SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 70
Original Article
Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability?
Relationships
between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness
Alexander K. Hill a,1, Rodrigo A. Cárdenas b, John R.
Wheatley a, Lisa L.M. Welling a,2, Robert P. Burriss a,3,
Peter Claes c, Coren L. Apicella d, Michael A. McDaniel e,
Anthony C. Little f, Mark D. Shriver a, David A. Puts a,g,⁎
a Department of Anthropology, The Pennsylvania State
University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
b Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State
University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
c KU Leuven, ESAT/PSI—UZ Leuven, MIRC—iMinds, Medical
IT Department, Belgium
d Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania,
Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
e Department of Management, Virginia Commonwealth
University, Richmond, VA 23284, USA
f Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
g Center for Brain, Behavior, and Cognition, The Pennsylvania
State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
a b s t r a c ta r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Initial receipt 26 November 2015
Final revision received 25 October 2016
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
3D images
Fluctuating asymmetry
Geometric morphometrics
Hunter-gatherers
Mate choice
Sexual selection
Fluctuating asymmetry (FA), deviation from perfect bilateral
symmetry, is thought to reflect an organism's rela-
tive inability to maintain stable morphological development in
the face of environmental and genetic stressors.
Previous research has documented negative relationships
between FA and attractiveness judgments in humans,
but scant research has explored relationships between the human
voice and this putative marker of genetic qual-
ity in either sex. Only one study (and in women only) has
explored relationships between vocal attractiveness
and asymmetry of the face, a feature-rich trait space central in
prior work on human genetic quality and mate
choice. We therefore examined this relationship in three studies
comprising 231 men and 240 women from
two Western samples as well as Hadza hunter–gatherers of
Tanzania. Voice recordings were collected and
rated for attractiveness, and FA was computed from two-
dimensional facial images as well as, for a subset of
men, three-dimensional facial scans. Through meta-analysis of
our results and those of prior studies, we found
a negative association between FA and vocal attractiveness that
was highly robust and statistically significant
whether we included effect sizes from previously published
work, or only those from the present research,
and regardless of the inclusion of any individual sample or
method of assessing FA (e.g., facial or limb FA).
Weighted mean correlations between FA and vocal
attractiveness across studies were −.23 for men and −.29
for women. This research thus offers strong support for the
hypothesis that voices provide cues to genetic quality
in humans.
© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Mate choice is predicated, in part, on heritable fitness benefits
(Petrie, 1994), and organisms are attentive to biomarkers that
putative-
ly indicate whether a potential mate possesses genes that would
con-
tribute additively to offspring fitness (Andersson, 1994)—that
is,
whether a mate is high in genetic quality (Hunt, Bussiere,
Jennions, &
Brooks, 2004). One such biomarker is fluctuating asymmetry
(FA;
e.g., Møller, 1992; Swaddle, 1996; Thornhill & Gangestad,
1999a; Van
Valen, 1962, 1973). FA is exhibited in bilaterally symmetrical
traits if
the difference between the left and right sides has a population
normal
distribution with a mean of zero (Palmer & Strobeck, 1986).
Substantial
evidence from humans (e.g., Gangestad, Thornhill, & Yeo,
1994; Livshits
& Kobyliansky, 1991) as well as an array of vertebrates and
insects
(Parsons, 1990) indicates that the degree to which bilaterally
symmet-
rical traits deviate from perfect symmetry reflects
developmental insta-
bility resulting from harmful mutations and environmental
perturbations. Experimental exposure to common environmental
stressors,
for example, can increase asymmetry in laboratory rats (e.g.,
Mooney,
Siegel, & Gest, 1985; Sciulli, Doyle, Kelley, Siegel, & Siegel,
1979).
In humans, individuals with known genetic or chromosomal
abnor-
malities have greater dental FA than do controls (e.g., Barden,
1980;
Peretz et al., 1988), and FA displays a negative relationship
with genetic
heterozygosity (Livshits & Kobyliansky, 1991), which can
increase fitness
by lowering the risk of expressing deleterious recessive alleles
and by in-
creasing acquired immunity through the major
histocompatibility
Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx
⁎ Corresponding author. Department of Anthropology, The
Pennsylvania State Universi-
ty, 218 Carpenter Building, University Park, PA 16802, USA.
Tel.: +1 814 867 0453.
E-mail address: [email protected] (D.A. Puts).
1 Present address: Department of Anthropology, University of
Washington, Seattle, WA
98195, USA.
2 Present address: Department of Psychology, Oakland
University, Rochester, MI 48309,
USA.
3 Present address: Department of Psychology, University of
Basel, Basel, Switzerland.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
1090-5138/© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Evolution and Human Behavior
journal homepage: www.ehbonline.org
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
complex (MHC; reviewed in Kempenaers, 2007). Lower FA in
ear height,
wrist breadth, and digit length has predicted reduced resting
metabolic
rate (Manning, Koukourakis & Brodie, 1997) and better mental
health
(Martin, Manning, & Dowrick, 1999) in men, and lower facial
FA shows
a similar relationship with perceptions of physical health (Jones
et al.,
2001). Moreover, FA is positively correlated with female body
mass, age,
and age at first childbirth, all indications of reduced fecundity
(Manning,
Scutt, Whitehouse & Leinster, 1997). A recent meta-analysis
also found
a small negative relationship between FA and general
intelligence
(Banks, Batchelor, & McDaniel, 2010). Together, these results
suggest
that bilateral symmetry may reflect the fit between an
organism's geno-
type and environment during development.
As a putative indicator of a genotype's resilience to
environmental
insults, FA is related to mate choice across a variety of
nonhuman spe-
cies. For example, among field crickets (Gryllus campestris),
males suc-
cessful in obtaining mates display lower FA than do less
successful
males (Simmons, 1995). Likewise, female zebra finches
(Taeniopygia
guttata) prefer to be near males wearing symmetrically colored
experi-
mental leg bands (Swaddle, 1996).
In humans, several studies have reported significant correlations
be-
tween FA and perceptions of facial attractiveness (e.g., Abend,
Pflüger,
Koppensteiner, Coquerelle, & Grammer, 2015; Grammer &
Thornhill,
1994; Komori, Kawamura, & Ishihara, 2009; Perrett et al.,
1999; Rhodes
et al., 2001). In addition, women who view themselves as
attractive
show a greater preference for symmetry in male faces (Little,
Burt,
Penton-Voak, & Perrett, 2001), suggesting that symmetrical
mates are
valued and are more attainable by women of higher mate value.
Some
studies (e.g., Little & Jones, 2012; Little, Jones, Burt & Perrett,
2007),
but not others (e.g., Peters, Simmons, & Rhodes, 2009), have
found
that women in the fertile phase of the ovulatory cycle exhibit a
greater
preference for symmetrical faces and bodies, again suggesting
an associ-
ation between symmetry and genetic quality, since genetic
benefits can
be obtained only when conception is possible. There is also
evidence
that humans mate assortatively for facial symmetry, indicating
that
symmetry or correlated traits may influence mate choice
(Burriss,
Roberts, Welling, Puts, & Little, 2011).
While bilateral symmetry appears to be important in predicting
mating decisions, preferences for symmetrical mates may not
always
reflect preferences for symmetry per se. Bilateral symmetry has
been
found to predict attractiveness even in the absence of visual
cues to
asymmetry itself, and thus asymmetry must be related to other
pheno-
typic cues. For example, women in the fertile phase of the
ovulatory
cycle have shown a preference for symmetrical men when
exposed ex-
clusively to the men's body odor (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998;
Thornhill & Gangestad, 1999b). Another study found a female
prefer-
ence for low FA in men even when study participants were
shown
only the left or right half of each male face, preventing them
from
assessing symmetry directly (Scheib, Gangestad, & Thornhill,
1999).
Developmental stability may also be conveyed vocally. Hughes,
Harrison, and Gallup (2002) found both sexes to exhibit a
negative rela-
tionship between upper limb FA (particularly finger length FA)
and at-
tractiveness assessed from vocal stimuli alone. Abend et al.
(2015)
similarly obtained a negative relationship between women's
vocal at-
tractiveness and measures of FA from their upper and lower
limbs as
well as the head and face. These studies join other research
demonstrat-
ing the salience of voice as an influence on mate choice in
humans
(reviewed in Feinberg, 2008; Pisanski & Feinberg, 2013; Puts,
Jones, &
DeBruine, 2012).
Moreover, vocal and facial attractiveness may covary (e.g.,
Saxton,
Burriss, Murray, Rowland, & Roberts, 2009) in part because
both the
face and vocal folds are bilaterally symmetrical, and both may
therefore
reflect deleterious environmental influences during
development. For
example, asymmetric shape, size, or tension in the vocal folds
could re-
sult in audible changes in the voice, such as hoarseness
(Eysholdt,
Rosanowski, & Hoppe, 2003), and indeed, Hughes, Pastizzo,
and Gallup
(2008) found that limb FA in men predicted a measure of vocal
hoarseness. Developmental instability may also affect the size
and
shape of vocal structures, as well as tension on the vocal folds
or the po-
sitioning of the larynx in the vocal tract, which would influence
vocal
pitch and timbre, respectively (for fuller consideration of
potential
links between FA and vocal attractiveness, please see
Discussion).
Some research indicates that women's vocal attractiveness is
related
to fertility and reproductive potential (Bryant & Haselton, 2009;
Pipitone & Gallup, 2008, 2011; Puts et al., 2013; Wheatley et
al., 2014),
though scant research has explored relationships between vocal
attrac-
tiveness and FA in either sex. To our knowledge, Abend et al.
(2015) is
the only study conducted to date investigating relationships
between
vocal attractiveness and facial FA. Furthermore, not all research
on FA
supports the prediction that individuals will tend to prefer mates
lower in FA. For example, some research on humans (e.g.,
Langlois,
Roggman, & Musselman, 1994; Swaddle & Cuthill, 1995), as
well as non-
human species (e.g., Oakes & Barnard, 1994; Tomkins &
Simmons,
1998), has failed to show preferences for mates with low FA,
while
other research has found a statistically significant relationship
between
FA and attractiveness only in particular situations (Springer et
al., 2007).
There are likely several reasons for these discrepancies. First,
FA is a
weak index of developmental instability, such that associations
be-
tween FA and measures of mate quality are difficult to detect
using
modest sample sizes (Van Dongen & Gangestad, 2011). Second,
rela-
tionships with FA may be complicated by directional asymmetry
(DA),
which characterizes traits for which the difference between the
left
and right sides has a population normal distribution with a mean
that
deviates from zero (Møller, 1994). Directional asymmetry, such
as oc-
curs in testes size in many bird species (Møller, 1994) and in
the
lower limbs of humans (Auerbach & Ruff, 2006), may constitute
part
of an organism's normal development (Graham, Emlen,
Freeman,
Leamy, & Kieser, 1998; Martin, Puts, & Breedlove, 2008).
Since the pres-
ence of DA may complicate measures of FA, it is critical to take
DA into
consideration in research examining FA. While some studies
have
done so (e.g., Abend et al., 2015; Brown et al., 2008; Manning,
1995),
others have not (e.g., Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997; Hughes et
al.,
2002; Thornhill, Gangestad, & Comer, 1995). Finally, prior
research has
disproportionately sampled from Western societies, especially
individ-
uals of European genetic ancestry. Not only do such individuals
differ
in facial morphology from those of other populations (Farkas,
Katic, &
Forrest, 2005), but Westerners may also have greater access to
modern
medical and cosmetic technologies that can buffer them from
environ-
mental stressors, weakening relationships between FA and
measures
of attractiveness (Apicella, 2011).
In the present paper, we tested whether somatic FA predicts
vocal
attractiveness by collecting data in three studies that were
designed in
part to address the limitations of prior research while enabling
us to ex-
plore the robustness of these relationships across methodologies
and
populations. We measured FA from the face due to the
centrality of fa-
cial symmetry in prior work as well as the status of the face as a
feature-rich space that is developmentally distinct from the
upper
limb, measured by Hughes et al. (2002) and the upper and lower
limb
included in Abend et al. (2015). In Studies 1 and 2, we
measured FA
from two-dimensional (2D) facial photographs, following most
prior
studies of facial asymmetry and attractiveness (Jones et al.,
2001).
Study 1 data were collected from U.S. undergraduate students,
whereas
Study 2 data were collected from the Hadza, a Tanzanian
forager popu-
lation less protected by modern medicine from environmental
insults
than are samples drawn from the West. Compared to Western
samples,
Hadza facial and vocal characteristics should thus more strongly
reflect
individuals' constitutional resilience to environmental stressors,
such as
pathogens. Finally, in Study 3, we explored facial FA and vocal
attrac-
tiveness among U.S. undergraduates using the 2D methods
employed
in the first two studies as well as three-dimensional (3D) images
mea-
sured via spatially dense geometric morphometrics, which
provides
far greater shape information than does 2D imagery (Abend et
al.,
2015; Brown et al., 2008).
2 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
2. Study 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
One hundred twenty-four male (mean age = 20.08 ± 1.76 years)
and 201 female (mean age = 19.97 ± 1.55 years) students from
Mich-
igan State University participated as part of a larger study of
siblings
(see, e.g., Puts et al., 2013; Wheatley et al., 2014). If same-sex
siblings
participated, we randomly selected one from each sibling group
(gener-
ally pairs). In addition, only students self-identifying as
“White” were
used, as facial morphology varies by ethnicity (e.g., Farkas et
al., 2005),
and we did not have sufficient numbers of non-White
participants to
permit a meaningful analysis stratified by ancestry group. Both
male
and female participants provided voice recordings and 2D facial
photo-
graphs at two data collection sessions scheduled one week
apart, with
all procedures approved by the Michigan State University
institutional
review board.
2.1.2. Face measurement
Two-dimensional facial photographs were taken of participants
at
each session, with only photographs from session 1 used unless
there
was no session 1 photograph for a participant, in which case the
session
2 photograph was used (the methodology followed to take these
photo-
graphs was identical between the sessions). Participants were
instructed
to assume a neutral expression after having removed any
jewelry, eye-
glasses, or makeup (with wet wipes provided at the time of data
collec-
tion). While not all male participants were clean-shaven, care
was taken
to ensure high-quality facial landmarks for all photographs (see
below).
Facial photographs were taken with a tripod-mounted Canon
PowerShot
S10 digital camera at a distance of approximately 1 m, a height
adjusted to
the participant, and using constant lighting across participants.
All facial
images were normalized on interpupillary distance and rotated
so that
both pupils lay on the same horizontal plane.
Each facial photograph was landmarked by two research
assistants
(RAs) with 14 x,y-coordinates in the software ImageJ, after
which land-
mark coordinates were averaged between RAs. Horizontal
asymmetry
measurements were obtained from landmarks as in Grammer and
Thornhill (1994) by first determining midpoints for six pairs of
bilateral
landmarks (Fig. 1). If a face is perfectly symmetrical about the
midsagittal
plane, then all midpoints will fall on the vertical line
representing this
plane, and the horizontal differences between these midpoints
will be
zero. Thus, to measure horizontal asymmetry, horizontal (i.e.,
x-
coordinate) distances between midpoints were computed for all
15 non-
redundant pairs of midpoints and then summed. Vertical
asymmetry was
computed from the sum of the vertical (i.e., y-coordinate)
distances be-
tween the 6 landmark pairs (Scheib et al., 1999). As in Scheib et
al.
(1999), the horizontal and vertical asymmetry measures were
finally
summed to produce an overall measure of facial asymmetry.
Inter-measurer reliability was assessed by calculating for each
facial
distance relative error magnitude (REM) values, which
incorporate
measurement size so as to arrive at a more precise assessment of
consis-
tency (Weinberg, Scott, Neiswanger, Brandon, & Marazita,
2004). The
mean for the x-coordinates was 1.67 (SD = 1.08) and the mean
for
the y-coordinates was 1.21 (SD = .74), indicating strong
agreement.
DA was calculated for each sex and each facial trait separately
and
then used to correct for DA when calculating individual FA
values. DA
for each facial trait j was calculated as
DAj ¼ ∑
n
i¼1
Lij−Rij
Lij þ Rij
! "
=2
2
4
3
5=n
where L and R represent the left and right sides of the trait for
each par-
ticipant i, and n represents the number of participants for whom
data
were available. Thus, for each trait in each sex, DA was the
average
signed left–right difference in distance across participants
expressed
as a proportion of the size of the trait. We were then able, in
accordance
with methods used by Gangestad et al. (1994) and Abend et al.
(2015),
to calculate FA for each trait j in each individual i as
FAij ¼
Lij−Rij
Lij þ Rij
! "
=2
2
4
3
5−DA j
######
######
FA was computed exclusively from front-on photographs (Table
S1,
available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org), and
we elim-
inated the facial image of one male participant whose facial FA
exceeded
the mean by 4.0 SD, possibly due to head tilt. See Palmer and
Strobeck
(1986) for a discussion of variation in methods of calculating
FA.
2.1.3. Voice recording
ParticipantswererecordedspeakingthefirstsixsentencesoftheRain
bow
Passage (Fairbanks, 1960) into a Shure SM58 vocal cardioid
microphone in a
quiet recording booth. A curved wire projecting from the
microphone stand
kept each participant's mouth approximately 9.5 cm from the
microphone.
Voices were recorded via a Sound Devices USBPre 2
preamplifier onto a com-
puter using GoldWave software in mono at a sampling rate of
44,100 Hz and
16-bit quantization, and saved as uncompressed WAV files.
2.1.4. Vocal and facial attractiveness ratings
Unfamiliar members of the opposite sex (n = 569, mean age =
19.4 years for male raters; n = 558, mean age = 19.1 years for
female
raters) recruited from The Pennsylvania State University rated
voice re-
cordings and facial images on attractiveness for a short-term
relation-
ship made on a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all
attractive,
7 = very attractive). This portion of the study was approved by
The
Pennsylvania State University's institutional review board.
Raters first
read the definition of a short-term relationship given by Little,
Cohen,
Jones and Belsky (2007, p. 970): “You are looking for the type
of person
who would be attractive in a short-term relationship. This
implies that
Fig. 1. Landmarks used to compute horizontal and vertical
facial asymmetry from two-
dimensional images. Left and right pupils were landmarked to
standardize interpupillary
distance and register faces horizontally.
3A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
the relationship may not last a long time. Examples of this type
of rela-
tionship would include a single date accepted on the spur of the
mo-
ment, an affair within a long-term relationship, and possibility
of a
one-night stand.” We chose to use ratings of short-term
attractiveness
made by members of the opposite sex because short-term
relationships
represent the mating context in which genetic quality is most
salient
relative to other mate choice criteria, such as investment
(Kenrick,
Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990). At private workstations, each
rater then
assessed stimulus sets comprising approximately 25 facial
photographs
presented on a computer screen and 25 recordings of the first
sentence
of the Rainbow Passage presented through Sennheiser HD 280
Profes-
sional headphones. Recordings and photographs were randomly
allo-
cated to a set, each of which was rated by at least 15 raters
(mean =
18.9). The order in which participants completed the rating
tasks
(faces or voices first) was random across participants, as was
the
order in which stimuli were presented within each task. We
calculated
mean short-term sexual attractiveness scores for each
participant using
the first 15 ratings per stimulus (Table S1, available on the
journal's
website at www.ehbonline.org).
2.2. Results
In both men (n = 124, r = −.21, p = .017) and women (n = 201,
r = −.24, p = .001), there was a significant negative relationship
be-
tween vocal attractiveness and facial FA (Tables 1 and 3, Fig.
2). We
also explored the possibility that the relationship between facial
FA
and vocal attractiveness could be attributable to correlations
between
(1) vocal and facial attractiveness (men: n = 118, r = .04, p =
.685;
women: n = 189, r = .21, p = .005) and (2) facial attractiveness
and fa-
cial FA (men: n = 118, r = −.04, p = .708; women: n = 189, r =
−.15,
p = .043). Attractiveness is an important mediator of mating
success
(Apicella & Feinberg, 2009; Rhodes, Simmons, & Peters, 2005),
and facial
attractiveness has also correlated with both symmetry (Grammer
&
Thornhill, 1994; Perrett et al., 1999) and vocal attractiveness
(Abend
et al., 2015; Collins & Missing, 2003; Lander, 2008; Wheatley
et al.,
2014). Hence, it is possible that facial symmetry might increase
vocal at-
tractiveness indirectly by increasing facial attractiveness. For
instance,
people with more symmetrical and thus more attractive faces
may conse-
quently feel more confident and therefore speak more
attractively. How-
ever, after controlling for facial attractiveness in multiple
regression
models predicting vocal attractiveness, we found that the
relationships
between facial FA and vocal attractiveness remained in both
men
(β = −.23, p = .014; model: F(2115) = 3.23, R = .23, p = .043)
and
women (β = −.25, p b .001; model: F(2186) = 10.75, R = .32, p
b .0001).
3. Study 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
Twenty-eight male (mean age = 29.32 ± 5.84 years) and 39
female
(mean age = 29.03 ± 6.37 years) participants were recruited
from the ap-
proximately 1000 Hadza hunter–gatherers living in remote
savannah–
woodland habitats of Northern Tanzania. The Hadza have a
sexual division
of labor in which women collect fruits and nuts and dig for
tubers, while
men collect honey and hunt animals (Marlowe, 2004). The
Hadza are
among an increasingly small number of human populations still
subsisting
by hunting and gathering and are thought to possess a social
organization
similar to that exhibited by early anatomically modern humans.
For a de-
scription of the Hadza and hunter–gatherers in general, see
Apicella and
Crittenden (2016).
Adult participants were recruited from eight Hadza camps
visited
opportunistically in the summer of 2006. Only individuals
between
the ages of 18 and 40 were used in the present study, and one
male
and one female were removed from the dataset because they
possessed
facial FA more than 3.2 SD from the mean FA for their sex. The
research-
er located the first Hadza camp by driving in the area of
Hadzaland, with
subsequent camps selected in a snowball sampling fashion based
on in-
formation provided by participants about the proximity of the
next clos-
est camp. Verbal consent was obtained for all participants, and
the study
was approved by Harvard University's ethics internal review
board.
3.1.2. Face measurement
Adult male and female participants had their faces photographed
out-
side during the day with a Sony Cyber-shot 18 megapixel digital
camera.
Participants were asked to maintain a neutral expression and
look directly
into the camera. Horizontal asymmetry was estimated from x,y-
coordinates of 6 bilateral points made by a single measurer
following tech-
niques used in previous studies (Grammer & Thornhill, 1994;
Penton-Voak
et al., 2001; Scheib et al., 1999). Symmetry was computed by
taking left and
right deviation from the midline, calculated from interpupillary
distance,
and then summing the absolute value of the individual scores
(Table S1,
available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org). DA
was calculat-
ed in the manner used in Study 1 and subtracted from the FA
measures. For
women, the resulting values were natural log-transformed to
correct skew.
3.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings
Voice recordings were made privately inside a Land Rover
vehicle.
Participants were asked to speak the Swahili word “hujambo,”
which
loosely translates to “hello,” into a Sennheiser MKH-60
microphone. Re-
cordings were encoded in mono directly onto a computer hard
disk
using Sonic Foundry's Sound Forge at 44,100 Hz sampling rate
and 16-
bit quantization, and saved as uncompressed WAV files. The
recordings
were used as stimuli for vocal attractiveness ratings, which, as
in Study
1, were made on a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all
attractive,
7 = very attractive) by unfamiliar members of the opposite sex
