SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 47
Research in Higher Education, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1992
NONLANGUAGE FACTORS AFFECTING
UNDERGRADUATES' JUDGMENTS OF
NONNATIVE ENGLISH-SPEAKING TEACHING
ASSISTANTS
Donald L Rubin
• . . . . . , , . . . . . . • • . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . + . . . . . . . , . . .
. • . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
. . . . , . , . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . • . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . .
, . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
In response to dramatic changes in the demographics of
graduate education, consid-
erable effort is being devoted to training teaching assistants
who are nonnative
speakers of English (NNSTAs). Three studies extend earlier
research that showed
the potency of nonlanguage factors such as ethnicity in
affecting undergraduates'
reactions to NNSTAs. Study 1 examined effects of instructor
ethnicity, even when
the instructor's language was completely standard. Study 2
identified predictors of
teacher ratings and listening comprehension from among several
attitudinal and
background variables. Study 3 was a pilot intervention effort in
which undergradu-
ates served as teaching coaches for NNSTAs. This intervention,
however, exerted
no detectable effect on undergraduates' attitudes. Taken
together, these findings
warrant that intercultural sensitization for undergraduates must
complement skills
training for NNSTAs, but that this sensitization will not accrue
from any superficial
intervention program.
. . . . . . o . . , . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
• . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . •
Observers who have been even moderately sensitive to the
voices o f North
American college students in recent years can appreciate the
depth o f under-
graduates' concerns about the quality of instruction offered at
many campuses
by nonnative English-speaking teaching assistants (NNSTAs).
The NNSTA
"crisis" is one of relatively few instructional issues in higher
education that has
captured the attention of the popular press (Hess, 1987;
Verhovek, 1989).
In several states, legislatures have directly intervened in
policies regarding
NNSTAs (e.g., "Instructors' Broken English Prompts Illinois
Law," 1987;
Thomas and Monoson, 1991).
To the thinking of the press and o f the policymakers--and
probably also o f
the undergraduate students--the root o f the problem is
NNSTAs' poor English-
language proficiency (Bailey, 1984). Accordingly, many
universities have re-
Donald L. Rubin, The University of Georgia. Address
Correspondence to: Speech Communica-
tion and Language Education, The University of Georgia,
Athens, GA 30602.
511
0 3 6 1 - 0 3 6 5 / 9 2 / 0 8 0 0 - 0 5 1 1 5 0 6 . 5 0 / 0 © 1 9 9 2
H u m a n S c i e n c e s P r e s s , I n c .
512 RUBtN
sponded by erecting procedures for screening NNSTAs'
language skills (see
discussions of these tests in Abraham and Plakans, 1988; Dunn
and Constan-
tinides, 1991; Gallego, Goodwin, and Turner, 1991; Heller,
1985; Yule and
Hoffman, 1990). Those deemed deficient are channeled into
remediation pro-
grams (Turitz, 1984). For the most part, these remedial
workshops appear to
focus on discrete language behaviors, especially pronunciation
and use of id-
ioms (Verhovek, 1989). This focus is not illogical, since many
of the current
groups of NNSTAs have had little exposure to English as a
medium of oral
communication. A more recent and educationally progressive
approach com-
bines language remediation with training in generic
instructional communica-
tion skills and in cultural norms associated with American
undergraduates
(e.g., Althen, 1991; Byrd, Constantinides, and Pennington,
1989; Pica, Barnes,
and Finger, 1990).
The common assumption of all these perspectives on NNSTAs is
that it is the
internationals who bear the onus for poor classroom
communication, and there-
fore it is the internationals who must undergo transformation in
the North
American mold. An alternative perspective, in contrast,
recognizes that com-
munication is a reciprocal process. If communication outcomes
are poor in
classes taught by NNSTAs, then perhaps the responsibility
ought to be shared
alike among NNSTAs and the American undergraduate student
body.
Scattered evidence warrants the plausibility of this alternative
view, the view
that the NNSTA "problem" is at least partially a problem of
undergraduates'
negative stereotyping. For example, Orth (1982) found that
undergraduate stu-
dents' ratings of their NNSTAs' speaking proficiency were only
weakly related
to expert ratings of the NNSTAs' language proficiency. Instead,
undergraduate
ratings of NNSTAs were biased by the grades they anticipated
receiving from
those NNSTAs. For many undergraduates, introductory courses
in mathematics
and the natural sciences have reputations as extremely inhumane
courses de-
signed to winnow out marginal students. And it is well known
that NNSTAs
are disproportionately assigned responsibility for such high-
anxiety classes
(Constantinides, 1987). In addition, with an increasing
proportion of NNSTAs
originating from East Asia (Zikopoulos, 1988), it is likely that
powerful ethnic
stereotypes also factor into negative perceptions of language
proficiency and
teaching ability (Brown, 1988).
Rubin and Smith (1990) undertook to explore the role of
NNSTAs' ethnicity,
the subject matter about which they lectured, and their level of
accentedness as
determinants of undergraduates' responses. Using a matched
guise technique
typical of language and attitude research (see review of this
body of research in
Edwards, 1982), two native speakers of Cantonese each
recorded highly ac-
cented and moderately accented versions of simulated classroom
lectures. One
lecture was about a natural science topic (the uses and growing
scarcity of
helium), and the second lecture was about a humanities topic
(the role of the
NNSTAs 513
Mahabarata in Indian society). Undergraduate subjects heard
one or the other of
the lecture topics delivered with either of the levels of accent,
accompanied by
a photograph of either a European or an Asian instructor.
Dependent measures included a cloze test of listening
comprehension (Bach-
man, 1985), ratings of teacher quality, and four subscales of a
measure of
homophily (McCroskey, Richmond, and Daly, 1975).
Homophily is a construct
that pertains to listeners' perceptions of similarity to message
sources. Homo-
phily is an important variable mediating cross-cultural
communication (Dodd,
1986). For example, in the diffusion of innovations from one
culture to an-
other, people are more likely to accept new ideas from
homophilous sources.
By the same token, homophily has been found to be an
important factor in
classroom communication (Elliot, 1979). Students respond more
positively to
teachers of optimal homophily. In addition to these measures,
subjects also
replied to questions about their contact with NNSTAs.
Rubin and Smith's (1990) results showed that the effects of
accent were not
explained by any simple response pattern. On the one hand,
manipulated ac-
centedness was not accurately discerned on a questionnaire item
designed to
check the effectiveness of the experimental manipulation;
students couldn't al-
ways discriminate between high and moderate levels of accent.
On the other
hand, manipulated accent did affect undergraduates' perceptions
of NNSTAs'
et h ni cit y--but only when other cues were ambiguous, that is,
when the photo-
graph was European and the topic pertained to humanities.
Of greater significance, listeners' perceptions of the instructors'
a c c e n t - -
whether accurate perceptions or n o t - - w e r e the strongest
predictors of teacher
ratings. When students believed an instructor's accent to be
"foreign," they
simultaneously perceived him or her to be a poor teacher. Also
of particular
interest, the best predictor of undergraduates' listening
comprehension scores
was the number of courses they had taken that had been
instructed by nonnative
instructors. Those students who had persevered with their
nonnative English-
speaking instructors had been rewarded by improved skill in
listening to ac-
cented speech.
On the basis of these findings, Rubin and Smith (1990)
concluded that high
effort/low results programs to remediate pronunciation ought
not constitute the
main investment of university resources. Instead, it makes sense
to simul-
taneously educate undergraduates about the nature of NNSTA
speech, perhaps
to provide instruction in listening to accented speech, and to
facilitate the
amount of exposure to NNSTAs experienced by undergraduates.
The present article reports three studies that elaborate on and
extend the work
reported by Rubin and Smith (1990). In that study, speakers
altered their de-
gree o f accentedness, but they could not completely adopt a
native-like Stand-
ard American English (SAE) accent. Study 1, therefore,
replicates Rubin and
Smith, but eliminates accent as a factor. Instead, Study 1 asks
undergraduates
514 RUBIN
to rate SAE-speaking instructors o f varying ethnicity. Building
on the espe-
cially interesting correlational results in the 1990 study, Study 2
utilizes a mul-
tiple regression analysis to predict teacher ratings and
comprehension scores.
Finally, Study 3 is a small-scale attempt to implement a training
program for
mitigating undergraduates' attitudes toward NNSTAs.
S T U D Y 1
Pronunciation training is tedious and is rarely 100 percent
effective. Even the
most motivated and industrious student of a foreign language is
likely to retain
a vestige of nonnative pronunciation. North American
undergraduates appar-
ently are not very discerning in discriminating levels of
accentedness. There-
fore, it is possible that even those NNSTAs who have excelled
in pronunciation
drills may still suffer a high level of stigmatization by North
American under-
graduates.
But what if it were possible to completely supplant a nonnative
accent with
SAE speech? Would even this unlikely achievement be
sufficient to overcome
negative ethnic stereotypes and attitudes toward subject matter?
The purpose of
Study 1 is to ascertain the effects of instructor ethnicity and of
lecture subject
matter when the instructor's actual language behavior is
standard, and therefore
not culpable as the source of potentially negative student
evaluations.
Methods
Participants
Undergraduates were recruited from basic speech
communication classes at a
large southeastern university. They participated in this study in
lieu of a re-
quired out-of-class assignment. Data collected from
international students were
omitted from analysis. Complete sets of usable data were
obtained from 62
North American undergraduates.
Procedures and Stimulus Materials
Each subject listened to a single tape-recorded speech sample
lasting about
four minutes. The speech was presented as part of a lecture by a
university
"instructor." As subjects listened to the lecture, a slide
photograph representing
the instructor was projected on a screen.
Instructor ethnicity was operationalized by projecting a
photograph of either
a Caucasian or an Asian (Chinese) woman. To avoid
confounding ethnicity
with physical attractiveness, both models were similarly
dressed, were of simi-
lar size and hair style, and were photographed in the same
setting and pose
(standing at a lecturn in front o f a chalkboard). No differences
between the
NNSTAs 515
photographs in rated physical attractiveness were found in any
uses o f these
photographs (although other factors like speech topic sometimes
exerted signifi-
cant effects on judgments of attractiveness).
Lecture top ic--physi c a l science or humanities--was
operationalized by
using the same two scripts as in Rubin and Smith (1990). Both
scripts were
adapted from articles originally appearing in the New York
Times. The topics of
helium scarcity and of the Mahabarata were selected to
represent science and
humanities, respectively, because it was judged that most
undergraduates
would have relatively little prior knowledge about them. Both
passages were
edited to approximately 450 words, and both recorded speech
samples were
approximately four minutes long. They were recorded by a
single speaker, a
doctoral student in speech communication, a native speaker of
English raised in
central Ohio, who was well regarded by her own undergraduate
students for
especially effective and clear classroom delivery.
Subjects were randomly assigned to one o f the resulting lecture
topics (at two
levels) by ethnicity (at two levels) treatment combinations.
Complete sets of
data were obtained from 13 participants in the Asian
instructor/science topic
group, 16 in the Caucasian instructor/science topic group, 17 in
the Asian in-
structor/humanities topic treatment, and 16 in the Caucasian
instructor/human-
ities topic treatment.
Measurement and Analysis
Immediately after hearing the lecture, subjects completed a
cloze test of lis-
tening comprehension. They were presented with a written text
of the lecture
with every seventh word deleted, save in the first sentence,
which was kept
intact. Only exact recall was scored as correct (see Bachman,
1985).
Next, subjects completed the McCroskey et al. (1975)
homophily instru-
ment. The instrument is comprised of 16 semantic differential
items. The items
pose polar opposite descriptions at either end o f seven equal-
appearing interval
scales (e.g., "shares my values: : : : : : : :doesn't share my
values"). The items
were arranged on the questionnaire in random order and with
randomly alter-
nating polarities. The instrument was developed to yield four
subscales, each of
which is comprised o f four semantic differential items: (1)
attitude homophily,
(2) background homophily, (3) value homophily, and (4)
appearance homo-
phily.
Interspersed among the homophily items were additional
semantic differen-
tial items checking the ethnicity manipulation
("Caucasian/European ethnici-
t y : : : : : : : :Oriental/Asian ethnicity") and measuring
impressions of accent
("Speaks with American accent: : : : : : : :Speakers with foreign
accent") and
of teaching competence ("Poor teacher: : : : : : : :Effective
teacher"), as well as
516 RUBIN
TABLE 1. Cell Means and Standard Deviations for 8 Dependent
Variables
(Study I)
Humanities Science Lecture Topic:
Instructor Ethnicity: Caucasian Asian Caucasian Asian
N = 16 17 16 t3
Perceived accent 3.44 (2.56) 4.94 (1.98) 2.75 (2.44) 3.77 (1.88)
Perceived ethnicity 2.75 (1.98) 5.53 (1.97) 2.06 (1,39) 6.23
(0.93)
Comprehension 11.94 (4.34) 9.93 (5,70) 1 2 . 5 (5.9) 7.31
(4.70)
Teaching qualifications 8.69 (3.61) 8.18 (t.81) 7.25 (2.46) 9.15
(2.73)
Attitude homophily 5.56 (5.51) 6.00 (4.95) 2.38 (4.24) 3.38
(4.37)
Background homophily 1 2 . 7 5 (4.12) 12.18 (4.03) 13.25
(4.48) 10.31 (3.54)
Values homophily 15.88 (4.53) 14.94 (2.88) 14.81 (3.21) 14.46
(3.86)
Appearance homophily 1 1 . 1 3 (5.04) 10.71 (5.77) 10.13
(4.77) 7.69 (3.22)
several unrelated filler scales. In all, the instrument contained
28 semantic dif-
ferential items.
Dependent variables, then, were cloze test scores, the four
homophily sub-
scales, perceptions of accent, and ratings of teaching
competence, as well as
the manipulation check scale that registered perceptions o f
ethnicity. The ma-
nipulation check was subjected to a 2 x 2 A N O V A with
subjects nested in
levels of ethnicity (Caucasian, Asian) and lecture topic
(science, humanities).
The remaining dependent variables were submitted to an
analogous 2 x 2
MANOVA. The regression solution, whereby each M A N O V
A sum o f squares
is adjusted for each of the others, was used to control potential
nonorthog-
onality due to unequal cell frequencies (Norusis, 1990). Since
the M A N O V A
revealed no interaction effect, the significant main effect was
followed up with
discriminant analysis to determine which combination o f
dependent variables
best discriminated among groups.
Results
Cell means and standard deviations for the eight dependent
variables appear
in Table 1. Table 2 shows their intercorrelations. The A N O V
A for the manipu-
lation check showed that manipulated ethnicity indeed exerted a
main effect
on perceived ethnicity (F1,ss = 66.91, p < .001). As expected,
the Asian in-
structor was perceived to be more Oriental/Asian than the
Caucasian instructor
(M,sia, = 5.79, Mcau = 2.41). Neither the topic factor nor the
interaction
between topic and ethnicity significantly affected students'
perceptions o f in-
structor ethnicity.
Table 3 summarizes the M A N O V A for the seven other
dependent variables.
N N S T A s 5 1 7
.=
"F.
<
I I
< I
I
518 RUBIN
TABLE 3. Multitopic X Ethnicity MANOVA of 7 Dependent
Variables
(Study 1)
Source of Variation df Wilk's Lambda F D
Topic 7 0.866 1.15 .35
Ethnicity 7 0.727 2.78 .02
Topic x Ethnicity 7 0.879 1.03 .42
Error 52
As it indicates, the only statistically significant effect to e m e r
g e was a m a i n
effect f o r instructor ethnicity ( W i l k ' s LambdaT,52 =
.727, equivalent F =
2.78, p < .05).
T o follow up this significant multivariate m a i n effect, a
step-wise discrimi-
nant analysis, s u m m a r i z e d in T a b l e 4, was p e r f o r
