SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 24
Society may not realize how everyone around the world engages
in the same/similar services or activities; people identify them
with various titles. In Japan, they offer and advertise family
rental services (actors) to mainly fill voids in their lives. Here
in the U.S., we hire people to assist with our daily living both
outside and inside of our homes with our children as well as
elders, for example, daycares/nannies, in-home caregivers, and
counselors.
Emotion Review
Vol. 8, No. 1 (January 2016) 8 –13
© The Author(s) 2015
ISSN 1754-0739
DOI: 10.1177/1754073915601228
er.sagepub.com
Memories of events that evoked powerful emotions are vivid
and lasting but not necessarily accurate (Levine & Edelstein,
2009; Reisberg & Heuer, 2007). Under some conditions, emo-
tion even increases people’s susceptibility to false memories.
This is of great concern in legal settings where people often
need to recall information that elicited intense emotion.
Consider
a victim describing the perpetrator of a crime, a witness testify-
ing about a terrifying experience, or a juror recalling gruesome
photographs of a crime scene. Distortions in memory for emo-
tional information in these contexts can result in false accusa-
tions, lawsuits, and wrongful criminal convictions (Loftus,
Doyle, & Dysart, 2013). A prominent example is the repressed
memory controversy that peaked in the 1990s. Hundreds of
adults claimed to have recovered memories, often during ther-
apy sessions, of having been victims of childhood sexual abuse.
Close investigation later revealed that many therapists used
highly suggestive techniques to help patients “remember”
instances of abuse, leading patients to form emotional but false
memories (Loftus & Ketcham, 1996). These events revealed
that even highly emotional memories can be inaccurate and that
memory errors can have a devastating human toll.
The issue of when and why emotion leaves people vulnera-
ble to misremembering events has been vigorously debated,
with investigators variously claiming that people are most sus-
ceptible to memory errors when experiencing intense emotion,
positive emotion, or negative emotion. Here we review research
on the malleability of memory and on the relation between emo-
tion and false memory, focusing on studies relevant to the inter-
disciplinary field of emotion and law. We identify discrepancies
in the literature, and argue that taking into account the effects
of
discrete emotions on attention and motivation helps to resolve
seemingly contradictory findings. We also discuss implications
Emotion and False Memory
Robin L. Kaplan
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, USA
Ilse Van Damme
Department of Psychology, KU Leuven (University of Leuven),
Belgium
Linda J. Levine
Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of
California, Irvine, USA
Elizabeth F. Loftus
Department of Psychology and Social Behavior and Department
of Criminology, Law, and Society, University of California,
Irvine, USA
Abstract
Emotional memories are vivid and lasting but not necessarily
accurate. Under some conditions, emotion even increases
people’s
susceptibility to false memories. This review addresses when
and why emotion leaves people vulnerable to misremembering
events.
Recent research suggests that pregoal emotions—those
experienced before goal attainment or failure (e.g., hope,
fear)—narrow
the scope of people’s attention to information that is central to
their goals. This narrow focus can impair memory for peripheral
details, leaving people vulnerable to misinformation concerning
those details. In contrast, postgoal emotions—those experienced
after goal attainment or failure (e.g., happiness, sadness)—
broaden the scope of attention leaving people more resistant to
misinformation. Implications for legal contexts, such as
emotion-related errors in eyewitness testimony, are discussed.
Keywords
emotion, eyewitness testimony, false memory, misinformation
effect
Author note: The views expressed here are those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent the policies of the U.S. Bureau
of Labor Statistics.
Corresponding author: Linda J. Levine, Department of
Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California,
Irvine, 4209 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway, Irvine,
CA 92697-7085, USA. Email: [email protected]
601228 EMR0010.1177/1754073915601228Emotion
ReviewKaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory
research-article2016
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F17540739
15601228&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2015-10-23
Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 9
of emotion-related memory errors in legal contexts and outline
directions for further research.
The Malleability of Memory
When evaluating eyewitness testimony from victims or wit-
nesses of a crime, people commonly assume that memory is a
simple recording of past events that can be played back at will.
This assumption has been shown to be false. Researchers have
analyzed exonerations based on DNA evidence and found that
inaccurate memory and mistaken eyewitness identification are
the most common causes of false convictions in the United
States (Garrett, 2011). Errors occur because memories can be
altered by people’s subsequent knowledge or beliefs. In a well-
known study, Loftus, Miller, and Burns (1978) presented par-
ticipants with a slideshow depicting a car at a yield sign. Later,
some participants were informed that the car was at a stop sign
rather than at a yield sign. Those exposed to this misleading
information were significantly more likely to endorse having
seen the nonexistent stop sign. This study inspired hundreds of
investigations of what is now called “the misinformation effect”
which revealed the ease with which memories can be distorted
by the introduction of misleading information after an event has
been experienced (for a review, see Loftus, 2005).
Exposing people to misleading information about events can
also lead them to “remember” patently false events with great
confidence, ranging from childhood sexual abuse to alien
abduc-
tions (Loftus & Bernstein, 2005; McNally et al., 2004).
Researchers have instilled memories for entire events that never
happened using techniques such as increasing their plausibility
(e.g., Hart & Schooler, 2006), repeated questioning (e.g., Loftus
& Pickrell, 1995), repeated imagination of events (e.g.,
Sharman
& Scoboria, 2009), and guided imagery (e.g., Hyman &
Pentland,
1996). The resulting “rich false memories” are often confidently
held, highly detailed, and emotional (Loftus & Bernstein, 2005).
Far from providing an exact recording of events, then, peo-
ple’s memories are malleable. Memories may include errors
concerning what happened, when an event happened, where it
happened, and who was involved, or memories may be entirely
fabricated. Importantly, laboratory studies on false memory
mimic real-world practices in the criminal justice system. In
police interrogations, witnesses are often subject to repeated
questioning and may be asked to imagine events or confirm
false information. Misleading information provided to wit-
nesses by authority figures often appears plausible (Loftus &
Ketcham, 1996). Because people can be exposed to, and asked
to elaborate on, inaccurate information in a number of phases of
criminal investigations, memory distortions are common
(Loftus et al., 2013).
Emotional Arousal and Memory
Events that cause people to become involved in the legal system
are often extremely emotional, raising the issue of how emotion
affects people’s susceptibility to false memories. Researchers
examining this issue have focused primarily on emotional
arousal, the dimension of emotion that ranges from calm relaxa-
tion to intense excitement or tension. Their findings show that
several cognitive and biological processes contribute to vivid
and lasting memories of emotionally arousing events. People
pay more attention to emotional than neutral events and, as a
result, emotional events are more likely to be encoded in mem-
ory. Once encoded, stress hormones in the brain consolidate or
stabilize emotional memories making them more enduring
(McGaugh, 2004). People also think about and recount emo-
tional events more often than neutral events. Each of these pro-
cesses enhances memory for emotional information (Laney,
Campbell, Heuer, & Reisberg, 2004; Levine & Edelstein, 2009)
and can reduce people’s susceptibility to false memories
(English & Nielson, 2010).
Emotional arousal can also impair memory and increase sus-
ceptibility to misinformation, however (e.g., Drivdahl,
Zaragoza,
& Learned, 2009). Whether emotion enhances or impairs mem-
ory depends greatly on how important the information being
remembered is to the individual. With increasing emotional
arousal, attention narrows to features of events that are of
central
importance. This results in enhanced memory for central infor-
mation at the expense of peripheral details (Christianson &
