SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 73
Duplicity Among the Dark Triad: Three Faces of Deceit
Daniel N. Jones
University of Texas at El Paso
Delroy L. Paulhus
University of British Columbia
Although all 3 of the Dark Triad members are predisposed to
engage in exploitative interpersonal
behavior, their motivations and tactics vary. Here we explore
their distinctive dynamics with 5 behavioral
studies of dishonesty (total N � 1,750). All 3 traits predicted
cheating on a coin-flipping task when there
was little risk of being caught (Study 1). Only psychopathy
predicted cheating when punishment was a
serious risk (Study 2). Machiavellian individuals also cheated
under high risk— but only if they were
ego-depleted (Study 3). Both psychopathy and
Machiavellianism predicted cheating when it required an
intentional lie (Study 4). Finally, those high in narcissism
showed the highest levels of self-deceptive bias
(Study 5). In sum, duplicitous behavior is far from uniform
across the Dark Triad members. The
frequency and nature of their dishonesty is moderated by 3
contextual factors: level of risk, ego depletion,
and target of deception. This evidence for distinctive forms of
duplicity helps clarify differences among
the Dark Triad members as well as illuminating different shades
of dishonesty.
Keywords: cheating, Dark Triad, deception, honesty
Supplemental materials:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139.supp
Concern about others’ honesty is among the strongest
situational
influences on social behavior (Rauthmann et al., 2014), and
hon-
esty attribution is the single most influential factor in person
perception (Paunonen, 2006). However, the measurement of
hon-
esty as a trait is characterized by a fractured history reaching
back
to Hartshorne and May (1928). After assessing diverse forms of
dishonest behaviors (e.g., lying, cheating, stealing) on large
sam-
ples of children, those seminal researchers dismissed the notion
of
a unitary trait construct. When properly interpreted, however,
their
data do indicate significant convergence across various forms of
duplicity (Burton, 1963). Such tendencies also tend to converge
at
the virtuous pole (Jayawickreme, Meindl, Helzer, Furr, &
Fleeson,
2014). In contemporary personality research, Ashton and Lee
(2001) have been relentless in confirming this trait notion by
isolating an Honesty-Humility (H-H) factor in their HEXACO
model of personality. The incremental value of adding this
factor
in the prediction of personal integrity appears to be well
supported
(e.g., Lee, Ashton, Morrison, Cordery, & Dunlop, 2010; Spain,
Harms, & Lebreton, 2014).
Under the broad umbrella of the H-H factor, three narrower
traits—narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy— have
re-
ceived extensive theoretical and empirical attention. Paulhus
and
Williams (2002) coined the term Dark Triad to encourage re-
searchers to study these three traits as a constellation.1 Their
rationale was that only a concurrent analysis can clarify any
unique
contributions of the triad members to outcome variables. Many
researchers have taken up that challenge and the body of
research
has expanded geometrically (for the most recent review, see
Paul-
hus, 2014). Of particular interest to the current report is that, in
a
series of factor analyses, all three of the Dark Triad loaded on
the
H-H factor (Book, Visser, & Volk, 2015; Lee & Ashton, 2005;
Lee
et al., 2013). Given that scores on the H-H factor predict honest
behavior in the workplace (Lee, Ashton, & de Vries, 2005), we
were encouraged to tease apart distinctive forms of dishonesty
linked with the Dark Triad members (See also Mededovic,
2012).
One handicap to Dark Triad research has been the excessive
length of the original measures: They add up to a taxing 124
items!
Fortunately, two brief inventories tapping all three traits have
become available. First is the Dirty Dozen (DD) (Jonason &
Webster, 2010), a 12-item instrument. Although a significant
body
of research has exploited this measure (Jonason, Webster,
Schmitt,
Li, & Crysel, 2012), the consequences of its brevity have raised
criticism (Carter, Campbell, Muncer, & Carter, 2015; Lee et al.,
2013; Miller et al., 2012; Rauthmann & Kolar, 2012). The
second
combination measure is the 27-item Short Dark Triad (SD3) de-
veloped by Jones and Paulhus (2014). When the two measures
are
compared head-to-head, the SD3 validities tend to be higher and
more closely match the original Dark Triad measures (Lee et
al.,
2013; Maples, Lamkin, & Miller, 2014; Jones & Paulhus, 2014).
To confirm this previous pattern, we used the traditional
measures
as well as both brief measures of the Dark Triad in several of
the
studies detailed below.
1 Note that the present research concerns subclinical levels of
these
variables (LeBreton et al., 2006) not clinical levels, where the
term per-
sonality disorder applies (see Campbell & Miller, 2011).
This article was published Online First March 2, 2017.
Daniel N. Jones, Department of Psychology, University of
Texas at El
Paso; Delroy L. Paulhus, Department of Psychology, University
of British
Columbia.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Delroy L.
Paulhus, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia, 2136
West Mall, Vancouver BC, V6T 1Z4 Canada. E-mail:
[email protected]
.ubc.ca
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2017 American
Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 113, No. 2, 329 –342 0022-3514/17/$12.00
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139
329
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139
Whether the D3 are operationalized with the traditional mea-
sures, the Dirty Dozen or Short Dark Triad, large bodies of
research confirm the importance of distinguishing the three con-
structs2— despite their positive intercorrelations (e.g.,
O’Boyle,
Forsyth, Banks, & McDaniel, 2012). Successful applications in-
clude such research domains as interpersonal aggression,
person-
nel selection, forensic diagnosis, and academic misbehavior as
well as sexual and romantic relationships. In all of these
studies,
the triad members manifest distinctive patterns of interpersonal
malevolence (see Paulhus, 2014, Jones & Paulhus, 2010).
A critical point for the present paper is that previous work has
linked all three Dark Triad members to a predisposition to
deceive
(Baughman et al., 2014; Giammarco, Atkinson, Baughman, Ve-
selka, & Vernon, 2013; Jonason, Lyons, Baughman, & Vernon,
2015; O’Boyle et al., 2012). To date, however, only two studies
on
the Dark Triad have exploited behavioral measures of
dishonesty
(Jones, 2013, 2014a). All five studies presented below maintain
the
rigor and objectivity maximized by the use of behavioral out-
comes.
The Present Research
Dishonesty has long been linked to callousness, that is, lack of
empathy (Davis, 1996; Brown et al., 2010; Frick, 2009; Giam-
marco & Vernon, 2015; Trout, 2009). Absent of concern for its
effect on others, achievement striving is unmitigated and
cheating
behavior is unconstrained. Given our assumption that
callousness
is a common element of the Dark Triad (Jones & Paulhus,
2011a),
all three should show a predisposition to dishonesty—at least,
when repercussions are unlikely (i.e., low risk)
Under more risky conditions, however, the dynamics of cheat-
ing may play out in a different way for each Dark Triad
member.
Individuals with psychopathic traits have difficulty resisting an
immediate reward— even when risk of punishment is high
(Crysel,
Crosier, & Webster, 2013; Jones, 2014b). Hence, in the studies
below, we predict that the dishonesty of those with
psychopathic
traits will continue even under high risk conditions.
By contrast, the hallmark of those high in Machiavellianism is
strategic manipulation (Christie & Geis, 1970). To exploit scho-
lastic situations, for example, they opt for surreptitious
plagiarism
rather than opportunistic copying during exams (Williams, Na-
thanson, & Paulhus, 2010). Such strategic thinking requires
cog-
nitive resources. When depleted of these resources, individuals
high in Machiavellianism may lose their strategic advantage and
behave more like those high in psychopathy.
Finally, those high in narcissism have a grandiose belief in their
superiority to others (Kohut, 1966). Rather than an instrumental
motivation to acquire resources, their egoistic motivation
requires
repeated confirmation of their intellectual superiority (Campbell
&
Foster, 2007; Jones & Paulhus, 2011a). In short, their
dishonesty is
self-deceptive in nature (Paulhus et al., 2003; von Hippel &
Trivers, 2011). Even when confronted with contradictory evi-
dence, narcissistic individuals tend to exaggerate their general
knowledge (Paulhus et al., 2003) and group performance
(Robins
& John, 1997). Psychopathic individuals also showed this ten-
dency, but to a lesser degree (Paulhus & Williams, 2002).
To summarize these qualitative differences in dishonesty, we
have laid them out in Table 1. Three distinctions loom large in
the
present set of studies on dishonesty: (a) instrumental versus
ego-
istic motivation, (b) impulsivity versus caution, and (c) self-
versus
other-deception. Those broad themes were highlighted in five
studies of deceptive behavior. Study 1 examined which traits
predicted dishonesty when there was minimal risk of being
caught.
Study 2 examined which individuals would continue to cheat
when
punishment was a serious risk. Study 3 examined the effects of
ego
depletion on dishonesty. Study 4 examined who was most likely
to
engage in blatant misrepresentation for financial benefit.
Finally,
Study 5 examined which individuals would exhibit self-
deception
on an anonymous overclaiming task.
Study 1: All Three Show (Low-Risk) Dishonesty
Method
Power analysis. Although power analysis is challenging with
novel research, we took guidance from the meta-analysis by
Rich-
ard, Bond, and Stokes-Zoota (2003). Across the body of social
psychology and personality research, their meta-analysis
yielded a
mean correlation effect size of .21. They deduced that achieving
80% power of reaching significance with that effect size would
require 173 participants. To be conservative, we aimed at a sub-
stantially larger sample.
Participants. The collection of human subjects data was ap-
proved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of
Texas – El Paso. The protocol number and title were as follows:
[391743–7] Moral Machiavellianism survey. The same approval
was used to collect data for all the studies presented in this
article.
Participants were 292 adults from MTurk (51% women, Mean
age � 32.86, SD � 11.78). Overall, 68% reported European
Heritage, 7% African Heritage, 6% East Asian, 7% South Asian,
7% Latino, 5% other mixed ethnicities. Unless otherwise indi-
cated, participants were compensated at a rate typical for
MTurk
(i.e., $0.25; see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011).
Measures. Across all studies, personality scales were col-
lected on Likert-type scales anchored by 1 (strongly disagree)
and
5 (strongly agree). The only exception is the NPI-16 used in
Study
3, which used a dichotomous response format.
Short Dark Triad. To assess the Dark Triad traits in an effi-
cient fashion, we used the Short Dark Triad (SD3) inventory
(Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Although recent in appearance, the
distinctive construct validity of this inventory has already been
supported by over 100 studies. Although not interchangeable,
the
SD3 tends to yield results that run parallel to those with the
original Dark Triad measures (Jones & Olderbak, 2014; Jones &
Paulhus, 2014). The three subscales have predicted unique
patterns
of interpersonal malevolence in dozens of studies: Using self-
reports, those outcomes include partner aggression (Hamel,
Jones,
Dutton, & Graham-Kevan, 2015), racism (Jonason, 2015), bully-
ing (Baughman et al., 2012), cyber-aggression (Pabian, De
Backer,
& Vandebosch, 2015), malevolent intentions (Veselka, Giam-
marco, & Vernon, 2014), and immorality (Jonason, Strosser,
Krull,
Duinefeld, & Baruffi, 2014).
A number of behavioral studies have also added to the construct
validity of the SD3: These behaviors included distinctive
patterns
2 The distinction is even supported by behavior genetics
research (Ver-
non, Villani, Vickers, & Harris, 2008; Veselka, Schermer, &
Vernon,
2011).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
330 JONES AND PAULHUS
of mimicry (Ashton-James & Levordashka, 2014), selfish
financial
behavior (Jones, 2013, 2014a; Roeser et al., 2016), and
laboratory
aggression (Buckels, Jones, & Paulhus, 2013). In all of these
studies, the triad members, as operationalized with the SD3,
man-
ifested distinctive patterns of malevolence.
The SD3 inventory measures the Dark Triad with nine items per
trait. In the present sample, the subscales showed typical means
and acceptable alpha reliabilities: Machiavellianism (M � 3.31,
SD � .60, � � .74); psychopathy (M � 2.14, SD � .70, � �
.81);
narcissism (M � 2.76, SD � .57, � � .71). Subscale intercorre-
lations were in the usual range: Machiavellianism with psychop-
athy (r � .54); psychopathy with narcissism (r � .41); Mach
with
narcissism (r � .27).
Dirty Dozen. We also included a second brief inventory,
namely, the so-called ‘Dirty Dozen’ (DD) (Jonason & Webster,
2010). Using only four items per subscale, the Dirty Dozen has
nonetheless generated a large body of research (see Jonason,
Webster, Schmitt, Li, & Crysel, 2012). The inevitable tradeoff
of
fidelity for efficiency is discussed by Maples et al. (2014) as
well
as Jonason, Li, and Czarna (2013).
In the present sample, internal consistencies for the Dirty Dozen
subscales were all strong: narcissism (� � .82):
Machiavellianism
(� � .78), and psychopathy (� � .76). The subscale
intercorrela-
tions were as follows: Machiavellianism with narcissism (r �
.44);
Machiavellianism with psychopathy (r � .60); psychopathy with
narcissism (r � .31). More detailed descriptives on the Dirty
Dozen are provided in the Supplementary Materials.
Procedure. Participants completed an online package of ques-
tionnaires: It comprised the SD3, DD, the Ten Item Personality
Inventory (TIPI; Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003) as well as
several questions about luck and gambling. The purpose of the
TIPI and gambling questions were to obfuscate the true purpose
of
the study, that is, to examine the link between the Dark Triad
and
cheating.
After completing the questionnaires, participants were sent to a
new page, which had instructions in the center that read: “On
the
next page is a coin flipping task – if you win, you get a bonus!
Unfortunately, there’s a glitch in the software, so it’s possible
to
flip the coin multiple times. So please only flip the coin once to
be
fair.” Participants then went to the next page, which had (a)
choice
buttons labeled “heads” and “tails”, (b) a “flip” button” repre-
sented by a rotating coin and (c) a heading that read “Are you a
lucky person?” This procedure was developed by Quoidbach and
Chakroff (2011) in order to study dishonesty in a virtual
environ-
ment.
Participants had to press “heads” or “tails” and were then asked
to press the flip button. The result of the coin flip, however,
was
preprogrammed as follows: (a) on the first flip, it did not match
participant’s choice, (b) a similar mismatch occurred on the
second
flip, and (c) on the third flip, it finally matched the participant’s
choice. This process allowed dishonest participants the chance
to
flip until they obtained the desired outcome. Our primary
depen-
dent variable was whether or not participants flipped the coin
more
than once in order to obtain the bonus.
Results and Discussion
Overall, 13% of the sample cheated. As predicted, SD3
narcissism, r � .21, p � .001, Machiavellianism, r � .18, p �
.001, and psychopathy, r � .23, p � .001 all had significant
point-biserial correlations with dishonesty. A series of z tests
provided no evidence that the three correlations were different
(all ps � .10). Note from Table 2, however, that when entered
simultaneously into a logistic regression, no one trait reached a
.05 significance. In other words, the three correlations with
dishonesty were so similar that claiming predominance for any
one of them would be arbitrary. This preliminary result suggests
that the three members of the Dark Triad were equally likely to
cheat in this study.
A similar but weaker pattern was found for the Dirty Dozen
scales: narcissism, r � .19, p � .002, Machiavellianism, r �
.11, p � .07, and psychopathy, r � .17, p � .004. Such weaker
results are consistent with recent criticisms leveled against the
scale (Carter et al., 2015; Jones & Paulhus, 2014; Kajonius,
Persson, Rosenberg, & Garcia, 2015; Maples, Lamkin, &
Miller, 2014; Miller et al., 2012). Not only do DD results tend
to be weaker than SD3 results, they tend to be misaligned with
the gold standard measures. Therefore, we relegate further
results for the Dirty Dozen to our Supplementary Materials and
will focus on the SD3 in the subsequent studies. Finally, none
of the Big Five factors, as measured by the TIPI, showed any
correlation with coin flip cheating (r values ranged from �.11
to .09, all p � .06). Therefore, we did not include the TIPI in
any of the subsequent studies.
Table 1
Summary of Predictions
D3 trait
Low-risk
deception
High-risk
deception
Ego-depleted
deception
Intentional
fraud Self deception
Machiavellianism Yes No Yes Yes No
Psychopathy Yes Yes Yes Yes Somewhat
Narcissism Yes No No No Yes
Table 2
Cheating as a Function of the Dark Triad Under Low Risk in
Study 1
Predictors r rx.y B WALD OR 95% CI p
Machiavellianism .18� .06 .23 1.07 1.26 .81, 1.97 .301
