SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 24
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416689563
Current Directions in Psychological
Science
2017, Vol. 26(2) 126 –131
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0963721416689563
www.psychologicalscience.org/CDPS
People routinely navigate choices with important conse-
quences for their physical health. They go to the gym,
smoke a cigarette with friends, work on their tan, order a
salad instead of a double bacon cheeseburger, and get
that recommended colonoscopy. Concerns about death
would seem relevant to these and other everyday health
decisions. Yet it was not until the terror management
health model (TMHM) that this relevance was program-
matically considered (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008). The
model was born, in fact, from observations of reciprocal
neglect. Health psychology largely ignored motivational
implications associated with awareness of mortality,
whereas terror management theory, despite inspiring
considerable research about social psychological conse-
quences of this awareness, was relatively silent about
decisions with implications for physical health. In the
present article, we consider how the TMHM bridges this
intersection of health and death. We articulate the theo-
retical foundation for the model and then highlight
insights it has generated for how health-relevant contexts
implicate awareness of mortality, how this awareness
affects everyday health decisions, and how it can be used
to foster better health outcomes.
The Genesis of the Terror Management
Health Model
Terror management theory (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, &
Solomon, 1986) builds from a tradition of existential and
psychodynamic theory (e.g., Becker, 1973) to posit that
people need to psychologically manage the unsettling
implications of knowing not just that death is inevitable
but that it could happen at any time. They do this by
identifying with cultural belief systems (i.e., worldviews),
which enable people to view themselves as valuable
members (reflecting self-esteem) of a cultural reality that
persists beyond their own physical demise. The theory
has inspired hundreds of studies around the globe and
been applied to an array of human social behaviors (see,
e.g., Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Greenberg, 2015).
After the initial wave of research on terror manage-
ment theory, studies increasingly suggested that people
689563CDPXXX10.1177/0963721416689563Arndt,
GoldenbergHealth and Death
research-article2017
Corresponding Author:
Jamie Arndt, McAlester Hall, Department of Psychological
Sciences,
University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65201
E-mail: [email protected]
Where Health and Death Intersect:
Insights From a Terror Management
Health Model
Jamie Arndt1 and Jamie L. Goldenberg2
1Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri,
and 2Department of Psychology,
University of South Florida
Abstract
This article offers an integrative understanding of the
intersection between health and death from the perspective of
the terror management health model. After highlighting the
potential for health-related situations to elicit concerns
about mortality, we turn to the question, how do thoughts of
death influence health-related decision making? Across
varied health domains, the answer depends on whether these
cognitions are in conscious awareness or not. When
mortality concerns are conscious, people form healthy
intentions and engage in healthy behavior if efficacy and
coping resources are present. In contrast, when contending with
accessible but nonconscious thoughts of death,
health-relevant decisions are guided more by the implications of
the behavior for the individual’s sense of cultural
value. Finally, we present research suggesting how these
processes can be leveraged to facilitate health promotion
and reduce health risk
Keywords
health, decision making, risky behavior, terror management,
death, mortality salience
http://www.psychologicalscience.org/cdps
http://sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F09637214
16689563&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-04-06
Health and Death 127
defend against conscious and nonconscious awareness
of mortality in different ways (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, &
Solomon, 1999). When thoughts of mortality are con-
scious, people try to remove them from focal attention.
(After all, ending up as fertilizer is a thought on which
people generally don’t like to dwell.) Such proximal
defenses push death-related thought to the mental back-
ground. It is when thoughts of death are active but out-
side of conscious awareness that people more strongly
engage in distal defenses that address the problem of
death on an abstract and symbolic level. For example,
people cling more vigorously to their cultural beliefs (i.e.,
worldview defense) and try harder to live up to cultural
standards (i.e., self-esteem striving). Proximal and distal
defenses are often inferred in experimental research by
measuring outcomes immediately after a mortality
reminder or after a delay, respectively. Conceptual and
meta-analytic reviews (i.e., statistical approaches that
average across different studies) have supported the
unique time course of death-thought activation and the
distinct effects elicited (e.g., Steinman & Updegraff, 2015).
The TMHM (Fig. 1) builds from these ideas. It begins
with the assumption that health conditions have varying
potential to make people think about death. The model
then integrates insights about how people manage con-
scious and nonconscious death-related cognitions with
the recognition that health decisions can be influenced
by concerns central (proximal) and more tangential (dis-
tal) to the health context. The foundational idea is that
when mortality concerns are conscious, health decisions
are largely guided by the proximal motivational goal of
reducing perceived vulnerability to a health threat and
thus concerns about mortality. In contrast, when mortal-
ity cognition is active but outside of focal attention,
health-relevant decisions are guided by distal motiva-
tional goals concerning the symbolic value of the self.
TMHM Research
The link between health and death
Every time people undergo routine cancer screenings,
there is the possibility that what they discover could mark
the beginning of the end. It is perhaps not surprising that
over 60% of people in a population-level survey reported
that when they think of cancer, they automatically think
of death (Moser et al., 2014). Even presentations of the
word “cancer” that participants report not having seen
(i.e., subliminal primes) increase the cognitive availability
of death-related thought (Arndt, Cook, Goldenberg, &
Cox, 2007). Performing breast self-exams (among women;
Goldenberg, Arndt, Hart, & Routledge, 2008) or reading
about risks of cancer from smoking (Hansen, Winzeler, &
Topolinski, 2010) or unprotected sun exposure (Cooper,
Goldenberg, & Arndt, 2014) also makes thoughts about
death accessible. But cancer is just one of many health
domains sharing this connection. Appeals about binge
drinking ( Jessop & Wade, 2008) or risky sex (Grover,
Miller, Solomon, Webster, & Saucier, 2010) and even
insurance advertisements (Fransen, Fennis, Pruyn, & Das,
Health Scenarios/Threats
Conscious Death-
Thought Activation
Motivation: Reduce
Vulnerability/
Awareness of Death
Health-Behavior-
Oriented Outcomes
Health-Defeating
Outcomes
Health-Facilitating
Outcomes
Threat-Avoidance
Outcomes
Motivation: Bolster Meaning
and Symbolic Self-Conception
Nonconscious Death-
Thought Activation
Fig. 1. The terror management health model.
128 Arndt, Goldenberg
2008) also activate thoughts of death. Such findings
prompt the critical question, how do cognitions about
mortality influence health-related decision making and
behavior? Across domains such as tanning, smoking, can-
cer screening, nutrition, and fitness, the answer often
depends on whether thoughts of death are in conscious
awareness or not.
The proximal and distal health
implications of mortality salience
Routledge, Arndt, and Goldenberg’s (2004) studies on sun
protection provide an illustration of divergent health-
relevant responses to conscious and nonconscious death-
related thought. Women reported greater interest in sun
protection immediately after answering two short ques-
tions about their mortality (vs. a control topic), presum-
ably because it would reduce their vulnerability to a
health risk. However, after a delay, they indicated stronger
interest in tanning, in line with appearance-based esteem
contingencies assessed as part of the experiment.
McCabe, Vail, Arndt, and Goldenberg’s (2014) studies
on product endorsement provide another illustration.
One study featured an ostensible taste test. Participants
sampled a brand of bottled water purportedly endorsed
by a medical doctor (to appeal to health) or a popular
celebrity (to appeal to social status). Immediately after
reminders of mortality, participants drank more of the
water if it had been endorsed by a medical doctor,
whereas after a delay, they drank more of the celebrity-
endorsed water. Such effects highlight the distinction
between health and esteem motivations that follow from
conscious and nonconscious thoughts of death.
Because people are motivated to reduce vulnerability
to health concerns when consciously thinking about
death, explicit thoughts of mortality render health-
promoting (proximal) responses such as exercising more,
using sun protection, and undergoing a screening exam
more likely when people have sufficient coping resources,
optimism, or beliefs in the efficacy of the behavior (and
themselves) to effectively mitigate the health concern.
When lacking these resources, people may respond to
conscious thoughts of death by avoiding or denying the
health threat (e.g., Cooper, Goldenberg, & Arndt, 2010).
Thus, the effect of conscious concerns about mortality on
health decisions depends on factors of immediate rele-
vance to the health context, much as has been found in
research based on rationally oriented models of health
behavior (e.g., Prentice-Dunn & Rogers, 1986).
In contrast, the relevance of the behavior for esteem
and cultural identification often directs health decisions
once thoughts of death are no longer conscious. For
example, when distracted from mortality reminders, indi-
viduals who derive self-esteem from fitness increase
exercise intentions (Arndt, Schimel, & Goldenberg, 2003),
whereas those who derive self-esteem from smoking
report less interest in quitting (Hansen et al., 2010). These
findings mesh well with evidence that self-esteem and
self-presentational motives influence health-related deci-
sion making (e.g., Leary, Tchividijian, & Kraxberger, 1994;
Mahler, Kulik, Gibbons, Gerrard, & Harrell, 2003) but
extend it by demonstrating the role of mortality concerns
in these processes. The TMHM further suggests that
worldview beliefs function similarly. Consider, for
instance, people subscribing to a fundamentalist religious
worldview. Terror management processes may play a
role in their willingness to rely on faith alone for medical
treatment (Vess, Arndt, Cox, Routledge, & Goldenberg,
2009). Taken together, this work helps to delineate when
health decisions will be influenced by factors tangential
to the health context and why people sometimes do the
