SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 5
Download to read offline
Published on Psychology Today (http://www.psychologytoday.com)



Why Donʼt We Help? Less Is More, at
Least When It Comes to Bystanders
By Melissa Burkley, Ph.D.
Created Nov 4 2009 - 11:27am




                                      On October 24th, 2009, as many as 20 witnesses
watched as a 15 year old girl was brutally assaulted and raped outside a homecoming
dance in Richmond, CA. The viciousness of the attack was shocking, but what was even
more shocking was the fact that so many people witnessed the attack and yet failed to
intervene or call police. As one of the police officers involved in the case states, "what
makes it even more disturbing is the presence of others. People came by, saw what was
happening and failed to report it." Some of the bystanders reportedly even laughed and
took photos of the assault with their cell phones.

How could people just stand by and watch something this horrible happen to a young,
innocent girl? Some have suggested that the eyewitnesses' failure to report the incident
likely resulted from a concern over being labeled as a snitch. Although this is possible,
social psychological research on the bystander effect suggests a different cause - there
were too many eyewitnesses present. The bystander effect refers to the fact that people
are less likely to offer help when they are in a group than when they are alone. Research
on this effect was inspired by a real-world account that seems hauntingly similar to the
recent event in Richmond.

In 1964, 28 year old Kitty Genovese was raped and stabbed to death in front of her
apartment complex.                            The attack lasted over 30 minutes and was
witnessed by several dozen people who failed to report the incident. Some failed to realize
that an actual crime was going on, claiming they thought it was a "lover's quarrel",
whereas others realized they were witnessing a crime, but failed to report it because they
assumed that someone else had already called the police. A similar incident took place in
New Bedford, MA, in 1983 when several men raped a woman on a pool table in front of
several witnesses in a bar. The 1988 film "The Accused" depicted this incident and Jodie
Foster went on to earn an Academy Award for her performance as the rape victim. More
recently, a 22 year old college student died from water intoxication in 2005 when four of
his fellow fraternity brothers failed to intervene during a deadly hazing incident.

To determine the underlying reasons why these witnesses failed to help, John Darley and
Bibb Latane conducted a series of lab experiments to examine how the presence of others
influences people's helping behavior in an emergency situation. The results of these
studies suggest there are two clear reasons why the eyewitnesses in the Richmond case
may have failed to help.

1. Pluralistic Ignorance
One of the first steps in anyone's decision to help another is the recognition that someone
is actually in need of help. To do this, the bystander must realize that they are witnessing
an emergency situation and that a victim is in need of assistance. Consequently, a major
reason why eyewitnesses fail to intervene is that they do not even realize they are
witnessing a crime. When we are in an ambiguous situation and we are not sure whether
there is an emergency or not, we often look to others to see how they are reacting. We
assume that others may know something that we don't, so we gauge their reactions before
we decide how we will respond. If those around us are acting as if it is an emergency, then
we will treat it like an emergency and act accordingly. But if those around us are acting
calm, then we may fail to recognize the immediacy of the situation and therefore fail to
intervene.

For example, imagine you are at the community pool and you see a child splashing wildly
in the water. Your first instinct would probably be to look around you and see how others
are responding. If others appear shocked and are yelling for help, you may conclude that
the child is drowning and dive in to help. But, if those around you are ignoring the child or
laughing, you may conclude that they child is just playing around. To avoid looking foolish,
you would probably just continue watching and would fail to dive in and help. This seems
like a reasonable approach and for the most part, it prevents us from making a fool out of
ourselves. But the problem is that this tendency to look to others in order to determine how
to respond can be biased by a phenomenon known as pluralistic ignorance. Pluralistic
ignorance describes a situation where a majority of group members privately believe one
thing, but assume (incorrectly) that most others believe the opposite.

For example, pluralistic ignorance explains why my undergraduate students often fail to
ask questions in class. Let's say that one of my students is confused about the class
material I just covered and wants to ask me to clarify. Before raising her hand, she will
likely look around to room to see if any of her fellow students seem confused or have their
hand up as well. If no one else looks puzzled, she will conclude that she is the only one in
the room that didn't get the material. To avoid looking stupid, she may choose to keep her
hand down and not ask me her question. But as a teacher, I have discovered that if one
student is unsure about the material, odds are most of the students are. So in this
situation, my class is suffering from pluralistic ignorance because each one assumes they
are the only one confused, when in fact all the students are confused and all of them are
incorrectly concluding that they are the only one. The same process can occur when we
witness an ambiguous emergency situation. All the bystanders may look to each other to
determine if they are witnessing a crime, and if no one reacts, then everyone will wrongly
conclude that this is not an emergency and no one will step up and help. The fact that
several of the eyewitnesses in the Richmond case were laughing and taking photos with
their cell phones suggests that they simply failed to realize they were witnessing a brutal
rape and instead may have thought it was a prank.

