SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 85
Article
Can you connect with
me now? How the
presence of mobile
communication
technology influences
face-to-face
conversation quality
Andrew K. Przybylski
Netta Weinstein
University of Essex, UK
Abstract
Recent advancements in communication technology have
enabled billions of people to
connect over great distances using mobile phones, yet little is
known about how the
frequent presence of these devices in social settings influences
face-to-face interactions.
In two experiments, we evaluated the extent to which the mere
presence of mobile
communication devices shape relationship quality in dyadic
settings. In both, we found
evidence they can have negative effects on closeness,
connection, and conversation
quality. These results demonstrate that the presence of mobile
phones can interfere with
human relationships, an effect that is most clear when
individuals are discussing person-
ally meaningful topics.
Keywords
Closeness, connection, conversation quality, face-to-face
interactions, mobile phones,
relationship quality
Corresponding author:
Andrew K. Przybylski, Department of Psychology, University of
Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ,
UK.
Email: [email protected]
Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships
30(3) 237–246
ª The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0265407512453827
spr.sagepub.com
J S P R
http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
http://spr.sagepub.com
Recent advancements in communication technology have
enabled billions of people in
the developed and developing world to connect with others
using mobile phones
(Mieczakowski, Goldhaber, & Clarkson, 2011). The widespread
availability and use of
mobile phones mean that these devices are commonly present in
public and private
settings and during casual and intimate interactions, often as
subtle background objects.
Despite their ubiquity, it is not known how the presence of
mobile communication
technology influences face-to-face interactions. The present
paper empirically explores
this issue for the first time and examines the effects of merely
having a mobile phone
present during in-person conversations.
Psychological research on phone use broadly suggests that it is
often aimed as a
source of entertainment and a means for sociability (O’Keefe &
Sulanowski, 1995), and
indicates the use of phones is largely a way to feel closer with
family members, to
express care for others, and to be available to others (Leung &
Wei, 2000). Despite the
fact that people seem attracted to mobile phones as a means to
interpersonal closeness,
little psychological research to date has systematically
investigated the actual influence
these devices have in or outside the context of relationships.
Instead, the thrust of
research in this area has examined effects mobile
communication technology has on
attention. Specifically, this research indicates that use of mobile
phones can reduce the
quality of attention to real-world events such as operating motor
vehicles (Strayer,
Drews, & Johnston, 2003; Strayer & Johnston, 2001).
Most research focusing on mobile phones and relationships
suggests they have the
potential to influence a range of interpersonal processes.
Interviews reveal mobile
phones provide a continual sense of connection to the wider
social world—a feeling that
persists even if a mobile is in ‘‘silent mode’’ (Plant, 2000).
Indeed, the presence of
phones is often felt during intimate social outings. As an
example, Geser (2002) observes
that a significant portion of couples eating together repeatedly
interrupt their meals to
check for text or voice messages. In reviewing a wide range of
survey data, Srivastava
(2005) concluded mobile phones might exert these pervasive
influences because people
associate phones with wide-ranging social networks. The
presence of a mobile phone
may orient individuals to thinking of other people and events
outside their immediate social
context. In doing so, they divert attention away from a presently
occurring interpersonal
experience to focus on a multitude of other concerns and
interests.
In line with this, a number of prominent theorists have argued
that mobile commu-
nication technology can have a decidedly negative influence on
interpersonal relation-
ships (Turkle, 2011). Turkle cites a wide range of qualitative
evidence collected in
interviews that phones can direct attention away from face-to-
face conversations by
making concerns about maintaining wider social networks
salient. Taken together with
quantitative research that demonstrates environmental cues can
activate relational
schema and affect behavior without a person’s awareness (Shah,
2003), there is reason
to believe that merely having a phone at hand may degrade
firsthand social interactions.
Effects such as these would be apparent when partners attempt
to engage one another
in a meaningful way. Particularly when strangers are
interacting, a superficial con-
versation may do little to foster closeness and trust in the
relationship. On the other hand,
dyads engaged in a meaningful conversation may develop their
relationship as they
become better acquainted with one another and self-disclose
relevant and personal
238 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
information (Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, 1997). In
these cases, the presence
of a diverting influence such as a mobile phone may inhibit
relationship formation by
reducing individuals’ engagement and attention for their
partners, and discouraging part-
ners’ perceptions that any self-disclosure had been met with
care and empathy. Indeed,
such an impediment to relationship formation may be frustrating
and isolating. To test
this idea, we investigated the degree to which the presence of
mobile communication
technology influences the quality of human interactions across
different kinds of
conversations.
Present studies
In the present research, we evaluated the idea that the presence
of mobile communication
technology may present a barrier to human interactions,
especially when people are
having meaningful conversations. To investigate this, we
conducted two experiments in
which pairs of strangers engaged in a brief relationship
formation task, adapted from
previous research (Aron et al., 1997). We also manipulated the
innocuous presence
versus absence of a mobile phone in the laboratory room. The
first experiment examined
the general effects of mobile phone presence on relational
processes, and the second
experiment investigated these dynamics for people having
casual and meaningful
interactions.
Experiment 1
Participants and procedure
Seventy-four participants (26 women; M age ¼ 21.88, SD ¼
4.81) were randomly
assigned to one of two conditions: (a) phone absent or (b) phone
present. For those
assigned to the phone present condition, a nondescript mobile
phone rested on a book,
which was placed on a nearby desk outside participants’ direct
visual field. In the phone
absent condition, a pocket notebook replaced the phone (see
Figure 1). We used a rela-
tionship formation task adapted from previous research (Aron et
al., 1997), which was
meant to emulate the content of many real-life conversations. A
pilot study indicated that
the assigned conversation, ‘‘Discuss an interesting event that
occurred to you over the
past month,’’ was a moderately intimate topic. Participants left
personal belongings in
a common waiting area before being led to a private booth along
with a randomly
assigned partner. Dyads were then asked to spend 10 minutes
discussing the topic
together. Following this encounter, participants completed
measures used in previous
research to assess relationship quality over time (Reis, Sheldon,
Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan,
2000) and emotional sensitivity in both committed and newly
formed relationships
(Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). Funneled debriefing of
participants in both experiments
indicated mobile phone placement was unobtrusive.
Measures
Relationship quality. Relationship quality was measured using a
seven-item version of the
connectedness subscale of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory
(McAuley, Duncan, &
Przybylski and Weinstein 239
Tammen, 1987), and included items such as ‘‘It is likely that
my partner and I could
become friends if we interacted a lot’’ (M ¼ 2.78, SD ¼ 0.79, a
¼ .85).
Partner closeness. Closeness between participants was measured
using the Inclusion of
Other in the Self Scale (Aron et al., 1992), which instructed
participants to select one of
seven increasingly overlapping circle pairs representing
themselves and their conversation
partner. (In the present study, participants responding averaged:
M ¼ 3.57, SD ¼ 1.46.)
Covariate: Positive affect. Positive and negative affect was
assessed using the nine-item
Emmons Mood Indicator (Diener & Emmons, 1984) to account
for the potential con-
founding effect of overall positive mood on relational outcomes.
Items included pleased,
worried/anxious, and frustrated (a ¼ .83), paired with a seven-
point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely).
Results
Data analytic strategy. Analyses required accommodations for
nesting persons within
dyads (assuming nonindependence between the two interacting
partners). Analyses were
Figure 1. Overhead view of lab room. 1. Mobile phone or pocket
notebook. 2. Book on desk.
3. Chairs used by participants.
240 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
therefore conducted with hierarchical linear modeling (HLM;
Raudenbush & Bryk,
2002). Condition (1: phone present, �1: phone absent) was
defined at level 2 (dyad
level), while covariates (gender, age, and positive affect) were
defined at level 1 (person
level). Unconditional models were first assessed to determine
whether sufficient var-
iance existed between- and within-dyad. Intraclass correlation
(ICC) derived from these
models showed that across outcomes, 43% to 51% of the total
variance occurred between
persons. Given the substantial variance accounted for at each
level, full models were
tested. Across analyses, the general level 1 equation was as
follows:
OV ij ¼ Boj þ B1X 1ij þ B2X 2ij þ B3X 3ij þ eij
where Boj reflects the average value of the relational outcome,
B1 reflects the estimated
population slope of gender, B2 reflects that of age, B3 reflects
that of positive affect, and
eij represents level 1 error.
The level 2 equation was:
Boj ¼ Goo þ G01X1j þ u0j
where Goo reflects the person level intercept for an average
person and G01 refers to the
effect of the phone condition. As Raudenbush & Bryk (2002)
recommended, level 1
variables were centered on individual rather than sample means;
level 2 variables were
not centered.
Relationship quality. At level 1, positive affect related to better
relationship quality,
b ¼ 0.80, t(69) ¼ 2.99, p < .001, though there were no effects of
either gender, b ¼�20,
t(69) ¼�0.61, p ¼ .55, or age, b ¼�0.04, t(69) ¼�0.03, p ¼
.12. Controlling for these,
individuals in the mobile phone condition reported lower
relationship quality after the
interaction, b ¼�0.99, t(35) ¼ 3.08, p ¼ .004.
Partner closeness. Results for perceived closeness with one’s
partner were largely
consistent with relationship quality. None of the level 1
predictors related to perceived
closeness with one’s partner, bs ¼ �0.00 to �.09, ts(69) ¼
�0.06 to �0.64, ps ¼ .52
to .95. Yet at level 2, those in the mobile phone condition
reported less closeness with
their partners, b ¼ �0.39, t(35) ¼�2.56, p ¼ .02.
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 found that dyadic partners who got to know one
another in the presence of
a mobile phone (via sharing a moderately meaningful
discussion) felt less close with
their partners and reported a lower quality of relationships than
did partners who shared a
conversation without a mobile phone present. Experiment 2
explored which relational
contexts mobile phones most mattered by manipulating the
content of discussion to be
either casual or meaningful. We hypothesized mobile phones
would impede relational
outcomes when partners are attempting to build an intimate
connection and have less effect
in casual conversation, where little self-disclosure takes place.
In addition, this second
experiment explored two new relational outcomes that have
been shown as important
Przybylski and Weinstein 241
indicators for building intimate relationships: interpersonal trust
(Campbell, Simpson,
Boldry, & Rubin, 2010) and perceived partner empathy (Reis,
Clark, & Holmes, 2004).
Participants and procedure
Sixty-eight participants (43 women; M age ¼ 23.21, SD ¼ 4.99)
were randomly assigned
to one of the cells of a 2 (absent vs. present phone) � 2 (casual
vs. meaningful con-
versation) between-subjects design. A modified version of the
10-minute relationship
formation task from Experiment 1 was used for all 34 dyads.
Participants in the casual
conversation condition were instructed to discuss their thoughts
and feelings about
plastic holiday trees (casual condition); those assigned to the
important conversation
condition discussed the most meaningful events of the past year
(meaningful
condition).
Measures
To examine a more diverse set of relational outcomes that
included processes indicative
of intimate relationships, relationship quality was assessed in
the same way as in
Experiment 1 (M ¼ 4.98, SD ¼ 1.09, a ¼ .86) but was paired
with two additional
measures of relational functioning: trust and empathy.
Partner trust. Trust was assessed with an item asking
participants to rate their agreement
to the statement: ‘‘I felt like I could really trust my
conversation partner’’ (M ¼ 3.25,
SD ¼ 1.01).
Partner empathy. Empathy was measured with the nine-item
Empathic Concern Scale
(Davis, 1995), which included items such as: ‘‘To what extent
do you think your partner
accurately understood your thoughts and feelings about the
topic?’’ (M ¼ 4.98, SD ¼ 1.09,
a ¼ .92).
Results
Analytic strategy. HLM analyses were conducted as in
Experiment 1, although in the
present study, phone manipulation (1: phone present, �1: phone
absent) was interacted
with conversation type (1: meaningful, �1: casual), both
entered at level 2. Preliminary
unconditional models showed 39% to 47% of variance was at
the between-person level.
Figure 2 presents the means of observed for relationship
quality, partner trust, and part-
ner empathy across conditions.
Relationship quality. There were no effects of gender, age, or
positive affect on relationship
quality, bs ¼ 0.01 to 0.09, ts(60) ¼ 0.13 to 1.28, ps ¼ .21 to
.90. At level 2 and consistent
with Experiment 1, mobile phone presence predicted lower
relationship quality,
b ¼�0.19, t(30) ¼�2.29, p ¼ .03, and new to this study, results
showed an interaction
between mobile phone presence and conversation type, b
¼�0.26, t(30) ¼�3.02, p ¼
.006 (no effect of conversation type, b ¼ 0.03, t(30) ¼ 0.29, p ¼
.78). Simple slopes
242 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
analyses showed no effect of phone when the conversation was
casual, b ¼ 0.02, t(14) ¼
0.50, p ¼ .63. On the other hand, the presence of the mobile
phone predicted lower rela-
tionship quality when the conversation was meaningful,
b¼�0.45, t(16)¼�4.21, p¼ .001.
Partner trust. None of the covariates (gender, age, or positive
affect) related to trust,
bs ¼ 0.02 to 0.13, ts(60) ¼ �0.13 to 1.50, ps ¼ .13 to .90. At
level 2, phone presence
predicted less trust between partners, b ¼ �0.36, t(30) ¼ �3.76,
p < .001, and mean-
ingful conversations marginally encouraged trust, b ¼ 0.21,
t(30) ¼ 2.00, p ¼ .06. These
main effects were qualified by their interaction, b ¼ �0.33,
t(30) ¼ �3.48, p ¼ .002.
Simple slopes analyses complemented those of relationship
quality, indicating that when
the conversation was casual the presence of a mobile phone had
no effect on partners’
trust, b ¼ �0.07, t(14) ¼ �0.26, p ¼ .80, yet partners who
attempted to share a
meaningful conversation in the presence of a phone reported
less trust than those who did
so in its absence, b ¼�0.69, t(16) ¼�5.24, p < .001.
Figure 2. This figure shows that the presence of a mobile phone
in the laboratory room leads to
lower levels of relationship quality, trust, and empathy. All
critical t-tests are significant at p < .05.
Error bars are based on standard error.
Przybylski and Weinstein 243
Perceived empathy. A final series of analyses were conducted
predicting perceived
empathy from partners. Older participants reported perceiving
marginally higher
empathy from their partners, b ¼ 0.04, t(60) ¼ 1.92, p ¼ .06,
though there was no
relation with gender, b ¼ �0.08, t(60) ¼ �0.29, p ¼ .77, or
positive affect, b ¼ 0.21,
t(60) ¼ 1.55, p ¼ .13. At level 2, participants reported lower
perceived empathy when the
phone was present compared to absent, b ¼�0.37, t(30)
¼�3.60, p ¼ .002, yet no effect
of conversation type, b ¼ 0.18, t(30) ¼ 1.49, p ¼ .15. These
relations were qualified by
an interaction, b ¼�0.36, t(30) ¼�3.41, p ¼ .002. As before,
simple slopes analyses
showed no differences in perceived empathy between the two
phone conditions when the
conversation was a casual one, b ¼�0.01, t(14) ¼�0.06, p ¼
.95; however, when the
conversation was meaningful the presence of a phone predicted
less perceived empathy
following the conversation, b ¼�0.72, t(16) ¼�4.66, p < .001.
Conclusions
These results demonstrated that the mere presence of mobile
communication technology
might interfere with human relationship formation, lending
some empirical support to
concerns voiced by theorists (Turkle, 2011). Evidence derived
from both experiments
indicates the mere presence of mobile phones inhibited the
development of interpersonal
closeness and trust, and reduced the extent to which individuals
felt empathy and
understanding from their partners. Results from the second
experiment indicated that
these effects were most pronounced if individuals were
discussing a personally
meaningful topic. More specifically, results of this experiment
showed that meaningful
conversation topics tended to encourage intimacy and trust
under neutral conditions.
This difference between those in the casual and meaningful
conversation conditions was
absent in the presence of a mobile phone, which appeared to
interfere in conditions that
were otherwise conducive to intimacy. More interesting, the
debriefing procedure
suggests that these effects might happen outside of conscious
awareness.
The present findings are qualified by a number of limitations
that provide avenues for
future research. The first and most important question this
research leaves open concerns
the mechanism through which a mobile phone impedes
relationship formation. Given
that the effects do not appear to depend on conscious awareness,
it is possible that phones
operate as a prime that activates implicit representations of
wider social networks, which
in turn crowd out face-to-face conversations. It is also possible
that people form individ-
ual and enduring implicit associations with phones; and such
attitudes, behaviors, and
cognitions interrupt here-and-now interactions. In both cases,
these mechanisms could
be examined in future research using sequential priming and or
lexical decision tasks
(e.g., Shah, 2003).
In a similar vein, an expanding body of qualitative research
(e.g., Srivastava, 2005)
suggests people form important and diverse kinds of
connections to their personal
devices. Future work might replicate and extend the present
studies by exploring how the
presence of a range of personally owned communication devices
such as portable or
tablet computers shape conversations. In the present
experiments, we examined mobile
phones in the context of casual and meaningful conversations,
but the presence of mobile
phones may show a curvilinear relationship such that they do
not obstruct relationship
244 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
formation in deeply meaningful conversations; future research
may explore these
processes across conversation types. In addition, future research
should test behavior
directly to answer the question: do mobile phones change the
way partners behave
toward one another (e.g., via the amount of self-disclosure,
nonverbal relational beha-
viors, attentiveness), or whether they primarily shape subjective
perceptions of conver-
sations. Finally, these experiments explored the effects of
mobile phone presence on
relationships among heretofore strangers. Future research may
focus on the extent to
which mobile devices would have such effects in established
relationships, such as those
between business associates, parents and children, or romantic
partners.
All things considered, this research presents interesting
empirical findings that inform
the billions of conversations that take place daily, as many are
accompanied by a phone
placed casually on a table or bar. These results indicate that
mobile communication
devices such as phones may, by their mere presence,
paradoxically hold the potential to
facilitate as well as to disrupt human bonding and intimacy.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding
agency in the public, commercial, or
not-for-profit sectors.
References
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other
in the Self Scale and the structure
of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 596–612.
Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R.
J. (1997). The experimental generation
of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary
findings. Personality & Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363–377.
Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J. G., & Rubin, H.
(2010). Trust, variability in relationship
evaluations, and relationship processes. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 99, 14–31.
Davis, M. H. (1995). A multidimensional approach to individual
differences in empathy. Corte
Madera, CA: Select Press.
Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of
positive and negative affect. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.
Geser, H. (2002). Sociology of the mobile phone. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Zurich,
Switzerland.
Leung, L., & Wei, R. (2000). More than just talk on the move:
Uses and gratifications of the
cellular phone. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly,
77, 308–320.
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. V. (1989).
Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic
Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport setting: A
confirmatory factor analysis. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 60, 48–58.
Mieczakowski, A., Goldhaber, T., & Clarkson, J. (2011).
Culture, communication, and change:
Report on an investigation of the use and impact of modern
media and technology in our lives.
In Culture, communication, and change. Cambridge, UK:
Engineering Design Centre, University
of Cambridge.
O’Keefe, G. J., & Sulanowski, B. K. (1995). More than just
talk: Uses, gratifications, and the
telephone. Journalism and Mass Communications Quarterly, 72,
922–933.
Przybylski and Weinstein 245
Plant, S. (2000). On the mobile. The effects of mobile
telephones on social and individual life.
Retrieved from
http://www.Motorola.com/mot/documents/0,1028,333,00.pdf
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models: Applications and data
analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived
partner responsiveness as an organizing
construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J.
Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of
closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan,
R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The
role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,
26, 419–435.
Shah, J. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How
significant others implicitly affect goal
appraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
424–439.
Srivastava, L. (2005). Mobile phones and the evolution of social
behavior. Behaviour & Information
Technology, 24, 111–129.
Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., & Johnston, W. A. (2003). Cell
phone-induced failures of visual attention
during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 9, 23–32.
Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction:
Dual-task studies of simulated
driving and conversing on a cellular phone. Psychological
Science, 12, 462–466.
Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from
technology and less from each other.
New York, NY: Basic Books.
246 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
http://www.Motorola.com/mot/documents/0,1028,333,00.pdf
<<
/ASCII85EncodePages false
/AllowTransparency false
/AutoPositionEPSFiles true
/AutoRotatePages /None
/Binding /Left
/CalGrayProfile (Gray Gamma 2.2)
/CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2)
/sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Warning
/CompatibilityLevel 1.3
/CompressObjects /Off
/CompressPages true
/ConvertImagesToIndexed true
/PassThroughJPEGImages false
/CreateJDFFile false
/CreateJobTicket false
/DefaultRenderingIntent /Default
/DetectBlends true
/DetectCurves 0.1000
/ColorConversionStrategy /LeaveColorUnchanged
/DoThumbnails false
/EmbedAllFonts true
/EmbedOpenType false
/ParseICCProfilesInComments true
/EmbedJobOptions true
/DSCReportingLevel 0
/EmitDSCWarnings false
/EndPage -1
/ImageMemory 1048576
/LockDistillerParams true
/MaxSubsetPct 100
/Optimize true
/OPM 1
/ParseDSCComments true
/ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true
/PreserveCopyPage true
/PreserveDICMYKValues true
/PreserveEPSInfo true
/PreserveFlatness false
/PreserveHalftoneInfo false
/PreserveOPIComments false
/PreserveOverprintSettings true
/StartPage 1
/SubsetFonts true
/TransferFunctionInfo /Apply
/UCRandBGInfo /Remove
/UsePrologue false
/ColorSettingsFile ()
/AlwaysEmbed [ true
]
/NeverEmbed [ true
]
/AntiAliasColorImages false
/CropColorImages false
/ColorImageMinResolution 266
/ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleColorImages true
/ColorImageDownsampleType /Bicubic
/ColorImageResolution 200
/ColorImageDepth -1
/ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1
/ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000
/EncodeColorImages true
/ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterColorImages false
/ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/ColorACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/ColorImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.76
/HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2]
>>
/JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000ColorImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasGrayImages false
/CropGrayImages false
/GrayImageMinResolution 266
/GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleGrayImages true
/GrayImageDownsampleType /Bicubic
/GrayImageResolution 200
/GrayImageDepth -1
/GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2
/GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000
/EncodeGrayImages true
/GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterGrayImages false
/GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/GrayACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/GrayImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.76
/HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2]
>>
/JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000GrayImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasMonoImages false
/CropMonoImages false
/MonoImageMinResolution 900
/MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleMonoImages true
/MonoImageDownsampleType /Average
/MonoImageResolution 600
/MonoImageDepth -1
/MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000
/EncodeMonoImages true
/MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode
/MonoImageDict <<
/K -1
>>
/AllowPSXObjects false
/CheckCompliance [
/None
]
/PDFX1aCheck false
/PDFX3Check false
/PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false
/PDFXNoTrimBoxError true
/PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false
/PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051
v2)
/PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier (CGATS TR 001)
/PDFXOutputCondition ()
/PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org)
/PDFXTrapped /Unknown
/Description <<
/ENU
<FEFF00550073006500200074006800650073006500200053006
1006700650020007300740061006e006400610072006400200073
0065007400740069006e0067007300200066006f0072002000630
0720065006100740069006e006700200077006500620020005000
440046002000660069006c00650073002e0020005400680065007
30065002000730065007400740069006e0067007300200063006f
006e006600690067007500720065006400200066006f007200200
04100630072006f006200610074002000760037002e0030002e00
20004300720065006100740065006400200062007900200054007
2006f00790020004f007400730020006100740020005300610067
00650020005500530020006f006e002000310031002f003100300
02f0032003000300036002e000d000d0032003000300050005000
49002f003600300030005000500049002f004a005000450047002
0004d0065006400690075006d002f004300430049005400540020
00470072006f0075007000200034>
>>
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(Common)
(1.0)
]
/OtherNamespaces [
<<
/AsReaderSpreads false
/CropImagesToFrames true
/ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue
/FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false
/IncludeGuidesGrids false
/IncludeNonPrinting false
/IncludeSlug false
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(InDesign)
(4.0)
]
/OmitPlacedBitmaps false
/OmitPlacedEPS false
/OmitPlacedPDF false
/SimulateOverprint /Legacy
>>
<<
/AllowImageBreaks true
/AllowTableBreaks true
/ExpandPage false
/HonorBaseURL true
/HonorRolloverEffect false
/IgnoreHTMLPageBreaks false
/IncludeHeaderFooter false
/MarginOffset [
0
0
0
0
]
/MetadataAuthor ()
/MetadataKeywords ()
/MetadataSubject ()
/MetadataTitle ()
/MetricPageSize [
0
0
]
/MetricUnit /inch
/MobileCompatible 0
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(GoLive)
(8.0)
]
/OpenZoomToHTMLFontSize false
/PageOrientation /Portrait
/RemoveBackground false
/ShrinkContent true
/TreatColorsAs /MainMonitorColors
/UseEmbeddedProfiles false
/UseHTMLTitleAsMetadata true
>>
<<
/AddBleedMarks false
/AddColorBars false
/AddCropMarks false
/AddPageInfo false
/AddRegMarks false
/BleedOffset [
9
9
9
9
]
/ConvertColors /ConvertToRGB
/DestinationProfileName (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/DestinationProfileSelector /UseName
/Downsample16BitImages true
/FlattenerPreset <<
/ClipComplexRegions true
/ConvertStrokesToOutlines false
/ConvertTextToOutlines false
/GradientResolution 300
/LineArtTextResolution 1200
/PresetName ([High Resolution])
/PresetSelector /HighResolution
/RasterVectorBalance 1
>>
/FormElements true
/GenerateStructure false
/IncludeBookmarks false
/IncludeHyperlinks false
/IncludeInteractive false
/IncludeLayers false
/IncludeProfiles true
/MarksOffset 9
/MarksWeight 0.125000
/MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(CreativeSuite)
(2.0)
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK
/PageMarksFile /RomanDefault
/PreserveEditing true
/UntaggedCMYKHandling /UseDocumentProfile
/UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile
/UseDocumentBleed false
>>
]
/SyntheticBoldness 1.000000
>> setdistillerparams
<<
/HWResolution [288 288]
/PageSize [612.000 792.000]
>> setpagedevice
Reading Guide #8 332 Fall 2017
Rus, M., & Galbeaza, A. B. (2013). Psychological aspects of
child sexual abuse.
Contemporary Readings in Law and Social Justice, 5(2), 499-
510.
1. How do Rus and Galbeaza (2013) describe abuse?
Use of force in order to try to dominate child, to
compel him/her to do dangerous things that he/she does not
want to do, expose him/her to hazardous situations or to
situations perceived by him/her dangerous. Any action that
causes injury or psycho-emotional disorders is abuse.
2. What are the ways abusers can manipulate their targets?
Physical abuse Emotional abuse Sexual abuse
Economic abuse Neglection
3. What are the defense mechanisms a child sexual abuse
survivor might display as an adult?
Disintegration of personality may be one of
the most fundamental defense mechanisms used by incest
victims.
4. Name 5 of the direct signs of distress abuse can cause in an
individual.
Low self-esteem. Symptoms of depression.
Narcissism, Alcohol, drugs and mental illness Shame
5. What is the picture of sexual abuse in Romania?
There are many tourists who go to Romania for sex tourism,
especially pedophiles. Some travel societies offer pictures of
children at a certain price.
Abuse/ignorance of children is widespread, and 9.1% of
children surveyed have said they have been sexually abused
6. What is the take home message in the Rus and Galbeaza
(2013) article?
Child sexual abuse can cause great psychological
and physical damage to children. This effect will last a lifetime
and will lead to suicide in severe cases.
THE CONSUMER IN A CONNECTED WORLD
Brain Drain: The Mere Presence of One’s Own
Smartphone Reduces Available Cognitive Capacity
ADRIAN F. WARD, KRISTEN DUKE, AYELET GNEEZY,
AND MAARTEN W. BOS
ABSTRACT Our smartphones enable—and encourage—constant
connection to information, entertainment, and
each other. They put the world at our fingertips, and rarely
leave our sides. Although these devices have immense po-
tential to improve welfare, their persistent presence may come
at a cognitive cost. In this research, we test the “brain
drain” hypothesis that the mere presence of one’s own
smartphone may occupy limited-capacity cognitive resources,
thereby leaving fewer resources available for other tasks and
undercutting cognitive performance. Results from two
experiments indicate that even when people are successful at
maintaining sustained attention—as when avoiding
the temptation to check their phones—the mere presence of
these devices reduces available cognitive capacity. More-
over, these cognitive costs are highest for those highest in
smartphone dependence. We conclude by discussing the
practical implications of this smartphone-induced brain drain
for consumer decision-making and consumer welfare.
Adr
211
ver
and
@di
Sch
the
Eas
Cen
JAC
© 2
We all understand the joys of our always-wired world—the
connections, the validations, the laughs . . . the info. . . . But we
are only beginning to get
our minds around the costs.
—Andrew Sullivan (2016)
T
he proliferation of smartphones has ushered in an
era of unprecedented connectivity. Consumers around
the globe are now constantly connected to faraway
friends, endless entertainment, and virtually unlimited in-
formation. With smartphones in hand, they check the
weather from bed, trade stocks—and gossip—while stuck
in traffic, browse potential romantic partners between ap-
pointments, make online purchases while standing in-store,
and live-stream each others’ experiences, in real time, from
opposite sides of the globe. Just a decade ago, this state of
constant connection would have been inconceivable; today,
it is seemingly indispensable.1 Smartphone owners interact
with their phones an average of 85 times a day, including
immediately upon waking up, just before going to sleep,
and even in the middle of the night (Perlow 2012; Andrews
et al. 2015; dscout 2016). Ninety-one percent report that
ian F. Ward ([email protected]) is an assistant professor of m
0 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712. Kristen Duke
([email protected]) i
sity of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA
92093. Ayelet G
marketing at the Rady School of Management, University of
California, Sa
sneyresearch.com) is a research scientist at Disney Research,
4720 Forbes A
wartz, Yael Horwitz, and the Atkinson Behavioral Lab for
research assistanc
1. In 2007, only 4% of American adults owned smartphones
(Radwanick 201
age of 35—own smartphones (Pew Research Center 2017).
Penetration is s
tern and Asian countries. South Korea, for example, has a
national smartphon
ter 2016).
R, volume 2, number 2. Published online April 3, 2017.
http://dx.doi.org/10
017 the Association for Consumer Research. All rights reserved.
2378-1815
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
they never leave home without their phones (Deutsche
Telekom 2012), and 46% say that they couldn’t live without
them (Pew Research Center 2015). These revolutionary de-
vices enable on-demand access to friends, family, col-
leagues, companies, brands, retailers, cat videos, and much
more. They represent all that the connected world has to of-
fer, condensed into a device that fits in the palm of one’s
hand—and almost never leaves one’s side.
The sharp penetration of smartphones, both across
global markets and into consumers’ everyday lives, repre-
sents a phenomenon high in “meaning and mattering”
(e.g., Kernan 1979; Mick 2006)—one that has the potential
to affect the welfare of billions of consumers worldwide.
As individuals increasingly turn to smartphone screens
for managing and enhancing their daily lives, we must
ask how dependence on these devices affects the ability to
arketing in the McCombs School of Business, University of
Texas at Austin,
s a PhD candidate in marketing at the Rady School of
Management, Uni-
neezy ([email protected]) is an associate professor of behavioral
sciences
n Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093. Maarten W.
Bos (mbos
venue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. The authors thank Jiyoung Lee,
Stephanie
e.
2). As of January 2017, 77% of American adults—and 92% of
those under
imilarly high in most Western nations, and even higher in
several Middle
e ownership rate of 88%, including 100% of those under 35
(Pew Research
.1086/691462
/2017/0202-0009$10.00
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 141
think and function in the world off-screen. Smartphones
promise to create a surplus of resources, productivity, and
time (e.g., Turkle 2011; Lee 2016); however, they may also
create unexpected deficits. Prior research on the costs and
benefits associated with smartphones has focused on how
consumers’ interactions with their smartphones can both
facilitate and interrupt off-screen performance (e.g., Isik-
man et al. 2016; Sciandra and Inman 2016). In the present
research, we focus on a previously unexplored (but com-
mon) situation: when smartphones are not in use, but are
merely present.
We propose that the mere presence of one’s own smart-
phone may induce “brain drain” by occupying limited-
capacity cognitive resources for purposes of attentional con-
trol. Because the same finite pool of attentional resources
supports both attentional control and other cognitive pro-
cesses, resources recruited to inhibit automatic attention
to one’s phone are made unavailable for other tasks, and
performance on these tasks will suffer. We differentiate be-
tween the orientation and allocation of attention and argue
that the mere presence of smartphones may reduce the
availability of attentional resources even when consumers
are successful at controlling the conscious orientation of at-
tention.
COGNITIVE CAPACITY AND
CONSUMER BEHAVIOR
Consumers’ finite capacity for cognitive processing is one
of the most fundamental influences on “real world” con-
sumer behavior (e.g., Bettman 1979; Bettman, Johnson,
and Payne 1991). Individuals are constantly surrounded
by potentially meaningful information; however, their abil-
ity to use this information is consistently constrained by
cognitive systems that are capable of attending to and pro-
cessing only a small amount of the information available at
any given time (e.g., Craik and Lockhart 1972; Newell and
Simon 1972). This capacity limit shapes a wide range of be-
haviors, from in-the-moment decision-making strategies
and performance (e.g., Lane 1982; Lynch and Srull 1982)
to long-term goal pursuit and self-regulation (e.g., Hof-
mann, Strack, and Deutsch 2008; Benjamin, Brown, and
Shapiro 2013).
Consumers’ cognitive capabilities—and constraints—are
largely determined by the availability of domain-general,
limited-capacity attentional resources associated with both
workingmemory and fluid intelligence (e.g., Halford, Cowan,
and Andrews 2007; Jaeggi et al. 2008). “Working memory”
(WM) refers to the theoretical cognitive system that sup-
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
ports complex cognition by actively selecting, maintaining,
and processing information relevant to current tasks and/
or goals. “Working memory capacity” (WMC) reflects the
availability of attentional resources, which serve the “cen-
tral executive” function of controlling and regulating cogni-
tive processes across domains (Baddeley and Hitch 1974;
Miyake and Shah 1999; Engle 2002; Baddeley 2003). “Fluid
intelligence” (Gf) represents the ability to reason and solve
novel problems, independent of any contributions from ac-
quired skills and knowledge stored in “crystallized intelli-
gence” (Cattell 1987). Similar to WM, Gf stresses the ability
to select, store, andmanipulate information in a goal-directed
manner. Also similar to WM, Gf is constrained by the avail-
ability of attentional resources (e.g., Engle et al. 1999; Halford
et al. 2007). Crucially, the limited capacity of these domain-
general resources dictates that using attentional resources
for one cognitive process or task leaves fewer available for
other tasks; in other words, occupying cognitive resources re-
duces available cognitive capacity.
Given the chronic mismatch between the abundance of
environmental information and the limited ability to pro-
cess that information, individuals need to be selective in
their allocation of attentional resources (e.g., Kahneman
1973; Johnston and Dark 1986). The priority of a stimu-
lus—that is, the likelihood that it will attract attention—
is determined by both its physical “salience” (e.g., location,
perceptual contrast) and its goal “relevance” (i.e., potential
importance for goal-directed behavior) (e.g., Corbetta and
Shulman 2002; Fecteau and Munoz 2006). Preferential at-
tention to temporarily relevant stimuli, such as those asso-
ciated with a current task or decision, is supported by WM;
when a goal is active in WM, stimuli relevant to that goal
are more likely to attract attention (e.g., Moskowitz 2002;
Soto et al. 2005; Vogt et al. 2010). Frequently relevant stim-
uli, such as those associated with long-term and/or self-
relevant goals, may automatically attract attention even
when the goals associated with these stimuli are not active
in WM (Shiffrin and Schneider 1977; Johnston and Dark
1986); for example, individuals automatically orient to
the sounds of their own names in ignored audio channels
(Moray 1959), and mothers, more so than nonmothers,
automatically attend to infants’ emotional expressions
(Thompson-Booth et al. 2014). Automatic attention gen-
erally helps individuals make the most of their limited
cognitive capacity by directing attention to frequently goal-
relevant stimuli without requiring these goals to be con-
stantly kept in mind. However, automatic attention may
undermine performance when an environmental stimulus is
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
142 Brain Drain Ward et al.
frequently relevant to an individual’s goals but currently irrel-
evant to the task at hand; inhibiting automatic attention—
keeping attractive but task-irrelevant stimuli from interfer-
ingwith the contents of consciousness—occupies attentional
resources (e.g., Engle 2002).
Smartphones serve as consumers’ personal access points
to all the connected world has to offer. We suggest that the
increasing integration of these devices into the minutiae of
daily life both reflects and creates a sense that they are fre-
quently relevant to their owners’ goals; it lays the founda-
tion for automatic attention. Consistent with this position,
research indicates that signals from one’s own phone (but
not someone else’s) activate the same involuntary atten-
tion system that responds to the sound of one’s own name
(Roye, Jacobsen, and Schröger 2007). When these devices
are salient in the environment, their status as high-priority
(relevant and salient) stimuli suggests that they will exert
a gravitational pull on the orientation of attention. And
when consumers are engaged in tasks for which their smart-
phones are task-irrelevant, the ability of these devices to
automatically attract attention may undermine performance
in two ways (Clapp, Rubens, and Gazzaley 2009; Clapp and
Gazzaley 2012). First, smartphones may redirect the orien-
tation of conscious attention away from the focal task and
toward thoughts or behaviors associated with one’s phone.
Prior research provides ample evidence that individuals
spontaneously attend to their phones at inopportune times
(e.g., Oulasvirta et al. 2011), and that this digital distraction
adversely affects both performance (End et al. 2009) and en-
joyment (Isikman et al. 2016). Second, smartphones may re-
distribute the allocation of attentional resources between en-
gaging with the focal task and inhibiting attention to one’s
phone. Because inhibiting automatic attention occupies at-
tentional resources, performance on tasks that rely on these
resources may suffer even when consumers do not con-
sciously attend to their phones. We explore this possibility
in the current research.
SMARTPHONE USE AND CONSCIOUS
DISTRACTION (THE ORIENTATION
OF ATTENTION)
Research on the relationship between mobile devices and
cognitive functioning has largely focused on downstream
consequences of device-related changes in the orientation
of attention. For example, research on mobile device use
while driving indicates that interacting with one’s phone
while behind the wheel causes performance deficits such
as delayed reaction times and inattentional blindness (e.g.,
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
Strayer and Johnston 2001; Caird et al. 2008); these defi-
cits mirror those associated with distracting “live” conver-
sations (Recarte and Nunes 2003). Similarly, research in
the educational sphere demonstrates that using mobile de-
vices and social media while learning new material reduces
comprehension and impairs academic performance (e.g.,
Froese et al. 2012). However, mobile device use does not af-
fect performance on self-paced tasks, which allow individu-
als to compensate for device-related distractions by pick-
ing up where they left off (e.g., Fox, Rosen, and Crawford
2009; Bowman et al. 2010). Taken together, these findings
suggest that many of the cognitive impairments associated
with mobile device use may simply represent the general
deleterious effects of diverting conscious attention away
from a focal task. What may be special about smartphones,
however, is the frequency with which they seem to create
these diversions; their omnipresence and personal rele-
vance may combine to create a particularly potent draw
on the orientation of attention.
A more limited body of work explores the cognitive
consequences of smartphone-related distractions in the ab-
sence of behavioral interaction (i.e., when consumers con-
sciously think about phone-related stimuli, but do not actu-
ally use their phones). Research on the attentional cost of
receiving cellphone notifications indicates that awareness
of amissed textmessage or call impairs performance on tasks
requiring sustained attention, arguably because unaddressed
notifications prompt message-related (and task-unrelated)
thoughts (Stothart, Mitchum, and Yehnert 2015). Related re-
search shows that individuals who hear their phones ring
while being separated from them report decreased enjoy-
ment of focal tasks as a consequence of increased attention
to phone-related thoughts (Isikman et al. 2016). Forced sep-
aration from one’s ringing phone can also increase heart rate
and anxiety and decrease cognitive performance (Clayton,
Leshner, andAlmond2015). To our knowledge, only one prior
study has investigated the cognitive effects of the mere pre-
sence of a mobile device—one that is not ringing, buzzing,
or otherwise actively interfering with a focal task. Thornton
et al. (2014, 485–86) found that a visually salient cellphone
can impair performance on tasks requiring sustained atten-
tion by eliciting awareness of the “broad social and informa-
tional network . . . that one is not part of at the moment.”
Together, these investigations of phone-related distractions
provide evidence thatmobile devices can adversely affect cog-
nitive performance even when consumers are not actively