Narcissism .21� .12 .37 3.16 1.45 .96, 2.19 .076
Psychopathy .23� .12 .41 3.15 1.50 .96, 2.34 .076
Note. N � 292. The tabled values emerged from a logistic
regression.
� p � .05, two-tailed.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
331DUPLICITY AMONG THE DARK TRIAD
Study 2: Under High-Risk, Only Psychopathy
Predicts Dishonesty
Method
Power analysis. Instead of the meta-analysis (Richard et al.,
2003), we used the effect sizes from Study 1 to calculate the
sample needs of Study 2. The lowest simultaneous OR obtained
in
the Binary Logistic Regression from Study 1 was for
Machiavel-
lianism (OR � 1.47). Using this effect size, a power analysis
suggested 340 participants were needed to achieve 80% power.
Because Study 2 also examined an interaction, we collected an
even larger sample of 441 participants. Note that we sampled
545
to achieve this final sample size after exclusion based on
attention
checks.
Overview. Study 2 used a similar coin-flip paradigm but
varied the risk of getting caught. Based on the previous
literature,
key differences among the Dark Triad should emerge under
high-
risk conditions. On MTurk, the most serious risk to workers is
having their efforts “rejected”: They are fully aware that com-
plaints about bad work are documented as a bad reputation and
may limit their ability to find future work on MTurk (see
Paolacci
& Chandler, 2014).
Perceived risk of getting caught was manipulated by randomly
assigning participants to one of two warning conditions. As in
Study 1, most participants under low risk should be tempted to
coin-flip more than once. Under high risk, however, only
individ-
uals high in psychopathy should persist in cheating— because
of
their inability to delay gratification (Jonason, 2015; Jones,
2014a;
Jones & Paulhus, 2011b).
An interaction should emerge such that individuals high in
Machiavellianism will cheat at low levels but not at high levels
of
risk. This prediction was based on previous literature showing
the
strategic flexibility of Machiavellians (Bereczkei et al., 2013;
Christie & Geis, 1970; Esperger & Bereczkei, 2012; Jones &
Paulhus, 2011a). For psychopathy, no such interaction should
emerge: Individuals high in psychopathy should be just as likely
to
cheat in high and low risk conditions (see Jones, 2014b). We
made
no strong predictions for narcissism in the high risk condition,
but
along with most participants, individuals high in narcissism
should
cheat in the low risk condition.
Participants. Participants were 545 adults solicited from Am-
azon’s MTurk. A total of 104 participants were removed for
failing
attention checks or for taking the survey twice. Note that these
rates of attention check failure are within the typical parameters
for
MTurk samples (Deetlefs, Chylinski, & Ortmann, 2015). The
final
sample was 441 workers (46% women, Mean age � 34.52, SD �
11.18; 77% European Heritage, 4% African Heritage, 7% East
Asian, 6% Latino(a), 6% other).
Dark Triad. As in Study 1, we used the Short Dark Triad or
SD3 (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). The SD3 had significant (all p �
.001) and moderate size intercorrelations: Machiavellianism
with
psychopathy � .58, Machiavellianism with narcissism � .37,
and
narcissism with psychopathy � .45). Means and internal consis-
tencies were similar to those in Study 1: psychopathy (M �
2.11,
SD � .64, � � .79); Machiavellianism (M � 3.09, SD � .68, �
�
.82); narcissism (M � 2.63, SD � .64, � � .77).
Measures and procedure. Participants first completed the
SD3 and were randomly assigned to one of two messages: The
low
risk message was identical to that of Study 1. The high risk
message read, “We will be conducting random checks to see if
anyone flipped the coin more than once. Those who do will have
their work rejected.” In both messages, however, participants
were
alerted to the computer glitch and the fact that the coin could be
flipped more than once.
Results
The warning manipulation was successful, with a higher per-
centage of participants cheating (i.e., performing more than one
coin-flip) in the low risk condition (17%) than in the high risk
condition (10%), �2 � 5.09, p � .02. We then examined
cheating
as a function of condition and the Dark Triad, as well as the
three
Dark Triad � condition interactions in a binary logistic
regression.
As displayed in Table 3, results indicated a main effect for psy-
chopathy, and an interaction between Machiavellianism and
con-
dition (p � .05, two-tailed).
We went on to examine the point-biserial correlations of cheat-
ing within conditions. As expected, both Machiavellianism, r �
.21, p � .002 and psychopathy, r � .17, p � .01 were associated
with cheating in the low risk condition. Unexpectedly,
narcissism
failed to reach significance. As predicted, only psychopathy, r
�
.15, p � .03 was significant in the high risk condition; Machia-
vellianism, r � .05, p � .46, and narcissism, r � .10, p � .16
were
not.
We went further to conduct logistic regressions within
condition.
The results indicated that under low risk, Machiavellianism and
psy-
chopathy were the best predictors of cheating
(Machiavellianism: B �
.87, Wald � 5.91, OR � 2.39, 95% CI for OR � 1.18, 4.81, p �
.015;
narcissism: B � �.68, Wald � 3.50, OR � 0.51, 95% CI for OR
�
0.25, 1.03, p � .506; psychopathy: B � .67, Wald � 3.23, OR �
1.94,
95% CI for OR � 0.94, 4.01, p � .072).
In the high-risk condition, only psychopathy showed an odds
ratio �1.00 and the only significant B coefficient:
(Machiavellian-
ism: B � �.32, Wald � 0.45, OR � 0.73, 95% CI for OR �
0.29,
1.84, p � .503; Narcissism: B � .36, Wald � 0.85, OR � 1.44,
95% CI for OR � 0.67, 3.10, p � .356; Psychopathy: B � .89,
Wald � 3.30, OR � 2.43, 95% CI for OR � 0.93, 6.35, p �
.069).
Finally, we tested the difference in strength between the cheat-
ing correlations (Machiavellianism vs. psychopathy) in the high
risk condition. Results indicated that, as predicted, the
correlation
Table 3
Cheating as a Function of Dark Triad and Instructions in Study
2: Logistic Regression
Predictors r rx.y B WALD OR 95% CI p
Condition �.10� �.10� �.27 2.79 .76 .55, 1.05 .095
Machiavellianism .15� .07 .19 .87 1.21 .81, 1.79 .351
Narcissism .05 �.04 �.10 .35 .90 .65, 1.26 .552
Psychopathy .16� .10� .50 6.41 1.65 1.12, 2.43 .011
Mach � Cond �.09 �.10� �.40 4.01 .67 .45, .99 .045
Narcissism � Cond �.03 .08 .33 3.80 1.40 .99, 1.95 .051
Psychopathy � Cond .04 .00 .07 .38 1.08 .73, 1.58 .715
Note. N � 441. The tabled values emerged from a logistic
regression.
Columns 7– 8 provide confidence intervals and significant
levels for the
odds ratios in Column 6.
� p � .05, two-tailed.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
332 JONES AND PAULHUS
of cheating with psychopathy was (marginally) higher than that
with Machiavellianism, based on a two-tailed test, t � 1.89, p �
.08. The cheating correlation with narcissism, however, was not
significantly different from that with psychopathy or
Machiavel-
lianism (ps � .20).
Discussion
The results of Study 2 extend those from Study 1 in demon-
strating the tenacious link between psychopathy and dishonesty.
Even in the high risk condition, those with psychopathic traits
persisted in risking their MTurk reputation for minimal gain
($0.25). This finding supports the risk-taking element of
psychop-
athy, well-known even from early writings on the topic (Hare &
Neumann, 2008). This failure to consider long-term
consequences
puts such individuals at risk for self-destructive as well as inter-
personally malevolent behavior (e.g., Lynam, 1996).
By contrast, individuals high in Machiavellianism hesitated to
cheat under risk (see Figure 1). For them, the long-term reputa-
tional loss was not worth the short-term financial gain. The
finding
is consistent with evidence for the adaptive flexibility of
Machi-
avellians (Adams et al., 2014; Bereczkei et al., 2013; Jones &
Paulhus, 2011b).
Although not predicted, narcissism showed a marginally signif-
icant interaction. Hence it may be the case that, although less
than
Machiavellianism, narcissism may also entail some degree of
flexibility (e.g., Wallace & Baumeister, 2002). Overall, these
findings support the idea that the Dark Triad members can show
distinct behavioral patterns, consistent with their distinct
theoret-
ical roots (Jones & Paulhus, 2011a).
Study 3: Ego Depletion Induces
Psychopathic Behavior
Among Machiavellians
To implement their strategic planning, individuals of a Machi-
avellian character require higher-level cognitive resources
(Jones
& Paulhus, 2011a). Any context that blocks or undermines exec-
utive functioning should serve to impair their impulse control
(Gino et al., 2011). By contrast, temporary reductions in
executive
functioning should have little effect on psychopathic
individuals:
After all, their impulse control is chronically low (Hare & Neu-
mann, 2008), even for those at the subclinical levels (LeBreton,
Binning, & Adorno, 2006).
A straightforward method for testing such a hypothesis would
be to experimentally undermine the executive resources of re-
search participants. If this manipulation changed the behavior of
those high in Machiavellianism, but not those high in
psychopathy
or narcissism, it would provide strong evidence that
Machiavellian
individuals rely on those executive resources more than do indi-
viduals high in other dark traits.
One approach to impairing executive resources is to apply a
cognitive load during the target behavior (e.g., Finn, Gunn, &
Gerst, …
Surname: 1
Genome Editing and Eugenics
https://learn.westcoastuniversity.edu/webapps/assignment/uploa
dAssignment?content_id=_4516681_1&course_id=_55060_1&gr
oup_id=&mode=view
Prenatal Testing Scenario and Reflection.
This is the assignment - answer all the questions at the
beginning of the template completely. The last section of the
template asks you to write a 250 word response that includes
using one of the accepted theories ONLY (Social Contract,
Virtue Ethics, Care Ethics, Act Utilitarianism, Rule
Utilitarianism or Kant) to defend your position pro or con on
the issue that week. In your response, please include 4 parts A-
D for full credit, here is the exact last part again:
Which ethical theory (of the 6 approved ones ONLY – Kant; Act
Utilitarianism; Rule Utilitarianism; Care Ethics; Virtue Ethics;
and Social Contract) would you apply to this topic to defend
your stance? Explain fully. 250 words.
A. In this reflection you must first define the key terms of the
debate (for example, define what Euthanasia/Abortion/Stem
Cell/Public Health/PreNatal Testing/Animal Research/Health
Care, etc. means and what are the different types depending on
which topic you are discussing)
B. Define and explain the ethical theory you chose (show me
you know exactly what the theory is and does – in your own
words)
C. Present the evidence both pro and con and follow up how
using your ethical theory is the best way to determine whether
or not your stance is really ethical
D. Conclusion – any final thoughts and opinions would go here
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for
this publication at:
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692
IntimatelyConnected: The Importance of
Partner Responsiveness for Experiencing
Sexual Desire
Article in Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology · July 2016
DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000069
READS
629
6 authors, including:
Gurit E Birnbaum
Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya
41 PUBLICATIONS 1,033 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Moran Mizrahi
Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya
7 PUBLICATIONS 13 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Yaniv Kanat-Maymon
Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya
38 PUBLICATIONS 613 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All in-text referencesunderlined in blue are linked
to publications on ResearchGate,
letting you access and read them immediately.
Available from: Gurit E Birnbaum
Retrieved on: 18 August 2016
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692_Intimately
_Connected_The_Importance_of_Partner_Responsiveness_for_E
xperiencing_Sexual_Desire?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_2
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692_Intimately
_Connected_The_Importance_of_Partner_Responsiveness_for_E
xperiencing_Sexual_Desire?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_3
https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_1
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_4
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_5
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente
r_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_6
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_7
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r
greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_4
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r
greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_5
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente
r_IDC_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_6
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r
greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_7
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat-
Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_4
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat-
Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_5
https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente
r_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_6
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat-
Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3
D%3D&el=1_x_7
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Intimately Connected: The Importance of Partner
Responsiveness for Experiencing Sexual Desire
Gurit E. Birnbaum, Harry T. Reis, Moran Mizrahi, Yaniv Kanat-
Maymon, Omri Sass, and Chen
Granovski-Milner
Online First Publication, July 11, 2016.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069
CITATION
Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., Mizrahi, M., Kanat-Maymon, Y.,
Sass, O., & Granovski-Milner, C.
(2016, July 11). Intimately Connected: The Importance of
Partner Responsiveness for
Experiencing Sexual Desire. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. Advance online
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069
Intimately Connected: The Importance of Partner
Responsiveness for
Experiencing Sexual Desire
Gurit E. Birnbaum
Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya
Harry T. Reis
University of Rochester
Moran Mizrahi and Yaniv Kanat-Maymon
Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya
Omri Sass
Cornell Tech
Chen Granovski-Milner
Bar-Ilan University
Sexual desire tends to subside gradually over time, with many
couples failing to maintain desire in their
long-term relationships. Three studies employed complementary
methodologies to examine whether
partner responsiveness, an intimacy-building behavior, could
instill desire for one’s partner. In Study 1,
participants were led to believe that they would interact online
with their partner. In reality, they
interacted with either a responsive or an unresponsive
confederate. In Study 2, participants interacted
face-to-face with their partner, and judges coded their displays
of responsiveness and sexual desire. Study
3 used a daily experiences methodology to examine the
mechanisms underlying the responsiveness–
desire linkage. Overall, responsiveness was associated with
increased desire, but more strongly in
women. Feeling special and perceived partner mate value
explained the responsiveness– desire link,
suggesting that responsive partners were seen as making one
feel valued as well as better potential mates
for anyone and thus as more sexually desirable.
Keywords: gender differences, mate value, responsiveness,
romantic relationships, sexual desire
Sexual desire is among the strongest forces in human nature—
one that may induce overwhelming pleasures and intensely
mean-
ingful experiences or profound yearning and disappointment. As
such, it plays a major role not only in attracting potential
partners
to each other but also in promoting an enduring bond between
them. Indeed, the absence of sexual desire, which is prevalent in
both subclinical samples and clinical practice (e.g., Laumann,
Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Rosen, 2000), has long
been
considered as an important index of disrupted relational
harmony
(Kaplan, 1979; Leiblum & Rosen, 1988), depriving relationships
of intimacy (McCarthy, Bodnar, & Handal, 2004) and often
lead-
ing to breakup and divorce (e.g., Regan, 2000). It is therefore of
little surprise that both clinicians and researchers have searched
for
effective strategies to prevent against the waning of sexual
desire
in long-term relationships (e.g., McCarthy & Farr, 2012; Muise,
Impett, Kogan, & Desmarais, 2013).
Because sex is a prominent pathway through which people seek
a sense of felt understanding and caring (Birnbaum & Reis,
2006),
it is easy to understand why scholars have acknowledged the
contribution of intimacy-related processes to sexual desire.
How-
ever, the relevant literature has been largely based on clinical
impressions rather than systematic research (see review by Fer-
reira, Narciso, & Novo, 2012). Furthermore, findings from the
few
studies that have focused on the intimacy– desire linkage (e.g.,
Birnbaum, Cohen, & Wertheimer, 2007; McCabe, 1997; Patton
&
Waring, 1985) are hard to interpret because of several method-
ological issues, such as the use of correlational designs, which
preclude causal conclusions about the link between intimacy
and