In one of Darley and Latane's classic studies, they tried to recreate this phenomenon in
the lab. For their study, they had participants complete a questionnaire and after a few
minutes, smoke started to pour into the room underneath a door in the back. Some
participants were the only one in the room when this happened, but for others, there were
two other students completing questionnaires in the room as well. In actuality, these two
"students" were working for the researchers and were instructed to keep calm not matter
what happens. The key question in this study was would the participant notice the smoke
and go get help or would they simply write it off as nothing concerning and continue
working on their questionnaire. The result showed that when the participant was alone,
75% of them left to report the smoke. But when there were two other people in the room
who remained calm, only 10% left to get help. In some cases, the smoke got so thick the
participant could barely read the questionnaire in front of them! Yet, as long as their fellow
bystanders remained in calm, they did as well. Thus, when we are alone, we are more
likely to assume an ambiguous situation represents and emergency and act accordingly.
When we are in the presence of other bystanders, we are likely to look to those others for
guidance and if they are not responding or are laughing or are taking photos of the event,
we will mistakenly conclude it is not an emergency and will fail to help.

So we now that one reason why people fail to help is because they don't realize they are
witnessing a crime, but how can we use this information to our advantage? First, if you
find yourself in an ambiguous situation, resist the urge to look to others and go with your
gut instinct. If you think there is even a possibility that someone is in need, act on it. At
worst, you will embarrass yourself for a few minutes, but at best, you will save a life.
Second, if you are unfortunate enough to find yourself the victim and are in need of help,
make sure you make it clear to those around you that this is an emergency situation. For
instance, self-defense instructors will often tell women that if they are being attacked by a
man, they should yell out "fire" instead of "help". This is because the word "help" is used in
many situations that are non-life-threatening so when we hear it, it does not automatically
indicate that there is an emergency. On the other hand, we only scream the word "fire"
when there is an actual fire, and in some cases it is illegal to yell this word when there is
not an actual fire (e.g., in a crowded theater). So by yelling "fire," you immediately make
everyone around you aware that they are in an emergency situation.

2. Diffusion of Responsibility
Even if people recognize that they are witnessing a crime, they may still fail to intervene if
they do not take personal responsibility for helping the victim. The problem is that the
more bystanders there are, the less responsible each individual feels. When you are the
only eyewitness present, 100% of the responsibility for providing help rests on your
shoulders. But if there are five eyewitnesses, only 20% of the responsibility is yours. The
responsibility becomes defused or dispersed among the group members. In these
situations, people may assume that someone else will help or that someone else is better
qualified to provide assistance. But if everyone assumes this, then no one will intervene.
Darley and Latane also investigate this phenomenon in a lab study.

Specifically, they had participants take part in a group discussion over an intercom system.
Some participants talked one-on-one over the intercom with another person and some
talked over the intercom with a group of 5 other people. During the discussion, one of the
voices on the intercom stated they were having a seizure and called out for help. In
actuality, this was a prerecorded voice. For those who were led to believe they were the
only person who overheard the seizure, 85% sought help. But for those who thought they
were one of six people who overheard the seizure, only 31% sought help. So even when
we are aware that an emergency is occurring, we are still less likely to help if other
bystanders are present. So what about these people who overheard the seizure and didn't
help? Were they just indifferent? Follow up interviews at the end of the study suggested
that they were in fact concerned. Most mentioned overhearing the seizure, many had
trembling hands and were clearly shaken from the experience and several inquired as to
whether the victim finally received help. This tells us that they were not indifferent or
heartless; they were concerned but simply didn't feel responsible enough to do anything
about it. Interestingly, the researchers also asked if the participants thought that the
presence of other bystanders affected their decision to get help or not and the most said it
did not. So even though the presence of others clearly affects our helping behavior, we are
unaware of this influence.