Loftus, 1991), a phenomenon referred to as emotional memory
narrowing (Kensinger, 2009) or tunnel memory (Safer,
Christianson, Autry, & Osterland, 1998). Researchers have doc-
umented emotional memory narrowing both in laboratory stud-
ies (e.g., Kensinger, 2009) and across a wide range of real-
world
events including natural disasters (Bahrick, Parker, Fivush, &
Levitt, 1998), physical injuries (Peterson & Bell, 1996), and
crime scenes (Reisberg & Heuer, 2007). Of particular relevance
to criminal investigations, laboratory studies provide evidence
of
“weapon focus,” in which witnesses focus on and remember the
features of a crime scene that threaten safety, such as the
weapon
used to commit the crime, at the expense of details such as the
perpetrator’s face or clothing that are of importance to police
(Loftus, Loftus, & Messo, 1987; Steblay, 1992). Rather than
enhancing memory generally, then, emotion often leads to a
trade-off in attention to, and memory for, central versus periph-
eral information.
Under conditions of severe emotional arousal or stress, peo-
ple may focus almost exclusively on survival, endurance, and
efforts to regulate emotion. This extremely narrow attentional
focus can result in poor memory even for the events that elic-
ited emotion (e.g., Deffenbacher, Bornstein, Penrod, &
McGorty, 2004) and leave people highly susceptible to misin-
formation. In a striking example, researchers assessed memory
for an interrogation in over 800 military personnel who were
going through survival training as part of the U.S. Navy
Survival School (Morgan, Southwick, Steffian, Hazlett, &
Loftus, 2013). Trainees were placed in a mock prisoner of war
camp and endured a stressful interrogation that involved threats
and physical assault. Researchers later interviewed participants
about their experiences and provided inaccurate information
such as a photograph that falsely depicted their interrogator.
Participants’ memory was poor both for details of the interro-
gation and for the interrogator. In some conditions, more than
10 Emotion Review Vol. 8 No. 1
half of the trainees falsely identified their interrogator (also see
Houston, Clifford, Phillips, & Memon, 2013). Thus, memories
of events that are highly stressful and evoke extreme levels of
emotional arousal are vulnerable to substantial error following
exposure to misinformation.
Laney and Loftus (2008) found that it is even possible to cre-
ate entirely false memories for emotional events such as being
hospitalized overnight or witnessing a violent fight between
one’s parents. When they compared memories for emotional
events described by participants who actually experienced them
versus by participants who were induced to believe they had
experienced them, true and false memories were indistinguisha-
ble on most dimensions. Similarly, people can generate detailed
but false memories of committing a crime. Using suggestive
interviewing techniques, Shaw and Porter (2015) induced under-
graduates to generate emotional false memories of having
engaged in theft, assault, or assault with a weapon that led to
police contact in their early adolescence. After three suggestive
interviews, including supposed corroboration by caregivers and
guided imagery, the majority of participants indicated that they
remembered having committed the crime and provided a
detailed
false account. False memories of committing a crime were simi-
lar to memories of emotional events that had actually occurred
with respect to the presence of sensory details (e.g., visual,
audi-
tory, and tactile details) and reported anxiety during the event.
This study showed that detailed false memories of committing
crimes can be generated in a controlled experimental setting and
reveals the types of conditions that may lead to false
confessions.
In summary, whether emotional arousal promotes or impairs
memory accuracy depends on the features of events people are
asked to remember and the intensity of arousal they experience.
In addition to being influenced by people’s knowledge and
beliefs, memories are influenced by the focus of people’s atten-
tion—a process powerfully swayed by emotion. Emotional
arousal enhances memory for information that captures atten-
tion, features of events that are of the greatest importance to the
individual. However, this memory advantage often comes at the
expense of memory for peripheral details and can leave people
vulnerable to false memories concerning such details (for a
review, see Levine & Edelstein, 2009). Under conditions of
extreme stress, this narrow attentional focus can lead to overall
memory deficits and pronounced susceptibility to misinforma-
tion (Morgan et al., 2013). People are also able to piece
together
fragments of autobiographical information with information
gathered from external sources to generate emotionally rich and
coherent memories of events that never occurred (Shaw &
Porter, 2015). The sobering implication of these findings for the
legal system is that, just because a remembered event is
reported
with high confidence and strong emotion, does not mean it rep-
resents authentic experience.
Emotional Valence and Memory
The negative or positive tone of an individual’s emotional
response to an event may also affect his or her susceptibility
to false memories. According to Schwarz and Clore (2003),
emotional valence signals the type of information-processing
strategy likely to be most adaptive in the current circum-
stances. People experience negative emotion when problems
arise. Negative feelings signal the need to focus on problems
and carefully monitor the environment for information rele-
vant to addressing them. People experience positive emotion
when no problem requires their immediate attention. Positive
feelings signal that it is adaptive to attend to a broad range of
stimuli to take advantage of new opportunities.
The consequences of these differing information-processing
strategies for memory are not clear because the research
literature
includes contradictory theories and findings. Some investigators
have found that people experiencing positive emotion are more
vulnerable to incorporating misleading information into memory
than people experiencing negative emotion. This may occur
because positive emotion promotes a broader and less detail-ori-
ented information-processing style than negative emotion,
allow-
ing people to confuse events they witnessed with similar events
that they did not witness (e.g., Forgas, Laham, & Vargas, 2005;
Kensinger & Schacter, 2006; Levine & Bluck, 2004).
Other investigators have argued that, by narrowing the scope
of attention to central information, negative emotional arousal
(rather than emotional arousal generally or positive emotion)
leaves people susceptible to memory errors concerning periph-
eral details (Berntsen, 2002; Waring & Kensinger, 2009; also
see Porter, Taylor, & ten Brinke, 2008). For instance, people
remembering negative photographs had fewer false memories
about central details of the images, but more false memories
about peripheral details, than people remembering positive or
neutral photographs (Van Damme & Smets, 2014). These find-
ings raise concerns about the accuracy of people’s memory for
negative, arousing information in legal settings. Witnesses
recalling a crime scene, and jurors presented with upsetting tes-
timony during a trial, are likely to remember central,
threatening
details. However, they may be susceptible to misinformation
concerning peripheral details, such as the setting in which the
crime occurred, that are important for establishing a defendant’s
innocence or guilt.
Like arousal, then, emotional valence influences memory
but is a fickle guide to whether particular memories will be
resistant or susceptible to distortion. Research findings conflict
concerning whether people are more susceptible to false memo-
ries when experiencing positive emotion or negative emotion.
Moreover, research on valence neglects potential effects of dis-
crete emotions on memory. Events that come to the attention of
the legal community are typically emotionally arousing and
negative but individuals may react to these events with fear,
anger, or despair. These considerations have prompted investi-
gators to take a more nuanced approach to examining the effects
of emotion on memory that takes into account a third dimension
of emotion, its motivational intensity.
Motivational Intensity and Memory
To understand the effects of emotion on memory it is essential
to consider the goals and motivations associated with specific
Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 11
emotions and the relevance of the information being remem-
bered to these goals. Motivational or goal relevance approaches
distinguish between pregoal and postgoal emotions and pro-
vide insight into the specific types of information likely to be
remembered in different emotional states (e.g., Harmon-Jones,
Price, & Gable, 2012; Kaplan, Van Damme, & Levine, 2012;
Levine & Edelstein, 2009; Levine & Pizarro, 2004; Montagrin,
Brosch, & Sander, 2013). According to appraisal theories (e.g.,