seemingly irrational things they do when it comes to tak-
ing care of their health.
Leveraging the Terror Management
Health Model to Improve Health
Decisions
The implications of the TMHM framework invite consid-
eration of a number of different ways to improve health-
related decision making. Indeed, research has begun to
examine how death-related cognition can be used as a
motivational catalyst to facilitate health promotion and
reduce health risk.
Augmenting conventional approaches
to health-related cognition
One research direction involves using conscious death-
related thought to bolster the influence of conventional
approaches to health-related cognition. For example,
Cooper et al. (2014) presented beachgoers with health
communications that did or did not highlight the risk of
death from skin cancer and did or did not elaborate on
the efficacy of sun protection to mitigate these risks.
When appeals emphasized sun-protection efficacy and
raised the conscious risk of mortality, sun-protection
intentions were greater. Such findings offer promise for
using explicit mortality concerns to augment educational
health campaigns that incorporate fear-related messages.
Notably, there are differing views about the potential
of the TMHM to inform research on the use of persuasive
fear messages (see Hunt & Shehryar, 2011; Tannenbaum
et al., 2015). When considering this potential, it is impor -
tant to recognize that appeals need not explicitly men-
tion death to conjure up death-related cognition;
implicating serious health consequences can do so as
Health and Death 129
well. Further, whether people are actively thinking about
death is an important issue for evaluating whether the
appeal encourages health- or esteem-based responses
and necessitates careful attention. Fine-grained measure-
ment of this issue may be necessary, although it is likely
challenging in the context of much health-communica-
tion research. But carefully considering the source of fear
and the potential for health communications to activate
conscious or nonconscious death-related thought may
help to illuminate when and why such appeals are effec-
tive, when they fall flat, and when they backfire (Ruiter,
Kessels, Peters, & Kok, 2014).
Targeting malleable bases of cultural
value
The TMHM suggests that when mortality concerns are
active but not conscious, efforts to change health behav-
ior may benefit from targeting malleable bases of cultural
value. For example, when smokers viewed a public ser-
vice announcement concerning the social consequences
of smoking (e.g., “Who wants to date someone with bad
breath?”), participants reminded of mortality reported
increased intentions to quit (Arndt et al., 2009; see also
Wong, Nisbett, & Harvell, 2017). Conveying positive
social norms can also be useful in this regard. Grocery
store patrons were reminded of mortality or a control
topic; then, based on research from the prototype-will-
ingness model (Gibbons & Gerrard, 1995), they visual-
ized exemplars of healthy eaters or did not. As determined
from their shopping receipts, those who were primed
with mortality and visualized healthy eaters purchased
healthier foods (McCabe et al., 2015).
The utility of targeting how individuals derive a sense
of value in conjunction with mortality reminders also
shows promise in the context of safe sun behavior. The
guiding idea is that if people can be steered away from
thinking of tanned skin as attractive, subtle primes of
mortality might lead to more interest in sun protection.
Using such an approach, Cox et al. (2009) observed
requests for sunscreen samples with higher SPF among
(Caucasian) beachgoers. Furthermore, framing a UV pho-
tograph of participants’ faces as revealing damaging
effects on appearance, rather than health, interacted with
mortality reminders to lead participants to take more
samples of sunscreen and report greater intentions to use
it (Morris, Cooper, Goldenberg, Arndt, & Gibbons, 2014).
Thus, there seems to be potential for nonconscious
thoughts of mortality to engage healthier behavioral
practices if aspects of social value are targeted.
Recognizing the body problem
The TMHM also fosters recognition of underappreciated
barriers to promoting health behavior. Goldenberg,
McCoy, Pyszczynski, Greenberg, and Solomon (2000)
suggested that the physicality of the body undermines
people’s capacity to maintain the symbolic, cultural value
of the self as a means to manage concerns associated
with mortality. This helps illuminate when and why peo-
ple may avoid health behaviors that involve intimate con-
frontation with the body’s physicality or creatureliness
(e.g., breast self-exams and mammograms; Goldenberg
et al., 2008). That these behaviors are threatening not
only because of their health implications (i.e., what one
might find) but also because of a non-health-related
threat suggests that, like other distal defenses, highlight-
ing the symbolic aspects of the self may benefit efforts to
foster health behavior. Opportunities to affirm symbolic
representations of the body may be effective when health
contexts elicit both mortality concerns and discomfort
with the body’s physicality (Morris, Cooper, Goldenberg,
Arndt, & Routledge, 2013).
The potential for behavioral
durability
An important question is whether the effects observed in
TMHM research are just a brief blip on the behavioral -
change radar. Concerns about inevitable mortality are an
ever-present condition with which people must contend,
and moreover, people are reminded of mortality—some-
times blatantly and sometimes subtly—on a routine basis.
Two recent studies provided initial insight as to how an
enduring influence of awareness of death may affect
health behavior as it unfolds over time.
In Morris, Goldenberg, Arndt, and McCabe (2016),
when participants were primed with mortality and rode
an exercise bike, they later reported exercising more in
the 2 weeks that followed than did participants who were
not reminded of mortality, and this led them to report
basing their self-esteem more on fitness. In a second
study, smokers who visualized a prototypical unhealthy
smoker after being reminded of mortality reported more
attempts to quit smoking in the following 3 weeks and
became more committed to an identity as a nonsmoker,
and this in turn inspired continued attempts over the next
3 weeks. These studies lay the groundwork for a longitu-
dinal model in which death-related thought encourages
identity-relevant behavior, the behavior fosters more
identity relevance, and this in turn promotes more of the
(healthy) behavior.
Becoming comfortably numb
Research has also begun to examine other processes
through which death-related cognition might influence
health-relevant choices. For example, perhaps because of
the potential for anxiety involved, death reminders can
motivate people to become “comfortably numb” (to
130 Arndt, Goldenberg
borrow from a colleague who borrowed from Pink Floyd)
and increase desire for intoxicants like marijuana (Nagar
& Rabinovitz, 2015) and purchasing and consumption of
alcohol (Ein-Dor et al., 2014). Such risky behavior may be
most likely for those who lack secure terror management
buffers. Indeed, nightclub patrons with low self-esteem
drank more alcohol (as indicated by breathalyzer analy-
sis) when primed with mortality reminders (Wisman,
Heflick, & Goldenberg, 2015).
Conclusion
The TMHM integrates research on existential motivation,
self-threats and psychological defense, risky behavior,
fear appeals, and vulnerability, esteem, and normative
factors influencing health-related decision making. Like
other applied theoretical research, research guided by
the TMHM enriches our understanding of the target
domain as well as the basic theory. Although additional
research is needed in the areas outlined above, the model
offers a foundation for understanding how people man-
age existential insecurity as well as harnessing the effects
of death-related thought to engage productive health-
behavior change.
Recommended Reading
Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). (See References). A theo-
retical review article introducing the TMHM.
Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). (See
References). A recent comprehensive review of terror man-
agement theory research for those interested in the differ -
ent directions of research inspired by the theory.
Spina, M., Arndt, J., Boyd, P., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2016).
Bridging health and death: Insights and questions from
a terror management health model. In L. A. Harvell &
G. S. Nisbett (Eds.), Denying death: An interdisciplinary
approach to Terror Management Theory (pp. 47–61). New
York, NY: Routledge. A recent review of TMHM research.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
Most of the research reviewed here that involved Jamie Arndt
and Jamie Goldenberg was supported by National Cancer Insti-
tute Grant R01CA096581.
References
Arndt, J., Cook, A., Goldenberg, J. L., & Cox, C. R. (2007).
Cancer and the threat of death: The cognitive dynamics
of death thought suppression and its impact on behav-
ioral health intentions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 92, 12–29.
Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Goldenberg, J. L., Vess, M., Routledge,
C., & Cohen, F. (2009). Blowing in the (social) wind:
Implications of extrinsic esteem contingencies for terror
management and health. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 96, 1191–1205.
Arndt, J., Schimel, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2003). Death can be
good for your health: Fitness intentions as proximal and
distal defense against mortality salience. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 33, 1726–1746.
Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York, NY: The
Free Press.
Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2010).
Examination
of the terror management health model: The interac-
tive effect of conscious death thought and health-coping
variables on decisions in potentially fatal health domains.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 937–946.
Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2014). Perceived
efficacy, conscious fear of death, and intentions to tan: Not
all fear appeals are created equal. British Journal of Health
Psychology, 19, 1–15.
Cox, C. R., Cooper, D. P., Vess, M., Arndt, J., Goldenberg, J.
L.,
& Routledge, C. (2009). Bronze is beautiful but pale can be
pretty: The effects of appearance standards and mortality
salience on sun-tanning outcomes. Health Psychology, 28,
746–752.
Ein-Dor, T., Hirschberger, G., Perry, A., Levin, N., Cohen, R.,
Horesh,
H., & Rothschild, E. (2014). Implicit death primes increase
alco-
hol consumption. Health Psychology, 33, 748–751.
Fransen, M. L., Fennis, B. M., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Das, E.
(2008).
Rest in peace? Brand-induced mortality salience and con-
sumer behavior. Journal of Business Research, 61, 1053–1061.
Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Predicting young adults’
health risk behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69, 505–517.
Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). The implications of death
for health: A terror management health model for behav-
ioral health promotion. Psychological Review, 115, 1032–
1053.
Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., Hart, J., & Routledge, C. (2008).
