
research subjects fairly, and even if IRBs are taking care to
assure that subjects are selected fairly within
a particular institution, unjust social patterns may nevertheless
appear in the overall distribution of the
burdens and benefits of research. Although individual
institutions or investigators may not be able to
resolve a problem that is pervasive in their social setting, they
can consider distributive justice in selecting
research subjects.
Some populations, especially institutionalized ones, are already
burdened in many ways by their infirmities
and environments. When research is proposed that involves
risks and does not include a therapeutic
component, other less burdened classes of persons should be
called upon first to accept these risks of
research, except where the research is directly related to the
specific conditions of the class involved.
Also, even though public funds for research may often flow in
the same directions as public funds for
health care, it seems unfair that populations dependent on
public health care constitute a pool of preferred
research subjects if more advantaged populations are likely to
be the recipients of the benefits.
One special instance of injustice results from the involvement
of vulnerable subjects. Certain groups, such
as racial minorities, the economically disadvantaged, the very
sick, and the institutionalized may
continually be sought as research subjects, owing to their ready
availability in settings where research is
conducted. Given their dependent status and their frequently
compromised capacity for free consent, they
should be protected against the danger of being involved in
research solely for administrative
convenience, or because they are easy to manipulate as a result
of their illness or socioeconomic
condition.
[1] Since 1945, various codes for the proper and responsible
conduct of human experimentation in
medical research have been adopted by different organizations.
The best known of these codes are the
Nuremberg Code of 1947, the Helsinki Declaration of 1964
(revised in 1975), and the 1971 Guidelines
(codified into Federal Regulations in 1974) issued by the U.S.
Department of Health, Education, and
Welfare Codes for the conduct of social and behavioral research
have also been adopted, the best known
being that of the American Psychological Association,
published in 1973.
[2] Although practice usually involves interventions designed
solely to enhance the well-being of a
particular individual, interventions are sometimes applied to
one individual for the enhancement of the
well-being of another (e.g., blood donation, skin grafts, organ
transplants) or an intervention may have the
dual purpose of enhancing the well-being of a particular
individual, and, at the same time, providing some
benefit to others (e.g., vaccination, which protects both the
person who is vaccinated and society
generally). The fact that some forms of practice have elements
other than immediate benefit to the
individual receiving an intervention, however, should not
confuse the general distinction between research
and practice. Even when a procedure applied in practice may
benefit some other person, it remains an
intervention designed to enhance the well-being of a particular
individual or groups of individuals; thus, it
is practice and need not be reviewed as research.
[3] Because the problems related to social experimentation may
differ substantially from those of
biomedical and behavioral research, the Commission
specifically declines to make any policy
determination regarding such research at this time. Rather, the
Commission believes that the problem
ought to be addressed by one of its successor bodies.
Content created by Office for Human Research Protections
Content last reviewed on March 15, 2016
7860-U1D2: Qualitative Research
According to Leedy & Ormrod (2012), qualitative research
incorporates looking at qualities or characteristics that are
unlikely to be condensed to numerical values. Ideally, a
qualitative research intends to survey multiple complexities and
nuances of a specific phenomenon. Therefore, the qualitative
research is mostly observed in research that involves
complicated human circumstances or complicated human
creations. On the other hand, quantitative research aims to
establish the quantities or amounts of either one or multiple
variables desired. The rationale of the study is to describe
qualitative research, its key concepts, and the meaning of
scientific merits.
The qualitative research was majorly used in the
Experimental Methods in Political Science. However, the
quantitative research was also incorporated in the study.
According to McDermott (2002), most political scientists prefer
quantitative analysis. The data was gathered in qualitative forms
but coded in quantitative analysis appropriate for further related
analysis. For example, the behavioral measures required an
experiment to establish the behavior of subjects such as
videotapes. The videotapes are later examined for
characteristics including the facial expressions and dominance
in the group. Besides, the physiological measures incorporate
data such as blood pressure, galvanic skin responses, and the
heart rate.
The Purposive Sampling in Qualitative Research Synthesis
just like the title suggests, it is entirely a qualitative form of
research. Suri (2011) reveals that an improved number of
researches especially from the healthcare and education sector
have recognized the benefit of using the qualitative research.
Notably, the majority of the growing research appears to be
dominated by quantitative research. The study on Ethical
Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects
of Research has used the quantitative techniques to scrutinize
the research protocols (Human Research Protections, 2016). The
essence of using the quantitative research is based on the
availability to scrutinize the research protocols.
The key concept investigated in the Experimental Methods
in Political Science is the review of the used experiments
particularly in political science. Ideally, the first section
provides a general synopsis of the experimental measures and
designs alongside the threats to external and internal validity
(McDermott, 2002). The study has also included the costs and
benefits of using the experimentation. The study focuses on the
experiments done in diverse fields such as political economy,
individual choice literature, and behavioral economics. The
study includes several forms of experimentation such as
simulation studies, field experiments, and field studies.
The key concept in Purposeful Sampling in Qualitative
Research Synthesis is the ability to make informed decisions
concerning sampling that is essential in enhancing the quality of
research synthesis (Suri, 2011). As a result, the research holds
the sparse discussion on diverse strategies important for
purposeful sampling as desirable in research synthesis. The key
concept in Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection
of Human Subjects of Research is to establish some of the basic
ethical principles that need to underlie the conduct of both
behavioral and biomedical research that majorly involves the
human subjects (Human Research Protections, 2016).
There are several ways through which the scientific
methods could have been improved in the article. For example,
the technique of analyzing the research merit should involve an
evaluation of the abilities to coordinate the scientific activities
(Estrela & Cury, 2013). Besides, training human resources and
producing appropriate research is essential in enhancing
scientific merit. Additionally, the research ought to ensure that
it is tied to the unshakeable innovative and creative capacities
and the ability to shift paradigms. The extent to which resources
are invested in the study needs to be considered as a major
indicator of improving the scientific merit of the research.
The persistent link for the article Experimental Methods in
Political Science is 10.1146/annurev.polisci.5.091001.170657.
The persistent link for the article Purposeful Sampling in
Qualitative Research Synthesis is 10.3316/QRJ1102063. The
persistent link for article Ethical Principles and Guidelines for
the Protection of Human Subjects of Research is
https://videocast.nih.gov/pdf/ohrp_appendix_belmont_report_vo
l_2.pdf.
References
Estrela, C., & Cury, A. (2013). Evaluation of scientific
merit. Brazilian Oral Research, 27(4), 297-298.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1806-83242013000400001
Human Research Protections. (2016). Ethical Principles and
Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research.
U.S. Department of Health & Human Services.
Leedy, P., & Ormrod, J. (2012). Practical research: Planning
and Design-Chapter 4 Planning Your Research Project (10th
Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Merrill Prentice Hall.
McDermott, R. (2002). Experiments Methods in Political
Science. Annual Review of Political Science, 5(1), 31-61.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.5.091001.170657
Suri, H. (2011). Purposeful Sampling in Qualitative Research
Synthesis. Qualitative Research Journal, 11(2), 63-75.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3316/qrj1102063
Unit 3
[D]
INTRODUCTION
QUANTITATIVE TOOLS
Constructs
While substantial phenomena exist in the physical world and
can therefore be measured directly, insubstantial
phenomena exist in a symbolic or abstract form and cannot be
measured directly. Insubstantial phenomena are
frequently called constructs. You can think of a construct as a
scientific concept or an abstraction designed to
explain a natural phenomenon (Kerlinger, 1986). Constructs
allow us to talk about abstract ideas. The following
are examples of constructs:
• Intelligence.
• Self-esteem.
• Depression.
• Job satisfaction
• Anxiety.
Constructs are used to understand, organize, and study the
world we live in.
Operational Definitions
As abstractions, constructs are inherently difficult to measure.
Because we cannot touch them, we have to
develop alternative ways to gather information about them.
When we try to measure constructs, or what Leedy
and Ormrod (2013) call insubstantial measurements, we try to
bridge the gap between what we can observe
and the constructs, or abstraction, that a theory proposes.
This process of obtaining systematic data on abstract ideas is
called operationalizing the constructs, or creating
operational definitions for the constructs. Operational
definitions allow us to devise methods, procedures, and
instruments that enable the quantification of constructs.
For example, let us say we are
interested in studying the relationship
between intelligence and school
achievement. Both intelligence and
school achievement are constructs, or
scientific ideas we have created to help
us describe and explain natural
phenomena (in this case, behaviors).
Our theory suggests the existence of a
relationship between these two
constructs. In order to test our theory,
we must obtain measurable data or
information on intelligence and school
achievement.
The Tools of Research
Tools
The graphic depicts the relationship between intelligence and
school achievement. The left portion of the
graphic implies that intelligence is a theoretical construct that is
measured by the WISC intelligence test and
teacher assessments. The right portion of the graphic implies
that school achievement is a theoretical construct
that is measured by the Iowa Test of Basic Skills and student
grade point averages, both of which are
observable phenomena.
How do we measure abstractions? We define observable
phenomena from which we can obtain data. In the
example, we might decide to measure intelligence using an
intelligence test. In selecting an operational
definition, we must recognize its limitations. We assume that
intelligence tests measure certain aspects of
intelligence, but clearly these tests do not tap the entirety of the
construct of intelligence. In fact, critics of
intelligence tests would argue that they measure very minor
aspects of intelligence. We can improve our
chances of tapping the intelligence construct by using more than
one operational definition. Can you think of
the reasons why?
The Importance of Selecting Good Operational Definitions
Selecting good operational definitions is critical to supporting
the construct validity of our research. Researchers
quite frequently use only one operational definition per
construct. The validity of this common practice rests on
the ability of the operational definition to capture the essence
(or at least the most relevant aspects) of the
construct. If that definition does not tap the construct we are
interested in (for example, using head
circumference to measure intelligence), our results will be
meaningless. Fortunately, such obviously poor
operational definitions are rare. Operational definitions that
generate some controversy (for example, using the
results of standardized achievement tests to measure school
performance) are far more common.
Variables
In the foregoing discussion, we identified how we can measure
constructs. Measuring constructs turns them into
variables in our research. A variable is something that can vary
or, in other words, take on at least two different
values. This idea is critical to understanding and identifying
variables. For example, gender cannot be a variable
in a study when all the participants are women.
We should note that not all variables are constructs. Leedy and
Ormrod's (2013) distinction between substantial
and insubstantial measurements highlights the point that some
variables used in research are not constructs.
Substantial measurements, such as the physical attributes of a
person, such as hair color, ring size, head
circumference, height, or thing, such as a car model, can be
used as variables, but they are not abstract
constructs. You will read more about variables in the chapters
on quantitative and qualitative research.
Hypothesis Testing and Data Analysis
How are hypotheses tested? Traditional scientific hypothesis
testing is associated with quantitative research
designs. The essence of hypothesis testing is straightforward
and involves some variation of the following steps:
1. State the research hypothesis and the null hypothesis.
2. Select a significance level (for example, 0.01, or 0.5).
3. Select a test statistic. Based on the level of significance and
other information, such as degrees of
freedom and directional or nondirectional testing, determine the
critical value of the test statistic (for
example, F, r, t) and the corresponding decision rule.
4. Collect your data. Applying the statistical test to the data,
compute an observed value of your chosen
test statistic.
5. Compare the observed value of the test statistic (number 4)
with the preset critical value (number 3).
6. Make the appropriate decision, to either reject or fail to