Ellsworth & Doughterty, 2016; Scherer, 1999), emotions are
inherently tied to changes in the status of people’s goals. People
experience emotions when they perceive that goal attainment
or failure is imminent or has occurred, making it necessary for
them to modify their beliefs, plans, or goals. Once evoked,
emotions direct cognition and motivate action in a manner that
is useful for ensuring, preventing, or adjusting to those changes
in the status of goals.
Pregoal emotions, such as desire, hope, anger, fear, and dis-
gust, reflect appraisals that goal attainment or failure is antici-
pated or threatened. These emotions, irrespective of their
positive or negative valence, are characterized by high motiva-
tional intensity and an impetus to act. When experiencing pre-
goal emotion, people’s attention narrows to information that is
central to their goals. For example, anger prepares people to
remove obstacles to their goals, and fear prepares people to
avoid or escape threats to their goals (Cunningham & Brosch,
2012). Because the angry individual focuses on the agent
obstructing a goal, and the fearful individual focuses on the
source of threat, the range of information attended to, encoded
in, or retrieved from memory is narrow. This narrow focus
leads to poorer memory for details that are not relevant to the
individual’s current goal, resulting in greater vulnerability to
misinformation concerning those details. In contrast, postgoal
emotions, such as happiness and sadness, are lower in motiva-
tional intensity because they reflect appraisals that goal attain-
ment or failure has already occurred. When experiencing
postgoal emotions, attention broadens as people consider the
consequences of goal attainment or failure, change their beliefs
and expectations, and orient toward new goals. Thus, postgoal
emotions increase the breadth of information processing,
allowing people to encode and retrieve peripheral details
(Kaplan et al., 2012).
Examining the motivations associated with specific emo-
tions, and their implications for cognition, represents a promis-
ing theoretical approach that helps to resolve seemingly
contradictory findings in the literature. For example, research
showing cognitive narrowing for negative emotion, and broad-
ening for positive emotion, has often contrasted pregoal nega-
tive emotion (e.g., fear, disgust) with postgoal positive emotion
(e.g., happiness, amusement). Conflicting findings may reflect
the fact that emotions of the same valence can vary in motiva-
tional intensity. For example, Gable and Harmon-Jones (2010)
showed participants photographs that evoked disgust (pregoal),
sadness (postgoal), or a neutral state. They then assessed the
breadth of participants’ attention with a separate task. Viewing
disgusting images narrowed attention, and viewing sad pictures
broadened attention, relative to viewing neutral pictures.
Although research is sparse on how the motivations associ-
ated with specific emotions affect memory, we believe this
approach is important for understanding memory distortion in
legal settings. During a trial, jurors may be exposed to testi-
mony or gruesome photographs that evoke fear, anger, or dis-
gust. They may also be exposed to testimony or photographs
that convey the devastating impact a crime had on victims,
evoking sadness (Bandes & Salerno, 2014). Further research is
needed to understand how pregoal emotions such as anger and
fear, and postgoal emotions such as sadness, affect the specific
types of information that witnesses and jurors attend to, encode,
and retrieve and their susceptibility to false memories.
In a recent study, we assessed people’s susceptibility to
memory distortion when they were instructed to elaborate on
information associated with pregoal and postgoal emotions
(Van Damme, Kaplan, Levine, & Loftus, 2015). While watch-
ing a slideshow depicting an interaction between a woman and
her boyfriend, participants were asked to empathize with the
woman’s emotional state. For different groups of participants,
the woman was described as feeling hopeful (pregoal positive),
fearful (pregoal negative), happy (postgoal positive), or devas-
tated (postgoal negative). After watching the slideshow, partici-
pants were asked to reflect on the interaction they had
witnessed,
including events that had actually occurred in the slideshow
(true events) and events that did not occur (false events). Later,
participants’ memory was tested for true and false events that
were relevant or irrelevant to the woman’s goals. Participants in
the pregoal conditions (hope, fear) remembered more false
events that were irrelevant to the woman’s goals than partici-
pants in the postgoal conditions (happiness, devastation).
Participants in the pregoal conditions were also more confident
in correctly rejecting goal-relevant false events than
participants
in the postgoal conditions. These findings are consistent with
the view that, irrespective of emotional valence, pregoal emo-
tion enhances memory for information relevant to goal pursuit
at the expense of irrelevant information (Harmon-Jones, Price,
& Gable, 2012; Kaplan et al., 2012; Montagrin et al., 2013).
Conclusions
Scholars across a multitude of disciplines have called for a
more
complete understanding of the role emotion plays in legal pro-
cesses (Bornstein & Wiener, 2010). Research shows that memo-
ries are shaped, and can be misshaped, by people’s knowledge
and beliefs, attention, and motivation—all processes that are
influenced by emotion. People’s most vivid and lasting memo-
ries are typically emotional ones. These memories are selective,
however. When experiencing intense emotion, people attend to
the features of events that are most relevant to their current
goals, promoting detailed and accurate memory for that central
information. This advantage comes at the expense of memory
for information that is not relevant to their goals and can leave
people vulnerable to misinformation concerning such details.
Under conditions of extreme stress, attentional narrowing can
lead to overall memory deficits and pronounced susceptibility
to misinformation.
12 Emotion Review Vol. 8 No. 1
These findings have important implications for practitioners
in the legal system. Eyewitnesses are often asked to remember
details of events that were extremely stressful long after the
events occurred. They may be exposed to misleading informa-
tion about the events from other witnesses’ reports, leading
questions by investigators, and pretrial news reports (Frenda,
Nichols, & Loftus, 2011). Because memory for emotional
events is both selective and malleable, law enforcement profes-
sionals should take great care that witnesses are not exposed to
information that might alter their memories. For jurors and
judges listening to emotional testimony, these findings mean
that testimony should not be given more credibility simply
because it is conveyed with emotion. Witnesses may convey
information with substantial emotion but be inaccurate. They
may even be genuinely emotional about memories that are
entirely false (Laney & Loftus, 2008; McNally et al., 2004;
Shaw & Porter, 2015). Information about memory processes
should be a standard component of the instructions given to
jurors and of the education received by law enforcement person-
nel, attorneys, and judges.
Investigators have also examined the specific dimensions of
emotional experience that render people most susceptible to
false memories. The findings are complex and investigators
have variously claimed that people are rendered most suscepti-
ble to misinformation by intense emotional arousal, positive
emotion, or negative emotion. We have argued that a more
nuanced approach is needed that takes into account the motiva-
tions associated with discrete emotions. Emotions high in moti-
vational intensity, such as fear, disgust, and anger, powerfully
direct attention to features of events that are of central impor-
tance to the individual for avoiding threats and removing obsta-
cles to their goals. When people’s attentional resources are
limited, misinformation effects are easier to obtain (Loftus,
2005). Thus, this narrow focus leaves people susceptible to
false
memories concerning features of events that are peripheral to
their goals. Investigating the effects of discrete emotions on
people’s susceptibility to false memories represents an impor-
tant research direction for the field of memory and the law.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
References
Bahrick, L. E., Parker, J. F., Fivush, R., & Levitt, M. (1998).
The effects
of stress on young children’s memory for a natural disaster.
Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 308–331.
doi:10.1037/1076–
898X.4.4.308
Bandes, S. A., & Salerno, J. M. (2014). Emotion, proof and
prejudice: The
cognitive science of gruesome photos and victim impact
statements.
Arizona State Law Journal, 46, 1003–1056.
Berntsen, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical
events: Cen-
tral details are remembered more frequently from shocking than
from
happy experiences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1010–1020.