(n = 29,
mean age = 19.38 years for male raters; n = 26, mean age =
20.38 years for female raters) recruited from The Pennsylvania
State
University (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www.
ehbonline.org). Raters listened to all opposite-sex recordings
through
Sennheiser HD 280 Professional headphones, and the order in
which
stimuli were presented was randomized for each rater.
3.2. Results
In contrast with the results of Study 1, we found no significant
rela-
tionship between vocal attractiveness and facial FA in either
men (n =
28, r = −.16, p = .431; Table 1) or women (n = 39, r = −.08, p =
.626;
Table 3), though the sample size of Hadza participants was
much small-
er, and in both sexes the non-significant correlation coefficients
were in
the same direction as in Study 1.
4. Study 3
4.1. Methods
4.1.1. Participants
Seventy-nine self-identified White men (mean age = 19.92 ±
1.51)
from The Pennsylvania State University participated as part of
two
Table 1
Meta-analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in men.
Study name r Lower limit Upper limit z p n
Study 1 −0.21 −0.38 −0.04 −2.39 0.017 124
Study 2 −0.16 −0.50 0.23 −0.78 0.435 28
Study 3 (2D facial FA) −0.10 −0.32 0.12 −0.89 0.372 79
Study 3 (3D facial FA) −0.17 −0.42 0.11 −1.20 0.229 52
Hughes et al. (2002) −0.36 −0.58 −0.10 −2.62 0.009 50
Overall1 −0.21 −0.32 −0.09 −3.41 0.001 281
Overall2 −0.23 −0.35 −0.11 −3.64 b0.001 254
1 Using 2D facial FA from Study 3.
2 Using 3D facial FA from Study 3.
4 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
broader studies, one on long-term romantic relationships
(supplemen-
tary online material 2, sample A), the other on interpersonal
dynamics
(supplementary online material 2, sample B). Data-collection
proce-
dures were approved by the University's institutional review
board.
4.1.2. Face measurement
Consistent with methods used in Studies 1 and 2, two-
dimensional
facial photographs were taken using an Olympus Evolt E-300
digital
camera. Participants sat in front of a light-colored background;
removed
any facial jewelry, eyeglasses, or hats; used a headband to
remove any
hair obstructing the face; and looked directly into the camera
lens
while maintaining a neutral facial expression. The resultant
photo-
graphs were used to obtain facial FA per the methods of Study 1
(Table S1, available on the journal's website at
www.ehbonline.org).
Also as in Study 1, inter-measurer reliability was assessed by
calculating
REM values (Weinberg et al., 2004) for each facial distance.
The mean
for the x-coordinates was .87 (SD = 1.05), and the mean for the
y-
coordinates was .79 (SD = .67), indicating strong agreement.
In addition to standard two-dimensional photographs, three-
dimensional facial images were collected for a subset of
participants
(n = 52, mean age = 19.87 ± 1.76) using the 3dMD face system
(3dMD, Atlanta, GA). Participants were asked to close their
mouths
and maintain a neutral facial expression while providing their
images,
which were then exported from the 3dMD Patient software in
OBJ file
format and imported into a scan-cleaning program for cropping
and
trimming, as well as removal of hair, ears, and any dissociated
polygons.
We subsequently partitioned facial shape into patterns of
symmetry
and asymmetry following the work of Claes, Walters and
Clement
(2012). First, an anthropometric mask was non-rigidly mapped
onto
the original 3D facial scans, as well as onto the reflected scan
representing the mirror image of the original (Fig. 3), which
was con-
structed by changing the sign of the x-coordinate (Claes, 2007;
Claes,
Walters, & Clement, 2012; Claes, Walters, Vandermeulen, &
Clement,
2011; Klingenberg & McIntyre, 1998; Mardia, Bookstein, &
Moreton,
2000). This established homologous spatially dense quasi-
landmark
configurations for all original and reflected 3D images (Claes et
al.,
2011). Secondly, following Mardia et al. (2000), a generalized
Procrus-
tes superimposition was performed (Rohlf & Slice, 1990),
eliminating
differences in position, orientation, and scale between the
original and
reflected configurations. Finally, the overall consensus
configuration
was perfectly symmetrical, and a single shape could be
decomposed
into its asymmetrical and bilaterally symmetrical parts (Mardia
et al.,
2000). The average of an original image and its reflected
configuration
constitutes the symmetrical component, whereas the difference
be-
tween both configurations constitutes the asymmetrical
component
(Kimmerle & Jantz, 2005; Klingenberg, Barluenga, & Meyer,
2002;
Fig. 3). Because the consensus configuration is, by
construction, sym-
metrical with respect to a plane, it is used to analyze variation
of
Fig. 2. Relationship between vocal attractiveness and facial FA
among (a) men (n = 124, r = −.21, p = .017) and (b) women (n =
201, r = −.24, p = .001) from Study 1.
Fig. 3. Overall asymmetry is calculated as the Procrustes
distance between original and reflected images of each
individual. The heatmap (third image from left) shows the
regional
variation in asymmetry with red color highlighting regions with
higher levels of asymmetry and blue color highlighting regions
with lower levels of asymmetry. The reader is
encouraged to consult Claes, Walters, Shriver, et al. (2012) for
a more detailed description.
5A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
symmetry in populations and provides an estimate of the
midsagittal
plane with a frame of reference that has clear anatomical
meaning
(Klingenberg et al., 2002). As a result, Procrustes distances in
the tan-
gent shape space between mapped 3D images and their
reflections
after superimposition were used as measures of facial
asymmetry. Be-
cause the asymmetry shape-space is centered around the average
asymmetry in the sample (i.e., the average asymmetry is
subtracted
from the asymmetry measurements), the DA component was
eliminat-
ed in computing facial asymmetry.
4.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings
Voice recordings were made from the first six sentences of the
Rain-
bow Passage (Fairbanks, 1960), collected according to the
protocol used
in Study 1. Vocal attractiveness was assessed by 167 female
students
(mean age = 18.80 ± .95 years) recruited from The Pennsylvania
State University (Table S1, available on the journal's website at
www.
ehbonline.org), who listened to only the first sentence of the
Rainbow
Passage (to avoid fatigue) on Sennheiser HD 280 Professional
head-
phones. Voice stimuli were split randomly into two sets so that
each lis-
tener rated half of the stimuli, but all stimuli were evaluated an
equal
number of times. Presentation order of stimuli was randomized
for
each listener. Recordings from sample A were rated on
attractiveness
using a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = very unattractive, 7 =
very at-
tractive). Recordings from sample B were rated on how
attractive the
men are for both a “short-term, purely sexual relationship, such
as a
one-night stand” and a “long-term, committed relationship, such
as
marriage,” using a 10-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all,
10 =
more than anyone else I know), and these scores were averaged
to pro-
duce a measure of overall vocal attractiveness. Ratings were
standard-
ized within each sample to place them on the same scale.
4.2. Results
As in Study 2, the correlations between vocal attractiveness and
FA
were in the predicted direction but were not statistically
significant
(2D: n = 79, r = −.10, p = .373; 3D Procrustes: n = 52, r = −.17,
p = .227; Table 1). Interestingly, measures of FA made from 2D
and
3D images were not significantly correlated (n = 52, r = −.08, p
=
.581), though this may be attributable to the appreciably larger
amount
of spatial information captured in 3D meshes, which, in theory,
repre-
sent a methodological improvement over 2D photographs (see
also Dis-
cussion). As a result, 3D images have been able to confirm the
oft-
replicated negative relationship between FA and attractiveness
in both
sexes obtained using 2D imagery (Brown et al., 2008). Despite
this,
however, the strength of the relationship between vocal
attractiveness
and FA in Study 3 did not differ between 2D and 3D
measurements
(n = 52, z = .19, p = .425).
5. Meta-analysis
In addition to examining independently the three studies
reported
here, we meta-analyzed the data across the studies, as well as
the re-
sults of Hughes et al. (2002) and Abend et al. (2015), to
produce sum-
mary estimates of the relationship between FA and vocal
attractiveness. Effect sizes were obtained for males from all
studies re-
ported herein plus Hughes et al. (2002), and for females from
Studies
1 and 2 as well as Hughes et al. (2002) and Abend et al. (2015).
This re-
sulted in total samples of 281 men (utilizing 2D facial FA
measurements
from Study 3) or 254 men (utilizing 3D facial FA measurements
from
Study 3), as well as 328 women.
Because of methodological differences in sampling, variable
mea-
surement (see Palmer & Strobeck, 1997 for a discussion of the
effect of
measurement error on FA), and data treatment across studies, a
random
effects model was used in the analyses, which allows for the
presence of
moderator variables (e.g., differences in the precision with
which FA in
different parts of the phenotype indexes developmental
instability).
Correlation coefficients between FA and vocal attractiveness
served as
the effect size. Variance estimates based on a small number of
studies
are at best speculative, and the accuracy of sampling variance
estimates,
though unbiased, improves with increasing numbers of samples.
Both
within-sample Q estimates and I2 estimates (Higgins,
Thompson,
Deeks, & Altman, 2003) suggest that all variances across
studies are
best attributed to random sampling error. Better estimates of
variance
across studies must await the accumulation of additional
research. The
data were analyzed in Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software
version
3 (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005), and males
and fe-
males were treated separately. This software is in the Hedges
and
Olkin (1985) tradition and performs calculations using the
Fisher r-to-
z transformation and then reports the results in the
untransformed cor-
relation coefficient metric. Moreover, in order to determine
whether the
removal of a study would affect the meta-analytic results, we
conducted
for each sex an “omit-one-study” analysis.
The point estimate for the population correlation between FA
and
vocal attractiveness in men was r = −.23 and −.21 using 3D and
2D fa-
cial FA from Study 3, respectively (Table 1, Fig. 4a). Excluding
individual
samples in the omit-one-study analysis resulted in effect sizes
ranging
from r = −.25 to r = −.20 using 3D facial FA from Study 3
(Table 2,
Fig. 4c), and r = −.24 to r = −.17 using 2D facial FA from Study
3
(Table S2, Fig. S1a, available on the journal's website at www.
ehbonline.org). In all analyses, the 95% CI excluded a
correlation of zero.
For women, the point estimate for the population correlation be-
tween FA and vocal attractiveness was r = −.28 and −.29 using
limb
FA and 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015), respectively
(Table 3,
Fig. 4b). Excluding individual samples in the omit-one-study
analysis
resulted in effect sizes ranging from r = −.32 to r = −.25 (Tables
4
and S3, Fig. S1b, available on the journal's website at
www.ehbonline.
org). In all analyses, the 95% CI excluded a correlation of zero.
We also
meta-analyzed only the two studies reported in the present paper
that
included women and obtained a population correlation of r =
−.21
(95% CI: −.33 to −.09, p b .001).
6. Discussion
In the present research, we found relationships between vocal
attrac-
tiveness and fluctuating asymmetry in both sexes. Though
analyses
should be re-conducted as more data accumulate, our meta-
analyses
gave point estimates for population correlations between vocal
attractive-
ness and FA of −.23 for men and −.29 for women, using 3D
facial FA from
Study 3 and Abend et al. (2015). All effect sizes across samples
were in the
same (negative) direction, although they ranged in magnitude,
with
greater deviations from the population estimate unsurprisingly
tending
to derive from smaller samples. Our estimate indicates that 146
male
and 91 female observations would be required to find a
statistically signif-
icant effect (two-tailed power analyses for α = .05 and β = .20).
As such,
conclusions based on the meta-analysis that cumulated across
studies are
likely to be more robust than conclusions drawn from under-
powered
primary studies. Importantly, the negative association between
FA and
vocal attractiveness was highly robust and manifest in every
meta-
analysis of the data. The correlation was statistically significant
regardless
of the inclusion of any individual study or sample, applied to
FA measured
from the face as well as from the body, and to both short-term
and overall
attractiveness, and was present whether we included effect sizes
from
previously published work or only those from the present
research.
Our findings thus provide important new evidence supporting
the
salience of the human voice in conveying information on mate
quality.
Furthermore, we are able to exclude several potential alternative
inter-
pretations. First, it is possible that behavioral tendencies may
spuriously
link vocal attractiveness with measurements of facial symmetry.
For ex-
ample, shyness might result in both a wavering, less attractive
voice, as
well as a tendency not to face the camera directly, which would
affect
symmetry measurements. However, in taking 2D images, we
were care-
ful to ensure that participants faced the camera fully,
subsequently
6 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
verifying this through visual inspection of the photographs and
removal
of those in which head turn was apparent. More importantly, the
rela-
tionship between vocal attractiveness and FA was in the
predicted di-
rection, albeit non-significantly in Study 3, when FA was
measured
from 3D imagery, which is not influenced by the spatial
orientation of
the head. A pertinent question is how developmental stability
can be
indexed by FA measured from both 2D and 3D images if the two
types
of measurements were not correlated in Study 3. It is possible
that,
while developmental instability influences FA in both two and
three di-
mensions, it does so differently such that the relationship
between the
two is not necessarily positive. For instance, increased
asymmetry in
the coronal plane, which is measured in 2D images, might be
related
to decreased asymmetry in the sagittal plane, which is measured
in
3D but not 2D images. That FA from both types of image
reliability
indexes developmental stability accords with results of Tigue,
Pisanski,
O'Connor, Fraccaro, and Feinberg (2012), who found that, while
3D im-
ages elicited more favorable assessments of attractiveness than
did 2D
images, attractiveness ratings from the two types of stimuli
were none-
theless correlated. This notwithstanding, we encourage future
re-
searchers to employ 3D imagery to capture appreciably more
spatial
information and to characterize more precisely subtle shape
variation
present in facial morphology.
Second, it is possible that developmental instability only
indirectly
influences vocal attractiveness by increasing confidence
associated
with having a symmetrical, attractive face. However, this does
not ap-
pear to be the case. In Study 1, we were able to statistically
control for
facial attractiveness and found that the relationships between
facial FA
and vocal attractiveness remained undiminished in both sexes
after
doing so. Moreover, these findings indicate that voices and
faces provide
partly unique information about mate quality, in that voices
provided
information about developmental instability that was not
encompassed
by facial attractiveness. Vocal and facial attractiveness
correlated mod-
erately in Study 1 women, suggesting that women's voices and
faces
provide partly redundant information about mate quality.
Previous re-
search has shown similar levels of agreement across these
domains in
women (e.g., Abend et al., 2015; Collins & Missing, 2003;
Feinberg
et al., 2005; Lander, 2008; Saxton et al., 2009; Wheatley et al.,
2014),
and auditory and visual cues considered in isolation may be less
Fig. 4. Correlations and 95% confidence intervals for
relationships between vocal attractiveness and facial FA.
Primary meta-analytic results are shown in panels (a) for men
and (b) for
women. Overall effects refer to results of random effect models.
In panel (a), superscripts 1 and 2 denote results using 2D and
3D facial FA, respectively, from Study 3. In panel (b),
superscripts 1 and 2 denote results using limb FA and 3D facial
FA, respectively, from Abend et al. (2015). Results of “omit-
one-study” analyses are illustrated for (c) men and
(d) women, with FA in Study 3 and Abend et al. (2015)
computed from 3D facial images.
Table 2
“Omit-one-study” analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in
men using 3D facial FA from
Study 3.
Study omitted r Lower limit Upper limit z p n
Study 1 −0.25 −0.40 −0.07 −2.75 0.006 130
Study 2 −0.24 −0.36 −0.11 −3.57 b0.001 226
Study 3 (3D facial FA) −0.24 −0.37 −0.11 −3.47 0.001 202
Hughes et al. (2002) −0.20 −0.33 −0.06 −2.77 0.006 204
Table 3
Meta-analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in women.
Study name r Lower limit Upper limit z p n
Study 1 −0.24 −0.36 −0.10 −3.40 0.001 201
Study 2 −0.08 −0.39 0.24 −0.48 0.630 39
Abend et al. (2015; limb FA) −0.44 −0.66 −0.16 −2.98 0.003 42
Abend et al. (2015; 3D facial FA) −0.45 −0.66 −0.16 −2.99
0.003 42
Hughes et al. (2002) −0.41 −0.62 −0.13 −2.84 0.005 46
Overall1 −0.28 −0.41 −0.14 −3.87 b0.001 328
Overall2 −0.29 −0.42 −0.14 −3.86 b0.001 328
1 Using limb FA from Abend et al. (2015).
2 Using 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015).
7A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
informative about mate quality than are stimuli combining these
and
other traits (Campanella & Belin, 2007). In Study 1 men, vocal
and facial
attractiveness did not correlate significantly. Although ours is
the larg-
est study of which we are aware to test this correlation in men,
our re-
sults corroborate most (e.g., Lander, 2008; Saxton et al., 2009;
Wells,
Baguley, Sergeant, & Dunn, 2013), but not all (Saxton, Caryl, &
Roberts,
2006; Skrinda et al., 2014), previous studies exploring this
correlation.
Further research is required to determine the extent to which
men's fa-
cial and vocal attractiveness correlate, which components of
mate qual-
ity may underlie any such correlation, and how these
relationships may
vary across samples.
Finally, our results provide further evidence that human
preferences
for symmetrical mates are not merely consequences of
symmetrical
stimuli being more easily processed by the visual system
(Enquist &
Johnstone, 1997). It is now becoming clear that people prefer
not only
the faces (Jones, DeBruine, & Little, 2007; Little & Jones,
2003; Scheib
et al., 1999) and odors (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Thornhill
&
Gangestad, 1999b) but also the voices (Abend et al., 2015;
Hughes
et al., 2002, 2008; present study) of mates with low levels of
FA, even
when symmetry cannot be directly assessed visually.
The present paper thus adds to an efflorescing literature
exploring
relationships between FA and attractiveness in humans. While
some
contributions to this literature have questioned relationships
between
FA and fitness-relevant outcomes (e.g., Clarke, 1998), a recent
meta-
analysis of 293 estimates of the relationship between FA and
measures
of health and quality taken from 94 studies on humans suggests
a robust
correlation (Van Dongen & Gangestad, 2011).
Much previous research on FA and mate preferences has
focused on
putative genetic benefits provided by symmetrical partners.
Heritability
estimates for FA vary across species and across traits within
species
(Leamy, 1997; Møller & Thornhill, 1997; Polak et al., 2003),
but it is pos-
sible that individuals derive non-genetic benefits from
symmetrical
mates, as well (Gangestad & Thornhill, 2013). For example, the
meta-
analysis of Van Dongen and Gangestad (2011) estimated
correlations
of 0.11 and 0.20 between FA and reduced intelligence and
schizophre-
nia/schizotypy, respectively, both of which may have influenced
invest-
ment capacity ancestrally. However, other research suggests
that
symmetrical males may allocate relatively greater reproductive
effort
to mating than to investment in mates or offspring (e.g.,
Gangestad &
Simpson, 2000; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997, 2013), suggesting
that ge-
netic benefits to offspring may have driven the evolution of
preferences
for low-FA partners, at least in women.
7. Future directions
Future studies should investigate how developmental instability
re-
lates to the anatomy of the vocal tract, and which vocal
anatomical char-
acteristics and acoustic features of the voice mediate
relationships
between somatic asymmetry and vocal attractiveness (see, for
example,
Hughes et al., 2008). While such an exploration is beyond the
scope of
the present paper, some informed speculation is possible. One
possibil-
ity is that developmental instability affects vocal attractiveness
by
influencing the symmetry of the vocal apparatus, for example
the
vocal folds or the shape of the supralaryngeal vocal tract.
Anatomical
asymmetries or asymmetric tension between the two vocal folds
will
cause the vocal folds to vibrate differently or at different rates,
produc-
ing perturbations in periodicity as well as incomplete closure
and
leading to perceptible vocal changes, such as hoarseness,
breathiness,
and roughness (Eysholdt et al., 2003). Facial asymmetries, such
as
those caused by cleft lip and palate, can affect aspects of
articulation
and resonance, as well. Because vocalization is integral to
human com-
munication, there may have been strong selection for the vocal
appara-
tus to be buffered against such perturbations, but research
suggests
pronounced asymmetries in the human vocal folds (e.g., Hirano,
Yukizane, Kurita, & Hibi, 1989). This is consistent with the
idea that
traits shaped by intersexual selection tend to display greater FA
than
do non-sexually selected traits (Møller & Hoglund, 1991).
Developmen-
tal instability may influence other aspects of the size and shape
of vocal
structures (e.g., the length, thickness, or density of the vocal
folds), as
well as neurological characteristics that could affect vocal
behavior,
such as tension on the vocal folds or the positioning of the
larynx in
the vocal tract, which would influence vocal pitch and timbre,
respec-
tively. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of vocal anatomy
during
phonation, precise acoustic measurements, and large samples
may be
particularly useful in exploring these possibilities.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Mental
Health
[grant number T32 MH70343-05], the National Science
Foundation
[REG grant to C.L.A. and GRFP grant to J.R.W.], and The
Pennsylvania
State University and Harvard University.
Supplementary materials
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008.
Acknowledgments
We thank Arslan Zaidi for assistance designing Fig. 3, Tobias
Kordsmeyer for insights on measuring facial FA, Matt Winn for
input
on the Discussion, and Lisa DeBruine and two anonymous
reviewers
for comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.
References
Abend, P., Pflüger, L. S., Koppensteiner, M., Coquerelle, M., &
Grammer, K. (2015). The
sound of female shape: a redundant signal of vocal and facial
attractiveness.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 174–181.
Andersson, M. B. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Apicella, C. L. (2011). On the universality of attractiveness. In
M. Brockman (Ed.), Future
science: essays from the cutting edge (pp. 88–100). New York,
NY: Oxford University
Press.
Apicella, C. L., & Crittenden, A. (2016). hunter–gatherer
families and parenting. In D. M.
Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd
ed.)Vol. 1. (pp. 578–597).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Apicella, C. L., & Feinberg, D. R. (2009). Voice pitch alters
mate-choice-relevant perception
in hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences,
276(1659), 1077–1082.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1542.
Auerbach, B. M., & Ruff, C. B. (2006). Limb bone bilateral
asymmetry: Variability and com-
monality among modern humans. Journal of Human Evolution,
50(2), 203–218.
Banks, G. C., Batchelor, J. H., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010).
Smarter people are (a bit) more
symmetrical: A meta-analysis of the relationship between
intelligence and fluctuat-
ing asymmetry. Intelligence, 38(4), 393–401.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.
04.003.
Barden, H. S. (1980). Fluctuating dental asymmetry: A measure
of developmental insta-
bility in Down syndrome. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 52(2), 169–173.
Table 4
“Omit-one-study” analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in
women using 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015).
Study omitted r Lower limit Upper limit z p n
Study 1 −0.32 −0.52 −0.10 −2.74 0.006 127
Study 2 −0.32 −0.45 −0.17 −4.22 b0.001 289
Abend et al. (2015; 3D facial FA) −0.25 −0.38 −0.11 −3.38
0.001 286
Hughes et al. (2002) −0.26 −0.41 −0.09 −2.94 0.003 282
8 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0010
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1542
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0030
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.003
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0040
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0040
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005).
Comprehensive meta-analysis
version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat, 104.
Brown, W. M., Price, M. E., Kang, J., Pound, N., Zhao, Y., &
Yu, H. (2008). Fluctuating asym-
metry and preferences for sex-typical bodily characteristics.
Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
105(35), 12938–12943.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710420105.