So once again, how can we use the knowledge garnered from this study to our
advantage? First, if you find yourself in an emergency situation with several fellow
bystanders, realize that your first instinct (and the first instinct of those around you) will be
to deny responsibility for helping the victim. By simply being aware of the diffusion of
responsibility process, it may snap you out of the biased way of thinking and cause you to
realize that you and everyone present is each 100% responsible for helping the victim.
Second, if you find yourself in need of help, it is up to you to actively make one of your
eyewitnesses feel personally responsible for your well-being. When we are in need of help
and there is a crowd watching, we often plead for help to anyone that is listening, thinking
that at least one person will step up to intervene. But self-defense instructors advise that
you instead pick one person out of the crowd, look them dead in the eye, and tell that one
person you need help. By pleading to a specific individual, you suddenly make that person
feel completely responsible for your safety and this increases the odds that they will help.
The same technique can be used if you are trying to get several others to help you assist
a victim. Point to one person and tell them to go get help; point to another and tell them to
call 911. Giving specific instructions to specific people counteracts the diffusion of
responsibility process.
Conclusion
We typically think that the more people who witness a crime, the more people there will be
to help the victim, but these classic social psychology experiments call this assumption
into question. By making yourself and others aware of the factors that lead to such
bystander apathy, we can hopefully make events like those that occurred in Richmond, CA
and Bedford, MA a thing of the past.

Source URL: http://www.psychologytoday.com/node/34502

Links:
[1] http://www.psychologytoday.com/files/teaser/2009/11/helpwantedsign.jpg
[2] http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-social-thinker

More Related Content

What's hot

Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_d
Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_dAu psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_d
Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_dAuntDix3
 
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...lygus.lt Butkeviciute
 
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!frodo2222
 
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...dallasfederallawfirm
 
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular Girl
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular GirlOk! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular Girl
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular GirlConnected: Media Psychology
 
Confirmation bias
Confirmation biasConfirmation bias
Confirmation biasDarrenTofu
 
Domestic Violence Outline revised
Domestic Violence Outline revisedDomestic Violence Outline revised
Domestic Violence Outline revisedWanda Posey
 
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)mabrandt
 
The Nature of Suicide Bereavement
The Nature of Suicide BereavementThe Nature of Suicide Bereavement
The Nature of Suicide BereavementFranklin Cook
 
Be somebody to somebody
Be somebody to somebodyBe somebody to somebody
Be somebody to somebodySusan Hoekstra
 
Self destructive behaviors and survivors of suicide
Self destructive behaviors and survivors  of suicideSelf destructive behaviors and survivors  of suicide
Self destructive behaviors and survivors of suicidesbuffo
 
Truth, service, love, and decision making
Truth, service, love, and decision makingTruth, service, love, and decision making
Truth, service, love, and decision makingJohnel Esponilla
 
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"Franklin Cook
 
When is a fish story not a fish story
When is a fish story not a fish storyWhen is a fish story not a fish story
When is a fish story not a fish storyBLeo0001
 
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1Wim Demeere
 

What's hot (19)

Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_d
Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_dAu psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_d
Au psy492 m7_a3_e_portf_carrington_d
 
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...
John o'reily (ireland) @profiling victims of human trafficking stop traffick!...
 
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!
Victim Puking on a Nice Guy or Gal!
 
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...
Legal Options for Charges of Aggravated Sex Assault By Dallas Sexual Assault ...
 
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular Girl
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular GirlOk! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular Girl
Ok! Magazine Mar 18 2013_Picking On the Popular Girl
 
Confirmation bias
Confirmation biasConfirmation bias
Confirmation bias
 
Domestic Violence Outline revised
Domestic Violence Outline revisedDomestic Violence Outline revised
Domestic Violence Outline revised
 
Dv,sa,dfsa
Dv,sa,dfsaDv,sa,dfsa
Dv,sa,dfsa
 
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)
Donald harvey serial killer project psych (Zachery Reedy)
 
The Nature of Suicide Bereavement
The Nature of Suicide BereavementThe Nature of Suicide Bereavement
The Nature of Suicide Bereavement
 
Be somebody to somebody
Be somebody to somebodyBe somebody to somebody
Be somebody to somebody
 
Self destructive behaviors and survivors of suicide
Self destructive behaviors and survivors  of suicideSelf destructive behaviors and survivors  of suicide
Self destructive behaviors and survivors of suicide
 
Social Conditioning Part 1
Social Conditioning Part 1Social Conditioning Part 1
Social Conditioning Part 1
 
Truth, service, love, and decision making
Truth, service, love, and decision makingTruth, service, love, and decision making
Truth, service, love, and decision making
 
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"
Enigmatic Nature of Suicide May Answer the Question "Why?"
 