m e d . C o m p r e h e n s i o n test
score w a s the first variable to enter, b a s e d on the highest
pooled within-group
correlation ( - .63) with the cannonical discriminant function.
Using c o m p r e -
hension test scores alone, 61.2 percent o f participants could be
correctly classi-
fied into groups (in this case the grouping factor b e i n g w h e
t h e r they v i e w e d an
Asian instructor o r a C a u c a s i a n instructor). Inspection o
f T a b l e 1 shows that
c o m p r e h e n s i o n cell m e a n s were l o w e r f o r the
groups e x p o s e d to an Asian vis-
age, and higher f o r groups that saw a Caucasian instructor.
T h e final discriminant m o d e l also included p e r c e i v e d
accent, entered at step
2. Inspection o f cell m e a n s in T a b l e 1 shows that accent
w a s p e r c e i v e d as m o r e
foreign and less standard in the case o f the Asian instructor's
photograph. An
additional 9.8 p e r c e n t o f participants were correctly
classified into t r e a t m e n t
groups w h e n p e r c e i v e d accent was added to the m o d e
l .
Discussion
T h e s e results p r o v i d e d r a m a t i c e v i d e n c e that
North A m e r i c a n undergraduates
are reacting to factors extraneous to just language proficiency w
h e n they are
TABLE 4. Stepwise Discriminant Analysis of Manipulated
Ethnicity by 7 Predictor Variables
(Study 1)
Model Model Correlation with
Step Variable Entered Wilk's Lambda F-to-Enter P-Value
DiscriminantFunction
1 Comprehension .901 6.6t .01 - .631
2 Perceived accent .798 7.56 .001 .573
NNSTAs 519
asked to react to NNSTAs. In Study 1, only a single language
variety was used:
SAE. Still, when they were faced with an ethnically Asian
instructor, partici-
pants responded in the direction one would expect had they been
listening to
nonstandard speech. Evidence from the discriminant analysis
suggests that par-
ticipants stereotypically attributed accent differences--
differences that did not
exist in t r u t h - - t o the instructors' speech. Yet more
serious, listening compre-
hension appeared to be undermined simply by identifying
(visually) the instruc-
tor as Asian.
The pessimistic conclusion warranted here is that at least among
this particu-
lar sample of undergraduates even vigorous pronunciation
training for NNSTAs
will matter little. Ethnically Asian instructors who speak SAE
apparently con-
front similar dysfunctional attitudes as those who do speak with
marked nonna-
tive accents.
S T U D Y 2
Those sympathetic to the plight of NNSTAs can derive some
slight comfort
from the results of Study 1: At least ratings of instructional
competence were
not significantly involved in discriminating between Asian and
Caucasian
teachers. Indeed, it is uncertain which attitudinal and
experiential attributes of
undergraduates do affect the way they rate NNSTAs' teaching
effectiveness.
Rubin and Smith (1990) found that perceived accentedness, but
not actual ac-
cent, was negatively related to teacher ratings. In addition, they
found that
actual experience attending classes taught by NNSTAs was
positively related to
accuracy in listening comprehension.
It is reasonable to expect that homophily would also mediate
teacher ratings
such that students are less antagonistic toward NNSTAs whom
they perceive as
relatively similar to them. Consistent with the homophily
hypothesis, Bailey
(1984) reports that students sharing the same major as a NNSTA
are likely to
rate that NNSTA higher than one from a different academic
area.
The purpose o f Study 2, accordingly, was to determine the
relative contribu-
tions o f factors that do predict undergraduates' ratings of
NNSTAs and their
comprehension of NNSTA speech. Based on the earlier studies,
it was pre-
sumed that it would be fruitful to locate these factors in
undergraduates' own
attitudinal systems and experience.
Methods
Participants
Participants were drawn from the same undergraduate pool as in
Study 1.
Again, data from international students were excluded. Usable
sets of data were
collected from a total of 148 students over the course of four
academic terms.
520 RUBIN
Procedures and Stimulus Materials
To best generalize the results of the regression analysis used in
this phase of
the research, a wide range of stimulus conditions (i.e.,
nonfactorial) was desir-
able. Each subject was nonsystematically assigned to one of the
ten stimulus
audio tapes developed either in conjunction with Study 1,
above, or in conjunc-
tion with Rubin and Smith (1990). That is, each subject listened
to either high
or moderate Chinese accent (produced by one or another of the
two native
Chinese speakers), or else to SAE accent, in combination with
either the sci-
ence or the humanities tape. About half the participants listened
to their simu-
lated lectures in conjunction with a photographic depiction of
an Asian teaching
assistant. About one-third listened to the lectures while viewing
a Caucasian/
European face, while one-sixth merely listened to the lectures
without any pho-
tographic presentation.
Measurement and Analysis
Subjects in this study responded to the same questionnaire
prepared for Study
1. This included items comprising the four homophily subscales,
and items
measuring perceptions of accent, ethnicity, and teaching
quality. In addition, a
"background questionnaire" asked students to report (among
other items not
under consideration here) the number of classes in which they
had been in-
structed by NNSTAs and the number of weeks they had traveled
outside the
United States and attitudes toward nonnative speakers in
general. This back-
ground questionnaire also included six Likert-type items
devised to measure
general attitudes toward normative English speakers. For each
of these Likert-
type items, participants read attitude statements and then
responded in one of
five categories ranging from "strongly agree" to "strongly
disagree." Three of
these attitude statements pertained to interpersonal relations
with international
students (e.g., "I would be willing to have a nonnative English
speaker as my
roommate"). The others pertained to benefits that internationals
contribute to
the university campus (e.g., "Our campus benefits by having
nonnative English
speakers attending").
Two step-wise regressions were run. The predictor variables in
both analyses
were identical: (1) perceived accent, (2) perceived ethnicity, (3)
attitude homo-
phily, (4) value homophily, (5) background homophily, (6)
appearance homo-
phily, (7) total college credits, (8) number of classes instructed
by NNSTAs,
(9) a composite scale of attitudes toward interpersonal relations
with nonnative
English speakers, and (10) a composite scale of attitudes toward
the overall
contribution of internationals. In one regression, the criterion
variable was rated
teacher qualification. In the second regression, the criterion
variable was listen-
ing comprehension (cloze) score. For the purposes of the step-
wise procedure,
the level of significance for F-to-enter and F-to-exit was set at
P < . 05.
NNSTAs 521
Results
Table 5 contains the zero-order correlation coefficients among
the 10 predic-
tor variables and between each predictor and the 2 criterion
variables. Table 6
summarizes the final results of the step-wise regressions for
judged teacher
qualification and for listening comprehension. The total R 2 for
the first model
indicates that it accounted for 11.6 percent o f the variance in
teacher ratings
(multiple r = .34). Inspection of the Beta-weights indicates that
perceived
accent was inversely related to teacher ratings (i.e., the more
foreign the accent
was judged, the lower the rating of teacher effectiveness), while
attitude homo-
phily was positively related to teacher ratings.
For the multiple regression of listening comprehension scores,
only a single
variable, number o f courses students had taken from NNSTAs,
was a statis-
tically significant predictor. It accounted for 9.1 percent of the
variance in
comprehension (r = .30). the positive Beta-weight indicates that
students who
had taken more courses from NNSTAs generally had higher
comprehension
scores.
Discussion
Results o f Study 2 are consistent with those of Rubin and
Smith (1990). The
degree to which undergraduates perceive NNSTAs' accents to be
markedly for-
eign (and Study 1 confirms that those perceptions are liable to
be inaccurate)
undermines those students' evaluations of NNSTAs. On the
other hand, when
students believe that their instructors' attitudes are similar to
their own, they
more highly regard their instructors' teaching skills. In this
latter finding, the
present results confirm those of Elliot (1979), who likewise
found an associa-
tion between students' perceived homophily with their
instructors and class-
room outcomes.
Actual listening comprehension was not affected by any of the
attitudinal
variables, but was moderately related to undergraduates'
experience sitting in
classes taught by NNSTAs. Those students who are willing to
subject them-
selves to NNSTA-instructed classes apparently learn more than
just course con-
tent; they also learn how to listen more effectively.
Now it is possible that more advanced students simply have
better listening
skills, and as Table 5 suggests, there is some overlap (r = .30)
between num-
ber of classes taken with NNSTAs and students' overall
scholastic experience.
To examine this alternative explanation in a post hoc manner,
the regression for
listening comprehension scores was rerun, this time forcing
total number of
credits into the equation first. After doing so, number of
NNSTA classes re-
mains a significant predictor. The multiple correlation between
this two-predic-
tor model and comprehension was .34. This was only a modest
gain of .04 over
522 RUBIN
° J
.e
t ' l
° ~
J
[..,
~c
Z
<
>
<
<
I' I I
I I I
~'3 ~ ' , ~ t"xl ' , 0 r"- t ' ~
~ t ~ ~ C',I ~
I I I
I I
I
I
4
I
I
I I
. , ~ J
~ . _ ~ ~ ~.~,~ ~ ~
t ~
¢xl
V V
NNSTAs 523
TABLE 6. Summary of Stepwise Multivariate Regressions
Predicting Teacher
Ratings and Listening Comprehension
(Study 2)
Criterion Step Predictor R 2 Increment Beta F-to-Enter
Teacher ratings 1 Attitude homophily .09 .30 14.59
2 Perceived accent .02 - . 1 5 3.88
Listening 1 # NNSTA classes .09 .30 14.58
comprehension
the single-predictor model presented in Table 6 in which
number o f classes
taught by NNSTAs was the sole significant variable.
S T U D Y 3
The results o f Study 2 suggest that exposure o f North
American undergradu-
ates to NNSTAs is a good thing. The study, however, offers no
clues as to why
some undergraduates are willing to stick with a class taught by
a NNSTA,
while others of their cohorts are among the 40 percent o f
students that Rubin
and Smith (1990) found had avoided a NNSTA-instructed class
on at least one
occasion. But if students could gain some low-stress, or even
some ego-reward-
ing, exposure to NNSTAs, perhaps they would recognize higher
attitude homo-
phily with internationals. Perhaps such exposure might also
render perceived
differences in accent and in ethnicity less salient.
Study 3, therefore, undertook to pilot an intervention experience
in which
undergraduates were given structured experience interacting
with NNSTAs.
The goal o f the intervention was to cast the undergraduates in
a helping role
rather than in an adversarial role vis-a-vis their N N S T A
"partners." The litera-
ture on cross-cultural training is a rich source o f guidance for
structuring such
experiences. Gudykunst (1977), for example, notes that
intercultural contact b y
itself is no guarantee that attitudes between hostile groups will
ameliorate. In-
stead, successful intercultural contact must be facilitated by a
neutral party,
must level differences in status between parties, must be
informal, and must be
gratifying. When peers from diverse cultures work on c o m m o
n academic tasks
in small groups, improved ethnic relations are likely to prevail
(Sharan, 1980).
Indeed, a recent trend in N N S T A skills improvement
programs involves un-
dergraduates in various phases o f the process (e.g., Civikly
and Muchisky,
1991; Schneider and Stevens, 1991; v o m Saal and Sarwak,
1989). Some pro-
grams use undergraduates as assessors to help screen NNSTAs
for classroom
assignment, or to place them in developmental workshops.
Other programs
524 RUBIN
involve North American undergraduates as workshop trainers,
or to provide
feedback as classroom observers.
The deliberate goal of such programs is to improve NNSTA
communication
skills. According to anecdotal reports, however, a felicitously
incidental out-
come pertains to changes in undergraduates' attitudes.
Undergraduate partici-
pants in NNSTA training programs come to see themselves as
allies of the
internationals. And as campus opinion leaders, those
undergraduates directly
interacting with NNSTAs in these helping relationships can
propagate their pos-
itive attitudes among their peers.
Methods
Participants
Participants for this study were recruited from the same pool as
those in
Studies 1 and 2. Because this was a pilot effort, only a small
number of partici-
pants were secured. Complete sets of data were collected from
25 undergradu-
ates, 15 of whom were randomly assigned to the training
condition, with the
remaining 10 comprising a comparison group.
Procedures and Instruments
The training program was an attempt to model an intervention
that could be
implemented with large numbers of undergraduates. It was,
therefore, ad-
mittedly superficial. After pretesting, members of the training
group were
briefed about the importance of helping NNSTAs by giving
them feedback
from native English-speaking undergraduates. Throughout the
program, those
in the training group were encouraged to think of themselves as
NNSTAs'
allies and benefactors.
Each participant in the training program observed two classes
taught by
NNSTAs. The NNSTAs were current or former participants in
NNSTA instruc-
tional development workshops who expressed interest in
receiving additional
feedback from American students. All the NNSTAs with whom
the under-
graduates interacted were Asian. The undergraduate subjects
recorded their re-
actions to each class on a structured observation/evaluation
sheet that had previ-
ously been used in the NNSTA training workshops. Participants
and NNSTAs
met after each observed class. During these sessions
undergraduates explained
their recorded observation/evaluations to the NNSTAs in the
presence of a neu-
tral facilitator.
The two observation/discussion sessions were approximately
one week apart.
Posttesting occurred within three days o f the final session. No
special interven-
tion activities were arranged for comparison group members.
The time lag be-
tween pre- and posttest for comparison group subjects was
identical to the train-
ing group's.
NNSTAs 525
All subjects were pretested and posttested with audiotapes
developed first in
Rubin and Smith (1990). To minimize pretest/posttest
variability due to differ-
ences in test forms, all testing employed a single language
guise, the moderate
accent versions, prepared by the same native Cantonese speaker.
Accent, there-
fore, was not a variable in this study. Nor was ethnicity, since
subjects were
not shown any photograph of the NNSTA whom they were
evaluating. Topic
was also excluded as a variable in Study 3. About half of the
subjects in each
of the groups heard the science topic at pretest, and half heard
the humanities
topic. Topics were flip-flopped at posttest.
Measurement instruments were the same as those used in
Studies 1 and 2.
These yielded measures of (1) judged teaching quality, (2)
attitude homophily,
(3) value homophily, (4) background homophily, and (5)
appearance homo-
phily, as well as (6) a cloze test of listening comprehension.
Analysis
The six dependent measures were all subjected to a 2 x 2
MANOVA. Par-
ticipants were nested in treatment groups (experimental or
comparison) and
crossed with the repeated measure, time of testing (pretest or
posttest). A sig-
nificant interaction between group and time o f testing would be
necessary in
order to probe for effects of training.
Results
Cell means and standard deviations for the six dependent
variables appear in
Table 7. Table 8 contains the zero-order correlations among the
variables. The
2 x 2 MANOVA is summarized in Table 9. No statistically
significant main or
interaction effect emerged in this analysis.
TABLE 7. Cell Means and Standard Deviations for 6 Dependent
Variables
(Study 3)
Treatment: NNSTA "Mentoring" No NNSTA Intervention
(n = 15) (n = 10)
Time: Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Comprehension 9.73 (3.95) 9.67 (4.89) 10.40 (4.01) 10.70
(4.52)
Teaching qualifications 8.33 (1.84) 7.87 (1.68) 6.22 (3.07) 7.30
(1.57)
Attitude homophily 9.60 (4.26) 10.67 (3.92) 9.56 (3.64) 9.70
(4.32)
Background homophily 12.80 (1.74) 13.47 (2.53) 12.56 (2.35)
14.00 (3.16)
Values homophily 13.80 (3.84) 12.60 (2.85) 12.89 (5.23) 12.90
(4.23)
Appearance homophily 9.00 (4.94) 9.53 (4.81) 6.44 (2.79) 8.70
(3.37)
5 2 6 R U B I N
0 ~
0
>.
0
0
© p..,
I I
I I I I
I
I
r~ E.- < m > <
v
NNSTAs 527
TABLE 9. Treatment x Time of Testing MANOVA of 6
Dependent Variables
(Study 3)
Source of Variation df Wilk's Lambda F p