using them. Similar to earlier research on distracted driving
and learning while multitasking, however, these studies
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
2. A pilot study confirmed that these physical locations predict
indi-
viduals’ top-of-mind awareness of their smartphones, with a
nearby and
in sight → nearby and out of sight → not nearby linear trend
(F(1,
111) 5 14.58, p < .001, partial h2 5 .116) and no quadratic trend
(p 5
.996). Interestingly, the majority of respondents (67.5%)
indicated that
they typically keep their smartphones nearby and in sight,
where these de-
vices are most salient. See the appendix for method and detailed
analyses.
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 143
connect the cognitive costs of smartphones to their (re-
markable) ability to attract the conscious orientation of at-
tention. When individuals interact with or think about their
phones rather than attend to the task at hand, their perfor-
mance suffers.
SMARTPHONE PRESENCE AND COGNITIVE
CAPACITY (THE ALLOCATION OF
ATTENTIONAL RESOURCES)
We suggest that smartphones may also impair cognitive
performance by affecting the allocation of attentional re-
sources, even when consumers successfully resist the urge
to multitask, mind-wander, or otherwise (consciously) at-
tend to their phones—that is, when their phones are merely
present. Despite the frequency with which individuals use
their smartphones, we note that these devices are quite of-
ten present but not in use—and that the attractiveness
of these high-priority stimuli should predict not just their
ability to capture the orientation of attention, but also the
cognitive costs associated with inhibiting this automatic at-
tention response.
We propose that the mere presence of one’s smartphone
may impose a “brain drain” as limited-capacity attentional
resources are recruited to inhibit automatic attention to
one’s phone, and are thus unavailable for engaging with
the task at hand. Research on controlled versus automatic
processing provides evidence that the mere presence of per-
sonally relevant stimuli can impair performance on cognitive
tasks (e.g., Geller and Shaver 1976; Bargh 1982; Wingenfeld
et al. 2006). Importantly, these performance deficits occur
without conscious attention to the potentially interfering
stimuli and as a function of inhibiting these stimuli from in-
terfering with the contents of consciousness (e.g., Shallice
1972; Lavie et al. 2004). Consistent with this evidence, we
posit that themere presence of consumers’ own smartphones
can reduce the availability of attentional resources (i.e., cog-
nitive capacity) even when consumers are successful at con-
trolling the conscious orientation of attention (i.e., resisting
overt distraction).
If smartphones undermine cognitive performance by oc-
cupying attentional resources, the cognitive consequences
of smartphone presence should be sensitive to variation
in both the salience and the personal relevance of these de-
vices, which together determine their priority in attracting
attention (e.g., Fecteau and Munoz 2006). Prior research
suggests that smartphones are chronically salient for many
individuals, even when they are located out of sight in one’s
pocket or bag (e.g., Deb 2015). However, we expect that in-
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
creasing the salience of one’s smartphone—for example, by
placing it nearby and in the field of vision—will amplify the
cognitive costs associated with its presence, as more atten-
tional resources are required to inhibit its influence on the
orientation of attention. We also expect that these costs
will vary according to the personal relevance of one’s smart-
phone. We operationalize relevance in terms of “smart-
phone dependence,” or the extent to which individuals rely
on their phones in their everyday lives. We posit that indi-
vidual differences in dependence on one’s smartphone will
moderate the effects of smartphone salience on available
cognitive capacity, such that individuals who most depend
on their phones will suffer the most from their presence—
and benefit the most from their absence.
OVERVIEW OF THE EXPERIMENTS
In two experiments, we test the hypothesis that the mere
presence of one’s own smartphone reduces available cogni-
tive capacity. We manipulate smartphone salience by ask-
ing participants to place their devices nearby and in sight
(high salience, “desk” condition), nearby and out of sight
(medium salience, “pocket/bag” condition), or in a separate
room (low salience, “other room” condition).2 Our data in-
dicate that the mere presence of one’s smartphone ad-
versely affects two domain-general measures of cognitive
capacity—available working memory capacity (WMC) and
functional fluid intelligence (Gf)—even when participants
are not using their phones and do not report thinking
about them (experiment 1). Data from experiment 2 repli-
cate this effect on available cognitive capacity, show no ef-
fect on a behavioral measure of sustained attention, and
provide evidence that individual differences in consumers’
dependence on their devices moderate the effects of smart-
phone salience on available WMC.
EXPERIMENT 1: SMARTPHONE SALIENCE
AFFECTS AVAILABLE COGNITIVE CAPACITY
In experiment 1, we test the proposition that themere pres-
ence of one’s own smartphone reduces available cognitive
capacity, as reflected in performance on tests of WMC
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
144 Brain Drain Ward et al.
and Gf. Each of these domain-general cognitive constructs
is constrained by the availability of attentional resources,
and the moment-to-moment availability of these resources
predicts performance on tests of both WMC (Engle, Cantor,
and Carullo 1992; Ilkowska and Engle 2010) and Gf (Horn
1972; Mani et al. 2013). If the mere presence of one’s
own smartphone taxes the limited-capacity attentional re-
sources that constrain both WMC and Gf, then the salience
of this device should predict performance on tasks associ-
ated with these constructs. We test this hypothesis in ex-
periment 1.
Method
Participants. Five hundred forty-eight undergraduates
(53.3% female;Mage 5 21.1 years; SDage 5 2.4 years) partic-
ipated for course credit. Data collection spanned two
weeks. Duplicate data from repeat participants were dis-
carded prior to analysis. We applied the same three data
selection criteria in experiments 1 and 2; see the appendix
(available online) for additional detail. In experiment 1,
three participants were excluded for indicating they did
not own smartphones, eight participants were excluded
for failing to follow instructions, and seventeen participants
were excluded due to excessive error rates on the OSpan
task (less than 85% accuracy; see Unsworth et al. 2005).
Our final sample consisted of 520 smartphone users.
Procedure. We manipulated smartphone salience by ran-
domly assigning participants to one of three phone location
conditions: desk, pocket/bag, or other room. Participants
in the “other room” condition left all of their belongings
in the lobby before entering the testing room (as per typical
lab protocol). Participants in the “desk” condition left most
of their belongings in the lobby but took their phones into
the testing room “for use in a later study;” once in the test-
ing room, they were instructed to place their phones face
down in a designated location on their desks. Participants
in the “pocket/bag” condition carried all of their belongings
into the testing room with them and kept their phones
wherever they “naturally” would. Of the 174 participants
in this condition, 91 (52.3%) reported keeping their phones
in their pockets, and 83 (47.7%) reported keeping their
phones in their bags; a planned contrast revealed no differ-
ence between these groups on our key dependent variable
(p5 .17), and they were pooled for all subsequent analyses.
Participants in all conditions were instructed to “turn your
phones completely on silent; this means turn off the ring
and vibrate so that your phone won’t make any sounds.”
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
After participants entered the testing room, they com-
pleted two tasks intended to measure available cognitive
capacity: the Automated Operation Span task (OSpan;
Unsworth et al. 2005) and a 10-item subset of Raven’s Stan-
dard Progressive Matrices (RSPM; Raven, Raven, and Court
1998). The OSpan task, a prominent measure of WMC, as-
sesses the ability to keep track of task-relevant information
while engaging in complex cognitive tasks. This particular
measure was designed to stress the domain-general nature
of the attentional resources at the heart of the WM system
(Turner and Engle 1989); in each trial set, participants
complete a series of math problems (information process-
ing) while simultaneously updating and remembering a
randomly generated letter sequence (information mainte-
nance). Performance on the OSpan assesses the domain-
general attentional resources “available to the individual
on a moment-to-moment basis” (Engle et al. 1992). The
RSPM test, a nonverbal measure of Gf, was developed to
isolate individuals’ capacity for understanding and solv-
ing novel problems (fluid intelligence), independent of any
influence of accumulated knowledge or domain-specific skill
(crystallized intelligence). In each trial, participants are
shown an incomplete pattern matrix and asked to select
the element that best completes the pattern. Much like the
OSpan task, performance on the RSPM test is sensitive to
the current availability of attentional resources (e.g., Mani
et al. 2013). Complete details of the tasks and measures
used in experiments 1 and 2 are provided in the appendix.
Participants also completed an exploratory test of the
“ending-digit drop-off” effect, modeled after the procedure
of Bizer and Schindler (2005). In this task, participants are
shown a series of products with .99-ending and .00-ending
prices and asked to report the quantity they would be able
to purchase for $73. Overestimating purchasing power for
a .99-ending price relative to a matched .00-ending price
(e.g., $3.99 vs. $4.00) constitutes evidence of the drop-off
effect. We thought this effect might be more pronounced
for those whose phones were made salient. However, we
failed to replicate the basic effect and did not find any
evidence of ending-digit drop-off in any condition (F(1,
514)5 .20, p5 .65). See the appendix for detailed analyses
and results.
Next, participants completed a questionnaire that in-
cluded items related to their experiences in the lab and their
lay beliefs about the connection between smartphones
and performance. These questions assessed how often they
thought about their phones during the experiment, to what
extent they thought the locations of their phones affected
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 145
their performance in the lab, how they thought phone loca-
tion might have affected their performance, and to what
extent they believed their phones affected their perfor-
mance and attention spans more generally; all responses
were measured using 7-point Likert scales. Finally, partici-
pants answered a series of demographic questions (gender,
age, ethnicity, nationality) and provided information about
their cellphone make/model and data plan.
Results and Discussion
All analyses in experiment 1 include a “Week” factor to ac-
count for variation across research assistants; this factor
does not interact with Phone Location in any analysis (all
F < 1.27, all p > .28).
Cognitive Capacity.We assessed the effects of smartphone
salience on available cognitive capacity using two measures
of domain-general cognitive function: OSpan task perfor-
mance and RSPM test score. Because both tasks rely on
limited-capacity attentional resources, both should be sen-
sitive to fluctuations in the availability of these resources.
A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) testing
the effects of Phone Location (desk, pocket/bag, other
room) on the optimal linear combination of these measures
revealed a significant effect of Phone Location on cognitive
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
capacity (Pillai’s Trace5 .027, F(4, 1028)5 3.51, p5 .007,
partial h2 5 .014). Paired comparisons revealed that partic-
ipants in the “other room” condition performed better than
those in the “desk” condition (p5 .002). Participants in the
“pocket/bag” condition did not perform significantly differ-
ently from those in either the “desk” (p 5 .09) or “other
room” (p 5 .11) conditions. However, planned contrasts
revealed a significant desk→ pocket/bag→ other room lin-
ear trend (Pillai’s Trace5 .023, F(2, 513)5 6.07, p5 .002,
partial h2 5 .023) and no quadratic trend (Pillai’s Trace 5
.004, F(2, 513) 5 .96, p 5 .39), suggesting that as smart-
phone salience increases, available cognitive capacity de-
creases.
Follow-up univariate ANOVAs separately testing the ef-
fect of Phone Location on OSpan performance and RSPM
score were consistent with our focal multivariate analysis.
Phone Location significantly affected both OSpan perfor-
mance (F(2, 514) 5 3.74, p 5 .02, partial h2 5 .014) and
RSPM score (F(2, 514) 5 3.96, p 5 .02, partial h2 5
.015). See figure 1 for means, and the appendix for detailed
analyses and results.
Conscious Thought. A one-way ANOVA on participants’
responses to the question “While completing today’s tasks,
how often were you thinking about your cellphone?” (1 5
Figure 1. Experiment 1: effect of randomly assigned phone
location condition on available WMC (OSpan Score, panel A)
and functional Gf
(Correctly Solved Raven’s Matrices, panel B). Participants in
the “desk” condition (high salience) displayed the lowest
available cognitive
capacity; those in the “other room” condition (low salience)
displayed the highest available cognitive capacity. Error bars
represent standard
errors of the means. Asterisks indicate significant differences
between conditions, with *p < .05 and **p < .01.
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
146 Brain Drain Ward et al.
not at all to 7 5 constantly/the whole time) revealed no
effect of Phone Location on phone-related thoughts (F(2,
514) 5 .84, p 5 .43). Notably, the modal self-reported fre-
quency of thinking about one’s phone in each condition was
“not at all.” Combined with the significant effect of Phone
Location on available cognitive capacity, these results sup-
port our proposition that the mere presence of one’s smart-
phone may impair cognitive functioning even when it does
not occupy the contents of consciousness.
Perceived Influence of Smartphone Presence. There were
no differences between conditions on any measures related
to the perceived effects of smartphones on performance
(“Howmuch / in what way do you think the position of your
cellphone affected your performance on today’s tasks?”; “In
general, how much do you think your cellphone usually
affects your performance and attention span?”), either in
the context of the experiment (all F < 1.58, all p > .21) or
in general (F(2, 494) 5 2.26, p 5 .11). Across conditions,
amajority of participants indicated that the location of their
phones during the experiment did not affect their perfor-
mance (“not at all”; 75.9%) and “neither helped nor hurt
[their] performance” (85.6%). This contrast between per-
ceived influence and actual performance suggests that par-
ticipants failed to anticipate or acknowledge the cognitive
consequences associated with the mere presence of their
phones.
Discussion. The results of experiment 1 indicate that the
mere presence of participants’ own smartphones impaired
their performance on tasks that are sensitive to the avail-
ability of limited-capacity attentional resources. In contrast
to prior research, participants in our experiment did not in-
teract with or receive notifications from their phones. In
addition, self-reported frequency of thoughts about these
devices did not differ across conditions. Taken together,
these results suggest that the mere presence of one’s smart-
phone may reduce available cognitive capacity and impair
cognitive functioning, even when consumers are successful
at remaining focused on the task at hand.
EXPERIMENT 2: SMARTPHONE DEPENDENCE
MODERATES THE EFFECT OF SMARTPHONE
SALIENCE ON COGNITIVE CAPACITY
The results of experiment 1 support the proposition that
the mere presence of one’s smartphone reduces available
cognitive capacity, even when it is not in use. In experi-
ment 2, we replicate the basic design of experiment 1, with
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
the following exceptions. First, we conduct a stronger test
of the proposed impairment-without-interruption effect
by examining the effects of smartphone salience on both
cognitive capacity (WMC) and a behavioral measure of sus-
tained attention. Consistent with both the proposed theo-
retical framework and participants’ self-reports in experi-
ment 1, we predict that increasing smartphone salience
will adversely affect the availability of attentional resources
without interrupting sustained attention. Second, one could
argue that participants who had access to their phones in
experiment 1 surreptitiously checked for notifications, were
consciously distracted by unanswerable messages, and dis-
played impaired performance as a result (as in Clayton et al.
2015; Stothart et al. 2015; Isikman et al. 2016). We did
not observe any behavior or this sort, and did not find any
differences between conditions in the frequency of phone-
related thoughts. In experiment 2, we further address this
alternate explanation by randomly assigning participants
to either silence their phones (as in experiment 1) or turn
them off completely. We predict that the salience of partic-
ipants’ smartphones will influence available cognitive ca-
pacity even when these devices are turned off and will not
influence sustained attention even when they are turned
on. Third, we test a potential moderator of the effects of
smartphone salience on available cognitive capacity: indi-
vidual differences in the personal relevance of one’s phone,
operationalized in terms of “smartphone dependence.” We
predict that individuals who are more dependent on their
phones will be more affected by their presence.
Method
Participants. Two hundred and ninety-six undergraduates
(56.9% female;Mage 5 21.3 years; SDage 5 2.6 years) partic-
ipated for course credit. Eleven participants were excluded
for reporting that they did not own smartphones, four par-
ticipants were excluded due to excessive error rates (<85%
OSpan accuracy), and six participants were excluded due to
missing (5) or extreme (1) response times in a Go/No-Go task
(see below). Our final sample consisted of 275 participants.
Procedure. This experiment followed a 3 (Phone Location:
desk, pocket/bag, other room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off )
between-subjects design. Phone Location instructions and
randomization procedures were identical to those used in
experiment 1, with the exception that participants in the
“desk” condition were instructed to place their phones fac-
ing up. Of the 91 participants in the “pocket/bag” condi-
tion, 68 (74.7%) reported keeping their phones in their
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
3. Two items did not clearly load onto either primary factor and
were
excluded from further analyses (Costello and Osborne 2005).
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 147
pockets, and 23 (25.3%) reported keeping their phones in
their bags; as in experiment 1, a planned contrast revealed
no difference between these groups on our key dependent
variable (p 5 .55), and they were pooled for all subsequent
analyses. Participants were randomly assigned to one of
two Phone Power conditions prior to entering the testing
room. The instructions for participants in the “power on”
condition were identical to those used in experiment 1;
we instructed participants in the “power off” condition to
completely turn off their devices.
After placing their phones in the proper location and
power mode, participants completed our two key depen-
dent measures: the OSpan task and the Cue-Dependent
Go/No-Go task (order counterbalanced across partici-
pants). In the Go/No-Go task, participants are presented
with a series of “go” and “no go” targets, and instructed
to respond to “go” targets as quickly as possible without
making errors, but to refrain from responding to “no go”
targets. In this task, both omission errors (failure to re-
spond to “go” targets) and reaction time (speed of respond-
ing to these targets) serve as measures of sustained atten-
tion (Bezdjian et al. 2009). After completing both tasks,
participants reported the subjective difficulty of each task.
Next, they completed a battery of exploratory questions
intended to assess individual differences in use of and con-
nection to one’s smartphone, including a 13-item inventory
related to reliance on one’s phone (see appendix for all
items and analyses). Finally, participants answered a series
of demographic questions (gender, age, ethnicity, national-
ity) and provided information about their cellphone make/
model and data plan.
Results and Discussion
All analyses in experiment 2 include a Task Order reflecting
our counterbalanced experimental design; this factor does
not interact with Phone Location or Phone Location �
Phone Power in any analysis (all F < 1.51, all p > .22).
Cognitive Capacity. As in experiment 1, performance on
the OSpan task measures the attentional resources avail-
able to the individual on a moment-to-moment basis (Engle
et al. 1992). A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other
room)� 2 (Phone Power: on, off) between-subjects ANOVA
revealed a significant effect of Location on OSpan perfor-
mance (F(2, 263)5 3.53, p5 .03, partial h2 5 .026). There
was no effect of Power (F(1, 263) 5 .05, p 5 .83) or of the
Power � Location interaction (F(2, 263) 5 1.05, p 5 .35).
Paired comparisons revealed that participants in the “other
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
room” condition performed significantly better on theOSpan
task than did those in the “desk” condition (Mdiff5 4.67, p5
.008). Participants in the “pocket/bag” condition did not per-
form significantly differently from those in either the “desk”
(Mdiff5 2.30, p5 .20) or “other room” (Mdiff5 2.37, p5 .17)
conditions. See figure 2 for means.
Planned contrasts revealed a significant desk → pocket/
bag→ other room linear trend (F(1, 263)5 7.05, p5 .008,
partial h2 5 .026) and no quadratic trend (F(1, 263)5 .001,
p 5 .98). Consistent with experiment 1, this pattern of re-
sults indicates that increasing the salience of one’s smart-
phone impairs OSpan performance, and decreasing the sa-
lience of one’s smartphone improves performance. Further,
the null effects of Power and the Power � Location in-
teraction suggest that decreases in performance are not re-
lated to incoming notifications (or the possibility of receiv-
ing notifications), ruling out this alternative explanation
of the effects found in experiment 1.
Moderation by Smartphone Dependence. Our framework
suggests that the effects of smartphone salience on avail-
able cognitive capacity should be moderated by individual
differences in dependence on these devices. We tested this
prediction by investigating responses to an exploratory
13-item inventory of individual differences in reliance on
one’s phone. A principal components factor analysis with
Varimax rotation revealed that these items loaded onto
two distinct factors, together explaining 52.67% of the var-
iance.3 Factor 1 (Smartphone Dependence; six items) ex-
plained 31.02% of the variance and captured our primary
concept of interest: the degree of dependence on one’s smart-
phone (e.g., “I would have trouble getting through a normal
day without my cellphone”). Factor 2 (Emotional Attach-
ment; five items) explained 21.65% of the variance and ac-
counted for the emotional aspects of smartphone use (e.g.,
“Usingmy cellphonemakesme feel happy”). Reliability anal-
yses indicated high reliability for both Smartphone Depen-
dence (a5 .89) and Emotional Attachment (a5 .79) as dis-
tinct factors. See appendix table 1 for all items and factor
loadings.
