SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 85
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National
Stereotypes: A FourYear Longitudinal Study on Ingrian Finn
Immigrants’ Pre-and Postmigration Stereotypes of a Typical
Finn Jan-Erik Lönnqvist1 , Inga Jasinskaja-Lahti2 , and Markku
Verkasalo1 Abstract In a 4-year longitudinal study, we
investigated stereotype change in the context of increased
intergroup contact. Specifically, using one pre- and two
postmigration measurement points, we followed some 200
Ingrian Finns and their families migrating from Russia to
Finland. Stereotypes of a typical Finn were conceptualized
within the framework provided by Schwartz’s values theory. At
the group level, migrants’ stereotype profiles were consensual,
similar to Finns’s autostereotypes, somewhat accurate, and
highly stable. However, mean-level changes indicated a process
of disillusionment: Finns were increasingly perceived as less
benevolent and more hedonistic. We argue that personal contact
changes aspects of stereotypes related to communal
characteristics, whereas contact with cultural institutions
influences perceptions of conservativeness. Probably due to
political climate, Finns were increasingly perceived as adhering
to tradition and security values over stimulation. Although
individual-level stereotypes were only moderately stable and
stereotype change was heterogeneous, we could not predict
individual-level changes. Keywords national stereotypes,
intergroup contact, migration, values From Herodotus’s 5th
century B.C.E. depiction of Egyptians as the wisest people of
all mankind to Hollywood’s contemporary portrayal of the bad
Arab, national stereotypes—beliefs about the characteristics of
people from different countries—have fascinated scientists and
laypeople alike. It could therefore be considered surprising how
little is actually known about the processes Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 766
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) through which
national stereotypes are formed. Yet such knowledge could in
today’s shrinking world be of considerable importance. The
present study asks, in a migration setting, how increased contact
affects national stereotypes. This could shed some light on the
mechanisms through which national stereotypes are formed. The
present 4-year longitudinal study investigated how Ingrian Finn
and Russian migrants’ premigration stereotypes of native
Finnish host nationals changed postmigration. Stereotype
change was investigated both at the level of the group and at the
level of the individual. We were interested both in the content
of the stereotype and the consensus with which it was held.
Stability and Change in Group-Level National Stereotypes On
the level of the group (that is, aggregated across individuals),
the content of national stereotypes has been reported to be
highly stable. Most important in this respect is a series of
studies concerning the ethnic and national stereotypes of
Princeton University students (Gilbert, 1951; Karlins, Coffman,
& Walters, 1969; Katz & Braly, 1933; Madon et al., 2001), the
results of which can perhaps best be summarized as suggesting
that the content of stereotypes does change, but that this
happens very slowly, taking decades. Regarding shorter periods
of time, ranging from a couple of weeks to 5 years, the content
of group-level stereotypes has been shown to be highly stable
(e.g., Garcia-Marques, Santos, & Mackie, 2006; Realo et al.,
2009; Rothbart & John, 1993). Also testifying to the stability of
national stereotypes, they are strongly affected by relatively
stable factors such as climatic warmth and national prosperity
(McCrae, Terraciano, McCrae, & Allik, 2007), whereas more
short-term factors, such as government policies, appear to have
only little impact (Terraciano & McCrae, 2007). National
stereotypes have not previously been investigated in a migration
context. Examining the stability of national stereotypes in such
a context of heightened intergroup contact could help illuminate
some of the processes through which national stereotypes are
formed. The migration context should be especially well suited
for investigating the kernel of truth hypothesis (e.g., Allport,
1954/1978; Brigham, 1971), which suggests that national
stereotypes are to some extent accurate, reflecting real
differences between people. If this is correct, then increased
contact should lead to increased accuracy. Despite the
plausibility of the kernel of truth hypothesis, it has been
challenged by some recent empirical results. Terracciano et al.
(2005) had respondents from 49 countries provide self-ratings
of personality and rate the personality of a typical member of
their cultural group. Comparison of these two sets of ratings
revealed that stereotypes did not correspond with average self-
ratings. One reason for this could be that people generally have
little experience with other cultures and may therefore lack a
reference point for rating their own culture (Robins, 2005). A
related possibility is that autostereotypes (stereotypes of one’s
own nation) are erroneous, but heterostereotypes (stereotypes of
other nations) are more truthful (Robins, 2005). Taking a
different approach to studying the accuracy of national
stereotypes, Church and Katigbak (2002) asked Americans and
Filipinos who had lived in both America and the Philippines for
at least 3 years to judge the personality traits of Filipinos and
Americans. These ratings were not correlated with average self-
ratings of personality obtained from American and Filipino
samples. Challenging the above results according to which
national stereotypes are not accurate, a seven-country study by
Realo et al. (2009) showed that when the same measurement
instrument was used to assess both personality and national
character stereotype, relatively good agreement between self-
ratings and autostereotypes was found. Such agreement was also
reported for Ingrian Finn and Russian potential migrants’
premigration stereotypes regarding the personal values of a
typical Finnish host national (Lönnqvist, Yijälä, Jasinskaja-
Lahti, & Verkasalo, 2012). The present research design, in
which we longitudinally monitor these same migrants
throughout the migration process, should be ideal for
investigating the kernel of truth hypothesis. Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
Lönnqvist et al. 767 Increased firsthand experience with a
country and its residents could also have other effects than
increased accuracy. Heterostereotypes of Americans (Terraciano
& McCrae, 2007) and Russians (Realo et al., 2009) have been
shown to converge with autostereotypes. Direct contact could
lead to increased knowledge of the autostereotype, and the
heterostereotype could thus be expected to become more similar
to the autostereotype. There may also be migration-specific
effects on national stereotypes. Based on previous results, one
could expect stereotypes to become either more or less positive.
Migration will necessarily involve increased intergroup contact,
and if satisfactory, this contact could lead to increased
favorability towards the outgroup (for a meta-analysis, see
Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). However, the opposite effect could
be expected if the postmigration contact is negative or
discriminatory. Typically, the contact hypothesis literature
conceives of intergroup contact as general contact evaluated,
e.g., for its pleasantness or superficiality. But intergroup
contact may for migrants also involve experiences of outgroup
rejection and ethnic discrimination. For example, our recent
studies conducted with the same sample of participants have
shown that immigrants who, after migration, perceived more
negative intergroup contact than they had expected, were more
likely to disidentify from the national majority group and to
increasingly show negative attitudes toward this group
(Jasinskaja-Lahti, Mähönen, & Liebkind, 2012; Jasinskaja-
Lahti, Mähönen, & Ketokivi, in press; Mähönen, & Jasinskaja-
Lahti, 2012). Similarly, a study on Jewish adolescents from
Russia and Ukraine participating in an Israeli immigration
program showed that premigration idealization of the receiving
country was typically replaced by postmigration disillusionment
(Tartakovsky, 2009a). The stereotypes of host nationals could
thus become more negative postmigration. Besides content,
another important aspect of national stereotypes is the extent to
which they are consensual (i.e., the extent to which people
agree on which characteristics describe a group). In general, the
more consensual stereotypes are, the more often those
stereotypes will be confronted, and the more they will thereby
shape the behavior of the stereotyped group (e.g., Chen &
Bargh, 1997). The above mentioned Princeton trilogy and its
follow-ups have provided mixed evidence regarding the long-
term stability of stereotype consensus, with some studies
suggesting decreased (Devine & Elliot, 1995 ; Dovidio &
Gaertner, 1986) and others increased (Madon et al., 2001)
consensus. Similarly, regarding the effect of increased
intergroup contact on stereotype consensus, the empirical
evidence does not allow for any straightforward conclusions.
Some studies have shown consensus to decrease with increased
familiarity with the stereotyped group (e.g., Katz & Schank,
1938), but others have uncovered the opposite pattern (e.g.,
Schoenfeld, 1942). Stability and Change at the Level of the
Individual There are extremely few studies on the
intraindividual stability of stereotypes. One unpublished study,
described by Rothbart and John (1993), showed that although
the group-level stability of ethnic stereotypes was, across a 4-
year interval, extremely high, the individual-level stability of
stereotypes was only moderate. Another study showed a similar
pattern of results, with extremely high group-level stability, but
only moderate intraindividual stability over a time period of a
couple of weeks (Garcia-Marques et al., 2006). Examining
stereotype change at the intraindividual level could in several
ways contribute to our understanding regarding the processes
that influence stereotypes. First, intraindividual analysis allows
for the examination of how similar the process of stereotype
change is across individuals. Due to lack of empirical research,
very little is known about the extent to which stereotype change
is consensual. However, this type of knowledge could be vital,
as it directly speaks to the extent to which stereotype change is
influenced by common experiences: The more Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 768
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) consensual
stereotype change is, the more likely it is to be influenced by
common experiences of societal and institutionalized forces,
such as television programs, newspapers, books, films, and
political leaders. In the present context, the question is whether
the migration process has a uniform impact on stereotypes or
whether some migrants’ stereotypes change in one way and
others’ in some other way. Related to the above, if the process
of stereotype change is not uniform across individuals, it could
be meaningful to examine the associations of stereotype change
with other individual-level variables. Because migrants’
experiences of intergroup contact are likely to vary between
individuals, the intergroup contact hypothesis described above
could also find support at this level of analysis: Increased
quantity and especially good quality of interaction with native
Finns could render the stereotype of Finns more positive. By
contrast, experiences of discrimination could have the opposite
effect. Also, the disillusionment process, described above at the
level of the group, could be expected to be found at the level of
the individual—those with more positive premigration feelings
toward Finns could experience more profound postmigration
changes in their stereotype of Finns. On the other hand, such
migrants may have experienced more premigration contact with
Finns, rendering their stereotypes less susceptible to change.
Purpose of the Present Research The present study is part of
larger longitudinal research project (Intervening at the
Premigration Stage: Providing Tools for Promoting Integration
and Adaptation Throughout the Migration Process [INPRES])
that followed a sample of Ingrian Finns and their families in the
context of migration from Russia to Finland. National
stereotypes were measured within the framework provided by
Schwartz’s (1992) theory on personal values. Values are
concepts or beliefs that act as standards of what is most
desirable in the appraisal of events, actions, and people. Values
diverge from attitudes in that they transcend particular
situations, are organized within an individual according to
importance, establish criterions of desirability, and are fewer
and more crucial to personality than are attitudes. Due to the
scarcity of previous longitudinal research on stereotype change,
the complete lack of such research in a context of increased
intergroup contact and the often inconsistent findings reviewed
above, exact predictions were difficult to make. However, we
did expect stereotypes to be relatively stable at the level of the
group but individuals to show much less stability
(GarciaMarques et al., 2006; Rothbart & John, 1993). Regarding
the content of stereotypes, during the migration process, Ingrian
Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes of Finns could, as a result of
increased contact, be expected to become more accurate
(supporting the kernel of truth hypothesis; Realo et al., 2009;
cf., Church & Katigbak, 2002; Terraciano et al., 2005) but also
to become more similar to the autostereotypes of Finns (Realo
et al., 2009; Terraciano et al., 2005). Regarding the latter, these
were, in the present study, measured in a sample of Finnish
students—previous research suggests students’ national
stereotypes to be generalizable to the whole population
(Terraciano et al., 2005). Based on the contact hypothesis
(Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), the content of migrants’ stereotype
of Finns could be expected to become more positive. However,
the opposite could also be true, as migrants may during the
postmigration phase become disillusioned with their host
country (Tartakovsky, 2009a). Another set of questions relates
to consensus. How consensual are stereotypes, and how
consensual is stereotype change over time? Regarding the
former question, previous findings are very mixed (e.g., Katz &
Schanck, 1938; Schoenfeld, 1942), and there is no research on
the latter question. This forced a rather exploratory approach to
these questions. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo
Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 769 It seems
plausible to expect that individuals’ stereotypes may change in
different ways and that such individual differences could be
predictable. Those with more premigration experience of
Finland could already have a rather stable stereotype, less
susceptible to change. Furthermore, their premigration
stereotype could be more positive (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). In
our previous studies, using the premigration data of the INPRES
project, the more positive the premigration contact of potential
migrants with Finnish host nationals, the more positive were
their attitudes toward Finns and the less they anticipated
discrimination and adaptation problems after migration
(Jasinskaja-Lahti & Yijälä, 2011; Yijälä & Jasinskaja-Lahti,
2010). However, as stated earlier, these positive experiences
and expectations could also render them more vulnerable to
postmigration disillusionment (Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2012;
Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., in press Mähönen & Jasinskaja-Lahti,
2012; Tartakovsky, 2009a, 2009b). Importantly, not all
intergroup contact may influence stereotypes similarly:
Satisfactory contact was expected to be associated with
increased favorability toward the national majority group
(Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), but the opposite effect was expected
when postmigration contact had been experienced as
discriminatory. Regarding methodology, we decided to analyze
the Ingrian Finn migrants (so-called primary applicants of
Finnish descent) and their family members (spouses of mostly
Russian ethnicity), as separate samples, as the two groups could
have been expected to differ in some respects (e.g., the Ingrian
Finns could have experienced more contact with Finns prior to
migration). This methodological decision also served to keep all
observations independent. Method Participants and Procedure
Finnish students. Autostereotypes of Finns were measured in a
sample of 38 Finnish students (8 males; mean age = 34.8, SD =
11.1; range = 21 to 58) taking part in an introductory course in
psychology arranged by the University of Helsinki. Participants
rated online the values of a typical Finn. The course was an
Open University course (anyone can attend these courses), and
therefore, the mean age was somewhat higher than in more
typical student samples, arguably contributing to the
representativeness of the sample. The low number of males in
our sample may raise some concerns; however, mean male and
female stereotype profiles were virtually identical (r = .96;
independent samples t tests for equality in means also revealed
no differences between males and females, for all 10 values, t <
1.8, p > .05). The ratings were collected in the summer of 2011,
at around the same time that most of the migrants completed the
second follow-up. Emigrating Ingrian Finns and Russians.
Ingrian Finns and Russians were participants of the INPRES
project investigating the migration of Ingrian Finns and their
family members to Finland between 2008 and 2011. The Ingrian
Finns are mostly offspring of Finnish colonizers who settled
down in Ingria (now Leningrad Oblast) in the 17th century.
After the Russian conquest in 1703, many remigrated back to
Finland. Of those who remained, many went missing in the
forced population transfers before and after World War II, and
many migrated to Finland soon after the breakdown of the
Soviet Union in the first years of the 1990s. However, there are
still about 30,000 Ingrian Finns remaining in Russia. After
completing an official language test, Ingrian Finns are qualified
for guaranteed habitation permit in the Finnish Law of Return.
The baseline sample of the project consisted altogether of 325
potential migrants living in the Republic of Karelia or the
Leningrad area. We measured participants’ stereotype of Finns
three times, once before migration (T0) and twice postmigration
(T1 and T2). Premigration experience of Finland and attitude
toward Finns were measured at T0 and experienced intergroup
contact at T2. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo
Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 770 Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology 44(5) The T0 data were collected at the Finnish
language courses that were part of the immigration training
program organized by the Finnish authorities in Russia during
April and May 2008. Everyone attending these courses (N =
192) completed the questionnaires in class. The language
students were instructed to take home an additional
questionnaire to be completed by family members (spouses)
joining the respondent in Finland (N = 84). In addition, the
questionnaire was mailed to those potential migrants who had
already passed the mandatory language test and were waiting to
be assigned a particular place of residence in Finland (N = 49).
These participants were identified via the register of the Finnish
Consulate in Russia (St. Petersburg), and they participated via
post survey. The mean age of the participants at T0 was 43.1
years (SD = 14.4 years), ranging between 19 and 85 years. Most
participants were married or cohabiting (70%) and had children
(77%). Furthermore, most of them had full-time employment
(61%), whereas only 3% were unemployed or temporarily
dismissed at T0. Every second (46%) of these 325 participants
thought that they would be able to migrate within the following
7 or 8 months. As the participants of the baseline study
migrated to Finland according to their personal schedules, the
follow-up data could not be collected at once. Instead, for the
first postmigration assessment (T1), three searches from the
Finnish population register were performed between October
2009 and December 2010 in order to include as many baseline
participants as possible. Each round consisted of an initial
request to participate, including the postmigration
questionnaires and a return envelope, followed by two
reminders. The participants were identified by their name and
date of birth. At the end of the third search, a total of 233
respondents (70%) of our baseline sample had migrated to
Finland by December 2010, of whom 227 respondents took part
in the study at T1.The mean age in the T1 follow-up sample was
44.3 years (SD = 13.9). Most participants were females (63%),
and they were still married or cohabiting (69%). Despite their
high level of education prior to migration (only 15% had no
education beyond secondary school), they had not yet been
employed in Finland but were typically unemployed (49%), on
pension (10%), or studying (12%) at T1. The second follow-up
(T2) was conducted 12 to 20 months after T1 assessment, using
the same procedure as described above. In total, 194 (82% of all
T0 participants that had migrated to Finland by December 2011
and 86% of T1 participants) migrants participated at T2. The
mean age in the second follow-up sample was 45.4 years (SD =
13.4). Most participants were females (63%), were typically
still married or cohabiting (62%), and were typically still
unemployed (49%), on pension (8%), or studying (19%). In
order to examine possible selection bias due to sample attrition,
t tests on relevant demographic factors (gender, age, marital
status, socioeconomic status, employment status, level of
education, Finnish language proficiency) and other T0 variables
used in this study were performed. Our final sample did not
differ in terms of sociodemographic factors or in terms of their
responses to T0 scales from those respondents participating
only at T0. For the analyses presented here, we included only
participants who took part at all three measurement occasions—
that is, 136 ethnic migrants (Ingrian Finns; 37 males, mean age
at T0 = 44.4, SD = 14.1) and their 56 spouses (46 Russians and
10 participants of other ethnic origins from within the former
Soviet region; 34 males, mean age = 40.6, SD = 11.1). We refer
to these two groups of migrants as Ingrian Finns and Russians,
respectively. The migrants had arrived in Finland between 3 and
15 months (Ingrian Finns: M = 9.6 months, SD = 3.9; Russians:
M = 10.5 months, SD = 3.8) before the T1 assessment and
between 13 and 28 months before the T2 assessment (Ingrian
Finns: M = 22.0 months, SD = 4.1; Russians: M = 23.0 months,
SD = 3.9). In both samples, the average time that had elapsed
between T0 and T1 was 19 months, and the average time
between T1 and T2 was 12 months. Representative sample of
Finns. As a criterion for the accuracy of stereotypes of Finns,
we used self-rating of values. These were gathered from strict
probability samples, representative of the Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
Lönnqvist et al. 771 population 15 years and older, in the third
round of the European Social Survey (ESS; Jowell & the Central
Coordinating Team, 2007). Finland had 1,646 (782 males; mean
age 48.2, SD = 18.6) participants. The data were collected in the
fall of 2006. Measures Portraits Values Questionnaire. At the
individual level, people in most cultures (recently 77; Schwartz,
2009) distinguish between at least 10 basic values. Schwartz
(1992) has labeled these universal value types as benevolence,
tradition, conformity, security, power, achievement, hedonism,
stimulation, self-direction, and universalism. Our measure of
the 10 basic values was the 21-item Portrait Values
Questionnaire (PVQ21). It is based on descriptions of different
people, whose goals, aspirations, and wishes are characterized
in two sentences (Davidov, Schmith, & Schwartz, 2008). For
example, the item “It is important to her to show her abilities.
She wants people to admire what she does” measures
achievement values. In the ESS, respondents indicate “how
much like you this person is” by using a 6-point scale ranging
from 1 (very much like me) to 6 (not like me at all). For the
present study, all items were reverse scored, so that the higher
the number, the stronger the agreement with the item. When two
values or less were missing, the missing values were replaced
using the regression method. By mistake, the PVQ21 was, in the
migrant samples, administered using a 5-point scale ranging
from 1 (not like me at all) to 5 (like me); the response option 6
(very much like me) was left outside the printable area of the
questionnaire. To facilitate comparison between samples, scores
of 5 or 6 were recoded into 5 in the Finnish student sample and
in the ESS data. The average internal consistency reliability of
our Finnish students’ ratings of the typical Finn was, across the
10 basic values, .57 (range = .23 [self-direction] to .84
[hedonism]). In the Ingrian Finn and Russian migrant samples,
the corresponding ratings showed similar and acceptable levels
of internal consistency reliability (average T0/T1/T2 αs = .56
[range = .29 (Stimulation) to .77 (Security)] / .53 [range = .16
(Stimulation) to .71 (Achievement)] / .60 [range = .28
(Stimulation) to .73 (Achievement)] and .62 [range = .40
(Achievement) to .81 (Security)] / .55 [range = .40 (Tradition)
to .73 (Achievement)] / .61 [range = .44 (Power) to .79
(Conformity)], respectively). Although some of the reliabilities
were low, this cannot be considered very surprising, as each of
the basic values, despite representing broad constructs, is
measured by only two items (three for universalism). The
reliabilities are in line with the reliabilities reported in earlier
studies (e.g., Schwartz, 2007; Schwartz & Rubel, 2005),
suggesting no particular problems in the current samples or with
the instructions to rate stereotypes. In fact, low alpha
reliabilities may not threaten measurement validity; McCrae,
Kurtz, Yamagata, and Terracciano (2011) demonstrated that
internal consistency reliability is not related to various validity
criteria. The PVQ21, like all other measures, was administered
in the Russian language to our samples of Ingrian Finns and
Russians. This may raise concerns regarding the comparability
of scores on the Russian and Finnish translations of the PVQ21
(the Finnish student sample and the representative sample of
Finns responded in Finnish). Indeed, the Finnish and Russian
translations of the PVQ21 display configural and partial metric
invariance but not scalar invariance (Davidov, 2010). This
allows researchers to compare the correlates of these two
questionnaires across countries but not to compare mean scores.
We used the scores obtained with the Finnish translation only
for purposes of computing profile correlations, not to compare
mean scores. Supporting the comparability of values profiles
collected with the Russian and Finnish translations of the PVQ,
the average self-rating values profiles of the Ingrian Finns (not
shown) and the representative sample of Finns were almost
identical (r = .94; for evidence that value profiles should not
differ much across cultures, see Fischer & Schwartz, 2011).
Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on
April 5, 2016 772 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5)
Because some of our predictions concerned changes in the
valence of the stereotype, it is important to note that some
values may be more desirable than others. Across cultures and
measurement instruments (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001), the most
important value tends to be benevolence, strongly implying that
it is the most socially desirable value (see Edwards, 1953).
Consistent with this, at least when judging others, communal
characteristics are generally considered the most desirable
(Abele & Wojciszke, 2007), and benevolence values are highly
correlated with measures of socially desirable responding,
which indicates the degree to which a person strives to make a
good impression (Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005; Schwartz,
Verkasalo, Antonovsky, & Sagiv, 1997). By contrast, hedonism,
on the opposite side of the values circle (see Figure 1 in
Schwartz, 1992), could be considered the least socially
desirable value, as it focuses on gratification of one’s own
sensual desires. Consistent with this, hedonism scores are
negatively correlated with socially desirable responding
(Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005; Schwartz et al., 1996).
Premigration experience of Finland and attitude toward Finns.
Premigration experience of Finland was assessed with an item
asking participants how often they had visited Finland. The
response options were 0 (never), 1 (once or twice), 2 (three or
four times), 3 (five or six times), 4 (seven or eight times), and 5
(nine or more times). The item was used both as a continuous
variable and as a dichotomous variable that indicated whether or
not the participant had ever visited Finland prior to migration.
In the sample of Ingrian Finns, the median response was five or
six previous visits to Finland, and only 12 (9%) had never
visited Finland. In the sample of Russians, the median response
was one or two previous visits to Finland, and 14 (25%) had
never visited. A feeling thermometer (e.g., Paolini, Hewstone,
Cairns, & Voci, 2004; Verkuyten, 2007) was used to assess
overall feeling toward Finns. Ratings were made on scale
ranging from 0 (very cold or negative feelings) to 100 (very
positive or warm feelings). The mean of the one-item measure
was, in the sample of Ingrian Finns, 87 (SD = 14) and, in the
sample of Russians, 89 (SD = 18). Intergroup contact. Quantity
of intergroup contact was measured with five items: participants
indicated on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much) how
much contact they had experienced with native Finns in each of
the following contexts: at college, as neighbors, as close friend,
as authorities, and as employers/teachers. The first three items
were from a scale developed by Islam and Hewstone (1993), to
which we added the last two items. In the sample of Ingrian
Finns/Russians, the descriptive statistics were M = 2.83, SD =
1.31, α = .68; M = 2.74, SD = 1.30, α = .68. Quality of contact
was measured by having participants rate each of the above five
contexts on three items. Specifically, on a scale from 1 (not at
all) to 5 (very), participants responded to the item “When you
meet native Finns, in general do you find the contact . . .”
followed by three adjectives: pleasant, cooperative, and
superficial (the last one was reverse-coded). The descriptives of
this 15-item scale, adapted from a study by Voci and Hewstone
(2003), were, in the sample of Ingrian Finns and Russians, M =
3.53, SD = 0.79, α = .78, and M = 3.49, SD = 0.68, α = .89,
respectively. To assess more negative experiences of intergroup
contact, we measured perceived discrimination using four items:
“I think native Finns do not accept me because I come from
Russia”; “I think native Finns have something against me
because I come from Russia”; “Native Finns have harassed me
or insulted me because I come from Russia”; and “Native Finns
have threatened me or attacked me because I come from
Russia.” The items, adapted from the discrimination scale by
Berry, Phinney, Sam, and Vedder (2006), were rated on a scale
from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). In the
sample of Ingrian Finns and Russians, the descriptive statistics
were M = 1.96, SD = 0.77, α = .77, and M = 2.03, SD = 0.67, α
= .65, respectively. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at
Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 773
Results The means and standard deviations of the personal
values of a representative sample of Finns are shown in Table 1.
These statistics are also shown for Finns’ stereotype of Finns as
well as Ingrian Finn and Russian migrants’ stereotypes of a
typical Finn at the three measurement points. Group-Level
Results Before examining the 10 basic values individually, we
combined information over all 10 values by conducting analyses
using the values profiles that the 10 basic values constitute. We
first examined whether within the different samples there
existed a shared stereotype of Finns and whether this
stereotype, within the groups of migrants, became more or less
consensual over time. Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs)
were used to assess interrater consensus. All ICCs reported are
from two-way mixed effects models: Both objects (the 10 basic
values) and raters contributed systematic variability, and rater