the desire for sex, and the possibility of motivated distortion in
participants’ reports of sensitive topics like sexual desire (de
Jong
& Reis, 2015). To be sure, no prior research examining the
intimacy– desire link has used an experimental manipulation of
intimacy displays or has assessed behavioral expressions of
sexual
desire, which would rule out a motivated construal process
expla-
Gurit E. Birnbaum, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology,
Interdisciplin-
ary Center (IDC) Herzliya; Harry T. Reis, Department of
Clinical and
Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester; Moran
Mizrahi
and Yaniv Kanat-Maymon, Baruch Ivcher School of
Psychology, Interdis-
ciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya; Omri Sass, Jacobs Institute,
Cornell Tech;
Chen Granovski-Milner, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan
University.
This research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation
(Grant
86/10 awarded to Gurit E. Birnbaum) and by the Binational
Science
Foundation (Grant 2011381 awarded to Gurit E. Birnbaum and
Harry T.
Reis). We thank Noam Segal, Reut Bivas, May Recanati, Romi
Orr, Or
Dvir, Amit Ben-Zvi, Efrat Na’aman, Sapir Damti, Shiran
Halavi, Adi
Harel, Noa Hoss, Sivan Got, Michal Gazit, Adi Manvich, Yulia
Bogdanov,
Yuval Gilad, Zach Gerber, Maya Trajtenberg, Gal Lazarovich,
Lital Lib-
erty, and Shir Kogan for their assistance in the collection and
coding of the
data.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Gurit E.
Birnbaum, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology,
Interdisciplinary Center
(IDC) Herzliya, P.O. Box 167, Herzliya 46150, Israel. E-mail:
[email protected]
gmail.com
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2016 American
Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 110, No. 6, 000 0022-3514/16/$12.00
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069
1
nation (Reis & Gable, 2000). Another issue that has ambiguated
interpretation of prior studies is vagueness concerning the
concep-
tual and operational definitions of intimacy (Reis, Clark, & Hol-
mes, 2004).
To address these concerns, we relied on complementary meth-
ods and a clear, behaviorally focused definition. Moreover, be-
cause intimacy may be manifested in numerous ways, some of
which may be relatively uninfluential, we suggest that couple
interactions may best profit from a heightened sense of respon-
siveness. This key intimacy-building behavior (Reis & Clark,
2013) may be more likely than less personal manifestations of
intimacy (e.g., familiarity, comfort with each other’s company)
to
engender a sense that the partner is valuable and that the
relation-
ship is special, both of which seem conductive to increasing
desire.
In line with this reasoning, in the present set of studies, we
aimed
to investigate the contribution of partner responsiveness to the
desire to have sex with that partner. Our research extends
previous
research in several ways. First, we sought to establish a causal
connection between expressions of responsiveness and sexual
de-
sire in ongoing relationships. Second, in an attempt to delineate
the
mechanisms underlying this connection, we focused on whether
the sense of uniqueness and perceptions of a partner’s mate
value
that partner responsiveness might evoke explain its expected
effect
on sexual desire. Third, given that women’s sexual responses
are
more attuned to the relational context than those of men (e.g.,
Baumeister, 2000; Birnbaum & Laser-Brandt, 2002), we also
examined whether men and women diverge in their sexual reac-
tions to a responsive partner, and we addressed the role of
unique-
ness and perceived partner mate value as potential mechanisms
for
understanding these gender differences.
The Contribution of Expressions of Intimacy to
Sexual Desire
Perceived partner responsiveness is inherent to the development
of intimacy in sexual contexts, in which people seek
understand-
ing, validation, and caring (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006). People
who
perceive that their partners understand and appreciate their
needs
can view sexual interactions as one way to enhance intimate
experiences with responsive partners and, accordingly, may
expe-
rience greater desire for sex with them. In contrast, people who
perceive that their partners are unresponsive to their needs may
avoid sexual activity with them, thereby forgoing the potential
intimacy provided by sex. This reasoning aligns with the
conten-
tion that passion is fueled by cues of rising intimacy (e.g.,
displays
of affection and understanding; Baumeister & Bratslavsky,
1999),
and is corroborated by cross-sectional studies showing that in
established relationships, intimacy relates positively to sexual
de-
sire (e.g., Birnbaum et al., 2007; Štulhofer, Ferreira, &
Landripet,
2014). It is also supported by a diary study indicating that daily
increases in intimacy reported by both partners predicted higher
relationship passion and a higher probability of engaging in sex
(Rubin & Campbell, 2012).
Challenging this view, recent studies have revealed that during
the earliest phase of acquaintanceship, a responsive potential
part-
ner, who seems to want to be close, is less likely than a
relatively
nonresponsive partner to arouse sexual interest in some people
(Birnbaum, Ein-Dor, Reis, & Segal, 2014; Birnbaum & Reis,
2012). Still, it is not known whether findings about
responsiveness
and sexual desire based on the early stages of romantic relation-
ships apply to established relationships, given that the
contextual
meaning of partner responsiveness likely changes across
different
stages of relationship development. In initial encounters, which
tend to be suffused with uncertainty (Afifi & Lucas, 2008),
some
people may be cautious when interpreting a stranger’s
expressions
of responsiveness. For example, they may attribute
responsiveness
to ulterior motives (e.g., as a manipulation to obtain sexual
favors,
a self-presentation strategy) or to neediness, and thus perceive a
responsive potential partner as less sexually desirable than an
unresponsive partner (Birnbaum et al., 2014; Birnbaum & Reis,
2012).
However, as relationships develop, responsiveness may acquire
a different meaning and signal to partners that one genuinely
understands, values, and supports important aspects of their
self-
concept and is willing to invest resources in the relationship
(Birnbaum & Reis, 2012; Reis & Clark, 2013), and therefore
may
become more uniformly desired in a partner (Clark & Lemay,
2010). Indeed, unlike less intimate expressions that signal one’s
general intention to “act nice,” which may be more typical of
initial encounters, a partner’s provision of responsiveness in on-
going relationships not only signifies general communal tenden-
cies but also indicates the partner’s specific awareness of who
one
is at a relatively deep level, and what one truly wants. That is,
in
social relations model terminology (Kenny, 1990),
responsiveness
in an established relationship may signify relationship-specific
caring and concern, above and beyond dispositional tendencies
to
be responsive (“You don’t just care about people, you care
about
me in particular”). Recognizing this specific awareness in a
partner
makes the relationship feel special (Birnbaum & Reis, 2012;
Kenny, 1990; Reis & Clark, 2013), which is, at least in Western
life, what people seek from their romantic relationships
(Eastwick,
Finkel, Mochon, & Ariely, 2007). This recognition thereby in-
creases the responsive partner’s perceived mate value and thus
desirability (Birnbaum et al., 2014; Clark & Lemay, 2010).
In line with this theorizing, extensive evidence indicates that in
established relationships, perceived partner responsiveness is
as-
sociated with relationship well-being (see Reis & Clark, 2013,
for
a review). For example, among committed romantic couples, re-
sponsiveness during videotaped discussions of negative and
posi-
tive events predicts changes in relationship well-being over 2
months (Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman, 2006). In other studies,
perceived partner responsiveness has been shown to foster trust
and commitment in romantic relationships (Wieselquist,
Rusbult,
Foster, & Agnew, 1999; see Clark & Lemay, 2010, for a
review).
Theorists generally agree that perceived partner responsiveness
benefits relationship well-being because it signifies the belief
that
a partner can be counted on to reliably support and promote
important personal needs— one of the major functions of close
relationships (Clark & Lemay, 2010; Reis & Clark, 2013).
The Present Research
This research demonstrates that responsiveness can have bene-
ficial effects on many aspects of ongoing relationships.
Neverthe-
less, that work virtually ignores sexuality in long-term partner-
ships. One study that has tackled the sexual arena is provided
by
Muise, Impett, Kogan, et al. (2013), who found that the
motivation
to meet a partner’s sexual needs (i.e., sexual communal
strength)
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2 BIRNBAUM ET AL.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/258189622_Day-to-
Day_Changes_in_Intimacy_Predict_Heightened_Relationship_P
assion_Sexual_Occurrence_and_Sexual_Satisfaction_A_Dyadic
_Diary_Analysis?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do
es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se
xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId=
rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
predicts heightened feelings of sexual desire in long-term
relation-
ships. Although sexual communal strength is conceptually
relevant
to responsiveness, it is focused specifically on sexual
responsive-
ness rather than general responsiveness. Even though these two
constructs are somewhat correlated (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006),
they
are not isomorphic; a partner who understands one’s needs in
the
bedroom may not be willing to meet one’s needs outside of it,
and,
vice versa, a partner who is responsive in ordinary social
interac-
tion may not be sexually responsive. Such divergences may
cause
frustration that adversely affects sexual interactions over time.
In the present research, we used complementary methods, in-
cluding experimental and daily diary designs, to examine
whether
and why partner responsiveness outside the bedroom affects
sexual
desire in ongoing relationships. In doing so, we took into
account
that sexual reactions to a partner’s responsiveness might vary,
reflecting gender-specific differences in the meaning of
provision
of responsiveness. This possibility is consistent with the
assertion
that women, who typically have more to lose from a poor
mating
choice than men do (i.e., because of greater investment in each
offspring; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Trivers, 1972), have evolved
relatively greater sensitivity to cues of partners’ willingness to
provide care and invest resources in the relationship than men
(Buss, 1989; Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990).
Women should therefore have a better appreciation of a current
partner’s cues of investment (i.e., expressions of
responsiveness,
which are inherently geared toward promoting a partner’s
welfare)
than men, and they should value responsiveness somewhat
more.
To be sure, although both men and women who pursue long-
term
mating value “good partner” and “good parent” indicators (e.g.,
being loving, kind, and understanding; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, &
Linsenmeier, 2002), women’s perceptions of partner’s
attractive-
ness are more influenced than those of men by evidence of a
partner’s willingness to invest in the relationship (Bleske-
Rechek,
Remiker, Swanson, & Zeug, 2006; Brase, 2006). For example,
both sexes are negatively affected by seeing a potential long-
term
partner ignore a baby in distress, but this effect is stronger in
women (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2006).
Based on these findings, we hypothesized that a partner’s re-
sponsiveness would have a differential effect on men’s and
wom-
en’s relationship and partner perceptions, and consequently on
their desire for sex with this partner. More specifically, because
responsiveness signals that a partner has “special” (that is, over
and above that of casual acquaintances) concern with one’s wel-
fare in a way that is informed about one’s needs and wishes
(Reis
et al., 2004), it is expected to make both men and women feel
valued and cared for and appreciate their responsive partner
more
as a mate. These perceptions, in turn, should lead them to desire
their partner more. However, to the extent that women
emphasize
behaviors that signify caregiving and investment more than men
do (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2006; Brase, 2006), provision of
respon-
siveness should have a stronger effect on their relationship-
specific
perceptions and desires.
This article reports three studies examining the contribution of
partner responsiveness to sexual desire directed toward this
part-
ner. In all studies, participants rated their partners’
responsiveness
during a recent interaction and their desire to have sex with
them.
In Study 1, participants were led to believe that they would
interact
online with their partner. In reality, they discussed a recent per-
sonally meaningful life event with a confederate who sent either
responsive or unresponsive standardized messages. In Study 2,
participants discussed a personal event face-to-face with their
partner, thereby allowing interactions to unfold in a natural,
spon-
taneous way. Following this procedure, partners were invited to
express physical intimacy (e.g., caressing, kissing) with each
other.
These interactions were videotaped and coded by independent
judges for displays of responsiveness and desire. Study 3
investi-
gated the processes by which partner responsiveness affects
men’s
and women’s desire to have sex with this partner. For this
purpose,
over a span of 42 consecutive days, we asked both members of
romantic couples to complete daily measures of their partner’s
responsiveness and mate value, feelings of being special, and
sexual desire. Our specific predictions were as follows:
1. Partner responsiveness would be associated with in-
creased desire.
2. Feeling special and perceiving that one’s partner has high
mate value would explain the responsiveness– desire link,
such that responsive partners would enhance participants’
feeling valued and would be seen as better mates and as
more sexually desirable.
3. Provision of responsiveness would have a stronger effect
on women’s self- and partner perceptions and desire than
on men’s perceptions and desire.
Study 1
Study 1 was designed to establish a causal link between partner
responsiveness and the desire to engage in sex with this partner.
To
do so, we employed an experimental design in which
participants
were led to believe that they would interact online with their
partner. In reality, they discussed with a confederate over
Instant
Messenger a recent personal negative or positive event. The
con-
federate responded to this disclosure by sending either
responsive
or unresponsive standardized messages. Following this
discussion,
participants rated how understood, validated, and cared for they
felt during the interaction (i.e., perceived partner
responsiveness;
Reis et al., 2004) and their desire to engage in sexual activity
with
their partner.
Asking participants to disclose either a positive or a negative
event enabled us to explore the potential contribution of event
type to the desire to have sex with one’s partner. In doing so,
we
followed previous research indicating that responses to positive
event discussions were more strongly associated with
relationship
well-being than were responses to negative event discussions,
possibly because of the diminished sense of self-worth implied
by
the need for support (Gable et al., 2006; Rafaeli & Gleason,
2009).
We hypothesized that a similar pattern would be observed in
sexual desire, because of the potential dampening effect of
respon-
siveness to negative event disclosures on sexual desire, such
that
responsiveness to positive events disclosures would be more
likely
to instigate desire than responsiveness to negative events
disclo-
sures.
In this study, as well as in Study 2, sample size was determined
via a priori power analysis using G�Power software package
(Faul,
Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) to ensure 80% power to
detect
a medium effect size, f, of 0.25 at p � .05. All data were
collected
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
3PARTNER RESPONSIVENESS AND SEXUAL DESIRE
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/280798613_A_Multiv
ariate_Hierarchical_Model_for_Studying_Psychological_Chang
e_Within_Married_Couples?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247233476_Cues_of_p
arental_investment_as_a_factor_in_attractiveness?el=1_x_8&en
richId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247233476_Cues_of_p
arental_investment_as_a_factor_in_attractiveness?el=1_x_8&en
richId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237536985_Women_
More_than_Men_Attend_to_Indicators_of_Good_Character_Tw
o_Experimental_Demonstrations?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237536985_Women_
More_than_Men_Attend_to_Indicators_of_Good_Character_Tw
o_Experimental_Demonstrations?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237536985_Women_
More_than_Men_Attend_to_Indicators_of_Good_Character_Tw
o_Experimental_Demonstrations?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-
cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg==
…