Treatment 6 0.722 1.09 .41
Time of testing 6 0.617 1.76 .17
Treatment x Time 6 0.839 0.54 .77
Error 17
Discussion and Conclusion
Previous studies had demonstrated the potency o f
undergraduates' own per-
ceptions and stereotypes in determining their response to
NNSTAs. Therefore,
an intervention program aimed at ameliorating undergraduates'
attitudes seems
one reasonable approach to reducing the communication
interference between
North American students and their international instructors. The
experimental
intervention tested in Study 3, however, was plainly not up to
the task. The
lack o f significant results was not just a matter o f low
statistical power due to a
small sample; cell means showed no consistent movement from
pretest to post-
test. Indeed, there is some anecdotal evidence (based on
undergraduates' feed-
back to the NNSTAs with whom they were paired) that students
in the training
group actually became more critical o f N N S T A s '
communication behaviors
during the course o f this experiment.
The treatment administered here was motivated by the intent to
cast under-
graduates into a sort o f mentoring role with respect to
NNSTAs, as opposed to
the antagonistic role they often assume in regular classroom
situations. But the
stereotypes and prejudices that this experimental program
sought to interdict are
powerful, indeed. Study 1 showed that the sight o f an Asian
visage was suffi-
cient to elicit among these students' perceptions o f foreign
accent, even though
the actual speech sample was produced by an expert speaker o f
SAE. Even
more revealing, when students were shown an Asian NNSTA,
their listening
comprehension scores were negatively impacted. Once again,
the speech in
each case was SAE.
Most likely the intervention attempted in Study 3 was o f too
short a duration
and too weak an intensity to bring about any profound
attitudinal change. Civ-
ikly and Muchisky (1991), however, describe a similar project
in which stu-
dents worked with NNSTAs for an entire term under apparently
more struc-
tured conditions. Their results, however, are likewise no cause
for celebration.
When asked what they had learned from participating in this
project, only 26
percent o f those undergraduates offered any comments that
indicated increased
intercultural sensitivity.
528 RUBIN
S U M M A R Y
Brislin (1981) notes that contact alone is surely not a potent
enough force to
change deeply held prejudices. Instead, changing intergroup
attitudes requires
an environment rich in informal and pleasant contact, some
degree of interper-
sonal intimacy and equality, and support from the participants'
own reference
groups. Future programs for improving undergraduate attitudes
toward NNSTAs
should incorporate these elements. Still, such programs will
require labor-inten-
sive and time-intensive efforts, and will not be practical for the
sort of large-
scale sensitization needed on college campuses. Principles for
engineering
large-scale attitude change in this particular intercultural
context have yet to be
articulated, much less tested.
Assuming that positive attitudes toward NNSTAs--or at least
open atti-
t u d e s - w i l l reduce attrition of NNSTA-taught classes,
there are very pragmatic
reasons to pursue any program that promises to improve those
attitudes. Ac-
cording to Rubin and Smith (1990), 42 percent of
undergraduates in one sample
had on at least one occasion (sometimes more) disenrolled in
one way or an-
other from classes they discovered were taught by NNSTAs.
Aside from the
logistical repercussions and the underutilization of teaching
talent this attrition
represents, the undergraduates themselves are poorly served by
their decisions.
Study 2 showed that the best predictor of listening
comprehension was the
number of NNSTA-taught classes one had attended. Those
undergraduates who
stuck with their NNSTAs were rewarded by achieving superior
communication
skills in the form of listening abilities.
Methodologically, each of the three studies reported here
follows in the tradi-
tion of much language and attitude research by employing the
matched guise
technique (Lambert et al., 1960) as a measurement device. This
has the advan-
tage of controlling for extraneous factors like speakers' vocal
characteristics. In
other studies, the matched guise technique also permits factorial
permutations
of language style with nonlanguage factors like ethnicity
(Brown, 1988; Rubin
and Smith, 1990) or school achievement or social class (Piche et
al., 1977). On
the other hand, the matched guise technique is vulnerable to the
criticism that it
requires artificial stimuli (e.g., usually denuded of gestural
communication)
and testing conditions (Robinson, 1972).
Certainly more studies are warranted in which undergraduates
rate NNSTAs
with whom they have shared an entire college term of study. In
one such study,
elements of NNSTAs' interpersonal styles affected judged
teaching competence
more than did NNSTAs' measured language proficiency (Dalle
and Inglis,
1989). The problem inherent in such in situ studies is that--even
when ratings
of North American teaching assistants can be contrasted with
NNSTA ratings
(e.g., Briggs and Hofer, 1991)--it is difficult to isolate elements
of idiosyncra-
tic teaching behaviors, language proficiency, engendered
stereotypes, or course
difficulty that might contribute to any differences found.
NNSTAs 529
It would be useful, in addition, to gather data about
undergraduates' reac-
tions to non-Asian NNSTAs. The present studies deliberately
narrowed their
purview to East Asian instructors. This seems a reasonable
methodological
choice because o f the rising influx o f university teachers
from those particular
ethnic groups. Quite possibly, the findings of these studies
would not general-
ize to more Western NNSTAs, say instructors from France or
Germany.
Finally, it would be worth replicating these studies with more
heterogenous
groups o f undergraduates. Nonnative English speakers were
deliberately ex-
cluded from the sample here. It is possible that their reactions
to NNSTAs are
considerably different from mainstream culture North
Americans. Moreover,
the geographic region in which this study was conducted has a
relatively low
proportion o f nonnative English speakers and o f Asians.
These students are
exposed to relatively little nonnative accented speech in their
daily affairs. Fur-
thermore, few o f these students had ever encountered a native
English-speaking
Asian-American instructor. No doubt stereotypical reactions
would be different
among students with a greater variety of cross-cultural
experience.
Acknowledgments. Kim A. Smith assisted in collecting data for
Studies 1 and 2,
Kimberly Lichner assisted in collecting data for Study 3 and in
data analysis, and Kath-
leen B. Smith assisted in collecting data for Study 3.
REFERENCES
Abraham, R. G., and Plakans, B. S. (1988). Evaluating a
screening/training program
for NNS Teaching Assistants. TESOL Quarterly 22: 505-508.
Althen, G. (1991). Teaching "culture" to international teaching
assistants. In J. Nyquist,
R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the
Professoriate of Tomor-
row to Teach (pp. 350-355). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Bacbrnan, L. F. (1985). Performance on cloze tests with fixed-
ratio and rational dele-
tions. TESOL QuarteHy 19: 535-556.
Bailey, K. M. (1984). The "foreign TA problem." In K. Bailey,
F. Pialorsi, and J.
Zukowski/Faust (eds.), Foreign Teaching Assistants in U.S.
Universities (pp. 3-15).
Washington, DC: National Association for Foreign Student
Affairs.
Briggs, S., and Hofer, B. (1991). Undergraduate perceptions of
ITA effectiveness. In J.
Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing
the Professoriate of
Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 435-445). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Brislin, R. W. (1981). Cross-Cultural Encounters. New York:
Pergamon.
Brown, K. (1988). American college student attitudes toward
non-native instructors.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.
(DAI 49:1710A-1711A).
Byrd, P., Constantinides, J. C., and Pennington, M. (1989). The
Foreign Teaching
Assistant's Manual. New York: Macmillan.
Civikly, J. M., and Muchisky, D. M. (1991). A collaborative
approach to ITA training:
The ITAs, faculty, TAs, undergraduate interns, and
undergraduate students. In J.
Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing
the Professoriate of
Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 356-360). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Constantinides, J. C. (March 1987). The "foreign TA problem"--
An update. NAFSA
Newsletter 38: 3, 5.
530 RUBIN
Dalle, T., and Inglis, M. (March 1989). What really affects
undergraduates' evaluations
of nonnative teaching assistants' teaching? Paper presented at
the TESOL Annual
Convention, San Antonio, Texas.
Dodd, C. (1986). Dynamics of Intercultural Communication
(2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA:
Wm. C. Brown.
Dunn, T. G., and Constantinides, J. C. (1991). Standardized test
scores and placement
of international teaching assistants. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D.
Wulff, and J.
Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate of Tomorrow to
Teach (pp. 413-419).
Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Edwards, J. R. (1982). Language attitudes and their implications
among English
speakers. In E. B. Ryan and H. Giles (eds.), Attitudes Towards
Language Variation
(pp. 20-33). London: Edward Arnold.
Elliot, S. (1979). Perceived homophily as a predictor of
classroom learning. In D.
Nimmo (ed.), Human Communication Yearbook 3 (pp. 585-
602). New Brunswick,
NJ: Transaction Books.
Gallego, J. C., Goodwin, J., and Turner, J. (1991). ITA Oral
Assessment: The exam-
inee's perspective. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J.
Sprague (eds.), Pre-
paring the Professoriate of Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 404-412).
Dubuque, IA: Ken-
dall-Hunt.
Gudykunst, W. (1977). Intercultural contact and attitude
change: A review of the litera-
ture and suggestions for future research. In N. Jain (ed.),
International and Inter-
cultural Communication Annual, vol. 4 (pp. 1-16). Falls Church,
VA: Speech Com-
munication Association.
Heller, S. (September 11, 1985). Colleges try tests and training
to make sure foreign
T.A.'s can be understood. Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. 1,
32, 33.
Hess, J. (November 2, 1987). Instructors' fluency in English
debated at University of
Florida. Atlanta Constitution, pp. 1, 9.
Instructors' broken English prompts Illinois law (October 4,
1987). Atlanta Journal and
Constitution.
Lambert, W., Hodgson, R., Gardner, R., and Fillenbaum, S.
(1960). Evaluational reac-
tions to spoken languages. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology 60: 44-51.
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., and Daly, J. A. (1975). The
development of a
measure of perceived homophily in interpersonal
communication. Human Communi-
cation Research 1: 321-332.
Norusis, M. J. (1990). SPSS-PC+ Advanced Statistics 4.0.
Chicago: SPSS, Inc.
Orth, J. L. (1982). University undergraduate evaluational
reactions to the speech of
foreign teaching assistants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
University of Texas.
(DAI (1983), 43: 12, 3897-A).
Pica, T., Barnes, G. A., and Finger, A. G. (1990). Teaching
Matters: Skills and Strate-
gies for International Teaching Assistants. New York: Newbury
House.
Piche, G. L., Michlin, M. L., Rubin, D. L., and Sullivan, A.
(1977). Effects of dialect-
ethnicity, social class, and quality of written composition on
teachers' subjective
judgments of students. Communication Monographs 44: 60-72.
Robinson, W. P. (1972). Language and Social Behavior.
Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin
Books.
Rubin, D. L., and Smith, K. A. (1990). Effects of accent,
ethnicity, and lecture topic
on undergraduates' perceptions of non-native English speaking
teaching assistants.
International Journal of lntercultural Relations 14: 337-353.
Schneider, K., and Stevens, S. G. (1991). American
undergraduate students as trainers
in an international teaching assistant training program. In J.
Nyquist, R. Abbott, D.
NNSTAs 531
Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate o f
Tomorrow to Teach (pp.
361-367). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Sharan, S. (1980). Cooperative learning in small groups: Recent
methods and effects on
achievement, attitudes, and ethnic relations. Review o f
Educational Research 50:
241-272.
Thomas, C. F., and Monoson, P. K. (1991). Issues related to
state-mandated English
language proficiency requirements. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D.
Wulff, and J.
Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate o f Tomorrow to
Teach (pp. 382-392).
Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt.
Turitz, N. J. (1984). A survey of training programs for foreign
teaching assistants in
American universities. In K. Bailey, F. Pialorsi, and J.
Zukowski/Faust (eds.), For-
eign Teaching Assistants in U.S. Universities (pp. 43-50).
Washington, DC: National
Association for Foreign Student Affairs.
Verhovek, S. H. (November 5, 1989). An American accent f o r
teachers from abroad.
New York Times, Section 4A, pp. 22-23.
vom Saal, D. R., and Sarwak, S. (November 1989).
Strengthening the ITA program
through the involvement of undergraduates. Paper presented at
the Second National
Conference on the Training and Employment of Teaching
Assistants, Seattle.
Whitla, D. K. (1968). Evaluation of decision making: A study of
college admissions. In
D. Whitla (ed.), Handbook o f Measurement and Assessment in
Behavioral Sciences
(pp. 456-490). Reading, MA; Addison Wesley.
Yule, G., and Hoffman, P. (1990). Predicting success for
international teaching assist-
ants in a U.S. university. TESOL Quarterly 24: 227-243.
Zikopoulos, M. (ed.) (1988). Open Doors 1987/88: Report on
International Educational
Exchange. New York: Institute of International Education.
Received February 27, 1991.
Language, Culture, and Power
Questions for the Author Assignment Instructions
This assignment consists of short writing exercises (about 300
words; not fewer than 250)
written in response to assigned texts and addressed to the
author. In each exercise, you will
formulate a question about something that confused you or
interested you while reading a
recently assigned text(s).
While the overall effect of each exercise should be to pose a
question, an adequate paper will
contain more than sentences ending with question marks. You
should explain how the question
occurred to you when reading, which will likely require quoting
or paraphrasing the text (and
citing page numbers). You should also attempt to answer the
question yourself, either by
speculating on what the author might answer or offering your
own perspective.
It may be helpful to imagine that you are having a conversation
with the author. Address the
author by beginning your submission as “Dear [author’s first
name]” and as “you,” express your
interest, and write the kinds of things you would say to start a
conversation. For example, you
might write: “I appreciate how you drew attention to the social
context of language by describing
your relationship to the speaker (p. 5), but I was wondering…”
The style of the paper should be only slightly more refined than
“freewriting,” which involves
writing continuously without pausing to correct mistakes or
organize ideas. Setting a timer for
5-10 minutes (depending on your writing speed), writing until
you run out of time, and then
submitting the assignment is a great way to complete these
assignments. In fact, it is the
preferred way. You might feel compelled to make revisions
after you finish writing. Minor
revisions are okay, but one of the purposes of this assignment is
to create an opportunity for you
to practice producing words—not editing, just producing. (That
said, please ensure that you
have checked your submission for spelling, syntax, and
punctuation.)
Another purpose is to create an opportunity for you to practice
reading both critically and
creatively. These papers are not to prove that you read the text.
They are conversations between
you and the author about ideas that you are incorporating into
your own thinking, and perhaps
into your own paper. In fact, as your paper develops, you are
encouraged to formulate questions
that relate to it directly.
Grading: Your grade for this assignment will be based on the
number of papers you submit. Each
paper is worth 1%, and you will receive the full 1% if you
complete the assignment as described
above. Submissions that indicate that you have not engaged the
text will not be given points.
You are permitted to submit more than ten of these QfA
exercises (i.e. you can get up to
two bonus points!)
Submission Instructions: Submit to the appropriate Moodle
topic before the beginning of class
on the day in which we will cover the reading you have
responded to. Late submissions will not
be accepted; Moodle will prevent submission of your Question
after the start of class.

