SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 61
Studying School Shootings: Challenges
and Considerations for Research
H. Jaymi Elsass1 & Jaclyn Schildkraut2 &
Mark C. Stafford1
Received: 4 June 2015 /Accepted: 19 October 2015 /
Published online: 29 October 2015
# Southern Criminal Justice Association 2015
Abstract Studying school shootings can be both a fruitful and
challenging endeavor.
The random nature of these events provides a number of
challenges for studying this
phenomenon. This paper explores these concerns as they relate
to developing and
implementing studies, as well as interpreting related findings by
drawing on previous
research that examined the effects of the 1999 Columbine High
School, the 2007
Virginia Tech, and the 2008 Northern Illinois University
shootings. Ways in which
these issues may be overcome and, more generally, the research
can be moved forward
also are discussed.
Keywords School shootings . Research methodologies .
Theoretical orientation .
Columbine
Beginning in the mid-1990s, culminating with the 1999 shooting
at Columbine High
School, and reiterated through more recent events, including
Virginia Tech (2007) and
Sandy Hook (2012), school shootings have become
characterized by many as a social
problem (Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut et al. 2015; see also
BWashington Post-ABC
News Poll^ 2015). Each event is intentional, designed to cause
numerous deaths, and is
highly publicized, particularly through the media (Addington
2003; Elsass et al. 2014;
Schildkraut 2014; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014). These
events also have the poten-
tial to affect people who are both local and spatially distant
from the shooting
(Addington 2003). Given their vast reach, coupled with varied
responses by groups
within society (see, for example, Schildkraut and Hernandez
2014), understanding the
associated impacts of school shootings is particularly important.
Researchers have made strides in examining the effects of
specific school shooting
events, including Columbine (Addington 2003; Brener et al.
2002; Stretesky and
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
DOI 10.1007/s12103-015-9311-9
* H. Jaymi Elsass
[email protected]
1 Texas State University, 601 University Drive, San Marcos, TX
78666, USA
2 State University of New York at Oswego, Oswego, NY, USA
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s12103-015-
9311-9&domain=pdf
Hogan 2001) and Virginia Tech and Northern Illinois University
(NIU) in 2008
(Kaminski et al. 2010; see also Fallahi et al. 2009). Each of
these studies makes an
important contribution to the limited, yet growing, body of
literature on school
shootings. While each has yielded findings that prompt further
inquiry, these studies
also share a number of limitations that require caution in the
interpretation of their
results. Still, they provide an opportunity to advance the
research on a rare
phenomenon.
As such, this paper focuses on examining such challenges to
studying school
shootings, as well as offers considerations for how research
designs can be improved
in future studies. Specifically, these prior studies are examined,
underscoring specific
methodological challenges encountered during the research
process. From there, addi-
tional considerations are offered with regards to the
interpretation of specific findings,
and methods through which these issues can be addressed.
Finally, discussion is offered
about approaches to moving the larger body of research in
school shootings forward to
provide a more comprehensive and robust analysis of these
events.
Understanding the [Mediatized] Problem of School Shootings
Despite that crime rates in the United States are declining, and
homicide specifically is
especially rare, many people believe that school shootings are
becoming epidemic,
occurring more frequently than they actually are (Muschert and
Ragnedda 2010;
Newman 2006; Schildkraut et al. 2015). In reality, however,
there are approximately
10 events per year, on average (Schildkraut 2012). One of the
main factors that drives
the disproportional beliefs about the frequency of school
shootings is the amount of
attention they garner in the media (Elsass et al. 2014). Despite
that these events
typically last for 10 min or less, media coverage often extends
days, weeks, and, with
the most salient cases, a month or more (Muschert 2002;
Schildkraut 2012, 2014;
Schildkraut and Muschert 2014).1 As Kellner (2003, 2008a,
2008b) notes, these events
become, at varying intensities, Bmedia spectacles,^ with every
facet of the story
splashed across headlines or permeating television and
computer screens. These spec-
tacles essentially take relatively uncommon events,
sensationalize them, and make the
events appear far more commonplace than they actually are
(Kellner 2008a; Surette
1992).
When the story first broke of the Columbine High School
shooting, for example,
CNN aired six hours of uninterrupted live coverage (Muschert
2002). Three major
news networks – ABC, CBS, and NBC – devoted more than half
of their nightly news
airtime to coverage of the shooting over the following month
(Robinson 2011), with 53
stories in the first week alone (Maguire et al. 2002). In total,
319 stories were aired over
all nightly news broadcasts throughout the remainder of 1999
(Robinson 2011).
Coverage of other highly salient events, including the Virginia
Tech and Sandy Hook
shootings, have followed similar patterns.
In addition to television news, disproportional coverage of
school shooting events
also is evident through other media. In the year following
Columbine, over 10,000
1 The 1999 Columbine High School shooting is a notable
exception. It has been estimated that the total event
lasted just under 50 min (Columbine Review Commission 2001).
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 445
articles were published about the shooting in the nation’s top 50
newspapers (Newman
2006), including 170 articles in The New York Times alone
(Chyi and McCombs 2004;
Muschert and Carr 2006). The Times also published over 130
articles about both
Virginia Tech (Schildkraut 2012, 2014) and Sandy Hook
(Schildkraut 2014;
Schildkraut and Muschert 2014) in the first 30 days after each
shooting. While other
events, such as those at NIU in 2008 or Chardon High School in
2012, also have
captured national attention, they did so with lower frequencies
of coverage (Elsass et al.
2014; Schildkraut 2014). Thus, while school shootings have
been overrepresented in
the media, the disproportional coverage also is exacerbated by
the focus on the most
extreme (and deadly) examples (Burns and Crawford 1999;
Elsass et al. 2014;
Schildkraut and Muschert 2014; see also Maguire et al. 2002;
Robinson 2011).
Cohen (1963) has noted that the media Bmay not be successful
much of the time in
telling people what to think, but it is stunningly successful in
telling people what to
think about^ (p. 13). Such a sentiment is particularly relevant as
the media serves as the
main source of information for up to 95 % of the general public
(Surette 1992). The
agenda-setting function of the media often is achieved through
story selection decisions
and how they are framed (see, for example, McCombs 1997;
McCombs and Shaw
1972). Specifically, researchers have found that up to 50 % of
news coverage is
dedicated to stories about crime, and despite that property-
related offenses are consid-
erably more common, a disproportionate amount of this
attention is focused on the
most serious and violent crimes (Chermak 1995; Gruenewald et
al. 2009; Paulsen
2003; Schildkraut and Donley 2012). As such, the emphasis on
Bhigh amplitude^ or
sensational cases, such as school shootings, both in the amount
and prominence of the
coverage allocated, has the ability to shape public perceptions
about a particular event
or phenomenon (Johnstone et al. 1994; see also Chermak 1994).
Previous Research on School Shootings
Though the body of research examining school shootings is
limited, several researchers
have taken important first steps in studying the effects. At the
time of the Columbine
shooting, researchers at the Rochester Institute of Technology
(RIT) were conducting a
campus dating violence study (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The
researchers adapted
their study in order to examine the effects of Columbine on
students' perceptions of
safety (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The findings of the study
indicated that, when
compared to those surveyed prior to the shootings, respondents'
perceived safety was
significantly lower after Columbine (Stretesky and Hogan
2001). Despite methodolog-
ical limitations, Stretesky and Hogan’s (2001) work provides
one of the earliest
considerations of the impacts of such rare events.
Two other studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002) also
examined the impact of
the Columbine shootings, but did so using national data sources.
Addington (2003)
utilized the School Crime Supplement of the National Crime
Victimization Survey
(NCVS-SCS), while Brener et al. (2002) employed data from
the 1999 Youth Risk
Behavior Survey (YRBS). Aside from differences in study
design and respective
challenges, these studies found similar patterns related to the
effects of the shooting.
Addington (2003) found that students expressed greater fear
while at school after
Columbine as compared to those surveyed prior to it, though the
effect size was small.
446 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
Similarly, Brener et al. (2002) found that following the
shooting, many respondents
expressed being too fearful to attend school, and consequently,
were more likely to
avoid school altogether.
Researchers also have examined the effects of shootings taking
place on
college campuses. Fallahi et al. (2009) researched the effect of
the 2007
Virginia Tech shootings on students at Central Connecticut
State University.
They found that students who consumed more media coverage
of the shooting
both were more fearful and more likely to believe a similar
event would happen
again (Fallahi et al. 2009). These same respondents, however,
were less inclined
to express the belief that a similar attack would happen on their
campus
(Fallahi et al. 2009).
Similarly, Kaminski et al. (2010) examined the impact of both
Virginia Tech and
2008 NIU shootings on students at the University of South
Carolina (USC).
They found that students reported greater fear of murder and of
being attacked
with a weapon following both shootings (Kaminski et al. 2010).
Demographic
variables, such as gender, age, and race/ethnicity, also were
significantly corre-
lated with changes in fear, with females reporting being more
fearful than
males (Kaminski et al. 2010; see also Fallahi et al. 2009).
Additionally,
Kaminski et al. (2010) also found that older students and non-
whites expressed
greater fear, as compared to younger respondents and whites,
respectively.
Further, students living on campus expressed greater fear
following the shoot-
ings (Kaminski et al. 2010).
While each of these studies makes an important contribution to
the growing body of
research on school shootings, methodological issues present in
each work, coupled with
the complexities inherent in studying a phenomenon that is
extremely (and statistically)
rare in nature, produces a number of serious limitations. The
careful examination of
such limitations allows for discussion about how to minimize
their effects. Such a
discourse will assist school shootings researchers in better
handling common method-
ological issues, thereby helping to propel the body of literature
forward.
Defining the School Shootings Problem
Perhaps the biggest challenge in studying school shootings,
particularly when
trying to determine their prevalence, is the absence of a precise
definition for
the phenomenon. Both in the academic community and beyond,
little agreement
has arisen with regard to how best to define these events.
Specifically, a
number of different definitions, each with their own inherent
limitations, have
been put forth by various agencies and organizations. In a joint
report between
the U.S. Department of Education and the Secret Service, for
example, the
researchers define Bincidents of targeted school violence,^ as a
category which
includes (but is not limited to) school shootings, as.
Any incident where (i) a current student or recent former
student attacked
someone at his or her school with lethal means (e.g., a gun or
knife); and, (ii)
where the student attacker purposefully chose his or her school
as the location of
the attack. (Vossekuil et al. 2002, p. 7)
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 447
In the School-Associated Violent Death Study published by the
Center for Disease
Control (CDC), in conjunction with the U.S. Department of
Education and the U.S.
Department of Justice, criteria for inclusion in the report relies
on defining events as.
A case is defined as a fatal injury (e.g., homicide, suicide, or
legal intervention)
that occurs on school property, on the way to/from school, or
during or on the
way to/from a school-sponsored event. Only violent deaths
associated with U.S.
elementary and secondary schools, both public and private, are
included. (Centers
for Disease Control 2014)
While the CDC does not claim that all events in the report are
school shootings,
statistics reported are given in aggregates of total incidents;
when used to explain
school shootings, they provide individuals with an overinflated
perception of their
prevalence. In the same vein, the overly broad crafting of their
definition suggests that
events such as a suicide at a bus stop involving a firearm
essentially could be counted
as a school shooting.
Following the attack at Sandy Hook Elementary School,
Everytown for Gun Safety
(2014), a national advocacy group, purported that Bin the two
years since the mass
shooting in Newtown, Connecticut, there have been at least 94
school shootings
including fatal and nonfatal assaults, suicides, and unintentional
shootings – an average
of nearly one a week.^ This claim, however, was problematic
based on the definition
the organization used to identify events:
Incidents were classified as school shootings when a firearm
was discharged
inside a school building or on school or campus grounds, as
documented by the
press or confirmed through further inquiries with law
enforcement. Incidents in
which guns were brought into schools but not fired, or were
fired off school
grounds after having been possessed in schools, were not
included. (Everytown
for Gun Safety 2014)
As a result of this choice of wording, accidental weapons
discharges were included
in their count, as were events that differed in motivation from
school shootings, such as
those involving gangs. In sum, since there is no universally
accepted definition, there is,
in turn, disagreement regarding the number of events that are
(or are not occurring)
each year.
Theoretical Approaches to Studying School Shootings
For many researchers, utilizing a deductive approach to study a
particular phenomenon
is beneficial as it offers theoretical propositions to help guide
the research hypotheses to
be tested (Bryman 2012). For researchers studying school
shootings, however, finding
an appropriate, and more importantly, testable, theory can
present a challenge. Specif-
ically, the majority of criminological theories – including
biological, psychological,
strain, and learning– focus on understanding why individuals
engage in delinquent or
criminal activities. While a case study approach can be used to
look for common
attributes among school shooters, the majority commit suicide;
therefore, the amount of
448 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
available data is limited, as is the sample size, to test specific
theoretical propositions.2
Similarly, understanding why people become victims of school
shootings is limited by
a lack of theoretical insight; only one group of theories –
routine activities – underscore
the opportunity for victimization to occur.
Instead, researchers commonly have focused on understanding
individuals' re-
sponses to school shooting events (Addington 2003; Warr
2000). This broader exam-
ination is advantageous for several reasons. First, given the
extensive reach of the
media, a greater number of people may indirectly become
victims of these events
(Addington 2003; Skogan and Maxfield 1981; Warr 2000). As
such, this provides the
opportunity to examine the effects of these events on a larger
sample. Additionally, it is
these very responses that have one of the greatest abilities to
influence policy decisions
that stem from the events. Most commonly, responses to school
shootings as they relate
to fear of crime or perceived safety have been examined
(Addington 2003; Brener et al.
2002; Fallahi et al. 2009; Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and
Hogan 2001). Other
outcomes, such as the use of avoidance behaviors (Addington
2003; Brener et al.
2002), also have been examined.
Perceptions of Safety and Fear of Crime
One area of inquiry that has been focused on by researchers is
fear of crime, coupled
with associated perceptions of risk or safety, among the greater
public. Although the
body of literature on fear of crime is ample, spanning over 40
years, these studies focus
on more ordinary crimes and, by design, are ill-suited to capture
the effects of rare
crimes, such as school shootings (Warr 2000). Addington (2003)
acknowledges this
challenge, noting that Bno theoretical or empirical precedent is
available to predict
Columbine’s [or other similar events'] effect on fear [or
perceived risk of
victimization]^ (p. 368). Therefore, studies examining the
effects of Columbine and
other school shootings have placed greater emphasis on
understanding individual
reactions to the event, rather than the causes of the event itself.
One issue that complicates this area of inquiry, however, stems
from researchers'
handling of these outcome measures. Perceived risk and fear of
crime are related, yet
distinct constructs, and, therefore, it is important to
differentiate between them, as the
former is hypothesized to cause the latter (Warr 2000; Warr and
Stafford 1983).
Highlighting this difference, Ferraro (1995) further contends
that perceived risk is
cognitive, while fear of crime is emotional.
A number of the studies examining fear and perceived risk
related to school shootings
assert that they are distinct constructs in their reviews of the
literature, but fail to include
measures of each. Addington (2003) discussed the role of
perceived risk and social distance
in her study of the effects of the Columbine High School
shooting, but through no fault of
her own, was unable to include measures of perceived risk, as
they are not available in the
NCVS-SCS. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) faced the opposite
problem. Their survey instru-
ment focused solely on perceived risk, which they referred to as
Bperceptions of safety^
(Stretesky and Hogan 2001, p. 430). This forced the researchers
to forgo an examination of
the prospective effects on fear of crime post-Columbine and
instead focus on the potential
2 In a study examining the broader category of rampage
shooters following Columbine, Schildkraut (2014)
found that 55 % of perpetrators in the study committed suicide.
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 449
impact of the event on perceived risk. Since these constructs are
measuring two different
outcomes, the results from Addington’s (2003) and Stretesky
and Hogan’s (2001) studies are
not readily comparable. Instead, when possible, measures of
both fear and perceived risk
should be included in order for researchers to glean a more
complete understanding of
individuals' responses to such events.
The issue of understanding the impact of school shootings on
fear of crime or
perceptions of risk further is exacerbated by the way in which
these outcomes are
measured. Both are multifaceted constructs, and this complexity
must be acknowledged
in its measurement to ensure validity. Addington (2003), for
example, utilized two
questions from the NCVS-SCS to measure fear of crime, which
in itself is problematic.
Stretesky and Hogan (2001) also used two dependent variables,
but were able to
address the multidimensional nature of safety perceptions by
using composite measures
rather than single questions. Still, as different studies have
measured fear of crime, or
some other related aspect such as perceived risk, in different
ways, it begs the question:
What constitutes the best way to measure fear of crime?
Routine Activities and Avoidance Behaviors
According to Warr (2000), fear also may be assessed by
examining avoidance behav-
iors. When an individual is fearful, they may change or limit
their routine activities or
employ avoidance behaviors in an attempt to combat the
perceived threat (Hindelang
et al. 1978). The use of avoidance behaviors is not solely
limited to how unsafe one
perceives their environment to be; impressions of safety also
may be influenced by
associating, either directly or indirectly (via the media in some
instances), with victims
who share similar demographic characteristics to the individual
(Hindelang et al. 1978;
see also Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Thus, the use of avoidance
behaviors, such as
avoiding certain areas of campus, not being out at night, or not
traveling alone, serves
to minimize exposure to potential victimization (Hindelang et
al. 1978).
Two studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002) specifically
examined changes in self-
reported avoidance behaviors following the Columbine
shooting. Brener et al. (2002) found
that students were more likely to report missing at least one day
of school following
Columbine as compared to those surveyed before the shooting.
Additionally, Addington
(2003) utilized questions from the NCVS-SCS that focused on
avoiding specific areas of
school. This supplementary analysis was included as an
alternate examination of the impact
of the shooting on students' fear (Addington 2003; see also
Warr 2000). Unlike Brener et al.
(2002), however, Addington (2003) did not find a significant
difference in the use of
avoidance behaviors between the pre- and post-Columbine
groups. In their examination
of the impact of the Virginia Tech and NIU shootings on college
students, Kaminski et al.
(2010) found that respondents expressed being more fearful of
routine activities, such as
walking alone on campus, but did not assess whether they
employed any form of avoidance
behaviors to combat these beliefs.
Methodological Complexities
When studying school shootings, researchers are faced with a
number of unique
methodological concerns that can affect studies' findings. Due
to the rarity of these
450 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
events, each decision made has the potential to greatly limit
confidence in the findings
regarding the phenomena. Specifically, careful contemplation
must be given to deci-
sions related to data collection, sampling sizes, and timing, as
well as potential spatial
and temporal effects that may influence outcome measures.
Beyond these decisions'
impacts on the findings, such issues also are imperative in
shaping the research from its
inception.
Data Collection Concerns
Data collection perhaps is the most difficult facet of a research
study to plan when
attempting to study events of episodic violent crime. Due to the
unanticipated nature of
school shootings, researchers are presented with a number of
study design options to
investigate an event’s effect. The first option is to utilize
national surveys, such as the
NCVS-SCS or the YRBS (see Addington 2003; Brener et al.
2002). Alternatively,
researchers may be conducting a survey at the time an incident
takes place, and can
adapt their data collection around the event (see generally,
Kaminski et al. 2010;
Stretesky and Hogan 2001).
Primary Data Collection It sometimes is the case when studying
this kind of phe-
nomenon that after data collection has begun, a school shooting
takes place, thereby
changing the purpose and focus of the original research
endeavor. While the ideal
research design would be to utilize the same group of
respondents for both pre- and
post-event samples, it is rare that researchers are able to survey
the same individuals
before and after. This is especially relevant if IRB regulations
require anonymity,
thereby thwarting the possibility of resurveying pre-event
respondents or if the shooting
occurs at a time that makes data collection particularly difficult,
like during a change in
semesters. Therefore, once an event occurs, investigators are
likely to be faced with the
task of limiting differences between pre- and post-event
samples, thus necessarily
increasing one’s reliance on convenience or purposive sampling
techniques.
Stretesky and Hogan (2001) encountered this challenge in their
examination of
changes in fear of crime as a result of the Columbine High
School shooting. They
originally were studying campus dating violence at RIT through
the dissemination of
separate surveys to male and female respondents, with only the
female survey contain-
ing questions about perceived safety (Stretesky and Hogan
2001). After the shootings at
Columbine, the researchers adapted their original research
design to focus on changes
in perceptions of safety as a result of the event; however,
because only the female
survey contained such questions, they only were able to
investigate the one group’s
changes in perceptions of safety, thereby reducing the
generalizability of the findings
(Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The impact of reduced
representativeness can be dimin-
ished through a few different methods, however, including the
use of internal compar-
ison groups (Shadish et al. 2002). Accordingly, Stretesky and
Hogan (2001) instituted
random selection from a designated sampling frame that
included only open enrollment
liberal arts classes to achieve this end.
Analysis of Secondary Data One way in which
representativeness can be increased is
through the analysis of secondary data sources. Brener et al.
(2002) used the YRBS and
employed Ba three-stage cluster-sample design to obtain a
national representative
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 451
sample of students in grades 9 through 12^ (p. 147).
Additionally, increased represen-
tativeness was achieved in Addington’s (2003) study by
capitalizing on the NCVS-
SCS' random allocation design, which is attained through the
survey’s rotating panel
design and interview schedule (p. 372).
Yet, the analysis of secondary data in these instances is not
without its disadvan-
tages. Though large sample sizes may be more representative of
the population, they
can also cause statistics to appear significant when they are not
(Shadish et al. 2002). In
the case of large samples, t-tests have so much power that even
a very small difference
can produce results reaching statistical significance (Shadish et
al. 2002). The likeli-
hood of a Type I error can be reduced though a number of
methods, including the
Bonferroni correction, the use of conservative multiple
comparison follow-up tests in
analysis of variance, and the use of a multivariate analysis of
variance if multiple
dependent variables are tested (Shadish et al. 2002).
Still, the tradeoff for using national-level secondary data can be
considerable. When
studying possible changes in attitudes as a result of a rare
violent crime, such as a
school shooting, Warr (2000) notes that capturing the full
effects of the phenomenon is
difficult without a pretest measure. This information, in most
instances, is lacking in
annual or national surveys because the temporal or spatial