doi:10.3758/
BF03194319
Bornstein, B. H., & Wiener, R. L. (2010). Emotion and the law:
A field
whose time has come. In B. H. Bornstein & R. L. Wiener (Eds.),
Emotion and the law: Psychological perspectives (pp. 1–12).
New
York, NY: Springer.
Christianson, S.-Å., & Loftus, E. F. (1991). Remembering
emotional
events: The fate of detailed information. Cognition & Emotion,
5, 81–
108. doi:10.1080/02699939108411027
Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational
salience: Amygdala
tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions
in Psy-
chological Science, 21, 54–59. doi:10.1177/0963721411430832
Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., &
McGorty, E. K.
(2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on
eyewit-
ness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 687–706.
doi:10.1007/
s10979–004–0565-x
Drivdahl, S. B., Zaragoza, M. S., & Learned, D. M. (2009). The
role of emo-
tional elaboration in the creation of false memories. Applied
Cognitive
Psychology, 23, 13–35. doi:10.1002/acp.1446
Ellsworth, P. C., & Doughterty, A. (2016). Appraisal and
reappraisals in the
courtroom. Emotion Review, 8(1): pp. 20–25.
English, S. M., & Nielson, K. A. (2010). Reduction of the
misinformation
effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117, 237–
242.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.014
Forgas, J. P., Laham, S. M., & Vargas, P. T. (2005). Mood
effects on
eyewitness memory: Affective influences on susceptibility to
misin-
formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 574–
588.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.11.005
Frenda, S. J., Nichols, R. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2011). Current
issues and
advances in misinformation research. Current Directions in
Psycho-
logical Science, 20, 20–23. doi:10.1177/0963721410396620
Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but
the
nasty narrows. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215.
doi:10.1177/
0956797609359622
Garrett, B. L. (2011). Convicting the innocent: Where criminal
prosecutions
go wrong. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Harmon-Jones, E., Price, T. F., & Gable, P. A. (2012). The
influence of
affective states on cognitive broadening/narrowing: Considering
the
importance of motivational intensity. Social and Personality
Psychol-
ogy Compass, 6, 314–327. doi:10.1111/j.1751–
9004.2012.00432.x
Hart, R. E., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). Increasing belief in the
experience
of an invasive procedure that never happened: The role of
plausibil-
ity and schematicity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 661–
669.
doi:10.1002/acp.1218
Houston, K. A., Clifford, B. R., Phillips, L. H., & Memon, A.
(2013). The
emotional eyewitness: The effects of emotion on specific
aspects of
eyewitness recall and recognition performance. Emotion, 13,
118–128.
doi.org/10.1037/a0029220
Hyman, I. E., Jr., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental
imagery in the
creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and
Lan-
guage, 35, 101–117.
Kaplan, R. L., Van Damme, I., & Levine, L. J. (2012).
Motivation mat-
ters: Differing effects of pre-goal and post-goal emotions on
atten-
tion and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(404). doi:10.3389/
fpsyg.2012.00404
Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of
emotion.
Emotion Review, 1, 99–113. doi:10.1177/1754073908100432
Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). When the Red Sox
shocked
the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories.
Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 13, 757–763. doi:10.3758/BF03193993
Laney, C., Campbell, H. V., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2004).
Memory
for thematically arousing events. Memory & Cognition, 32,
1149–1159.
doi:10.3758/BF03196888
Laney, C., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Emotional content of true
and false mem-
ories. Memory, 16, 500–516.
Levine, L. J., & Bluck, S. (2004). Painting with broad strokes:
Happiness
and the malleability of event memory. Cognition and Emotion,
18,
559–574. doi:10.1080/02699930341000446
Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 13
Levine, L. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory
narrowing:
A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition & Emotion,
23, 833–
875. doi:10.1080/02699930902738863
Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory
research:
A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22, 530–554.
doi:10.1521/
soco.22.5.530.50767
Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human
mind: A 30-year
investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning and
Memory, 12,
361–366.
Loftus, E. F., & Bernstein, D. M. (2005). Rich false memories:
The royal
road to success. In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Experimental cognitive
psychol-
ogy and its applications (pp. 101–113). Washington, DC:
American
Psychological Association Press.
Loftus, E. F., Doyle, J. M., & Dysart, J. (2013). Eyewitness
testimony: Civil
& criminal (5th ed.). Charlottesville, VA: Lexis Law.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1996). The myth of repressed
memory. New
York, NY: MacMillan.
Loftus, E. F., Loftus, G. R., & Messo, J. (1987). Some facts
about
weapon focus. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 55–62.
doi:10.1007/
BF01044839
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic
integration of
verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of
Experimental Psy-
chology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31.
doi:10.1037/0278–
7393.4.1.19
Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false
memories. Psy-
chiatric Annals, 25, 720–725. doi:10.3928/0048–5713–
19951201–07
McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the
consolidation of mem-
ories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of
Neurosci-
ence, 27, 1–28. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157
McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., Maclin, M. L.,
Pitman, R. K., &
Orr, S. P. (2004). Psychophysiological responding during script-
driven
imagery in people reporting abduction by space aliens.
Psychological
Science, 15, 493–497. doi:10.1111/j.0956–7976.2004.00707.x
Montagrin, A., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2013). Goal
conduciveness as
a key determinant of memory facilitation. Emotion, 13, 622–
628.
doi:10.1037/a003306
Morgan, C. A., III, Southwick, S., Steffian, G., Hazlett, G. A.,
& Loftus, E. F.
(2013). Misinformation can influence memory for recently
experienced,
highly stressful events. International Journal of Law and
Psychiatry, 36,
11–17. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.11.002
Peterson, C., & Bell, M. (1996). Children’s memory for
traumatic injury.
Child Development, 67, 3045–3070. doi:10.1111/j.1467–
8624.1996.
tb01902.x
Porter, S., Taylor, K., & ten Brinke, L. (2008). Memory for
media: Investi-
gation of false memories for negatively and positively charged
public
events. Memory, 16, 658–666. doi:10.1080/09658210802154626
Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2007). The influence of emotion on
memory in
forensic settings. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross & R.
C. L. Lind-
say (Eds.), The handbook of eyewitness psychology, Vol. 1:
Memory for
events (pp. 81–116). London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Safer, M. A., Christianson, S., Autry, M. W., & Osterland, K.
(1998).
Tunnel memory for traumatic events. Applied Cognitive
Psychology,
12, 99–117. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099–
0720(199804)12:2<99::AID-
ACP509>3.0.CO;2–7
Scherer, K. R. (1999). Appraisal theories. In T. Dalgleish & M.
Power
(Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 637–663).
Chichester,
UK: Wiley.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20
years later.
Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303.
Sharman, S. J., & Scoboria, A. (2009). Imagination equally
influences false
memories of high and low plausibility events. Applied
Cognitive Psy-
chology, 23, 813–827. doi:10.1002/acp.1515
Shaw, J., & Porter, S. (2015). Constructing rich false memo-
ries of committing crime. Psychological Science, 26, 291–301.
doi:10.1177/0956797614562862
Steblay, N. (1992). A meta-analytic review of the weapon focus
effect. Law
and Human Behavior, 16, 413–424.
Van Damme, I., Kaplan, R. L., Levine, L. J., & Loftus, E. F.
(2015). The rel-
evance of goal-irrelevance for false memories: Emotional
elaboration
in the misinformation paradigm. Manuscript in preparation.
Van Damme, I., & Smets, K. (2014). The power of emotion
versus the
power of suggestion: Memory for emotional events in the
misinforma-
tion paradigm. Emotion, 14, 310–320. doi:10.1037/a0034629
Waring, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Effects of emotional
valence and
arousal upon memory trade-offs with aging. Psychology and
Aging, 24,
412–422. doi:10.1037/a0015526

