SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 53
Assignment: Macroeconomic Analysis
Professor Sinha
April 13, 2016
The assignment is due in my mailbox on May 6th, 2016 at 5pm.
The mailbox is located in
Lowenstein LL 924. No late submissions will be accepted.
1 Data
1. For January 1981 to February 2016, use the data on
unemployment, inflation and interest
rates to plot these three lines. Unemployment and inflation can
be plotted on the same graph,
and the interest rate will be plotted on a separate graph.
2. Plot the GDP data for January 1981 to October 2015. Please
note that the GDP data is
quarterly, in contrast to the above series, which are monthly.
3. Using the recession dates from NBER (link given below),
discuss how the unemployment,
inflation and GDP numbers changed over 2 years before a
recession, and for 2 years after a
recession for the three most recent recession in the U.S. For
each recession, make a separate
table with the value of these variables. You can use the values
at the start of every quarter
(January, April, July and October).
4. Corresponding to these series for the three most recent
recession, also tabulate how the interest
rate changed, two years before the start of the recession, and 2
years after it ended (also use
quarterly data here).
5. How was the recession of 2007-2009 different from the
previous two recessions in terms of
these variables?
NBER recessions: http://www.nber.org/cycles.html
2 Analyzing the Crisis
1. Read the article on the crisis posted on Blackboard. Pgs. 49-
58 provide you with some
context of the crisis. For the purpose of the questions below,
you will be specifically focusing
on Pgs. 58-70.
1
2. Suppose the economy was at equilibrium before the start of
the recession. Show this using
the IS-LM, Keynesian cross, AD-SRAS-LRAS and money
market spaces.
3. Following the start of the recession, what happened to
aggregate demand and output? (You
should be able to see this from the plots in section 1 above).
Show this shift in AD and IS
curves.
4. If the Federal Reserve and government had not intervened,
explain how the economy would
have adjusted back to the long-run equilibrium.
5. How could monetary policy have ensured a faster return to
the long-run equilibrium? Use
the money market and LM curves to show this.
6. The Federal Reserve undertook several measures to deal with
the crisis. Using the section
on "Policy Responses to the financial crisis" (Pg. 58), discuss
two unconventional policy
measures undertaken.
7. Using the discussion of fiscal policy on pgs. 63-65, discuss
two fiscal policy actions that were
taken. Which components of planned expenditure would be
affected by these fiscal actions?
8. In your view, why were unconventional monetary policy
actions needed?
9. Using the interest rate data plotted above, has monetary
policy returned to "normal" opera-
tions after the 2007 crisis, relative to the previous two
recessions?
2
Research Report
Integrated speech and phonological awareness intervention for
pre-school
children with Down syndrome
Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt†, Gail Gillon‡ and Susan
Foster-Cohen§
†Department of Communication Disorders, University of
Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
‡College of Education, University of Canterbury, Christchurch,
New Zealand
§The Champion Centre, Burwood Hospital, Christchurch, New
Zealand
(Received 14 October 2008; accepted 27 April 2009)
Abstract
Background: Children with Down syndrome experience
difficulty with both spoken and written language
acquisition, however controlled intervention studies to improve
these difficulties are rare and have typically focused
on improving one language domain.
Aims: To investigate the effectiveness of an integrated
intervention approach on the speech, letter knowledge, and
phonological awareness development of ten pre-school children
with Down syndrome aged between 4;4 and 5;5.
Methods & Procedures: A multiple single-subject design was
used to evaluate treatment effectiveness. Baseline and
intervention measures for speech and pre- and post-intervention
measures for letter knowledge and phonological
awareness were compared. The intervention comprised three
components: a parent-implemented home programme;
centre-based speech–language therapy sessions, and ‘Learning
through Computer’ sessions with a total intervention
time of 20 hours over 18 weeks. Letter knowledge and
phonological awareness activities were linked to each child’s
speech targets.
Outcomes & Results: Results indicated significant treatment
effects on speech measures for all ten participants. Six of
the ten participants showed increases on letter knowledge and
nine showed increased awareness of initial phonemes
in words but responses were not above binomial chance level
(that is, 70% correct) for phonological awareness tasks.
Individual results are presented and implications for parents and
therapists are discussed.
Conclusions and Implications: The findings of this study
suggest an intervention approach that integrates speech,
letter knowledge, and phonological awareness targets is
effective in remediating speech error patterns at the single-
word level in young children with Down syndrome.
Phonological awareness and letter knowledge appeared to be
stimulated through the intervention, but significant
improvement above chance levels on untrained phonological
awareness tasks was not evident. Follow-up investigation is
necessary to determine longer-term outcomes.
Keywords: Down syndrome, speech intervention, phonological
awareness intervention.
What this paper adds
Children with Down syndrome experience difficulty with both
spoken and written language acquisition, exhibiting
persistent speech difficulties and poor underlying skills critical
for early reading development. Intervention research
to address these deficits is scarce and previous studies have
typically focused on remediating one language domain.
The present study advances our knowledge through
investigating the effectiveness of an integrated intervention
approach which aims to improve speech, letter knowledge, and
phonological awareness development in pre-school
children with Down syndrome. This study suggests an
intervention which includes a letter knowledge and
phonological awareness focus is effective in remediating speech
errors at the single-word level in pre-school children
with Down syndrome and may stimulate letter knowledge and
early phonological awareness in this population.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online q 2010 Royal
College of Speech & Language Therapists
http://www.informahealthcare.com
DOI: 10.3109/13682820903003514
Address correspondence to: Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt,
Department of Communication Disorders, University of
Canterbury, Private Bag
4800, Christchurch, New Zealand; e-mail: [email protected]
INT. J. LANG. COMM. DIS., MAY–JUNE 2010,
VOL. 45, NO. 3, 320–335
Introduction
Individuals with Down syndrome (DS) experience
difficulty with both spoken and written language
acquisition. They demonstrate poorer verbal skills
compared with non-verbal skills and perform more
poorly on language measures and speech production
measures than do peers with other cognitive impair-
ments matched for mental age (Smith and Stoel-
Gammon 1983, Roberts et al. 2008). These findings
lead to the conclusion that individuals with DS present
with speech and language impairment in addition to
cognitive impairment. Children with concurrent speech
and language impairment are known to be at high risk
for reading difficulties (for example, Catts et al. 2001)
which suggests that reading development in children
with DS may be particularly compromised.
Reading achievement is a valid goal for young
children with DS and is associated with superior spoken
language abilities (Cardoso-Martins et al. 2008),
however research investigating variables that influence
early reading acquisition is limited. Recent research
suggests children with DS perform poorly on
phonological awareness (for example, Snowling et al.
2002) — an area critical for early reading success.
Controlled intervention studies to improve spoken
or written language development in children with DS
are rare and have typically focused on improving only
one language domain. However, given these children’s
concurrent speech and language deficits and weakness
in the underlying skills critical for reading, an
integrated intervention approach that may lead to
improvements in both spoken and written language is
intuitively appealing. An integrated intervention aims
to simultaneously facilitate the development of
different skills, in this instance speech production,
letter name and sound knowledge, and phonological
awareness. The current study sought to investigate the
impact of such an intervention on the speech, letter
knowledge, and phonological awareness development
of pre-school children with DS.
Speech difficulties in children with DS
An integrated intervention that will simultaneously
improve speech articulation in addition to stimulating
phonological awareness is important, given the well
documented speech production difficulties in children
with DS. Although early vocalizations and babble
appear similar to those produced by their peers with
typical development, phonological development and
early word production are substantially delayed in
children with DS (Smith and Stoel-Gammon 1983).
The speech of children with DS is reported to contain
the same type of error patterns and is qualitatively
similar to that of typically developing younger children
(Smith and Stoel-Gammon 1983), however, slower
speech development in individuals with DS results in a
widening gap across time.
As well as delay, Dodd et al. (1994) report
characteristics of phonological disorder, including
significantly more atypical speech error patterns and
greater inconsistency in the speech errors of individuals
with DS compared with mental age-matched peers.
Given the phonological speech error patterns observed
in children with DS, a phonological approach to
intervention where children’s conscious awareness of
the sounds structure of the spoken word is enhanced, in
addition to practice in articulating correct speech
patterns, may be effective in improving the articulation
of words.
Interventions to enhance speech production
A number of interventions have focused on physical
and motor based activities to improve the speech
production of children with DS. For example, in a
radical approach to try to improve speech intelligibility,
children with DS may undergo tongue reduction
surgery. Parsons et al. (1987) compared the articulation
errors of 18 children with DS pre-, post- and 6 months
after tongue reduction surgery and found no significant
differences across the three measures, nor between the
surgery group and a non-surgery contrast group.
Non-speech oral motor exercises and treatments
have been recommended for children with DS to
increase physical awareness and strength of the oral and
facial muscles used during speech (Kumin 2006).
However, Barnes et al. (2006) reported a disassociation
between oral motor structure and function, with
children with DS demonstrating better oral function
skills than speech function skills, supporting the
hypothesis that remediating oral-motor deficits will
not improve speech production. Reviews of the
literature investigating the use of non-speech oral
motor exercises indicated that the evidence does not
support their effectiveness in the treatment of speech
disorders (Lof and Watson 2008), nonetheless, their use
is still widely reported by clinicians working with
children with speech sound disorders, including
children with DS (Lof and Watson 2008). Thus,
there is an urgent need to explore other types of
interventions such as phonologically based interven-
tions that may hold more promise to improving speech
difficulties in children with DS.
Phonological intervention approaches have been
extensively used with young children with spoken
language impairment in the absence of DS (Hesketh
et al. 2000, Gillon 2005). However, only two
phonological intervention approaches to improving
Integrated intervention for children with DS 321
speech difficulties in children with DS have been
documented (Cholmain 1994, Dodd et al. 1994).
Cholmain’s study included six children with DS aged
4;1–5;6 with language ages of 1;3–2;8. The interven-
tion was implemented via a modified cycles approach
(Hodson and Paden 1991) to remediate phonological
patterns, and was delivered using amplification with a
focus on contrastive versus homophonic production of
the targeted patterns in words, and a home-based
programme of speech targets practice and joint story
reading. Despite previous stability in the 3–12 months
before intervention, response to the intervention was
rapid with all the children demonstrating positive
change in the first two weeks of the intervention.
Nine children with DS aged between two and
6 years took part in the Dodd et al. (1994) study which
used a core vocabulary approach to reduce non-
developmental errors and variability in the children’s
speech. Parents were trained to provide corrective
feedback to their child who was required to produce a
consistent pronunciation of each target word, with no
non-developmental errors present. Fewer errors and
inconsistencies were reported post-intervention. These
two studies demonstrate a phonological intervention
approach can be effective for young children with DS
and that parents can play a key role in modelling target
production and providing corrective feedback to
improve their child’s speech. The current study extends
this early work through explicitly linking goals to
stimulate areas of phonological development that are
critical for reading success, with goals to stimulate
correct phonological production.
Integrated phonological awareness intervention
Integrated phonological awareness intervention which
incorporated phonological awareness, letter knowledge
and speech production goals has been shown to be
effective for young children with speech disorders and
normal cognitive ability (Gillon 2005, McNeill et al.
2009). Intervention to improve phonological awareness
in children with DS has been the focus of a number of
recent studies. Reported findings included improve-
ment in reading skills, alphabet knowledge, and
phonological awareness (van Bysterveldt et al. 2006,
Goetz et al. 2008), however the effects of the
interventions on speech production were not examined.
Recent evidence suggests that achieving a precise
phonological representation of a word is particularly
demanding for children with DS ( Jarrold et al. 2009).
Improving explicit awareness of the underlying
representation of a spoken word and a child’s ability
to consciously access this representation through
phoneme awareness tasks (such as identifying the
initial sound in a word or segmenting a word into
phonemes), may improve the child’s speech production
of the target word.
There is increasing research into the role of
phonological awareness in reading for children with
DS, and a positive relationship between phonological
awareness and reading for individuals with DS has been
reported by researchers (Cupples and Iacono 2000,
Snowling et al. 2002, Goetz et al. 2008). For example,
Cupples and Iacono (2000) examined the relationship
between phonological awareness and reading skills in
young children with DS aged 6 to 10 years and found
better phonological awareness skills were associated
with better reading of both real and non-words.
Roch and Jarrold (2008) compared real word and
non-word reading ability in individuals with DS. They
found that based on the relationship between these
skills in the typically developing control group, non-
word reading skills of participants with DS were poorer
than would be predicted from their real word reading
ability. However, reported correlation coefficients
between non-word reading and composite phonologi-
cal awareness scores were equivalent between the two
groups. Thus, while real and non-word reading
appeared differently related, non-word reading and
phonological awareness skills were similarly related in
both groups. Therefore, the research appears to suggest
that while individuals with DS do exhibit strength in
visual reading strategies relative to other reading sills,
phonological awareness skills also play a role in
supporting reading in this population.
Letter knowledge and phonological awareness
The importance of phonological awareness and letter
knowledge in early reading and spelling acquisition is
now well established, with both skills strongly
predictive of later reading outcomes (Wagner et al.
1994). Letter name and sound knowledge are distinct
but overlapping skills, with letter name knowledge
typically preceding and predictive of letter sound
knowledge and both skills associated with reading-
related measures (McBride-Chang 1999). Letter
knowledge and phonological awareness are also
positively associated and reciprocal gains in these skills
have been demonstrated when their instruction has
been integrated and explicitly linked (Oudeans 2003).
Joint book reading, a key element of the home literacy
environment (for example, Bus et al. 1995) is an
important medium through which specific early literacy
skills such as phonological awareness and letter
knowledge may be targeted (Justice and Ezell 2000,
van Bysterveldt et al. 2006).
Given the potential of phonological intervention
and the important role of parents in supporting such
an approach to improve speech in children with DS,
322 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
as well as the success of an integrated phonological
awareness approach for children with other types of
speech and language impairment, the current study
sought to investigate the impact of an intervention
approach which combines all these components, on the
speech and phonological awareness development of pre-
school children with DS. Specifically it was hypoth-
esized that the experimental integrated phonological
awareness intervention would improve the following
skills:
. Speech production accuracy in trained and
untrained speech targets.
. Letter name and letter sound knowledge.
. Phonological awareness skills on untrained
phoneme level tasks.
Method
Research design
A multiple single-subject repeated measures (duplicated
AB) design was used to evaluate treatment effectiveness.
Participant selection process
Ten children (five girls and five boys) with DS who
ranged in age from 4 years 4 months to 5 years 5
months (mean ! 4 years 11 months, standard
deviation (SD) ! 4.08 months) participated in the
study. The participants who were from a range of socio-
economic backgrounds were recruited from a group of
thirteen 4- and 5-year-old children enrolled in a
transition to school programme for children with DS at
a specialist early intervention centre. Throughout the
intervention participants attended weekly sessions at the
centre, in small groups of up to six children where they
received an individual programme based on the
national curriculum. Each child received services from
a team of six specialists/therapists which included a
physiotherapist or occupational therapist, a cognitive
therapist, a speech–language therapist, a music
therapist, an early childhood teacher and a computer
specialist who they saw individually and sequentially
throughout the morning.
Following parent consent, criteria for inclusion in
the intervention study were (1) a diagnosis of Down
syndrome with no known current major medical
conditions or additional developmental disabilities, (2)
enrolment in the centre’s transition to school clinic, (3)
standard New Zealand English as a first and only
language and (4) no additional speech therapy for the
duration of the intervention. These criteria excluded
three children from the study, one with a serious
medical condition and two who intended to leave the
centre before the completion of the intervention period.
Procedure
Standardized and experimental measures were used to
assess the speech, expressive and receptive language,
phonological awareness, letter knowledge and hearing
of participants. Formal assessment of participants’
mental age was incompatible with the philosophy of
the early intervention centre. The lead researcher
(a qualified speech–language therapist) administered all
the experimental tasks. Two senior student Speech–
Language Therapists assisted in the administration of
the standardized assessments under clinical supervision
procedures.
Participants received audiological assessment twice
during the intervention period. Seven participants had
some degree of hearing loss ranging from slight to
moderate. Average hearing thresholds across all
frequencies tested, both ears (if tested) and both times
(if tested) ranged from 17.5 to 39.37 dB. Six of the ten
participants wore corrective glasses and one child was
scheduled to have cataract surgery at the completion of
the intervention.
Participants were assessed on the following
measures of receptive and expressive language:
Standardized assessments:
. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test — III (PPVT-
III) (Dunn and Dunn 1997). This receptive
vocabulary test requires the child to point to
one of four pictures named by the examiner.
The assessment was administered and scored
according to the examiner manual. As standard
score floor effects were apparent, both raw scores
and standard scores are reported.
. Pre-School Language Scale — Fourth Edition
(Australian Language Adaptation) (PLS-4)
(Zimmerman et al. 2002). This norm-referenced
test assesses the receptive (Auditory Comprehen-
sion) and expressive (Expressive Communication)
language of children aged from birth to 6 years 11
months. The assessment was administered and
scored according to the examiner manual. Standard
score floor effects were apparent for the majority of
children therefore a Total Language Score (TLS)
presented as a language age score is also reported.
Participants’ speech production was initially assessed 6
weeks before the start of the intervention using the
following measures:
Hodson Assessment of Phonological Patterns —
Third Edition (HAPP-3) (Hodson 2004). This is a
single word articulation test comprising 50 single and
multi syllabic words elicited by naming manipulatives
and line drawings. For all speech data, where a
spontaneous response could not be elicited by the
picture or stimulus item, a response was elicited
Integrated intervention for children with DS 323
following delayed imitation. Speech data were recorded
using a high-quality digital voice recording device
(Belkin F8E462). All responses were transcribed via
broad transcription. These samples were analysed using
Computer Profiling (PROPH; Long and Fey 2005).
Initial assessment data for measures of speech and
language are reported in Table 1.
The following measures were administered pre- and
post-intervention to all ten participants:
Letter knowledge and phonological awareness
experimental measures
. Letter name and letter sound knowledge tasks.
Letter name and sound knowledge was assessed
using the Gillon Preschool Phonology and Letter
Knowledge probes (Gillon 2005). These probes
comprise a lower-case presentation of all 26 letters
presented six at a time on an A4-sized grid.
The child is required to point to the letter name
or sound the assessor names, for example
‘Which one says ssss?’, or ‘Show me the letter t’.
To reduce the effects of a position response