Bryant, G. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2009). Vocal cues of
ovulation in human females. Biology
Letters, 5(1), 12–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0507.
Burriss, R. P., Roberts, S. C., Welling, L. L., Puts, D. A., &
Little, A. C. (2011). Heterosexual ro-
mantic couples mate assortatively for facial symmetry, but not
masculinity.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(5), 601–613.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0146167211399584.
Campanella, S., & Belin, P. (2007). Integrating face and voice
in person perception. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 535–543.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.001.
Claes, P. (2007). A robust statistical surface registration
framework using implicit function
representations—Application in craniofacial reconstruction.
Claes, P., Walters, M., & Clement, J. (2012b). Improved facial
outcome assessment using a
3D anthropometric mask. International Journal of Oral and
Maxillofacial Surgery, 41(3),
324–330.
Claes, P., Walters, M., Shriver, M. D., Puts, D., Gibson, G.,
Clement, J., ... Suetens, P. (2012a). Sex-
ual dimorphism in multiple aspects of 3D facial symmetry and
asymmetry defined by
spatially dense geometric morphometrics. Journal of Anatomy,
221(2), 97–114.
Claes, P., Walters, M., Vandermeulen, D., & Clement, J. G.
(2011). Spatially-dense 3D facial
asymmetry assessment in both typical and disordered growth.
Journal of Anatomy,
219(4), 444–455.
Clarke, G. M. (1998). Developmental stability and fitness: The
evidence is not quite so
clear. The American Naturalist, 152(5), 762–766.
Collins, S. A., & Missing, C. (2003). Vocal and visual
attractiveness are related in women.
Animal Behaviour, 65(5), 997–1004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2123.
Enquist, M., & Johnstone, R. A. (1997). Generalization and the
evolution of symmetry pref-
erences. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B:
Biological Sciences, 264(1386),
1345–1348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0186.
Eysholdt, U., Rosanowski, F., & Hoppe, U. (2003). Vocal fold
vibration irregularities caused
by different types of laryngeal asymmetry. European Archives
of Oto-Rhino-
Laryngology, 260(8), 412–417.
Fairbanks, G. (1960). Voice and articulation handbook. New
York: Harper.
Farkas, L. G., Katic, M. J., & Forrest, C. R. (2005).
International anthropometric study of fa-
cial morphology in various ethnic groups/races. Journal of
Craniofacial Surgery, 16(4),
615–646.
Feinberg, D. R. (2008). Are human faces and voices ornaments
signaling common under-
lying cues to mate value? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues,
News, and Reviews, 17(2),
112–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/evan.20166.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Moore, F. R.,
Law Smith, M. J., Cornwell, R. E., ...
Perrett, D. I. (2005). The voice and face of woman: One
ornament that signals quality?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 398–408.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
evolhumbehav.2005.04.001.
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of
human mating: Trade-offs and
strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(04),
573–587.
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). The evolutionary
psychology of extrapair sex:
The role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 18(2), 69–88.
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle
variation in women's prefer-
ences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London
B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–933.
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2013). Human sexual
selection and developmental sta-
bility. In J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary
social psychology
(pp. 169–195).
Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A. (1994). Facial
attractiveness, developmental
stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethology and
Sociobiology, 15(2), 73–85.
Graham, J. H., Emlen, J. M., Freeman, D. C., Leamy, L. J., &
Kieser, J. A. (1998). Directional
asymmetry and the measurement of developmental instability.
Biological Journal of
the Linnean Society, 64(1), 1–16.
Grammer, K., & Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens)
facial attractiveness and sex-
ual selection: The role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of
Comparative
Psychology, 108(3), 233.
Hedges, L., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical models for meta-
analysis. New York: Academic
Press.
Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2001). The power of statistical
tests in meta-analysis.
Psychological Methods, 6, 203–217.
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G.
(2003). Measuring inconsistency
in meta-analyses. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 327(7414),
557.
Hirano, M., Yukizane, K., Kurita, S., & Hibi, S. (1989).
Asymmetry of the laryngeal frame-
work: A morphologic study of cadaver larynges. Annals of
Otology, Rhinology &
Laryngology, 98(2), 135–140.
Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G. (2002). The
sound of symmetry: Voice as a
marker of developmental instability. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 23(3), 173–180.
Hughes, S. M., Pastizzo, M. J., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). The
sound of symmetry revisited:
Subjective and objective analyses of voice. Journal of
Nonverbal Behavior, 32(2),
93–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0042-6.
Hunt, J., Bussiere, L. F., Jennions, M. D., & Brooks, R. (2004).
What is genetic quality? Trends
in Ecology & Evolution, 19(6), 329–333.
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Little, A. C. (2007). The role
of symmetry in attraction to av-
erage faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1273–1277.
Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Tiddeman, B.,
Burt, D., & Perrett, D. (2001). Facial
symmetry and judgements of apparent health: Support for a
“good genes” explana-
tion of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evolution and
Human Behavior,
22(6), 417–429.
Kempenaers, B. (2007). Mate choice and genetic quality: A
review of the heterozygosity
theory. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 189–278.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990).
Evolution, traits, and the
stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental investment
model. Journal of
Personality, 58, 97–116.
Kimmerle, E. H., & Jantz, R. L. (2005). Secular trends in
craniofacial asymmetry studied by
geometric morphometry and generalized Procrustes methods.
Modern morphometrics
in physical anthropology (pp. 247–263). Springer.
Klingenberg, C. P., & McIntyre, G. S. (1998). Geometric
morphometrics of developmental
instability: Analyzing patterns of fluctuating asymmetry with
Procrustes methods.
Evolution, 1363–1375.
Klingenberg, C. P., Barluenga, M., & Meyer, A. (2002). Shape
analysis of symmetric struc-
tures: Quantifying variation among individuals and asymmetry.
Evolution, 56(10),
1909–1920.
Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2009). Averageness
or symmetry: Which is
more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychologica,
131(2), 136–142. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008.
Lander, K. (2008). Relating visual and vocal attractiveness for
moving and static faces.
Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 817–822.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.001.
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Musselman, L. (1994). What
is average and what is not
average about attractive faces? Psychological Science, 5(4),
214–220.
Leamy, L. (1997). Is developmental stability heritable? Journal
of Evolutionary Biology,
10(1), 21–29.
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2003). Evidence against
perceptual bias views for symmetry
preferences in human faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society
B: Biological Sciences,
270(1526), 1759–1763.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2445.
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Variation in facial
masculinity and symmetry preferences across
the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(7),
999–1008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007.
Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I.
(2001). Self-perceived attractive-
ness influences human female preferences for sexual
dimorphism and symmetry in
male faces. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 268(1462),
39–44. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1327.
Little, A. C., Cohen, D. L., Jones, B. C., & Belsky, J. (2007a).
Human preferences for facial
masculinity change with relationship type and environmental
harshness. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 61(6), 967–973.
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007b).
Preferences for symmetry in
faces change across the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology,
76(3), 209–216.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.003.
Livshits, G., & Kobyliansky, E. (1991). Fluctuating asymmetry
as a possible measure of de-
velopmental homeostasis in humans: A review. Human Biology,
441–466.
Manning, J. T. (1995). Fluctuating asymmetry and body weight
in men and women: Im-
plications for sexual selection. Ethology and Sociobiology,
16(2), 145–153.
Manning, J., Koukourakis, K., & Brodie, D. (1997b).
Fluctuating asymmetry, metabolic rate
and sexual selection in human males. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 18(1), 15–21.
Manning, J., Scutt, D., Whitehouse, G., & Leinster, S. (1997a).
Breast asymmetry and phe-
notypic quality in women. Evolution and Human Behavior,
18(4), 223–236.
Mardia, K. V., Bookstein, F. L., & Moreton, I. J. (2000).
Statistical assessment of bilateral
symmetry of shapes. Biometrika, 285–300.
Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter–
gatherers. Human Nature,
15(4), 365–376.
Martin, S., Manning, J., & Dowrick, C. (1999). Fluctuating
asymmetry, relative digit length,
and depression in men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(3),
203–214.
Martin, J. T., Puts, D. A., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Hand
asymmetry in heterosexual and
homosexual men and women: Relationship to 2D:4D digit ratios
and other sexually
dimorphic anatomical traits. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
37(1), 119–132. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9279-8.
Møller, A. P. (1992). Female swallow preference for
symmetrical male. Nature, 357.
Møller, A. P. (1994). Directional selection on directional
asymmetry: Testes size and sec-
ondary sexual characters in birds. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London B: Biolog-
ical Sciences, 258(1352), 147–151.
Møller, A. P., & Hoglund, J. (1991). Patterns of fluctuating
asymmetry in avian feather or-
naments: Implications for models of sexual selection.
Proceedings of the Royal Society
of London B: Biological Sciences, 245(1312), 1–5.
Møller, A., & Thornhill, R. (1997). A meta-analysis of the
heritability of developmental sta-
bility. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 10(1), 1–16.
Mooney, M. P., Siegel, M. I., & Gest, T. R. (1985). Prenatal
stress and increased fluctuating
asymmetry in the parietal bones of neonatal rats. American
Journal of Physical Anthro-
pology, 68(1), 131–134.
Oakes, E. J., & Barnard, P. (1994). Fluctuating asymmetry and
mate choice in paradise
whydahs, Vidua paradisaea: An experimental manipulation.
Animal Behaviour,
48(4), 937–943.
Palmer, A. R., & Strobeck, C. (1986). Fluctuating asymmetry:
Measurement, analysis, pat-
terns. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 391–421.
Palmer, A., & Strobeck, C. (1997). Fluctuating asymmetry and
developmental stability:
Heritability of observable variation vs. heritability of inferred
cause. Journal of
Evolutionary Biology, 10(1), 39–49.
Parsons, P. (1990). Fluctuating asymmetry: An epigenetic
measure of stress. Biological
Reviews, 65(2), 131–145.
Penton-Voak, I. S., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Baker, S.,
Tiddeman, B., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I.
(2001). Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and
male facial attrac-
tiveness. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 268(1476),
1617–1623. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1703.
Peretz, B., Ever-Hadani, P., Casamassimo, P., Eidelman, E.,
Shellhart, C., & Hagerman, R.
(1988). Crown size asymmetry in males with fra (X) or Martin–
Bell syndrome.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 30(1–2), 185–190.
9A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0045
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0045
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710420105
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0507
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399584
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399584
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.001
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0070
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0070
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0090
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0090
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0186
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0110
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/evan.20166
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.001
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0130
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0130
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0135
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0135
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0150
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0150
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0165
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0165
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0170
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0170
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0175
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0175
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0185
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0185
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0042-6
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0195
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0195
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0200
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0200
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0210
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0210
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.001
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0245
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0245
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0250
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0250
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2445
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1327
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.003
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0280
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0280
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0285
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0285
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0290
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0290
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0295
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0295
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0300
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0300
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0305
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0305
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0310
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0310
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9279-8
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0320
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0335
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0335
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0350
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0350
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0360
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0360
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1703
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0370
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0370
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J.,
Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. (1999). Sym-
metry and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 20(5), 295–307.
Peters, M., Simmons, L. W., & Rhodes, G. (2009). Preferences
across the menstrual cycle
for masculinity and symmetry in photographs of male faces and
bodies. PloS One,
4(1), e4138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004138.
Petrie, M. (1994). Improved growth and survival of offspring of
peacocks with more elab-
orate trains. Nature, 371(6498), 598–599.
Pipitone, N. R., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2008). Women's voice
attractiveness varies across the
menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268–274.
Pipitone, N. R., & Gallup, G. (2011). The unique impact of
menstruation on the female
voice: Implications for the evolution of menstrual cycle cues.
Ethology, 118,
281–291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02010.x.
Pisanski, K., & Feinberg, D. R. (2013). Cross-cultural variation
in mate preferences for average-
ness, symmetry, body size, and masculinity. Cross-Cultural
Research, 47(2), 162–197.
Polak, M., Møller, A., Gangestad, S. W., Kroeger, D. E.,
Manning, J. T., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Does
an individual asymmetry parameter exist? A meta-analysis. In
M. Polak (Ed.), Develop-
mental instability: causes and consequences (pp. 81–96).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., Cardenas, R. A., Burriss, R. P.,
Welling, L. L., Wheatley, J. R., &
Dawood, K. (2013). Women's attractiveness changes with
estradiol and progesterone
across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 63(1), 13–
19. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007.
Puts, D. A., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2012). Sexual
selection on human faces and
voices. Journal of Sex Research, 49(2–3), 227–243.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00224499.2012.658924.
Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005).
Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does
attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human
Behavior, 26(2),
186–201.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014.
Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Lee, K., McKay, R., &
Akamatsu, S. (2001). Attractive-
ness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-Western
cultures: In search of bio-
logically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30(5), 611–625.
http://dx.doi.org/
10.1068/p3123.
Rohlf, F. J., & Slice, D. (1990). Extensions of the Procrustes
method for the optimal super-
imposition of landmarks. Systematic Biology, 39(1), 40–59.
Saxton, T., Burriss, R., Murray, A., Rowland, H., & Roberts, S.
C. (2009). Face, body and
speech cues independently predict judgments of attractiveness.
Journal of
Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 23–35.
Saxton, T. K., Caryl, P. G., & Roberts, S. C. (2006). Vocal and
facial attractiveness judgments
of children, adolescents and adults: The ontogeny of mate
choice. Ethology, 112(12),
1179–1185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-
0310.2006.01278.x.
Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1999). Facial
attractiveness, symmetry and
cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London
B: Biological Sciences,
266(1431), 1913–1917.
Sciulli, P., Doyle, W., Kelley, C., Siegel, P., & Siegel, M.
(1979). The interaction of stressors in
the induction of increased levels of fluctuating asymmetry in
the laboratory rat.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 50(2), 279–284.
Simmons, L. (1995). Correlates of male quality in the field
cricket, Gryllus campestris L.:
Age, size, and symmetry determine pairing success in field
populations. Behavioral
Ecology, 6(4), 376–381.
Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kecko, S., Moore, F. R., Kaasik, A.,
Meija, L., ... Krams, I. (2014). Body
height, immunity, facial and vocal attractiveness in young men.
Naturwissenschaften,
101(12), 1017–1025.
Springer, I. N., Wannicke, B., Warnke, P. H., Zernial, O.,
Wiltfang, J., Russo, P. A., ... Wolfart,
S. (2007). Facial attractiveness: Visual impact of symmetry
increases significantly to-
wards the midline. Annals of Plastic Surgery, 59(2), 156–162.
Swaddle, J. P. (1996). Reproductive success and symmetry in
zebra finches. Animal
Behaviour, 51(1), 203–210.
Swaddle, J. P., & Cuthill, I. C. (1995). Asymmetry and human
facial attractiveness: Symme-
try may not always be beautiful. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London B: Biological
Sciences, 261(1360), 111–116.
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999a). Facial
attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
3(12), 452–460.
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999b). The scent of
symmetry: A human sex phero-
mone that signals fitness? Evolution and Human Behavior,
20(3), 175–201.
Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., & Comer, R. (1995). Human
female orgasm and mate fluc-
tuating asymmetry. Animal Behaviour, 50(6), 1601–1615.
Tigue, C. C., Pisanski, K., O'Connor, J. J., Fraccaro, P. J., &
Feinberg, D. R. (2012). Men's judg-
ments of women's facial attractiveness from two- and three-
dimensional images are
similar. Journal of Vision, 12(12), 3.
Tomkins, J. L., & Simmons, L. W. (1998). Female choice and
manipulations of forceps size
and symmetry in the earwig Forficula auricularia L. Animal
Behaviour, 56(2), 347–356.
Van Dongen, S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2011). Human fluctuating
asymmetry in relation to
health and quality: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 32(6), 380–398.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.002.
Van Valen, L. (1962). A study of fluctuating asymmetry.
Evolution, 125–142.
Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary
Theory, 1, 1–30.
Weinberg, S. M., Scott, N. M., Neiswanger, K., Brandon, C. A.,
& Marazita, M. L. (2004). Dig-
ital three-dimensional photogrammetry: Evaluation of
anthropometric precision and
accuracy using a Genex 3D camera system. The Cleft Palate-
Craniofacial Journal, 41(5),
507–518.
Wells, T., Baguley, T., Sergeant, M., & Dunn, A. (2013).
Perceptions of human attractive-
ness comprising face and voice cues. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 42(5), 805–811.
Wheatley, J. R., Apicella, C. A., Burriss, R. P., Cárdenas, R. A.,
Bailey, D. H., Welling, L. L., &
Puts, D. A. (2014). Women's faces and voices are cues to
reproductive potential in in-
dustrial and forager societies. Evolution and Human Behavior,
35(4), 264–271.
10 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
xxx–xxx
Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial
fluctuating
asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human
Behavior (2016),
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0375
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0375
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004138
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0385
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0385
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0390
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0390
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02010.x
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0400
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0400
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3123
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0430
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0430
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01278.x
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0465
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0465
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0470
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0470
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0480
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0480
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0485
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0485
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0490
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0490
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0500
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0500
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.002
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0510
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0515
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0525
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0525
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0530
http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0530
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008Are there
vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships
between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attract...1.
Introduction2. Study 12.1. Methods2.1.1. Participants2.1.2.
Face measurement2.1.3. Voice recording2.1.4. Vocal and facial
attractiveness ratings2.2. Results3. Study 23.1. Methods3.1.1.
Participants3.1.2. Face measurement3.1.3. Vocal attractiveness
ratings3.2. Results4. Study 34.1. Methods4.1.1.
Participants4.1.2. Face measurement4.1.3. Vocal attractiveness
ratings4.2. Results5. Meta-analysis6. Discussion7. Future
directionsFundingSupplementary
materialsAcknowledgmentsReferences
Original ArticleAre there vocal cues to human developmenta.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Original ArticleAre there vocal cues to human developmenta.docx

Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twins
Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twinsFinger length ratios in female monozygotic twins
Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twinsTeresa Levy
 
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy Indian dental academy
 
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant ExtractsCytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant ExtractsIOSRJPBS
 
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientation
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientationDermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientation
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientationTeresa Levy
 
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipWeinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipTamara Weinstein
 
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery courses
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery coursesGenetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery courses
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery coursesIndian dental academy
 
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report Complete
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report CompleteRayner Tai - Empirical Report Complete
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report CompleteRayner Tai
 
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...Healthcare and Medical Sciences
 
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profiles
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profilesCognitive, behavioral and personality profiles
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profilesTeresa Levy
 
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrienta
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrientaGenetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrienta
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrientaMatthewTennant613
 
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...ricguer
 
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docx
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docxP1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docx
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docxgerardkortney
 
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docxSONU61709
 
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16Samuel Jensen
 
Language evolution and genomic imprinting
Language evolution and genomic imprintingLanguage evolution and genomic imprinting
Language evolution and genomic imprintingWill Brown
 
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...ricguer
 
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdf
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdfWhy is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdf
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdfarrowmobile
 
An expanded view of complex traits from polygenic to omnigenic
 An expanded view of complex traits  from polygenic to omnigenic An expanded view of complex traits  from polygenic to omnigenic
An expanded view of complex traits from polygenic to omnigenicBARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 

Similar to Original ArticleAre there vocal cues to human developmenta.docx (20)

Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twins
Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twinsFinger length ratios in female monozygotic twins
Finger length ratios in female monozygotic twins
 
RSepKJabcdeaARR2.docx
RSepKJabcdeaARR2.docxRSepKJabcdeaARR2.docx
RSepKJabcdeaARR2.docx
 
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
GENETICS & MALOCCLUSION - II /orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
 
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant ExtractsCytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
 
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientation
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientationDermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientation
Dermatoglyphics, handedness sex, and sexual orientation
 
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and FriendshipWeinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
Weinstein 2014 Neuropeptides and Friendship
 
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery courses
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery coursesGenetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery courses
Genetics & malocclusion-2/ oral surgery courses
 
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report Complete
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report CompleteRayner Tai - Empirical Report Complete
Rayner Tai - Empirical Report Complete
 
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...
Using Role Play to (Re) Evaluate the Relationship between Masculinity And / O...
 
