SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 83
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 1
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief
What is a Policy Brief? The Policy Brief is a “short, neutral
summary of what is known about a particular issue or problem.
Policy briefs are a form of report designed to facilitate policy-
making” (Eisele, 2006). The main purpose is to “succinctly
evaluate policy options regarding a specific issue, for a specific
policy-maker audience” (Eisele, n.d.). Policy-makers need to
make practical decisions under time-constraints, so the brief
should provide evidence and actionable recommendations
(Eisele, n.d.). The issue brief distils or synthesizes a large
amount of complex detail, so the reader can easily understand
the heart of the issue, its background, the players
(“stakeholders”) and any recommendations, or even educated
guesses about the future of the issue. It may have tables and
graphs; usually, it has a short list of references, so the reader
knows something about the sources on which it is based, and
where to go for more information. Most of the time, the brief
has its own “brief”--a one page “executive summary,” allowing
the reader to quickly grasp the essence of the report (Eisele,
n.d.). In short, “the purpose of the policy brief is to convince
the target audience of the
urgency of the current problem and the need to adopt the
preferred alternative or
course of action outlined and therefore, serve as an impetus for
action” (Young & Quinn,
n.d.).
What are the components of a Policy Brief? (Lifted from Tsai,
2006)
Executive summary The executive summary aims to convince
the reader further that the brief is worth in-depth investigation.
It is especially important for an audience that is short of time to
clearly see the relevance and importance of the brief in reading
the summary. As such, a 1 to 2 paragraph executive summary
commonly includes: 1. A description of the problem addressed;
2. A statement on why the current approach/policy option needs
to be changed; 3. Your recommendations for action.
Context and importance of the problem The purpose of this
element of the brief is to convince the target audience that a
current and urgent problem exists which requires them to take
action. The context and importance of the problem is both the
introductory and first building block of the brief. As such, it
usually includes the following: 1. A clear statement of the
problem or issue in focus. 2. A short overview of the root
causes of the problem 3. A clear statement of the policy
implications of the problem that clearly establishes the current
importance and policy relevance of the issue. It is worth noting
that the length of the problem description may vary
considerably from brief to brief depending on the stage on the
policy process in focus, e.g. there may be a need to have a much
more extensive problem description for policy at the evaluation
stage than for one at the option choosing stage.
Policy Brief versus
Research Paper
(Tsai, 2006)
Some might say that a policy brief is more “professional”
because it is geared towards readers who have a limited amount
of time to make a practical decision, while a research paper is
more “academic” because it pays more attention to the scholarly
roots of particular arguments and judges their merit on
intellectual and logical criteria.
Front-loaded!
Policy briefs are front-loaded: the
conclusions are on
the front page! The front page needs an executive summary,
providing a concise (1 or 2 paragraphs) overview of the
brief’s aim and core recommendations (Kopenski, 2010).
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 2
Critique of policy option(s) The aim of this element is to detail
shortcomings of the current approach or options being
implemented and therefore, illustrate both the need for change
and focus of where change needs to occur. In doing so, the
critique of policy options usually includes the following: 1. A
short overview of the policy option(s) in focus 2. An argument
illustrating why and how the current or proposed approach is
failing. It is important for the sake of credibility to recognize
all opinions in the debate of the issue.
Policy recommendations The aim of the policy
recommendations element is to provide a detailed and
convincing proposal of how the failings of the current policy
approach need to change. As such this is achieved by including:
1. A breakdown of the specific practical steps or measures that
need to be implemented 2. Sometimes also includes a closing
paragraph re-emphasizing the importance of action.
Appendices Although the brief is a short and targeted document,
authors sometimes decide that their argumen t needs further
support and so include an appendix. Appendices should be
included only when absolutely necessary.
What a persuasive Policy Brief should be (Lifted from Young
and Quinn, n.d.) As with all good marketing tools, the key to
success is targeting the particular audience for your message.
The most common audience for a policy brief is the decision-
maker but, it is also not unusual to use the document to support
broader advocacy initiatives targeting a wide but knowledgeable
audience (e.g. decision makers, journalists, diplomats,
administrators, researchers). In constructing a policy brief that
can effectively serve its intended purpose, it is common for a
brief to be:
FOCUSED All aspects of the policy brief (from the message to
the layout) need to strategically focused on achieving the
intended goal of convincing the target audience. For example,
the argument provided must build on what they do know about
the problem, provide insight about what they don’t know about
the problem and be presented in language that reflects their
values, i.e. using ideas, evidence and language that will
convince them.
PROFESSIONAL, NOT ACADEMIC The common audience for
a policy brief is not interested in the research/analysis
procedures conducted to produce the evidence, but are very
interested to know the writer’s perspective on the problem and
potential solutions based on the new evidence.
EVIDENCED-BASED The policy brief is a communication tool
produced by policy analysts and therefore all potential
audiences not only expect a rational argument but will only be
convinced by argumentation supported by evidence that the
problem exists and the consequences of adopting particular
alternatives.
LIMITED To provide adequately comprehensive but targeted
argument within a limited space, the focus of the brief needs to
be limited to a particular problem or area of a problem.
Do’s and Dont’s
(Kopenski, 2010)
Avoid technical, legalistic, economic or academic jargon!
Presentation needs to be professional as suits a public
document. Use correct grammar and spelling, and appropriate
spacing, font, point, headings and sub-headings. Facilitate
readability through images, catch-phrases, layout choices, and
the provision of data as graphs or charts.
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 3
SUCCINT The type of audiences targeted commonly do not
have the time or inclination to read an in-depth 20 page
argument on a policy problem. Therefore, it is common that
policy briefs do not exceed 6 – 8 pages in length (i.e. usually
not longer than 3,000 words).
UNDERSTANDABLE This not only refers to using clear and
simple language (i.e. not the jargon and concepts of an
academic discipline) but also to providing a well explained and
easy to follow argument targeting a wide but knowledgeable
audience.
ACCESSIBLE The writer of the policy brief should facilitate
the ease of use of the document by the target audience and
therefore, should subdivide the text using clear descriptive titles
to guide the reader.
PROMOTIONAL The policy brief should catch the eye of the
potential audience in order to create a favourable impression
(e.g. professional, innovative etc) In this way many brief
writers many of the features of the promotional leaflet (use of
colour, use of logos, photographs, slogans, illustrative quotes
etc).
PRACTICAL AND FEASIBLE The policy brief is an action-
oriented tool targeting policy practitioners. As such the brief
must provide arguments based on what is actually happening in
practice with a particular policy and propose recommendations
which seem realistic to the target audience
6 Steps for a compelling Policy Brief (Lifted from Young and
Quinn, n.d.) 1. Issue: examine the issue you will be dealing
with. Answer these questions: is the issue general or specific?
How general/specific? 2. Audience: take your primary audience
into serious consideration. Your brief should be tailored to the
needs of your audience. It makes a fundamental difference for
how you must frame your analysis and your recommendation. Is
your audience an individual (i.e. Prime Minister) or an
organization (i.e. the Government as a whole)? 3. Actors:
identify the relevant actors for the issue you are dealing with.
This is an essential step, since you will have to analyze their
interests in order to make sensible and viable policy
recommendations. Identifying the relevant actors is also
essential to produce a good assessment of the context and of the
interests that are plug into the issue. 4. Interests: once you have
identified the relevant actors, it is necessary to analyze their
interests. What are the actors' interests? Which of the relevant
actors have similar interests to your audience? Which ones have
different interests? How different? This step is important both
for the context part of your brief and for the critique of policy
options/policy recommendations. Without a clear identification
of the actors involved in the issue and their interests, your brief
will result vague, and therefore not useful. 5.
Recommendations: your policy recommendations should reflect
the above analysis. Remember that, according to the issue and
the audience, your recommendation(s) might not suggest the
best policy, but instead the most viable one. This should not
limit your recommendation to just compromise policies. If you
want to recommend radical change, you can; remember though
that such radical action has to be implemented in some ways. 6.
How-To: the last step is to suggest your audience the way to
'sell' the policy to its public (the public could be other members
of the organizations, voters, other parties, etc.). This last step
helps your audience build support/consensus to implement the
policy you recommended.
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 4
Online Resources
You can check the following policy briefs online to serve as
your guides in writing
your own policy briefs:
Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations
Policy Brief on the 2007-08 Rice Price Crisis:
http://www.fao.org/docrep/013/am172e/am172e00.pdf The
Poverty and Economic Policy Policy Briefs on Food and Fuel
Crises on Cambodia, Ghana and the Philippines:
http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16735
http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16736
http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16737 The
Poverty and Economic Policy Brief on Effects of the GFC in the
Philippines: http://portal.pep-
net.org/documents/download/id/16739 The Royal Children’s
Hospital Policy Brief on Evidence-Based Practice:
http://www.rch.org.au/emplibrary/ccch/Policy_Brief_21_-
_Evidence_based_practice_final_web.pdf
Submission Guidelines: 1. All submissions should be in MS
Word Format (.doc, .docx), 2 pages in length and should be in a
2-column form. 2. Images, tables, charts and graphs present in
the layout of the Policy Brief should be submitted separately,
i.e., all .jpg, .png, .bmp, .gif, .psd, etc., should be submitted
individually. 3. Check policy briefs from the Poverty and
Economic Policy (PEP) Website (www.pep-net.org),
particularly those from the Community-Based Monitoring
System (CBMS) to serve as guides in preparing your policy
briefs.
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 5
References: Eisele, F (n.d.). Preparing a Policy Brief Issue
[PDF Document]. Retrieved from
https://www.courses.psu.edu/hpa/hpa301_fre1/IBInstructions_fa
02.PDF Kopenski, Marc (March 2010). Policy Briefs. Retrieved
from the Richmond University Website
http://www.richmond.ac.uk/content/library/subjects/politics/poli
cy-briefs.aspx Tsai (May 2006). Guidelines for Writing a
Policy Brief [PDF Document]. Retrieved from
http://jhunix.hcf.jhu.edu/~ktsai/policybrief.html. Young, E. and
Quinn, L (n.d.). The Policy Brief [PDF Documnet]. Retrieved
from http://www.policy.hu/ipf/fel-pubs/samples/PolicyBrief-
described.pdf Young, E. and Quinn, L (n.d.). The Policy Brief:
Instructions [PDF Document]. Retrieved from
http://sobek.colorado.edu/~salucci/teaching/teaching_portfolio/a
ssets/Policy_Brief_instructions.pdf
Prepared by the Community-Based Monitoring System (CBMS)
Network Coordinating Team
10th Floor Angelo King International Center
Estrada corner Arellano Streets, Malate, Manila
Philippines 1004
Telephone: (02) 5262067
Email: [email protected]; [email protected]
Website: http://www.pep-net.org;
http://www.cbmsphilippines.webs.com/
Small Group Research
42(5) 536 –561
© The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1046496410397382
http://sgr.sagepub.com
Creativity in Virtual
Work: Effects
of Demographic
Differences
Luis L. Martins1
and Christina E. Shalley2
Abstract
Organizations are increasingly using virtual teams, in which
individuals
work with their teammates across distance and differences,
using a
variety of information and communication technologies. In this
study,
the authors examined how demographic differences (i.e.,
differences
in race, sex, age, and nationality) between individuals working
virtually
affected their collective creativity. Specifically, the authors
examined how
demographic differences interacted with the nature of
interaction processes
(establishment of rapport, participation equality, and process
conflict) and
difference in technical experience, to affect creativity in short-
term virtual
work interactions. Differences in age interacted with the
processes and
with differences in technical experience to affect creativity.
Differences in
nationality had a strong negative direct effect and interacted
with differences
in technical experience to affect creativity. Differences in sex
and race did
not significantly affect creativity. Implications of findings for
managing virtual
teams are discussed.
Keywords
creativity, demographic differences, virtual teams
1The University of Texas at Austin, USA
2Georgia Institute of Technology, USA
Corresponding Author:
Luis L. Martins, University of Texas at Austin, McCombs
School of Business,
1 University Station, B6300, Austin, TX 78712-0210
Email: [email protected]
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F10464964
10397382&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2011-02-28
Martins and Shalley 537
With an increase in global competition, companies have been
looking to cre-
ativity and innovation to give them a competitive edge
(Amabile, 1988;
Magadley & Birdi, 2009). Thus, effectively utilizing knowledge
resources
wherever they may reside in the organization has become an
important stra-
tegic priority for organizations (e.g., Dew & Hearn, 2009).
Until recently,
much of organizational knowledge was locked within
individuals and units
separated by various boundaries. However, with advances in
information and
communication technologies, organizations are increasingly
using virtual
teams to break down boundaries and connect employees
regardless of their
geographic location and subunit affiliation, so that they can
combine their
knowledge and perspectives to produce creative solutions to
various business
problems (e.g., McDonough, Kahn, & Barczak, 2001;
Townsend, DeMarie, &
Hendrickson, 1998). Indeed, increased creativity and innovation
have been
touted as among the primary benefits of using virtual teams
(e.g., Zakaria,
Amelinckx, & Wilemon, 2004). Thus, for example, consulting
firms such as
Bain and Company and McKinsey and Company use information
technology
(e.g., e-mail, instant messaging, and databases with the contact
information
and areas of expertise of every consultant) and other
communication tools to
enable consultants to reach peers in the company’s globally
distributed work-
force to work collaboratively on client problems as the needs
arise, which
contributes to their organizations’ ability to provide innovative
solutions to
their clients (Hansen, Nohria, & Tierney, 1999).
A natural consequence of the increase in the prominence of
global virtual
teams is that individuals are increasingly working virtually with
others who
are demographically different from themselves (Griffith &
Neale, 2001;
Griffith, Sawyer, & Neale, 2003). The extant literature on
creativity has gen-
erally proposed that demographic differences have the potential
to be benefi-
cial for creativity because demographically different individuals
working
together are able to bring to the task different perspectives and
approaches
(e.g., Milliken & Martins, 1996). On the other hand, researchers
have found
that, in the short run, demographic differences make it harder
for team mem-
bers to work together, thus potentially reducing their creative
performance
(e.g., see van Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007, for a recent
review). Whereas
there is a growing literature on the effects of demographic
differences on
creativity in more traditional face-to-face teams (e.g., Hoffman
& Maier,
1961; Nemeth, 1986), the effects have not been examined much
in virtual
teams. This study addresses this gap in the literature by
examining how
demographic differences interact with group process and input
conditions to
affect creativity in short-term virtual work interactions.
538 Small Group Research 42(5)
Of the variety of approaches that can be used to study dynamics
in virtual
teams, we focused on dyadic interactions within virtual teams
(i.e., on virtual
collaborations between any two members of a team). Although
there are
groupware systems that enable whole groups to interact at the
same time, much
of the interaction in virtual teams involves two team members at
any one time
collaborating virtually on a component of their team’s task
(e.g., Majchrzak,
Malhotra, Stamps, & Lipnack, 2004). Thus, though meetings of
whole virtual
teams occur episodically, their members’ day-to-day
interactions are typically
dyadic. For example, it is typical for a knowledge worker based
in the United
States to work with a colleague in Spain, one in Ireland, and
one in India, at
different times on different parts of the same task or project.
Indeed, Dew and
Hearn (2009) found that aggregating the creativity of pairs of
collaborators
within teams produced similar results as for the whole team,
leading them to
suggest that “structuring innovation teams into networked,
nominal pairs may
be just as productive as purely nominal group structures” (p.
521). Further-
more, the limited examination of the role played by
demographic differences
in dyadic interactions has been pointed out as a deficiency in
the literature on
diversity in teams in general (e.g., Tsui, Xin, & Egan, 1995) and
in virtual
teams in particular (Martins, Gilson, & Maynard, 2004).
Our examination of how the interactions of demographic
differences with
group process and input conditions affect creativity in virtual
collaborations
contributes to the literature in several ways. Whereas there is a
strong interest
among managers in using virtual teams to enhance creativity in
their organi-
zations, very few studies have empirically examined creativity
in a virtual
work context (e.g., Nemiro, 2002; Ocker, 2005), and fewer still
have looked
at how demographic differences affect creativity or innovation
in virtual con-
texts (e.g., Giambatista & Bhappu, 2010; Gibson & Gibbs,
2006). Recently,
researchers have argued that most work in organizations is now
virtual to a
greater or lesser extent, depending on the amount of time the
employees
spend working together on a task, the extent to which they use
technology to
support their interactions, and their geographic and temporal