When is a fish story not a fish story
When is a fish story not a fish storyWhen is a fish story not a fish story
When is a fish story not a fish story
 
Your Dark Side and Being Calm
Your Dark Side and Being CalmYour Dark Side and Being Calm
Your Dark Side and Being Calm
 
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1
Wim Demeere's Patreon newsletter: Vol 2, Issue 1
 
Friday Plenary. What is the restorative space that a survivor volunteer and c...
Friday Plenary. What is the restorative space that a survivor volunteer and c...Friday Plenary. What is the restorative space that a survivor volunteer and c...
Friday Plenary. What is the restorative space that a survivor volunteer and c...
 

Viewers also liked

The Bystander Effect
The Bystander EffectThe Bystander Effect
The Bystander EffectDylan Thomas
 
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_finalAshok Sharma
 
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015Brian Lowe
 
Bystander intervention fhsu times talk
Bystander intervention fhsu times talkBystander intervention fhsu times talk
Bystander intervention fhsu times talkAndy Tincknell
 
The Unknown
The UnknownThe Unknown
The Unknownfubu233
 
Sexual Violence & Bystander Intervention
Sexual Violence & Bystander InterventionSexual Violence & Bystander Intervention
Sexual Violence & Bystander InterventionKpereira38
 
Radiation induced bystander effect
Radiation induced bystander effectRadiation induced bystander effect
Radiation induced bystander effectmasoudnajafi67
 
Bystander effect
Bystander effectBystander effect
Bystander effectEvanJFranz
 
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace Bullying
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace BullyingFive Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace Bullying
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace BullyingAnderson-davis, Inc.
 
Prosocial behavior 2011 class
Prosocial behavior 2011 classProsocial behavior 2011 class
Prosocial behavior 2011 classSpencer Gross
 
Bystander Awareness
Bystander AwarenessBystander Awareness
Bystander Awarenessmikewilhelm
 
Bystander effect
Bystander effectBystander effect
Bystander effectAlice Fang
 
Bullying And Harassment
Bullying And HarassmentBullying And Harassment
Bullying And HarassmentJessica Booth
 
Human Trafficking PowerPoint Presentation
Human Trafficking PowerPoint PresentationHuman Trafficking PowerPoint Presentation
Human Trafficking PowerPoint Presentationlbrowning9
 

Viewers also liked (19)

The Bystander Effect
The Bystander EffectThe Bystander Effect
The Bystander Effect
 
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final
09 ppt-confronting bullyingatworkd-lacy_final
 
The Bystander Borderline
The Bystander BorderlineThe Bystander Borderline
The Bystander Borderline
 
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015
Bystander intervention FHSU times talk 2015
 
Bystander intervention fhsu times talk
Bystander intervention fhsu times talkBystander intervention fhsu times talk
Bystander intervention fhsu times talk
 
The Unknown
The UnknownThe Unknown
The Unknown
 
Sexual Violence & Bystander Intervention
Sexual Violence & Bystander InterventionSexual Violence & Bystander Intervention
Sexual Violence & Bystander Intervention
 
Radiation induced bystander effect
Radiation induced bystander effectRadiation induced bystander effect
Radiation induced bystander effect
 
Bystanders in Bullying: What We Know and Where to Go
Bystanders in Bullying: What We Know and Where to GoBystanders in Bullying: What We Know and Where to Go
Bystanders in Bullying: What We Know and Where to Go
 
Psy Report
Psy ReportPsy Report
Psy Report
 
Bystander effect
Bystander effectBystander effect
Bystander effect
 
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace Bullying
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace BullyingFive Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace Bullying
Five Steps to Prevent Abusive Conduct and Workplace Bullying
 
Prosocial behavior
Prosocial behaviorProsocial behavior
Prosocial behavior
 
Helping others
Helping othersHelping others
Helping others
 
Prosocial behavior 2011 class
Prosocial behavior 2011 classProsocial behavior 2011 class
Prosocial behavior 2011 class
 