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profiles
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profilesCognitive, behavioral and personality profiles
Cognitive, behavioral and personality profiles
 
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrienta
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrientaGenetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrienta
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Female SexualOrienta
 
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
 
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docx
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docxP1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docx
P1 IZOArchives of Sexual Behavior pp744-aseb-460154 Februar.docx
 
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
 
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16
Defreece-Jensen-4-14-16
 
Birth order
Birth orderBirth order
Birth order
 
Language evolution and genomic imprinting
Language evolution and genomic imprintingLanguage evolution and genomic imprinting
Language evolution and genomic imprinting
 
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...
Are environmental levels of bpa associated with reproductive function in fert...
 
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdf
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdfWhy is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdf
Why is it important to study reactions norms to understand phenotypi.pdf
 
An expanded view of complex traits from polygenic to omnigenic
 An expanded view of complex traits  from polygenic to omnigenic An expanded view of complex traits  from polygenic to omnigenic
An expanded view of complex traits from polygenic to omnigenic
 

More from vannagoforth

1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx
1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx
1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docxvannagoforth
 
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docxvannagoforth
 
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docxvannagoforth
 
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docxvannagoforth
 
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docxvannagoforth
 
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docxvannagoforth
 
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docxvannagoforth
 
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docxvannagoforth
 
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docxvannagoforth
 

More from vannagoforth (20)

1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx
1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx
1. Primary sources2. Secondary sources3. La Malinche4. Bacon’s.docx
 
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx
1. Prepare an outline, an introduction, and a summary.docx
 
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx
1. Normative moral philosophy typically focuses on the determining t.docx
 
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx
1. Paper should be 5-pages min. + 1 page works cited2. Should have.docx
 
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx
1. Name and describe the three steps of the looking-glass self.2.docx
 
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx
1. Provide an example of a business or specific person(s) that effec.docx
 
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx
1. Mexico and Guatemala. Research the political and economic situati.docx
 
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx
1. Many scholars have set some standards to judge a system for taxat.docx
 
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx
1. List and (in 1-2 sentences) describe the 4 interlocking factors t.docx
 
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx
1. Please explain how the Constitution provides for a system of sepa.docx
 
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx
1. Please watch the following The Diving Bell & The Butterfly, Amel.docx
 
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx
1. Most sociologists interpret social life from one of the three maj.docx
 
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx
1. Members of one species cannot successfully interbreed and produc.docx
 
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx
1. Of the three chemical bonds discussed in class, which of them is .docx
 
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx
1. Look at your diagrams for hydrogen, lithium, and sodium. What do .docx
 
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx
1. Name the following molecules2. Sketch the following molecules.docx
 
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx
1. List the horizontal and vertical levels of systems that exist in .docx
 
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx
1. Kemal Ataturk carried out policies that distanced the new Turkish.docx
 
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx
1. If we consider a gallon of gas as having 100 units of energy, and.docx
 
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx
1. In 200-250 words, analyze the basic issues of human biology as th.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxabhijeetpadhi001
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupJonathanParaisoCruz
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptxMICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
MICROBIOLOGY biochemical test detailed.pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized GroupMARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
MARGINALIZATION (Different learners in Marginalized Group
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 