More Related Content

Similar to Three Faces of Deceit: Distinctive Dark Triad Dynamics

An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docx
An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docxAn Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docx
An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docxgalerussel59292
 
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxdaniahendric
 
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxSALU18
 
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Kyle Erwin
 
Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature Reviewjssgrf
 
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docx
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docxDiscussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docx
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docxtheresiarede
 
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docxpriestmanmable
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLSantosConleyha
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLBenitoSumpter862
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitamit657720
 
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on © The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on TaunyaCoffman887
 
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxInternational Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxbagotjesusa
 
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxInternational Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxBHANU281672
 
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docx
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docxOriginal ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docx
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docxvannagoforth
 
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersHandbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersSpringer
 
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL Danny Athenour
 

Similar to Three Faces of Deceit: Distinctive Dark Triad Dynamics (19)

An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docx
An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docxAn Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docx
An Examination of the Early Strains” of Imprisonment Among .docx
 
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
 
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docxADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2016VOL. 24, NO. 3, 248–260ht.docx
 
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
 
Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature Review
 
FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28FINAL thesis 4.28
FINAL thesis 4.28
 
Social psychology
Social psychologySocial psychology
Social psychology
 
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docx
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docxDiscussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docx
Discussion 1 Attachment TheoryThe adolescent stage can be d.docx
 
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx
300 words agree or disagree to each questionQ1.There are.docx
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
 
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longitNarcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
Narcissism, bullying, and social dominance in youth a longit
 
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on © The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on
© The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on
 
Key Debates in Psychology
Key Debates in PsychologyKey Debates in Psychology
Key Debates in Psychology
 
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxInternational Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
 
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docxInternational Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
International Journal of Developmental Science 14 (2020) 1–7.docx
 