separation (e.g.,
Griffith & Neale, 2001; Martins et al., 2004). Therefore, these
researchers
have suggested that we need to move beyond comparing virtual
to face-to-
face teams and, instead, empirically examine variation in
behavioral phe-
nomena within virtual teams. The moderated effects examined
in this study
advance understanding of the circumstances affecting the ability
of virtual
collaborators to leverage their knowledge resources, which prior
researchers
(e.g., Martins et al., 2004; Ocker, 2005) have suggested is an
important area
in need of further research. Specifically, the examination of
moderated effects
Martins and Shalley 539
enriches theory on the relationships between demographic
differences and
creativity by investigating how various process conditions and
an input fac-
tor determine whether demographic differences benefit or hurt
creativity in
virtual work (van Knippenberg, De Dreu, & Homan, 2004; van
Knippenberg &
Schippers, 2007).
Theory and Hypotheses
It is important to specify the boundary conditions of the model
we develop
and test in this study, as has been recommended in both the
diversity and the
virtual team literatures (Martins et al., 2004; Webber &
Donahue, 2001). Our
unit of analysis was dyadic interactions, which comprise most
of the day-to-
day work dynamics within virtual teams. The demographic
differences we
focused on were differences in race, sex, age, and nationality,
which are among
the major dimensions of demographic difference examined in
prior research
(e.g., Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989). The virtual working technology
we focused on
was computer-mediated communication (CMC; specifically,
electronic chat
room), which forms a large component of virtual work,
particularly among
geographically and temporally distributed individuals (Griffith
et al., 2003).
In addition, we focused on short-term dyadic collaborations
aimed at solving
immediate managerial problems. This short-term time
perspective was chosen
for a few reasons. First, virtual interactions for short-term
problem solving are
prevalent in virtual teams (Martins et al., 2004). In addition,
virtual teams
have been found to have a shorter lifecycle than face-to-face
teams, as they
are brought together as needed to work on specific tasks
(Jarvenpaa & Leidner,
1999). Finally, since membership in virtual work groups is often
fluid, dyadic
interactions with any one team member often are of a one-time
or short-term
nature (Zakaria et al., 2004). Finally, we looked at variation
within virtual
work, as has been recommended by researchers (e.g., Griffith et
al., 2003),
rather than comparing virtual to face-to-face work.
Effects of Demographic Differences on Creativity
Creativity is defined as the production of novel, potentially
useful ideas about
work products, practices, services, or procedures (Amabile,
1996; Shalley,
1995). It is a major part of work quality and effectiveness and is
increasingly
valued for a variety of tasks, occupations, and industries
(Amabile, 1988). In
collaborative work, creativity requires the pooling together and
effective
integration of different perspectives, knowledge, skills, and
abilities on a
540 Small Group Research 42(5)
task (Hargadon & Bechky, 2006; Taggar, 2002; Woodman,
Sawyer, &
Griffin, 1993).
Research on diversity suggests two opposing expectations
regarding the
effects of demographic differences on creativity, based on the
contrasting
predictions of what have been termed the information/deci sion-
making per-
spective and the social categorization and similarity/attraction
perspective
(Williams & O’Reilly, 1998). Using the information/decision-
making per-
spective, researchers have proposed that differences in
perspective and expe-
riences underlying demographic differences should result in a
greater range
of information, ideas, and approaches to a problem being
generated and,
in turn, improved creative problem solving (e.g., Nemeth, 1986;
Pelled,
Eisenhardt, & Xin, 1999). Also, they have found that working
with different
others may stimulate consideration of nonobvious alternatives
that could
potentially lead to higher creativity (McLeod & Lobel, 1992).
Similarly, the
group brainstorming literature (see Paulus, 2000) has found that
differences
may be beneficial for the generation of more novel ideas. For
example,
McLeod and Lobel found that ethnically diverse groups
produced higher-
quality ideas. Also, culturally heterogeneous groups were found
to generate
more alternatives in the long run (Watson, Kumar, &
Michaelsen, 1993). Thus,
this perspective suggests that demographic differences have the
potential to
contribute to creativity by increasing the number of unique
ideas brought to
bear on a task (Milliken, Bartel, & Kurtzberg, 2003).
On the other hand, using the social categorization and
similarity/attraction
perspective, the literature suggests that demographic differences
can lead to
a variety of process losses leading to negative effects on team
performance.
This expectation is based on a variety of sociocognitive
theories, especially
the argument that individuals are attracted to those who they
perceive to be
demographically similar to themselves (Byrne, 1971) and those
who they
categorize as belonging to the same social category as
themselves (Tajfel,
1981). Social categorization and stereotyping based on
demographic charac-
teristics are particularly prevalent when teams are first formed
or in short-
lived teams, since individuals tend to use these cognitive
mechanisms to
make sense of other team members until their stereotypes are
invalidated
through extended positive interactions (e.g., Allport, 1954).
Thus, in the short
term, demographic differences within teams have been found to
result in
greater conflict, communication difficulties, and other negative
processes, as
well as lower cohesion and social integration (e.g., Harrison,
Price, & Bell, 1998;
Pelled et al., 1999), and consequently, lower creative
performance (Ancona &
Caldwell, 1992).
Martins and Shalley 541
Depending on what theoretical and process foundations are
used, demo-
graphic differences can both help and hurt performance (for
reviews, see
Milliken & Martins, 1996; van Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007;
Williams &
O’Reilly, 1998). Commenting on this conundrum, van
Knippenberg and
Schippers noted that these competing predictions and findings
are primarily
caused by focusing on main effects instead of potential
moderators. Consis-
tent with some prior work (e.g., Jackson & Joshi, 2004;
Milliken & Martins,
1996; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998), they argue for models that
are more com-
plex and that consider moderator variables in explaining the
effects of diver-
sity. We examine the moderating effects of processes and input
factors that
are likely to determine the extent of cognitive elaboration and
combination of
various perspectives on a collaborative task, which are critical
to translating
the potential benefits of demographic differences into actual
performance
benefits (e.g., van Knippenberg et al., 2004). We argue that in
order for the
potential creativity benefits due to differing perspectives to be
realized, it
is important that interactions among demographically different
individuals
enable the surfacing, pooling together, and integration of their
differing
perspectives (Gilson & Shalley, 2004; Milliken et al., 2003;
Shalley &
Perry-Smith, 2008).
Demographic Differences and Creativity:
Moderation by Processes and Inputs
The literature (e.g., Ocker, 2005) suggests that input and
process factors that
facilitate positive exchanges and the building of a positive
relationship are
important in determining the quality of interactions in virtual
teams. This is
particularly true for problem solving, which benefits from
multiple perspec-
tives but requires collaborators to work through their
differences in attitudes
and values to arrive at a consensus on solutions (Straus &
McGrath, 1994).
For instance, Taggar (2002) found that a team’s creativity-
relevant process
moderated the relationship between the average creativity of its
members and
the team’s creative output. Therefore, processes and inputs that
facilitate
information elaboration may be the key to reducing the negative
effects and
accentuating the benefits of demographic differences in virtual
interactions
(van Knippenberg, et al., 2004). For example, a team’s process
skills have
been found to be important to leveraging its members’ creative
resources
(e.g., Stroebe & Diehl, 1994). Also, Payne (1990) found that
communication
patterns in research teams had critical effects on their creative
performance.
The nature of the interaction process in virtual collaborations
also may affect
how members approach a task as well as affect their attention to
the heuristic
542 Small Group Research 42(5)
aspects of the task. The specific process factors we examined as
moderators
of the effects of demographic differences on creativity are the
degree to
which virtual collaborators spend time establishing rapport (i.e.,
bonding
through informal conversation) before beginning work on the
task, equality
of participation in their task-related discussions, and degree of
experienced
process conflict. The input factor we examined as a moderator
is the differ-
ence in technical experience between collaborators, which prior
research has
found to be one of the most influential inputs affecting the
quality of virtual
interactions (Sarker & Sahay, 2002). Difference in technical
experience
refers to the difference between virtual collaborators in their
experience with
using the information and communication technologies required
to collaborate
virtually.
Establishment of rapport. Whether individuals spend time
establishing
rapport with each other before working on their task can be
important for the
development of a good working relationship in virtual
collaborations (Coutu,
1998; Saunders, 2000). For example, by spending a few minutes
with intro-
ductions and discussing how they should approach working on
the task,
virtual collaborators can establish a bond of trust that may make
it easier
for them to work together effectively (Jarvenpaa & Leidner,
1999). Such
an establishment of rapport may create a psychologically safe
environment
(Edmondson, 1999) in which demographically different virtual
team members
are comfortable raising and discussing their differing
perspectives on a
problem without feeling interpersonally threatened (Griffith &
Neale, 2001).
In keeping with this argument, prior research has found that
virtual teams
whose members spend time at the onset of their work getting to
know each other
experience greater trust among members down the road, which
facilitates the
overall effectiveness of their working together (Jarvenpaa &
Leidner, 1999;
Suchan & Hayzak, 2001). This also should help them to
overcome the interaction
difficulties in working virtually. Therefore, we expect that
demographically
different virtual collaborators who establish rapport to a greater
degree will
be better able to surface, discuss, and integrate differing
perspectives, which
in turn enhances creativity. In contrast, when demographically
different virtual
collaborators do not establish rapport, they may find that their
differences
in perspective lead to difficulties in working together, which in
turn diminishes
creativity.
Hypothesis 1: The relationship between demographic
differences and
creativity will be positive when there is greater establishment of
rap-
port in virtual collaborations and negative when there is less
estab-
lishment of rapport.
Martins and Shalley 543
Participation equality. Participation equality reflects the extent
to which
each member of a dyad engaged in virtual collaboration
participates equally
in task interactions. In a diverse group, participation equality
may enable
“cognitive elaboration and information exchange within work
groups, draw-
ing out the different knowledge and skills represented” (Webber
& Donahue,
2001, p. 158). Thus, more equal participation enables better
surfacing and
discussion of different ideas, resulting in greater creativity
(Taggar, 2002).
For example, Kruempel (2000) found that, in order for effective
knowledge
production to occur in a virtual team, the perspectives of all
team members
needed to be raised and debated. Also, Gilson and Shalley
(2004) found that
teams that valued participation by all members were more
creative.
Consideration of the variety of views and ideas represented by
demo-
graphically different collaborators should lead to an expanded
source of
knowledge to use in decision making. Also, the intellectual
stimulation of
considering others’ ideas should encourage exploratory
thinking, which
results in greater creativity. However, effective collaboration
and participa-
tion are necessary for virtual teams to successfully integrate
various team
members’ ideas and perspectives (Sarker, Lau, & Sahay, 2001).
Thus, when
demographically different virtual collaborators do achieve
equality of par-
ticipation they may be better able to leverage their diversity of
perspectives
to generate creative solutions. In contrast, when
demographically different
virtual collaborators have unequal inputs they will be less likely
to be work-
ing with a broad range of information and perspectives, which
diminishes
their collective creativity.
Hypothesis 2: The relationship between demographic
differences and
creativity will be positive when there is greater participation
equal-
ity in virtual collaborations and negative when there is less
partici-
pation equality.
Process conflict. Process conflict is defined as “controversies
about aspects
of how task accomplishment will proceed” (Jehn & Mannix,
2001, p. 239;
italics in original). Greater process conflict increases
uncertainty and reduces
the ability of groups working on a task to pool together their
ideas effectively
to come up with collective solutions to problems (Jehn &
Mannix, 2001).
Thus, for demographically different virtual collaborators, a high
level of
process conflict between them may worsen the interaction
difficulties caused
by their demographic differences and virtual interaction, which
leads to process
losses (Montoya-Weiss, Massey, & Song, 2001). This argument
is consistent
with Griffith and Neale’s (2001) observation that “more virtual
environments
544 Small Group Research 42(5)
require more attention to procedural matters for success” (p.
401). The greater
the process conflict, the greater the increase in process losses,
which
negatively affects the joint creativity of demographically
different virtual
collaborators. In contrast, based on the diversity literature (e.g.,
Pelled et al.,
1999; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998), when virtual collaborators
do not
experience a great deal of process conflict they may be better
able to reduce
the interaction difficulties caused by their demographic
differences and
virtual interaction, and therefore, enhance effective discussion
and integration
of differing perspec tives to arrive at creative solutions.
Consistent with this
argument, creativity researchers have found that effective
collaboration is a
key determinant of creativity and innovation in teams (Pirola-
Merlo & Mann,
2004). Thus, virtual collaborators who have effective processes
for
integrating their efforts for productive teamwork may be better
able to
overcome the low media richness of virtual work technologies
and to
integrate the differing perspectives on a task resulting from
their
demographic differences, in order to produce creative outcomes.
Hypothesis 3: The relationship between demographic
differences and
creativity will be negative when there is greater process conflict
in
virtual collaborations and positive when there is less process
conflict.
Differences in technical experience. Individuals collaborating
virtually may
be expected to differ in their extent of experience in using the
technologies
needed to interact virtually. Prior research has found that teams
whose
members all have high levels of competence in using virtual
work technologies
perform better than those in which some members are more
proficient in
using the technologies than others (Kayworth & Leidner, 2000;
Sarker &
Sahay, 2002). Similarities in technical experience may thus
create a positive
context for demographically different collaborators to surface
and discuss
their differing perspectives. Differences in technical experience,
on the other
hand, may create communication and interaction problems, as
individuals
with stronger technical abilities may feel frustrated or limited in
their virtual
collaborations with others who are not as proficient in using
virtual working
technologies. These difficulties would exacerbate any
interaction diffi-
culties due to demographic differences and the low media
richness of
virtual communication technologies. Thus, differences in
technical experience
between virtual collaborators may create process barriers to
effective virtual
interaction, causing frustration and miscues that reduce
creativity. Similar
levels of technical experience, on the other hand, may provide a
common platform
that establishes the nature of the interaction between virtual
collaborators
Martins and Shalley 545
(i.e., both individuals will face the same difficulties if they both
have low
technical experience or may have the same level of proficiency
if they both
have high technical experience). Therefore, when there are wide
differences
between virtual collaborators in their technical expertise, they
may have
greater difficulty in operating effectively in a virtual context. In
such a
circumstance, it may be expected that they will have difficulties
in establishing
interactions that surface and utilize differing perspectives,
which diminishes
their creativity.
Hypothesis 4: The relationship between demographic
differences and
creativity will be negative when there is greater difference in
tech-
nical experience between virtual collaborators and positive
when
there is less difference in technical experience.
Method
Sample, Task, and Procedures
The sample consisted of 94 MBA students in an organizational
behavior
course at a medium-sized urban university in the United States.
As part of
their normal course curriculum, the class worked on a virtual
work project.
The students were asked to volunteer to participate in this
research by filling
out a survey; all did. The sample was demographically diverse:
33% were
women, 45% international (representing 24 countries), and 36%
nonwhite.
The participants were in the age range of 23 to 42 years (M =
27.6 years,
median = 27 years). All participants were proficient in spoken
and written
English; the average TOEFL score for international students
was 637 (out of
a possible 677).
We used a complex heuristic task for which responses were
open ended,
did not have correct answers, and required participants to “seek
consensus on
a preferred alternative” (Straus & McGrath, 1994) that has been
used in a num-
ber of prior creativity studies (e.g., Shalley, 1991; Shalley &
Perry-Smith,
2001; Zhou, 1998). Participants were asked to generate
solutions to various
human resource problems (e.g., employee theft, motivating the
sales force)
that typically arise within organizations and that managers need
to be able to
effectively solve. The participants were told that we were
particularly inter-
ested in creative solutions, so they should try to think of unique
ways to solve
the problems that also would work well in the company.
Participants were randomly assigned a partner to collaborate
with, which
yielded 47 dyads engaged in virtual collaboration. Members of
59.57% of the
546 Small Group Research 42(5)
dyads were of different races, 53.19% were of different sexes,
and 63.83%
were of different nationalities. The difference in age between
virtual collabo-
rators ranged from 0 to 16 years (M = 3.89 years). The
participants were only
allowed to communicate with their partner in an electronic chat
room.
Participants were given instructions on how to log on to their
assigned chat
room to connect with their partner to work on the task, were