More Related Content

Similar to How Emotions Shape Eyewitness Memory

Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docx
Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docxLiterature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docx
Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docxSHIVA101531
 
Leslie Handy: Alien Abduction
Leslie Handy: Alien AbductionLeslie Handy: Alien Abduction
Leslie Handy: Alien Abductionpsych493
 
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015#ogilvychange
 
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docx
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docxRead the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docx
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docxaudeleypearl
 

Similar to How Emotions Shape Eyewitness Memory (6)

False memory
False memoryFalse memory
False memory
 
Writing Sample
Writing SampleWriting Sample
Writing Sample
 
Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docx
Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docxLiterature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docx
Literature ReviewIn an article written by Eakin et al. in 2003 f.docx
 
Leslie Handy: Alien Abduction
Leslie Handy: Alien AbductionLeslie Handy: Alien Abduction
Leslie Handy: Alien Abduction
 
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015
O Behave! Issue 10 - January 2015
 
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docx
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docxRead the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docx
Read the two peer-reviewed journal articles on eyewitness testimon.docx
 

More from rosemariebrayshaw

Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docx
Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docxSouthampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docx
Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16 Is.docx
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16  Is.docxSouthwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16  Is.docx
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16 Is.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docx
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docxSpadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docx
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse SPAN100 Title Spanish I.docx
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse  SPAN100 Title  Spanish I.docxSPAN100Course SummaryCourse  SPAN100 Title  Spanish I.docx
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse SPAN100 Title Spanish I.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docx
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docxSources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docx
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources of General Information about the Topic A paragr.docx
Sources of General Information about the Topic  A paragr.docxSources of General Information about the Topic  A paragr.docx
Sources of General Information about the Topic A paragr.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docx
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docxSouthside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docx
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxSources and Tips for Assignment 1  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docx
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docxSource for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docx
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docx
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docxSoria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docx
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docx
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docxSPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docx
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docx
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docxSouth University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docx
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docx
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docxSources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docx
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docx
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docxSooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docx
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docx
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docxSophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docx
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docx
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docxSoria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docx
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxSources and Tips for Assignment 3  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docx
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docxSome of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docx
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docx
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docxSources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docx
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Source Shutterstock.com .docx
Source Shutterstock.com .docxSource Shutterstock.com .docx
Source Shutterstock.com .docxrosemariebrayshaw
 

More from rosemariebrayshaw (20)

Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docx
Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docxSouthampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docx
Southampton Business School Postgraduate Module Grade Descrip.docx
 
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16 Is.docx
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16  Is.docxSouthwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16  Is.docx
Southwestern Business Administration JournalVolume 16 Is.docx
 
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docx
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docxSpadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docx
Spadoni • revised Jan. 2020 —continued— Checklist for .docx
 
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse SPAN100 Title Spanish I.docx
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse  SPAN100 Title  Spanish I.docxSPAN100Course SummaryCourse  SPAN100 Title  Spanish I.docx
SPAN100Course SummaryCourse SPAN100 Title Spanish I.docx
 
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docx
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docxSources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docx
Sources and Resources for RC004Informed Advocacy in Early .docx
 
Sources of General Information about the Topic A paragr.docx
Sources of General Information about the Topic  A paragr.docxSources of General Information about the Topic  A paragr.docx
Sources of General Information about the Topic A paragr.docx
 
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docx
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docxSouthside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docx
Southside Community Services Mrs. Bargas Case History© 2018 Lau.docx
 
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxSources and Tips for Assignment 1  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 1 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
 
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docx
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docxSource for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docx
Source for ArticleMilliken, A. (2018). Ethical awareness What .docx
 
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docx
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docxSoria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docx
Soria 2Victoria SoriaDean WintherEnglish 101 10 March 20.docx
 
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docx
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docxSPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docx
SPC1017 Rubric Informative SpeechName JhoanSpeech Top.docx
 
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docx
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docxSouth University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docx
South University College of Nursing and Public Health Graduate.docx
 
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docx
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docxSources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docx
Sources to UseSuskie, L. (2014, March 17). What is good.docx
 
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docx
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docxSooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docx
Sooner or later you’ll find your-self leading a team where one.docx
 
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docx
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docxSophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docx
Sophia Bosoni, Tombra Esite & Junhui LiuFebruary 6, 2020 Innov.docx
 
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docx
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docxSoria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docx
Soria 2Victoria Soria Dean WintherEnglish 101 04 Februar.docx
 
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docxSources and Tips for Assignment 3  (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
Sources and Tips for Assignment 3 (History 105; Prof. Stansbury)—.docx
 
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docx
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docxSome of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docx
Some of the bibliography docsKarls Marxhttpswww.marxi.docx
 
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docx
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docxSources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docx
Sources of Risk for Chronic Conditions in the State of Flo.docx
 
Source Shutterstock.com .docx
Source Shutterstock.com .docxSource Shutterstock.com .docx
Source Shutterstock.com .docx
 

Recently uploaded

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 

Recently uploaded (20)