More Related Content

Similar to Research in Higher Education, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1992 NONLANG.docx

Academic Literacy On Two Continents The Role Of Community Norms
Academic Literacy On Two Continents  The Role Of Community NormsAcademic Literacy On Two Continents  The Role Of Community Norms
Academic Literacy On Two Continents The Role Of Community NormsAlicia Buske
 
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...englishonecfl
 
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...englishonecfl
 
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdf
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdfApplied linguistics and language teacher education.pdf
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdfKarla Long
 
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert LensRemedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lensijtsrd
 
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...Dr. Seyed Hossein Fazeli
 
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesTeaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesNhinie Indasari
 
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnReciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnannes86
 
Ex Ed English- research paper
Ex Ed English- research paperEx Ed English- research paper
Ex Ed English- research paperPendarvis Ben
 
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course Program Model And Learner Out...
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course  Program Model And Learner Out...A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course  Program Model And Learner Out...
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course Program Model And Learner Out...Kimberly Pulley
 
Understanding Asian Students
Understanding Asian StudentsUnderstanding Asian Students
Understanding Asian StudentsArnau Cerdà
 
Strategies to teach english
Strategies to teach englishStrategies to teach english
Strategies to teach englishCarol1430
 
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfAnnaLizaTadeo1
 
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfAnnaLizaTadeo1
 
The influence of pronunciation learning
The influence of pronunciation learningThe influence of pronunciation learning
The influence of pronunciation learningJoyce Nguyen
 

Similar to Research in Higher Education, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1992 NONLANG.docx (20)

Kum
KumKum
Kum
 
Academic Literacy On Two Continents The Role Of Community Norms
Academic Literacy On Two Continents  The Role Of Community NormsAcademic Literacy On Two Continents  The Role Of Community Norms
Academic Literacy On Two Continents The Role Of Community Norms
 
Sla stages
Sla stagesSla stages
Sla stages
 
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...
Awareness Raising In The Tefl Phonology Classroom Student Voices And Sociocul...
 