effects of such events
typically have only a short-term impact at the local level. This
is an obvious limitation
of using national survey data to study this phenomenon, as they
are unlikely to contain
all of the necessary measures.
Spatial Event Proximity Spatial proximity to a school shooting
presents an important
challenge, as the effects of the event may be diffused the further
the respondent is from
an incident’s location (Warr 2000). Researchers (e.g., Heath
1984; Liska and Baccaglini
1990) have found that people respond differently when crime is
local (see also
Addington 2003). As Heath (1984) noted, Bthe worse things are
elsewhere, the better
we feel about our immediate environment^ (p. 270). Since both
Addington (2003) and
Brener et al. (2002) opted to use national data in their
examinations of Columbine, the
majority of respondents surveyed were from areas outside of
Littleton, the Denver
metropolitan area, or even the state of Colorado. Therefore, it is
possible that the event
did not strongly affect respondents who were spatially distant
from the shooting
(Addington 2003). One potential way to make such a
determination regarding respon-
dents' spatial proximity to a school shooting would be to
include a control measure for
the location of the respondent (e.g., city, state, or region, based
on data availability).
Temporal Event Proximity Temporal proximity to a school
shooting also provides a
challenge for researchers. While it is unknown specifically how
long the aftermath of
these events lasts, it is likely to be short (Warr 2000; see also
Addington 2003;
Stretesky and Hogan 2001). For example, researchers who have
examined this phe-
nomenon have found that coverage in the media typically lasts
around 30 days (Chyi
and McCombs 2004; Muschert and Carr 2006; Schildkraut 2012;
Schildkraut and
Muschert 2014). By comparison, topics that take more time to
develop, such as politics,
have an average lifespan of 18.5 months (McCombs and Zhu
1995). Thus, given the
volatile nature of school shootings, as well as the subsequent
reactions to them, it is
possible that with the passage of time, any effects or changes in
outcome measures may
be diminished. At the same time, however, it is possible that,
particularly with highly
452 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
salient events, the effects may increase over time, thus not
being able to be captured in
cross-sectional surveys.
Even when studies are able to collect data immediately prior to
and after an event,
there still are potential limitations in assessing changes in the
outcome measures.
Surveying respondents directly after an event in order to capture
its immediate
impact may not only fail to find significant effects due to a
possible delay, as may
have been a problem with Kaminski et al. (2010) study, but also
is unable to assess any
long-term developing effects. The data collection period for
Stretesky and Hogan
(2001) was 5 days prior to Columbine and 15 days after the
shooting. Given this
shortened time frame, the authors note that it was impossible to
determine the length of
the effects of Columbine (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The issue
of seasonality, as
highlighted by Addington (2003), is a supplementary
confounding factor that must be
considered with regard to the timing of data collection. Perhaps
then, it would be most
fruitful to survey respondents as quickly as possible after a
school shooting takes place
and incorporate into the study design multiple post-event
collection periods to track the
progression of event effects over time.
Similarly, when surveys are designed for a purpose other than
capturing the effects
of school shootings, questions included may present challenges
for disentangling the
actual impact of the event. In Brener et al. (2002) study, for
example, the researchers
utilized a measure assessing whether students had missed school
in the 30 days
prior to taking the survey because of feeling unsafe. Such a
question is
problematic, however, based on when a respondent actually
completed the
survey. If the survey was completed on April 21 (the shooting
occurred on
April 20), the question arises as to whether the reported
avoidance is attribut-
able to Columbine, which would likely be freshest in the
respondent’s mind,
rather than other events that took place during the preceding 29
days. Con-
versely, if the survey was completed on May 19, and similar
changes in the avoidance of
school were found, it is equally difficult to determine if the
change is a result of the
shooting, or if some other event occurring after Columbine
caused a respondent’s
absence. One potential way to address this issue, as is employed
by Addington
(2003), is to include controls for interview month and day. This,
however, requires the
information to be available in the dataset used; it is unclear if
this information was
available to Brener et al. (2002).
Finally, when considering the temporally-associated issues
related to data collection,
one also must consider the possibility of other events occurring
during the same period
(Kaminski et al. 2010). Though Columbine often is regarded as
the archetypal school
shooting to which all other events are measured (Altheide 2009;
Larkin 2007, 2009;
Muschert 2007b; Muschert and Larkin 2007), it is not the only
event that occurred
during the data collection period of several of the studies
mentioned thus far. Addington
(2003) utilized data from the first six months of 1999, during
which time, besides
Columbine, another shooting occurred on May 20 in Conyers,
Georgia at Heritage
High School. Brener et al. (2002) data collection period also
encompassed this shoot-
ing. While Columbine is considerably more noteworthy than the
Conyers
shooting, it is possible that the latter event still had an effect,
particularly if
any of the respondents surveyed were in the vicinity of the
shooting. As such,
including control measures, such as the day and month
(Addington 2003) or the
occurrence of another event, can be helpful.
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 453
Sampling Considerations
A supplementary concern involves the investigation of a school
shooting directly
impacting one population, like high school students, but its
effect being studied on a
separate population, such as college students. Why should a
shooting at a high school
affect college students' fear, as was investigated by Stretesky
and Hogan (2001)? While
demographic overlap between victims and study respondents
beyond age partially may
account for why these kinds of comparisons are valid,
researchers still must consider
who to sample in the first place.
Yet, regardless of sampling method, and even when employing
techniques to
minimize the impact of separate pre- and post-event samples,
differences often remain
on a number of characteristics. Brener et al. (2002) and
Stretesky and Hogan (2001)
faced opposite ends of a similar problem regarding pre- and
post-Columbine sample
sizes. In the study conducted by Brener et al. (2002),
approximately 78.4 % of the
surveys had been collected prior to the shooting at Columbine
High School. This
resulted in the pre-Columbine sample including 12,049 cases
and the post-Columbine
sample including 3137 cases (Brener et al. 2002). The effect of
the difference in sample
size pre- and post-event is minimized however, because both
samples were so large, as
in essence, large sample sizes limit the influence of outliers as
they are more
representative of the population (Tabachnick and Fidell 2006).
In the same vein,
Stretesky and Hogan’s (2001) study utilized drastically different
pre- and post-
Columbine sample sizes, with only 20 respondents in the
before-event sample
and 113 respondents in the after-event sample. They opted to
utilize a qualita-
tive research technique – incorporating the use of focus groups
– to increase
confidence in their quantitative findings.
The timing of drawing samples in relation to event occurrence
also can impact a
study. Kaminski et al. (2010) employed a research design that
utilized four distinct
sampling intervals – prior to and following the shooting on
April 16, 2007 at Virginia
Tech and the same with the February 14, 2008 shooting at NIU.
Since the post-Virginia
Tech sample of respondents and the pre-NIU sample of
respondents were surveyed in
close temporal proximity to one another – only a 7 month
difference between the two
sample-collection periods – it is possible that there was some
carryover of fear in the
pre-NIU sample due to the commission of the Virginia Tech
shooting. As Kaminski
et al. (2010) note,
The pre-NIU coefficient was statistically significant and larger
than that for the
post-VT estimate. Taken at face value, this suggests that not
only was there no
decay in fear following the VT shooting incident, but that fear
of walking alone
on campus during the day actually increased between Survey 2
(post-VT) and
Survey 3 (pre-NIU). (p. 94)
It is possible, then, that it the effects of the shooting at Virginia
Tech on fear was
delayed. In fact, Kaminski et al. (2010) point out that if delayed
effects did occur, it
could be caused by a wide array of factors that affected their
study, including a possible
desensitization of respondents to similar repeated events,
increased campus safety
measures employed after the Virginia Tech shooting, and the
timing of the survey
administration, among others.
454 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
Understanding The Findings
Fear, Risk, and Non-Normal Events
Though several studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002;
Kaminski et al. 2010;
Stretesky and Hogan 2001) reported finding effects, albeit
minimal, of their respective
school shootings on measures of fear of crime or perceptions of
safety, these results
must be interpreted with caution for two key reasons. First,
respondents systematically
may be excluded from the study, the extent of which can be
difficult, if not impossible,
to determine. Addington’s (2003) research on changes in fear of
crime highlights this
issue by finding that Bafter Columbine, students were more
likely to avoid school
entirely^ (p. 371). Moreover, of the students who do return to
school after a shooting
incident, those who are fearful may refuse to answer certain
survey questions that make
them feel afraid or uncomfortable during post-event data
collection. This plausibly can
result in missing data from some subsection of the respondents,
thereby reducing the
variability of the sample, and possibly biasing results.
Perhaps more problematic, however, is the way in which
measures of fear of crime
or perceived risk of victimization, have been constructed. The
outcome measures in
these studies, similar to the larger body of fear of crime (see,
for instance, Ferraro 1995;
LaGrange and Ferraro 1989), focus on assessing perceptions as
they relate to more
commonly occurring crimes. Addington (2003), for example,
assessed respondents'
level of fear while at school, as well as while traveling to and
from school. This begs
the question of why respondents' fear of crime while traveling
to or from school should
be impacted by the Columbine shooting, which occurred solely
on the campus itself.
Similarly, in their examination of perceptions of safety,
Stretesky and Hogan (2001)
incorporated questions related to routine activities, including
walking alone, being
home alone, or taking public transportation, each at night.3
School shootings, however, cannot be said to be normally
occurring in any sense;
thus, the construct validity of the fear or risk measures is
reduced. As Addington (2003)
points out,
The theoretical models utilizing perceived risk were developed
to explain fear of
victimization arising from daily experiences as opposed to a
particular event like
Columbine. It is not clear how analogous Columbine is to more
common
recurring experiences. Prior research also operationalized
indirect victimization
as fairly minor or routine crimes occurring in the neighborhood
or involving
one’s friends or family. In contrast, Columbine was an
unusually violent crime
that for the vast majority of Americans occurred in a distant
place to strangers. (p.
369, emphasis added)
As such, the lack of, or minimal significant findings about the
effects of a non-
normally occurring crime may best be attributed to trying to
capture its impact using
measures related to more routine events.
In order to better assess the full impact of school shooting
events on both fear of
crime and perceptions of risk, researchers must reconceptualize
the way in which these
3 For a complete list of questions included, see Stretesky and
Hogan (2001, p. 437).
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 455
outcomes are measured. First, measures of both fear and risk
must be multidimensional
in composition, as a single measure of either will be insufficient
to capture such a
complex issue. Additionally, these questions specifically must
be consistent with school
shootings, as opposed to routine crimes, such as burglary,
robbery, or even homicide
more generally. As Kaminski et al. (2010) note,
Although campus shootings may increase student fear of
walking alone on
campus or fear of crime on campus generally, they may more
strongly increase
fear of being threatened with a gun or fear of being murdered on
campus, since
guns and murder were characteristics specific to the Virginia
Tech and NIU
events. (p. 91)
The panel of questions proposed by Ferraro (1995), which has
been validated
through a number of subsequent studies, provides a good
starting point to design such
a survey. This panel, however, merely provides a template, as it
is was designed to
assess more routine crimes. Thus, researchers could benefit
from adapting this panel to
maintain questions about fear or perceived risk of murder while
incorporating measures
specific to school shootings, such as being shot while on
campus, being threatened with
a gun (and perhaps disaggregating further to identify fear or
risk of being threatened
with a handgun, shotgun, or rifle), or being targeted
indiscriminately by disgruntled
classmates.
Mediatized Outcomes
As most people never will be a direct victim of a school
shooting, nor spatially
proximate to the site of an event, media attention has the
potential to close these gaps
(Addington 2003; Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and Hogan
2001). More specifically,
increased media coverage provides a way by which the public
can identify with both
the victims and the event as a whole (Addington 2003). As such,
the media can have
the ability to lead to the indirect victimization for millions of
news consumers
(Addington 2003; Skogan and Maxfield 1981; Warr 2000).
Understanding this poten-
tial effect, then, must be a consideration for researchers.
Both Addington (2003) and Stretesky and Hogan (2001), for
example, posit a
potential media effect in their studies, due in part to the
heightened media attention
the Columbine shooting received. While Addington (2003)
contextualizes the link as
indirect, Stretesky and Hogan (2001) propose a direct
relationship. In fact, their study is
focused around the research question, BDoes the media
depiction of the Columbine
shooting change perceptions of safety and risk on the part of
[RIT] students during the
time period in question?^ (Stretesky and Hogan 2001, p. 430).
Despite these proposi-
tions, neither study included media-related variables.
Even if media variables are included, consideration must be
given to the differences
between consumption and content. In order to adequately assess
the impact of the
media, both should be considered. Further, measures assessing
the different modes of
media (e.g., television, newspaper, or social media) are needed,
as often, different
categories of news consumers frequent specific sources (see, for
example, Pew Re-
search Center for the People and the Press 2008). In sum,
researchers not only must
account for how much media people consume, but also what
they are taking in and
456 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
through what mode in order to understand the potential effects
of indirect victimization
via the media on study outcome measures.
The Role of Prior Victimization
As noted, the media may act as a form of indirect victimization
for the general public
who tune in when a school shooting occurs. Direct prior
victimization also has the
ability to influence people’s perceptions about school shootings,
and as such, must be
considered. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) included some measure
of previous victimi-
zation in their studies, recognizing that respondents' past
victimization could affect their
fear of crime, perceived risk of victimization, and avoidance
behaviors, irrespective of
the occurrence of a school shooting. As Stretesky and Hogan
(2001) note, individuals
who previously have been victimized are more likely to report
feeling unsafe. The
majority of the studies examined here, however, did not
incorporate controls for prior
victimization, which also could confound the results.
Additionally, the influence of time with regard to respondents'
past victimization
must be taken into account. More recent victimization may have
stronger effects on fear
of crime, perceived risk of victimization, and avoidance
behaviors than victimization
experiences occurring in the more distant past. Furthermore, it
is unknown how prior
personal victimization may affect fear and perceived risk of
crime as they relate
specifically to school shootings. Similar to the aforementioned
concerns related to fear
of normally occurring crime, past personal victimization, such
as individuals having
had their house broken into while they were home, may have
little relation to fear of
being shot in a classroom. Therefore, it is imperative that
researchers studying school
shootings also incorporate questions with similar situational
characteristics of these
events, such as bullying or being confronted with a firearm, in
addition to more general
measures of prior victimization.
Combating The Challenges Of Studying School Shootings
Each of these studies has made an important contribution to the
available body of
literature on school shootings. Though we have highlighted
areas for consideration
within each study that warrant caution in understanding their
methodologies and
interpreting their findings, these limitations simultaneously
provide a considerable
opportunity to continue to advance the research. Drawing on
these, a series of Bbest
practices^ can be offered that researchers may use in future
studies to overcome prior
constraints. Table 1 provides a summary of these
recommendations for moving the
research forward.
Employ a Universally Accepted Definition
While various definitions of school shootings (or other closely
associated identifiers)
exist, the absence of a precise, universally accepted definition
is problematic for several
reasons. First, without clearly defined parameters of what
constitutes such an event,
some incidents may be labeled incorrectly as a school shooting,
despite that they do not
align with other attacks. For instance, under the broadly
constructed definition put forth
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 457
by Everytown for Gun Safety (2014), a gang shooting at an
Orlando, Florida area high
school is identified as a school shooting (Stableford 2014;
Weiner 2013). Not only does
this confuse the definitional understanding of these events, it
also inflates the statistics,
giving the public the perception that such attacks are occurring
more frequently than
they actually are. Further, since many of these definitions are
proposed by government
agencies or national organizations, such definitions and related
statistics are believed to
be accurate, though they may not be.
In order to overcome this limitation, we propose that Katherine
Newman and
colleagues' definition in their book, Rampage: The Social Roots
of School Shootings,
should be used as the universally accepted definition through
which to study these
events. Specifically, Newman et al. (2004) define rampage
school shootings as events
that.
…Take place on a school-related public stage before an
audience; involve
multiple victims, some of whom are shot simply for their
symbolic significance
or at random; and involve one or more shooters who are
students or former
students of the school. (p. 50, emphasis added)
Newman et al. (2004) definition has been widely accepted and
typically serves as the
basis among researchers in the academic community (e.g., Fox
and Harding 2005;
Kalish and Kimmel 2010; Langman 2009; Muschert 2007b;
Muschert and Ragnedda
2010; Tonso 2009; Wike and Fraser 2009) who study such
events. Given its validation
through these works, it can be considered the most ideal
definition presented to
understand school shootings.
Ensure the Accuracy of the Data
Data associated with mass shootings is predicated on a precise
definition. When
inaccuracy exists, however, the data itself will be impacted.
Most commonly, defini-
tions have been crafted in a way that leads to an inflation in the
statistics, indicating that
there is a greater number of events occurring than actually are.
Based on Everytown for
Gun Safety’s aforementioned definition, the organization claims
that 142 school
shootings occurred in the 22 months after Sandy Hook, despite
that other researchers
(e.g., Schildkraut and Elsass 2016) found fewer events occurred
in 134 years based on a
more precise classification.
Table 1 Best practices for studying school shootings
1. Employ a universally accepted definition of the phenomenon.
2. Ensure that the data being used is accurate and that it is
collected in accordance with the agreed upon
definition.
3. Utilize theoretical perspectives to support the findings.
4. Ground the findings in the context of homicide in the U.S.
and relevant statistical risk of victimization.
5. Expand the resource focus to explore new avenues and
unanswered research questions.
6. Combine quantitative and qualitative methodologies to
increase the robustness of the body of literature and
individual studies.
458 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
It is important to note, however, that even the most accurate
definition does not
guarantee inclusion of all events. The heavy reliance of
researchers on media accounts
to identify these events can be problematic based on the
continual changing of
terminology used in the discourse (e.g., school shooting, active
shooter, rampage,
etc.). Further, despite the highly sensational nature of these
events, not all events are
covered and of those that are, the number of stories and amount
of information released
may vary (Schildkraut 2014). Despite such cautions, improving
the overall accuracy of
the data will benefit researchers in being able to offer more
insight and possible
generalizability of their findings related to these events.
Utilize Theoretical Perspectives to Support the Findings
Like all research, findings from these studies should be
supported by theory in order to
increase their validity. As identified earlier, the way in which
theoretical concepts are
measured is important, as many theories have clearly defined
constructs that are
instrumental in their application. Deviation in how such
constructs are measured,
however, may reduce confidence in researchers’ findings.
Instead, future studies should
emphasize measurement in accordance with specific theoretical
propositions substan-
tiated in previous literature.
Further, a broader range of theories should be used to study
these events. Rather than
anecdote or speculation, for example, theories from the
biological or psychological
perspectives may be used to further assess why school shooters
engage in such crimes.
Similarly, learning theories also may be employed to better
understand how they come
to engage in these behaviors. Routine activities theory, as
proposed by Cohen and
Felson (1979), also can provide a strong foundation not only for
identifying how the
opportunity arises for these events to occur, but potential ways
in which future school
shootings may be prevented. While there is no Bone size fits
all^ theory through which
to understand these events, the broad range of criminological
explanations, steeped in
rich, academic history, can provide different opportunities to
strengthen the findings of
future research.
It may be necessary to develop a new theoretical paradigm
through whose lens
school shootings may be examined, apart from the traditional
criminological ap-
proaches. Due to the rarity of this phenomenon, it is likely that
criminological theories
developed for the purpose of explaining more normally
occurring crimes are ill-
equipped to be applied to school shootings. It then is vital that
researchers begin to
consider theory construction with rare violent events,
specifically school shootings, as
the focus.
Ground the Findings in the Bigger Picture of Homicide and
Relative Risk
of Victimization
The employment of a precise definition and accurate statistics
can provide a greater
understanding about these events and their prevalence. As it has
been found that
individuals believe that school shootings are happening more
frequently and that they
have a high statistical likelihood of becoming victims (e.g.,
Elsass et al. 2014;
Schildkraut et al. 2015), findings from research on these events
must be
contextualized in terms of homicide in the U.S., an already
statistically rare
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 459
occurrence compared to other types of crimes. This also will
enable researchers
to translate their findings in terms of a person’s actual risk of
becoming the
victim of a school shooting.
This context is particularly important, given the reliance of
media accounts.
Schildkraut (2014), for example, found in her examination of all
mass shootings
between 2000 and 2014 that of 1930 statistical references coded
in 564 articles, just
3 mentions of the U.S. violent crime rate were made. The
absence of this information,
however, is critical as it is likely a main contributing factor to
the disproportional beliefs
about these events that have been found in other studies (e.g.,
Elsass et al. 2014;
Schildkraut et al. 2015). Therefore, in order to potentially
reshape perceptions about
these events' occurrence and people’s likelihood of
victimization, context must be
offered as it relates to crime in the U.S. more broadly.
Expand the Research Focus
The studies examined here are important to the body of
literature on school shootings
as they focus on understanding how members of society respond
to these events in the
form of fear of crime or heightened perceptions of risk
victimization. Beyond these,
however, the majority of research on school shootings focuses
most heavily on
understanding the way in which these stories are framed in the
media. For example,
a study by Chyi and McCombs (2004) examined the way in
which coverage of the
Columbine shooting was framed in The New York Times, and
how these
patterns shifted over the life of the story. These findings later
were compared
with 13 other shootings occurring in a similar time period
(Muschert and Carr
2006) and the Sandy Hook shooting (Schildkraut and Muschert
2014). Others
have examined how specific narratives about these events,
perpetrators, or
victims are constructed within the media (e.g., Frymer 2009;
Kupchik and
Bracy 2009; Lawrence and Mueller 2003; Schildkraut 2012).
Each study is a
worthwhile endeavor, given that the media acts as the main
source of informa-
tion for up to 95 % of the general public (Graber 1980; Surette
1992) and the
manner in which stories are framed can have a considerable
impact on the
attitudes examined in the studies by Addington (2003); Brener