reject the null hypothesis. Specifically, if the
obtained value is greater than the critical value, reject the null
hypothesis. If the obtained value is less
than the critical value of the test hypothesis, you would fail to
reject the null hypothesis.
In other words, researchers use statistics to compute an obtained
value of their test statistic, which they
compare with a previously established critical value. The
critical value is based on the alpha they selected before
they began their research. Some of the terms used in traditional
hypothesis testing may be unfamiliar to you, so
review the following definitions:
• Alpha: The risk you are willing to accept in the event your
statistical analysis indicates a statistically
significant finding, but that finding is not real—it occurred due
to chance or some other factor. In other
words, alpha is the probability of making a Type I error, which
is the same as the p level.
• p level: The likelihood that your obtained results were due to
chance if the null hypothesis is true.
• Statistical test: The method you use to statistically analyze
your data, such as a t-test or Pearson's
correlation.
• Test statistic: The ruler you use to test your hypothesis. It is
the obtained value from the result of your
statistical test, such as the obtained t value or r.
• Critical value: The value of your test statistic at which you
can reject the null hypothesis.
Types of Errors in Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis testing is based on probability. Regardless of the
statistical test or test statistic used, the researcher
is testing the null hypothesis against the laws of probability.
Using probability, you will always have four possible
outcomes:
• Error 1: You were wrong. You rejected the null hypothesis
when the null was the correct alternative. You
concluded there was a relationship or difference when the
observed differences actually occurred purely
by chance. A Type I Error occurs when you incorrectly reject
the null hypothesis. Alpha (α) or your p value
is the probability of this happening to you.
• Error 2: You were wrong. You retained, or did not reject, the
null when it was actually false. You were not
able to find the relationship or difference that really exists. A
Type II Error occurs when you incorrectly
accept the null hypothesis. The probability of this happening to
you is known as beta (β).
• Correct Decision 1: You were right. You rejected the null
when it was indeed false. You successfully
found the difference or relationship that really exists. Power is
the probability of this happening.
• Correct Decision 2: You were right. You retained the null
when it was true. No difference or relationship
exists, and you found none.
Thus, we try to guard against two types of error when testing
hypotheses. A Type I Error is defined as a false
positive or rejecting the null hypothesis when it should have
been accepted. A Type II Error is defined as a false
negative or failing to reject the null hypothesis when it should
have been rejected (Williams, 1992). Statistical
power refers to the probability of finding relationships or
differences that indeed exist. Much research in the
social sciences is underpowered, meaning that the designs are
not sensitive enough to detect relationships that
may indeed exist (Cohen, 1988). See Lipsey's 1997 chapter,
"Design Sensitivity: Statistical Power for Applied
Experimental Research," in Handbook of Social Science
Research for recommendations on how to increase the
statistical power of research designs.
The following is a paraphrased excerpt from Percy and
Kostere's (2008) Qualitative Research Approaches in
Psychology .
QUALITATIVE TOOLS
Level of Analysis
Level of analysis specifies the type of phenomena being
investigated, according to the following hierarchy:
intra-psychic phenomena; individual whole-person phenomena;
inter-personal phenomena, family phenomena
(including couples or dyads in committed relationships); small-
group phenomena (work group, team);
organizational phenomena (large corporation, large church,
government agency); social-cultural phenomena
(society as a whole, a culture or subculture).
Key Phenomena
Key phenomena in a qualitative study correspond to the
variables in a quantitative study. They might be specific
phenomena, cases, factors, or simply focal elements that are the
focus of the study. They are the primary
constructs under investigation and they should be defined in a
similar way to the way that we define
quantitative constructs. These definitions should be consistent
with similar constructs used in previous research,
whether qualitative or quantitative, and consistent with the
meaning of the terms in the research question.
Types of Data
Qualitative data is not collected from tests and measures like
quantitative data, but consists of verbal and
behavioral output as recorded in interviews, documents,
videotapes or photographs, journals, notes of
observations, and so on. The most common type of qualitative
data is verbal data from interviews.
Role of the Researcher
In qualitative research, the researcher is a tool of the research.
The researcher has to use his or her own
knowledge, training, and experience to collect the data in an
efficient and sufficient manner, to bring the data
together, and to make it into something meaningful and useful.
Data Preparation
Qualitative data must be physically prepared. For example, if
in-depth interviews will generate audiotapes,
those must be transcribed. If videotapes are used, they too must
be transcribed, or perhaps behaviors must be
coded. Transcripts must be organized physically (electronic
copies, paper copies). In some approaches such as
grounded theory, this phase (preparing and organizing the data)
is already part of the data analysis procedures.
In other approaches, such as case study or survey, the various
kinds of data must first be organized and
prepared so that it is usable.
Data Analysis
Depending on the approach used, qualitative data will be
analyzed in different ways, Much like there are
different statistical tests used for different quantitative
approaches, the different qualitative approaches
demand different data analysis techniques. Some examples
include data coding, data reduction, and thematic
analysis.
References
Cohen, J. L. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Cozby, P. C. (1993). Methods in behavioral research (5th ed.).
Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Kerlinger, F. N. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research (3rd
ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Leedy, P. D., & Ormrod, J. E. (2013). Practical research:
Planning and design (10th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Pearson.
Lipsey, J. W. (1997). Design sensitivity: Statistical power for
applied experimental research. In L. Bickman & D.
Rog, Handbook of social science research (pp. 39–68).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Percy, W. H., & Kostere, K. (2008). Qualitative research
approaches in psychology . Minneapolis, MN:
Williams, F. (1992). Reasoning with statistics: How to read
quantitative research (4th ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich.
OBJECTIVES
To successfully complete this learning unit, you will be
expected to:
1. Identify variables in a research study.
2. Delineate quantitative instruments used to measure variables.
3. Explain the importance of operational definitions to scientific
merit.
4. Evaluate the data collection method or methods.
[u03s1] Unit 3 Study 1
STUDIES
Readings
Read the introduction to Unit 3, "The Tools of Research." This
will provide basic explanations and examples
of the key components of quantitative and qualitative research.
Use your Leedy and Ormrod text to complete the following:
• Read Chapter 1, "The Nature and Tools of Research,"
beginning with page 7 at the heading "Tools
for Research," through page 25. This reading covers some of the
tangible tools researchers use, such
as libraries and computers, as well as "cognitive tools," such as
critical thinking and logic.
• Read Chapter 8, "Analyzing Quantitative Data," pages 211–
250. This chapter reviews the types of
quantitative data, descriptive statistics, and inferential
statistics.
Use Trochim's Research Methods Knowledge Base Web site to
read the following pages. This information
contains more information on qualitative phenomena, data
collection, and analysis:
• Qualitative Measures.
• The Qualitative Debate.
• Qualitative Data.
• Qualitative Methods.
Also on the Research Methods Knowledge Base Web site, read
the following pages for further information
on quantitative variables, levels of measurement, hypotheses,
and hypothesis testing. Additionally, you will
learn about the relationship between qualitative and quantitative
data, and other cognitive tools.
• Variables.
• Levels of Measurement.
• Hypotheses.
• Types of Data.
• Deduction and Induction.
• Inferential Statistics. There are several links to specific kinds
of statistical tests on this page that you
might find useful for understanding the data analyses reported
in various quantitative research
articles.
PSY Learners Additional Required Reading
In addition to the other required study activities for this unit,
PSY learners are also required to compete the
following:
• Read Percy, Kostere, and Kostere's 2015 document,
Qualitative Research Approaches in Psychology.
This document provides an overview of qualitative methods and
the major qualitative approaches.
Data collection and data analyses are covered under each
approach. You may find it helpful to refer
to this document throughout this course.
Optional Program-Specific Content
Some programs have opted to provide program-specific content
designed to help you better understand
how the subject matter in this study is incorporated into your
particular field of study. Check below to see if
your program has any suggested readings for you.
SOBT Learners
• Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A-G., & Buchner, A. (2007).
G*power 3: A flexible statistical power
analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39
(2), 1175–191. This reading provides the foundation for using
G*Power to determine sample size for
quantitative studies.
• Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A-G. (2009).
Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1:
Tests for correlation and regression analysis. Behavior Research
Methods, 41(4), 1149–1150. This
reading provides the foundation for using G*Power for
determining sample size for correlation and
regression analysis studies.
• Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). "Building theories from case study
research." Academy of Management
Review 14(4), 532–550. This article provides an overall process
and structure for case study research.
Probably the most cited case study methods paper in business
and management papers.
[u03s2] Unit 3 Study 2
PROJECT PREPARATION
Resources
Research Topic and Methodology Form.
Research Topic and Methodology Scoring Guide.
In preparation for the Unit 4 assignment due next week, make
sure during this unit that you have thoroughly
read and understand the approved research study you selected
for the Unit 2 assignment. Be prepared to
complete the Unit 4 assignment by identifying and
understanding the research topic, research problem,
research question, and basic methodology. You may also find it
beneficial to view the Research Topic and
Methodology Form that you will use to complete the Unit 4
assignment. Also, view the assignment
description and scoring guide to learn how you will be
evaluated.
QUANTITATIVE TOOLS
Resources
Discussion Participation Scoring Guide.
APA Style and Format.
Persistent Links and DOIs.
Make sure the quantitative article that you selected in Unit 1
will allow you to thoroughly address all of the
points required for this discussion. Using the information from
this week's readings, complete the following:
• Identify the instrument or instruments used to quantify the
data, the level of measurement for each
instrument, and the statistics used to analyze the data.
• Identify and describe the constructs, variables, and operational
definitions included in the research.
Do not just list terms. Include a description of how the
researcher defined these.
• Describe the cognitive tool used to interpret the data.
Possibilities include deductive logic, inductive
reasoning, scientific method, or critical thinking.
• Discuss the usefulness of the operational definitions for the
constructs in this study. How could they
have been defined differently? Were the operational definitions
sufficient to allow the researcher to
answer the research question? Make sure to justify your answer.
• Explain the importance of operational definitions to scientific
merit.
• List the persistent link for the article in your response. Refer
to the Persistent Links and DOIs guide,
linked in Resources, to learn how to locate this information in
the library databases.
• Cite all sources in APA style and provide an APA-formatted
reference list at the end of your post.
Response Guidelines
After reviewing the discussion postings, choose one peer to
respond to. For your response:
• Follow the persistent link to the article being discussed.
• Using the language of research, explain how you agree or
disagree with your peer's evaluation,
offering your own suggestions for improving the research
design.
[u03d2
QUALITATIVE TOOLS
Resources
Discussion Participation Scoring Guide.
APA Style and Format.
Persistent Links and DOIs.
Make sure the qualitative article that you selected in Unit 1 will
allow you to thoroughly address all of the
points required for this discussion. Using the information from
this week's readings, complete the following:
• Identify and describe key concept in the research.
• Describe and evaluate the data collection method or methods.
Was the data collection method
appropriate to allow the researcher to answer the research
question? Why or why not?
• Describe and evaluate the data analysis procedure or
procedures. Was the data analysis procedure
appropriate to allow the researcher to answer the research
question?
• Explain the importance of appropriate data collection and data
analyses procedures to scientific
merit.
• List the persistent link for the article in your response. Refer
to the Persistent Links and DOIs guide,
linked in Resources, to learn how to locate this information in
the library database.
• Cite all sources in APA style and provide an APA-formatted
reference list at the end of your post.
Response Guidelines
After reviewing the discussion postings, choose one peer to
respond to. For your response:
• Follow the persistent link to the article being discussed.
• Using the language of research, explain how you agree or
disagree with your peer's evaluation,
offering your own suggestions for improving the research
design.