effects were fixed (raters did not constitute a random sample
drawn from a larger population). The single-measures ICCs
indicate the percentage of shared variance between any two
raters’ stereotype profiles. The average measures ICCs show the
variance that the average profile would be expected to share
with an average profile derived from a similar sample of raters.
The ICCs shown in Table 2 indicate that within all groups,
participants shared a rather similar stereotype of the values of a
typical Finn. Furthermore, the highly overlapping confidence
intervals suggest that there were no differences in consensus,
either between groups of participants or over time. We next
examined the group-level accuracy and stability of stereotype
profiles. This was done by correlating among each other the
actual personal values of the representative sample of Finn (ESS
sample) and Finns’, Ingrian Finns’, and Russians’ stereotypes of
the typical Finn. For such correlational profile analyses, using
Schwartz’s (1992) values theory as a general framework
provides two advantages: the direction of keying is meaningful
(an arbitrary direction of keying would influence indices of
profile similarity; Tellegen, 1965), and only the shape of the
profiles matters (differences in elevation present response bias
and should be ignored; Schwartz, 1992). These considerations
make Pearson’s correlation coefficient, shown in Table 3, an
appropriate statistic. There are five important things to notice
about these correlations. (a) Ingrian Finns’ and Table 1. Means
and Standard Deviations of Finns’ Personal Values and Finns’,
Ingrian Finns’, and Russians Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal
Values. Ingrian Finns’ Stereotype (N = 125 to 131) Russians’
Stereotype (N = 52 to 56) Basic Values Finns’ Values (N =
1,646) Finns’ Stereotype (N = 38) T0 T1 T2 T0 T1 T2
Benevolence 1.18 (0.16) 1.09 (0.18) 1.02 (0.18) 0.96 (0.21)
0.92 (0.18) 0.98 (0.17) 0.95 (0.21) 0.93 (0.19) Universalism
1.19 (0.17) 0.99 (0.17) 1.14 (0.14) 1.14 (0.16) 1.11 (0.14) 1.13
(0.13) 1.13 (0.14) 1.14 (0.15) Self-Direction 1.11 (0.16) 1.01
(0.14) 0.99 (0.16) 0.98 (0.19) 0.99 (0.17) 0.98 (0.16) 0.97
(0.19) 0.96 (0.20) Stimulation 0.89 (0.23) 0.75 (0.20) 0.77
(0.17) 0.73 (0.16) 0.75 (0.16) 0.74 (0.18) 0.72 (0.21) 0.74
(0.18) Hedonism 0.95 (0.24) 0.90 (0.25) 0.79 (0.21) 0.84 (0.22)
0.84 (0.22) 0.82 (0.21) 0.85 (0.21) 0.90 (0.20) Achievement
0.84 (0.24) 0.93 (0.23) 0.93 (0.18) 0.91 (0.20) 0.92 (0.20) 0.97
(0.16) 0.94 (0.21) 0.92 (0.18) Power 0.72 (0.21) 0.98 (0.23)
0.77 (0.20) 0.77 (0.21) 0.80 (0.23) 0.82 (0.23) 0.83 (0.20) 0.86
(0.19) Security 1.11 (0.20) 1.30 (0.17) 1.24 (0.15) 1.27 (0.15)
1.26 (0.16) 1.21 (0.16) 1.27 (0.16) 1.25 (0.18) Conformity 1.03
(0.23) 1.12 (0.26) 1.14 (0.21) 1.18 (0.20) 1.17 (0.19) 1.17
(0.16) 1.19 (0.19) 1.20 (0.19) Tradition 1.00 (0.22) 1.09 (0.25)
1.15 (0.17) 1.20 (0.19) 1.19 (0.16) 1.14 (0.17) 1.18 (0.18) 1.20
(0.17) Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group -
UOP on April 5, 2016 774 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
44(5) Russians’ ratings of the typical Finn were, across
measurement points, rather accurate, whereas Finns’ ratings
were not. (b) Both Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes,
besides being rather accurate, resembled Finns’ autostereotype.
In fact, Finns’ self-ratings (ESS sample) and Finns’
autostereotypes (student sample) both contributed to the
prediction of Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ heterostereotypes of
the typical Finn. For instance, in the prediction of Ingrian
Finns’ premigration stereotypes, a regression model including
only Finns’ self-ratings as a predictor variable predicted 50%
(R2 ) of the variance (p = .02), and this number increased to
80% (p = .005 ) when Finns’ autostereotypes were added to the
model (change in R2 = 30, p = .01). Similarly, Finns’ self-rating
could predict Ingrian Finns’ heterostereotypes beyond that
which was predicted by Finns’ autostereotypes (R2 increased
from .68 [p = .003] to .80 [p = .005], change in R2 = 12, p =
.08), although the effect did not quite reach statistical
significance. Virtually identical results were obtained for all
three measurement points and in the sample of Russians. (c)
Both Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes remained
remarkably consistent over time, with all profile-profile
correlations across the three measurement points exceeding .97.
(d) Neither Ingrian Finns’ nor Russians’ stereotypes became
more accurate over time (first column of Table 3), (e) nor did
they become more similar to Finns’ autostereotype (second
column of Table 3). Table 2. Intraclass Correlation Coefficients
for Stereotype Ratings. Single Measures ICC Average Measures
ICC Finns’ stereotype .32 (.17 to .62) .95 (.89 to .98) Ingrian
Finns’ stereotype T0 .51 (.33 to .78) .99 (.98 to 1.00) T1 .50
(.32 to .77) .99 (.98 to 1.00) T2 .47 (.29 to .75) .99 (.97 to 1.00)
Russians’ stereotype T0 .39 (.22 to .71) .97 (.94 to .99) T1 .39
(.22 to .71) .97 (.94 to .99) T2 .62 (.33 to .97) .99 (.96 to 1.00)
Note: The numbers in parenthesis are 95% confidence intervals.
Table 3. Group-Level Profile Similarity Coefficients Between
Finns’ Personal Values and Finns’, Ingrian Finns’, and
Russians’ Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Finns’ Values
(N = 1,646) Finns’ Stereotype (N = 38) Ingrian Finns’
Stereotype (N = 125) Russians’ Stereotype (N = 52) 1. 2. 3. T0
4. T1 5. T2 6. T0 7. T1 8. T2 1. 2. .49 3. .71* .83* 4. .64* .83*
.98* 5. .58 .83* .97* 1.00* 6. .62 .83* .98* .99* .98* 7. .57
.86* .97 .99* .99* .99* 8. .54 .83* .94* .98* .99* .97* .99* *p
< .05. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group -
UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 775 To assess mean-level
changes in the content of Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’
stereotypes, we evaluated the data with a repeated measures
regression analysis (see Misangyi, LePine, Algina, & Goeddeke,
2006). The repeated measures regression procedure fit, for each
migrant, a regression line to his three ratings over time.
Specifically, one basic value at a time, each migrant’s
stereotype was predicted by the time intervals of 0
(corresponding to years lived in Finland at T0), years in Finland
at first follow-up, and years in Finland at second follow-up.
This procedure derived for each migrant an intercept value,
which indicated how the migrant on average rated Finns, and a
slope value, which indicated how the migrant’s ratings changed
as a function of time. Furthermore, we also included, in the
same regression, the curvilinear effects of time, because some
changes could conceivable be only temporary. The average
intercepts and slopes are shown in Table 4. These reveal that on
average, Ingrian Finns’ perceptions of the typical Finn became
less positive over time, with a decrease in perceived
benevolence and an increase in perceived hedonism.
Furthermore, Ingrian Finns also perceived Finns as increasingly
conservative (tradition and security up and stimulation down).
Almost identical results were obtained in the sample of
Russians, but these tended not to be statistically significant due
to smaller sample size. Curvilinear effects were neglectable.
Individual-Level Results The above group-level analyses
revealed high stereotype profile stability across time. We next
investigated whether such high-profile stability could also be
found at the level of the individual. At this level, we were able
to differentiate between overall similarity and distinctive
similarity. As noted by Cronbach (1955), there are two
components that influence similarities in profile shape:
stereotype accuracy and differential accuracy. Stereotype
accuracy refers to the similarities that any two profiles will
share. Applied to the present case, the Ingrian Finns clearly
shared a stereotype regarding the values profile of a typical
Finn. This means that any two randomly chosen Ingrian Finns’
stereotypes of the typical Finn will, even across time, be
somewhat similar. If one is interested in the stability of an
individual’s stereotype profile, then such similarity between
random profiles will inflate stability estimates. Therefore, one
should instead look at distinctive similarity, which controls for
the shared variance of all profiles. In cross-cultural data, this
means standardizing the data using cross-cultural norms
(Terracciano et al., 2005). In Table 4. Change in Stereotypes:
Means, Standard Deviations, and Range of Regression Estimates
From Within-Individual Analyses. Ingrian Finns’ Stereotype (N
= 120) Russians’ Stereotype (N = 53) Intercept Beta Beta range
Beta2 Intercept Beta Beta range Beta2 Basic Values M (SD) M
(SD) Betamin to Betamax M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Betamin to
Betamax M (SD) Benevolence 1.01 (0.18) -0.11 (0.58)* -1.60 to
1.49 0.02 (0.24) 0.98 (0.17) -0.05 (0.51) -1.50 to 1.41 0.02
(0.22) Universalism 1.14(0.14) 0.00 (0.43) -1.93 to 1.16 -0.01
(0.19) 1.14 (0.13) -0.01 (0.38) -1.13 to 0.90 0.01 (0.15) Self-
direction 0.99 (.17) 0.00 (0.49) -1.30 to 2.24 0.01 (0.21) 0.98
(0.16) -0.02 (0.43) -1.32 to 0.79 0.01 (0.15) Stimulation
0.78(0.17) -0.14 (0.55)* -2.95 to 1.44 0.06 (0.24)* 0.74 (0.18) -
0.04 (0.42) -1.22 to 1.25 0.01 (0.16) Hedonism 0.79(0.21) 0.11
(0.58)* -1.44 to 2.65 -0.04 (0.26) 0.82 (0.21) 0.04 (0.49) -1.11
to 1.74 -0.01 (0.20) Achievement 0.94(0.18) -0.04 (0.51) -1.89
to 1.65 0.01 (0.22) 0.97 (0.16) -0.05 (0.49) -1.64 to 1.31 0.01
(0.20) Power 0.77(0.21) -0.03 (0.54) -1.23 to 2.22 0.02 (0.22)
0.82 (0.23) -0.02 (0.54) -1.46 to 1.11 0.02 (0.20) Security
1.24(0.15) 0.10 (0.45)* -0.95 to 2.79 -0.04 (0.20) 1.22 (0.16)
0.10 (0.31)* -0.47 to 1.15 -0.03 (0.13) Conformity 1.15(0.21)
0.06 (0.60) -1.58 to 2.88 -0.03 (0.27) 1.18 (0.16) 0.00 (0.61) -
2.43 to 1.22 0.02 (0.30) Tradition 1.16(0.17) 0.10 (0.55)* -1.76
to 2.88 -0.04 (0.26) 1.14 (0.17) 0.07 (0.39) -0.89 to 1.15 -0.01
(0.15) *p < .05. One-sample t test against zero. Downloaded
from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
776 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) individual-level
data, this means computing the indices of profile similarity
from data first standardized across individuals. The resulting
index of profile similarity indicates how the pattern of how a
profile (specific individual’s T0 stereotype) deviates from other
profiles (average of other individuals’ T0 stereotypes) reflects
how another profile (specific individual’s T1 stereotype)
deviates from other profiles (average of other individuals’ T1
stereotypes). Both overall and distinctive profile similarity
coefficients (Pearson’s rs) are presented in Table 5 (we used the
Fisher r-to-Z-to-r averaging procedure). The psychometrically
preferable distinctive similarity coefficients indicated, as
expected, that stereotype profiles were only moderately stable at
the level of the individual. The rank-order stabilities (test-retest
correlations) of specific values are given in Table 6. Consistent
with the only moderate stability of the individual-level values
profiles, the rank-order stabilities of some of the basic values
were not very high. This means that migrants changed their
relative ordering compared to other migrants; for example, the
same migrants who premigration perceived the Finns as more
benevolent than did other migrants did not necessarily do so
postmigration. To assess whether changes in the stereotype were
consistent across individuals (was it the same people whose
perceptions of Finns’ benevolence and stimulation decreased,
and whose perceptions of Finns’ hedonism, security, and
tradition increased?), we computed the ICC for Table 5.
Average Individual-Level Profile Similarity Coefficients
(Pearson rs) Over Time for Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’
Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Ingrian Finns (N = 125)
Russians (N = 52) Time Period Overall Distinctive Overall
Distinctive T0 to T1 .75 .37 .74 .44 T1 to T2 .77 .48 .72 .42 T0
to T2 .70 .31 .65 .30 Note: All significant at p < .001; one-
sample t test against zero. Table 6. Rank-Order Correlation
Coefficients for Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ Stereotypes of
Finns’ Personal Values. Ingrian Finns (N = 125 to 131)
Russians (N = 52 to 56) Basic Values T0 to T1 T1 to T2 T0 to
T2 T0 to T1 T1 to T2 T0 to T2 Benevolence .36 .31 .05 .38 .08
.33 Universalism .35 .48 .25 .37 .48 .22 Self-direction .40 .49
.38 .40 .48 .19 Stimulation .32 .46 .43 .54 .64 .56 Hedonism .53
.42 .31 .42 .32 .17 Achievement .47 .59 .32 .33 .39 .29 Power
.43 .55 .39 .29 .49 .54 Security .39 .42 .23 .59 .52 .35
Conformism .31 .45 .39 .37 .28 .35 Tradition .33 .38 .26 .42 .56
.32 Average .39 .46 .30 .42 .44 .34 Note: For Ingrian Finns
(Russians), all correlations r ≥ .23 (r ≥ .28) are statistically
significant at p < .05. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at
Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 777 value
change. This analysis was based on the 10 Beta values (one Beta
estimate for each of the basic values) that each individual
obtained in the within-individual regression analyses. The
obtained ICC revealed that any two random Ingrian Finn
migrants’ value change profiles shared 0% of their variance (p =
.79). A similar result was obtained in the sample of Russian
migrants (p = .81). The migrants did not experience uniform
changes in their stereotypes of the values profile of a typical
Finn. Rather, some migrants’ stereotypes changed regarding
some values, and other migrants’ regarding some other values.
Predictors of Stereotype Change The above results suggest that
stereotypes were only moderately stable at the level of the
individual and that there was no uniform pattern to how
stereotypes changed. Such circumstances could have been
expected to facilitate the search for individual-level predictors
of stereotype change. Recall that we predicted that positive and
negative experiences with Finns could potentially influence the
stereotype of Finns. To examine this, we correlated quantity of
contact with Finns, quality of contact with Finns, and perceived
discrimination by Finns with the slopes obtained from the
within-participants regression analyses. None of the resulting
30 correlations reached conventional levels of statistical
significance (in both samples, all p > .05). To assess the overall
magnitude of stereotype change, we created for each individual
a sum score by adding up the absolute values of the individual’s
slope coefficients (derived from the withinindividual
regression), but this index was also not correlated with the three
variables reflecting direct experience with Finns (in both
samples, all p > .05). Finally, we did not find evidence that
those migrants with more frequent premigration contact (coded
either as a continuous or dichotomous variable) would have had
a different initial stereotype (indexed by the intercepts from the
within-participant regressions). Across both samples, only one
correlation out of the 40 (10 Basic Values × 2 Indices of
Contact × 2 Samples) tested reached statistical significance. The
same result was found when examining stereotype change (as
indexed by the betas from within-participants regressions): 1
correlation (out of 40) was statistically significant. Regarding
the predictive power of premigration positive feelings, scores
on the thermometer scale were not correlated with any of the
intercept values from withinparticipants regression analyses,
and with only 1 (out of 20 [10 Slopes × 2 Samples]) of the beta
values. Furthermore, neither premigration contact nor feelings
toward Finns predicted the overall magnitude of stereotype
change. Discussion The data presented here force us to
acknowledge that there are no simple answers to questions
regarding stability and change in stereotypes. In the present
context, the extent of stability and change in migrants’
stereotypes depended on whether one examined (a) the group or
the individual, (b) profile scores that combined information
over all 10 basic values or single basic values, and (c) the rank-
order or absolute stability of single values. Based on our
results, we argue that the specific mechanisms involved in
stereotype change may be different for different aspects of the
stereotype. Mechanisms of Stereotype Change We initially
suspected that migrants’ stereotypes could become more
accurate or more similar to Finns’ autostereotypes as a result of
increased intergroup contact. However, group-level profile
scores were extremely stable. This could be because the
premigration stereotypes were already Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 778
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) (a) rather accurate,
as shown by comparison with self-ratings from a representative
sample of Finns, and also (b) similar to Finns’ autostereotype.
In fact, around 80% of the variance of premigration stereotypes
could be explained by correspondence with Finns’ self-ratings
and Finns’ autostereotype. Upon increased contact with Finns,
and thereby also increased knowledge of Finns’ autostereotype,
migrants most likely found support for both those aspects of
their stereotype that corresponded with Finns’ self-ratings as
well as those aspects that corresponded with Finns’
autostereotype (for confirmatory biases in information
processing, see Kunda & Oleson, 1995; Lord, Ross, & Lepper,
1979). Apart from the above longitudinal perspective, it is
worthwhile to note that the results support the idea that
heterostereotypes may, among well-informed individuals, be
more accurate than autostereotypes, for instance due to the
availability of a reference point (Robins, 2005). However,
heterostereotypes, despite being somewhat accurate, resembled
Finns’ autostereotypes even more than they resembled Finns’
self-ratings. We also have more confidence in this latter result
(as compared to the results pertaining to accuracy), as it is
similar to previous results according to which heterostereotypes
of the personality traits of Americans (Terraciano & McCrae,
2007) and Russians (Realo et al., 2009) converge with
autostereotypes. One reason for this could be that both
autostereotypes and heterostereotypes are to some extent based
on similar knowledge of facts about the target of the ratings.
For instance, at least some Finns and migrants to Finland are
likely to know that Finland tends to be ranked as being one of
the least corrupt countries in the world (e.g., Zook, 2009), and
this may be reflected in high ratings of the typical Finns’
conformity and security values. Although group-level stereotype
profile scores were extremely stable, meaningful mean-level
changes could be observed for some of the 10 basic values. One
clearly observable trend was that stereotypes became less
desirable over time, with perceptions of native Finns’
benevolence decreasing and perceptions of their hedonism
increasing. Tartakovsky (2009a) identified a disillusionment
process in his study on Jewish adolescents migrating to Israel.
Consistent with the results of that study, in which the attitude
toward Israel became more negative postmigration, our
participants revealed an increasingly negative stereotype of
Finns. In general, for increased contact to lead to increased
favorability toward the outgroup, the contact should optimally
be positive, equal status, and cooperative (e.g., Pettigrew &
Tropp, 2006). The contact between Ingrian Finns and native
Finns has most likely not fulfilled these criteria. In fact, much
of the contact is likely to have occurred in a context in which
the migrant has been in a low-status position, for instance
applying for jobs, educational programs, public housing, or
well-fare benefits. In such unequal status contexts, high-power
individuals tend to behave in a more selfish way (De Cremer &
Van Dijk, 2005), show less social engagement (Kraus &
Keltner, 2009), and interrupt others more often during
interpersonal interaction (Hall, Coats, & LeBeau, 2005). High-
power individuals are also stereotypically perceived as colder
and less friendly (Conway, Pizzamiglio, & Mount, 1996). The
results of our previous studies on the quality of intergroup
contact and anticipated and perceived discrimination, conducted
with the same sample of migrants as this study (Jasinskaja-
Lahti, Mähönen, & Liebkind, 2012; Jasinskaja-Lahti, Mähönen,
& Ketokivi, in press; Mähönen, & Jasinskaja-Lahti, 2012), also
suggest that the intergroup contact experienced by our
participants had not been positive; immigrants have reported
less positive contact and more discrimination after migration
than they have anticipated premigration. The stressfulness of
the acculturation experience could also in part be responsible
for the stereotypes of Finns becoming increasingly negative.
The second year after migration may be especially difficult,
with psychological adjustment and adaptation again beginning
to improve in the third year (Tartakovsky, 2009b). As our
migrants had, on average, lived in Finland for only 22 months at
T2, it is possible that they were still at the bottom of the
suggested U-curve of adjustment. This could have had an
adverse effect on their perceptions of Finns. A fourth
Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on
April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 779 measurement point, planned
for 2013, will reveal whether stereotypes of Finns will again
become more positive as migrants increasingly adapt to their
new environment. Through what mechanism did ratings of
Finns’ benevolence become more negative? The rank-order
stabilities of the 10 basic values revealed that migrants were
more likely to change their relative positions regarding some
values than other values. Most unstable in this regard were
ratings of benevolence (lowest test-retest r = .05). Benevolence
values also changed the most in terms of absolute stability.
Migrants were thus more likely to change their ratings of Finns’
benevolence than their ratings of Finns’ other values. One
explanation for this could be that “perceivers first of all want to
know whether another person is benevolent” (Abele &
Bruckmüller, 2011, p. 935). Research on person perception
shows that people initially strive to infer another person’s
benevolence in order to make approach and avoidance decisions
(e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and that information related to
this dimension is preferentially processed on a cognitive level
(Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011). Our migrant participants may
more or less automatically have judged the benevolence of those
Finns with whom they had been in contact, and this contact may
not always have been positive, influencing the migrants’
stereotypes of Finns. Another trend that could be observed in
mean-level changes was that the stereotype of Finns moved in
the direction of increased adherence to conservation values (as
opposed to openness to change values). This trend was evident
in increased perceptions of adherence to security and tradition
and decreased perceptions of adherence to stimulation. One of
the most central themes to conservation values is that they serve
to maintain or justify the status quo. These shifts could thus
also partly reflect a process of disillusionment; migrants may by
Finns be perceived as threatening the traditions, conventions,
and norms established by the system (e.g., Huddy & Sears,
1995), and migrants could pick up such a fearful attitude. In
contrast to ratings of benevolence, migrants kept, at least to
some extent, their relative positions in their ratings of security,
tradition, and stimulation. That is, although migrants showed
mean level-shifts in their ratings of these values, these shifts
were more or less common to all migrants. The factors that
caused such mean-level changes were therefore likely to be
rather similar across migrants, perhaps including common
experience of Finnish television, laws, newspapers, officials,
political leaders, or other cultural institutions. Related to the
uniform changes in migrants’ perceptions of Finns’ adherence to
conservation values, of particular interest is that the period of
migration happened to coincide with the breakthrough of the
True Finns, a political party with national identity or
Finnishness as its pivotal concept (Arter, 2010). During the 4-
year span of the present study, the national vote share of this
party rose steadily from around 5% to 20%, as reflected for
instance in the 2007 and 2011 parliamentary elections. The
number of seats that the True Finns won rose from 5 to 39 out
of 200 (Nurmi & Nurmi, 2012). Furthermore, like other populist
radical right-wing parties, the True Finns commanded
disproportionately much media attention (see Mazzoleni,
Stewart, & Horsfield, 2003). Although still lacking xenophobic
extremism, the monoculturalist orientation of the True Finns—
the party explicitly states that multiculturalism should not be
promoted—and the espousing of a Christian society grounded in
traditional family values (Arter, 2010) could have led migrants
to increasingly view Finns as adhering to tradition and security
values. Regarding changes in other values than the above, no
clear patterns emerged. Based on previous research, one could,
for instance, have expected characteristics more central to the
stereotype to be more stable (Garcia-Marques et al., 2006). As a
proxy of centrality, one could use the premigration stereotype
mean scores; basic values with more extreme scores could be
argued to be more central to the stereotype. But the results did
not support the idea that perceptions of more central values
(e.g., security) would have been more stable. Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 780
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) Predicting
Stereotype Change at the Level of the Individual Regarding
consensus, the results of the present research can be
summarized as showing that (a) Finns’ auto- and migrants’
heterostereotypes across time showed similar levels of moderate
within-group consensus (ICCs ranging from .30 to .62); (b)
consensus neither increased nor decreased during the migration
process, suggesting a negligible role of increased contact on
consensus; and (c) stereotype change during the migration
process was not consensual. The last point is particularly
important, as it confirms that stereotypes did not change merely
as a function of migrants’ common experiences with cultural
institutions, but rather that stereotype change was determined
also by personal experiences. This is consistent with the low
rank-order stabilities of ratings of some of the values and with
the only moderate stability of individuallevel profile indices
(ICCs ranging from .30 to .48). Although stereotypes were only
moderately stable and changed in different ways across
individuals, we were unable to predict such changes: Neither
positive nor negative contact with native Finns predicted
changes in the stereotype of Finns. This was true regarding both
changes in the content of the stereotype and overall quantity of
change. Furthermore, premigration measures of prior contact
with Finns, as well as attitudes toward Finns, failed to predict
either the initial stereotype of Finns or changes in that
stereotype. Consistent with this lack of individuallevel results,
none of the results obtained in the samples of Ingrian Finn and
Russia migrants differed notably. For some aspects of the
stereotype—that is, those that showed similar changes across
individuals, such as perceptions of Finns’ adherence to
tradition—the result that we could not find individual-level
predictors was not that surprising. However, for other aspects of
the stereotype, such as benevolence values, the fact that neither
quantity nor quality of intergroup contact predicted stereotype
change can be considered surprising. Such results appear to
contradict previous research that has shown cross-group
friendships to predict more positive attitudes toward the
outgroup (Paolini et al., 2004). One reason that changes in
stereotypes were independent of reports of intergroup contact
could be that the latter may be highly biased. Such bias may be
partly conscious, as the reporting of negative intergroup contact
is socially undesirable (Krieger, Smith, Naishadham, Hartman,
& Barbeau, 2005), and this may be particularly true if the
outgroup is composed of host nationals whose acceptance
migrants may seek. Importantly, a large body of research
demonstrates that peoples’ memories for events are often
distorted and reconstructed in ways incongruent with what
actually happened (e.g., Kunda, 1999). For instance, self-
enhancement and self-protection motives (e.g., Wilson & Ross,
2003), as well as belongingness and relational needs (e.g.,
Gardner, Pickett, & Brewer, 2000; Karney & Coombs, 2000),
may influence memory reconstruction and selective memory
biases. Most pertinent to the present research, there is ample
evidence that people tend to deny or minimize experiences of
discrimination (e.g., Crosby, 1984; Taylor, Wright,
Moghaddam, & Lalonde, 1990), at least in part to protect their
self-esteem (e.g., Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997). However, we
acknowledge that a different line of research suggests that
attributing negative feedback to prejudice buffers the
consequences of such feedback on self-esteem (e.g., Crocker,
Voelkl, Testa, & Major, 1991). However, this latter line of
research may be less relevant, as our measure of experienced
discrimination explicitly measured discrimination attributable to
prejudice, including items such as ”I think native Finns do not
accept me because I come from Russia.” Both denying direct
discrimination due to ethnicity (Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997) and
attributing negative treatment with unknown causes to prejudice
(Crocker et al., 1991) may therefore serve to protect self-
esteem. Summarizing the above, because judgments of
intergroup contact and particularly discrimination are strongly
associated with how people perceive and feel about themselves
(see also, Dion & Earn, 1975; Major, Spencer, Schmader,
Wolfe, & Crocker, 1997), such judgments may be Downloaded
from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
Lönnqvist et al. 781 based on highly motivated information-
processing biases. By contrast, judgments of the typical Finn’s
personal values are likely to be less personally relevant and
therefore less suspect to motivational biases. The extent to
which migrants’ stereotypes of host nationals are associated
with reports of intergroup contact should be assessed in other
contexts. The associations between migrants’ stereotypes and
their well-being or adjustment should also be examined.
Stereotype judgments could turn out to be a useful, perhaps less
motivationally biased, compliment to more traditional measures
of intergroup contact. Conclusion Perhaps one of the most basic
questions in research on national stereotypes is whether the
content of stereotypes is affected by personal contact. In
essence, do the impressions that are formed in direct contact
with outgroup members generalize to other group members? Or
are stereotypes largely independent of direct personal
experience? Our results suggest that it depends on the
characteristic being assessed. Both in terms of absolute and
rank-order stability, ratings of benevolence were highly
malleable, suggesting that actual experience with Finns changed
migrants’ perceptions. This result thus suggests that the more or
less automatic judgments (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011) that
people make of others’ communal characteristics may influence
stereotypes. Regarding perceptions of other characteristics, our
results indicate that Finns became increasingly perceived as
more conservative. But here, the migrants kept their relative
positions, suggesting that differing personal experiences may
not have been responsible for these changes in the perception of
Finns. Rather, common experiences of Finnish cultural
institutions are likely to have caused these changes—in
particular, as argued above, we believe such shifts in the
stereotype of Finns to have been caused by the rise of the True
Finns during the time-span of the present research. The present
results thus suggest that the mechanisms of stereotype change
may be different for different characteristics. Personal contact
may influence some aspects of the stereotype, whereas other
aspects of the stereotype may be more influenced by common
cultural experiences. Regarding correspondence of migrants’
stereotypes with Finns’ self-ratings and Finns’ autostereotype,
firm conclusions were difficult to draw. Premigration
stereotypes were both accurate and similar to autostereotypes,
and this most likely contributed to their extremely high
stability. Studying a sample of migrants whose premigration
stereotypes were either more inaccurate or deviated more from
host nationals’ autostereotype could prove to be illuminating
regarding the influence of accuracy and autostereotypes on
stereotype formation. More generally, perhaps one of the most
promising research designs for future research on the
mechanisms of national stereotypes could be the longitudinal