We tested the potential moderating role of Smartphone
Dependence in a univariate generalized linear model pre-
dicting OSpan performance from all variables included in
our original 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room)
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
148 Brain Drain Ward et al.
� 2 (Phone Power: on, off) ANOVA, mean-centered Smart-
phone Dependence score, and all independent variable �
SmartphoneDependence interaction terms (Baron andKenny
1986). This analysis revealed a significant Phone Location �
Smartphone Dependence interaction (F(2, 247) 5 3.25,
p 5 .04, partial h2 5 .026), indicating that the effects of
smartphone salience on OSpan performance are moder-
ated by individual differences in dependence on one’s smart-
phone. Follow-up analyses probing the conditional effects of
Location at the sample mean of the moderator and plus/
minus one SD from the mean revealed no effect of Location
on OSpan performance at low levels of Smartphone Depen-
dence (21 SD; p 5 .28); however, this effect was significant
at both mean (p5 .05) and high levels (p5 .007) of Depen-
dence. See figure 3 for estimated marginal means. Similar re-
sults for other measures of smartphone dependence (e.g.,
number of texts sent per day) are reported in the appendix.
Interestingly, a parallel moderation analysis indicated
that Emotional Attachment did not moderate the effects
of Phone Location on OSpan performance (p 5 .61). Al-
though we are cautious about making strong claims based
on null effects and reiterate that these factors were derived
from an exploratory inventory, this disparity between Smart-
phone Dependence and Emotional Attachment suggests that
the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive ca-
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
pacity may be determined by the extent to which consumers
feel they need their phones, as opposed to howmuch they like
them. These results are consistent with the proposition that
the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive ca-
pacity stem from the singularly important role these devices
play in many consumers’ lives.
Sustained Attention. We analyzed the effects of smart-
phone salience on two behavioral measures of sustained
attention: omission errors and reaction time in the Go/
No-Go task. A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other
room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off) ANOVA revealed no ef-
fects of Location, Power, or their interaction on either of
these measures (all F < 1.05, all p > .35). See figure 2 for
reaction time means, and the appendix for full results.
Perceived Difficulty. Finally, we analyzed perceived task
difficulty in order to see if the cognitive consequences of
smartphone salience were reflected in participants’ subjec-
tive experiences. A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag,
other room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off ) ANOVA revealed
a marginal effect of Location on perceived difficulty for
the memory section of the OSpan task (F(1, 256) 5 2.38,
p 5 .09, partial h2 5 .018). Paired comparisons revealed
that participants in the “other room” condition found it sig-
Figure 2. Experiment 2: effect of randomly assigned phone
location condition on available cognitive capacity (OSpan
Score) and sustained
attention (Mean Reaction Time, Go/No-Go). Participants in the
“desk” condition (high salience) displayed the lowest available
cognitive
capacity; those in the “other room” condition ( low salience)
displayed the highest available cognitive capacity. Phone
location did not affect
sustained attention. Error bars represent standard errors of the
means. Asterisks indicate significant differences between
conditions, with
**p < .01.
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 149
nificantly easier to remember information in this task rel-
ative to participants in the “desk” condition (Mdiff 5 .49,
p 5 .04) and marginally easier relative to those in the
“pocket/bag” condition (Mdiff 5 .40, p 5 .09). This pattern
of results is consistent with participants’ actual perfor-
mance on the OSpan task and suggests that the cognitive
benefits of escaping the mere presence of one’s phone
may be reflected, at least partially, in subjective experience.
However, the lay beliefs reported in experiment 1 suggest
that even when consumers notice these benefits, they
may not attribute them to the presence (or absence) of
their phones. There were no differences between condi-
tions on any of the other perceived difficulty or perceived
performance measures (all F < 1.82, all p > .16).
Discussion. Consistent with the behavioral and self-report
results observed in experiment 1, the results of experiment 2
suggest that the mere presence of consumers’ own smart-
phones may adversely affect cognitive functioning even
when consumers are not consciously attending to them. Ex-
periment 2 also provides evidence that these cognitive costs
are moderated by individual differences in dependence on
these devices. Ironically, the more consumers depend on
their smartphones, the more they seem to suffer from their
presence—or, more optimistically, themore theymay stand
to benefit from their absence.
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The proliferation of smartphones represents a profound
shift in the relationship between consumers and technol-
ogy. Across human history, the vast majority of innovations
have occupied a defined space in consumers’ lives; they
have been constrained by the functions they perform and
the locations they inhabit. Smartphones transcend these
limitations. They are consumers’ constant companions, of-
fering unprecedented connection to information, enter-
tainment, and each other. They play an integral role in
the lives of billions of consumers worldwide and, as a re-
sult, have vast potential to influence consumer welfare—
both for better and for worse.
The present research identifies a potentially costly side
effect of the integration of smartphones into daily life:
smartphone-induced “brain drain.” We provide evidence
that the mere presence of consumers’ smartphones can ad-
versely affect two measures of cognitive capacity—available
working memory capacity and functional fluid intelligence—
without interrupting sustained attention or increasing the
frequency of phone-related thoughts. Consumers who were
engaged with ongoing cognitive tasks were able to keep their
phones not just out of their hands, but also out of their (con-
scious) minds; however, the mere presence of these devices
left fewer attentional resources available for engaging with
the task at hand.
Figure 3. Experiment 2: estimated marginal means representing
the effect of phone location on available cognitive capacity
(OSpan Score)
at low (21 SD), mean, and high (11 SD) levels of smartphone
dependence. Phone location affects available cognitive capacity
at mean and
high levels of smartphone dependence, but not at low levels of
smartphone dependence. Asterisks indicate significant
differences between
conditions, with *p < .05 and **p < .01.
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
150 Brain Drain Ward et al.
Further, we find that the effects of smartphone salience
on available cognitive capacity are moderated by individual
differences in the personal relevance of these devices (op-
erationalized in terms of smartphone dependence); those
who depend most on their devices suffer the most from
their salience, and benefit the most from their absence.
The role of dependence in determining mere presence ef-
fects suggests that similar cognitive costs would not be in-
curred by the presence of just any product, device, or even
phone. We submit that few, if any, stimuli are both so per-
sonally relevant and so perpetually present as consumers’
own smartphones. However, we leave open the door for
our insights to apply more broadly to future connective
technologies that may become equally central to consum-
ers’ lives as technology continues to advance.
Our research also offers insight into the tactics that
might mitigate “brain drain”—as well as those that might
not. For example, we find that the effect of smartphone sa-
lience on cognitive capacity is robust to both the visibility
of the phone’s screen (face down in experiment 1, face up
in experiment 2) and the phone’s power (silent vs. powered
off in experiment 2), suggesting that intuitive “fixes” such
as placing one’s phone face down or turning it off are likely
futile. However, our data suggest at least one simple solu-
tion: separation. Although this approach may seem at odds
with prior research indicating that being separated from
one’s phone undermines performance by increasing anxi-
ety (Cheever et al. 2014; Clayton et al. 2015), we note that
participants in those studies were unexpectedly separated
from their phones (Cheever et al. 2014) and forced to hear
them ring while being unable to answer (Clayton et al.
2015). In contrast, participants in our experiments ex-
pected to be separated from their phones (this was the
norm in the lab) and were not confronted with unanswer-
able notifications or calls while separated. We therefore
suggest that defined and protected periods of separation,
such as these, may allow consumers to perform better not
just by reducing interruptions but also by increasing avail-
able cognitive capacity.
Our theoretical framework draws on prior research out-
lining the role of limited-capacity attentional resources
in inhibiting responses to high-priority but task-irrelevant
stimuli (Shallice 1972; Bargh 1982; Lavie et al. 2004; Clapp
et al. 2009). However, our data are equally consistent with
an alternate explanation: that these attentional resources
are recruited for purposes of hypervigilance, or monitoring
high-priority stimuli in the absence of conscious awareness
(e.g., Legrain et al. 2011; Jacob, Jacobs, and Silvanto 2015).
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
This interpretation is consistent with the common phe-
nomenon of “phantom vibration syndrome,” or the feeling
that one’s phone is vibrating when it actually is not (e.g.,
Rothberg et al. 2010; Deb 2015). Data suggest that 89%
of mobile phone users experience phantom vibrations at
least occasionally (Drouin, Kaiser, and Miller 2012), and
that this over-responsiveness to innocuous sensations is
particularly prevalent in those whose devices are particu-
larly meaningful (e.g., Rothberg et al. 2010). Because the
same limited-capacity attentional resources are implicated
in both hypervigilance and inhibition, our data cannot dis-
tinguish between the two theoretical explanations. In fact,
it is plausible that these processes may operate in tandem,
as goal-directed attentional control processes both monitor
for signals of potentially important information from high-
priority stimuli, and (attempt to) prevent these stimuli
from interrupting conscious attention until such signals
appear.
Implications and Future Directions
Consumers’ limited cognitive resources shape innumerable
aspects of their daily lives, from their approaches to deci-
sions (Bettman et al. 1991) to their enjoyment of experi-
ences (Weber et al. 2009). Our data suggest that the mere
presence of consumers’ own smartphones may further con-
strain their already limited cognitive capacity by taxing the
attentional resources that reside at the core of both work-
ing memory capacity and fluid intelligence. The specific
cognitive capacity measures used in our experiments are
associated with domain-general capabilities that support
fundamental processes such as learning, logical reasoning,
abstract thought, problem solving, and creativity (e.g.,
Cattell 1987; Kane et al. 2004). Because consumers’ smart-
phones are so frequently present, the mere presence effects
observed in our experiments have the potential to influence
consumer welfare across a wide range of contexts: when
consumers work, shop, take classes, watch movies, dine
with friends, attend concerts, play games, receive massages,
read books, and more (Isikman et al. 2016). Moreover, re-
sults from our pilot study (reported prior to experiment 1)
indicate that the majority of consumers typically keep their
smartphones nearby and in sight, where smartphone sa-
lience is particularly high.
Consumer Choice. Prior research indicates that occupying
cognitive resources by increasing cognitive load causes con-
sumers to rely less on analytic and deliberative “system 2”
processing, and more on intuitive and heuristic-based “sys-
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
Volume 2 Number 2 2017 151
tem 1” approaches (Evans 2008). To the extent that both
cognitive load and the mere presence of consumers’ smart-
phones reduce available cognitive capacity, we may expect
consumers to be more likely to adopt choice strategies asso-
ciated with system 1 when their smartphones are present
but irrelevant to the choice task. Reliance on system 1 pro-
cessing could, for example, enhance the appeal of affect-rich
choice alternatives (Rottenstreich, Sood, and Brenner 2007),
amplify the preference for simple (and possibly inferior) so-
lutions (Drolet, Luce, and Simonson 2009), increase consum-
ers’willingness to make attribute trade-offs (Drolet and Luce
2004), and heighten susceptibility to anchoring effects (Deck
and Jahedi 2015). Building on these connections, future re-
search could explore whether the presence of smartphones
accentuates individuals’ preference for options favored by
system 1 processing.
Advertising Effectiveness. The availability of cognitive re-
sources also predicts elaboration likelihood (e.g., Petty and
Cacioppo 1986) and susceptibility to deceptive advertising
(Xie and Boush 2011). Consistent with a potential shift to-
ward reliance on system 1 processing, consumers who view
advertising messages in the presence of their smartphones
may be less likely to elaborate on advertising messages and
more likely to be influenced by heuristics such as likability of
the communicator (e.g., Chaiken 1980). Note that the pro-
posed theoretical framework suggests that this may not be
the case for advertising delivered via smartphone, because
the cognitive costs associated with mere presence should be
incurred when consumers’ phones are present but not in use.
Education. Younger adults—92% of whom are smartphone
owners—rely heavily on smartphones (Pew Research Center
2016). Given that many of them are in school, the potential
detrimental effects of smartphones on their cognitive func-
tioning may have an outsized effect on long-term welfare.
As educational institutions increasingly embrace “connected
classrooms” (e.g., Petrina 2007), the presence of students’
mobile devices in educational environments may undermine
both learning and test performance—particularly when these
devices are present but not in use. Future research could fo-
cus onhow children, adolescents, and young adults are affected
by the mere presence of personally relevant technologies in
the classroom.
Intentional Disconnection. Althoughwe have primarily fo-
cused on the cognitive costs associated with the presence of
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
smartphones, our research is equally relevant to the poten-
tial implications of their absence. Discussions of “disconnec-
tion” in popular culture reflect increasing consumer interest
in intentionally reducing—or at least controlling—the ex-
tent to which they interact with their devices (e.g., Perlow
2012; Harmon and Mazmanian 2013). Some consumers
are replacing their smartphones with feature phones (i.e.,
phones lacking the advanced functionality of smartphones;
Thomas 2016), others are supplementing their smart-
phones with stripped-down devices that offer “a short break
from connectedness” (http://www.thelightphone.com/), and
still others are turning to apps that track, filter, and limit
smartphone usage (e.g., https://inthemoment.io/). Our re-
search suggests that thesemeasures may be doubly beneficial
for the digitally weary; by redefining the relevance of their de-
vices, these consumers may both reduce digital distraction
and increase available cognitive capacity. More broadly, our
research contributes to the growing discussion among con-
sumers and marketers alike about the influence of technol-
ogy on consumers—and consumers on technology—in an
increasingly connected world.
One’s smartphone is more than just a phone, a camera,
or a collection of apps. It is the one thing that connects ev-
erything—the hub of the connected world. The presence of
one’s smartphone enables on-demand access to informa-
tion, entertainment, social stimulation, and more. However,
our research suggests that these benefits—and the depen-
dence they engender—may come at a cognitive cost.
REFERENCES
Andrews, Sally, David A. Ellis, Heather Shaw, and Lukasz
Piwek (2015),
“BeyondSelf-Report: Tools toCompareEstimated andReal-World
Smart-
phone Use,” PLoS One, 10 (10), 1–9.
Baddeley, Alan D. (2003), “Working Memory: Looking Back
and Looking
Forward,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4 (10), 829–39.
Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch (1974), “Working
Memory,” Psychol-
ogy of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89.
Bargh, John A. (1982), “Attention and Automaticity in the
Processing of
Self-Relevant Information,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
43 (3), 425–36.
Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny (1986), “The
Moderator–Mediator
Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research:
Conceptual, Stra-
tegic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality
and Social
Psychology, 51 (6), 1173–82.
Benjamin, Daniel, Sebastian Brown, and Jesse Shapiro (2013),
“Who Is ‘Be-
havioral’? Cognitive Ability and Anomalous Preferences,”
Journal of the
European Economic Association, 11 (6), 1231–55.
Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory of
Consumer
Choice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Bettman, James R., Eric J. Johnson, and John W. Payne (1991),
“Consumer
DecisionMaking,” inHandbook of Consumer Behavior, ed.
Thomas S. Rob-
ertson and Harold H. Kassarjian, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM
and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
152 Brain Drain Ward et al.
Bezdjian, Serena, Laura A. Baker, Dora Isabel Lozano, and
Adrian Raine
(2009), “Assessing Inattention and Impulsivity in Children
during the
Go/NoGo Task,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
27 (2),
365–83.
Bizer, George Y., and Robert M. Schindler (2005), “Direct
Evidence of
Ending-Digit Drop-off in Price Information Processing,”
Psychology
and Marketing, 22 (10), 771–83.
Bowman, Laura L., Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, and
Michael Gendron
(2010), “Can Students Really Multitask? An Experimental Study
of In-
stant Messaging while Reading,” Computers and Education, 54
(4), 927–
31.
Caird, Jeff K., Chelsea R. Willness, Piers Steel, and Chip
Scialfa (2008), “A
Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Cell Phones on Driver
Performance,”
Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40 (4), 1282–93.
Cattell, Raymond B. (1987), Intelligence: Its Structure, Growth
and Action,
New York: Elsevier.
Chaiken, Shelly (1980), “Heuristic versus Systematic
Information Process-
ing and the Use of Source versus Message Cues in Persuasion,”
Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (5), 752–66.
Cheever, Nancy A., Larry D. Rosen, L. Mark Carrier, and
Amber Chavez
(2014), “Out of Sight Is Not Out of Mind: The Impact of
Restricting
Wireless Mobile Device Use on Anxiety Levels among Low,
Moderate
and High Users,” Computers in Human Behavior, 37 (August),
290–97.
Clapp, Wesley C., and Adam Gazzaley (2012), “Distinct
Mechanisms for
the Impact of Distraction and Interruption on Working Memory
in
Aging,” Neurobiology of Aging, 33 (1), 134–48.
Clapp, Wesley C., Michael T. Rubens, and Adam Gazzaley
(2009), “Mech-
anisms of Working Memory Disruption by External
Interference,” Ce-
rebral Cortex, 20 (4), 859–72.
Clayton, Russell B., Glenn Leshner, and Anthony Almond
(2015), “The Ex-
tended iSelf: The Impact of iPhone Separation on Cognition,
Emotion,
and Physiology,” Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 20 (2),
119–35.
Corbetta, Maurizio, and Gordon L. Shulman (2002), “Control of
Goal-
Directed and Stimulus-Driven Attention in the Brain,” Nature
Reviews
Neuroscience, 3 (3), 201–15.
Costello, Anna B., and Jason W. Osborne (2005), “Best
Practices in Ex-
ploratory Factor Analysis: Four Recommendations for Getting
the
Most from Your Analysis,” Practical Assessment, Research, and
Evalua-
tion, 10 (7), 1–9.
Craik, Fergus I. M., and Robert S. Lockhart (1972), “Levels of
Processing: A
Framework for Memory Research,” Journal of Verbal Learning
and Ver-
bal Behavior, 11 (6), 671–84.
Deb, Amrita (2015), “Phantom Vibration and Phantom Ringing
among
Mobile Phone Users: A Systematic Review of Literature,” Asia-
Pacific
Psychiatry, 7 (3), 231–39.
Deck, Cary, and Salar Jahedi (2015), “The Effect of Cognitive
Load on Eco-
nomics Decision Making: A Survey and New Experiments,”
European
Economic Review, 78 (C), 97–119.
Deutsche Telekom AG (2012), “Smart Payments—How the Cell
Phone
Becomes a Wallet,” research report, http://www.studie-
life.de/en/life
-reports/smart-payments/.
Drolet, Aimee, and Mary Frances Luce (2004), “The
Rationalizing Effects
of Cognitive Load on Emotion-Based Trade-off Avoidance,”
Journal of
Consumer Research, 31 (1), 63–77.
Drolet, Aimee, Mary Frances Luce, and Itamar Simonson
(2009), “When
Does Choice Reveal Preference? Moderators of Heuristic versus
Goal-
Based Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (1), 137–47.
This content downloaded from 075.18
All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms
Drouin, Michelle, Daren H. Kaiser, and Daniel A. Miller
(2012), “Phantom
Vibrations among Undergraduates: Prevalence and Associated
Psycho-
logical Characteristics,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28 (4),
1490–96.
dscout (2016), “Mobile Touches: dscout’s Inaugural Study on
Humans and
Their Tech,” research report,
https://blog.dscout.com/hubfs/downloads
/dscout_mobile_touches_study_2016.pdf.
End, Christian M., Shaye Worthman, Mary Bridget Mathews,
and Katharina
Wetterau (2009), “Costly Cell Phones: The Impact of Cell
Phone Rings
on Academic Performance,” Teaching of Psychology, 37 (1),
55–57.
Engle, Randall W. (2002), “Working Memory Capacity as
Executive Atten-
tion,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11 (1), 19–
23.
Engle, RandallW., Judy Cantor, and Julie J. Carullo (1992),
“Individual Dif-
ferences in Working Memory and Comprehension: A Test of
Four Hy-
potheses,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and
Cognition, 18 (5), 972–92.
Engle, Randall W., Stephen W. Tuholski, James E. Laughlin,
and Andrew
R. A. Conway (1999), “WorkingMemory, Short-TermMemory,
and Gen-
eral Fluid Intelligence: A Latent-Variable Approach,” Journal
of Exper-
imental Psychology: General, 128 (3), 309–31.
Evans, Jonathan St. B. T. (2008), “Dual-Processing Accounts of
Reasoning,
Judgment, and Social Cognition,” Annual Review of
Psychology, 59, 255–
78.
Fecteau, Jillian H., and Douglas P. Munoz (2006), “Salience,
Relevance,
and Firing: A Priority Map for Target Selection,” Trends in
Cognitive Sci-
ences, 10 (8), 382–90.
Fox, Annie Beth, Jonathan Rosen, andMary Crawford (2009),
“Distractions,
Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students’
Perfor-
mance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?”
CyberPsychology
and Behavior, 12 (1), 51–53.
Froese, Arnold D., Christina N. Carpenter, Denyse A. Inman,
Jessica R.
Schooley, Rebecca B. Barnes, Paul W. Brecht, and Jasmin D.
Chacon
(2012), “Effects of Classroom Cell Phone Use on Expected and
Actual
Learning,” College Student Journal, 46 (2), 323–32.
Geller, Valerie, and Phillip Shaver (1976), “Cognitive
Consequences of Self-
Awareness,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12 (1),
99–108.
Halford, Graeme S., Nelson Cowan, and Glenda Andrews
(2007), “Separat-
ing Cognitive Capacity from Knowledge: A New Hypothesis,”
Trends in
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx
ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx

More Related Content

Similar to ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx

Decline of interpersonal communication
Decline of interpersonal communicationDecline of interpersonal communication
Decline of interpersonal communicationNaeemMushtaq4
 
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent children
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent childrenDoes social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent children
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent childrenCormac O Connor
 
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...Sajid Ali Khan
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Myunggoon Choi
 
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-Lewy
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-LewyTexting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-Lewy
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-LewyBibi Loizzo
 
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)D912f5118a6142d37f (1)
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)Maliha Gul
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Myunggoon Choi
 
Social Media Research
Social Media ResearchSocial Media Research
Social Media ResearchLuke Willson
 
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docxRunning Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docx
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docxHuman Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docx
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docxwellesleyterresa
 
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPER
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPERMobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPER
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPERKelsey Harris
 
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300Primary Research Paper for PSY 300
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300Shelby Lazorka
 
Modern Media Technologies
Modern Media Technologies Modern Media Technologies
Modern Media Technologies JENNINGS YONGO
 
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomolecule
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomoleculeCommunication skill , cell and structures of biomolecule
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomoleculeBrianJuma5
 
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docx
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docxComputers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docx
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docxdonnajames55
 

Similar to ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx (20)

Decline of interpersonal communication
Decline of interpersonal communicationDecline of interpersonal communication
Decline of interpersonal communication
 
Mobile
MobileMobile
Mobile
 
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent children
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent childrenDoes social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent children
Does social media create a schism between parents and their adolescent children
 
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...
Sajid Ali Khan Psychosocial estimation-of-mobile-phone-usage-a-case-study-of-...
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
 
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-Lewy
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-LewyTexting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-Lewy
Texting and Anxiety by Bibi Loizzo and Naomi D. Chakofsky-Lewy
 
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)D912f5118a6142d37f (1)
D912f5118a6142d37f (1)
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
 
Social Media Research
Social Media ResearchSocial Media Research
Social Media Research
 
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docxRunning Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE                .docx
Running Head THE INFLUENCE OF MEDIA ON BODY IMAGE .docx
 
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docx
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docxHuman Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docx
Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989O R I G I N A L.docx
 
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPER
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPERMobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPER
Mobile Social Media in Social Internations FINAL PAPER
 
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
 
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
7. Laurisa Mae B. Tabotabo.pdf
 
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300Primary Research Paper for PSY 300
Primary Research Paper for PSY 300
 
Modern Media Technologies
Modern Media Technologies Modern Media Technologies
Modern Media Technologies
 
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomolecule
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomoleculeCommunication skill , cell and structures of biomolecule
Communication skill , cell and structures of biomolecule
 
مثال 6.pdf
مثال 6.pdfمثال 6.pdf
مثال 6.pdf
 
C427 Research Paper
C427 Research PaperC427 Research Paper
C427 Research Paper
 
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docx
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docxComputers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docx
Computers in Human Behavior 28 (2012) 2083–2090Contents list.docx
 

More from festockton

Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docx
Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docxLearning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docx
Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docxfestockton
 
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docx
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docxLeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docx
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docxfestockton
 
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docx
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docxLeadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docx
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docxfestockton
 
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docxLearning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docxfestockton
 
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docx
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docxLearning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docx
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docxfestockton
 
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docxLearning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docxfestockton
 
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docx
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docxLEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docx
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docxfestockton
 
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docx
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docxLeadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docx
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docxfestockton
 
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docx
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docxLeadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docx
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docxfestockton
 
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docx
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docxLean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docx
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docxfestockton
 
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docx
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docxLeadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docx
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docxfestockton
 
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docx
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docxLeadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docx
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docxfestockton
 
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docx
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docxLawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docx
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docxfestockton
 
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719 1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docx
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719  1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docxlaw43665_fm_i-xx i 010719  1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docx
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719 1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docxfestockton
 
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docxLeaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docxfestockton
 
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docx
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docxLast year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docx
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docxfestockton
 
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docxLeaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docxfestockton
 
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docx
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docxLeaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docx
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docxfestockton
 
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docx
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docxLaw enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docx
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docxfestockton
 
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docx
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docxLAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docx
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docxfestockton
 

More from festockton (20)

Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docx
Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docxLearning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docx
Learning ResourcesRequired ReadingsToseland, R. W., & Ri.docx
 
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docx
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docxLeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docx
LeamosEscribamos Completa el párrafo con las formas correctas de lo.docx
 
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docx
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docxLeadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docx
Leadership via vision is necessary for success. Discuss in detail .docx
 
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docxLearning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real.docx
 
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docx
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docxLearning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docx
Learning Accomplishment Profile-Diagnostic Spanish Language Edit.docx
 
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docxLearning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docx
Learning about Language by Observing and ListeningThe real voy.docx
 
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docx
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docxLEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docx
LEARNING OUTCOMES1. Have knowledge and understanding of the pri.docx
 
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docx
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docxLeadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docx
Leadership Style What do people do when they are leadingAssignme.docx
 
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docx
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docxLeadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docx
Leadership Throughout HistoryHistory is filled with tales of leade.docx
 
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docx
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docxLean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docx
Lean Inventory Management1. Why do you think lean inventory manage.docx
 
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docx
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docxLeadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docx
Leadership varies widely by culture and personality. An internationa.docx
 
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docx
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docxLeadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docx
Leadership is the ability to influence people toward the attainment .docx
 
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docx
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docxLawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docx
Lawday. Court of Brightwaltham holden on Monday next after Ascension.docx
 
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719 1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docx
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719  1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docxlaw43665_fm_i-xx i 010719  1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docx
law43665_fm_i-xx i 010719 1032 AMStakeholders, Eth.docx
 
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docxLeaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. There .docx
 
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docx
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docxLast year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docx
Last year Angelina Jolie had a double mastectomy because of re.docx
 
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docxLeaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docx
Leaders face many hurdles when leading in multiple countries. Ther.docx
 
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docx
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docxLeaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docx
Leaders today must be able to create a compelling vision for the org.docx
 
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docx
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docxLaw enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docx
Law enforcement professionals and investigators use digital fore.docx
 
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docx
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docxLAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docx
LAW and Economics 4 questionsLaw And EconomicsTextsCoote.docx
 

Recently uploaded

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 

Recently uploaded (20)

POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 

ArticleCan you connect withme now How thepresence of .docx

  • 1. Article Can you connect with me now? How the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation quality Andrew K. Przybylski Netta Weinstein University of Essex, UK Abstract Recent advancements in communication technology have enabled billions of people to connect over great distances using mobile phones, yet little is known about how the frequent presence of these devices in social settings influences face-to-face interactions. In two experiments, we evaluated the extent to which the mere presence of mobile communication devices shape relationship quality in dyadic settings. In both, we found evidence they can have negative effects on closeness, connection, and conversation quality. These results demonstrate that the presence of mobile phones can interfere with human relationships, an effect that is most clear when individuals are discussing person- ally meaningful topics.
  • 2. Keywords Closeness, connection, conversation quality, face-to-face interactions, mobile phones, relationship quality Corresponding author: Andrew K. Przybylski, Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, UK. Email: [email protected] Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3) 237–246 ª The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0265407512453827 spr.sagepub.com J S P R http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav http://spr.sagepub.com Recent advancements in communication technology have enabled billions of people in the developed and developing world to connect with others
  • 3. using mobile phones (Mieczakowski, Goldhaber, & Clarkson, 2011). The widespread availability and use of mobile phones mean that these devices are commonly present in public and private settings and during casual and intimate interactions, often as subtle background objects. Despite their ubiquity, it is not known how the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face interactions. The present paper empirically explores this issue for the first time and examines the effects of merely having a mobile phone present during in-person conversations. Psychological research on phone use broadly suggests that it is often aimed as a source of entertainment and a means for sociability (O’Keefe & Sulanowski, 1995), and indicates the use of phones is largely a way to feel closer with family members, to express care for others, and to be available to others (Leung & Wei, 2000). Despite the fact that people seem attracted to mobile phones as a means to interpersonal closeness,
  • 4. little psychological research to date has systematically investigated the actual influence these devices have in or outside the context of relationships. Instead, the thrust of research in this area has examined effects mobile communication technology has on attention. Specifically, this research indicates that use of mobile phones can reduce the quality of attention to real-world events such as operating motor vehicles (Strayer, Drews, & Johnston, 2003; Strayer & Johnston, 2001). Most research focusing on mobile phones and relationships suggests they have the potential to influence a range of interpersonal processes. Interviews reveal mobile phones provide a continual sense of connection to the wider social world—a feeling that persists even if a mobile is in ‘‘silent mode’’ (Plant, 2000). Indeed, the presence of phones is often felt during intimate social outings. As an example, Geser (2002) observes that a significant portion of couples eating together repeatedly interrupt their meals to
  • 5. check for text or voice messages. In reviewing a wide range of survey data, Srivastava (2005) concluded mobile phones might exert these pervasive influences because people associate phones with wide-ranging social networks. The presence of a mobile phone may orient individuals to thinking of other people and events outside their immediate social context. In doing so, they divert attention away from a presently occurring interpersonal experience to focus on a multitude of other concerns and interests. In line with this, a number of prominent theorists have argued that mobile commu- nication technology can have a decidedly negative influence on interpersonal relation- ships (Turkle, 2011). Turkle cites a wide range of qualitative evidence collected in interviews that phones can direct attention away from face-to- face conversations by making concerns about maintaining wider social networks salient. Taken together with quantitative research that demonstrates environmental cues can activate relational
  • 6. schema and affect behavior without a person’s awareness (Shah, 2003), there is reason to believe that merely having a phone at hand may degrade firsthand social interactions. Effects such as these would be apparent when partners attempt to engage one another in a meaningful way. Particularly when strangers are interacting, a superficial con- versation may do little to foster closeness and trust in the relationship. On the other hand, dyads engaged in a meaningful conversation may develop their relationship as they become better acquainted with one another and self-disclose relevant and personal 238 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3) information (Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, 1997). In these cases, the presence of a diverting influence such as a mobile phone may inhibit relationship formation by reducing individuals’ engagement and attention for their partners, and discouraging part- ners’ perceptions that any self-disclosure had been met with care and empathy. Indeed,
  • 7. such an impediment to relationship formation may be frustrating and isolating. To test this idea, we investigated the degree to which the presence of mobile communication technology influences the quality of human interactions across different kinds of conversations. Present studies In the present research, we evaluated the idea that the presence of mobile communication technology may present a barrier to human interactions, especially when people are having meaningful conversations. To investigate this, we conducted two experiments in which pairs of strangers engaged in a brief relationship formation task, adapted from previous research (Aron et al., 1997). We also manipulated the innocuous presence versus absence of a mobile phone in the laboratory room. The first experiment examined the general effects of mobile phone presence on relational processes, and the second experiment investigated these dynamics for people having
  • 8. casual and meaningful interactions. Experiment 1 Participants and procedure Seventy-four participants (26 women; M age ¼ 21.88, SD ¼ 4.81) were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: (a) phone absent or (b) phone present. For those assigned to the phone present condition, a nondescript mobile phone rested on a book, which was placed on a nearby desk outside participants’ direct visual field. In the phone absent condition, a pocket notebook replaced the phone (see Figure 1). We used a rela- tionship formation task adapted from previous research (Aron et al., 1997), which was meant to emulate the content of many real-life conversations. A pilot study indicated that the assigned conversation, ‘‘Discuss an interesting event that occurred to you over the past month,’’ was a moderately intimate topic. Participants left personal belongings in a common waiting area before being led to a private booth along with a randomly
  • 9. assigned partner. Dyads were then asked to spend 10 minutes discussing the topic together. Following this encounter, participants completed measures used in previous research to assess relationship quality over time (Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000) and emotional sensitivity in both committed and newly formed relationships (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). Funneled debriefing of participants in both experiments indicated mobile phone placement was unobtrusive. Measures Relationship quality. Relationship quality was measured using a seven-item version of the connectedness subscale of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (McAuley, Duncan, & Przybylski and Weinstein 239 Tammen, 1987), and included items such as ‘‘It is likely that my partner and I could become friends if we interacted a lot’’ (M ¼ 2.78, SD ¼ 0.79, a ¼ .85). Partner closeness. Closeness between participants was measured
  • 10. using the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (Aron et al., 1992), which instructed participants to select one of seven increasingly overlapping circle pairs representing themselves and their conversation partner. (In the present study, participants responding averaged: M ¼ 3.57, SD ¼ 1.46.) Covariate: Positive affect. Positive and negative affect was assessed using the nine-item Emmons Mood Indicator (Diener & Emmons, 1984) to account for the potential con- founding effect of overall positive mood on relational outcomes. Items included pleased, worried/anxious, and frustrated (a ¼ .83), paired with a seven- point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). Results Data analytic strategy. Analyses required accommodations for nesting persons within dyads (assuming nonindependence between the two interacting partners). Analyses were Figure 1. Overhead view of lab room. 1. Mobile phone or pocket notebook. 2. Book on desk. 3. Chairs used by participants. 240 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3)
  • 11. therefore conducted with hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Condition (1: phone present, �1: phone absent) was defined at level 2 (dyad level), while covariates (gender, age, and positive affect) were defined at level 1 (person level). Unconditional models were first assessed to determine whether sufficient var- iance existed between- and within-dyad. Intraclass correlation (ICC) derived from these models showed that across outcomes, 43% to 51% of the total variance occurred between persons. Given the substantial variance accounted for at each level, full models were tested. Across analyses, the general level 1 equation was as follows: OV ij ¼ Boj þ B1X 1ij þ B2X 2ij þ B3X 3ij þ eij where Boj reflects the average value of the relational outcome, B1 reflects the estimated population slope of gender, B2 reflects that of age, B3 reflects that of positive affect, and eij represents level 1 error. The level 2 equation was: Boj ¼ Goo þ G01X1j þ u0j where Goo reflects the person level intercept for an average
  • 12. person and G01 refers to the effect of the phone condition. As Raudenbush & Bryk (2002) recommended, level 1 variables were centered on individual rather than sample means; level 2 variables were not centered. Relationship quality. At level 1, positive affect related to better relationship quality, b ¼ 0.80, t(69) ¼ 2.99, p < .001, though there were no effects of either gender, b ¼�20, t(69) ¼�0.61, p ¼ .55, or age, b ¼�0.04, t(69) ¼�0.03, p ¼ .12. Controlling for these, individuals in the mobile phone condition reported lower relationship quality after the interaction, b ¼�0.99, t(35) ¼ 3.08, p ¼ .004. Partner closeness. Results for perceived closeness with one’s partner were largely consistent with relationship quality. None of the level 1 predictors related to perceived closeness with one’s partner, bs ¼ �0.00 to �.09, ts(69) ¼ �0.06 to �0.64, ps ¼ .52 to .95. Yet at level 2, those in the mobile phone condition reported less closeness with their partners, b ¼ �0.39, t(35) ¼�2.56, p ¼ .02. Experiment 2 Experiment 1 found that dyadic partners who got to know one
  • 13. another in the presence of a mobile phone (via sharing a moderately meaningful discussion) felt less close with their partners and reported a lower quality of relationships than did partners who shared a conversation without a mobile phone present. Experiment 2 explored which relational contexts mobile phones most mattered by manipulating the content of discussion to be either casual or meaningful. We hypothesized mobile phones would impede relational outcomes when partners are attempting to build an intimate connection and have less effect in casual conversation, where little self-disclosure takes place. In addition, this second experiment explored two new relational outcomes that have been shown as important Przybylski and Weinstein 241 indicators for building intimate relationships: interpersonal trust (Campbell, Simpson, Boldry, & Rubin, 2010) and perceived partner empathy (Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004).
  • 14. Participants and procedure Sixty-eight participants (43 women; M age ¼ 23.21, SD ¼ 4.99) were randomly assigned to one of the cells of a 2 (absent vs. present phone) � 2 (casual vs. meaningful con- versation) between-subjects design. A modified version of the 10-minute relationship formation task from Experiment 1 was used for all 34 dyads. Participants in the casual conversation condition were instructed to discuss their thoughts and feelings about plastic holiday trees (casual condition); those assigned to the important conversation condition discussed the most meaningful events of the past year (meaningful condition). Measures To examine a more diverse set of relational outcomes that included processes indicative of intimate relationships, relationship quality was assessed in the same way as in Experiment 1 (M ¼ 4.98, SD ¼ 1.09, a ¼ .86) but was paired with two additional measures of relational functioning: trust and empathy. Partner trust. Trust was assessed with an item asking
  • 15. participants to rate their agreement to the statement: ‘‘I felt like I could really trust my conversation partner’’ (M ¼ 3.25, SD ¼ 1.01). Partner empathy. Empathy was measured with the nine-item Empathic Concern Scale (Davis, 1995), which included items such as: ‘‘To what extent do you think your partner accurately understood your thoughts and feelings about the topic?’’ (M ¼ 4.98, SD ¼ 1.09, a ¼ .92). Results Analytic strategy. HLM analyses were conducted as in Experiment 1, although in the present study, phone manipulation (1: phone present, �1: phone absent) was interacted with conversation type (1: meaningful, �1: casual), both entered at level 2. Preliminary unconditional models showed 39% to 47% of variance was at the between-person level. Figure 2 presents the means of observed for relationship quality, partner trust, and part- ner empathy across conditions. Relationship quality. There were no effects of gender, age, or positive affect on relationship quality, bs ¼ 0.01 to 0.09, ts(60) ¼ 0.13 to 1.28, ps ¼ .21 to .90. At level 2 and consistent with Experiment 1, mobile phone presence predicted lower relationship quality,
  • 16. b ¼�0.19, t(30) ¼�2.29, p ¼ .03, and new to this study, results showed an interaction between mobile phone presence and conversation type, b ¼�0.26, t(30) ¼�3.02, p ¼ .006 (no effect of conversation type, b ¼ 0.03, t(30) ¼ 0.29, p ¼ .78). Simple slopes 242 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3) analyses showed no effect of phone when the conversation was casual, b ¼ 0.02, t(14) ¼ 0.50, p ¼ .63. On the other hand, the presence of the mobile phone predicted lower rela- tionship quality when the conversation was meaningful, b¼�0.45, t(16)¼�4.21, p¼ .001. Partner trust. None of the covariates (gender, age, or positive affect) related to trust, bs ¼ 0.02 to 0.13, ts(60) ¼ �0.13 to 1.50, ps ¼ .13 to .90. At level 2, phone presence predicted less trust between partners, b ¼ �0.36, t(30) ¼ �3.76, p < .001, and mean- ingful conversations marginally encouraged trust, b ¼ 0.21, t(30) ¼ 2.00, p ¼ .06. These main effects were qualified by their interaction, b ¼ �0.33, t(30) ¼ �3.48, p ¼ .002. Simple slopes analyses complemented those of relationship quality, indicating that when the conversation was casual the presence of a mobile phone had no effect on partners’ trust, b ¼ �0.07, t(14) ¼ �0.26, p ¼ .80, yet partners who attempted to share a
  • 17. meaningful conversation in the presence of a phone reported less trust than those who did so in its absence, b ¼�0.69, t(16) ¼�5.24, p < .001. Figure 2. This figure shows that the presence of a mobile phone in the laboratory room leads to lower levels of relationship quality, trust, and empathy. All critical t-tests are significant at p < .05. Error bars are based on standard error. Przybylski and Weinstein 243 Perceived empathy. A final series of analyses were conducted predicting perceived empathy from partners. Older participants reported perceiving marginally higher empathy from their partners, b ¼ 0.04, t(60) ¼ 1.92, p ¼ .06, though there was no relation with gender, b ¼ �0.08, t(60) ¼ �0.29, p ¼ .77, or positive affect, b ¼ 0.21, t(60) ¼ 1.55, p ¼ .13. At level 2, participants reported lower perceived empathy when the phone was present compared to absent, b ¼�0.37, t(30) ¼�3.60, p ¼ .002, yet no effect of conversation type, b ¼ 0.18, t(30) ¼ 1.49, p ¼ .15. These relations were qualified by an interaction, b ¼�0.36, t(30) ¼�3.41, p ¼ .002. As before, simple slopes analyses showed no differences in perceived empathy between the two phone conditions when the conversation was a casual one, b ¼�0.01, t(14) ¼�0.06, p ¼
  • 18. .95; however, when the conversation was meaningful the presence of a phone predicted less perceived empathy following the conversation, b ¼�0.72, t(16) ¼�4.66, p < .001. Conclusions These results demonstrated that the mere presence of mobile communication technology might interfere with human relationship formation, lending some empirical support to concerns voiced by theorists (Turkle, 2011). Evidence derived from both experiments indicates the mere presence of mobile phones inhibited the development of interpersonal closeness and trust, and reduced the extent to which individuals felt empathy and understanding from their partners. Results from the second experiment indicated that these effects were most pronounced if individuals were discussing a personally meaningful topic. More specifically, results of this experiment showed that meaningful conversation topics tended to encourage intimacy and trust under neutral conditions. This difference between those in the casual and meaningful
  • 19. conversation conditions was absent in the presence of a mobile phone, which appeared to interfere in conditions that were otherwise conducive to intimacy. More interesting, the debriefing procedure suggests that these effects might happen outside of conscious awareness. The present findings are qualified by a number of limitations that provide avenues for future research. The first and most important question this research leaves open concerns the mechanism through which a mobile phone impedes relationship formation. Given that the effects do not appear to depend on conscious awareness, it is possible that phones operate as a prime that activates implicit representations of wider social networks, which in turn crowd out face-to-face conversations. It is also possible that people form individ- ual and enduring implicit associations with phones; and such attitudes, behaviors, and cognitions interrupt here-and-now interactions. In both cases, these mechanisms could be examined in future research using sequential priming and or
  • 20. lexical decision tasks (e.g., Shah, 2003). In a similar vein, an expanding body of qualitative research (e.g., Srivastava, 2005) suggests people form important and diverse kinds of connections to their personal devices. Future work might replicate and extend the present studies by exploring how the presence of a range of personally owned communication devices such as portable or tablet computers shape conversations. In the present experiments, we examined mobile phones in the context of casual and meaningful conversations, but the presence of mobile phones may show a curvilinear relationship such that they do not obstruct relationship 244 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3) formation in deeply meaningful conversations; future research may explore these processes across conversation types. In addition, future research should test behavior directly to answer the question: do mobile phones change the
  • 21. way partners behave toward one another (e.g., via the amount of self-disclosure, nonverbal relational beha- viors, attentiveness), or whether they primarily shape subjective perceptions of conver- sations. Finally, these experiments explored the effects of mobile phone presence on relationships among heretofore strangers. Future research may focus on the extent to which mobile devices would have such effects in established relationships, such as those between business associates, parents and children, or romantic partners. All things considered, this research presents interesting empirical findings that inform the billions of conversations that take place daily, as many are accompanied by a phone placed casually on a table or bar. These results indicate that mobile communication devices such as phones may, by their mere presence, paradoxically hold the potential to facilitate as well as to disrupt human bonding and intimacy. Funding
  • 22. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. References Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612. Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363–377. Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J. G., & Rubin, H. (2010). Trust, variability in relationship evaluations, and relationship processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 14–31. Davis, M. H. (1995). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Corte Madera, CA: Select Press. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117.
  • 23. Geser, H. (2002). Sociology of the mobile phone. Unpublished manuscript, University of Zurich, Switzerland. Leung, L., & Wei, R. (2000). More than just talk on the move: Uses and gratifications of the cellular phone. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 77, 308–320. McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. V. (1989). Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport setting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 60, 48–58. Mieczakowski, A., Goldhaber, T., & Clarkson, J. (2011). Culture, communication, and change: Report on an investigation of the use and impact of modern media and technology in our lives. In Culture, communication, and change. Cambridge, UK: Engineering Design Centre, University of Cambridge. O’Keefe, G. J., & Sulanowski, B. K. (1995). More than just talk: Uses, gratifications, and the telephone. Journalism and Mass Communications Quarterly, 72, 922–933.
  • 24. Przybylski and Weinstein 245 Plant, S. (2000). On the mobile. The effects of mobile telephones on social and individual life. Retrieved from http://www.Motorola.com/mot/documents/0,1028,333,00.pdf Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 419–435. Shah, J. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How significant others implicitly affect goal
  • 25. appraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 424–439. Srivastava, L. (2005). Mobile phones and the evolution of social behavior. Behaviour & Information Technology, 24, 111–129. Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., & Johnston, W. A. (2003). Cell phone-induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 23–32. Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular phone. Psychological Science, 12, 462–466. Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from each other. New York, NY: Basic Books. 246 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(3) http://www.Motorola.com/mot/documents/0,1028,333,00.pdf << /ASCII85EncodePages false /AllowTransparency false /AutoPositionEPSFiles true /AutoRotatePages /None /Binding /Left /CalGrayProfile (Gray Gamma 2.2) /CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
  • 26. /CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2) /sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Warning /CompatibilityLevel 1.3 /CompressObjects /Off /CompressPages true /ConvertImagesToIndexed true /PassThroughJPEGImages false /CreateJDFFile false /CreateJobTicket false /DefaultRenderingIntent /Default /DetectBlends true /DetectCurves 0.1000 /ColorConversionStrategy /LeaveColorUnchanged /DoThumbnails false /EmbedAllFonts true /EmbedOpenType false /ParseICCProfilesInComments true /EmbedJobOptions true /DSCReportingLevel 0 /EmitDSCWarnings false /EndPage -1 /ImageMemory 1048576 /LockDistillerParams true /MaxSubsetPct 100 /Optimize true /OPM 1 /ParseDSCComments true /ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true /PreserveCopyPage true /PreserveDICMYKValues true /PreserveEPSInfo true /PreserveFlatness false /PreserveHalftoneInfo false /PreserveOPIComments false /PreserveOverprintSettings true
  • 27. /StartPage 1 /SubsetFonts true /TransferFunctionInfo /Apply /UCRandBGInfo /Remove /UsePrologue false /ColorSettingsFile () /AlwaysEmbed [ true ] /NeverEmbed [ true ] /AntiAliasColorImages false /CropColorImages false /ColorImageMinResolution 266 /ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleColorImages true /ColorImageDownsampleType /Bicubic /ColorImageResolution 200 /ColorImageDepth -1 /ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1 /ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000 /EncodeColorImages true /ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterColorImages false /ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /ColorACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.15 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /ColorImageDict << /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30
  • 28. >> /JPEG2000ColorImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasGrayImages false /CropGrayImages false /GrayImageMinResolution 266 /GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Bicubic /GrayImageResolution 200 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages false /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.15 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /GrayImageDict << /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /JPEG2000GrayImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256
  • 29. /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasMonoImages false /CropMonoImages false /MonoImageMinResolution 900 /MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Average /MonoImageResolution 600 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.00000 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict << /K -1 >> /AllowPSXObjects false /CheckCompliance [ /None ] /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000
  • 30. ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated 050SWOP051 v2) /PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier (CGATS TR 001) /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org) /PDFXTrapped /Unknown /Description << /ENU <FEFF00550073006500200074006800650073006500200053006 1006700650020007300740061006e006400610072006400200073 0065007400740069006e0067007300200066006f0072002000630 0720065006100740069006e006700200077006500620020005000 440046002000660069006c00650073002e0020005400680065007 30065002000730065007400740069006e0067007300200063006f 006e006600690067007500720065006400200066006f007200200 04100630072006f006200610074002000760037002e0030002e00 20004300720065006100740065006400200062007900200054007 2006f00790020004f007400730020006100740020005300610067 00650020005500530020006f006e002000310031002f003100300 02f0032003000300036002e000d000d0032003000300050005000 49002f003600300030005000500049002f004a005000450047002 0004d0065006400690075006d002f004300430049005400540020 00470072006f0075007000200034> >> /Namespace [ (Adobe) (Common) (1.0) ] /OtherNamespaces [ << /AsReaderSpreads false /CropImagesToFrames true /ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue
  • 31. /FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false /IncludeGuidesGrids false /IncludeNonPrinting false /IncludeSlug false /Namespace [ (Adobe) (InDesign) (4.0) ] /OmitPlacedBitmaps false /OmitPlacedEPS false /OmitPlacedPDF false /SimulateOverprint /Legacy >> << /AllowImageBreaks true /AllowTableBreaks true /ExpandPage false /HonorBaseURL true /HonorRolloverEffect false /IgnoreHTMLPageBreaks false /IncludeHeaderFooter false /MarginOffset [ 0 0 0 0 ] /MetadataAuthor () /MetadataKeywords () /MetadataSubject () /MetadataTitle () /MetricPageSize [ 0 0 ]
  • 32. /MetricUnit /inch /MobileCompatible 0 /Namespace [ (Adobe) (GoLive) (8.0) ] /OpenZoomToHTMLFontSize false /PageOrientation /Portrait /RemoveBackground false /ShrinkContent true /TreatColorsAs /MainMonitorColors /UseEmbeddedProfiles false /UseHTMLTitleAsMetadata true >> << /AddBleedMarks false /AddColorBars false /AddCropMarks false /AddPageInfo false /AddRegMarks false /BleedOffset [ 9 9 9 9 ] /ConvertColors /ConvertToRGB /DestinationProfileName (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /DestinationProfileSelector /UseName /Downsample16BitImages true /FlattenerPreset << /ClipComplexRegions true /ConvertStrokesToOutlines false /ConvertTextToOutlines false /GradientResolution 300
  • 33. /LineArtTextResolution 1200 /PresetName ([High Resolution]) /PresetSelector /HighResolution /RasterVectorBalance 1 >> /FormElements true /GenerateStructure false /IncludeBookmarks false /IncludeHyperlinks false /IncludeInteractive false /IncludeLayers false /IncludeProfiles true /MarksOffset 9 /MarksWeight 0.125000 /MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings /Namespace [ (Adobe) (CreativeSuite) (2.0) ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK /PageMarksFile /RomanDefault /PreserveEditing true /UntaggedCMYKHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UseDocumentBleed false >> ] /SyntheticBoldness 1.000000 >> setdistillerparams << /HWResolution [288 288] /PageSize [612.000 792.000] >> setpagedevice
  • 34. Reading Guide #8 332 Fall 2017 Rus, M., & Galbeaza, A. B. (2013). Psychological aspects of child sexual abuse. Contemporary Readings in Law and Social Justice, 5(2), 499- 510. 1. How do Rus and Galbeaza (2013) describe abuse? Use of force in order to try to dominate child, to compel him/her to do dangerous things that he/she does not want to do, expose him/her to hazardous situations or to situations perceived by him/her dangerous. Any action that causes injury or psycho-emotional disorders is abuse. 2. What are the ways abusers can manipulate their targets? Physical abuse Emotional abuse Sexual abuse Economic abuse Neglection 3. What are the defense mechanisms a child sexual abuse survivor might display as an adult? Disintegration of personality may be one of the most fundamental defense mechanisms used by incest victims.
  • 35. 4. Name 5 of the direct signs of distress abuse can cause in an individual. Low self-esteem. Symptoms of depression. Narcissism, Alcohol, drugs and mental illness Shame 5. What is the picture of sexual abuse in Romania? There are many tourists who go to Romania for sex tourism, especially pedophiles. Some travel societies offer pictures of children at a certain price. Abuse/ignorance of children is widespread, and 9.1% of children surveyed have said they have been sexually abused 6. What is the take home message in the Rus and Galbeaza (2013) article? Child sexual abuse can cause great psychological and physical damage to children. This effect will last a lifetime and will lead to suicide in severe cases. THE CONSUMER IN A CONNECTED WORLD Brain Drain: The Mere Presence of One’s Own