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 

How Emotions Shape Eyewitness Memory

  • 1. Society may not realize how everyone around the world engages in the same/similar services or activities; people identify them with various titles. In Japan, they offer and advertise family rental services (actors) to mainly fill voids in their lives. Here in the U.S., we hire people to assist with our daily living both outside and inside of our homes with our children as well as elders, for example, daycares/nannies, in-home caregivers, and counselors. Emotion Review Vol. 8, No. 1 (January 2016) 8 –13 © The Author(s) 2015 ISSN 1754-0739 DOI: 10.1177/1754073915601228 er.sagepub.com Memories of events that evoked powerful emotions are vivid and lasting but not necessarily accurate (Levine & Edelstein, 2009; Reisberg & Heuer, 2007). Under some conditions, emo- tion even increases people’s susceptibility to false memories. This is of great concern in legal settings where people often need to recall information that elicited intense emotion. Consider a victim describing the perpetrator of a crime, a witness testify- ing about a terrifying experience, or a juror recalling gruesome photographs of a crime scene. Distortions in memory for emo- tional information in these contexts can result in false accusa- tions, lawsuits, and wrongful criminal convictions (Loftus, Doyle, & Dysart, 2013). A prominent example is the repressed memory controversy that peaked in the 1990s. Hundreds of adults claimed to have recovered memories, often during ther-
  • 2. apy sessions, of having been victims of childhood sexual abuse. Close investigation later revealed that many therapists used highly suggestive techniques to help patients “remember” instances of abuse, leading patients to form emotional but false memories (Loftus & Ketcham, 1996). These events revealed that even highly emotional memories can be inaccurate and that memory errors can have a devastating human toll. The issue of when and why emotion leaves people vulnera- ble to misremembering events has been vigorously debated, with investigators variously claiming that people are most sus- ceptible to memory errors when experiencing intense emotion, positive emotion, or negative emotion. Here we review research on the malleability of memory and on the relation between emo- tion and false memory, focusing on studies relevant to the inter- disciplinary field of emotion and law. We identify discrepancies in the literature, and argue that taking into account the effects of discrete emotions on attention and motivation helps to resolve seemingly contradictory findings. We also discuss implications Emotion and False Memory Robin L. Kaplan U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, USA Ilse Van Damme Department of Psychology, KU Leuven (University of Leuven), Belgium Linda J. Levine Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, USA Elizabeth F. Loftus
  • 3. Department of Psychology and Social Behavior and Department of Criminology, Law, and Society, University of California, Irvine, USA Abstract Emotional memories are vivid and lasting but not necessarily accurate. Under some conditions, emotion even increases people’s susceptibility to false memories. This review addresses when and why emotion leaves people vulnerable to misremembering events. Recent research suggests that pregoal emotions—those experienced before goal attainment or failure (e.g., hope, fear)—narrow the scope of people’s attention to information that is central to their goals. This narrow focus can impair memory for peripheral details, leaving people vulnerable to misinformation concerning those details. In contrast, postgoal emotions—those experienced after goal attainment or failure (e.g., happiness, sadness)— broaden the scope of attention leaving people more resistant to misinformation. Implications for legal contexts, such as emotion-related errors in eyewitness testimony, are discussed. Keywords emotion, eyewitness testimony, false memory, misinformation effect Author note: The views expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the policies of the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. Corresponding author: Linda J. Levine, Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, 4209 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, USA. Email: [email protected] 601228 EMR0010.1177/1754073915601228Emotion
  • 4. ReviewKaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory research-article2016 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F17540739 15601228&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2015-10-23 Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 9 of emotion-related memory errors in legal contexts and outline directions for further research. The Malleability of Memory When evaluating eyewitness testimony from victims or wit- nesses of a crime, people commonly assume that memory is a simple recording of past events that can be played back at will. This assumption has been shown to be false. Researchers have analyzed exonerations based on DNA evidence and found that inaccurate memory and mistaken eyewitness identification are the most common causes of false convictions in the United States (Garrett, 2011). Errors occur because memories can be altered by people’s subsequent knowledge or beliefs. In a well- known study, Loftus, Miller, and Burns (1978) presented par- ticipants with a slideshow depicting a car at a yield sign. Later, some participants were informed that the car was at a stop sign rather than at a yield sign. Those exposed to this misleading information were significantly more likely to endorse having seen the nonexistent stop sign. This study inspired hundreds of investigations of what is now called “the misinformation effect” which revealed the ease with which memories can be distorted by the introduction of misleading information after an event has been experienced (for a review, see Loftus, 2005). Exposing people to misleading information about events can also lead them to “remember” patently false events with great confidence, ranging from childhood sexual abuse to alien
  • 5. abduc- tions (Loftus & Bernstein, 2005; McNally et al., 2004). Researchers have instilled memories for entire events that never happened using techniques such as increasing their plausibility (e.g., Hart & Schooler, 2006), repeated questioning (e.g., Loftus & Pickrell, 1995), repeated imagination of events (e.g., Sharman & Scoboria, 2009), and guided imagery (e.g., Hyman & Pentland, 1996). The resulting “rich false memories” are often confidently held, highly detailed, and emotional (Loftus & Bernstein, 2005). Far from providing an exact recording of events, then, peo- ple’s memories are malleable. Memories may include errors concerning what happened, when an event happened, where it happened, and who was involved, or memories may be entirely fabricated. Importantly, laboratory studies on false memory mimic real-world practices in the criminal justice system. In police interrogations, witnesses are often subject to repeated questioning and may be asked to imagine events or confirm false information. Misleading information provided to wit- nesses by authority figures often appears plausible (Loftus & Ketcham, 1996). Because people can be exposed to, and asked to elaborate on, inaccurate information in a number of phases of criminal investigations, memory distortions are common (Loftus et al., 2013). Emotional Arousal and Memory Events that cause people to become involved in the legal system are often extremely emotional, raising the issue of how emotion affects people’s susceptibility to false memories. Researchers examining this issue have focused primarily on emotional arousal, the dimension of emotion that ranges from calm relaxa- tion to intense excitement or tension. Their findings show that several cognitive and biological processes contribute to vivid
  • 6. and lasting memories of emotionally arousing events. People pay more attention to emotional than neutral events and, as a result, emotional events are more likely to be encoded in mem- ory. Once encoded, stress hormones in the brain consolidate or stabilize emotional memories making them more enduring (McGaugh, 2004). People also think about and recount emo- tional events more often than neutral events. Each of these pro- cesses enhances memory for emotional information (Laney, Campbell, Heuer, & Reisberg, 2004; Levine & Edelstein, 2009) and can reduce people’s susceptibility to false memories (English & Nielson, 2010). Emotional arousal can also impair memory and increase sus- ceptibility to misinformation, however (e.g., Drivdahl, Zaragoza, & Learned, 2009). Whether emotion enhances or impairs mem- ory depends greatly on how important the information being remembered is to the individual. With increasing emotional arousal, attention narrows to features of events that are of central importance. This results in enhanced memory for central infor- mation at the expense of peripheral details (Christianson & Loftus, 1991), a phenomenon referred to as emotional memory narrowing (Kensinger, 2009) or tunnel memory (Safer, Christianson, Autry, & Osterland, 1998). Researchers have doc- umented emotional memory narrowing both in laboratory stud- ies (e.g., Kensinger, 2009) and across a wide range of real- world events including natural disasters (Bahrick, Parker, Fivush, & Levitt, 1998), physical injuries (Peterson & Bell, 1996), and crime scenes (Reisberg & Heuer, 2007). Of particular relevance to criminal investigations, laboratory studies provide evidence of “weapon focus,” in which witnesses focus on and remember the features of a crime scene that threaten safety, such as the weapon
  • 7. used to commit the crime, at the expense of details such as the perpetrator’s face or clothing that are of importance to police (Loftus, Loftus, & Messo, 1987; Steblay, 1992). Rather than enhancing memory generally, then, emotion often leads to a trade-off in attention to, and memory for, central versus periph- eral information. Under conditions of severe emotional arousal or stress, peo- ple may focus almost exclusively on survival, endurance, and efforts to regulate emotion. This extremely narrow attentional focus can result in poor memory even for the events that elic- ited emotion (e.g., Deffenbacher, Bornstein, Penrod, & McGorty, 2004) and leave people highly susceptible to misin- formation. In a striking example, researchers assessed memory for an interrogation in over 800 military personnel who were going through survival training as part of the U.S. Navy Survival School (Morgan, Southwick, Steffian, Hazlett, & Loftus, 2013). Trainees were placed in a mock prisoner of war camp and endured a stressful interrogation that involved threats and physical assault. Researchers later interviewed participants about their experiences and provided inaccurate information such as a photograph that falsely depicted their interrogator. Participants’ memory was poor both for details of the interro- gation and for the interrogator. In some conditions, more than 10 Emotion Review Vol. 8 No. 1 half of the trainees falsely identified their interrogator (also see Houston, Clifford, Phillips, & Memon, 2013). Thus, memories of events that are highly stressful and evoke extreme levels of emotional arousal are vulnerable to substantial error following exposure to misinformation. Laney and Loftus (2008) found that it is even possible to cre-