Uncovering an existential barrier to breast cancer screening.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 260–274.
Goldenberg, J. L., McCoy, S. K., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg,
J.,
& Solomon, S. (2000). The body as a source of self-esteem:
The effect of mortality salience on identification with one’s
body, interest in sex, and appearance monitoring. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 118–130.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The
causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A ter-
ror management theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public
self and private self (pp. 189–212). New York, NY: Springer.
Grover, K. W., Miller, C. T., Solomon, S., Webster, R. J., &
Saucier, D. A. (2010). Mortality salience and perceptions
of people with AIDS: Understanding the role of prejudice.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32, 315–327.
Hansen, J., Winzeler, S., & Topolinski, S. (2010). When the
death makes you smoke: A terror management perspective
on the effectiveness of cigarette on-pack warnings. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 226–228.
Health and Death 131
Hunt, D. M., & Shehryar, O. (2011). Integrating terror manage-
ment theory into fear appeal research. Social & Personality
Psychology Compass, 5, 372–382.
Jessop, D. C., & Wade, J. (2008). Fear appeals and binge
drinking: A terror management theory perspective. British
Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 773–788.
Leary, M. R., Tchividijian, L. R., & Kraxberger, B. E. (1994).
Self-
presentation can be hazardous to your health: Impression
management and health risk. Health Psychology, 13,
461–470.
Mahler, H. I., Kulik, J. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., &
Harrell,
J. (2003). Effects of appearance-based intervention on sun
protection intentions and self-reported behaviors. Health
Psychology, 22, 199–209.
McCabe, S., Arndt, J., Goldenberg, J. L., Vess, M., Vail, K. E.,
III,
Gibbons, F. X., & Rogers, R. (2015). The effect of visualizing
healthy eaters and mortality reminders on nutritious grocery
purchases: An integrative terror management and prototype
willingness analysis. Health Psychology, 34, 279–282.
McCabe, S., Vail, K. E., III, Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L.
(2014). Hails from the crypt: A terror management health
model investigation of health and celebrity endorsements.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 289–300.
Morris, K. L., Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., &
Gibbons, F. X. (2014). Improving the efficacy of appear -
ance-based sun exposure intervention with the terror man-
agement health model. Psychology & Health, 29, 1245–1264.
Morris, K. L., Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., &
Routledge, C. (2013). Objectification as self-affirmation
in the context of a death-relevant health threat. Self and
Identity, 12, 610–620.
Morris, K. L., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., & McCabe, S.
(2016).
The enduring influence of death on health: Insight from the
terror management health model. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Moser, R. P., Arndt, J., Han, P., Waters, E., Amsellem, M., &
Hesse, B. (2014). Perceptions of cancer as a death sen-
tence: Prevalence and consequences. Journal of Health
Psychology, 19, 1518–1524.
Nagar, M., & Rabinovitz, S. (2015). Smoke your troubles away:
Exploring the effects of death cognitions on cannabis
craving and consumption. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs,
47, 91–99.
Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (1986). Protection motiva-
tion theory and preventive health: Beyond the health belief
model. Health Education Research, 1, 153–161.
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual -
process model of defense against conscious and uncon-
scious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror
management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845.
Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty
years of terror management theory: From genesis to revela-
tion. In M. Zanna & J. Olsen (Eds.), Advances in experi-
mental social psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 1–70). Cambridge,
MA: Academic Press.
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2004). A time
to tan: Proximal and distal effects of mortality salience on
sun exposure intentions. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 30, 1347–1358.
Ruiter, R. A., Kessels, L. T., Peters, G. J. Y., & Kok, G. (2014).
Sixty years of fear appeal research: Current state of the
evidence. International Journal of Psychology, 49, 63–70.
Steinman, C. T., & Updegraff, J. A. (2015). Delay and death-
thought accessibility: A meta-analysis. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1682–1696.
Tannenbaum, M. B., Hepler, J., Zimmerman, R. S., Saul, L.,
Jacobs, S., Wilson, K., & Albarracín, D. (2015). Appealing
to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and
theories. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1178–1204.
Vess, M., Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Routledge, C., & Goldenberg,
J. L. (2009). Exploring the existential function of religion: The
effect of religious fundamentalism and mortality salience
on faith-based medical refusals. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 97, 334–350.
Wisman, A., Heflick, N., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2015). The great
escape: The role of self-esteem and self-related cogni-
tion in terror management. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 60, 121–132.
Wong, N. C. H., Nisbett, G. S., & Harvell, L. A. (2017).
Smoking
is so ew!: College smokers reactions to health- versus social-
focused anti-smoking threat messages. Health Commu-
nication, 32, 451–460.
APA Citation
EXAMPLE:
Hunt, R. R., Smith, R. E., & Dunlap, K. R. (2011). How does
distinctive processing reduce false recall? Journal of Memory
and Language, 65, 378-389.
(APA format is very specific. I recommend picking up an APA
manual 7th edition. They’re pretty cheap and an amazing
resource. Otherwise, you can find APA format info by googling
“Owl Purdue APA format.”)
NOTE: Different types of sources require different methods of
citation. So, the citation format for a journal article will differ
from that of a book chapter or an internet source.
Research Question
What is the underlying question the researchers were aiming to
answer?
EXAMPLES:
Does masturbation frequency change as a function of age?
Is there a meaningful relationship between sexual preferences
and religious background?
What is the prevalence of HIV in a given population?
Do oysters act as an aphrodisiac?
Importance (why would other researchers be interested in this
study?)
What can be gained from the information provided in this
study? How might this inform future research?
Hypotheses
Hypotheses are specific predictions about the general research
question. For example, if the research question is, “Does
masturbation frequency change as a function of age,” then a
hypothesis might be, “As age increases, masturbation frequency
decreases.”
Design & Variables
Design: Descriptive, correlational, meta-analytic, or
experimental?
Independent variable(s): This is “manipulated” variable. Only
experimental designs involve independent variables. If the
research question is, “Do oysters act as an aphrodisiac?” then
the independent variable would be the administration of oysters.
For example, you might have one group of participants who
consume a half-dozen of oysters, another group who consumes a
dozen oysters, and a third group who consumes no oysters (i.e.,
a control group).
Dependent variable(s): This is the “variable of interest.” In
other words, this is the thing that the researchers are trying to
acquire information about. While only experimental designs
include independent variables, all research designs will include
at least one (and sometimes many more) dependent variable. In
the above example, the researchers are wanting to know if the
consumption of oysters increases sexual desire. In order to
determine this, they might measure self-reported sexual desire
levels and/or physiological signs of sexual desire, like blood
flow, perspiration, and pupillary dilation. Sexual desire would
be the dependent variable, and these things would be used to
measure it.
Number of participants (n = ____)
This is just the number of individuals who acted as participants
in the study.
Materials & Measures
In psychology, we often have to use indirect measures to
acquire information about a dependent variable. In the above
example involving oysters and sexual desire, the way one might
go about measuring sexual desire could include self-reports,
questionnaires, tools that measure blood flow, eye tracker s that
measure pupil dilation, etc. Any materials that were used to
gather data should be listed/briefly described here.
NOTE: Type of materials used will often differ depending on
the research design. For example, descriptive research often
employs behavioral observations, questionnaires, and/or
surveys.
Brief Description of Procedure
Here, you should provide a brief chronological account of what
participants actually did in the study.
EXAMPLE:
Participants completed informed consents and were randomly
assigned to one of three conditions. Measures of sexual desire
were gathered prior to oyster exposure in order to get baseline
measures for each participant. Depending on condition,
participants then consumed either a half-dozen oysters, a dozen
oysters, or zero oysters. Next, participants again completed the
sexual desire measures so that any change in desire due to
oyster consumption could be inferred.
NOTE: Procedures can differ greatly depending on the research
design. For example, a meta-analytic design would involve
analyzing several experimental studies on a particular subject
and then summarizing the collective results.
Results
What did the researchers find? Was there a significant
correlation or experimental effect? If the design was
descriptive, what kind of frequency data did they find?
Limitations (Is there anything about this research that might
affect the generalizability of the results?)
There are always limitations to every research design. More
specifically, there are some limitations that will apply to all
studies employing a given design (e.g., all descriptive research),
and there will be limitations that apply to a particular study. For
example, descriptive and correlational research can be said to
have low internal validity because it is difficult (or impossible)
to control for extraneous variables. Experimental designs, on
the other hand, can be said to have lower external validity
because it often involves a great degree of variable control.
Another common limitation is sample size. Results from a small
sample may be less generalizable than those from a larger
sample. If the researchers utilized a sample of convenience (i.e.,
one that was convenient but might not be representative of the
entire population of interest), this this could also be considered
a limitation.
NOTE: I want you to come up with something to put here. This
might take some critical thought!
How does this inform your group project?
Why is this study relevant to your own project topic?
httpsdoi.org10.11770963721416689563Current Directions