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...
Primary Stress And Intelligibility Research To Motivate The Teaching Of Supra...
 
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdf
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdfApplied linguistics and language teacher education.pdf
Applied linguistics and language teacher education.pdf
 
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert LensRemedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
 
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...
Use of the metacognitive english language learning strategies based on person...
 
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tensesTeaching and learning english verb tenses
Teaching and learning english verb tenses
 
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughnReciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
Reciprocal teaching klingner and vaughn
 
Ex Ed English- research paper
Ex Ed English- research paperEx Ed English- research paper
Ex Ed English- research paper
 
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course Program Model And Learner Out...
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course  Program Model And Learner Out...A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course  Program Model And Learner Out...
A High School Intensive Summer Mandarin Course Program Model And Learner Out...
 
J0363057063
J0363057063J0363057063
J0363057063
 
Understanding Asian Students
Understanding Asian StudentsUnderstanding Asian Students
Understanding Asian Students
 
Strategies to teach english
Strategies to teach englishStrategies to teach english
Strategies to teach english
 
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
 
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdfACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
 
From Theory to Practice for Teachers of English Learners.docx
From Theory to Practice for Teachers of English Learners.docxFrom Theory to Practice for Teachers of English Learners.docx
From Theory to Practice for Teachers of English Learners.docx
 
MAdissertation
MAdissertationMAdissertation
MAdissertation
 
The influence of pronunciation learning
The influence of pronunciation learningThe influence of pronunciation learning
The influence of pronunciation learning
 

More from ronak56

According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docx
According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docxAccording to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docx
According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docxronak56
 
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docxAccording to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docxronak56
 
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docx
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docxAccording to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docx
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docxronak56
 
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docxAccording to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docxronak56
 
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docx
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docxAccording to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docx
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docxronak56
 
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docx
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docxAccording to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docx
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docxronak56
 
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docx
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docxAccording to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docx
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docxronak56
 
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docxAccording to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docxronak56
 
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docx
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docxAccording to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docx
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docxronak56
 
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docx
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docxAccording to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docx
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docxronak56
 
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docx
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docxAccording to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docx
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docxronak56
 
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docx
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docxAccording to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docx
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docxronak56
 
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docxAccording to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docxronak56
 
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docx
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docxAccording to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docx
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docxronak56
 
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docx
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docxAccording to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docx
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docxronak56
 
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docxronak56
 
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docx
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docxAccording to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docx
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docxronak56
 
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docxronak56
 
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docxronak56
 
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docx
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docxAccess the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docx
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docxronak56
 

More from ronak56 (20)

According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docx
According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docxAccording to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docx
According to the textbook, the Federal Disaster Assistance Act of 19.docx
 
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docxAccording to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5 Eng.docx
 
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docx
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docxAccording to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docx
According to the text, economic outcomes measured by economic gr.docx
 
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docxAccording to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docx
According to the Council on Social Work Education, Competency 5.docx
 
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docx
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docxAccording to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docx
According to the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC), part of.docx
 
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docx
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docxAccording to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docx
According to the article, Answer these two questions. Why did Ma.docx
 
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docx
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docxAccording to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docx
According to Neuman’s theory, a human being is a total person as a c.docx
 
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docxAccording to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is the pr.docx
 
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docx
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docxAccording to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docx
According to your readings, cloud computing represents one of th.docx
 
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docx
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docxAccording to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docx
According to this idea that gender is socially constructed, answer.docx
 
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docx
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docxAccording to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docx
According to Thiel (2015, p. 40), CSR literature lacks consensus fo.docx
 
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docx
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docxAccording to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docx
According to recent surveys, China, India, and the Philippines are t.docx
 
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docxAccording to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docx
According to Rolando et al. (2012), alcohol socialization is th.docx
 
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docx
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docxAccording to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docx
According to the author, Social Security is an essential program, .docx
 
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docx
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docxAccording to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docx
According to Morrish, the blame for the ever-growing problem of disc.docx
 
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), Cultural intelligence is an outsiders .docx
 
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docx
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docxAccording to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docx
According to Edgar Schein, organizational culture are the shared.docx
 
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics are .docx
 
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docxAccording to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docx
According to DuBrin (2015), the following strategies or tactics .docx
 
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docx
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docxAccess the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docx
Access the Mental Measurements Yearbook, located in the Univer.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceSamikshaHamane
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupJonathanParaisoCruz
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 

Recently uploaded (20)

ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 

Research in Higher Education, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1992 NONLANG.docx