pattern, a correct response was credited when the
appropriate letter was identified on two
occasions. The phonological awareness and letter
knowledge tasks did not require a verbal response.
. Initial phoneme identity (Gillon 2005). In this
task the therapist names a large colourful picture
of an animal and draws the child’s attention to
the first sound of that word. The child is then
required to identify this as the initial sound in a
word from a choice of three pictures presented
and named by the examiner. Corrective feedback
is given if required. The test comprised two
training items and ten test items.
. Initial phoneme identity with words (Gillon and
McNeill 2007). In this task the therapist presents
a large letter and draws the child’s attention to
the sound it makes. The child is then required
to identify this as the initial sound in a word and
match this to one of three cards consisting of a
picture with the word written underneath,
presented and named by the examiner. Corrective
feedback is given if required. The test comprised
one training item and twelve test items.
(Phonological awareness measures may be freely
downloaded from: http://www.education.canterbury.
ac.nz/people/gillon/resources.shtml).
Intervention target selection
For each participant, four speech targets for interven-
tion and a control target were selected based on the
initial speech assessment data (Table 2). Each speech
target was then assessed with twelve speech cards to
determine the percentage usage of dominant phonolo-
gical error patterns in the target words presented.
Pre- and post-intervention measures
Twelve speech cards consisting of pictures with words
of a similar phonological structure were prepared for
each speech target (that is, 48 cards per child). Six were
randomly selected to be the trained items and included
in the intervention activities with the remaining six
assessed only as a generalization measure. Correct
production of the target phoneme in the appropriate
word position was credited correct; however non-
targeted phonemes in the word may have been in error.
For example a child whose speech target was the correct
articulation of final /p/ would be credited as correct
with the production of the word ‘cup’ as /tLp/.
An additional set of twelve speech cards was prepared as
a control measure for each child. This set was not
included in the intervention activities and was assessed
only. The cards were modelled on the format used by
Gillon and McNeill (2007). (Examples of these speech
Table 1. Participants’ assessment data
P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 P10
Child’s age 4;11 5;0 5;05 4;10 5;02 5;05 4;04 4;09 5;02 4;08
Gender F M F F M F F M M M
PPVT-III RSa 42 3 36 13 18 28 19 39 28 31
PPVT-III SSb 81 40* 68 48* 53 60* 65 80* 62 73
PLS-4:ACc 66 50 50 50 50 50 60 74 51 73
PLS-4:ECd 62 50 53 50 50 55 50 79 52 77
PLS-4:TLSe 3;4 1;5 2;8* 1;8* 2;2 2;5* 2;1 3;3* 3;0 3;1
PCC-Rf 66.7 36.2* 72.1 22.4* 43.8 43.5* 38.7 76.1 53.2 53.3
PVCg 97.8 92.0* 89.1 93.3* 86.4 84.6* 100 91.5 85.2 93.5
Total wordsh 39 25 39 26 21 26 14 50 47 46
Note. Age (years; months). P, participant. aPeabody Picture
Vocabulary Test — III Raw Score. bPeabody Picture
Vocabulary Test — III Standard Score. cPreschool Language
Scale —
4 Auditory Comprehension Standard Score. dPreschool
Language Scale — 4 Expressive Communication Standard
Score. ePreschool Language Scale — 4 Total Language Score is
language age in years; months. fPer cent consonants correct —
Revised. gPer cent vowels correct. hTotal number of words
analysed in the sample using computerized profiling (PROPH;
Long and Fey 2005). *Unwillingness to participate in the
assessment.
324 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
cards are freely available from: http://www.education.
canterbury.ac.nz/people/gillon/resources.shtml).
Participants were assessed on all their target and
control speech cards (60 trials) on five occasions
throughout the study. The first two administrations
completed one week prior and immediately prior to the
start of the intervention, comprised the final two
baseline measures pre-intervention. Intervention phase
assessments were completed at the end of cycle one and
the start and end of cycle two. Participants’ correct
scores on speech targets ranged between zero and
16.6% correct at the two pre-intervention assessments,
with all participants scoring zero per cent correct on
control targets pre-intervention.
Intervention
Intervention procedures
The experimental integrated intervention implemented
in this study comprised the following three com-
ponents:
. A parent-implemented home programme. Before
the intervention, parents attended an information
and training evening following the format
detailed in van Bysterveldt et al. (2006) whereby
parents were trained to use print referencing
techniques at home to bring their child’s
attention to targeted letters and sounds during
joint story book reading. The intervention was
implemented four times per week for 10 minutes
per session throughout the 18 week intervention
period (that is, 12 hours in total). Parents’
implementation of the print referencing tech-
niques was monitored on a weekly basis during
the intervention.
. Speech therapy (SLT) sessions.
. Learning through computer (LTC). Participants
attended the early intervention centre one
morning per week where they saw six different
therapists in 20-minute sessions throughout the
morning. In keeping with the service delivery
model of the early intervention centre, the LTC
sessions supported the SLT goals for the child,
Table 2. Examples of participants’ dominant phonological error
patterns present in target words, percentage usage and speech
sound
targets
P PCC-R Examples of dominant phonological error patterns
Usage (%) Target sounds selected (initial position unless stated)
1 40.8 Initial cluster reduction 84 /fl/ /gr/ /sp/ /l/
Gliding 91
2 8.3a Final consonant deletion 100 /k/ /m/ /t/ /f/
Deletion of initial consonants 28
Context sensitive voicing 50
Deletion of fricatives 75
3 45.0 Cluster reduction 64 /kr/ /sp/ /sw/ /t
R
/
Stopping 64
4 18.9 Early stopping 100 /t/ final /k/ final /n/
Final consonant deletion 69 final /p/
Deletion of final stops 68
Deletion of final nasals 100
5 18.8 Cluster reduction 100 /l/ /f/ /sp/ /h/
Deletion of initial h 91
Liquid deletion 60
Deletion of initial fricatives 50
6 26.8 Final consonant deletion 65 /v/ final /m/ final /p/
Later stopping 67 final /d/
Deletion of final stops 67
7 11.6 Final consonant deletion 78 /p/ /n/ /t/ /b/ all final
Deletion of final stops 82
Deletion of final nasals 100
8 54.3 Later stopping 95 /kr/ /tr/ /d3/ /t
R
/
Cluster simplification 96
9 44.1 Later stopping 71 /l/ /v/ /t
R
/ final /k/
Gliding 33
Liquid deletion 25
Deletion/substitution of final k 56
10 54.0 S nasal cluster reduction 100 /h/ /v/ /sn/ and/sm/
Substitutions/distortions of fricatives 50 /t
R
/
Distortions/substitutions of affricates 58
Deletion/substitutions/distortions of initial h 60
Note: aSeverely restricted sample.
Integrated intervention for children with DS 325
thus the experimental intervention was delivered
during the SLT and LTC sessions.
The intervention was delivered in two, 6 weekly cycles
separated by a 6-week break (that is, 4 hours total SLT
and 4 hours total LTC). SLT sessions characteristically
included four 5-minute activities as this was compatible
with the children’s attention span and ability to engage
with the task. Sessions began with a letter knowledge
activity, followed by one or two phoneme matching
tasks. Each session also typically contained at least one
game where letter name, letter sound and phoneme
matching were integrated into the one activity.
A description of the intervention activities is provided
in Appendix A.
The SLT sessions integrated speech goals with
phonological awareness and letter knowledge goals and
was implemented by the lead researcher, focusing on
two speech targets per session. For example if the child’s
speech target was the correct articulation of final /p/ in
CVC words then the letter p was used in letter sound
knowledge activities, and phoneme matching tasks
included CVC words ending with /p/. The lead
researcher provided a correct model and feedback
during all activities. Children were encouraged to
engage in tasks that required the articulation of their
target speech words, letter name and letter sound
knowledge tasks and phoneme matching tasks based on
the structure described in Gillon and McNeill (2007)
and adapted by the researcher for use with pre-school
children with DS. LTC sessions comprised these
phonological awareness and letter knowledge tasks
adapted for presentation on a computer to pre-school
children with DS.
Travel issues and participants’ health influenced
attendance at the centre, with participants attending an
average of 75% of centre based sessions (approximately
6 hours) equating to a total mean intervention time of
18 hours 8 minutes (SD ! 1 hour 18 minutes).
Reliability
Twenty per cent of speech assessment and speech probe
data were re-transcribed by an independent SLT. Point-
by-point analysis showed mean agreement of 92.1%
with a range of 87.7–95.0% for assessment data and
mean agreement of 98.9% with a range of 83.3–100%
agreement for speech card probe data. Any differences
were resolved by consensus after repeated listenings.
Treatment fidelity
Print referencing sessions
Parents were videotaped administering the print
referencing techniques and received structured feedback
two to four times during the intervention period.
Additionally, a trained independent reviewer reviewed
23 video-taped sessions and recorded that nine of the
ten parents consistently and accurately implemented
the intervention. One parent who did not consistently
use all three key parts of the technique during the
session received extra training and corrective feedback
however the implementation remained inconsistent.
Parents completed a weekly report and although
all parents reported abandoning sessions due to
non-compliance, they all completed four 10-minute
sessions of print referencing per week for the full
18-week period. Sessions were largely presented by
mothers; 100% in six families, 90% in three families
and 70% in one family. Seven reviewed sessions (32%)
were affected by or abandoned due to child behaviours
including screaming, grabbing the book, running away
or refusing to listen to the story.
Speech therapy sessions
Thirty-nine randomly selected intervention sessions
(just over 41% of the total sessions) which included at
least two sessions from each participant were evaluated
by a trained independent reviewer. A minimum of
twelve productions by the researcher of each of the three
intervention elements: letter name instruction, letter
sound instruction, and phoneme matching was
required, with 79.4% (31 sessions) adhering to the
treatment fidelity protocol. Additionally the reviewer
noted the child’s overall compliance and engagement
within the session and the occurrence of interaction
breakdowns. The reviewer identified 28 different
challenging behaviours such as biting, pinching,
shouting, throwing toys, hiding, damaging equipment
etc. Interaction breakdowns affected 25 of the 39
sessions reviewed (just over 64%) with 90 separate
challenging behavioural events recorded, ranging from
one to ten per session (mean ! 2.3, SD ! 2.79)
which prevented one or more elements being
completed in approximately 18% (7) of the sessions.
Four participants averaged less than one event per
session, with the remaining six participants averaging
between one and seven (mean ! 3.6, SD ! 2.96)
challenging behavioural events per session. No pattern
was observed between the presentation of the different
activities and the occurrence of interaction breakdowns.
The presence of additional visiting specialists impacted
the presentation of one element in the remaining
session.
The wide variation in participants’ speech abilities
and the impact of behaviour and motivation meant
minimum target production across participants was not
appropriate. Average production across participants
ranged from 0.5 to 29 for letter names, from 0.3 to 22
326 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
for letter sounds and from five to 32 for whole words,
with considerable within-participant variability across
sessions. Corrective feedback, modelling or cueing by
bringing the child’s attention to the visual support
(that is, letter cards or speech cards) and phoneme–
grapheme connections, was required from the
researcher on at least 50% of occasions when the
child’s letter name, letter sound or target word
production was in error. All reviewed sessions adhered
to the corrective feedback standard.
Learning through computer sessions
The computer specialist’s clinical notes from a
randomly selected 32 (just over 34%) sessions were
reviewed by the researcher. Sessions were required to
contain at least two of the activities developed by the
researcher and the computer specialist known to
include letter names, letter sounds and phoneme
matching. All the reviewed sessions adhered to the
protocol. Interaction breakdowns which resulted in
disengagement with the task were recorded in seven
(just over 21%) of the reviewed sessions.
Results
Scoring
Where the assessment was unable to be completed due
to a participant’s inability to understand the task or an
unwillingness to attempt or complete the task, a score of
zero was given for that item and the next item was
presented. Where participants did not respond during
the speech sound assessment, stimulus cards were
placed on the bottom of the pile and represented later in
the assessment. A further non-response resulted in a
zero score for that item.
Speech production accuracy
No statistical difference was found between partici-
pants’ performances on trained and untrained target
speech items ( p $ 0.1) therefore these data were
combined for all analyses. The two standard deviation
band method (Portney and Watkins 2009) was used
to determine whether a statistical difference existed
between scores within the baseline and intervention
phases. The mean of the baseline phase and a two
standard deviation band are plotted across both phases
with intervention data points outside the banded area
indicative of significant change (Portney and Watkins
2009). A linear regression line was also calculated
depicting the line which best describes the relation-
ship between speech production performance and
time during the baseline phase, and providing
an estimate of predicted speech performance during
the intervention phase.
Additional analysis was undertaken using the
statistical process control (SPC) model described in
Portney and Watkins (2009) to control for the
contribution of time-series data to the probability of
Type I error (false-positive), to verify the existence of
stable baseline performances and to determine whether
significant change has occurred between baseline and
intervention phases. A moving range score (X-mR)
based on the mean variability between adjacent baseline
data points is calculated to determine the upper and
lower control limits (UCL and LCL). Data within these
control limits of common cause variation indicate a
stable baseline. Data points outside these limits
comprise special cause variation which is indicative of
significant change.
Effect sizes and confidence intervals were generated
for seven participants. Three participants achieved zero
scores at all points during the baseline phase, thus
neither SPC analysis nor effect size analysis was
appropriate. A standardized mean difference statistic
was calculated using the procedure described in Shadish
et al. (2008). This measure has been developed in an
effort to provide a mechanism to compare the
effectiveness of single-case designs with between-
group designs. This procedure yielded a g-statistic of
g ! 2.59 (Hedges adjustment for sample size d ! 2.48)
which is comparable with those reported by Shadish
et al. (2008) in their meta analysis of single-case
experimental designs.
The speech production gains of one child
(participant 4) analysed via the two standard deviation
band method and the statistical process control
method are presented in Figures 1 and 2, respectively.
A summary of change scores, significance and effect
sizes for target speech sounds for all participants is
presented in Table 3.
Correct production of target sounds ranged from
zero to 10.41% at pre-intervention (mean ! 2.91,
SD ! 3.42) and from 20.8% to 70.8% post-
intervention (mean ! 51.42, SD ! 16.54) indicat-
ing gains of between 20.8% and 66.64%
(mean ! 48.51, SD ! 15). A paired samples t-test
performed on the grouped data revealed significant
differences between the pre- and post-intervention
scores (t(9) ! 10.02, p , 0.001). Per cent consonants
correct (PCC-R) and per cent vowels correct (PVC) and
percentage change for all sounds in participants’ speech
target words are presented in Table 4. The PCC-R
metric, which scores distortions (both clinical and non-
clinical) as correct, is recommended by Shriberg et al.
(1997) as the measure which is most appropriate to
compare the speech of individuals with diverse speech
and age profiles. Post-intervention gains ranged from
Integrated intervention for children with DS 327
22.5 to 53.8 for PCC-R and from 5.3 to 46.4 for PVC.
All participants scored zero per cent correct on control
sounds pre-intervention and six participants remained
at zero per cent correct scores post-intervention with
four participants demonstrating a small improvement
in the production of their control sounds post-
intervention. All participants showed greater change
on target sounds than control sounds, with average
difference scores for target sounds ranging from 2.5 to
eight out of twelve and for control sounds from zero to
three out of twelve. Spearman rank-order correlations
revealed no significant relationships between percentage
change on target phonemes and pre-intervention
measures of PPVT (r ! 20.05, p ! 0.86), hearing
thresholds (r ! 0.41, p ! 0.21) or chronological age
(r ! 0.18, p ! 0.58). Analysis revealed no relationship
Figure 1. Baseline and intervention phase performance for
speech sound measures for Participant 4 using 2 Standard
Deviation Band analysis.
Performance in the intervention phase must be above the shaded
2 standard deviation band to demonstrate significant
improvement (in this
example, speech sound measures pre-intervention were mean !
2.45, SD ! 1.85).
Figure 2. Baseline and intervention phase performance for
speech sound measures for participant 4 using Statistical
Process Control analysis.
Performance in the intervention phase must be above the upper
control limit (UCL) to demonstrate significant improvement (in
this example
the UCL ! 7.98 and the lower control limit (LCL) ! 23.0776.
328 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
between gains on speech targets and gains on letter
knowledge (r ! 20.33, p ! 0.38), initial phoneme
identity (IPI) (r ! 0.20, p ! 0.53) or initial phoneme
identity with words (IPIW) (r ! 20.45, p ! 0.17).
Letter name knowledge
Letter name knowledge was assessed pre- and post-
intervention with results presented in Figure 3. At pre-
intervention two participants had complete or near
complete letter name knowledge. Of the remaining eight
participants, five showed improvement in scores at post-
intervention, with large gains demonstrated by one
participant. Three participants who were unable to
complete the task at both testing times were assigned a
score of zero. Responses included closing the assessment
book, pointing to several letters at once and providing no
response. No significant relationships were found
between gain scores on letter name knowledge and pre-
intervention measures of PPVT (r ! 0.03, p ! 0.88),
hearing thresholds (r ! 0.33, p ! 0.38) or chronological
age(r ! 20.18,p ! 0.61).Duetoceilingeffectsthedata
from two participants with high scores at pre-intervention
were excluded from this analysis of change scores.
Spearman rank-order correlations revealed pre-interven-
tionscoresofletternameknowledgewerenotsignificantly
correlated with hearing thresholds (r ! 20.48,
p ! 0.13) or chronological age (r ! 0.03, p ! 0.89),
howeverthecorrelationwithPPVTscoresapproachedthe
level of significance (r ! 0.55, p ! 0.06). Analysis also
revealed significant and near significant relationships
between gain scores on the letter name knowledge task
and gain scores on the IPI (r ! 0.86, p , 0.001) and
IPIW tasks (r ! 0.65, p ! 0.05).
Letter sound knowledge
Results of the pre- and post-intervention assessment
of letter sound knowledge are presented in Figure 4.
At pre-intervention two participants demonstrated high
scores on the letter sound knowledge task with one
other participant able to identify one letter sound
correctly. Post-intervention two of these participants
showed some increases in scores but no additional
children were able to demonstrate any letter sound
knowledge. Five participants at pre-intervention and
four participants at post-intervention were unable to
complete the task and were assigned a score of zero.
Letter name knowledge was more advanced than
letter sound knowledge at both testing times, and
appeared to be prerequisite for letter sound knowledge in
the three children able to demonstrate this knowledge,
with no child able to demonstrate knowledge of a letter’s
sound without knowledge of that letter’s name.
Initial phoneme identity
Pre- and post-intervention assessment results are
presented in Figure 5. Three participants at pre-
intervention and nine participants at post-intervention
achieved scores in the Initial Phoneme Identity (IPI)
Table 3. Summary of change and effect sizes in target speech
sounds
Effect size
P Baseline Intervention 2 SD significant? SPC significant?
Cohen’s d 295% CI "95% CI
1 0.52 (1.04) 13.88 (13.22) Yes Yes 1.42 20.24 3.09
2 0 14.90 (23.12) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a.
3 0 61.77 (8.66) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a.
4 2.45 (1.85) 45.11 (17.30) Yes Yes 3.46 1.11 5.82
5 1.47 (2.94) 15.26 (5.22) Yes Yes 3.25 0.98 5.51
6 1.04 (2.08) 52.07 (22.52) Yes Yes 3.19 0.94 5.43
7 3.85 (2.70) 38.88 (15.91) Yes Yes 3.42 1.05 5.75
8 0 22.77 (15.73) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a.
9 11.40 (3.50) 53.11 (14.47) Yes Yes 3.96 1.40 6.52
10 4.68 (3.55) 43.74 (16.66) Yes Yes 3.24 0.97 5.50
Notes: Baseline and intervention scores are the mean and
standard deviation (SD) of the baseline and intervention scores.
P, participant; 2 SD significant?, significant using the two SD
band method; SPC significant?, significant using the Statistical
Process Control method; CI, confidence intervals.
Table 4. PCC-R and PVC of all sounds in participants’ speech
target words
PCC-R PVC
P Pre- Post- Difference Pre- Post- Difference
1 40.8 64.9 "24.1 83.7 91.5 "7.8
2 8.3a 62.1 "53.8 28.6a 75.0 "46.4
3 45.0 82.4 "37.4 89.7 97.9 "8.2
4 18.9 68.1 "49.2 65.2 80.4 "15.2
5 18.8 41.9 "23.1 77.8 89.7 "11.9
6 26.8 75.3 "48.5 52.4 95.0 "42.6
7 11.6 57.3 "45.7 26.5 60.0 "33.5
8 54.3 76.8 "22.5 91.1 100 "8.9
9 44.1 73.4 "29.3 68.2 93.1 "24.9
10 54.0 78.0 "24.0 82.6 97.9 "15.3
Notes: P, participant; PCC-R, per cent consonants correct —
revised; PVC, per cent
vowels correct. aSeverely restricted sample characterized by
non-speech sounds.
Integrated intervention for children with DS 329
Figure 3. Letter name knowledge pre- and post-intervention.
Figure 4. Letter sound knowledge pre- and post-intervention.