Original ArticleAre there vocal cues to human developmenta.docx

  • 1. Original Article Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness Alexander K. Hill a,1, Rodrigo A. Cárdenas b, John R. Wheatley a, Lisa L.M. Welling a,2, Robert P. Burriss a,3, Peter Claes c, Coren L. Apicella d, Michael A. McDaniel e, Anthony C. Little f, Mark D. Shriver a, David A. Puts a,g,⁎ a Department of Anthropology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA b Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA c KU Leuven, ESAT/PSI—UZ Leuven, MIRC—iMinds, Medical IT Department, Belgium d Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA e Department of Management, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA 23284, USA f Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK g Center for Brain, Behavior, and Cognition, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA a b s t r a c ta r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Initial receipt 26 November 2015 Final revision received 25 October 2016 Available online xxxx Keywords:
  • 2. 3D images Fluctuating asymmetry Geometric morphometrics Hunter-gatherers Mate choice Sexual selection Fluctuating asymmetry (FA), deviation from perfect bilateral symmetry, is thought to reflect an organism's rela- tive inability to maintain stable morphological development in the face of environmental and genetic stressors. Previous research has documented negative relationships between FA and attractiveness judgments in humans, but scant research has explored relationships between the human voice and this putative marker of genetic qual- ity in either sex. Only one study (and in women only) has explored relationships between vocal attractiveness and asymmetry of the face, a feature-rich trait space central in prior work on human genetic quality and mate choice. We therefore examined this relationship in three studies comprising 231 men and 240 women from two Western samples as well as Hadza hunter–gatherers of Tanzania. Voice recordings were collected and rated for attractiveness, and FA was computed from two- dimensional facial images as well as, for a subset of men, three-dimensional facial scans. Through meta-analysis of our results and those of prior studies, we found a negative association between FA and vocal attractiveness that was highly robust and statistically significant whether we included effect sizes from previously published work, or only those from the present research, and regardless of the inclusion of any individual sample or method of assessing FA (e.g., facial or limb FA). Weighted mean correlations between FA and vocal attractiveness across studies were −.23 for men and −.29 for women. This research thus offers strong support for the
  • 3. hypothesis that voices provide cues to genetic quality in humans. © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Mate choice is predicated, in part, on heritable fitness benefits (Petrie, 1994), and organisms are attentive to biomarkers that putative- ly indicate whether a potential mate possesses genes that would con- tribute additively to offspring fitness (Andersson, 1994)—that is, whether a mate is high in genetic quality (Hunt, Bussiere, Jennions, & Brooks, 2004). One such biomarker is fluctuating asymmetry (FA; e.g., Møller, 1992; Swaddle, 1996; Thornhill & Gangestad, 1999a; Van Valen, 1962, 1973). FA is exhibited in bilaterally symmetrical traits if the difference between the left and right sides has a population normal distribution with a mean of zero (Palmer & Strobeck, 1986). Substantial evidence from humans (e.g., Gangestad, Thornhill, & Yeo, 1994; Livshits & Kobyliansky, 1991) as well as an array of vertebrates and insects (Parsons, 1990) indicates that the degree to which bilaterally symmet- rical traits deviate from perfect symmetry reflects developmental insta- bility resulting from harmful mutations and environmental
  • 4. perturbations. Experimental exposure to common environmental stressors, for example, can increase asymmetry in laboratory rats (e.g., Mooney, Siegel, & Gest, 1985; Sciulli, Doyle, Kelley, Siegel, & Siegel, 1979). In humans, individuals with known genetic or chromosomal abnor- malities have greater dental FA than do controls (e.g., Barden, 1980; Peretz et al., 1988), and FA displays a negative relationship with genetic heterozygosity (Livshits & Kobyliansky, 1991), which can increase fitness by lowering the risk of expressing deleterious recessive alleles and by in- creasing acquired immunity through the major histocompatibility Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx ⁎ Corresponding author. Department of Anthropology, The Pennsylvania State Universi- ty, 218 Carpenter Building, University Park, PA 16802, USA. Tel.: +1 814 867 0453. E-mail address: [email protected] (D.A. Puts). 1 Present address: Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, USA. 2 Present address: Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, MI 48309, USA.
  • 5. 3 Present address: Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 1090-5138/© 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Evolution and Human Behavior journal homepage: www.ehbonline.org Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 complex (MHC; reviewed in Kempenaers, 2007). Lower FA in ear height, wrist breadth, and digit length has predicted reduced resting metabolic rate (Manning, Koukourakis & Brodie, 1997) and better mental health (Martin, Manning, & Dowrick, 1999) in men, and lower facial FA shows a similar relationship with perceptions of physical health (Jones et al.,
  • 6. 2001). Moreover, FA is positively correlated with female body mass, age, and age at first childbirth, all indications of reduced fecundity (Manning, Scutt, Whitehouse & Leinster, 1997). A recent meta-analysis also found a small negative relationship between FA and general intelligence (Banks, Batchelor, & McDaniel, 2010). Together, these results suggest that bilateral symmetry may reflect the fit between an organism's geno- type and environment during development. As a putative indicator of a genotype's resilience to environmental insults, FA is related to mate choice across a variety of nonhuman spe- cies. For example, among field crickets (Gryllus campestris), males suc- cessful in obtaining mates display lower FA than do less successful males (Simmons, 1995). Likewise, female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) prefer to be near males wearing symmetrically colored experi- mental leg bands (Swaddle, 1996). In humans, several studies have reported significant correlations be- tween FA and perceptions of facial attractiveness (e.g., Abend, Pflüger, Koppensteiner, Coquerelle, & Grammer, 2015; Grammer & Thornhill, 1994; Komori, Kawamura, & Ishihara, 2009; Perrett et al., 1999; Rhodes
  • 7. et al., 2001). In addition, women who view themselves as attractive show a greater preference for symmetry in male faces (Little, Burt, Penton-Voak, & Perrett, 2001), suggesting that symmetrical mates are valued and are more attainable by women of higher mate value. Some studies (e.g., Little & Jones, 2012; Little, Jones, Burt & Perrett, 2007), but not others (e.g., Peters, Simmons, & Rhodes, 2009), have found that women in the fertile phase of the ovulatory cycle exhibit a greater preference for symmetrical faces and bodies, again suggesting an associ- ation between symmetry and genetic quality, since genetic benefits can be obtained only when conception is possible. There is also evidence that humans mate assortatively for facial symmetry, indicating that symmetry or correlated traits may influence mate choice (Burriss, Roberts, Welling, Puts, & Little, 2011). While bilateral symmetry appears to be important in predicting mating decisions, preferences for symmetrical mates may not always reflect preferences for symmetry per se. Bilateral symmetry has been found to predict attractiveness even in the absence of visual cues to asymmetry itself, and thus asymmetry must be related to other pheno- typic cues. For example, women in the fertile phase of the
  • 8. ovulatory cycle have shown a preference for symmetrical men when exposed ex- clusively to the men's body odor (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Thornhill & Gangestad, 1999b). Another study found a female prefer- ence for low FA in men even when study participants were shown only the left or right half of each male face, preventing them from assessing symmetry directly (Scheib, Gangestad, & Thornhill, 1999). Developmental stability may also be conveyed vocally. Hughes, Harrison, and Gallup (2002) found both sexes to exhibit a negative rela- tionship between upper limb FA (particularly finger length FA) and at- tractiveness assessed from vocal stimuli alone. Abend et al. (2015) similarly obtained a negative relationship between women's vocal at- tractiveness and measures of FA from their upper and lower limbs as well as the head and face. These studies join other research demonstrat- ing the salience of voice as an influence on mate choice in humans (reviewed in Feinberg, 2008; Pisanski & Feinberg, 2013; Puts, Jones, & DeBruine, 2012). Moreover, vocal and facial attractiveness may covary (e.g., Saxton, Burriss, Murray, Rowland, & Roberts, 2009) in part because both the
  • 9. face and vocal folds are bilaterally symmetrical, and both may therefore reflect deleterious environmental influences during development. For example, asymmetric shape, size, or tension in the vocal folds could re- sult in audible changes in the voice, such as hoarseness (Eysholdt, Rosanowski, & Hoppe, 2003), and indeed, Hughes, Pastizzo, and Gallup (2008) found that limb FA in men predicted a measure of vocal hoarseness. Developmental instability may also affect the size and shape of vocal structures, as well as tension on the vocal folds or the po- sitioning of the larynx in the vocal tract, which would influence vocal pitch and timbre, respectively (for fuller consideration of potential links between FA and vocal attractiveness, please see Discussion). Some research indicates that women's vocal attractiveness is related to fertility and reproductive potential (Bryant & Haselton, 2009; Pipitone & Gallup, 2008, 2011; Puts et al., 2013; Wheatley et al., 2014), though scant research has explored relationships between vocal attrac- tiveness and FA in either sex. To our knowledge, Abend et al. (2015) is the only study conducted to date investigating relationships between vocal attractiveness and facial FA. Furthermore, not all research on FA
  • 10. supports the prediction that individuals will tend to prefer mates lower in FA. For example, some research on humans (e.g., Langlois, Roggman, & Musselman, 1994; Swaddle & Cuthill, 1995), as well as non- human species (e.g., Oakes & Barnard, 1994; Tomkins & Simmons, 1998), has failed to show preferences for mates with low FA, while other research has found a statistically significant relationship between FA and attractiveness only in particular situations (Springer et al., 2007). There are likely several reasons for these discrepancies. First, FA is a weak index of developmental instability, such that associations be- tween FA and measures of mate quality are difficult to detect using modest sample sizes (Van Dongen & Gangestad, 2011). Second, rela- tionships with FA may be complicated by directional asymmetry (DA), which characterizes traits for which the difference between the left and right sides has a population normal distribution with a mean that deviates from zero (Møller, 1994). Directional asymmetry, such as oc- curs in testes size in many bird species (Møller, 1994) and in the lower limbs of humans (Auerbach & Ruff, 2006), may constitute part of an organism's normal development (Graham, Emlen, Freeman,
  • 11. Leamy, & Kieser, 1998; Martin, Puts, & Breedlove, 2008). Since the pres- ence of DA may complicate measures of FA, it is critical to take DA into consideration in research examining FA. While some studies have done so (e.g., Abend et al., 2015; Brown et al., 2008; Manning, 1995), others have not (e.g., Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997; Hughes et al., 2002; Thornhill, Gangestad, & Comer, 1995). Finally, prior research has disproportionately sampled from Western societies, especially individ- uals of European genetic ancestry. Not only do such individuals differ in facial morphology from those of other populations (Farkas, Katic, & Forrest, 2005), but Westerners may also have greater access to modern medical and cosmetic technologies that can buffer them from environ- mental stressors, weakening relationships between FA and measures of attractiveness (Apicella, 2011). In the present paper, we tested whether somatic FA predicts vocal attractiveness by collecting data in three studies that were designed in part to address the limitations of prior research while enabling us to ex- plore the robustness of these relationships across methodologies and populations. We measured FA from the face due to the centrality of fa-
  • 12. cial symmetry in prior work as well as the status of the face as a feature-rich space that is developmentally distinct from the upper limb, measured by Hughes et al. (2002) and the upper and lower limb included in Abend et al. (2015). In Studies 1 and 2, we measured FA from two-dimensional (2D) facial photographs, following most prior studies of facial asymmetry and attractiveness (Jones et al., 2001). Study 1 data were collected from U.S. undergraduate students, whereas Study 2 data were collected from the Hadza, a Tanzanian forager popu- lation less protected by modern medicine from environmental insults than are samples drawn from the West. Compared to Western samples, Hadza facial and vocal characteristics should thus more strongly reflect individuals' constitutional resilience to environmental stressors, such as pathogens. Finally, in Study 3, we explored facial FA and vocal attrac- tiveness among U.S. undergraduates using the 2D methods employed in the first two studies as well as three-dimensional (3D) images mea- sured via spatially dense geometric morphometrics, which provides far greater shape information than does 2D imagery (Abend et al., 2015; Brown et al., 2008). 2 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016)
  • 13. xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 2. Study 1 2.1. Methods 2.1.1. Participants One hundred twenty-four male (mean age = 20.08 ± 1.76 years) and 201 female (mean age = 19.97 ± 1.55 years) students from Mich- igan State University participated as part of a larger study of siblings (see, e.g., Puts et al., 2013; Wheatley et al., 2014). If same-sex siblings participated, we randomly selected one from each sibling group (gener- ally pairs). In addition, only students self-identifying as “White” were used, as facial morphology varies by ethnicity (e.g., Farkas et al., 2005), and we did not have sufficient numbers of non-White participants to permit a meaningful analysis stratified by ancestry group. Both male and female participants provided voice recordings and 2D facial
  • 14. photo- graphs at two data collection sessions scheduled one week apart, with all procedures approved by the Michigan State University institutional review board. 2.1.2. Face measurement Two-dimensional facial photographs were taken of participants at each session, with only photographs from session 1 used unless there was no session 1 photograph for a participant, in which case the session 2 photograph was used (the methodology followed to take these photo- graphs was identical between the sessions). Participants were instructed to assume a neutral expression after having removed any jewelry, eye- glasses, or makeup (with wet wipes provided at the time of data collec- tion). While not all male participants were clean-shaven, care was taken to ensure high-quality facial landmarks for all photographs (see below). Facial photographs were taken with a tripod-mounted Canon PowerShot S10 digital camera at a distance of approximately 1 m, a height adjusted to the participant, and using constant lighting across participants. All facial images were normalized on interpupillary distance and rotated so that both pupils lay on the same horizontal plane.
  • 15. Each facial photograph was landmarked by two research assistants (RAs) with 14 x,y-coordinates in the software ImageJ, after which land- mark coordinates were averaged between RAs. Horizontal asymmetry measurements were obtained from landmarks as in Grammer and Thornhill (1994) by first determining midpoints for six pairs of bilateral landmarks (Fig. 1). If a face is perfectly symmetrical about the midsagittal plane, then all midpoints will fall on the vertical line representing this plane, and the horizontal differences between these midpoints will be zero. Thus, to measure horizontal asymmetry, horizontal (i.e., x- coordinate) distances between midpoints were computed for all 15 non- redundant pairs of midpoints and then summed. Vertical asymmetry was computed from the sum of the vertical (i.e., y-coordinate) distances be- tween the 6 landmark pairs (Scheib et al., 1999). As in Scheib et al. (1999), the horizontal and vertical asymmetry measures were finally summed to produce an overall measure of facial asymmetry. Inter-measurer reliability was assessed by calculating for each facial distance relative error magnitude (REM) values, which incorporate measurement size so as to arrive at a more precise assessment of consis-
  • 16. tency (Weinberg, Scott, Neiswanger, Brandon, & Marazita, 2004). The mean for the x-coordinates was 1.67 (SD = 1.08) and the mean for the y-coordinates was 1.21 (SD = .74), indicating strong agreement. DA was calculated for each sex and each facial trait separately and then used to correct for DA when calculating individual FA values. DA for each facial trait j was calculated as DAj ¼ ∑ n i¼1 Lij−Rij Lij þ Rij ! " =2 2 4 3 5=n where L and R represent the left and right sides of the trait for each par- ticipant i, and n represents the number of participants for whom data were available. Thus, for each trait in each sex, DA was the
  • 17. average signed left–right difference in distance across participants expressed as a proportion of the size of the trait. We were then able, in accordance with methods used by Gangestad et al. (1994) and Abend et al. (2015), to calculate FA for each trait j in each individual i as FAij ¼ Lij−Rij Lij þ Rij ! " =2 2 4 3 5−DA j ###### ###### FA was computed exclusively from front-on photographs (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org), and we elim- inated the facial image of one male participant whose facial FA exceeded
  • 18. the mean by 4.0 SD, possibly due to head tilt. See Palmer and Strobeck (1986) for a discussion of variation in methods of calculating FA. 2.1.3. Voice recording ParticipantswererecordedspeakingthefirstsixsentencesoftheRain bow Passage (Fairbanks, 1960) into a Shure SM58 vocal cardioid microphone in a quiet recording booth. A curved wire projecting from the microphone stand kept each participant's mouth approximately 9.5 cm from the microphone. Voices were recorded via a Sound Devices USBPre 2 preamplifier onto a com- puter using GoldWave software in mono at a sampling rate of 44,100 Hz and 16-bit quantization, and saved as uncompressed WAV files. 2.1.4. Vocal and facial attractiveness ratings Unfamiliar members of the opposite sex (n = 569, mean age = 19.4 years for male raters; n = 558, mean age = 19.1 years for female raters) recruited from The Pennsylvania State University rated voice re- cordings and facial images on attractiveness for a short-term relation- ship made on a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all attractive, 7 = very attractive). This portion of the study was approved by The Pennsylvania State University's institutional review board. Raters first
  • 19. read the definition of a short-term relationship given by Little, Cohen, Jones and Belsky (2007, p. 970): “You are looking for the type of person who would be attractive in a short-term relationship. This implies that Fig. 1. Landmarks used to compute horizontal and vertical facial asymmetry from two- dimensional images. Left and right pupils were landmarked to standardize interpupillary distance and register faces horizontally. 3A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 the relationship may not last a long time. Examples of this type of rela- tionship would include a single date accepted on the spur of the mo- ment, an affair within a long-term relationship, and possibility of a one-night stand.” We chose to use ratings of short-term attractiveness made by members of the opposite sex because short-term
  • 20. relationships represent the mating context in which genetic quality is most salient relative to other mate choice criteria, such as investment (Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990). At private workstations, each rater then assessed stimulus sets comprising approximately 25 facial photographs presented on a computer screen and 25 recordings of the first sentence of the Rainbow Passage presented through Sennheiser HD 280 Profes- sional headphones. Recordings and photographs were randomly allo- cated to a set, each of which was rated by at least 15 raters (mean = 18.9). The order in which participants completed the rating tasks (faces or voices first) was random across participants, as was the order in which stimuli were presented within each task. We calculated mean short-term sexual attractiveness scores for each participant using the first 15 ratings per stimulus (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org). 2.2. Results In both men (n = 124, r = −.21, p = .017) and women (n = 201, r = −.24, p = .001), there was a significant negative relationship be- tween vocal attractiveness and facial FA (Tables 1 and 3, Fig. 2). We
  • 21. also explored the possibility that the relationship between facial FA and vocal attractiveness could be attributable to correlations between (1) vocal and facial attractiveness (men: n = 118, r = .04, p = .685; women: n = 189, r = .21, p = .005) and (2) facial attractiveness and fa- cial FA (men: n = 118, r = −.04, p = .708; women: n = 189, r = −.15, p = .043). Attractiveness is an important mediator of mating success (Apicella & Feinberg, 2009; Rhodes, Simmons, & Peters, 2005), and facial attractiveness has also correlated with both symmetry (Grammer & Thornhill, 1994; Perrett et al., 1999) and vocal attractiveness (Abend et al., 2015; Collins & Missing, 2003; Lander, 2008; Wheatley et al., 2014). Hence, it is possible that facial symmetry might increase vocal at- tractiveness indirectly by increasing facial attractiveness. For instance, people with more symmetrical and thus more attractive faces may conse- quently feel more confident and therefore speak more attractively. How- ever, after controlling for facial attractiveness in multiple regression models predicting vocal attractiveness, we found that the relationships between facial FA and vocal attractiveness remained in both men (β = −.23, p = .014; model: F(2115) = 3.23, R = .23, p = .043) and
  • 22. women (β = −.25, p b .001; model: F(2186) = 10.75, R = .32, p b .0001). 3. Study 2 3.1. Methods 3.1.1. Participants Twenty-eight male (mean age = 29.32 ± 5.84 years) and 39 female (mean age = 29.03 ± 6.37 years) participants were recruited from the ap- proximately 1000 Hadza hunter–gatherers living in remote savannah– woodland habitats of Northern Tanzania. The Hadza have a sexual division of labor in which women collect fruits and nuts and dig for tubers, while men collect honey and hunt animals (Marlowe, 2004). The Hadza are among an increasingly small number of human populations still subsisting by hunting and gathering and are thought to possess a social organization similar to that exhibited by early anatomically modern humans. For a de- scription of the Hadza and hunter–gatherers in general, see Apicella and Crittenden (2016). Adult participants were recruited from eight Hadza camps visited opportunistically in the summer of 2006. Only individuals between
  • 23. the ages of 18 and 40 were used in the present study, and one male and one female were removed from the dataset because they possessed facial FA more than 3.2 SD from the mean FA for their sex. The research- er located the first Hadza camp by driving in the area of Hadzaland, with subsequent camps selected in a snowball sampling fashion based on in- formation provided by participants about the proximity of the next clos- est camp. Verbal consent was obtained for all participants, and the study was approved by Harvard University's ethics internal review board. 3.1.2. Face measurement Adult male and female participants had their faces photographed out- side during the day with a Sony Cyber-shot 18 megapixel digital camera. Participants were asked to maintain a neutral expression and look directly into the camera. Horizontal asymmetry was estimated from x,y- coordinates of 6 bilateral points made by a single measurer following tech- niques used in previous studies (Grammer & Thornhill, 1994; Penton-Voak et al., 2001; Scheib et al., 1999). Symmetry was computed by taking left and right deviation from the midline, calculated from interpupillary distance, and then summing the absolute value of the individual scores (Table S1,
  • 24. available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org). DA was calculat- ed in the manner used in Study 1 and subtracted from the FA measures. For women, the resulting values were natural log-transformed to correct skew. 3.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings Voice recordings were made privately inside a Land Rover vehicle. Participants were asked to speak the Swahili word “hujambo,” which loosely translates to “hello,” into a Sennheiser MKH-60 microphone. Re- cordings were encoded in mono directly onto a computer hard disk using Sonic Foundry's Sound Forge at 44,100 Hz sampling rate and 16- bit quantization, and saved as uncompressed WAV files. The recordings were used as stimuli for vocal attractiveness ratings, which, as in Study 1, were made on a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all attractive, 7 = very attractive) by unfamiliar members of the opposite sex (n = 29, mean age = 19.38 years for male raters; n = 26, mean age = 20.38 years for female raters) recruited from The Pennsylvania State University (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www. ehbonline.org). Raters listened to all opposite-sex recordings through Sennheiser HD 280 Professional headphones, and the order in which stimuli were presented was randomized for each rater.