  • 1. Research in Higher Education, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1992 NONLANGUAGE FACTORS AFFECTING UNDERGRADUATES' JUDGMENTS OF NONNATIVE ENGLISH-SPEAKING TEACHING ASSISTANTS Donald L Rubin • . . . . . , , . . . . . . • • . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . + . . . . . . . , . . . . • . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . , . , . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . • . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . In response to dramatic changes in the demographics of graduate education, consid- erable effort is being devoted to training teaching assistants who are nonnative speakers of English (NNSTAs). Three studies extend earlier research that showed the potency of nonlanguage factors such as ethnicity in affecting undergraduates' reactions to NNSTAs. Study 1 examined effects of instructor ethnicity, even when the instructor's language was completely standard. Study 2 identified predictors of teacher ratings and listening comprehension from among several attitudinal and background variables. Study 3 was a pilot intervention effort in which undergradu- ates served as teaching coaches for NNSTAs. This intervention,
  • 2. however, exerted no detectable effect on undergraduates' attitudes. Taken together, these findings warrant that intercultural sensitization for undergraduates must complement skills training for NNSTAs, but that this sensitization will not accrue from any superficial intervention program. . . . . . . o . . , . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . • . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . • Observers who have been even moderately sensitive to the voices o f North American college students in recent years can appreciate the depth o f under- graduates' concerns about the quality of instruction offered at many campuses by nonnative English-speaking teaching assistants (NNSTAs). The NNSTA "crisis" is one of relatively few instructional issues in higher education that has captured the attention of the popular press (Hess, 1987; Verhovek, 1989). In several states, legislatures have directly intervened in policies regarding NNSTAs (e.g., "Instructors' Broken English Prompts Illinois Law," 1987; Thomas and Monoson, 1991). To the thinking of the press and o f the policymakers--and probably also o f the undergraduate students--the root o f the problem is NNSTAs' poor English- language proficiency (Bailey, 1984). Accordingly, many universities have re-
  • 3. Donald L. Rubin, The University of Georgia. Address Correspondence to: Speech Communica- tion and Language Education, The University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602. 511 0 3 6 1 - 0 3 6 5 / 9 2 / 0 8 0 0 - 0 5 1 1 5 0 6 . 5 0 / 0 © 1 9 9 2 H u m a n S c i e n c e s P r e s s , I n c . 512 RUBtN sponded by erecting procedures for screening NNSTAs' language skills (see discussions of these tests in Abraham and Plakans, 1988; Dunn and Constan- tinides, 1991; Gallego, Goodwin, and Turner, 1991; Heller, 1985; Yule and Hoffman, 1990). Those deemed deficient are channeled into remediation pro- grams (Turitz, 1984). For the most part, these remedial workshops appear to focus on discrete language behaviors, especially pronunciation and use of id- ioms (Verhovek, 1989). This focus is not illogical, since many of the current groups of NNSTAs have had little exposure to English as a medium of oral communication. A more recent and educationally progressive approach com- bines language remediation with training in generic instructional communica- tion skills and in cultural norms associated with American undergraduates
  • 4. (e.g., Althen, 1991; Byrd, Constantinides, and Pennington, 1989; Pica, Barnes, and Finger, 1990). The common assumption of all these perspectives on NNSTAs is that it is the internationals who bear the onus for poor classroom communication, and there- fore it is the internationals who must undergo transformation in the North American mold. An alternative perspective, in contrast, recognizes that com- munication is a reciprocal process. If communication outcomes are poor in classes taught by NNSTAs, then perhaps the responsibility ought to be shared alike among NNSTAs and the American undergraduate student body. Scattered evidence warrants the plausibility of this alternative view, the view that the NNSTA "problem" is at least partially a problem of undergraduates' negative stereotyping. For example, Orth (1982) found that undergraduate stu- dents' ratings of their NNSTAs' speaking proficiency were only weakly related to expert ratings of the NNSTAs' language proficiency. Instead, undergraduate ratings of NNSTAs were biased by the grades they anticipated receiving from those NNSTAs. For many undergraduates, introductory courses in mathematics and the natural sciences have reputations as extremely inhumane courses de- signed to winnow out marginal students. And it is well known
  • 5. that NNSTAs are disproportionately assigned responsibility for such high- anxiety classes (Constantinides, 1987). In addition, with an increasing proportion of NNSTAs originating from East Asia (Zikopoulos, 1988), it is likely that powerful ethnic stereotypes also factor into negative perceptions of language proficiency and teaching ability (Brown, 1988). Rubin and Smith (1990) undertook to explore the role of NNSTAs' ethnicity, the subject matter about which they lectured, and their level of accentedness as determinants of undergraduates' responses. Using a matched guise technique typical of language and attitude research (see review of this body of research in Edwards, 1982), two native speakers of Cantonese each recorded highly ac- cented and moderately accented versions of simulated classroom lectures. One lecture was about a natural science topic (the uses and growing scarcity of helium), and the second lecture was about a humanities topic (the role of the NNSTAs 513 Mahabarata in Indian society). Undergraduate subjects heard one or the other of the lecture topics delivered with either of the levels of accent, accompanied by
  • 6. a photograph of either a European or an Asian instructor. Dependent measures included a cloze test of listening comprehension (Bach- man, 1985), ratings of teacher quality, and four subscales of a measure of homophily (McCroskey, Richmond, and Daly, 1975). Homophily is a construct that pertains to listeners' perceptions of similarity to message sources. Homo- phily is an important variable mediating cross-cultural communication (Dodd, 1986). For example, in the diffusion of innovations from one culture to an- other, people are more likely to accept new ideas from homophilous sources. By the same token, homophily has been found to be an important factor in classroom communication (Elliot, 1979). Students respond more positively to teachers of optimal homophily. In addition to these measures, subjects also replied to questions about their contact with NNSTAs. Rubin and Smith's (1990) results showed that the effects of accent were not explained by any simple response pattern. On the one hand, manipulated ac- centedness was not accurately discerned on a questionnaire item designed to check the effectiveness of the experimental manipulation; students couldn't al- ways discriminate between high and moderate levels of accent. On the other hand, manipulated accent did affect undergraduates' perceptions of NNSTAs'
  • 7. et h ni cit y--but only when other cues were ambiguous, that is, when the photo- graph was European and the topic pertained to humanities. Of greater significance, listeners' perceptions of the instructors' a c c e n t - - whether accurate perceptions or n o t - - w e r e the strongest predictors of teacher ratings. When students believed an instructor's accent to be "foreign," they simultaneously perceived him or her to be a poor teacher. Also of particular interest, the best predictor of undergraduates' listening comprehension scores was the number of courses they had taken that had been instructed by nonnative instructors. Those students who had persevered with their nonnative English- speaking instructors had been rewarded by improved skill in listening to ac- cented speech. On the basis of these findings, Rubin and Smith (1990) concluded that high effort/low results programs to remediate pronunciation ought not constitute the main investment of university resources. Instead, it makes sense to simul- taneously educate undergraduates about the nature of NNSTA speech, perhaps to provide instruction in listening to accented speech, and to facilitate the amount of exposure to NNSTAs experienced by undergraduates. The present article reports three studies that elaborate on and extend the work
  • 8. reported by Rubin and Smith (1990). In that study, speakers altered their de- gree o f accentedness, but they could not completely adopt a native-like Stand- ard American English (SAE) accent. Study 1, therefore, replicates Rubin and Smith, but eliminates accent as a factor. Instead, Study 1 asks undergraduates 514 RUBIN to rate SAE-speaking instructors o f varying ethnicity. Building on the espe- cially interesting correlational results in the 1990 study, Study 2 utilizes a mul- tiple regression analysis to predict teacher ratings and comprehension scores. Finally, Study 3 is a small-scale attempt to implement a training program for mitigating undergraduates' attitudes toward NNSTAs. S T U D Y 1 Pronunciation training is tedious and is rarely 100 percent effective. Even the most motivated and industrious student of a foreign language is likely to retain a vestige of nonnative pronunciation. North American undergraduates appar- ently are not very discerning in discriminating levels of accentedness. There- fore, it is possible that even those NNSTAs who have excelled in pronunciation drills may still suffer a high level of stigmatization by North
  • 9. American under- graduates. But what if it were possible to completely supplant a nonnative accent with SAE speech? Would even this unlikely achievement be sufficient to overcome negative ethnic stereotypes and attitudes toward subject matter? The purpose of Study 1 is to ascertain the effects of instructor ethnicity and of lecture subject matter when the instructor's actual language behavior is standard, and therefore not culpable as the source of potentially negative student evaluations. Methods Participants Undergraduates were recruited from basic speech communication classes at a large southeastern university. They participated in this study in lieu of a re- quired out-of-class assignment. Data collected from international students were omitted from analysis. Complete sets of usable data were obtained from 62 North American undergraduates. Procedures and Stimulus Materials Each subject listened to a single tape-recorded speech sample lasting about four minutes. The speech was presented as part of a lecture by a university "instructor." As subjects listened to the lecture, a slide
  • 10. photograph representing the instructor was projected on a screen. Instructor ethnicity was operationalized by projecting a photograph of either a Caucasian or an Asian (Chinese) woman. To avoid confounding ethnicity with physical attractiveness, both models were similarly dressed, were of simi- lar size and hair style, and were photographed in the same setting and pose (standing at a lecturn in front o f a chalkboard). No differences between the NNSTAs 515 photographs in rated physical attractiveness were found in any uses o f these photographs (although other factors like speech topic sometimes exerted signifi- cant effects on judgments of attractiveness). Lecture top ic--physi c a l science or humanities--was operationalized by using the same two scripts as in Rubin and Smith (1990). Both scripts were adapted from articles originally appearing in the New York Times. The topics of helium scarcity and of the Mahabarata were selected to represent science and humanities, respectively, because it was judged that most undergraduates would have relatively little prior knowledge about them. Both passages were
  • 11. edited to approximately 450 words, and both recorded speech samples were approximately four minutes long. They were recorded by a single speaker, a doctoral student in speech communication, a native speaker of English raised in central Ohio, who was well regarded by her own undergraduate students for especially effective and clear classroom delivery. Subjects were randomly assigned to one o f the resulting lecture topics (at two levels) by ethnicity (at two levels) treatment combinations. Complete sets of data were obtained from 13 participants in the Asian instructor/science topic group, 16 in the Caucasian instructor/science topic group, 17 in the Asian in- structor/humanities topic treatment, and 16 in the Caucasian instructor/human- ities topic treatment. Measurement and Analysis Immediately after hearing the lecture, subjects completed a cloze test of lis- tening comprehension. They were presented with a written text of the lecture with every seventh word deleted, save in the first sentence, which was kept intact. Only exact recall was scored as correct (see Bachman, 1985). Next, subjects completed the McCroskey et al. (1975) homophily instru- ment. The instrument is comprised of 16 semantic differential
  • 12. items. The items pose polar opposite descriptions at either end o f seven equal- appearing interval scales (e.g., "shares my values: : : : : : : :doesn't share my values"). The items were arranged on the questionnaire in random order and with randomly alter- nating polarities. The instrument was developed to yield four subscales, each of which is comprised o f four semantic differential items: (1) attitude homophily, (2) background homophily, (3) value homophily, and (4) appearance homo- phily. Interspersed among the homophily items were additional semantic differen- tial items checking the ethnicity manipulation ("Caucasian/European ethnici- t y : : : : : : : :Oriental/Asian ethnicity") and measuring impressions of accent ("Speaks with American accent: : : : : : : :Speakers with foreign accent") and of teaching competence ("Poor teacher: : : : : : : :Effective teacher"), as well as 516 RUBIN TABLE 1. Cell Means and Standard Deviations for 8 Dependent Variables (Study I) Humanities Science Lecture Topic: Instructor Ethnicity: Caucasian Asian Caucasian Asian
  • 13. N = 16 17 16 t3 Perceived accent 3.44 (2.56) 4.94 (1.98) 2.75 (2.44) 3.77 (1.88) Perceived ethnicity 2.75 (1.98) 5.53 (1.97) 2.06 (1,39) 6.23 (0.93) Comprehension 11.94 (4.34) 9.93 (5,70) 1 2 . 5 (5.9) 7.31 (4.70) Teaching qualifications 8.69 (3.61) 8.18 (t.81) 7.25 (2.46) 9.15 (2.73) Attitude homophily 5.56 (5.51) 6.00 (4.95) 2.38 (4.24) 3.38 (4.37) Background homophily 1 2 . 7 5 (4.12) 12.18 (4.03) 13.25 (4.48) 10.31 (3.54) Values homophily 15.88 (4.53) 14.94 (2.88) 14.81 (3.21) 14.46 (3.86) Appearance homophily 1 1 . 1 3 (5.04) 10.71 (5.77) 10.13 (4.77) 7.69 (3.22) several unrelated filler scales. In all, the instrument contained 28 semantic dif- ferential items. Dependent variables, then, were cloze test scores, the four homophily sub- scales, perceptions of accent, and ratings of teaching competence, as well as the manipulation check scale that registered perceptions o f ethnicity. The ma- nipulation check was subjected to a 2 x 2 A N O V A with subjects nested in levels of ethnicity (Caucasian, Asian) and lecture topic (science, humanities). The remaining dependent variables were submitted to an analogous 2 x 2 MANOVA. The regression solution, whereby each M A N O V
  • 14. A sum o f squares is adjusted for each of the others, was used to control potential nonorthog- onality due to unequal cell frequencies (Norusis, 1990). Since the M A N O V A revealed no interaction effect, the significant main effect was followed up with discriminant analysis to determine which combination o f dependent variables best discriminated among groups. Results Cell means and standard deviations for the eight dependent variables appear in Table 1. Table 2 shows their intercorrelations. The A N O V A for the manipu- lation check showed that manipulated ethnicity indeed exerted a main effect on perceived ethnicity (F1,ss = 66.91, p < .001). As expected, the Asian in- structor was perceived to be more Oriental/Asian than the Caucasian instructor (M,sia, = 5.79, Mcau = 2.41). Neither the topic factor nor the interaction between topic and ethnicity significantly affected students' perceptions o f in- structor ethnicity. Table 3 summarizes the M A N O V A for the seven other dependent variables. N N S T A s 5 1 7
  • 15. .= "F. < I I < I I 518 RUBIN TABLE 3. Multitopic X Ethnicity MANOVA of 7 Dependent Variables (Study 1) Source of Variation df Wilk's Lambda F D Topic 7 0.866 1.15 .35 Ethnicity 7 0.727 2.78 .02 Topic x Ethnicity 7 0.879 1.03 .42 Error 52 As it indicates, the only statistically significant effect to e m e r g e was a m a i n effect f o r instructor ethnicity ( W i l k ' s LambdaT,52 = .727, equivalent F = 2.78, p < .05). T o follow up this significant multivariate m a i n effect, a step-wise discrimi- nant analysis, s u m m a r i z e d in T a b l e 4, was p e r f o r m e d . C o m p r e h e n s i o n test