et al. (2002);
Fallahi et al. (2009); Kaminski et al. (2010), and Stretesky and
Hogan (2001).
Still, this limited focus creates the opportunity to expand the
research in future
studies.
Moving forward, scholars may wish to consider other aspects of
the school shoot-
ings phenomenon that largely has been absent from the
literature. One such opportunity
would be to examine the perpetrators themselves more in depth.
Both Langman (2009)
and Muschert (2007a) have offered typologies through which to
understand the
shooters. Langman (2009), for example, presented three
different categories of school
shooters – the traumatized, the psychotic, and the psychopathic.
Muschert (2007a), on
the other hand, offered a different typology through which to
analyze school shootings.
Specifically, he differentiated rampage shootings, as defined by
Newman et al. (2004),
from mass murders, terrorist attacks, targeted shootings, and
government shootings
(Muschert 2007a). These studies each were limited by a small
sample size (e.g.,
Langman had only 10 shooters from 8 different events in his
study); therefore, a
renewed application to a broader group of perpetrators may
provide additional insights
460 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
about these events. From there, a greater understanding about
why these events occur
and how they may either be predicted or prevented (e.g.,
through the identification of
warning signs) may be gleaned.
Combine Both Qualitative and Quantitative Methodologies
In order to continue to advance this important body of
literature, new methodological
approaches must be employed. One possible way in which this
can be accomplished is
by employing a multi-stage research design that utilizes both
quantitative and
qualitative methodologies. While the studies reviewed here have
illustrated the
benefits of quantitative survey research, such information can
benefit from being
supplemented with qualitative data to provide a more robust and
rich understanding
of responses. With studies such as those by Addington (2003)
and Brener et al. (2002),
which found through quantitative analysis that students were
more fearful after the
Columbine shooting, supplementing the research with
qualitative follow-up will allow
for researchers to understand the Bwhy^ behind the data.
One way in which this could be accomplished would be for
researchers to conduct
interviews or focus groups with individuals and gather
information about reactions to
school shootings, either individual events or the collective
phenomenon (see, for
example, Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Likewise, observational
research could be
conducted with individuals or groups, looking for cues as
participants read news stories
or watch coverage of the events. Regardless of the specific
methodology chosen, the
use of a multi-stage, mixed-method design will allow
researchers to significantly
advance the breadth of the literature on school shootings.
Conclusion
The studies examined throughout the current work have taken
important first steps in
developing the research on school shootings, particularly as the
phenomenon has
become a highly prevalent issue in the public discourse. Despite
inherent challenges
arising from methodological issues, these studies provide an
important glimpse into
how these events can impact people’s attitudes about school
shootings. At the same
time, recognizing these roadblocks enables future researchers to
reconceptualize the
way in which these events are studied, creating the opportunity
to develop a more
robust examination of school shootings, from the moment the
story breaks to how
information is processed and received by the public, and in turn,
how these beliefs
influence related public policy. Only then can researchers
continue to advance this
limited, yet vital, body of literature forward.
Acknowledgments The authors wish to thank the reviewers for
their invaluable feedback on this
manuscript. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the
2014 annual meeting of the American
Society of Criminology.
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 461
References
Addington, L. A. (2003). Students' fear after columbine:
Findings from a randomized experiment. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology, 19(4), 367–387.
Altheide, D. L. (2009). The columbine shooting and the
discourse of fear. American Behavioral Scientist,
52(10), 1354–1370.
Brener, N. D., Simon, T. R., Anderson, M., Barrios, L. C., &
Small, M. L. (2002). Effect of the incident at
columbine on students' violence- and suicide-related behaviors.
American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 22(3), 146–150.
Bryman, A. (2012). Social research methods (4th ed., ). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Burns, R., & Crawford, C. (1999). School shootings, the media,
and public fear: Ingredients for a moral panic.
Crime, Law & Social Change, 32, 147–168.
Centers for Disease Control. (2014). School-assisted violent
death study. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/
ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolviolence/SAVD.html
Chermak, S. (1994). Body count news: How crime is presented
in the news media. Justice Quarterly, 11(4),
561–582.
Chermak, S. (1995). Victims in the news: Crime and the
American news media. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Chyi, H. I., & McCombs, M. E. (2004). Media salience and the
process of framing: Coverage of the
Columbine school shootings. Journalism and Mass
Communication Quarterly, 81(1), 22–35.
Cohen, B. C. (1963). The press and foreign policy. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. K. (1979). Social change and crime
rate trends: A routine activities approach.
American Sociological Review, 44(4), 588–608.
Columbine Review Commission (2001). The report of governor
Bill Owens' Columbine Review Commission.
Denver, CO: State of Colorado. Retrieved from
http://www.state.co.us/columbine/.
Elsass, H. J., Schildkraut, J., & Stafford, M. C. (2014).
Breaking news of social problems: Examining media
consumption and student beliefs about school shootings.
Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law and Society,
15(2).
Everytown for Gun Safety. (2014). Analysis of school
shootings: December 15, 2012 – December 9, 2014.
Retrieved from
http://everytown.org/documents/2014/10/analysis-of-school-
shootings.pdf
Fallahi, C. R., Austad, C. S., Fallon, M., & Leishman, L.
(2009). A survey of the perceptions of the Virginia
Tech tragedy. Journal of School Violence, 8(2), 120–135.
Ferraro, K. F. (1995). Fear of crime: Interpreting victimization
risk. New York: SUNY Press.
Fox, C., & Harding, D. (2005). School shootings as
organizational deviance. Sociology of Education, 78(1),
69–97.
Frymer, B. (2009). The media spectacle of Columbine:
Alienated youth as an object of fear. American
Behavioral Scientist, 52(10), 1387–1404.
Graber, D. A. (1980). Crime news and the public. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Gruenewald, J., Pizarro, J., & Chermak, S. (2009). Race,
gender, and the newsworthiness of homicide
incidents. Journal of Criminal Justice, 37(3), 262–272.
Heath, L. (1984). Impact of newspaper crime reports on fear of
crime: Multimethodological investigation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 263–276.
Hindelang, M. J., Gottfredson, M. R., & Garofalo, J. (1978).
Victims of personal crime: An empirical
foundation for a theory of personal victimization. Cambridge,
MA: Ballinger Publishing Company.
Johnstone, J., Hawkins, D., & Michener, A. (1994). Homicide
reporting in Chicago dailies. Journalism
Quarterly, 71(4), 860–872.
Kalish, R., & Kimmel, M. (2010). Suicide by mass murder:
Masculinity, aggrieved entitlement, and rampage
school shootings. Health Sociology Review, 19(4), 451–464.
Kaminski, R. J., Koons-Witt, B. A., Thompson, N. S., & Weiss,
D. (2010). The impacts of Virginia Tech and
Northern Illinois University shootings on the fear of crime on
campus. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(1),
88–98.
Kellner, D. (2003). Media spectacle. London: Routledge.
Kellner, D. (2008a). Guys and guns amok: Domestic terrorism
and school shootings from the Oklahoma City
bombing to the Virginia Tech massacre. Boulder, CO: Paradigm
Publishers.
Kellner, D. (2008b). Media spectacle and the Bmassacre at
Virginia Tech^. In B. Agger, & T. W. Luke (Eds.),
There is a gunman on campus (pp. 29–54). Lanham, MD: Rowan
& Littlefield Publishers, Inc.
462 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolvi
olence/SAVD.html
http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolvi
olence/SAVD.html
http://www.state.co.us/columbine/
http://everytown.org/documents/2014/10/analysis-of-school-
shootings.pdf
Kupchik, A., & Bracy, N. L. (2009). The news media on school
crime and violence: Constructing danger-
ousness and fueling fear. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice,
7(2), 136–155.
LaGrange, R. L., & Ferraro, K. F. (1989). Assessing age and
gender differences in perceived risk and fear of
crime. Criminology, 27(4), 697–719.
Langman, P. (2009). Rampage school shooters: A typology.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(1), 79–86.
Larkin, R. W. (2007). Comprehending Columbine. Philadelphia,
PA: Temple University Press.
Larkin, R. W. (2009). The Columbine legacy: Rampage
shootings as political acts. American Behavioral
Scientist, 52(9), 1309–1326.
Lawrence, R., & Mueller, D. (2003). School shootings and the
man-bites-dog criterion of newsworthiness.
Youth Violence & Juvenile Justice, 1(4), 330–345.
Liska, A. E., & Baccaglini, W. (1990). Feeling safe by
comparison: Crime in the newspapers. Social Problems,
37(3), 360–374.
Maguire, B., Weatherby, G. A., & Mathers, R. A. (2002).
Network news coverage of school shootings. The
Social Science Journal, 39(3), 465–470.
McCombs, M. E. (1997). Building consensus: The news media’s
agenda-setting roles. Polymer Composites,
14(4), 433–443.
McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting
function of the mass media. The Public Opinion
Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187.
McCombs, M. E., & Zhu, J. (1995). Capacity, diversity, and
volatility of the public agenda: Trends from 1954
to 1994. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 59(4), 495–525.
Muschert, G. W. (2002). Media and massacre: The social
construction of the columbine story, Unpublished
doctoral dissertation. Boulder, CO.: University of Colorado at
Boulder.
Muschert, G. W. (2007a). Research in school shootings.
Sociology Compass, 1(1), 60–80.
Muschert, G. W. (2007b). The Columbine victims and the myth
of the juvenile superpredator. Youth Violence
and Juvenile Justice, 5(4), 351–366.
Muschert, G. W., & Carr, D. (2006). Media salience and frame
changing across events: Coverage of nine
school shootings, 1997–2001. Journalism and Mass
Communication Quarterly, 83(4), 747–766.
Muschert, G. W., & Larkin, R. W. (2007). The Columbine High
School shootings. In S. Chermak, & F. Y.
Bailey (Eds.), Crimes and trials of the century (pp. 253–266).
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Muschert, G. W., & Ragnedda, M. (2010). Media and violence
control: The framing of school shootings. In
W. Heitmeyer, H. G. Haupt, S. Malthaner, & A. Kirschner
(Eds.), The control of violence in modern
society: Multidisciplinary perspectives, from school shootings
to ethnic violence (pp. 345–361). New
York: Springer Publishing.
Newman, K. S. (2006). School shootings are a serious problem.
In S. Hunnicutt (Ed.), School shootings (pp.
10–17). Farmington Hills, MI: Greenhaven Press.
Newman, K. S., Fox, C., Harding, D. J., Mehta, J., & Roth, W.
(2004). Rampage: The social roots of school
shootings. New York: Basic Books.
Paulsen, D. (2003). Murder in black and white: The newspaper
coverage of homicide in Houston. Homicide
Studies, 7(3), 289–317.
Pew Research Center for the People & the Press. (2008).
Audience segments in a changing news environment:
Key audiences now blend online and traditional sources.
Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/
files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf.
Robinson, M. B. (2011). Media coverage of crime and criminal
justice. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic
Press.
Schildkraut, J. (2012). Media and massacre: A comparative
analysis of the reporting of the 2007 Virginia Tech
shootings. Fast Capitalism, 9(1) Retrieved from
http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/
schildkraut9_1.html.
Schildkraut, J. (2014). Mass murder and the mass media: An
examination of the media discourse on U.S,
rampage shootings, 2000–2012. (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). San Marcos, TX: Texas State
University.
Schildkraut, J., & Donley, A. M. (2012). Murder in black: A
media distortion analysis of homicides in
Baltimore in 2010. Homicide Studies, 16(2), 175–196.
Schildkraut, J., & Elsass, H. J. (2016). Mass shootings: Media,
myths, and realities. Santa Barbara, CA:
Praeger.
Schildkraut, J., & Hernandez, T. C. (2014). Laws that bit the
bullet: A review of legislative responses to school
shootings. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39(2), 358–
374.
Schildkraut, J., & Muschert, G. W. (2014). Media salience and
the framing of mass murder in schools: A
comparison of the Columbine and Sandy Hook massacres.
Homicide Studies, 18(1), 23–43.
Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 463
http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf
http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf
http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/schildkraut9
_1.html
http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/schildkraut9
_1.html
Schildkraut, J., Elsass, H. J., & Stafford, M. C. (2015). Could it
happen here? Moral panics, school shootings,
and fear of crime among college students. Crime, Law and
Social Change, 63(1), 91–110.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002).
Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for
generalized causal inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
Company.
Skogan, W. G., & Maxfield, M. G. (1981). Coping with crime:
Individual and neighborhood reactions.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Stableford, D. (2014). What is a ‘school shooting’? It depends
who you ask. Yahoo News. Retrieved from
http://news.yahoo.com/how-many-school-shootings-since-
sandy-hook-150152463.html
Stretesky, P. B., & Hogan, M. J. (2001). Columbine and student
perceptions of safety: A quasi-experimental
study. Journal of Criminal Justice, 29(5), 429–443.
Surette, R. (1992). Media, crime, & criminal justice: Images
and reality. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole
Publishing Company.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2006). Using multivariate
statistics (5th ed.). Needham Heights, MA:
Pearson.
Tonso, K. L. (2009). Violent masculinities as tropes for school
shooters: The Montréal massacre, the
Columbine attack, and rethinking schools. American Behavioral
Scientist, 52(9), 1266–1285.
Vossekuil, B., Fein, R. A., Reddy, M., Borum, R., &
Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final report and findings of
the safe school initiative: Implications for the prevention of
school attacks in the United States.
Washington, D.C.: United States Secret Service and United
States Department of Education.
Warr, M. (2000). Fear of crime in the United States: Avenues
for research and policy. In D. Duffee (Ed.),
Criminal justice 2000, Measurement and analysis of crime and
justice (vol. 4, pp. 451–489). Washington,
DC: National Institute of Justice.
Warr, M., & Stafford, M. (1983). Fear of victimization: A look
at the proximate causes. Social Forces, 61(4),
1033–1043.
Washington Post-ABC News Poll. (2015). The Washington post.
Retrieved from http://www.washingtonpost.
com/wp-srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html
Weiner, J. (2013). West Orange High shooter a Bdocumented
gang member,^ cops say. Orlando Sentinel.
Retrieved from http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12-
06/news/os-west-orange-high-shooting-gang-
20131206_1_gang-member-gang-affiliation-west-orange-high-
school
Wike, T. L., & Fraser, M. W. (2009). School shootings: Making
sense of the senseless. Aggression and Violent
Behavior, 14(3), 162–169.
H. Jaymi Elsass is a doctoral candidate in the School of
Criminal Justice at Texas State University. Her
primary research interests include episodic violent crime, moral
panics, fear of crime, and juvenile crime. She
has published in Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law & Society,
Crime, Law & Social Change, and an edited
volume.
Jaclyn Schildkraut is an Assistant Professor of Public Justice at
the State University of New York at Oswego.
Her research interests include school shootings, homicide
trends, mediatization effects, and crime theories.
She has published in American Journal of Criminal Justice,
Homicide Studies, Fast Capitalism, Crime, Law &
Social Change, and Criminal Justice Studies, as well as several
edited volumes
Mark C. Stafford is a Professor and the Doctoral Program
Director in the School of Criminal Justice at Texas
State University. He also has held faculty positions at the
University of Texas at Austin and Washington State
University. He has published in Criminology, Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, Quantitative
Criminology, and Journal of Criminal Justice.
464 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464
http://news.yahoo.com/how-many-school-shootings-since-
sandy-hook-150152463.html
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-
srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-
srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html
http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12-06/news/os-west-
orange-high-shooting-gang-20131206_1_gang-member-gang-
affiliation-west-orange-high-school
http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12-06/news/os-west-
orange-high-shooting-gang-20131206_1_gang-member-gang-
affiliation-west-orange-high-school
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without
permission.
Studying School Shootings: Challenges and Considerations for
ResearchAbstractUnderstanding the [Mediatized] Problem of
School ShootingsPrevious Research on School
ShootingsDefining the School Shootings ProblemTheoretical
Approaches to Studying School ShootingsPerceptions of Safety
and Fear of CrimeRoutine Activities and Avoidance
BehaviorsMethodological ComplexitiesData Collection
ConcernsSampling ConsiderationsUnderstanding The
FindingsFear, Risk, and Non-Normal EventsMediatized
OutcomesThe Role of Prior VictimizationCombating The
Challenges Of Studying School ShootingsEmploy a Universally
Accepted DefinitionEnsure the Accuracy of the DataUtilize
Theoretical Perspectives to Support the FindingsGround the
Findings in the Bigger Picture of Homicide and Relative Risk of
VictimizationExpand the Research FocusCombine Both
Qualitative and Quantitative
MethodologiesConclusionReferences
discussion
William Ford Jr., Chairman of Ford Motor Co. said, "A good
company delivers excellent products and services, and a great
company does all that and strives to make the world a better
place."
Supported by evidence from your textbook, the "Starbucks"
case study, and other research, describe two forces that you
believe shape the relationship between business and society.
Provide two examples, one for each force you select. Be
specific in your answer and discuss strengths and weaknesses
via examples and applications. Be certain to cite in APA format
all sources used.
Mass Shootings, Copycat Killers, and the Media
ProQuest document link
Abstract:
Omar Mateen's mass shooting slaughter at Orlando's Pulse
nightclub, where he left 49 innocents dead and 53 more
wounded, has led to the predictable debates over gun control,
the availability and efficacy of mental health resources, our
national response to the threat of international terrorism at
home and abroad, and the readiness and reaction of law
enforcement when such active shooter incidents occur. These
are vital discussions to be had, for no matter your position on
any one of the topics one thing, by now, should be clear: what
happened in Orlando, like what happened in San Bernardino,
and Sandy Hook, and Virginia Tech, and Aurora, CO, and on
and on and on... will happen again. Nothing that arises out of
this particular incident, or the conversations it spurs, or any
legislation it drives, will lead to the "Never Again!" moment
that finally sticks; right now, there are a number of people in
the U.S. alone - men mostly, but surely a woman or two at any
given time - just waiting to strike and lay claim to their piece of
infamy.
A recent Chicago Tribune article ("Are media complicit in mass
shootings?" by reporter Alan Zarambo) looked at how media
coverage of mass slayings might be influencing others to
emulate the killers. Zarambo cites research by Arizona State
University researcher Sherry Towers showing that shootings
often occur in "clusters." Towers' study found:
Full text:
Omar Mateen's mass shooting slaughter at Orlando's Pulse
nightclub, where he left 49 innocents dead and 53 more
wounded, has led to the predictable debates over gun control,
the availability and efficacy of mental health resources, our
national response to the threat of international terrorism at
home and abroad, and the readiness and reaction of law
enforcement when such active shooter incidents occur. These
are vital discussions to be had, for no matter your position on
any one of the topics one thing, by now, should be clear: what
happened in Orlando, like what happened in San Bernardino,
and Sandy Hook, and Virginia Tech, and Aurora, CO, and on
and on and on... will happen again. Nothing that arises out of
this particular incident, or the conversations it spurs, or any
legislation it drives, will lead to the "Never Again!" moment
that finally sticks; right now, there are a number of people in
the U.S. alone - men mostly, but surely a woman or two at any
given time - just waiting to strike and lay claim to their piece of
infamy.
As fatalistic as that sounds, it really isn't. Examination of, and
debate around, the issues of mass shootings is a crucial first
step to reducing the number and lethality of such events, even if
outright elimination of the phenomena is presently unrealistic.
We can argue whether Mateen's crimes were a terroristic act
driven by allegiance to ISIS ideals of Islamic jihad, a
homophobic hate crime, or a symbolic gesture of horrifically
externalized self-hatred over his own conflicted sexuality (or a
terrible combo of all three, as I believe) but what we learn
through reasoned argument is our best hope to understand and,
hopefully, intervene to head off the next attack.
Two additional concerns... for law enforcement and
society Sometimes lost amid the obvious debate topics already
mentioned above are two that are less prevalent, though more
immediate and readily addressed. The first is the issue of
copycat crimes inspired by the actions of high-profile killers
who, whether in death or capture, achieve a degree of attention,
notoriety, and even respect few will ever know. While most of
us recoil in horror at wanton destruction of human life, reviling
those who would take it, others look on with reverence, envious
of the limelight the shooter basks in. As improbable and sick as
it sounds, certain impressionable and deluded individuals will
never grasp the revulsion most of us feel. Others see the rest of
us as weak, ineffectual
15 September 2016 Page 1 of 3 ProQuest
sheep, blind to their version of truth, our sympathy for the
victims emblematic of our acceptance of a sickness that must be
excised from society. They may see shooters as martyrs, selfless
in their actions and worthy of imitation (or to be topped). This
creates obvious problems for law enforcement. A recent
Chicago Tribune article ("Are media complicit in mass
shootings?" by reporter Alan Zarambo) looked at how media
coverage of mass slayings might be influencing others to
emulate the killers. Zarambo cites research by Arizona State
University researcher Sherry Towers showing that shootings
often occur in "clusters." Towers' study found:
That these clusters exist in today's world, barring the possibility
of mere (if then strangely predictable) coincidence, certainly
has to do with the reach of modern media, including social
media and its effect on the sense of individual self-importance
and how it accelerates the pace and reach of breaking
news. For law enforcement, this means that the weeks
following a mass shooting are a critical time for increased
vigilance. Patrol officers should give greater attention to the
otherwise routine calls for service that might indicate a copycat
situation, school resource officers should heighten their
awareness of the angry and disaffected, and how they may
emulate a high-profile antihero, and intel officers should
probably look more closely at the characters in their areas of
responsibility who fit certain profiles - politically,
psychologically, and temperamentally - of concern for acting
out. This is a time that begs increased information sharing and
foresight in planning, especially as the disaffected are
emboldened or even challenged to make their own violent
contributions.
The second major concern is the question of how media reports
on these incidents. While I don't believe media to be complicit
in the attacks, that coverage of one high-profile mass shooting
tends to spawn more in the days and weeks following is
becoming more and more evident (In fact, I would be interested
in seeing if there is a correlation over coverage of day-to-day
violence, as in Chicago and some other large urban areas seeing
a significant uptick in shootings, that normalizes it to the
degree inhibitions are lowered among others who then see
violence as a viable option for settling disputes).
There are those who argue the killers/shooters should not even
be named, that media should withhold certain information about
the offender to instead put greater emphasis on the victims. This
has, to a very limited degree, been tried with a couple recent
shootings wherein law enforcement agencies have provided very
little information directly, in the hopes of not offering shooters
the attention they seek. The down side is this: I want to know
about the shooter. I want information about his background,
history, motives, and actions. Information is important to
understanding - and good public policy making - even if that
information may also be consumed and misused by those who
look to the wrong role models.
Of course, there will certainly be those who disagree, and we
welcome respectful disagreement. Or perhaps there is wise
middle ground that can strike a balance and reduce the risk of
copycat killers. But these incidents are not going away. We
should find that middle ground sooner rather than later.
Publication title: Officer.com Publication year: 2016
Publication date: Jul 6, 2016 Year: 2016
Publisher: SouthComm Business Media LLC Place of
publication: Fort Atkinson Country of publication: United
States Publication subject: Criminology And Law Enforcement
15 September 2016 Page 2 of 3 ProQuest
Source type: Trade Journals Language of publication: English
Document type: News ProQuest document ID: 1808327229
Document URL:
https://libraryresources.columbiasouthern.edu/login?url=http://s
earch.proquest.com/docview/1808327229?acco untid=33337
Copyright: © Copyright 2016 Cygnus Business Media Last
updated: 2016-08-03 Database: Criminal Justice Database
_____________________________________________________
__________
uest LLC. All rights
reserved. - Terms and Conditions