Figure 5. Initial phoneme identity pre- and post-intervention.
330 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
task however all scores were below chance level as
determined by the binomial test (Portney and Watkins
2009), with a score of 7/10 or higher required for a
statistically significant result ( p , 0.05). Three partici-
pants at pre-intervention and one participant at post-
intervention were unable to complete the task and were
assigned a score of zero. Spearman rank-order
correlations revealed IPI scores were significantly
correlated with PPVT scores pre-intervention
(r ! 0.79, p ! 0.003) and approaching significance
post-intervention (r ! 0.61, p ! 0.05). A strong
negative correlation was found between IPI scores and
hearing thresholds at pre-intervention (r ! 20.81,
p ! 0.001) but not at post-intervention (r ! 20.08,
p ! 0.81) nor with chronological age pre- (r ! 20.47,
p ! 0.16) or post-intervention (r ! 20.49, p ! 0.13).
No significant relationships were found between IPI
gain scores and pre-intervention measures of PPVT
(r ! 20.10, p ! 0.75), hearing thresholds (r ! 0.54,
p ! 0.08) or chronological age (r ! 0.08, p ! 0.81).
Initial phoneme identity with words
Pre- and post-intervention assessment results are
presented in Figure 6. Four participants at pre-
intervention and eight participants at post-intervention
achieved scores in the Initial Phoneme Identity with
Words (IPIW) task, however all scores were below
chance level as determined by the binomial test
(Portney and Watkins 2009) with a score of 8/12 or
higher required for a statistically significant result
(p , 0.05). Four participants at pre-intervention and
two participants at post-intervention were unable to
complete the task and were assigned a score of
zero. Spearman rank-order correlations revealed no
significant relationships between scores on IPIW
and PPVT pre- (r ! 0.07, p ! 0.83) and post-
intervention (r ! 0.47, p ! 0.16), hearing thresholds
pre- (r ! 0.14, p ! 0.68) and post-intervention
(r ! 20.29, p ! 0.38) or chronological age pre-
(r ! 20.33, p ! 0.34) and post-intervention
(r ! 20.35, p ! 0.29) nor between IPIW gains scores
and PPVT (r ! 0.44, p ! 0.18), hearing thresholds
(r ! 20.33, p ! 0.32) or chronological age
(r ! 20.16, p ! 0.63).
Discussion
This study used a multiple single-subject design to
investigate the effectiveness of an integrated phonolo-
gical awareness intervention for ten children with DS
aged 4 and 5 years. The intervention aimed
simultaneously to facilitate the development of speech
production, letter name and sound knowledge, and
phonological awareness. The intervention included the
following three components:
. Parent-implemented print referencing during
joint story reading.
. Letter knowledge and phoneme awareness
activities conducted by the speech–language
therapist (SLT) in a play-based format.
. Letter knowledge and phoneme awareness
activities conducted by the computer
specialist (CS).
The intervention was implemented by the SLT and
CS at an early intervention centre during two
20-minute sessions per week, in two 6-week therapy
blocks separated by a 6-week break (that is, 8 hours in
total). The parents implemented the print referencing
component in four 10-minute sessions per week
across the 18-week intervention period (approximately
12 hours in total).
The first hypothesis stated that the research
intervention would improve speech production accu-
racy in the trained and untrained words featuring each
Figure 6. Initial phoneme identity with words pre- and post-
intervention.
Integrated intervention for children with DS 331
child’s target speech sounds. The results from the
intervention supported this hypothesis. All ten
participants demonstrated statistically significant
improvement in production accuracy on both trained
and untrained words.
There was, however, considerable variability
between the speech production skills of the participants
pre- and post-intervention and in their response to
intervention. This variability, however, could not be
accounted for by participants’ receptive language scores,
chronological age or hearing thresholds. The interven-
tion appeared to be effective irrespective of whether the
targeted speech errors were evident in developmentally
earlier or later resolving speech error patterns or
whether the participants used single speech error
patterns or multiple speech error patterns at once.
Although speech targets were in activities which also
integrated letter name and sound knowledge and
phoneme matching, analysis revealed no relationship
between gains on speech targets and gains on letter
knowledge, IPI or IPIW.
The study findings indicated that significant
improvement in the speech articulation of single
words can be achieved after a short intervention, that is,
approximately 20 hours over 18 weeks (intervention
time mean ! 18 hours 8 minutes, standard deviation
(SD) ! 1 hour 13 minutes) in children whose rate
of speech development is slow and whose performance
has remained stable over the preceding two months.
The improvement in speech production by the children
with DS is consistent with the findings of Cholmain
(1994) and Dodd et al. (1994) who reported
participants made considerable speech gains over a
relatively short period of time, despite stable perform-
ance pre-intervention.
Grunwell (1990) described four different types of
generalization of speech skills: Lexical, Socioenviron-
mental, Syntactic, and Phonological. Lexical generaliz-
ation refers to use of the remediated pattern in
untrained words and was observed in all participants
with children performing equally well on the trained
and untrained words. Phonological generalization is
observed when the remediated pattern is demonstrated
with other phonemes in the same sound class.
Participant 4 in this study provides an example of
phonological generalization and use of multiple
patterns. Targeted speech patterns for participant 4
included voiced/voiceless contrasts presented in words
with initial /t/, many of which were of CVC structure.
Additionally, some words presented to target final
consonant deletion included initial /k/, and these were
typically voiced to /g/ or /d/. Analysis across time
demonstrated remediation of the final consonant
deletion pattern in the articulation of single words
and establishment of voicing contrasts within the same
word, for example, for the target word /k ^ p/
addressing deletion of final stops, participant 4’s
production progressed as follows; /d‰/ ! /d ^ p/ !
/k ^ p/ and for the target word /tæp/ addressing voicing
contrasts the progression was similar /dæ/ ! /tæ/ ! /
dæp/ ! /tæp/. Other improvements in participants’
speech that were not the focus of a targeted pattern,
such as velar fronting in the example above, are
evidenced by increased per cent consonant correct
(PCC-R) and per cent vowels correct (PVC) scores
from all sounds in target words.
The second hypothesis was that the research
intervention would improve participants’ letter name
and letter sound knowledge. This hypothesis was
partially supported. Letter sound knowledge proved
difficult for most participants. The intervention
appeared only to stimulate further letter sound
knowledge in two participants who entered the study
already able to demonstrate some letter sound knowl-
edge. The intervention had more effect on teaching
participants letter names. Six of the ten participants
demonstrated that they knew more letter names
at the end of the intervention compared with
pre-intervention.
Participants with the strongest letter knowledge at
post-intervention were three of the four participants
who had language ages of at least 3 years suggesting a
stronger language foundation may facilitate the
acquisition of alphabet knowledge. A pattern of
learning letter names before letter sounds is consistent
with findings for children without DS (McBride-Chang
1999). Analysis of the videotaped intervention sessions
showed many participants were able to identify
correctly one or two letter names and sounds that
were the focus of their intervention session. However,
these skills were evident in therapy sessions only and did
not generalize to the assessment session when the
activity was presented in a different context. Difficulty
in transferring improvement skills from a therapy
context to an assessment context is common in children
with high learning needs and may suggest the
knowledge has not yet consolidated or further
scaffolding and increased therapy time is required
(Roberts et al. 2008).
The third hypothesis examined whether the
research intervention would improve phonological
awareness skills on untrained phoneme level tasks.
The data partially supported this hypothesis. Perform-
ance improved post-intervention compared with pre-
intervention for most participants but their improved
performance did not meet the conservative binomial
level of 70% correct at post-intervention when
untrained phonological awareness items were intro-
duced. This suggests phonological awareness was being
stimulated during the intervention period, but
332 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
participants had not reached mastery of identifying
initial sounds in words and therefore could not
demonstrate the transference of knowledge to novel
items.
Phonological awareness in young children typically
proceeds along a developmental continuum of increas-
ing ability and stability (Lonigan et al. 1998)
throughout the preschool years. Lonigan et al. (1998)
reported some measurable, though inconsistent,
phonological awareness skills in children as young as
2 or 3 years of age and more consolidated phonological
awareness skills in children aged 4 years and over.
Participants’ receptive language abilities, as determined
by their PPVT scores, were associated with both pre-
and post-intervention measures of letter name and
sound knowledge and IPI, but not with change scores.
However, gain scores on letter name knowledge and
both phonological awareness tasks were related, which
suggests that the presentation of these activities in an
integrated format may facilitate the reciprocal gains
reported by Oudeans (2003). Consistent with the
findings of Lonigan et al. (1998) it is plausible that
some participants in the current study did demonstrate
a pattern of emerging but unstable phonological
awareness. Follow-up assessment of letter knowledge
and phonological awareness skills is necessary to
investigate any potential longer-term influence of the
intervention on these skills. Follow-up assessment of
speech measures is also required to determine whether
treatment gains were maintained.
Clinical implications
Speech production errors are common in children with
DS and these errors often persist into adulthood.
However, empirically based interventions to improve
speech production in this population are rare. Children
with DS also exhibit weakness in the underlying skills of
phonological awareness and letter knowledge tasks that
are critical for early reading success. The findings of this
study suggest an intervention approach which integrates
speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness
targets was effective in remediating speech errors for
pre-school children with DS and may potentially
stimulate letter knowledge and phonological awareness
skills in the future. Speech gains were demonstrated by
all children regardless of language age, however the
greatest gains on letter knowledge and phonological
awareness were demonstrated by children who had
language ages of at least 3 years. The relationships
between gains in letter knowledge and phonological
awareness support the appropriateness of an approach
in which instruction in these skills is integrated. In light
of the persistent nature of speech difficulties in
individuals with DS and the superior language abilities
associated with reading in this population, evidence
based interventions which can combine several
treatment goals may provide a valuable alternative to
traditional therapy techniques which aim to improve
only one language domain.
Acknowledgements
Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of
interest.
The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of
the
paper.
References
BARNES, E., ROBERTS, J., MIRRETT, P., SIDERIS, J. and
MISENHEIMER, J.,
2006, A comparison of oral structure and oral-motor
function in young males with Fragile X syndrome and Down
syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research,
49, 903–917.
BUS, A. G., VANIJZENDOORN, M. H. and PELLEGRINI, A.
D., 1995, Joint
book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta-
analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review
of Educational Research, 65, 1–21.
CARDOSO-MARTINS, C., PETERSON, R., OLSON, R. and
PENNINGTON, B.,
2008, Component reading skills in Down syndrome.
Reading and Writing, 21, 277–292.
CATTS, H., FEY, M., ZHANG, X. and TOMBLIN, B., 2001,
Estimating the
risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children:
a research-based model and its clinical implementation.
Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38–51.
CHOLMAIN, C. N., 1994, Working on phonology with young
children with Down syndrome — a pilot study. Journal of
Clinical Speech and Language Studies, 1, 14–35.
CUPPLES, L. and IACONO, T., 2000, Phonological awareness
and oral
reading skill in children with Down syndrome. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 595.
DODD, B., MCCORMACK, P. and WOODYATT, G., 1994,
Evaluation of
an intervention program: Relation between children’s
phonology and parents’ communicative behavior. American
Journal on Mental Retardation, 98, 632–645.
DUNN, L. M. and DUNN, L. M., 1997, Peabody Picture
Vocabulary
Test — III (Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service).
GIBBON, F. and GRUNWELL, P., 1990, Specific
developmental language
learning disabilities. In P. GRUNWELL (ed), Developmental
speech disorders:clinical issues and practical implications
(Edinborough; New York: Churchill Livingstone), 135–161.
GILLON, G., 2005, Facilitating phoneme awareness
development in
3- and 4–year-old children with speech impairment.
Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 36,
308–324.
GILLON, G. T. and MCNEILL, B. C., 2007, Integrated
Phonological
Awareness: An Intervention Program for Preschool Children
with Speech–Language Impairment (Christchurch: Depart-
ment of Communication Disorders, University of Canter-
bury) (available at: http://www.education.canterbury.ac.nz/
people/gillon/resources.shtml).
GOETZ, K., HULME, C., BRIGSTOCKE, S., CARROLL, J. M.,
NASIR, L. and
SNOWLING, M., 2008, Training reading and phoneme
awareness skills in children with Down syndrome. Reading
and Writing, 21, 395–412.
HESKETH, A., ADAMS, C., NIGHTINGALE, C. and HALL,
R., 2000,
Phonological awareness therapy and articulatory training
approaches for children with phonological disorders:
Integrated intervention for children with DS 333
A comparative study. International Journal of Language and
Communication Disorders, 35, 337–354.
HODSON, B. and PADEN, E., 1991, Targeting intelligible
speech: A
phonological approach to remediation, 2nd edn (Austin, TX:
Pro-Ed).
HODSON, B., 2004, Hodson Assessment of Phonological
Patterns —
Third Edition HAPP-3 (Austin, TX: PRO-ED).
JARROLD, C., THORN, A. and STEPHENS, E., 2009, The
relationships
among verbal-short term memory, phonological awareness,
and new word learning: Evidence from typical development
and Down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 102, 196–218.
JUSTICE, L. M. and EZELL, H. K., 2000, Enhancing children’s
print
and word awareness through home-based parent interven-
tion. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9,
257–269.
KUMIN, L., 2006, Speech intelligibility and childhood verbal
apraxia
of speech in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome
Research and Practice, 10, 10–22.
LOF, G. L. and WATSON, M. M., 2008, A nationwide survey of
nonspeech oral motor exercise use: implications for evidence-
based practice. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in
Schools, 39, 392–407.
LONG, S. and FEY, M. E., 2005, Computerized Profiling
(Version
9.7.0) (Cleveland, OH: Case Western Reserve University).
LONIGAN, C. J., BURGESS, S. R., ANTHONY, J. L. and
BARKER, T. A., 1998,
Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old
children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294–311.
MCBRIDE-CHANG, C., 1999, The ABCs of the ABCs: the
development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.
MCNEILL, B. C., GILLON, G. T. and DODD, B., in press,
2009,
The effectiveness of an integrated phonological awareness
approach for children with childhood apraxia of speech
(CAS). Child Language Teaching and Therapy.
OUDEANS, M. K., 2003, Integration of letter-sound
correspondences
and phonological awareness skills of blending and segment-
ing: a pilot study examining the effects of instructional
sequence on word reading for kindergarten children with
low phonological awareness. Learning Disability Quarterly,
26, 258–308.
PARSONS, C. L., IACONO, T. A. and ROZNER, L., 1987,
Effect of tongue
reduction on articulation in children with Down syndrome.
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 328–332.
PORTNEY, L. G. and WATKINS, M. P., 2009, Foundations of
Clinical
Research: Applications to Practice (Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Pearson Education).
ROBERTS, J. E., CHAPMAN, R. S., MARTIN, G. E. and
MOSKOWITZ, L.,
2008, Language of preschool and school-age children with
Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. (pp 77–115) In J.
E. Roberts, R. S. Chapman and S. F. Warren (eds), Speech
and Language Development and Intervention in Down
Syndrome and Fragile X Syndrome (Baltimore, MA: Paul H.
Brookes).
ROCH, M. and JARROLD, C., 2008, A comparison between
word and
nonword reading in Down syndrome: the role of
phonological awareness. Journal of Communication Disorders,
41, 305–318.
SHADISH, W. R., RINDSKOPF, D. M. and HEDGES, L. V.,
2008, The state
of the science in the meta-analysis of single-case experimental
designs. Evidence-Based Communication Assessment and
Intervention, 2, 188–196.
SHRIBERG, L. D., AUSTIN, D., LEWIS, B. A., MCSWEENEY,
J. L. and
WILSON, D. L., 1997, The percentage of consonants correct
(PCC) metric: extensions and reliability data. Journal of
Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 708–722.
SMITH, B. L. and STOEL-GAMMON, C., 1983, A longitudinal
study of
the development of stop consonant production in normal
and Down’s syndrome children. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders, 48, 114–118.
SNOWLING, M. J., HULME, C. and MERCER, R. C., 2002, A
deficit in
rime awareness in children with Down syndrome. Reading
and Writing, 15, 471–495.
VAN BYSTERVELDT, A. K., GILLON, G. T. and MORAN, C.,
2006,
Enhancing phonological awareness and letter knowledge in
preschool children with Down syndrome. International
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53,
301–329.
WAGNER, R. K., TORGESEN, J. K. and RASHOTTE, C. A.,
1994,
The development of reading-related phonological processing
abilities: new evidence of bi-directional causality from a
latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
30, 73–87.
ZIMMERMAN, I. L., STEINER, V. G. and POND, R. E., 2002,
Preschool
Language Scale — Fourth Edition — Australian Language
Adaptation (PLS-4) (San Antonio, TX: Psychological).
334 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
Appendix A: Letter name and sound knowledge example
The speech-language therapist (SLT) placed poster-sized cards
of
the two targets being targeted (for example, l and k) on the floor
and
hid six to ten small cards of each letter around the room in
obvious
locations, for example, under a cushion for a ‘hide and seek’-
type
format and the SLT and child took turns to turn them over.
The SLT said the letter name and sound and the child matched it
to the corresponding large letter card. The child was encouraged
to
say the letter name and sound, with any production reinforced
and
corrected by the SLT. The child then placed a mechanical toy on
the
large letter card and activated it to jump:
SLT: What have you found?
SLT: Ooh, you’ve found t.
SLT: t says /t/. Can you see t, /t/ anywhere?
Child: There. (child points)
SLT: Oh, good matching, you found . . . ? (prompting
for the target letter name)
Child: t.
SLT: You’re right, and it says . . . ? (prompting for the
target letter sound)
Child: /t/.
SLT: Well done, you matched t, /t/.
Child places small letter card onto corresponding large
letter.
SLT: Let’s bounce Tigger on t.
Phoneme identity and matching example
The SLT placed poster-sized cards of the two targets being
taught
(for example, l and k) on the floor and two poster-sized ‘bingo’-
type
boards on the floor beside the corresponding letter target. The
bingo
boards displayed six target words with the word written
underneath
each picture in large bold font. An identical set was cut up for
the
child to match. For example, where the target was to address
final
consonant deletion of /k/, the bingo board included the words
sock,
hook, work, beak, wink, and book. These were placed face down
and
the SLT and child took turns to turn them over. If the child
spontaneously said the word, this was reinforced by the SLT.
Corrective feedback was provided if required including drawing
the
child’s attention to the written text. Where the child did not
spontaneously say the word, it was said by the therapist and the
child
matched it to the corresponding large letter. Once the words had
been matched to the target letter they were matched to the
corresponding picture on the bingo board.
SLT: Let’s see what we’ve got. You choose one.
Child: (child chooses a card and names it) tap.
SLT: You’re right, that says tap and there’s the letter t
that says /t/. (points to the text).
SLT: See, it starts with t. (points to the large letter).
SLT: Let’s check, is it the same? (compares to large
letter).
Child: Yes.
SLT: You’re right, now we need to find the other
picture of . . . ? (prompts child to respond)
Child: tap.
SLT: You’re right, tap.
Child: (places card on board) tap.
SLT: Good matching.
For some children, two bingo boards were too
challenging or distracting so the activity was modified
to present one target at a time.
Combined activity
SLT placed a poster-sized sheet on the floor with a racetrack
circuit
on it. The track was divided into squares on which were placed
six to
ten small letter cards for each of the two target letters/sounds.
Two
piles of word cards, each pile comprising the six target words
for
each sound, were placed in the middle of the track. The child,
parent and SLT took turns throwing dice and moving a toy car
around the track. When a person landed on a small letter card a
corresponding word card was chosen. Parents were encouraged
to
provide feedback to the child following the SLT’s model:
SLT: Your turn to throw the dice.
Child throws dice.
SLT: Oh you got a . . . ?
Child: Three.
SLT: Great, let’s count, one, two, three. You landed on
. . . ? (prompting for the target letter name)
Child: l.
SLT: You did, you landed on l, and it makes the /l/
sound. Now choose a word that starts with l. Which
pile is it on?
Child: (chooses a card) lamp.
SLT: Good talking, you chose lamp. It starts with
l that makes the /l/ sound. Let’s say it together; lamp.
Show Mum what you got.
Child: lamp.
Mum: What does it start with?
Child: l.
Mum: Yes, l says /l/. Good job!
Integrated intervention for children with DS 335