study of migrants. Many processes are likely to be context
specific, but some may generalize across contexts. For instance,
we believe that the disillusionment process may be specific to
voluntary migration contexts. Even more specifically, the
observed changes in the perception of host nationals’ adherence
to conservation values may have been solely due to the
particular upsurge of one political party. However, some of our
results may have more general bearing, such as the result that
personal contact may influence some aspects of the stereotype—
specifically, those aspects related to communal characteristics—
whereas other aspects of the stereotype—perhaps those that are
judged less automatically in interpersonal contact—may be
more strongly affected by cultural institutions.
Acknowledgments We would like to thank Antti Hulsi (Institute
of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland),
Kaisa Kanerva (Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of
Helsinki, Finland), Tamara Kinunen Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 782
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) (St. Petersburg
State University, Russia), Michail Vinokurov (State University
of Petrozavodsk, Russia), as well as Elina Leinonen and Jesse
Haapoja (Department of Social Psychology, University of
Helsinki, Finland) for their help in data collection and coding.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared no
potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The
authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
research was supported by the Academy of Finland Research
Grants 127641 and 123297. References Abele, A. E., &
Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”?
Preferential processing of communal information. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935-948. Abele, A. E., &
Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the
perspective of self and others. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 93, 751-763. Allport, G. W. (1954/1978). The
nature of prejudice (25th anniversary ed.). New York: Basic
Books. Arter, D. (2010). The breakthrough of another West
European populist radical right party? The case of the True
Finns. Government and Opposition, 45, 484-504. Baumeister, R.
F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Berry, J. W., Phinney, J.
S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (Eds.) (2006). Immigrant youth in
cultural transition: Acculturation, identity and adaptation across
national contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Brigham, J.
C. (1971). Ethnic stereotypes. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 15-
38. Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral
confirmation processes: The selffulfilling nature of
automatically-activated stereotypes. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 33, 541-560. Church, A. T., & Katigbak, M.
S. (2002). The Five-Factor Model in the Philippines:
Investigating trait structure and levels across cultures. In R. R.
McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The Five-Factor Model of personality
across cultures (pp. 129-154). New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers. Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T.,
& Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality and agency:
Implications for the stereotypes of gender and other groups.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 25-38.
Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social
stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 218-228.
Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Process affecting scores on
“understanding others” and “assumed similarity”. Psychological
Bulletin, 52, 177-193. Crosby, F. (1984). The denial of personal
discrimination. American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 371-386.
Davidov, E. (2010). Testing for comparability of human values
across countries and time with the third round of the European
Social Survey. International Journal of Comparative Sociology,
51, 171-191. Davidov, E., Schmidt, P., & Schwartz, S. H.
(2008). Bringing values back in: The adequacy of the European
Social Survey to measure values in 20 countries. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 72, 420-445. De Cremer, D., & Van Dijk, E. (2005).
When and why leaders put themselves first: Leader behaviour in
resource allocations as a function of being entitled. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 553-563. Devine, P. G., &
Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The
Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 21, 1139-1150. Dion, K. L., & Earn, B. M. (1975). The
phenomenology of being a target of prejudice. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 944-950. Downloaded
from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
Lönnqvist et al. 783 Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1986).
Prejudice, discrimination, and racism: Historical trends and
contemporary approaches. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner
(Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 1-34).
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Edwards, A. L. (1953). The
relationship between the judged desirability of a trait and the
probability that the trait will be endorsed. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 37, 90-93. Fischer, R., & Schwartz, S. H. (2011).
Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or
artifactual sources. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42,
1127-1144. Garcia-Marques, L., Santos, A. S. C., & Mackie, D.
M. (2006). Stereotypes: Static abstractions or dynamic
knowledge structures? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 91, 814-831. Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., &
Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclusion and selective memory:
How the need to belong influences memory for social events.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 486-496.
Gilbert, G. M. (1951). Stereotype persistence and change among
college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
46, 245-254. Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005).
Nonverbal behaviour and the vertical dimension of social
relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898-
924. Huddy, L., & Sears, D. O. (1995). Opposition to bilingual
education: Prejudice or the defense of realistic interests? Social
Psychology Quarterly, 58, 133-143. Karlins, M., Coffman, T.
L., & Walters, G. (1969). On the fading of social stereotypes:
Studies in three generations of college students. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 1-16. Katz, D., & Braly,
K. W. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one-hundred college
students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280-
290. Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2009). Signs of
socioeconomic status: A thin-slicing approach. Psychological
Science, 20, 99-106. Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993).
Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety,
perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An
integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
19, 700-710. Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., Mähönen, T. A., & Ketokivi,
M. (in press). The dynamics of ethnic discrimination, identities,
and outgroup attitudes: A pre-post longitudinal study of ethnic
remigrants. European Journal of Social Psychology. Jasinskaja-
Lahti, I., Mähönen, T. A., & Liebkind, K. (2012). Identity and
attitudinal reactions to perceptions of intergroup contact among
ethnic migrants: A longitudinal study. British Journal of
Psychology, 51, 312-329. Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., & Yijälä, A.
(2011). The model of pre-acculturative stress. A pre-migration
study of potential migrants from Russia to Finland.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35, 499-510.
Jowell, R., & the Central Coordinating Team. (2007). European
Social Survey 2006/2007: Technical report. London: Centre for
Comparative Social Surveys, City University, The Data
Archive: Norwegian Social Science Data Service (NSD).
Karney, B. R., & Coombs, R. H. (2000). Memory bias in
evaluations of close relationships: Consistency or improvement?
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 959-970. Katz,
D., & Schanck, R. L. (1938). Social psychology. New York:
Wiley. Krieger, N., Smith, K., Naishadham, D., Hartman, C., &
Barbeau, E. M. (2005). Experiences of discrimination: Validity
and reliability of a self-report measure for population health
research on racism and health. Social Science and Medicine, 61,
1576-1596. Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of
people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kunda, Z., & Oleson, K. C.
(1995). Maintaining stereotypes in the face of disconfirmation:
Constructing grounds for subtyping deviants. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 565-579. Lindeman, M.,
& Verkasalo, M. (2005). Measuring values with the Short
Schwartz Values Survey. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85,
170-178. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group -
UOP on April 5, 2016 784 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
44(5) Lönnqvist, J.- E., Yijälä, A., Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., &
Verkasalo, M. (2012). Accuracy and contrast in national value
stereotypes—A case study using bi-cultural experts.
International Journal of Intergroup Relations, 36, 271-278.
Lord, C., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. E. (1979). Biased assimilation
and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on
subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37, 2098-2109. Madon, S., Guyll, M.,
Aboufadel, K., Montiel, E., Smith, A., Palumbo, P., & Jussim,
J. (2001). Ethnic and national stereotypes: The Princeton trilogy
revisited and revised. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 27, 996-1010. Mähönen, T. A. & Jasinskaja-Lahti, I.
(2012). Anticipated and perceived intergroup relations as
predictors of immigrants’ identification patterns: A follow-up
study. European Psychologist, 17, 120-130. Major, B., Spencer,
S., Schmader, T., Wolfe, C., & Crocker, J. (1997). Coping with
negative stereotypes about intellectual performance: The role of
psychological disengagement. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 24, 34-50. Mazzoleni, G., Stewart, J., &
Horsfield, B. (2003). The media and neo-populism: A
contemporary comparative analysis. Westport, CT: Praeger.
McCrae, R. R., Kurtz, J. E., Yamagata, S., & Terracciano, A.
(2011). Internal consistency, retest reliability, and their
implications for personality scale validity. Personality and
Social Psychology Review, 15, 28-50. McCrae, R. R.,
Terracciano, A., Realo, A., & Allik, J. (2007). Climatic warmth
and national wealth: Some culture-level determinants of
national character stereotypes. European Journal of Personality,
21, 953-976. Misangyi, V. F., LePine, J. A., Algina, J., &
Goeddeke, F., Jr. (2006). The adequacy of repeated-measures
regression for multilevel research. Organizational Research
Methods, 9, 5-28. Nurmi, H., & Nurmi, L. (2012). The
parliamentary election in Finland, April 2011. Electoral Studies,
31, 234-238. Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., & Voci, A.
(2004). Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on
judgements of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland:
The mediating role of an anxietyreduction mechanism.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 770-786.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of
intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 90, 751-783. Realo, A., Allik, J., Lönnqvist, J.- E.,
Verkasalo, M., Kwiatkowska, A., Kööts, L., . . ., Renge, V.
(2009). Mechanisms of the national character stereotype: How
people in six neighboring countries of Russia describe
themselves and the typical Russian. European Journal of
Personality, 23, 229-249. Robins, R. W. (2005). The nature of
personality: Genes, culture, and national character. Science,
310, 62-63. Rothbart, M., & John, O. P. (1993). Intergroup
relations and stereotype change: A social-cognitive analysis and
some longitudinal findings. In P. M. Sniderman, P. E. Tetlock,
& E. G. Carmines (Eds.), Prejudice, politics and the American
dilemma (pp. 32-59). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1997). Why minority group
members perceive or do not perceive the discrimination that
confronts them: The role of self-esteem and perceived control.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 373-389.
Schoenfeld, N. (1942). An experimental study of some problems
relating to stereotypes. Archives of Psychology, No. 270.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure
of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20
countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Value orientations: Measurement,
antecedents and consequences across nations. In R. Jowell, C.
Roberts, R. Fitzgerald & G. Eva (Eds.), Measuring attitudes
cross-nationally: Lessons from the European Social Survey (pp.
161-193). London: Sage. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at
Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 785
Schwartz, S. H. (2009). Culture matters: National value
cultures, sources and consequences. In C.- Y. Chiu, Y. Y. Hong,
S. Shavitt, & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Understanding culture: Theory,
research and application (pp. 127-150). New York: Psychology
Press. Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies
across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 268-290. Schwartz, S. H., &
Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities: Cross-
cultural and multimethod studies. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 89, 1010-1028. Schwartz, S.H., Verkasalo,
M., Antonovsky, A., & Sagiv, L. (1997). Value priorities and
social desirability: Much substance, some style. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 36, 3-18. Tartakovsky, E. (2009a).
Cultural identities of adolescent immigrants: A three year
longitudinal study including the pre-migration period. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 654-671. Tartakovsky, E.
(2009b). The psychological well-being of unaccompanied
minors: A longitudinal study of adolescents immigrating from
Russia and Ukraine to Israel without parents. Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 19, 177-204. Taylor, D. M., Wright,
S. C., Moghaddam, F. M., & Lalonde, R. N. (1990). The
personal/group discrimination discrepancy: Perceiving my
group, but not myself, to be a target of discrimination.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 254-262.
Tellegen, A. (1965). Direction of measurement: A source of
misinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 233-243.
Terracciano, A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Adam, N., Adamovova,
L., Ahn, C., & Ahn, H. N. . . ., Robert, R. (2005). National
character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49
cultures. Science, 310, 96-100. Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R.
R. (2007). Perceptions of American and the Iraq Invasion:
Implications for understanding national character stereotypes.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 695-710. Verkuyten,
M. (2007). Religious group identification and inter-religious
relations: A study among TurkishDutch Muslims. Group
Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 341-357. Voci, A., &
Hewstone, M. (2003). Intergroup contact and prejudice toward
immigrants in Italy: The mediational role of anxiety and the
moderational role of group salience. Group Processes and
Intergroup Relations, 6, 37-54. Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M.
(2003). The identity function of autobiographical memory: Time
is on our side. Memory, 11, 137-149. Yijälä, A., & Jasinskaja-
Lahti, I. (2010). Pre-migration acculturation attitudes among
potential ethnic migrants from Russia to Finland. International
Journal of Intercultural Relations, 34, 326-339. Zook, D. C.
(2009). The curious case of Finland’s clean politics. Journal of
Democracy, 20, 157-168. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at
Apollo Gro
Pacific Islands Families Study: The Association of Infant
Health Risk Indicators and Acculturation of Pacific Island
Mothers Living in New Zealand Jim Borrows1 , Maynard
Williams1 , Philip Schluter2 , Janis Paterson3 , and S. Langitoto
Helu4 Abstract The Pacific Islands Families study follows a
cohort of 1,398 Pacific infants born in Auckland, New Zealand.
This article examines associations between maternal
acculturation, measured by an abbreviated version of the
General Ethnicity Questionnaire, and selected infant and
maternal health risk indicators. Findings reveal that those with
strong alignment to Pacific culture had significantly better
infant and maternal risk factor outcomes than those with weak
cultural alignment. In terms of Berry’s classical acculturation
model, separators had the best infant and maternal outcomes;
integrators had reasonable infant and maternal outcomes, while
assimilators and marginalisors appeared to have the poorest
infant and maternal outcomes. These findings suggest that
retaining strong cultural links for Pacific immigrants is likely to
have positive health benefits. Keywords acculturation, infant
health risk, Pacific health, culture and health Introduction and
Background People of Pacific ethnicities resident in New
Zealand are overrepresented in many adverse social and health
statistics. Pacific peoples generally fare worse than the New
Zealand population as a whole in statistics relating to health,
unemployment, housing, crime, income, education, and nutrition
(Bathgate, Donnell, & Mitikulena, 1994; Cook, Didham, &
Khawaja, 1999). Despite the 1 Faculty of Health and
Environmental Sciences, AUT University, Auckland, New
Zealand 2 School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies,
AUT University, Auckland, New Zealand, and the University of
Queensland, School of Nursing and Midwifery, Australia 3
School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies, AUT
University, Auckland, New Zealand 4 School of Population
Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of
Auckland, New Zealand Corresponding Author: Jim Borrows,
C/-Professor Philip Schluter, School of Public Health and
Psychosocial Studies, AUT University, Private Bag 92006,
Auckland, New Zealand. Email: [email protected] Downloaded
from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
700 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(5) growth and
employment opportunities in New Zealand, Pacific people are
more likely to be living in poor circumstances with restricted
access to higher education, home ownership, and access to
functional amenities such as automobiles and telephones. Such
statistics have significant consequences for Pacific families
given that socioeconomic disadvantage has been consistently
linked with negative health outcomes (Chen, 2004; Power,
2002). Specifically, the raison d’etre for the Pacific Island
Families (PIF) Study, the health of Pacific families, and
especially their infants continues to be an issue of major
concern for New Zealanders. The total neonatal death rate for
Pacific infants at 4.7 per 1,000 live births is twice that of the
rate for New Zealanders of European ancestry but still less than
the 5.0 of the indigenous Maori population (New Zealand
Health Information Service, 2006). Similarly, Pacific infants
have high rates of hospitalization, particularly for respiratory
illnesses (Ministry of Health & Ministry of Pacific Island
Affairs, 2004), and present at hospital with higher severity of
illness than other New Zealand children (Grant et al., 2001).
These negative infant statistics are somewhat perplexing,
especially in a country where primary health care services are
available at low cost (free for pre-schoolers) and emergency and
hospital care services, including birthing services, are provided
free of charge. Also, New Zealand (Abel, Park, Tipene-Leach,
Finau, & Lennan, 2001) and Pacific ethnographies (Lukere &
Jolly, 2002) show that neonatal and infant care practices are not
directly contradictory to accepted Western infant care practices.
In Pacific Island settings, themselves changed by 200 years of
Western contact, the family is perceived as central in providing
traditional protocols for support and advice to ensure infant
well-being. Explanation for the current Pacific child health
circumstances is likely driven by multiple variables including
the immigration process itself. Previous research from the PIF
study demonstrated that acculturative orientation had a
persistent association with aspects of health status and
behaviour for cohort participants (e.g., Abbott & Williams,
2006; Low et al., 2005; Paterson, Feehan, Butler, Williams, &
Cowley-Malcolm, 2007), hence the emphasis in this article on
testing the association between maternal acculturation and
infant and maternal health risk factors. Culture, Health, and
Acculturation The interrelationship between culture and health,
including associated psychological processes, has been a
recurrent theme in the social science literature over much of the
last century (Helman, 2000; Sam, 2006a; Stroebe & Stroebe,
1995; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2001).
There is now acceptance in the medical and health professional
domains that culture should be acknowledged as an important
determinant of health status (Corin, 1994; Snowden, 2005;
Spector, 2002; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,
2001) and that concepts derived from anthropologic and cross-
cultural research may provide an alternative framework for
identifying health issues that require resolution (Kleinman,
Eisenberg, & Good, 1978; Savage, 2000). In particular, there is
some agreement that many people from minority cultures may
not have faith in, or necessarily benefit from, the medical
interventions that are being offered by the host society
(MacLachlan, 1997). Also recognized is the importance of the
interrelationship between migration and health, including
seminal New Zealand/Pacific migration studies (Stanhope &
Prior, 1976), early international studies (Carballo, Divino, &
Zeric, 1998; Ostbye, Welby, Prior, Salmond, & Stokes, 1989),
and more recent studies aimed at explaining the link between
migration and health (Sam, 2006a). That is, the realization that
the well-being of a migrant group is determined by interlinking
factors that relate to the society of origin, the migration itself,
and the society of resettlement. All three sets of factors need to
be considered if one seeks to reduce or merely to understand the
level of health disorder in any immigrant group. Despite the
recognition of the importance of Downloaded from
jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016
Borrows et al. 701 culture and migration in determining health
status and the explanatory acculturation/health hypotheses that
this has generated (Carballo et al., 1998; Sam, 2006a), there
have been few empirical attempts to link health with both
migration and culture in relation to other demographic, social,
and psychological factors operating in given communities in
New Zealand or international studies (Snowden, 2005).
However, it is now clear that migration at an individual level is
a significant life event for individuals impacting on subsequent
health behaviour and outcomes. Closely related to culture and
migration is the concept of acculturation—that is, “culture
change that is initiated by the conjunction of two or more
autonomous culture systems” (Social Science Research Council,
1954, as cited in Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 2002, p.
350). The social psychology literature is replete with alternative
models of the acculturative process, most of which are
multidimensional, involving numerous topics and factors
(Stanley, 2003). These multidimensional topics range from
those at the personal level, such as personality qualities and
psychological adjustment (Ward & Leon, 2004), language
retention and community socialization, and external
acculturation drivers such as migration experience, micro- and
macro-societal policies, and regional setting (Persky & Birman,
2005). Outside of these models, but still incorporating
multidimensionality, are the two most common models of
acculturation theory: unidirectional and bidirectional models of
acculturation. Berry restated Redfield and colleagues’
hypothesis that acculturative adaptations lead to culture changes
in either or both of the migrating and host society groups. He
went on further to note that it is not inevitable that intergroup
contact proceeds uniformly through sequential to ultimate
assimilation as there are many other ways of going about it or
indeed is potentially bidirectional and reciprocal (Berry, 2006).
Such insights generated by this bidirectional model challenges
the ethnic melting-pot assumptions and promotes exploration
and resolution of political sensitivities among ethnicities
(Flannery, Reise, & Jiajuan, 2001). These observations by
Berry, Sam, and others, which hint at multiple individual and
group acculturation strategies, have been complemented more
recently by Boski, who calls for the development of a
theoretical model of integration, a key concept in the
psychology of acculturation, in which five meanings for this
concept identified in the existing literature are positioned as in-
depth directed layers of the bicultural psyche (Boski, 2008).
That is, the subtleties in the acculturation process at the group
and individual level deserve further and more detailed
examination. There are many studies that have examined
acculturation strategies in nondominant groups. In most studies,
preference for integration is expressed over other acculturation
strategies, although notable exceptions with Turks both in
Germany and in Canada, and in Hispanic immigrant women in
the United States, have been cited (Ataca & Berry, 2002; Berry,
2006; Jones, Bond, Gardner, & Hernandez, 2002). All these
recent contributions that counter the assimilation and melting-
pot models could be seen as underpinning Pacific community
perspectives on cultural maintenance within New Zealand
society. In New Zealand, there is widespread official
government dogma and minority community perception that
cultural maintenance is important to health outcomes and that
culturally specific information for minority groups on which to
base optimal policy and services is necessary. The untested
assumption is that such an approach will lead to improved
health and social outcomes for Pacific peoples. An alternative
“popular hypothesis” in New Zealand would more likely support
international perspectives and studies cited above that would
expect more positive health outcomes for those effectively
embedded in mainstream culture than for those embedded in
Pacific culture or those marginalized from both cultures. This
dominant cultural and official “cultural maintenance” viewpoint
is politically persuasive in New Zealand and as a result became
the focus of refutation or support in terms of our working
hypothesis outlined as the second aim for this study presented
below. Based on all these considerations, we applied Berry’s
acculturation model to the relationships between acculturation
and health, in this case operationalised as poor outcomes for
maternal and Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo
Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 702 Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology 42(5) infant health risk factors. Thus, in the context
of understanding the process and outcomes of acculturation
strategies adopted by Pacific families, this study had two
principal aims: namely, to (a) investigate the association
between mother and infant health variables that might act as
infant risk indicators and adaptation to living in New Zealand
and (b) test the New Zealand view that strong cultural
alignment to the original Pacific culture is associated with
significantly better outcomes in terms of maternal and infant
health risk factors and that weak cultural alignment is
associated with significantly poorer outcomes in terms of
maternal and infant health risk factors. For reasons outlined in
the Method section, an abbreviated version of the General
Ethnicity Questionnaire (GEQ; Tsai, Ying, & Lee, 2000)
acculturation measurement instrument was employed. As a
result, a secondary aim was to establish the validity and
reliability of the modified instrument. Migration and Pacific
People in Contemporary New Zealand Society To give a context
to this study, it is necessary to describe the place played in New
Zealand’s migration history by people of the Pacific Islands (as
distinct from indigenous Maori descent) and their place in
contemporary society. Polynesian settlement of the Pacific was
completed around 1200-1300 AD when Te Ika o Maui (the
mythical fish of Maui), the North Island of New Zealand, was
the last Pacific archipelago to be discovered and settled by the
ancient Polynesians (Prickett, 2001). These Polynesian
ancestors became the New Zealand indigenous Maori. Major
European settlement, and subsequent colonization, commenced
from the late 18th century. Polynesian post-Maori contacts in
the 18th and 19th centuries were limited, and at the 1945 New
Zealand Census of Population and Dwellings, only about 2,000
people were recorded as being of Pacific origin. A second great
wave of Polynesian migration took place in the relatively short
period between the 1950s and 1980s, when Pacific peoples
arrived from the islands of Samoa, Tonga, Cook Islands, Niue,
Fiji, and the Tokelaus. This modern Polynesian migration was
based principally on opportunity provided by largely economic
imperatives in New Zealand (Macpherson, Spoonley, & Anae,
2001) or economic sustainability of small island groups such as
the Tokelaus (Prior, Welby, Ostbye, Salmond, & Stokes, 1987;
Salmond, Joseph, Prior, Stanley, & Wessen, 1985),
supplemented more recently by matters relating to renewing or
continuing links of kinship and family. Currently, Pacific
peoples are a very significant and growing proportion of New
Zealand’s population. More than 6% (231,801 people) in New
Zealand were of Pacific ethnicity at the time of the 2001 Census
(Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2002a), and Pacific
people are projected to make up more than 8% of the population
by 2021 (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2005). The
biggest concentration of Pacific people is in Auckland, New
Zealand’s largest metropolitan area. Sixty percent of people of
Pacific ethnicity were born in New Zealand; of those born
overseas, 40% had arrived in New Zealand by 1981 and 30%
between 1981 and 1990 (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau,
2002a). This latest migration of Pacific people influences the
nature of both New Zealand and the home island societies. For
example, in the islands, it is significant in terms of reducing the
overall population and in providing economic support to home
communities by way of individual and family remittances to
relatives. Table 1 illustrates the large proportion of Pacific
people residing in New Zealand in relation to their respective
home island populations. Since the migration wave of the late
20th century, Pacific people have actively participated in the
New Zealand economy and society. In economic terms, Pacific
people have relatively high labour force participation rates,
particularly in the manufacturing sector. This sector has
declined since the mid-1980s as a proportion of total
employment but has been offset with Pacific people employment
participation in the growing consumer service industries (such
as hotels, restaurants, and retail) and the employment of
younger people in more skilled technical and professional
Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on
April 5, 2016 Borrows et al. 703 occupations (Statistics New
Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2002b). However, people of Pacific
ethnicities remain underrepresented in managerial and
professional occupations yet overrepresented in trades and
elementary occupations. Overall current labour force
participation rates for people of Pacific ethnicities are at 62.9%,
lower than the national rate of 68.5%, and unemployment rates
are at 6.9%, higher than the national rate of 3.7% (Department
of Labour—Te Tari Mahi, 2007). Maori rates for 2007 in labour
force participation and unemployment are 67.6% and 7.6%,
respectively. In terms of demography, Pacific people living in
New Zealand have a relatively young age structure and a high
fertility rate. While people of Pacific ethnicities currently have
a lower life expectancy than the total population, it is higher
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx
Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx

Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docx
Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docxCommunication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docx
Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docxclarebernice
 
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in Adolescence
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in AdolescenceMehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in Adolescence
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in AdolescenceClare Mehta
 
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School Context
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School ContextAttitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School Context
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School ContextCynthia Velynne
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. MaruyBenitoSumpter862
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. MaruySantosConleyha
 
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special IssueAdvances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special IssueKatie Naple
 
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docx
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docxReceived 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docx
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docxsodhi3
 
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...David Brooks
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revisionKat Wortham
 
The Division of Race in Neighborhood Structure
The Division of Race in Neighborhood StructureThe Division of Race in Neighborhood Structure
The Division of Race in Neighborhood StructurePaul Blazevich
 
Fractionalization alesinassrn fractionalization
Fractionalization   alesinassrn fractionalizationFractionalization   alesinassrn fractionalization
Fractionalization alesinassrn fractionalizationEspi Sul
 
literature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikliterature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikBesire Paralik
 
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For Practice
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For PracticeAnalysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For Practice
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For PracticeMartha Brown
 
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments A Review And Research Agenda
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments  A Review And Research AgendaAcculturation And Overseas Assignments  A Review And Research Agenda
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments A Review And Research AgendaJim Webb
 
Hofstede and globe the ecological fallacy
Hofstede and globe  the ecological fallacyHofstede and globe  the ecological fallacy
Hofstede and globe the ecological fallacyBrendan McSweeney
 
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docx
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docxResearch on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docx
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docxverad6
 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docx
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docxJournal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docx
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docxtawnyataylor528
 
Ethnic group conflict
Ethnic group conflictEthnic group conflict
Ethnic group conflictTrudy Urbain
 

Similar to Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx (20)

Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docx
Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docxCommunication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docx
Communication and StereotypicalImpressionsPatrick C. Hug.docx
 
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in Adolescence
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in AdolescenceMehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in Adolescence
Mehta & Strough_2010_ Gender Segregation and Gender-typing in Adolescence
 
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School Context
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School ContextAttitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School Context
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality In A School Context
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
 
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special IssueAdvances In Research On Homelessness  An Overview Of The Special Issue
Advances In Research On Homelessness An Overview Of The Special Issue
 
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docx
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docxReceived 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docx
Received 032911Revised 011712Accepted 030812At.docx
 
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...
The significance of language to multiracial individuals and identity part ii ...
 