More Related Content

Similar to httpsdoi.org10.11770963721416689563Current Directions

Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docxtoltonkendal
 
Communication in Public Health
Communication in Public HealthCommunication in Public Health
Communication in Public HealthAymery Constant
 
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...Michele Battle-Fisher
 
Decisional architectures june 2013
Decisional architectures june 2013Decisional architectures june 2013
Decisional architectures june 2013Bradford Hesse
 
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Study
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says StudyConcern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Study
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Studytimothyweem
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Lossijtsrd
 
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docxLesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docxSHIVA101531
 
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docx
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docxTHEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docx
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docxchristalgrieg
 
Death by Assistance
Death by AssistanceDeath by Assistance
Death by AssistanceKumiko Sasa
 
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013Michelle N. Meyer
 
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docxRunning Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docxtoddr4
 
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docxRunning Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docxhealdkathaleen
 
Physician Assistant Suicide
Physician Assistant SuicidePhysician Assistant Suicide
Physician Assistant Suicidejoehartza
 
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxJunxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxtawnyataylor528
 
Literature Review Worksheet
Literature Review WorksheetLiterature Review Worksheet
Literature Review WorksheetSam Landfried
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLSantosConleyha
 

Similar to httpsdoi.org10.11770963721416689563Current Directions (18)

Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
 
Communication in Public Health
Communication in Public HealthCommunication in Public Health
Communication in Public Health
 
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...
Severity in Scope versus altruism: working against organ donation's realizati...
 