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docx
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docxOriginal ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docx
Original ArticleNeed for Cognitive Closure andPolitical .docx
 
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disordersHandbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders
 
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL
Disgust and Syrian Refugees FINAL
 

More from infantkimber

During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docx
During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docxDuring this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docx
During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docxinfantkimber
 
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docx
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docxDuring this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docx
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docxinfantkimber
 
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docx
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docxDuring this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docx
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docxinfantkimber
 
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docx
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docxDuring this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docx
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docxinfantkimber
 
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docx
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docxDuring the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docx
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docxinfantkimber
 
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docx
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docxDuring the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docx
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docxinfantkimber
 
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docx
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docxDuring the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docx
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docxinfantkimber
 
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docx
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docxDuring the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docx
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docxinfantkimber
 
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docx
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docxDuring the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docx
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docxinfantkimber
 
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docx
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docxDuring the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docx
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docxinfantkimber
 
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docx
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docxDuring the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docx
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docxinfantkimber
 
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docx
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docxDuring the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docx
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docxinfantkimber
 
During the Reagan Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docx
During the Reagan  Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docxDuring the Reagan  Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docx
During the Reagan Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docxinfantkimber
 
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docx
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docxDuring the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docx
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docxinfantkimber
 
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docx
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docxDuring the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docx
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docxinfantkimber
 
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docx
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docxDuring the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docx
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docxinfantkimber
 
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docxDuring the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docxinfantkimber
 
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docxDuring the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docxinfantkimber
 
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docx
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docxDuring the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docx
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docxinfantkimber
 
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docx
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docxDueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docx
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docxinfantkimber
 

More from infantkimber (20)

During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docx
During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docxDuring this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docx
During this time when the Internet provides essential communicat.docx
 
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docx
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docxDuring this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docx
During this period, the Impressionists became very interested in Jap.docx
 
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docx
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docxDuring this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docx
During this Module, you will be required to complete a Discussio.docx
 
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docx
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docxDuring this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docx
During this module, we review the concept of technology structures a.docx
 
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docx
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docxDuring the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docx
During the two decades following WWII, significant political, social.docx
 
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docx
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docxDuring the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docx
During the unprecedented times caused by the novel coronavirus d.docx
 
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docx
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docxDuring the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docx
During the time of the High Renaissance, culture and values varied d.docx
 
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docx
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docxDuring the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docx
During the Reconstruction Era, the Southern states created many .docx
 
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docx
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docxDuring the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docx
During the semester in which the student is enrolled in MUL2380, o.docx
 
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docx
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docxDuring the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docx
During the Reconstruction period, Congress became consumed with part.docx
 
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docx
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docxDuring the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docx
During the late 1960s and early 1970s, cognitive psychology rese.docx
 
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docx
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docxDuring the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docx
During the Renaissance and Baroque periods the type of patron divers.docx
 
During the Reagan Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docx
During the Reagan  Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docxDuring the Reagan  Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docx
During the Reagan Bush years there were many changes in America. P.docx
 
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docx
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docxDuring the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docx
During the quarter, you should keep a typed or handwritten journal r.docx
 
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docx
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docxDuring the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docx
During the quarter, Andronache Inc. collected $100 of cash from cust.docx
 
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docx
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docxDuring the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docx
During the previous 30 years, the airline industry has experienced m.docx
 
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docxDuring the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaki.docx
 
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docxDuring the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docx
During the previous weeks, you have developed your public speaking s.docx
 
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docx
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docxDuring the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docx
During the last quarter of the century before 1900, a great movement.docx
 
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docx
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docxDueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docx
DueApr 25 by 11pmPoints50Submittinga file .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptxPoojaSen20
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptxPSYCHIATRIC   History collection FORMAT.pptx
PSYCHIATRIC History collection FORMAT.pptx
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 