  • 25. 3.2. Results In contrast with the results of Study 1, we found no significant rela- tionship between vocal attractiveness and facial FA in either men (n = 28, r = −.16, p = .431; Table 1) or women (n = 39, r = −.08, p = .626; Table 3), though the sample size of Hadza participants was much small- er, and in both sexes the non-significant correlation coefficients were in the same direction as in Study 1. 4. Study 3 4.1. Methods 4.1.1. Participants Seventy-nine self-identified White men (mean age = 19.92 ± 1.51) from The Pennsylvania State University participated as part of two Table 1 Meta-analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in men. Study name r Lower limit Upper limit z p n Study 1 −0.21 −0.38 −0.04 −2.39 0.017 124 Study 2 −0.16 −0.50 0.23 −0.78 0.435 28 Study 3 (2D facial FA) −0.10 −0.32 0.12 −0.89 0.372 79 Study 3 (3D facial FA) −0.17 −0.42 0.11 −1.20 0.229 52 Hughes et al. (2002) −0.36 −0.58 −0.10 −2.62 0.009 50
  • 26. Overall1 −0.21 −0.32 −0.09 −3.41 0.001 281 Overall2 −0.23 −0.35 −0.11 −3.64 b0.001 254 1 Using 2D facial FA from Study 3. 2 Using 3D facial FA from Study 3. 4 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 broader studies, one on long-term romantic relationships (supplemen- tary online material 2, sample A), the other on interpersonal dynamics (supplementary online material 2, sample B). Data-collection proce- dures were approved by the University's institutional review board. 4.1.2. Face measurement Consistent with methods used in Studies 1 and 2, two- dimensional facial photographs were taken using an Olympus Evolt E-300
  • 27. digital camera. Participants sat in front of a light-colored background; removed any facial jewelry, eyeglasses, or hats; used a headband to remove any hair obstructing the face; and looked directly into the camera lens while maintaining a neutral facial expression. The resultant photo- graphs were used to obtain facial FA per the methods of Study 1 (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline.org). Also as in Study 1, inter-measurer reliability was assessed by calculating REM values (Weinberg et al., 2004) for each facial distance. The mean for the x-coordinates was .87 (SD = 1.05), and the mean for the y- coordinates was .79 (SD = .67), indicating strong agreement. In addition to standard two-dimensional photographs, three- dimensional facial images were collected for a subset of participants (n = 52, mean age = 19.87 ± 1.76) using the 3dMD face system (3dMD, Atlanta, GA). Participants were asked to close their mouths and maintain a neutral facial expression while providing their images, which were then exported from the 3dMD Patient software in OBJ file format and imported into a scan-cleaning program for cropping and trimming, as well as removal of hair, ears, and any dissociated polygons.
  • 28. We subsequently partitioned facial shape into patterns of symmetry and asymmetry following the work of Claes, Walters and Clement (2012). First, an anthropometric mask was non-rigidly mapped onto the original 3D facial scans, as well as onto the reflected scan representing the mirror image of the original (Fig. 3), which was con- structed by changing the sign of the x-coordinate (Claes, 2007; Claes, Walters, & Clement, 2012; Claes, Walters, Vandermeulen, & Clement, 2011; Klingenberg & McIntyre, 1998; Mardia, Bookstein, & Moreton, 2000). This established homologous spatially dense quasi- landmark configurations for all original and reflected 3D images (Claes et al., 2011). Secondly, following Mardia et al. (2000), a generalized Procrus- tes superimposition was performed (Rohlf & Slice, 1990), eliminating differences in position, orientation, and scale between the original and reflected configurations. Finally, the overall consensus configuration was perfectly symmetrical, and a single shape could be decomposed into its asymmetrical and bilaterally symmetrical parts (Mardia et al., 2000). The average of an original image and its reflected configuration constitutes the symmetrical component, whereas the difference be- tween both configurations constitutes the asymmetrical
  • 29. component (Kimmerle & Jantz, 2005; Klingenberg, Barluenga, & Meyer, 2002; Fig. 3). Because the consensus configuration is, by construction, sym- metrical with respect to a plane, it is used to analyze variation of Fig. 2. Relationship between vocal attractiveness and facial FA among (a) men (n = 124, r = −.21, p = .017) and (b) women (n = 201, r = −.24, p = .001) from Study 1. Fig. 3. Overall asymmetry is calculated as the Procrustes distance between original and reflected images of each individual. The heatmap (third image from left) shows the regional variation in asymmetry with red color highlighting regions with higher levels of asymmetry and blue color highlighting regions with lower levels of asymmetry. The reader is encouraged to consult Claes, Walters, Shriver, et al. (2012) for a more detailed description. 5A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
  • 30. symmetry in populations and provides an estimate of the midsagittal plane with a frame of reference that has clear anatomical meaning (Klingenberg et al., 2002). As a result, Procrustes distances in the tan- gent shape space between mapped 3D images and their reflections after superimposition were used as measures of facial asymmetry. Be- cause the asymmetry shape-space is centered around the average asymmetry in the sample (i.e., the average asymmetry is subtracted from the asymmetry measurements), the DA component was eliminat- ed in computing facial asymmetry. 4.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings Voice recordings were made from the first six sentences of the Rain- bow Passage (Fairbanks, 1960), collected according to the protocol used in Study 1. Vocal attractiveness was assessed by 167 female students (mean age = 18.80 ± .95 years) recruited from The Pennsylvania State University (Table S1, available on the journal's website at www. ehbonline.org), who listened to only the first sentence of the Rainbow Passage (to avoid fatigue) on Sennheiser HD 280 Professional head- phones. Voice stimuli were split randomly into two sets so that each lis- tener rated half of the stimuli, but all stimuli were evaluated an equal
  • 31. number of times. Presentation order of stimuli was randomized for each listener. Recordings from sample A were rated on attractiveness using a 7-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = very unattractive, 7 = very at- tractive). Recordings from sample B were rated on how attractive the men are for both a “short-term, purely sexual relationship, such as a one-night stand” and a “long-term, committed relationship, such as marriage,” using a 10-point Likert scale (anchors: 1 = not at all, 10 = more than anyone else I know), and these scores were averaged to pro- duce a measure of overall vocal attractiveness. Ratings were standard- ized within each sample to place them on the same scale. 4.2. Results As in Study 2, the correlations between vocal attractiveness and FA were in the predicted direction but were not statistically significant (2D: n = 79, r = −.10, p = .373; 3D Procrustes: n = 52, r = −.17, p = .227; Table 1). Interestingly, measures of FA made from 2D and 3D images were not significantly correlated (n = 52, r = −.08, p = .581), though this may be attributable to the appreciably larger amount of spatial information captured in 3D meshes, which, in theory, repre- sent a methodological improvement over 2D photographs (see
  • 32. also Dis- cussion). As a result, 3D images have been able to confirm the oft- replicated negative relationship between FA and attractiveness in both sexes obtained using 2D imagery (Brown et al., 2008). Despite this, however, the strength of the relationship between vocal attractiveness and FA in Study 3 did not differ between 2D and 3D measurements (n = 52, z = .19, p = .425). 5. Meta-analysis In addition to examining independently the three studies reported here, we meta-analyzed the data across the studies, as well as the re- sults of Hughes et al. (2002) and Abend et al. (2015), to produce sum- mary estimates of the relationship between FA and vocal attractiveness. Effect sizes were obtained for males from all studies re- ported herein plus Hughes et al. (2002), and for females from Studies 1 and 2 as well as Hughes et al. (2002) and Abend et al. (2015). This re- sulted in total samples of 281 men (utilizing 2D facial FA measurements from Study 3) or 254 men (utilizing 3D facial FA measurements from Study 3), as well as 328 women. Because of methodological differences in sampling, variable mea-
  • 33. surement (see Palmer & Strobeck, 1997 for a discussion of the effect of measurement error on FA), and data treatment across studies, a random effects model was used in the analyses, which allows for the presence of moderator variables (e.g., differences in the precision with which FA in different parts of the phenotype indexes developmental instability). Correlation coefficients between FA and vocal attractiveness served as the effect size. Variance estimates based on a small number of studies are at best speculative, and the accuracy of sampling variance estimates, though unbiased, improves with increasing numbers of samples. Both within-sample Q estimates and I2 estimates (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003) suggest that all variances across studies are best attributed to random sampling error. Better estimates of variance across studies must await the accumulation of additional research. The data were analyzed in Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software version 3 (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005), and males and fe- males were treated separately. This software is in the Hedges and Olkin (1985) tradition and performs calculations using the Fisher r-to- z transformation and then reports the results in the
  • 34. untransformed cor- relation coefficient metric. Moreover, in order to determine whether the removal of a study would affect the meta-analytic results, we conducted for each sex an “omit-one-study” analysis. The point estimate for the population correlation between FA and vocal attractiveness in men was r = −.23 and −.21 using 3D and 2D fa- cial FA from Study 3, respectively (Table 1, Fig. 4a). Excluding individual samples in the omit-one-study analysis resulted in effect sizes ranging from r = −.25 to r = −.20 using 3D facial FA from Study 3 (Table 2, Fig. 4c), and r = −.24 to r = −.17 using 2D facial FA from Study 3 (Table S2, Fig. S1a, available on the journal's website at www. ehbonline.org). In all analyses, the 95% CI excluded a correlation of zero. For women, the point estimate for the population correlation be- tween FA and vocal attractiveness was r = −.28 and −.29 using limb FA and 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015), respectively (Table 3, Fig. 4b). Excluding individual samples in the omit-one-study analysis resulted in effect sizes ranging from r = −.32 to r = −.25 (Tables 4 and S3, Fig. S1b, available on the journal's website at www.ehbonline. org). In all analyses, the 95% CI excluded a correlation of zero. We also
  • 35. meta-analyzed only the two studies reported in the present paper that included women and obtained a population correlation of r = −.21 (95% CI: −.33 to −.09, p b .001). 6. Discussion In the present research, we found relationships between vocal attrac- tiveness and fluctuating asymmetry in both sexes. Though analyses should be re-conducted as more data accumulate, our meta- analyses gave point estimates for population correlations between vocal attractive- ness and FA of −.23 for men and −.29 for women, using 3D facial FA from Study 3 and Abend et al. (2015). All effect sizes across samples were in the same (negative) direction, although they ranged in magnitude, with greater deviations from the population estimate unsurprisingly tending to derive from smaller samples. Our estimate indicates that 146 male and 91 female observations would be required to find a statistically signif- icant effect (two-tailed power analyses for α = .05 and β = .20). As such, conclusions based on the meta-analysis that cumulated across studies are likely to be more robust than conclusions drawn from under- powered primary studies. Importantly, the negative association between FA and
  • 36. vocal attractiveness was highly robust and manifest in every meta- analysis of the data. The correlation was statistically significant regardless of the inclusion of any individual study or sample, applied to FA measured from the face as well as from the body, and to both short-term and overall attractiveness, and was present whether we included effect sizes from previously published work or only those from the present research. Our findings thus provide important new evidence supporting the salience of the human voice in conveying information on mate quality. Furthermore, we are able to exclude several potential alternative inter- pretations. First, it is possible that behavioral tendencies may spuriously link vocal attractiveness with measurements of facial symmetry. For ex- ample, shyness might result in both a wavering, less attractive voice, as well as a tendency not to face the camera directly, which would affect symmetry measurements. However, in taking 2D images, we were care- ful to ensure that participants faced the camera fully, subsequently 6 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues
  • 37. to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://www.ehbonline.org/#_blank http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 verifying this through visual inspection of the photographs and removal of those in which head turn was apparent. More importantly, the rela- tionship between vocal attractiveness and FA was in the predicted di- rection, albeit non-significantly in Study 3, when FA was measured from 3D imagery, which is not influenced by the spatial orientation of the head. A pertinent question is how developmental stability can be indexed by FA measured from both 2D and 3D images if the two types of measurements were not correlated in Study 3. It is possible that, while developmental instability influences FA in both two and three di- mensions, it does so differently such that the relationship between the two is not necessarily positive. For instance, increased
  • 38. asymmetry in the coronal plane, which is measured in 2D images, might be related to decreased asymmetry in the sagittal plane, which is measured in 3D but not 2D images. That FA from both types of image reliability indexes developmental stability accords with results of Tigue, Pisanski, O'Connor, Fraccaro, and Feinberg (2012), who found that, while 3D im- ages elicited more favorable assessments of attractiveness than did 2D images, attractiveness ratings from the two types of stimuli were none- theless correlated. This notwithstanding, we encourage future re- searchers to employ 3D imagery to capture appreciably more spatial information and to characterize more precisely subtle shape variation present in facial morphology. Second, it is possible that developmental instability only indirectly influences vocal attractiveness by increasing confidence associated with having a symmetrical, attractive face. However, this does not ap- pear to be the case. In Study 1, we were able to statistically control for facial attractiveness and found that the relationships between facial FA and vocal attractiveness remained undiminished in both sexes after
  • 39. doing so. Moreover, these findings indicate that voices and faces provide partly unique information about mate quality, in that voices provided information about developmental instability that was not encompassed by facial attractiveness. Vocal and facial attractiveness correlated mod- erately in Study 1 women, suggesting that women's voices and faces provide partly redundant information about mate quality. Previous re- search has shown similar levels of agreement across these domains in women (e.g., Abend et al., 2015; Collins & Missing, 2003; Feinberg et al., 2005; Lander, 2008; Saxton et al., 2009; Wheatley et al., 2014), and auditory and visual cues considered in isolation may be less Fig. 4. Correlations and 95% confidence intervals for relationships between vocal attractiveness and facial FA. Primary meta-analytic results are shown in panels (a) for men and (b) for women. Overall effects refer to results of random effect models. In panel (a), superscripts 1 and 2 denote results using 2D and 3D facial FA, respectively, from Study 3. In panel (b), superscripts 1 and 2 denote results using limb FA and 3D facial FA, respectively, from Abend et al. (2015). Results of “omit- one-study” analyses are illustrated for (c) men and (d) women, with FA in Study 3 and Abend et al. (2015) computed from 3D facial images. Table 2 “Omit-one-study” analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in men using 3D facial FA from
  • 40. Study 3. Study omitted r Lower limit Upper limit z p n Study 1 −0.25 −0.40 −0.07 −2.75 0.006 130 Study 2 −0.24 −0.36 −0.11 −3.57 b0.001 226 Study 3 (3D facial FA) −0.24 −0.37 −0.11 −3.47 0.001 202 Hughes et al. (2002) −0.20 −0.33 −0.06 −2.77 0.006 204 Table 3 Meta-analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in women. Study name r Lower limit Upper limit z p n Study 1 −0.24 −0.36 −0.10 −3.40 0.001 201 Study 2 −0.08 −0.39 0.24 −0.48 0.630 39 Abend et al. (2015; limb FA) −0.44 −0.66 −0.16 −2.98 0.003 42 Abend et al. (2015; 3D facial FA) −0.45 −0.66 −0.16 −2.99 0.003 42 Hughes et al. (2002) −0.41 −0.62 −0.13 −2.84 0.005 46 Overall1 −0.28 −0.41 −0.14 −3.87 b0.001 328 Overall2 −0.29 −0.42 −0.14 −3.86 b0.001 328 1 Using limb FA from Abend et al. (2015). 2 Using 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015). 7A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008
  • 41. informative about mate quality than are stimuli combining these and other traits (Campanella & Belin, 2007). In Study 1 men, vocal and facial attractiveness did not correlate significantly. Although ours is the larg- est study of which we are aware to test this correlation in men, our re- sults corroborate most (e.g., Lander, 2008; Saxton et al., 2009; Wells, Baguley, Sergeant, & Dunn, 2013), but not all (Saxton, Caryl, & Roberts, 2006; Skrinda et al., 2014), previous studies exploring this correlation. Further research is required to determine the extent to which men's fa- cial and vocal attractiveness correlate, which components of mate qual- ity may underlie any such correlation, and how these relationships may vary across samples. Finally, our results provide further evidence that human preferences for symmetrical mates are not merely consequences of symmetrical stimuli being more easily processed by the visual system (Enquist & Johnstone, 1997). It is now becoming clear that people prefer not only the faces (Jones, DeBruine, & Little, 2007; Little & Jones, 2003; Scheib et al., 1999) and odors (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Thornhill &
  • 42. Gangestad, 1999b) but also the voices (Abend et al., 2015; Hughes et al., 2002, 2008; present study) of mates with low levels of FA, even when symmetry cannot be directly assessed visually. The present paper thus adds to an efflorescing literature exploring relationships between FA and attractiveness in humans. While some contributions to this literature have questioned relationships between FA and fitness-relevant outcomes (e.g., Clarke, 1998), a recent meta- analysis of 293 estimates of the relationship between FA and measures of health and quality taken from 94 studies on humans suggests a robust correlation (Van Dongen & Gangestad, 2011). Much previous research on FA and mate preferences has focused on putative genetic benefits provided by symmetrical partners. Heritability estimates for FA vary across species and across traits within species (Leamy, 1997; Møller & Thornhill, 1997; Polak et al., 2003), but it is pos- sible that individuals derive non-genetic benefits from symmetrical mates, as well (Gangestad & Thornhill, 2013). For example, the meta- analysis of Van Dongen and Gangestad (2011) estimated correlations of 0.11 and 0.20 between FA and reduced intelligence and schizophre-
  • 43. nia/schizotypy, respectively, both of which may have influenced invest- ment capacity ancestrally. However, other research suggests that symmetrical males may allocate relatively greater reproductive effort to mating than to investment in mates or offspring (e.g., Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997, 2013), suggesting that ge- netic benefits to offspring may have driven the evolution of preferences for low-FA partners, at least in women. 7. Future directions Future studies should investigate how developmental instability re- lates to the anatomy of the vocal tract, and which vocal anatomical char- acteristics and acoustic features of the voice mediate relationships between somatic asymmetry and vocal attractiveness (see, for example, Hughes et al., 2008). While such an exploration is beyond the scope of the present paper, some informed speculation is possible. One possibil- ity is that developmental instability affects vocal attractiveness by influencing the symmetry of the vocal apparatus, for example the vocal folds or the shape of the supralaryngeal vocal tract. Anatomical asymmetries or asymmetric tension between the two vocal folds will
  • 44. cause the vocal folds to vibrate differently or at different rates, produc- ing perturbations in periodicity as well as incomplete closure and leading to perceptible vocal changes, such as hoarseness, breathiness, and roughness (Eysholdt et al., 2003). Facial asymmetries, such as those caused by cleft lip and palate, can affect aspects of articulation and resonance, as well. Because vocalization is integral to human com- munication, there may have been strong selection for the vocal appara- tus to be buffered against such perturbations, but research suggests pronounced asymmetries in the human vocal folds (e.g., Hirano, Yukizane, Kurita, & Hibi, 1989). This is consistent with the idea that traits shaped by intersexual selection tend to display greater FA than do non-sexually selected traits (Møller & Hoglund, 1991). Developmen- tal instability may influence other aspects of the size and shape of vocal structures (e.g., the length, thickness, or density of the vocal folds), as well as neurological characteristics that could affect vocal behavior, such as tension on the vocal folds or the positioning of the larynx in the vocal tract, which would influence vocal pitch and timbre, respec- tively. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of vocal anatomy during
  • 45. phonation, precise acoustic measurements, and large samples may be particularly useful in exploring these possibilities. Funding This work was supported by the National Institutes of Mental Health [grant number T32 MH70343-05], the National Science Foundation [REG grant to C.L.A. and GRFP grant to J.R.W.], and The Pennsylvania State University and Harvard University. Supplementary materials Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http://dx. doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008. Acknowledgments We thank Arslan Zaidi for assistance designing Fig. 3, Tobias Kordsmeyer for insights on measuring facial FA, Matt Winn for input on the Discussion, and Lisa DeBruine and two anonymous reviewers for comments on previous drafts of this manuscript. References Abend, P., Pflüger, L. S., Koppensteiner, M., Coquerelle, M., & Grammer, K. (2015). The sound of female shape: a redundant signal of vocal and facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 174–181.
  • 46. Andersson, M. B. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Apicella, C. L. (2011). On the universality of attractiveness. In M. Brockman (Ed.), Future science: essays from the cutting edge (pp. 88–100). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Apicella, C. L., & Crittenden, A. (2016). hunter–gatherer families and parenting. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.)Vol. 1. (pp. 578–597). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Apicella, C. L., & Feinberg, D. R. (2009). Voice pitch alters mate-choice-relevant perception in hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 276(1659), 1077–1082. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1542. Auerbach, B. M., & Ruff, C. B. (2006). Limb bone bilateral asymmetry: Variability and com- monality among modern humans. Journal of Human Evolution, 50(2), 203–218. Banks, G. C., Batchelor, J. H., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Smarter people are (a bit) more symmetrical: A meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and fluctuat- ing asymmetry. Intelligence, 38(4), 393–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010. 04.003. Barden, H. S. (1980). Fluctuating dental asymmetry: A measure
  • 47. of developmental insta- bility in Down syndrome. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 52(2), 169–173. Table 4 “Omit-one-study” analysis of FA and vocal attractiveness in women using 3D facial FA from Abend et al. (2015). Study omitted r Lower limit Upper limit z p n Study 1 −0.32 −0.52 −0.10 −2.74 0.006 127 Study 2 −0.32 −0.45 −0.17 −4.22 b0.001 289 Abend et al. (2015; 3D facial FA) −0.25 −0.38 −0.11 −3.38 0.001 286 Hughes et al. (2002) −0.26 −0.41 −0.09 −2.94 0.003 282 8 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://dx.doi.org/ http://dx.doi.org/ http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0005 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0010 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0015 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020
  • 48. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1542 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0030 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0030 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.003 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0040 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0040 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat, 104. Brown, W. M., Price, M. E., Kang, J., Pound, N., Zhao, Y., & Yu, H. (2008). Fluctuating asym- metry and preferences for sex-typical bodily characteristics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(35), 12938–12943. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710420105. Bryant, G. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2009). Vocal cues of ovulation in human females. Biology Letters, 5(1), 12–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0507. Burriss, R. P., Roberts, S. C., Welling, L. L., Puts, D. A., & Little, A. C. (2011). Heterosexual ro- mantic couples mate assortatively for facial symmetry, but not masculinity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(5), 601–613. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0146167211399584.