  • 8. ate entirely false memories for emotional events such as being hospitalized overnight or witnessing a violent fight between one’s parents. When they compared memories for emotional events described by participants who actually experienced them versus by participants who were induced to believe they had experienced them, true and false memories were indistinguisha- ble on most dimensions. Similarly, people can generate detailed but false memories of committing a crime. Using suggestive interviewing techniques, Shaw and Porter (2015) induced under- graduates to generate emotional false memories of having engaged in theft, assault, or assault with a weapon that led to police contact in their early adolescence. After three suggestive interviews, including supposed corroboration by caregivers and guided imagery, the majority of participants indicated that they remembered having committed the crime and provided a detailed false account. False memories of committing a crime were simi- lar to memories of emotional events that had actually occurred with respect to the presence of sensory details (e.g., visual, audi- tory, and tactile details) and reported anxiety during the event. This study showed that detailed false memories of committing crimes can be generated in a controlled experimental setting and reveals the types of conditions that may lead to false confessions. In summary, whether emotional arousal promotes or impairs memory accuracy depends on the features of events people are asked to remember and the intensity of arousal they experience. In addition to being influenced by people’s knowledge and beliefs, memories are influenced by the focus of people’s atten- tion—a process powerfully swayed by emotion. Emotional arousal enhances memory for information that captures atten- tion, features of events that are of the greatest importance to the individual. However, this memory advantage often comes at the expense of memory for peripheral details and can leave people
  • 9. vulnerable to false memories concerning such details (for a review, see Levine & Edelstein, 2009). Under conditions of extreme stress, this narrow attentional focus can lead to overall memory deficits and pronounced susceptibility to misinforma- tion (Morgan et al., 2013). People are also able to piece together fragments of autobiographical information with information gathered from external sources to generate emotionally rich and coherent memories of events that never occurred (Shaw & Porter, 2015). The sobering implication of these findings for the legal system is that, just because a remembered event is reported with high confidence and strong emotion, does not mean it rep- resents authentic experience. Emotional Valence and Memory The negative or positive tone of an individual’s emotional response to an event may also affect his or her susceptibility to false memories. According to Schwarz and Clore (2003), emotional valence signals the type of information-processing strategy likely to be most adaptive in the current circum- stances. People experience negative emotion when problems arise. Negative feelings signal the need to focus on problems and carefully monitor the environment for information rele- vant to addressing them. People experience positive emotion when no problem requires their immediate attention. Positive feelings signal that it is adaptive to attend to a broad range of stimuli to take advantage of new opportunities. The consequences of these differing information-processing strategies for memory are not clear because the research literature includes contradictory theories and findings. Some investigators have found that people experiencing positive emotion are more vulnerable to incorporating misleading information into memory
  • 10. than people experiencing negative emotion. This may occur because positive emotion promotes a broader and less detail-ori- ented information-processing style than negative emotion, allow- ing people to confuse events they witnessed with similar events that they did not witness (e.g., Forgas, Laham, & Vargas, 2005; Kensinger & Schacter, 2006; Levine & Bluck, 2004). Other investigators have argued that, by narrowing the scope of attention to central information, negative emotional arousal (rather than emotional arousal generally or positive emotion) leaves people susceptible to memory errors concerning periph- eral details (Berntsen, 2002; Waring & Kensinger, 2009; also see Porter, Taylor, & ten Brinke, 2008). For instance, people remembering negative photographs had fewer false memories about central details of the images, but more false memories about peripheral details, than people remembering positive or neutral photographs (Van Damme & Smets, 2014). These find- ings raise concerns about the accuracy of people’s memory for negative, arousing information in legal settings. Witnesses recalling a crime scene, and jurors presented with upsetting tes- timony during a trial, are likely to remember central, threatening details. However, they may be susceptible to misinformation concerning peripheral details, such as the setting in which the crime occurred, that are important for establishing a defendant’s innocence or guilt. Like arousal, then, emotional valence influences memory but is a fickle guide to whether particular memories will be resistant or susceptible to distortion. Research findings conflict concerning whether people are more susceptible to false memo- ries when experiencing positive emotion or negative emotion. Moreover, research on valence neglects potential effects of dis- crete emotions on memory. Events that come to the attention of the legal community are typically emotionally arousing and
  • 11. negative but individuals may react to these events with fear, anger, or despair. These considerations have prompted investi- gators to take a more nuanced approach to examining the effects of emotion on memory that takes into account a third dimension of emotion, its motivational intensity. Motivational Intensity and Memory To understand the effects of emotion on memory it is essential to consider the goals and motivations associated with specific Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 11 emotions and the relevance of the information being remem- bered to these goals. Motivational or goal relevance approaches distinguish between pregoal and postgoal emotions and pro- vide insight into the specific types of information likely to be remembered in different emotional states (e.g., Harmon-Jones, Price, & Gable, 2012; Kaplan, Van Damme, & Levine, 2012; Levine & Edelstein, 2009; Levine & Pizarro, 2004; Montagrin, Brosch, & Sander, 2013). According to appraisal theories (e.g., Ellsworth & Doughterty, 2016; Scherer, 1999), emotions are inherently tied to changes in the status of people’s goals. People experience emotions when they perceive that goal attainment or failure is imminent or has occurred, making it necessary for them to modify their beliefs, plans, or goals. Once evoked, emotions direct cognition and motivate action in a manner that is useful for ensuring, preventing, or adjusting to those changes in the status of goals. Pregoal emotions, such as desire, hope, anger, fear, and dis- gust, reflect appraisals that goal attainment or failure is antici- pated or threatened. These emotions, irrespective of their positive or negative valence, are characterized by high motiva- tional intensity and an impetus to act. When experiencing pre-
  • 12. goal emotion, people’s attention narrows to information that is central to their goals. For example, anger prepares people to remove obstacles to their goals, and fear prepares people to avoid or escape threats to their goals (Cunningham & Brosch, 2012). Because the angry individual focuses on the agent obstructing a goal, and the fearful individual focuses on the source of threat, the range of information attended to, encoded in, or retrieved from memory is narrow. This narrow focus leads to poorer memory for details that are not relevant to the individual’s current goal, resulting in greater vulnerability to misinformation concerning those details. In contrast, postgoal emotions, such as happiness and sadness, are lower in motiva- tional intensity because they reflect appraisals that goal attain- ment or failure has already occurred. When experiencing postgoal emotions, attention broadens as people consider the consequences of goal attainment or failure, change their beliefs and expectations, and orient toward new goals. Thus, postgoal emotions increase the breadth of information processing, allowing people to encode and retrieve peripheral details (Kaplan et al., 2012). Examining the motivations associated with specific emo- tions, and their implications for cognition, represents a promis- ing theoretical approach that helps to resolve seemingly contradictory findings in the literature. For example, research showing cognitive narrowing for negative emotion, and broad- ening for positive emotion, has often contrasted pregoal nega- tive emotion (e.g., fear, disgust) with postgoal positive emotion (e.g., happiness, amusement). Conflicting findings may reflect the fact that emotions of the same valence can vary in motiva- tional intensity. For example, Gable and Harmon-Jones (2010) showed participants photographs that evoked disgust (pregoal), sadness (postgoal), or a neutral state. They then assessed the breadth of participants’ attention with a separate task. Viewing disgusting images narrowed attention, and viewing sad pictures broadened attention, relative to viewing neutral pictures.
  • 13. Although research is sparse on how the motivations associ- ated with specific emotions affect memory, we believe this approach is important for understanding memory distortion in legal settings. During a trial, jurors may be exposed to testi- mony or gruesome photographs that evoke fear, anger, or dis- gust. They may also be exposed to testimony or photographs that convey the devastating impact a crime had on victims, evoking sadness (Bandes & Salerno, 2014). Further research is needed to understand how pregoal emotions such as anger and fear, and postgoal emotions such as sadness, affect the specific types of information that witnesses and jurors attend to, encode, and retrieve and their susceptibility to false memories. In a recent study, we assessed people’s susceptibility to memory distortion when they were instructed to elaborate on information associated with pregoal and postgoal emotions (Van Damme, Kaplan, Levine, & Loftus, 2015). While watch- ing a slideshow depicting an interaction between a woman and her boyfriend, participants were asked to empathize with the woman’s emotional state. For different groups of participants, the woman was described as feeling hopeful (pregoal positive), fearful (pregoal negative), happy (postgoal positive), or devas- tated (postgoal negative). After watching the slideshow, partici- pants were asked to reflect on the interaction they had witnessed, including events that had actually occurred in the slideshow (true events) and events that did not occur (false events). Later, participants’ memory was tested for true and false events that were relevant or irrelevant to the woman’s goals. Participants in the pregoal conditions (hope, fear) remembered more false events that were irrelevant to the woman’s goals than partici- pants in the postgoal conditions (happiness, devastation). Participants in the pregoal conditions were also more confident in correctly rejecting goal-relevant false events than participants