Wix ethical
Wix ethicalWix ethical
Wix ethical
 
Decisional architectures june 2013
Decisional architectures june 2013Decisional architectures june 2013
Decisional architectures june 2013
 
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Study
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says StudyConcern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Study
Concern Over Climate Change May Trigger Anxiety and Depression, Says Study
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
 
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docxLesson 11  Mental Health StigmaReadings  Please note that th.docx
Lesson 11 Mental Health StigmaReadings Please note that th.docx
 
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docx
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docxTHEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docx
THEORETICAL CONCEPT The Health Belief Model (HBM) is by far the mo.docx
 
MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9
MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9
MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9
 
Death by Assistance
Death by AssistanceDeath by Assistance
Death by Assistance
 
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013
Meyer from personalized med to personalized research-april 2013
 
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docxRunning Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
 
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docxRunning Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH      1                 .docx
Running Head ADVANCE NURSING RESEARCH 1 .docx
 
Physician Assistant Suicide
Physician Assistant SuicidePhysician Assistant Suicide
Physician Assistant Suicide
 
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docxJunxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
Junxian KuangLaura SinaiENG099101572018In the essay O.docx
 
Literature Review Worksheet
Literature Review WorksheetLiterature Review Worksheet
Literature Review Worksheet
 
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
10.11770146167203260716 ARTICLEPERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOL
 

More from PazSilviapm

Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docx
Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docxCase Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docx
Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docxPazSilviapm
 
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docx
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docxCASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docx
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docxPazSilviapm
 
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docx
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docxCASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docx
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docx
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docxCase Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docx
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study Horizon Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docx
Case Study Horizon  Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docxCase Study Horizon  Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docx
Case Study Horizon Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docx
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docxCase Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docx
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docx
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docxCase Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docx
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docx
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docxCase Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docx
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docx
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docxCase Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docx
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docx
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docxCase Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docx
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docx
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docxCase Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docx
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docxPazSilviapm
 
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docx
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docxCase FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docx
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docx
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docxCase Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docx
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docxPazSilviapm
 
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docx
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docxcase scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docx
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docx
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docxCase Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docx
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docx
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docxCase Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docx
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docx
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docxCase AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docx
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docx
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docxCase and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docx
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docxPazSilviapm
 
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docx
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docxCase C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docx
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docxPazSilviapm
 
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docx
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docxCase Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docx
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docxPazSilviapm
 

More from PazSilviapm (20)

Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docx
Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docxCase Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docx
Case Study Clinical LeadersDavid Rochester enjoys his role as a C.docx
 
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docx
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docxCASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docx
CASE STUDY Clinical  Journal Entry  1 to 2 pages A 21 month .docx
 
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docx
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docxCASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docx
CASE STUDY 5Exploring Innovation in Action The Dimming of the Lig.docx
 
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docx
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docxCase Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docx
Case Study 2A 40 year-old female presents to the office with the c.docx
 
Case Study Horizon Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docx
Case Study Horizon  Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docxCase Study Horizon  Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docx
Case Study Horizon Horizon Consulting Patti Smith looked up at .docx
 
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docx
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docxCase Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docx
Case Study EvaluationBeing too heavy or too thin, having a disabil.docx
 
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docx
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docxCase Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docx
Case Study Disney Corporation1, What does Disney do best to connec.docx
 
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docx
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docxCase Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docx
Case Study 3  Exemplar of Politics and Public Management Rightly Un.docx
 
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docx
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docxCase Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docx
Case Study 2 Structure and Function of the Kidney Rivka is an ac.docx
 
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docx
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docxCase Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docx
Case Study 2 Plain View, Open Fields, Abandonment, and Border Searc.docx
 
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docx
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docxCase Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docx
Case Study 2 Collaboration Systems at Isuzu Australia LimitedDue .docx
 
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docx
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docxCase FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docx
Case FormatI. Write the Executive SummaryOne to two para.docx
 
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docx
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docxCase Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docx
Case Study #2 Diabetes Hannah is a 10-year-old girl who has recentl.docx
 
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docx
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docxcase scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docx
case scenario being used for this discussion postABS 300 Week One.docx
 
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docx
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docxCase Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docx
Case Study #2Alleged improper admission orders resulting in mor.docx
 
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docx
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docxCase Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docx
Case Study 1Denise is a sixteen-year old 11th grade student wh.docx
 
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docx
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docxCase AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docx
Case AssignmentI. First read the following definitions of biodiver.docx
 
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docx
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docxCase and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docx
Case and questions are In the attchmentExtra resources given.H.docx
 
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docx
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docxCase C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docx
Case C Hot GiftsRose Stone moved into an urban ghetto in order .docx
 
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docx
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docxCase Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docx
Case Assignment must be 850 words and use current APA format with a .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersChitralekhaTherkar
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxPoojaSen20
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of PowdersMicromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
Micromeritics - Fundamental and Derived Properties of Powders
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 