briefly introduced
to their partner face to face (they could see each other, but
could not speak),
and were given 60 min to work on the collaborative task. Before
working on
the task, they were asked to complete a brief survey that
collected informa-
tion on demographics and extent of prior technical experience
with computer-
based interaction (e.g., chat rooms, bulletin boards, and e-mail).
Measures
Demographic differences. All participants were asked to
indicate their race,
sex, age, and nationality. As has been done in previous research
on demo-
graphic differences (Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989), we used
dichotomous mea-
sures for differences in race, sex, and nationality (with 0
indicating no
difference, and 1 indicating a difference in the respective
characteristic) and
computed difference in age as the squared difference between
the ages of
the two collaborators.
Creativity. According to Amabile (1996), a product is creative
if observers
independently agree that it is novel and appropriate. Two
graduate research
assistants independently rated the creativity of all solutions
generated on a
7-point scale (1 = not at all creative to 7 = extremely creative).
Interrater reli-
ability was assessed using rWG. The mean rWG(j) for the
creativity ratings was
.96, which is well above the commonly used cutoff of .70. Thus,
the overall
creativity score for each pair of virtual collaborators was
computed as the
average of the two raters’ creativity ratings for the solutions
generated by the
collaborators. Since there may be an association between the
number of solu-
tions provided and the overall level of creativity, we tested the
number of
problems solved as a potential control variable. However, since
it was not
significant as a control, it was excluded from the analyses.
Virtual interaction process factors. We retained a transcript of
the interac-
tions within each chat room as the virtual collaborators worked,
so that we
could code aspects of their interaction process. Two raters
independently
coded the following factors on a 7-point scale: establishment of
rapport,
equality of participation in problem solving, and extent of
process conflict.
The factors were defined for both raters, and their
understanding of the defi-
nitions was ascertained. Establishme nt of rapport was defined as
spending
Martins and Shalley 547
time at the start of the virtual collaboration discussing non-task-
related issues
that served to create a positive working relationship (e.g.,
spending time
briefly getting to know each other, reassuring each other about
the upcoming
virtual collaboration). Equality of participation was coded as
the extent to which
each of the virtual collaborators had an equal amount of input
and influence
over the task. Process conflict was defined as the extent of
disagreement
between the virtual collaborators in how the task should be done
(e.g., dis-
agreement was high if one person wanted to read all the memos
before com-
mencing discussion and the other wanted to tackle them one by
one). As was
done with the creativity ratings, interrater agreement for the
three factors was
assessed using rWG. The mean rWG for the process ratings was
.94 for estab-
lishment of rapport, .91 for equality of participation, and .86 for
extent of
process conflict. Thus, for each of the three process factors, the
ratings
assigned by the two raters were averaged to obtain overall
scores.
Virtual interaction input factor. For technical experience,
respondents were
asked to indicate on a 5-point scale (1 = none to 5 = a great
deal) their degree
of experience with using e-mail, chat rooms, bulletin boards,
and any other
electronic collaboration technologies (e.g., document sharing).
Responses
were averaged across the four items (alpha = .71). For each set
of collabora-
tors, difference in technical experience was computed as the
variance between
the scores of its members.
Results
Correlations and descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1.
The hypothe-
ses were tested using hierarchical regression analysis
(significant findings
are reported in Table 2). Due to sample size constraints, we ran
separate
regressions for differences in age and nationality and for
differences in race
and sex. Also, we entered each moderator variable separately.
The limita-
tions of the separate tests for the dimensions of difference and
moderator
variables are noted in the discussion section below. The
variables were
entered into the hierarchical regression equation in four steps.
In the first set
of analyses, difference in age and difference in nationality were
entered in the
first step (Table 2, Model 1a-d), the respective moderator
variable was entered
in the second step (Table 2, Models 2a-d), the interaction term
for difference
in age and the respective moderator was entered in the third step
(Table 2,
Models 3a-d), and the interaction term for difference in
nationality and the
respective moderator was entered in the fourth step (Table 2,
Models 4a-d).
The second set of analyses repeated the process, but with
differences in race
and sex as the demographic difference variables. A centering
procedure was
548
T
a
b
le
1
.
D
es
cr
ip
ti
ve
s
an
d
C
o
rr
el
at
io
ns
V
ar
ia
bl
e
M
ea
n
SD
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
1.
D
iff
er
en
ce
in
r
ac
e
0
.6
0
0
.5
0
2.
D
iff
er
en
ce
in
s
ex
0
.5
3
0
.5
0
-.
25
3.
D
iff
er
en
ce
in
a
ge
(
sq
ua
re
d)
29
.6
3
53
.4
0
-.
07
.0
1
4.
D
iff
er
en
ce
in
n
at
io
na
lit
y
0
.6
4
0
.4
9
.4
6
-.
26
-.
26
5.
E
st
ab
lis
hm
en
t
o
f
ra
pp
o
rt
a
4
.0
2
0
.8
1
-.
06
-.
09
-.
11
.0
2
6.
P
ar
ti
ci
pa
ti
o
n
eq
ua
lit
ya
4
.6
9
0
.7
4
-.
17
.2
5
-.
08
-.
17
.0
2
7.
P
ro
ce
ss
c
o
nf
lic
ta
3
.4
9
1
.0
6
.0
5
-.
15
.0
6
.1
4
.1
1
-.
65
8.
D
iff
er
en
ce
in
t
ec
hn
ic
al
e
xp
er
ie
nc
eb
1
.4
8
1
.3
1
.1
3
-.
20
-.
03
.2
6
.1
3
-.
09
.2
9
9.
C
re
at
iv
it
yc
3
.9
8
0
.5
4
-.
09
.0
9
.0
1
-.
38
.3
1
.3
5
-.
20
.1
1
N
o
te
: N
=
4
7.
C
o
rr
el
at
io
ns
a
bo
ve
.2
8
ar
e
si
gn
ifi
ca
nt
a
t
th
e
.0
5
le
ve
l.
a.
E
st
ab
lis
hm
en
t
o
f
ra
pp
o
rt
, p
ar
ti
ci
pa
ti
o
n
eq
ua
lit
y,
a
nd
p
ro
ce
ss
c
o
nf
lic
t
w
er
e
ra
te
d
o
n
a
7-
po
in
t
sc
al
e.
b.
T
he
e
xt
en
t
o
f
te
ch
ni
ca
l e
xp
er
ie
nc
e
w
as
r
at
ed
o
n
a
5-
po
in
t
sc
al
e.
c.
T
he
le
ve
l o
f
cr
ea
ti
vi
ty
w
as
r
at
ed
o
n
a
7-
po
in
t
sc
al
e.
Martins and Shalley 549
Table 2. Results of Hierarchical Regression Analyses
Model 1a 2a 3a 4a
Step 1: Independent variables
Difference in age -.11 -.07 .03 .03
Difference in nationality -.42*** -.41*** -.40*** -.40***
Step 2: Moderator
Establishment of rapport — .30** .39*** .39***
Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms
Establishment of rapport ×
Difference in age
— — .32** .31**
Establishment of
Rapport × Difference in
Nationality
— — — -.03
R2 .17 .26 .34 .34
Adj. R2 .13 .20 .28 .26
F 4.26** 4.79*** 5.28*** 4.13***
R2 change — .09** .09** .00
Model 1b 2b 3b 4b
Step 1: Independent variables
Difference in age -.11 -.07 -.02 -.01
Difference in nationality -.42*** -.36** -.34** -.35**
Step 2: Moderator
Participation equality — .27** .20 .17
Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms
Participation Equality ×
Difference in Age
— — .29** .32**
Participation Equality ×
Difference in Nationality
— — — .14
R2 .17 .24 .31 .33
Adj. R2 .13 .18 .24 .24
F 4.26** 4.34*** 4.58*** 3.87***
R2 change — .07** .07** .02
Model 1c 2c 3c 4c
Step 1: Independent variables
Difference in age -.11 -.09 -.18 -.18
Difference in nationality -.42*** -.40*** -.34** -.35**
Step 2: Moderator
Process conflict — -.15 -.08 -.08
Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms
Process Conflict ×
Difference in Age
— — -.38*** -.38***
Process Conflict ×
Difference in Nationality
— — -.01
R2 .17 .19 .32 .32
Adj. R2 .13 .13 .25 .23
F 4.26** 3.21** 4.74*** 3.70***
R2 change — .02 .13*** .00
(continued)
550 Small Group Research 42(5)
followed in the computation of interaction terms (Aiken, West,
& Reno,
1991). All significant moderated effects found were examined
further, using
recommended procedures (Aiken et al., 1991).
Hypothesis 1 stated that there would be a positive relationship
between
demographic differences and creativity when there was high
establishment of
rapport and a negative relationship when there was low
establishment of rap-
port. This hypothesis was supported (p < .05) for differences in
age (Table 2,
Model 3a; F = 5.28, p < .01) but not for differences in
nationality, race, or
sex. Difference in age was positively related to creativity when
there was
more establishment of rapport but was negatively related to
creativity when
there was less rapport established.
Hypothesis 2 stated that there would be a positive relationship
between
demographic differences and creativity when there was more
equal participation
by both virtual collaborators and a negative relationship when
there was less
equal participation. This hypothesis was supported (p < .05) for
differences
in age (Table 2, Model 3b; F = 4.58, p < .01) but not for
differences in nation-
ality, race, or sex. Difference in age was positively related to
creativity when
there was relatively equal participation in the virtual w ork
interaction but was
negatively related when there was less equal participation.
Table 2. (continued)
Model 1d 2d 3d 4d
Step 1: Independent variables
Difference in age -.11 -.12 -.11 -.13
Difference in nationality -.42*** -.49*** -.47*** -.55****
Step 2: Moderator
Difference in technical
experience
— .25* .17 .22
Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms
Difference in Technical
Experience × Difference
in Age
— — –.28** -.35***
Difference in Technical
Experience × Difference
in Nationality
— — — -.33**
R2 .17 .22 .29 .39
Adj. R2 .13 .17 .23 .32
F 4.26** 3.98** 4.26*** 5.14****
R2 change — .06* .07** .10**
Note: N = 47.
*p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < .01. ****p < .001.
Martins and Shalley 551
Hypothesis 3 stated that there would be a negative relationship
between
demographic differences and creativity when there was high
process conflict
and a positive relationship when there was low process conflict.
This hypothe-
sis was supported (p < .01) for differences in age (Table 2,
Model 3c; F = 4.74,
p < .01), but not for differences in nationality, race, or sex.
Difference in age
was negatively related to creativity when there was high process
conflict in
the virtual interaction but was positively related to creativity
when there was
low process conflict.
Hypothesis 4 stated that there would be a negative relationship
between
demographic differences and creativity when there was a larger
difference in
technical experience between virtual collaborators and a
positive relationship
when there was a smaller difference in technical experience.
This hypothesis
was supported (p < .05) for differences in age (Table 2, Model
3d; F = 4.26,
p < .01), and to some extent for differences in nationality
(Table 2, Model 4d;
F = 5.14, p < .01), but not for differences in race or sex.
Difference in age was
negatively related to creativity when there was a large
difference in technical
experience between virtual collaborators but was slightly
positively related
when there was a smaller difference in technical experience.
Difference in
nationality was more negatively related to creativity when there
was a greater
difference in technical experience than when there was a
smaller difference.
Discussion
The pattern of our findings is relatively consistent when looked
at from the
perspective of the various dimensions of difference we
examined. We found
that our hypotheses were consistently supported for differences
in age. The
effect of the difference in age between virtual collaborators on
creativity was
contingent on various aspects of their interaction and on their
difference in
technical experience. All the interaction process factors we
examined moder-
ated the effects of age difference on creativity. We found that a
difference in
age led to greater creativity when virtual collaborators had
spent some time
establishing rapport, when they had equal participation in the
discussion, and
when process conflict was low. These findings support the idea
that when
demographically different virtual collaborators are able to
utilize effective
processes, they are better able to deal with interaction
difficulties that may
arise from their differences and virtual interaction, and thus,
benefit from the
differences in perspectives associated with their age
differences. In contrast,
when such processes are not in place, the interaction difficulties
caused by
age differences and virtual interactions may lead to lower
creative perfor-
mance. We found that the difference in technical experience
between virtual
552 Small Group Research 42(5)
collaborators was an important moderator of the effects of
difference in age
on creativity, such that it exacerbated the negative effect of
difference in age
on creativity. Difference in technical experience likely cr eated
communica-
tion problems that further increased the difficulty of interaction
beyond that
already caused by a difference in age and the virtual interaction
medium,
thereby reducing creativity. Overall, our findings for age
differences are consis-
tent with the suggestion in prior research that contextual
conditions may be
important in determining the effects of age differences on
outcomes (Ferris,
Judge, Chachere, & Liden, 1991; Shore, Cleveland, & Goldberg,
2003).
Given the strong effects for age differences in our model, we
conducted
post hoc analyses to examine alternative explanations for our
findings. To
determine whether the effects of differences in age were really
due to differ-
ences in experience between virtual collaborators, we reran the
regression
analyses using difference in work experience in place of
difference in age.
We found that whereas difference in work experience
functioned similarly in
the analysis to difference in age, the results were much stronger
for differ-
ence in age. This suggests that difference in work experience
may explain the
effects of difference in age to a large extent but that other
factors associated
with difference in age may also play a part in generating the
effects obtained.
Another argument that can be made to explain the effect of age
difference is
that the older of the virtual collaborators brought to the task
greater life and
work experience, thus increasing the creativity of the
collaborative product.
However, we found that creativity did not correlate significantly
with the
virtual collaborators’ average age (r = -.07, p = ns) or average
amount of
work experience (r = .02, p = ns). Taken together, our post hoc
analyses sug-
gest that the effects we found were more likely due to
differences between
the ages of the two collaborators (which partly reflects
difference in work
experience) rather than due to higher- or lower-average age of
the two virtual
collaborators.
For differences in nationality, we found only one significant
interaction
effect, and that was not entirely in the predicted direction.
Essentially, for vir-
tual collaborators from different nationalities, even relatively
equal technical
experience did not produce a positive effect of difference in
nationality on
creativity, though it did reduce the strength of the negative
effect experienced
by the virtual collaborators with unequal technical experience.
In addition,
though we did not predict a direct effect of a difference in
nationality on
creativity, we did find a relatively strong direct effect. The
finding is consis-
tent with prior research that has found that national differences
create com-
munication problems in cross-national teams (e.g., Kayworth &
Leidner,
2000) and points out that these problems may be amplified by
differences in
Martins and Shalley 553
technical experience between employees working virtually.
Furthermore, the
negative effect for difference in nationality on creativity was
not moderated
by the interaction processes we examined. This lack of
moderation suggests
that the interaction difficulties encountered in cross-national
virtual teams
may not be easily overcome in short-term virtual interactions,
such as those
examined in this study. Given that short-term problem-solving
interactions
among globally distributed employees are becoming
increasingly common in
organizations, our finding suggests that much more research
needs to be con-
ducted in order to understand and manage such interactions.
For differences in race and sex we did not find any significant
effects. In
general, the effects of demographic differences hinge on the
cognitive avail-
ability of demographic characteristics that feed into social
categorization
processes (e.g., van Knippenberg et al., 2004). Since virtual
communication
technologies are low in the richness of information carried, this
reduces the
cognitive availability of demographic differences in virtual
interactions (e.g.,
Nowak, 2003; Sproull & Kiesler, 1986). Thus, rather than react
to surface-
level characteristics using social category stereotypes,
demographically dif-
ferent collaborators may instead focus on differences in virtual
interaction
patterns that may accompany their demographic differences.
Furthermore,
since researchers have found that there are no readily detectable
differences
in the behavior of men and women in a virtual context, such as
in the extent
of interaction (Gefen & Straub, 1997), it is understandable that
there were no
significant effects for difference in sex.
In contrast, differences in age and nationality have been found
to affect
interaction patterns in virtual teams. Individuals from different
nationalities
may encounter difficulties in interacting virtually due to cross -
national dif-
ferences in communication styles and differences in word
connotations even
when communicating in the same language (e.g., Maznevski &
Chudoba,
2000; Zakaria et al., 2004). Furthermore, difficulties may arise
in virtual
interactions between individuals from different nationalities due
to differ-
ences in reliance on body language, facial expressions, gestures,
and physical
distance in communication in their respective countries (Farmer
& Hyatt,
1994). Also, national differences in usage of the English
language may cause
difficulties in communication in virtual teams (e.g., Zakaria et
al., 2004). In
a similar vein, prior research has found that an individual’s age
affects his or
her attitude toward, and comfort with the use of, information
technologies
such as e-mail (e.g., Agarwal & Prasad, 1999; Burton-Jones &
Hubona,
2005). Also, individuals of different ages may communicate
differently, in
terms of the formalness of their communication style, their use
of slang or
554 Small Group Research 42(5)
certain terms, and norms regarding communication in general
(e.g., Lancaster
& Stillman, 2002).
Limitations, Directions for Future
Research, and Contributions
An obvious limitation of this study is the sample size, which
was largely a
consequence of the logistical and financial difficulty in setting
up a large
number of virtual collaborations with the type of students we
used in the
study—graduate business school students—and coding hour-
long chat logs.
As a consequence, we were unable to examine all demographic
differences
and moderator variables together in the same regression
equation. Although
the small sample may reduce confidence in the nonsignificant
findings, it
does make us more confident about the significant effects we
found. In addi-
tion, it is possible that the context in which our study was
conducted (an MBA
program) may have made differences in age salient. However,
given that the
participants on average had more than 5 years of work
experience, this con-
cern is mitigated to some extent. But the effects we found
should be tested in
other samples, in particular in field settings, to establish
broader generaliz-
ability of the findings.
Since the effects of demographic diversity can be different
depending on
time and the type of task, future research should examine these
effects using
different types of tasks that vary in complexity and need for
consensus, as