More Related Content

Similar to ACC: Starbucks Cash Flow Analysis

National Patient Safety Goals.pdf
National Patient Safety Goals.pdfNational Patient Safety Goals.pdf
National Patient Safety Goals.pdfbkbk37
 
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESS
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESSCHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESS
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESSWilheminaRossi174
 
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docx
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docxTopic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docx
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docxturveycharlyn
 
Belmont report
Belmont reportBelmont report
Belmont reportPradeep H
 
Institutional ethics committee.pptx
Institutional ethics committee.pptxInstitutional ethics committee.pptx
Institutional ethics committee.pptxumarfarooque63
 
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docx
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docxDQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docx
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docxelinoraudley582231
 
Pressure Biosciences Investor Presentation
Pressure Biosciences Investor PresentationPressure Biosciences Investor Presentation
Pressure Biosciences Investor PresentationRedChip Companies, Inc.
 
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017Bruno Vayssié
 
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivas
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivasBioetica, problemas y perspectivas
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivasseminary
 
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social Sciences
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social SciencesEvidence Based Practice in the Applied Social Sciences
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social SciencesRobin Paynter
 
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bites
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bitesTranscript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bites
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bitesARDC
 
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdfnipuns1983
 
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for Neuroscience, Ethics, and Society
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for  Neuroscience, Ethics, and SocietyGRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for  Neuroscience, Ethics, and Society
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for Neuroscience, Ethics, and SocietyKarlos Svoboda
 
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge After a disappointi.docx
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge  After a disappointi.docxHistory of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge  After a disappointi.docx
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge After a disappointi.docxpauline234567
 
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptx
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptxRESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptx
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptxJessicaAngeleo3
 
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptx
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptxInstitutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptx
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptxRushikeshShinde74
 
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptx
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptxOrganization of IRBs and SOPs.pptx
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptxMelkamAndargie
 

Similar to ACC: Starbucks Cash Flow Analysis (20)

Medical Ethics With Research
Medical Ethics With ResearchMedical Ethics With Research
Medical Ethics With Research
 
National Patient Safety Goals.pdf
National Patient Safety Goals.pdfNational Patient Safety Goals.pdf
National Patient Safety Goals.pdf
 
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESS
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESSCHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESS
CHAPTER 4 ETHICS AND THE INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD (IRB) PROCESS
 
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docx
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docxTopic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docx
Topic Is a Career in Aviation(pilot) worth the sacrifices..docx
 
MEDICAL ETHICS IN RESEARCH PROPOSALS ( Indian contest )
MEDICAL ETHICS INRESEARCH PROPOSALS  ( Indian contest )    MEDICAL ETHICS INRESEARCH PROPOSALS  ( Indian contest )
MEDICAL ETHICS IN RESEARCH PROPOSALS ( Indian contest )
 
Belmont report
Belmont reportBelmont report
Belmont report
 
Institutional ethics committee.pptx
Institutional ethics committee.pptxInstitutional ethics committee.pptx
Institutional ethics committee.pptx
 
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docx
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docxDQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docx
DQ 2 Responses1.With the advances science and technology, it i.docx
 
Pressure Biosciences Investor Presentation
Pressure Biosciences Investor PresentationPressure Biosciences Investor Presentation
Pressure Biosciences Investor Presentation
 
Ethics report
Ethics reportEthics report
Ethics report
 
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017
Preliminary program of ISPOR Boston 2017
 
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivas
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivasBioetica, problemas y perspectivas
Bioetica, problemas y perspectivas
 
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social Sciences
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social SciencesEvidence Based Practice in the Applied Social Sciences
Evidence Based Practice in the Applied Social Sciences
 
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bites
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bitesTranscript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bites
Transcript webinar-9-5-17-health-and-medical-short-bites
 
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf
2a. The Institutional Review Boards (IRB) are entrusted to protect t.pdf
 
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for Neuroscience, Ethics, and Society
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for  Neuroscience, Ethics, and SocietyGRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for  Neuroscience, Ethics, and Society
GRAY MATTERS Integrative Approaches for Neuroscience, Ethics, and Society
 
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge After a disappointi.docx
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge  After a disappointi.docxHistory of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge  After a disappointi.docx
History of the NCO 1700 Valley Forge After a disappointi.docx
 
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptx
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptxRESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptx
RESEARCH REPORT 2018.pptx
 
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptx
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptxInstitutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptx
Institutional review Board (IRB) by Mr. Rushikesh Shinde.pptx
 
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptx
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptxOrganization of IRBs and SOPs.pptx
Organization of IRBs and SOPs.pptx
 

More from nettletondevon

Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxYour NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxnettletondevon
 
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxYour namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxnettletondevon
 
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxYour nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxnettletondevon
 
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxYour name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxnettletondevon
 
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxYour NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxYour Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
Your Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docxYour Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docx
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docxnettletondevon
 
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxYour muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxnettletondevon
 
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxYour midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxnettletondevon
 
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxYour local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxnettletondevon
 
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxYour letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxnettletondevon
 
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxYour legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxYour Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxYour job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxYour job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxYour job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxYour Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxnettletondevon
 
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxYour instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxnettletondevon
 
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxYour initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxnettletondevon
 
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxYour initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxnettletondevon
 

More from nettletondevon (20)

Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxYour NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
 
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxYour namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
 
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxYour nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
 
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxYour name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
 
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxYour NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
 
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxYour Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
 
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
Your Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docxYour Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docx
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
 
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxYour muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
 
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxYour midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
 
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxYour local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
 