More Related Content

Similar to Assignment Macroeconomic AnalysisProfessor SinhaApril 13,.docx

The Movement Program_Whitepaper
The Movement Program_WhitepaperThe Movement Program_Whitepaper
The Movement Program_WhitepaperAlex Doman
 
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...Dr.Ebtessam Nada
 
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert LensRemedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lensijtsrd
 
Relations between language and executive function in spanish spea
Relations between language and executive function in spanish speaRelations between language and executive function in spanish spea
Relations between language and executive function in spanish speaCristina GM
 
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docx
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docxRESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docx
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docxrgladys1
 
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic languageRESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic languageanitramcroberts
 
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docxAssignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docxbraycarissa250
 
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...Georgia State School of Public Health
 
Summary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis PaperSummary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis PaperBronwyn Thomas
 
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15LamptonLWA
 
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) Approach
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) ApproachLetters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) Approach
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) ApproachRead Australia (Wiring Brains Education)
 
Parent-Infant Speech Interactions
Parent-Infant Speech InteractionsParent-Infant Speech Interactions
Parent-Infant Speech Interactionsabbey73
 
PAGE Running head SAMPLE 1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docx
PAGE  Running head SAMPLE  1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docxPAGE  Running head SAMPLE  1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docx
PAGE Running head SAMPLE 1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docxgerardkortney
 
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docxodiliagilby
 

Similar to Assignment Macroeconomic AnalysisProfessor SinhaApril 13,.docx (20)

The Movement Program_Whitepaper
The Movement Program_WhitepaperThe Movement Program_Whitepaper
The Movement Program_Whitepaper
 
Journal.pone.0158753
Journal.pone.0158753Journal.pone.0158753
Journal.pone.0158753
 
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...
Value of early intervention for hearing impairment on speech and language aqu...
 