well as examine these effects over the entire lifecycle of a team.
Also, we
focused on short-lived virtual interactions, which can contribute
to our under-
standing of short-term virtual teams as well as early interactions
in longer-
term virtual teams. On average, virtual teams tend to have a
shorter lifecycle
than face-to-face teams (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999); therefore,
our findings
should apply well to the average virtual team. However, future
research
should examine longer lifecycle virtual teams that will enable
an examina-
tion of different stages in a team’s lifecycle; such an
examination is impor-
tant because how demographic differences affect outcomes may
vary by the
amount of time that a team has spent working together (Harrison
et al., 1998).
In addition, we looked at only two-party virtual collaborations.
If more peo-
ple were involved, the need for effective processes would be
expected to be
even stronger, but this remains an empirical question. Finally, it
should be
pointed out that we examined the effect of demographic
differences on cre-
ativity for CMC exclusively. Although this medium represents a
large part of
virtual work, other ways of working virtually also need to be
examined.
Martins and Shalley 555
This study contributes to research and practice in several ways.
It is one
of the first studies to examine how process and input factors
influence the
relationship between demographic differences and creativity in
a virtual
work context. As such, it contributes to research on creativity,
virtual work,
and diversity. Shalley, Zhou, and Oldham (2004), in their
integration of the
creativity literature, called for more research on team creativity
since prior
research on creativity had tended to focus on individual
creativity, with
only a few studies having examined team creativity (Gilson &
Shalley,
2004; Taggar, 2002). Furthermore, since most employees are
now working
virtually, at least to some extent in their collaboration with
coworkers on
projects (Griffith & Neale, 2001), research is needed that
explores what
aspects are most important for creativity in virtual teams
(Martins et al.,
2004). In this study we start to address this topic by providing
insights into
how demographic differences may affect the performance of
team members
working together virtually on a problem-solving task. In
addition, our study
contributes to developing an understanding of the circumstances
(i.e., mod-
erators) that enable demographically different coworkers to
overcome
interaction difficulties resulting from their differences and from
the limita-
tions of virtual collaboration, and consequently, to improve the
quality of
their performance.
Our findings indicate that demographic differences can be
effectively
used to tease out creative contributions as long as organizations
focus on
important team input and process factors. Given that working
virtually
requires a certain level of technical expertise, attention should
be paid on the
front end to making sure that employees are comfortable with
the technology
and can easily use it to interact with others in their team. This is
particularly
important, as our findings indicate that for virtual teams that are
working
across national boundaries differences in technical abilities may
cause inter-
action difficulties that worsen the interaction difficulties teams
face while
working across nationalities. Also, developing routines that
encourage the
formation of rapport early on in virtual interactions may benefit
performance
when there are large age differences between individuals
working together
virtually. This could be done by encouraging employees to
initially make
time in a virtual interaction to chat and get to know each other.
Finally,
knowledge of our results can lead to the design of process
interventions to
improve creativity. For example, when individuals work
virtually, managers
should pay attention to facilitating the process by providing
training up front
in communication and process management skills. Overall, our
findings also
suggest that developing more elaborate research models is
necessary in order
to better understand the dynamics and outcomes of virtual
teams.
556 Small Group Research 42(5)
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interests with
respect to the authorship
and/or publication of this article.
Financial Disclosure/Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research
and/or authorship of this
article.
References
Agarwal, R., & Prasad, J. (1999). Are individual differences
germane to the acceptance
of new information technologies? Decision Sciences, 30, 361-
391. doi:10.1111/
j.1540-5915.1999.tb01614.x
Aiken, L., West, S., & Reno, R. (1991). Multiple regression:
Testing and interpreting
interactions. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity and innovation in
organizations. In
B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in
organizational behavior (Vol. 10,
pp. 123-167). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO:
Westview.
Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Demography and
design: Predictors of
new product team performance. Organization Science, 3, 321-
341. doi:10.1287/
orsc.3.3.321
Burton-Jones, A., & Hubona, G. (2005). Individual differences
and usage behavior:
Revisiting a technology acceptance model assumption. ACM
SIGMIS Database,
36(2), 58-77. doi:10.1145/1066149.1066155
Byrne, D. E. (1971). The attraction paradigm. San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Coutu, D. L. (1998). Trust in virtual teams. Harvard Business
Review, 76, 20-21.
Dew, R., & Hearn, G. (2009). A new model of the learning
process for innovation
teams: Networked nominal pairs. International Journal of
Innovation Manage-
ment, 13, 521-535. doi:10.1142/S136391960900239X
Edmondson, A. (1999). Psychological safety and learning
behavior in work teams.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 350-383.
doi:10.2307/2666999
Farmer, S. M., & Hyatt, C. W. (1994). Effects of task language
demands and task
complexity on computer-mediated work groups. Small Group
Research, 25, 331-366.
doi:10.1177/1046496494253001
Ferris, G. R., Judge, T. A., Chachere, J. G., & Liden, R. C.
(1991). The age context
of performance-evaluation decisions. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 6, 616-622.
Gefen, D., & Straub, D. W. (1997). Gender differences in the
perception and use
of e-mail: An extension to the technology acceptance model.
MIS Quarterly, 21,
389-400. doi:10.2307/249720
Martins and Shalley 557
Giambatista, R. C., & Bhappu, A. D. (2010). Diversity’s
harvest: Interactions of diver-
sity sources and communication technology on creative group
performance. Orga-
nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111, 116-
126. doi:10.1016/
j.obhdp.2009.11.003
Gilson, L. L., & Shalley, C. E. (2004). A little creativity goes a
long way: An exami-
nation of teams’ engagement in creative processes. Journal of
Management, 30,
453-470. doi:10.1016/j.jm.2003.07.001
Griffith, T. L., & Neale, M. A. (2001). Information processing
in traditional, hybrid,
and virtual teams: From nascent knowledge to transactive
memory. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 23, 379-421.
Griffith, T. L., Sawyer, J. E., & Neale, M. A. (2003).
Virtualness and knowledge in
teams: Managing the love triangle of organizations, individuals,
and information
technology. MIS Quarterly, 27, 265-287. doi:10.2307/30036531
Hansen, M. T., Nohria, N., & Tierney, T. (1999). What’s your
strategy for managing
knowledge? Harvard Business Review, 77(2), 106-116.
Hargadon, A. B., & Bechky, B. A. (2006). When collections of
creatives become creative
collectives: A field study of problem solving at work.
Organization Science, 17,
484-500. doi:10.1287/orsc.1060.0200
Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., & Bell, M. P. (1998). Beyond
relational demography:
Time and the effects of surface- and deep-level diversity on
work group cohesion.
Academy of Management Journal, 41, 96-107.
Hoffman, L. R., & Maier, N. R. F. (1961). Quality and
acceptance of problem solu-
tions by members of homogeneous and heterogeneous groups.
Journal of Abnor-
mal and Social Psychology, 62, 401-407. doi:10.1037/h0044025
Jackson, S. E., & Joshi, A. (2004). Diversity in social context:
A multi-attribute, mul-
tilevel analysis of team diversity and sales performance. Journal
of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 25, 675-702. doi:10.1002/job.265
Jarvenpaa, S. L., & Leidner, D. E. (1999). Communication and
trust in global virtual
teams. Organization Science, 10, 791-815.
doi:10.1287/orsc.10.6.791
Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of
conflict: A longitudi-
nal study of intragroup conflict and group performance.
Academy of Management
Journal, 44, 238-251. doi:10.2307/3069453
Kayworth, T., & Leidner, D. (2000). The global virtual
manager: A prescription
for success. European Management Journal, 18, 183-194.
doi:10.1016/S0263-
2373(99)00090-0
Kruempel, K. (2000). Making the right (interactive) moves for
knowledge-producing
tasks in computer-mediated groups. IEEE Transactions on
Professional Commu-
nication, 43, 185-195. doi:10.1109/47.843645
Lancaster, L. C., & Stillman, D. (2002). When generations
collide. San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
558 Small Group Research 42(5)
Magadley, W., & Birdi, K. (2009). Innovation labs: An
examination into the use of
physical spaces to enhance organizati onal creativity. Creativity
and Innovation
Management, 18, 315-325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8691.2009.00540.x
Majchrzak, A., Malhotra, A., Stamps, J., & Lipnack, J. (2004).
Can absence make a
team grow stronger? Harvard Business Review, 82(5), 131-137.
Martins, L. L., Gilson, L. L., & Maynard, M. T. (2004). Virtual
teams: What do
we know and where do we go from here? Journal of
Management, 30, 805-835.
doi:10.1016/j.jm.2004.05.002
Maznevski, M. L., & Chudoba, K. M. (2000). Bridging space
over time: Global
virtual team dynamics and effectiveness. Organization Science,
11, 473-492.
doi:10.1287/orsc.11.5.473.15200
McDonough, E. F., Kahn, K. B., & Barczak, G. (2001). An
investigation of the use of
global, virtual, and colocated new product development teams.
Journal of Product
Innovation Management, 18, 110-120. doi:10.1111/1540-
5885.1820110
McLeod, P. L., & Lobel, S. A. (1992). The effects of ethnic
diversity on idea generation
in small groups. Academy of Management Best Paper
Proceedings, pp. 227-231.
Milliken, F. J., Bartel, C. A., & Kurtzberg, T. (2003). Diversity
and creativity in work
groups: A dynamic perspective on the affective and cognitive
processes that link
diversity and performance. In P. Paulus & B. Nijstad (Eds.),
Group creativity
(pp. 32-62). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Milliken, F. J., & Martins, L. L. (1996). Searching for common
threads: Understand-
ing the multiple effects of diversity in organizational groups.
Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 21, 402-433. doi:10.2307/258667
Montoya-Weiss, M. M., Massey, A. P., & Song, M. (2001).
Getting it together: Tem-
poral coordination and conflict management in global virtual
teams. Academy of
Management Journal, 44, 1251-1262. doi:10.2307/3069399
Nemeth, C. J. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and
minority influence.
Psychological Review, 93(1), 23-32. doi:10.1037/0033-
295x.93.1.23
Nemiro, J. E. (2002). The creative process in virtual teams.
Creativity Research Journal,
14(1), 69-83. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1401_6
Nowak, K. L. (2003). Sex categorization in computer mediated
communication
(CMC): Exploring the utopian promise. Media Psychology, 5,
83-103. doi:10.1207/
S1532785XMEP0501_4
Ocker, R. J. (2005). Influences on creativity in asynchronous
virtual teams: A qualita-
tive analysis of experimental teams. IEEE Transactions on
Professional Commu-
nication, 48(1), 22-39. doi:10.1109/TPC.2004.843294
Paulus, P. (2000). Groups, teams, and creativity: The creative
potential of idea-
generating groups. Applied Psychology, 49, 237-262.
doi:10.1111/1464-0597.00013
Payne, R. (1990). The effectiveness of research teams: A
review. In M. West &
J. Farr (Eds.), Innovation and creativity at work: Psychological
and organiza-
tional strategies (pp. 101-122). New York, NY: John Wiley.
Martins and Shalley 559
Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Exploring
the black box: An
analysis of work group diversity, conflict, and performance.
Administrative Sci-
ence Quarterly, 44, 1-28. doi:10.2307/2667029
Pirola-Merlo, A., & Mann, L. (2004). The relationship between
individual creativity
and team creativity: Aggregating across people and time.
Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 25, 235-257. doi:10.1002/job.240
Sarker, S., Lau, F., & Sahay, S. (2001). Using an adapted
grounded theory approach
for inductive theory building about virtual team development.
Database for
Advances in Information Systems, 32(1), 38-56.
doi:10.1145/506740.506745
Sarker, S., & Sahay, S. (2002). Information systems
development by US-Norwegian
virtual teams: Implications of time and space. Proceedings of
the thirty-fifth
annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences
(January), pp. 1-10.
Saunders, C. S. (2000). Virtual teams: Piecing together the
puzzle. In R. W. Zmud
(Ed.) Framing the domain of IT management: Projecting the
future through the
past, pp.29-50. Cincinnati, OH: Pinnaflex.
Shalley, C. E. (1991). Effects of productivity goals, creativity
goals, and personal
discretion on individual creativity. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 76, 179-185.
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.76.2.179
Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of coaction, expected evaluation,
and goal setting
on creativity and productivity. Academy of Management
Journal, 38, 483-503.
doi:10.2307/256689
Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-
psychological factors on
creative performance: The role of informational and controlling
expected evalu-
ation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision
Processes, 84, 1-22. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2918
Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2008). The emergence of
team creative cogni-
tion: The role of diverse outside ties, socio-cognitive network
centrality, and team
evolution. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal, 2, 23-41. doi:
10.1002/sej
Shalley, C. E., Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2004). The effects of
personal and con-
textual characteristics on creativity: Where should we go from
here? Journal of
Management, 30, 933-958. doi:10.1016/j.jm.2004.06.007
Shore, L. M., Cleveland, J. N., & Goldberg, C. B. (2003). Work
attitudes and deci-
sions as a function of manager age and employee age. Journal of
Applied Psychol-
ogy, 88, 529-537. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.529
Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues:
Electronic mail in
organizational communication. Management Science, 32, 1492-
1512. doi:10.1287/
mnsc.32.11.1492
Straus, S. G., & McGrath, J. E. (1994). Does the medium
matter? The interaction of
task type and technology on group performance and member
reactions. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 79, 87-97. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.79.1.87
560 Small Group Research 42(5)
Stroebe, W., & Diehl, M. (1994). Why groups are less effective
than their members:
On productivity losses in idea-generating groups. European
Review of Social Psy-
chology, 5, 271-303. doi:10.1080/14792779543000084
Suchan, J., & Hayzak, G. (2001). The communication
characteristics of virtual teams:
A case study. IEEE Transactions on Professional
Communication, 44, 174-186.
doi:10.1109/47.946463
Taggar, S. (2002). Individual creativity and group ability to
utilize individual creative
resources: A multilevel model. Academy of Management
Journal, 45, 315-330.
doi:10.2307/3069349
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies
in social psychology.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Townsend, A. M., DeMarie, S. M., & Hendrickson, A. R.
(1998). Virtual teams:
Technology and the workplace of the future. Academy of
Management Executive,
12(3), 17-29.
Tsui, A. S., & O’Reilly, C. A., III. (1989). Beyond simple
demographic effects: The
importance of relational demography in superior-subordinate
dyads. Academy of
Management Journal, 32, 402-423. doi:10.2307/256368
Tsui, A. S., Xin, K. R., & Egan, T. D. (1995). Relational
demography: The missing
link in vertical dyad linkage. In S. E. Jackson & M. N.
Ruderman (Eds.), Diver-
sity in work teams: Research paradigms for a changing
workplace (pp. 97-129).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
van Knippenberg, D., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Homan, A. C.
(2004). Work group diver-
sity and group performance: An integrative model and research
agenda. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 89, 1008-1022. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.89.6.1008
van Knippenberg, D., & Schippers, M. C. (2007). Work group
diversity. Annual Review
of Psychology, 58, 515-541.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085546
Watson, W. E., Kumar, K., & Michaelsen, L. K. (1993).
Cultural diversity’s impact
on interaction process and performance: Comparing
homogeneous and diverse
task groups. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 590-602.
Webber, S. S., & Donahue, L. M. (2001). Impact of highly and
less job-related diversity
on work group cohesion and performance: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Manage-
ment, 27, 141-162. doi:10.1177/014920630102700202
Williams K. Y., & O’Reilly, C. A. (1998). Demography and
diversity in organiza-
tions: A review of 40 years of research. In B. M. Staw & L. L.
Cummings (Eds.),
Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 77-140).
Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993).
Toward a theory of organiza-
tional creativity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 293-
321. doi:10.2307/258761
Zakaria, N., Amelinckx, A., & Wilemon, D. (2004). Working
together apart? Build-
ing a knowledge-sharing culture for global virtual teams.
Creativity & Innovation
Management, 13, 15-29. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8691.2004.00290.x
Martins and Shalley 561
Zhou, J. (1998). Feedback valence, feedback style, task
autonomy, and achievement
orientation: Interactive effects on creative performance. Journal
of Applied Psy-
chology, 83, 261-276. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.261
Bios
Luis L. Martins is an associate professor at the McCombs
School of Business, Uni-
versity of Texas at Austin, USA. He received his PhD from the
Stern School of
Business, New York University. His research examines the
dynamics of diversity,
particularly in the context of virtual teams and global virtual
work.
Christina E. Shalley is the Thomas R. Williams-Wachovia and
ADVANCE profes-
sor at the College of Management at the Georgia Institute of
Technology, USA. She
received her PhD in business administration from the University
of Illinois, Urbana–
Champaign. Her research examines the effects of social and
contextual factors on
creativity and innovation.
Research Alternative Assignment:
This option involves writing a review and critique of an
academic research article. A review should include:
· A statement in your own words of what you thought the author
hoped to discover.
· A brief description of the research approach that was used
including the nature of the sample, the way key variables were
measured, and the type of analysis used.
· A summary of the key results of the study.
· A description of what you think the results of the article
suggest for how organizations and/or their managers might
change the way they operate in the future.
· Your overall evaluation of the article. What did you like about
the article? Why? What did you not like? Why?
Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief  1  Guidelines for