Bystander Awareness
Bystander AwarenessBystander Awareness
Bystander Awareness
 
Bystander effect
Bystander effectBystander effect
Bystander effect
 
Bullying And Harassment
Bullying And HarassmentBullying And Harassment
Bullying And Harassment
 
Human Trafficking PowerPoint Presentation
Human Trafficking PowerPoint PresentationHuman Trafficking PowerPoint Presentation
Human Trafficking PowerPoint Presentation
 

More from Dr. J's AP Psych Class

Questions for lyle lanley youre my hero
Questions for lyle lanley youre my heroQuestions for lyle lanley youre my hero
Questions for lyle lanley youre my heroDr. J's AP Psych Class
 
Questions for psych today bystander article
Questions for psych today bystander articleQuestions for psych today bystander article
Questions for psych today bystander articleDr. J's AP Psych Class
 
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles times
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles timesFacebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles times
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles timesDr. J's AP Psych Class
 

More from Dr. J's AP Psych Class (20)

Stupid brain article
Stupid brain articleStupid brain article
Stupid brain article
 
Prologue ch 1 review q's
Prologue ch 1 review q'sPrologue ch 1 review q's
Prologue ch 1 review q's
 
Simpsonsclips social psych
Simpsonsclips social psychSimpsonsclips social psych
Simpsonsclips social psych
 
Chapter 18 study questions
Chapter 18 study questionsChapter 18 study questions
Chapter 18 study questions
 
Questions for lyle lanley youre my hero
Questions for lyle lanley youre my heroQuestions for lyle lanley youre my hero
Questions for lyle lanley youre my hero
 
Questions for psych today bystander article
Questions for psych today bystander articleQuestions for psych today bystander article
Questions for psych today bystander article
 
Chapter 17 study questions
Chapter 17 study questionsChapter 17 study questions
Chapter 17 study questions
 
Abnormal humor
Abnormal humorAbnormal humor
Abnormal humor
 
Which one of us is truly crazy
Which one of us is truly crazyWhich one of us is truly crazy
Which one of us is truly crazy
 
Chapter 16 study questions ap psych
Chapter 16 study questions ap psychChapter 16 study questions ap psych
Chapter 16 study questions ap psych
 
Gardner mult int
Gardner mult intGardner mult int
Gardner mult int
 
Chapter 11 study qs
Chapter 11 study qsChapter 11 study qs
Chapter 11 study qs
 
11 9 mensa test
11 9 mensa test11 9 mensa test
11 9 mensa test
 
The breakfast club assignment full
The breakfast club assignment fullThe breakfast club assignment full
The breakfast club assignment full
 
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles times
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles timesFacebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles times
Facebook profile reflects the true you | booster shots | los angeles times
 
Ap psych chapter 15 study questions
Ap psych chapter 15 study questionsAp psych chapter 15 study questions
Ap psych chapter 15 study questions
 
The moral life of babies ny times
The moral life of babies   ny timesThe moral life of babies   ny times
The moral life of babies ny times
 
Moral development dilemmas ap psych
Moral development dilemmas   ap psychMoral development dilemmas   ap psych
Moral development dilemmas ap psych
 
Chapter 4 review questions
Chapter 4 review questionsChapter 4 review questions
Chapter 4 review questions
 
Chapter 12 review qs
Chapter 12 review qsChapter 12 review qs
Chapter 12 review qs
 