Three Faces of Deceit: Distinctive Dark Triad Dynamics

  • 1. Duplicity Among the Dark Triad: Three Faces of Deceit Daniel N. Jones University of Texas at El Paso Delroy L. Paulhus University of British Columbia Although all 3 of the Dark Triad members are predisposed to engage in exploitative interpersonal behavior, their motivations and tactics vary. Here we explore their distinctive dynamics with 5 behavioral studies of dishonesty (total N � 1,750). All 3 traits predicted cheating on a coin-flipping task when there was little risk of being caught (Study 1). Only psychopathy predicted cheating when punishment was a serious risk (Study 2). Machiavellian individuals also cheated under high risk— but only if they were ego-depleted (Study 3). Both psychopathy and Machiavellianism predicted cheating when it required an intentional lie (Study 4). Finally, those high in narcissism showed the highest levels of self-deceptive bias (Study 5). In sum, duplicitous behavior is far from uniform across the Dark Triad members. The frequency and nature of their dishonesty is moderated by 3 contextual factors: level of risk, ego depletion, and target of deception. This evidence for distinctive forms of duplicity helps clarify differences among the Dark Triad members as well as illuminating different shades of dishonesty. Keywords: cheating, Dark Triad, deception, honesty
  • 2. Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139.supp Concern about others’ honesty is among the strongest situational influences on social behavior (Rauthmann et al., 2014), and hon- esty attribution is the single most influential factor in person perception (Paunonen, 2006). However, the measurement of hon- esty as a trait is characterized by a fractured history reaching back to Hartshorne and May (1928). After assessing diverse forms of dishonest behaviors (e.g., lying, cheating, stealing) on large sam- ples of children, those seminal researchers dismissed the notion of a unitary trait construct. When properly interpreted, however, their data do indicate significant convergence across various forms of duplicity (Burton, 1963). Such tendencies also tend to converge at the virtuous pole (Jayawickreme, Meindl, Helzer, Furr, & Fleeson, 2014). In contemporary personality research, Ashton and Lee (2001) have been relentless in confirming this trait notion by isolating an Honesty-Humility (H-H) factor in their HEXACO model of personality. The incremental value of adding this factor in the prediction of personal integrity appears to be well supported (e.g., Lee, Ashton, Morrison, Cordery, & Dunlop, 2010; Spain, Harms, & Lebreton, 2014). Under the broad umbrella of the H-H factor, three narrower
  • 3. traits—narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy— have re- ceived extensive theoretical and empirical attention. Paulhus and Williams (2002) coined the term Dark Triad to encourage re- searchers to study these three traits as a constellation.1 Their rationale was that only a concurrent analysis can clarify any unique contributions of the triad members to outcome variables. Many researchers have taken up that challenge and the body of research has expanded geometrically (for the most recent review, see Paul- hus, 2014). Of particular interest to the current report is that, in a series of factor analyses, all three of the Dark Triad loaded on the H-H factor (Book, Visser, & Volk, 2015; Lee & Ashton, 2005; Lee et al., 2013). Given that scores on the H-H factor predict honest behavior in the workplace (Lee, Ashton, & de Vries, 2005), we were encouraged to tease apart distinctive forms of dishonesty linked with the Dark Triad members (See also Mededovic, 2012). One handicap to Dark Triad research has been the excessive length of the original measures: They add up to a taxing 124 items! Fortunately, two brief inventories tapping all three traits have become available. First is the Dirty Dozen (DD) (Jonason & Webster, 2010), a 12-item instrument. Although a significant body of research has exploited this measure (Jonason, Webster, Schmitt, Li, & Crysel, 2012), the consequences of its brevity have raised
  • 4. criticism (Carter, Campbell, Muncer, & Carter, 2015; Lee et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2012; Rauthmann & Kolar, 2012). The second combination measure is the 27-item Short Dark Triad (SD3) de- veloped by Jones and Paulhus (2014). When the two measures are compared head-to-head, the SD3 validities tend to be higher and more closely match the original Dark Triad measures (Lee et al., 2013; Maples, Lamkin, & Miller, 2014; Jones & Paulhus, 2014). To confirm this previous pattern, we used the traditional measures as well as both brief measures of the Dark Triad in several of the studies detailed below. 1 Note that the present research concerns subclinical levels of these variables (LeBreton et al., 2006) not clinical levels, where the term per- sonality disorder applies (see Campbell & Miller, 2011). This article was published Online First March 2, 2017. Daniel N. Jones, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at El Paso; Delroy L. Paulhus, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Delroy L. Paulhus, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver BC, V6T 1Z4 Canada. E-mail: [email protected]
  • 9. em in at ed br oa dl y. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 113, No. 2, 329 –342 0022-3514/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139 329 mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139 Whether the D3 are operationalized with the traditional mea- sures, the Dirty Dozen or Short Dark Triad, large bodies of research confirm the importance of distinguishing the three con- structs2— despite their positive intercorrelations (e.g., O’Boyle, Forsyth, Banks, & McDaniel, 2012). Successful applications in- clude such research domains as interpersonal aggression, person- nel selection, forensic diagnosis, and academic misbehavior as well as sexual and romantic relationships. In all of these studies,
  • 10. the triad members manifest distinctive patterns of interpersonal malevolence (see Paulhus, 2014, Jones & Paulhus, 2010). A critical point for the present paper is that previous work has linked all three Dark Triad members to a predisposition to deceive (Baughman et al., 2014; Giammarco, Atkinson, Baughman, Ve- selka, & Vernon, 2013; Jonason, Lyons, Baughman, & Vernon, 2015; O’Boyle et al., 2012). To date, however, only two studies on the Dark Triad have exploited behavioral measures of dishonesty (Jones, 2013, 2014a). All five studies presented below maintain the rigor and objectivity maximized by the use of behavioral out- comes. The Present Research Dishonesty has long been linked to callousness, that is, lack of empathy (Davis, 1996; Brown et al., 2010; Frick, 2009; Giam- marco & Vernon, 2015; Trout, 2009). Absent of concern for its effect on others, achievement striving is unmitigated and cheating behavior is unconstrained. Given our assumption that callousness is a common element of the Dark Triad (Jones & Paulhus, 2011a), all three should show a predisposition to dishonesty—at least, when repercussions are unlikely (i.e., low risk) Under more risky conditions, however, the dynamics of cheat- ing may play out in a different way for each Dark Triad member. Individuals with psychopathic traits have difficulty resisting an immediate reward— even when risk of punishment is high
  • 11. (Crysel, Crosier, & Webster, 2013; Jones, 2014b). Hence, in the studies below, we predict that the dishonesty of those with psychopathic traits will continue even under high risk conditions. By contrast, the hallmark of those high in Machiavellianism is strategic manipulation (Christie & Geis, 1970). To exploit scho- lastic situations, for example, they opt for surreptitious plagiarism rather than opportunistic copying during exams (Williams, Na- thanson, & Paulhus, 2010). Such strategic thinking requires cog- nitive resources. When depleted of these resources, individuals high in Machiavellianism may lose their strategic advantage and behave more like those high in psychopathy. Finally, those high in narcissism have a grandiose belief in their superiority to others (Kohut, 1966). Rather than an instrumental motivation to acquire resources, their egoistic motivation requires repeated confirmation of their intellectual superiority (Campbell & Foster, 2007; Jones & Paulhus, 2011a). In short, their dishonesty is self-deceptive in nature (Paulhus et al., 2003; von Hippel & Trivers, 2011). Even when confronted with contradictory evi- dence, narcissistic individuals tend to exaggerate their general knowledge (Paulhus et al., 2003) and group performance (Robins & John, 1997). Psychopathic individuals also showed this ten- dency, but to a lesser degree (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). To summarize these qualitative differences in dishonesty, we have laid them out in Table 1. Three distinctions loom large in the
  • 12. present set of studies on dishonesty: (a) instrumental versus ego- istic motivation, (b) impulsivity versus caution, and (c) self- versus other-deception. Those broad themes were highlighted in five studies of deceptive behavior. Study 1 examined which traits predicted dishonesty when there was minimal risk of being caught. Study 2 examined which individuals would continue to cheat when punishment was a serious risk. Study 3 examined the effects of ego depletion on dishonesty. Study 4 examined who was most likely to engage in blatant misrepresentation for financial benefit. Finally, Study 5 examined which individuals would exhibit self- deception on an anonymous overclaiming task. Study 1: All Three Show (Low-Risk) Dishonesty Method Power analysis. Although power analysis is challenging with novel research, we took guidance from the meta-analysis by Rich- ard, Bond, and Stokes-Zoota (2003). Across the body of social psychology and personality research, their meta-analysis yielded a mean correlation effect size of .21. They deduced that achieving 80% power of reaching significance with that effect size would require 173 participants. To be conservative, we aimed at a sub- stantially larger sample.
  • 13. Participants. The collection of human subjects data was ap- proved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Texas – El Paso. The protocol number and title were as follows: [391743–7] Moral Machiavellianism survey. The same approval was used to collect data for all the studies presented in this article. Participants were 292 adults from MTurk (51% women, Mean age � 32.86, SD � 11.78). Overall, 68% reported European Heritage, 7% African Heritage, 6% East Asian, 7% South Asian, 7% Latino, 5% other mixed ethnicities. Unless otherwise indi- cated, participants were compensated at a rate typical for MTurk (i.e., $0.25; see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Measures. Across all studies, personality scales were col- lected on Likert-type scales anchored by 1 (strongly disagree) and 5 (strongly agree). The only exception is the NPI-16 used in Study 3, which used a dichotomous response format. Short Dark Triad. To assess the Dark Triad traits in an effi- cient fashion, we used the Short Dark Triad (SD3) inventory (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Although recent in appearance, the distinctive construct validity of this inventory has already been supported by over 100 studies. Although not interchangeable, the SD3 tends to yield results that run parallel to those with the original Dark Triad measures (Jones & Olderbak, 2014; Jones & Paulhus, 2014). The three subscales have predicted unique patterns of interpersonal malevolence in dozens of studies: Using self- reports, those outcomes include partner aggression (Hamel, Jones, Dutton, & Graham-Kevan, 2015), racism (Jonason, 2015), bully-
  • 14. ing (Baughman et al., 2012), cyber-aggression (Pabian, De Backer, & Vandebosch, 2015), malevolent intentions (Veselka, Giam- marco, & Vernon, 2014), and immorality (Jonason, Strosser, Krull, Duinefeld, & Baruffi, 2014). A number of behavioral studies have also added to the construct validity of the SD3: These behaviors included distinctive patterns 2 The distinction is even supported by behavior genetics research (Ver- non, Villani, Vickers, & Harris, 2008; Veselka, Schermer, & Vernon, 2011). T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh
  • 18. er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 330 JONES AND PAULHUS of mimicry (Ashton-James & Levordashka, 2014), selfish financial behavior (Jones, 2013, 2014a; Roeser et al., 2016), and laboratory
  • 19. aggression (Buckels, Jones, & Paulhus, 2013). In all of these studies, the triad members, as operationalized with the SD3, man- ifested distinctive patterns of malevolence. The SD3 inventory measures the Dark Triad with nine items per trait. In the present sample, the subscales showed typical means and acceptable alpha reliabilities: Machiavellianism (M � 3.31, SD � .60, � � .74); psychopathy (M � 2.14, SD � .70, � � .81); narcissism (M � 2.76, SD � .57, � � .71). Subscale intercorre- lations were in the usual range: Machiavellianism with psychop- athy (r � .54); psychopathy with narcissism (r � .41); Mach with narcissism (r � .27). Dirty Dozen. We also included a second brief inventory, namely, the so-called ‘Dirty Dozen’ (DD) (Jonason & Webster, 2010). Using only four items per subscale, the Dirty Dozen has nonetheless generated a large body of research (see Jonason, Webster, Schmitt, Li, & Crysel, 2012). The inevitable tradeoff of fidelity for efficiency is discussed by Maples et al. (2014) as well as Jonason, Li, and Czarna (2013). In the present sample, internal consistencies for the Dirty Dozen subscales were all strong: narcissism (� � .82): Machiavellianism (� � .78), and psychopathy (� � .76). The subscale intercorrela- tions were as follows: Machiavellianism with narcissism (r � .44); Machiavellianism with psychopathy (r � .60); psychopathy with narcissism (r � .31). More detailed descriptives on the Dirty Dozen are provided in the Supplementary Materials.
  • 20. Procedure. Participants completed an online package of ques- tionnaires: It comprised the SD3, DD, the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003) as well as several questions about luck and gambling. The purpose of the TIPI and gambling questions were to obfuscate the true purpose of the study, that is, to examine the link between the Dark Triad and cheating. After completing the questionnaires, participants were sent to a new page, which had instructions in the center that read: “On the next page is a coin flipping task – if you win, you get a bonus! Unfortunately, there’s a glitch in the software, so it’s possible to flip the coin multiple times. So please only flip the coin once to be fair.” Participants then went to the next page, which had (a) choice buttons labeled “heads” and “tails”, (b) a “flip” button” repre- sented by a rotating coin and (c) a heading that read “Are you a lucky person?” This procedure was developed by Quoidbach and Chakroff (2011) in order to study dishonesty in a virtual environ- ment. Participants had to press “heads” or “tails” and were then asked to press the flip button. The result of the coin flip, however, was preprogrammed as follows: (a) on the first flip, it did not match participant’s choice, (b) a similar mismatch occurred on the second flip, and (c) on the third flip, it finally matched the participant’s
  • 21. choice. This process allowed dishonest participants the chance to flip until they obtained the desired outcome. Our primary depen- dent variable was whether or not participants flipped the coin more than once in order to obtain the bonus. Results and Discussion Overall, 13% of the sample cheated. As predicted, SD3 narcissism, r � .21, p � .001, Machiavellianism, r � .18, p � .001, and psychopathy, r � .23, p � .001 all had significant point-biserial correlations with dishonesty. A series of z tests provided no evidence that the three correlations were different (all ps � .10). Note from Table 2, however, that when entered simultaneously into a logistic regression, no one trait reached a .05 significance. In other words, the three correlations with dishonesty were so similar that claiming predominance for any one of them would be arbitrary. This preliminary result suggests that the three members of the Dark Triad were equally likely to cheat in this study. A similar but weaker pattern was found for the Dirty Dozen scales: narcissism, r � .19, p � .002, Machiavellianism, r � .11, p � .07, and psychopathy, r � .17, p � .004. Such weaker results are consistent with recent criticisms leveled against the scale (Carter et al., 2015; Jones & Paulhus, 2014; Kajonius, Persson, Rosenberg, & Garcia, 2015; Maples, Lamkin, & Miller, 2014; Miller et al., 2012). Not only do DD results tend to be weaker than SD3 results, they tend to be misaligned with the gold standard measures. Therefore, we relegate further results for the Dirty Dozen to our Supplementary Materials and will focus on the SD3 in the subsequent studies. Finally, none of the Big Five factors, as measured by the TIPI, showed any correlation with coin flip cheating (r values ranged from �.11
  • 22. to .09, all p � .06). Therefore, we did not include the TIPI in any of the subsequent studies. Table 1 Summary of Predictions D3 trait Low-risk deception High-risk deception Ego-depleted deception Intentional fraud Self deception Machiavellianism Yes No Yes Yes No Psychopathy Yes Yes Yes Yes Somewhat Narcissism Yes No No No Yes Table 2 Cheating as a Function of the Dark Triad Under Low Risk in Study 1 Predictors r rx.y B WALD OR 95% CI p Machiavellianism .18� .06 .23 1.07 1.26 .81, 1.97 .301 Narcissism .21� .12 .37 3.16 1.45 .96, 2.19 .076 Psychopathy .23� .12 .41 3.15 1.50 .96, 2.34 .076 Note. N � 292. The tabled values emerged from a logistic regression. � p � .05, two-tailed.