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxYour letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
 
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxYour legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
 
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxYour Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
 
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxYour job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
 
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxYour job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
 
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxYour job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
 
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxYour Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
 
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxYour instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
 
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxYour initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
 
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxYour initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
 

Recently uploaded

The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...fonyou31
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...anjaliyadav012327
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxThe byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxShobhayan Kirtania
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 

Recently uploaded (20)

The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxThe byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 

ACC: Starbucks Cash Flow Analysis

  • 1. ACC: Financial Statement Analysis Due on 7/27/17 – 5pm, CT. Look at Starbucks company’s cash flow statement. Look at a minimum of two years. What are the major sources of cash in operating, investing, and financing? How about the major uses? - Please include reference. Thanks. See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534 Examining the Validity Structure of Qualitative Research. Article · January 1997 CITATIONS 291 READS
  • 2. 13,076 1 author: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on theserelated projects: Multiple and Mixed Paradigms View project Toward a theory of causation in mixed methods research. View project R. Burke Johnson University of South Alabama 63 PUBLICATIONS 6,256 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by R. Burke Johnson on 23 January 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534_Examinin g_the_Validity_Structure_of_Qualitative_Research?enrichId=rg req-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534_Examinin g_the_Validity_Structure_of_Qualitative_Research?enrichId=rg
  • 3. req-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/Multiple-and-Mixed- Paradigms?enrichId=rgreq- 43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/Toward-a-theory-of- causation-in-mixed-methods-research?enrichId=rgreq- 43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- 43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_South_A labama?enrichId=rgreq-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf
  • 4. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?enrichId=rgre q-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw%3D %3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Examining the validity structure of qualitative research R Burke Johnson Education; Winter 1997; 118, 2; Research Library pg. 282 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/R_Johnson3?el=1_x_100& enrichId=rgreq-43249586ca0542a4c72d4c1a99341635- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI0NjEyNjUzNDtBUzox ODg4ODQ1MDM3NjkxMDFAMTQyMjA0NDk4MzkwNw== Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
  • 5. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
  • 6. reproduction prohibited without permission. View publication statsView publication stats https://www.researchgate.net/publication/246126534 HHS.gov The Belmont Report Office of the Secretary Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research The National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research April 18, 1979 AGENCY: Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. ACTION: Notice of Report for Public Comment. SUMMARY: On July 12, 1974, the National Research Act (Pub. L. 93-348) was signed into law, there-by creating the National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. One of the charges to the Commission was to identify the basic ethical principles that should underlie the conduct of biomedical and behavioral research involving human subjects and to develop
  • 7. guidelines which should be followed to assure that such research is conducted in accordance with those principles. In carrying out the above, the Commission was directed to consider: (i) the boundaries between biomedical and behavioral research and the accepted and routine practice of medicine, (ii) the role of assessment of risk-benefit criteria in the determination of the appropriateness of research involving human subjects, (iii) appropriate guidelines for the selection of human subjects for participation in such research and (iv) the nature and definition of informed consent in various research settings. The Belmont Report attempts to summarize the basic ethical principles identified by the Commission in the course of its deliberations. It is the outgrowth of an intensive four-day period of discussions that were held in February 1976 at the Smithsonian Institution's Belmont Conference Center supplemented by the monthly deliberations of the Commission that were held over a period of nearly four years. It is a statement of basic ethical principles and guidelines that should assist in resolving the ethical problems that surround the conduct of research with human subjects. By publishing the Report in the Federal
  • 8. Register, and providing reprints upon request, the Secretary intends that it may be made readily available to scientists, members of Institutional Review Boards, and Federal employees. The two-volume Appendix, containing the lengthy reports of experts and specialists who assisted the Commission in fulfilling this part of its charge, is available as DHEW Publication No. (OS) 78- 0013 and No. (OS) 78-0014, for sale by the Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C. 20402. Office for Human Research Protections U.S. Department of Health & Human Services Unlike most other reports of the Commission, the Belmont Report does not make specific recommendations for administrative action by the Secretary of Health, Education, and Welfare. Rather, the Commission recommended that the Belmont Report be adopted in its entirety, as a statement of the Department's policy. The Department requests public comment on this recommendation. National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and
  • 9. Behavioral Research Members of the Commission • Kenneth John Ryan, M.D., Chairman, Chief of Staff, Boston Hospital for Women. • Joseph V. Brady, Ph.D., Professor of Behavioral Biology, Johns Hopkins University. • Robert E. Cooke, M.D., President, Medical College of Pennsylvania. • Dorothy I. Height, President, National Council of Negro Women, Inc. • Albert R. Jonsen, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Bioethics, University of California at San Francisco. • Patricia King, J.D., Associate Professor of Law, Georgetown University Law Center. • Karen Lebacqz, Ph.D., Associate Professor of Christian Ethics, Pacific School of Religion. • *** David W. Louisell, J.D., Professor of Law, University of California at Berkeley. • Donald W. Seldin, M.D., Professor and Chairman, Department of Internal Medicine, University of Texas at Dallas. • ***Eliot Stellar, Ph.D., Provost of the University and Professor of Physiological Psychology, University of
  • 10. Pennsylvania. • *** Robert H. Turtle, LL.B., Attorney, VomBaur, Coburn, Simmons & Turtle, Washington, D.C. • ***Deceased. Table of Contents • Ethical Principles and Guidelines for Research Involving Human Subjects • A. Boundaries Between Practice and Research • B. Basic Ethical Principles 1. Respect for Persons 2. Beneficence 3. Justice • C. Applications 1. Informed Consent 2. Assessment of Risk and Benefits 3. Selection of Subjects Ethical Principles & Guidelines for Research Involving Human Subjects Scientific research has produced substantial social benefits. It
  • 11. has also posed some troubling ethical questions. Public attention was drawn to these questions by reported abuses of human subjects in biomedical experiments, especially during the Second World War. During the Nuremberg War Crime Trials, the Nuremberg code was drafted as a set of standards for judging physicians and scientists who had conducted biomedical experiments on concentration camp prisoners. This code became the prototype of many later codes[1] intended to assure that research involving human subjects would be carried out in an ethical manner. The codes consist of rules, some general, others specific, that guide the investigators or the reviewers of research in their work. Such rules often are inadequate to cover complex situations; at times they come into conflict, and they are frequently difficult to interpret or apply. Broader ethical principles will provide a basis on which specific rules may be formulated, criticized and interpreted. Three principles, or general prescriptive judgments, that are relevant to research involving human subjects are identified in this statement. Other principles may also be relevant. These three are comprehensive,
  • 12. however, and are stated at a level of generalization that should assist scientists, subjects, reviewers and interested citizens to understand the ethical issues inherent in research involving human subjects. These principles cannot always be applied so as to resolve beyond dispute particular ethical problems. The objective is to provide an analytical framework that will guide the resolution of ethical problems arising from research involving human subjects. This statement consists of a distinction between research and practice, a discussion of the three basic ethical principles, and remarks about the application of these principles. [RETURN TO TABLE OF CONTENTS] Part A: Boundaries Between Practice & Research A. Boundaries Between Practice and Research It is important to distinguish between biomedical and behavioral research, on the one hand, and the practice of accepted therapy on the other, in order to know what activities ought to undergo review for the protection of human subjects of research. The distinction
  • 13. between research and practice is blurred partly because both often occur together (as in research designed to evaluate a therapy) and partly because notable departures from standard practice are often called "experimental" when the terms "experimental" and "research" are not carefully defined. For the most part, the term "practice" refers to interventions that are designed solely to enhance the well- being of an individual patient or client and that have a reasonable expectation of success. The purpose of medical or behavioral practice is to provide diagnosis, preventive treatment or therapy to particular individuals [2]. By contrast, the term "research' designates an activity designed to test an hypothesis, permit conclusions to be drawn, and thereby to develop or contribute to generalizable knowledge (expressed, for example, in theories, principles, and statements of relationships). Research is usually described in a formal protocol that sets forth an objective and a set of procedures designed to reach that objective. When a clinician departs in a significant way from standard or accepted practice, the innovation does not,
  • 14. in and of itself, constitute research. The fact that a procedure is "experimental," in the sense of new, untested or different, does not automatically place it in the category of research. Radically new procedures of this description should, however, be made the object of formal research at an early stage in order to determine whether they are safe and effective. Thus, it is the responsibility of medical practice committees, for example, to insist that a major innovation be incorporated into a formal research project [3]. Research and practice may be carried on together when research is designed to evaluate the safety and efficacy of a therapy. This need not cause any confusion regarding whether or not the activity requires review; the general rule is that if there is any element of research in an activity, that activity should undergo review for the protection of human subjects. Part B: Basic Ethical Principles B. Basic Ethical Principles The expression "basic ethical principles" refers to those general judgments that serve as a basic justification for the many particular ethical prescriptions and