More Related Content

Similar to Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief 1 Guidelines for

Communicating research for policy
Communicating research for policyCommunicating research for policy
Communicating research for policyAndrew Dunne
 
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docx
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docxClass,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docx
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docxclarebernice
 
Writing for policymakers
Writing for policymakersWriting for policymakers
Writing for policymakersresyst
 
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docx
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docxProject Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docx
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docxpoulterbarbara
 
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs 1AN ESSENTIA.docx
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs  1AN ESSENTIA.docxAn Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs  1AN ESSENTIA.docx
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs 1AN ESSENTIA.docxdaniahendric
 
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxhow-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxssuser59e4b8
 
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptx
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptxhow to write a policy brief reserch.pptx
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptxjose83668
 
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxhow-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxssuser4858bf1
 
Business Report Writing.pptx
Business Report Writing.pptxBusiness Report Writing.pptx
Business Report Writing.pptxanniemalik37
 
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docx
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docxThe fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docx
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docxmehek4
 
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action Plan
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action PlanENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action Plan
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action PlanJodie Nicotra
 
2 policy brief n press release (yuti)
2   policy brief n press release (yuti)2   policy brief n press release (yuti)
2 policy brief n press release (yuti)Syahyuti Si-Buyuang
 
Chapter 3.pptx
Chapter 3.pptxChapter 3.pptx
Chapter 3.pptxFitsumKS
 
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business letter
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business letterUnit II- Reports & Proposals. business letter
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business lettermohitvarshney725
 

Similar to Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief 1 Guidelines for (20)

Communicating research for policy
Communicating research for policyCommunicating research for policy
Communicating research for policy
 
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docx
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docxClass,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docx
Class,The balanced scorecard has four categoriesquadrants, na.docx
 
Writing for policymakers
Writing for policymakersWriting for policymakers
Writing for policymakers
 
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docx
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docxProject Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docx
Project Description1. Describe the broad objective or purpo.docx
 
What is research
What is researchWhat is research
What is research
 
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs 1AN ESSENTIA.docx
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs  1AN ESSENTIA.docxAn Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs  1AN ESSENTIA.docx
An Essential Guide to Writing Policy Briefs 1AN ESSENTIA.docx
 
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxhow-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
 
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptx
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptxhow to write a policy brief reserch.pptx
how to write a policy brief reserch.pptx
 
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptxhow-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
how-to-write-a-policy-brief_1 (2).pptx
 
The art of policy briefs
The art of policy briefsThe art of policy briefs
The art of policy briefs
 
Business Report Writing.pptx
Business Report Writing.pptxBusiness Report Writing.pptx
Business Report Writing.pptx
 
Report writing.pdf
Report writing.pdfReport writing.pdf
Report writing.pdf
 
Report writing.pdf
Report writing.pdfReport writing.pdf
Report writing.pdf
 
Report writing.pdf
Report writing.pdfReport writing.pdf
Report writing.pdf
 
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docx
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docxThe fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docx
The fight for Pre-Exposure Prophylaxis in the United Kingdom. Co.docx
 
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action Plan
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action PlanENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action Plan
ENGL 318 Detailed Assignment Sheet: Report and Communication Action Plan
 
2 policy brief n press release (yuti)
2   policy brief n press release (yuti)2   policy brief n press release (yuti)
2 policy brief n press release (yuti)
 
Chapter 3.pptx
Chapter 3.pptxChapter 3.pptx
Chapter 3.pptx
 
Report EAPP.pptx
Report EAPP.pptxReport EAPP.pptx
Report EAPP.pptx
 
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business letter
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business letterUnit II- Reports & Proposals. business letter
Unit II- Reports & Proposals. business letter
 

More from JeanmarieColbert3

Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docx
Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docxHai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docx
Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docx
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docxGuys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docx
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docx
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docxhare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docx
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docx
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docxHacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docx
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docx
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docxHA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docx
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docx
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docxHA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docx
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1. para la clase a la.docx
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1.  para la clase a la.docxhacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1.  para la clase a la.docx
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1. para la clase a la.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docx
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docxH07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docx
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docx
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docxGuidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docx
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docx
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docxGuidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docx
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docx
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docxHA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docx
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docx
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docxGuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docx
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docx
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docxGuidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docx
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docx
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docxGuided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docx
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docx
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docxGuided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docx
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docx
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docxGuided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docx
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docx
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docxGuided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docx
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docx
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docxGuided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docx
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docx
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docxGuided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docx
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docxJeanmarieColbert3
 
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docx
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docxGuided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docx
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docxJeanmarieColbert3
 

More from JeanmarieColbert3 (20)

Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docx
Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docxHai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docx
Hai,this is Anusha. am looking for a help with my research.docx
 
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docx
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docxGuys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docx
Guys I need your help with my international law class, Its a course.docx
 
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docx
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docxhare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docx
hare some memories of encounters with people who had very different .docx
 
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docx
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docxHacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docx
Hacker or SupporterAnswer ONE of the following questionsQuestio.docx
 
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docx
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docxHA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docx
HA415 Unit 6Discussion TopicHealthcare systems are huge, compl.docx
 
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docx
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docxHA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docx
HA410 Unit 7 AssignmentUnit outcomes addressed in this Assignment.docx
 
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1. para la clase a la.docx
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1.  para la clase a la.docxhacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1.  para la clase a la.docx
hacer oír salir suponer traer ver 1. para la clase a la.docx
 
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docx
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docxH07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docx
H07 Medical Coding IDirections  Be sure to make an electronic c.docx
 
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docx
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docxGuidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docx
Guidelines1.Paper consisting of 2,000-2,250 words; however,.docx
 
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docx
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docxGuidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docx
Guidelines12-point fontCambria fontSingle space50 words ma.docx
 
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docx
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docxHA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docx
HA425 Unit 2 discussion- Organizational Behavior and Management in H.docx
 
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docx
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docxGuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docx
GuidelinesPaper  is based on one novel , Frankenstein. We ha.docx
 
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docx
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docxGuidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docx
Guidelines1.Paper word count should be 1,000-1,250. Refer.docx
 
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docx
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docxGuided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docx
Guided Response Respond to at least two of your classmates.  Ch.docx
 
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docx
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docxGuided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docx
Guided ResponseReview the philosophies of education that your.docx
 
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docx
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docxGuided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docx
Guided Response  When responding to your peers, suggest ways to.docx
 
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docx
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docxGuided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docx
Guided Response As you read the responses of your classmates, con.docx
 
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docx
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docxGuided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docx
Guided ResponseReview several of your classmates’ posts and res.docx
 
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docx
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docxGuided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docx
Guided ResponseYou must reply to at least one classmate. As y.docx
 
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docx
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docxGuided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docx
Guided ResponseRespond to at least one classmate that has been .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 

Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief 1 Guidelines for

  • 1. Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 1 Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief What is a Policy Brief? The Policy Brief is a “short, neutral summary of what is known about a particular issue or problem. Policy briefs are a form of report designed to facilitate policy- making” (Eisele, 2006). The main purpose is to “succinctly evaluate policy options regarding a specific issue, for a specific policy-maker audience” (Eisele, n.d.). Policy-makers need to make practical decisions under time-constraints, so the brief should provide evidence and actionable recommendations (Eisele, n.d.). The issue brief distils or synthesizes a large amount of complex detail, so the reader can easily understand the heart of the issue, its background, the players (“stakeholders”) and any recommendations, or even educated guesses about the future of the issue. It may have tables and graphs; usually, it has a short list of references, so the reader knows something about the sources on which it is based, and where to go for more information. Most of the time, the brief has its own “brief”--a one page “executive summary,” allowing the reader to quickly grasp the essence of the report (Eisele, n.d.). In short, “the purpose of the policy brief is to convince the target audience of the urgency of the current problem and the need to adopt the preferred alternative or course of action outlined and therefore, serve as an impetus for action” (Young & Quinn, n.d.).
  • 2. What are the components of a Policy Brief? (Lifted from Tsai, 2006) Executive summary The executive summary aims to convince the reader further that the brief is worth in-depth investigation. It is especially important for an audience that is short of time to clearly see the relevance and importance of the brief in reading the summary. As such, a 1 to 2 paragraph executive summary commonly includes: 1. A description of the problem addressed; 2. A statement on why the current approach/policy option needs to be changed; 3. Your recommendations for action. Context and importance of the problem The purpose of this element of the brief is to convince the target audience that a current and urgent problem exists which requires them to take action. The context and importance of the problem is both the introductory and first building block of the brief. As such, it usually includes the following: 1. A clear statement of the problem or issue in focus. 2. A short overview of the root causes of the problem 3. A clear statement of the policy implications of the problem that clearly establishes the current importance and policy relevance of the issue. It is worth noting that the length of the problem description may vary considerably from brief to brief depending on the stage on the policy process in focus, e.g. there may be a need to have a much more extensive problem description for policy at the evaluation stage than for one at the option choosing stage. Policy Brief versus Research Paper (Tsai, 2006) Some might say that a policy brief is more “professional” because it is geared towards readers who have a limited amount of time to make a practical decision, while a research paper is
  • 3. more “academic” because it pays more attention to the scholarly roots of particular arguments and judges their merit on intellectual and logical criteria. Front-loaded! Policy briefs are front-loaded: the conclusions are on the front page! The front page needs an executive summary, providing a concise (1 or 2 paragraphs) overview of the brief’s aim and core recommendations (Kopenski, 2010). Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 2 Critique of policy option(s) The aim of this element is to detail shortcomings of the current approach or options being implemented and therefore, illustrate both the need for change and focus of where change needs to occur. In doing so, the critique of policy options usually includes the following: 1. A short overview of the policy option(s) in focus 2. An argument illustrating why and how the current or proposed approach is failing. It is important for the sake of credibility to recognize all opinions in the debate of the issue. Policy recommendations The aim of the policy recommendations element is to provide a detailed and convincing proposal of how the failings of the current policy approach need to change. As such this is achieved by including: 1. A breakdown of the specific practical steps or measures that need to be implemented 2. Sometimes also includes a closing paragraph re-emphasizing the importance of action. Appendices Although the brief is a short and targeted document, authors sometimes decide that their argumen t needs further support and so include an appendix. Appendices should be included only when absolutely necessary.
  • 4. What a persuasive Policy Brief should be (Lifted from Young and Quinn, n.d.) As with all good marketing tools, the key to success is targeting the particular audience for your message. The most common audience for a policy brief is the decision- maker but, it is also not unusual to use the document to support broader advocacy initiatives targeting a wide but knowledgeable audience (e.g. decision makers, journalists, diplomats, administrators, researchers). In constructing a policy brief that can effectively serve its intended purpose, it is common for a brief to be: FOCUSED All aspects of the policy brief (from the message to the layout) need to strategically focused on achieving the intended goal of convincing the target audience. For example, the argument provided must build on what they do know about the problem, provide insight about what they don’t know about the problem and be presented in language that reflects their values, i.e. using ideas, evidence and language that will convince them. PROFESSIONAL, NOT ACADEMIC The common audience for a policy brief is not interested in the research/analysis procedures conducted to produce the evidence, but are very interested to know the writer’s perspective on the problem and potential solutions based on the new evidence. EVIDENCED-BASED The policy brief is a communication tool produced by policy analysts and therefore all potential audiences not only expect a rational argument but will only be convinced by argumentation supported by evidence that the problem exists and the consequences of adopting particular alternatives. LIMITED To provide adequately comprehensive but targeted argument within a limited space, the focus of the brief needs to be limited to a particular problem or area of a problem.
  • 5. Do’s and Dont’s (Kopenski, 2010) Avoid technical, legalistic, economic or academic jargon! Presentation needs to be professional as suits a public document. Use correct grammar and spelling, and appropriate spacing, font, point, headings and sub-headings. Facilitate readability through images, catch-phrases, layout choices, and the provision of data as graphs or charts. Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 3 SUCCINT The type of audiences targeted commonly do not have the time or inclination to read an in-depth 20 page argument on a policy problem. Therefore, it is common that policy briefs do not exceed 6 – 8 pages in length (i.e. usually not longer than 3,000 words). UNDERSTANDABLE This not only refers to using clear and simple language (i.e. not the jargon and concepts of an academic discipline) but also to providing a well explained and easy to follow argument targeting a wide but knowledgeable audience. ACCESSIBLE The writer of the policy brief should facilitate the ease of use of the document by the target audience and therefore, should subdivide the text using clear descriptive titles to guide the reader. PROMOTIONAL The policy brief should catch the eye of the potential audience in order to create a favourable impression (e.g. professional, innovative etc) In this way many brief writers many of the features of the promotional leaflet (use of colour, use of logos, photographs, slogans, illustrative quotes etc). PRACTICAL AND FEASIBLE The policy brief is an action- oriented tool targeting policy practitioners. As such the brief
  • 6. must provide arguments based on what is actually happening in practice with a particular policy and propose recommendations which seem realistic to the target audience 6 Steps for a compelling Policy Brief (Lifted from Young and Quinn, n.d.) 1. Issue: examine the issue you will be dealing with. Answer these questions: is the issue general or specific? How general/specific? 2. Audience: take your primary audience into serious consideration. Your brief should be tailored to the needs of your audience. It makes a fundamental difference for how you must frame your analysis and your recommendation. Is your audience an individual (i.e. Prime Minister) or an organization (i.e. the Government as a whole)? 3. Actors: identify the relevant actors for the issue you are dealing with. This is an essential step, since you will have to analyze their interests in order to make sensible and viable policy recommendations. Identifying the relevant actors is also essential to produce a good assessment of the context and of the interests that are plug into the issue. 4. Interests: once you have identified the relevant actors, it is necessary to analyze their interests. What are the actors' interests? Which of the relevant actors have similar interests to your audience? Which ones have different interests? How different? This step is important both for the context part of your brief and for the critique of policy options/policy recommendations. Without a clear identification of the actors involved in the issue and their interests, your brief will result vague, and therefore not useful. 5. Recommendations: your policy recommendations should reflect the above analysis. Remember that, according to the issue and the audience, your recommendation(s) might not suggest the best policy, but instead the most viable one. This should not limit your recommendation to just compromise policies. If you want to recommend radical change, you can; remember though that such radical action has to be implemented in some ways. 6.
  • 7. How-To: the last step is to suggest your audience the way to 'sell' the policy to its public (the public could be other members of the organizations, voters, other parties, etc.). This last step helps your audience build support/consensus to implement the policy you recommended. Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 4 Online Resources You can check the following policy briefs online to serve as your guides in writing your own policy briefs: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations Policy Brief on the 2007-08 Rice Price Crisis: http://www.fao.org/docrep/013/am172e/am172e00.pdf The Poverty and Economic Policy Policy Briefs on Food and Fuel Crises on Cambodia, Ghana and the Philippines: http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16735 http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16736 http://portal.pep-net.org/documents/download/id/16737 The Poverty and Economic Policy Brief on Effects of the GFC in the Philippines: http://portal.pep- net.org/documents/download/id/16739 The Royal Children’s Hospital Policy Brief on Evidence-Based Practice: http://www.rch.org.au/emplibrary/ccch/Policy_Brief_21_- _Evidence_based_practice_final_web.pdf Submission Guidelines: 1. All submissions should be in MS Word Format (.doc, .docx), 2 pages in length and should be in a 2-column form. 2. Images, tables, charts and graphs present in the layout of the Policy Brief should be submitted separately, i.e., all .jpg, .png, .bmp, .gif, .psd, etc., should be submitted individually. 3. Check policy briefs from the Poverty and Economic Policy (PEP) Website (www.pep-net.org),
  • 8. particularly those from the Community-Based Monitoring System (CBMS) to serve as guides in preparing your policy briefs. Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief | 5 References: Eisele, F (n.d.). Preparing a Policy Brief Issue [PDF Document]. Retrieved from
  • 9. https://www.courses.psu.edu/hpa/hpa301_fre1/IBInstructions_fa 02.PDF Kopenski, Marc (March 2010). Policy Briefs. Retrieved from the Richmond University Website http://www.richmond.ac.uk/content/library/subjects/politics/poli cy-briefs.aspx Tsai (May 2006). Guidelines for Writing a Policy Brief [PDF Document]. Retrieved from http://jhunix.hcf.jhu.edu/~ktsai/policybrief.html. Young, E. and Quinn, L (n.d.). The Policy Brief [PDF Documnet]. Retrieved from http://www.policy.hu/ipf/fel-pubs/samples/PolicyBrief- described.pdf Young, E. and Quinn, L (n.d.). The Policy Brief: Instructions [PDF Document]. Retrieved from http://sobek.colorado.edu/~salucci/teaching/teaching_portfolio/a ssets/Policy_Brief_instructions.pdf Prepared by the Community-Based Monitoring System (CBMS) Network Coordinating Team 10th Floor Angelo King International Center Estrada corner Arellano Streets, Malate, Manila Philippines 1004 Telephone: (02) 5262067 Email: [email protected]; [email protected] Website: http://www.pep-net.org; http://www.cbmsphilippines.webs.com/ Small Group Research 42(5) 536 –561 © The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
  • 10. DOI: 10.1177/1046496410397382 http://sgr.sagepub.com Creativity in Virtual Work: Effects of Demographic Differences Luis L. Martins1 and Christina E. Shalley2 Abstract Organizations are increasingly using virtual teams, in which individuals work with their teammates across distance and differences, using a variety of information and communication technologies. In this study, the authors examined how demographic differences (i.e., differences in race, sex, age, and nationality) between individuals working virtually affected their collective creativity. Specifically, the authors examined how demographic differences interacted with the nature of interaction processes (establishment of rapport, participation equality, and process conflict) and difference in technical experience, to affect creativity in short- term virtual work interactions. Differences in age interacted with the processes and with differences in technical experience to affect creativity. Differences in
  • 11. nationality had a strong negative direct effect and interacted with differences in technical experience to affect creativity. Differences in sex and race did not significantly affect creativity. Implications of findings for managing virtual teams are discussed. Keywords creativity, demographic differences, virtual teams 1The University of Texas at Austin, USA 2Georgia Institute of Technology, USA Corresponding Author: Luis L. Martins, University of Texas at Austin, McCombs School of Business, 1 University Station, B6300, Austin, TX 78712-0210 Email: [email protected] http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F10464964 10397382&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2011-02-28 Martins and Shalley 537 With an increase in global competition, companies have been looking to cre- ativity and innovation to give them a competitive edge (Amabile, 1988; Magadley & Birdi, 2009). Thus, effectively utilizing knowledge resources wherever they may reside in the organization has become an important stra- tegic priority for organizations (e.g., Dew & Hearn, 2009).
  • 12. Until recently, much of organizational knowledge was locked within individuals and units separated by various boundaries. However, with advances in information and communication technologies, organizations are increasingly using virtual teams to break down boundaries and connect employees regardless of their geographic location and subunit affiliation, so that they can combine their knowledge and perspectives to produce creative solutions to various business problems (e.g., McDonough, Kahn, & Barczak, 2001; Townsend, DeMarie, & Hendrickson, 1998). Indeed, increased creativity and innovation have been touted as among the primary benefits of using virtual teams (e.g., Zakaria, Amelinckx, & Wilemon, 2004). Thus, for example, consulting firms such as Bain and Company and McKinsey and Company use information technology (e.g., e-mail, instant messaging, and databases with the contact information and areas of expertise of every consultant) and other communication tools to enable consultants to reach peers in the company’s globally distributed work- force to work collaboratively on client problems as the needs arise, which contributes to their organizations’ ability to provide innovative solutions to their clients (Hansen, Nohria, & Tierney, 1999). A natural consequence of the increase in the prominence of
  • 13. global virtual teams is that individuals are increasingly working virtually with others who are demographically different from themselves (Griffith & Neale, 2001; Griffith, Sawyer, & Neale, 2003). The extant literature on creativity has gen- erally proposed that demographic differences have the potential to be benefi- cial for creativity because demographically different individuals working together are able to bring to the task different perspectives and approaches (e.g., Milliken & Martins, 1996). On the other hand, researchers have found that, in the short run, demographic differences make it harder for team mem- bers to work together, thus potentially reducing their creative performance (e.g., see van Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007, for a recent review). Whereas there is a growing literature on the effects of demographic differences on creativity in more traditional face-to-face teams (e.g., Hoffman & Maier, 1961; Nemeth, 1986), the effects have not been examined much in virtual teams. This study addresses this gap in the literature by examining how demographic differences interact with group process and input conditions to affect creativity in short-term virtual work interactions. 538 Small Group Research 42(5)
  • 14. Of the variety of approaches that can be used to study dynamics in virtual teams, we focused on dyadic interactions within virtual teams (i.e., on virtual collaborations between any two members of a team). Although there are groupware systems that enable whole groups to interact at the same time, much of the interaction in virtual teams involves two team members at any one time collaborating virtually on a component of their team’s task (e.g., Majchrzak, Malhotra, Stamps, & Lipnack, 2004). Thus, though meetings of whole virtual teams occur episodically, their members’ day-to-day interactions are typically dyadic. For example, it is typical for a knowledge worker based in the United States to work with a colleague in Spain, one in Ireland, and one in India, at different times on different parts of the same task or project. Indeed, Dew and Hearn (2009) found that aggregating the creativity of pairs of collaborators within teams produced similar results as for the whole team, leading them to suggest that “structuring innovation teams into networked, nominal pairs may be just as productive as purely nominal group structures” (p. 521). Further- more, the limited examination of the role played by demographic differences in dyadic interactions has been pointed out as a deficiency in the literature on diversity in teams in general (e.g., Tsui, Xin, & Egan, 1995) and
  • 15. in virtual teams in particular (Martins, Gilson, & Maynard, 2004). Our examination of how the interactions of demographic differences with group process and input conditions affect creativity in virtual collaborations contributes to the literature in several ways. Whereas there is a strong interest among managers in using virtual teams to enhance creativity in their organi- zations, very few studies have empirically examined creativity in a virtual work context (e.g., Nemiro, 2002; Ocker, 2005), and fewer still have looked at how demographic differences affect creativity or innovation in virtual con- texts (e.g., Giambatista & Bhappu, 2010; Gibson & Gibbs, 2006). Recently, researchers have argued that most work in organizations is now virtual to a greater or lesser extent, depending on the amount of time the employees spend working together on a task, the extent to which they use technology to support their interactions, and their geographic and temporal separation (e.g., Griffith & Neale, 2001; Martins et al., 2004). Therefore, these researchers have suggested that we need to move beyond comparing virtual to face-to- face teams and, instead, empirically examine variation in behavioral phe- nomena within virtual teams. The moderated effects examined in this study advance understanding of the circumstances affecting the ability
  • 16. of virtual collaborators to leverage their knowledge resources, which prior researchers (e.g., Martins et al., 2004; Ocker, 2005) have suggested is an important area in need of further research. Specifically, the examination of moderated effects Martins and Shalley 539 enriches theory on the relationships between demographic differences and creativity by investigating how various process conditions and an input fac- tor determine whether demographic differences benefit or hurt creativity in virtual work (van Knippenberg, De Dreu, & Homan, 2004; van Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007). Theory and Hypotheses It is important to specify the boundary conditions of the model we develop and test in this study, as has been recommended in both the diversity and the virtual team literatures (Martins et al., 2004; Webber & Donahue, 2001). Our unit of analysis was dyadic interactions, which comprise most of the day-to- day work dynamics within virtual teams. The demographic differences we focused on were differences in race, sex, age, and nationality, which are among the major dimensions of demographic difference examined in
  • 17. prior research (e.g., Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989). The virtual working technology we focused on was computer-mediated communication (CMC; specifically, electronic chat room), which forms a large component of virtual work, particularly among geographically and temporally distributed individuals (Griffith et al., 2003). In addition, we focused on short-term dyadic collaborations aimed at solving immediate managerial problems. This short-term time perspective was chosen for a few reasons. First, virtual interactions for short-term problem solving are prevalent in virtual teams (Martins et al., 2004). In addition, virtual teams have been found to have a shorter lifecycle than face-to-face teams, as they are brought together as needed to work on specific tasks (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999). Finally, since membership in virtual work groups is often fluid, dyadic interactions with any one team member often are of a one-time or short-term nature (Zakaria et al., 2004). Finally, we looked at variation within virtual work, as has been recommended by researchers (e.g., Griffith et al., 2003), rather than comparing virtual to face-to-face work. Effects of Demographic Differences on Creativity Creativity is defined as the production of novel, potentially useful ideas about work products, practices, services, or procedures (Amabile, 1996; Shalley,
  • 18. 1995). It is a major part of work quality and effectiveness and is increasingly valued for a variety of tasks, occupations, and industries (Amabile, 1988). In collaborative work, creativity requires the pooling together and effective integration of different perspectives, knowledge, skills, and abilities on a 540 Small Group Research 42(5) task (Hargadon & Bechky, 2006; Taggar, 2002; Woodman, Sawyer, & Griffin, 1993). Research on diversity suggests two opposing expectations regarding the effects of demographic differences on creativity, based on the contrasting predictions of what have been termed the information/deci sion- making per- spective and the social categorization and similarity/attraction perspective (Williams & O’Reilly, 1998). Using the information/decision- making per- spective, researchers have proposed that differences in perspective and expe- riences underlying demographic differences should result in a greater range of information, ideas, and approaches to a problem being generated and, in turn, improved creative problem solving (e.g., Nemeth, 1986; Pelled, Eisenhardt, & Xin, 1999). Also, they have found that working
  • 19. with different others may stimulate consideration of nonobvious alternatives that could potentially lead to higher creativity (McLeod & Lobel, 1992). Similarly, the group brainstorming literature (see Paulus, 2000) has found that differences may be beneficial for the generation of more novel ideas. For example, McLeod and Lobel found that ethnically diverse groups produced higher- quality ideas. Also, culturally heterogeneous groups were found to generate more alternatives in the long run (Watson, Kumar, & Michaelsen, 1993). Thus, this perspective suggests that demographic differences have the potential to contribute to creativity by increasing the number of unique ideas brought to bear on a task (Milliken, Bartel, & Kurtzberg, 2003). On the other hand, using the social categorization and similarity/attraction perspective, the literature suggests that demographic differences can lead to a variety of process losses leading to negative effects on team performance. This expectation is based on a variety of sociocognitive theories, especially the argument that individuals are attracted to those who they perceive to be demographically similar to themselves (Byrne, 1971) and those who they categorize as belonging to the same social category as themselves (Tajfel, 1981). Social categorization and stereotyping based on
  • 20. demographic charac- teristics are particularly prevalent when teams are first formed or in short- lived teams, since individuals tend to use these cognitive mechanisms to make sense of other team members until their stereotypes are invalidated through extended positive interactions (e.g., Allport, 1954). Thus, in the short term, demographic differences within teams have been found to result in greater conflict, communication difficulties, and other negative processes, as well as lower cohesion and social integration (e.g., Harrison, Price, & Bell, 1998; Pelled et al., 1999), and consequently, lower creative performance (Ancona & Caldwell, 1992). Martins and Shalley 541 Depending on what theoretical and process foundations are used, demo- graphic differences can both help and hurt performance (for reviews, see Milliken & Martins, 1996; van Knippenberg & Schippers, 2007; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998). Commenting on this conundrum, van Knippenberg and Schippers noted that these competing predictions and findings are primarily caused by focusing on main effects instead of potential moderators. Consis- tent with some prior work (e.g., Jackson & Joshi, 2004;