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert LensRemedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
Relations between language and executive function in spanish spea
Relations between language and executive function in spanish speaRelations between language and executive function in spanish spea
Relations between language and executive function in spanish spea
 
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docx
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docxRESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docx
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language.docx
 
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic languageRESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language
RESEARCH ARTICLEA systematic review of pragmatic language
 
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docxAssignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docx
Assignment Content1. Top of FormProfessional dispositions ha.docx
 
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...
Promoting Community Wide Engagement in Evidence-Based Prevention by Dr. Judit...
 
Summary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis PaperSummary Analysis Paper
Summary Analysis Paper
 
School Language Policies. By Tracey Tokuhama-Espinosa. Rome. April 2016
School Language Policies. By Tracey Tokuhama-Espinosa. Rome. April 2016School Language Policies. By Tracey Tokuhama-Espinosa. Rome. April 2016
School Language Policies. By Tracey Tokuhama-Espinosa. Rome. April 2016
 
Therapeutic Priority for Developing Communication in Children with Special Ed...
Therapeutic Priority for Developing Communication in Children with Special Ed...Therapeutic Priority for Developing Communication in Children with Special Ed...
Therapeutic Priority for Developing Communication in Children with Special Ed...
 
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15
Janet brennan new eng curric 12 march15
 
Letters and sounds_-_dfes-00281-2007
Letters and sounds_-_dfes-00281-2007Letters and sounds_-_dfes-00281-2007
Letters and sounds_-_dfes-00281-2007
 
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) Approach
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) ApproachLetters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) Approach
Letters and Sounds_-_DFES - Complement the Speech Sound Pics (SSP) Approach
 
Parent-Infant Speech Interactions
Parent-Infant Speech InteractionsParent-Infant Speech Interactions
Parent-Infant Speech Interactions
 
PAGE Running head SAMPLE 1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docx
PAGE  Running head SAMPLE  1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docxPAGE  Running head SAMPLE  1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docx
PAGE Running head SAMPLE 1Sample Annotated Bibliography.docx
 
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx
· Independent Design Project Literature Review and Research Log .docx
 

More from braycarissa250

1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx
1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx
1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docxbraycarissa250
 
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docxbraycarissa250
 
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docxbraycarissa250
 

More from braycarissa250 (20)

1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx
1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx
1.Does BPH predispose this patient to cancer2. Why are pati.docx
 
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx
1.Do you think that mass media mostly reflects musical taste, or.docx
 
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx
1.Discuss theoretical and conceptual frameworks. How are the.docx
 
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx
1.Discuss the medical model of corrections. Is this model of c.docx
 
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx
1.Discussion Question How do we perceive sacred spaceplace in Ame.docx
 
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx
1.Cybercriminals use many different types of malware to attack s.docx
 
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx
1.Define emotional intelligence. What are the benefits of emotional .docx
 
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx
1.Define Strategic Planning and Swot Analysis2.List and define.docx
 
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx
1.Choose a writer; indicate hisher contribution to the Harlem Renai.docx
 
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx
1.Being sure that one has the resources necessary to accomplish the .docx
 
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx
1.Based on how you will evaluate your EBP project, which indepen.docx
 
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx
1.Be organized. 2.   Spend less time doing a summary, but more o.docx
 
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx
1.After discussion with your preceptor, name one financial aspec.docx
 
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx
1.A 52-year-old obese Caucasian male presents to the clinic wit.docx
 
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx
1.1Arguments, Premises, and ConclusionsHow Logical Are You·.docx
 
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx
1.4 Participate in health care policy development to influence nursi.docx
 
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx
1.5 - 2 pages single-spaced. Use 1-inch margins, 12 font, Microsoft .docx
 
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx
1.5 Pages on the following topics Diversity, Race and Gender Equity.docx
 
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx
1.0. Introduction Effective project management is consid.docx
 
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx
1.1 What is the OSI security architecture1.2 What is the differ.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxUnboundStockton
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docxBlooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
Blooming Together_ Growing a Community Garden Worksheet.docx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 