  • 15. evaluations of human actions. Three basic principles, among those generally accepted in our cultural tradition, are particularly relevant to the ethics of research involving human subjects: the principles of respect of persons, beneficence and justice. 1. Respect for Persons. -- Respect for persons incorporates at least two ethical convictions: first, that individuals should be treated as autonomous agents, and second, that persons with diminished autonomy are entitled to protection. The principle of respect for persons thus divides into two separate moral requirements: the requirement to acknowledge autonomy and the requirement to protect those with diminished autonomy. An autonomous person is an individual capable of deliberation about personal goals and of acting under the direction of such deliberation. To respect autonomy is to give weight to autonomous persons' considered opinions and choices while refraining from obstructing their actions unless they are clearly detrimental to others. To show lack of respect for an autonomous agent is to repudiate that person's
  • 16. considered judgments, to deny an individual the freedom to act on those considered judgments, or to withhold information necessary to make a considered judgment, when there are no compelling reasons to do so. However, not every human being is capable of self- determination. The capacity for self-determination matures during an individual's life, and some individuals lose this capacity wholly or in part because of illness, mental disability, or circumstances that severely restrict liberty. Respect for the immature and the incapacitated may require protecting them as they mature or while they are incapacitated. Some persons are in need of extensive protection, even to the point of excluding them from activities which may harm them; other persons require little protection beyond making sure they undertake activities freely and with awareness of possible adverse consequence. The extent of protection afforded should depend upon the risk of harm and the likelihood of benefit. The judgment that any individual lacks autonomy should be periodically reevaluated and will vary in different situations. In most cases of research involving human subjects, respect for
  • 17. persons demands that subjects enter into the research voluntarily and with adequate information. In some situations, however, application of the principle is not obvious. The involvement of prisoners as subjects of research provides an instructive example. On the one hand, it would seem that the principle of respect for persons requires that prisoners not be deprived of the opportunity to volunteer for research. On the other hand, under prison conditions they may be subtly coerced or unduly influenced to engage in research activities for which they would not otherwise volunteer. Respect for persons would then dictate that prisoners be protected. Whether to allow prisoners to "volunteer" or to "protect" them presents a dilemma. Respecting persons, in most hard cases, is often a matter of balancing competing claims urged by the principle of respect itself. 2. Beneficence. -- Persons are treated in an ethical manner not only by respecting their decisions and protecting them from harm, but also by making efforts to secure their well-being. Such treatment falls under the principle of beneficence. The term "beneficence" is often understood to cover acts of kindness or charity that go beyond strict obligation. In this document,
  • 18. beneficence is understood in a stronger sense, as an obligation. Two general rules have been formulated as complementary expressions of beneficent actions in this sense: (1) do not harm and (2) maximize possible benefits and minimize possible harms. The Hippocratic maxim "do no harm" has long been a fundamental principle of medical ethics. Claude Bernard extended it to the realm of research, saying that one should not injure one person regardless of the benefits that might come to others. However, even avoiding harm requires learning what is harmful; and, in the process of obtaining this information, persons may be exposed to risk of harm. Further, the Hippocratic Oath requires physicians to benefit their patients "according to their best judgment." Learning what will in fact benefit may require exposing persons to risk. The problem posed by these imperatives is to decide when it is justifiable to seek certain benefits despite the risks involved, and when the benefits should be foregone because of the risks. The obligations of beneficence affect both individual
  • 19. investigators and society at large, because they extend both to particular research projects and to the entire enterprise of research. In the case of particular projects, investigators and members of their institutions are obliged to give forethought to the maximization of benefits and the reduction of risk that might occur from the research investigation. In the case of scientific research in general, members of the larger society are obliged to recognize the longer term benefits and risks that may result from the improvement of knowledge and from the development of novel medical, psychotherapeutic, and social procedures. The principle of beneficence often occupies a well-defined justifying role in many areas of research involving human subjects. An example is found in research involving children. Effective ways of treating childhood diseases and fostering healthy development are benefits that serve to justify research involving children -- even when individual research subjects are not direct beneficiaries. Research also makes it possible to avoid the harm that may result from the application of previously accepted routine practices that on closer investigation turn out to be dangerous. But the role of the principle of beneficence is not
  • 20. always so unambiguous. A difficult ethical problem remains, for example, about research that presents more than minimal risk without immediate prospect of direct benefit to the children involved. Some have argued that such research is inadmissible, while others have pointed out that this limit would rule out much research promising great benefit to children in the future. Here again, as with all hard cases, the different claims covered by the principle of beneficence may come into conflict and force difficult choices. 3. Justice. -- Who ought to receive the benefits of research and bear its burdens? This is a question of justice, in the sense of "fairness in distribution" or "what is deserved." An injustice occurs when some benefit to which a person is entitled is denied without good reason or when some burden is imposed unduly. Another way of conceiving the principle of justice is that equals ought to be treated equally. However, this statement requires explication. Who is equal and who is unequal? What considerations justify departure from equal distribution? Almost all commentators allow that distinctions based on experience, age, deprivation, competence, merit and position do sometimes constitute criteria justifying
  • 21. differential treatment for certain purposes. It is necessary, then, to explain in what respects people should be treated equally. There are several widely accepted formulations of just ways to distribute burdens and benefits. Each formulation mentions some relevant property on the basis of which burdens and benefits should be distributed. These formulations are (1) to each person an equal share, (2) to each person according to individual need, (3) to each person according to individual effort, (4) to each person according to societal contribution, and (5) to each person according to merit. Questions of justice have long been associated with social practices such as punishment, taxation and political representation. Until recently these questions have not generally been associated with scientific research. However, they are foreshadowed even in the earliest reflections on the ethics of research involving human subjects. For example, during the 19th and early 20th centuries the burdens of serving as research subjects fell largely upon poor ward patients, while the benefits of improved medical care flowed
  • 22. primarily to private patients. Subsequently, the exploitation of unwilling prisoners as research subjects in Nazi concentration camps was condemned as a particularly flagrant injustice. In this country, in the 1940's, the Tuskegee syphilis study used disadvantaged, rural black men to study the untreated course of a disease that is by no means confined to that population. These subjects were deprived of demonstrably effective treatment in order not to interrupt the project, long after such treatment became generally available. Against this historical background, it can be seen how conceptions of justice are relevant to research involving human subjects. For example, the selection of research subjects needs to be scrutinized in order to determine whether some classes (e.g., welfare patients, particular racial and ethnic minorities, or persons confined to institutions) are being systematically selected simply because of their easy availability, their compromised position, or their manipulability, rather than for reasons directly related to the problem being studied. Finally, whenever research supported by public funds leads to the development of therapeutic devices and procedures, justice
  • 23. demands both that these not provide advantages only to those who can afford them and that such research should not unduly involve persons from groups unlikely to be among the beneficiaries of subsequent applications of the research. Part C: Applications C. Applications Applications of the general principles to the conduct of research leads to consideration of the following requirements: informed consent, risk/benefit assessment, and the selection of subjects of research. 1. Informed Consent. -- Respect for persons requires that subjects, to the degree that they are capable, be given the opportunity to choose what shall or shall not happen to them. This opportunity is provided when adequate standards for informed consent are satisfied. While the importance of informed consent is unquestioned, controversy prevails over the nature and possibility of an informed consent. Nonetheless, there is widespread agreement that the consent process can be analyzed as containing three elements: information, comprehension and voluntariness.
  • 24. Information. Most codes of research establish specific items for disclosure intended to assure that subjects are given sufficient information. These items generally include: the research procedure, their purposes, risks and anticipated benefits, alternative procedures (where therapy is involved), and a statement offering the subject the opportunity to ask questions and to withdraw at any time from the research. Additional items have been proposed, including how subjects are selected, the person responsible for the research, etc. However, a simple listing of items does not answer the question of what the standard should be for judging how much and what sort of information should be provided. One standard frequently invoked in medical practice, namely the information commonly provided by practitioners in the field or in the locale, is inadequate since research takes place precisely when a common understanding does not exist. Another standard, currently popular in malpractice law, requires the practitioner to reveal the information that reasonable persons would wish to know in order to make a decision regarding their care. This, too, seems
  • 25. insufficient since the research subject, being in essence a volunteer, may wish to know considerably more about risks gratuitously undertaken than do patients who deliver themselves into the hand of a clinician for needed care. It may be that a standard of "the reasonable volunteer" should be proposed: the extent and nature of information should be such that persons, knowing that the procedure is neither necessary for their care nor perhaps fully understood, can decide whether they wish to participate in the furthering of knowledge. Even when some direct benefit to them is anticipated, the subjects should understand clearly the range of risk and the voluntary nature of participation. A special problem of consent arises where informing subjects of some pertinent aspect of the research is likely to impair the validity of the research. In many cases, it is sufficient to indicate to subjects that they are being invited to participate in research of which some features will not be revealed until the research is concluded. In all cases of research involving incomplete disclosure, such research is justified only if it is clear that (1) incomplete disclosure is truly necessary to accomplish the goals of the research, (2) there are no undisclosed risks to subjects that are more than minimal,
  • 26. and (3) there is an adequate plan for debriefing subjects, when appropriate, and for dissemination of research results to them. Information about risks should never be withheld for the purpose of eliciting the cooperation of subjects, and truthful answers should always be given to direct questions about the research. Care should be taken to distinguish cases in which disclosure would destroy or invalidate the research from cases in which disclosure would simply inconvenience the investigator. Comprehension. The manner and context in which information is conveyed is as important as the information itself. For example, presenting information in a disorganized and rapid fashion, allowing too little time for consideration or curtailing opportunities for questioning, all may adversely affect a subject's ability to make an informed choice. Because the subject's ability to understand is a function of intelligence, rationality, maturity and language, it is necessary to adapt the presentation of the information to the subject's capacities. Investigators are responsible for ascertaining that the subject has comprehended the information. While there is always an