httpsdoi.org10.11770963721416689563Current Directions

  • 1. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416689563 Current Directions in Psychological Science 2017, Vol. 26(2) 126 –131 © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721416689563 www.psychologicalscience.org/CDPS People routinely navigate choices with important conse- quences for their physical health. They go to the gym, smoke a cigarette with friends, work on their tan, order a salad instead of a double bacon cheeseburger, and get that recommended colonoscopy. Concerns about death would seem relevant to these and other everyday health decisions. Yet it was not until the terror management health model (TMHM) that this relevance was program- matically considered (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008). The model was born, in fact, from observations of reciprocal neglect. Health psychology largely ignored motivational implications associated with awareness of mortality, whereas terror management theory, despite inspiring considerable research about social psychological conse- quences of this awareness, was relatively silent about decisions with implications for physical health. In the present article, we consider how the TMHM bridges this intersection of health and death. We articulate the theo- retical foundation for the model and then highlight insights it has generated for how health-relevant contexts implicate awareness of mortality, how this awareness
  • 2. affects everyday health decisions, and how it can be used to foster better health outcomes. The Genesis of the Terror Management Health Model Terror management theory (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986) builds from a tradition of existential and psychodynamic theory (e.g., Becker, 1973) to posit that people need to psychologically manage the unsettling implications of knowing not just that death is inevitable but that it could happen at any time. They do this by identifying with cultural belief systems (i.e., worldviews), which enable people to view themselves as valuable members (reflecting self-esteem) of a cultural reality that persists beyond their own physical demise. The theory has inspired hundreds of studies around the globe and been applied to an array of human social behaviors (see, e.g., Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Greenberg, 2015). After the initial wave of research on terror manage- ment theory, studies increasingly suggested that people 689563CDPXXX10.1177/0963721416689563Arndt, GoldenbergHealth and Death research-article2017 Corresponding Author: Jamie Arndt, McAlester Hall, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65201 E-mail: [email protected] Where Health and Death Intersect: Insights From a Terror Management Health Model
  • 3. Jamie Arndt1 and Jamie L. Goldenberg2 1Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, and 2Department of Psychology, University of South Florida Abstract This article offers an integrative understanding of the intersection between health and death from the perspective of the terror management health model. After highlighting the potential for health-related situations to elicit concerns about mortality, we turn to the question, how do thoughts of death influence health-related decision making? Across varied health domains, the answer depends on whether these cognitions are in conscious awareness or not. When mortality concerns are conscious, people form healthy intentions and engage in healthy behavior if efficacy and coping resources are present. In contrast, when contending with accessible but nonconscious thoughts of death, health-relevant decisions are guided more by the implications of the behavior for the individual’s sense of cultural value. Finally, we present research suggesting how these processes can be leveraged to facilitate health promotion and reduce health risk Keywords health, decision making, risky behavior, terror management, death, mortality salience http://www.psychologicalscience.org/cdps http://sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F09637214 16689563&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-04-06 Health and Death 127
  • 4. defend against conscious and nonconscious awareness of mortality in different ways (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999). When thoughts of mortality are con- scious, people try to remove them from focal attention. (After all, ending up as fertilizer is a thought on which people generally don’t like to dwell.) Such proximal defenses push death-related thought to the mental back- ground. It is when thoughts of death are active but out- side of conscious awareness that people more strongly engage in distal defenses that address the problem of death on an abstract and symbolic level. For example, people cling more vigorously to their cultural beliefs (i.e., worldview defense) and try harder to live up to cultural standards (i.e., self-esteem striving). Proximal and distal defenses are often inferred in experimental research by measuring outcomes immediately after a mortality reminder or after a delay, respectively. Conceptual and meta-analytic reviews (i.e., statistical approaches that average across different studies) have supported the unique time course of death-thought activation and the distinct effects elicited (e.g., Steinman & Updegraff, 2015). The TMHM (Fig. 1) builds from these ideas. It begins with the assumption that health conditions have varying potential to make people think about death. The model then integrates insights about how people manage con- scious and nonconscious death-related cognitions with the recognition that health decisions can be influenced by concerns central (proximal) and more tangential (dis- tal) to the health context. The foundational idea is that when mortality concerns are conscious, health decisions are largely guided by the proximal motivational goal of reducing perceived vulnerability to a health threat and thus concerns about mortality. In contrast, when mortal- ity cognition is active but outside of focal attention,
  • 5. health-relevant decisions are guided by distal motiva- tional goals concerning the symbolic value of the self. TMHM Research The link between health and death Every time people undergo routine cancer screenings, there is the possibility that what they discover could mark the beginning of the end. It is perhaps not surprising that over 60% of people in a population-level survey reported that when they think of cancer, they automatically think of death (Moser et al., 2014). Even presentations of the word “cancer” that participants report not having seen (i.e., subliminal primes) increase the cognitive availability of death-related thought (Arndt, Cook, Goldenberg, & Cox, 2007). Performing breast self-exams (among women; Goldenberg, Arndt, Hart, & Routledge, 2008) or reading about risks of cancer from smoking (Hansen, Winzeler, & Topolinski, 2010) or unprotected sun exposure (Cooper, Goldenberg, & Arndt, 2014) also makes thoughts about death accessible. But cancer is just one of many health domains sharing this connection. Appeals about binge drinking ( Jessop & Wade, 2008) or risky sex (Grover, Miller, Solomon, Webster, & Saucier, 2010) and even insurance advertisements (Fransen, Fennis, Pruyn, & Das, Health Scenarios/Threats Conscious Death- Thought Activation Motivation: Reduce Vulnerability/ Awareness of Death
  • 6. Health-Behavior- Oriented Outcomes Health-Defeating Outcomes Health-Facilitating Outcomes Threat-Avoidance Outcomes Motivation: Bolster Meaning and Symbolic Self-Conception Nonconscious Death- Thought Activation Fig. 1. The terror management health model. 128 Arndt, Goldenberg 2008) also activate thoughts of death. Such findings prompt the critical question, how do cognitions about mortality influence health-related decision making and behavior? Across domains such as tanning, smoking, can- cer screening, nutrition, and fitness, the answer often depends on whether thoughts of death are in conscious awareness or not. The proximal and distal health implications of mortality salience
  • 7. Routledge, Arndt, and Goldenberg’s (2004) studies on sun protection provide an illustration of divergent health- relevant responses to conscious and nonconscious death- related thought. Women reported greater interest in sun protection immediately after answering two short ques- tions about their mortality (vs. a control topic), presum- ably because it would reduce their vulnerability to a health risk. However, after a delay, they indicated stronger interest in tanning, in line with appearance-based esteem contingencies assessed as part of the experiment. McCabe, Vail, Arndt, and Goldenberg’s (2014) studies on product endorsement provide another illustration. One study featured an ostensible taste test. Participants sampled a brand of bottled water purportedly endorsed by a medical doctor (to appeal to health) or a popular celebrity (to appeal to social status). Immediately after reminders of mortality, participants drank more of the water if it had been endorsed by a medical doctor, whereas after a delay, they drank more of the celebrity- endorsed water. Such effects highlight the distinction between health and esteem motivations that follow from conscious and nonconscious thoughts of death. Because people are motivated to reduce vulnerability to health concerns when consciously thinking about death, explicit thoughts of mortality render health- promoting (proximal) responses such as exercising more, using sun protection, and undergoing a screening exam more likely when people have sufficient coping resources, optimism, or beliefs in the efficacy of the behavior (and themselves) to effectively mitigate the health concern. When lacking these resources, people may respond to conscious thoughts of death by avoiding or denying the health threat (e.g., Cooper, Goldenberg, & Arndt, 2010). Thus, the effect of conscious concerns about mortality on
  • 8. health decisions depends on factors of immediate rele- vance to the health context, much as has been found in research based on rationally oriented models of health behavior (e.g., Prentice-Dunn & Rogers, 1986). In contrast, the relevance of the behavior for esteem and cultural identification often directs health decisions once thoughts of death are no longer conscious. For example, when distracted from mortality reminders, indi- viduals who derive self-esteem from fitness increase exercise intentions (Arndt, Schimel, & Goldenberg, 2003), whereas those who derive self-esteem from smoking report less interest in quitting (Hansen et al., 2010). These findings mesh well with evidence that self-esteem and self-presentational motives influence health-related deci- sion making (e.g., Leary, Tchividijian, & Kraxberger, 1994; Mahler, Kulik, Gibbons, Gerrard, & Harrell, 2003) but extend it by demonstrating the role of mortality concerns in these processes. The TMHM further suggests that worldview beliefs function similarly. Consider, for instance, people subscribing to a fundamentalist religious worldview. Terror management processes may play a role in their willingness to rely on faith alone for medical treatment (Vess, Arndt, Cox, Routledge, & Goldenberg, 2009). Taken together, this work helps to delineate when health decisions will be influenced by factors tangential to the health context and why people sometimes do the seemingly irrational things they do when it comes to tak- ing care of their health. Leveraging the Terror Management Health Model to Improve Health Decisions The implications of the TMHM framework invite consid-