  • 49. Campanella, S., & Belin, P. (2007). Integrating face and voice in person perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 535–543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.001. Claes, P. (2007). A robust statistical surface registration framework using implicit function representations—Application in craniofacial reconstruction. Claes, P., Walters, M., & Clement, J. (2012b). Improved facial outcome assessment using a 3D anthropometric mask. International Journal of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery, 41(3), 324–330. Claes, P., Walters, M., Shriver, M. D., Puts, D., Gibson, G., Clement, J., ... Suetens, P. (2012a). Sex- ual dimorphism in multiple aspects of 3D facial symmetry and asymmetry defined by spatially dense geometric morphometrics. Journal of Anatomy, 221(2), 97–114. Claes, P., Walters, M., Vandermeulen, D., & Clement, J. G. (2011). Spatially-dense 3D facial asymmetry assessment in both typical and disordered growth. Journal of Anatomy, 219(4), 444–455. Clarke, G. M. (1998). Developmental stability and fitness: The evidence is not quite so clear. The American Naturalist, 152(5), 762–766. Collins, S. A., & Missing, C. (2003). Vocal and visual attractiveness are related in women. Animal Behaviour, 65(5), 997–1004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2123.
  • 50. Enquist, M., & Johnstone, R. A. (1997). Generalization and the evolution of symmetry pref- erences. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1386), 1345–1348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0186. Eysholdt, U., Rosanowski, F., & Hoppe, U. (2003). Vocal fold vibration irregularities caused by different types of laryngeal asymmetry. European Archives of Oto-Rhino- Laryngology, 260(8), 412–417. Fairbanks, G. (1960). Voice and articulation handbook. New York: Harper. Farkas, L. G., Katic, M. J., & Forrest, C. R. (2005). International anthropometric study of fa- cial morphology in various ethnic groups/races. Journal of Craniofacial Surgery, 16(4), 615–646. Feinberg, D. R. (2008). Are human faces and voices ornaments signaling common under- lying cues to mate value? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 17(2), 112–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/evan.20166. Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Moore, F. R., Law Smith, M. J., Cornwell, R. E., ... Perrett, D. I. (2005). The voice and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 398–408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. evolhumbehav.2005.04.001.
  • 51. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(04), 573–587. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). The evolutionary psychology of extrapair sex: The role of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(2), 69–88. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women's prefer- ences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–933. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2013). Human sexual selection and developmental sta- bility. In J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 169–195). Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A. (1994). Facial attractiveness, developmental stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(2), 73–85. Graham, J. H., Emlen, J. M., Freeman, D. C., Leamy, L. J., & Kieser, J. A. (1998). Directional asymmetry and the measurement of developmental instability. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 64(1), 1–16. Grammer, K., & Thornhill, R. (1994). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sex- ual selection: The role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of
  • 52. Comparative Psychology, 108(3), 233. Hedges, L., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical models for meta- analysis. New York: Academic Press. Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2001). The power of statistical tests in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 6, 203–217. Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557. Hirano, M., Yukizane, K., Kurita, S., & Hibi, S. (1989). Asymmetry of the laryngeal frame- work: A morphologic study of cadaver larynges. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology, 98(2), 135–140. Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G. (2002). The sound of symmetry: Voice as a marker of developmental instability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(3), 173–180. Hughes, S. M., Pastizzo, M. J., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). The sound of symmetry revisited: Subjective and objective analyses of voice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32(2), 93–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0042-6. Hunt, J., Bussiere, L. F., Jennions, M. D., & Brooks, R. (2004). What is genetic quality? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 19(6), 329–333.
  • 53. Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Little, A. C. (2007). The role of symmetry in attraction to av- erage faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(8), 1273–1277. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Tiddeman, B., Burt, D., & Perrett, D. (2001). Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health: Support for a “good genes” explana- tion of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(6), 417–429. Kempenaers, B. (2007). Mate choice and genetic quality: A review of the heterozygosity theory. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 189–278. Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990). Evolution, traits, and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental investment model. Journal of Personality, 58, 97–116. Kimmerle, E. H., & Jantz, R. L. (2005). Secular trends in craniofacial asymmetry studied by geometric morphometry and generalized Procrustes methods. Modern morphometrics in physical anthropology (pp. 247–263). Springer. Klingenberg, C. P., & McIntyre, G. S. (1998). Geometric morphometrics of developmental instability: Analyzing patterns of fluctuating asymmetry with Procrustes methods. Evolution, 1363–1375. Klingenberg, C. P., Barluenga, M., & Meyer, A. (2002). Shape analysis of symmetric struc-
  • 54. tures: Quantifying variation among individuals and asymmetry. Evolution, 56(10), 1909–1920. Komori, M., Kawamura, S., & Ishihara, S. (2009). Averageness or symmetry: Which is more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychologica, 131(2), 136–142. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008. Lander, K. (2008). Relating visual and vocal attractiveness for moving and static faces. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 817–822. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.001. Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces? Psychological Science, 5(4), 214–220. Leamy, L. (1997). Is developmental stability heritable? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 10(1), 21–29. Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2003). Evidence against perceptual bias views for symmetry preferences in human faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270(1526), 1759–1763. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2445. Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Variation in facial masculinity and symmetry preferences across the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(7), 999–1008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007.
  • 55. Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Self-perceived attractive- ness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 268(1462), 39–44. http://dx.doi. org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1327. Little, A. C., Cohen, D. L., Jones, B. C., & Belsky, J. (2007a). Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61(6), 967–973. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007b). Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 76(3), 209–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.003. Livshits, G., & Kobyliansky, E. (1991). Fluctuating asymmetry as a possible measure of de- velopmental homeostasis in humans: A review. Human Biology, 441–466. Manning, J. T. (1995). Fluctuating asymmetry and body weight in men and women: Im- plications for sexual selection. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(2), 145–153. Manning, J., Koukourakis, K., & Brodie, D. (1997b). Fluctuating asymmetry, metabolic rate and sexual selection in human males. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(1), 15–21.
  • 56. Manning, J., Scutt, D., Whitehouse, G., & Leinster, S. (1997a). Breast asymmetry and phe- notypic quality in women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(4), 223–236. Mardia, K. V., Bookstein, F. L., & Moreton, I. J. (2000). Statistical assessment of bilateral symmetry of shapes. Biometrika, 285–300. Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter– gatherers. Human Nature, 15(4), 365–376. Martin, S., Manning, J., & Dowrick, C. (1999). Fluctuating asymmetry, relative digit length, and depression in men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(3), 203–214. Martin, J. T., Puts, D. A., & Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Hand asymmetry in heterosexual and homosexual men and women: Relationship to 2D:4D digit ratios and other sexually dimorphic anatomical traits. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(1), 119–132. http://dx. doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9279-8. Møller, A. P. (1992). Female swallow preference for symmetrical male. Nature, 357. Møller, A. P. (1994). Directional selection on directional asymmetry: Testes size and sec- ondary sexual characters in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biolog- ical Sciences, 258(1352), 147–151. Møller, A. P., & Hoglund, J. (1991). Patterns of fluctuating
  • 57. asymmetry in avian feather or- naments: Implications for models of sexual selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 245(1312), 1–5. Møller, A., & Thornhill, R. (1997). A meta-analysis of the heritability of developmental sta- bility. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 10(1), 1–16. Mooney, M. P., Siegel, M. I., & Gest, T. R. (1985). Prenatal stress and increased fluctuating asymmetry in the parietal bones of neonatal rats. American Journal of Physical Anthro- pology, 68(1), 131–134. Oakes, E. J., & Barnard, P. (1994). Fluctuating asymmetry and mate choice in paradise whydahs, Vidua paradisaea: An experimental manipulation. Animal Behaviour, 48(4), 937–943. Palmer, A. R., & Strobeck, C. (1986). Fluctuating asymmetry: Measurement, analysis, pat- terns. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 391–421. Palmer, A., & Strobeck, C. (1997). Fluctuating asymmetry and developmental stability: Heritability of observable variation vs. heritability of inferred cause. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 10(1), 39–49. Parsons, P. (1990). Fluctuating asymmetry: An epigenetic measure of stress. Biological Reviews, 65(2), 131–145. Penton-Voak, I. S., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Baker, S.,
  • 58. Tiddeman, B., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attrac- tiveness. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 268(1476), 1617–1623. http://dx.doi. org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1703. Peretz, B., Ever-Hadani, P., Casamassimo, P., Eidelman, E., Shellhart, C., & Hagerman, R. (1988). Crown size asymmetry in males with fra (X) or Martin– Bell syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 30(1–2), 185–190. 9A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0045 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0045 http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710420105 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0507 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399584 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399584 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.001 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0070 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0070 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0075 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080
  • 59. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0080 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0085 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0090 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0090 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2123 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0186 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0105 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0110 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0115 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/evan.20166 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.001 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.001 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0130 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0130 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0135 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0135 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0140 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0145 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0150 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0150 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0155 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160
  • 60. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0160 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0165 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0165 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0170 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0170 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0175 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0175 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0180 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0185 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0185 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0042-6 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0195 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0195 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0200 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0200 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0205 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0210 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0210 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0215 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0220 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0225 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0230 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.03.008
  • 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.001 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0245 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0245 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0250 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0250 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2445 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1327 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0270 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.003 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0280 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0280 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0285 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0285 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0290 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0290 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0295 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0295 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0300 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0300 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0305 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0305 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0310 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0310 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9279-8 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0320 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0325 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0330 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0335 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0335
  • 62. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0340 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0345 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0350 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0350 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0355 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0360 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0360 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1703 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0370 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0370 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 Perrett, D. I., Burt, D. M., Penton-Voak, I. S., Lee, K. J., Rowland, D. A., & Edwards, R. (1999). Sym- metry and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(5), 295–307. Peters, M., Simmons, L. W., & Rhodes, G. (2009). Preferences across the menstrual cycle for masculinity and symmetry in photographs of male faces and bodies. PloS One, 4(1), e4138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004138. Petrie, M. (1994). Improved growth and survival of offspring of peacocks with more elab- orate trains. Nature, 371(6498), 598–599. Pipitone, N. R., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2008). Women's voice attractiveness varies across the
  • 63. menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268–274. Pipitone, N. R., & Gallup, G. (2011). The unique impact of menstruation on the female voice: Implications for the evolution of menstrual cycle cues. Ethology, 118, 281–291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02010.x. Pisanski, K., & Feinberg, D. R. (2013). Cross-cultural variation in mate preferences for average- ness, symmetry, body size, and masculinity. Cross-Cultural Research, 47(2), 162–197. Polak, M., Møller, A., Gangestad, S. W., Kroeger, D. E., Manning, J. T., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Does an individual asymmetry parameter exist? A meta-analysis. In M. Polak (Ed.), Develop- mental instability: causes and consequences (pp. 81–96). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., Cardenas, R. A., Burriss, R. P., Welling, L. L., Wheatley, J. R., & Dawood, K. (2013). Women's attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 63(1), 13– 19. http://dx.doi.org/10. 1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007. Puts, D. A., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2012). Sexual selection on human faces and voices. Journal of Sex Research, 49(2–3), 227–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 00224499.2012.658924. Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does
  • 64. attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 186–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014. Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Lee, K., McKay, R., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Attractive- ness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-Western cultures: In search of bio- logically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30(5), 611–625. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1068/p3123. Rohlf, F. J., & Slice, D. (1990). Extensions of the Procrustes method for the optimal super- imposition of landmarks. Systematic Biology, 39(1), 40–59. Saxton, T., Burriss, R., Murray, A., Rowland, H., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). Face, body and speech cues independently predict judgments of attractiveness. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 23–35. Saxton, T. K., Caryl, P. G., & Roberts, S. C. (2006). Vocal and facial attractiveness judgments of children, adolescents and adults: The ontogeny of mate choice. Ethology, 112(12), 1179–1185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439- 0310.2006.01278.x. Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 266(1431), 1913–1917.
  • 65. Sciulli, P., Doyle, W., Kelley, C., Siegel, P., & Siegel, M. (1979). The interaction of stressors in the induction of increased levels of fluctuating asymmetry in the laboratory rat. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 50(2), 279–284. Simmons, L. (1995). Correlates of male quality in the field cricket, Gryllus campestris L.: Age, size, and symmetry determine pairing success in field populations. Behavioral Ecology, 6(4), 376–381. Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kecko, S., Moore, F. R., Kaasik, A., Meija, L., ... Krams, I. (2014). Body height, immunity, facial and vocal attractiveness in young men. Naturwissenschaften, 101(12), 1017–1025. Springer, I. N., Wannicke, B., Warnke, P. H., Zernial, O., Wiltfang, J., Russo, P. A., ... Wolfart, S. (2007). Facial attractiveness: Visual impact of symmetry increases significantly to- wards the midline. Annals of Plastic Surgery, 59(2), 156–162. Swaddle, J. P. (1996). Reproductive success and symmetry in zebra finches. Animal Behaviour, 51(1), 203–210. Swaddle, J. P., & Cuthill, I. C. (1995). Asymmetry and human facial attractiveness: Symme- try may not always be beautiful. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 261(1360), 111–116. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999a). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
  • 66. 3(12), 452–460. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999b). The scent of symmetry: A human sex phero- mone that signals fitness? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(3), 175–201. Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., & Comer, R. (1995). Human female orgasm and mate fluc- tuating asymmetry. Animal Behaviour, 50(6), 1601–1615. Tigue, C. C., Pisanski, K., O'Connor, J. J., Fraccaro, P. J., & Feinberg, D. R. (2012). Men's judg- ments of women's facial attractiveness from two- and three- dimensional images are similar. Journal of Vision, 12(12), 3. Tomkins, J. L., & Simmons, L. W. (1998). Female choice and manipulations of forceps size and symmetry in the earwig Forficula auricularia L. Animal Behaviour, 56(2), 347–356. Van Dongen, S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2011). Human fluctuating asymmetry in relation to health and quality: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(6), 380–398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.002. Van Valen, L. (1962). A study of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution, 125–142. Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory, 1, 1–30. Weinberg, S. M., Scott, N. M., Neiswanger, K., Brandon, C. A., & Marazita, M. L. (2004). Dig- ital three-dimensional photogrammetry: Evaluation of
  • 67. anthropometric precision and accuracy using a Genex 3D camera system. The Cleft Palate- Craniofacial Journal, 41(5), 507–518. Wells, T., Baguley, T., Sergeant, M., & Dunn, A. (2013). Perceptions of human attractive- ness comprising face and voice cues. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(5), 805–811. Wheatley, J. R., Apicella, C. A., Burriss, R. P., Cárdenas, R. A., Bailey, D. H., Welling, L. L., & Puts, D. A. (2014). Women's faces and voices are cues to reproductive potential in in- dustrial and forager societies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(4), 264–271. 10 A.K. Hill et al. / Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (2016) xxx–xxx Please cite this article as: Hill, A.K., et al., Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0375 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0375 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004138 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0385 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0385 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0390 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0390 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02010.x http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0400
  • 68. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0400 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0405 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.014 http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3123 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0430 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0430 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0435 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01278.x http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0445 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0450 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0455 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0460 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0465 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0465 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0470 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0470 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0475 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0480
  • 69. http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0480 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0485 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0485 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0490 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0490 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0495 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0500 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0500 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.002 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0510 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0515 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0520 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0525 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0525 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0530 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1090-5138(16)30302-6/rf0530 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.008Are there vocal cues to human developmental stability? Relationships between facial fluctuating asymmetry and voice attract...1. Introduction2. Study 12.1. Methods2.1.1. Participants2.1.2. Face measurement2.1.3. Voice recording2.1.4. Vocal and facial attractiveness ratings2.2. Results3. Study 23.1. Methods3.1.1. Participants3.1.2. Face measurement3.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings3.2. Results4. Study 34.1. Methods4.1.1. Participants4.1.2. Face measurement4.1.3. Vocal attractiveness ratings4.2. Results5. Meta-analysis6. Discussion7. Future directionsFundingSupplementary materialsAcknowledgmentsReferences