  • 27. obligation to ascertain that the information about risk to subjects is complete and adequately comprehended, when the risks are more serious, that obligation increases. On occasion, it may be suitable to give some oral or written tests of comprehension. Special provision may need to be made when comprehension is severely limited -- for example, by conditions of immaturity or mental disability. Each class of subjects that one might consider as incompetent (e.g., infants and young children, mentally disable patients, the terminally ill and the comatose) should be considered on its own terms. Even for these persons, however, respect requires giving them the opportunity to choose to the extent they are able, whether or not to participate in research. The objections of these subjects to involvement should be honored, unless the research entails providing them a therapy unavailable elsewhere. Respect for persons also requires seeking the permission of other parties in order to protect the subjects from harm. Such persons are thus respected both by acknowledging their own wishes and by the use of third parties to protect them from harm.
  • 28. The third parties chosen should be those who are most likely to understand the incompetent subject's situation and to act in that person's best interest. The person authorized to act on behalf of the subject should be given an opportunity to observe the research as it proceeds in order to be able to withdraw the subject from the research, if such action appears in the subject's best interest. Voluntariness. An agreement to participate in research constitutes a valid consent only if voluntarily given. This element of informed consent requires conditions free of coercion and undue influence. Coercion occurs when an overt threat of harm is intentionally presented by one person to another in order to obtain compliance. Undue influence, by contrast, occurs through an offer of an excessive, unwarranted, inappropriate or improper reward or other overture in order to obtain compliance. Also, inducements that would ordinarily be acceptable may become undue influences if the subject is especially vulnerable. Unjustifiable pressures usually occur when persons in positions of authority or commanding influence -- especially where possible sanctions are involved -- urge a course of action for a subject. A continuum of
  • 29. such influencing factors exists, however, and it is impossible to state precisely where justifiable persuasion ends and undue influence begins. But undue influence would include actions such as manipulating a person's choice through the controlling influence of a close relative and threatening to withdraw health services to which an individual would otherwise be entitled. 2. Assessment of Risks and Benefits. -- The assessment of risks and benefits requires a careful arrayal of relevant data, including, in some cases, alternative ways of obtaining the benefits sought in the research. Thus, the assessment presents both an opportunity and a responsibility to gather systematic and comprehensive information about proposed research. For the investigator, it is a means to examine whether the proposed research is properly designed. For a review committee, it is a method for determining whether the risks that will be presented to subjects are justified. For prospective subjects, the assessment will assist the determination whether or not to participate.
  • 30. The Nature and Scope of Risks and Benefits. The requirement that research be justified on the basis of a favorable risk/benefit assessment bears a close relation to the principle of beneficence, just as the moral requirement that informed consent be obtained is derived primarily from the principle of respect for persons. The term "risk" refers to a possibility that harm may occur. However, when expressions such as "small risk" or "high risk" are used, they usually refer (often ambiguously) both to the chance (probability) of experiencing a harm and the severity (magnitude) of the envisioned harm. The term "benefit" is used in the research context to refer to something of positive value related to health or welfare. Unlike, "risk," "benefit" is not a term that expresses probabilities. Risk is properly contrasted to probability of benefits, and benefits are properly contrasted with harms rather than risks of harm. Accordingly, so-called risk/benefit assessments are concerned with the probabilities and magnitudes of possible harm and anticipated benefits. Many kinds of possible harms and benefits need to be taken into account. There are, for example, risks of psychological harm, physical harm, legal harm, social harm and
  • 31. economic harm and the corresponding benefits. While the most likely types of harms to research subjects are those of psychological or physical pain or injury, other possible kinds should not be overlooked. Risks and benefits of research may affect the individual subjects, the families of the individual subjects, and society at large (or special groups of subjects in society). Previous codes and Federal regulations have required that risks to subjects be outweighed by the sum of both the anticipated benefit to the subject, if any, and the anticipated benefit to society in the form of knowledge to be gained from the research. In balancing these different elements, the risks and benefits affecting the immediate research subject will normally carry special weight. On the other hand, interests other than those of the subject may on some occasions be sufficient by themselves to justify the risks involved in the research, so long as the subjects' rights have been protected. Beneficence thus requires that we protect against risk of harm to subjects and also that we be concerned about the loss of the substantial benefits that might be gained from research. The Systematic Assessment of Risks and Benefits. It is
  • 32. commonly said that benefits and risks must be "balanced" and shown to be "in a favorable ratio." The metaphorical character of these terms draws attention to the difficulty of making precise judgments. Only on rare occasions will quantitative techniques be available for the scrutiny of research protocols. However, the idea of systematic, nonarbitrary analysis of risks and benefits should be emulated insofar as possible. This ideal requires those making decisions about the justifiability of research to be thorough in the accumulation and assessment of information about all aspects of the research, and to consider alternatives systematically. This procedure renders the assessment of research more rigorous and precise, while making communication between review board members and investigators less subject to misinterpretation, misinformation and conflicting judgments. Thus, there should first be a determination of the validity of the presuppositions of the research; then the nature, probability and magnitude of risk should be distinguished with as much clarity as possible. The method of ascertaining risks should be explicit, especially where there is no alternative to the use of such vague categories as small or slight risk. It should also be
  • 33. determined whether an investigator's estimates of the probability of harm or benefits are reasonable, as judged by known facts or other available studies. Finally, assessment of the justifiability of research should reflect at least the following considerations: (i) Brutal or inhumane treatment of human subjects is never morally justified. (ii) Risks should be reduced to those necessary to achieve the research objective. It should be determined whether it is in fact necessary to use human subjects at all. Risk can perhaps never be entirely eliminated, but it can often be reduced by careful attention to alternative procedures. (iii) When research involves significant risk of serious impairment, review committees should be extraordinarily insistent on the justification of the risk (looking usually to the likelihood of benefit to the subject -- or, in some rare cases, to the manifest voluntariness of the participation). (iv) When vulnerable populations are involved in research, the appropriateness of involving them should itself be demonstrated. A number of variables go into such judgments, including the nature and degree of risk, the condition of the particular population involved, and the nature and level of
  • 34. the anticipated benefits. (v) Relevant risks and benefits must be thoroughly arrayed in documents and procedures used in the informed consent process. 3. Selection of Subjects. -- Just as the principle of respect for persons finds expression in the requirements for consent, and the principle of beneficence in risk/benefit assessment, the principle of justice gives rise to moral requirements that there be fair procedures and outcomes in the selection of research subjects. Justice is relevant to the selection of subjects of research at two levels: the social and the individual. Individual justice in the selection of subjects would require that researchers exhibit fairness: thus, they should not offer potentially beneficial research only to some patients who are in their favor or select only "undesirable" persons for risky research. Social justice requires that distinction be drawn between classes of subjects that ought, and ought not, to participate in any particular kind of research, based on the ability of members of that class to bear burdens and on the appropriateness of placing further burdens on already burdened persons. Thus, it can be considered a matter of
  • 35. social justice that there is an order of preference in the selection of classes of subjects (e.g., adults before children) and that some classes of potential subjects (e.g., the institutionalized mentally infirm or prisoners) may be involved as research subjects, if at all, only on certain conditions. Injustice may appear in the selection of subjects, even if individual subjects are selected fairly by investigators and treated fairly in the course of research. Thus injustice arises from social, racial, sexual and cultural biases institutionalized in society. Thus, even if individual researchers are treating their research subjects fairly, and even if IRBs are taking care to assure that subjects are selected fairly within a particular institution, unjust social patterns may nevertheless appear in the overall distribution of the burdens and benefits of research. Although individual institutions or investigators may not be able to resolve a problem that is pervasive in their social setting, they can consider distributive justice in selecting research subjects. Some populations, especially institutionalized ones, are already burdened in many ways by their infirmities
  • 36. and environments. When research is proposed that involves risks and does not include a therapeutic component, other less burdened classes of persons should be called upon first to accept these risks of research, except where the research is directly related to the specific conditions of the class involved. Also, even though public funds for research may often flow in the same directions as public funds for health care, it seems unfair that populations dependent on public health care constitute a pool of preferred research subjects if more advantaged populations are likely to be the recipients of the benefits. One special instance of injustice results from the involvement of vulnerable subjects. Certain groups, such as racial minorities, the economically disadvantaged, the very sick, and the institutionalized may continually be sought as research subjects, owing to their ready availability in settings where research is conducted. Given their dependent status and their frequently compromised capacity for free consent, they should be protected against the danger of being involved in research solely for administrative convenience, or because they are easy to manipulate as a result
  • 37. of their illness or socioeconomic condition. [1] Since 1945, various codes for the proper and responsible conduct of human experimentation in medical research have been adopted by different organizations. The best known of these codes are the Nuremberg Code of 1947, the Helsinki Declaration of 1964 (revised in 1975), and the 1971 Guidelines (codified into Federal Regulations in 1974) issued by the U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Codes for the conduct of social and behavioral research have also been adopted, the best known being that of the American Psychological Association, published in 1973. [2] Although practice usually involves interventions designed solely to enhance the well-being of a particular individual, interventions are sometimes applied to one individual for the enhancement of the well-being of another (e.g., blood donation, skin grafts, organ transplants) or an intervention may have the dual purpose of enhancing the well-being of a particular individual, and, at the same time, providing some benefit to others (e.g., vaccination, which protects both the person who is vaccinated and society