More Related Content

Similar to Studying School Shootings Challengesand Considerations for .docx

An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...Liz Adams
 
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNING
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNINGJOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNING
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNINGMuhammad Baari
 
Achievement Goals And Current Events Knowledge
Achievement Goals And Current Events KnowledgeAchievement Goals And Current Events Knowledge
Achievement Goals And Current Events KnowledgeDarian Pruitt
 
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docx
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docxEmpirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docx
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docxSALU18
 
INTS 3300 Literature Review
INTS 3300 Literature ReviewINTS 3300 Literature Review
INTS 3300 Literature ReviewAlex Agnew
 
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docxdurantheseldine
 
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace Bullying
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace BullyingA Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace Bullying
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace BullyingBria Davis
 
Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222AdiOnita
 
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
 University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docxaryan532920
 
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example Topi...
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example  Topi...Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example  Topi...
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example Topi...Chelsea Cote
 
Driving Age Essay.pdf
Driving Age Essay.pdfDriving Age Essay.pdf
Driving Age Essay.pdfEvelin Santos
 
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdf
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdfRomeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdf
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdfVicki Ardon
 
Research Proposal Edited
Research Proposal EditedResearch Proposal Edited
Research Proposal EditedKristin Bayer
 
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docx
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docxESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docx
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docxSALU18
 
Definition and measurement of cyberbullying
Definition and measurement of cyberbullyingDefinition and measurement of cyberbullying
Definition and measurement of cyberbullyingMarcelo Pesallaccia
 
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docx
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docxPSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docx
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docxpotmanandrea
 

Similar to Studying School Shootings Challengesand Considerations for .docx (20)

Webb new
Webb newWebb new
Webb new
 
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
An Overview of Measurement Issues in School Violence and School Safety Resear...
 