  • 36. Smartphone Reduces Available Cognitive Capacity ADRIAN F. WARD, KRISTEN DUKE, AYELET GNEEZY, AND MAARTEN W. BOS ABSTRACT Our smartphones enable—and encourage—constant connection to information, entertainment, and each other. They put the world at our fingertips, and rarely leave our sides. Although these devices have immense po- tential to improve welfare, their persistent presence may come at a cognitive cost. In this research, we test the “brain drain” hypothesis that the mere presence of one’s own smartphone may occupy limited-capacity cognitive resources, thereby leaving fewer resources available for other tasks and undercutting cognitive performance. Results from two experiments indicate that even when people are successful at maintaining sustained attention—as when avoiding the temptation to check their phones—the mere presence of these devices reduces available cognitive capacity. More- over, these cognitive costs are highest for those highest in smartphone dependence. We conclude by discussing the practical implications of this smartphone-induced brain drain for consumer decision-making and consumer welfare. Adr 211 ver and @di Sch
  • 37. the Eas Cen JAC © 2 We all understand the joys of our always-wired world—the connections, the validations, the laughs . . . the info. . . . But we are only beginning to get our minds around the costs. —Andrew Sullivan (2016) T he proliferation of smartphones has ushered in an era of unprecedented connectivity. Consumers around the globe are now constantly connected to faraway friends, endless entertainment, and virtually unlimited in- formation. With smartphones in hand, they check the weather from bed, trade stocks—and gossip—while stuck in traffic, browse potential romantic partners between ap- pointments, make online purchases while standing in-store, and live-stream each others’ experiences, in real time, from opposite sides of the globe. Just a decade ago, this state of constant connection would have been inconceivable; today, it is seemingly indispensable.1 Smartphone owners interact with their phones an average of 85 times a day, including immediately upon waking up, just before going to sleep, and even in the middle of the night (Perlow 2012; Andrews et al. 2015; dscout 2016). Ninety-one percent report that ian F. Ward ([email protected]) is an assistant professor of m 0 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712. Kristen Duke ([email protected]) i sity of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093. Ayelet G
  • 38. marketing at the Rady School of Management, University of California, Sa sneyresearch.com) is a research scientist at Disney Research, 4720 Forbes A wartz, Yael Horwitz, and the Atkinson Behavioral Lab for research assistanc 1. In 2007, only 4% of American adults owned smartphones (Radwanick 201 age of 35—own smartphones (Pew Research Center 2017). Penetration is s tern and Asian countries. South Korea, for example, has a national smartphon ter 2016). R, volume 2, number 2. Published online April 3, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10 017 the Association for Consumer Research. All rights reserved. 2378-1815 This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms they never leave home without their phones (Deutsche Telekom 2012), and 46% say that they couldn’t live without them (Pew Research Center 2015). These revolutionary de- vices enable on-demand access to friends, family, col- leagues, companies, brands, retailers, cat videos, and much more. They represent all that the connected world has to of- fer, condensed into a device that fits in the palm of one’s hand—and almost never leaves one’s side. The sharp penetration of smartphones, both across global markets and into consumers’ everyday lives, repre- sents a phenomenon high in “meaning and mattering” (e.g., Kernan 1979; Mick 2006)—one that has the potential to affect the welfare of billions of consumers worldwide. As individuals increasingly turn to smartphone screens
  • 39. for managing and enhancing their daily lives, we must ask how dependence on these devices affects the ability to arketing in the McCombs School of Business, University of Texas at Austin, s a PhD candidate in marketing at the Rady School of Management, Uni- neezy ([email protected]) is an associate professor of behavioral sciences n Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093. Maarten W. Bos (mbos venue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. The authors thank Jiyoung Lee, Stephanie e. 2). As of January 2017, 77% of American adults—and 92% of those under imilarly high in most Western nations, and even higher in several Middle e ownership rate of 88%, including 100% of those under 35 (Pew Research .1086/691462 /2017/0202-0009$10.00 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). Volume 2 Number 2 2017 141 think and function in the world off-screen. Smartphones promise to create a surplus of resources, productivity, and time (e.g., Turkle 2011; Lee 2016); however, they may also create unexpected deficits. Prior research on the costs and benefits associated with smartphones has focused on how consumers’ interactions with their smartphones can both facilitate and interrupt off-screen performance (e.g., Isik-
  • 40. man et al. 2016; Sciandra and Inman 2016). In the present research, we focus on a previously unexplored (but com- mon) situation: when smartphones are not in use, but are merely present. We propose that the mere presence of one’s own smart- phone may induce “brain drain” by occupying limited- capacity cognitive resources for purposes of attentional con- trol. Because the same finite pool of attentional resources supports both attentional control and other cognitive pro- cesses, resources recruited to inhibit automatic attention to one’s phone are made unavailable for other tasks, and performance on these tasks will suffer. We differentiate be- tween the orientation and allocation of attention and argue that the mere presence of smartphones may reduce the availability of attentional resources even when consumers are successful at controlling the conscious orientation of at- tention. COGNITIVE CAPACITY AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR Consumers’ finite capacity for cognitive processing is one of the most fundamental influences on “real world” con- sumer behavior (e.g., Bettman 1979; Bettman, Johnson, and Payne 1991). Individuals are constantly surrounded by potentially meaningful information; however, their abil- ity to use this information is consistently constrained by cognitive systems that are capable of attending to and pro- cessing only a small amount of the information available at any given time (e.g., Craik and Lockhart 1972; Newell and Simon 1972). This capacity limit shapes a wide range of be- haviors, from in-the-moment decision-making strategies and performance (e.g., Lane 1982; Lynch and Srull 1982) to long-term goal pursuit and self-regulation (e.g., Hof-
  • 41. mann, Strack, and Deutsch 2008; Benjamin, Brown, and Shapiro 2013). Consumers’ cognitive capabilities—and constraints—are largely determined by the availability of domain-general, limited-capacity attentional resources associated with both workingmemory and fluid intelligence (e.g., Halford, Cowan, and Andrews 2007; Jaeggi et al. 2008). “Working memory” (WM) refers to the theoretical cognitive system that sup- This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms ports complex cognition by actively selecting, maintaining, and processing information relevant to current tasks and/ or goals. “Working memory capacity” (WMC) reflects the availability of attentional resources, which serve the “cen- tral executive” function of controlling and regulating cogni- tive processes across domains (Baddeley and Hitch 1974; Miyake and Shah 1999; Engle 2002; Baddeley 2003). “Fluid intelligence” (Gf) represents the ability to reason and solve novel problems, independent of any contributions from ac- quired skills and knowledge stored in “crystallized intelli- gence” (Cattell 1987). Similar to WM, Gf stresses the ability to select, store, andmanipulate information in a goal-directed manner. Also similar to WM, Gf is constrained by the avail- ability of attentional resources (e.g., Engle et al. 1999; Halford et al. 2007). Crucially, the limited capacity of these domain- general resources dictates that using attentional resources for one cognitive process or task leaves fewer available for other tasks; in other words, occupying cognitive resources re- duces available cognitive capacity. Given the chronic mismatch between the abundance of environmental information and the limited ability to pro- cess that information, individuals need to be selective in their allocation of attentional resources (e.g., Kahneman 1973; Johnston and Dark 1986). The priority of a stimu-
  • 42. lus—that is, the likelihood that it will attract attention— is determined by both its physical “salience” (e.g., location, perceptual contrast) and its goal “relevance” (i.e., potential importance for goal-directed behavior) (e.g., Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Fecteau and Munoz 2006). Preferential at- tention to temporarily relevant stimuli, such as those asso- ciated with a current task or decision, is supported by WM; when a goal is active in WM, stimuli relevant to that goal are more likely to attract attention (e.g., Moskowitz 2002; Soto et al. 2005; Vogt et al. 2010). Frequently relevant stim- uli, such as those associated with long-term and/or self- relevant goals, may automatically attract attention even when the goals associated with these stimuli are not active in WM (Shiffrin and Schneider 1977; Johnston and Dark 1986); for example, individuals automatically orient to the sounds of their own names in ignored audio channels (Moray 1959), and mothers, more so than nonmothers, automatically attend to infants’ emotional expressions (Thompson-Booth et al. 2014). Automatic attention gen- erally helps individuals make the most of their limited cognitive capacity by directing attention to frequently goal- relevant stimuli without requiring these goals to be con- stantly kept in mind. However, automatic attention may undermine performance when an environmental stimulus is 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). 142 Brain Drain Ward et al. frequently relevant to an individual’s goals but currently irrel- evant to the task at hand; inhibiting automatic attention— keeping attractive but task-irrelevant stimuli from interfer- ingwith the contents of consciousness—occupies attentional resources (e.g., Engle 2002).
  • 43. Smartphones serve as consumers’ personal access points to all the connected world has to offer. We suggest that the increasing integration of these devices into the minutiae of daily life both reflects and creates a sense that they are fre- quently relevant to their owners’ goals; it lays the founda- tion for automatic attention. Consistent with this position, research indicates that signals from one’s own phone (but not someone else’s) activate the same involuntary atten- tion system that responds to the sound of one’s own name (Roye, Jacobsen, and Schröger 2007). When these devices are salient in the environment, their status as high-priority (relevant and salient) stimuli suggests that they will exert a gravitational pull on the orientation of attention. And when consumers are engaged in tasks for which their smart- phones are task-irrelevant, the ability of these devices to automatically attract attention may undermine performance in two ways (Clapp, Rubens, and Gazzaley 2009; Clapp and Gazzaley 2012). First, smartphones may redirect the orien- tation of conscious attention away from the focal task and toward thoughts or behaviors associated with one’s phone. Prior research provides ample evidence that individuals spontaneously attend to their phones at inopportune times (e.g., Oulasvirta et al. 2011), and that this digital distraction adversely affects both performance (End et al. 2009) and en- joyment (Isikman et al. 2016). Second, smartphones may re- distribute the allocation of attentional resources between en- gaging with the focal task and inhibiting attention to one’s phone. Because inhibiting automatic attention occupies at- tentional resources, performance on tasks that rely on these resources may suffer even when consumers do not con- sciously attend to their phones. We explore this possibility in the current research. SMARTPHONE USE AND CONSCIOUS DISTRACTION (THE ORIENTATION
  • 44. OF ATTENTION) Research on the relationship between mobile devices and cognitive functioning has largely focused on downstream consequences of device-related changes in the orientation of attention. For example, research on mobile device use while driving indicates that interacting with one’s phone while behind the wheel causes performance deficits such as delayed reaction times and inattentional blindness (e.g., This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms Strayer and Johnston 2001; Caird et al. 2008); these defi- cits mirror those associated with distracting “live” conver- sations (Recarte and Nunes 2003). Similarly, research in the educational sphere demonstrates that using mobile de- vices and social media while learning new material reduces comprehension and impairs academic performance (e.g., Froese et al. 2012). However, mobile device use does not af- fect performance on self-paced tasks, which allow individu- als to compensate for device-related distractions by pick- ing up where they left off (e.g., Fox, Rosen, and Crawford 2009; Bowman et al. 2010). Taken together, these findings suggest that many of the cognitive impairments associated with mobile device use may simply represent the general deleterious effects of diverting conscious attention away from a focal task. What may be special about smartphones, however, is the frequency with which they seem to create these diversions; their omnipresence and personal rele- vance may combine to create a particularly potent draw on the orientation of attention. A more limited body of work explores the cognitive consequences of smartphone-related distractions in the ab- sence of behavioral interaction (i.e., when consumers con- sciously think about phone-related stimuli, but do not actu-
  • 45. ally use their phones). Research on the attentional cost of receiving cellphone notifications indicates that awareness of amissed textmessage or call impairs performance on tasks requiring sustained attention, arguably because unaddressed notifications prompt message-related (and task-unrelated) thoughts (Stothart, Mitchum, and Yehnert 2015). Related re- search shows that individuals who hear their phones ring while being separated from them report decreased enjoy- ment of focal tasks as a consequence of increased attention to phone-related thoughts (Isikman et al. 2016). Forced sep- aration from one’s ringing phone can also increase heart rate and anxiety and decrease cognitive performance (Clayton, Leshner, andAlmond2015). To our knowledge, only one prior study has investigated the cognitive effects of the mere pre- sence of a mobile device—one that is not ringing, buzzing, or otherwise actively interfering with a focal task. Thornton et al. (2014, 485–86) found that a visually salient cellphone can impair performance on tasks requiring sustained atten- tion by eliciting awareness of the “broad social and informa- tional network . . . that one is not part of at the moment.” Together, these investigations of phone-related distractions provide evidence thatmobile devices can adversely affect cog- nitive performance even when consumers are not actively using them. Similar to earlier research on distracted driving and learning while multitasking, however, these studies 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). 2. A pilot study confirmed that these physical locations predict indi- viduals’ top-of-mind awareness of their smartphones, with a nearby and in sight → nearby and out of sight → not nearby linear trend (F(1,
  • 46. 111) 5 14.58, p < .001, partial h2 5 .116) and no quadratic trend (p 5 .996). Interestingly, the majority of respondents (67.5%) indicated that they typically keep their smartphones nearby and in sight, where these de- vices are most salient. See the appendix for method and detailed analyses. Volume 2 Number 2 2017 143 connect the cognitive costs of smartphones to their (re- markable) ability to attract the conscious orientation of at- tention. When individuals interact with or think about their phones rather than attend to the task at hand, their perfor- mance suffers. SMARTPHONE PRESENCE AND COGNITIVE CAPACITY (THE ALLOCATION OF ATTENTIONAL RESOURCES) We suggest that smartphones may also impair cognitive performance by affecting the allocation of attentional re- sources, even when consumers successfully resist the urge to multitask, mind-wander, or otherwise (consciously) at- tend to their phones—that is, when their phones are merely present. Despite the frequency with which individuals use their smartphones, we note that these devices are quite of- ten present but not in use—and that the attractiveness of these high-priority stimuli should predict not just their ability to capture the orientation of attention, but also the cognitive costs associated with inhibiting this automatic at- tention response. We propose that the mere presence of one’s smartphone
  • 47. may impose a “brain drain” as limited-capacity attentional resources are recruited to inhibit automatic attention to one’s phone, and are thus unavailable for engaging with the task at hand. Research on controlled versus automatic processing provides evidence that the mere presence of per- sonally relevant stimuli can impair performance on cognitive tasks (e.g., Geller and Shaver 1976; Bargh 1982; Wingenfeld et al. 2006). Importantly, these performance deficits occur without conscious attention to the potentially interfering stimuli and as a function of inhibiting these stimuli from in- terfering with the contents of consciousness (e.g., Shallice 1972; Lavie et al. 2004). Consistent with this evidence, we posit that themere presence of consumers’ own smartphones can reduce the availability of attentional resources (i.e., cog- nitive capacity) even when consumers are successful at con- trolling the conscious orientation of attention (i.e., resisting overt distraction). If smartphones undermine cognitive performance by oc- cupying attentional resources, the cognitive consequences of smartphone presence should be sensitive to variation in both the salience and the personal relevance of these de- vices, which together determine their priority in attracting attention (e.g., Fecteau and Munoz 2006). Prior research suggests that smartphones are chronically salient for many individuals, even when they are located out of sight in one’s pocket or bag (e.g., Deb 2015). However, we expect that in- This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms creasing the salience of one’s smartphone—for example, by placing it nearby and in the field of vision—will amplify the cognitive costs associated with its presence, as more atten- tional resources are required to inhibit its influence on the orientation of attention. We also expect that these costs will vary according to the personal relevance of one’s smart- phone. We operationalize relevance in terms of “smart-
  • 48. phone dependence,” or the extent to which individuals rely on their phones in their everyday lives. We posit that indi- vidual differences in dependence on one’s smartphone will moderate the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive capacity, such that individuals who most depend on their phones will suffer the most from their presence— and benefit the most from their absence. OVERVIEW OF THE EXPERIMENTS In two experiments, we test the hypothesis that the mere presence of one’s own smartphone reduces available cogni- tive capacity. We manipulate smartphone salience by ask- ing participants to place their devices nearby and in sight (high salience, “desk” condition), nearby and out of sight (medium salience, “pocket/bag” condition), or in a separate room (low salience, “other room” condition).2 Our data in- dicate that the mere presence of one’s smartphone ad- versely affects two domain-general measures of cognitive capacity—available working memory capacity (WMC) and functional fluid intelligence (Gf)—even when participants are not using their phones and do not report thinking about them (experiment 1). Data from experiment 2 repli- cate this effect on available cognitive capacity, show no ef- fect on a behavioral measure of sustained attention, and provide evidence that individual differences in consumers’ dependence on their devices moderate the effects of smart- phone salience on available WMC. EXPERIMENT 1: SMARTPHONE SALIENCE AFFECTS AVAILABLE COGNITIVE CAPACITY In experiment 1, we test the proposition that themere pres- ence of one’s own smartphone reduces available cognitive capacity, as reflected in performance on tests of WMC
  • 49. 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). 144 Brain Drain Ward et al. and Gf. Each of these domain-general cognitive constructs is constrained by the availability of attentional resources, and the moment-to-moment availability of these resources predicts performance on tests of both WMC (Engle, Cantor, and Carullo 1992; Ilkowska and Engle 2010) and Gf (Horn 1972; Mani et al. 2013). If the mere presence of one’s own smartphone taxes the limited-capacity attentional re- sources that constrain both WMC and Gf, then the salience of this device should predict performance on tasks associ- ated with these constructs. We test this hypothesis in ex- periment 1. Method Participants. Five hundred forty-eight undergraduates (53.3% female;Mage 5 21.1 years; SDage 5 2.4 years) partic- ipated for course credit. Data collection spanned two weeks. Duplicate data from repeat participants were dis- carded prior to analysis. We applied the same three data selection criteria in experiments 1 and 2; see the appendix (available online) for additional detail. In experiment 1, three participants were excluded for indicating they did not own smartphones, eight participants were excluded for failing to follow instructions, and seventeen participants were excluded due to excessive error rates on the OSpan task (less than 85% accuracy; see Unsworth et al. 2005). Our final sample consisted of 520 smartphone users. Procedure. We manipulated smartphone salience by ran- domly assigning participants to one of three phone location conditions: desk, pocket/bag, or other room. Participants
  • 50. in the “other room” condition left all of their belongings in the lobby before entering the testing room (as per typical lab protocol). Participants in the “desk” condition left most of their belongings in the lobby but took their phones into the testing room “for use in a later study;” once in the test- ing room, they were instructed to place their phones face down in a designated location on their desks. Participants in the “pocket/bag” condition carried all of their belongings into the testing room with them and kept their phones wherever they “naturally” would. Of the 174 participants in this condition, 91 (52.3%) reported keeping their phones in their pockets, and 83 (47.7%) reported keeping their phones in their bags; a planned contrast revealed no differ- ence between these groups on our key dependent variable (p5 .17), and they were pooled for all subsequent analyses. Participants in all conditions were instructed to “turn your phones completely on silent; this means turn off the ring and vibrate so that your phone won’t make any sounds.” This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms After participants entered the testing room, they com- pleted two tasks intended to measure available cognitive capacity: the Automated Operation Span task (OSpan; Unsworth et al. 2005) and a 10-item subset of Raven’s Stan- dard Progressive Matrices (RSPM; Raven, Raven, and Court 1998). The OSpan task, a prominent measure of WMC, as- sesses the ability to keep track of task-relevant information while engaging in complex cognitive tasks. This particular measure was designed to stress the domain-general nature of the attentional resources at the heart of the WM system (Turner and Engle 1989); in each trial set, participants complete a series of math problems (information process- ing) while simultaneously updating and remembering a randomly generated letter sequence (information mainte- nance). Performance on the OSpan assesses the domain- general attentional resources “available to the individual
  • 51. on a moment-to-moment basis” (Engle et al. 1992). The RSPM test, a nonverbal measure of Gf, was developed to isolate individuals’ capacity for understanding and solv- ing novel problems (fluid intelligence), independent of any influence of accumulated knowledge or domain-specific skill (crystallized intelligence). In each trial, participants are shown an incomplete pattern matrix and asked to select the element that best completes the pattern. Much like the OSpan task, performance on the RSPM test is sensitive to the current availability of attentional resources (e.g., Mani et al. 2013). Complete details of the tasks and measures used in experiments 1 and 2 are provided in the appendix. Participants also completed an exploratory test of the “ending-digit drop-off” effect, modeled after the procedure of Bizer and Schindler (2005). In this task, participants are shown a series of products with .99-ending and .00-ending prices and asked to report the quantity they would be able to purchase for $73. Overestimating purchasing power for a .99-ending price relative to a matched .00-ending price (e.g., $3.99 vs. $4.00) constitutes evidence of the drop-off effect. We thought this effect might be more pronounced for those whose phones were made salient. However, we failed to replicate the basic effect and did not find any evidence of ending-digit drop-off in any condition (F(1, 514)5 .20, p5 .65). See the appendix for detailed analyses and results. Next, participants completed a questionnaire that in- cluded items related to their experiences in the lab and their lay beliefs about the connection between smartphones and performance. These questions assessed how often they thought about their phones during the experiment, to what extent they thought the locations of their phones affected 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 52. Volume 2 Number 2 2017 145 their performance in the lab, how they thought phone loca- tion might have affected their performance, and to what extent they believed their phones affected their perfor- mance and attention spans more generally; all responses were measured using 7-point Likert scales. Finally, partici- pants answered a series of demographic questions (gender, age, ethnicity, nationality) and provided information about their cellphone make/model and data plan. Results and Discussion All analyses in experiment 1 include a “Week” factor to ac- count for variation across research assistants; this factor does not interact with Phone Location in any analysis (all F < 1.27, all p > .28). Cognitive Capacity.We assessed the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive capacity using two measures of domain-general cognitive function: OSpan task perfor- mance and RSPM test score. Because both tasks rely on limited-capacity attentional resources, both should be sen- sitive to fluctuations in the availability of these resources. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) testing the effects of Phone Location (desk, pocket/bag, other room) on the optimal linear combination of these measures revealed a significant effect of Phone Location on cognitive This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms capacity (Pillai’s Trace5 .027, F(4, 1028)5 3.51, p5 .007, partial h2 5 .014). Paired comparisons revealed that partic- ipants in the “other room” condition performed better than those in the “desk” condition (p5 .002). Participants in the
  • 53. “pocket/bag” condition did not perform significantly differ- ently from those in either the “desk” (p 5 .09) or “other room” (p 5 .11) conditions. However, planned contrasts revealed a significant desk→ pocket/bag→ other room lin- ear trend (Pillai’s Trace5 .023, F(2, 513)5 6.07, p5 .002, partial h2 5 .023) and no quadratic trend (Pillai’s Trace 5 .004, F(2, 513) 5 .96, p 5 .39), suggesting that as smart- phone salience increases, available cognitive capacity de- creases. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs separately testing the ef- fect of Phone Location on OSpan performance and RSPM score were consistent with our focal multivariate analysis. Phone Location significantly affected both OSpan perfor- mance (F(2, 514) 5 3.74, p 5 .02, partial h2 5 .014) and RSPM score (F(2, 514) 5 3.96, p 5 .02, partial h2 5 .015). See figure 1 for means, and the appendix for detailed analyses and results. Conscious Thought. A one-way ANOVA on participants’ responses to the question “While completing today’s tasks, how often were you thinking about your cellphone?” (1 5 Figure 1. Experiment 1: effect of randomly assigned phone location condition on available WMC (OSpan Score, panel A) and functional Gf (Correctly Solved Raven’s Matrices, panel B). Participants in the “desk” condition (high salience) displayed the lowest available cognitive capacity; those in the “other room” condition (low salience) displayed the highest available cognitive capacity. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. Asterisks indicate significant differences between conditions, with *p < .05 and **p < .01. 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 54. 146 Brain Drain Ward et al. not at all to 7 5 constantly/the whole time) revealed no effect of Phone Location on phone-related thoughts (F(2, 514) 5 .84, p 5 .43). Notably, the modal self-reported fre- quency of thinking about one’s phone in each condition was “not at all.” Combined with the significant effect of Phone Location on available cognitive capacity, these results sup- port our proposition that the mere presence of one’s smart- phone may impair cognitive functioning even when it does not occupy the contents of consciousness. Perceived Influence of Smartphone Presence. There were no differences between conditions on any measures related to the perceived effects of smartphones on performance (“Howmuch / in what way do you think the position of your cellphone affected your performance on today’s tasks?”; “In general, how much do you think your cellphone usually affects your performance and attention span?”), either in the context of the experiment (all F < 1.58, all p > .21) or in general (F(2, 494) 5 2.26, p 5 .11). Across conditions, amajority of participants indicated that the location of their phones during the experiment did not affect their perfor- mance (“not at all”; 75.9%) and “neither helped nor hurt [their] performance” (85.6%). This contrast between per- ceived influence and actual performance suggests that par- ticipants failed to anticipate or acknowledge the cognitive consequences associated with the mere presence of their phones. Discussion. The results of experiment 1 indicate that the mere presence of participants’ own smartphones impaired their performance on tasks that are sensitive to the avail- ability of limited-capacity attentional resources. In contrast to prior research, participants in our experiment did not in-