Assignment Macroeconomic AnalysisProfessor SinhaApril 13,.docx

  • 1. Assignment: Macroeconomic Analysis Professor Sinha April 13, 2016 The assignment is due in my mailbox on May 6th, 2016 at 5pm. The mailbox is located in Lowenstein LL 924. No late submissions will be accepted. 1 Data 1. For January 1981 to February 2016, use the data on unemployment, inflation and interest rates to plot these three lines. Unemployment and inflation can be plotted on the same graph, and the interest rate will be plotted on a separate graph. 2. Plot the GDP data for January 1981 to October 2015. Please note that the GDP data is quarterly, in contrast to the above series, which are monthly. 3. Using the recession dates from NBER (link given below), discuss how the unemployment, inflation and GDP numbers changed over 2 years before a recession, and for 2 years after a recession for the three most recent recession in the U.S. For each recession, make a separate
  • 2. table with the value of these variables. You can use the values at the start of every quarter (January, April, July and October). 4. Corresponding to these series for the three most recent recession, also tabulate how the interest rate changed, two years before the start of the recession, and 2 years after it ended (also use quarterly data here). 5. How was the recession of 2007-2009 different from the previous two recessions in terms of these variables? NBER recessions: http://www.nber.org/cycles.html 2 Analyzing the Crisis 1. Read the article on the crisis posted on Blackboard. Pgs. 49- 58 provide you with some context of the crisis. For the purpose of the questions below, you will be specifically focusing on Pgs. 58-70. 1 2. Suppose the economy was at equilibrium before the start of
  • 3. the recession. Show this using the IS-LM, Keynesian cross, AD-SRAS-LRAS and money market spaces. 3. Following the start of the recession, what happened to aggregate demand and output? (You should be able to see this from the plots in section 1 above). Show this shift in AD and IS curves. 4. If the Federal Reserve and government had not intervened, explain how the economy would have adjusted back to the long-run equilibrium. 5. How could monetary policy have ensured a faster return to the long-run equilibrium? Use the money market and LM curves to show this. 6. The Federal Reserve undertook several measures to deal with the crisis. Using the section on "Policy Responses to the financial crisis" (Pg. 58), discuss two unconventional policy measures undertaken. 7. Using the discussion of fiscal policy on pgs. 63-65, discuss two fiscal policy actions that were taken. Which components of planned expenditure would be affected by these fiscal actions?
  • 4. 8. In your view, why were unconventional monetary policy actions needed? 9. Using the interest rate data plotted above, has monetary policy returned to "normal" opera- tions after the 2007 crisis, relative to the previous two recessions? 2 Research Report Integrated speech and phonological awareness intervention for pre-school children with Down syndrome Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt†, Gail Gillon‡ and Susan Foster-Cohen§ †Department of Communication Disorders, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand ‡College of Education, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand §The Champion Centre, Burwood Hospital, Christchurch, New Zealand (Received 14 October 2008; accepted 27 April 2009) Abstract Background: Children with Down syndrome experience
  • 5. difficulty with both spoken and written language acquisition, however controlled intervention studies to improve these difficulties are rare and have typically focused on improving one language domain. Aims: To investigate the effectiveness of an integrated intervention approach on the speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness development of ten pre-school children with Down syndrome aged between 4;4 and 5;5. Methods & Procedures: A multiple single-subject design was used to evaluate treatment effectiveness. Baseline and intervention measures for speech and pre- and post-intervention measures for letter knowledge and phonological awareness were compared. The intervention comprised three components: a parent-implemented home programme; centre-based speech–language therapy sessions, and ‘Learning through Computer’ sessions with a total intervention time of 20 hours over 18 weeks. Letter knowledge and phonological awareness activities were linked to each child’s speech targets. Outcomes & Results: Results indicated significant treatment effects on speech measures for all ten participants. Six of the ten participants showed increases on letter knowledge and nine showed increased awareness of initial phonemes in words but responses were not above binomial chance level (that is, 70% correct) for phonological awareness tasks. Individual results are presented and implications for parents and therapists are discussed. Conclusions and Implications: The findings of this study suggest an intervention approach that integrates speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness targets is effective in remediating speech error patterns at the single- word level in young children with Down syndrome. Phonological awareness and letter knowledge appeared to be stimulated through the intervention, but significant improvement above chance levels on untrained phonological awareness tasks was not evident. Follow-up investigation is
  • 6. necessary to determine longer-term outcomes. Keywords: Down syndrome, speech intervention, phonological awareness intervention. What this paper adds Children with Down syndrome experience difficulty with both spoken and written language acquisition, exhibiting persistent speech difficulties and poor underlying skills critical for early reading development. Intervention research to address these deficits is scarce and previous studies have typically focused on remediating one language domain. The present study advances our knowledge through investigating the effectiveness of an integrated intervention approach which aims to improve speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness development in pre-school children with Down syndrome. This study suggests an intervention which includes a letter knowledge and phonological awareness focus is effective in remediating speech errors at the single-word level in pre-school children with Down syndrome and may stimulate letter knowledge and early phonological awareness in this population. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online q 2010 Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists http://www.informahealthcare.com DOI: 10.3109/13682820903003514 Address correspondence to: Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt, Department of Communication Disorders, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand; e-mail: [email protected] INT. J. LANG. COMM. DIS., MAY–JUNE 2010, VOL. 45, NO. 3, 320–335
  • 7. Introduction Individuals with Down syndrome (DS) experience difficulty with both spoken and written language acquisition. They demonstrate poorer verbal skills compared with non-verbal skills and perform more poorly on language measures and speech production measures than do peers with other cognitive impair- ments matched for mental age (Smith and Stoel- Gammon 1983, Roberts et al. 2008). These findings lead to the conclusion that individuals with DS present with speech and language impairment in addition to cognitive impairment. Children with concurrent speech and language impairment are known to be at high risk for reading difficulties (for example, Catts et al. 2001) which suggests that reading development in children with DS may be particularly compromised. Reading achievement is a valid goal for young children with DS and is associated with superior spoken language abilities (Cardoso-Martins et al. 2008), however research investigating variables that influence early reading acquisition is limited. Recent research suggests children with DS perform poorly on phonological awareness (for example, Snowling et al. 2002) — an area critical for early reading success. Controlled intervention studies to improve spoken or written language development in children with DS are rare and have typically focused on improving only one language domain. However, given these children’s concurrent speech and language deficits and weakness in the underlying skills critical for reading, an
  • 8. integrated intervention approach that may lead to improvements in both spoken and written language is intuitively appealing. An integrated intervention aims to simultaneously facilitate the development of different skills, in this instance speech production, letter name and sound knowledge, and phonological awareness. The current study sought to investigate the impact of such an intervention on the speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness development of pre-school children with DS. Speech difficulties in children with DS An integrated intervention that will simultaneously improve speech articulation in addition to stimulating phonological awareness is important, given the well documented speech production difficulties in children with DS. Although early vocalizations and babble appear similar to those produced by their peers with typical development, phonological development and early word production are substantially delayed in children with DS (Smith and Stoel-Gammon 1983). The speech of children with DS is reported to contain the same type of error patterns and is qualitatively similar to that of typically developing younger children (Smith and Stoel-Gammon 1983), however, slower speech development in individuals with DS results in a widening gap across time. As well as delay, Dodd et al. (1994) report characteristics of phonological disorder, including significantly more atypical speech error patterns and greater inconsistency in the speech errors of individuals with DS compared with mental age-matched peers. Given the phonological speech error patterns observed
  • 9. in children with DS, a phonological approach to intervention where children’s conscious awareness of the sounds structure of the spoken word is enhanced, in addition to practice in articulating correct speech patterns, may be effective in improving the articulation of words. Interventions to enhance speech production A number of interventions have focused on physical and motor based activities to improve the speech production of children with DS. For example, in a radical approach to try to improve speech intelligibility, children with DS may undergo tongue reduction surgery. Parsons et al. (1987) compared the articulation errors of 18 children with DS pre-, post- and 6 months after tongue reduction surgery and found no significant differences across the three measures, nor between the surgery group and a non-surgery contrast group. Non-speech oral motor exercises and treatments have been recommended for children with DS to increase physical awareness and strength of the oral and facial muscles used during speech (Kumin 2006). However, Barnes et al. (2006) reported a disassociation between oral motor structure and function, with children with DS demonstrating better oral function skills than speech function skills, supporting the hypothesis that remediating oral-motor deficits will not improve speech production. Reviews of the literature investigating the use of non-speech oral motor exercises indicated that the evidence does not support their effectiveness in the treatment of speech disorders (Lof and Watson 2008), nonetheless, their use is still widely reported by clinicians working with children with speech sound disorders, including
  • 10. children with DS (Lof and Watson 2008). Thus, there is an urgent need to explore other types of interventions such as phonologically based interven- tions that may hold more promise to improving speech difficulties in children with DS. Phonological intervention approaches have been extensively used with young children with spoken language impairment in the absence of DS (Hesketh et al. 2000, Gillon 2005). However, only two phonological intervention approaches to improving Integrated intervention for children with DS 321 speech difficulties in children with DS have been documented (Cholmain 1994, Dodd et al. 1994). Cholmain’s study included six children with DS aged 4;1–5;6 with language ages of 1;3–2;8. The interven- tion was implemented via a modified cycles approach (Hodson and Paden 1991) to remediate phonological patterns, and was delivered using amplification with a focus on contrastive versus homophonic production of the targeted patterns in words, and a home-based programme of speech targets practice and joint story reading. Despite previous stability in the 3–12 months before intervention, response to the intervention was rapid with all the children demonstrating positive change in the first two weeks of the intervention. Nine children with DS aged between two and 6 years took part in the Dodd et al. (1994) study which used a core vocabulary approach to reduce non- developmental errors and variability in the children’s speech. Parents were trained to provide corrective
  • 11. feedback to their child who was required to produce a consistent pronunciation of each target word, with no non-developmental errors present. Fewer errors and inconsistencies were reported post-intervention. These two studies demonstrate a phonological intervention approach can be effective for young children with DS and that parents can play a key role in modelling target production and providing corrective feedback to improve their child’s speech. The current study extends this early work through explicitly linking goals to stimulate areas of phonological development that are critical for reading success, with goals to stimulate correct phonological production. Integrated phonological awareness intervention Integrated phonological awareness intervention which incorporated phonological awareness, letter knowledge and speech production goals has been shown to be effective for young children with speech disorders and normal cognitive ability (Gillon 2005, McNeill et al. 2009). Intervention to improve phonological awareness in children with DS has been the focus of a number of recent studies. Reported findings included improve- ment in reading skills, alphabet knowledge, and phonological awareness (van Bysterveldt et al. 2006, Goetz et al. 2008), however the effects of the interventions on speech production were not examined. Recent evidence suggests that achieving a precise phonological representation of a word is particularly demanding for children with DS ( Jarrold et al. 2009). Improving explicit awareness of the underlying representation of a spoken word and a child’s ability to consciously access this representation through phoneme awareness tasks (such as identifying the initial sound in a word or segmenting a word into
  • 12. phonemes), may improve the child’s speech production of the target word. There is increasing research into the role of phonological awareness in reading for children with DS, and a positive relationship between phonological awareness and reading for individuals with DS has been reported by researchers (Cupples and Iacono 2000, Snowling et al. 2002, Goetz et al. 2008). For example, Cupples and Iacono (2000) examined the relationship between phonological awareness and reading skills in young children with DS aged 6 to 10 years and found better phonological awareness skills were associated with better reading of both real and non-words. Roch and Jarrold (2008) compared real word and non-word reading ability in individuals with DS. They found that based on the relationship between these skills in the typically developing control group, non- word reading skills of participants with DS were poorer than would be predicted from their real word reading ability. However, reported correlation coefficients between non-word reading and composite phonologi- cal awareness scores were equivalent between the two groups. Thus, while real and non-word reading appeared differently related, non-word reading and phonological awareness skills were similarly related in both groups. Therefore, the research appears to suggest that while individuals with DS do exhibit strength in visual reading strategies relative to other reading sills, phonological awareness skills also play a role in supporting reading in this population. Letter knowledge and phonological awareness
  • 13. The importance of phonological awareness and letter knowledge in early reading and spelling acquisition is now well established, with both skills strongly predictive of later reading outcomes (Wagner et al. 1994). Letter name and sound knowledge are distinct but overlapping skills, with letter name knowledge typically preceding and predictive of letter sound knowledge and both skills associated with reading- related measures (McBride-Chang 1999). Letter knowledge and phonological awareness are also positively associated and reciprocal gains in these skills have been demonstrated when their instruction has been integrated and explicitly linked (Oudeans 2003). Joint book reading, a key element of the home literacy environment (for example, Bus et al. 1995) is an important medium through which specific early literacy skills such as phonological awareness and letter knowledge may be targeted (Justice and Ezell 2000, van Bysterveldt et al. 2006). Given the potential of phonological intervention and the important role of parents in supporting such an approach to improve speech in children with DS, 322 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. as well as the success of an integrated phonological awareness approach for children with other types of speech and language impairment, the current study sought to investigate the impact of an intervention approach which combines all these components, on the speech and phonological awareness development of pre- school children with DS. Specifically it was hypoth- esized that the experimental integrated phonological
  • 14. awareness intervention would improve the following skills: . Speech production accuracy in trained and untrained speech targets. . Letter name and letter sound knowledge. . Phonological awareness skills on untrained phoneme level tasks. Method Research design A multiple single-subject repeated measures (duplicated AB) design was used to evaluate treatment effectiveness. Participant selection process Ten children (five girls and five boys) with DS who ranged in age from 4 years 4 months to 5 years 5 months (mean ! 4 years 11 months, standard deviation (SD) ! 4.08 months) participated in the study. The participants who were from a range of socio- economic backgrounds were recruited from a group of thirteen 4- and 5-year-old children enrolled in a transition to school programme for children with DS at a specialist early intervention centre. Throughout the intervention participants attended weekly sessions at the centre, in small groups of up to six children where they received an individual programme based on the national curriculum. Each child received services from a team of six specialists/therapists which included a physiotherapist or occupational therapist, a cognitive therapist, a speech–language therapist, a music
  • 15. therapist, an early childhood teacher and a computer specialist who they saw individually and sequentially throughout the morning. Following parent consent, criteria for inclusion in the intervention study were (1) a diagnosis of Down syndrome with no known current major medical conditions or additional developmental disabilities, (2) enrolment in the centre’s transition to school clinic, (3) standard New Zealand English as a first and only language and (4) no additional speech therapy for the duration of the intervention. These criteria excluded three children from the study, one with a serious medical condition and two who intended to leave the centre before the completion of the intervention period. Procedure Standardized and experimental measures were used to assess the speech, expressive and receptive language, phonological awareness, letter knowledge and hearing of participants. Formal assessment of participants’ mental age was incompatible with the philosophy of the early intervention centre. The lead researcher (a qualified speech–language therapist) administered all the experimental tasks. Two senior student Speech– Language Therapists assisted in the administration of the standardized assessments under clinical supervision procedures. Participants received audiological assessment twice during the intervention period. Seven participants had some degree of hearing loss ranging from slight to moderate. Average hearing thresholds across all frequencies tested, both ears (if tested) and both times (if tested) ranged from 17.5 to 39.37 dB. Six of the ten
  • 16. participants wore corrective glasses and one child was scheduled to have cataract surgery at the completion of the intervention. Participants were assessed on the following measures of receptive and expressive language: Standardized assessments: . Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test — III (PPVT- III) (Dunn and Dunn 1997). This receptive vocabulary test requires the child to point to one of four pictures named by the examiner. The assessment was administered and scored according to the examiner manual. As standard score floor effects were apparent, both raw scores and standard scores are reported. . Pre-School Language Scale — Fourth Edition (Australian Language Adaptation) (PLS-4) (Zimmerman et al. 2002). This norm-referenced test assesses the receptive (Auditory Comprehen- sion) and expressive (Expressive Communication) language of children aged from birth to 6 years 11 months. The assessment was administered and scored according to the examiner manual. Standard score floor effects were apparent for the majority of children therefore a Total Language Score (TLS) presented as a language age score is also reported. Participants’ speech production was initially assessed 6 weeks before the start of the intervention using the following measures: Hodson Assessment of Phonological Patterns — Third Edition (HAPP-3) (Hodson 2004). This is a
  • 17. single word articulation test comprising 50 single and multi syllabic words elicited by naming manipulatives and line drawings. For all speech data, where a spontaneous response could not be elicited by the picture or stimulus item, a response was elicited Integrated intervention for children with DS 323 following delayed imitation. Speech data were recorded using a high-quality digital voice recording device (Belkin F8E462). All responses were transcribed via broad transcription. These samples were analysed using Computer Profiling (PROPH; Long and Fey 2005). Initial assessment data for measures of speech and language are reported in Table 1. The following measures were administered pre- and post-intervention to all ten participants: Letter knowledge and phonological awareness experimental measures . Letter name and letter sound knowledge tasks. Letter name and sound knowledge was assessed using the Gillon Preschool Phonology and Letter Knowledge probes (Gillon 2005). These probes comprise a lower-case presentation of all 26 letters presented six at a time on an A4-sized grid. The child is required to point to the letter name or sound the assessor names, for example ‘Which one says ssss?’, or ‘Show me the letter t’. To reduce the effects of a position response pattern, a correct response was credited when the appropriate letter was identified on two