Bystander effect article

  • 1. Published on Psychology Today (http://www.psychologytoday.com) Why Donʼt We Help? Less Is More, at Least When It Comes to Bystanders By Melissa Burkley, Ph.D. Created Nov 4 2009 - 11:27am On October 24th, 2009, as many as 20 witnesses watched as a 15 year old girl was brutally assaulted and raped outside a homecoming dance in Richmond, CA. The viciousness of the attack was shocking, but what was even more shocking was the fact that so many people witnessed the attack and yet failed to intervene or call police. As one of the police officers involved in the case states, "what makes it even more disturbing is the presence of others. People came by, saw what was happening and failed to report it." Some of the bystanders reportedly even laughed and took photos of the assault with their cell phones. How could people just stand by and watch something this horrible happen to a young, innocent girl? Some have suggested that the eyewitnesses' failure to report the incident likely resulted from a concern over being labeled as a snitch. Although this is possible, social psychological research on the bystander effect suggests a different cause - there were too many eyewitnesses present. The bystander effect refers to the fact that people are less likely to offer help when they are in a group than when they are alone. Research on this effect was inspired by a real-world account that seems hauntingly similar to the recent event in Richmond. In 1964, 28 year old Kitty Genovese was raped and stabbed to death in front of her
  • 2. apartment complex. The attack lasted over 30 minutes and was witnessed by several dozen people who failed to report the incident. Some failed to realize that an actual crime was going on, claiming they thought it was a "lover's quarrel", whereas others realized they were witnessing a crime, but failed to report it because they assumed that someone else had already called the police. A similar incident took place in New Bedford, MA, in 1983 when several men raped a woman on a pool table in front of several witnesses in a bar. The 1988 film "The Accused" depicted this incident and Jodie Foster went on to earn an Academy Award for her performance as the rape victim. More recently, a 22 year old college student died from water intoxication in 2005 when four of his fellow fraternity brothers failed to intervene during a deadly hazing incident. To determine the underlying reasons why these witnesses failed to help, John Darley and Bibb Latane conducted a series of lab experiments to examine how the presence of others influences people's helping behavior in an emergency situation. The results of these studies suggest there are two clear reasons why the eyewitnesses in the Richmond case may have failed to help. 1. Pluralistic Ignorance One of the first steps in anyone's decision to help another is the recognition that someone is actually in need of help. To do this, the bystander must realize that they are witnessing an emergency situation and that a victim is in need of assistance. Consequently, a major reason why eyewitnesses fail to intervene is that they do not even realize they are witnessing a crime. When we are in an ambiguous situation and we are not sure whether there is an emergency or not, we often look to others to see how they are reacting. We assume that others may know something that we don't, so we gauge their reactions before we decide how we will respond. If those around us are acting as if it is an emergency, then we will treat it like an emergency and act accordingly. But if those around us are acting calm, then we may fail to recognize the immediacy of the situation and therefore fail to intervene. For example, imagine you are at the community pool and you see a child splashing wildly in the water. Your first instinct would probably be to look around you and see how others are responding. If others appear shocked and are yelling for help, you may conclude that the child is drowning and dive in to help. But, if those around you are ignoring the child or laughing, you may conclude that they child is just playing around. To avoid looking foolish, you would probably just continue watching and would fail to dive in and help. This seems like a reasonable approach and for the most part, it prevents us from making a fool out of ourselves. But the problem is that this tendency to look to others in order to determine how to respond can be biased by a phenomenon known as pluralistic ignorance. Pluralistic
  • 3. ignorance describes a situation where a majority of group members privately believe one thing, but assume (incorrectly) that most others believe the opposite. For example, pluralistic ignorance explains why my undergraduate students often fail to ask questions in class. Let's say that one of my students is confused about the class material I just covered and wants to ask me to clarify. Before raising her hand, she will likely look around to room to see if any of her fellow students seem confused or have their hand up as well. If no one else looks puzzled, she will conclude that she is the only one in the room that didn't get the material. To avoid looking stupid, she may choose to keep her hand down and not ask me her question. But as a teacher, I have discovered that if one student is unsure about the material, odds are most of the students are. So in this situation, my class is suffering from pluralistic ignorance because each one assumes they are the only one confused, when in fact all the students are confused and all of them are incorrectly concluding that they are the only one. The same process can occur when we witness an ambiguous emergency situation. All the bystanders may look to each other to determine if they are witnessing a crime, and if no one reacts, then everyone will wrongly conclude that this is not an emergency and no one will step up and help. The fact that several of the eyewitnesses in the Richmond case were laughing and taking photos with their cell phones suggests that they simply failed to realize they were witnessing a brutal rape and instead may have thought it was a prank. In one of Darley and Latane's classic studies, they tried to recreate this phenomenon in the lab. For their study, they had participants complete a questionnaire and after a few minutes, smoke started to pour into the room underneath a door in the back. Some participants were the only one in the room when this happened, but for others, there were two other students completing questionnaires in the room as well. In actuality, these two "students" were working for the researchers and were