  • 21. Milliken & Martins, 1996; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998), they argue for models that are more com- plex and that consider moderator variables in explaining the effects of diver- sity. We examine the moderating effects of processes and input factors that are likely to determine the extent of cognitive elaboration and combination of various perspectives on a collaborative task, which are critical to translating the potential benefits of demographic differences into actual performance benefits (e.g., van Knippenberg et al., 2004). We argue that in order for the potential creativity benefits due to differing perspectives to be realized, it is important that interactions among demographically different individuals enable the surfacing, pooling together, and integration of their differing perspectives (Gilson & Shalley, 2004; Milliken et al., 2003; Shalley & Perry-Smith, 2008). Demographic Differences and Creativity: Moderation by Processes and Inputs The literature (e.g., Ocker, 2005) suggests that input and process factors that facilitate positive exchanges and the building of a positive relationship are important in determining the quality of interactions in virtual teams. This is particularly true for problem solving, which benefits from multiple perspec- tives but requires collaborators to work through their
  • 22. differences in attitudes and values to arrive at a consensus on solutions (Straus & McGrath, 1994). For instance, Taggar (2002) found that a team’s creativity- relevant process moderated the relationship between the average creativity of its members and the team’s creative output. Therefore, processes and inputs that facilitate information elaboration may be the key to reducing the negative effects and accentuating the benefits of demographic differences in virtual interactions (van Knippenberg, et al., 2004). For example, a team’s process skills have been found to be important to leveraging its members’ creative resources (e.g., Stroebe & Diehl, 1994). Also, Payne (1990) found that communication patterns in research teams had critical effects on their creative performance. The nature of the interaction process in virtual collaborations also may affect how members approach a task as well as affect their attention to the heuristic 542 Small Group Research 42(5) aspects of the task. The specific process factors we examined as moderators of the effects of demographic differences on creativity are the degree to which virtual collaborators spend time establishing rapport (i.e., bonding
  • 23. through informal conversation) before beginning work on the task, equality of participation in their task-related discussions, and degree of experienced process conflict. The input factor we examined as a moderator is the differ- ence in technical experience between collaborators, which prior research has found to be one of the most influential inputs affecting the quality of virtual interactions (Sarker & Sahay, 2002). Difference in technical experience refers to the difference between virtual collaborators in their experience with using the information and communication technologies required to collaborate virtually. Establishment of rapport. Whether individuals spend time establishing rapport with each other before working on their task can be important for the development of a good working relationship in virtual collaborations (Coutu, 1998; Saunders, 2000). For example, by spending a few minutes with intro- ductions and discussing how they should approach working on the task, virtual collaborators can establish a bond of trust that may make it easier for them to work together effectively (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999). Such an establishment of rapport may create a psychologically safe environment (Edmondson, 1999) in which demographically different virtual team members
  • 24. are comfortable raising and discussing their differing perspectives on a problem without feeling interpersonally threatened (Griffith & Neale, 2001). In keeping with this argument, prior research has found that virtual teams whose members spend time at the onset of their work getting to know each other experience greater trust among members down the road, which facilitates the overall effectiveness of their working together (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999; Suchan & Hayzak, 2001). This also should help them to overcome the interaction difficulties in working virtually. Therefore, we expect that demographically different virtual collaborators who establish rapport to a greater degree will be better able to surface, discuss, and integrate differing perspectives, which in turn enhances creativity. In contrast, when demographically different virtual collaborators do not establish rapport, they may find that their differences in perspective lead to difficulties in working together, which in turn diminishes creativity. Hypothesis 1: The relationship between demographic differences and creativity will be positive when there is greater establishment of rap- port in virtual collaborations and negative when there is less estab- lishment of rapport.
  • 25. Martins and Shalley 543 Participation equality. Participation equality reflects the extent to which each member of a dyad engaged in virtual collaboration participates equally in task interactions. In a diverse group, participation equality may enable “cognitive elaboration and information exchange within work groups, draw- ing out the different knowledge and skills represented” (Webber & Donahue, 2001, p. 158). Thus, more equal participation enables better surfacing and discussion of different ideas, resulting in greater creativity (Taggar, 2002). For example, Kruempel (2000) found that, in order for effective knowledge production to occur in a virtual team, the perspectives of all team members needed to be raised and debated. Also, Gilson and Shalley (2004) found that teams that valued participation by all members were more creative. Consideration of the variety of views and ideas represented by demo- graphically different collaborators should lead to an expanded source of knowledge to use in decision making. Also, the intellectual stimulation of considering others’ ideas should encourage exploratory thinking, which results in greater creativity. However, effective collaboration
  • 26. and participa- tion are necessary for virtual teams to successfully integrate various team members’ ideas and perspectives (Sarker, Lau, & Sahay, 2001). Thus, when demographically different virtual collaborators do achieve equality of par- ticipation they may be better able to leverage their diversity of perspectives to generate creative solutions. In contrast, when demographically different virtual collaborators have unequal inputs they will be less likely to be work- ing with a broad range of information and perspectives, which diminishes their collective creativity. Hypothesis 2: The relationship between demographic differences and creativity will be positive when there is greater participation equal- ity in virtual collaborations and negative when there is less partici- pation equality. Process conflict. Process conflict is defined as “controversies about aspects of how task accomplishment will proceed” (Jehn & Mannix, 2001, p. 239; italics in original). Greater process conflict increases uncertainty and reduces the ability of groups working on a task to pool together their ideas effectively to come up with collective solutions to problems (Jehn & Mannix, 2001). Thus, for demographically different virtual collaborators, a high
  • 27. level of process conflict between them may worsen the interaction difficulties caused by their demographic differences and virtual interaction, which leads to process losses (Montoya-Weiss, Massey, & Song, 2001). This argument is consistent with Griffith and Neale’s (2001) observation that “more virtual environments 544 Small Group Research 42(5) require more attention to procedural matters for success” (p. 401). The greater the process conflict, the greater the increase in process losses, which negatively affects the joint creativity of demographically different virtual collaborators. In contrast, based on the diversity literature (e.g., Pelled et al., 1999; Williams & O’Reilly, 1998), when virtual collaborators do not experience a great deal of process conflict they may be better able to reduce the interaction difficulties caused by their demographic differences and virtual interaction, and therefore, enhance effective discussion and integration of differing perspec tives to arrive at creative solutions. Consistent with this argument, creativity researchers have found that effective collaboration is a key determinant of creativity and innovation in teams (Pirola- Merlo & Mann,
  • 28. 2004). Thus, virtual collaborators who have effective processes for integrating their efforts for productive teamwork may be better able to overcome the low media richness of virtual work technologies and to integrate the differing perspectives on a task resulting from their demographic differences, in order to produce creative outcomes. Hypothesis 3: The relationship between demographic differences and creativity will be negative when there is greater process conflict in virtual collaborations and positive when there is less process conflict. Differences in technical experience. Individuals collaborating virtually may be expected to differ in their extent of experience in using the technologies needed to interact virtually. Prior research has found that teams whose members all have high levels of competence in using virtual work technologies perform better than those in which some members are more proficient in using the technologies than others (Kayworth & Leidner, 2000; Sarker & Sahay, 2002). Similarities in technical experience may thus create a positive context for demographically different collaborators to surface and discuss their differing perspectives. Differences in technical experience, on the other hand, may create communication and interaction problems, as
  • 29. individuals with stronger technical abilities may feel frustrated or limited in their virtual collaborations with others who are not as proficient in using virtual working technologies. These difficulties would exacerbate any interaction diffi- culties due to demographic differences and the low media richness of virtual communication technologies. Thus, differences in technical experience between virtual collaborators may create process barriers to effective virtual interaction, causing frustration and miscues that reduce creativity. Similar levels of technical experience, on the other hand, may provide a common platform that establishes the nature of the interaction between virtual collaborators Martins and Shalley 545 (i.e., both individuals will face the same difficulties if they both have low technical experience or may have the same level of proficiency if they both have high technical experience). Therefore, when there are wide differences between virtual collaborators in their technical expertise, they may have greater difficulty in operating effectively in a virtual context. In such a circumstance, it may be expected that they will have difficulties in establishing
  • 30. interactions that surface and utilize differing perspectives, which diminishes their creativity. Hypothesis 4: The relationship between demographic differences and creativity will be negative when there is greater difference in tech- nical experience between virtual collaborators and positive when there is less difference in technical experience. Method Sample, Task, and Procedures The sample consisted of 94 MBA students in an organizational behavior course at a medium-sized urban university in the United States. As part of their normal course curriculum, the class worked on a virtual work project. The students were asked to volunteer to participate in this research by filling out a survey; all did. The sample was demographically diverse: 33% were women, 45% international (representing 24 countries), and 36% nonwhite. The participants were in the age range of 23 to 42 years (M = 27.6 years, median = 27 years). All participants were proficient in spoken and written English; the average TOEFL score for international students was 637 (out of a possible 677). We used a complex heuristic task for which responses were
  • 31. open ended, did not have correct answers, and required participants to “seek consensus on a preferred alternative” (Straus & McGrath, 1994) that has been used in a num- ber of prior creativity studies (e.g., Shalley, 1991; Shalley & Perry-Smith, 2001; Zhou, 1998). Participants were asked to generate solutions to various human resource problems (e.g., employee theft, motivating the sales force) that typically arise within organizations and that managers need to be able to effectively solve. The participants were told that we were particularly inter- ested in creative solutions, so they should try to think of unique ways to solve the problems that also would work well in the company. Participants were randomly assigned a partner to collaborate with, which yielded 47 dyads engaged in virtual collaboration. Members of 59.57% of the 546 Small Group Research 42(5) dyads were of different races, 53.19% were of different sexes, and 63.83% were of different nationalities. The difference in age between virtual collabo- rators ranged from 0 to 16 years (M = 3.89 years). The participants were only allowed to communicate with their partner in an electronic chat room.
  • 32. Participants were given instructions on how to log on to their assigned chat room to connect with their partner to work on the task, were briefly introduced to their partner face to face (they could see each other, but could not speak), and were given 60 min to work on the collaborative task. Before working on the task, they were asked to complete a brief survey that collected informa- tion on demographics and extent of prior technical experience with computer- based interaction (e.g., chat rooms, bulletin boards, and e-mail). Measures Demographic differences. All participants were asked to indicate their race, sex, age, and nationality. As has been done in previous research on demo- graphic differences (Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989), we used dichotomous mea- sures for differences in race, sex, and nationality (with 0 indicating no difference, and 1 indicating a difference in the respective characteristic) and computed difference in age as the squared difference between the ages of the two collaborators. Creativity. According to Amabile (1996), a product is creative if observers independently agree that it is novel and appropriate. Two graduate research assistants independently rated the creativity of all solutions
  • 33. generated on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all creative to 7 = extremely creative). Interrater reli- ability was assessed using rWG. The mean rWG(j) for the creativity ratings was .96, which is well above the commonly used cutoff of .70. Thus, the overall creativity score for each pair of virtual collaborators was computed as the average of the two raters’ creativity ratings for the solutions generated by the collaborators. Since there may be an association between the number of solu- tions provided and the overall level of creativity, we tested the number of problems solved as a potential control variable. However, since it was not significant as a control, it was excluded from the analyses. Virtual interaction process factors. We retained a transcript of the interac- tions within each chat room as the virtual collaborators worked, so that we could code aspects of their interaction process. Two raters independently coded the following factors on a 7-point scale: establishment of rapport, equality of participation in problem solving, and extent of process conflict. The factors were defined for both raters, and their understanding of the defi- nitions was ascertained. Establishme nt of rapport was defined as spending
  • 34. Martins and Shalley 547 time at the start of the virtual collaboration discussing non-task- related issues that served to create a positive working relationship (e.g., spending time briefly getting to know each other, reassuring each other about the upcoming virtual collaboration). Equality of participation was coded as the extent to which each of the virtual collaborators had an equal amount of input and influence over the task. Process conflict was defined as the extent of disagreement between the virtual collaborators in how the task should be done (e.g., dis- agreement was high if one person wanted to read all the memos before com- mencing discussion and the other wanted to tackle them one by one). As was done with the creativity ratings, interrater agreement for the three factors was assessed using rWG. The mean rWG for the process ratings was .94 for estab- lishment of rapport, .91 for equality of participation, and .86 for extent of process conflict. Thus, for each of the three process factors, the ratings assigned by the two raters were averaged to obtain overall scores. Virtual interaction input factor. For technical experience, respondents were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale (1 = none to 5 = a great deal) their degree of experience with using e-mail, chat rooms, bulletin boards,
  • 35. and any other electronic collaboration technologies (e.g., document sharing). Responses were averaged across the four items (alpha = .71). For each set of collabora- tors, difference in technical experience was computed as the variance between the scores of its members. Results Correlations and descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. The hypothe- ses were tested using hierarchical regression analysis (significant findings are reported in Table 2). Due to sample size constraints, we ran separate regressions for differences in age and nationality and for differences in race and sex. Also, we entered each moderator variable separately. The limita- tions of the separate tests for the dimensions of difference and moderator variables are noted in the discussion section below. The variables were entered into the hierarchical regression equation in four steps. In the first set of analyses, difference in age and difference in nationality were entered in the first step (Table 2, Model 1a-d), the respective moderator variable was entered in the second step (Table 2, Models 2a-d), the interaction term for difference in age and the respective moderator was entered in the third step (Table 2, Models 3a-d), and the interaction term for difference in nationality and the
  • 36. respective moderator was entered in the fourth step (Table 2, Models 4a-d). The second set of analyses repeated the process, but with differences in race and sex as the demographic difference variables. A centering procedure was 548 T a b le 1 . D es cr ip ti ve s an d C o rr
  • 53. ty w as r at ed o n a 7- po in t sc al e. Martins and Shalley 549 Table 2. Results of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Model 1a 2a 3a 4a Step 1: Independent variables Difference in age -.11 -.07 .03 .03 Difference in nationality -.42*** -.41*** -.40*** -.40***
  • 54. Step 2: Moderator Establishment of rapport — .30** .39*** .39*** Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms Establishment of rapport × Difference in age — — .32** .31** Establishment of Rapport × Difference in Nationality — — — -.03 R2 .17 .26 .34 .34 Adj. R2 .13 .20 .28 .26 F 4.26** 4.79*** 5.28*** 4.13*** R2 change — .09** .09** .00 Model 1b 2b 3b 4b Step 1: Independent variables Difference in age -.11 -.07 -.02 -.01 Difference in nationality -.42*** -.36** -.34** -.35** Step 2: Moderator Participation equality — .27** .20 .17 Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms Participation Equality × Difference in Age — — .29** .32** Participation Equality × Difference in Nationality — — — .14
  • 55. R2 .17 .24 .31 .33 Adj. R2 .13 .18 .24 .24 F 4.26** 4.34*** 4.58*** 3.87*** R2 change — .07** .07** .02 Model 1c 2c 3c 4c Step 1: Independent variables Difference in age -.11 -.09 -.18 -.18 Difference in nationality -.42*** -.40*** -.34** -.35** Step 2: Moderator Process conflict — -.15 -.08 -.08 Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms Process Conflict × Difference in Age — — -.38*** -.38*** Process Conflict × Difference in Nationality — — -.01 R2 .17 .19 .32 .32 Adj. R2 .13 .13 .25 .23 F 4.26** 3.21** 4.74*** 3.70*** R2 change — .02 .13*** .00 (continued) 550 Small Group Research 42(5) followed in the computation of interaction terms (Aiken, West,
  • 56. & Reno, 1991). All significant moderated effects found were examined further, using recommended procedures (Aiken et al., 1991). Hypothesis 1 stated that there would be a positive relationship between demographic differences and creativity when there was high establishment of rapport and a negative relationship when there was low establishment of rap- port. This hypothesis was supported (p < .05) for differences in age (Table 2, Model 3a; F = 5.28, p < .01) but not for differences in nationality, race, or sex. Difference in age was positively related to creativity when there was more establishment of rapport but was negatively related to creativity when there was less rapport established. Hypothesis 2 stated that there would be a positive relationship between demographic differences and creativity when there was more equal participation by both virtual collaborators and a negative relationship when there was less equal participation. This hypothesis was supported (p < .05) for differences in age (Table 2, Model 3b; F = 4.58, p < .01) but not for differences in nation- ality, race, or sex. Difference in age was positively related to creativity when there was relatively equal participation in the virtual w ork interaction but was negatively related when there was less equal participation.
  • 57. Table 2. (continued) Model 1d 2d 3d 4d Step 1: Independent variables Difference in age -.11 -.12 -.11 -.13 Difference in nationality -.42*** -.49*** -.47*** -.55**** Step 2: Moderator Difference in technical experience — .25* .17 .22 Steps 3 & 4: Interaction terms Difference in Technical Experience × Difference in Age — — –.28** -.35*** Difference in Technical Experience × Difference in Nationality — — — -.33** R2 .17 .22 .29 .39 Adj. R2 .13 .17 .23 .32 F 4.26** 3.98** 4.26*** 5.14**** R2 change — .06* .07** .10** Note: N = 47. *p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < .01. ****p < .001.
  • 58. Martins and Shalley 551 Hypothesis 3 stated that there would be a negative relationship between demographic differences and creativity when there was high process conflict and a positive relationship when there was low process conflict. This hypothe- sis was supported (p < .01) for differences in age (Table 2, Model 3c; F = 4.74, p < .01), but not for differences in nationality, race, or sex. Difference in age was negatively related to creativity when there was high process conflict in the virtual interaction but was positively related to creativity when there was low process conflict. Hypothesis 4 stated that there would be a negative relationship between demographic differences and creativity when there was a larger difference in technical experience between virtual collaborators and a positive relationship when there was a smaller difference in technical experience. This hypothesis was supported (p < .05) for differences in age (Table 2, Model 3d; F = 4.26, p < .01), and to some extent for differences in nationality (Table 2, Model 4d; F = 5.14, p < .01), but not for differences in race or sex. Difference in age was negatively related to creativity when there was a large difference in technical
  • 59. experience between virtual collaborators but was slightly positively related when there was a smaller difference in technical experience. Difference in nationality was more negatively related to creativity when there was a greater difference in technical experience than when there was a smaller difference. Discussion The pattern of our findings is relatively consistent when looked at from the perspective of the various dimensions of difference we examined. We found that our hypotheses were consistently supported for differences in age. The effect of the difference in age between virtual collaborators on creativity was contingent on various aspects of their interaction and on their difference in technical experience. All the interaction process factors we examined moder- ated the effects of age difference on creativity. We found that a difference in age led to greater creativity when virtual collaborators had spent some time establishing rapport, when they had equal participation in the discussion, and when process conflict was low. These findings support the idea that when demographically different virtual collaborators are able to utilize effective processes, they are better able to deal with interaction difficulties that may arise from their differences and virtual interaction, and thus, benefit from the
  • 60. differences in perspectives associated with their age differences. In contrast, when such processes are not in place, the interaction difficulties caused by age differences and virtual interactions may lead to lower creative perfor- mance. We found that the difference in technical experience between virtual 552 Small Group Research 42(5) collaborators was an important moderator of the effects of difference in age on creativity, such that it exacerbated the negative effect of difference in age on creativity. Difference in technical experience likely cr eated communica- tion problems that further increased the difficulty of interaction beyond that already caused by a difference in age and the virtual interaction medium, thereby reducing creativity. Overall, our findings for age differences are consis- tent with the suggestion in prior research that contextual conditions may be important in determining the effects of age differences on outcomes (Ferris, Judge, Chachere, & Liden, 1991; Shore, Cleveland, & Goldberg, 2003). Given the strong effects for age differences in our model, we conducted post hoc analyses to examine alternative explanations for our findings. To