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNING
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNINGJOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNING
JOURNAL OF CHEMISTRY'S MEDIA LEARNING
 
Jurnal media
Jurnal mediaJurnal media
Jurnal media
 
Achievement Goals And Current Events Knowledge
Achievement Goals And Current Events KnowledgeAchievement Goals And Current Events Knowledge
Achievement Goals And Current Events Knowledge
 
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docx
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docxEmpirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docx
Empirical Article Review and Class DiscussionOrientation to the .docx
 
INTS 3300 Literature Review
INTS 3300 Literature ReviewINTS 3300 Literature Review
INTS 3300 Literature Review
 
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx
1465American Behavioral ScientistVolume 52 Number 10Ju.docx
 
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace Bullying
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace BullyingA Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace Bullying
A Summary Review Of Literature Relating To Workplace Bullying
 
Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222Adriana inter d222
Adriana inter d222
 
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
 University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
 
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example Topi...
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example  Topi...Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example  Topi...
Driving Age Essay. Should the driving age be raised to 21 Essay Example Topi...
 
Driving Age Essay.pdf
Driving Age Essay.pdfDriving Age Essay.pdf
Driving Age Essay.pdf
 
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdf
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdfRomeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdf
Romeo And Juliet Death Essay.pdf
 
Research Proposal Edited
Research Proposal EditedResearch Proposal Edited
Research Proposal Edited
 
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docx
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docxESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docx
ESSAY INSTRUCTIONSI. TITLE & NUMBER OF THE COURSE ENGL 1301 
.docx
 
Definition and measurement of cyberbullying
Definition and measurement of cyberbullyingDefinition and measurement of cyberbullying
Definition and measurement of cyberbullying
 
Coffey_3300_L3-A1
Coffey_3300_L3-A1Coffey_3300_L3-A1
Coffey_3300_L3-A1
 
poster
posterposter
poster
 
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docx
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docxPSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docx
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTERESTVOL. 4, NO.docx
 

More from picklesvalery

NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docx
NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docxNPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docx
NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docx
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docxNow that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docx
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docx
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docxNow that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docx
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docx
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docxNow that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docx
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docx
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docxNow that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docx
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docx
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docxNow that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docx
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docxpicklesvalery
 
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docx
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docxNow that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docx
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docxpicklesvalery
 
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docx
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docxNovel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docx
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docxpicklesvalery
 
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docx
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docxNotifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docx
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docxpicklesvalery
 
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docx
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docxNovember-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docx
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docxpicklesvalery
 
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docx
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docxNOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docx
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docxpicklesvalery
 
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docx
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docxNOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docx
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docxpicklesvalery
 
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docx
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docxNOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docx
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docx
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docxNote Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docx
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docx
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docxNote I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docx
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docx
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docxNote Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docx
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docx
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docxNote Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docx
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docx
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docxNote A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docx
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docxpicklesvalery
 
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docx
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docxNotable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docx
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docxpicklesvalery
 
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docx
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docxNote 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docx
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docxpicklesvalery
 

More from picklesvalery (20)

NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docx
NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docxNPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docx
NPV, IRR, Payback period,— PA1Correlates with CLA2 (NPV portion.docx
 
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docx
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docxNow that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docx
Now that you have had the opportunity to review various Cyber At.docx
 
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docx
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docxNow that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docx
Now that you have completed a series of assignments that have led yo.docx
 
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docx
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docxNow that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docx
Now that you have completed your paper (ATTACHED), build and deliver.docx
 
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docx
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docxNow that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docx
Now that you have identified the revenue-related internal contro.docx
 
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docx
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docxNow that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docx
Now that you have read about Neandertals and modern Homo sapiens.docx
 
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docx
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docxNow that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docx
Now that you have had an opportunity to explore ethics formally, cre.docx
 
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docx
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docxNovel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docx
Novel Literary Exploration EssayWrite a Literary Exploration Ess.docx
 
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docx
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docxNotifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docx
Notifications My CommunityHomeBBA 3551-16P-5A19-S3, Inform.docx
 
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docx
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docxNovember-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docx
November-December 2013 • Vol. 22No. 6 359Beverly Waller D.docx
 
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docx
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docxNOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docx
NOTEPlease pay attention to the assignment instructionsZero.docx
 
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docx
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docxNOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docx
NOTE Use below Textbooks only. 400 WordsTopic Which doctrine.docx
 
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docx
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docxNOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docx
NOTE Everything in BOLD are things that I need to turn in for m.docx
 
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docx
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docxNote Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docx
Note Be sure to focus only on the causes of the problem in this.docx
 
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docx
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docxNote I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docx
Note I’ll provide my sources in the morning, and lmk if you hav.docx
 
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docx
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docxNote Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docx
Note Here, the company I mentioned was Qualcomm 1. Email is the.docx
 
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docx
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docxNote Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docx
Note Please follow instructions to the T.Topic of 3 page pape.docx
 
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docx
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docxNote A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docx
Note A full-sentence outline differs from bullet points because e.docx
 
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docx
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docxNotable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docx
Notable photographers 1980 to presentAlmas, ErikAraki, No.docx
 
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docx
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docxNote 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docx
Note 2 political actions that are in line with Socialism and explain.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Shubhangi Sonawane
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docxPoojaSen20
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfSanaAli374401
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Disha Kariya
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 