  • 16. score w a s the first variable to enter, b a s e d on the highest pooled within-group correlation ( - .63) with the cannonical discriminant function. Using c o m p r e - hension test scores alone, 61.2 percent o f participants could be correctly classi- fied into groups (in this case the grouping factor b e i n g w h e t h e r they v i e w e d an Asian instructor o r a C a u c a s i a n instructor). Inspection o f T a b l e 1 shows that c o m p r e h e n s i o n cell m e a n s were l o w e r f o r the groups e x p o s e d to an Asian vis- age, and higher f o r groups that saw a Caucasian instructor. T h e final discriminant m o d e l also included p e r c e i v e d accent, entered at step 2. Inspection o f cell m e a n s in T a b l e 1 shows that accent w a s p e r c e i v e d as m o r e foreign and less standard in the case o f the Asian instructor's photograph. An additional 9.8 p e r c e n t o f participants were correctly classified into t r e a t m e n t groups w h e n p e r c e i v e d accent was added to the m o d e l . Discussion T h e s e results p r o v i d e d r a m a t i c e v i d e n c e that North A m e r i c a n undergraduates are reacting to factors extraneous to just language proficiency w h e n they are TABLE 4. Stepwise Discriminant Analysis of Manipulated Ethnicity by 7 Predictor Variables (Study 1)
  • 17. Model Model Correlation with Step Variable Entered Wilk's Lambda F-to-Enter P-Value DiscriminantFunction 1 Comprehension .901 6.6t .01 - .631 2 Perceived accent .798 7.56 .001 .573 NNSTAs 519 asked to react to NNSTAs. In Study 1, only a single language variety was used: SAE. Still, when they were faced with an ethnically Asian instructor, partici- pants responded in the direction one would expect had they been listening to nonstandard speech. Evidence from the discriminant analysis suggests that par- ticipants stereotypically attributed accent differences-- differences that did not exist in t r u t h - - t o the instructors' speech. Yet more serious, listening compre- hension appeared to be undermined simply by identifying (visually) the instruc- tor as Asian. The pessimistic conclusion warranted here is that at least among this particu- lar sample of undergraduates even vigorous pronunciation training for NNSTAs will matter little. Ethnically Asian instructors who speak SAE apparently con- front similar dysfunctional attitudes as those who do speak with marked nonna-
  • 18. tive accents. S T U D Y 2 Those sympathetic to the plight of NNSTAs can derive some slight comfort from the results of Study 1: At least ratings of instructional competence were not significantly involved in discriminating between Asian and Caucasian teachers. Indeed, it is uncertain which attitudinal and experiential attributes of undergraduates do affect the way they rate NNSTAs' teaching effectiveness. Rubin and Smith (1990) found that perceived accentedness, but not actual ac- cent, was negatively related to teacher ratings. In addition, they found that actual experience attending classes taught by NNSTAs was positively related to accuracy in listening comprehension. It is reasonable to expect that homophily would also mediate teacher ratings such that students are less antagonistic toward NNSTAs whom they perceive as relatively similar to them. Consistent with the homophily hypothesis, Bailey (1984) reports that students sharing the same major as a NNSTA are likely to rate that NNSTA higher than one from a different academic area. The purpose o f Study 2, accordingly, was to determine the relative contribu- tions o f factors that do predict undergraduates' ratings of
  • 19. NNSTAs and their comprehension of NNSTA speech. Based on the earlier studies, it was pre- sumed that it would be fruitful to locate these factors in undergraduates' own attitudinal systems and experience. Methods Participants Participants were drawn from the same undergraduate pool as in Study 1. Again, data from international students were excluded. Usable sets of data were collected from a total of 148 students over the course of four academic terms. 520 RUBIN Procedures and Stimulus Materials To best generalize the results of the regression analysis used in this phase of the research, a wide range of stimulus conditions (i.e., nonfactorial) was desir- able. Each subject was nonsystematically assigned to one of the ten stimulus audio tapes developed either in conjunction with Study 1, above, or in conjunc- tion with Rubin and Smith (1990). That is, each subject listened to either high or moderate Chinese accent (produced by one or another of the two native
  • 20. Chinese speakers), or else to SAE accent, in combination with either the sci- ence or the humanities tape. About half the participants listened to their simu- lated lectures in conjunction with a photographic depiction of an Asian teaching assistant. About one-third listened to the lectures while viewing a Caucasian/ European face, while one-sixth merely listened to the lectures without any pho- tographic presentation. Measurement and Analysis Subjects in this study responded to the same questionnaire prepared for Study 1. This included items comprising the four homophily subscales, and items measuring perceptions of accent, ethnicity, and teaching quality. In addition, a "background questionnaire" asked students to report (among other items not under consideration here) the number of classes in which they had been in- structed by NNSTAs and the number of weeks they had traveled outside the United States and attitudes toward nonnative speakers in general. This back- ground questionnaire also included six Likert-type items devised to measure general attitudes toward normative English speakers. For each of these Likert- type items, participants read attitude statements and then responded in one of five categories ranging from "strongly agree" to "strongly disagree." Three of
  • 21. these attitude statements pertained to interpersonal relations with international students (e.g., "I would be willing to have a nonnative English speaker as my roommate"). The others pertained to benefits that internationals contribute to the university campus (e.g., "Our campus benefits by having nonnative English speakers attending"). Two step-wise regressions were run. The predictor variables in both analyses were identical: (1) perceived accent, (2) perceived ethnicity, (3) attitude homo- phily, (4) value homophily, (5) background homophily, (6) appearance homo- phily, (7) total college credits, (8) number of classes instructed by NNSTAs, (9) a composite scale of attitudes toward interpersonal relations with nonnative English speakers, and (10) a composite scale of attitudes toward the overall contribution of internationals. In one regression, the criterion variable was rated teacher qualification. In the second regression, the criterion variable was listen- ing comprehension (cloze) score. For the purposes of the step- wise procedure, the level of significance for F-to-enter and F-to-exit was set at P < . 05. NNSTAs 521 Results
  • 22. Table 5 contains the zero-order correlation coefficients among the 10 predic- tor variables and between each predictor and the 2 criterion variables. Table 6 summarizes the final results of the step-wise regressions for judged teacher qualification and for listening comprehension. The total R 2 for the first model indicates that it accounted for 11.6 percent o f the variance in teacher ratings (multiple r = .34). Inspection of the Beta-weights indicates that perceived accent was inversely related to teacher ratings (i.e., the more foreign the accent was judged, the lower the rating of teacher effectiveness), while attitude homo- phily was positively related to teacher ratings. For the multiple regression of listening comprehension scores, only a single variable, number o f courses students had taken from NNSTAs, was a statis- tically significant predictor. It accounted for 9.1 percent of the variance in comprehension (r = .30). the positive Beta-weight indicates that students who had taken more courses from NNSTAs generally had higher comprehension scores. Discussion Results o f Study 2 are consistent with those of Rubin and Smith (1990). The degree to which undergraduates perceive NNSTAs' accents to be
  • 23. markedly for- eign (and Study 1 confirms that those perceptions are liable to be inaccurate) undermines those students' evaluations of NNSTAs. On the other hand, when students believe that their instructors' attitudes are similar to their own, they more highly regard their instructors' teaching skills. In this latter finding, the present results confirm those of Elliot (1979), who likewise found an associa- tion between students' perceived homophily with their instructors and class- room outcomes. Actual listening comprehension was not affected by any of the attitudinal variables, but was moderately related to undergraduates' experience sitting in classes taught by NNSTAs. Those students who are willing to subject them- selves to NNSTA-instructed classes apparently learn more than just course con- tent; they also learn how to listen more effectively. Now it is possible that more advanced students simply have better listening skills, and as Table 5 suggests, there is some overlap (r = .30) between num- ber of classes taken with NNSTAs and students' overall scholastic experience. To examine this alternative explanation in a post hoc manner, the regression for listening comprehension scores was rerun, this time forcing total number of credits into the equation first. After doing so, number of
  • 24. NNSTA classes re- mains a significant predictor. The multiple correlation between this two-predic- tor model and comprehension was .34. This was only a modest gain of .04 over 522 RUBIN ° J .e t ' l ° ~ J [.., ~c Z < > < < I' I I
  • 25. I I I ~'3 ~ ' , ~ t"xl ' , 0 r"- t ' ~ ~ t ~ ~ C',I ~ I I I I I I I 4 I I I I . , ~ J ~ . _ ~ ~ ~.~,~ ~ ~ t ~ ¢xl V V NNSTAs 523 TABLE 6. Summary of Stepwise Multivariate Regressions Predicting Teacher
  • 26. Ratings and Listening Comprehension (Study 2) Criterion Step Predictor R 2 Increment Beta F-to-Enter Teacher ratings 1 Attitude homophily .09 .30 14.59 2 Perceived accent .02 - . 1 5 3.88 Listening 1 # NNSTA classes .09 .30 14.58 comprehension the single-predictor model presented in Table 6 in which number o f classes taught by NNSTAs was the sole significant variable. S T U D Y 3 The results o f Study 2 suggest that exposure o f North American undergradu- ates to NNSTAs is a good thing. The study, however, offers no clues as to why some undergraduates are willing to stick with a class taught by a NNSTA, while others of their cohorts are among the 40 percent o f students that Rubin and Smith (1990) found had avoided a NNSTA-instructed class on at least one occasion. But if students could gain some low-stress, or even some ego-reward- ing, exposure to NNSTAs, perhaps they would recognize higher attitude homo- phily with internationals. Perhaps such exposure might also render perceived differences in accent and in ethnicity less salient.
  • 27. Study 3, therefore, undertook to pilot an intervention experience in which undergraduates were given structured experience interacting with NNSTAs. The goal o f the intervention was to cast the undergraduates in a helping role rather than in an adversarial role vis-a-vis their N N S T A "partners." The litera- ture on cross-cultural training is a rich source o f guidance for structuring such experiences. Gudykunst (1977), for example, notes that intercultural contact b y itself is no guarantee that attitudes between hostile groups will ameliorate. In- stead, successful intercultural contact must be facilitated by a neutral party, must level differences in status between parties, must be informal, and must be gratifying. When peers from diverse cultures work on c o m m o n academic tasks in small groups, improved ethnic relations are likely to prevail (Sharan, 1980). Indeed, a recent trend in N N S T A skills improvement programs involves un- dergraduates in various phases o f the process (e.g., Civikly and Muchisky, 1991; Schneider and Stevens, 1991; v o m Saal and Sarwak, 1989). Some pro- grams use undergraduates as assessors to help screen NNSTAs for classroom assignment, or to place them in developmental workshops. Other programs
  • 28. 524 RUBIN involve North American undergraduates as workshop trainers, or to provide feedback as classroom observers. The deliberate goal of such programs is to improve NNSTA communication skills. According to anecdotal reports, however, a felicitously incidental out- come pertains to changes in undergraduates' attitudes. Undergraduate partici- pants in NNSTA training programs come to see themselves as allies of the internationals. And as campus opinion leaders, those undergraduates directly interacting with NNSTAs in these helping relationships can propagate their pos- itive attitudes among their peers. Methods Participants Participants for this study were recruited from the same pool as those in Studies 1 and 2. Because this was a pilot effort, only a small number of partici- pants were secured. Complete sets of data were collected from 25 undergradu- ates, 15 of whom were randomly assigned to the training condition, with the remaining 10 comprising a comparison group. Procedures and Instruments The training program was an attempt to model an intervention
  • 29. that could be implemented with large numbers of undergraduates. It was, therefore, ad- mittedly superficial. After pretesting, members of the training group were briefed about the importance of helping NNSTAs by giving them feedback from native English-speaking undergraduates. Throughout the program, those in the training group were encouraged to think of themselves as NNSTAs' allies and benefactors. Each participant in the training program observed two classes taught by NNSTAs. The NNSTAs were current or former participants in NNSTA instruc- tional development workshops who expressed interest in receiving additional feedback from American students. All the NNSTAs with whom the under- graduates interacted were Asian. The undergraduate subjects recorded their re- actions to each class on a structured observation/evaluation sheet that had previ- ously been used in the NNSTA training workshops. Participants and NNSTAs met after each observed class. During these sessions undergraduates explained their recorded observation/evaluations to the NNSTAs in the presence of a neu- tral facilitator. The two observation/discussion sessions were approximately one week apart. Posttesting occurred within three days o f the final session. No
  • 30. special interven- tion activities were arranged for comparison group members. The time lag be- tween pre- and posttest for comparison group subjects was identical to the train- ing group's. NNSTAs 525 All subjects were pretested and posttested with audiotapes developed first in Rubin and Smith (1990). To minimize pretest/posttest variability due to differ- ences in test forms, all testing employed a single language guise, the moderate accent versions, prepared by the same native Cantonese speaker. Accent, there- fore, was not a variable in this study. Nor was ethnicity, since subjects were not shown any photograph of the NNSTA whom they were evaluating. Topic was also excluded as a variable in Study 3. About half of the subjects in each of the groups heard the science topic at pretest, and half heard the humanities topic. Topics were flip-flopped at posttest. Measurement instruments were the same as those used in Studies 1 and 2. These yielded measures of (1) judged teaching quality, (2) attitude homophily, (3) value homophily, (4) background homophily, and (5) appearance homo- phily, as well as (6) a cloze test of listening comprehension.
  • 31. Analysis The six dependent measures were all subjected to a 2 x 2 MANOVA. Par- ticipants were nested in treatment groups (experimental or comparison) and crossed with the repeated measure, time of testing (pretest or posttest). A sig- nificant interaction between group and time o f testing would be necessary in order to probe for effects of training. Results Cell means and standard deviations for the six dependent variables appear in Table 7. Table 8 contains the zero-order correlations among the variables. The 2 x 2 MANOVA is summarized in Table 9. No statistically significant main or interaction effect emerged in this analysis. TABLE 7. Cell Means and Standard Deviations for 6 Dependent Variables (Study 3) Treatment: NNSTA "Mentoring" No NNSTA Intervention (n = 15) (n = 10) Time: Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest Comprehension 9.73 (3.95) 9.67 (4.89) 10.40 (4.01) 10.70 (4.52) Teaching qualifications 8.33 (1.84) 7.87 (1.68) 6.22 (3.07) 7.30 (1.57) Attitude homophily 9.60 (4.26) 10.67 (3.92) 9.56 (3.64) 9.70
  • 32. (4.32) Background homophily 12.80 (1.74) 13.47 (2.53) 12.56 (2.35) 14.00 (3.16) Values homophily 13.80 (3.84) 12.60 (2.85) 12.89 (5.23) 12.90 (4.23) Appearance homophily 9.00 (4.94) 9.53 (4.81) 6.44 (2.79) 8.70 (3.37) 5 2 6 R U B I N 0 ~ 0 >. 0 0 © p.., I I I I I I I I r~ E.- < m > < v