  • 9. eration of a number of different ways to improve health- related decision making. Indeed, research has begun to examine how death-related cognition can be used as a motivational catalyst to facilitate health promotion and reduce health risk. Augmenting conventional approaches to health-related cognition One research direction involves using conscious death- related thought to bolster the influence of conventional approaches to health-related cognition. For example, Cooper et al. (2014) presented beachgoers with health communications that did or did not highlight the risk of death from skin cancer and did or did not elaborate on the efficacy of sun protection to mitigate these risks. When appeals emphasized sun-protection efficacy and raised the conscious risk of mortality, sun-protection intentions were greater. Such findings offer promise for using explicit mortality concerns to augment educational health campaigns that incorporate fear-related messages. Notably, there are differing views about the potential of the TMHM to inform research on the use of persuasive fear messages (see Hunt & Shehryar, 2011; Tannenbaum et al., 2015). When considering this potential, it is impor - tant to recognize that appeals need not explicitly men- tion death to conjure up death-related cognition; implicating serious health consequences can do so as Health and Death 129 well. Further, whether people are actively thinking about death is an important issue for evaluating whether the
  • 10. appeal encourages health- or esteem-based responses and necessitates careful attention. Fine-grained measure- ment of this issue may be necessary, although it is likely challenging in the context of much health-communica- tion research. But carefully considering the source of fear and the potential for health communications to activate conscious or nonconscious death-related thought may help to illuminate when and why such appeals are effec- tive, when they fall flat, and when they backfire (Ruiter, Kessels, Peters, & Kok, 2014). Targeting malleable bases of cultural value The TMHM suggests that when mortality concerns are active but not conscious, efforts to change health behav- ior may benefit from targeting malleable bases of cultural value. For example, when smokers viewed a public ser- vice announcement concerning the social consequences of smoking (e.g., “Who wants to date someone with bad breath?”), participants reminded of mortality reported increased intentions to quit (Arndt et al., 2009; see also Wong, Nisbett, & Harvell, 2017). Conveying positive social norms can also be useful in this regard. Grocery store patrons were reminded of mortality or a control topic; then, based on research from the prototype-will- ingness model (Gibbons & Gerrard, 1995), they visual- ized exemplars of healthy eaters or did not. As determined from their shopping receipts, those who were primed with mortality and visualized healthy eaters purchased healthier foods (McCabe et al., 2015). The utility of targeting how individuals derive a sense of value in conjunction with mortality reminders also shows promise in the context of safe sun behavior. The guiding idea is that if people can be steered away from
  • 11. thinking of tanned skin as attractive, subtle primes of mortality might lead to more interest in sun protection. Using such an approach, Cox et al. (2009) observed requests for sunscreen samples with higher SPF among (Caucasian) beachgoers. Furthermore, framing a UV pho- tograph of participants’ faces as revealing damaging effects on appearance, rather than health, interacted with mortality reminders to lead participants to take more samples of sunscreen and report greater intentions to use it (Morris, Cooper, Goldenberg, Arndt, & Gibbons, 2014). Thus, there seems to be potential for nonconscious thoughts of mortality to engage healthier behavioral practices if aspects of social value are targeted. Recognizing the body problem The TMHM also fosters recognition of underappreciated barriers to promoting health behavior. Goldenberg, McCoy, Pyszczynski, Greenberg, and Solomon (2000) suggested that the physicality of the body undermines people’s capacity to maintain the symbolic, cultural value of the self as a means to manage concerns associated with mortality. This helps illuminate when and why peo- ple may avoid health behaviors that involve intimate con- frontation with the body’s physicality or creatureliness (e.g., breast self-exams and mammograms; Goldenberg et al., 2008). That these behaviors are threatening not only because of their health implications (i.e., what one might find) but also because of a non-health-related threat suggests that, like other distal defenses, highlight- ing the symbolic aspects of the self may benefit efforts to foster health behavior. Opportunities to affirm symbolic representations of the body may be effective when health contexts elicit both mortality concerns and discomfort with the body’s physicality (Morris, Cooper, Goldenberg,
  • 12. Arndt, & Routledge, 2013). The potential for behavioral durability An important question is whether the effects observed in TMHM research are just a brief blip on the behavioral - change radar. Concerns about inevitable mortality are an ever-present condition with which people must contend, and moreover, people are reminded of mortality—some- times blatantly and sometimes subtly—on a routine basis. Two recent studies provided initial insight as to how an enduring influence of awareness of death may affect health behavior as it unfolds over time. In Morris, Goldenberg, Arndt, and McCabe (2016), when participants were primed with mortality and rode an exercise bike, they later reported exercising more in the 2 weeks that followed than did participants who were not reminded of mortality, and this led them to report basing their self-esteem more on fitness. In a second study, smokers who visualized a prototypical unhealthy smoker after being reminded of mortality reported more attempts to quit smoking in the following 3 weeks and became more committed to an identity as a nonsmoker, and this in turn inspired continued attempts over the next 3 weeks. These studies lay the groundwork for a longitu- dinal model in which death-related thought encourages identity-relevant behavior, the behavior fosters more identity relevance, and this in turn promotes more of the (healthy) behavior. Becoming comfortably numb Research has also begun to examine other processes through which death-related cognition might influence
  • 13. health-relevant choices. For example, perhaps because of the potential for anxiety involved, death reminders can motivate people to become “comfortably numb” (to 130 Arndt, Goldenberg borrow from a colleague who borrowed from Pink Floyd) and increase desire for intoxicants like marijuana (Nagar & Rabinovitz, 2015) and purchasing and consumption of alcohol (Ein-Dor et al., 2014). Such risky behavior may be most likely for those who lack secure terror management buffers. Indeed, nightclub patrons with low self-esteem drank more alcohol (as indicated by breathalyzer analy- sis) when primed with mortality reminders (Wisman, Heflick, & Goldenberg, 2015). Conclusion The TMHM integrates research on existential motivation, self-threats and psychological defense, risky behavior, fear appeals, and vulnerability, esteem, and normative factors influencing health-related decision making. Like other applied theoretical research, research guided by the TMHM enriches our understanding of the target domain as well as the basic theory. Although additional research is needed in the areas outlined above, the model offers a foundation for understanding how people man- age existential insecurity as well as harnessing the effects of death-related thought to engage productive health- behavior change. Recommended Reading Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). (See References). A theo-
  • 14. retical review article introducing the TMHM. Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). (See References). A recent comprehensive review of terror man- agement theory research for those interested in the differ - ent directions of research inspired by the theory. Spina, M., Arndt, J., Boyd, P., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2016). Bridging health and death: Insights and questions from a terror management health model. In L. A. Harvell & G. S. Nisbett (Eds.), Denying death: An interdisciplinary approach to Terror Management Theory (pp. 47–61). New York, NY: Routledge. A recent review of TMHM research. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Funding Most of the research reviewed here that involved Jamie Arndt and Jamie Goldenberg was supported by National Cancer Insti- tute Grant R01CA096581. References Arndt, J., Cook, A., Goldenberg, J. L., & Cox, C. R. (2007). Cancer and the threat of death: The cognitive dynamics of death thought suppression and its impact on behav- ioral health intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 12–29. Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Goldenberg, J. L., Vess, M., Routledge, C., & Cohen, F. (2009). Blowing in the (social) wind: Implications of extrinsic esteem contingencies for terror
  • 15. management and health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1191–1205. Arndt, J., Schimel, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2003). Death can be good for your health: Fitness intentions as proximal and distal defense against mortality salience. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1726–1746. Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York, NY: The Free Press. Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2010). Examination of the terror management health model: The interac- tive effect of conscious death thought and health-coping variables on decisions in potentially fatal health domains. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 937–946. Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2014). Perceived efficacy, conscious fear of death, and intentions to tan: Not all fear appeals are created equal. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 1–15. Cox, C. R., Cooper, D. P., Vess, M., Arndt, J., Goldenberg, J. L., & Routledge, C. (2009). Bronze is beautiful but pale can be pretty: The effects of appearance standards and mortality salience on sun-tanning outcomes. Health Psychology, 28, 746–752. Ein-Dor, T., Hirschberger, G., Perry, A., Levin, N., Cohen, R., Horesh, H., & Rothschild, E. (2014). Implicit death primes increase alco- hol consumption. Health Psychology, 33, 748–751.