  • 27. in at ed br oa dl y. 331DUPLICITY AMONG THE DARK TRIAD Study 2: Under High-Risk, Only Psychopathy Predicts Dishonesty Method Power analysis. Instead of the meta-analysis (Richard et al., 2003), we used the effect sizes from Study 1 to calculate the sample needs of Study 2. The lowest simultaneous OR obtained in the Binary Logistic Regression from Study 1 was for Machiavel- lianism (OR � 1.47). Using this effect size, a power analysis suggested 340 participants were needed to achieve 80% power. Because Study 2 also examined an interaction, we collected an even larger sample of 441 participants. Note that we sampled 545 to achieve this final sample size after exclusion based on attention checks.
  • 28. Overview. Study 2 used a similar coin-flip paradigm but varied the risk of getting caught. Based on the previous literature, key differences among the Dark Triad should emerge under high- risk conditions. On MTurk, the most serious risk to workers is having their efforts “rejected”: They are fully aware that com- plaints about bad work are documented as a bad reputation and may limit their ability to find future work on MTurk (see Paolacci & Chandler, 2014). Perceived risk of getting caught was manipulated by randomly assigning participants to one of two warning conditions. As in Study 1, most participants under low risk should be tempted to coin-flip more than once. Under high risk, however, only individ- uals high in psychopathy should persist in cheating— because of their inability to delay gratification (Jonason, 2015; Jones, 2014a; Jones & Paulhus, 2011b). An interaction should emerge such that individuals high in Machiavellianism will cheat at low levels but not at high levels of risk. This prediction was based on previous literature showing the strategic flexibility of Machiavellians (Bereczkei et al., 2013; Christie & Geis, 1970; Esperger & Bereczkei, 2012; Jones & Paulhus, 2011a). For psychopathy, no such interaction should emerge: Individuals high in psychopathy should be just as likely to cheat in high and low risk conditions (see Jones, 2014b). We made no strong predictions for narcissism in the high risk condition,
  • 29. but along with most participants, individuals high in narcissism should cheat in the low risk condition. Participants. Participants were 545 adults solicited from Am- azon’s MTurk. A total of 104 participants were removed for failing attention checks or for taking the survey twice. Note that these rates of attention check failure are within the typical parameters for MTurk samples (Deetlefs, Chylinski, & Ortmann, 2015). The final sample was 441 workers (46% women, Mean age � 34.52, SD � 11.18; 77% European Heritage, 4% African Heritage, 7% East Asian, 6% Latino(a), 6% other). Dark Triad. As in Study 1, we used the Short Dark Triad or SD3 (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). The SD3 had significant (all p � .001) and moderate size intercorrelations: Machiavellianism with psychopathy � .58, Machiavellianism with narcissism � .37, and narcissism with psychopathy � .45). Means and internal consis- tencies were similar to those in Study 1: psychopathy (M � 2.11, SD � .64, � � .79); Machiavellianism (M � 3.09, SD � .68, � � .82); narcissism (M � 2.63, SD � .64, � � .77). Measures and procedure. Participants first completed the SD3 and were randomly assigned to one of two messages: The low risk message was identical to that of Study 1. The high risk message read, “We will be conducting random checks to see if
  • 30. anyone flipped the coin more than once. Those who do will have their work rejected.” In both messages, however, participants were alerted to the computer glitch and the fact that the coin could be flipped more than once. Results The warning manipulation was successful, with a higher per- centage of participants cheating (i.e., performing more than one coin-flip) in the low risk condition (17%) than in the high risk condition (10%), �2 � 5.09, p � .02. We then examined cheating as a function of condition and the Dark Triad, as well as the three Dark Triad � condition interactions in a binary logistic regression. As displayed in Table 3, results indicated a main effect for psy- chopathy, and an interaction between Machiavellianism and con- dition (p � .05, two-tailed). We went on to examine the point-biserial correlations of cheat- ing within conditions. As expected, both Machiavellianism, r � .21, p � .002 and psychopathy, r � .17, p � .01 were associated with cheating in the low risk condition. Unexpectedly, narcissism failed to reach significance. As predicted, only psychopathy, r � .15, p � .03 was significant in the high risk condition; Machia- vellianism, r � .05, p � .46, and narcissism, r � .10, p � .16 were not. We went further to conduct logistic regressions within condition.
  • 31. The results indicated that under low risk, Machiavellianism and psy- chopathy were the best predictors of cheating (Machiavellianism: B � .87, Wald � 5.91, OR � 2.39, 95% CI for OR � 1.18, 4.81, p � .015; narcissism: B � �.68, Wald � 3.50, OR � 0.51, 95% CI for OR � 0.25, 1.03, p � .506; psychopathy: B � .67, Wald � 3.23, OR � 1.94, 95% CI for OR � 0.94, 4.01, p � .072). In the high-risk condition, only psychopathy showed an odds ratio �1.00 and the only significant B coefficient: (Machiavellian- ism: B � �.32, Wald � 0.45, OR � 0.73, 95% CI for OR � 0.29, 1.84, p � .503; Narcissism: B � .36, Wald � 0.85, OR � 1.44, 95% CI for OR � 0.67, 3.10, p � .356; Psychopathy: B � .89, Wald � 3.30, OR � 2.43, 95% CI for OR � 0.93, 6.35, p � .069). Finally, we tested the difference in strength between the cheat- ing correlations (Machiavellianism vs. psychopathy) in the high risk condition. Results indicated that, as predicted, the correlation Table 3 Cheating as a Function of Dark Triad and Instructions in Study 2: Logistic Regression Predictors r rx.y B WALD OR 95% CI p Condition �.10� �.10� �.27 2.79 .76 .55, 1.05 .095 Machiavellianism .15� .07 .19 .87 1.21 .81, 1.79 .351 Narcissism .05 �.04 �.10 .35 .90 .65, 1.26 .552
  • 32. Psychopathy .16� .10� .50 6.41 1.65 1.12, 2.43 .011 Mach � Cond �.09 �.10� �.40 4.01 .67 .45, .99 .045 Narcissism � Cond �.03 .08 .33 3.80 1.40 .99, 1.95 .051 Psychopathy � Cond .04 .00 .07 .38 1.08 .73, 1.58 .715 Note. N � 441. The tabled values emerged from a logistic regression. Columns 7– 8 provide confidence intervals and significant levels for the odds ratios in Column 6. � p � .05, two-tailed. T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by
  • 36. is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 332 JONES AND PAULHUS of cheating with psychopathy was (marginally) higher than that with Machiavellianism, based on a two-tailed test, t � 1.89, p � .08. The cheating correlation with narcissism, however, was not significantly different from that with psychopathy or Machiavel- lianism (ps � .20). Discussion
  • 37. The results of Study 2 extend those from Study 1 in demon- strating the tenacious link between psychopathy and dishonesty. Even in the high risk condition, those with psychopathic traits persisted in risking their MTurk reputation for minimal gain ($0.25). This finding supports the risk-taking element of psychop- athy, well-known even from early writings on the topic (Hare & Neumann, 2008). This failure to consider long-term consequences puts such individuals at risk for self-destructive as well as inter- personally malevolent behavior (e.g., Lynam, 1996). By contrast, individuals high in Machiavellianism hesitated to cheat under risk (see Figure 1). For them, the long-term reputa- tional loss was not worth the short-term financial gain. The finding is consistent with evidence for the adaptive flexibility of Machi- avellians (Adams et al., 2014; Bereczkei et al., 2013; Jones & Paulhus, 2011b). Although not predicted, narcissism showed a marginally signif- icant interaction. Hence it may be the case that, although less than Machiavellianism, narcissism may also entail some degree of flexibility (e.g., Wallace & Baumeister, 2002). Overall, these findings support the idea that the Dark Triad members can show distinct behavioral patterns, consistent with their distinct theoret- ical roots (Jones & Paulhus, 2011a). Study 3: Ego Depletion Induces Psychopathic Behavior Among Machiavellians
  • 38. To implement their strategic planning, individuals of a Machi- avellian character require higher-level cognitive resources (Jones & Paulhus, 2011a). Any context that blocks or undermines exec- utive functioning should serve to impair their impulse control (Gino et al., 2011). By contrast, temporary reductions in executive functioning should have little effect on psychopathic individuals: After all, their impulse control is chronically low (Hare & Neu- mann, 2008), even for those at the subclinical levels (LeBreton, Binning, & Adorno, 2006). A straightforward method for testing such a hypothesis would be to experimentally undermine the executive resources of re- search participants. If this manipulation changed the behavior of those high in Machiavellianism, but not those high in psychopathy or narcissism, it would provide strong evidence that Machiavellian individuals rely on those executive resources more than do indi- viduals high in other dark traits. One approach to impairing executive resources is to apply a cognitive load during the target behavior (e.g., Finn, Gunn, & Gerst, … Surname: 1 Genome Editing and Eugenics https://learn.westcoastuniversity.edu/webapps/assignment/uploa dAssignment?content_id=_4516681_1&course_id=_55060_1&gr oup_id=&mode=view
  • 39. Prenatal Testing Scenario and Reflection. This is the assignment - answer all the questions at the beginning of the template completely. The last section of the template asks you to write a 250 word response that includes using one of the accepted theories ONLY (Social Contract, Virtue Ethics, Care Ethics, Act Utilitarianism, Rule Utilitarianism or Kant) to defend your position pro or con on the issue that week. In your response, please include 4 parts A- D for full credit, here is the exact last part again: Which ethical theory (of the 6 approved ones ONLY – Kant; Act Utilitarianism; Rule Utilitarianism; Care Ethics; Virtue Ethics; and Social Contract) would you apply to this topic to defend your stance? Explain fully. 250 words. A. In this reflection you must first define the key terms of the debate (for example, define what Euthanasia/Abortion/Stem Cell/Public Health/PreNatal Testing/Animal Research/Health Care, etc. means and what are the different types depending on which topic you are discussing) B. Define and explain the ethical theory you chose (show me you know exactly what the theory is and does – in your own words) C. Present the evidence both pro and con and follow up how using your ethical theory is the best way to determine whether or not your stance is really ethical D. Conclusion – any final thoughts and opinions would go here See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692 IntimatelyConnected: The Importance of
  • 40. Partner Responsiveness for Experiencing Sexual Desire Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · July 2016 DOI: 10.1037/pspi0000069 READS 629 6 authors, including: Gurit E Birnbaum Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya 41 PUBLICATIONS 1,033 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Moran Mizrahi Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya 7 PUBLICATIONS 13 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Yaniv Kanat-Maymon Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya
  • 41. 38 PUBLICATIONS 613 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All in-text referencesunderlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately. Available from: Gurit E Birnbaum Retrieved on: 18 August 2016 https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692_Intimately _Connected_The_Importance_of_Partner_Responsiveness_for_E xperiencing_Sexual_Desire?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_2 https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305211692_Intimately _Connected_The_Importance_of_Partner_Responsiveness_for_E xperiencing_Sexual_Desire?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_3 https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_1 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
  • 42. ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_4 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_5 https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente r_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_6 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Gurit_Birnbaum?enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_7 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_4 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_5 https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente r_IDC_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_6 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moran_Mizrahi?enrichId=r greq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a-
  • 43. XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_7 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat- Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_4 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat- Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_5 https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Interdisciplinary_Cente r_Herzliya?enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_6 https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Yaniv_Kanat- Maymon?enrichId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg%3 D%3D&el=1_x_7 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Intimately Connected: The Importance of Partner Responsiveness for Experiencing Sexual Desire Gurit E. Birnbaum, Harry T. Reis, Moran Mizrahi, Yaniv Kanat- Maymon, Omri Sass, and Chen Granovski-Milner Online First Publication, July 11, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069
  • 44. CITATION Birnbaum, G. E., Reis, H. T., Mizrahi, M., Kanat-Maymon, Y., Sass, O., & Granovski-Milner, C. (2016, July 11). Intimately Connected: The Importance of Partner Responsiveness for Experiencing Sexual Desire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069 Intimately Connected: The Importance of Partner Responsiveness for Experiencing Sexual Desire Gurit E. Birnbaum Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya Harry T. Reis University of Rochester Moran Mizrahi and Yaniv Kanat-Maymon Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya Omri Sass Cornell Tech Chen Granovski-Milner Bar-Ilan University Sexual desire tends to subside gradually over time, with many couples failing to maintain desire in their long-term relationships. Three studies employed complementary methodologies to examine whether partner responsiveness, an intimacy-building behavior, could
  • 45. instill desire for one’s partner. In Study 1, participants were led to believe that they would interact online with their partner. In reality, they interacted with either a responsive or an unresponsive confederate. In Study 2, participants interacted face-to-face with their partner, and judges coded their displays of responsiveness and sexual desire. Study 3 used a daily experiences methodology to examine the mechanisms underlying the responsiveness– desire linkage. Overall, responsiveness was associated with increased desire, but more strongly in women. Feeling special and perceived partner mate value explained the responsiveness– desire link, suggesting that responsive partners were seen as making one feel valued as well as better potential mates for anyone and thus as more sexually desirable. Keywords: gender differences, mate value, responsiveness, romantic relationships, sexual desire Sexual desire is among the strongest forces in human nature— one that may induce overwhelming pleasures and intensely mean- ingful experiences or profound yearning and disappointment. As such, it plays a major role not only in attracting potential partners to each other but also in promoting an enduring bond between them. Indeed, the absence of sexual desire, which is prevalent in both subclinical samples and clinical practice (e.g., Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Rosen, 2000), has long been considered as an important index of disrupted relational harmony (Kaplan, 1979; Leiblum & Rosen, 1988), depriving relationships of intimacy (McCarthy, Bodnar, & Handal, 2004) and often
  • 46. lead- ing to breakup and divorce (e.g., Regan, 2000). It is therefore of little surprise that both clinicians and researchers have searched for effective strategies to prevent against the waning of sexual desire in long-term relationships (e.g., McCarthy & Farr, 2012; Muise, Impett, Kogan, & Desmarais, 2013). Because sex is a prominent pathway through which people seek a sense of felt understanding and caring (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006), it is easy to understand why scholars have acknowledged the contribution of intimacy-related processes to sexual desire. How- ever, the relevant literature has been largely based on clinical impressions rather than systematic research (see review by Fer- reira, Narciso, & Novo, 2012). Furthermore, findings from the few studies that have focused on the intimacy– desire linkage (e.g., Birnbaum, Cohen, & Wertheimer, 2007; McCabe, 1997; Patton & Waring, 1985) are hard to interpret because of several method- ological issues, such as the use of correlational designs, which preclude causal conclusions about the link between intimacy and the desire for sex, and the possibility of motivated distortion in participants’ reports of sensitive topics like sexual desire (de Jong & Reis, 2015). To be sure, no prior research examining the intimacy– desire link has used an experimental manipulation of intimacy displays or has assessed behavioral expressions of sexual desire, which would rule out a motivated construal process expla-