  • 38. generally). The fact that some forms of practice have elements other than immediate benefit to the individual receiving an intervention, however, should not confuse the general distinction between research and practice. Even when a procedure applied in practice may benefit some other person, it remains an intervention designed to enhance the well-being of a particular individual or groups of individuals; thus, it is practice and need not be reviewed as research. [3] Because the problems related to social experimentation may differ substantially from those of biomedical and behavioral research, the Commission specifically declines to make any policy determination regarding such research at this time. Rather, the Commission believes that the problem ought to be addressed by one of its successor bodies. Content created by Office for Human Research Protections Content last reviewed on March 15, 2016 7860-U1D2: Qualitative Research According to Leedy & Ormrod (2012), qualitative research
  • 39. incorporates looking at qualities or characteristics that are unlikely to be condensed to numerical values. Ideally, a qualitative research intends to survey multiple complexities and nuances of a specific phenomenon. Therefore, the qualitative research is mostly observed in research that involves complicated human circumstances or complicated human creations. On the other hand, quantitative research aims to establish the quantities or amounts of either one or multiple variables desired. The rationale of the study is to describe qualitative research, its key concepts, and the meaning of scientific merits. The qualitative research was majorly used in the Experimental Methods in Political Science. However, the quantitative research was also incorporated in the study. According to McDermott (2002), most political scientists prefer quantitative analysis. The data was gathered in qualitative forms but coded in quantitative analysis appropriate for further related analysis. For example, the behavioral measures required an experiment to establish the behavior of subjects such as videotapes. The videotapes are later examined for characteristics including the facial expressions and dominance in the group. Besides, the physiological measures incorporate data such as blood pressure, galvanic skin responses, and the heart rate. The Purposive Sampling in Qualitative Research Synthesis just like the title suggests, it is entirely a qualitative form of research. Suri (2011) reveals that an improved number of researches especially from the healthcare and education sector have recognized the benefit of using the qualitative research. Notably, the majority of the growing research appears to be dominated by quantitative research. The study on Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research has used the quantitative techniques to scrutinize the research protocols (Human Research Protections, 2016). The essence of using the quantitative research is based on the availability to scrutinize the research protocols.
  • 40. The key concept investigated in the Experimental Methods in Political Science is the review of the used experiments particularly in political science. Ideally, the first section provides a general synopsis of the experimental measures and designs alongside the threats to external and internal validity (McDermott, 2002). The study has also included the costs and benefits of using the experimentation. The study focuses on the experiments done in diverse fields such as political economy, individual choice literature, and behavioral economics. The study includes several forms of experimentation such as simulation studies, field experiments, and field studies. The key concept in Purposeful Sampling in Qualitative Research Synthesis is the ability to make informed decisions concerning sampling that is essential in enhancing the quality of research synthesis (Suri, 2011). As a result, the research holds the sparse discussion on diverse strategies important for purposeful sampling as desirable in research synthesis. The key concept in Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research is to establish some of the basic ethical principles that need to underlie the conduct of both behavioral and biomedical research that majorly involves the human subjects (Human Research Protections, 2016). There are several ways through which the scientific methods could have been improved in the article. For example, the technique of analyzing the research merit should involve an evaluation of the abilities to coordinate the scientific activities (Estrela & Cury, 2013). Besides, training human resources and producing appropriate research is essential in enhancing scientific merit. Additionally, the research ought to ensure that it is tied to the unshakeable innovative and creative capacities and the ability to shift paradigms. The extent to which resources are invested in the study needs to be considered as a major indicator of improving the scientific merit of the research. The persistent link for the article Experimental Methods in Political Science is 10.1146/annurev.polisci.5.091001.170657. The persistent link for the article Purposeful Sampling in
  • 41. Qualitative Research Synthesis is 10.3316/QRJ1102063. The persistent link for article Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research is https://videocast.nih.gov/pdf/ohrp_appendix_belmont_report_vo l_2.pdf. References Estrela, C., & Cury, A. (2013). Evaluation of scientific merit. Brazilian Oral Research, 27(4), 297-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1806-83242013000400001 Human Research Protections. (2016). Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research. U.S. Department of Health & Human Services. Leedy, P., & Ormrod, J. (2012). Practical research: Planning and Design-Chapter 4 Planning Your Research Project (10th Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Merrill Prentice Hall. McDermott, R. (2002). Experiments Methods in Political Science. Annual Review of Political Science, 5(1), 31-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.5.091001.170657 Suri, H. (2011). Purposeful Sampling in Qualitative Research Synthesis. Qualitative Research Journal, 11(2), 63-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.3316/qrj1102063 Unit 3 [D] INTRODUCTION QUANTITATIVE TOOLS
  • 42. Constructs While substantial phenomena exist in the physical world and can therefore be measured directly, insubstantial phenomena exist in a symbolic or abstract form and cannot be measured directly. Insubstantial phenomena are frequently called constructs. You can think of a construct as a scientific concept or an abstraction designed to explain a natural phenomenon (Kerlinger, 1986). Constructs allow us to talk about abstract ideas. The following are examples of constructs: • Intelligence. • Self-esteem. • Depression. • Job satisfaction • Anxiety. Constructs are used to understand, organize, and study the world we live in. Operational Definitions As abstractions, constructs are inherently difficult to measure. Because we cannot touch them, we have to develop alternative ways to gather information about them. When we try to measure constructs, or what Leedy and Ormrod (2013) call insubstantial measurements, we try to bridge the gap between what we can observe and the constructs, or abstraction, that a theory proposes. This process of obtaining systematic data on abstract ideas is called operationalizing the constructs, or creating operational definitions for the constructs. Operational definitions allow us to devise methods, procedures, and instruments that enable the quantification of constructs.
  • 43. For example, let us say we are interested in studying the relationship between intelligence and school achievement. Both intelligence and school achievement are constructs, or scientific ideas we have created to help us describe and explain natural phenomena (in this case, behaviors). Our theory suggests the existence of a relationship between these two constructs. In order to test our theory, we must obtain measurable data or information on intelligence and school achievement. The Tools of Research Tools The graphic depicts the relationship between intelligence and school achievement. The left portion of the graphic implies that intelligence is a theoretical construct that is measured by the WISC intelligence test and teacher assessments. The right portion of the graphic implies that school achievement is a theoretical construct that is measured by the Iowa Test of Basic Skills and student grade point averages, both of which are observable phenomena. How do we measure abstractions? We define observable phenomena from which we can obtain data. In the example, we might decide to measure intelligence using an intelligence test. In selecting an operational definition, we must recognize its limitations. We assume that
  • 44. intelligence tests measure certain aspects of intelligence, but clearly these tests do not tap the entirety of the construct of intelligence. In fact, critics of intelligence tests would argue that they measure very minor aspects of intelligence. We can improve our chances of tapping the intelligence construct by using more than one operational definition. Can you think of the reasons why? The Importance of Selecting Good Operational Definitions Selecting good operational definitions is critical to supporting the construct validity of our research. Researchers quite frequently use only one operational definition per construct. The validity of this common practice rests on the ability of the operational definition to capture the essence (or at least the most relevant aspects) of the construct. If that definition does not tap the construct we are interested in (for example, using head circumference to measure intelligence), our results will be meaningless. Fortunately, such obviously poor operational definitions are rare. Operational definitions that generate some controversy (for example, using the results of standardized achievement tests to measure school performance) are far more common. Variables In the foregoing discussion, we identified how we can measure constructs. Measuring constructs turns them into variables in our research. A variable is something that can vary or, in other words, take on at least two different values. This idea is critical to understanding and identifying variables. For example, gender cannot be a variable in a study when all the participants are women.
  • 45. We should note that not all variables are constructs. Leedy and Ormrod's (2013) distinction between substantial and insubstantial measurements highlights the point that some variables used in research are not constructs. Substantial measurements, such as the physical attributes of a person, such as hair color, ring size, head circumference, height, or thing, such as a car model, can be used as variables, but they are not abstract constructs. You will read more about variables in the chapters on quantitative and qualitative research. Hypothesis Testing and Data Analysis How are hypotheses tested? Traditional scientific hypothesis testing is associated with quantitative research designs. The essence of hypothesis testing is straightforward and involves some variation of the following steps: 1. State the research hypothesis and the null hypothesis. 2. Select a significance level (for example, 0.01, or 0.5). 3. Select a test statistic. Based on the level of significance and other information, such as degrees of freedom and directional or nondirectional testing, determine the critical value of the test statistic (for example, F, r, t) and the corresponding decision rule. 4. Collect your data. Applying the statistical test to the data, compute an observed value of your chosen test statistic. 5. Compare the observed value of the test statistic (number 4) with the preset critical value (number 3).
  • 46. 6. Make the appropriate decision, to either reject or fail to reject the null hypothesis. Specifically, if the obtained value is greater than the critical value, reject the null hypothesis. If the obtained value is less than the critical value of the test hypothesis, you would fail to reject the null hypothesis. In other words, researchers use statistics to compute an obtained value of their test statistic, which they compare with a previously established critical value. The critical value is based on the alpha they selected before they began their research. Some of the terms used in traditional hypothesis testing may be unfamiliar to you, so review the following definitions: • Alpha: The risk you are willing to accept in the event your statistical analysis indicates a statistically significant finding, but that finding is not real—it occurred due to chance or some other factor. In other words, alpha is the probability of making a Type I error, which is the same as the p level. • p level: The likelihood that your obtained results were due to chance if the null hypothesis is true. • Statistical test: The method you use to statistically analyze your data, such as a t-test or Pearson's correlation. • Test statistic: The ruler you use to test your hypothesis. It is the obtained value from the result of your statistical test, such as the obtained t value or r. • Critical value: The value of your test statistic at which you can reject the null hypothesis.