  • 18. occasions. The phonological awareness and letter knowledge tasks did not require a verbal response. . Initial phoneme identity (Gillon 2005). In this task the therapist names a large colourful picture of an animal and draws the child’s attention to the first sound of that word. The child is then required to identify this as the initial sound in a word from a choice of three pictures presented and named by the examiner. Corrective feedback is given if required. The test comprised two training items and ten test items. . Initial phoneme identity with words (Gillon and McNeill 2007). In this task the therapist presents a large letter and draws the child’s attention to the sound it makes. The child is then required to identify this as the initial sound in a word and match this to one of three cards consisting of a picture with the word written underneath, presented and named by the examiner. Corrective feedback is given if required. The test comprised one training item and twelve test items. (Phonological awareness measures may be freely downloaded from: http://www.education.canterbury. ac.nz/people/gillon/resources.shtml). Intervention target selection For each participant, four speech targets for interven- tion and a control target were selected based on the initial speech assessment data (Table 2). Each speech target was then assessed with twelve speech cards to determine the percentage usage of dominant phonolo-
  • 19. gical error patterns in the target words presented. Pre- and post-intervention measures Twelve speech cards consisting of pictures with words of a similar phonological structure were prepared for each speech target (that is, 48 cards per child). Six were randomly selected to be the trained items and included in the intervention activities with the remaining six assessed only as a generalization measure. Correct production of the target phoneme in the appropriate word position was credited correct; however non- targeted phonemes in the word may have been in error. For example a child whose speech target was the correct articulation of final /p/ would be credited as correct with the production of the word ‘cup’ as /tLp/. An additional set of twelve speech cards was prepared as a control measure for each child. This set was not included in the intervention activities and was assessed only. The cards were modelled on the format used by Gillon and McNeill (2007). (Examples of these speech Table 1. Participants’ assessment data P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 P10 Child’s age 4;11 5;0 5;05 4;10 5;02 5;05 4;04 4;09 5;02 4;08 Gender F M F F M F F M M M PPVT-III RSa 42 3 36 13 18 28 19 39 28 31 PPVT-III SSb 81 40* 68 48* 53 60* 65 80* 62 73 PLS-4:ACc 66 50 50 50 50 50 60 74 51 73 PLS-4:ECd 62 50 53 50 50 55 50 79 52 77 PLS-4:TLSe 3;4 1;5 2;8* 1;8* 2;2 2;5* 2;1 3;3* 3;0 3;1 PCC-Rf 66.7 36.2* 72.1 22.4* 43.8 43.5* 38.7 76.1 53.2 53.3 PVCg 97.8 92.0* 89.1 93.3* 86.4 84.6* 100 91.5 85.2 93.5 Total wordsh 39 25 39 26 21 26 14 50 47 46
  • 20. Note. Age (years; months). P, participant. aPeabody Picture Vocabulary Test — III Raw Score. bPeabody Picture Vocabulary Test — III Standard Score. cPreschool Language Scale — 4 Auditory Comprehension Standard Score. dPreschool Language Scale — 4 Expressive Communication Standard Score. ePreschool Language Scale — 4 Total Language Score is language age in years; months. fPer cent consonants correct — Revised. gPer cent vowels correct. hTotal number of words analysed in the sample using computerized profiling (PROPH; Long and Fey 2005). *Unwillingness to participate in the assessment. 324 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. cards are freely available from: http://www.education. canterbury.ac.nz/people/gillon/resources.shtml). Participants were assessed on all their target and control speech cards (60 trials) on five occasions throughout the study. The first two administrations completed one week prior and immediately prior to the start of the intervention, comprised the final two baseline measures pre-intervention. Intervention phase assessments were completed at the end of cycle one and the start and end of cycle two. Participants’ correct scores on speech targets ranged between zero and 16.6% correct at the two pre-intervention assessments, with all participants scoring zero per cent correct on control targets pre-intervention. Intervention
  • 21. Intervention procedures The experimental integrated intervention implemented in this study comprised the following three com- ponents: . A parent-implemented home programme. Before the intervention, parents attended an information and training evening following the format detailed in van Bysterveldt et al. (2006) whereby parents were trained to use print referencing techniques at home to bring their child’s attention to targeted letters and sounds during joint story book reading. The intervention was implemented four times per week for 10 minutes per session throughout the 18 week intervention period (that is, 12 hours in total). Parents’ implementation of the print referencing tech- niques was monitored on a weekly basis during the intervention. . Speech therapy (SLT) sessions. . Learning through computer (LTC). Participants attended the early intervention centre one morning per week where they saw six different therapists in 20-minute sessions throughout the morning. In keeping with the service delivery model of the early intervention centre, the LTC sessions supported the SLT goals for the child, Table 2. Examples of participants’ dominant phonological error patterns present in target words, percentage usage and speech sound targets
  • 22. P PCC-R Examples of dominant phonological error patterns Usage (%) Target sounds selected (initial position unless stated) 1 40.8 Initial cluster reduction 84 /fl/ /gr/ /sp/ /l/ Gliding 91 2 8.3a Final consonant deletion 100 /k/ /m/ /t/ /f/ Deletion of initial consonants 28 Context sensitive voicing 50 Deletion of fricatives 75 3 45.0 Cluster reduction 64 /kr/ /sp/ /sw/ /t R / Stopping 64 4 18.9 Early stopping 100 /t/ final /k/ final /n/ Final consonant deletion 69 final /p/ Deletion of final stops 68 Deletion of final nasals 100 5 18.8 Cluster reduction 100 /l/ /f/ /sp/ /h/ Deletion of initial h 91 Liquid deletion 60 Deletion of initial fricatives 50 6 26.8 Final consonant deletion 65 /v/ final /m/ final /p/ Later stopping 67 final /d/ Deletion of final stops 67 7 11.6 Final consonant deletion 78 /p/ /n/ /t/ /b/ all final Deletion of final stops 82 Deletion of final nasals 100 8 54.3 Later stopping 95 /kr/ /tr/ /d3/ /t
  • 23. R / Cluster simplification 96 9 44.1 Later stopping 71 /l/ /v/ /t R / final /k/ Gliding 33 Liquid deletion 25 Deletion/substitution of final k 56 10 54.0 S nasal cluster reduction 100 /h/ /v/ /sn/ and/sm/ Substitutions/distortions of fricatives 50 /t R / Distortions/substitutions of affricates 58 Deletion/substitutions/distortions of initial h 60 Note: aSeverely restricted sample. Integrated intervention for children with DS 325 thus the experimental intervention was delivered during the SLT and LTC sessions. The intervention was delivered in two, 6 weekly cycles separated by a 6-week break (that is, 4 hours total SLT and 4 hours total LTC). SLT sessions characteristically included four 5-minute activities as this was compatible with the children’s attention span and ability to engage
  • 24. with the task. Sessions began with a letter knowledge activity, followed by one or two phoneme matching tasks. Each session also typically contained at least one game where letter name, letter sound and phoneme matching were integrated into the one activity. A description of the intervention activities is provided in Appendix A. The SLT sessions integrated speech goals with phonological awareness and letter knowledge goals and was implemented by the lead researcher, focusing on two speech targets per session. For example if the child’s speech target was the correct articulation of final /p/ in CVC words then the letter p was used in letter sound knowledge activities, and phoneme matching tasks included CVC words ending with /p/. The lead researcher provided a correct model and feedback during all activities. Children were encouraged to engage in tasks that required the articulation of their target speech words, letter name and letter sound knowledge tasks and phoneme matching tasks based on the structure described in Gillon and McNeill (2007) and adapted by the researcher for use with pre-school children with DS. LTC sessions comprised these phonological awareness and letter knowledge tasks adapted for presentation on a computer to pre-school children with DS. Travel issues and participants’ health influenced attendance at the centre, with participants attending an average of 75% of centre based sessions (approximately 6 hours) equating to a total mean intervention time of 18 hours 8 minutes (SD ! 1 hour 18 minutes). Reliability
  • 25. Twenty per cent of speech assessment and speech probe data were re-transcribed by an independent SLT. Point- by-point analysis showed mean agreement of 92.1% with a range of 87.7–95.0% for assessment data and mean agreement of 98.9% with a range of 83.3–100% agreement for speech card probe data. Any differences were resolved by consensus after repeated listenings. Treatment fidelity Print referencing sessions Parents were videotaped administering the print referencing techniques and received structured feedback two to four times during the intervention period. Additionally, a trained independent reviewer reviewed 23 video-taped sessions and recorded that nine of the ten parents consistently and accurately implemented the intervention. One parent who did not consistently use all three key parts of the technique during the session received extra training and corrective feedback however the implementation remained inconsistent. Parents completed a weekly report and although all parents reported abandoning sessions due to non-compliance, they all completed four 10-minute sessions of print referencing per week for the full 18-week period. Sessions were largely presented by mothers; 100% in six families, 90% in three families and 70% in one family. Seven reviewed sessions (32%) were affected by or abandoned due to child behaviours including screaming, grabbing the book, running away or refusing to listen to the story. Speech therapy sessions
  • 26. Thirty-nine randomly selected intervention sessions (just over 41% of the total sessions) which included at least two sessions from each participant were evaluated by a trained independent reviewer. A minimum of twelve productions by the researcher of each of the three intervention elements: letter name instruction, letter sound instruction, and phoneme matching was required, with 79.4% (31 sessions) adhering to the treatment fidelity protocol. Additionally the reviewer noted the child’s overall compliance and engagement within the session and the occurrence of interaction breakdowns. The reviewer identified 28 different challenging behaviours such as biting, pinching, shouting, throwing toys, hiding, damaging equipment etc. Interaction breakdowns affected 25 of the 39 sessions reviewed (just over 64%) with 90 separate challenging behavioural events recorded, ranging from one to ten per session (mean ! 2.3, SD ! 2.79) which prevented one or more elements being completed in approximately 18% (7) of the sessions. Four participants averaged less than one event per session, with the remaining six participants averaging between one and seven (mean ! 3.6, SD ! 2.96) challenging behavioural events per session. No pattern was observed between the presentation of the different activities and the occurrence of interaction breakdowns. The presence of additional visiting specialists impacted the presentation of one element in the remaining session. The wide variation in participants’ speech abilities and the impact of behaviour and motivation meant minimum target production across participants was not appropriate. Average production across participants ranged from 0.5 to 29 for letter names, from 0.3 to 22
  • 27. 326 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. for letter sounds and from five to 32 for whole words, with considerable within-participant variability across sessions. Corrective feedback, modelling or cueing by bringing the child’s attention to the visual support (that is, letter cards or speech cards) and phoneme– grapheme connections, was required from the researcher on at least 50% of occasions when the child’s letter name, letter sound or target word production was in error. All reviewed sessions adhered to the corrective feedback standard. Learning through computer sessions The computer specialist’s clinical notes from a randomly selected 32 (just over 34%) sessions were reviewed by the researcher. Sessions were required to contain at least two of the activities developed by the researcher and the computer specialist known to include letter names, letter sounds and phoneme matching. All the reviewed sessions adhered to the protocol. Interaction breakdowns which resulted in disengagement with the task were recorded in seven (just over 21%) of the reviewed sessions. Results Scoring Where the assessment was unable to be completed due to a participant’s inability to understand the task or an unwillingness to attempt or complete the task, a score of
  • 28. zero was given for that item and the next item was presented. Where participants did not respond during the speech sound assessment, stimulus cards were placed on the bottom of the pile and represented later in the assessment. A further non-response resulted in a zero score for that item. Speech production accuracy No statistical difference was found between partici- pants’ performances on trained and untrained target speech items ( p $ 0.1) therefore these data were combined for all analyses. The two standard deviation band method (Portney and Watkins 2009) was used to determine whether a statistical difference existed between scores within the baseline and intervention phases. The mean of the baseline phase and a two standard deviation band are plotted across both phases with intervention data points outside the banded area indicative of significant change (Portney and Watkins 2009). A linear regression line was also calculated depicting the line which best describes the relation- ship between speech production performance and time during the baseline phase, and providing an estimate of predicted speech performance during the intervention phase. Additional analysis was undertaken using the statistical process control (SPC) model described in Portney and Watkins (2009) to control for the contribution of time-series data to the probability of Type I error (false-positive), to verify the existence of stable baseline performances and to determine whether significant change has occurred between baseline and intervention phases. A moving range score (X-mR)
  • 29. based on the mean variability between adjacent baseline data points is calculated to determine the upper and lower control limits (UCL and LCL). Data within these control limits of common cause variation indicate a stable baseline. Data points outside these limits comprise special cause variation which is indicative of significant change. Effect sizes and confidence intervals were generated for seven participants. Three participants achieved zero scores at all points during the baseline phase, thus neither SPC analysis nor effect size analysis was appropriate. A standardized mean difference statistic was calculated using the procedure described in Shadish et al. (2008). This measure has been developed in an effort to provide a mechanism to compare the effectiveness of single-case designs with between- group designs. This procedure yielded a g-statistic of g ! 2.59 (Hedges adjustment for sample size d ! 2.48) which is comparable with those reported by Shadish et al. (2008) in their meta analysis of single-case experimental designs. The speech production gains of one child (participant 4) analysed via the two standard deviation band method and the statistical process control method are presented in Figures 1 and 2, respectively. A summary of change scores, significance and effect sizes for target speech sounds for all participants is presented in Table 3. Correct production of target sounds ranged from zero to 10.41% at pre-intervention (mean ! 2.91, SD ! 3.42) and from 20.8% to 70.8% post- intervention (mean ! 51.42, SD ! 16.54) indicat- ing gains of between 20.8% and 66.64%
  • 30. (mean ! 48.51, SD ! 15). A paired samples t-test performed on the grouped data revealed significant differences between the pre- and post-intervention scores (t(9) ! 10.02, p , 0.001). Per cent consonants correct (PCC-R) and per cent vowels correct (PVC) and percentage change for all sounds in participants’ speech target words are presented in Table 4. The PCC-R metric, which scores distortions (both clinical and non- clinical) as correct, is recommended by Shriberg et al. (1997) as the measure which is most appropriate to compare the speech of individuals with diverse speech and age profiles. Post-intervention gains ranged from Integrated intervention for children with DS 327 22.5 to 53.8 for PCC-R and from 5.3 to 46.4 for PVC. All participants scored zero per cent correct on control sounds pre-intervention and six participants remained at zero per cent correct scores post-intervention with four participants demonstrating a small improvement in the production of their control sounds post- intervention. All participants showed greater change on target sounds than control sounds, with average difference scores for target sounds ranging from 2.5 to eight out of twelve and for control sounds from zero to three out of twelve. Spearman rank-order correlations revealed no significant relationships between percentage change on target phonemes and pre-intervention measures of PPVT (r ! 20.05, p ! 0.86), hearing thresholds (r ! 0.41, p ! 0.21) or chronological age (r ! 0.18, p ! 0.58). Analysis revealed no relationship Figure 1. Baseline and intervention phase performance for
  • 31. speech sound measures for Participant 4 using 2 Standard Deviation Band analysis. Performance in the intervention phase must be above the shaded 2 standard deviation band to demonstrate significant improvement (in this example, speech sound measures pre-intervention were mean ! 2.45, SD ! 1.85). Figure 2. Baseline and intervention phase performance for speech sound measures for participant 4 using Statistical Process Control analysis. Performance in the intervention phase must be above the upper control limit (UCL) to demonstrate significant improvement (in this example the UCL ! 7.98 and the lower control limit (LCL) ! 23.0776. 328 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. between gains on speech targets and gains on letter knowledge (r ! 20.33, p ! 0.38), initial phoneme identity (IPI) (r ! 0.20, p ! 0.53) or initial phoneme identity with words (IPIW) (r ! 20.45, p ! 0.17). Letter name knowledge Letter name knowledge was assessed pre- and post- intervention with results presented in Figure 3. At pre- intervention two participants had complete or near complete letter name knowledge. Of the remaining eight participants, five showed improvement in scores at post- intervention, with large gains demonstrated by one participant. Three participants who were unable to complete the task at both testing times were assigned a score of zero. Responses included closing the assessment
  • 32. book, pointing to several letters at once and providing no response. No significant relationships were found between gain scores on letter name knowledge and pre- intervention measures of PPVT (r ! 0.03, p ! 0.88), hearing thresholds (r ! 0.33, p ! 0.38) or chronological age(r ! 20.18,p ! 0.61).Duetoceilingeffectsthedata from two participants with high scores at pre-intervention were excluded from this analysis of change scores. Spearman rank-order correlations revealed pre-interven- tionscoresofletternameknowledgewerenotsignificantly correlated with hearing thresholds (r ! 20.48, p ! 0.13) or chronological age (r ! 0.03, p ! 0.89), howeverthecorrelationwithPPVTscoresapproachedthe level of significance (r ! 0.55, p ! 0.06). Analysis also revealed significant and near significant relationships between gain scores on the letter name knowledge task and gain scores on the IPI (r ! 0.86, p , 0.001) and IPIW tasks (r ! 0.65, p ! 0.05). Letter sound knowledge Results of the pre- and post-intervention assessment of letter sound knowledge are presented in Figure 4. At pre-intervention two participants demonstrated high scores on the letter sound knowledge task with one other participant able to identify one letter sound correctly. Post-intervention two of these participants showed some increases in scores but no additional children were able to demonstrate any letter sound knowledge. Five participants at pre-intervention and four participants at post-intervention were unable to complete the task and were assigned a score of zero. Letter name knowledge was more advanced than letter sound knowledge at both testing times, and
  • 33. appeared to be prerequisite for letter sound knowledge in the three children able to demonstrate this knowledge, with no child able to demonstrate knowledge of a letter’s sound without knowledge of that letter’s name. Initial phoneme identity Pre- and post-intervention assessment results are presented in Figure 5. Three participants at pre- intervention and nine participants at post-intervention achieved scores in the Initial Phoneme Identity (IPI) Table 3. Summary of change and effect sizes in target speech sounds Effect size P Baseline Intervention 2 SD significant? SPC significant? Cohen’s d 295% CI "95% CI 1 0.52 (1.04) 13.88 (13.22) Yes Yes 1.42 20.24 3.09 2 0 14.90 (23.12) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a. 3 0 61.77 (8.66) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a. 4 2.45 (1.85) 45.11 (17.30) Yes Yes 3.46 1.11 5.82 5 1.47 (2.94) 15.26 (5.22) Yes Yes 3.25 0.98 5.51 6 1.04 (2.08) 52.07 (22.52) Yes Yes 3.19 0.94 5.43 7 3.85 (2.70) 38.88 (15.91) Yes Yes 3.42 1.05 5.75 8 0 22.77 (15.73) Yes Yes n.a. n.a. n.a. 9 11.40 (3.50) 53.11 (14.47) Yes Yes 3.96 1.40 6.52 10 4.68 (3.55) 43.74 (16.66) Yes Yes 3.24 0.97 5.50 Notes: Baseline and intervention scores are the mean and standard deviation (SD) of the baseline and intervention scores. P, participant; 2 SD significant?, significant using the two SD band method; SPC significant?, significant using the Statistical
  • 34. Process Control method; CI, confidence intervals. Table 4. PCC-R and PVC of all sounds in participants’ speech target words PCC-R PVC P Pre- Post- Difference Pre- Post- Difference 1 40.8 64.9 "24.1 83.7 91.5 "7.8 2 8.3a 62.1 "53.8 28.6a 75.0 "46.4 3 45.0 82.4 "37.4 89.7 97.9 "8.2 4 18.9 68.1 "49.2 65.2 80.4 "15.2 5 18.8 41.9 "23.1 77.8 89.7 "11.9 6 26.8 75.3 "48.5 52.4 95.0 "42.6 7 11.6 57.3 "45.7 26.5 60.0 "33.5 8 54.3 76.8 "22.5 91.1 100 "8.9 9 44.1 73.4 "29.3 68.2 93.1 "24.9 10 54.0 78.0 "24.0 82.6 97.9 "15.3 Notes: P, participant; PCC-R, per cent consonants correct — revised; PVC, per cent vowels correct. aSeverely restricted sample characterized by non-speech sounds. Integrated intervention for children with DS 329 Figure 3. Letter name knowledge pre- and post-intervention. Figure 4. Letter sound knowledge pre- and post-intervention. Figure 5. Initial phoneme identity pre- and post-intervention. 330 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al.