Studying School Shootings Challengesand Considerations for .docx

  • 1. Studying School Shootings: Challenges and Considerations for Research H. Jaymi Elsass1 & Jaclyn Schildkraut2 & Mark C. Stafford1 Received: 4 June 2015 /Accepted: 19 October 2015 / Published online: 29 October 2015 # Southern Criminal Justice Association 2015 Abstract Studying school shootings can be both a fruitful and challenging endeavor. The random nature of these events provides a number of challenges for studying this phenomenon. This paper explores these concerns as they relate to developing and implementing studies, as well as interpreting related findings by drawing on previous research that examined the effects of the 1999 Columbine High School, the 2007 Virginia Tech, and the 2008 Northern Illinois University shootings. Ways in which these issues may be overcome and, more generally, the research can be moved forward also are discussed. Keywords School shootings . Research methodologies . Theoretical orientation . Columbine Beginning in the mid-1990s, culminating with the 1999 shooting
  • 2. at Columbine High School, and reiterated through more recent events, including Virginia Tech (2007) and Sandy Hook (2012), school shootings have become characterized by many as a social problem (Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut et al. 2015; see also BWashington Post-ABC News Poll^ 2015). Each event is intentional, designed to cause numerous deaths, and is highly publicized, particularly through the media (Addington 2003; Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut 2014; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014). These events also have the poten- tial to affect people who are both local and spatially distant from the shooting (Addington 2003). Given their vast reach, coupled with varied responses by groups within society (see, for example, Schildkraut and Hernandez 2014), understanding the associated impacts of school shootings is particularly important. Researchers have made strides in examining the effects of specific school shooting events, including Columbine (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002; Stretesky and Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 DOI 10.1007/s12103-015-9311-9 * H. Jaymi Elsass [email protected] 1 Texas State University, 601 University Drive, San Marcos, TX 78666, USA 2 State University of New York at Oswego, Oswego, NY, USA http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s12103-015-
  • 3. 9311-9&domain=pdf Hogan 2001) and Virginia Tech and Northern Illinois University (NIU) in 2008 (Kaminski et al. 2010; see also Fallahi et al. 2009). Each of these studies makes an important contribution to the limited, yet growing, body of literature on school shootings. While each has yielded findings that prompt further inquiry, these studies also share a number of limitations that require caution in the interpretation of their results. Still, they provide an opportunity to advance the research on a rare phenomenon. As such, this paper focuses on examining such challenges to studying school shootings, as well as offers considerations for how research designs can be improved in future studies. Specifically, these prior studies are examined, underscoring specific methodological challenges encountered during the research process. From there, addi- tional considerations are offered with regards to the interpretation of specific findings, and methods through which these issues can be addressed. Finally, discussion is offered about approaches to moving the larger body of research in school shootings forward to provide a more comprehensive and robust analysis of these events. Understanding the [Mediatized] Problem of School Shootings
  • 4. Despite that crime rates in the United States are declining, and homicide specifically is especially rare, many people believe that school shootings are becoming epidemic, occurring more frequently than they actually are (Muschert and Ragnedda 2010; Newman 2006; Schildkraut et al. 2015). In reality, however, there are approximately 10 events per year, on average (Schildkraut 2012). One of the main factors that drives the disproportional beliefs about the frequency of school shootings is the amount of attention they garner in the media (Elsass et al. 2014). Despite that these events typically last for 10 min or less, media coverage often extends days, weeks, and, with the most salient cases, a month or more (Muschert 2002; Schildkraut 2012, 2014; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014).1 As Kellner (2003, 2008a, 2008b) notes, these events become, at varying intensities, Bmedia spectacles,^ with every facet of the story splashed across headlines or permeating television and computer screens. These spec- tacles essentially take relatively uncommon events, sensationalize them, and make the events appear far more commonplace than they actually are (Kellner 2008a; Surette 1992). When the story first broke of the Columbine High School shooting, for example, CNN aired six hours of uninterrupted live coverage (Muschert 2002). Three major news networks – ABC, CBS, and NBC – devoted more than half of their nightly news
  • 5. airtime to coverage of the shooting over the following month (Robinson 2011), with 53 stories in the first week alone (Maguire et al. 2002). In total, 319 stories were aired over all nightly news broadcasts throughout the remainder of 1999 (Robinson 2011). Coverage of other highly salient events, including the Virginia Tech and Sandy Hook shootings, have followed similar patterns. In addition to television news, disproportional coverage of school shooting events also is evident through other media. In the year following Columbine, over 10,000 1 The 1999 Columbine High School shooting is a notable exception. It has been estimated that the total event lasted just under 50 min (Columbine Review Commission 2001). Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 445 articles were published about the shooting in the nation’s top 50 newspapers (Newman 2006), including 170 articles in The New York Times alone (Chyi and McCombs 2004; Muschert and Carr 2006). The Times also published over 130 articles about both Virginia Tech (Schildkraut 2012, 2014) and Sandy Hook (Schildkraut 2014; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014) in the first 30 days after each shooting. While other events, such as those at NIU in 2008 or Chardon High School in 2012, also have captured national attention, they did so with lower frequencies
  • 6. of coverage (Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut 2014). Thus, while school shootings have been overrepresented in the media, the disproportional coverage also is exacerbated by the focus on the most extreme (and deadly) examples (Burns and Crawford 1999; Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014; see also Maguire et al. 2002; Robinson 2011). Cohen (1963) has noted that the media Bmay not be successful much of the time in telling people what to think, but it is stunningly successful in telling people what to think about^ (p. 13). Such a sentiment is particularly relevant as the media serves as the main source of information for up to 95 % of the general public (Surette 1992). The agenda-setting function of the media often is achieved through story selection decisions and how they are framed (see, for example, McCombs 1997; McCombs and Shaw 1972). Specifically, researchers have found that up to 50 % of news coverage is dedicated to stories about crime, and despite that property- related offenses are consid- erably more common, a disproportionate amount of this attention is focused on the most serious and violent crimes (Chermak 1995; Gruenewald et al. 2009; Paulsen 2003; Schildkraut and Donley 2012). As such, the emphasis on Bhigh amplitude^ or sensational cases, such as school shootings, both in the amount and prominence of the coverage allocated, has the ability to shape public perceptions about a particular event
  • 7. or phenomenon (Johnstone et al. 1994; see also Chermak 1994). Previous Research on School Shootings Though the body of research examining school shootings is limited, several researchers have taken important first steps in studying the effects. At the time of the Columbine shooting, researchers at the Rochester Institute of Technology (RIT) were conducting a campus dating violence study (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The researchers adapted their study in order to examine the effects of Columbine on students' perceptions of safety (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The findings of the study indicated that, when compared to those surveyed prior to the shootings, respondents' perceived safety was significantly lower after Columbine (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Despite methodolog- ical limitations, Stretesky and Hogan’s (2001) work provides one of the earliest considerations of the impacts of such rare events. Two other studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002) also examined the impact of the Columbine shootings, but did so using national data sources. Addington (2003) utilized the School Crime Supplement of the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS-SCS), while Brener et al. (2002) employed data from the 1999 Youth Risk Behavior Survey (YRBS). Aside from differences in study design and respective challenges, these studies found similar patterns related to the effects of the shooting.
  • 8. Addington (2003) found that students expressed greater fear while at school after Columbine as compared to those surveyed prior to it, though the effect size was small. 446 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 Similarly, Brener et al. (2002) found that following the shooting, many respondents expressed being too fearful to attend school, and consequently, were more likely to avoid school altogether. Researchers also have examined the effects of shootings taking place on college campuses. Fallahi et al. (2009) researched the effect of the 2007 Virginia Tech shootings on students at Central Connecticut State University. They found that students who consumed more media coverage of the shooting both were more fearful and more likely to believe a similar event would happen again (Fallahi et al. 2009). These same respondents, however, were less inclined to express the belief that a similar attack would happen on their campus (Fallahi et al. 2009). Similarly, Kaminski et al. (2010) examined the impact of both Virginia Tech and 2008 NIU shootings on students at the University of South Carolina (USC). They found that students reported greater fear of murder and of
  • 9. being attacked with a weapon following both shootings (Kaminski et al. 2010). Demographic variables, such as gender, age, and race/ethnicity, also were significantly corre- lated with changes in fear, with females reporting being more fearful than males (Kaminski et al. 2010; see also Fallahi et al. 2009). Additionally, Kaminski et al. (2010) also found that older students and non- whites expressed greater fear, as compared to younger respondents and whites, respectively. Further, students living on campus expressed greater fear following the shoot- ings (Kaminski et al. 2010). While each of these studies makes an important contribution to the growing body of research on school shootings, methodological issues present in each work, coupled with the complexities inherent in studying a phenomenon that is extremely (and statistically) rare in nature, produces a number of serious limitations. The careful examination of such limitations allows for discussion about how to minimize their effects. Such a discourse will assist school shootings researchers in better handling common method- ological issues, thereby helping to propel the body of literature forward. Defining the School Shootings Problem Perhaps the biggest challenge in studying school shootings, particularly when
  • 10. trying to determine their prevalence, is the absence of a precise definition for the phenomenon. Both in the academic community and beyond, little agreement has arisen with regard to how best to define these events. Specifically, a number of different definitions, each with their own inherent limitations, have been put forth by various agencies and organizations. In a joint report between the U.S. Department of Education and the Secret Service, for example, the researchers define Bincidents of targeted school violence,^ as a category which includes (but is not limited to) school shootings, as. Any incident where (i) a current student or recent former student attacked someone at his or her school with lethal means (e.g., a gun or knife); and, (ii) where the student attacker purposefully chose his or her school as the location of the attack. (Vossekuil et al. 2002, p. 7) Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 447 In the School-Associated Violent Death Study published by the Center for Disease Control (CDC), in conjunction with the U.S. Department of Education and the U.S. Department of Justice, criteria for inclusion in the report relies on defining events as. A case is defined as a fatal injury (e.g., homicide, suicide, or
  • 11. legal intervention) that occurs on school property, on the way to/from school, or during or on the way to/from a school-sponsored event. Only violent deaths associated with U.S. elementary and secondary schools, both public and private, are included. (Centers for Disease Control 2014) While the CDC does not claim that all events in the report are school shootings, statistics reported are given in aggregates of total incidents; when used to explain school shootings, they provide individuals with an overinflated perception of their prevalence. In the same vein, the overly broad crafting of their definition suggests that events such as a suicide at a bus stop involving a firearm essentially could be counted as a school shooting. Following the attack at Sandy Hook Elementary School, Everytown for Gun Safety (2014), a national advocacy group, purported that Bin the two years since the mass shooting in Newtown, Connecticut, there have been at least 94 school shootings including fatal and nonfatal assaults, suicides, and unintentional shootings – an average of nearly one a week.^ This claim, however, was problematic based on the definition the organization used to identify events: Incidents were classified as school shootings when a firearm was discharged inside a school building or on school or campus grounds, as
  • 12. documented by the press or confirmed through further inquiries with law enforcement. Incidents in which guns were brought into schools but not fired, or were fired off school grounds after having been possessed in schools, were not included. (Everytown for Gun Safety 2014) As a result of this choice of wording, accidental weapons discharges were included in their count, as were events that differed in motivation from school shootings, such as those involving gangs. In sum, since there is no universally accepted definition, there is, in turn, disagreement regarding the number of events that are (or are not occurring) each year. Theoretical Approaches to Studying School Shootings For many researchers, utilizing a deductive approach to study a particular phenomenon is beneficial as it offers theoretical propositions to help guide the research hypotheses to be tested (Bryman 2012). For researchers studying school shootings, however, finding an appropriate, and more importantly, testable, theory can present a challenge. Specif- ically, the majority of criminological theories – including biological, psychological, strain, and learning– focus on understanding why individuals engage in delinquent or criminal activities. While a case study approach can be used to look for common attributes among school shooters, the majority commit suicide;
  • 13. therefore, the amount of 448 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 available data is limited, as is the sample size, to test specific theoretical propositions.2 Similarly, understanding why people become victims of school shootings is limited by a lack of theoretical insight; only one group of theories – routine activities – underscore the opportunity for victimization to occur. Instead, researchers commonly have focused on understanding individuals' re- sponses to school shooting events (Addington 2003; Warr 2000). This broader exam- ination is advantageous for several reasons. First, given the extensive reach of the media, a greater number of people may indirectly become victims of these events (Addington 2003; Skogan and Maxfield 1981; Warr 2000). As such, this provides the opportunity to examine the effects of these events on a larger sample. Additionally, it is these very responses that have one of the greatest abilities to influence policy decisions that stem from the events. Most commonly, responses to school shootings as they relate to fear of crime or perceived safety have been examined (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002; Fallahi et al. 2009; Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Other outcomes, such as the use of avoidance behaviors (Addington
  • 14. 2003; Brener et al. 2002), also have been examined. Perceptions of Safety and Fear of Crime One area of inquiry that has been focused on by researchers is fear of crime, coupled with associated perceptions of risk or safety, among the greater public. Although the body of literature on fear of crime is ample, spanning over 40 years, these studies focus on more ordinary crimes and, by design, are ill-suited to capture the effects of rare crimes, such as school shootings (Warr 2000). Addington (2003) acknowledges this challenge, noting that Bno theoretical or empirical precedent is available to predict Columbine’s [or other similar events'] effect on fear [or perceived risk of victimization]^ (p. 368). Therefore, studies examining the effects of Columbine and other school shootings have placed greater emphasis on understanding individual reactions to the event, rather than the causes of the event itself. One issue that complicates this area of inquiry, however, stems from researchers' handling of these outcome measures. Perceived risk and fear of crime are related, yet distinct constructs, and, therefore, it is important to differentiate between them, as the former is hypothesized to cause the latter (Warr 2000; Warr and Stafford 1983). Highlighting this difference, Ferraro (1995) further contends that perceived risk is cognitive, while fear of crime is emotional.
  • 15. A number of the studies examining fear and perceived risk related to school shootings assert that they are distinct constructs in their reviews of the literature, but fail to include measures of each. Addington (2003) discussed the role of perceived risk and social distance in her study of the effects of the Columbine High School shooting, but through no fault of her own, was unable to include measures of perceived risk, as they are not available in the NCVS-SCS. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) faced the opposite problem. Their survey instru- ment focused solely on perceived risk, which they referred to as Bperceptions of safety^ (Stretesky and Hogan 2001, p. 430). This forced the researchers to forgo an examination of the prospective effects on fear of crime post-Columbine and instead focus on the potential 2 In a study examining the broader category of rampage shooters following Columbine, Schildkraut (2014) found that 55 % of perpetrators in the study committed suicide. Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 449 impact of the event on perceived risk. Since these constructs are measuring two different outcomes, the results from Addington’s (2003) and Stretesky and Hogan’s (2001) studies are not readily comparable. Instead, when possible, measures of both fear and perceived risk should be included in order for researchers to glean a more complete understanding of
  • 16. individuals' responses to such events. The issue of understanding the impact of school shootings on fear of crime or perceptions of risk further is exacerbated by the way in which these outcomes are measured. Both are multifaceted constructs, and this complexity must be acknowledged in its measurement to ensure validity. Addington (2003), for example, utilized two questions from the NCVS-SCS to measure fear of crime, which in itself is problematic. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) also used two dependent variables, but were able to address the multidimensional nature of safety perceptions by using composite measures rather than single questions. Still, as different studies have measured fear of crime, or some other related aspect such as perceived risk, in different ways, it begs the question: What constitutes the best way to measure fear of crime? Routine Activities and Avoidance Behaviors According to Warr (2000), fear also may be assessed by examining avoidance behav- iors. When an individual is fearful, they may change or limit their routine activities or employ avoidance behaviors in an attempt to combat the perceived threat (Hindelang et al. 1978). The use of avoidance behaviors is not solely limited to how unsafe one perceives their environment to be; impressions of safety also may be influenced by associating, either directly or indirectly (via the media in some instances), with victims
  • 17. who share similar demographic characteristics to the individual (Hindelang et al. 1978; see also Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Thus, the use of avoidance behaviors, such as avoiding certain areas of campus, not being out at night, or not traveling alone, serves to minimize exposure to potential victimization (Hindelang et al. 1978). Two studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002) specifically examined changes in self- reported avoidance behaviors following the Columbine shooting. Brener et al. (2002) found that students were more likely to report missing at least one day of school following Columbine as compared to those surveyed before the shooting. Additionally, Addington (2003) utilized questions from the NCVS-SCS that focused on avoiding specific areas of school. This supplementary analysis was included as an alternate examination of the impact of the shooting on students' fear (Addington 2003; see also Warr 2000). Unlike Brener et al. (2002), however, Addington (2003) did not find a significant difference in the use of avoidance behaviors between the pre- and post-Columbine groups. In their examination of the impact of the Virginia Tech and NIU shootings on college students, Kaminski et al. (2010) found that respondents expressed being more fearful of routine activities, such as walking alone on campus, but did not assess whether they employed any form of avoidance behaviors to combat these beliefs. Methodological Complexities
  • 18. When studying school shootings, researchers are faced with a number of unique methodological concerns that can affect studies' findings. Due to the rarity of these 450 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 events, each decision made has the potential to greatly limit confidence in the findings regarding the phenomena. Specifically, careful contemplation must be given to deci- sions related to data collection, sampling sizes, and timing, as well as potential spatial and temporal effects that may influence outcome measures. Beyond these decisions' impacts on the findings, such issues also are imperative in shaping the research from its inception. Data Collection Concerns Data collection perhaps is the most difficult facet of a research study to plan when attempting to study events of episodic violent crime. Due to the unanticipated nature of school shootings, researchers are presented with a number of study design options to investigate an event’s effect. The first option is to utilize national surveys, such as the NCVS-SCS or the YRBS (see Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002). Alternatively, researchers may be conducting a survey at the time an incident takes place, and can
  • 19. adapt their data collection around the event (see generally, Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Primary Data Collection It sometimes is the case when studying this kind of phe- nomenon that after data collection has begun, a school shooting takes place, thereby changing the purpose and focus of the original research endeavor. While the ideal research design would be to utilize the same group of respondents for both pre- and post-event samples, it is rare that researchers are able to survey the same individuals before and after. This is especially relevant if IRB regulations require anonymity, thereby thwarting the possibility of resurveying pre-event respondents or if the shooting occurs at a time that makes data collection particularly difficult, like during a change in semesters. Therefore, once an event occurs, investigators are likely to be faced with the task of limiting differences between pre- and post-event samples, thus necessarily increasing one’s reliance on convenience or purposive sampling techniques. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) encountered this challenge in their examination of changes in fear of crime as a result of the Columbine High School shooting. They originally were studying campus dating violence at RIT through the dissemination of separate surveys to male and female respondents, with only the female survey contain- ing questions about perceived safety (Stretesky and Hogan
  • 20. 2001). After the shootings at Columbine, the researchers adapted their original research design to focus on changes in perceptions of safety as a result of the event; however, because only the female survey contained such questions, they only were able to investigate the one group’s changes in perceptions of safety, thereby reducing the generalizability of the findings (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The impact of reduced representativeness can be dimin- ished through a few different methods, however, including the use of internal compar- ison groups (Shadish et al. 2002). Accordingly, Stretesky and Hogan (2001) instituted random selection from a designated sampling frame that included only open enrollment liberal arts classes to achieve this end. Analysis of Secondary Data One way in which representativeness can be increased is through the analysis of secondary data sources. Brener et al. (2002) used the YRBS and employed Ba three-stage cluster-sample design to obtain a national representative Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 451 sample of students in grades 9 through 12^ (p. 147). Additionally, increased represen- tativeness was achieved in Addington’s (2003) study by capitalizing on the NCVS- SCS' random allocation design, which is attained through the survey’s rotating panel
  • 21. design and interview schedule (p. 372). Yet, the analysis of secondary data in these instances is not without its disadvan- tages. Though large sample sizes may be more representative of the population, they can also cause statistics to appear significant when they are not (Shadish et al. 2002). In the case of large samples, t-tests have so much power that even a very small difference can produce results reaching statistical significance (Shadish et al. 2002). The likeli- hood of a Type I error can be reduced though a number of methods, including the Bonferroni correction, the use of conservative multiple comparison follow-up tests in analysis of variance, and the use of a multivariate analysis of variance if multiple dependent variables are tested (Shadish et al. 2002). Still, the tradeoff for using national-level secondary data can be considerable. When studying possible changes in attitudes as a result of a rare violent crime, such as a school shooting, Warr (2000) notes that capturing the full effects of the phenomenon is difficult without a pretest measure. This information, in most instances, is lacking in annual or national surveys because the temporal or spatial effects of such events typically have only a short-term impact at the local level. This is an obvious limitation of using national survey data to study this phenomenon, as they are unlikely to contain all of the necessary measures.