301_A3-2
301_A3-2301_A3-2
301_A3-2
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revision
 
The Division of Race in Neighborhood Structure
The Division of Race in Neighborhood StructureThe Division of Race in Neighborhood Structure
The Division of Race in Neighborhood Structure
 
Fractionalization alesinassrn fractionalization
Fractionalization   alesinassrn fractionalizationFractionalization   alesinassrn fractionalization
Fractionalization alesinassrn fractionalization
 
literature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralikliterature review - besire paralik
literature review - besire paralik
 
Rotterdam 2013
Rotterdam 2013Rotterdam 2013
Rotterdam 2013
 
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For Practice
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For PracticeAnalysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For Practice
Analysis Of LGBT Identity Development Models And Implications For Practice
 
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments A Review And Research Agenda
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments  A Review And Research AgendaAcculturation And Overseas Assignments  A Review And Research Agenda
Acculturation And Overseas Assignments A Review And Research Agenda
 
Hofstede and globe the ecological fallacy
Hofstede and globe  the ecological fallacyHofstede and globe  the ecological fallacy
Hofstede and globe the ecological fallacy
 
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docx
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docxResearch on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docx
Research on Aging33(4) 361 –378© The Author(s) 2011 Re.docx
 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docx
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docxJournal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docx
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1997, Vol. 73, N.docx
 
Ethnic group conflict
Ethnic group conflictEthnic group conflict
Ethnic group conflict
 

More from whittemorelucilla

Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxDatabase reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxDataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxDataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxDataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxDataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxDataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxDataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxDataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxDatabase Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docxDatabases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxDatabase SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxDATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxDatabase Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxDatabase Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxDatabase Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxDatabase Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxDatabase Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docxDatabase Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docxwhittemorelucilla
 
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxDataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxwhittemorelucilla
 

More from whittemorelucilla (20)

Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docxDatabase reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
Database reports provide us with the ability to further analyze ou.docx
 
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docxDataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
DataInformationKnowledge1.  Discuss the relationship between.docx
 
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docxDataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
DataHole 12 Score6757555455555455575775655565656555655656556566643.docx
 
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docxDataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
DataDestination PalletsTotal CasesCases redCases whiteCases organi.docx
 
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docxDataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
DataIllinois Tool WorksConsolidated Statement of Income($ in milli.docx
 
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docxDataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
DataIDSalaryCompa-ratioMidpoint AgePerformance RatingServiceGender.docx
 
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docxDataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
DataCity1997 Median Price1997 Change1998 Forecast1993-98 Annualize.docx
 
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docxDataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
DataClientRoom QualityFood QualityService Quality1GPG2GGG3GGG4GPG5.docx
 
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docxDatabase Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
Database Project CharterBusiness CaseKhalia HartUnive.docx
 
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docxDatabases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223  .docx
Databases selected Multiple databases...Full Text (1223 .docx
 
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docxDatabase SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
Database SystemsDesign, Implementation, and ManagementCo.docx
 
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docxDATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
DATABASE SYSTEMS DEVELOPMENT & IMPLEMENTATION PLAN1DATABASE SYS.docx
 
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docxDatabase Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
Database Security Assessment Transcript You are a contracting office.docx
 
Data.docx
Data.docxData.docx
Data.docx
 
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docxDatabase Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
Database Design Mid Term ExamSpring 2020Name ________________.docx
 
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docxDatabase Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
Database Justification MemoCreate a 1-page memo for the .docx
 
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docxDatabase Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
Database Concept Maphttpwikieducator.orgCCNCCCN.docx
 
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docxDatabase Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
Database Dump Script(Details of project in file)Mac1) O.docx
 
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docxDatabase Design 1.  What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
Database Design 1. What is a data model A. method of sto.docx
 
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docxDataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
DataAGEGENDERETHNICMAJORSEMHOUSEGPAHRSNEWSPAPTVHRSSLEEPWEIGHTHEIGH.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...PsychoTech Services
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajanpragatimahajan3
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 

Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes.docx

  • 1. Group-Level and Intraindividual Stability of National Stereotypes: A FourYear Longitudinal Study on Ingrian Finn Immigrants’ Pre-and Postmigration Stereotypes of a Typical Finn Jan-Erik Lönnqvist1 , Inga Jasinskaja-Lahti2 , and Markku Verkasalo1 Abstract In a 4-year longitudinal study, we investigated stereotype change in the context of increased intergroup contact. Specifically, using one pre- and two postmigration measurement points, we followed some 200 Ingrian Finns and their families migrating from Russia to Finland. Stereotypes of a typical Finn were conceptualized within the framework provided by Schwartz’s values theory. At the group level, migrants’ stereotype profiles were consensual, similar to Finns’s autostereotypes, somewhat accurate, and highly stable. However, mean-level changes indicated a process of disillusionment: Finns were increasingly perceived as less benevolent and more hedonistic. We argue that personal contact changes aspects of stereotypes related to communal characteristics, whereas contact with cultural institutions influences perceptions of conservativeness. Probably due to political climate, Finns were increasingly perceived as adhering to tradition and security values over stimulation. Although individual-level stereotypes were only moderately stable and stereotype change was heterogeneous, we could not predict individual-level changes. Keywords national stereotypes, intergroup contact, migration, values From Herodotus’s 5th century B.C.E. depiction of Egyptians as the wisest people of all mankind to Hollywood’s contemporary portrayal of the bad Arab, national stereotypes—beliefs about the characteristics of people from different countries—have fascinated scientists and laypeople alike. It could therefore be considered surprising how little is actually known about the processes Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 766 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) through which national stereotypes are formed. Yet such knowledge could in
  • 2. today’s shrinking world be of considerable importance. The present study asks, in a migration setting, how increased contact affects national stereotypes. This could shed some light on the mechanisms through which national stereotypes are formed. The present 4-year longitudinal study investigated how Ingrian Finn and Russian migrants’ premigration stereotypes of native Finnish host nationals changed postmigration. Stereotype change was investigated both at the level of the group and at the level of the individual. We were interested both in the content of the stereotype and the consensus with which it was held. Stability and Change in Group-Level National Stereotypes On the level of the group (that is, aggregated across individuals), the content of national stereotypes has been reported to be highly stable. Most important in this respect is a series of studies concerning the ethnic and national stereotypes of Princeton University students (Gilbert, 1951; Karlins, Coffman, & Walters, 1969; Katz & Braly, 1933; Madon et al., 2001), the results of which can perhaps best be summarized as suggesting that the content of stereotypes does change, but that this happens very slowly, taking decades. Regarding shorter periods of time, ranging from a couple of weeks to 5 years, the content of group-level stereotypes has been shown to be highly stable (e.g., Garcia-Marques, Santos, & Mackie, 2006; Realo et al., 2009; Rothbart & John, 1993). Also testifying to the stability of national stereotypes, they are strongly affected by relatively stable factors such as climatic warmth and national prosperity (McCrae, Terraciano, McCrae, & Allik, 2007), whereas more short-term factors, such as government policies, appear to have only little impact (Terraciano & McCrae, 2007). National stereotypes have not previously been investigated in a migration context. Examining the stability of national stereotypes in such a context of heightened intergroup contact could help illuminate some of the processes through which national stereotypes are formed. The migration context should be especially well suited for investigating the kernel of truth hypothesis (e.g., Allport, 1954/1978; Brigham, 1971), which suggests that national
  • 3. stereotypes are to some extent accurate, reflecting real differences between people. If this is correct, then increased contact should lead to increased accuracy. Despite the plausibility of the kernel of truth hypothesis, it has been challenged by some recent empirical results. Terracciano et al. (2005) had respondents from 49 countries provide self-ratings of personality and rate the personality of a typical member of their cultural group. Comparison of these two sets of ratings revealed that stereotypes did not correspond with average self- ratings. One reason for this could be that people generally have little experience with other cultures and may therefore lack a reference point for rating their own culture (Robins, 2005). A related possibility is that autostereotypes (stereotypes of one’s own nation) are erroneous, but heterostereotypes (stereotypes of other nations) are more truthful (Robins, 2005). Taking a different approach to studying the accuracy of national stereotypes, Church and Katigbak (2002) asked Americans and Filipinos who had lived in both America and the Philippines for at least 3 years to judge the personality traits of Filipinos and Americans. These ratings were not correlated with average self- ratings of personality obtained from American and Filipino samples. Challenging the above results according to which national stereotypes are not accurate, a seven-country study by Realo et al. (2009) showed that when the same measurement instrument was used to assess both personality and national character stereotype, relatively good agreement between self- ratings and autostereotypes was found. Such agreement was also reported for Ingrian Finn and Russian potential migrants’ premigration stereotypes regarding the personal values of a typical Finnish host national (Lönnqvist, Yijälä, Jasinskaja- Lahti, & Verkasalo, 2012). The present research design, in which we longitudinally monitor these same migrants throughout the migration process, should be ideal for investigating the kernel of truth hypothesis. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 767 Increased firsthand experience with a
  • 4. country and its residents could also have other effects than increased accuracy. Heterostereotypes of Americans (Terraciano & McCrae, 2007) and Russians (Realo et al., 2009) have been shown to converge with autostereotypes. Direct contact could lead to increased knowledge of the autostereotype, and the heterostereotype could thus be expected to become more similar to the autostereotype. There may also be migration-specific effects on national stereotypes. Based on previous results, one could expect stereotypes to become either more or less positive. Migration will necessarily involve increased intergroup contact, and if satisfactory, this contact could lead to increased favorability towards the outgroup (for a meta-analysis, see Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). However, the opposite effect could be expected if the postmigration contact is negative or discriminatory. Typically, the contact hypothesis literature conceives of intergroup contact as general contact evaluated, e.g., for its pleasantness or superficiality. But intergroup contact may for migrants also involve experiences of outgroup rejection and ethnic discrimination. For example, our recent studies conducted with the same sample of participants have shown that immigrants who, after migration, perceived more negative intergroup contact than they had expected, were more likely to disidentify from the national majority group and to increasingly show negative attitudes toward this group (Jasinskaja-Lahti, Mähönen, & Liebkind, 2012; Jasinskaja- Lahti, Mähönen, & Ketokivi, in press; Mähönen, & Jasinskaja- Lahti, 2012). Similarly, a study on Jewish adolescents from Russia and Ukraine participating in an Israeli immigration program showed that premigration idealization of the receiving country was typically replaced by postmigration disillusionment (Tartakovsky, 2009a). The stereotypes of host nationals could thus become more negative postmigration. Besides content, another important aspect of national stereotypes is the extent to which they are consensual (i.e., the extent to which people agree on which characteristics describe a group). In general, the more consensual stereotypes are, the more often those
  • 5. stereotypes will be confronted, and the more they will thereby shape the behavior of the stereotyped group (e.g., Chen & Bargh, 1997). The above mentioned Princeton trilogy and its follow-ups have provided mixed evidence regarding the long- term stability of stereotype consensus, with some studies suggesting decreased (Devine & Elliot, 1995 ; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1986) and others increased (Madon et al., 2001) consensus. Similarly, regarding the effect of increased intergroup contact on stereotype consensus, the empirical evidence does not allow for any straightforward conclusions. Some studies have shown consensus to decrease with increased familiarity with the stereotyped group (e.g., Katz & Schank, 1938), but others have uncovered the opposite pattern (e.g., Schoenfeld, 1942). Stability and Change at the Level of the Individual There are extremely few studies on the intraindividual stability of stereotypes. One unpublished study, described by Rothbart and John (1993), showed that although the group-level stability of ethnic stereotypes was, across a 4- year interval, extremely high, the individual-level stability of stereotypes was only moderate. Another study showed a similar pattern of results, with extremely high group-level stability, but only moderate intraindividual stability over a time period of a couple of weeks (Garcia-Marques et al., 2006). Examining stereotype change at the intraindividual level could in several ways contribute to our understanding regarding the processes that influence stereotypes. First, intraindividual analysis allows for the examination of how similar the process of stereotype change is across individuals. Due to lack of empirical research, very little is known about the extent to which stereotype change is consensual. However, this type of knowledge could be vital, as it directly speaks to the extent to which stereotype change is influenced by common experiences: The more Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 768 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) consensual stereotype change is, the more likely it is to be influenced by common experiences of societal and institutionalized forces,
  • 6. such as television programs, newspapers, books, films, and political leaders. In the present context, the question is whether the migration process has a uniform impact on stereotypes or whether some migrants’ stereotypes change in one way and others’ in some other way. Related to the above, if the process of stereotype change is not uniform across individuals, it could be meaningful to examine the associations of stereotype change with other individual-level variables. Because migrants’ experiences of intergroup contact are likely to vary between individuals, the intergroup contact hypothesis described above could also find support at this level of analysis: Increased quantity and especially good quality of interaction with native Finns could render the stereotype of Finns more positive. By contrast, experiences of discrimination could have the opposite effect. Also, the disillusionment process, described above at the level of the group, could be expected to be found at the level of the individual—those with more positive premigration feelings toward Finns could experience more profound postmigration changes in their stereotype of Finns. On the other hand, such migrants may have experienced more premigration contact with Finns, rendering their stereotypes less susceptible to change. Purpose of the Present Research The present study is part of larger longitudinal research project (Intervening at the Premigration Stage: Providing Tools for Promoting Integration and Adaptation Throughout the Migration Process [INPRES]) that followed a sample of Ingrian Finns and their families in the context of migration from Russia to Finland. National stereotypes were measured within the framework provided by Schwartz’s (1992) theory on personal values. Values are concepts or beliefs that act as standards of what is most desirable in the appraisal of events, actions, and people. Values diverge from attitudes in that they transcend particular situations, are organized within an individual according to importance, establish criterions of desirability, and are fewer and more crucial to personality than are attitudes. Due to the scarcity of previous longitudinal research on stereotype change,
  • 7. the complete lack of such research in a context of increased intergroup contact and the often inconsistent findings reviewed above, exact predictions were difficult to make. However, we did expect stereotypes to be relatively stable at the level of the group but individuals to show much less stability (GarciaMarques et al., 2006; Rothbart & John, 1993). Regarding the content of stereotypes, during the migration process, Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes of Finns could, as a result of increased contact, be expected to become more accurate (supporting the kernel of truth hypothesis; Realo et al., 2009; cf., Church & Katigbak, 2002; Terraciano et al., 2005) but also to become more similar to the autostereotypes of Finns (Realo et al., 2009; Terraciano et al., 2005). Regarding the latter, these were, in the present study, measured in a sample of Finnish students—previous research suggests students’ national stereotypes to be generalizable to the whole population (Terraciano et al., 2005). Based on the contact hypothesis (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), the content of migrants’ stereotype of Finns could be expected to become more positive. However, the opposite could also be true, as migrants may during the postmigration phase become disillusioned with their host country (Tartakovsky, 2009a). Another set of questions relates to consensus. How consensual are stereotypes, and how consensual is stereotype change over time? Regarding the former question, previous findings are very mixed (e.g., Katz & Schanck, 1938; Schoenfeld, 1942), and there is no research on the latter question. This forced a rather exploratory approach to these questions. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 769 It seems plausible to expect that individuals’ stereotypes may change in different ways and that such individual differences could be predictable. Those with more premigration experience of Finland could already have a rather stable stereotype, less susceptible to change. Furthermore, their premigration stereotype could be more positive (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). In our previous studies, using the premigration data of the INPRES
  • 8. project, the more positive the premigration contact of potential migrants with Finnish host nationals, the more positive were their attitudes toward Finns and the less they anticipated discrimination and adaptation problems after migration (Jasinskaja-Lahti & Yijälä, 2011; Yijälä & Jasinskaja-Lahti, 2010). However, as stated earlier, these positive experiences and expectations could also render them more vulnerable to postmigration disillusionment (Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2012; Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., in press Mähönen & Jasinskaja-Lahti, 2012; Tartakovsky, 2009a, 2009b). Importantly, not all intergroup contact may influence stereotypes similarly: Satisfactory contact was expected to be associated with increased favorability toward the national majority group (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), but the opposite effect was expected when postmigration contact had been experienced as discriminatory. Regarding methodology, we decided to analyze the Ingrian Finn migrants (so-called primary applicants of Finnish descent) and their family members (spouses of mostly Russian ethnicity), as separate samples, as the two groups could have been expected to differ in some respects (e.g., the Ingrian Finns could have experienced more contact with Finns prior to migration). This methodological decision also served to keep all observations independent. Method Participants and Procedure Finnish students. Autostereotypes of Finns were measured in a sample of 38 Finnish students (8 males; mean age = 34.8, SD = 11.1; range = 21 to 58) taking part in an introductory course in psychology arranged by the University of Helsinki. Participants rated online the values of a typical Finn. The course was an Open University course (anyone can attend these courses), and therefore, the mean age was somewhat higher than in more typical student samples, arguably contributing to the representativeness of the sample. The low number of males in our sample may raise some concerns; however, mean male and female stereotype profiles were virtually identical (r = .96; independent samples t tests for equality in means also revealed no differences between males and females, for all 10 values, t <
  • 9. 1.8, p > .05). The ratings were collected in the summer of 2011, at around the same time that most of the migrants completed the second follow-up. Emigrating Ingrian Finns and Russians. Ingrian Finns and Russians were participants of the INPRES project investigating the migration of Ingrian Finns and their family members to Finland between 2008 and 2011. The Ingrian Finns are mostly offspring of Finnish colonizers who settled down in Ingria (now Leningrad Oblast) in the 17th century. After the Russian conquest in 1703, many remigrated back to Finland. Of those who remained, many went missing in the forced population transfers before and after World War II, and many migrated to Finland soon after the breakdown of the Soviet Union in the first years of the 1990s. However, there are still about 30,000 Ingrian Finns remaining in Russia. After completing an official language test, Ingrian Finns are qualified for guaranteed habitation permit in the Finnish Law of Return. The baseline sample of the project consisted altogether of 325 potential migrants living in the Republic of Karelia or the Leningrad area. We measured participants’ stereotype of Finns three times, once before migration (T0) and twice postmigration (T1 and T2). Premigration experience of Finland and attitude toward Finns were measured at T0 and experienced intergroup contact at T2. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 770 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) The T0 data were collected at the Finnish language courses that were part of the immigration training program organized by the Finnish authorities in Russia during April and May 2008. Everyone attending these courses (N = 192) completed the questionnaires in class. The language students were instructed to take home an additional questionnaire to be completed by family members (spouses) joining the respondent in Finland (N = 84). In addition, the questionnaire was mailed to those potential migrants who had already passed the mandatory language test and were waiting to be assigned a particular place of residence in Finland (N = 49). These participants were identified via the register of the Finnish
  • 10. Consulate in Russia (St. Petersburg), and they participated via post survey. The mean age of the participants at T0 was 43.1 years (SD = 14.4 years), ranging between 19 and 85 years. Most participants were married or cohabiting (70%) and had children (77%). Furthermore, most of them had full-time employment (61%), whereas only 3% were unemployed or temporarily dismissed at T0. Every second (46%) of these 325 participants thought that they would be able to migrate within the following 7 or 8 months. As the participants of the baseline study migrated to Finland according to their personal schedules, the follow-up data could not be collected at once. Instead, for the first postmigration assessment (T1), three searches from the Finnish population register were performed between October 2009 and December 2010 in order to include as many baseline participants as possible. Each round consisted of an initial request to participate, including the postmigration questionnaires and a return envelope, followed by two reminders. The participants were identified by their name and date of birth. At the end of the third search, a total of 233 respondents (70%) of our baseline sample had migrated to Finland by December 2010, of whom 227 respondents took part in the study at T1.The mean age in the T1 follow-up sample was 44.3 years (SD = 13.9). Most participants were females (63%), and they were still married or cohabiting (69%). Despite their high level of education prior to migration (only 15% had no education beyond secondary school), they had not yet been employed in Finland but were typically unemployed (49%), on pension (10%), or studying (12%) at T1. The second follow-up (T2) was conducted 12 to 20 months after T1 assessment, using the same procedure as described above. In total, 194 (82% of all T0 participants that had migrated to Finland by December 2011 and 86% of T1 participants) migrants participated at T2. The mean age in the second follow-up sample was 45.4 years (SD = 13.4). Most participants were females (63%), were typically still married or cohabiting (62%), and were typically still unemployed (49%), on pension (8%), or studying (19%). In
  • 11. order to examine possible selection bias due to sample attrition, t tests on relevant demographic factors (gender, age, marital status, socioeconomic status, employment status, level of education, Finnish language proficiency) and other T0 variables used in this study were performed. Our final sample did not differ in terms of sociodemographic factors or in terms of their responses to T0 scales from those respondents participating only at T0. For the analyses presented here, we included only participants who took part at all three measurement occasions— that is, 136 ethnic migrants (Ingrian Finns; 37 males, mean age at T0 = 44.4, SD = 14.1) and their 56 spouses (46 Russians and 10 participants of other ethnic origins from within the former Soviet region; 34 males, mean age = 40.6, SD = 11.1). We refer to these two groups of migrants as Ingrian Finns and Russians, respectively. The migrants had arrived in Finland between 3 and 15 months (Ingrian Finns: M = 9.6 months, SD = 3.9; Russians: M = 10.5 months, SD = 3.8) before the T1 assessment and between 13 and 28 months before the T2 assessment (Ingrian Finns: M = 22.0 months, SD = 4.1; Russians: M = 23.0 months, SD = 3.9). In both samples, the average time that had elapsed between T0 and T1 was 19 months, and the average time between T1 and T2 was 12 months. Representative sample of Finns. As a criterion for the accuracy of stereotypes of Finns, we used self-rating of values. These were gathered from strict probability samples, representative of the Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 771 population 15 years and older, in the third round of the European Social Survey (ESS; Jowell & the Central Coordinating Team, 2007). Finland had 1,646 (782 males; mean age 48.2, SD = 18.6) participants. The data were collected in the fall of 2006. Measures Portraits Values Questionnaire. At the individual level, people in most cultures (recently 77; Schwartz, 2009) distinguish between at least 10 basic values. Schwartz (1992) has labeled these universal value types as benevolence, tradition, conformity, security, power, achievement, hedonism, stimulation, self-direction, and universalism. Our measure of
  • 12. the 10 basic values was the 21-item Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ21). It is based on descriptions of different people, whose goals, aspirations, and wishes are characterized in two sentences (Davidov, Schmith, & Schwartz, 2008). For example, the item “It is important to her to show her abilities. She wants people to admire what she does” measures achievement values. In the ESS, respondents indicate “how much like you this person is” by using a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (very much like me) to 6 (not like me at all). For the present study, all items were reverse scored, so that the higher the number, the stronger the agreement with the item. When two values or less were missing, the missing values were replaced using the regression method. By mistake, the PVQ21 was, in the migrant samples, administered using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not like me at all) to 5 (like me); the response option 6 (very much like me) was left outside the printable area of the questionnaire. To facilitate comparison between samples, scores of 5 or 6 were recoded into 5 in the Finnish student sample and in the ESS data. The average internal consistency reliability of our Finnish students’ ratings of the typical Finn was, across the 10 basic values, .57 (range = .23 [self-direction] to .84 [hedonism]). In the Ingrian Finn and Russian migrant samples, the corresponding ratings showed similar and acceptable levels of internal consistency reliability (average T0/T1/T2 αs = .56 [range = .29 (Stimulation) to .77 (Security)] / .53 [range = .16 (Stimulation) to .71 (Achievement)] / .60 [range = .28 (Stimulation) to .73 (Achievement)] and .62 [range = .40 (Achievement) to .81 (Security)] / .55 [range = .40 (Tradition) to .73 (Achievement)] / .61 [range = .44 (Power) to .79 (Conformity)], respectively). Although some of the reliabilities were low, this cannot be considered very surprising, as each of the basic values, despite representing broad constructs, is measured by only two items (three for universalism). The reliabilities are in line with the reliabilities reported in earlier studies (e.g., Schwartz, 2007; Schwartz & Rubel, 2005), suggesting no particular problems in the current samples or with
  • 13. the instructions to rate stereotypes. In fact, low alpha reliabilities may not threaten measurement validity; McCrae, Kurtz, Yamagata, and Terracciano (2011) demonstrated that internal consistency reliability is not related to various validity criteria. The PVQ21, like all other measures, was administered in the Russian language to our samples of Ingrian Finns and Russians. This may raise concerns regarding the comparability of scores on the Russian and Finnish translations of the PVQ21 (the Finnish student sample and the representative sample of Finns responded in Finnish). Indeed, the Finnish and Russian translations of the PVQ21 display configural and partial metric invariance but not scalar invariance (Davidov, 2010). This allows researchers to compare the correlates of these two questionnaires across countries but not to compare mean scores. We used the scores obtained with the Finnish translation only for purposes of computing profile correlations, not to compare mean scores. Supporting the comparability of values profiles collected with the Russian and Finnish translations of the PVQ, the average self-rating values profiles of the Ingrian Finns (not shown) and the representative sample of Finns were almost identical (r = .94; for evidence that value profiles should not differ much across cultures, see Fischer & Schwartz, 2011). Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 772 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) Because some of our predictions concerned changes in the valence of the stereotype, it is important to note that some values may be more desirable than others. Across cultures and measurement instruments (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001), the most important value tends to be benevolence, strongly implying that it is the most socially desirable value (see Edwards, 1953). Consistent with this, at least when judging others, communal characteristics are generally considered the most desirable (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007), and benevolence values are highly correlated with measures of socially desirable responding, which indicates the degree to which a person strives to make a good impression (Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005; Schwartz,
  • 14. Verkasalo, Antonovsky, & Sagiv, 1997). By contrast, hedonism, on the opposite side of the values circle (see Figure 1 in Schwartz, 1992), could be considered the least socially desirable value, as it focuses on gratification of one’s own sensual desires. Consistent with this, hedonism scores are negatively correlated with socially desirable responding (Lindeman & Verkasalo, 2005; Schwartz et al., 1996). Premigration experience of Finland and attitude toward Finns. Premigration experience of Finland was assessed with an item asking participants how often they had visited Finland. The response options were 0 (never), 1 (once or twice), 2 (three or four times), 3 (five or six times), 4 (seven or eight times), and 5 (nine or more times). The item was used both as a continuous variable and as a dichotomous variable that indicated whether or not the participant had ever visited Finland prior to migration. In the sample of Ingrian Finns, the median response was five or six previous visits to Finland, and only 12 (9%) had never visited Finland. In the sample of Russians, the median response was one or two previous visits to Finland, and 14 (25%) had never visited. A feeling thermometer (e.g., Paolini, Hewstone, Cairns, & Voci, 2004; Verkuyten, 2007) was used to assess overall feeling toward Finns. Ratings were made on scale ranging from 0 (very cold or negative feelings) to 100 (very positive or warm feelings). The mean of the one-item measure was, in the sample of Ingrian Finns, 87 (SD = 14) and, in the sample of Russians, 89 (SD = 18). Intergroup contact. Quantity of intergroup contact was measured with five items: participants indicated on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much) how much contact they had experienced with native Finns in each of the following contexts: at college, as neighbors, as close friend, as authorities, and as employers/teachers. The first three items were from a scale developed by Islam and Hewstone (1993), to which we added the last two items. In the sample of Ingrian Finns/Russians, the descriptive statistics were M = 2.83, SD = 1.31, α = .68; M = 2.74, SD = 1.30, α = .68. Quality of contact was measured by having participants rate each of the above five
  • 15. contexts on three items. Specifically, on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very), participants responded to the item “When you meet native Finns, in general do you find the contact . . .” followed by three adjectives: pleasant, cooperative, and superficial (the last one was reverse-coded). The descriptives of this 15-item scale, adapted from a study by Voci and Hewstone (2003), were, in the sample of Ingrian Finns and Russians, M = 3.53, SD = 0.79, α = .78, and M = 3.49, SD = 0.68, α = .89, respectively. To assess more negative experiences of intergroup contact, we measured perceived discrimination using four items: “I think native Finns do not accept me because I come from Russia”; “I think native Finns have something against me because I come from Russia”; “Native Finns have harassed me or insulted me because I come from Russia”; and “Native Finns have threatened me or attacked me because I come from Russia.” The items, adapted from the discrimination scale by Berry, Phinney, Sam, and Vedder (2006), were rated on a scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). In the sample of Ingrian Finns and Russians, the descriptive statistics were M = 1.96, SD = 0.77, α = .77, and M = 2.03, SD = 0.67, α = .65, respectively. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 773 Results The means and standard deviations of the personal values of a representative sample of Finns are shown in Table 1. These statistics are also shown for Finns’ stereotype of Finns as well as Ingrian Finn and Russian migrants’ stereotypes of a typical Finn at the three measurement points. Group-Level Results Before examining the 10 basic values individually, we combined information over all 10 values by conducting analyses using the values profiles that the 10 basic values constitute. We first examined whether within the different samples there existed a shared stereotype of Finns and whether this stereotype, within the groups of migrants, became more or less consensual over time. Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were used to assess interrater consensus. All ICCs reported are from two-way mixed effects models: Both objects (the 10 basic