  • 35. task however all scores were below chance level as determined by the binomial test (Portney and Watkins 2009), with a score of 7/10 or higher required for a statistically significant result ( p , 0.05). Three partici- pants at pre-intervention and one participant at post- intervention were unable to complete the task and were assigned a score of zero. Spearman rank-order correlations revealed IPI scores were significantly correlated with PPVT scores pre-intervention (r ! 0.79, p ! 0.003) and approaching significance post-intervention (r ! 0.61, p ! 0.05). A strong negative correlation was found between IPI scores and hearing thresholds at pre-intervention (r ! 20.81, p ! 0.001) but not at post-intervention (r ! 20.08, p ! 0.81) nor with chronological age pre- (r ! 20.47, p ! 0.16) or post-intervention (r ! 20.49, p ! 0.13). No significant relationships were found between IPI gain scores and pre-intervention measures of PPVT (r ! 20.10, p ! 0.75), hearing thresholds (r ! 0.54, p ! 0.08) or chronological age (r ! 0.08, p ! 0.81). Initial phoneme identity with words Pre- and post-intervention assessment results are presented in Figure 6. Four participants at pre- intervention and eight participants at post-intervention achieved scores in the Initial Phoneme Identity with Words (IPIW) task, however all scores were below chance level as determined by the binomial test (Portney and Watkins 2009) with a score of 8/12 or higher required for a statistically significant result (p , 0.05). Four participants at pre-intervention and two participants at post-intervention were unable to
  • 36. complete the task and were assigned a score of zero. Spearman rank-order correlations revealed no significant relationships between scores on IPIW and PPVT pre- (r ! 0.07, p ! 0.83) and post- intervention (r ! 0.47, p ! 0.16), hearing thresholds pre- (r ! 0.14, p ! 0.68) and post-intervention (r ! 20.29, p ! 0.38) or chronological age pre- (r ! 20.33, p ! 0.34) and post-intervention (r ! 20.35, p ! 0.29) nor between IPIW gains scores and PPVT (r ! 0.44, p ! 0.18), hearing thresholds (r ! 20.33, p ! 0.32) or chronological age (r ! 20.16, p ! 0.63). Discussion This study used a multiple single-subject design to investigate the effectiveness of an integrated phonolo- gical awareness intervention for ten children with DS aged 4 and 5 years. The intervention aimed simultaneously to facilitate the development of speech production, letter name and sound knowledge, and phonological awareness. The intervention included the following three components: . Parent-implemented print referencing during joint story reading. . Letter knowledge and phoneme awareness activities conducted by the speech–language therapist (SLT) in a play-based format. . Letter knowledge and phoneme awareness activities conducted by the computer specialist (CS).
  • 37. The intervention was implemented by the SLT and CS at an early intervention centre during two 20-minute sessions per week, in two 6-week therapy blocks separated by a 6-week break (that is, 8 hours in total). The parents implemented the print referencing component in four 10-minute sessions per week across the 18-week intervention period (approximately 12 hours in total). The first hypothesis stated that the research intervention would improve speech production accu- racy in the trained and untrained words featuring each Figure 6. Initial phoneme identity with words pre- and post- intervention. Integrated intervention for children with DS 331 child’s target speech sounds. The results from the intervention supported this hypothesis. All ten participants demonstrated statistically significant improvement in production accuracy on both trained and untrained words. There was, however, considerable variability between the speech production skills of the participants pre- and post-intervention and in their response to intervention. This variability, however, could not be accounted for by participants’ receptive language scores, chronological age or hearing thresholds. The interven- tion appeared to be effective irrespective of whether the targeted speech errors were evident in developmentally earlier or later resolving speech error patterns or whether the participants used single speech error
  • 38. patterns or multiple speech error patterns at once. Although speech targets were in activities which also integrated letter name and sound knowledge and phoneme matching, analysis revealed no relationship between gains on speech targets and gains on letter knowledge, IPI or IPIW. The study findings indicated that significant improvement in the speech articulation of single words can be achieved after a short intervention, that is, approximately 20 hours over 18 weeks (intervention time mean ! 18 hours 8 minutes, standard deviation (SD) ! 1 hour 13 minutes) in children whose rate of speech development is slow and whose performance has remained stable over the preceding two months. The improvement in speech production by the children with DS is consistent with the findings of Cholmain (1994) and Dodd et al. (1994) who reported participants made considerable speech gains over a relatively short period of time, despite stable perform- ance pre-intervention. Grunwell (1990) described four different types of generalization of speech skills: Lexical, Socioenviron- mental, Syntactic, and Phonological. Lexical generaliz- ation refers to use of the remediated pattern in untrained words and was observed in all participants with children performing equally well on the trained and untrained words. Phonological generalization is observed when the remediated pattern is demonstrated with other phonemes in the same sound class. Participant 4 in this study provides an example of phonological generalization and use of multiple patterns. Targeted speech patterns for participant 4 included voiced/voiceless contrasts presented in words
  • 39. with initial /t/, many of which were of CVC structure. Additionally, some words presented to target final consonant deletion included initial /k/, and these were typically voiced to /g/ or /d/. Analysis across time demonstrated remediation of the final consonant deletion pattern in the articulation of single words and establishment of voicing contrasts within the same word, for example, for the target word /k ^ p/ addressing deletion of final stops, participant 4’s production progressed as follows; /d‰/ ! /d ^ p/ ! /k ^ p/ and for the target word /tæp/ addressing voicing contrasts the progression was similar /dæ/ ! /tæ/ ! / dæp/ ! /tæp/. Other improvements in participants’ speech that were not the focus of a targeted pattern, such as velar fronting in the example above, are evidenced by increased per cent consonant correct (PCC-R) and per cent vowels correct (PVC) scores from all sounds in target words. The second hypothesis was that the research intervention would improve participants’ letter name and letter sound knowledge. This hypothesis was partially supported. Letter sound knowledge proved difficult for most participants. The intervention appeared only to stimulate further letter sound knowledge in two participants who entered the study already able to demonstrate some letter sound knowl- edge. The intervention had more effect on teaching participants letter names. Six of the ten participants demonstrated that they knew more letter names at the end of the intervention compared with pre-intervention. Participants with the strongest letter knowledge at post-intervention were three of the four participants
  • 40. who had language ages of at least 3 years suggesting a stronger language foundation may facilitate the acquisition of alphabet knowledge. A pattern of learning letter names before letter sounds is consistent with findings for children without DS (McBride-Chang 1999). Analysis of the videotaped intervention sessions showed many participants were able to identify correctly one or two letter names and sounds that were the focus of their intervention session. However, these skills were evident in therapy sessions only and did not generalize to the assessment session when the activity was presented in a different context. Difficulty in transferring improvement skills from a therapy context to an assessment context is common in children with high learning needs and may suggest the knowledge has not yet consolidated or further scaffolding and increased therapy time is required (Roberts et al. 2008). The third hypothesis examined whether the research intervention would improve phonological awareness skills on untrained phoneme level tasks. The data partially supported this hypothesis. Perform- ance improved post-intervention compared with pre- intervention for most participants but their improved performance did not meet the conservative binomial level of 70% correct at post-intervention when untrained phonological awareness items were intro- duced. This suggests phonological awareness was being stimulated during the intervention period, but 332 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. participants had not reached mastery of identifying
  • 41. initial sounds in words and therefore could not demonstrate the transference of knowledge to novel items. Phonological awareness in young children typically proceeds along a developmental continuum of increas- ing ability and stability (Lonigan et al. 1998) throughout the preschool years. Lonigan et al. (1998) reported some measurable, though inconsistent, phonological awareness skills in children as young as 2 or 3 years of age and more consolidated phonological awareness skills in children aged 4 years and over. Participants’ receptive language abilities, as determined by their PPVT scores, were associated with both pre- and post-intervention measures of letter name and sound knowledge and IPI, but not with change scores. However, gain scores on letter name knowledge and both phonological awareness tasks were related, which suggests that the presentation of these activities in an integrated format may facilitate the reciprocal gains reported by Oudeans (2003). Consistent with the findings of Lonigan et al. (1998) it is plausible that some participants in the current study did demonstrate a pattern of emerging but unstable phonological awareness. Follow-up assessment of letter knowledge and phonological awareness skills is necessary to investigate any potential longer-term influence of the intervention on these skills. Follow-up assessment of speech measures is also required to determine whether treatment gains were maintained. Clinical implications Speech production errors are common in children with DS and these errors often persist into adulthood. However, empirically based interventions to improve
  • 42. speech production in this population are rare. Children with DS also exhibit weakness in the underlying skills of phonological awareness and letter knowledge tasks that are critical for early reading success. The findings of this study suggest an intervention approach which integrates speech, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness targets was effective in remediating speech errors for pre-school children with DS and may potentially stimulate letter knowledge and phonological awareness skills in the future. Speech gains were demonstrated by all children regardless of language age, however the greatest gains on letter knowledge and phonological awareness were demonstrated by children who had language ages of at least 3 years. The relationships between gains in letter knowledge and phonological awareness support the appropriateness of an approach in which instruction in these skills is integrated. In light of the persistent nature of speech difficulties in individuals with DS and the superior language abilities associated with reading in this population, evidence based interventions which can combine several treatment goals may provide a valuable alternative to traditional therapy techniques which aim to improve only one language domain. Acknowledgements Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper. References
  • 43. BARNES, E., ROBERTS, J., MIRRETT, P., SIDERIS, J. and MISENHEIMER, J., 2006, A comparison of oral structure and oral-motor function in young males with Fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49, 903–917. BUS, A. G., VANIJZENDOORN, M. H. and PELLEGRINI, A. D., 1995, Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta- analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21. CARDOSO-MARTINS, C., PETERSON, R., OLSON, R. and PENNINGTON, B., 2008, Component reading skills in Down syndrome. Reading and Writing, 21, 277–292. CATTS, H., FEY, M., ZHANG, X. and TOMBLIN, B., 2001, Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: a research-based model and its clinical implementation. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38–51. CHOLMAIN, C. N., 1994, Working on phonology with young children with Down syndrome — a pilot study. Journal of Clinical Speech and Language Studies, 1, 14–35. CUPPLES, L. and IACONO, T., 2000, Phonological awareness and oral reading skill in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 595. DODD, B., MCCORMACK, P. and WOODYATT, G., 1994, Evaluation of an intervention program: Relation between children’s
  • 44. phonology and parents’ communicative behavior. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 98, 632–645. DUNN, L. M. and DUNN, L. M., 1997, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test — III (Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service). GIBBON, F. and GRUNWELL, P., 1990, Specific developmental language learning disabilities. In P. GRUNWELL (ed), Developmental speech disorders:clinical issues and practical implications (Edinborough; New York: Churchill Livingstone), 135–161. GILLON, G., 2005, Facilitating phoneme awareness development in 3- and 4–year-old children with speech impairment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 308–324. GILLON, G. T. and MCNEILL, B. C., 2007, Integrated Phonological Awareness: An Intervention Program for Preschool Children with Speech–Language Impairment (Christchurch: Depart- ment of Communication Disorders, University of Canter- bury) (available at: http://www.education.canterbury.ac.nz/ people/gillon/resources.shtml). GOETZ, K., HULME, C., BRIGSTOCKE, S., CARROLL, J. M., NASIR, L. and SNOWLING, M., 2008, Training reading and phoneme awareness skills in children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing, 21, 395–412. HESKETH, A., ADAMS, C., NIGHTINGALE, C. and HALL, R., 2000, Phonological awareness therapy and articulatory training
  • 45. approaches for children with phonological disorders: Integrated intervention for children with DS 333 A comparative study. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 337–354. HODSON, B. and PADEN, E., 1991, Targeting intelligible speech: A phonological approach to remediation, 2nd edn (Austin, TX: Pro-Ed). HODSON, B., 2004, Hodson Assessment of Phonological Patterns — Third Edition HAPP-3 (Austin, TX: PRO-ED). JARROLD, C., THORN, A. and STEPHENS, E., 2009, The relationships among verbal-short term memory, phonological awareness, and new word learning: Evidence from typical development and Down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 196–218. JUSTICE, L. M. and EZELL, H. K., 2000, Enhancing children’s print and word awareness through home-based parent interven- tion. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 257–269. KUMIN, L., 2006, Speech intelligibility and childhood verbal apraxia of speech in children with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 10, 10–22.
  • 46. LOF, G. L. and WATSON, M. M., 2008, A nationwide survey of nonspeech oral motor exercise use: implications for evidence- based practice. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 392–407. LONG, S. and FEY, M. E., 2005, Computerized Profiling (Version 9.7.0) (Cleveland, OH: Case Western Reserve University). LONIGAN, C. J., BURGESS, S. R., ANTHONY, J. L. and BARKER, T. A., 1998, Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294–311. MCBRIDE-CHANG, C., 1999, The ABCs of the ABCs: the development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308. MCNEILL, B. C., GILLON, G. T. and DODD, B., in press, 2009, The effectiveness of an integrated phonological awareness approach for children with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). Child Language Teaching and Therapy. OUDEANS, M. K., 2003, Integration of letter-sound correspondences and phonological awareness skills of blending and segment- ing: a pilot study examining the effects of instructional sequence on word reading for kindergarten children with low phonological awareness. Learning Disability Quarterly, 26, 258–308. PARSONS, C. L., IACONO, T. A. and ROZNER, L., 1987, Effect of tongue reduction on articulation in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 328–332.
  • 47. PORTNEY, L. G. and WATKINS, M. P., 2009, Foundations of Clinical Research: Applications to Practice (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education). ROBERTS, J. E., CHAPMAN, R. S., MARTIN, G. E. and MOSKOWITZ, L., 2008, Language of preschool and school-age children with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. (pp 77–115) In J. E. Roberts, R. S. Chapman and S. F. Warren (eds), Speech and Language Development and Intervention in Down Syndrome and Fragile X Syndrome (Baltimore, MA: Paul H. Brookes). ROCH, M. and JARROLD, C., 2008, A comparison between word and nonword reading in Down syndrome: the role of phonological awareness. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 305–318. SHADISH, W. R., RINDSKOPF, D. M. and HEDGES, L. V., 2008, The state of the science in the meta-analysis of single-case experimental designs. Evidence-Based Communication Assessment and Intervention, 2, 188–196. SHRIBERG, L. D., AUSTIN, D., LEWIS, B. A., MCSWEENEY, J. L. and WILSON, D. L., 1997, The percentage of consonants correct (PCC) metric: extensions and reliability data. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 708–722. SMITH, B. L. and STOEL-GAMMON, C., 1983, A longitudinal study of the development of stop consonant production in normal
  • 48. and Down’s syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 114–118. SNOWLING, M. J., HULME, C. and MERCER, R. C., 2002, A deficit in rime awareness in children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing, 15, 471–495. VAN BYSTERVELDT, A. K., GILLON, G. T. and MORAN, C., 2006, Enhancing phonological awareness and letter knowledge in preschool children with Down syndrome. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53, 301–329. WAGNER, R. K., TORGESEN, J. K. and RASHOTTE, C. A., 1994, The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: new evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87. ZIMMERMAN, I. L., STEINER, V. G. and POND, R. E., 2002, Preschool Language Scale — Fourth Edition — Australian Language Adaptation (PLS-4) (San Antonio, TX: Psychological). 334 Anne Katherine van Bysterveldt et al. Appendix A: Letter name and sound knowledge example The speech-language therapist (SLT) placed poster-sized cards of the two targets being targeted (for example, l and k) on the floor
  • 49. and hid six to ten small cards of each letter around the room in obvious locations, for example, under a cushion for a ‘hide and seek’- type format and the SLT and child took turns to turn them over. The SLT said the letter name and sound and the child matched it to the corresponding large letter card. The child was encouraged to say the letter name and sound, with any production reinforced and corrected by the SLT. The child then placed a mechanical toy on the large letter card and activated it to jump: SLT: What have you found? SLT: Ooh, you’ve found t. SLT: t says /t/. Can you see t, /t/ anywhere? Child: There. (child points) SLT: Oh, good matching, you found . . . ? (prompting for the target letter name) Child: t. SLT: You’re right, and it says . . . ? (prompting for the target letter sound) Child: /t/. SLT: Well done, you matched t, /t/. Child places small letter card onto corresponding large
  • 50. letter. SLT: Let’s bounce Tigger on t. Phoneme identity and matching example The SLT placed poster-sized cards of the two targets being taught (for example, l and k) on the floor and two poster-sized ‘bingo’- type boards on the floor beside the corresponding letter target. The bingo boards displayed six target words with the word written underneath each picture in large bold font. An identical set was cut up for the child to match. For example, where the target was to address final consonant deletion of /k/, the bingo board included the words sock, hook, work, beak, wink, and book. These were placed face down and the SLT and child took turns to turn them over. If the child spontaneously said the word, this was reinforced by the SLT. Corrective feedback was provided if required including drawing the child’s attention to the written text. Where the child did not spontaneously say the word, it was said by the therapist and the child matched it to the corresponding large letter. Once the words had been matched to the target letter they were matched to the corresponding picture on the bingo board. SLT: Let’s see what we’ve got. You choose one. Child: (child chooses a card and names it) tap.
  • 51. SLT: You’re right, that says tap and there’s the letter t that says /t/. (points to the text). SLT: See, it starts with t. (points to the large letter). SLT: Let’s check, is it the same? (compares to large letter). Child: Yes. SLT: You’re right, now we need to find the other picture of . . . ? (prompts child to respond) Child: tap. SLT: You’re right, tap. Child: (places card on board) tap. SLT: Good matching. For some children, two bingo boards were too challenging or distracting so the activity was modified to present one target at a time. Combined activity SLT placed a poster-sized sheet on the floor with a racetrack circuit on it. The track was divided into squares on which were placed six to ten small letter cards for each of the two target letters/sounds. Two piles of word cards, each pile comprising the six target words for
  • 52. each sound, were placed in the middle of the track. The child, parent and SLT took turns throwing dice and moving a toy car around the track. When a person landed on a small letter card a corresponding word card was chosen. Parents were encouraged to provide feedback to the child following the SLT’s model: SLT: Your turn to throw the dice. Child throws dice. SLT: Oh you got a . . . ? Child: Three. SLT: Great, let’s count, one, two, three. You landed on . . . ? (prompting for the target letter name) Child: l. SLT: You did, you landed on l, and it makes the /l/ sound. Now choose a word that starts with l. Which pile is it on? Child: (chooses a card) lamp. SLT: Good talking, you chose lamp. It starts with l that makes the /l/ sound. Let’s say it together; lamp. Show Mum what you got. Child: lamp. Mum: What does it start with? Child: l.
  • 53. Mum: Yes, l says /l/. Good job! Integrated intervention for children with DS 335