  • 33. NNSTAs 527 TABLE 9. Treatment x Time of Testing MANOVA of 6 Dependent Variables (Study 3) Source of Variation df Wilk's Lambda F p Treatment 6 0.722 1.09 .41 Time of testing 6 0.617 1.76 .17 Treatment x Time 6 0.839 0.54 .77 Error 17 Discussion and Conclusion Previous studies had demonstrated the potency o f undergraduates' own per- ceptions and stereotypes in determining their response to NNSTAs. Therefore, an intervention program aimed at ameliorating undergraduates' attitudes seems one reasonable approach to reducing the communication interference between North American students and their international instructors. The experimental intervention tested in Study 3, however, was plainly not up to the task. The lack o f significant results was not just a matter o f low statistical power due to a small sample; cell means showed no consistent movement from pretest to post- test. Indeed, there is some anecdotal evidence (based on undergraduates' feed- back to the NNSTAs with whom they were paired) that students in the training group actually became more critical o f N N S T A s '
  • 34. communication behaviors during the course o f this experiment. The treatment administered here was motivated by the intent to cast under- graduates into a sort o f mentoring role with respect to NNSTAs, as opposed to the antagonistic role they often assume in regular classroom situations. But the stereotypes and prejudices that this experimental program sought to interdict are powerful, indeed. Study 1 showed that the sight o f an Asian visage was suffi- cient to elicit among these students' perceptions o f foreign accent, even though the actual speech sample was produced by an expert speaker o f SAE. Even more revealing, when students were shown an Asian NNSTA, their listening comprehension scores were negatively impacted. Once again, the speech in each case was SAE. Most likely the intervention attempted in Study 3 was o f too short a duration and too weak an intensity to bring about any profound attitudinal change. Civ- ikly and Muchisky (1991), however, describe a similar project in which stu- dents worked with NNSTAs for an entire term under apparently more struc- tured conditions. Their results, however, are likewise no cause for celebration. When asked what they had learned from participating in this project, only 26 percent o f those undergraduates offered any comments that
  • 35. indicated increased intercultural sensitivity. 528 RUBIN S U M M A R Y Brislin (1981) notes that contact alone is surely not a potent enough force to change deeply held prejudices. Instead, changing intergroup attitudes requires an environment rich in informal and pleasant contact, some degree of interper- sonal intimacy and equality, and support from the participants' own reference groups. Future programs for improving undergraduate attitudes toward NNSTAs should incorporate these elements. Still, such programs will require labor-inten- sive and time-intensive efforts, and will not be practical for the sort of large- scale sensitization needed on college campuses. Principles for engineering large-scale attitude change in this particular intercultural context have yet to be articulated, much less tested. Assuming that positive attitudes toward NNSTAs--or at least open atti- t u d e s - w i l l reduce attrition of NNSTA-taught classes, there are very pragmatic reasons to pursue any program that promises to improve those attitudes. Ac- cording to Rubin and Smith (1990), 42 percent of
  • 36. undergraduates in one sample had on at least one occasion (sometimes more) disenrolled in one way or an- other from classes they discovered were taught by NNSTAs. Aside from the logistical repercussions and the underutilization of teaching talent this attrition represents, the undergraduates themselves are poorly served by their decisions. Study 2 showed that the best predictor of listening comprehension was the number of NNSTA-taught classes one had attended. Those undergraduates who stuck with their NNSTAs were rewarded by achieving superior communication skills in the form of listening abilities. Methodologically, each of the three studies reported here follows in the tradi- tion of much language and attitude research by employing the matched guise technique (Lambert et al., 1960) as a measurement device. This has the advan- tage of controlling for extraneous factors like speakers' vocal characteristics. In other studies, the matched guise technique also permits factorial permutations of language style with nonlanguage factors like ethnicity (Brown, 1988; Rubin and Smith, 1990) or school achievement or social class (Piche et al., 1977). On the other hand, the matched guise technique is vulnerable to the criticism that it requires artificial stimuli (e.g., usually denuded of gestural communication) and testing conditions (Robinson, 1972).
  • 37. Certainly more studies are warranted in which undergraduates rate NNSTAs with whom they have shared an entire college term of study. In one such study, elements of NNSTAs' interpersonal styles affected judged teaching competence more than did NNSTAs' measured language proficiency (Dalle and Inglis, 1989). The problem inherent in such in situ studies is that--even when ratings of North American teaching assistants can be contrasted with NNSTA ratings (e.g., Briggs and Hofer, 1991)--it is difficult to isolate elements of idiosyncra- tic teaching behaviors, language proficiency, engendered stereotypes, or course difficulty that might contribute to any differences found. NNSTAs 529 It would be useful, in addition, to gather data about undergraduates' reac- tions to non-Asian NNSTAs. The present studies deliberately narrowed their purview to East Asian instructors. This seems a reasonable methodological choice because o f the rising influx o f university teachers from those particular ethnic groups. Quite possibly, the findings of these studies would not general- ize to more Western NNSTAs, say instructors from France or Germany.
  • 38. Finally, it would be worth replicating these studies with more heterogenous groups o f undergraduates. Nonnative English speakers were deliberately ex- cluded from the sample here. It is possible that their reactions to NNSTAs are considerably different from mainstream culture North Americans. Moreover, the geographic region in which this study was conducted has a relatively low proportion o f nonnative English speakers and o f Asians. These students are exposed to relatively little nonnative accented speech in their daily affairs. Fur- thermore, few o f these students had ever encountered a native English-speaking Asian-American instructor. No doubt stereotypical reactions would be different among students with a greater variety of cross-cultural experience. Acknowledgments. Kim A. Smith assisted in collecting data for Studies 1 and 2, Kimberly Lichner assisted in collecting data for Study 3 and in data analysis, and Kath- leen B. Smith assisted in collecting data for Study 3. REFERENCES Abraham, R. G., and Plakans, B. S. (1988). Evaluating a screening/training program for NNS Teaching Assistants. TESOL Quarterly 22: 505-508. Althen, G. (1991). Teaching "culture" to international teaching assistants. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the
  • 39. Professoriate of Tomor- row to Teach (pp. 350-355). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Bacbrnan, L. F. (1985). Performance on cloze tests with fixed- ratio and rational dele- tions. TESOL QuarteHy 19: 535-556. Bailey, K. M. (1984). The "foreign TA problem." In K. Bailey, F. Pialorsi, and J. Zukowski/Faust (eds.), Foreign Teaching Assistants in U.S. Universities (pp. 3-15). Washington, DC: National Association for Foreign Student Affairs. Briggs, S., and Hofer, B. (1991). Undergraduate perceptions of ITA effectiveness. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate of Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 435-445). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Brislin, R. W. (1981). Cross-Cultural Encounters. New York: Pergamon. Brown, K. (1988). American college student attitudes toward non-native instructors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota. (DAI 49:1710A-1711A). Byrd, P., Constantinides, J. C., and Pennington, M. (1989). The Foreign Teaching Assistant's Manual. New York: Macmillan. Civikly, J. M., and Muchisky, D. M. (1991). A collaborative approach to ITA training: The ITAs, faculty, TAs, undergraduate interns, and undergraduate students. In J.
  • 40. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate of Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 356-360). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Constantinides, J. C. (March 1987). The "foreign TA problem"-- An update. NAFSA Newsletter 38: 3, 5. 530 RUBIN Dalle, T., and Inglis, M. (March 1989). What really affects undergraduates' evaluations of nonnative teaching assistants' teaching? Paper presented at the TESOL Annual Convention, San Antonio, Texas. Dodd, C. (1986). Dynamics of Intercultural Communication (2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. Dunn, T. G., and Constantinides, J. C. (1991). Standardized test scores and placement of international teaching assistants. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate of Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 413-419). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Edwards, J. R. (1982). Language attitudes and their implications among English speakers. In E. B. Ryan and H. Giles (eds.), Attitudes Towards Language Variation (pp. 20-33). London: Edward Arnold.
  • 41. Elliot, S. (1979). Perceived homophily as a predictor of classroom learning. In D. Nimmo (ed.), Human Communication Yearbook 3 (pp. 585- 602). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. Gallego, J. C., Goodwin, J., and Turner, J. (1991). ITA Oral Assessment: The exam- inee's perspective. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Pre- paring the Professoriate of Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 404-412). Dubuque, IA: Ken- dall-Hunt. Gudykunst, W. (1977). Intercultural contact and attitude change: A review of the litera- ture and suggestions for future research. In N. Jain (ed.), International and Inter- cultural Communication Annual, vol. 4 (pp. 1-16). Falls Church, VA: Speech Com- munication Association. Heller, S. (September 11, 1985). Colleges try tests and training to make sure foreign T.A.'s can be understood. Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. 1, 32, 33. Hess, J. (November 2, 1987). Instructors' fluency in English debated at University of Florida. Atlanta Constitution, pp. 1, 9. Instructors' broken English prompts Illinois law (October 4, 1987). Atlanta Journal and Constitution. Lambert, W., Hodgson, R., Gardner, R., and Fillenbaum, S.
  • 42. (1960). Evaluational reac- tions to spoken languages. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60: 44-51. McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., and Daly, J. A. (1975). The development of a measure of perceived homophily in interpersonal communication. Human Communi- cation Research 1: 321-332. Norusis, M. J. (1990). SPSS-PC+ Advanced Statistics 4.0. Chicago: SPSS, Inc. Orth, J. L. (1982). University undergraduate evaluational reactions to the speech of foreign teaching assistants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas. (DAI (1983), 43: 12, 3897-A). Pica, T., Barnes, G. A., and Finger, A. G. (1990). Teaching Matters: Skills and Strate- gies for International Teaching Assistants. New York: Newbury House. Piche, G. L., Michlin, M. L., Rubin, D. L., and Sullivan, A. (1977). Effects of dialect- ethnicity, social class, and quality of written composition on teachers' subjective judgments of students. Communication Monographs 44: 60-72. Robinson, W. P. (1972). Language and Social Behavior. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin Books. Rubin, D. L., and Smith, K. A. (1990). Effects of accent, ethnicity, and lecture topic
  • 43. on undergraduates' perceptions of non-native English speaking teaching assistants. International Journal of lntercultural Relations 14: 337-353. Schneider, K., and Stevens, S. G. (1991). American undergraduate students as trainers in an international teaching assistant training program. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. NNSTAs 531 Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate o f Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 361-367). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Sharan, S. (1980). Cooperative learning in small groups: Recent methods and effects on achievement, attitudes, and ethnic relations. Review o f Educational Research 50: 241-272. Thomas, C. F., and Monoson, P. K. (1991). Issues related to state-mandated English language proficiency requirements. In J. Nyquist, R. Abbott, D. Wulff, and J. Sprague (eds.), Preparing the Professoriate o f Tomorrow to Teach (pp. 382-392). Dubuque, IA: Kendall-Hunt. Turitz, N. J. (1984). A survey of training programs for foreign teaching assistants in American universities. In K. Bailey, F. Pialorsi, and J. Zukowski/Faust (eds.), For- eign Teaching Assistants in U.S. Universities (pp. 43-50).
  • 44. Washington, DC: National Association for Foreign Student Affairs. Verhovek, S. H. (November 5, 1989). An American accent f o r teachers from abroad. New York Times, Section 4A, pp. 22-23. vom Saal, D. R., and Sarwak, S. (November 1989). Strengthening the ITA program through the involvement of undergraduates. Paper presented at the Second National Conference on the Training and Employment of Teaching Assistants, Seattle. Whitla, D. K. (1968). Evaluation of decision making: A study of college admissions. In D. Whitla (ed.), Handbook o f Measurement and Assessment in Behavioral Sciences (pp. 456-490). Reading, MA; Addison Wesley. Yule, G., and Hoffman, P. (1990). Predicting success for international teaching assist- ants in a U.S. university. TESOL Quarterly 24: 227-243. Zikopoulos, M. (ed.) (1988). Open Doors 1987/88: Report on International Educational Exchange. New York: Institute of International Education. Received February 27, 1991. Language, Culture, and Power Questions for the Author Assignment Instructions
  • 45. This assignment consists of short writing exercises (about 300 words; not fewer than 250) written in response to assigned texts and addressed to the author. In each exercise, you will formulate a question about something that confused you or interested you while reading a recently assigned text(s). While the overall effect of each exercise should be to pose a question, an adequate paper will contain more than sentences ending with question marks. You should explain how the question occurred to you when reading, which will likely require quoting or paraphrasing the text (and citing page numbers). You should also attempt to answer the question yourself, either by speculating on what the author might answer or offering your own perspective. It may be helpful to imagine that you are having a conversation with the author. Address the author by beginning your submission as “Dear [author’s first name]” and as “you,” express your interest, and write the kinds of things you would say to start a conversation. For example, you might write: “I appreciate how you drew attention to the social context of language by describing your relationship to the speaker (p. 5), but I was wondering…” The style of the paper should be only slightly more refined than “freewriting,” which involves writing continuously without pausing to correct mistakes or organize ideas. Setting a timer for 5-10 minutes (depending on your writing speed), writing until you run out of time, and then
  • 46. submitting the assignment is a great way to complete these assignments. In fact, it is the preferred way. You might feel compelled to make revisions after you finish writing. Minor revisions are okay, but one of the purposes of this assignment is to create an opportunity for you to practice producing words—not editing, just producing. (That said, please ensure that you have checked your submission for spelling, syntax, and punctuation.) Another purpose is to create an opportunity for you to practice reading both critically and creatively. These papers are not to prove that you read the text. They are conversations between you and the author about ideas that you are incorporating into your own thinking, and perhaps into your own paper. In fact, as your paper develops, you are encouraged to formulate questions that relate to it directly. Grading: Your grade for this assignment will be based on the number of papers you submit. Each paper is worth 1%, and you will receive the full 1% if you complete the assignment as described above. Submissions that indicate that you have not engaged the text will not be given points. You are permitted to submit more than ten of these QfA exercises (i.e. you can get up to two bonus points!) Submission Instructions: Submit to the appropriate Moodle topic before the beginning of class on the day in which we will cover the reading you have responded to. Late submissions will not be accepted; Moodle will prevent submission of your Question
  • 47. after the start of class.