instructed to keep calm not matter what happens. The key question in this study was would the participant notice the smoke and go get help or would they simply write it off as nothing concerning and continue working on their questionnaire. The result showed that when the participant was alone, 75% of them left to report the smoke. But when there were two other people in the room who remained calm, only 10% left to get help. In some cases, the smoke got so thick the participant could barely read the questionnaire in front of them! Yet, as long as their fellow bystanders remained in calm, they did as well. Thus, when we are alone, we are more likely to assume an ambiguous situation represents and emergency and act accordingly. When we are in the presence of other bystanders, we are likely to look to those others for guidance and if they are not responding or are laughing or are taking photos of the event, we will mistakenly conclude it is not an emergency and will fail to help. So we now that one reason why people fail to help is because they don't realize they are witnessing a crime, but how can we use this information to our advantage? First, if you find yourself in an ambiguous situation, resist the urge to look to others and go with your gut instinct. If you think there is even a possibility that someone is in need, act on it. At worst, you will embarrass yourself for a few minutes, but at best, you will save a life. Second, if you are unfortunate enough to find yourself the victim and are in need of help, make sure you make it clear to those around you that this is an emergency situation. For instance, self-defense instructors will often tell women that if they are being attacked by a man, they should yell out "fire" instead of "help". This is because the word "help" is used in many situations that are non-life-threatening so when we hear it, it does not automatically indicate that there is an emergency. On the other hand, we only scream the word "fire" when there is an actual fire, and in some cases it is illegal to yell this word when there is
  • 4. not an actual fire (e.g., in a crowded theater). So by yelling "fire," you immediately make everyone around you aware that they are in an emergency situation. 2. Diffusion of Responsibility Even if people recognize that they are witnessing a crime, they may still fail to intervene if they do not take personal responsibility for helping the victim. The problem is that the more bystanders there are, the less responsible each individual feels. When you are the only eyewitness present, 100% of the responsibility for providing help rests on your shoulders. But if there are five eyewitnesses, only 20% of the responsibility is yours. The responsibility becomes defused or dispersed among the group members. In these situations, people may assume that someone else will help or that someone else is better qualified to provide assistance. But if everyone assumes this, then no one will intervene. Darley and Latane also investigate this phenomenon in a lab study. Specifically, they had participants take part in a group discussion over an intercom system. Some participants talked one-on-one over the intercom with another person and some talked over the intercom with a group of 5 other people. During the discussion, one of the voices on the intercom stated they were having a seizure and called out for help. In actuality, this was a prerecorded voice. For those who were led to believe they were the only person who overheard the seizure, 85% sought help. But for those who thought they were one of six people who overheard the seizure, only 31% sought help. So even when we are aware that an emergency is occurring, we are still less likely to help if other bystanders are present. So what about these people who overheard the seizure and didn't help? Were they just indifferent? Follow up interviews at the end of the study suggested that they were in fact concerned. Most mentioned overhearing the seizure, many had trembling hands and were clearly shaken from the experience and several inquired as to whether the victim finally received help. This tells us that they were not indifferent or heartless; they were concerned but simply didn't feel responsible enough to do anything about it. Interestingly, the researchers also asked if the participants thought that the presence of other bystanders affected their decision to get help or not and the most said it did not. So even though the presence of others clearly affects our helping behavior, we are unaware of this influence. So once again, how can we use the knowledge garnered from this study to our advantage? First, if you find yourself in an emergency situation with several fellow bystanders, realize that your first instinct (and the first instinct of those around you) will be to deny responsibility for helping the victim. By simply being aware of the diffusion of responsibility process, it may snap you out of the biased way of thinking and cause you to realize that you and everyone present is each 100% responsible for helping the victim. Second, if you find yourself in need of help, it is up to you to actively make one of your eyewitnesses feel personally responsible for your well-being. When we are in need of help and there is a crowd watching, we often plead for help to anyone that is listening, thinking that at least one person will step up to intervene. But self-defense instructors advise that you instead pick one person out of the crowd, look them dead in the eye, and tell that one person you need help. By pleading to a specific individual, you suddenly make that person feel completely responsible for your safety and this increases the odds that they will help. The same technique can be used if you are trying to get several others to help you assist a victim. Point to one person and tell them to go get help; point to another and tell them to call 911. Giving specific instructions to specific people counteracts the diffusion of responsibility process.
  • 5. Conclusion We typically think that the more people who witness a crime, the more people there will be to help the victim, but these classic social psychology experiments call this assumption into question. By making yourself and others aware of the factors that lead to such bystander apathy, we can hopefully make events like those that occurred in Richmond, CA and Bedford, MA a thing of the past. Source URL: http://www.psychologytoday.com/node/34502 Links: [1] http://www.psychologytoday.com/files/teaser/2009/11/helpwantedsign.jpg [2] http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-social-thinker