  • 61. determine whether the effects of differences in age were really due to differ- ences in experience between virtual collaborators, we reran the regression analyses using difference in work experience in place of difference in age. We found that whereas difference in work experience functioned similarly in the analysis to difference in age, the results were much stronger for differ- ence in age. This suggests that difference in work experience may explain the effects of difference in age to a large extent but that other factors associated with difference in age may also play a part in generating the effects obtained. Another argument that can be made to explain the effect of age difference is that the older of the virtual collaborators brought to the task greater life and work experience, thus increasing the creativity of the collaborative product. However, we found that creativity did not correlate significantly with the virtual collaborators’ average age (r = -.07, p = ns) or average amount of work experience (r = .02, p = ns). Taken together, our post hoc analyses sug- gest that the effects we found were more likely due to differences between the ages of the two collaborators (which partly reflects difference in work experience) rather than due to higher- or lower-average age of the two virtual collaborators.
  • 62. For differences in nationality, we found only one significant interaction effect, and that was not entirely in the predicted direction. Essentially, for vir- tual collaborators from different nationalities, even relatively equal technical experience did not produce a positive effect of difference in nationality on creativity, though it did reduce the strength of the negative effect experienced by the virtual collaborators with unequal technical experience. In addition, though we did not predict a direct effect of a difference in nationality on creativity, we did find a relatively strong direct effect. The finding is consis- tent with prior research that has found that national differences create com- munication problems in cross-national teams (e.g., Kayworth & Leidner, 2000) and points out that these problems may be amplified by differences in Martins and Shalley 553 technical experience between employees working virtually. Furthermore, the negative effect for difference in nationality on creativity was not moderated by the interaction processes we examined. This lack of moderation suggests that the interaction difficulties encountered in cross-national virtual teams may not be easily overcome in short-term virtual interactions,
  • 63. such as those examined in this study. Given that short-term problem-solving interactions among globally distributed employees are becoming increasingly common in organizations, our finding suggests that much more research needs to be con- ducted in order to understand and manage such interactions. For differences in race and sex we did not find any significant effects. In general, the effects of demographic differences hinge on the cognitive avail- ability of demographic characteristics that feed into social categorization processes (e.g., van Knippenberg et al., 2004). Since virtual communication technologies are low in the richness of information carried, this reduces the cognitive availability of demographic differences in virtual interactions (e.g., Nowak, 2003; Sproull & Kiesler, 1986). Thus, rather than react to surface- level characteristics using social category stereotypes, demographically dif- ferent collaborators may instead focus on differences in virtual interaction patterns that may accompany their demographic differences. Furthermore, since researchers have found that there are no readily detectable differences in the behavior of men and women in a virtual context, such as in the extent of interaction (Gefen & Straub, 1997), it is understandable that there were no significant effects for difference in sex.
  • 64. In contrast, differences in age and nationality have been found to affect interaction patterns in virtual teams. Individuals from different nationalities may encounter difficulties in interacting virtually due to cross - national dif- ferences in communication styles and differences in word connotations even when communicating in the same language (e.g., Maznevski & Chudoba, 2000; Zakaria et al., 2004). Furthermore, difficulties may arise in virtual interactions between individuals from different nationalities due to differ- ences in reliance on body language, facial expressions, gestures, and physical distance in communication in their respective countries (Farmer & Hyatt, 1994). Also, national differences in usage of the English language may cause difficulties in communication in virtual teams (e.g., Zakaria et al., 2004). In a similar vein, prior research has found that an individual’s age affects his or her attitude toward, and comfort with the use of, information technologies such as e-mail (e.g., Agarwal & Prasad, 1999; Burton-Jones & Hubona, 2005). Also, individuals of different ages may communicate differently, in terms of the formalness of their communication style, their use of slang or
  • 65. 554 Small Group Research 42(5) certain terms, and norms regarding communication in general (e.g., Lancaster & Stillman, 2002). Limitations, Directions for Future Research, and Contributions An obvious limitation of this study is the sample size, which was largely a consequence of the logistical and financial difficulty in setting up a large number of virtual collaborations with the type of students we used in the study—graduate business school students—and coding hour- long chat logs. As a consequence, we were unable to examine all demographic differences and moderator variables together in the same regression equation. Although the small sample may reduce confidence in the nonsignificant findings, it does make us more confident about the significant effects we found. In addi- tion, it is possible that the context in which our study was conducted (an MBA program) may have made differences in age salient. However, given that the participants on average had more than 5 years of work experience, this con- cern is mitigated to some extent. But the effects we found should be tested in other samples, in particular in field settings, to establish broader generaliz- ability of the findings.
  • 66. Since the effects of demographic diversity can be different depending on time and the type of task, future research should examine these effects using different types of tasks that vary in complexity and need for consensus, as well as examine these effects over the entire lifecycle of a team. Also, we focused on short-lived virtual interactions, which can contribute to our under- standing of short-term virtual teams as well as early interactions in longer- term virtual teams. On average, virtual teams tend to have a shorter lifecycle than face-to-face teams (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999); therefore, our findings should apply well to the average virtual team. However, future research should examine longer lifecycle virtual teams that will enable an examina- tion of different stages in a team’s lifecycle; such an examination is impor- tant because how demographic differences affect outcomes may vary by the amount of time that a team has spent working together (Harrison et al., 1998). In addition, we looked at only two-party virtual collaborations. If more peo- ple were involved, the need for effective processes would be expected to be even stronger, but this remains an empirical question. Finally, it should be pointed out that we examined the effect of demographic differences on cre- ativity for CMC exclusively. Although this medium represents a large part of
  • 67. virtual work, other ways of working virtually also need to be examined. Martins and Shalley 555 This study contributes to research and practice in several ways. It is one of the first studies to examine how process and input factors influence the relationship between demographic differences and creativity in a virtual work context. As such, it contributes to research on creativity, virtual work, and diversity. Shalley, Zhou, and Oldham (2004), in their integration of the creativity literature, called for more research on team creativity since prior research on creativity had tended to focus on individual creativity, with only a few studies having examined team creativity (Gilson & Shalley, 2004; Taggar, 2002). Furthermore, since most employees are now working virtually, at least to some extent in their collaboration with coworkers on projects (Griffith & Neale, 2001), research is needed that explores what aspects are most important for creativity in virtual teams (Martins et al., 2004). In this study we start to address this topic by providing insights into how demographic differences may affect the performance of team members working together virtually on a problem-solving task. In
  • 68. addition, our study contributes to developing an understanding of the circumstances (i.e., mod- erators) that enable demographically different coworkers to overcome interaction difficulties resulting from their differences and from the limita- tions of virtual collaboration, and consequently, to improve the quality of their performance. Our findings indicate that demographic differences can be effectively used to tease out creative contributions as long as organizations focus on important team input and process factors. Given that working virtually requires a certain level of technical expertise, attention should be paid on the front end to making sure that employees are comfortable with the technology and can easily use it to interact with others in their team. This is particularly important, as our findings indicate that for virtual teams that are working across national boundaries differences in technical abilities may cause inter- action difficulties that worsen the interaction difficulties teams face while working across nationalities. Also, developing routines that encourage the formation of rapport early on in virtual interactions may benefit performance when there are large age differences between individuals working together virtually. This could be done by encouraging employees to
  • 69. initially make time in a virtual interaction to chat and get to know each other. Finally, knowledge of our results can lead to the design of process interventions to improve creativity. For example, when individuals work virtually, managers should pay attention to facilitating the process by providing training up front in communication and process management skills. Overall, our findings also suggest that developing more elaborate research models is necessary in order to better understand the dynamics and outcomes of virtual teams. 556 Small Group Research 42(5) Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article. Financial Disclosure/Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article. References Agarwal, R., & Prasad, J. (1999). Are individual differences germane to the acceptance
  • 70. of new information technologies? Decision Sciences, 30, 361- 391. doi:10.1111/ j.1540-5915.1999.tb01614.x Aiken, L., West, S., & Reno, R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE. Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 10, pp. 123-167). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview. Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Demography and design: Predictors of new product team performance. Organization Science, 3, 321- 341. doi:10.1287/ orsc.3.3.321 Burton-Jones, A., & Hubona, G. (2005). Individual differences and usage behavior: Revisiting a technology acceptance model assumption. ACM SIGMIS Database, 36(2), 58-77. doi:10.1145/1066149.1066155 Byrne, D. E. (1971). The attraction paradigm. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Coutu, D. L. (1998). Trust in virtual teams. Harvard Business Review, 76, 20-21.
  • 71. Dew, R., & Hearn, G. (2009). A new model of the learning process for innovation teams: Networked nominal pairs. International Journal of Innovation Manage- ment, 13, 521-535. doi:10.1142/S136391960900239X Edmondson, A. (1999). Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 350-383. doi:10.2307/2666999 Farmer, S. M., & Hyatt, C. W. (1994). Effects of task language demands and task complexity on computer-mediated work groups. Small Group Research, 25, 331-366. doi:10.1177/1046496494253001 Ferris, G. R., Judge, T. A., Chachere, J. G., & Liden, R. C. (1991). The age context of performance-evaluation decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 6, 616-622. Gefen, D., & Straub, D. W. (1997). Gender differences in the perception and use of e-mail: An extension to the technology acceptance model. MIS Quarterly, 21, 389-400. doi:10.2307/249720 Martins and Shalley 557 Giambatista, R. C., & Bhappu, A. D. (2010). Diversity’s harvest: Interactions of diver- sity sources and communication technology on creative group
  • 72. performance. Orga- nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111, 116- 126. doi:10.1016/ j.obhdp.2009.11.003 Gilson, L. L., & Shalley, C. E. (2004). A little creativity goes a long way: An exami- nation of teams’ engagement in creative processes. Journal of Management, 30, 453-470. doi:10.1016/j.jm.2003.07.001 Griffith, T. L., & Neale, M. A. (2001). Information processing in traditional, hybrid, and virtual teams: From nascent knowledge to transactive memory. Research in Organizational Behavior, 23, 379-421. Griffith, T. L., Sawyer, J. E., & Neale, M. A. (2003). Virtualness and knowledge in teams: Managing the love triangle of organizations, individuals, and information technology. MIS Quarterly, 27, 265-287. doi:10.2307/30036531 Hansen, M. T., Nohria, N., & Tierney, T. (1999). What’s your strategy for managing knowledge? Harvard Business Review, 77(2), 106-116. Hargadon, A. B., & Bechky, B. A. (2006). When collections of creatives become creative collectives: A field study of problem solving at work. Organization Science, 17, 484-500. doi:10.1287/orsc.1060.0200 Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., & Bell, M. P. (1998). Beyond relational demography: Time and the effects of surface- and deep-level diversity on
  • 73. work group cohesion. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 96-107. Hoffman, L. R., & Maier, N. R. F. (1961). Quality and acceptance of problem solu- tions by members of homogeneous and heterogeneous groups. Journal of Abnor- mal and Social Psychology, 62, 401-407. doi:10.1037/h0044025 Jackson, S. E., & Joshi, A. (2004). Diversity in social context: A multi-attribute, mul- tilevel analysis of team diversity and sales performance. Journal of Organiza- tional Behavior, 25, 675-702. doi:10.1002/job.265 Jarvenpaa, S. L., & Leidner, D. E. (1999). Communication and trust in global virtual teams. Organization Science, 10, 791-815. doi:10.1287/orsc.10.6.791 Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of conflict: A longitudi- nal study of intragroup conflict and group performance. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 238-251. doi:10.2307/3069453 Kayworth, T., & Leidner, D. (2000). The global virtual manager: A prescription for success. European Management Journal, 18, 183-194. doi:10.1016/S0263- 2373(99)00090-0 Kruempel, K. (2000). Making the right (interactive) moves for knowledge-producing tasks in computer-mediated groups. IEEE Transactions on Professional Commu-
  • 74. nication, 43, 185-195. doi:10.1109/47.843645 Lancaster, L. C., & Stillman, D. (2002). When generations collide. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 558 Small Group Research 42(5) Magadley, W., & Birdi, K. (2009). Innovation labs: An examination into the use of physical spaces to enhance organizati onal creativity. Creativity and Innovation Management, 18, 315-325. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 8691.2009.00540.x Majchrzak, A., Malhotra, A., Stamps, J., & Lipnack, J. (2004). Can absence make a team grow stronger? Harvard Business Review, 82(5), 131-137. Martins, L. L., Gilson, L. L., & Maynard, M. T. (2004). Virtual teams: What do we know and where do we go from here? Journal of Management, 30, 805-835. doi:10.1016/j.jm.2004.05.002 Maznevski, M. L., & Chudoba, K. M. (2000). Bridging space over time: Global virtual team dynamics and effectiveness. Organization Science, 11, 473-492. doi:10.1287/orsc.11.5.473.15200 McDonough, E. F., Kahn, K. B., & Barczak, G. (2001). An investigation of the use of global, virtual, and colocated new product development teams.
  • 75. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 18, 110-120. doi:10.1111/1540- 5885.1820110 McLeod, P. L., & Lobel, S. A. (1992). The effects of ethnic diversity on idea generation in small groups. Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings, pp. 227-231. Milliken, F. J., Bartel, C. A., & Kurtzberg, T. (2003). Diversity and creativity in work groups: A dynamic perspective on the affective and cognitive processes that link diversity and performance. In P. Paulus & B. Nijstad (Eds.), Group creativity (pp. 32-62). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Milliken, F. J., & Martins, L. L. (1996). Searching for common threads: Understand- ing the multiple effects of diversity in organizational groups. Academy of Manage- ment Review, 21, 402-433. doi:10.2307/258667 Montoya-Weiss, M. M., Massey, A. P., & Song, M. (2001). Getting it together: Tem- poral coordination and conflict management in global virtual teams. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 1251-1262. doi:10.2307/3069399 Nemeth, C. J. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence. Psychological Review, 93(1), 23-32. doi:10.1037/0033- 295x.93.1.23 Nemiro, J. E. (2002). The creative process in virtual teams. Creativity Research Journal,
  • 76. 14(1), 69-83. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1401_6 Nowak, K. L. (2003). Sex categorization in computer mediated communication (CMC): Exploring the utopian promise. Media Psychology, 5, 83-103. doi:10.1207/ S1532785XMEP0501_4 Ocker, R. J. (2005). Influences on creativity in asynchronous virtual teams: A qualita- tive analysis of experimental teams. IEEE Transactions on Professional Commu- nication, 48(1), 22-39. doi:10.1109/TPC.2004.843294 Paulus, P. (2000). Groups, teams, and creativity: The creative potential of idea- generating groups. Applied Psychology, 49, 237-262. doi:10.1111/1464-0597.00013 Payne, R. (1990). The effectiveness of research teams: A review. In M. West & J. Farr (Eds.), Innovation and creativity at work: Psychological and organiza- tional strategies (pp. 101-122). New York, NY: John Wiley. Martins and Shalley 559 Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Exploring the black box: An analysis of work group diversity, conflict, and performance. Administrative Sci- ence Quarterly, 44, 1-28. doi:10.2307/2667029 Pirola-Merlo, A., & Mann, L. (2004). The relationship between
  • 77. individual creativity and team creativity: Aggregating across people and time. Journal of Organiza- tional Behavior, 25, 235-257. doi:10.1002/job.240 Sarker, S., Lau, F., & Sahay, S. (2001). Using an adapted grounded theory approach for inductive theory building about virtual team development. Database for Advances in Information Systems, 32(1), 38-56. doi:10.1145/506740.506745 Sarker, S., & Sahay, S. (2002). Information systems development by US-Norwegian virtual teams: Implications of time and space. Proceedings of the thirty-fifth annual Hawaii international conference on system sciences (January), pp. 1-10. Saunders, C. S. (2000). Virtual teams: Piecing together the puzzle. In R. W. Zmud (Ed.) Framing the domain of IT management: Projecting the future through the past, pp.29-50. Cincinnati, OH: Pinnaflex. Shalley, C. E. (1991). Effects of productivity goals, creativity goals, and personal discretion on individual creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 179-185. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.76.2.179 Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on creativity and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 483-503. doi:10.2307/256689
  • 78. Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social- psychological factors on creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected evalu- ation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84, 1-22. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2918 Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2008). The emergence of team creative cogni- tion: The role of diverse outside ties, socio-cognitive network centrality, and team evolution. Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal, 2, 23-41. doi: 10.1002/sej Shalley, C. E., Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2004). The effects of personal and con- textual characteristics on creativity: Where should we go from here? Journal of Management, 30, 933-958. doi:10.1016/j.jm.2004.06.007 Shore, L. M., Cleveland, J. N., & Goldberg, C. B. (2003). Work attitudes and deci- sions as a function of manager age and employee age. Journal of Applied Psychol- ogy, 88, 529-537. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.529 Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science, 32, 1492- 1512. doi:10.1287/ mnsc.32.11.1492 Straus, S. G., & McGrath, J. E. (1994). Does the medium matter? The interaction of
  • 79. task type and technology on group performance and member reactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 87-97. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.79.1.87 560 Small Group Research 42(5) Stroebe, W., & Diehl, M. (1994). Why groups are less effective than their members: On productivity losses in idea-generating groups. European Review of Social Psy- chology, 5, 271-303. doi:10.1080/14792779543000084 Suchan, J., & Hayzak, G. (2001). The communication characteristics of virtual teams: A case study. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication, 44, 174-186. doi:10.1109/47.946463 Taggar, S. (2002). Individual creativity and group ability to utilize individual creative resources: A multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 315-330. doi:10.2307/3069349 Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Townsend, A. M., DeMarie, S. M., & Hendrickson, A. R. (1998). Virtual teams: Technology and the workplace of the future. Academy of Management Executive, 12(3), 17-29.
  • 80. Tsui, A. S., & O’Reilly, C. A., III. (1989). Beyond simple demographic effects: The importance of relational demography in superior-subordinate dyads. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 402-423. doi:10.2307/256368 Tsui, A. S., Xin, K. R., & Egan, T. D. (1995). Relational demography: The missing link in vertical dyad linkage. In S. E. Jackson & M. N. Ruderman (Eds.), Diver- sity in work teams: Research paradigms for a changing workplace (pp. 97-129). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. van Knippenberg, D., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Homan, A. C. (2004). Work group diver- sity and group performance: An integrative model and research agenda. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1008-1022. doi:10.1037/0021- 9010.89.6.1008 van Knippenberg, D., & Schippers, M. C. (2007). Work group diversity. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 515-541. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085546 Watson, W. E., Kumar, K., & Michaelsen, L. K. (1993). Cultural diversity’s impact on interaction process and performance: Comparing homogeneous and diverse task groups. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 590-602. Webber, S. S., & Donahue, L. M. (2001). Impact of highly and less job-related diversity on work group cohesion and performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Manage-
  • 81. ment, 27, 141-162. doi:10.1177/014920630102700202 Williams K. Y., & O’Reilly, C. A. (1998). Demography and diversity in organiza- tions: A review of 40 years of research. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 77-140). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993). Toward a theory of organiza- tional creativity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 293- 321. doi:10.2307/258761 Zakaria, N., Amelinckx, A., & Wilemon, D. (2004). Working together apart? Build- ing a knowledge-sharing culture for global virtual teams. Creativity & Innovation Management, 13, 15-29. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8691.2004.00290.x Martins and Shalley 561 Zhou, J. (1998). Feedback valence, feedback style, task autonomy, and achievement orientation: Interactive effects on creative performance. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 83, 261-276. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.261 Bios Luis L. Martins is an associate professor at the McCombs School of Business, Uni- versity of Texas at Austin, USA. He received his PhD from the
  • 82. Stern School of Business, New York University. His research examines the dynamics of diversity, particularly in the context of virtual teams and global virtual work. Christina E. Shalley is the Thomas R. Williams-Wachovia and ADVANCE profes- sor at the College of Management at the Georgia Institute of Technology, USA. She received her PhD in business administration from the University of Illinois, Urbana– Champaign. Her research examines the effects of social and contextual factors on creativity and innovation. Research Alternative Assignment: This option involves writing a review and critique of an academic research article. A review should include: · A statement in your own words of what you thought the author hoped to discover. · A brief description of the research approach that was used including the nature of the sample, the way key variables were measured, and the type of analysis used. · A summary of the key results of the study. · A description of what you think the results of the article suggest for how organizations and/or their managers might change the way they operate in the future. · Your overall evaluation of the article. What did you like about the article? Why? What did you not like? Why?