  • 22. Spatial Event Proximity Spatial proximity to a school shooting presents an important challenge, as the effects of the event may be diffused the further the respondent is from an incident’s location (Warr 2000). Researchers (e.g., Heath 1984; Liska and Baccaglini 1990) have found that people respond differently when crime is local (see also Addington 2003). As Heath (1984) noted, Bthe worse things are elsewhere, the better we feel about our immediate environment^ (p. 270). Since both Addington (2003) and Brener et al. (2002) opted to use national data in their examinations of Columbine, the majority of respondents surveyed were from areas outside of Littleton, the Denver metropolitan area, or even the state of Colorado. Therefore, it is possible that the event did not strongly affect respondents who were spatially distant from the shooting (Addington 2003). One potential way to make such a determination regarding respon- dents' spatial proximity to a school shooting would be to include a control measure for the location of the respondent (e.g., city, state, or region, based on data availability). Temporal Event Proximity Temporal proximity to a school shooting also provides a challenge for researchers. While it is unknown specifically how long the aftermath of these events lasts, it is likely to be short (Warr 2000; see also Addington 2003; Stretesky and Hogan 2001). For example, researchers who have examined this phe- nomenon have found that coverage in the media typically lasts
  • 23. around 30 days (Chyi and McCombs 2004; Muschert and Carr 2006; Schildkraut 2012; Schildkraut and Muschert 2014). By comparison, topics that take more time to develop, such as politics, have an average lifespan of 18.5 months (McCombs and Zhu 1995). Thus, given the volatile nature of school shootings, as well as the subsequent reactions to them, it is possible that with the passage of time, any effects or changes in outcome measures may be diminished. At the same time, however, it is possible that, particularly with highly 452 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 salient events, the effects may increase over time, thus not being able to be captured in cross-sectional surveys. Even when studies are able to collect data immediately prior to and after an event, there still are potential limitations in assessing changes in the outcome measures. Surveying respondents directly after an event in order to capture its immediate impact may not only fail to find significant effects due to a possible delay, as may have been a problem with Kaminski et al. (2010) study, but also is unable to assess any long-term developing effects. The data collection period for Stretesky and Hogan (2001) was 5 days prior to Columbine and 15 days after the shooting. Given this
  • 24. shortened time frame, the authors note that it was impossible to determine the length of the effects of Columbine (Stretesky and Hogan 2001). The issue of seasonality, as highlighted by Addington (2003), is a supplementary confounding factor that must be considered with regard to the timing of data collection. Perhaps then, it would be most fruitful to survey respondents as quickly as possible after a school shooting takes place and incorporate into the study design multiple post-event collection periods to track the progression of event effects over time. Similarly, when surveys are designed for a purpose other than capturing the effects of school shootings, questions included may present challenges for disentangling the actual impact of the event. In Brener et al. (2002) study, for example, the researchers utilized a measure assessing whether students had missed school in the 30 days prior to taking the survey because of feeling unsafe. Such a question is problematic, however, based on when a respondent actually completed the survey. If the survey was completed on April 21 (the shooting occurred on April 20), the question arises as to whether the reported avoidance is attribut- able to Columbine, which would likely be freshest in the respondent’s mind, rather than other events that took place during the preceding 29 days. Con- versely, if the survey was completed on May 19, and similar changes in the avoidance of
  • 25. school were found, it is equally difficult to determine if the change is a result of the shooting, or if some other event occurring after Columbine caused a respondent’s absence. One potential way to address this issue, as is employed by Addington (2003), is to include controls for interview month and day. This, however, requires the information to be available in the dataset used; it is unclear if this information was available to Brener et al. (2002). Finally, when considering the temporally-associated issues related to data collection, one also must consider the possibility of other events occurring during the same period (Kaminski et al. 2010). Though Columbine often is regarded as the archetypal school shooting to which all other events are measured (Altheide 2009; Larkin 2007, 2009; Muschert 2007b; Muschert and Larkin 2007), it is not the only event that occurred during the data collection period of several of the studies mentioned thus far. Addington (2003) utilized data from the first six months of 1999, during which time, besides Columbine, another shooting occurred on May 20 in Conyers, Georgia at Heritage High School. Brener et al. (2002) data collection period also encompassed this shoot- ing. While Columbine is considerably more noteworthy than the Conyers shooting, it is possible that the latter event still had an effect, particularly if any of the respondents surveyed were in the vicinity of the shooting. As such,
  • 26. including control measures, such as the day and month (Addington 2003) or the occurrence of another event, can be helpful. Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 453 Sampling Considerations A supplementary concern involves the investigation of a school shooting directly impacting one population, like high school students, but its effect being studied on a separate population, such as college students. Why should a shooting at a high school affect college students' fear, as was investigated by Stretesky and Hogan (2001)? While demographic overlap between victims and study respondents beyond age partially may account for why these kinds of comparisons are valid, researchers still must consider who to sample in the first place. Yet, regardless of sampling method, and even when employing techniques to minimize the impact of separate pre- and post-event samples, differences often remain on a number of characteristics. Brener et al. (2002) and Stretesky and Hogan (2001) faced opposite ends of a similar problem regarding pre- and post-Columbine sample sizes. In the study conducted by Brener et al. (2002), approximately 78.4 % of the surveys had been collected prior to the shooting at Columbine High School. This
  • 27. resulted in the pre-Columbine sample including 12,049 cases and the post-Columbine sample including 3137 cases (Brener et al. 2002). The effect of the difference in sample size pre- and post-event is minimized however, because both samples were so large, as in essence, large sample sizes limit the influence of outliers as they are more representative of the population (Tabachnick and Fidell 2006). In the same vein, Stretesky and Hogan’s (2001) study utilized drastically different pre- and post- Columbine sample sizes, with only 20 respondents in the before-event sample and 113 respondents in the after-event sample. They opted to utilize a qualita- tive research technique – incorporating the use of focus groups – to increase confidence in their quantitative findings. The timing of drawing samples in relation to event occurrence also can impact a study. Kaminski et al. (2010) employed a research design that utilized four distinct sampling intervals – prior to and following the shooting on April 16, 2007 at Virginia Tech and the same with the February 14, 2008 shooting at NIU. Since the post-Virginia Tech sample of respondents and the pre-NIU sample of respondents were surveyed in close temporal proximity to one another – only a 7 month difference between the two sample-collection periods – it is possible that there was some carryover of fear in the pre-NIU sample due to the commission of the Virginia Tech shooting. As Kaminski
  • 28. et al. (2010) note, The pre-NIU coefficient was statistically significant and larger than that for the post-VT estimate. Taken at face value, this suggests that not only was there no decay in fear following the VT shooting incident, but that fear of walking alone on campus during the day actually increased between Survey 2 (post-VT) and Survey 3 (pre-NIU). (p. 94) It is possible, then, that it the effects of the shooting at Virginia Tech on fear was delayed. In fact, Kaminski et al. (2010) point out that if delayed effects did occur, it could be caused by a wide array of factors that affected their study, including a possible desensitization of respondents to similar repeated events, increased campus safety measures employed after the Virginia Tech shooting, and the timing of the survey administration, among others. 454 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 Understanding The Findings Fear, Risk, and Non-Normal Events Though several studies (Addington 2003; Brener et al. 2002; Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and Hogan 2001) reported finding effects, albeit minimal, of their respective
  • 29. school shootings on measures of fear of crime or perceptions of safety, these results must be interpreted with caution for two key reasons. First, respondents systematically may be excluded from the study, the extent of which can be difficult, if not impossible, to determine. Addington’s (2003) research on changes in fear of crime highlights this issue by finding that Bafter Columbine, students were more likely to avoid school entirely^ (p. 371). Moreover, of the students who do return to school after a shooting incident, those who are fearful may refuse to answer certain survey questions that make them feel afraid or uncomfortable during post-event data collection. This plausibly can result in missing data from some subsection of the respondents, thereby reducing the variability of the sample, and possibly biasing results. Perhaps more problematic, however, is the way in which measures of fear of crime or perceived risk of victimization, have been constructed. The outcome measures in these studies, similar to the larger body of fear of crime (see, for instance, Ferraro 1995; LaGrange and Ferraro 1989), focus on assessing perceptions as they relate to more commonly occurring crimes. Addington (2003), for example, assessed respondents' level of fear while at school, as well as while traveling to and from school. This begs the question of why respondents' fear of crime while traveling to or from school should be impacted by the Columbine shooting, which occurred solely on the campus itself.
  • 30. Similarly, in their examination of perceptions of safety, Stretesky and Hogan (2001) incorporated questions related to routine activities, including walking alone, being home alone, or taking public transportation, each at night.3 School shootings, however, cannot be said to be normally occurring in any sense; thus, the construct validity of the fear or risk measures is reduced. As Addington (2003) points out, The theoretical models utilizing perceived risk were developed to explain fear of victimization arising from daily experiences as opposed to a particular event like Columbine. It is not clear how analogous Columbine is to more common recurring experiences. Prior research also operationalized indirect victimization as fairly minor or routine crimes occurring in the neighborhood or involving one’s friends or family. In contrast, Columbine was an unusually violent crime that for the vast majority of Americans occurred in a distant place to strangers. (p. 369, emphasis added) As such, the lack of, or minimal significant findings about the effects of a non- normally occurring crime may best be attributed to trying to capture its impact using measures related to more routine events. In order to better assess the full impact of school shooting events on both fear of
  • 31. crime and perceptions of risk, researchers must reconceptualize the way in which these 3 For a complete list of questions included, see Stretesky and Hogan (2001, p. 437). Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 455 outcomes are measured. First, measures of both fear and risk must be multidimensional in composition, as a single measure of either will be insufficient to capture such a complex issue. Additionally, these questions specifically must be consistent with school shootings, as opposed to routine crimes, such as burglary, robbery, or even homicide more generally. As Kaminski et al. (2010) note, Although campus shootings may increase student fear of walking alone on campus or fear of crime on campus generally, they may more strongly increase fear of being threatened with a gun or fear of being murdered on campus, since guns and murder were characteristics specific to the Virginia Tech and NIU events. (p. 91) The panel of questions proposed by Ferraro (1995), which has been validated through a number of subsequent studies, provides a good starting point to design such a survey. This panel, however, merely provides a template, as it is was designed to
  • 32. assess more routine crimes. Thus, researchers could benefit from adapting this panel to maintain questions about fear or perceived risk of murder while incorporating measures specific to school shootings, such as being shot while on campus, being threatened with a gun (and perhaps disaggregating further to identify fear or risk of being threatened with a handgun, shotgun, or rifle), or being targeted indiscriminately by disgruntled classmates. Mediatized Outcomes As most people never will be a direct victim of a school shooting, nor spatially proximate to the site of an event, media attention has the potential to close these gaps (Addington 2003; Kaminski et al. 2010; Stretesky and Hogan 2001). More specifically, increased media coverage provides a way by which the public can identify with both the victims and the event as a whole (Addington 2003). As such, the media can have the ability to lead to the indirect victimization for millions of news consumers (Addington 2003; Skogan and Maxfield 1981; Warr 2000). Understanding this poten- tial effect, then, must be a consideration for researchers. Both Addington (2003) and Stretesky and Hogan (2001), for example, posit a potential media effect in their studies, due in part to the heightened media attention the Columbine shooting received. While Addington (2003) contextualizes the link as
  • 33. indirect, Stretesky and Hogan (2001) propose a direct relationship. In fact, their study is focused around the research question, BDoes the media depiction of the Columbine shooting change perceptions of safety and risk on the part of [RIT] students during the time period in question?^ (Stretesky and Hogan 2001, p. 430). Despite these proposi- tions, neither study included media-related variables. Even if media variables are included, consideration must be given to the differences between consumption and content. In order to adequately assess the impact of the media, both should be considered. Further, measures assessing the different modes of media (e.g., television, newspaper, or social media) are needed, as often, different categories of news consumers frequent specific sources (see, for example, Pew Re- search Center for the People and the Press 2008). In sum, researchers not only must account for how much media people consume, but also what they are taking in and 456 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 through what mode in order to understand the potential effects of indirect victimization via the media on study outcome measures. The Role of Prior Victimization As noted, the media may act as a form of indirect victimization
  • 34. for the general public who tune in when a school shooting occurs. Direct prior victimization also has the ability to influence people’s perceptions about school shootings, and as such, must be considered. Stretesky and Hogan (2001) included some measure of previous victimi- zation in their studies, recognizing that respondents' past victimization could affect their fear of crime, perceived risk of victimization, and avoidance behaviors, irrespective of the occurrence of a school shooting. As Stretesky and Hogan (2001) note, individuals who previously have been victimized are more likely to report feeling unsafe. The majority of the studies examined here, however, did not incorporate controls for prior victimization, which also could confound the results. Additionally, the influence of time with regard to respondents' past victimization must be taken into account. More recent victimization may have stronger effects on fear of crime, perceived risk of victimization, and avoidance behaviors than victimization experiences occurring in the more distant past. Furthermore, it is unknown how prior personal victimization may affect fear and perceived risk of crime as they relate specifically to school shootings. Similar to the aforementioned concerns related to fear of normally occurring crime, past personal victimization, such as individuals having had their house broken into while they were home, may have little relation to fear of being shot in a classroom. Therefore, it is imperative that
  • 35. researchers studying school shootings also incorporate questions with similar situational characteristics of these events, such as bullying or being confronted with a firearm, in addition to more general measures of prior victimization. Combating The Challenges Of Studying School Shootings Each of these studies has made an important contribution to the available body of literature on school shootings. Though we have highlighted areas for consideration within each study that warrant caution in understanding their methodologies and interpreting their findings, these limitations simultaneously provide a considerable opportunity to continue to advance the research. Drawing on these, a series of Bbest practices^ can be offered that researchers may use in future studies to overcome prior constraints. Table 1 provides a summary of these recommendations for moving the research forward. Employ a Universally Accepted Definition While various definitions of school shootings (or other closely associated identifiers) exist, the absence of a precise, universally accepted definition is problematic for several reasons. First, without clearly defined parameters of what constitutes such an event, some incidents may be labeled incorrectly as a school shooting, despite that they do not align with other attacks. For instance, under the broadly
  • 36. constructed definition put forth Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 457 by Everytown for Gun Safety (2014), a gang shooting at an Orlando, Florida area high school is identified as a school shooting (Stableford 2014; Weiner 2013). Not only does this confuse the definitional understanding of these events, it also inflates the statistics, giving the public the perception that such attacks are occurring more frequently than they actually are. Further, since many of these definitions are proposed by government agencies or national organizations, such definitions and related statistics are believed to be accurate, though they may not be. In order to overcome this limitation, we propose that Katherine Newman and colleagues' definition in their book, Rampage: The Social Roots of School Shootings, should be used as the universally accepted definition through which to study these events. Specifically, Newman et al. (2004) define rampage school shootings as events that. …Take place on a school-related public stage before an audience; involve multiple victims, some of whom are shot simply for their symbolic significance or at random; and involve one or more shooters who are students or former
  • 37. students of the school. (p. 50, emphasis added) Newman et al. (2004) definition has been widely accepted and typically serves as the basis among researchers in the academic community (e.g., Fox and Harding 2005; Kalish and Kimmel 2010; Langman 2009; Muschert 2007b; Muschert and Ragnedda 2010; Tonso 2009; Wike and Fraser 2009) who study such events. Given its validation through these works, it can be considered the most ideal definition presented to understand school shootings. Ensure the Accuracy of the Data Data associated with mass shootings is predicated on a precise definition. When inaccuracy exists, however, the data itself will be impacted. Most commonly, defini- tions have been crafted in a way that leads to an inflation in the statistics, indicating that there is a greater number of events occurring than actually are. Based on Everytown for Gun Safety’s aforementioned definition, the organization claims that 142 school shootings occurred in the 22 months after Sandy Hook, despite that other researchers (e.g., Schildkraut and Elsass 2016) found fewer events occurred in 134 years based on a more precise classification. Table 1 Best practices for studying school shootings 1. Employ a universally accepted definition of the phenomenon.
  • 38. 2. Ensure that the data being used is accurate and that it is collected in accordance with the agreed upon definition. 3. Utilize theoretical perspectives to support the findings. 4. Ground the findings in the context of homicide in the U.S. and relevant statistical risk of victimization. 5. Expand the resource focus to explore new avenues and unanswered research questions. 6. Combine quantitative and qualitative methodologies to increase the robustness of the body of literature and individual studies. 458 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 It is important to note, however, that even the most accurate definition does not guarantee inclusion of all events. The heavy reliance of researchers on media accounts to identify these events can be problematic based on the continual changing of terminology used in the discourse (e.g., school shooting, active shooter, rampage, etc.). Further, despite the highly sensational nature of these events, not all events are covered and of those that are, the number of stories and amount of information released may vary (Schildkraut 2014). Despite such cautions, improving the overall accuracy of the data will benefit researchers in being able to offer more insight and possible
  • 39. generalizability of their findings related to these events. Utilize Theoretical Perspectives to Support the Findings Like all research, findings from these studies should be supported by theory in order to increase their validity. As identified earlier, the way in which theoretical concepts are measured is important, as many theories have clearly defined constructs that are instrumental in their application. Deviation in how such constructs are measured, however, may reduce confidence in researchers’ findings. Instead, future studies should emphasize measurement in accordance with specific theoretical propositions substan- tiated in previous literature. Further, a broader range of theories should be used to study these events. Rather than anecdote or speculation, for example, theories from the biological or psychological perspectives may be used to further assess why school shooters engage in such crimes. Similarly, learning theories also may be employed to better understand how they come to engage in these behaviors. Routine activities theory, as proposed by Cohen and Felson (1979), also can provide a strong foundation not only for identifying how the opportunity arises for these events to occur, but potential ways in which future school shootings may be prevented. While there is no Bone size fits all^ theory through which to understand these events, the broad range of criminological explanations, steeped in
  • 40. rich, academic history, can provide different opportunities to strengthen the findings of future research. It may be necessary to develop a new theoretical paradigm through whose lens school shootings may be examined, apart from the traditional criminological ap- proaches. Due to the rarity of this phenomenon, it is likely that criminological theories developed for the purpose of explaining more normally occurring crimes are ill- equipped to be applied to school shootings. It then is vital that researchers begin to consider theory construction with rare violent events, specifically school shootings, as the focus. Ground the Findings in the Bigger Picture of Homicide and Relative Risk of Victimization The employment of a precise definition and accurate statistics can provide a greater understanding about these events and their prevalence. As it has been found that individuals believe that school shootings are happening more frequently and that they have a high statistical likelihood of becoming victims (e.g., Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut et al. 2015), findings from research on these events must be contextualized in terms of homicide in the U.S., an already statistically rare Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 459
  • 41. occurrence compared to other types of crimes. This also will enable researchers to translate their findings in terms of a person’s actual risk of becoming the victim of a school shooting. This context is particularly important, given the reliance of media accounts. Schildkraut (2014), for example, found in her examination of all mass shootings between 2000 and 2014 that of 1930 statistical references coded in 564 articles, just 3 mentions of the U.S. violent crime rate were made. The absence of this information, however, is critical as it is likely a main contributing factor to the disproportional beliefs about these events that have been found in other studies (e.g., Elsass et al. 2014; Schildkraut et al. 2015). Therefore, in order to potentially reshape perceptions about these events' occurrence and people’s likelihood of victimization, context must be offered as it relates to crime in the U.S. more broadly. Expand the Research Focus The studies examined here are important to the body of literature on school shootings as they focus on understanding how members of society respond to these events in the form of fear of crime or heightened perceptions of risk victimization. Beyond these, however, the majority of research on school shootings focuses
  • 42. most heavily on understanding the way in which these stories are framed in the media. For example, a study by Chyi and McCombs (2004) examined the way in which coverage of the Columbine shooting was framed in The New York Times, and how these patterns shifted over the life of the story. These findings later were compared with 13 other shootings occurring in a similar time period (Muschert and Carr 2006) and the Sandy Hook shooting (Schildkraut and Muschert 2014). Others have examined how specific narratives about these events, perpetrators, or victims are constructed within the media (e.g., Frymer 2009; Kupchik and Bracy 2009; Lawrence and Mueller 2003; Schildkraut 2012). Each study is a worthwhile endeavor, given that the media acts as the main source of informa- tion for up to 95 % of the general public (Graber 1980; Surette 1992) and the manner in which stories are framed can have a considerable impact on the attitudes examined in the studies by Addington (2003); Brener et al. (2002); Fallahi et al. (2009); Kaminski et al. (2010), and Stretesky and Hogan (2001). Still, this limited focus creates the opportunity to expand the research in future studies. Moving forward, scholars may wish to consider other aspects of the school shoot- ings phenomenon that largely has been absent from the