  • 55. teract with or receive notifications from their phones. In addition, self-reported frequency of thoughts about these devices did not differ across conditions. Taken together, these results suggest that the mere presence of one’s smart- phone may reduce available cognitive capacity and impair cognitive functioning, even when consumers are successful at remaining focused on the task at hand. EXPERIMENT 2: SMARTPHONE DEPENDENCE MODERATES THE EFFECT OF SMARTPHONE SALIENCE ON COGNITIVE CAPACITY The results of experiment 1 support the proposition that the mere presence of one’s smartphone reduces available cognitive capacity, even when it is not in use. In experi- ment 2, we replicate the basic design of experiment 1, with This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms the following exceptions. First, we conduct a stronger test of the proposed impairment-without-interruption effect by examining the effects of smartphone salience on both cognitive capacity (WMC) and a behavioral measure of sus- tained attention. Consistent with both the proposed theo- retical framework and participants’ self-reports in experi- ment 1, we predict that increasing smartphone salience will adversely affect the availability of attentional resources without interrupting sustained attention. Second, one could argue that participants who had access to their phones in experiment 1 surreptitiously checked for notifications, were consciously distracted by unanswerable messages, and dis- played impaired performance as a result (as in Clayton et al. 2015; Stothart et al. 2015; Isikman et al. 2016). We did not observe any behavior or this sort, and did not find any differences between conditions in the frequency of phone-
  • 56. related thoughts. In experiment 2, we further address this alternate explanation by randomly assigning participants to either silence their phones (as in experiment 1) or turn them off completely. We predict that the salience of partic- ipants’ smartphones will influence available cognitive ca- pacity even when these devices are turned off and will not influence sustained attention even when they are turned on. Third, we test a potential moderator of the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive capacity: indi- vidual differences in the personal relevance of one’s phone, operationalized in terms of “smartphone dependence.” We predict that individuals who are more dependent on their phones will be more affected by their presence. Method Participants. Two hundred and ninety-six undergraduates (56.9% female;Mage 5 21.3 years; SDage 5 2.6 years) partic- ipated for course credit. Eleven participants were excluded for reporting that they did not own smartphones, four par- ticipants were excluded due to excessive error rates (<85% OSpan accuracy), and six participants were excluded due to missing (5) or extreme (1) response times in a Go/No-Go task (see below). Our final sample consisted of 275 participants. Procedure. This experiment followed a 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off ) between-subjects design. Phone Location instructions and randomization procedures were identical to those used in experiment 1, with the exception that participants in the “desk” condition were instructed to place their phones fac- ing up. Of the 91 participants in the “pocket/bag” condi- tion, 68 (74.7%) reported keeping their phones in their 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 57. 3. Two items did not clearly load onto either primary factor and were excluded from further analyses (Costello and Osborne 2005). Volume 2 Number 2 2017 147 pockets, and 23 (25.3%) reported keeping their phones in their bags; as in experiment 1, a planned contrast revealed no difference between these groups on our key dependent variable (p 5 .55), and they were pooled for all subsequent analyses. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two Phone Power conditions prior to entering the testing room. The instructions for participants in the “power on” condition were identical to those used in experiment 1; we instructed participants in the “power off” condition to completely turn off their devices. After placing their phones in the proper location and power mode, participants completed our two key depen- dent measures: the OSpan task and the Cue-Dependent Go/No-Go task (order counterbalanced across partici- pants). In the Go/No-Go task, participants are presented with a series of “go” and “no go” targets, and instructed to respond to “go” targets as quickly as possible without making errors, but to refrain from responding to “no go” targets. In this task, both omission errors (failure to re- spond to “go” targets) and reaction time (speed of respond- ing to these targets) serve as measures of sustained atten- tion (Bezdjian et al. 2009). After completing both tasks, participants reported the subjective difficulty of each task. Next, they completed a battery of exploratory questions intended to assess individual differences in use of and con- nection to one’s smartphone, including a 13-item inventory related to reliance on one’s phone (see appendix for all items and analyses). Finally, participants answered a series
  • 58. of demographic questions (gender, age, ethnicity, national- ity) and provided information about their cellphone make/ model and data plan. Results and Discussion All analyses in experiment 2 include a Task Order reflecting our counterbalanced experimental design; this factor does not interact with Phone Location or Phone Location � Phone Power in any analysis (all F < 1.51, all p > .22). Cognitive Capacity. As in experiment 1, performance on the OSpan task measures the attentional resources avail- able to the individual on a moment-to-moment basis (Engle et al. 1992). A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room)� 2 (Phone Power: on, off) between-subjects ANOVA revealed a significant effect of Location on OSpan perfor- mance (F(2, 263)5 3.53, p5 .03, partial h2 5 .026). There was no effect of Power (F(1, 263) 5 .05, p 5 .83) or of the Power � Location interaction (F(2, 263) 5 1.05, p 5 .35). Paired comparisons revealed that participants in the “other This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms room” condition performed significantly better on theOSpan task than did those in the “desk” condition (Mdiff5 4.67, p5 .008). Participants in the “pocket/bag” condition did not per- form significantly differently from those in either the “desk” (Mdiff5 2.30, p5 .20) or “other room” (Mdiff5 2.37, p5 .17) conditions. See figure 2 for means. Planned contrasts revealed a significant desk → pocket/ bag→ other room linear trend (F(1, 263)5 7.05, p5 .008, partial h2 5 .026) and no quadratic trend (F(1, 263)5 .001, p 5 .98). Consistent with experiment 1, this pattern of re- sults indicates that increasing the salience of one’s smart- phone impairs OSpan performance, and decreasing the sa- lience of one’s smartphone improves performance. Further,
  • 59. the null effects of Power and the Power � Location in- teraction suggest that decreases in performance are not re- lated to incoming notifications (or the possibility of receiv- ing notifications), ruling out this alternative explanation of the effects found in experiment 1. Moderation by Smartphone Dependence. Our framework suggests that the effects of smartphone salience on avail- able cognitive capacity should be moderated by individual differences in dependence on these devices. We tested this prediction by investigating responses to an exploratory 13-item inventory of individual differences in reliance on one’s phone. A principal components factor analysis with Varimax rotation revealed that these items loaded onto two distinct factors, together explaining 52.67% of the var- iance.3 Factor 1 (Smartphone Dependence; six items) ex- plained 31.02% of the variance and captured our primary concept of interest: the degree of dependence on one’s smart- phone (e.g., “I would have trouble getting through a normal day without my cellphone”). Factor 2 (Emotional Attach- ment; five items) explained 21.65% of the variance and ac- counted for the emotional aspects of smartphone use (e.g., “Usingmy cellphonemakesme feel happy”). Reliability anal- yses indicated high reliability for both Smartphone Depen- dence (a5 .89) and Emotional Attachment (a5 .79) as dis- tinct factors. See appendix table 1 for all items and factor loadings. We tested the potential moderating role of Smartphone Dependence in a univariate generalized linear model pre- dicting OSpan performance from all variables included in our original 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room) 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 60. 148 Brain Drain Ward et al. � 2 (Phone Power: on, off) ANOVA, mean-centered Smart- phone Dependence score, and all independent variable � SmartphoneDependence interaction terms (Baron andKenny 1986). This analysis revealed a significant Phone Location � Smartphone Dependence interaction (F(2, 247) 5 3.25, p 5 .04, partial h2 5 .026), indicating that the effects of smartphone salience on OSpan performance are moder- ated by individual differences in dependence on one’s smart- phone. Follow-up analyses probing the conditional effects of Location at the sample mean of the moderator and plus/ minus one SD from the mean revealed no effect of Location on OSpan performance at low levels of Smartphone Depen- dence (21 SD; p 5 .28); however, this effect was significant at both mean (p5 .05) and high levels (p5 .007) of Depen- dence. See figure 3 for estimated marginal means. Similar re- sults for other measures of smartphone dependence (e.g., number of texts sent per day) are reported in the appendix. Interestingly, a parallel moderation analysis indicated that Emotional Attachment did not moderate the effects of Phone Location on OSpan performance (p 5 .61). Al- though we are cautious about making strong claims based on null effects and reiterate that these factors were derived from an exploratory inventory, this disparity between Smart- phone Dependence and Emotional Attachment suggests that the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive ca- This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms pacity may be determined by the extent to which consumers feel they need their phones, as opposed to howmuch they like them. These results are consistent with the proposition that the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive ca- pacity stem from the singularly important role these devices play in many consumers’ lives.
  • 61. Sustained Attention. We analyzed the effects of smart- phone salience on two behavioral measures of sustained attention: omission errors and reaction time in the Go/ No-Go task. A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off) ANOVA revealed no ef- fects of Location, Power, or their interaction on either of these measures (all F < 1.05, all p > .35). See figure 2 for reaction time means, and the appendix for full results. Perceived Difficulty. Finally, we analyzed perceived task difficulty in order to see if the cognitive consequences of smartphone salience were reflected in participants’ subjec- tive experiences. A 3 (Phone Location: desk, pocket/bag, other room) � 2 (Phone Power: on, off ) ANOVA revealed a marginal effect of Location on perceived difficulty for the memory section of the OSpan task (F(1, 256) 5 2.38, p 5 .09, partial h2 5 .018). Paired comparisons revealed that participants in the “other room” condition found it sig- Figure 2. Experiment 2: effect of randomly assigned phone location condition on available cognitive capacity (OSpan Score) and sustained attention (Mean Reaction Time, Go/No-Go). Participants in the “desk” condition (high salience) displayed the lowest available cognitive capacity; those in the “other room” condition ( low salience) displayed the highest available cognitive capacity. Phone location did not affect sustained attention. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. Asterisks indicate significant differences between conditions, with **p < .01. 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 62. Volume 2 Number 2 2017 149 nificantly easier to remember information in this task rel- ative to participants in the “desk” condition (Mdiff 5 .49, p 5 .04) and marginally easier relative to those in the “pocket/bag” condition (Mdiff 5 .40, p 5 .09). This pattern of results is consistent with participants’ actual perfor- mance on the OSpan task and suggests that the cognitive benefits of escaping the mere presence of one’s phone may be reflected, at least partially, in subjective experience. However, the lay beliefs reported in experiment 1 suggest that even when consumers notice these benefits, they may not attribute them to the presence (or absence) of their phones. There were no differences between condi- tions on any of the other perceived difficulty or perceived performance measures (all F < 1.82, all p > .16). Discussion. Consistent with the behavioral and self-report results observed in experiment 1, the results of experiment 2 suggest that the mere presence of consumers’ own smart- phones may adversely affect cognitive functioning even when consumers are not consciously attending to them. Ex- periment 2 also provides evidence that these cognitive costs are moderated by individual differences in dependence on these devices. Ironically, the more consumers depend on their smartphones, the more they seem to suffer from their presence—or, more optimistically, themore theymay stand to benefit from their absence. This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms GENERAL DISCUSSION The proliferation of smartphones represents a profound shift in the relationship between consumers and technol- ogy. Across human history, the vast majority of innovations have occupied a defined space in consumers’ lives; they
  • 63. have been constrained by the functions they perform and the locations they inhabit. Smartphones transcend these limitations. They are consumers’ constant companions, of- fering unprecedented connection to information, enter- tainment, and each other. They play an integral role in the lives of billions of consumers worldwide and, as a re- sult, have vast potential to influence consumer welfare— both for better and for worse. The present research identifies a potentially costly side effect of the integration of smartphones into daily life: smartphone-induced “brain drain.” We provide evidence that the mere presence of consumers’ smartphones can ad- versely affect two measures of cognitive capacity—available working memory capacity and functional fluid intelligence— without interrupting sustained attention or increasing the frequency of phone-related thoughts. Consumers who were engaged with ongoing cognitive tasks were able to keep their phones not just out of their hands, but also out of their (con- scious) minds; however, the mere presence of these devices left fewer attentional resources available for engaging with the task at hand. Figure 3. Experiment 2: estimated marginal means representing the effect of phone location on available cognitive capacity (OSpan Score) at low (21 SD), mean, and high (11 SD) levels of smartphone dependence. Phone location affects available cognitive capacity at mean and high levels of smartphone dependence, but not at low levels of smartphone dependence. Asterisks indicate significant differences between conditions, with *p < .05 and **p < .01. 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
  • 64. 150 Brain Drain Ward et al. Further, we find that the effects of smartphone salience on available cognitive capacity are moderated by individual differences in the personal relevance of these devices (op- erationalized in terms of smartphone dependence); those who depend most on their devices suffer the most from their salience, and benefit the most from their absence. The role of dependence in determining mere presence ef- fects suggests that similar cognitive costs would not be in- curred by the presence of just any product, device, or even phone. We submit that few, if any, stimuli are both so per- sonally relevant and so perpetually present as consumers’ own smartphones. However, we leave open the door for our insights to apply more broadly to future connective technologies that may become equally central to consum- ers’ lives as technology continues to advance. Our research also offers insight into the tactics that might mitigate “brain drain”—as well as those that might not. For example, we find that the effect of smartphone sa- lience on cognitive capacity is robust to both the visibility of the phone’s screen (face down in experiment 1, face up in experiment 2) and the phone’s power (silent vs. powered off in experiment 2), suggesting that intuitive “fixes” such as placing one’s phone face down or turning it off are likely futile. However, our data suggest at least one simple solu- tion: separation. Although this approach may seem at odds with prior research indicating that being separated from one’s phone undermines performance by increasing anxi- ety (Cheever et al. 2014; Clayton et al. 2015), we note that participants in those studies were unexpectedly separated from their phones (Cheever et al. 2014) and forced to hear them ring while being unable to answer (Clayton et al. 2015). In contrast, participants in our experiments ex- pected to be separated from their phones (this was the
  • 65. norm in the lab) and were not confronted with unanswer- able notifications or calls while separated. We therefore suggest that defined and protected periods of separation, such as these, may allow consumers to perform better not just by reducing interruptions but also by increasing avail- able cognitive capacity. Our theoretical framework draws on prior research out- lining the role of limited-capacity attentional resources in inhibiting responses to high-priority but task-irrelevant stimuli (Shallice 1972; Bargh 1982; Lavie et al. 2004; Clapp et al. 2009). However, our data are equally consistent with an alternate explanation: that these attentional resources are recruited for purposes of hypervigilance, or monitoring high-priority stimuli in the absence of conscious awareness (e.g., Legrain et al. 2011; Jacob, Jacobs, and Silvanto 2015). This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms This interpretation is consistent with the common phe- nomenon of “phantom vibration syndrome,” or the feeling that one’s phone is vibrating when it actually is not (e.g., Rothberg et al. 2010; Deb 2015). Data suggest that 89% of mobile phone users experience phantom vibrations at least occasionally (Drouin, Kaiser, and Miller 2012), and that this over-responsiveness to innocuous sensations is particularly prevalent in those whose devices are particu- larly meaningful (e.g., Rothberg et al. 2010). Because the same limited-capacity attentional resources are implicated in both hypervigilance and inhibition, our data cannot dis- tinguish between the two theoretical explanations. In fact, it is plausible that these processes may operate in tandem, as goal-directed attentional control processes both monitor for signals of potentially important information from high- priority stimuli, and (attempt to) prevent these stimuli from interrupting conscious attention until such signals appear.
  • 66. Implications and Future Directions Consumers’ limited cognitive resources shape innumerable aspects of their daily lives, from their approaches to deci- sions (Bettman et al. 1991) to their enjoyment of experi- ences (Weber et al. 2009). Our data suggest that the mere presence of consumers’ own smartphones may further con- strain their already limited cognitive capacity by taxing the attentional resources that reside at the core of both work- ing memory capacity and fluid intelligence. The specific cognitive capacity measures used in our experiments are associated with domain-general capabilities that support fundamental processes such as learning, logical reasoning, abstract thought, problem solving, and creativity (e.g., Cattell 1987; Kane et al. 2004). Because consumers’ smart- phones are so frequently present, the mere presence effects observed in our experiments have the potential to influence consumer welfare across a wide range of contexts: when consumers work, shop, take classes, watch movies, dine with friends, attend concerts, play games, receive massages, read books, and more (Isikman et al. 2016). Moreover, re- sults from our pilot study (reported prior to experiment 1) indicate that the majority of consumers typically keep their smartphones nearby and in sight, where smartphone sa- lience is particularly high. Consumer Choice. Prior research indicates that occupying cognitive resources by increasing cognitive load causes con- sumers to rely less on analytic and deliberative “system 2” processing, and more on intuitive and heuristic-based “sys- 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). Volume 2 Number 2 2017 151
  • 67. tem 1” approaches (Evans 2008). To the extent that both cognitive load and the mere presence of consumers’ smart- phones reduce available cognitive capacity, we may expect consumers to be more likely to adopt choice strategies asso- ciated with system 1 when their smartphones are present but irrelevant to the choice task. Reliance on system 1 pro- cessing could, for example, enhance the appeal of affect-rich choice alternatives (Rottenstreich, Sood, and Brenner 2007), amplify the preference for simple (and possibly inferior) so- lutions (Drolet, Luce, and Simonson 2009), increase consum- ers’willingness to make attribute trade-offs (Drolet and Luce 2004), and heighten susceptibility to anchoring effects (Deck and Jahedi 2015). Building on these connections, future re- search could explore whether the presence of smartphones accentuates individuals’ preference for options favored by system 1 processing. Advertising Effectiveness. The availability of cognitive re- sources also predicts elaboration likelihood (e.g., Petty and Cacioppo 1986) and susceptibility to deceptive advertising (Xie and Boush 2011). Consistent with a potential shift to- ward reliance on system 1 processing, consumers who view advertising messages in the presence of their smartphones may be less likely to elaborate on advertising messages and more likely to be influenced by heuristics such as likability of the communicator (e.g., Chaiken 1980). Note that the pro- posed theoretical framework suggests that this may not be the case for advertising delivered via smartphone, because the cognitive costs associated with mere presence should be incurred when consumers’ phones are present but not in use. Education. Younger adults—92% of whom are smartphone owners—rely heavily on smartphones (Pew Research Center 2016). Given that many of them are in school, the potential detrimental effects of smartphones on their cognitive func- tioning may have an outsized effect on long-term welfare. As educational institutions increasingly embrace “connected classrooms” (e.g., Petrina 2007), the presence of students’
  • 68. mobile devices in educational environments may undermine both learning and test performance—particularly when these devices are present but not in use. Future research could fo- cus onhow children, adolescents, and young adults are affected by the mere presence of personally relevant technologies in the classroom. Intentional Disconnection. Althoughwe have primarily fo- cused on the cognitive costs associated with the presence of This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms smartphones, our research is equally relevant to the poten- tial implications of their absence. Discussions of “disconnec- tion” in popular culture reflect increasing consumer interest in intentionally reducing—or at least controlling—the ex- tent to which they interact with their devices (e.g., Perlow 2012; Harmon and Mazmanian 2013). Some consumers are replacing their smartphones with feature phones (i.e., phones lacking the advanced functionality of smartphones; Thomas 2016), others are supplementing their smart- phones with stripped-down devices that offer “a short break from connectedness” (http://www.thelightphone.com/), and still others are turning to apps that track, filter, and limit smartphone usage (e.g., https://inthemoment.io/). Our re- search suggests that thesemeasures may be doubly beneficial for the digitally weary; by redefining the relevance of their de- vices, these consumers may both reduce digital distraction and increase available cognitive capacity. More broadly, our research contributes to the growing discussion among con- sumers and marketers alike about the influence of technol- ogy on consumers—and consumers on technology—in an increasingly connected world. One’s smartphone is more than just a phone, a camera, or a collection of apps. It is the one thing that connects ev- erything—the hub of the connected world. The presence of one’s smartphone enables on-demand access to informa-
  • 69. tion, entertainment, social stimulation, and more. However, our research suggests that these benefits—and the depen- dence they engender—may come at a cognitive cost. REFERENCES Andrews, Sally, David A. Ellis, Heather Shaw, and Lukasz Piwek (2015), “BeyondSelf-Report: Tools toCompareEstimated andReal-World Smart- phone Use,” PLoS One, 10 (10), 1–9. Baddeley, Alan D. (2003), “Working Memory: Looking Back and Looking Forward,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4 (10), 829–39. Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch (1974), “Working Memory,” Psychol- ogy of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89. Bargh, John A. (1982), “Attention and Automaticity in the Processing of Self-Relevant Information,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43 (3), 425–36. Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator–Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Stra- tegic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173–82. Benjamin, Daniel, Sebastian Brown, and Jesse Shapiro (2013), “Who Is ‘Be-
  • 70. havioral’? Cognitive Ability and Anomalous Preferences,” Journal of the European Economic Association, 11 (6), 1231–55. Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory of Consumer Choice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bettman, James R., Eric J. Johnson, and John W. Payne (1991), “Consumer DecisionMaking,” inHandbook of Consumer Behavior, ed. Thomas S. Rob- ertson and Harold H. Kassarjian, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 7.068.114 on January 25, 2020 12:00:54 PM and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c). 152 Brain Drain Ward et al. Bezdjian, Serena, Laura A. Baker, Dora Isabel Lozano, and Adrian Raine (2009), “Assessing Inattention and Impulsivity in Children during the Go/NoGo Task,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27 (2), 365–83. Bizer, George Y., and Robert M. Schindler (2005), “Direct Evidence of Ending-Digit Drop-off in Price Information Processing,” Psychology and Marketing, 22 (10), 771–83. Bowman, Laura L., Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, and Michael Gendron
  • 71. (2010), “Can Students Really Multitask? An Experimental Study of In- stant Messaging while Reading,” Computers and Education, 54 (4), 927– 31. Caird, Jeff K., Chelsea R. Willness, Piers Steel, and Chip Scialfa (2008), “A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Cell Phones on Driver Performance,” Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40 (4), 1282–93. Cattell, Raymond B. (1987), Intelligence: Its Structure, Growth and Action, New York: Elsevier. Chaiken, Shelly (1980), “Heuristic versus Systematic Information Process- ing and the Use of Source versus Message Cues in Persuasion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (5), 752–66. Cheever, Nancy A., Larry D. Rosen, L. Mark Carrier, and Amber Chavez (2014), “Out of Sight Is Not Out of Mind: The Impact of Restricting Wireless Mobile Device Use on Anxiety Levels among Low, Moderate and High Users,” Computers in Human Behavior, 37 (August), 290–97. Clapp, Wesley C., and Adam Gazzaley (2012), “Distinct Mechanisms for the Impact of Distraction and Interruption on Working Memory in Aging,” Neurobiology of Aging, 33 (1), 134–48.
  • 72. Clapp, Wesley C., Michael T. Rubens, and Adam Gazzaley (2009), “Mech- anisms of Working Memory Disruption by External Interference,” Ce- rebral Cortex, 20 (4), 859–72. Clayton, Russell B., Glenn Leshner, and Anthony Almond (2015), “The Ex- tended iSelf: The Impact of iPhone Separation on Cognition, Emotion, and Physiology,” Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20 (2), 119–35. Corbetta, Maurizio, and Gordon L. Shulman (2002), “Control of Goal- Directed and Stimulus-Driven Attention in the Brain,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3 (3), 201–15. Costello, Anna B., and Jason W. Osborne (2005), “Best Practices in Ex- ploratory Factor Analysis: Four Recommendations for Getting the Most from Your Analysis,” Practical Assessment, Research, and Evalua- tion, 10 (7), 1–9. Craik, Fergus I. M., and Robert S. Lockhart (1972), “Levels of Processing: A Framework for Memory Research,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Ver- bal Behavior, 11 (6), 671–84. Deb, Amrita (2015), “Phantom Vibration and Phantom Ringing
  • 73. among Mobile Phone Users: A Systematic Review of Literature,” Asia- Pacific Psychiatry, 7 (3), 231–39. Deck, Cary, and Salar Jahedi (2015), “The Effect of Cognitive Load on Eco- nomics Decision Making: A Survey and New Experiments,” European Economic Review, 78 (C), 97–119. Deutsche Telekom AG (2012), “Smart Payments—How the Cell Phone Becomes a Wallet,” research report, http://www.studie- life.de/en/life -reports/smart-payments/. Drolet, Aimee, and Mary Frances Luce (2004), “The Rationalizing Effects of Cognitive Load on Emotion-Based Trade-off Avoidance,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (1), 63–77. Drolet, Aimee, Mary Frances Luce, and Itamar Simonson (2009), “When Does Choice Reveal Preference? Moderators of Heuristic versus Goal- Based Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (1), 137–47. This content downloaded from 075.18 All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms Drouin, Michelle, Daren H. Kaiser, and Daniel A. Miller (2012), “Phantom Vibrations among Undergraduates: Prevalence and Associated Psycho- logical Characteristics,” Computers in Human Behavior, 28 (4), 1490–96.
  • 74. dscout (2016), “Mobile Touches: dscout’s Inaugural Study on Humans and Their Tech,” research report, https://blog.dscout.com/hubfs/downloads /dscout_mobile_touches_study_2016.pdf. End, Christian M., Shaye Worthman, Mary Bridget Mathews, and Katharina Wetterau (2009), “Costly Cell Phones: The Impact of Cell Phone Rings on Academic Performance,” Teaching of Psychology, 37 (1), 55–57. Engle, Randall W. (2002), “Working Memory Capacity as Executive Atten- tion,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11 (1), 19– 23. Engle, RandallW., Judy Cantor, and Julie J. Carullo (1992), “Individual Dif- ferences in Working Memory and Comprehension: A Test of Four Hy- potheses,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18 (5), 972–92. Engle, Randall W., Stephen W. Tuholski, James E. Laughlin, and Andrew R. A. Conway (1999), “WorkingMemory, Short-TermMemory, and Gen- eral Fluid Intelligence: A Latent-Variable Approach,” Journal of Exper- imental Psychology: General, 128 (3), 309–31. Evans, Jonathan St. B. T. (2008), “Dual-Processing Accounts of
  • 75. Reasoning, Judgment, and Social Cognition,” Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255– 78. Fecteau, Jillian H., and Douglas P. Munoz (2006), “Salience, Relevance, and Firing: A Priority Map for Target Selection,” Trends in Cognitive Sci- ences, 10 (8), 382–90. Fox, Annie Beth, Jonathan Rosen, andMary Crawford (2009), “Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students’ Perfor- mance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?” CyberPsychology and Behavior, 12 (1), 51–53. Froese, Arnold D., Christina N. Carpenter, Denyse A. Inman, Jessica R. Schooley, Rebecca B. Barnes, Paul W. Brecht, and Jasmin D. Chacon (2012), “Effects of Classroom Cell Phone Use on Expected and Actual Learning,” College Student Journal, 46 (2), 323–32. Geller, Valerie, and Phillip Shaver (1976), “Cognitive Consequences of Self- Awareness,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12 (1), 99–108. Halford, Graeme S., Nelson Cowan, and Glenda Andrews (2007), “Separat- ing Cognitive Capacity from Knowledge: A New Hypothesis,” Trends in