  • 47. Types of Errors in Hypothesis Testing Hypothesis testing is based on probability. Regardless of the statistical test or test statistic used, the researcher is testing the null hypothesis against the laws of probability. Using probability, you will always have four possible outcomes: • Error 1: You were wrong. You rejected the null hypothesis when the null was the correct alternative. You concluded there was a relationship or difference when the observed differences actually occurred purely by chance. A Type I Error occurs when you incorrectly reject the null hypothesis. Alpha (α) or your p value is the probability of this happening to you. • Error 2: You were wrong. You retained, or did not reject, the null when it was actually false. You were not able to find the relationship or difference that really exists. A Type II Error occurs when you incorrectly accept the null hypothesis. The probability of this happening to you is known as beta (β). • Correct Decision 1: You were right. You rejected the null when it was indeed false. You successfully found the difference or relationship that really exists. Power is the probability of this happening. • Correct Decision 2: You were right. You retained the null when it was true. No difference or relationship exists, and you found none. Thus, we try to guard against two types of error when testing hypotheses. A Type I Error is defined as a false positive or rejecting the null hypothesis when it should have been accepted. A Type II Error is defined as a false
  • 48. negative or failing to reject the null hypothesis when it should have been rejected (Williams, 1992). Statistical power refers to the probability of finding relationships or differences that indeed exist. Much research in the social sciences is underpowered, meaning that the designs are not sensitive enough to detect relationships that may indeed exist (Cohen, 1988). See Lipsey's 1997 chapter, "Design Sensitivity: Statistical Power for Applied Experimental Research," in Handbook of Social Science Research for recommendations on how to increase the statistical power of research designs. The following is a paraphrased excerpt from Percy and Kostere's (2008) Qualitative Research Approaches in Psychology . QUALITATIVE TOOLS Level of Analysis Level of analysis specifies the type of phenomena being investigated, according to the following hierarchy: intra-psychic phenomena; individual whole-person phenomena; inter-personal phenomena, family phenomena (including couples or dyads in committed relationships); small- group phenomena (work group, team); organizational phenomena (large corporation, large church, government agency); social-cultural phenomena (society as a whole, a culture or subculture). Key Phenomena Key phenomena in a qualitative study correspond to the variables in a quantitative study. They might be specific
  • 49. phenomena, cases, factors, or simply focal elements that are the focus of the study. They are the primary constructs under investigation and they should be defined in a similar way to the way that we define quantitative constructs. These definitions should be consistent with similar constructs used in previous research, whether qualitative or quantitative, and consistent with the meaning of the terms in the research question. Types of Data Qualitative data is not collected from tests and measures like quantitative data, but consists of verbal and behavioral output as recorded in interviews, documents, videotapes or photographs, journals, notes of observations, and so on. The most common type of qualitative data is verbal data from interviews. Role of the Researcher In qualitative research, the researcher is a tool of the research. The researcher has to use his or her own knowledge, training, and experience to collect the data in an efficient and sufficient manner, to bring the data together, and to make it into something meaningful and useful. Data Preparation Qualitative data must be physically prepared. For example, if in-depth interviews will generate audiotapes, those must be transcribed. If videotapes are used, they too must be transcribed, or perhaps behaviors must be coded. Transcripts must be organized physically (electronic copies, paper copies). In some approaches such as grounded theory, this phase (preparing and organizing the data) is already part of the data analysis procedures.
  • 50. In other approaches, such as case study or survey, the various kinds of data must first be organized and prepared so that it is usable. Data Analysis Depending on the approach used, qualitative data will be analyzed in different ways, Much like there are different statistical tests used for different quantitative approaches, the different qualitative approaches demand different data analysis techniques. Some examples include data coding, data reduction, and thematic analysis. References Cohen, J. L. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cozby, P. C. (1993). Methods in behavioral research (5th ed.). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield. Kerlinger, F. N. (1986). Foundations of behavioral research (3rd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Leedy, P. D., & Ormrod, J. E. (2013). Practical research: Planning and design (10th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Lipsey, J. W. (1997). Design sensitivity: Statistical power for applied experimental research. In L. Bickman & D. Rog, Handbook of social science research (pp. 39–68). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • 51. Percy, W. H., & Kostere, K. (2008). Qualitative research approaches in psychology . Minneapolis, MN: Williams, F. (1992). Reasoning with statistics: How to read quantitative research (4th ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. OBJECTIVES To successfully complete this learning unit, you will be expected to: 1. Identify variables in a research study. 2. Delineate quantitative instruments used to measure variables. 3. Explain the importance of operational definitions to scientific merit. 4. Evaluate the data collection method or methods. [u03s1] Unit 3 Study 1 STUDIES Readings Read the introduction to Unit 3, "The Tools of Research." This will provide basic explanations and examples of the key components of quantitative and qualitative research. Use your Leedy and Ormrod text to complete the following: • Read Chapter 1, "The Nature and Tools of Research," beginning with page 7 at the heading "Tools
  • 52. for Research," through page 25. This reading covers some of the tangible tools researchers use, such as libraries and computers, as well as "cognitive tools," such as critical thinking and logic. • Read Chapter 8, "Analyzing Quantitative Data," pages 211– 250. This chapter reviews the types of quantitative data, descriptive statistics, and inferential statistics. Use Trochim's Research Methods Knowledge Base Web site to read the following pages. This information contains more information on qualitative phenomena, data collection, and analysis: • Qualitative Measures. • The Qualitative Debate. • Qualitative Data. • Qualitative Methods. Also on the Research Methods Knowledge Base Web site, read the following pages for further information on quantitative variables, levels of measurement, hypotheses, and hypothesis testing. Additionally, you will learn about the relationship between qualitative and quantitative data, and other cognitive tools. • Variables. • Levels of Measurement. • Hypotheses. • Types of Data. • Deduction and Induction. • Inferential Statistics. There are several links to specific kinds of statistical tests on this page that you
  • 53. might find useful for understanding the data analyses reported in various quantitative research articles. PSY Learners Additional Required Reading In addition to the other required study activities for this unit, PSY learners are also required to compete the following: • Read Percy, Kostere, and Kostere's 2015 document, Qualitative Research Approaches in Psychology. This document provides an overview of qualitative methods and the major qualitative approaches. Data collection and data analyses are covered under each approach. You may find it helpful to refer to this document throughout this course. Optional Program-Specific Content Some programs have opted to provide program-specific content designed to help you better understand how the subject matter in this study is incorporated into your particular field of study. Check below to see if your program has any suggested readings for you. SOBT Learners • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2), 1175–191. This reading provides the foundation for using G*Power to determine sample size for quantitative studies.
  • 54. • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1150. This reading provides the foundation for using G*Power for determining sample size for correlation and regression analysis studies. • Eisenhardt, K. M. (1989). "Building theories from case study research." Academy of Management Review 14(4), 532–550. This article provides an overall process and structure for case study research. Probably the most cited case study methods paper in business and management papers. [u03s2] Unit 3 Study 2 PROJECT PREPARATION Resources Research Topic and Methodology Form. Research Topic and Methodology Scoring Guide. In preparation for the Unit 4 assignment due next week, make sure during this unit that you have thoroughly read and understand the approved research study you selected for the Unit 2 assignment. Be prepared to complete the Unit 4 assignment by identifying and understanding the research topic, research problem, research question, and basic methodology. You may also find it beneficial to view the Research Topic and
  • 55. Methodology Form that you will use to complete the Unit 4 assignment. Also, view the assignment description and scoring guide to learn how you will be evaluated. QUANTITATIVE TOOLS Resources Discussion Participation Scoring Guide. APA Style and Format. Persistent Links and DOIs. Make sure the quantitative article that you selected in Unit 1 will allow you to thoroughly address all of the points required for this discussion. Using the information from this week's readings, complete the following: • Identify the instrument or instruments used to quantify the data, the level of measurement for each instrument, and the statistics used to analyze the data. • Identify and describe the constructs, variables, and operational definitions included in the research. Do not just list terms. Include a description of how the researcher defined these. • Describe the cognitive tool used to interpret the data. Possibilities include deductive logic, inductive reasoning, scientific method, or critical thinking. • Discuss the usefulness of the operational definitions for the
  • 56. constructs in this study. How could they have been defined differently? Were the operational definitions sufficient to allow the researcher to answer the research question? Make sure to justify your answer. • Explain the importance of operational definitions to scientific merit. • List the persistent link for the article in your response. Refer to the Persistent Links and DOIs guide, linked in Resources, to learn how to locate this information in the library databases. • Cite all sources in APA style and provide an APA-formatted reference list at the end of your post. Response Guidelines After reviewing the discussion postings, choose one peer to respond to. For your response: • Follow the persistent link to the article being discussed. • Using the language of research, explain how you agree or disagree with your peer's evaluation, offering your own suggestions for improving the research design. [u03d2 QUALITATIVE TOOLS Resources Discussion Participation Scoring Guide.
  • 57. APA Style and Format. Persistent Links and DOIs. Make sure the qualitative article that you selected in Unit 1 will allow you to thoroughly address all of the points required for this discussion. Using the information from this week's readings, complete the following: • Identify and describe key concept in the research. • Describe and evaluate the data collection method or methods. Was the data collection method appropriate to allow the researcher to answer the research question? Why or why not? • Describe and evaluate the data analysis procedure or procedures. Was the data analysis procedure appropriate to allow the researcher to answer the research question? • Explain the importance of appropriate data collection and data analyses procedures to scientific merit. • List the persistent link for the article in your response. Refer to the Persistent Links and DOIs guide, linked in Resources, to learn how to locate this information in the library database. • Cite all sources in APA style and provide an APA-formatted reference list at the end of your post. Response Guidelines After reviewing the discussion postings, choose one peer to
  • 58. respond to. For your response: • Follow the persistent link to the article being discussed. • Using the language of research, explain how you agree or disagree with your peer's evaluation, offering your own suggestions for improving the research design.