  • 14. in the postgoal conditions. These findings are consistent with the view that, irrespective of emotional valence, pregoal emo- tion enhances memory for information relevant to goal pursuit at the expense of irrelevant information (Harmon-Jones, Price, & Gable, 2012; Kaplan et al., 2012; Montagrin et al., 2013). Conclusions Scholars across a multitude of disciplines have called for a more complete understanding of the role emotion plays in legal pro- cesses (Bornstein & Wiener, 2010). Research shows that memo- ries are shaped, and can be misshaped, by people’s knowledge and beliefs, attention, and motivation—all processes that are influenced by emotion. People’s most vivid and lasting memo- ries are typically emotional ones. These memories are selective, however. When experiencing intense emotion, people attend to the features of events that are most relevant to their current goals, promoting detailed and accurate memory for that central information. This advantage comes at the expense of memory for information that is not relevant to their goals and can leave people vulnerable to misinformation concerning such details. Under conditions of extreme stress, attentional narrowing can lead to overall memory deficits and pronounced susceptibility to misinformation. 12 Emotion Review Vol. 8 No. 1 These findings have important implications for practitioners in the legal system. Eyewitnesses are often asked to remember details of events that were extremely stressful long after the events occurred. They may be exposed to misleading informa- tion about the events from other witnesses’ reports, leading questions by investigators, and pretrial news reports (Frenda, Nichols, & Loftus, 2011). Because memory for emotional
  • 15. events is both selective and malleable, law enforcement profes- sionals should take great care that witnesses are not exposed to information that might alter their memories. For jurors and judges listening to emotional testimony, these findings mean that testimony should not be given more credibility simply because it is conveyed with emotion. Witnesses may convey information with substantial emotion but be inaccurate. They may even be genuinely emotional about memories that are entirely false (Laney & Loftus, 2008; McNally et al., 2004; Shaw & Porter, 2015). Information about memory processes should be a standard component of the instructions given to jurors and of the education received by law enforcement person- nel, attorneys, and judges. Investigators have also examined the specific dimensions of emotional experience that render people most susceptible to false memories. The findings are complex and investigators have variously claimed that people are rendered most suscepti- ble to misinformation by intense emotional arousal, positive emotion, or negative emotion. We have argued that a more nuanced approach is needed that takes into account the motiva- tions associated with discrete emotions. Emotions high in moti- vational intensity, such as fear, disgust, and anger, powerfully direct attention to features of events that are of central impor- tance to the individual for avoiding threats and removing obsta- cles to their goals. When people’s attentional resources are limited, misinformation effects are easier to obtain (Loftus, 2005). Thus, this narrow focus leaves people susceptible to false memories concerning features of events that are peripheral to their goals. Investigating the effects of discrete emotions on people’s susceptibility to false memories represents an impor- tant research direction for the field of memory and the law. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.
  • 16. References Bahrick, L. E., Parker, J. F., Fivush, R., & Levitt, M. (1998). The effects of stress on young children’s memory for a natural disaster. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 308–331. doi:10.1037/1076– 898X.4.4.308 Bandes, S. A., & Salerno, J. M. (2014). Emotion, proof and prejudice: The cognitive science of gruesome photos and victim impact statements. Arizona State Law Journal, 46, 1003–1056. Berntsen, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Cen- tral details are remembered more frequently from shocking than from happy experiences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1010–1020. doi:10.3758/ BF03194319 Bornstein, B. H., & Wiener, R. L. (2010). Emotion and the law: A field whose time has come. In B. H. Bornstein & R. L. Wiener (Eds.), Emotion and the law: Psychological perspectives (pp. 1–12). New York, NY: Springer. Christianson, S.-Å., & Loftus, E. F. (1991). Remembering emotional events: The fate of detailed information. Cognition & Emotion,
  • 17. 5, 81– 108. doi:10.1080/02699939108411027 Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psy- chological Science, 21, 54–59. doi:10.1177/0963721411430832 Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K. (2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewit- ness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 687–706. doi:10.1007/ s10979–004–0565-x Drivdahl, S. B., Zaragoza, M. S., & Learned, D. M. (2009). The role of emo- tional elaboration in the creation of false memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 13–35. doi:10.1002/acp.1446 Ellsworth, P. C., & Doughterty, A. (2016). Appraisal and reappraisals in the courtroom. Emotion Review, 8(1): pp. 20–25. English, S. M., & Nielson, K. A. (2010). Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117, 237– 242. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.014 Forgas, J. P., Laham, S. M., & Vargas, P. T. (2005). Mood effects on eyewitness memory: Affective influences on susceptibility to
  • 18. misin- formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 574– 588. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.11.005 Frenda, S. J., Nichols, R. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2011). Current issues and advances in misinformation research. Current Directions in Psycho- logical Science, 20, 20–23. doi:10.1177/0963721410396620 Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215. doi:10.1177/ 0956797609359622 Garrett, B. L. (2011). Convicting the innocent: Where criminal prosecutions go wrong. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Harmon-Jones, E., Price, T. F., & Gable, P. A. (2012). The influence of affective states on cognitive broadening/narrowing: Considering the importance of motivational intensity. Social and Personality Psychol- ogy Compass, 6, 314–327. doi:10.1111/j.1751– 9004.2012.00432.x Hart, R. E., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). Increasing belief in the experience of an invasive procedure that never happened: The role of plausibil- ity and schematicity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 661– 669.
  • 19. doi:10.1002/acp.1218 Houston, K. A., Clifford, B. R., Phillips, L. H., & Memon, A. (2013). The emotional eyewitness: The effects of emotion on specific aspects of eyewitness recall and recognition performance. Emotion, 13, 118–128. doi.org/10.1037/a0029220 Hyman, I. E., Jr., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Lan- guage, 35, 101–117. Kaplan, R. L., Van Damme, I., & Levine, L. J. (2012). Motivation mat- ters: Differing effects of pre-goal and post-goal emotions on atten- tion and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(404). doi:10.3389/ fpsyg.2012.00404 Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1, 99–113. doi:10.1177/1754073908100432 Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 757–763. doi:10.3758/BF03193993 Laney, C., Campbell, H. V., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (2004). Memory for thematically arousing events. Memory & Cognition, 32,
  • 20. 1149–1159. doi:10.3758/BF03196888 Laney, C., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Emotional content of true and false mem- ories. Memory, 16, 500–516. Levine, L. J., & Bluck, S. (2004). Painting with broad strokes: Happiness and the malleability of event memory. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 559–574. doi:10.1080/02699930341000446 Kaplan et al. Emotion and False Memory 13 Levine, L. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 833– 875. doi:10.1080/02699930902738863 Levine, L. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2004). Emotion and memory research: A grumpy overview. Social Cognition, 22, 530–554. doi:10.1521/ soco.22.5.530.50767 Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning and Memory, 12, 361–366. Loftus, E. F., & Bernstein, D. M. (2005). Rich false memories:
  • 21. The royal road to success. In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Experimental cognitive psychol- ogy and its applications (pp. 101–113). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press. Loftus, E. F., Doyle, J. M., & Dysart, J. (2013). Eyewitness testimony: Civil & criminal (5th ed.). Charlottesville, VA: Lexis Law. Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1996). The myth of repressed memory. New York, NY: MacMillan. Loftus, E. F., Loftus, G. R., & Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about weapon focus. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 55–62. doi:10.1007/ BF01044839 Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31. doi:10.1037/0278– 7393.4.1.19 Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psy- chiatric Annals, 25, 720–725. doi:10.3928/0048–5713– 19951201–07 McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of mem-
  • 22. ories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 27, 1–28. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157 McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., Maclin, M. L., Pitman, R. K., & Orr, S. P. (2004). Psychophysiological responding during script- driven imagery in people reporting abduction by space aliens. Psychological Science, 15, 493–497. doi:10.1111/j.0956–7976.2004.00707.x Montagrin, A., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2013). Goal conduciveness as a key determinant of memory facilitation. Emotion, 13, 622– 628. doi:10.1037/a003306 Morgan, C. A., III, Southwick, S., Steffian, G., Hazlett, G. A., & Loftus, E. F. (2013). Misinformation can influence memory for recently experienced, highly stressful events. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 36, 11–17. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.11.002 Peterson, C., & Bell, M. (1996). Children’s memory for traumatic injury. Child Development, 67, 3045–3070. doi:10.1111/j.1467– 8624.1996. tb01902.x Porter, S., Taylor, K., & ten Brinke, L. (2008). Memory for media: Investi- gation of false memories for negatively and positively charged
  • 23. public events. Memory, 16, 658–666. doi:10.1080/09658210802154626 Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2007). The influence of emotion on memory in forensic settings. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross & R. C. L. Lind- say (Eds.), The handbook of eyewitness psychology, Vol. 1: Memory for events (pp. 81–116). London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Safer, M. A., Christianson, S., Autry, M. W., & Osterland, K. (1998). Tunnel memory for traumatic events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 99–117. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099– 0720(199804)12:2<99::AID- ACP509>3.0.CO;2–7 Scherer, K. R. (1999). Appraisal theories. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 637–663). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 296–303. Sharman, S. J., & Scoboria, A. (2009). Imagination equally influences false memories of high and low plausibility events. Applied Cognitive Psy- chology, 23, 813–827. doi:10.1002/acp.1515
  • 24. Shaw, J., & Porter, S. (2015). Constructing rich false memo- ries of committing crime. Psychological Science, 26, 291–301. doi:10.1177/0956797614562862 Steblay, N. (1992). A meta-analytic review of the weapon focus effect. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 413–424. Van Damme, I., Kaplan, R. L., Levine, L. J., & Loftus, E. F. (2015). The rel- evance of goal-irrelevance for false memories: Emotional elaboration in the misinformation paradigm. Manuscript in preparation. Van Damme, I., & Smets, K. (2014). The power of emotion versus the power of suggestion: Memory for emotional events in the misinforma- tion paradigm. Emotion, 14, 310–320. doi:10.1037/a0034629 Waring, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Effects of emotional valence and arousal upon memory trade-offs with aging. Psychology and Aging, 24, 412–422. doi:10.1037/a0015526