  • 47. Gurit E. Birnbaum, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Interdisciplin- ary Center (IDC) Herzliya; Harry T. Reis, Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester; Moran Mizrahi and Yaniv Kanat-Maymon, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Interdis- ciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya; Omri Sass, Jacobs Institute, Cornell Tech; Chen Granovski-Milner, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University. This research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation (Grant 86/10 awarded to Gurit E. Birnbaum) and by the Binational Science Foundation (Grant 2011381 awarded to Gurit E. Birnbaum and Harry T. Reis). We thank Noam Segal, Reut Bivas, May Recanati, Romi Orr, Or Dvir, Amit Ben-Zvi, Efrat Na’aman, Sapir Damti, Shiran Halavi, Adi Harel, Noa Hoss, Sivan Got, Michal Gazit, Adi Manvich, Yulia Bogdanov, Yuval Gilad, Zach Gerber, Maya Trajtenberg, Gal Lazarovich, Lital Lib- erty, and Shir Kogan for their assistance in the collection and coding of the data. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gurit E. Birnbaum, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya, P.O. Box 167, Herzliya 46150, Israel. E-mail:
  • 52. ss em in at ed br oa dl y. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology © 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 110, No. 6, 000 0022-3514/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000069 1 nation (Reis & Gable, 2000). Another issue that has ambiguated interpretation of prior studies is vagueness concerning the concep- tual and operational definitions of intimacy (Reis, Clark, & Hol- mes, 2004). To address these concerns, we relied on complementary meth- ods and a clear, behaviorally focused definition. Moreover, be- cause intimacy may be manifested in numerous ways, some of which may be relatively uninfluential, we suggest that couple interactions may best profit from a heightened sense of respon- siveness. This key intimacy-building behavior (Reis & Clark, 2013) may be more likely than less personal manifestations of
  • 53. intimacy (e.g., familiarity, comfort with each other’s company) to engender a sense that the partner is valuable and that the relation- ship is special, both of which seem conductive to increasing desire. In line with this reasoning, in the present set of studies, we aimed to investigate the contribution of partner responsiveness to the desire to have sex with that partner. Our research extends previous research in several ways. First, we sought to establish a causal connection between expressions of responsiveness and sexual de- sire in ongoing relationships. Second, in an attempt to delineate the mechanisms underlying this connection, we focused on whether the sense of uniqueness and perceptions of a partner’s mate value that partner responsiveness might evoke explain its expected effect on sexual desire. Third, given that women’s sexual responses are more attuned to the relational context than those of men (e.g., Baumeister, 2000; Birnbaum & Laser-Brandt, 2002), we also examined whether men and women diverge in their sexual reac- tions to a responsive partner, and we addressed the role of unique- ness and perceived partner mate value as potential mechanisms for understanding these gender differences. The Contribution of Expressions of Intimacy to Sexual Desire Perceived partner responsiveness is inherent to the development
  • 54. of intimacy in sexual contexts, in which people seek understand- ing, validation, and caring (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006). People who perceive that their partners understand and appreciate their needs can view sexual interactions as one way to enhance intimate experiences with responsive partners and, accordingly, may expe- rience greater desire for sex with them. In contrast, people who perceive that their partners are unresponsive to their needs may avoid sexual activity with them, thereby forgoing the potential intimacy provided by sex. This reasoning aligns with the conten- tion that passion is fueled by cues of rising intimacy (e.g., displays of affection and understanding; Baumeister & Bratslavsky, 1999), and is corroborated by cross-sectional studies showing that in established relationships, intimacy relates positively to sexual de- sire (e.g., Birnbaum et al., 2007; Štulhofer, Ferreira, & Landripet, 2014). It is also supported by a diary study indicating that daily increases in intimacy reported by both partners predicted higher relationship passion and a higher probability of engaging in sex (Rubin & Campbell, 2012). Challenging this view, recent studies have revealed that during the earliest phase of acquaintanceship, a responsive potential part- ner, who seems to want to be close, is less likely than a relatively nonresponsive partner to arouse sexual interest in some people (Birnbaum, Ein-Dor, Reis, & Segal, 2014; Birnbaum & Reis, 2012). Still, it is not known whether findings about
  • 55. responsiveness and sexual desire based on the early stages of romantic relation- ships apply to established relationships, given that the contextual meaning of partner responsiveness likely changes across different stages of relationship development. In initial encounters, which tend to be suffused with uncertainty (Afifi & Lucas, 2008), some people may be cautious when interpreting a stranger’s expressions of responsiveness. For example, they may attribute responsiveness to ulterior motives (e.g., as a manipulation to obtain sexual favors, a self-presentation strategy) or to neediness, and thus perceive a responsive potential partner as less sexually desirable than an unresponsive partner (Birnbaum et al., 2014; Birnbaum & Reis, 2012). However, as relationships develop, responsiveness may acquire a different meaning and signal to partners that one genuinely understands, values, and supports important aspects of their self- concept and is willing to invest resources in the relationship (Birnbaum & Reis, 2012; Reis & Clark, 2013), and therefore may become more uniformly desired in a partner (Clark & Lemay, 2010). Indeed, unlike less intimate expressions that signal one’s general intention to “act nice,” which may be more typical of initial encounters, a partner’s provision of responsiveness in on- going relationships not only signifies general communal tenden- cies but also indicates the partner’s specific awareness of who one is at a relatively deep level, and what one truly wants. That is,
  • 56. in social relations model terminology (Kenny, 1990), responsiveness in an established relationship may signify relationship-specific caring and concern, above and beyond dispositional tendencies to be responsive (“You don’t just care about people, you care about me in particular”). Recognizing this specific awareness in a partner makes the relationship feel special (Birnbaum & Reis, 2012; Kenny, 1990; Reis & Clark, 2013), which is, at least in Western life, what people seek from their romantic relationships (Eastwick, Finkel, Mochon, & Ariely, 2007). This recognition thereby in- creases the responsive partner’s perceived mate value and thus desirability (Birnbaum et al., 2014; Clark & Lemay, 2010). In line with this theorizing, extensive evidence indicates that in established relationships, perceived partner responsiveness is as- sociated with relationship well-being (see Reis & Clark, 2013, for a review). For example, among committed romantic couples, re- sponsiveness during videotaped discussions of negative and posi- tive events predicts changes in relationship well-being over 2 months (Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman, 2006). In other studies, perceived partner responsiveness has been shown to foster trust and commitment in romantic relationships (Wieselquist, Rusbult, Foster, & Agnew, 1999; see Clark & Lemay, 2010, for a review). Theorists generally agree that perceived partner responsiveness benefits relationship well-being because it signifies the belief that
  • 57. a partner can be counted on to reliably support and promote important personal needs— one of the major functions of close relationships (Clark & Lemay, 2010; Reis & Clark, 2013). The Present Research This research demonstrates that responsiveness can have bene- ficial effects on many aspects of ongoing relationships. Neverthe- less, that work virtually ignores sexuality in long-term partner- ships. One study that has tackled the sexual arena is provided by Muise, Impett, Kogan, et al. (2013), who found that the motivation to meet a partner’s sexual needs (i.e., sexual communal strength) T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh
  • 61. er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 2 BIRNBAUM ET AL. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/258189622_Day-to- Day_Changes_in_Intimacy_Predict_Heightened_Relationship_P assion_Sexual_Occurrence_and_Sexual_Satisfaction_A_Dyadic _Diary_Analysis?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
  • 62. ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/224769564_When_Do es_Responsiveness_Pique_Sexual_Interest_Attachment_and_Se xual_Desire_in_Initial_Acquaintanceships?el=1_x_8&enrichId= rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz
  • 63. ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== predicts heightened feelings of sexual desire in long-term relation- ships. Although sexual communal strength is conceptually relevant to responsiveness, it is focused specifically on sexual responsive- ness rather than general responsiveness. Even though these two constructs are somewhat correlated (Birnbaum & Reis, 2006), they are not isomorphic; a partner who understands one’s needs in the bedroom may not be willing to meet one’s needs outside of it, and, vice versa, a partner who is responsive in ordinary social interac- tion may not be sexually responsive. Such divergences may cause frustration that adversely affects sexual interactions over time. In the present research, we used complementary methods, in- cluding experimental and daily diary designs, to examine whether and why partner responsiveness outside the bedroom affects sexual desire in ongoing relationships. In doing so, we took into account that sexual reactions to a partner’s responsiveness might vary, reflecting gender-specific differences in the meaning of provision of responsiveness. This possibility is consistent with the assertion that women, who typically have more to lose from a poor mating
  • 64. choice than men do (i.e., because of greater investment in each offspring; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Trivers, 1972), have evolved relatively greater sensitivity to cues of partners’ willingness to provide care and invest resources in the relationship than men (Buss, 1989; Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990). Women should therefore have a better appreciation of a current partner’s cues of investment (i.e., expressions of responsiveness, which are inherently geared toward promoting a partner’s welfare) than men, and they should value responsiveness somewhat more. To be sure, although both men and women who pursue long- term mating value “good partner” and “good parent” indicators (e.g., being loving, kind, and understanding; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002), women’s perceptions of partner’s attractive- ness are more influenced than those of men by evidence of a partner’s willingness to invest in the relationship (Bleske- Rechek, Remiker, Swanson, & Zeug, 2006; Brase, 2006). For example, both sexes are negatively affected by seeing a potential long- term partner ignore a baby in distress, but this effect is stronger in women (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2006). Based on these findings, we hypothesized that a partner’s re- sponsiveness would have a differential effect on men’s and wom- en’s relationship and partner perceptions, and consequently on their desire for sex with this partner. More specifically, because responsiveness signals that a partner has “special” (that is, over and above that of casual acquaintances) concern with one’s wel- fare in a way that is informed about one’s needs and wishes
  • 65. (Reis et al., 2004), it is expected to make both men and women feel valued and cared for and appreciate their responsive partner more as a mate. These perceptions, in turn, should lead them to desire their partner more. However, to the extent that women emphasize behaviors that signify caregiving and investment more than men do (Bleske-Rechek et al., 2006; Brase, 2006), provision of respon- siveness should have a stronger effect on their relationship- specific perceptions and desires. This article reports three studies examining the contribution of partner responsiveness to sexual desire directed toward this part- ner. In all studies, participants rated their partners’ responsiveness during a recent interaction and their desire to have sex with them. In Study 1, participants were led to believe that they would interact online with their partner. In reality, they discussed a recent per- sonally meaningful life event with a confederate who sent either responsive or unresponsive standardized messages. In Study 2, participants discussed a personal event face-to-face with their partner, thereby allowing interactions to unfold in a natural, spon- taneous way. Following this procedure, partners were invited to express physical intimacy (e.g., caressing, kissing) with each other. These interactions were videotaped and coded by independent judges for displays of responsiveness and desire. Study 3 investi-
  • 66. gated the processes by which partner responsiveness affects men’s and women’s desire to have sex with this partner. For this purpose, over a span of 42 consecutive days, we asked both members of romantic couples to complete daily measures of their partner’s responsiveness and mate value, feelings of being special, and sexual desire. Our specific predictions were as follows: 1. Partner responsiveness would be associated with in- creased desire. 2. Feeling special and perceiving that one’s partner has high mate value would explain the responsiveness– desire link, such that responsive partners would enhance participants’ feeling valued and would be seen as better mates and as more sexually desirable. 3. Provision of responsiveness would have a stronger effect on women’s self- and partner perceptions and desire than on men’s perceptions and desire. Study 1 Study 1 was designed to establish a causal link between partner responsiveness and the desire to engage in sex with this partner. To do so, we employed an experimental design in which participants were led to believe that they would interact online with their partner. In reality, they discussed with a confederate over Instant Messenger a recent personal negative or positive event. The con- federate responded to this disclosure by sending either responsive
  • 67. or unresponsive standardized messages. Following this discussion, participants rated how understood, validated, and cared for they felt during the interaction (i.e., perceived partner responsiveness; Reis et al., 2004) and their desire to engage in sexual activity with their partner. Asking participants to disclose either a positive or a negative event enabled us to explore the potential contribution of event type to the desire to have sex with one’s partner. In doing so, we followed previous research indicating that responses to positive event discussions were more strongly associated with relationship well-being than were responses to negative event discussions, possibly because of the diminished sense of self-worth implied by the need for support (Gable et al., 2006; Rafaeli & Gleason, 2009). We hypothesized that a similar pattern would be observed in sexual desire, because of the potential dampening effect of respon- siveness to negative event disclosures on sexual desire, such that responsiveness to positive events disclosures would be more likely to instigate desire than responsiveness to negative events disclo- sures. In this study, as well as in Study 2, sample size was determined via a priori power analysis using G�Power software package (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) to ensure 80% power to
  • 68. detect a medium effect size, f, of 0.25 at p � .05. All data were collected T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by th e A m er ic
  • 72. ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 3PARTNER RESPONSIVENESS AND SEXUAL DESIRE https://www.researchgate.net/publication/280798613_A_Multiv ariate_Hierarchical_Model_for_Studying_Psychological_Chang e_Within_Married_Couples?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq- cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247233476_Cues_of_p arental_investment_as_a_factor_in_attractiveness?el=1_x_8&en richId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247233476_Cues_of_p arental_investment_as_a_factor_in_attractiveness?el=1_x_8&en richId=rgreq-cc30770839814aebbe6d4cad1aa1362a- XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzMwNTIxMTY5MjtBUz ozODMxNDYxNzc2NDY1OTZAMTQ2ODM2MDU3NTI2Mg== https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237536985_Women_ More_than_Men_Attend_to_Indicators_of_Good_Character_Tw o_Experimental_Demonstrations?el=1_x_8&enrichId=rgreq-