  • 43. literature. One such opportunity would be to examine the perpetrators themselves more in depth. Both Langman (2009) and Muschert (2007a) have offered typologies through which to understand the shooters. Langman (2009), for example, presented three different categories of school shooters – the traumatized, the psychotic, and the psychopathic. Muschert (2007a), on the other hand, offered a different typology through which to analyze school shootings. Specifically, he differentiated rampage shootings, as defined by Newman et al. (2004), from mass murders, terrorist attacks, targeted shootings, and government shootings (Muschert 2007a). These studies each were limited by a small sample size (e.g., Langman had only 10 shooters from 8 different events in his study); therefore, a renewed application to a broader group of perpetrators may provide additional insights 460 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 about these events. From there, a greater understanding about why these events occur and how they may either be predicted or prevented (e.g., through the identification of warning signs) may be gleaned. Combine Both Qualitative and Quantitative Methodologies In order to continue to advance this important body of literature, new methodological
  • 44. approaches must be employed. One possible way in which this can be accomplished is by employing a multi-stage research design that utilizes both quantitative and qualitative methodologies. While the studies reviewed here have illustrated the benefits of quantitative survey research, such information can benefit from being supplemented with qualitative data to provide a more robust and rich understanding of responses. With studies such as those by Addington (2003) and Brener et al. (2002), which found through quantitative analysis that students were more fearful after the Columbine shooting, supplementing the research with qualitative follow-up will allow for researchers to understand the Bwhy^ behind the data. One way in which this could be accomplished would be for researchers to conduct interviews or focus groups with individuals and gather information about reactions to school shootings, either individual events or the collective phenomenon (see, for example, Stretesky and Hogan 2001). Likewise, observational research could be conducted with individuals or groups, looking for cues as participants read news stories or watch coverage of the events. Regardless of the specific methodology chosen, the use of a multi-stage, mixed-method design will allow researchers to significantly advance the breadth of the literature on school shootings. Conclusion
  • 45. The studies examined throughout the current work have taken important first steps in developing the research on school shootings, particularly as the phenomenon has become a highly prevalent issue in the public discourse. Despite inherent challenges arising from methodological issues, these studies provide an important glimpse into how these events can impact people’s attitudes about school shootings. At the same time, recognizing these roadblocks enables future researchers to reconceptualize the way in which these events are studied, creating the opportunity to develop a more robust examination of school shootings, from the moment the story breaks to how information is processed and received by the public, and in turn, how these beliefs influence related public policy. Only then can researchers continue to advance this limited, yet vital, body of literature forward. Acknowledgments The authors wish to thank the reviewers for their invaluable feedback on this manuscript. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 2014 annual meeting of the American Society of Criminology. Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 461 References Addington, L. A. (2003). Students' fear after columbine: Findings from a randomized experiment. Journal of
  • 46. Quantitative Criminology, 19(4), 367–387. Altheide, D. L. (2009). The columbine shooting and the discourse of fear. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(10), 1354–1370. Brener, N. D., Simon, T. R., Anderson, M., Barrios, L. C., & Small, M. L. (2002). Effect of the incident at columbine on students' violence- and suicide-related behaviors. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 22(3), 146–150. Bryman, A. (2012). Social research methods (4th ed., ). New York: Oxford University Press. Burns, R., & Crawford, C. (1999). School shootings, the media, and public fear: Ingredients for a moral panic. Crime, Law & Social Change, 32, 147–168. Centers for Disease Control. (2014). School-assisted violent death study. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolviolence/SAVD.html Chermak, S. (1994). Body count news: How crime is presented in the news media. Justice Quarterly, 11(4), 561–582. Chermak, S. (1995). Victims in the news: Crime and the American news media. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Chyi, H. I., & McCombs, M. E. (2004). Media salience and the process of framing: Coverage of the Columbine school shootings. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 81(1), 22–35. Cohen, B. C. (1963). The press and foreign policy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. K. (1979). Social change and crime
  • 47. rate trends: A routine activities approach. American Sociological Review, 44(4), 588–608. Columbine Review Commission (2001). The report of governor Bill Owens' Columbine Review Commission. Denver, CO: State of Colorado. Retrieved from http://www.state.co.us/columbine/. Elsass, H. J., Schildkraut, J., & Stafford, M. C. (2014). Breaking news of social problems: Examining media consumption and student beliefs about school shootings. Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law and Society, 15(2). Everytown for Gun Safety. (2014). Analysis of school shootings: December 15, 2012 – December 9, 2014. Retrieved from http://everytown.org/documents/2014/10/analysis-of-school- shootings.pdf Fallahi, C. R., Austad, C. S., Fallon, M., & Leishman, L. (2009). A survey of the perceptions of the Virginia Tech tragedy. Journal of School Violence, 8(2), 120–135. Ferraro, K. F. (1995). Fear of crime: Interpreting victimization risk. New York: SUNY Press. Fox, C., & Harding, D. (2005). School shootings as organizational deviance. Sociology of Education, 78(1), 69–97. Frymer, B. (2009). The media spectacle of Columbine: Alienated youth as an object of fear. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(10), 1387–1404. Graber, D. A. (1980). Crime news and the public. Chicago, IL:
  • 48. University of Chicago Press. Gruenewald, J., Pizarro, J., & Chermak, S. (2009). Race, gender, and the newsworthiness of homicide incidents. Journal of Criminal Justice, 37(3), 262–272. Heath, L. (1984). Impact of newspaper crime reports on fear of crime: Multimethodological investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 263–276. Hindelang, M. J., Gottfredson, M. R., & Garofalo, J. (1978). Victims of personal crime: An empirical foundation for a theory of personal victimization. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Company. Johnstone, J., Hawkins, D., & Michener, A. (1994). Homicide reporting in Chicago dailies. Journalism Quarterly, 71(4), 860–872. Kalish, R., & Kimmel, M. (2010). Suicide by mass murder: Masculinity, aggrieved entitlement, and rampage school shootings. Health Sociology Review, 19(4), 451–464. Kaminski, R. J., Koons-Witt, B. A., Thompson, N. S., & Weiss, D. (2010). The impacts of Virginia Tech and Northern Illinois University shootings on the fear of crime on campus. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(1), 88–98. Kellner, D. (2003). Media spectacle. London: Routledge. Kellner, D. (2008a). Guys and guns amok: Domestic terrorism and school shootings from the Oklahoma City bombing to the Virginia Tech massacre. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers. Kellner, D. (2008b). Media spectacle and the Bmassacre at
  • 49. Virginia Tech^. In B. Agger, & T. W. Luke (Eds.), There is a gunman on campus (pp. 29–54). Lanham, MD: Rowan & Littlefield Publishers, Inc. 462 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolvi olence/SAVD.html http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/youthviolence/schoolvi olence/SAVD.html http://www.state.co.us/columbine/ http://everytown.org/documents/2014/10/analysis-of-school- shootings.pdf Kupchik, A., & Bracy, N. L. (2009). The news media on school crime and violence: Constructing danger- ousness and fueling fear. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 7(2), 136–155. LaGrange, R. L., & Ferraro, K. F. (1989). Assessing age and gender differences in perceived risk and fear of crime. Criminology, 27(4), 697–719. Langman, P. (2009). Rampage school shooters: A typology. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(1), 79–86. Larkin, R. W. (2007). Comprehending Columbine. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. Larkin, R. W. (2009). The Columbine legacy: Rampage shootings as political acts. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(9), 1309–1326. Lawrence, R., & Mueller, D. (2003). School shootings and the man-bites-dog criterion of newsworthiness.
  • 50. Youth Violence & Juvenile Justice, 1(4), 330–345. Liska, A. E., & Baccaglini, W. (1990). Feeling safe by comparison: Crime in the newspapers. Social Problems, 37(3), 360–374. Maguire, B., Weatherby, G. A., & Mathers, R. A. (2002). Network news coverage of school shootings. The Social Science Journal, 39(3), 465–470. McCombs, M. E. (1997). Building consensus: The news media’s agenda-setting roles. Polymer Composites, 14(4), 433–443. McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of the mass media. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187. McCombs, M. E., & Zhu, J. (1995). Capacity, diversity, and volatility of the public agenda: Trends from 1954 to 1994. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 59(4), 495–525. Muschert, G. W. (2002). Media and massacre: The social construction of the columbine story, Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Boulder, CO.: University of Colorado at Boulder. Muschert, G. W. (2007a). Research in school shootings. Sociology Compass, 1(1), 60–80. Muschert, G. W. (2007b). The Columbine victims and the myth of the juvenile superpredator. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5(4), 351–366. Muschert, G. W., & Carr, D. (2006). Media salience and frame changing across events: Coverage of nine school shootings, 1997–2001. Journalism and Mass
  • 51. Communication Quarterly, 83(4), 747–766. Muschert, G. W., & Larkin, R. W. (2007). The Columbine High School shootings. In S. Chermak, & F. Y. Bailey (Eds.), Crimes and trials of the century (pp. 253–266). Westport, CT: Praeger. Muschert, G. W., & Ragnedda, M. (2010). Media and violence control: The framing of school shootings. In W. Heitmeyer, H. G. Haupt, S. Malthaner, & A. Kirschner (Eds.), The control of violence in modern society: Multidisciplinary perspectives, from school shootings to ethnic violence (pp. 345–361). New York: Springer Publishing. Newman, K. S. (2006). School shootings are a serious problem. In S. Hunnicutt (Ed.), School shootings (pp. 10–17). Farmington Hills, MI: Greenhaven Press. Newman, K. S., Fox, C., Harding, D. J., Mehta, J., & Roth, W. (2004). Rampage: The social roots of school shootings. New York: Basic Books. Paulsen, D. (2003). Murder in black and white: The newspaper coverage of homicide in Houston. Homicide Studies, 7(3), 289–317. Pew Research Center for the People & the Press. (2008). Audience segments in a changing news environment: Key audiences now blend online and traditional sources. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/ files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf. Robinson, M. B. (2011). Media coverage of crime and criminal justice. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.
  • 52. Schildkraut, J. (2012). Media and massacre: A comparative analysis of the reporting of the 2007 Virginia Tech shootings. Fast Capitalism, 9(1) Retrieved from http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/ schildkraut9_1.html. Schildkraut, J. (2014). Mass murder and the mass media: An examination of the media discourse on U.S, rampage shootings, 2000–2012. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). San Marcos, TX: Texas State University. Schildkraut, J., & Donley, A. M. (2012). Murder in black: A media distortion analysis of homicides in Baltimore in 2010. Homicide Studies, 16(2), 175–196. Schildkraut, J., & Elsass, H. J. (2016). Mass shootings: Media, myths, and realities. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger. Schildkraut, J., & Hernandez, T. C. (2014). Laws that bit the bullet: A review of legislative responses to school shootings. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39(2), 358– 374. Schildkraut, J., & Muschert, G. W. (2014). Media salience and the framing of mass murder in schools: A comparison of the Columbine and Sandy Hook massacres. Homicide Studies, 18(1), 23–43. Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 463 http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf http://www.people-press.org/files/legacy-pdf/444.pdf http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/schildkraut9
  • 53. _1.html http://www.uta.edu/huma/agger/fastcapitalism/9_1/schildkraut9 _1.html Schildkraut, J., Elsass, H. J., & Stafford, M. C. (2015). Could it happen here? Moral panics, school shootings, and fear of crime among college students. Crime, Law and Social Change, 63(1), 91–110. Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company. Skogan, W. G., & Maxfield, M. G. (1981). Coping with crime: Individual and neighborhood reactions. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Stableford, D. (2014). What is a ‘school shooting’? It depends who you ask. Yahoo News. Retrieved from http://news.yahoo.com/how-many-school-shootings-since- sandy-hook-150152463.html Stretesky, P. B., & Hogan, M. J. (2001). Columbine and student perceptions of safety: A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Criminal Justice, 29(5), 429–443. Surette, R. (1992). Media, crime, & criminal justice: Images and reality. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2006). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Pearson.
  • 54. Tonso, K. L. (2009). Violent masculinities as tropes for school shooters: The Montréal massacre, the Columbine attack, and rethinking schools. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(9), 1266–1285. Vossekuil, B., Fein, R. A., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final report and findings of the safe school initiative: Implications for the prevention of school attacks in the United States. Washington, D.C.: United States Secret Service and United States Department of Education. Warr, M. (2000). Fear of crime in the United States: Avenues for research and policy. In D. Duffee (Ed.), Criminal justice 2000, Measurement and analysis of crime and justice (vol. 4, pp. 451–489). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Warr, M., & Stafford, M. (1983). Fear of victimization: A look at the proximate causes. Social Forces, 61(4), 1033–1043. Washington Post-ABC News Poll. (2015). The Washington post. Retrieved from http://www.washingtonpost. com/wp-srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html Weiner, J. (2013). West Orange High shooter a Bdocumented gang member,^ cops say. Orlando Sentinel. Retrieved from http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12- 06/news/os-west-orange-high-shooting-gang- 20131206_1_gang-member-gang-affiliation-west-orange-high- school Wike, T. L., & Fraser, M. W. (2009). School shootings: Making sense of the senseless. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(3), 162–169.
  • 55. H. Jaymi Elsass is a doctoral candidate in the School of Criminal Justice at Texas State University. Her primary research interests include episodic violent crime, moral panics, fear of crime, and juvenile crime. She has published in Criminology, Criminal Justice, Law & Society, Crime, Law & Social Change, and an edited volume. Jaclyn Schildkraut is an Assistant Professor of Public Justice at the State University of New York at Oswego. Her research interests include school shootings, homicide trends, mediatization effects, and crime theories. She has published in American Journal of Criminal Justice, Homicide Studies, Fast Capitalism, Crime, Law & Social Change, and Criminal Justice Studies, as well as several edited volumes Mark C. Stafford is a Professor and the Doctoral Program Director in the School of Criminal Justice at Texas State University. He also has held faculty positions at the University of Texas at Austin and Washington State University. He has published in Criminology, Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, Quantitative Criminology, and Journal of Criminal Justice. 464 Am J Crim Just (2016) 41:444–464 http://news.yahoo.com/how-many-school-shootings-since- sandy-hook-150152463.html http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp- srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp- srv/politics/polls/postabcpoll_20121216.html http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12-06/news/os-west- orange-high-shooting-gang-20131206_1_gang-member-gang-
  • 56. affiliation-west-orange-high-school http://articles.orlandosentinel.com/2013-12-06/news/os-west- orange-high-shooting-gang-20131206_1_gang-member-gang- affiliation-west-orange-high-school Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Studying School Shootings: Challenges and Considerations for ResearchAbstractUnderstanding the [Mediatized] Problem of School ShootingsPrevious Research on School ShootingsDefining the School Shootings ProblemTheoretical Approaches to Studying School ShootingsPerceptions of Safety and Fear of CrimeRoutine Activities and Avoidance BehaviorsMethodological ComplexitiesData Collection ConcernsSampling ConsiderationsUnderstanding The FindingsFear, Risk, and Non-Normal EventsMediatized OutcomesThe Role of Prior VictimizationCombating The Challenges Of Studying School ShootingsEmploy a Universally Accepted DefinitionEnsure the Accuracy of the DataUtilize Theoretical Perspectives to Support the FindingsGround the Findings in the Bigger Picture of Homicide and Relative Risk of VictimizationExpand the Research FocusCombine Both Qualitative and Quantitative MethodologiesConclusionReferences discussion William Ford Jr., Chairman of Ford Motor Co. said, "A good company delivers excellent products and services, and a great company does all that and strives to make the world a better place." Supported by evidence from your textbook, the "Starbucks" case study, and other research, describe two forces that you
  • 57. believe shape the relationship between business and society. Provide two examples, one for each force you select. Be specific in your answer and discuss strengths and weaknesses via examples and applications. Be certain to cite in APA format all sources used. Mass Shootings, Copycat Killers, and the Media ProQuest document link Abstract: Omar Mateen's mass shooting slaughter at Orlando's Pulse nightclub, where he left 49 innocents dead and 53 more wounded, has led to the predictable debates over gun control, the availability and efficacy of mental health resources, our national response to the threat of international terrorism at home and abroad, and the readiness and reaction of law enforcement when such active shooter incidents occur. These are vital discussions to be had, for no matter your position on any one of the topics one thing, by now, should be clear: what happened in Orlando, like what happened in San Bernardino, and Sandy Hook, and Virginia Tech, and Aurora, CO, and on and on and on... will happen again. Nothing that arises out of this particular incident, or the conversations it spurs, or any legislation it drives, will lead to the "Never Again!" moment that finally sticks; right now, there are a number of people in the U.S. alone - men mostly, but surely a woman or two at any given time - just waiting to strike and lay claim to their piece of infamy. A recent Chicago Tribune article ("Are media complicit in mass shootings?" by reporter Alan Zarambo) looked at how media coverage of mass slayings might be influencing others to emulate the killers. Zarambo cites research by Arizona State University researcher Sherry Towers showing that shootings often occur in "clusters." Towers' study found: Full text: Omar Mateen's mass shooting slaughter at Orlando's Pulse nightclub, where he left 49 innocents dead and 53 more
  • 58. wounded, has led to the predictable debates over gun control, the availability and efficacy of mental health resources, our national response to the threat of international terrorism at home and abroad, and the readiness and reaction of law enforcement when such active shooter incidents occur. These are vital discussions to be had, for no matter your position on any one of the topics one thing, by now, should be clear: what happened in Orlando, like what happened in San Bernardino, and Sandy Hook, and Virginia Tech, and Aurora, CO, and on and on and on... will happen again. Nothing that arises out of this particular incident, or the conversations it spurs, or any legislation it drives, will lead to the "Never Again!" moment that finally sticks; right now, there are a number of people in the U.S. alone - men mostly, but surely a woman or two at any given time - just waiting to strike and lay claim to their piece of infamy. As fatalistic as that sounds, it really isn't. Examination of, and debate around, the issues of mass shootings is a crucial first step to reducing the number and lethality of such events, even if outright elimination of the phenomena is presently unrealistic. We can argue whether Mateen's crimes were a terroristic act driven by allegiance to ISIS ideals of Islamic jihad, a homophobic hate crime, or a symbolic gesture of horrifically externalized self-hatred over his own conflicted sexuality (or a terrible combo of all three, as I believe) but what we learn through reasoned argument is our best hope to understand and, hopefully, intervene to head off the next attack. Two additional concerns... for law enforcement and society Sometimes lost amid the obvious debate topics already mentioned above are two that are less prevalent, though more immediate and readily addressed. The first is the issue of copycat crimes inspired by the actions of high-profile killers who, whether in death or capture, achieve a degree of attention, notoriety, and even respect few will ever know. While most of us recoil in horror at wanton destruction of human life, reviling those who would take it, others look on with reverence, envious
  • 59. of the limelight the shooter basks in. As improbable and sick as it sounds, certain impressionable and deluded individuals will never grasp the revulsion most of us feel. Others see the rest of us as weak, ineffectual 15 September 2016 Page 1 of 3 ProQuest sheep, blind to their version of truth, our sympathy for the victims emblematic of our acceptance of a sickness that must be excised from society. They may see shooters as martyrs, selfless in their actions and worthy of imitation (or to be topped). This creates obvious problems for law enforcement. A recent Chicago Tribune article ("Are media complicit in mass shootings?" by reporter Alan Zarambo) looked at how media coverage of mass slayings might be influencing others to emulate the killers. Zarambo cites research by Arizona State University researcher Sherry Towers showing that shootings often occur in "clusters." Towers' study found: That these clusters exist in today's world, barring the possibility of mere (if then strangely predictable) coincidence, certainly has to do with the reach of modern media, including social media and its effect on the sense of individual self-importance and how it accelerates the pace and reach of breaking news. For law enforcement, this means that the weeks following a mass shooting are a critical time for increased vigilance. Patrol officers should give greater attention to the otherwise routine calls for service that might indicate a copycat situation, school resource officers should heighten their awareness of the angry and disaffected, and how they may emulate a high-profile antihero, and intel officers should probably look more closely at the characters in their areas of responsibility who fit certain profiles - politically, psychologically, and temperamentally - of concern for acting out. This is a time that begs increased information sharing and foresight in planning, especially as the disaffected are emboldened or even challenged to make their own violent contributions.
  • 60. The second major concern is the question of how media reports on these incidents. While I don't believe media to be complicit in the attacks, that coverage of one high-profile mass shooting tends to spawn more in the days and weeks following is becoming more and more evident (In fact, I would be interested in seeing if there is a correlation over coverage of day-to-day violence, as in Chicago and some other large urban areas seeing a significant uptick in shootings, that normalizes it to the degree inhibitions are lowered among others who then see violence as a viable option for settling disputes). There are those who argue the killers/shooters should not even be named, that media should withhold certain information about the offender to instead put greater emphasis on the victims. This has, to a very limited degree, been tried with a couple recent shootings wherein law enforcement agencies have provided very little information directly, in the hopes of not offering shooters the attention they seek. The down side is this: I want to know about the shooter. I want information about his background, history, motives, and actions. Information is important to understanding - and good public policy making - even if that information may also be consumed and misused by those who look to the wrong role models. Of course, there will certainly be those who disagree, and we welcome respectful disagreement. Or perhaps there is wise middle ground that can strike a balance and reduce the risk of copycat killers. But these incidents are not going away. We should find that middle ground sooner rather than later. Publication title: Officer.com Publication year: 2016 Publication date: Jul 6, 2016 Year: 2016 Publisher: SouthComm Business Media LLC Place of publication: Fort Atkinson Country of publication: United States Publication subject: Criminology And Law Enforcement 15 September 2016 Page 2 of 3 ProQuest Source type: Trade Journals Language of publication: English Document type: News ProQuest document ID: 1808327229 Document URL:
  • 61. https://libraryresources.columbiasouthern.edu/login?url=http://s earch.proquest.com/docview/1808327229?acco untid=33337 Copyright: © Copyright 2016 Cygnus Business Media Last updated: 2016-08-03 Database: Criminal Justice Database _____________________________________________________ __________ uest LLC. All rights reserved. - Terms and Conditions