  • 16. values) and raters contributed systematic variability, and rater effects were fixed (raters did not constitute a random sample drawn from a larger population). The single-measures ICCs indicate the percentage of shared variance between any two raters’ stereotype profiles. The average measures ICCs show the variance that the average profile would be expected to share with an average profile derived from a similar sample of raters. The ICCs shown in Table 2 indicate that within all groups, participants shared a rather similar stereotype of the values of a typical Finn. Furthermore, the highly overlapping confidence intervals suggest that there were no differences in consensus, either between groups of participants or over time. We next examined the group-level accuracy and stability of stereotype profiles. This was done by correlating among each other the actual personal values of the representative sample of Finn (ESS sample) and Finns’, Ingrian Finns’, and Russians’ stereotypes of the typical Finn. For such correlational profile analyses, using Schwartz’s (1992) values theory as a general framework provides two advantages: the direction of keying is meaningful (an arbitrary direction of keying would influence indices of profile similarity; Tellegen, 1965), and only the shape of the profiles matters (differences in elevation present response bias and should be ignored; Schwartz, 1992). These considerations make Pearson’s correlation coefficient, shown in Table 3, an appropriate statistic. There are five important things to notice about these correlations. (a) Ingrian Finns’ and Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of Finns’ Personal Values and Finns’, Ingrian Finns’, and Russians Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Ingrian Finns’ Stereotype (N = 125 to 131) Russians’ Stereotype (N = 52 to 56) Basic Values Finns’ Values (N = 1,646) Finns’ Stereotype (N = 38) T0 T1 T2 T0 T1 T2 Benevolence 1.18 (0.16) 1.09 (0.18) 1.02 (0.18) 0.96 (0.21) 0.92 (0.18) 0.98 (0.17) 0.95 (0.21) 0.93 (0.19) Universalism 1.19 (0.17) 0.99 (0.17) 1.14 (0.14) 1.14 (0.16) 1.11 (0.14) 1.13 (0.13) 1.13 (0.14) 1.14 (0.15) Self-Direction 1.11 (0.16) 1.01 (0.14) 0.99 (0.16) 0.98 (0.19) 0.99 (0.17) 0.98 (0.16) 0.97
  • 17. (0.19) 0.96 (0.20) Stimulation 0.89 (0.23) 0.75 (0.20) 0.77 (0.17) 0.73 (0.16) 0.75 (0.16) 0.74 (0.18) 0.72 (0.21) 0.74 (0.18) Hedonism 0.95 (0.24) 0.90 (0.25) 0.79 (0.21) 0.84 (0.22) 0.84 (0.22) 0.82 (0.21) 0.85 (0.21) 0.90 (0.20) Achievement 0.84 (0.24) 0.93 (0.23) 0.93 (0.18) 0.91 (0.20) 0.92 (0.20) 0.97 (0.16) 0.94 (0.21) 0.92 (0.18) Power 0.72 (0.21) 0.98 (0.23) 0.77 (0.20) 0.77 (0.21) 0.80 (0.23) 0.82 (0.23) 0.83 (0.20) 0.86 (0.19) Security 1.11 (0.20) 1.30 (0.17) 1.24 (0.15) 1.27 (0.15) 1.26 (0.16) 1.21 (0.16) 1.27 (0.16) 1.25 (0.18) Conformity 1.03 (0.23) 1.12 (0.26) 1.14 (0.21) 1.18 (0.20) 1.17 (0.19) 1.17 (0.16) 1.19 (0.19) 1.20 (0.19) Tradition 1.00 (0.22) 1.09 (0.25) 1.15 (0.17) 1.20 (0.19) 1.19 (0.16) 1.14 (0.17) 1.18 (0.18) 1.20 (0.17) Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 774 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) Russians’ ratings of the typical Finn were, across measurement points, rather accurate, whereas Finns’ ratings were not. (b) Both Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes, besides being rather accurate, resembled Finns’ autostereotype. In fact, Finns’ self-ratings (ESS sample) and Finns’ autostereotypes (student sample) both contributed to the prediction of Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ heterostereotypes of the typical Finn. For instance, in the prediction of Ingrian Finns’ premigration stereotypes, a regression model including only Finns’ self-ratings as a predictor variable predicted 50% (R2 ) of the variance (p = .02), and this number increased to 80% (p = .005 ) when Finns’ autostereotypes were added to the model (change in R2 = 30, p = .01). Similarly, Finns’ self-rating could predict Ingrian Finns’ heterostereotypes beyond that which was predicted by Finns’ autostereotypes (R2 increased from .68 [p = .003] to .80 [p = .005], change in R2 = 12, p = .08), although the effect did not quite reach statistical significance. Virtually identical results were obtained for all three measurement points and in the sample of Russians. (c) Both Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes remained remarkably consistent over time, with all profile-profile correlations across the three measurement points exceeding .97.
  • 18. (d) Neither Ingrian Finns’ nor Russians’ stereotypes became more accurate over time (first column of Table 3), (e) nor did they become more similar to Finns’ autostereotype (second column of Table 3). Table 2. Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Stereotype Ratings. Single Measures ICC Average Measures ICC Finns’ stereotype .32 (.17 to .62) .95 (.89 to .98) Ingrian Finns’ stereotype T0 .51 (.33 to .78) .99 (.98 to 1.00) T1 .50 (.32 to .77) .99 (.98 to 1.00) T2 .47 (.29 to .75) .99 (.97 to 1.00) Russians’ stereotype T0 .39 (.22 to .71) .97 (.94 to .99) T1 .39 (.22 to .71) .97 (.94 to .99) T2 .62 (.33 to .97) .99 (.96 to 1.00) Note: The numbers in parenthesis are 95% confidence intervals. Table 3. Group-Level Profile Similarity Coefficients Between Finns’ Personal Values and Finns’, Ingrian Finns’, and Russians’ Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Finns’ Values (N = 1,646) Finns’ Stereotype (N = 38) Ingrian Finns’ Stereotype (N = 125) Russians’ Stereotype (N = 52) 1. 2. 3. T0 4. T1 5. T2 6. T0 7. T1 8. T2 1. 2. .49 3. .71* .83* 4. .64* .83* .98* 5. .58 .83* .97* 1.00* 6. .62 .83* .98* .99* .98* 7. .57 .86* .97 .99* .99* .99* 8. .54 .83* .94* .98* .99* .97* .99* *p < .05. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 775 To assess mean-level changes in the content of Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ stereotypes, we evaluated the data with a repeated measures regression analysis (see Misangyi, LePine, Algina, & Goeddeke, 2006). The repeated measures regression procedure fit, for each migrant, a regression line to his three ratings over time. Specifically, one basic value at a time, each migrant’s stereotype was predicted by the time intervals of 0 (corresponding to years lived in Finland at T0), years in Finland at first follow-up, and years in Finland at second follow-up. This procedure derived for each migrant an intercept value, which indicated how the migrant on average rated Finns, and a slope value, which indicated how the migrant’s ratings changed as a function of time. Furthermore, we also included, in the same regression, the curvilinear effects of time, because some changes could conceivable be only temporary. The average
  • 19. intercepts and slopes are shown in Table 4. These reveal that on average, Ingrian Finns’ perceptions of the typical Finn became less positive over time, with a decrease in perceived benevolence and an increase in perceived hedonism. Furthermore, Ingrian Finns also perceived Finns as increasingly conservative (tradition and security up and stimulation down). Almost identical results were obtained in the sample of Russians, but these tended not to be statistically significant due to smaller sample size. Curvilinear effects were neglectable. Individual-Level Results The above group-level analyses revealed high stereotype profile stability across time. We next investigated whether such high-profile stability could also be found at the level of the individual. At this level, we were able to differentiate between overall similarity and distinctive similarity. As noted by Cronbach (1955), there are two components that influence similarities in profile shape: stereotype accuracy and differential accuracy. Stereotype accuracy refers to the similarities that any two profiles will share. Applied to the present case, the Ingrian Finns clearly shared a stereotype regarding the values profile of a typical Finn. This means that any two randomly chosen Ingrian Finns’ stereotypes of the typical Finn will, even across time, be somewhat similar. If one is interested in the stability of an individual’s stereotype profile, then such similarity between random profiles will inflate stability estimates. Therefore, one should instead look at distinctive similarity, which controls for the shared variance of all profiles. In cross-cultural data, this means standardizing the data using cross-cultural norms (Terracciano et al., 2005). In Table 4. Change in Stereotypes: Means, Standard Deviations, and Range of Regression Estimates From Within-Individual Analyses. Ingrian Finns’ Stereotype (N = 120) Russians’ Stereotype (N = 53) Intercept Beta Beta range Beta2 Intercept Beta Beta range Beta2 Basic Values M (SD) M (SD) Betamin to Betamax M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Betamin to Betamax M (SD) Benevolence 1.01 (0.18) -0.11 (0.58)* -1.60 to 1.49 0.02 (0.24) 0.98 (0.17) -0.05 (0.51) -1.50 to 1.41 0.02
  • 20. (0.22) Universalism 1.14(0.14) 0.00 (0.43) -1.93 to 1.16 -0.01 (0.19) 1.14 (0.13) -0.01 (0.38) -1.13 to 0.90 0.01 (0.15) Self- direction 0.99 (.17) 0.00 (0.49) -1.30 to 2.24 0.01 (0.21) 0.98 (0.16) -0.02 (0.43) -1.32 to 0.79 0.01 (0.15) Stimulation 0.78(0.17) -0.14 (0.55)* -2.95 to 1.44 0.06 (0.24)* 0.74 (0.18) - 0.04 (0.42) -1.22 to 1.25 0.01 (0.16) Hedonism 0.79(0.21) 0.11 (0.58)* -1.44 to 2.65 -0.04 (0.26) 0.82 (0.21) 0.04 (0.49) -1.11 to 1.74 -0.01 (0.20) Achievement 0.94(0.18) -0.04 (0.51) -1.89 to 1.65 0.01 (0.22) 0.97 (0.16) -0.05 (0.49) -1.64 to 1.31 0.01 (0.20) Power 0.77(0.21) -0.03 (0.54) -1.23 to 2.22 0.02 (0.22) 0.82 (0.23) -0.02 (0.54) -1.46 to 1.11 0.02 (0.20) Security 1.24(0.15) 0.10 (0.45)* -0.95 to 2.79 -0.04 (0.20) 1.22 (0.16) 0.10 (0.31)* -0.47 to 1.15 -0.03 (0.13) Conformity 1.15(0.21) 0.06 (0.60) -1.58 to 2.88 -0.03 (0.27) 1.18 (0.16) 0.00 (0.61) - 2.43 to 1.22 0.02 (0.30) Tradition 1.16(0.17) 0.10 (0.55)* -1.76 to 2.88 -0.04 (0.26) 1.14 (0.17) 0.07 (0.39) -0.89 to 1.15 -0.01 (0.15) *p < .05. One-sample t test against zero. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 776 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) individual-level data, this means computing the indices of profile similarity from data first standardized across individuals. The resulting index of profile similarity indicates how the pattern of how a profile (specific individual’s T0 stereotype) deviates from other profiles (average of other individuals’ T0 stereotypes) reflects how another profile (specific individual’s T1 stereotype) deviates from other profiles (average of other individuals’ T1 stereotypes). Both overall and distinctive profile similarity coefficients (Pearson’s rs) are presented in Table 5 (we used the Fisher r-to-Z-to-r averaging procedure). The psychometrically preferable distinctive similarity coefficients indicated, as expected, that stereotype profiles were only moderately stable at the level of the individual. The rank-order stabilities (test-retest correlations) of specific values are given in Table 6. Consistent with the only moderate stability of the individual-level values profiles, the rank-order stabilities of some of the basic values were not very high. This means that migrants changed their
  • 21. relative ordering compared to other migrants; for example, the same migrants who premigration perceived the Finns as more benevolent than did other migrants did not necessarily do so postmigration. To assess whether changes in the stereotype were consistent across individuals (was it the same people whose perceptions of Finns’ benevolence and stimulation decreased, and whose perceptions of Finns’ hedonism, security, and tradition increased?), we computed the ICC for Table 5. Average Individual-Level Profile Similarity Coefficients (Pearson rs) Over Time for Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Ingrian Finns (N = 125) Russians (N = 52) Time Period Overall Distinctive Overall Distinctive T0 to T1 .75 .37 .74 .44 T1 to T2 .77 .48 .72 .42 T0 to T2 .70 .31 .65 .30 Note: All significant at p < .001; one- sample t test against zero. Table 6. Rank-Order Correlation Coefficients for Ingrian Finns’ and Russians’ Stereotypes of Finns’ Personal Values. Ingrian Finns (N = 125 to 131) Russians (N = 52 to 56) Basic Values T0 to T1 T1 to T2 T0 to T2 T0 to T1 T1 to T2 T0 to T2 Benevolence .36 .31 .05 .38 .08 .33 Universalism .35 .48 .25 .37 .48 .22 Self-direction .40 .49 .38 .40 .48 .19 Stimulation .32 .46 .43 .54 .64 .56 Hedonism .53 .42 .31 .42 .32 .17 Achievement .47 .59 .32 .33 .39 .29 Power .43 .55 .39 .29 .49 .54 Security .39 .42 .23 .59 .52 .35 Conformism .31 .45 .39 .37 .28 .35 Tradition .33 .38 .26 .42 .56 .32 Average .39 .46 .30 .42 .44 .34 Note: For Ingrian Finns (Russians), all correlations r ≥ .23 (r ≥ .28) are statistically significant at p < .05. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 777 value change. This analysis was based on the 10 Beta values (one Beta estimate for each of the basic values) that each individual obtained in the within-individual regression analyses. The obtained ICC revealed that any two random Ingrian Finn migrants’ value change profiles shared 0% of their variance (p = .79). A similar result was obtained in the sample of Russian migrants (p = .81). The migrants did not experience uniform changes in their stereotypes of the values profile of a typical
  • 22. Finn. Rather, some migrants’ stereotypes changed regarding some values, and other migrants’ regarding some other values. Predictors of Stereotype Change The above results suggest that stereotypes were only moderately stable at the level of the individual and that there was no uniform pattern to how stereotypes changed. Such circumstances could have been expected to facilitate the search for individual-level predictors of stereotype change. Recall that we predicted that positive and negative experiences with Finns could potentially influence the stereotype of Finns. To examine this, we correlated quantity of contact with Finns, quality of contact with Finns, and perceived discrimination by Finns with the slopes obtained from the within-participants regression analyses. None of the resulting 30 correlations reached conventional levels of statistical significance (in both samples, all p > .05). To assess the overall magnitude of stereotype change, we created for each individual a sum score by adding up the absolute values of the individual’s slope coefficients (derived from the withinindividual regression), but this index was also not correlated with the three variables reflecting direct experience with Finns (in both samples, all p > .05). Finally, we did not find evidence that those migrants with more frequent premigration contact (coded either as a continuous or dichotomous variable) would have had a different initial stereotype (indexed by the intercepts from the within-participant regressions). Across both samples, only one correlation out of the 40 (10 Basic Values × 2 Indices of Contact × 2 Samples) tested reached statistical significance. The same result was found when examining stereotype change (as indexed by the betas from within-participants regressions): 1 correlation (out of 40) was statistically significant. Regarding the predictive power of premigration positive feelings, scores on the thermometer scale were not correlated with any of the intercept values from withinparticipants regression analyses, and with only 1 (out of 20 [10 Slopes × 2 Samples]) of the beta values. Furthermore, neither premigration contact nor feelings toward Finns predicted the overall magnitude of stereotype
  • 23. change. Discussion The data presented here force us to acknowledge that there are no simple answers to questions regarding stability and change in stereotypes. In the present context, the extent of stability and change in migrants’ stereotypes depended on whether one examined (a) the group or the individual, (b) profile scores that combined information over all 10 basic values or single basic values, and (c) the rank- order or absolute stability of single values. Based on our results, we argue that the specific mechanisms involved in stereotype change may be different for different aspects of the stereotype. Mechanisms of Stereotype Change We initially suspected that migrants’ stereotypes could become more accurate or more similar to Finns’ autostereotypes as a result of increased intergroup contact. However, group-level profile scores were extremely stable. This could be because the premigration stereotypes were already Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 778 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) (a) rather accurate, as shown by comparison with self-ratings from a representative sample of Finns, and also (b) similar to Finns’ autostereotype. In fact, around 80% of the variance of premigration stereotypes could be explained by correspondence with Finns’ self-ratings and Finns’ autostereotype. Upon increased contact with Finns, and thereby also increased knowledge of Finns’ autostereotype, migrants most likely found support for both those aspects of their stereotype that corresponded with Finns’ self-ratings as well as those aspects that corresponded with Finns’ autostereotype (for confirmatory biases in information processing, see Kunda & Oleson, 1995; Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979). Apart from the above longitudinal perspective, it is worthwhile to note that the results support the idea that heterostereotypes may, among well-informed individuals, be more accurate than autostereotypes, for instance due to the availability of a reference point (Robins, 2005). However, heterostereotypes, despite being somewhat accurate, resembled Finns’ autostereotypes even more than they resembled Finns’
  • 24. self-ratings. We also have more confidence in this latter result (as compared to the results pertaining to accuracy), as it is similar to previous results according to which heterostereotypes of the personality traits of Americans (Terraciano & McCrae, 2007) and Russians (Realo et al., 2009) converge with autostereotypes. One reason for this could be that both autostereotypes and heterostereotypes are to some extent based on similar knowledge of facts about the target of the ratings. For instance, at least some Finns and migrants to Finland are likely to know that Finland tends to be ranked as being one of the least corrupt countries in the world (e.g., Zook, 2009), and this may be reflected in high ratings of the typical Finns’ conformity and security values. Although group-level stereotype profile scores were extremely stable, meaningful mean-level changes could be observed for some of the 10 basic values. One clearly observable trend was that stereotypes became less desirable over time, with perceptions of native Finns’ benevolence decreasing and perceptions of their hedonism increasing. Tartakovsky (2009a) identified a disillusionment process in his study on Jewish adolescents migrating to Israel. Consistent with the results of that study, in which the attitude toward Israel became more negative postmigration, our participants revealed an increasingly negative stereotype of Finns. In general, for increased contact to lead to increased favorability toward the outgroup, the contact should optimally be positive, equal status, and cooperative (e.g., Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). The contact between Ingrian Finns and native Finns has most likely not fulfilled these criteria. In fact, much of the contact is likely to have occurred in a context in which the migrant has been in a low-status position, for instance applying for jobs, educational programs, public housing, or well-fare benefits. In such unequal status contexts, high-power individuals tend to behave in a more selfish way (De Cremer & Van Dijk, 2005), show less social engagement (Kraus & Keltner, 2009), and interrupt others more often during interpersonal interaction (Hall, Coats, & LeBeau, 2005). High-
  • 25. power individuals are also stereotypically perceived as colder and less friendly (Conway, Pizzamiglio, & Mount, 1996). The results of our previous studies on the quality of intergroup contact and anticipated and perceived discrimination, conducted with the same sample of migrants as this study (Jasinskaja- Lahti, Mähönen, & Liebkind, 2012; Jasinskaja-Lahti, Mähönen, & Ketokivi, in press; Mähönen, & Jasinskaja-Lahti, 2012), also suggest that the intergroup contact experienced by our participants had not been positive; immigrants have reported less positive contact and more discrimination after migration than they have anticipated premigration. The stressfulness of the acculturation experience could also in part be responsible for the stereotypes of Finns becoming increasingly negative. The second year after migration may be especially difficult, with psychological adjustment and adaptation again beginning to improve in the third year (Tartakovsky, 2009b). As our migrants had, on average, lived in Finland for only 22 months at T2, it is possible that they were still at the bottom of the suggested U-curve of adjustment. This could have had an adverse effect on their perceptions of Finns. A fourth Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 779 measurement point, planned for 2013, will reveal whether stereotypes of Finns will again become more positive as migrants increasingly adapt to their new environment. Through what mechanism did ratings of Finns’ benevolence become more negative? The rank-order stabilities of the 10 basic values revealed that migrants were more likely to change their relative positions regarding some values than other values. Most unstable in this regard were ratings of benevolence (lowest test-retest r = .05). Benevolence values also changed the most in terms of absolute stability. Migrants were thus more likely to change their ratings of Finns’ benevolence than their ratings of Finns’ other values. One explanation for this could be that “perceivers first of all want to know whether another person is benevolent” (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011, p. 935). Research on person perception
  • 26. shows that people initially strive to infer another person’s benevolence in order to make approach and avoidance decisions (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and that information related to this dimension is preferentially processed on a cognitive level (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011). Our migrant participants may more or less automatically have judged the benevolence of those Finns with whom they had been in contact, and this contact may not always have been positive, influencing the migrants’ stereotypes of Finns. Another trend that could be observed in mean-level changes was that the stereotype of Finns moved in the direction of increased adherence to conservation values (as opposed to openness to change values). This trend was evident in increased perceptions of adherence to security and tradition and decreased perceptions of adherence to stimulation. One of the most central themes to conservation values is that they serve to maintain or justify the status quo. These shifts could thus also partly reflect a process of disillusionment; migrants may by Finns be perceived as threatening the traditions, conventions, and norms established by the system (e.g., Huddy & Sears, 1995), and migrants could pick up such a fearful attitude. In contrast to ratings of benevolence, migrants kept, at least to some extent, their relative positions in their ratings of security, tradition, and stimulation. That is, although migrants showed mean level-shifts in their ratings of these values, these shifts were more or less common to all migrants. The factors that caused such mean-level changes were therefore likely to be rather similar across migrants, perhaps including common experience of Finnish television, laws, newspapers, officials, political leaders, or other cultural institutions. Related to the uniform changes in migrants’ perceptions of Finns’ adherence to conservation values, of particular interest is that the period of migration happened to coincide with the breakthrough of the True Finns, a political party with national identity or Finnishness as its pivotal concept (Arter, 2010). During the 4- year span of the present study, the national vote share of this party rose steadily from around 5% to 20%, as reflected for
  • 27. instance in the 2007 and 2011 parliamentary elections. The number of seats that the True Finns won rose from 5 to 39 out of 200 (Nurmi & Nurmi, 2012). Furthermore, like other populist radical right-wing parties, the True Finns commanded disproportionately much media attention (see Mazzoleni, Stewart, & Horsfield, 2003). Although still lacking xenophobic extremism, the monoculturalist orientation of the True Finns— the party explicitly states that multiculturalism should not be promoted—and the espousing of a Christian society grounded in traditional family values (Arter, 2010) could have led migrants to increasingly view Finns as adhering to tradition and security values. Regarding changes in other values than the above, no clear patterns emerged. Based on previous research, one could, for instance, have expected characteristics more central to the stereotype to be more stable (Garcia-Marques et al., 2006). As a proxy of centrality, one could use the premigration stereotype mean scores; basic values with more extreme scores could be argued to be more central to the stereotype. But the results did not support the idea that perceptions of more central values (e.g., security) would have been more stable. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 780 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) Predicting Stereotype Change at the Level of the Individual Regarding consensus, the results of the present research can be summarized as showing that (a) Finns’ auto- and migrants’ heterostereotypes across time showed similar levels of moderate within-group consensus (ICCs ranging from .30 to .62); (b) consensus neither increased nor decreased during the migration process, suggesting a negligible role of increased contact on consensus; and (c) stereotype change during the migration process was not consensual. The last point is particularly important, as it confirms that stereotypes did not change merely as a function of migrants’ common experiences with cultural institutions, but rather that stereotype change was determined also by personal experiences. This is consistent with the low rank-order stabilities of ratings of some of the values and with
  • 28. the only moderate stability of individuallevel profile indices (ICCs ranging from .30 to .48). Although stereotypes were only moderately stable and changed in different ways across individuals, we were unable to predict such changes: Neither positive nor negative contact with native Finns predicted changes in the stereotype of Finns. This was true regarding both changes in the content of the stereotype and overall quantity of change. Furthermore, premigration measures of prior contact with Finns, as well as attitudes toward Finns, failed to predict either the initial stereotype of Finns or changes in that stereotype. Consistent with this lack of individuallevel results, none of the results obtained in the samples of Ingrian Finn and Russia migrants differed notably. For some aspects of the stereotype—that is, those that showed similar changes across individuals, such as perceptions of Finns’ adherence to tradition—the result that we could not find individual-level predictors was not that surprising. However, for other aspects of the stereotype, such as benevolence values, the fact that neither quantity nor quality of intergroup contact predicted stereotype change can be considered surprising. Such results appear to contradict previous research that has shown cross-group friendships to predict more positive attitudes toward the outgroup (Paolini et al., 2004). One reason that changes in stereotypes were independent of reports of intergroup contact could be that the latter may be highly biased. Such bias may be partly conscious, as the reporting of negative intergroup contact is socially undesirable (Krieger, Smith, Naishadham, Hartman, & Barbeau, 2005), and this may be particularly true if the outgroup is composed of host nationals whose acceptance migrants may seek. Importantly, a large body of research demonstrates that peoples’ memories for events are often distorted and reconstructed in ways incongruent with what actually happened (e.g., Kunda, 1999). For instance, self- enhancement and self-protection motives (e.g., Wilson & Ross, 2003), as well as belongingness and relational needs (e.g., Gardner, Pickett, & Brewer, 2000; Karney & Coombs, 2000),
  • 29. may influence memory reconstruction and selective memory biases. Most pertinent to the present research, there is ample evidence that people tend to deny or minimize experiences of discrimination (e.g., Crosby, 1984; Taylor, Wright, Moghaddam, & Lalonde, 1990), at least in part to protect their self-esteem (e.g., Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997). However, we acknowledge that a different line of research suggests that attributing negative feedback to prejudice buffers the consequences of such feedback on self-esteem (e.g., Crocker, Voelkl, Testa, & Major, 1991). However, this latter line of research may be less relevant, as our measure of experienced discrimination explicitly measured discrimination attributable to prejudice, including items such as ”I think native Finns do not accept me because I come from Russia.” Both denying direct discrimination due to ethnicity (Ruggiero & Taylor, 1997) and attributing negative treatment with unknown causes to prejudice (Crocker et al., 1991) may therefore serve to protect self- esteem. Summarizing the above, because judgments of intergroup contact and particularly discrimination are strongly associated with how people perceive and feel about themselves (see also, Dion & Earn, 1975; Major, Spencer, Schmader, Wolfe, & Crocker, 1997), such judgments may be Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 781 based on highly motivated information- processing biases. By contrast, judgments of the typical Finn’s personal values are likely to be less personally relevant and therefore less suspect to motivational biases. The extent to which migrants’ stereotypes of host nationals are associated with reports of intergroup contact should be assessed in other contexts. The associations between migrants’ stereotypes and their well-being or adjustment should also be examined. Stereotype judgments could turn out to be a useful, perhaps less motivationally biased, compliment to more traditional measures of intergroup contact. Conclusion Perhaps one of the most basic questions in research on national stereotypes is whether the content of stereotypes is affected by personal contact. In