  • 16. Fransen, M. L., Fennis, B. M., Pruyn, A. T. H., & Das, E. (2008). Rest in peace? Brand-induced mortality salience and con- sumer behavior. Journal of Business Research, 61, 1053–1061. Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1995). Predicting young adults’ health risk behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 505–517. Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). The implications of death for health: A terror management health model for behav- ioral health promotion. Psychological Review, 115, 1032– 1053. Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., Hart, J., & Routledge, C. (2008). Uncovering an existential barrier to breast cancer screening. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 260–274. Goldenberg, J. L., McCoy, S. K., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (2000). The body as a source of self-esteem: The effect of mortality salience on identification with one’s body, interest in sex, and appearance monitoring. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 118–130. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A ter- ror management theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 189–212). New York, NY: Springer. Grover, K. W., Miller, C. T., Solomon, S., Webster, R. J., & Saucier, D. A. (2010). Mortality salience and perceptions of people with AIDS: Understanding the role of prejudice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 32, 315–327. Hansen, J., Winzeler, S., & Topolinski, S. (2010). When the
  • 17. death makes you smoke: A terror management perspective on the effectiveness of cigarette on-pack warnings. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 226–228. Health and Death 131 Hunt, D. M., & Shehryar, O. (2011). Integrating terror manage- ment theory into fear appeal research. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 372–382. Jessop, D. C., & Wade, J. (2008). Fear appeals and binge drinking: A terror management theory perspective. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 773–788. Leary, M. R., Tchividijian, L. R., & Kraxberger, B. E. (1994). Self- presentation can be hazardous to your health: Impression management and health risk. Health Psychology, 13, 461–470. Mahler, H. I., Kulik, J. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., & Harrell, J. (2003). Effects of appearance-based intervention on sun protection intentions and self-reported behaviors. Health Psychology, 22, 199–209. McCabe, S., Arndt, J., Goldenberg, J. L., Vess, M., Vail, K. E., III, Gibbons, F. X., & Rogers, R. (2015). The effect of visualizing healthy eaters and mortality reminders on nutritious grocery purchases: An integrative terror management and prototype willingness analysis. Health Psychology, 34, 279–282. McCabe, S., Vail, K. E., III, Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L.
  • 18. (2014). Hails from the crypt: A terror management health model investigation of health and celebrity endorsements. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 289–300. Morris, K. L., Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., & Gibbons, F. X. (2014). Improving the efficacy of appear - ance-based sun exposure intervention with the terror man- agement health model. Psychology & Health, 29, 1245–1264. Morris, K. L., Cooper, D. P., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2013). Objectification as self-affirmation in the context of a death-relevant health threat. Self and Identity, 12, 610–620. Morris, K. L., Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., & McCabe, S. (2016). The enduring influence of death on health: Insight from the terror management health model. Manuscript submitted for publication. Moser, R. P., Arndt, J., Han, P., Waters, E., Amsellem, M., & Hesse, B. (2014). Perceptions of cancer as a death sen- tence: Prevalence and consequences. Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 1518–1524. Nagar, M., & Rabinovitz, S. (2015). Smoke your troubles away: Exploring the effects of death cognitions on cannabis craving and consumption. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47, 91–99. Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (1986). Protection motiva- tion theory and preventive health: Beyond the health belief model. Health Education Research, 1, 153–161. Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual -
  • 19. process model of defense against conscious and uncon- scious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845. Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty years of terror management theory: From genesis to revela- tion. In M. Zanna & J. Olsen (Eds.), Advances in experi- mental social psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 1–70). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2004). A time to tan: Proximal and distal effects of mortality salience on sun exposure intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1347–1358. Ruiter, R. A., Kessels, L. T., Peters, G. J. Y., & Kok, G. (2014). Sixty years of fear appeal research: Current state of the evidence. International Journal of Psychology, 49, 63–70. Steinman, C. T., & Updegraff, J. A. (2015). Delay and death- thought accessibility: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1682–1696. Tannenbaum, M. B., Hepler, J., Zimmerman, R. S., Saul, L., Jacobs, S., Wilson, K., & Albarracín, D. (2015). Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1178–1204. Vess, M., Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Routledge, C., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2009). Exploring the existential function of religion: The effect of religious fundamentalism and mortality salience on faith-based medical refusals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 334–350. Wisman, A., Heflick, N., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2015). The great escape: The role of self-esteem and self-related cogni-
  • 20. tion in terror management. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 60, 121–132. Wong, N. C. H., Nisbett, G. S., & Harvell, L. A. (2017). Smoking is so ew!: College smokers reactions to health- versus social- focused anti-smoking threat messages. Health Commu- nication, 32, 451–460. APA Citation EXAMPLE: Hunt, R. R., Smith, R. E., & Dunlap, K. R. (2011). How does distinctive processing reduce false recall? Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 378-389. (APA format is very specific. I recommend picking up an APA manual 7th edition. They’re pretty cheap and an amazing resource. Otherwise, you can find APA format info by googling “Owl Purdue APA format.”) NOTE: Different types of sources require different methods of citation. So, the citation format for a journal article will differ from that of a book chapter or an internet source. Research Question What is the underlying question the researchers were aiming to answer? EXAMPLES: Does masturbation frequency change as a function of age? Is there a meaningful relationship between sexual preferences and religious background? What is the prevalence of HIV in a given population?
  • 21. Do oysters act as an aphrodisiac? Importance (why would other researchers be interested in this study?) What can be gained from the information provided in this study? How might this inform future research? Hypotheses Hypotheses are specific predictions about the general research question. For example, if the research question is, “Does masturbation frequency change as a function of age,” then a hypothesis might be, “As age increases, masturbation frequency decreases.” Design & Variables Design: Descriptive, correlational, meta-analytic, or experimental? Independent variable(s): This is “manipulated” variable. Only experimental designs involve independent variables. If the research question is, “Do oysters act as an aphrodisiac?” then the independent variable would be the administration of oysters. For example, you might have one group of participants who consume a half-dozen of oysters, another group who consumes a dozen oysters, and a third group who consumes no oysters (i.e., a control group). Dependent variable(s): This is the “variable of interest.” In other words, this is the thing that the researchers are trying to acquire information about. While only experimental designs include independent variables, all research designs will include at least one (and sometimes many more) dependent variable. In the above example, the researchers are wanting to know if the consumption of oysters increases sexual desire. In order to determine this, they might measure self-reported sexual desire
  • 22. levels and/or physiological signs of sexual desire, like blood flow, perspiration, and pupillary dilation. Sexual desire would be the dependent variable, and these things would be used to measure it. Number of participants (n = ____) This is just the number of individuals who acted as participants in the study. Materials & Measures In psychology, we often have to use indirect measures to acquire information about a dependent variable. In the above example involving oysters and sexual desire, the way one might go about measuring sexual desire could include self-reports, questionnaires, tools that measure blood flow, eye tracker s that measure pupil dilation, etc. Any materials that were used to gather data should be listed/briefly described here. NOTE: Type of materials used will often differ depending on the research design. For example, descriptive research often employs behavioral observations, questionnaires, and/or surveys. Brief Description of Procedure Here, you should provide a brief chronological account of what participants actually did in the study. EXAMPLE: Participants completed informed consents and were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. Measures of sexual desire were gathered prior to oyster exposure in order to get baseline measures for each participant. Depending on condition, participants then consumed either a half-dozen oysters, a dozen oysters, or zero oysters. Next, participants again completed the sexual desire measures so that any change in desire due to oyster consumption could be inferred.
  • 23. NOTE: Procedures can differ greatly depending on the research design. For example, a meta-analytic design would involve analyzing several experimental studies on a particular subject and then summarizing the collective results. Results What did the researchers find? Was there a significant correlation or experimental effect? If the design was descriptive, what kind of frequency data did they find? Limitations (Is there anything about this research that might affect the generalizability of the results?) There are always limitations to every research design. More specifically, there are some limitations that will apply to all studies employing a given design (e.g., all descriptive research), and there will be limitations that apply to a particular study. For example, descriptive and correlational research can be said to have low internal validity because it is difficult (or impossible) to control for extraneous variables. Experimental designs, on the other hand, can be said to have lower external validity because it often involves a great degree of variable control. Another common limitation is sample size. Results from a small sample may be less generalizable than those from a larger sample. If the researchers utilized a sample of convenience (i.e., one that was convenient but might not be representative of the entire population of interest), this this could also be considered a limitation. NOTE: I want you to come up with something to put here. This might take some critical thought! How does this inform your group project? Why is this study relevant to your own project topic?