  • 30. essence, do the impressions that are formed in direct contact with outgroup members generalize to other group members? Or are stereotypes largely independent of direct personal experience? Our results suggest that it depends on the characteristic being assessed. Both in terms of absolute and rank-order stability, ratings of benevolence were highly malleable, suggesting that actual experience with Finns changed migrants’ perceptions. This result thus suggests that the more or less automatic judgments (Abele & Bruckmüller, 2011) that people make of others’ communal characteristics may influence stereotypes. Regarding perceptions of other characteristics, our results indicate that Finns became increasingly perceived as more conservative. But here, the migrants kept their relative positions, suggesting that differing personal experiences may not have been responsible for these changes in the perception of Finns. Rather, common experiences of Finnish cultural institutions are likely to have caused these changes—in particular, as argued above, we believe such shifts in the stereotype of Finns to have been caused by the rise of the True Finns during the time-span of the present research. The present results thus suggest that the mechanisms of stereotype change may be different for different characteristics. Personal contact may influence some aspects of the stereotype, whereas other aspects of the stereotype may be more influenced by common cultural experiences. Regarding correspondence of migrants’ stereotypes with Finns’ self-ratings and Finns’ autostereotype, firm conclusions were difficult to draw. Premigration stereotypes were both accurate and similar to autostereotypes, and this most likely contributed to their extremely high stability. Studying a sample of migrants whose premigration stereotypes were either more inaccurate or deviated more from host nationals’ autostereotype could prove to be illuminating regarding the influence of accuracy and autostereotypes on stereotype formation. More generally, perhaps one of the most promising research designs for future research on the mechanisms of national stereotypes could be the longitudinal
  • 31. study of migrants. Many processes are likely to be context specific, but some may generalize across contexts. For instance, we believe that the disillusionment process may be specific to voluntary migration contexts. Even more specifically, the observed changes in the perception of host nationals’ adherence to conservation values may have been solely due to the particular upsurge of one political party. However, some of our results may have more general bearing, such as the result that personal contact may influence some aspects of the stereotype— specifically, those aspects related to communal characteristics— whereas other aspects of the stereotype—perhaps those that are judged less automatically in interpersonal contact—may be more strongly affected by cultural institutions. Acknowledgments We would like to thank Antti Hulsi (Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland), Kaisa Kanerva (Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland), Tamara Kinunen Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 782 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) (St. Petersburg State University, Russia), Michail Vinokurov (State University of Petrozavodsk, Russia), as well as Elina Leinonen and Jesse Haapoja (Department of Social Psychology, University of Helsinki, Finland) for their help in data collection and coding. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by the Academy of Finland Research Grants 127641 and 123297. References Abele, A. E., & Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential processing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935-948. Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751-763. Allport, G. W. (1954/1978). The
  • 32. nature of prejudice (25th anniversary ed.). New York: Basic Books. Arter, D. (2010). The breakthrough of another West European populist radical right party? The case of the True Finns. Government and Opposition, 45, 484-504. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (Eds.) (2006). Immigrant youth in cultural transition: Acculturation, identity and adaptation across national contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Brigham, J. C. (1971). Ethnic stereotypes. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 15- 38. Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation processes: The selffulfilling nature of automatically-activated stereotypes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541-560. Church, A. T., & Katigbak, M. S. (2002). The Five-Factor Model in the Philippines: Investigating trait structure and levels across cultures. In R. R. McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The Five-Factor Model of personality across cultures (pp. 129-154). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T., & Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality and agency: Implications for the stereotypes of gender and other groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 25-38. Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 218-228. Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Process affecting scores on “understanding others” and “assumed similarity”. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 177-193. Crosby, F. (1984). The denial of personal discrimination. American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 371-386. Davidov, E. (2010). Testing for comparability of human values across countries and time with the third round of the European Social Survey. International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 51, 171-191. Davidov, E., Schmidt, P., & Schwartz, S. H. (2008). Bringing values back in: The adequacy of the European Social Survey to measure values in 20 countries. Public Opinion
  • 33. Quarterly, 72, 420-445. De Cremer, D., & Van Dijk, E. (2005). When and why leaders put themselves first: Leader behaviour in resource allocations as a function of being entitled. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 553-563. Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1139-1150. Dion, K. L., & Earn, B. M. (1975). The phenomenology of being a target of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 944-950. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 783 Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1986). Prejudice, discrimination, and racism: Historical trends and contemporary approaches. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 1-34). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Edwards, A. L. (1953). The relationship between the judged desirability of a trait and the probability that the trait will be endorsed. Journal of Applied Psychology, 37, 90-93. Fischer, R., & Schwartz, S. H. (2011). Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 1127-1144. Garcia-Marques, L., Santos, A. S. C., & Mackie, D. M. (2006). Stereotypes: Static abstractions or dynamic knowledge structures? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 814-831. Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclusion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 486-496. Gilbert, G. M. (1951). Stereotype persistence and change among college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 245-254. Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behaviour and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898- 924. Huddy, L., & Sears, D. O. (1995). Opposition to bilingual education: Prejudice or the defense of realistic interests? Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 133-143. Karlins, M., Coffman, T. L., & Walters, G. (1969). On the fading of social stereotypes:
  • 34. Studies in three generations of college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 1-16. Katz, D., & Braly, K. W. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one-hundred college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280- 290. Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2009). Signs of socioeconomic status: A thin-slicing approach. Psychological Science, 20, 99-106. Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 700-710. Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., Mähönen, T. A., & Ketokivi, M. (in press). The dynamics of ethnic discrimination, identities, and outgroup attitudes: A pre-post longitudinal study of ethnic remigrants. European Journal of Social Psychology. Jasinskaja- Lahti, I., Mähönen, T. A., & Liebkind, K. (2012). Identity and attitudinal reactions to perceptions of intergroup contact among ethnic migrants: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychology, 51, 312-329. Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., & Yijälä, A. (2011). The model of pre-acculturative stress. A pre-migration study of potential migrants from Russia to Finland. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35, 499-510. Jowell, R., & the Central Coordinating Team. (2007). European Social Survey 2006/2007: Technical report. London: Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University, The Data Archive: Norwegian Social Science Data Service (NSD). Karney, B. R., & Coombs, R. H. (2000). Memory bias in evaluations of close relationships: Consistency or improvement? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 959-970. Katz, D., & Schanck, R. L. (1938). Social psychology. New York: Wiley. Krieger, N., Smith, K., Naishadham, D., Hartman, C., & Barbeau, E. M. (2005). Experiences of discrimination: Validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Social Science and Medicine, 61, 1576-1596. Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kunda, Z., & Oleson, K. C. (1995). Maintaining stereotypes in the face of disconfirmation:
  • 35. Constructing grounds for subtyping deviants. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 565-579. Lindeman, M., & Verkasalo, M. (2005). Measuring values with the Short Schwartz Values Survey. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 170-178. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 784 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 44(5) Lönnqvist, J.- E., Yijälä, A., Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., & Verkasalo, M. (2012). Accuracy and contrast in national value stereotypes—A case study using bi-cultural experts. International Journal of Intergroup Relations, 36, 271-278. Lord, C., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. E. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098-2109. Madon, S., Guyll, M., Aboufadel, K., Montiel, E., Smith, A., Palumbo, P., & Jussim, J. (2001). Ethnic and national stereotypes: The Princeton trilogy revisited and revised. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 996-1010. Mähönen, T. A. & Jasinskaja-Lahti, I. (2012). Anticipated and perceived intergroup relations as predictors of immigrants’ identification patterns: A follow-up study. European Psychologist, 17, 120-130. Major, B., Spencer, S., Schmader, T., Wolfe, C., & Crocker, J. (1997). Coping with negative stereotypes about intellectual performance: The role of psychological disengagement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 34-50. Mazzoleni, G., Stewart, J., & Horsfield, B. (2003). The media and neo-populism: A contemporary comparative analysis. Westport, CT: Praeger. McCrae, R. R., Kurtz, J. E., Yamagata, S., & Terracciano, A. (2011). Internal consistency, retest reliability, and their implications for personality scale validity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 28-50. McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., Realo, A., & Allik, J. (2007). Climatic warmth and national wealth: Some culture-level determinants of national character stereotypes. European Journal of Personality, 21, 953-976. Misangyi, V. F., LePine, J. A., Algina, J., & Goeddeke, F., Jr. (2006). The adequacy of repeated-measures
  • 36. regression for multilevel research. Organizational Research Methods, 9, 5-28. Nurmi, H., & Nurmi, L. (2012). The parliamentary election in Finland, April 2011. Electoral Studies, 31, 234-238. Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., & Voci, A. (2004). Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on judgements of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland: The mediating role of an anxietyreduction mechanism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 770-786. Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751-783. Realo, A., Allik, J., Lönnqvist, J.- E., Verkasalo, M., Kwiatkowska, A., Kööts, L., . . ., Renge, V. (2009). Mechanisms of the national character stereotype: How people in six neighboring countries of Russia describe themselves and the typical Russian. European Journal of Personality, 23, 229-249. Robins, R. W. (2005). The nature of personality: Genes, culture, and national character. Science, 310, 62-63. Rothbart, M., & John, O. P. (1993). Intergroup relations and stereotype change: A social-cognitive analysis and some longitudinal findings. In P. M. Sniderman, P. E. Tetlock, & E. G. Carmines (Eds.), Prejudice, politics and the American dilemma (pp. 32-59). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1997). Why minority group members perceive or do not perceive the discrimination that confronts them: The role of self-esteem and perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 373-389. Schoenfeld, N. (1942). An experimental study of some problems relating to stereotypes. Archives of Psychology, No. 270. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press. Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Value orientations: Measurement, antecedents and consequences across nations. In R. Jowell, C. Roberts, R. Fitzgerald & G. Eva (Eds.), Measuring attitudes cross-nationally: Lessons from the European Social Survey (pp.
  • 37. 161-193). London: Sage. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Lönnqvist et al. 785 Schwartz, S. H. (2009). Culture matters: National value cultures, sources and consequences. In C.- Y. Chiu, Y. Y. Hong, S. Shavitt, & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Understanding culture: Theory, research and application (pp. 127-150). New York: Psychology Press. Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 268-290. Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value priorities: Cross- cultural and multimethod studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1010-1028. Schwartz, S.H., Verkasalo, M., Antonovsky, A., & Sagiv, L. (1997). Value priorities and social desirability: Much substance, some style. British Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 3-18. Tartakovsky, E. (2009a). Cultural identities of adolescent immigrants: A three year longitudinal study including the pre-migration period. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 654-671. Tartakovsky, E. (2009b). The psychological well-being of unaccompanied minors: A longitudinal study of adolescents immigrating from Russia and Ukraine to Israel without parents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 177-204. Taylor, D. M., Wright, S. C., Moghaddam, F. M., & Lalonde, R. N. (1990). The personal/group discrimination discrepancy: Perceiving my group, but not myself, to be a target of discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 254-262. Tellegen, A. (1965). Direction of measurement: A source of misinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 233-243. Terracciano, A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Adam, N., Adamovova, L., Ahn, C., & Ahn, H. N. . . ., Robert, R. (2005). National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures. Science, 310, 96-100. Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2007). Perceptions of American and the Iraq Invasion: Implications for understanding national character stereotypes. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 695-710. Verkuyten, M. (2007). Religious group identification and inter-religious
  • 38. relations: A study among TurkishDutch Muslims. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 341-357. Voci, A., & Hewstone, M. (2003). Intergroup contact and prejudice toward immigrants in Italy: The mediational role of anxiety and the moderational role of group salience. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 37-54. Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2003). The identity function of autobiographical memory: Time is on our side. Memory, 11, 137-149. Yijälä, A., & Jasinskaja- Lahti, I. (2010). Pre-migration acculturation attitudes among potential ethnic migrants from Russia to Finland. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 34, 326-339. Zook, D. C. (2009). The curious case of Finland’s clean politics. Journal of Democracy, 20, 157-168. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Gro Pacific Islands Families Study: The Association of Infant Health Risk Indicators and Acculturation of Pacific Island Mothers Living in New Zealand Jim Borrows1 , Maynard Williams1 , Philip Schluter2 , Janis Paterson3 , and S. Langitoto Helu4 Abstract The Pacific Islands Families study follows a cohort of 1,398 Pacific infants born in Auckland, New Zealand. This article examines associations between maternal acculturation, measured by an abbreviated version of the General Ethnicity Questionnaire, and selected infant and maternal health risk indicators. Findings reveal that those with strong alignment to Pacific culture had significantly better infant and maternal risk factor outcomes than those with weak cultural alignment. In terms of Berry’s classical acculturation model, separators had the best infant and maternal outcomes; integrators had reasonable infant and maternal outcomes, while assimilators and marginalisors appeared to have the poorest infant and maternal outcomes. These findings suggest that retaining strong cultural links for Pacific immigrants is likely to have positive health benefits. Keywords acculturation, infant health risk, Pacific health, culture and health Introduction and Background People of Pacific ethnicities resident in New
  • 39. Zealand are overrepresented in many adverse social and health statistics. Pacific peoples generally fare worse than the New Zealand population as a whole in statistics relating to health, unemployment, housing, crime, income, education, and nutrition (Bathgate, Donnell, & Mitikulena, 1994; Cook, Didham, & Khawaja, 1999). Despite the 1 Faculty of Health and Environmental Sciences, AUT University, Auckland, New Zealand 2 School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies, AUT University, Auckland, New Zealand, and the University of Queensland, School of Nursing and Midwifery, Australia 3 School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies, AUT University, Auckland, New Zealand 4 School of Population Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auckland, New Zealand Corresponding Author: Jim Borrows, C/-Professor Philip Schluter, School of Public Health and Psychosocial Studies, AUT University, Private Bag 92006, Auckland, New Zealand. Email: [email protected] Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 700 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(5) growth and employment opportunities in New Zealand, Pacific people are more likely to be living in poor circumstances with restricted access to higher education, home ownership, and access to functional amenities such as automobiles and telephones. Such statistics have significant consequences for Pacific families given that socioeconomic disadvantage has been consistently linked with negative health outcomes (Chen, 2004; Power, 2002). Specifically, the raison d’etre for the Pacific Island Families (PIF) Study, the health of Pacific families, and especially their infants continues to be an issue of major concern for New Zealanders. The total neonatal death rate for Pacific infants at 4.7 per 1,000 live births is twice that of the rate for New Zealanders of European ancestry but still less than the 5.0 of the indigenous Maori population (New Zealand Health Information Service, 2006). Similarly, Pacific infants have high rates of hospitalization, particularly for respiratory illnesses (Ministry of Health & Ministry of Pacific Island
  • 40. Affairs, 2004), and present at hospital with higher severity of illness than other New Zealand children (Grant et al., 2001). These negative infant statistics are somewhat perplexing, especially in a country where primary health care services are available at low cost (free for pre-schoolers) and emergency and hospital care services, including birthing services, are provided free of charge. Also, New Zealand (Abel, Park, Tipene-Leach, Finau, & Lennan, 2001) and Pacific ethnographies (Lukere & Jolly, 2002) show that neonatal and infant care practices are not directly contradictory to accepted Western infant care practices. In Pacific Island settings, themselves changed by 200 years of Western contact, the family is perceived as central in providing traditional protocols for support and advice to ensure infant well-being. Explanation for the current Pacific child health circumstances is likely driven by multiple variables including the immigration process itself. Previous research from the PIF study demonstrated that acculturative orientation had a persistent association with aspects of health status and behaviour for cohort participants (e.g., Abbott & Williams, 2006; Low et al., 2005; Paterson, Feehan, Butler, Williams, & Cowley-Malcolm, 2007), hence the emphasis in this article on testing the association between maternal acculturation and infant and maternal health risk factors. Culture, Health, and Acculturation The interrelationship between culture and health, including associated psychological processes, has been a recurrent theme in the social science literature over much of the last century (Helman, 2000; Sam, 2006a; Stroebe & Stroebe, 1995; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2001). There is now acceptance in the medical and health professional domains that culture should be acknowledged as an important determinant of health status (Corin, 1994; Snowden, 2005; Spector, 2002; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2001) and that concepts derived from anthropologic and cross- cultural research may provide an alternative framework for identifying health issues that require resolution (Kleinman, Eisenberg, & Good, 1978; Savage, 2000). In particular, there is
  • 41. some agreement that many people from minority cultures may not have faith in, or necessarily benefit from, the medical interventions that are being offered by the host society (MacLachlan, 1997). Also recognized is the importance of the interrelationship between migration and health, including seminal New Zealand/Pacific migration studies (Stanhope & Prior, 1976), early international studies (Carballo, Divino, & Zeric, 1998; Ostbye, Welby, Prior, Salmond, & Stokes, 1989), and more recent studies aimed at explaining the link between migration and health (Sam, 2006a). That is, the realization that the well-being of a migrant group is determined by interlinking factors that relate to the society of origin, the migration itself, and the society of resettlement. All three sets of factors need to be considered if one seeks to reduce or merely to understand the level of health disorder in any immigrant group. Despite the recognition of the importance of Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Borrows et al. 701 culture and migration in determining health status and the explanatory acculturation/health hypotheses that this has generated (Carballo et al., 1998; Sam, 2006a), there have been few empirical attempts to link health with both migration and culture in relation to other demographic, social, and psychological factors operating in given communities in New Zealand or international studies (Snowden, 2005). However, it is now clear that migration at an individual level is a significant life event for individuals impacting on subsequent health behaviour and outcomes. Closely related to culture and migration is the concept of acculturation—that is, “culture change that is initiated by the conjunction of two or more autonomous culture systems” (Social Science Research Council, 1954, as cited in Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 2002, p. 350). The social psychology literature is replete with alternative models of the acculturative process, most of which are multidimensional, involving numerous topics and factors (Stanley, 2003). These multidimensional topics range from those at the personal level, such as personality qualities and
  • 42. psychological adjustment (Ward & Leon, 2004), language retention and community socialization, and external acculturation drivers such as migration experience, micro- and macro-societal policies, and regional setting (Persky & Birman, 2005). Outside of these models, but still incorporating multidimensionality, are the two most common models of acculturation theory: unidirectional and bidirectional models of acculturation. Berry restated Redfield and colleagues’ hypothesis that acculturative adaptations lead to culture changes in either or both of the migrating and host society groups. He went on further to note that it is not inevitable that intergroup contact proceeds uniformly through sequential to ultimate assimilation as there are many other ways of going about it or indeed is potentially bidirectional and reciprocal (Berry, 2006). Such insights generated by this bidirectional model challenges the ethnic melting-pot assumptions and promotes exploration and resolution of political sensitivities among ethnicities (Flannery, Reise, & Jiajuan, 2001). These observations by Berry, Sam, and others, which hint at multiple individual and group acculturation strategies, have been complemented more recently by Boski, who calls for the development of a theoretical model of integration, a key concept in the psychology of acculturation, in which five meanings for this concept identified in the existing literature are positioned as in- depth directed layers of the bicultural psyche (Boski, 2008). That is, the subtleties in the acculturation process at the group and individual level deserve further and more detailed examination. There are many studies that have examined acculturation strategies in nondominant groups. In most studies, preference for integration is expressed over other acculturation strategies, although notable exceptions with Turks both in Germany and in Canada, and in Hispanic immigrant women in the United States, have been cited (Ataca & Berry, 2002; Berry, 2006; Jones, Bond, Gardner, & Hernandez, 2002). All these recent contributions that counter the assimilation and melting- pot models could be seen as underpinning Pacific community
  • 43. perspectives on cultural maintenance within New Zealand society. In New Zealand, there is widespread official government dogma and minority community perception that cultural maintenance is important to health outcomes and that culturally specific information for minority groups on which to base optimal policy and services is necessary. The untested assumption is that such an approach will lead to improved health and social outcomes for Pacific peoples. An alternative “popular hypothesis” in New Zealand would more likely support international perspectives and studies cited above that would expect more positive health outcomes for those effectively embedded in mainstream culture than for those embedded in Pacific culture or those marginalized from both cultures. This dominant cultural and official “cultural maintenance” viewpoint is politically persuasive in New Zealand and as a result became the focus of refutation or support in terms of our working hypothesis outlined as the second aim for this study presented below. Based on all these considerations, we applied Berry’s acculturation model to the relationships between acculturation and health, in this case operationalised as poor outcomes for maternal and Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 702 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(5) infant health risk factors. Thus, in the context of understanding the process and outcomes of acculturation strategies adopted by Pacific families, this study had two principal aims: namely, to (a) investigate the association between mother and infant health variables that might act as infant risk indicators and adaptation to living in New Zealand and (b) test the New Zealand view that strong cultural alignment to the original Pacific culture is associated with significantly better outcomes in terms of maternal and infant health risk factors and that weak cultural alignment is associated with significantly poorer outcomes in terms of maternal and infant health risk factors. For reasons outlined in the Method section, an abbreviated version of the General Ethnicity Questionnaire (GEQ; Tsai, Ying, & Lee, 2000)
  • 44. acculturation measurement instrument was employed. As a result, a secondary aim was to establish the validity and reliability of the modified instrument. Migration and Pacific People in Contemporary New Zealand Society To give a context to this study, it is necessary to describe the place played in New Zealand’s migration history by people of the Pacific Islands (as distinct from indigenous Maori descent) and their place in contemporary society. Polynesian settlement of the Pacific was completed around 1200-1300 AD when Te Ika o Maui (the mythical fish of Maui), the North Island of New Zealand, was the last Pacific archipelago to be discovered and settled by the ancient Polynesians (Prickett, 2001). These Polynesian ancestors became the New Zealand indigenous Maori. Major European settlement, and subsequent colonization, commenced from the late 18th century. Polynesian post-Maori contacts in the 18th and 19th centuries were limited, and at the 1945 New Zealand Census of Population and Dwellings, only about 2,000 people were recorded as being of Pacific origin. A second great wave of Polynesian migration took place in the relatively short period between the 1950s and 1980s, when Pacific peoples arrived from the islands of Samoa, Tonga, Cook Islands, Niue, Fiji, and the Tokelaus. This modern Polynesian migration was based principally on opportunity provided by largely economic imperatives in New Zealand (Macpherson, Spoonley, & Anae, 2001) or economic sustainability of small island groups such as the Tokelaus (Prior, Welby, Ostbye, Salmond, & Stokes, 1987; Salmond, Joseph, Prior, Stanley, & Wessen, 1985), supplemented more recently by matters relating to renewing or continuing links of kinship and family. Currently, Pacific peoples are a very significant and growing proportion of New Zealand’s population. More than 6% (231,801 people) in New Zealand were of Pacific ethnicity at the time of the 2001 Census (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2002a), and Pacific people are projected to make up more than 8% of the population by 2021 (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2005). The biggest concentration of Pacific people is in Auckland, New
  • 45. Zealand’s largest metropolitan area. Sixty percent of people of Pacific ethnicity were born in New Zealand; of those born overseas, 40% had arrived in New Zealand by 1981 and 30% between 1981 and 1990 (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2002a). This latest migration of Pacific people influences the nature of both New Zealand and the home island societies. For example, in the islands, it is significant in terms of reducing the overall population and in providing economic support to home communities by way of individual and family remittances to relatives. Table 1 illustrates the large proportion of Pacific people residing in New Zealand in relation to their respective home island populations. Since the migration wave of the late 20th century, Pacific people have actively participated in the New Zealand economy and society. In economic terms, Pacific people have relatively high labour force participation rates, particularly in the manufacturing sector. This sector has declined since the mid-1980s as a proportion of total employment but has been offset with Pacific people employment participation in the growing consumer service industries (such as hotels, restaurants, and retail) and the employment of younger people in more skilled technical and professional Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at Apollo Group - UOP on April 5, 2016 Borrows et al. 703 occupations (Statistics New Zealand—Te Tari Tatau, 2002b). However, people of Pacific ethnicities remain underrepresented in managerial and professional occupations yet overrepresented in trades and elementary occupations. Overall current labour force participation rates for people of Pacific ethnicities are at 62.9%, lower than the national rate of 68.5%, and unemployment rates are at 6.9%, higher than the national rate of 3.7% (Department of Labour—Te Tari Mahi, 2007). Maori rates for 2007 in labour force participation and unemployment are 67.6% and 7.6%, respectively. In terms of demography, Pacific people living in New Zealand have a relatively young age structure and a high fertility rate. While people of Pacific ethnicities currently have a lower life expectancy than the total population, it is higher